07.04.2015 Views

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

Developmental surface dyslexias - Naama Friedmann

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

cortex 44 (2008) 1146–1160 1153<br />

comprehension, which will be presented in the next sections.<br />

The bottom rows of Table 4 include the threshold for each<br />

task above which the number of errors of a dyslexic participant<br />

is significantly larger (Crawford and Howell’s (1998)<br />

t-test, p < .05) than the age-matched control group. (For example,<br />

the adults control group had an average of 1.0% errors<br />

in reading aloud irregular/potentiophonic words, with SD of<br />

0.9%. For these data, given 16 participants in this control<br />

group, the lowest score that would be significantly different<br />

from the control would be 2.7% errors, using Crawford and<br />

Howell’s (1998) t-test, and this is presented in Table 2.)<br />

2.3.2. Lexical decision<br />

The participants differed with respect to their ability to decide<br />

which of two letter strings was a word spelled correctly, as<br />

seen in Table 4. Whereas nine of the participants performed<br />

within the normal range in this task ( p > .05 for the comparison<br />

of each of them with their controls), eight performed significantly<br />

poorer than their controls (t > 6, p < .001).<br />

2.3.3. Homophone and potentiophone comprehension<br />

The assessment of homophone/potentiophone comprehension<br />

showed that some of the participants performed well in<br />

this task, indicating preserved access to semantics, whereas<br />

others did poorly. As seen in Table 4, six of the participants<br />

performed within normal limits in the homophone selection<br />

task, whereas the other participants performed significantly<br />

poorer than their control groups ( p < .05), five of them<br />

performing at chance level on this task. Thus, the results<br />

yielded 3 patterns: impaired reading aloud, lexical decision,<br />

and comprehension; impaired reading aloud and comprehension,<br />

with intact lexical decision; and impaired reading<br />

aloud with intact lexical decision and comprehension.<br />

2.3.4. Naming<br />

The results of the naming task are reported in Table 5. The<br />

naming performance of 16 of the 17 participants was within<br />

the normal range, with performance ranging between 92 and<br />

100 items named correctly out of 100 items, indicating intact<br />

lexical access and retrieval. Especially relevant for <strong>surface</strong><br />

dyslexia, in which some individuals showed impaired phonological<br />

output lexicon, the naming ability of the participants in<br />

this study indicates that they had no impairment at the phonological<br />

output lexicon. None of the participants produced<br />

a phonemic paraphasia, indicating also an intact phonemic<br />

output buffer for all the participants.<br />

One participant, AS, performed below the normal range,<br />

with 88% correct naming, which might indicate a deficit in<br />

lexical retrieval as well. Unlike previously reported individuals<br />

with acquired <strong>surface</strong> dyslexia, the pattern of his errors<br />

and other responses in the naming task did not indicate<br />

a deficit in the phonological output lexicon, which is usually<br />

manifested in phonologically related paraphasias. His errors<br />

were close semantic paraphasias, hesitations, and don’t know<br />

responses, and only one of his paraphasias was a formal<br />

paraphasia, which was not only phonologically, but also semantically,<br />

related to the target word. AS’s response pattern<br />

is not characteristic of a deficit in the phonological lexicon<br />

itself, because a deficit at this level is expected to also yield<br />

phonemic paraphasias. According to his error pattern, AS’s<br />

mild lexical retrieval difficulties stem from a deficit either<br />

in the semantic lexicon, or in the access from the semantic<br />

lexicon to the phonological output lexicon – according to<br />

Butterworth (1989) and Caramazza and Hillis (1990), when<br />

there is no access to the phonological representation of<br />

the target word, a word which is semantically related to it,<br />

whose phonological representation is available, might be<br />

produced instead. The fact that he had frequency effect on<br />

naming (r pb ¼ .2, p ¼ .03) further supports a deficit in the access<br />

to the phonological output lexicon rather than a deficit<br />

in the semantic lexicon (Jescheniak and Levelt, 1994). It<br />

might be interesting to note that AS’s reading indicated<br />

that he also had a deficit in the access from the orthographic<br />

input lexicon to the phonological output lexicon, so his deficit<br />

might be a general difficulty accessing the phonological<br />

output lexicon. (This is unlike the other eight individuals<br />

Table 5 – Performance on naming 100 objects<br />

Participant Correct responses Don’t know Paraphasia Correct naming following<br />

Semantic Formal a Long hesitation Semantic paraphasia<br />

SH 93 1 1 3 2<br />

GL 96 1 1 1 1<br />

OF 95 3 2<br />

YR 92 4 1 3<br />

OM 96 4<br />

BZ 100<br />

AS 88 2 5 1 3 1<br />

OS 96 2 2<br />

AK 95 1 2 2 0<br />

AM 97 3<br />

AL b 96 1 1 1 1<br />

KR 92 5 2 1<br />

NF 99 1<br />

IR 100<br />

YD 96 4<br />

a All the formal paraphasias produced by our participants were phonologically and semantically related to the target.<br />

b AL’s don’t know response and his correct response following hesitation also included a definition of the target.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!