11.07.2015 Views

Rudiments of Numeracy by A.S.C.Ehrenberg - School of Mathematics

Rudiments of Numeracy by A.S.C.Ehrenberg - School of Mathematics

Rudiments of Numeracy by A.S.C.Ehrenberg - School of Mathematics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

1977] EHRENBERG - <strong>Rudiments</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Numeracy</strong> 293Such an ordering is <strong>of</strong>ten criticized as departing from the alphabetical listing <strong>of</strong> thecountries, making it difficult to look up the result for a particular country. But a statisticaltable is not a telephone directory. To use an isolated figure one must understand the context<strong>of</strong> the surrounding ones and see the general pattern <strong>of</strong> the data. If there are many suchtables and they are large, an alphabetical key will be worth giving. In any case it is probablyeasier to find an isolated name in a non-alphabeticalisting than to interpret an isolatednumber from an unstructured table.Spacing and Layout (Rule 5)The basic guidelines for table layout may seem simple: single spacing and occasionaldeliberate gaps to guide the eye; columns spaced evenly and close together, and occasionalhorizontal and vertical rules to mark major divisions. But many typists, printers andcomputers are programmedifferently. Double spacing in tables is common, as are columnsspaced unevenly according to the width <strong>of</strong> the headings, and occasional irregular gaps betweensingle-spaced rows because some row captions ran to two lines. One needs not only goodtypists or printers, but also thoughtful control <strong>of</strong> these facilities.The traditional printers' embargo on vertical rules is widely accepted but can be sidesteppedincreasingly through modern <strong>of</strong>f-set and duplicating methods (as illustrated <strong>by</strong> thetables in this paper). However, ruling <strong>of</strong>f every column routinely, as in Tables 1 and 6, iscounter-productive. In contrast, a few well-chosen rules can have a startling effect, as thereader may see <strong>by</strong> drawing a vertical and a horizontal rule <strong>by</strong> hand to separate the two sets<strong>of</strong> five programmes in Table 14.The niceties <strong>of</strong> good spacing and layout are many and complex. Some specific ones areillustrated <strong>by</strong> Table 15. A good general example (not illustrated here) occurs with tables wherethe data come in pairs <strong>of</strong> related figures, e.g. "observed" and "theoretical" values, or "lastyear" and "this year": The use <strong>of</strong> closely spaced pairs <strong>of</strong> columns is then very effective. Morework is needed to make the possible variety <strong>of</strong> such procedures more explicit.Graphs versus Tables (Rule 6)A claim <strong>of</strong>ten made in supposed contradiction to Rule 6 is that people (especially"otherpeople") find graphs easier to look at than tables, e.g. Fig. 1 rather than Table 13. Theyprobably do, but this is misleading because it does not reflecthat people usually extract andretain little information from a graph.It is no use merely saying "Here are some complex data-let's put them on a graph".What does one learn even from simple graphs (like Figs 1 or 2)? They are supposed to beeasy, but suppose one looks away, what does one remember? Some shapes or qualitativefeatures perhaps, but seldom any quantities. In any case, Fig. 3 reminds us that most graphsdo not show simple patterns or dominant numbers which can readily be grasped. Success ingraphicseems to be judged in produce rather than consumer terms: <strong>by</strong> how much informationone can get on to a graph (or how easily), rather than <strong>by</strong> how much any reader can get <strong>of</strong>fagain (or how easily).5. SOME PROBLEMS OF IMPLEMENTATIONPerhaps the most frequent comment on the rules <strong>of</strong> data presentation discussed here isthat they are "mere common sense", with the insidious implication that real statisticians donot have to bother with them. But at this stage the rules reflect neither common knowledgenor common practice. In that sense they are decidedly uncommon.The rules seem obvious only once they have been stated. No particular skills or knowledgeare then required to assess them-only one's common senses, i.e. whether they feel, look andsound right. The procedures therefore lack the technical mystique <strong>of</strong> a Durbin-Watson testor non-Euclidian space that tends to guarantee a certain instant popularity.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!