13.07.2015 Views

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

4 S. Kazi et al. / Intelligence xxx (2012) xxx–xxx2.2. Aims and predictions of the studyThis study was designed to examine possible organizationaland developmental differences between Greek andChinese children early in age and specify if these differencesmay be related to differences in the <strong>writing</strong> systems used inthe two countries. This would contribute to the resolution ofthe debate noted above about the nature and origin ofindividual differences in intelligence, because it would showhow differences in an important human invention, <strong>writing</strong>,may be transformed into differences in intellectual possibilities.It is also important to trace when in age these differencesappear, if at all. To our knowledge, so far no study has lookedinto these differences in the early preschool years.In sake of these aims, we developed the following tests:(a) Processing efficiency tasks varying in complexity(i.e., speeded stimulus recognition with and without interferencefrom other stimuli and speeded reaction choice). Theywere (i) tasks relevant to the <strong>writing</strong> systems of each culture,i.e., letters and ideograms, and (ii) tasks common to bothcountries, i.e., shapes, numbers and objects. (b) Phonologicaland spatial working memory. (c) Quantitative and spatialproblem solving. (d) Inferential processes, i.e., inductive anddeductive reasoning. (e) Self-awareness about cognitive processes.These tests were addressed to 4- to 7-year-old children.Admittedly, the precise matching of children across thetwo cultures in their previous exposure to reading beyondtotal time in formal education was not possible. It is noted,however, that all children started primary school educationat the same age and they attended preschool education.Moreover, the precise matching of parents in education andincome was not possible due to the vast differences betweenthe two counties in socio-political organization. <strong>How</strong>ever, toensure similarity between participants in the two countriesin as far as this was possible, we opted to examine children ofparents with university education in both countries. With thesereservations present, we hope that the study can uncover anypossible differential influences of reading differences betweenthe two cultures. It is recognized, however, that the restrictionof range in cognitive abilities that is associated with our optionto examine children of educated parents in the two countriesmay limit the generalization of our findings to groups of thepopulation with lower education.Based on the literature reviewed and the design of thestudy, the following predictions could be tested.1. Performance differences. Chinese might outperform Greekson tasks requiring visuo-spatial processing, such as readingrelevantspeeded performance, visuo-spatial workingmemory, and spatial reasoning, especially when theystart learning to read and write, in primary school, that is,at the age of 6 and 7 years. It is an open question if therewill be any differences in other aspects of reasoning at thisearly age, such as deductive reasoning or self-awareness,which were not investigated before. One might predict thatany positive effects of <strong>writing</strong> on processing efficiency andworking memory would need time to generalize to higherlevel processes, such as inference and self-awareness. In thiscase, an interaction between age and culture would have tobe expected. It is of course noted that there may be otherpossible differences in the educational systems of the twocountries which might interfere in subtle and not easilyspecified ways.2. Differences in the overall mental architecture. No importantdifferences in the architecture of the various processeswere expected. Specifically, we expected that all processesexamined (i.e., processing efficiency, working memory,problem solving, inferential processes, and self-awareness),will emerge as distinct factors, and all will be related to ahigher-order general factor, g, inbothcultures(seeFig. 6).This structure was found to be independent of age andculture (Demetriou et al., 2005; Shayer et al., 1988; Sternberg,2004).3. Differences in the strength of relations between processes. Allcomponent processes may be present in both ethnicgroups but the strength of their relations might vary.This prediction was tested in a hierarchical model ofstructural relations where g was decomposed into thedirect relations between processes (see Fig. 7). Specifically,processing efficiency, as a very general index of thequality of information processing (Coyle, Pillow, Snyder, &Kochunov, 2011; Jensen, 1998, 2006; Kail, 1991), would berelated to all other processes; working memory, as anindex of the overall representational constraints of themind, would be related to reasoning and self-awareness(Case, 1985; Pascual-Leone, 1970); finally, self-awareness,as a regulatory function, would also relate to reasoning(Demetriou & Kazi, 2006; Demetriou et al., 2008, 2011,submitted for publication). It is commonly accepted indevelopmental psychology that the optimum ability associatedwith a given age may be differentially realized acrossprocesses and domains, depending upon complexity, familiarity,and social scaffolding. As a result, the covariationbetween processes increases as they approach this optimum,reflecting their increasing alignment to the optimum(Fischer & Bidell, 1998; Flavell, Miller, & Miller, 2001; Shayeret al., 1988). Based on these considerations, we expectedthat the relations between reading-related speeded performance,working memory, and reasoning tasks would behigher in the Chinese than in the Greek group, to reflecttheir better alignment to underlying optimum cognitiveability (Demetriou et al., 2005, 2008, 2011, submitted forpublication).3. Method3.1. ParticipantsA total of 300 children, from four to seven years of age, wereexamined. There were 140 Greek and 160 Chinese children.Genders were about equally represented in each age group inboth ethnic groups (see Table 1). All children lived in urbanareas (Athens, Greece, and Changchun, Jilin Province, China)and came from parents with University education, for thereasons explained in the introduction. This sample is fullydescribed in Table 1. There was no attrition of children selectedfor the study.In both countries school curriculum is centrally controlledby the state and it is common for all children. Based on thiscurriculum, children do not directly learn to read and writeduring preschool but they are engaged in preparatory symbolrecognition and <strong>writing</strong>-relevant activities, aiming to controlPlease cite this article as: Kazi, S., et al., Mind–culture <strong>interactions</strong>: <strong>How</strong> <strong>writing</strong> <strong>molds</strong> mental fluidity in early development,Intelligence (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.001

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!