13.07.2015 Views

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

Mind–culture interactions: How writing molds ... - ResearchGate

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

6 S. Kazi et al. / Intelligence xxx (2012) xxx–xxx(quantitative, categorical and spatial) were administered separately.Thus, in each sub-battery the miniature pictures on thebuttons of SR-Box matched the stimuli projected on the screenfor the particular sub-battery.In all tasks described above, reaction times of wrongchoices and reaction times below a minimum (300 ms for theSimon task and 500 ms for the rest) and above a maximumvalue (5000 ms) were excluded from analysis. This is acommon practice in this type of research aiming to ensurethat the responses analyzed are relevant to the task (Jensen,2006). Moreover, the 4-year-old children were screened forinclusion in the study on the basis of their performance onthe Simon task. Specifically, only children succeeding on atleast 70% of the trials on this task were retained for furthertesting (49% and 45% of the Greek and Chinese 4-year-oldchildren). Screening was necessary for this age because pilotexaminations showed that about 50% of this age grouptended to respond randomly to speeded performance tasks.Therefore, this screening ensured that the youngest childrenincluded in the study were matched to the older children ontheir ability to respond reliably. There was no demographicbias in the origin of the children excluded under this criterion.No older child was dropped based on this criterion.3.2.2. Short-term memory measures3.2.2.1. Spatial memory (corsi). A 16×16 square layout wasshown on screen; a cartoon figure stepped randomly in severalof these squares and children recalled, in reverse order, wherethe cartoon figure stepped in. The task included seven levels(from one to seven cartoon appearances), with three items ineach level.3.2.2.2. Phonological memory. Two tasks were used: (a) wordsand (b) pseudowords. The first involved 34 familiar two-syllablewords in each of the two languages. The second involved 34two-syllable pseudowords, sounding like proper words in eachlanguage. It is noted that the Chinese pseudowords may bemore meaningful than the Greek pseudowords, because theywere constructed by inverting the order of two existingcharacters; these combinations may not always be nonsensicalin Chinese (Tavassoli, 2002) as in the European languages.Children recalled the items in their presentation order. The tasksincluded five levels (from one to five words or pseudowords,respectively), with two items in each level.Administration of all memory tasks stopped when participantsfailed to respond accurately to two items of the samelevel. The score on each of these three tasks equalled the higherlevel of difficulty attained on them.3.2.3. Cognitive measuresCognitive tests addressed the quantitative and the spatialSSS, and analogical and deductive reasoning.3.2.3.1. Quantitative tasks. (a) Counting from 3 to 9 objectsand (b) three arithmetic operations problems (i.e., findingthe sum of 1+2, 2+3, and 7+4) addressed the quantitativeSSS. The arithmetic operations problems were enacted by theexperimenter, who placed as many cubes as required in abox. For example, in the first problem, she first placed 1 cube,then 2 more, and called the child to specify the number ofcubes in the box. After each act the experiment covered theopening of the box to ensure that the objects in it could notbe counted. For counting, the score was the maximumnumber of objects counted correctly. For arithmetic operations,one point was given for each correct answer.3.2.3.2. Spatial task. This task involved three items requiringpicture assembly and mental rotation. Children werepresented with a model figure (i.e., a house), and they wereasked to reproduce it on the side, by properly arranging itscomponent parts (i.e., a square, a triangle, and a semicircle).Difficulty varied with the number, shape, and orientation of thecomponents involved, including three (as described above, onlyone component must be rotated), three (an inclined parallelogrammade of a square and two triangles, which would have tobe rotated), and five components (i.e., a diamond made of asquare and four triangles, all of which required rotation). Twoscores were given for each item: 1 or 0 for the reproduction ofthe figure and 1 or 0 for the overall orientation; these scoreswere summed up into a total score ranging from 0 to 2 score foreach item.3.2.3.3. Deductive reasoning task. This task involved threeitems requiring to map permission rules onto their relevantpictorial representation (Goswami, 1996). Modus ponens,conjunction, and disjunction arguments were given. Specifically,a story was told about a character having to obey a rulein order to obtain something that she wanted. For example,according to the story addressed to modus ponens, (a) “Oneday Sally wants to play outside. Her mum says that if shewants to play outside, she must put her coat on”. Accordingto the story addressed to conjunction “One day Vera wants toplay outside. Her mum says that if she wants to play outside,the weather must be nice and her room must be tide.”According to the story addressed to disjunction, “One dayPeter wants to eat a kind of fruit. His mum says that he canhave either watermelon or banana, only if he eats his lunch.”To ensure that they could follow the rule, children wereasked to repeat it. Then they were presented with 4 pictures,one showing the character obeying the rule and the restdisobeying it. Performance on each item was scored from 0 to3 to reflect their understanding of the rule and its matchingwith the proper pictorial representation. Scoring was based onboth the selection of the correct picture and the explanationsgiven for this selection: 0 for both wrong choice of picture andfailure to repeat the rule; 1 for choosing the right picture butfailing to repeat the premises of the rule or for choosing a wrongpicture but correctly repeating the premises; 2 for choosing theright picture and partially repeating the rule; 3 for choosing theright picture and fully repeating the premises.3.2.3.4. Analogical reasoning. Three analogical reasoning tasks,(i.e., with quantitative, spatial, and verbal content) were used.In the quantitative task, which included three items, childrenwere presented two dolls and two sticks, each involving adifferent proportion of white and red sections (bricks). Theinstruction to the child was as follows: “You see that each ofthese two dolls has a stick made of red and white bricks. Now,we will play a game where we will have to decide which of thedolls wins each time. The doll “having more red bricks comparedto the white bricks in his stick wins the game”. Children were alsoPlease cite this article as: Kazi, S., et al., Mind–culture <strong>interactions</strong>: <strong>How</strong> <strong>writing</strong> <strong>molds</strong> mental fluidity in early development,Intelligence (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.001

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!