04.01.2017 Views

Display and Interface Design

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A Meaning Processing Approach 21<br />

Similarly, theories of thinking <strong>and</strong> decision making tend to focus on violations<br />

of normative models of rationality (e.g., conjunction fallacy, gambler’s<br />

fallacy, etc.) <strong>and</strong> biases <strong>and</strong> heuristics in judgment <strong>and</strong> decision making<br />

(e.g., anchoring <strong>and</strong> adjustment, representativeness, hindsight). For example,<br />

Wickens (1992) writes:<br />

Many aspects of decision making are not as accurate as they could<br />

be. The limitations of information processing <strong>and</strong> memory, previously<br />

discussed, restrict the accuracy of diagnosis <strong>and</strong> choice. In addition,<br />

limits of attention <strong>and</strong> cognitive resources lead people to adopt<br />

decision-making heuristics, or “mental shortcuts,” which produce<br />

decisions that are often adequate but not usually as precise as they<br />

could be … . Finally, we will sometimes refer to general biases in the<br />

decision-making process. These biases are either described as risky—<br />

leading to a course of action based on insufficient information—or<br />

conservative—leading to the use of less information or less confidence<br />

in a decision than is warranted. (p. 261)<br />

In this context, in which meaning is constructed from ambiguous data<br />

using limited <strong>and</strong> biased information processes, it is not surprising that<br />

two people can interpret the same situation very differently. For example,<br />

two pilots might have different opinions about where the limits of a safe<br />

approach are or two drivers may have different ideas about when to initiate<br />

braking when approaching a line of traffic. Have you ever instinctively<br />

reached for the imaginary brake pedal when you were the passenger of a<br />

more aggressive driver?<br />

Thus, conventional wisdom tends to treat meaning as if it were synonymous<br />

with interpretation. The meaning is how an individual interprets the<br />

situation. This suggests that meaning is a property of awareness <strong>and</strong> this<br />

leads implicitly to the conclusion that the situation is, at best, of secondary<br />

interest <strong>and</strong>, at worst, meaningless or irrelevant. Somehow, meaning is constructed<br />

from ambiguous information, based on arbitrary relations to the<br />

ecology or situation. Meaning becomes a pure invention or construction of<br />

the mind.<br />

In this context, it is not surprising that much of the work in human factors,<br />

engineering psychology, <strong>and</strong> human–computer interaction (HCI) is framed in<br />

terms of internal mental processes. In this context, the job of the human factors<br />

engineer is to ensure that the designers take into account the limits of<br />

these internal computational processes (e.g., perceptual thresholds, memory<br />

limitations, decision biases, etc.). In terms of aiding problem solving, the target<br />

for design is often framed as a requirement to match the operator’s mental<br />

model.<br />

There are obvious values of the conventional approach. Certainly, if features<br />

of the interface are below perceptual thresholds, if the operator is<br />

overwhelmed by an avalanche of data, or if she is constantly surprised by<br />

unexpected events, then the value of the interface may be compromised.<br />

© 2011 by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!