20.07.2013 Views

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

( The similarity between avian song and human speech )<br />

may extend all the way to the molecular level.<br />

Birds that must<br />

learn how to sing,<br />

such as the zebra<br />

fi nch, produce the<br />

FOXP2 protein<br />

(red) in area X<br />

(blue) in their brain.<br />

Birds that know<br />

their songs at birth,<br />

like the ring dove,<br />

do not make<br />

the protein.<br />

duction of speech. We know now, however, that<br />

this strict division is a little too cut-and-dried because<br />

a number of other areas of the brain are<br />

involved in both understanding and producing<br />

speech. The human brain probably processes<br />

spoken information in many areas of the brain<br />

simultaneously.<br />

The idea that the brain uses parallel processing<br />

to understand and control speech is consistent<br />

with another observation: in the KEs, parts<br />

of the brain that are normally not involved in<br />

speech are active. This activity could be a direct<br />

consequence of the FOXP2 defect—a proper<br />

amount of the transcription factor would have<br />

made those areas behave normally—or it could<br />

be a sign that the brain is attempting to compensate<br />

for one of the other fl aws caused by the<br />

mutation.<br />

Differentiating between the direct and indirect<br />

effects of FOXP2 is no simple matter. For<br />

example, because the gene is active during the<br />

embryonic stage, its dysfunction could disturb<br />

brain development. The brain could be “wired”<br />

incorrectly, or certain specialized nerve cells<br />

could fail to form. On the opposite end of the<br />

spectrum of possibility, the brain might develop<br />

normally but run into problems with information<br />

processing later on—for example, during the<br />

phase when children learn to speak. The real effects<br />

of the FOXP2 mutation probably lie somewhere<br />

in between these two extremes.<br />

Bird Babble<br />

To probe further FOXP2’s effects on cognitive<br />

development, researchers are turning to<br />

animals for clues. The FOXP2 gene has been<br />

identifi ed in primates, whales, birds and even<br />

crocodiles; it is highly likely that all vertebrates<br />

have it. The sequence of the gene in these animals<br />

is almost identical to that in humans. For example,<br />

only three of the 715 amino acids in the<br />

mouse FOXP2 gene product differ from those in<br />

the human version. The timing and location of<br />

the gene’s expression in the brains of other species<br />

are also very similar. So what is the FOXP2<br />

gene doing in the brains of these animals, none<br />

of which is capable of speech?<br />

Although most animals have vocalizations<br />

that seem to be innate, a few species—among<br />

them songbirds, parrots, hummingbirds, some<br />

marine mammals and bats—do learn vocal patterns<br />

by imitating their parents. To some extent,<br />

this process is similar to that of a human infant<br />

making his or her fi rst efforts at learning speech.<br />

At fi rst, baby sparrows can imitate only minute<br />

elements of their future song, for instance. This<br />

type of vocalization is referred to as subsong, and<br />

it is similar to infant babbling. When the young<br />

animal hears an example of what is correct, it<br />

adapts its vocal output.<br />

Through intensive practice, young songbirds<br />

increasingly come to sound like their role models,<br />

mastering the repertoire by the onset of sexual<br />

maturity. As is the case with humans, songbirds<br />

are dependent on what they hear to develop normal<br />

vocalization. If songbirds are subjected to<br />

loud noises, if they become deaf or if the feedback<br />

from their “teacher” is interrupted, they<br />

never learn to sing properly.<br />

The similarities between learned avian song<br />

Zebra Finch Ring Dove<br />

70 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND <strong>June</strong>/<strong>July</strong> <strong>2007</strong><br />

COPYRIGHT <strong>2007</strong> SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC.<br />

GEHIRN & GEIST/SIGANIM

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!