20.07.2013 Views

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

Scientific American Mind-June/July 2007

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

JIM BRANDENBURG <strong>Mind</strong>en Pictures<br />

and human speech run even deeper. Both humans<br />

and songbirds have developed neuronal<br />

structures that specialize in the perception and<br />

production of sounds. Compared with humans,<br />

whose brains use parallel processing to comprehend<br />

speech, songbirds have a rather more modularly<br />

constructed brain in which various centers<br />

assume specifi c roles. In the avian brain, auditory<br />

stimuli reach the high vocal center, which<br />

controls the muscular movements of the vocal<br />

organ via the motor center; damage to this region<br />

prevents singing.<br />

Another important data path in bird brains<br />

extends from the high vocal center via area X—a<br />

song-learning center in the basal ganglia—to the<br />

thalamus and from there back to the cortex. This<br />

so-called corticobasal ganglia loop also exists<br />

in the brains of mammals, including humans,<br />

where it is vital for learning. In young birds, lesions<br />

in area X lead to abnormal twittering,<br />

whereas such lesions seem to have no effect in the<br />

adults of most songbird species— until they try to<br />

learn a new song. Apparently the corticobasal<br />

network is important for learning songs but not<br />

necessarily for singing them. In humans also,<br />

FOXP2 proteins are produced in large quantities<br />

in the basal ganglia, which is where the structural<br />

and functional anomalies occur in patients<br />

with FOXP2-related speech defects.<br />

Genetic Songwriter<br />

In the brains of zebra fi nches, area X contains<br />

more FOXP2 during the song-learning phase<br />

than during infancy or adulthood. Likewise in<br />

the canary, which changes its melody once a year<br />

after breeding season, FOXP2 is expressed particularly<br />

strongly in area X during this learning<br />

phase [see illustration on opposite page].<br />

Accordingly, FOXP2 may well be involved in<br />

song plasticity—the ability to learn new songs.<br />

To explore this possibility, our team genetically<br />

reduced the amount of FOXP2 in area X in zebra<br />

fi nches to artifi cially induce a situation similar to<br />

a FOXP2 mutation in humans. The crucial question<br />

is, What happens to a melody if less FOXP2<br />

is expressed in area X while it is being learned?<br />

Our initial results show that the zebra fi nches<br />

have diffi culty learning their songs when they<br />

have less than the normal amount of the FOXP2<br />

transcription factor. We concluded that this protein<br />

is necessary for zebra fi nches to learn a song,<br />

but it is less important for motor functions overall.<br />

Thus, a FOXP2 mutation does not simply<br />

cause the brain to develop abnormally. The defect<br />

continues to play a role once the brain has<br />

become fully developed—an important clue<br />

about what happens in humans who have the<br />

speech problems displayed by the KE family.<br />

Careful analogy of the zebra fi nch’s impaired<br />

song learning with the problems seen in the KE<br />

family leads us to believe that affected family<br />

members may have diffi culty imitating the sounds<br />

made by their parents. They are unable to harmonize<br />

their own speech with that of others. Should<br />

this suspicion be borne out, it would mean that<br />

the similarity between avian song learning and<br />

human speech acquisition extends all the way to<br />

the molecular level.<br />

The logical implication is that the evolution<br />

of language is not a unique feature of the human<br />

lineage. Many species share the structure and<br />

molecular makeup of the brain that was already<br />

in place as our ancestors began to speak. Only as<br />

existing genes and neuronal systems continued<br />

to develop was the path cleared for the uniquely<br />

human capacity for speech. M<br />

(Further Reading)<br />

Songbirds such<br />

as this warbling<br />

western meadowlark<br />

could help provide<br />

clues about<br />

how genes regulate<br />

human speech.<br />

◆ FOXP2 in Focus: What Can Genes Tell Us about Speech and Language?<br />

Gary F. Marcus and Simon E. Fisher in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 7,<br />

No. 6, pages 257–262; <strong>June</strong> 2003.<br />

◆ FOXP2 Expression in Avian Vocal Learners and Non-Learners. Sebastian<br />

Haesler, Kazuhiro Wada, A. Nshdejan, Edward E. Morrisey, Thierry Lints,<br />

Eric D. Jarvis and Constance Scharff in Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 24,<br />

No. 13, pages 3164–3175; March 31, 2004.<br />

◆ Singing Mice, Songbirds, and More: Models for FOXP2 Function and<br />

Dysfunction in Human Speech and Language. Stephanie A. White, Simon<br />

E. Fisher, Daniel H. Geschwind, Constance Scharff and Timothy E. Holy<br />

in Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 26, No. 41, pages 10376–10379;<br />

October 11, 2006.<br />

www.sciammind.com SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN MIND 71<br />

COPYRIGHT <strong>2007</strong> SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!