01.08.2013 Views

Download The Journal (PDF) - Division on Autism and ...

Download The Journal (PDF) - Division on Autism and ...

Download The Journal (PDF) - Division on Autism and ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training<br />

in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental<br />

Disabilities<br />

Focusing <strong>on</strong> individuals with<br />

autism, intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities<br />

Volume 45 Number 1<br />

DAD<br />

D<br />

March 2010


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

College of Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong> & Leadership<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sulting Editors<br />

Martin Agran<br />

Reuben Altman<br />

Phillip J. Belfiore<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> Borthwick-Duffy<br />

Michael P. Brady<br />

Fredda Brown<br />

Mary Lynne Calhoun<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> F. Cramer<br />

Caroline Dunn<br />

Lise Fox<br />

David L. Gast<br />

Herbert Goldstein<br />

Robert Henders<strong>on</strong><br />

Carolyn Hughes<br />

Larry K. Irvin<br />

James V. Kahn<br />

H. Earle Knowlt<strong>on</strong><br />

Barry W. Lavay<br />

Rena Lewis<br />

Kathleen J. Marshall<br />

Editorial Assistant: Hannah H. Hainline<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

College of Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong> & Leadership<br />

John McD<strong>on</strong>nell<br />

Gale M. Morris<strong>on</strong><br />

Gabriel A. Nardi<br />

John Nietupski<br />

James R. Patt<strong>on</strong><br />

Edward A. Polloway<br />

Thomas G. Roberts<br />

Robert S. Rueda<br />

Diane L. Ryndak<br />

Edward J. Sabornie<br />

Laurence R. Sargent<br />

Gary M. Sasso<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Scott Sparks<br />

Fred Spo<strong>on</strong>er<br />

Robert Stodden<br />

Keith Storey<br />

David L. Westling<br />

John J. Wheeler<br />

Mark Wolery<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities is sent to all members of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities of <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children. All <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> members must first be members of <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> membership dues are $25.00 for regular members <strong>and</strong> $13.00 for full time students. Membership is <strong>on</strong> a yearly basis. All inquiries<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning membership, subscripti<strong>on</strong>, advertising, etc. should be sent to the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 1110 North<br />

Glebe Road, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, VA 22201. Advertising rates are available up<strong>on</strong> request.<br />

Manuscripts should be typed, double spaced, <strong>and</strong> sent (five copies) to the Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, College of Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Leadership, Box 875411, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-5411. Each manuscript should have a cover sheet that gives the names,<br />

affiliati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> complete addresses of all authors.<br />

Editing policies are based <strong>on</strong> the Publicati<strong>on</strong> Manual, the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2001 revisi<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

provided <strong>on</strong> the inside back cover. Any signed article is the pers<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> of the author; likewise, any advertisement is the resp<strong>on</strong>sibility<br />

of the advertiser. Neither necessarily carries <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> endorsement unless specifically set forth by adopted resoluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities is abstracted <strong>and</strong> indexed in Psychological Abstracts, PsycINFO, e-psyche,<br />

Abstracts for Social Workers, Internati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> Research, Current C<strong>on</strong>tents/Social <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Sciences, Excerpta<br />

Medica, Social Sciences Citati<strong>on</strong> Index, Adolescent Mental Health Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Administrati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research<br />

Abstracts, <strong>and</strong> Language <strong>and</strong> Language Behavior Abstracts. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it is annotated <strong>and</strong> indexed by the ERIC Clearinghouse <strong>on</strong><br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> Gifted Children for publicati<strong>on</strong> in the m<strong>on</strong>thly print index Current Index to <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>s in Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the quarterly index,<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Child Educati<strong>on</strong> Resources.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities Vol. 45, No. 1, March 2010, Copyright 2010 by the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Austim <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Board of Directors<br />

Officers<br />

Past President J. David Smith<br />

President Emily Bouck<br />

President-Elect Teresa Taber-Doughty<br />

Vice President Richard Gargiulo<br />

Secretary T<strong>on</strong>i Merfeld<br />

Treasurer Gardner Umbarger<br />

Members<br />

Mark Francis<br />

Linda Laz<br />

Nicole Mucherino (Student Governor)<br />

Robert S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong><br />

Debora Wichmanowski<br />

Dianne Zager<br />

Executive Director<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Publicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Jack Hourcade<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Darlene Perner<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference Coordinator<br />

Cindy Perras<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purposes of this organizati<strong>on</strong> shall be to advance the educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, research<br />

in the educati<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, competency of educators in this field, public underst<strong>and</strong>ing of autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong> needed to help accomplish these goals. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> shall encourage <strong>and</strong> promote professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

growth, research, <strong>and</strong> the disseminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> utilizati<strong>on</strong> of research findings.<br />

EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES (ISSN 1547-0350) (USPS 0168-5000) is published<br />

quarterly in March, June, September, <strong>and</strong> December, by <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities, 1110 North Glebe Road, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22201-5704. Members’ dues to <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Developmental Disabilities include $8.00 for subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND<br />

DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES. Subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABIL-<br />

ITIES is available without membership; Individual—U.S. $40.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $44.00; Instituti<strong>on</strong>s—<br />

U.S. $175.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $179.50; single copy price is $25.00. U.S. Periodicals postage is paid at<br />

Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22204 <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al mailing offices.<br />

POSTMASTERS: Send address changes to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES,<br />

1110 North Glebe Road, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22201-5704.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities<br />

Editorial Policy<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities focuses <strong>on</strong> the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

ETADD invites research <strong>and</strong> expository manuscripts <strong>and</strong> critical review of the<br />

literature. Major emphasis is <strong>on</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> assessment, educati<strong>on</strong>al programming,<br />

characteristics, training of instructi<strong>on</strong>al pers<strong>on</strong>nel, habilitati<strong>on</strong>, preventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

community underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> provisi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Each manuscript is evaluated an<strong>on</strong>ymously by three reviewers. Criteria for acceptance<br />

include the following: relevance, reader interest, quality, applicability,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the field, <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omy <strong>and</strong> smoothness of expressi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> review<br />

process requires two to four m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Viewpoints expressed are those of the authors <strong>and</strong> do not necessarily c<strong>on</strong>form to<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s of the editors or of the officers of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Submissi<strong>on</strong> of Manuscripts<br />

1. Manuscript submissi<strong>on</strong> is a representati<strong>on</strong> that the manuscript is the author’s<br />

own work, has not been published, <strong>and</strong> is not currently under c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

publicati<strong>on</strong> elsewhere.<br />

2. Manuscripts must be prepared according to the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s in the Publicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Manual of the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong> (Fifth Editi<strong>on</strong>, 2001).<br />

Laser or high density dot printing are acceptable.<br />

3. Each manuscript must have a cover sheet giving the names <strong>and</strong> affiliati<strong>on</strong>s of all<br />

authors <strong>and</strong> the address of the principal author.<br />

4. Graphs <strong>and</strong> figures should be originals or sharp, high quality photographic<br />

prints suitable, if necessary, for a 50% reducti<strong>on</strong> in size.<br />

5. Five copies of the manuscript al<strong>on</strong>g with a transmittal letter should be sent to the<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, College of Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong> & Leadership, Box<br />

875411, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-5411.<br />

6. Up<strong>on</strong> receipt, each manuscript will be screened by the editor. Appropriate<br />

manuscripts will then be sent to c<strong>on</strong>sulting editors. Principal authors will receive<br />

notificati<strong>on</strong> of receipt of manuscript.<br />

7. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Editor reserves the right to make minor editorial changes which do not<br />

materially affect the meaning of the text.<br />

8. Manuscripts are the property of ETADD for a minimum period of six m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

All articles accepted for publicati<strong>on</strong> are copyrighted in the name of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities<br />

VOLUME 45 NUMBER 1 MARCH 2010<br />

Teaching Students with Moderate Intellectual Disabilities to Read: An Experimental<br />

Examinati<strong>on</strong> of a Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> 3<br />

JILL ALLOR, PATRICIA MATHES, KYLE ROBERTS, FRANCESCA JONES, <strong>and</strong><br />

TAMMI CHAMPLIN<br />

Friendships with Peers with Severe Disabilities: American <strong>and</strong> Iranian Sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

Students’ Ideas about Being a Friend 23<br />

MOHSEN SHOKOOHI-YEKTA, <strong>and</strong> JO M. HENDRICKSON<br />

L<strong>on</strong>g-term Outcomes of Services in Inclusive <strong>and</strong> Self-C<strong>on</strong>tained Settings for Siblings<br />

with Comparable Significant Disabilities 38<br />

DIANE RYNDAK, TERRI WARD, SANDRA ALPER, JILL F. STORCH, <strong>and</strong><br />

JENNIFER WILSON MONTGOMERY<br />

Mild Intellectual Disabilities: Legacies <strong>and</strong> Trends in C<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Practices 54<br />

EDWARD A. POLLOWAY, JACQUELINE LUBIN, J. DAVID SMITH, <strong>and</strong> JAMES R. PATTON<br />

An Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the Effects of a Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the Play<br />

Behaviors of Children with <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorder 69<br />

JENNIFER LONCOLA WALBERG <strong>and</strong> LESLEY A. CRAIG-UNKEFER<br />

Characteristics <strong>and</strong> Hypothesized Functi<strong>on</strong>s of Challenging Behavior in a Community-<br />

Based Sample 81<br />

ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR, JOHN W. ESCH, RACHAEL A. SAUTTER, <strong>and</strong><br />

KELISE K. STEWART<br />

A Collaborative Approach to Implement Positive Behavior Support Plans for Children<br />

with Problem Behaviors: A Comparis<strong>on</strong> of C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> versus C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Feedback Approach 94<br />

DILEK ERBAS<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong>: A Review 107<br />

JIE ZHANG <strong>and</strong> JOHN J. WHEELER<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards: An Effective Interventi<strong>on</strong> for Students with Disabilities 116<br />

CHANNON HORN<br />

Effects of C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay Procedure <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s Method of Swimming<br />

Rotati<strong>on</strong> Skills for Children with <strong>Autism</strong> 124<br />

I˙LKER YILMAZ, FERMAN KONUKMAN, BENYAMIN BIRKAN, ARZU ÖZEN,<br />

MEHMET YANARDAG˘ , <strong>and</strong> I˙LHAN C AMURSOY<br />

Use of Self-Modeling Static-Picture Prompts via a H<strong>and</strong>held Computer to Facilitate<br />

Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring in the General Educati<strong>on</strong> Classroom 136<br />

DAVID F. CIHAK, RACHEL WRIGHT, <strong>and</strong> KEVIN M. AYRES<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> for Children with <strong>Autism</strong>: A Comparis<strong>on</strong> with the<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council Recommended Practices 150<br />

ROBYN CONLEY DOWNS <strong>and</strong> ANDREW DOWNS<br />

Manuscripts Accepted for Future Publicati<strong>on</strong> in Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities 2<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities retains literary property rights <strong>on</strong> copyrighted articles. Up<br />

to 100 copies of the articles in this journal may be reproduced for n<strong>on</strong>profit distributi<strong>on</strong> without permissi<strong>on</strong> from<br />

the publisher. All other forms of reproducti<strong>on</strong> require permissi<strong>on</strong> from the publisher.


Manuscripts Accepted for Future Publicati<strong>on</strong> in Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

June 2010<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the picture exchange communicati<strong>on</strong> system behaviors across<br />

settings, pers<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> stimulus classes with three students with autism. Maud S. Dogoe, Devender<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, <strong>and</strong> Robin H. Lock, Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology <strong>and</strong> Leadership, College of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, P.O. Box 41701, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409.<br />

Training teachers to assess the challenging behaviors of student with autism using video telec<strong>on</strong>ferencing.<br />

Wendy Machalicek, Mark F. O’Reilly, M<strong>and</strong>y Rispoli, T<strong>on</strong>ya Davis, Russell Lang,<br />

Jessica Hetlinger Franco, <strong>and</strong> Jeffrey M. Chan, Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Counselor<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, GSE, PO Box 751, Portl<strong>and</strong> State University, Portl<strong>and</strong>, OR 97207.<br />

Review of interventi<strong>on</strong>s to increase functi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> symbolic play in children with autism. Russell<br />

Lang, Wendy Machalicek, Mark O’Reilly, Jeff Sigafoos, M<strong>and</strong>y Rispoli, Karrie Shogren, <strong>and</strong> April<br />

Regester, Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 1 University Stati<strong>on</strong> D5300, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> University of Texas at<br />

Austin, Austin, TX 78712.<br />

Promoting active engagement in the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom <strong>and</strong> access to the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum for students with cognitive disabilities. Martin Agran, Michael Wehmeyer,<br />

Michael Cavin, <strong>and</strong> Susan Palmer, University of Wyoming, Special Eudcati<strong>on</strong> Department, Dept.<br />

3374, 1000 E University Ave., Laramie, WY 82071-2000.<br />

Descriptive analysis of single subject research designs: 1983-2007. Diana Hamm<strong>on</strong>d <strong>and</strong> David L.<br />

Gast, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> University of Georgia, Department of Communicati<strong>on</strong> Sciences <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

516 Aderhold Hall, Athens, GA 30602.<br />

Sexuality educati<strong>on</strong> for individuals with autism spectrum disorders: Critical issues <strong>and</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong><br />

making guidelines. Jas<strong>on</strong> Travers <strong>and</strong> Matt Tincani, 1301 Cecil B. Moore Ave., Ritter Hall 367,<br />

Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122.<br />

An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the effects of sensory integrati<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong> by a preschool special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teacher. Penelope W<strong>on</strong>g B<strong>on</strong>ggat <strong>and</strong> Laura J. Hall, Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

San Diego State University, 5500 Campanile Drive, San Diego, CA 92182-1170.<br />

A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong> of parents’ opini<strong>on</strong>s about safety skills instructi<strong>on</strong>: An apparent<br />

discrepancy between importance <strong>and</strong> expectati<strong>on</strong>. Martin Agran <strong>and</strong> Michael Krupp, University of<br />

Wyoming, Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Department, Dept. 3374, 1000 E. University Ave., Laramie, WY 82071.<br />

A review of the literature <strong>on</strong> community-based instructi<strong>on</strong> across grade levels. Allis<strong>on</strong> Walker,<br />

Nicole M. Uphold, Shar<strong>on</strong> Richter, <strong>and</strong> David W. Test, Temple University, Curriculum, Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Technology in Educati<strong>on</strong>, Ritter Hall 362, 1301 Cecil B. Moore Avenue, Philadelphia, PA<br />

19122.<br />

Culturally diverse parents’ perspectives <strong>on</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. Dalun Zhang, Leena L<strong>and</strong>mark,<br />

Cheryl Grenwelge, <strong>and</strong> Linda M<strong>on</strong>toya, Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 4225 TAMU,<br />

College Stati<strong>on</strong>, TX 77843-4225.<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>al safety programs for children with intellectual disabilities. Yu-Ri Kim, Department of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, Sangmyung University, 7 H<strong>on</strong>gji-D<strong>on</strong>g, J<strong>on</strong>gno-Gu, Seoul, 110-743 KOREA.<br />

Computer-based video instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach students with intellecutal disabilities to use public bus<br />

transportati<strong>on</strong>. Linda Mechling <strong>and</strong> Eileen O’Brien, University of North Carolina Wilmingt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Department of Early Childhood <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 601 S. College Road, Wilmingt<strong>on</strong>, NC<br />

28404-5940.<br />

Address is supplied for author in boldface type.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 3–22<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Teaching Students with Moderate Intellectual Disabilities to<br />

Read: An Experimental Examinati<strong>on</strong> of a Comprehensive<br />

Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Jill H. Allor, Patricia G. Mathes, J. Kyle Roberts, Francesca G. J<strong>on</strong>es, <strong>and</strong><br />

Tammi M. Champlin<br />

Southern Methodist University<br />

Abstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> primary purpose of our research was to determine if a comprehensive, ph<strong>on</strong>ics-based, direct<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> reading program would be effective in teaching early reading <strong>and</strong> language skills to students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities (ID). Participants were 28 elementary students from 10 public schools in an<br />

urban school district <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e urban private school who were r<strong>and</strong>omly placed into treatment <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast<br />

groups. Students in the treatment c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> received daily, comprehensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong> in small groups<br />

of 1–4 students for approximately 40 minutes per sessi<strong>on</strong>. A broad array of measures was studied, including<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness, ph<strong>on</strong>ics, word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> oral language. Means favored the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> group <strong>on</strong> all measures, with moderate to str<strong>on</strong>g effect sizes. Statistically significant differences were<br />

found <strong>on</strong> most measures, including ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness, ph<strong>on</strong>ics, vocabulary, <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se<br />

findings dem<strong>on</strong>strated that students with moderate ID can learn basic reading skills given c<strong>on</strong>sistent, explicit,<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong> across an extended period of time.<br />

In recent years, there has been growing nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong> that literacy is a civil right.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> nati<strong>on</strong>al rhetoric suggests that all children<br />

have the right to scientifically-based<br />

reading instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> that it is not acceptable<br />

for any child to leave school with low<br />

literacy skills (No Child Left Behind Act,<br />

2002). However, within the rhetoric about all<br />

children, references to students with intellectual<br />

disabilities (ID), or mental retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

are typically vague or absent. One might questi<strong>on</strong><br />

how <strong>on</strong>e set of children could be overlooked<br />

in discussi<strong>on</strong>s about all children. In<br />

our experience, the answer appears to be that<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> work presented in this article was supported<br />

by Grant No. H324K040011-05 from the Institute of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Sciences. This article does not necessarily<br />

reflect the positi<strong>on</strong>s or policies of this funding<br />

agency <strong>and</strong> no official endorsement should be inferred.<br />

We thank the teachers <strong>and</strong> students who<br />

participated in this research. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

this article should be addressed to Jill H.<br />

Allor, Department of Teaching <strong>and</strong> Learning, Annette<br />

Caldwell Simm<strong>on</strong>s School of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Human Development, Southern Methodist University,<br />

P.O. Box 750381, Dallas, TX 75275-0381.<br />

all has really referred to all children who are<br />

believed to be capable of learning to read<br />

(Katims, 2000). We define reading as the ability<br />

to process individual words in c<strong>on</strong>nected<br />

text resulting in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the author’s<br />

intended meaning. With this definiti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

mind, many educators assume that children<br />

with ID are not capable of learning to read<br />

(Katims). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> expectati<strong>on</strong> has been that, at<br />

best, students with ID, particularly those with<br />

moderate ID, can learn to identify a specific<br />

list of words memorized by sight. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> result is<br />

that typically little effort is made to teach these<br />

students to become fully literate <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly 1 in<br />

5 children with mild or moderate ID achieves<br />

even minimal literacy skills (Katims, 2001).<br />

Research <strong>on</strong> Reading <strong>and</strong> Intellectual Disabilities<br />

Although much progress has been made in<br />

recent years regarding the educati<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with ID, to date, very little reading research<br />

has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted with these students.<br />

What research has been d<strong>on</strong>e has<br />

focused primarily <strong>on</strong> students with mild ID<br />

(see Browder, Wakeman, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-<br />

Delzell, & Algozzine, 2006) <strong>and</strong> has focused<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 3


<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> isolated subskills of reading, rather<br />

than <strong>on</strong> comprehensive reading interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that integrate all essential comp<strong>on</strong>ents of<br />

reading (defined below). Currently, no research<br />

has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted to determine<br />

whether students with ID can learn to read by<br />

fully processing the print <strong>and</strong> meaning of c<strong>on</strong>nected<br />

text, as is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with current theories<br />

of reading development (see reviews<br />

Browder & Xin, 1998; Browder et al., 2006;<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ners, 2003; Joseph & Seery, 2004).<br />

In spite of the paucity of research, the research<br />

that does exist is promising, suggesting<br />

that students with ID are capable of learning<br />

various aspects of reading. Sight word recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

has received the greatest attenti<strong>on</strong> from<br />

researchers <strong>and</strong> the prep<strong>on</strong>derance of evidence<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strates that students with even<br />

moderate <strong>and</strong> severe levels of ID can learn to<br />

automatically recognize a fairly large corpus<br />

of words with systematic instructi<strong>on</strong> (Browder<br />

et al., 2006). Even so, these students have little<br />

ability to generalize their learning bey<strong>on</strong>d the<br />

specific words included in instructi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

thus, are far from achieving even basic literacy<br />

(Browder et al.). Research <strong>on</strong> the effectiveness<br />

of ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> is primarily limited to<br />

students with mild ID, but that research is also<br />

promising. C<strong>on</strong>ners (1992) <strong>and</strong> Joseph <strong>and</strong><br />

Seery (2004) found fourteen studies that examined<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> these studies lend preliminary support<br />

to the effectiveness of ph<strong>on</strong>ics interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Unfortunately, these studies were all relatively<br />

brief, providing at most a few m<strong>on</strong>ths of instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> they focused <strong>on</strong> isolated subskills<br />

of ph<strong>on</strong>ics, rather than a comprehensive,<br />

systematic approach that might result in<br />

skilled decoding. Further, n<strong>on</strong>e of these studies<br />

focused specifically <strong>on</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness<br />

(PA). In 1996, O’C<strong>on</strong>nor, Notari-Syvers<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Vadasy, described the progress of<br />

several students with mild ID who participated<br />

in a PA interventi<strong>on</strong> study. Of the nine students<br />

with ID who participated in the sixm<strong>on</strong>th<br />

PA interventi<strong>on</strong>, three made substantial<br />

progress. In a recent study, students with<br />

ID receiving instructi<strong>on</strong> for approximately 10<br />

weeks made significantly more progress <strong>on</strong><br />

sounding out activities than a similar c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group (C<strong>on</strong>ners, Rosenquist, Sligh, Atwell, &<br />

Kiser, 2006). Studies <strong>on</strong> vocabulary <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

are even more limited, <strong>on</strong>ly in-<br />

cluding dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s of very basic skills,<br />

such as using a sight word in the c<strong>on</strong>text of a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al activity or matching a word to a<br />

picture (Browder et al.).<br />

Taken in its totality, the research base <strong>on</strong><br />

teaching students with ID to read is sparse <strong>and</strong><br />

inadequate. At the present time, there are no<br />

studies that have examined the effectiveness<br />

of a comprehensive reading interventi<strong>on</strong> delivered<br />

over a sustained period of time. Without<br />

this type of research, we cannot determine<br />

whether “all” as described in No Child Left<br />

Behind should or should not include students<br />

with ID. In short, we simply do not know what<br />

is possible for students with ID. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> missi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the research reported here is to take important<br />

steps toward addressing this questi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Specifically, we seek to determine what is possible<br />

for teaching children with moderate levels<br />

of ID to read.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Framework<br />

While there is little research <strong>on</strong> reading to<br />

guide decisi<strong>on</strong> making for children with moderate<br />

ID, much research has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with other populati<strong>on</strong>s who also find learning<br />

to read very difficult, <strong>and</strong> thus, should inform<br />

research <strong>on</strong> teaching students with ID to read.<br />

This research provides the c<strong>on</strong>ceptual framework<br />

for our study. As is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with current<br />

research, we see reading as an integrated<br />

process, rather than a set of isolated skills. In<br />

a relatively simple view of reading, good readers<br />

effortlessly recognize words <strong>and</strong> build<br />

mental representati<strong>on</strong>s of the message of the<br />

text (Ehri, 2005; Perfetti, L<strong>and</strong>i, & Oakhill,<br />

2005). Studies examining the underlying processes<br />

of word recogniti<strong>on</strong> are clear. Good<br />

readers fully process print, attending to the<br />

inner structure of each word that is read (Adams,<br />

1990; Ehri, 2005; Ehri & McCormick,<br />

1998; Torgesen, 2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y do this quickly<br />

<strong>and</strong> effortlessly. Researchers believe good<br />

readers are able to focus attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the<br />

meaning of print because word recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

processes are automatized. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> underlying<br />

processes of comprehensi<strong>on</strong> are arguably<br />

more complex, depending <strong>on</strong> a variety of factors<br />

including listening comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, linguistic<br />

abilities, relevant knowledge, <strong>and</strong> general<br />

intelligence (Perfetti et al.). Specific to<br />

written language underst<strong>and</strong>ing are factors in-<br />

4 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


cluding sensitivity to story structure, inference<br />

making, <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

(Perfetti et al.). We know that students<br />

progress through predictable stages as their<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

develop (Chall, 1996; Ehri; Ehri & McCormick,<br />

1998). In early stages students develop<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ological awareness <strong>and</strong> print awareness,<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with expressive <strong>and</strong> receptive oral language<br />

skills. In later stages decoding <strong>and</strong> morphographic<br />

knowledge increases, eventually<br />

leading to the quick <strong>and</strong> effortless retrieval of<br />

words from l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory, enabling students<br />

to read fluently <strong>and</strong>, most importantly,<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> making sense of the message of text.<br />

Good readers make inferences <strong>and</strong> m<strong>on</strong>itor<br />

their own comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, ensuring that stories<br />

<strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> are cohesive (Perfetti et<br />

al.).<br />

Research <strong>on</strong> Early Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Over the past 30 years numerous studies focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> the preventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> correcti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

reading problems with students who struggle<br />

to learn to read who do not have ID. A primary<br />

finding from this research is that interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

provided to small groups of children<br />

in the primary grades can be highly effective<br />

in preventing reading problems for most children<br />

<strong>and</strong> greatly reducing the depth of reading<br />

problems for those who c<strong>on</strong>tinue to experience<br />

difficulty, (e.g., Foorman & Torgesen,<br />

2001; Mathes et al., 2005; Mathes & Dent<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2002; Dent<strong>on</strong> & Mathes, 2003; Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading<br />

Panel, 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin,<br />

1998). Likewise, we now underst<strong>and</strong> the critical<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent students must acquire if they are<br />

to become competent readers. Effective interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in early reading target multiple comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of the reading process in an integrated<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensive manner, including<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts of print, oral language, ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

<strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness (PA), letter knowledge,<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, fluency, <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

(see Foorman & Torgesen, 2001; Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Reading Panel, 2000; Pressley, 1998;<br />

Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, & Seidenberg,<br />

2001; Snow et al., 1998). Many experimental<br />

studies dem<strong>on</strong>strate that teaching PA<br />

results in improved reading <strong>and</strong> spelling outcomes<br />

(see Ehri et al., 2001). Letter knowledge,<br />

including letter naming <strong>and</strong> letter-<br />

sound recogniti<strong>on</strong>, is also an important<br />

predictor of reading achievement (Share,<br />

Jorm, Maclean, & Matthews, 1984; Adams,<br />

1990), <strong>and</strong> these skills influence other key<br />

early literacy skills, such as PA <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

decoding (Blaiklock, 2004; Evans, Bell, Shaw,<br />

Moretti, & Page, 2006; Foy & Mann, 2006;<br />

Roberts, 2003; Treiman, Tincoff, & Richm<strong>on</strong>d-Welty,<br />

1996; Treiman, Tincoff, Rodriguez,<br />

Mouzaki, & Francis, 1998). Many children<br />

who have difficulty learning to read also<br />

struggle with the development of good oral<br />

language skills (Perfetti et al., 2005). One<br />

method dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective for students<br />

with language delays is interactive storybook<br />

reading (Arnold & Whitehurst, 1994;<br />

Dickins<strong>on</strong> & Smith, 1994; Karweit & Wasik,<br />

1996; Valdez-Menchaca & Whitehurst, 1992).<br />

Explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> in basic comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

strategies is also a critical comp<strong>on</strong>ent of successful<br />

early reading interventi<strong>on</strong>s (Mathes et<br />

al., 2005).<br />

As we approach the task of intervening with<br />

children with moderate ID, it is important that<br />

we provide instructi<strong>on</strong> that not <strong>on</strong>ly teaches<br />

the critical c<strong>on</strong>tent of reading, but also synthesizes<br />

what is known to be effective in teaching<br />

students with ID. For these students, a<br />

behavioral approach appears to be most appropriate<br />

(Adams & Engelmann, 1996; Browder<br />

et al., 2006; Joseph & Seery, 2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

role of the teacher in a behaviorist model is to<br />

explicitly teach c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> model skills, providing<br />

systematic review of skills <strong>and</strong> reinforcement<br />

for mastery.<br />

Purpose of the Study<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study was to analyze the<br />

effectiveness of a carefully crafted, comprehensive<br />

reading interventi<strong>on</strong> built <strong>on</strong> behavioral<br />

principles in teaching primary-grade students<br />

with moderate ID to read. Students in<br />

this study participated in our interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

<strong>on</strong>e to <strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> a half years. Specifically, we<br />

implemented <strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong>ed an explicit, systematic<br />

reading interventi<strong>on</strong> that had been<br />

empirically validated with students at-risk for<br />

learning disabilities (Mathes et al., 2005) <strong>and</strong><br />

with students who are both struggling readers<br />

<strong>and</strong> English Language Learners (Vaughn,<br />

Mathes et al., 2006). This interventi<strong>on</strong>, now<br />

published as Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in Reading<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 5


(Mathes & Torgesen, 2005a), was (a) rooted<br />

in behavioral theory, (b) comprised of all of<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tent dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be critical for<br />

struggling readers without ID, <strong>and</strong> (c) supplemented<br />

with additi<strong>on</strong>al language development<br />

support.<br />

This study adds to the literature in several<br />

ways. First, the reading interventi<strong>on</strong> is comprehensive<br />

in nature, with instructi<strong>on</strong> targeting<br />

oral language, ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness, alphabetic<br />

knowledge, ph<strong>on</strong>emic decoding,<br />

<strong>and</strong> basic comprehensi<strong>on</strong> strategies. Sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> in this study includes many<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents that have been previously validated<br />

with students at-risk for reading failure<br />

who have IQs in the average range, thus extending<br />

that research to the populati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

students with moderate ID. Third, the study<br />

extends these techniques with the additi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

oral language activities <strong>and</strong> modified teaching<br />

techniques. Fourth, the study employs a l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal,<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omized trial design. In this article,<br />

we report data collected after the students<br />

had been in the study for at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

full academic year. Future reports will follow<br />

the students for approximately four academic<br />

years. Finally, ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

decoding were measured repeatedly allowing<br />

for the use of advanced statistical techniques.<br />

We addressed the following specific<br />

research questi<strong>on</strong>: Does a comprehensive<br />

reading program taught to primary-grade students<br />

who have moderate ID (IQs ranging<br />

from 40–55) result in better reading outcomes<br />

than typical special educati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> measures of (a) ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness,<br />

(b) alphabetic knowledge, (c) word recogniti<strong>on</strong>/ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

decoding, <strong>and</strong> (d) oral language/comprehensi<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Previously Validated Interventi<strong>on</strong> Comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> included comp<strong>on</strong>ents previously<br />

validated for students without ID. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

first, <strong>and</strong> most comprehensive, is Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in Reading (Mathes & Torgesen,<br />

2005a; Mathes et al., 2005; Vaughn, Mathes et<br />

al., 2006). We also built up<strong>on</strong> oral language<br />

storybook techniques successfully used with<br />

English Language Learners (Vaughn, Cirino<br />

et al., 2006; Vaughn, Linan-Thomps<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Mathes, Duradola, & Cárdenas-Hagan, 2007).<br />

Finally, we used a simple game to provide<br />

students with extensive modeling, practice,<br />

<strong>and</strong> feedback in ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness segmentati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> blending, as well as the applicati<strong>on</strong><br />

of those skills to print (Allor, Gansle, &<br />

Denny, 2006). (See Method secti<strong>on</strong> for further<br />

details about the interventi<strong>on</strong>.)<br />

Method<br />

Research Design<br />

This study focused <strong>on</strong> students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities (i.e. IQs ranging from<br />

40-55) who were participants in a larger, l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

study examining the effectiveness of<br />

a comprehensive reading program for students<br />

with low IQs (ranging from 40–79: Allor,<br />

Roberts, Mathes, Roid, & Cheatham,<br />

2010). Students were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned<br />

within each school to either (a) an interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

group that participated in daily, small<br />

group reading instructi<strong>on</strong> delivered by research<br />

teachers or (b) a c<strong>on</strong>trast group receiving<br />

typical special educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Participants<br />

Schools. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study took place in 10 elementary<br />

schools in a large, southwestern urban<br />

school district <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e private school for<br />

students with special needs. District pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

worked with the researchers to select schools<br />

with a relatively large number of students with<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> that would provide a balanced sample,<br />

racially <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omically. An urban, private<br />

school that served students with special needs<br />

was added to increase the size of our sample of<br />

students with moderate ID.<br />

Teachers. Six certified special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teachers were hired to provide instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

students in the research study. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> highest<br />

degree held by five of the teachers was a bachelor’s<br />

degree, while <strong>on</strong>e teacher also held a<br />

master’s degree. Five were female <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e was<br />

male. Five were Caucasian <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e was African<br />

American. Five were jointly hired <strong>and</strong> supervised<br />

by district pers<strong>on</strong>nel <strong>and</strong> researchers,<br />

teaching at two or three different schools each<br />

day. One taught exclusively at the private<br />

school. At the outset of the study, two were<br />

new to teaching <strong>and</strong> the others had 5, 9, 12,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 35 years of teaching experience, respectively.<br />

Three of the teachers had prior experi-<br />

6 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 1<br />

Student Demographic Data by Group<br />

Variable<br />

ence working with students with reading difficulties,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e had prior experience with<br />

students with behavioral disorders, <strong>on</strong>e was<br />

bilingual, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e had prior experience<br />

teaching students with ID.<br />

Students. At the outset of the study, researchers<br />

<strong>and</strong> school district pers<strong>on</strong>nel identified<br />

all students in each of the schools with<br />

moderate ID (IQ scores between 40 <strong>and</strong> 55)<br />

<strong>and</strong> who were in grades 1 to 4. All students in<br />

this IQ range were included regardless of the<br />

cause or comorbid c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (i.e., Down Syndrome,<br />

autism, William’s Syndrome, physical<br />

disability, etc.). Students were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned<br />

within each school into either the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

group or the c<strong>on</strong>trast group. Due<br />

to the small number of students within each<br />

school, students were not matched <strong>on</strong> other<br />

variables. Twenty-four students began the<br />

study in the first year <strong>and</strong> another seven students<br />

joined the study at the beginning of the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d year (these seven were also r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

assigned to the treatment or c<strong>on</strong>trast group).<br />

Of these 31 students, two moved during the<br />

study <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e was removed from the sample<br />

due to misidentificati<strong>on</strong>, resulting in a sample<br />

of 28 students (treatment, n 16; c<strong>on</strong>trast,<br />

n 12). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean age of the participants was<br />

9.46 (SD 1.19) for the treatment group <strong>and</strong><br />

Treatment (n 16)<br />

n (%)<br />

9.25 (SD 1.76) for the c<strong>on</strong>trast group. This<br />

difference was not significant (t .106).<br />

Other demographic informati<strong>on</strong> is presented<br />

in Table 1. Chi-square analyses revealed no<br />

significant differences <strong>on</strong> any demographic<br />

variables, including race, gender, socioec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

status, <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al placement.<br />

Measures<br />

We employed two types of measurement<br />

schemes. First, we assessed at pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, we collected c<strong>on</strong>tinuous progress<br />

m<strong>on</strong>itoring data every four weeks during the<br />

first year of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> every six<br />

weeks during the sec<strong>on</strong>d year.<br />

Pre-post<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trast (n 12)<br />

n (%) 2 (df) a<br />

Gender 1.77 (1)<br />

M 14 (88%) 8 (67%)<br />

F 2 (12%) 4 (33%)<br />

Race .58 (3)<br />

Caucasian 4 (25%) 4 (33%)<br />

African American 9 (56%) 5 (42%)<br />

Hispanic 2 (13%) 2 (17%)<br />

Other 1 (6%) 1 (8%)<br />

Free Lunch Program Participati<strong>on</strong><br />

Free 5 (31%) 3 (25%) .15 (2)<br />

N<strong>on</strong>e 10 (63%) 8 (67%)<br />

Unknown 1 (6%) 1 (8%)<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Placement 1.39 (2)<br />

Self c<strong>on</strong>tained class for students with ID 12 (75%) 11 (92%)<br />

Self c<strong>on</strong>tained class for students with autism 1 (6%) 0 (0%)<br />

General educati<strong>on</strong> w/ resource 3 (19%) 1 (8%)<br />

a No differences were statistically significant.<br />

All students were assessed prior to the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> at the end of the Spring semester<br />

of the sec<strong>on</strong>d year. Pretesting during the first<br />

year occurred between October <strong>and</strong> February<br />

<strong>on</strong> a staggered schedule with students in the<br />

treatment <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast groups tested at approximately<br />

the same time. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> 7 students<br />

who entered the study in the sec<strong>on</strong>d year were<br />

pretested in August or September of that year.<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 7


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following measures comprised the comprehensive<br />

battery:<br />

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III (PPVT-III;<br />

Dunn & Dunn, 1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> PPVT-III measures<br />

receptive vocabulary. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> technical<br />

manual reports reliability coefficients ranging<br />

from .91 to .98 <strong>and</strong> adequate c<strong>on</strong>tent, criteri<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct validity.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT; Williams,<br />

1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> EVT measures expressive vocabulary.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> technical manual reports internal<br />

reliability alphas ranging from .90 to .98 with<br />

a median of .95 <strong>and</strong> test-retest reliability coefficients<br />

range from .77 to .90. Data <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent,<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>, clinical <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct validity<br />

are reported in the technical manual.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery-<br />

Revised. (WLPB-R; Woodcock, 1991). We included<br />

memory for sentences <strong>and</strong> listening<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> from the language composite.<br />

We included letter-word identificati<strong>on</strong> (real<br />

word reading), word attack (n<strong>on</strong>sense word<br />

reading), <strong>and</strong> passage comprehensi<strong>on</strong> from<br />

the reading composite. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> WLPB-R has good<br />

reliability (internal c<strong>on</strong>sistency ranged from<br />

.81–.92; test-retest ranged from .75 to .95).<br />

Adequate c<strong>on</strong>tent, c<strong>on</strong>current, predictive,<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct validity data are also reported in<br />

its technical manual.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Comprehensive Test of Ph<strong>on</strong>ological Processing<br />

(CTOPP; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte,<br />

1999). Five subtests of the CTOPP were<br />

used: Blending Words, Blending N<strong>on</strong>words,<br />

Segmenting Words, Sound Matching (first<br />

sound <strong>and</strong> last sound), <strong>and</strong> Rapid Letter Naming.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> CTOPP has good reliability (internal<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistency ranged from .83 to .95; test-retest<br />

ranged from .70 to .92). Adequate c<strong>on</strong>tent,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current, predictive, <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct validity<br />

data are also reported in its technical manual.<br />

Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE:<br />

Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999). Both<br />

subtests were administered: ph<strong>on</strong>emic decoding<br />

efficiency <strong>and</strong> sight word efficiency.<br />

Reliability coefficients are .95 <strong>and</strong> .96, respectively.<br />

Data <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent-descripti<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>current,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>struct identificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> item validity<br />

are reported in its technical manual.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>tinuous Progress M<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

In order to assess progress c<strong>on</strong>tinuously across<br />

a school year, we used Dynamic Indicators of<br />

Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS; Good &<br />

Kaminski, 2002). DIBELS measures are comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

used for collecting c<strong>on</strong>tinuous progress<br />

m<strong>on</strong>itoring data. We administered 4 subtests:<br />

Initial Sound Fluency (ISF), Ph<strong>on</strong>eme Segmentati<strong>on</strong><br />

Fluency (PSF), <strong>and</strong> N<strong>on</strong>sense<br />

Word Fluency (NWF). ISF, PSF, <strong>and</strong> NWF reliability<br />

coefficients range from .72 to .92 <strong>on</strong><br />

single probes <strong>and</strong> .91 to .98 <strong>on</strong> the means of<br />

multiple probes (3 to 5 probes). C<strong>on</strong>current<br />

<strong>and</strong> predictive validity with a variety of reading<br />

tests ranges from .36 to .82. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

Letter Naming Fluency (LNF) test was given at<br />

pre <strong>and</strong> posttest. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> alternative form reliability<br />

coefficient for LNF was .88. Validity coefficients<br />

for this measure ranged from .65 to .71.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> Overview<br />

Students in the interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> received<br />

approximately 40 to 50 minutes of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

daily in small groups of <strong>on</strong>e to four<br />

from <strong>on</strong>e of our six highly trained <strong>and</strong> supported<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> teachers across the durati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the study. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> was comprehensive,<br />

including systematic <strong>and</strong> explicit<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in multiple c<strong>on</strong>tent str<strong>and</strong>s (i.e.<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts of print, ph<strong>on</strong>ological <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

awareness, oral language, letter knowledge,<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, vocabulary, fluency,<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>) woven together so skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> strategies were integrated <strong>and</strong> applied in<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> built <strong>on</strong> a curriculum previously<br />

validated with students without ID,<br />

Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in Reading (Mathes &<br />

Torgesen, 2005a, 2005b), which was comprised<br />

of 240 less<strong>on</strong>s split evenly into two levels.<br />

However, students in the current research<br />

did not possess the prerequisite skills necessary<br />

to profit from this curriculum. Thus, we<br />

created an additi<strong>on</strong>al 60 less<strong>on</strong>s we called the<br />

Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level (Allor, Mathes, & J<strong>on</strong>es,<br />

2010). An oral language comp<strong>on</strong>ent was also<br />

developed <strong>and</strong> included in both the Foundati<strong>on</strong><br />

Level <strong>and</strong> Level One. In total, 300 less<strong>on</strong>s<br />

have been designed to take students from being<br />

n<strong>on</strong>readers with very little or no letter<br />

knowledge or ph<strong>on</strong>ological awareness to reading<br />

at approximately an ending 3 rd grade<br />

reading level. Since no students had yet begun<br />

Level Two at the time of the article, <strong>on</strong>ly details<br />

8 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


about the Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level <strong>and</strong> Level One are<br />

provided here.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> pre-test DIBELS scores, 13 students<br />

began the interventi<strong>on</strong> in the Foundati<strong>on</strong><br />

Level <strong>and</strong> three began in Level One. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> three<br />

students beginning in Level One were am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

the older students in the study (2 third graders<br />

<strong>and</strong> 1 fourth grader). Groups were determined<br />

by DIBELS pretest scores as well as<br />

other practical c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Thus, two of<br />

the 16 students were taught individually, while<br />

others were taught in groups of two to four.<br />

Grouping arrangements changed as needed,<br />

based <strong>on</strong> rate of progress.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Design <strong>and</strong> Features<br />

All of the less<strong>on</strong>s in the program were fullyspecified<br />

<strong>and</strong> employed the principles of Direct<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> (Carnine, Silbert, Kame’enui,<br />

& Tarver, 2004; Coyne, Kame’enui, & Simm<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

2001; Englemann, 1997; Englemann &<br />

Carnine, 1982; Kame’enui & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, 1990).<br />

We chose this model of instructi<strong>on</strong> because of<br />

its l<strong>on</strong>g st<strong>and</strong>ing record of success with various<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>s at-risk for school failure (Adams<br />

& Engelmann, 1996; Borman, Hewes, Overman,<br />

& Brown, 2003; Carls<strong>on</strong> & Francis, 2002;<br />

Ligas, 2002). Instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent was carefully<br />

analyzed <strong>and</strong> organized into a systematic<br />

scope <strong>and</strong> sequence intended to reduce student<br />

c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> target big ideas <strong>and</strong> key<br />

strategies. Errors were reduced through integrati<strong>on</strong><br />

of new learning with previous learning,<br />

<strong>on</strong>going review, <strong>and</strong> opportunities for<br />

group <strong>and</strong> individual resp<strong>on</strong>ding. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> goal<br />

was to integrate skills <strong>and</strong> strategies over time,<br />

resulting in a set of daily less<strong>on</strong> plans with<br />

overlapping c<strong>on</strong>tent str<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> extensive cumulative<br />

review <strong>and</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., c<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

of print, ph<strong>on</strong>ological <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness,<br />

letter knowledge, word recogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>nected text fluency, comprehensi<strong>on</strong> strategies,<br />

vocabulary, <strong>and</strong> oral language development).<br />

Following a behavioral approach, less<strong>on</strong>s<br />

provided for (a) frequent reinforcement<br />

<strong>on</strong> both an interval <strong>and</strong> intermittent schedule,<br />

(b) carefully orchestrated time delay techniques<br />

between stimuli presentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses, <strong>and</strong> (c) multiple opportunities<br />

to practice each item of c<strong>on</strong>tent.<br />

Each less<strong>on</strong> plan was highly detailed, providing<br />

exact wording to ensure teacher lan-<br />

guage was clear <strong>and</strong> kept to a minimum. By<br />

following these plans, teachers delivered explicit<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in integrated instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

str<strong>and</strong>s, resp<strong>on</strong>ding to individual student<br />

learning needs by scaffolding instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

when necessary. Thus, while less<strong>on</strong> plans were<br />

prescribed, the way in which less<strong>on</strong>s were actually<br />

delivered required teachers to make <strong>on</strong><br />

the spot decisi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> minor adjustments in<br />

the plans in order to focus <strong>on</strong> specific target<br />

areas needed by students within a group. Accompanying<br />

these less<strong>on</strong> plans, teachers were<br />

provided storybooks for read-alouds, pictures<br />

for vocabulary support, student activity books,<br />

magnetic pictures (Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level <strong>on</strong>ly),<br />

daily reading books using decodable stories<br />

(Level One <strong>on</strong>ly), a puppet with a fully articulated<br />

mouth, letter-sound picture cards, “automatic”<br />

word cards, <strong>and</strong> less<strong>on</strong> mastery tracking<br />

forms. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, the Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level<br />

included a game designed to provide students<br />

with opportunities to practice the PA skills of<br />

blending, segmenting, <strong>and</strong> letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

(See Allor et al., 2006 for details).<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Str<strong>and</strong>s<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cepts of print. During the Foundati<strong>on</strong><br />

Level, students developed various c<strong>on</strong>cepts of<br />

print. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se included pointing to the title <strong>and</strong><br />

author of a book, tracking from left to right,<br />

<strong>and</strong> pointing to individual words while repeating<br />

a sentence.<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>ological <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness. Activities<br />

in this str<strong>and</strong> spanned the Foundati<strong>on</strong><br />

Level <strong>and</strong> Level One <strong>and</strong> addressed skills al<strong>on</strong>g<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum of ph<strong>on</strong>ological <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

awareness, including clapping words in<br />

sentences, clapping syllables within a multisyllabic<br />

word, initial sound isolati<strong>on</strong>, ph<strong>on</strong>eme<br />

segmentati<strong>on</strong>, ph<strong>on</strong>eme blending, <strong>and</strong><br />

ph<strong>on</strong>eme discriminati<strong>on</strong>. Over time the complexity<br />

of words included in segmentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

blending activities increased.<br />

Letter knowledge. In this str<strong>and</strong>, students<br />

learned letter names <strong>and</strong> the sounds of individual<br />

letters <strong>and</strong> letter combinati<strong>on</strong>s, as well<br />

as worked <strong>on</strong> speeded retrieval (i.e., rapid<br />

automatic naming tasks). Starting in the 21 st<br />

less<strong>on</strong> of the Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level, students were<br />

taught to map ph<strong>on</strong>emes to letters, with new<br />

letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dences introduced ev-<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 9


ery few days <strong>and</strong> followed by daily cumulative<br />

review.<br />

Word recogniti<strong>on</strong>. This str<strong>and</strong> included<br />

both ph<strong>on</strong>etically regular <strong>and</strong> irregular words.<br />

Toward the end of the Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level, students<br />

were taught a small number of sight<br />

words; these words were high-frequency, ph<strong>on</strong>etically<br />

irregular words presented as tricky<br />

words to be recognized automatically. Students<br />

were also taught to decode very simple<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>etically regular words (i.e. closed syllable,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant-vowel-c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant: CVC) by<br />

blending the sounds represented by the letters.<br />

As students progressed through Level<br />

One, additi<strong>on</strong>al sight words were taught <strong>and</strong><br />

the time allowed to sound out the words was<br />

reduced, while the complexity of the words<br />

was increased (i.e. variant spelling patterns,<br />

blends, additi<strong>on</strong>al syllable types, <strong>and</strong> multisyllabic<br />

words). Students were also taught to be<br />

flexible decoders.<br />

Fluency with c<strong>on</strong>nected text. Beginning very<br />

early in Level One, word recogniti<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

were applied as students read decodable stories.<br />

As students acquired greater mastery of<br />

more elements, as well as the ability to decode<br />

more difficult words, this text became more<br />

challenging. To promote fluency, repeated<br />

reading of these stories was built into daily<br />

less<strong>on</strong>s. Typically students read a story in unis<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the first reading, followed by reading a<br />

page or two individually <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d reading.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> third reading was typically read in<br />

pairs, with the teacher timing the reading rate<br />

of <strong>on</strong>e student.<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> strategies. A major objective<br />

was for children to read strategically to increase<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing. Thus, prior to reading<br />

a story, students “browsed the story” looking at<br />

the pictures <strong>and</strong> predicting story c<strong>on</strong>tent. Students<br />

then read to find out if their predicti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were true. With expository text, teachers activated<br />

prior knowledge by asking students to<br />

tell what they already knew about the topic<br />

<strong>and</strong> to read to learn more. After reading the<br />

story, students then engaged in a number of<br />

activities depending <strong>on</strong> the students’ competence<br />

<strong>and</strong> text structure. Initially, students<br />

were <strong>on</strong>ly asked to tell about what they read.<br />

Informati<strong>on</strong> in any order was accepted. Over<br />

time, students sequenced informati<strong>on</strong> until<br />

they were able to sequence <strong>on</strong>ly the most important<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>. In later less<strong>on</strong>s, students<br />

identified story grammar elements for narrative<br />

text <strong>and</strong> new informati<strong>on</strong> learned in expository<br />

text.<br />

Vocabulary <strong>and</strong> oral language development.<br />

Language goals were addressed through storybook<br />

read-alouds, with direct teaching of<br />

spoken vocabulary <strong>and</strong> key background<br />

knowledge, as well as extensive discussi<strong>on</strong>. In<br />

the Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level, teachers explicitly taught<br />

vocabulary <strong>and</strong> engaged students in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong><br />

using open-ended questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> building<br />

<strong>on</strong> student language (Arnold & Whitehurst,<br />

1994). When students began Level One, the<br />

Storybook routine became more complex,<br />

with books organized into themes to facilitate<br />

vocabulary <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept review. One book was<br />

read from <strong>and</strong> discussed for 3 to 5 days, with<br />

two to three new vocabulary words taught<br />

each day. Students listened for <strong>and</strong> discussed<br />

the “target words” during the reading of the<br />

story. After the passage was read aloud, students<br />

provided an oral retell <strong>and</strong> discussed<br />

the story with the teacher who employed feedback<br />

<strong>and</strong> scaffolding to encourage the use of<br />

complete sentences <strong>and</strong> new vocabulary<br />

terms.<br />

Staff Development<br />

During the first year of the interventi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

teachers attended a total of six days of training<br />

<strong>on</strong> the interventi<strong>on</strong>, four at the beginning of<br />

the school year <strong>and</strong> two later in the school<br />

year. Teachers were visited by two experienced<br />

reading coaches every other m<strong>on</strong>th to<br />

address their individual needs <strong>and</strong> the needs<br />

of their students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> coaches were former<br />

teachers who had previously taught the Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in Reading (Mathes & Torgesen,<br />

2005a) curriculum under similar research<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Teachers also attended three<br />

meetings with the entire research team, including<br />

the coaches <strong>and</strong> lead research investigators<br />

who had created the curriculum.<br />

During the sec<strong>on</strong>d year, teachers participated<br />

in three days of training, two days at the<br />

beginning of the school year <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e day in<br />

the middle of the school year. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> number of<br />

coaching visits was reduced to two per semester.<br />

Research team meetings with the teachers<br />

were increased in frequency to <strong>on</strong>ce per<br />

m<strong>on</strong>th <strong>and</strong> focused <strong>on</strong> using student data to<br />

10 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 2<br />

Pretest Equivalencies<br />

Measure<br />

make instructi<strong>on</strong>al decisi<strong>on</strong>s, including both<br />

academic <strong>and</strong> behavioral modificati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Implementati<strong>on</strong> Fidelity <strong>and</strong> Intensity<br />

Three fidelity observati<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

each year to measure the degree to which the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was implemented. After each observati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the research assistants shared feedback<br />

with teachers. A 3-point rating scale was<br />

used to evaluate the fidelity of implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

across several categories including teaching<br />

to mastery, maintaining a good pace,<br />

maintaining student attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> providing<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> scaffolding. A score of 3<br />

indicated that the teacher implemented the<br />

category exactly as intended. A score of 2 indicated<br />

that the category was implemented<br />

acceptably but with some error. A score of 1<br />

indicated that the category was poorly represented.<br />

A score of 0 indicated that the behavior<br />

was expected but not observed. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> measure<br />

included a global checklist for readiness<br />

of materials, appropriate seating arrangement,<br />

<strong>and</strong> instructor warmth <strong>and</strong> enthusiasm.<br />

Interrater agreement was calculated <strong>and</strong> exceeded<br />

85%. Averaged across six fidelity ob-<br />

Treatment (n 16) C<strong>on</strong>trast (n 12)<br />

M SD M SD<br />

servati<strong>on</strong>s, teachers’ scores ranged from 2.29<br />

to 2.96 out of 3 with a mean of 2.75 (SD <br />

0.25). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean, calculated as a percentage<br />

score, was 90.9% (SD 8.63).<br />

Total instructi<strong>on</strong>al time for each student<br />

varied depending <strong>on</strong> when they began the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> attendance. As a result, instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for the students varied from 30 to 53<br />

weeks, with a mean of 42.8 weeks (SD <br />

10.34). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> average length of an instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> was 40 minutes (SD 6). Students<br />

participated in an average of 119 (SD 11)<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s during the study.<br />

Results<br />

t (1, 27) ns<br />

CTOPP<br />

Blending Words 2.00 3.50 .92 1.98 .96<br />

Blending N<strong>on</strong>words .88 1.78 .83 1.64 .86<br />

Segmenting Words .37 1.50 .00 .00 .86<br />

Sound Matching 2.00 2.45 1.50 4.30 .36<br />

EVT 34.94 13.28 30.08 13.85 .94<br />

PPVT 40.81 21.70 33.92 19.52 .87<br />

TOWRE<br />

Sight Word Efficiency 2.69 5.65 6.08 16.26 .78<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>emic Decoding Efficiency .38 1.50 2.42 8.37 .84<br />

WLPB<br />

Memory for Sentences 23.44 10.02 21.17 8.27 .64<br />

Listening Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> 2.81 3.25 1.25 1.55 1.54<br />

Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> 12.25 7.61 11.25 7.42 .35<br />

Passage Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> 2.62 3.30 1.58 2.28 .94<br />

Word Attack 1.00 2.85 .75 2.30 .25<br />

ns No significant differences found at .05 level <strong>on</strong> any measure<br />

Pretest Equivalence<br />

Pretest data were analyzed using independent<br />

t-tests. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se indicated no statistically significant<br />

differences between the treatment <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trast groups <strong>on</strong> any pretest measure. Pretest<br />

equivalency data are presented in Table 2.<br />

Growth from Pretest to Posttest<br />

Independent t-tests <strong>on</strong> difference scores of the<br />

pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest measures were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 11


TABLE 3<br />

Growth <strong>on</strong> Pretest to Posttest Measures<br />

Treatment (n 16) C<strong>on</strong>trast (n 12)<br />

Pretest Posttest Difference Pretest Posttest Difference<br />

t Effect Size<br />

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD<br />

Measure<br />

CTOPP<br />

Blending Words 2.00 3.50 4.44 4.94 2.44 4.66 .92 1.98 1.25 1.87 .33 1.61 1.68 .57<br />

Blending N<strong>on</strong>words .88 1.78 2.31 2.94 1.44 2.75 .83 1.64 .33 .89 .50 1.24 2.49 .87*<br />

Segmenting Words .37 1.50 3.06 3.87 2.69 3.75 .00 .00 .17 .58 .17 .58 2.64 .88*<br />

Sound Matching 2.00 2.45 4.69 3.93 2.69 2.91 1.50 4.30 2.33 2.81 .83 2.44 1.78 .68<br />

EVT 34.94 13.28 42.38 11.51 7.44 9.31 30.08 13.85 34.42 11.84 4.33 7.70 .94 .36<br />

PPVT 40.81 21.70 50.81 25.38 10.00 10.22 33.92 19.52 33.25 14.18 .67 19.64 1.87 .71*<br />

TOWRE<br />

Sight Word Efficiency 2.69 5.65 11.38 11.12 8.69 9.45 6.08 16.26 8.33 16.38 2.25 8.35 1.87 .72*<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>emic Decoding Efficiency .38 1.50 5.00 6.53 4.63 5.38 2.42 8.37 1.75 4.07 .67 5.23 2.61 1.00*<br />

WLPB-R<br />

Memory for Sentences 23.44 10.02 27.88 7.22 4.44 7.66 21.17 3.27 22.00 7.99 .83 5.87 1.36 .52<br />

Listening Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> 2.81 3.25 5.44 6.11 2.63 4.24 1.25 1.55 2.17 2.89 .92 2.35 1.25 .48<br />

Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> 12.25 7.61 18.75 7.72 6.50 4.05 11.25 7.42 14.00 8.11 2.75 3.38 2.60 .99*<br />

Passage Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> 2.62 3.30 5.31 3.93 2.69 2.63 1.58 2.28 2.67 2.93 1.08 1.83 1.81 .69*<br />

Word Attack 1.00 2.85 2.94 2.82 1.94 2.49 .75 2.30 1.08 2.94 .33 .78 2.15 .82*<br />

12 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010<br />

* p .05


to determine whether students in the treatment<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> made greater gains than students<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>trast c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Because of<br />

positive results in previous studies with Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in Reading, we anticipated the directi<strong>on</strong>ality<br />

of any differences (Mathes et al.,<br />

2005) <strong>and</strong>, therefore, we analyzed the data<br />

using a <strong>on</strong>e-tailed test of the null hypothesis<br />

(Gall, Gall, & Borg, 2007). T-test <strong>and</strong> effect<br />

size results are presented in Table 3. Statistically<br />

significant results were found <strong>on</strong> the following<br />

measures: CTOPP Blending N<strong>on</strong>words,<br />

CTOPP Segmenting Words, CTOPP<br />

Sound Matching, PPVT, TOWRE Sight Word<br />

Efficiency, TOWRE Ph<strong>on</strong>emic Decoding<br />

Efficiency, WLPB-R Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

WLPB-R Passage Comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

WLPB-R Word Attack. No statistically significant<br />

differences were found <strong>on</strong> CTOPP<br />

Blending Words, EVT, WLPB-R memory for<br />

sentences, <strong>and</strong> WLPB-R listening comprehensi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

although all means favored the treatment<br />

group <strong>and</strong> effect sizes were moderate to<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g.<br />

We also applied the B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

procedure because we employed multiple, related<br />

measures of various reading c<strong>on</strong>structs.<br />

This adjustment was made to help c<strong>on</strong>trol for<br />

Type I error (Dunn, 1961). We adjusted our<br />

critical p value by dividing .05 by the number<br />

of measures in a given c<strong>on</strong>struct, i.e. ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

awareness, ph<strong>on</strong>emic decoding, real word<br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong>, reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

oral language measures. After making this correcti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

differences <strong>on</strong> PPVT, TOWRE Sight<br />

Word Efficiency, <strong>and</strong> WLPB-R Passage Comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

were no l<strong>on</strong>ger statistically significant.<br />

Other findings remained the same. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

Analysis of Covariance tests were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> the gain scores using pretest<br />

measures as covariates. However, results were<br />

very similar to the t-test analyses, including<br />

significant findings <strong>on</strong> all of the same measures,<br />

as well as significant findings <strong>on</strong> CTOPP<br />

Blending Words, CTOPP Sound Matching,<br />

<strong>and</strong> WLPB-R Memory for Sentences. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore,<br />

these results were not included.<br />

Growth <strong>on</strong> C<strong>on</strong>tinuous Progress M<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

Measures<br />

We used a hierarchical linear modeling<br />

(HLM) approach to examine student gains <strong>on</strong><br />

the three DIBELS measures: initial sound fluency<br />

(ISF), ph<strong>on</strong>eme segmentati<strong>on</strong> fluency<br />

(PSF), <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>sense word fluency (NWF).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> advantage of HLM over simple regressi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

ANOVA, or repeated measures ANOVA<br />

is that it allows the researcher to look at hierarchically<br />

structured data <strong>and</strong> interpret results<br />

without ignoring these structures. This is accomplished<br />

by including a complex r<strong>and</strong>om<br />

part that can appropriately account for complex<br />

covariance structure in the data (Roberts,<br />

2004). In the present analysis, a two-level<br />

model was examined with measurement occasi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

at level-1 <strong>and</strong> students at level-2. Previously,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly t-tests were c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> gain<br />

scores to note differences between the treatment<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast groups. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se initial analyses<br />

were not performed in a HLM envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

because of a lack of power in the HLM<br />

design. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, independent samples<br />

t-tests were used because they were more appropriate<br />

<strong>and</strong> more parsim<strong>on</strong>ious. With the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuous progress m<strong>on</strong>itoring data, HLM<br />

growth curve analysis is more appropriate as it<br />

can model complex covariance structures <strong>and</strong><br />

effectively model explanatory variables that<br />

are able to mediate the changes in growth<br />

from student to student.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> HLM model investigated differences<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g the success of the interventi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with moderate ID (IQ scores between 40<br />

<strong>and</strong> 55). HLM combines the strength of simple<br />

ANOVA (mean difference analysis) <strong>and</strong><br />

regressi<strong>on</strong> (correlati<strong>on</strong>al analysis) to build a<br />

model that both c<strong>on</strong>siders differences across<br />

students (the sec<strong>on</strong>d-level or student-level)<br />

<strong>and</strong> incorporates a correlati<strong>on</strong>al comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

for each of these students (the time covariate).<br />

This assumpti<strong>on</strong> is fundamental to our<br />

analysis since we are hypothesizing that bel<strong>on</strong>ging<br />

to the interventi<strong>on</strong> group <strong>and</strong> the<br />

cross-level interacti<strong>on</strong> between time <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(a student-level variable) will have an<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> growth.<br />

For each of the three dependent variables,<br />

PSF, ISF, <strong>and</strong> NWF, two models were tested.<br />

Because these models included interacti<strong>on</strong> effects,<br />

it was important to code time with a<br />

meaningful zero (Hox, 2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore,<br />

time was centered with zero being the day that<br />

a student began the program <strong>and</strong> increasing<br />

numbers representing the number of weeks<br />

the student was involved in the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 13


TABLE 4<br />

Model Fit Estimates for Initial Sound Fluency with Students with IQs in the Moderate Range<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first model included <strong>on</strong>ly the time covariate<br />

with r<strong>and</strong>om effects for the intercept <strong>and</strong><br />

for the time variable <strong>and</strong> was presented as:<br />

y ti 00 10 * time u 0i u 1i * time e ti,<br />

(1)<br />

where y ti is the dependent variable for that<br />

model, 00 is the average fluency at time 0,<br />

10 is the average student increase in fluency<br />

for each week since the program began, u 0i is<br />

the r<strong>and</strong>om effect of 00, u 1i is the r<strong>and</strong>om<br />

effect for 10, <strong>and</strong> e ti is the r<strong>and</strong>om effect of<br />

the measurement occasi<strong>on</strong>s within individual<br />

students.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d model included a student-level<br />

(level-2) effect to identify whether or not the<br />

student was in the interventi<strong>on</strong> or c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group. In this model, the intercept 00 has a<br />

slightly different interpretati<strong>on</strong> than in the<br />

first model as it now represents the average<br />

fluency for a student in the c<strong>on</strong>trol group at<br />

time0 (the interventi<strong>on</strong> group receives a “1”<br />

for the level-2 grouping variable). This new<br />

model is:<br />

yti 00 10 time 01 group<br />

11 time group u0i u1itime eti, (2)<br />

where 01 is the effect of a student bel<strong>on</strong>g-<br />

M 0: Null model M 1 : group & interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

estimate s.e. p-value estimate s. e. p-value<br />

Fixed Effects:<br />

Intercept 00 5.000 1.326 0.001 5.579 2.060 0.007<br />

Time 10 0.238 0.046 0.001 0.142 0.066 0.033<br />

Group 01 1.026 2.735 0.711<br />

Time*Group 11 R<strong>and</strong>om Effects:<br />

0.167 0.088 0.058<br />

2 e 30.706 30.746<br />

2 u0 38.957 41.130<br />

2 u1 0.040 0.034<br />

COV (u0.u1) Fit:<br />

0.017 0.041<br />

AIC 1678.826 1678.642<br />

BIC 1699.882 1706.652<br />

2<br />

1666.826 1662.642<br />

ing to the interventi<strong>on</strong> group at time 0 <strong>and</strong><br />

11 represents the cross-level interacti<strong>on</strong> between<br />

time <strong>and</strong> the interventi<strong>on</strong> effect.<br />

Hox (2002) has noted that it is typical to<br />

include both of the main effects in a model in<br />

the presence of a statistically significant interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

effect. As can be seen from the results<br />

of our three models in Tables 4–6, the model<br />

structures are the same across all four analyses.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>ly change between each analysis<br />

was the dependent variable.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> analysis in Table 4 represents the effect<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> ISF across time for<br />

students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was no statistical difference<br />

between the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast groups<br />

at the initial time-point (1.026, p 0.711).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interacti<strong>on</strong> effect tested to see whether or<br />

not the amount of difference between the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> group <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast group<br />

changed over time. For example, a large positive<br />

value for 11 would mean that students<br />

involved in the interventi<strong>on</strong> tended to have<br />

larger gains in ISF over the c<strong>on</strong>trast group<br />

students the l<strong>on</strong>ger they were involved in the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. In this analysis, however, the<br />

value for this interacti<strong>on</strong> (0.167) was not statistically<br />

significant over time (p 0.058) indicating<br />

that students in the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol groups tended to have the same rate<br />

of change over time.<br />

14 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 5<br />

Model Fit Estimates for Ph<strong>on</strong>eme Segmentati<strong>on</strong> Fluency with Students with IQs in the Moderate Range<br />

Table 5 shows the effect of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> PSF across time for students. As can be<br />

seen from this analysis, there was no statistical<br />

difference between the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast<br />

groups at the initial time-point (0.199,<br />

p 0.927), thus indicating that they were<br />

statistically equivalent in terms of PSF when<br />

the program began. Also in model M1, the<br />

M 0: Null model M 1 : group & interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

estimate s.e. p-value estimate s. e. p-value<br />

Fixed Effects:<br />

Intercept 00 0.681 1.032 0.509 0.883 1.608 0.584<br />

Time 10 0.369 0.066 .001 0.124 0.081 0.130<br />

Group 01 0.199 2.139 0.927<br />

Time*Group 11 R<strong>and</strong>om Effects:<br />

0.417 0.108 .001<br />

2 e 27.819 27.832<br />

2 u0 20.332 22.102<br />

2 u1 0.107 0.062<br />

COV (u0.u1) Fit:<br />

0.231 0.247<br />

AIC 1666.922 1657.986<br />

BIC 1687.979 1685.996<br />

2<br />

1654.922 1641.986<br />

TABLE 6<br />

value for the interacti<strong>on</strong> effect (0.417) was<br />

statistically significant over time (p .001)<br />

thus indicating that students in the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

group tended to have a larger rate of<br />

growth in PSF over time than did the students<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group.<br />

Table 6 shows the effect of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> students’ NWF across time. Again, there<br />

Model Fit Estimates for N<strong>on</strong>sense Word Fluency with Students with IQs in the Moderate Range<br />

M 0: Null model M 1 : group & interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

estimate s.e. p-value estimate s. e. p-value<br />

Fixed Effects:<br />

Intercept 00 2.786 1.794 0.122 4.845 2.668 0.071<br />

Time 10 0.327 0.065 0.001 0.140 0.085 0.102<br />

Group 01 3.725 3.586 0.309<br />

Time*Group 11 R<strong>and</strong>om Effects:<br />

0.337 0.114 0.003<br />

2 e 37.252 37.266<br />

2 u0 73.907 73.004<br />

2 u1 0.088 0.064<br />

COV (u0.u1) Fit:<br />

0.455 0.828<br />

AIC 1673.274 1666.205<br />

BIC 1694.057 1693.847<br />

2<br />

1661.274 1650.205<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 15


was no statistical difference between the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast groups at the initial timepoint<br />

(3.725, p 0.309), thus indicating<br />

that they were statistically equivalent in terms<br />

of NWF when the program began. Also in<br />

model M1, the value for the interacti<strong>on</strong> effect<br />

(0.337) was statistically significant over time<br />

(p .003) thus indicating that students in the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> group tended to have a larger<br />

rate of growth in NWF over time than did the<br />

students in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group. It should be<br />

noted that <strong>on</strong>e student in the treatment group<br />

was excluded from this analysis because the<br />

student began the study above benchmark<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintained scores above benchmark.<br />

Graphs of scores for individual students <strong>on</strong><br />

PSF <strong>and</strong> NWF are presented in Figures 1 <strong>and</strong><br />

2. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> graphs <strong>on</strong> the left show the scores for<br />

the 12 students in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group; the<br />

graphs <strong>on</strong> the right show the scores for the 16<br />

students in the treatment group.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

In this article, we report the results from a<br />

l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study examining the effectiveness<br />

of a comprehensive early literacy interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with moderate intellectual disabilities<br />

(ID). This article reports <strong>on</strong> student<br />

progress after participating in the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

or typical special educati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e to <strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> a half years. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose<br />

of the study is to determine if students participating<br />

in the interventi<strong>on</strong> make significantly<br />

more progress <strong>on</strong> a variety of reading <strong>and</strong><br />

language measures than similar students participating<br />

in typical special educati<strong>on</strong>. Our<br />

outcomes str<strong>on</strong>gly support the effectiveness of<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> with students with moderate<br />

ID. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings are discussed in detail below.<br />

Research Questi<strong>on</strong>: Does a comprehensive reading<br />

program taught to primary-grade students who have<br />

moderate ID (IQs ranging from 40-55) result in<br />

better early reading outcomes than typical special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> measures of (a) ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

awareness (PA), (b) ph<strong>on</strong>ics, (c) word recogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (d) oral language/comprehensi<strong>on</strong>? <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> answer<br />

to this questi<strong>on</strong> is clearly yes. On all<br />

outcome measures, means favored the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

group, with moderate to str<strong>on</strong>g ESs<strong>on</strong><br />

all measures. Despite low statistical power due<br />

to the small sample size (16 in the interven-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> 12 in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group), statistically<br />

significant differences were found <strong>on</strong> multiple<br />

measures, including measures of ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

awareness, ph<strong>on</strong>ics, word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, vocabulary,<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> clearest, <strong>and</strong> arguably <strong>on</strong>e of the most<br />

important findings in the study were <strong>on</strong> measures<br />

of ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness (PA). Students<br />

participating in the interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

outperformed students in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group<br />

<strong>on</strong> measures of PA. Effect sizes <strong>on</strong> the four<br />

CTOPP subtests (gains from pretest to posttest)<br />

ranged from a moderate effect of .57 to a<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g effect of .88. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> differences <strong>on</strong> both<br />

Blending N<strong>on</strong>words <strong>and</strong> Segmenting Words<br />

were statistically significant (See Table 3). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

these differences remained significant<br />

after the B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i correcti<strong>on</strong>. Although<br />

differences <strong>on</strong> CTOPP Blending<br />

Words <strong>and</strong> Sound Matching were not statistically<br />

significant, effect sizes were moderate to<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g (.57 <strong>and</strong> .68, respectively). Results from<br />

the HLM analysis also revealed that the students<br />

in the interventi<strong>on</strong> group tended to<br />

have a higher rate of growth <strong>on</strong> DIBELS-PSF<br />

over time, with this interacti<strong>on</strong> statistically significant<br />

(p .001; see Table 5 <strong>and</strong> Figure 1).<br />

This finding is particularly compelling because<br />

it indicates that students in the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

group c<strong>on</strong>sistently outperformed students<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group over a l<strong>on</strong>g period<br />

of time. Unexpectedly, the same pattern of<br />

results was not evident <strong>on</strong> DIBELS-ISF, but<br />

this was likely because students with ID found<br />

the language <strong>and</strong> cognitive dem<strong>and</strong>s of the<br />

task challenging, preventing them from dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

their ability to isolate ph<strong>on</strong>emes.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sistent differences in favor of the treatment<br />

group were also evident <strong>on</strong> multiple<br />

measures of alphabetic decoding. Effect sizes<br />

<strong>on</strong> n<strong>on</strong>sense word reading measures, TOWRE<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>emic decoding <strong>and</strong> WLPB word attack,<br />

were 1.0 <strong>and</strong> .66, respectively, with statistically<br />

significant differences <strong>on</strong> the former measure.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se differences remained significant after<br />

the B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

HLM analyses of NWF measures<br />

across time revealed a statistically significant<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> in favor of the treatment group (p<br />

.003; See Table 6 <strong>and</strong> Figure 2).<br />

Data also indicate that students in the treatment<br />

group c<strong>on</strong>sistently made more growth<br />

<strong>on</strong> word recogniti<strong>on</strong> (i.e., real word reading)<br />

16 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Figure 1. Individual Graphs <strong>on</strong> Ph<strong>on</strong>eme Segmentati<strong>on</strong> Fluency.<br />

than students in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group. Two measures<br />

directly assessed this skill, TOWRE Sight<br />

Word Efficiency <strong>and</strong> WLPB-R Letter-Word<br />

Identificati<strong>on</strong>. Differences <strong>on</strong> these measures<br />

were statistically significant <strong>and</strong> ESs were<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g (.72 <strong>on</strong> TOWRE-word reading efficiency<br />

<strong>and</strong> .99 <strong>on</strong> WLPB-R Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

Outcomes for oral language <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

were also positive. Effect sizes were<br />

moderate to str<strong>on</strong>g <strong>on</strong> language measures,<br />

ranging from .36 to .71, with significant differences<br />

<strong>on</strong> receptive vocabulary (PPVT). Differences<br />

<strong>on</strong> passage comprehensi<strong>on</strong> were statistically<br />

significant. (See Table 3.)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

This study provides clear support for raising<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s related to reading for students<br />

Figure 2. Individual Graphs <strong>on</strong> N<strong>on</strong>sense Word Fluency.<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 17


with moderate ID. Students with moderate ID<br />

should not be left behind; they should be<br />

provided with scientifically-based reading instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> findings of this study str<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

support the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that students with<br />

moderate IDs can make important gains in<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> language skills when provided<br />

with intensive <strong>and</strong> comprehensive instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

over an extended period of time. A broad<br />

array of measures was studied, including PA,<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics, word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, comprehensi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> oral language. ESs <strong>on</strong> all measures were<br />

moderate to str<strong>on</strong>g, with means c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

favoring the interventi<strong>on</strong> group. Statistically<br />

significant differences were found <strong>on</strong> multiple<br />

measures, including ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness,<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics, word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with existing<br />

research <strong>and</strong> extend that research in several<br />

ways.<br />

First, explicit, systematic instructi<strong>on</strong> in PA<br />

<strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics that has proven to be effective for<br />

students with IQs in the average range (Ehri et<br />

al., 2001; Mathes et al., 2005) is also effective<br />

for students with moderate ID. Prior research<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching PA <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics to students with<br />

ID focused <strong>on</strong> those with mild ID <strong>and</strong> was<br />

limited to relatively brief instructi<strong>on</strong>al periods<br />

targeting isolated skills (Joseph & Seery, 2004;<br />

O’C<strong>on</strong>nor et al., 1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current study<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strates that with an integrated <strong>and</strong> systematic<br />

approach, students with moderate ID<br />

can successfully combine isolated skills in PA<br />

<strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics to decode unfamiliar words.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, this study is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with previous<br />

research dem<strong>on</strong>strating the effectiveness<br />

of systematic approaches in improving sight<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong> (Browder et al., 2006). In<br />

this study sight word instructi<strong>on</strong> was <strong>on</strong>e comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

of the comprehensive reading program<br />

implemented. Effect sizes <strong>on</strong> measures<br />

of sight word recogniti<strong>on</strong> were high <strong>and</strong> differences<br />

between the treatment <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trast<br />

groups were statistically significant.<br />

Third, we found that a comprehensive reading<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> can positively impact oral language<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. With moderate<br />

ESs <strong>on</strong> oral language measures <strong>and</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g,<br />

statistically significant differences <strong>on</strong> reading<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> receptive vocabulary, the<br />

current study extends previous research that<br />

had dem<strong>on</strong>strated <strong>on</strong>ly very basic, isolated<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skills (Browder, 2006). As is<br />

similar in research with students without ID, it<br />

is likely that gains in comprehensi<strong>on</strong> are<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly influenced by gains in word recogniti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It appeared that the students in the treatment<br />

group were able to identify more words<br />

than the students in the c<strong>on</strong>trast group, enabling<br />

them to answer a few basic comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

items <strong>on</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ardized measure.<br />

Fourth, the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal design of this study<br />

provides informati<strong>on</strong> about the level of reading<br />

performance that can be expected after<br />

<strong>on</strong>e to <strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> a half years of c<strong>on</strong>sistent instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in a comprehensive reading program.<br />

Eight of the 16 students in the treatment<br />

group were approximately halfway<br />

through Level One or further. At this level,<br />

students were able to identify the most comm<strong>on</strong><br />

sound for all individual letters <strong>and</strong> read<br />

words made up of those letters. For example,<br />

students were able to successfully say the<br />

sounds in words such as last, mom, slip, <strong>and</strong><br />

step, as well as blend those sounds together to<br />

form the word. Further, students at this level<br />

were working <strong>on</strong> basic comprehensi<strong>on</strong> strategies,<br />

such as retelling stories, sequencing main<br />

events, <strong>and</strong> story grammar. Generally, students<br />

in this study took approximately twice<br />

the amount of time to successfully complete<br />

less<strong>on</strong>s than struggling readers in previous<br />

studies. Further, a closer look at the graphs in<br />

Figures 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 reveals that gains <strong>on</strong> DIBELS<br />

measures of PA <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics (PSF <strong>and</strong> NWF)<br />

were typically not evident until students had<br />

been participating in the interventi<strong>on</strong> for approximately<br />

15 to 20 weeks of instructi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

time needed to evidence gain was much<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger in durati<strong>on</strong> than is typical of struggling<br />

readers without ID. Thus, while the c<strong>on</strong>tent of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for both groups is the same, what<br />

differentiates them is the persistence needed<br />

<strong>on</strong> the part of schools to provide this instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Practical Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> findings of this study have important<br />

practical implicati<strong>on</strong>s for educators in the<br />

field of intellectual disabilities. First, <strong>and</strong> most<br />

importantly, our findings support educators<br />

who choose to provide reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

that is comprehensive <strong>and</strong> not limited to sight<br />

word memorizati<strong>on</strong>, even with students with<br />

IQs in the moderate range. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, reading<br />

18 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


programs should be selected that are c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with the techniques of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

described in this study, including (a) systematic,<br />

explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> in all comp<strong>on</strong>ents of<br />

reading; (b) repetitive, routine activities implemented<br />

with c<strong>on</strong>sistent instructi<strong>on</strong>al language;<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) fast-paced, short activities that<br />

are highly motivating. Third, to be effective<br />

with students with ID, programs must be implemented<br />

with extremely high degrees of fidelity.<br />

This requires initial <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>going professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development. Fourth, practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

need to make data-based decisi<strong>on</strong>s about how<br />

to modify instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> provide positive behavioral<br />

support. In additi<strong>on</strong> to using existing<br />

progress m<strong>on</strong>itoring measures, such as<br />

DIBELS, observati<strong>on</strong> of student performance<br />

during less<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> other informal measures is<br />

key to making appropriate decisi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

One limitati<strong>on</strong> of the current study is the<br />

variability of student performance <strong>on</strong> outcome<br />

measures, as is comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g students<br />

with IDs. We met the challenge of eliciting<br />

optimal performance from our students<br />

<strong>on</strong> study measures by ensuring familiarity of<br />

examiners <strong>and</strong> disc<strong>on</strong>tinuing testing when<br />

necessary. We also addressed this issue by including<br />

repeated measures across time, when<br />

possible. This enabled us to employ data analytic<br />

techniques (i.e., HLM) that analyzed<br />

trends across time <strong>and</strong> minimized the impact<br />

of variability of the data. Due to this limitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

findings related to measures <strong>on</strong>ly administered<br />

at pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest should be interpreted<br />

cautiously. Further research is needed<br />

to develop reading <strong>and</strong> language tests that use<br />

repeated measures of progress, especially untimed<br />

measures as existing repeated measures<br />

are usually timed.<br />

Another limitati<strong>on</strong> of the study is the small<br />

sample size. This is a comm<strong>on</strong> problem when<br />

studying low-incidence populati<strong>on</strong>s because it<br />

is logistically challenging <strong>and</strong> resource-intensive<br />

to increase sample size with a low-incidence<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>. Even after carefully selecting<br />

schools with as many students with<br />

moderate IDs as possible <strong>and</strong> with the additi<strong>on</strong><br />

of a school that focuses <strong>on</strong> students with<br />

IDs, our sample remained quite small for a<br />

group design study. This is problematic be-<br />

cause it increases the probability of Type II<br />

error <strong>and</strong> it is possible that significant differences<br />

between the groups <strong>on</strong> some measures<br />

were not detected simply because of the small<br />

sample size. A competing limitati<strong>on</strong> is that by<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducting multiple t-tests <strong>on</strong> related measures<br />

we increased the possibility of Type I<br />

error. We addressed this limitati<strong>on</strong> by applying<br />

the B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure. Our<br />

findings held up under the scrutiny of this<br />

c<strong>on</strong>servative procedure.<br />

Further research is needed to address multiple<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s related to teaching students<br />

with ID to read. One need is further explorati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between IQ <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

to reading instructi<strong>on</strong>. Currently, we<br />

are examining this issue with our larger study<br />

in which we are following the progress of students<br />

with IQs ranging from 40 to 79 over four<br />

academic years. In that study, we are also addressing<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong> of the level of reading<br />

competence that can be achieved by students<br />

with low IQs. In this article, language measures<br />

were administered <strong>on</strong>ly at pretest <strong>and</strong><br />

posttest. Further analyses of language measures,<br />

especially measures across time, are also<br />

needed. Given the variability of student performance,<br />

language measures that can be administered<br />

frequently would be useful for research<br />

<strong>and</strong> for teachers to use in their<br />

classrooms for <strong>on</strong>going progress m<strong>on</strong>itoring.<br />

Finally, further research is needed to determine<br />

progress over a l<strong>on</strong>ger period of time,<br />

especially <strong>on</strong> measures of advanced reading,<br />

including fluency <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Summary<br />

In summary, students with moderate IDs can<br />

learn basic reading skills given c<strong>on</strong>sistent, explicit,<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

across a l<strong>on</strong>g period of time. Success requires<br />

that we apply key instructi<strong>on</strong>al features that<br />

have been dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective with<br />

struggling readers with average IQs, as well as<br />

techniques known to be effective for students<br />

with IDs. Teachers must be provided with upto-date<br />

materials <strong>and</strong> extensive professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued support in order<br />

to implement research-based instructi<strong>on</strong> with<br />

high degrees of fidelity. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, teachers<br />

must m<strong>on</strong>itor student progress in order to<br />

make academic <strong>and</strong> behavioral modificati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 19


needed to ensure success. Teachers also need<br />

access to coaches with expertise in reading.<br />

Although we hope this study raises expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for students with IDs, particularly moderate<br />

IDs, we also wish to emphasize that providing<br />

effective reading instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

students with IDs is extremely challenging.<br />

Finally, we need to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to explore what is<br />

possible for students with ID if they are provided<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent, comprehensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for an extended period of time.<br />

References<br />

Adams, G., & Engelmann, S. (1996). Research <strong>on</strong><br />

direct instructi<strong>on</strong>: 25 years bey<strong>on</strong>d DISTAR. Seattle,<br />

WA: Educati<strong>on</strong>al Achievement Systems<br />

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking <strong>and</strong><br />

learning about print. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts<br />

Institute of Technology.<br />

Allor, J. H., Gansle, K. A., & Denny, R. K. (2006).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> stop <strong>and</strong> go ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness game: Providing<br />

modeling, practice, <strong>and</strong> feedback. Preventing<br />

School Failure, 50(4), 23–30.<br />

Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., & J<strong>on</strong>es, F. G. (2010).<br />

Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in Reading: Foundati<strong>on</strong> Level. Unpublished<br />

manuscript.<br />

Allor, J. H., Roberts, J. K., Mathes, P. G., Roid, G., &<br />

Cheatham, J. P. (2010). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between<br />

IQ <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se to interventi<strong>on</strong>: An experimental<br />

examinati<strong>on</strong>. Unpublished manuscript.<br />

Arnold, D. S., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1994). Accelerating<br />

language development through picture<br />

reading: A summary of dialogic reading <strong>and</strong> its<br />

effects. In D. K. Dickins<strong>on</strong> (Ed.), Bridges to literacy:<br />

Children, families, <strong>and</strong> school (pp. 102–128).Cambridge,<br />

MA: Blackwell.<br />

Blaiklock, K. E. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> importance of letter<br />

knowledge in the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

awareness <strong>and</strong> reading. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Research in<br />

Reading, 27, 36–57.<br />

Borman, G., Hewes, G. M., Overman, L. T., &<br />

Brown, S. (2003). Comprehensive school reform<br />

<strong>and</strong> achievement: A meta-analysis. Review of Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Research, 73, 125–230.<br />

Browder, D. M., Wakeman, S. Y., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell,<br />

L., & Algozzine, B. (2006). Research<br />

<strong>on</strong> reading instructi<strong>on</strong> for individuals with significant<br />

cognitive disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 72,<br />

392–408.<br />

Browder, D. M., & Xin, Y. P. (1998). A meta-analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> review of sight word research <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al reading to individuals<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 32, 130–153.<br />

Carls<strong>on</strong>, C. D., & Francis, D. (2002). Increasing the<br />

reading achievement of as-risk children through<br />

direct instructi<strong>on</strong>: Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the Rodeo Institute<br />

for Teacher Excellence (RITE). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> for Students Placed at Risk, 7, 141–166.<br />

Carnine, D. W., Silbert, J., Kame’enui, E. J., &<br />

Tarver, S. G. (2004). Direct instructi<strong>on</strong> reading (4th<br />

ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pears<strong>on</strong> Merrill<br />

Prentice Hall.<br />

Chall, J. S. (1996). Stages of reading development (2nd<br />

ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ners, F. A. (2003). Reading skills <strong>and</strong> cognitive<br />

abilities of individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. In<br />

L. Abbeduto (Ed.), Internati<strong>on</strong>al review of research<br />

in mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (Vol. 27, pp. 191–229). San<br />

Diego: San Diego Academic Press.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ners, F. A., Rosenquist, C. J., Sligh, A. C., Atwell,<br />

J. A., & Kiser, T. (2006). Ph<strong>on</strong>ological reading<br />

skills acquisiti<strong>on</strong> by children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 121–<br />

137.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ners, F. A. (1992). Reading instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with moderate mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Review<br />

<strong>and</strong> analysis of research. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 96, 577–597.<br />

Coyne, M. D., Kame’enui, E. J., & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, D. C.<br />

(2001). Preventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> in beginning<br />

reading: Two complex systems. Learning Disabilities<br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Practice, 16, 62–73.<br />

Dent<strong>on</strong>, C. A., & Mathes, P. G. (2003). Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for struggling readers: Possibilities <strong>and</strong> challenges.<br />

In B.R. Foorman (Ed.), Preventing <strong>and</strong><br />

remediating reading difficulties: Bringing science to<br />

scale (pp. 229–251). Tim<strong>on</strong>ium, MD: York Press.<br />

Dickins<strong>on</strong>, D. K., & Smith, M. W. (1994). L<strong>on</strong>g-term<br />

effects of preschool teachers’ book readings <strong>on</strong><br />

low-income children’s vocabulary <strong>and</strong> story comprehensi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 105–<br />

122.<br />

Dunn, O. J. (1961). Multiple comparis<strong>on</strong>s am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

means, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the American Statistical Associati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

56, 52–64.<br />

Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture<br />

Vocabulary Test (3 rd ed). Circle<br />

Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory,<br />

findings, <strong>and</strong> issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9,<br />

167–188.<br />

Ehri, L. C., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word<br />

learning: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for instructi<strong>on</strong> with delayed<br />

<strong>and</strong> disabled readers. Reading & Writing<br />

Quarterly, 14, 135.<br />

Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster,<br />

B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001).<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>emic awareness instructi<strong>on</strong> that helps children<br />

learn to read: Evidence from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research<br />

Quarterly, 36, 250–287.<br />

Engelmann, S. (1997). Preventing failure in the pri-<br />

20 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


mary grades. Eugene, OR: Associati<strong>on</strong> for Direct<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Engelmann, S., & Carnine, D. W. (1982). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>: Principles <strong>and</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong>s. New York:<br />

Irvingt<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Evans, M. A., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page,<br />

J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds, <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

awareness: An examinati<strong>on</strong> of kindergarten<br />

children across letters <strong>and</strong> of letters across<br />

children. Reading <strong>and</strong> Writing 19, 959–989.<br />

Foorman, B. R., & Torgesen, J. (2001). Critical elements<br />

of classroom <strong>and</strong> small-group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

promote reading success in all children. Learning<br />

Disabilities Research & Practice, 16, 203–212.<br />

Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2006). Changes in letter<br />

sound knowledge are associated with development<br />

of ph<strong>on</strong>ological awareness in pre-school<br />

children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Research in Reading, 29, 143–<br />

161.<br />

Gall, J. P., Gall, M. D., & Borg, W. R. (2007). Applying<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al research: A practical guide (8th ed.).<br />

Bost<strong>on</strong>: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>. Good, R. H., & Kaminski,<br />

R. A. (2002). Dynamic indicators of basic early<br />

literacy skills, 6 th editi<strong>on</strong>. http://dibels.uoreg<strong>on</strong>.<br />

edu/<br />

Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques <strong>and</strong><br />

applicati<strong>on</strong>s. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Joseph, L. M., & Seery, M. E. (2004). Where is the<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics? A review of the literature <strong>on</strong> the use of<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>etic analysis with students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 25(2), 88–<br />

94.<br />

Kame’enui, E. J., & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, D. C. (1990). Designing<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies: Preventi<strong>on</strong> of academic<br />

learning problems. Columbus, OH: Merrill.<br />

Karweit, N., & Wasik, B. A. (1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of<br />

story reading programs <strong>on</strong> literacy <strong>and</strong> language<br />

development of disadvantaged preschoolers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong> for Students Placed at Risk, 1, 319–<br />

348.<br />

Katims, D. S. (2000). Literacy instructi<strong>on</strong> for people<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Historical highlights <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>temporary analysis. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

35, 3–15<br />

Katims, D. S. (2001). Literacy assessment of students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An exploratory investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities 36, 363–371.<br />

Ligas, M. (2002). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of Broward County<br />

alliance of quality schools project. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Students Placed at Risk, 7, 117–139.<br />

Mathes, P. G., & Dent<strong>on</strong>, C. A. (2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> preventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> of reading disability. Seminars<br />

in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 185–191.<br />

Mathes, P. G., Dent<strong>on</strong>, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anth<strong>on</strong>y,<br />

J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatschneider, C.<br />

(2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of theoretically different in-<br />

structi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> student characteristics <strong>on</strong> the skills<br />

of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly,<br />

40, 148–182.<br />

Mathes, P. G., & Torgesen, J. K. (2005a) Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in Reading, Level 1. Columbus, OH: SRA/<br />

McGraw-Hill.<br />

Mathes, P. G., & Torgesen, J. K. (2005b) Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in Reading, Level 2. Columbus, OH: SRA/<br />

McGraw-Hill.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to<br />

read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research<br />

literature <strong>on</strong> reading <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

reading instructi<strong>on</strong>. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute<br />

of Child Health <strong>and</strong> Human Development.<br />

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No.<br />

107–110, 115 Stat. 1425 (2002).<br />

O’C<strong>on</strong>nor, R. E., Notari-Syvers<strong>on</strong>, A., & Vadasy, P. F.<br />

(1996). Ladders to literacy: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of teacherled<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ological activities for kindergarten children<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

63, 117–130.<br />

Perfetti, C. A., L<strong>and</strong>i, N., & Oakhill, J. (2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skill. In<br />

M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> science of<br />

reading: A h<strong>and</strong>book (pp. 248–266). Malden, MA:<br />

Blackwell.<br />

Pressley, M. P. (1998). Elementary reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

that works. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, A., Pesetsky, D.,<br />

& Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological<br />

science informs the teaching of reading [M<strong>on</strong>ograph].<br />

Psychological Science, 2, 31–74.<br />

Roberts, T. A. (2003). Effects of alphabet-letter instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> young children’s word recogniti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 95, 41–51.<br />

Roberts, J. K. (2004). An introductory primer <strong>on</strong><br />

multilevel <strong>and</strong> hierarchical linear modeling.<br />

Learning Disabilities: A C<strong>on</strong>temporary <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 2(1),<br />

30–38.<br />

Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews,<br />

R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in<br />

reading acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology,<br />

76, 1309–1324.<br />

Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.).<br />

(1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy Press.<br />

Torgesen, J. K. (2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> preventi<strong>on</strong> of reading<br />

difficulties. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of School Psychology, 40, 7.<br />

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A.<br />

(1999). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-Ed. Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Richm<strong>on</strong>d-<br />

Welty, E. D. (1996). Letter names help children<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>nect print <strong>and</strong> speech. Developmental Psychology,<br />

32, 505–514.<br />

Treiman, R., Tincoff, K. R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki,<br />

A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> foundati<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

literacy: learning the sounds of letters. Child Development,<br />

69, 1524–1540.<br />

Comprehensive Reading Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 21


Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992).<br />

Accelerating language development through picture<br />

book reading: A systematic extensi<strong>on</strong> to Mexican<br />

day care. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1106–<br />

1114.<br />

Vaughn, S., Mathes, P., Linan-Thomps<strong>on</strong>, S., Cirino,<br />

P., Carls<strong>on</strong>, C., Pollard-Durodola, S., et al. (2006).<br />

Effectiveness of an English interventi<strong>on</strong> for firstgrade<br />

English language learners at risk for reading<br />

problems. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Elementary School <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>,<br />

107(2), 153–180.<br />

Vaughn, S., Cirino, P. T., Linan-Thomps<strong>on</strong>, S.,<br />

Mathes, P. G., Carls<strong>on</strong>, C. D., Cardenas-Hagan, et<br />

al. (2006). Effectiveness of a Spanish interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> an English interventi<strong>on</strong> for English-Language<br />

Learners at risk for reading problems. American Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Research <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 43, 449–479.<br />

Vaughn, S., Linan-Thomps<strong>on</strong>, Mathes, P. G. Duradola,<br />

S., & Cárdenas-Hagan, E. (2007). Firstgrade<br />

English language learners at-risk for reading<br />

problems: Effectiveness of an English<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. Elementary School <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 107, 153–180.<br />

Wagner, R., Torgesen, J. & Rashotte, C. (1999).<br />

Comprehensive Test of Ph<strong>on</strong>ological Processing. Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-ED.<br />

Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test.<br />

Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency<br />

Battery-Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.<br />

Received:I 4 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 12 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 30 March 2009<br />

22 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 23–37<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Friendships with Peers with Severe Disabilities: American <strong>and</strong><br />

Iranian Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Students’ Ideas about Being a Friend<br />

Mohsen Shokoohi-Yekta<br />

University of Tehran<br />

Jo M. Hendricks<strong>on</strong><br />

University of Iowa<br />

Abstract: We used the Student Friendship Percepti<strong>on</strong> Survey (SFPS) (Hamre-Nietupski, Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, Nietupski,<br />

& Shokoohi-Yekta, 1993; Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, Shokoohi-Yekta, Hamre-Nietupski, & Gable, 1996) to examine the<br />

opini<strong>on</strong>s of 656 American <strong>and</strong> 289 Iranian high school students (13–18 years old, M 15.0 <strong>and</strong> 15.5,<br />

respectively) in inclusive settings regarding friendships with peers with severe disabilities. Student opini<strong>on</strong>s<br />

related to (a) general friendship issues; (b) school settings/placement; (c) who should facilitate friendships;<br />

(d) teacher <strong>and</strong> school resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities; (e) parent involvement; (f) willingness to invest in friendships;<br />

(g) reas<strong>on</strong>s for friendships; <strong>and</strong> (h) difficulties being friends were analyzed. Iranian students favor more<br />

inclusive school envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> oversight by school pers<strong>on</strong>nel. Both American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students feel<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for initiating friendship acts, <strong>and</strong> both view knowing how to interact as a main challenge. Cultural<br />

factors <strong>and</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s for practice <strong>and</strong> research are discussed.<br />

In the United States the Individuals with Disabilities<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Act of 1997 (IDEA, 1999),<br />

currently the Individuals with Disabilities<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement Act (IDEIA, 2004),<br />

m<strong>and</strong>ates that students with disabilities be<br />

served in settings with students without disabilities<br />

to the maximum extent possible. In<br />

Iran the school system is under the jurisdicti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the Ministry of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training.<br />

Public educati<strong>on</strong> is available throughout<br />

Iran <strong>and</strong> the majority of Iranian students<br />

complete sec<strong>on</strong>dary school (Melchiorre, 2004).<br />

For the most part, public educati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., 1 st<br />

grade through university levels) is free, however<br />

acceptance to postsec<strong>on</strong>dary school requires<br />

passing entrance exams. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Bureau<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong> for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children <strong>and</strong><br />

Students exists within the Ministry of Educa-<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors wish to express sincere appreciati<strong>on</strong><br />

to Je<strong>on</strong>g-il-Cho for assistance with the literature<br />

search <strong>and</strong> to S. Kavoosi for her assistance with data<br />

collecti<strong>on</strong>. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article<br />

should be addressed to Mohsen Shokoohi-<br />

Yekta <strong>and</strong>/or Jo M. Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Department<br />

of Teaching <strong>and</strong> Learning, N259 Lindquist Center,<br />

University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242. Email: johendricks<strong>on</strong>@uiowa.edu<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> almost three decades ago <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Organizati<strong>on</strong> (SEO) was established<br />

<strong>and</strong> gradually inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> introduced.<br />

In recent years specialized pers<strong>on</strong>nel have c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

training to ensure students with disabilities<br />

benefit from inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> (Joibari,<br />

2003). To date inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>, especially in<br />

Iranian high schools, is in its infancy. In the<br />

current investigati<strong>on</strong>, we query high school students<br />

without disabilities attending inclusive<br />

schools (i.e., schools that to some extent serve<br />

individuals with disabilities in general educati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

regarding friendships with students with<br />

severe disabilities. Our objective is to identify<br />

similarities <strong>and</strong> differences in the views of American<br />

<strong>and</strong> Iranian adolescents <strong>on</strong> the value of<br />

such friendships <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> strategies to enhance<br />

friendship formati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Assumpti<strong>on</strong>s of the centrality <strong>and</strong> the significance<br />

of friendships to the well-being <strong>and</strong> life<br />

trajectories of all students regardless of disability<br />

status are well established in the literature<br />

(e.g., Asher, Parkhurst, Hymel, & Williams,<br />

1990; Bagwell, Schmidt, Newcomb, &<br />

Bukowski, 2001; Bierman, K., 2004; Buysse,<br />

Goldman, & Skinner, 2002; Hartup, 1992,<br />

1996; Overt<strong>on</strong> & Rausch, 2002; Rutter, 1989).<br />

Research indicates that friendships reported<br />

Friendships / 23


y students with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities are<br />

based <strong>on</strong> empathy, reciprocity, intimacy, trust,<br />

mutuality, <strong>and</strong> sharing experiences (Margalit<br />

& Efrati, 1996; Rosenblum, 1998; Heiman,<br />

2000; Turnbull, Blue-Banning, & Pereira,<br />

2000). Adolescent friendships, as Rosenblum<br />

notes, tend to be characterized by loyalty,<br />

trust, self-disclosure, openness, <strong>and</strong> affecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>versely, lack of friends is associated with<br />

deficiencies of social competence, peer rejecti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

l<strong>on</strong>eliness, decreased self-esteem, depressive<br />

symptoms, <strong>and</strong> inadequate social networks<br />

(Bierman, 2004; McVilly, Stancliffe,<br />

Parmenter, & Burt<strong>on</strong>-Smith, 2006; Odom et<br />

al., 2006). We know that friendship can mitigate<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>al, educati<strong>on</strong>al, vocati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> societal<br />

challenges. Adolescent friendships <strong>and</strong><br />

social occasi<strong>on</strong>s provide the venue for youths<br />

to practice age-appropriate skills (e.g., selfadvocacy,<br />

leadership, c<strong>on</strong>flict resoluti<strong>on</strong>) <strong>and</strong><br />

develop interactive styles (e.g., dependence or<br />

independence, active coping or passive coping)<br />

(Haghighatgou & Peters<strong>on</strong>, 1995).<br />

Although specific definiti<strong>on</strong>s of friendship<br />

vary, researchers agree that a youngster’s c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of friendship are (a) developmental<br />

(Furman & Bierman, 1984; O’Brien & Bierman,<br />

1988), (b) affected qualitatively by disability<br />

<strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al characteristics (Margalit & Efrati,<br />

1996; Odom et al., 2006; Parker & Asher,<br />

1993; Siperstein, Leffert, & Wenz-Gross, 1997),<br />

(c) associated with school climate <strong>and</strong> classroom<br />

c<strong>on</strong>textual variables (Buysse, Goldman, & Skinner,<br />

2003; Hamm, 2000; Han & Chadsey, 2004;<br />

Sasso, et al., 1992; Way & Chen, 2000), <strong>and</strong> (c)<br />

influenced by gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity (Hamm;<br />

Turnbull, Blue-Banning, & Pereira, 2000; Way &<br />

Chen). For the purpose of this study, we employ<br />

the definiti<strong>on</strong> of friendship used by Hendricks<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> colleagues (1996). Friendship is a social<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship between two people that is reciprocal,<br />

rewarding, <strong>and</strong> fun for both parties; it is a<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship that is characterized by multiple,<br />

voluntary c<strong>on</strong>tacts, <strong>and</strong> shared experiences<br />

across time.<br />

Friendships, intellectual disabilities, <strong>and</strong> inclusi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

For adolescents with severe disabilities,<br />

friendship formati<strong>on</strong> is arguably very closely<br />

linked to the school <strong>and</strong> its social c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

Just as self-determinati<strong>on</strong> capabilities of students<br />

are likely to be influenced by the opportunities<br />

the teacher provides (Shogren, et al.<br />

2007), friendship formati<strong>on</strong> is likely to be af-<br />

fected by opportunities to interact at school<br />

<strong>and</strong> the attitudes, knowledge, <strong>and</strong> behaviors<br />

of peers. Unfortunately, the empirically-based<br />

practices required for teaching academics<br />

(Turnbull, Turnbull, & Wehmeyer, 2007) are<br />

not m<strong>and</strong>ated for social domain competencies<br />

(e.g., social reciprocity, friendship formati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance). With regard to<br />

friendships of sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with severe<br />

disabilities, there is a dearth of solid scientific<br />

evidence. We know substantially more about<br />

preschool (Boyd, C<strong>on</strong>roy, Asmus, McKenney,<br />

& Mancil, 2008; Buyesse, et al., 1997; Odom,<br />

et al., 2006), elementary (Overt<strong>on</strong> & Rausch,<br />

2002) <strong>and</strong> middle-school children (Han &<br />

Chadsey, 2004; Siperstein, Leffert, & Wenz-<br />

Gross, 1997) <strong>and</strong> their dispositi<strong>on</strong>s toward<br />

<strong>and</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>s with students with disabilities<br />

than we do about sec<strong>on</strong>dary students. Siperstein<br />

<strong>and</strong> colleagues (1997), for example, report<br />

data <strong>on</strong> a large sample of American 7 th -<br />

<strong>and</strong> 8 th -grade students’ views <strong>and</strong> experiences<br />

related to inclusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> to students with intellectual<br />

disabilities (ID). Less than half of the<br />

students (38%) indicate they have a schoolmate<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly 10%<br />

report having a friend with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

N<strong>on</strong>etheless, similar to earlier studies<br />

(Harper, 1997; Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, et al. 1996), students<br />

without disabilities c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be open<br />

to the noti<strong>on</strong> of making friends with a classmate<br />

with a disability. Han <strong>and</strong> Chadsey<br />

(2004) <strong>and</strong> Siperstein <strong>and</strong> colleagues (1997)<br />

report that most (i.e., 89% <strong>and</strong> 88%, respectively)<br />

students without disabilities have a positive<br />

attitude about the idea that friendships<br />

can be formed between students with <strong>and</strong><br />

without disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>oretically speaking, in more inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> settings students with disabilities<br />

should have more opportunities to interact<br />

with peers without disabilities <strong>and</strong> thereby create<br />

a larger network of friends (Fryxell &<br />

Kennedy, 1995; Kennedy, et al., 1997; Hamre-<br />

Nietupski, et al., 1994; Turnbull, et al., 2007).<br />

This c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>, for example, is supported by<br />

Heiman’s (2000) study of 12- to 15-year-old<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Heiman’s survey data reveal that students<br />

served in special schools have fewer friends<br />

<strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>d more passively to l<strong>on</strong>eliness than<br />

students in mainstream schools. Han <strong>and</strong><br />

Chadsey (2004) note that the most frequent<br />

24 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


eas<strong>on</strong> students report for not having a friend<br />

with severe disabilities is limited opportunity<br />

for interacti<strong>on</strong>. Although it would be naive<br />

<strong>and</strong> err<strong>on</strong>eous to assume inclusive schools per<br />

se are sufficient for friendship formati<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

challenge of providing opportunity to interact<br />

in school settings seems imminently surmountable.<br />

What may be needed is the socalled<br />

political will. In this regard, Emers<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> McVilly (2004) <strong>and</strong> Rosenblum (1998)<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g others argue for agency <strong>and</strong> governmental<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al initiatives to specifically<br />

target the social domain.<br />

Friendships <strong>and</strong> comparative research. In the<br />

past, the majority of research pertaining to<br />

friendship, disability, <strong>and</strong> inclusi<strong>on</strong> has been<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the United States. More recently,<br />

internati<strong>on</strong>al interest in friendship<br />

formati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> variables affecting student<br />

friendships has become apparent in the literature.<br />

By way of example, Lee, Yoo, <strong>and</strong><br />

Bak’s (2003) study of the peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

of Korean elementary students in inclusive<br />

settings reveals qualitative differences in interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between students with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

disabilities (e.g., more physicality in exchanges<br />

of typical friends, game play characterized<br />

by students with disabilities placed in a<br />

receiver role). Indeed <strong>on</strong>e-third of the Korean<br />

children without disabilities menti<strong>on</strong> opportunity<br />

to spend time together in the classroom<br />

as a key factor in friendship formati<strong>on</strong>. Laws<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kelly (2005) examine the friendships intenti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of 9- to 12-year-old students in mainstream<br />

schools in the United Kingdom (U.K.);<br />

their results show that attitudes toward physical<br />

disability predict friendship intenti<strong>on</strong>s. According<br />

to Shams (2001) in a study<br />

of adolescents of Asian <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-Asian background<br />

in the U.K., a low preference for<br />

mixed ethnic friendships is indicative of a<br />

greater need for physical assistance (<strong>and</strong> advice)<br />

as well as increased l<strong>on</strong>eliness.<br />

Cross-cultural investigati<strong>on</strong>s of friendships<br />

<strong>and</strong> peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships often involve college<br />

students as the primary participants. Shokouhi-<br />

Behnam, Chambliss, <strong>and</strong> Caruso (1997) query<br />

American <strong>and</strong> Iranian college students living<br />

in the Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C. area of the U.S. <strong>on</strong><br />

familial <strong>and</strong> peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships. On the closestfriend<br />

subscale of the Shokouhi-Behnam et al.<br />

study, Euro-American students rate trust/<br />

justice <strong>and</strong> loyalty in friendships significantly<br />

higher than Iranian-Americans. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clude that Euro-American students rely<br />

more <strong>on</strong> friends outside of the family than<br />

Iranian-Americans.<br />

Women report loyalty in close friendships<br />

significantly higher than men do. In the Koh,<br />

Mendels<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Rhee (2003) study of Canadian<br />

<strong>and</strong> Korean students’ satisfacti<strong>on</strong> with friendships,<br />

the authors argue that friendship functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., compani<strong>on</strong>ship, self-validati<strong>on</strong>) vary<br />

<strong>and</strong> that student feelings about friendship can<br />

be differentially affected by culture. Koh <strong>and</strong><br />

colleagues point out that Korean <strong>and</strong> Canadian<br />

cultures vary <strong>on</strong> collectivism/individualism as<br />

well as ethnical homogeneity/heterogeneity; in<br />

turn these factors affect various aspects of friendship<br />

(e.g., functi<strong>on</strong>s, satisfacti<strong>on</strong>).<br />

Research <strong>on</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> friendships<br />

of school-aged children in Middle Eastern<br />

countries, specifically Iran, is sparse although<br />

some studies do pertain to social skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> related issues albeit in different resp<strong>on</strong>dent<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>s. Shahim (1999), for instance,<br />

administered the Social Skills Rating scales<br />

(Gresham & Elliott, 1990) to teachers <strong>and</strong><br />

parents of children with mild mental disabilities.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to high internal c<strong>on</strong>sistency<br />

of the Farsi translati<strong>on</strong> of the scales, Shahim<br />

reports that boys <strong>and</strong> girls with mental disabilities<br />

generally score similarly <strong>on</strong> social skill<br />

items. Iranian boys are seen to have more<br />

behavioral issues than girls <strong>and</strong> to not attend<br />

to instructi<strong>on</strong>s as well.<br />

Haghighatgou <strong>and</strong> Peters<strong>on</strong> (1995) measure<br />

the depressive symptoms <strong>and</strong> copying<br />

styles of junior <strong>and</strong> senior high school students<br />

in Tehran, Iran. First, they identify four<br />

factors--passivity, problem-solving, acting out,<br />

<strong>and</strong> denial, using a st<strong>and</strong>ard factor analytic<br />

procedure. Haghighatgou <strong>and</strong> Peters<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clude<br />

that depressi<strong>on</strong> is higher am<strong>on</strong>g Iranians<br />

(than U.S. adolescents) <strong>and</strong> that depressi<strong>on</strong><br />

significantly correlates with being female,<br />

low socio-ec<strong>on</strong>omic class, <strong>and</strong> a passive copying<br />

style. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors cauti<strong>on</strong> that in Iran<br />

solemnity <strong>and</strong> seriousness are highly valued,<br />

so the cultural interpretati<strong>on</strong> of depressive<br />

symptoms may differ from a western perspective.<br />

It also is possible that the prevalence of<br />

depressi<strong>on</strong> differs in Iran. Unfortunately, the<br />

above menti<strong>on</strong>ed studies are <strong>on</strong>ly indirectly<br />

related to friendships. We <strong>on</strong>ly could locate<br />

<strong>on</strong>e refereed study of Iranian friendships<br />

Friendships / 25


(Nasrabadi, Parsayekta, & Emami, 2004) in<br />

our literature search. Nasrabadi <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

c<strong>on</strong>duct interviews with male nurses<br />

<strong>and</strong> nursing students who reveal that smoking<br />

tobacco is associated with friendship, trust,<br />

<strong>and</strong> bel<strong>on</strong>ging to the group. Nasrabadi <strong>and</strong><br />

colleagues also c<strong>on</strong>clude that peer pressure<br />

is a crucial factor in smoking behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

that smoking actually is maintained through<br />

friendship networks.<br />

We fully c<strong>on</strong>cur with Pottie <strong>and</strong> Sumarah<br />

(2004) who note that interpers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

friendships, <strong>and</strong> bel<strong>on</strong>ging are critical<br />

to a fulfilling life. For adolescents with severe<br />

disabilities the factors that affect friendship<br />

formati<strong>on</strong> need to be identified so families,<br />

peers, educators, <strong>and</strong> other professi<strong>on</strong>als can<br />

engage in policy-making <strong>and</strong> scientificallybased<br />

acti<strong>on</strong>s that promote the student’s current<br />

<strong>and</strong> future quality of life. We know that a<br />

variety of factors may influence an adolescent’s<br />

willingness to be friends with peers with<br />

severe disabilities, however there is a dearth of<br />

cross-cultural data <strong>on</strong> adolescent friendship<br />

intenti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s related to peers<br />

with disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current study is the first<br />

cross-cultural study of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s about friendship<br />

formati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong>. Our goal is to<br />

gain a preliminary underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the views<br />

of western <strong>and</strong> middle-eastern adolescents as a<br />

baseline for the development of sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

school-based friendship formati<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

in these two distinct cultures.<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> schools. A total of 656<br />

American <strong>and</strong> 289 Iranian high school students<br />

served in general educati<strong>on</strong> inclusive<br />

schools participated in this study. American<br />

students came from 23 school districts in Iowa,<br />

Illinois, <strong>and</strong> Florida. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y ranged in age from<br />

13 to 18 years with an average age of 15.0 years<br />

old. Overall 48.5% of the American students<br />

were male, <strong>and</strong> 51.5% were female. Iranian<br />

students came from 10 inclusive schools in five<br />

school districts of Mashhad, Khorasan. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y<br />

ranged in age from 13 to 19 years old with an<br />

average age of 15.5 years. Overall, 48.8% of<br />

the Iranian students were male, <strong>and</strong> 51.2%<br />

were female.<br />

Most of the American students (95%) at-<br />

tended schools (n 23) with an enrollment<br />

of 250 to 1,000 students, <strong>and</strong> 100% of Iranian<br />

students attended schools (n 10) with an<br />

enrollment of 250 to 1000. Twenty-five school<br />

districts in the United States <strong>and</strong> 10 schools<br />

in Iran were c<strong>on</strong>tacted initially. Twenty-three<br />

U.S. districts (92%) agreed to participate, <strong>and</strong><br />

all of the Iranian schools (100%) agreed. In<br />

Iran schools are segregated by gender; five<br />

schools served young women, <strong>and</strong> five schools<br />

served young men.<br />

Instrument. We administered the Students<br />

Friendship Percepti<strong>on</strong> Survey (SFPS) which<br />

was developed by Hamre-Nietupski et al. (1993)<br />

<strong>and</strong> revised by Hendricks<strong>on</strong> et al. (1996). (For<br />

a more in-depth descripti<strong>on</strong> of the instrument<br />

see Hamre-Nietupski et al. <strong>and</strong> Hendricks<strong>on</strong>,<br />

et al.). Briefly, at the top of page <strong>on</strong>e, the<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>s of students with severe disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> students without disabilities were written:<br />

“Students with Severe Disabilities” are students<br />

with moderate, severe, or profound<br />

mental disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y also may have physical<br />

disabilities, visual impairments, hearing<br />

impairments, <strong>and</strong> behavior that is different<br />

or challenging compared to most students.<br />

“Students without Disabilities” means students<br />

who do not have disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y are<br />

often referred to as “typical,” “normal,” or<br />

“regular” students.<br />

Six short questi<strong>on</strong>s related to the c<strong>on</strong>cept of<br />

forming friendships with students with severe<br />

disabilities followed the definiti<strong>on</strong>s. Students<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded by circling 1, 2, 3, or 4 <strong>on</strong> a 4-point<br />

Likert-type scale (1 str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree to 4 <br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly agree).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> next four questi<strong>on</strong>s required rank ordering<br />

(e.g., 1 1st, 2 2nd, <strong>and</strong> 3 3rd)<br />

the settings where friendships are most likely to<br />

happen, who should take primary resp<strong>on</strong>sibility<br />

for facilitating friendships, what teachers/<br />

schools can do, <strong>and</strong> what parents can do to promote<br />

friendship formati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> students<br />

marked all of the resp<strong>on</strong>se choices that applied<br />

<strong>on</strong> the next three secti<strong>on</strong>s (i.e., what they would<br />

be willing to do, reas<strong>on</strong>s for being a friend, <strong>and</strong><br />

reas<strong>on</strong>s it might be difficult to be a friend).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first author, fluent in both English <strong>and</strong><br />

Farsi, translated the SFPS to Farsi. A team of<br />

three teachers <strong>and</strong> university faculty members<br />

trained in special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> fluent both<br />

26 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 1<br />

Student Beliefs about Friendships Between Students with Severe Disabilities <strong>and</strong> Students Without Disabilities<br />

in English <strong>and</strong> Farsi, reviewed <strong>and</strong> evaluated<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong>naire <strong>on</strong> the acceptability <strong>and</strong> accuracy<br />

of the translati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> for item <strong>and</strong><br />

overall instrument readability <strong>and</strong> clarity. Recommended<br />

revisi<strong>on</strong>s were incorporated into<br />

the final versi<strong>on</strong> of the SFPS (Farsi), <strong>and</strong> it was<br />

administered to a class of Iranian students as a<br />

field trial with no changes needed.<br />

Administrati<strong>on</strong> procedure. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> same procedure<br />

used by Hendricks<strong>on</strong> et al. (1996) was<br />

employed in the administrati<strong>on</strong> of the SFPS.<br />

Written instructi<strong>on</strong>s were provided to each<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> teacher after being interviewed<br />

in pers<strong>on</strong> or over the teleph<strong>on</strong>e. Survey<br />

administrators embedded the survey in a<br />

teacher-directed, explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> less<strong>on</strong><br />

related to forming friendships with individuals<br />

with disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> teacher <strong>and</strong> the students<br />

in class discussed the meanings of friendship,<br />

severe disabilities, <strong>and</strong> students without disabilities.<br />

After the questi<strong>on</strong>naires were distributed,<br />

the teacher read each item aloud. Approximately<br />

20–30 minutes were required to<br />

complete the survey. St<strong>and</strong>ard assent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sent<br />

procedures were employed. No names<br />

were collected to ensure an<strong>on</strong>ymity.<br />

Results<br />

General Friendship Issues<br />

Table 1 presents the average ratings (M),<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard deviati<strong>on</strong>s (SD), degrees of freedom<br />

(df), <strong>and</strong> t-scores for six general friendship<br />

formati<strong>on</strong> items. As noted, these items were<br />

rated using a 4-point Likert scale (1 <br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

M (SD)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

M (SD) df T-score<br />

1. It is possible for these friendships to happen. 3.27 (0.63) 3.23 (0.68) 943 1.00<br />

2. Such friendships benefit students with severe disabilities. 3.26 (0.77) 3.18 (0.82) 943 2.00*<br />

3. Students without disabilities should try to make friends<br />

with students with severe disabilities. 3.22 (0.73) 3.47 (0.76) 943 5.00*<br />

4. Such friendships benefit students without disabilities. 3.04 (0.79) 2.97 (0.97) 943 1.18<br />

5. Adults should help these friendships happen. 2.73 (0.91) 3.25 (0.78) 943 8.66*<br />

6. It is easy to help students with severe disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

students without disabilities become friends. 2.28 (0.84) 2.75 (0.83) 943 8.1*<br />

Note. Items rated <strong>on</strong> a1to4point Likert scale (1 str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree, 4 str<strong>on</strong>gly agree); df degrees of<br />

freedom; *p .05<br />

Str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree to 4 Str<strong>on</strong>gly Agree).<br />

First, both American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students<br />

agree that it is possible for friendships to<br />

form, that friendships benefit students with<br />

severe disabilities, <strong>and</strong> that students without<br />

disabilities should try to make friends with<br />

students with severe disabilities. However, as<br />

Table 1 shows, a significant difference is seen<br />

between American (am) <strong>and</strong> Iranian (ir) students<br />

<strong>on</strong> items 2, 3, 5, <strong>and</strong> 6. American students<br />

agree more str<strong>on</strong>gly with the noti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

friendships benefit the student with disabilities<br />

than Iranian students do (M am 3.26 vs.<br />

M ir 3.18). Iranian students feel more<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly than Americans that the student without<br />

a disability should reach out to the student<br />

with disabilities (M am 3.22 vs. M ir 3.47).<br />

Both groups agree that there are benefits to<br />

students without disabilities, however American<br />

students express significantly less agreement<br />

with the statement that adults should help<br />

these friendships happen (M am 2.73 vs. M ir <br />

3.25) <strong>and</strong> that such friendships would be easy to<br />

establish (M am 2.28 vs. M ir 2.75). Neither<br />

group perceives the friendships as easy to<br />

form.<br />

Table 2 presents the results related to the<br />

school setting or educati<strong>on</strong>al placement most<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducive to friendship formati<strong>on</strong>. Significant<br />

differences are seen in the items related to<br />

regular class placement for the entire day<br />

(df 943, t 6.36, p .05) <strong>and</strong> placement<br />

in a special class in a special school (df 943,<br />

t 5.62, p .05). On average Iranian sec<strong>on</strong>d-<br />

Friendships / 27


TABLE 2<br />

Student Beliefs about Educati<strong>on</strong>al Settings Most Likely to Facilitate Friendships<br />

Setting<br />

ary students more str<strong>on</strong>gly favor the regular<br />

classroom. Iranian students do view placement<br />

of students with severe disabilities in a<br />

special schools less favorably than American<br />

students.<br />

Tables 3–5 show the resp<strong>on</strong>ses of both<br />

groups to questi<strong>on</strong>s in the following categories:<br />

(a) who should facilitate friendships,<br />

(b) how teachers <strong>and</strong> schools can help, <strong>and</strong><br />

(c) how parents can facilitate such friendships.<br />

Results indicate that both groups highly<br />

value the role that they themselves (see Table<br />

3) play in friendships. Approximately 77% of<br />

Americans <strong>and</strong> 80% of Iranian students report<br />

that they should take resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for facilitating<br />

friendships. Chi-square analyses reveal<br />

American students emphasize youth clubs <strong>and</strong><br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>s more than the Iranians (P am <br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

M (SD)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

M (SD) df T-score<br />

1. A regular class for the entire day. 2.79 (1.14) 3.28 (1.05) 943 6.36*<br />

2. A regular class for part of the day. 2.67 (0.88) 2.73 (0.78) 943 1.01<br />

3. A special class in a regular school. 2.56 (0.96) 2.48 (0.89) 943 1.23<br />

4. A special class in a special school. 1.95 (1.24) 1.50 (0.94) 943 5.62*<br />

Note. Items rated <strong>on</strong> a1to4point Likert scale (1 str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree, 4 str<strong>on</strong>gly agree); df degrees of<br />

freedom; *p .05<br />

TABLE 3<br />

38.87% vs. P ir 20.4%). On the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

Iranian students c<strong>on</strong>sider professi<strong>on</strong>als (e.g.,<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teachers, guidance counselors,<br />

school psychologists, <strong>and</strong> social workers)<br />

as more resp<strong>on</strong>sible for facilitating these<br />

friendships. Close to 30% of American <strong>and</strong><br />

Iranian students feel that parents of students<br />

with severe disabilities should facilitate friendships,<br />

<strong>and</strong> about 20% of the resp<strong>on</strong>dents feel<br />

parents of students without disabilities should<br />

take some resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for the friendships.<br />

Results presented in Table 4 present student<br />

views regarding how teachers <strong>and</strong><br />

schools can facilitate friendships. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re are<br />

two top activities the majority of students in<br />

both the U. S. <strong>and</strong> Iran agree up<strong>on</strong>. Both<br />

think schools should use ways of teaching that<br />

have students work together <strong>and</strong> arrange social<br />

Student Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of Who Should Facilitate Friendships with Students with Severe Disabilities<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong> or Agency<br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. Student themselves. 502 (76.52) 231 (80.0) 1 1.34 0.25<br />

2. Special educati<strong>on</strong> teachers. 302 (46.03) 155 (53.7) 1 4.67 0.03*<br />

3. Guidance counselors, school psychologists,<br />

social workers. 290 (44.20) 181 (62.0) 1 27.23 0.00*<br />

4. Youth clubs <strong>and</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong>s. 255 (38.87) 59 (20.4) 1 30.80 0.00*<br />

5. Regular educati<strong>on</strong> teachers. 232 (35.36) 98 (34.0) 1 0.19 0.66<br />

6. Parents of students with severe disabilities. 202 (30.79) 83 (28.7) 1 0.41 0.52<br />

7. Parents of n<strong>on</strong>-disabled students. 129 (19.66) 56 (19.4) 1 0.01 0.92<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; dfdegrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

28 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 4<br />

Student Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of How Teachers <strong>and</strong> Schools Can Facilitate Friendships with Students with Severe<br />

Disabilities<br />

Types of Assistance<br />

activities in which all students may participate.<br />

American students resp<strong>on</strong>d significantly more<br />

often to the strategy of teaching so students work<br />

together than Iranian students do. American<br />

<strong>and</strong> Iranian students resp<strong>on</strong>d similarly to the<br />

importance of teaching all students how to interact<br />

with each other, teaching the importance of<br />

friendships, <strong>and</strong> organizing a circle of friends<br />

(range 27 to 36%).<br />

Significant differences are observed between<br />

the two groups <strong>on</strong> three other items.<br />

Iranians place greater importance of informati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> disabilities being presented to students,<br />

teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents <strong>and</strong> the need to encourage<br />

parents to create opportunities for such friendships.<br />

American students are more apt to suggest<br />

that students without disabilities tutor<br />

students with severe disabilities.<br />

Table 5 presents student percepti<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

how parents can facilitate friendships. An<br />

equal percent (56%) of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian<br />

students think a positive attitude toward such<br />

friendships at home is important. Seventy-three<br />

percent (72.6%) of Iranian students think parents<br />

should work with schools <strong>and</strong> teachers to think<br />

of ways to help friendships. Iranian resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

parent involvement (e.g., parents working<br />

with schools <strong>and</strong> being involved in support<br />

groups) is significantly higher than that of<br />

American students. American students more<br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. Use ways of teaching that have students work together. 432 (65.85) 170 (58.8) 1 4.29 0.04*<br />

2. Arrange social activities in which all students may<br />

participate. 371 (56.55) 176 (57.4) 1 1.55 0.21<br />

3. Teach students how to interact with each other. 233 (35.51) 98 (33.9) 1 0.23 0.63<br />

4. Teach the importance of such friendships. 233 (35.51) 91 (31.4) 1 1.45 0.23<br />

5. Organize a “Circle of Friends” around the student<br />

with disabilities to encourage interacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> support. 197 (30.03) 79 (27.3) 1 0.70 0.40<br />

6. Present informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> disabilities to students, other<br />

teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents. 184 (28.04) 118 (40.8) 1 15.07 0.00*<br />

7. Teach n<strong>on</strong>-disabled students to be tutors. 161 (24.54) 51 (17.6) 1 5.48 0.02*<br />

8. Encourage parents to make opportunities for these<br />

friendships. 113 (17.22) 89 (30.8) 1 21.98 0.00*<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; df degrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

frequently identify organizing <strong>and</strong> attending parent<br />

support groups (P am 40.85% vs. P ir <br />

26.3%) than Iranians. Americans also favor<br />

serving as leaders in youth organizati<strong>on</strong>s (P am <br />

28.96% vs. P ir 21.8%).<br />

Table 6 presents American <strong>and</strong> Iranian student<br />

reports of acti<strong>on</strong>s they are willing to take<br />

to facilitate making friends with students with<br />

severe disabilities. Although Chi square analyses<br />

show significant differences (p .05)<br />

between the choices of the two groups <strong>on</strong> 10<br />

of 12 items, we begin by discussing group<br />

similarities. First, fifty percent or more of both<br />

American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students identify four<br />

items most often: to be a peer tutor, to work <strong>on</strong> a<br />

team, to learn <strong>and</strong> to share informati<strong>on</strong> about disabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to be part of a group spending time<br />

together regularly. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> greatest number of Iranian<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses (61.2%) indicate that students<br />

believe that they need to learn (<strong>and</strong> share)<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>. Iranian students are least willing<br />

(13% to 19%) to stay overnight, visit the other<br />

student’s home, invite the student with disabilities<br />

to their home regularly, or to have the<br />

student stay at their home. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se same four<br />

activities are also the least frequent choices of<br />

American students. As seen in Table 7, for<br />

both groups the two most frequently selected<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>s for friendships are that “<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y need<br />

friends, too,” <strong>and</strong> “I like to help people.” On<br />

Friendships / 29


TABLE 5<br />

Student Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of How Parents Can Facilitate Friendships with Students with Severe Disabilities<br />

Parent Acti<strong>on</strong><br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. Have a positive attitude toward such friendships at home. 367 (55.94) 161 (55.8) 1 0.01 0.95<br />

2. Invite students, including those with disabilities, to their<br />

homes. 331 (50.45) 127 (43.9) 1 3.41 0.06<br />

3. Work with the schools <strong>and</strong> teachers to think of ways to<br />

help these friendships form. 330 (50.30) 210 (72.6) 1 40.95 0.00*<br />

4. Organize/attend parent support groups. 268 (40.85) 76 (26.3) 1 18.36 0.00*<br />

5. Provide transportati<strong>on</strong> for students with disabilities to<br />

after school activities. 227 (34.60) 86 (29.8) 1 2.13 0.14<br />

6. Serve as leaders in youth organizati<strong>on</strong>s (for example, be<br />

a scout leader). 190 (28.96) 63 (21.8) 1 5.25 0.02*<br />

7. Let teachers know they want these friendships to be<br />

made at school. 170 (25.91) 72 (24.9) 1 0.105 0.74<br />

8. Make friends with adults who have disabilities. 140 (21.34) 57 (19.7) 1 0.32 0.57<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; df degrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

TABLE 6<br />

Student Willingness to Take Acti<strong>on</strong>s That Facilitate Friendships with Students with Severe Disabilities<br />

Acti<strong>on</strong><br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. Be a peer tutor or buddy to a student with severe<br />

disabilities. 458 (70.78) 146 (50.5) 1 35.86 0.00*<br />

2. Work in a team with a student with severe disabilities. 447 (69.08) 149 (51.6) 1 26.54 0.00*<br />

3. Learn more about disabilities <strong>and</strong> share what I know<br />

with others. 428 (66.50) 117 (61.2) 1 54.10 0.00*<br />

4. Be part of a group of students who regularly spends<br />

time with students with severe disabilities. 411 (63.52) 143 (49.5) 1 16.31 0.00*<br />

5. Visit the home of a student with disabilities. 350 (54.09) 122 (42.2) 1 11.28 0.00*<br />

6. Spend time after school with a student with severe<br />

disabilities. 334 (53.16) 138 (47.8) 1 2.35 0.12<br />

7. Spend some time <strong>on</strong> weekends with a student with<br />

disabilities. 316 (48.84) 124 (42.9) 1 2.82 0.09<br />

8. Invite a student with severe disabilities to my home. 315 (48.68) 119 (41.2) 1 4.53 0.03*<br />

9. Stay overnight at the home of a student with severe<br />

disabilities. 198 (30.60) 36 (12.5) 1 35.08 0.00*<br />

10. Visit the home of a student with disabilities <strong>on</strong> a<br />

regular basis. 195 (30.13) 44 (15.2) 1 23.37 0.00*<br />

11. Have a student with disabilities stay overnight at my<br />

house. 194 (29.98) 55 (19.0) 1 4.03 0.04*<br />

12. Invite a student with disabilities to my home <strong>on</strong> a<br />

regular basis. 157 (24.26) 53 (18.3) 1 12.27 0.00*<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; df degrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

30 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 7<br />

Reas<strong>on</strong>s of Friendships with Students with Severe Disabilities<br />

Reas<strong>on</strong><br />

the other h<strong>and</strong>, Iranian students are significantly<br />

less likely to select reas<strong>on</strong>s that pertain<br />

to talking about <strong>on</strong>e’s self such as I am underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

<strong>and</strong> I have a good attitude. Similarly,<br />

when a benefit is to <strong>on</strong>e’s self (e.g., It<br />

could be fun.), there is a significant reducti<strong>on</strong><br />

in Iranian student resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Although total<br />

frequencies differ, American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students<br />

order 7 of 10 items in the same most-toleast<br />

sequence. (Items 4, 7, <strong>and</strong> 9 are out of<br />

sequence.)<br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y need friends, too. 534 (82.40) 214 (74.0) 1 8.67 0.00*<br />

2. I like to help people. 478 (73.76) 214 (74.0) 1 0.01 0.93<br />

3. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y would feel better with a friend. 465 (71.75) 190 (65.7) 1 3.43 0.06<br />

4. It could be fun. 434 (66.97) 24 (8.3) 1 27.53 0.00*<br />

5. I would feel good about myself. 427 (65.89) 144 (49.8) 1 21.67 0.00*<br />

6. We might have comm<strong>on</strong> interests. 401 (61.88) 136 (47.8) 1 17.95 0.00*<br />

7. Because I am underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> friendly. 366 (56.48) 89 (30.8) 1 52.78 0.00*<br />

8. I could learn about disabilities. 361 (55.70) 167 (57.8) 1 0.35 0.55<br />

9. Because I have a good attitude. 335 (51.69) 56 (19.4) 1 85.86 0.00*<br />

10. I would have another friend. 302 (46.60) 78 (27.0) 1 31.89 0.00*<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; df degrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

TABLE 8<br />

Difficulties of Friendships with Students with Severe Disabilities<br />

Difficulty<br />

Finally, difficulties for friendships are presented<br />

in Table 8. Forty percent or more American<br />

students identify six barriers to friendships,<br />

while Iranian students <strong>on</strong>ly identify two<br />

items, “I wouldn’t know what to say or do,”<br />

<strong>and</strong> “<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y couldn’t do the things I like to do,”<br />

at a rate of 42% to 34%, respectively. Both<br />

groups emphasize not knowing how to behave<br />

as a main barrier or difficulty. Overall, the two<br />

groups differ significantly <strong>on</strong> 8 of 10 items, all<br />

of which American students select more fre-<br />

American<br />

(n 656)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

Iranian<br />

(n 289)<br />

Number<br />

(Percentage)<br />

df Value<br />

Chi-square<br />

Significance<br />

Level<br />

1. I wouldn’t know what to say or do. 414 (67.09) 122 (42.2) 1 50.44 0.00*<br />

2. I would be nervous or uncomfortable. 339 (54.94) 32 (11.1) 1 156.65 0.00*<br />

3. People might tease me; I d<strong>on</strong>’t want to be made<br />

fun of. 317 (51.37) 50 (17.3) 1 94.83 0.00*<br />

4. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y couldn’t do the things I like to do. 307 (49.75) 97 (33.6) 1 20.88 0.00*<br />

5. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y wouldn’t fit in with my friends. 273 (44.24) 49 (17.0) 1 63.99 0.00*<br />

6. I would feel awkward or embarrassed. 251 (40.68) 34 (11.8) 1 76.32 0.00*<br />

7. I have too many other commitments. 180 (29.17) 37 (12.8) 1 28.96 0.00*<br />

8. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y might fight or hurt me. 129 (20.90) 42 (14.5) 1 5.22 0.02*<br />

9. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y are not as smart or intelligent as me. 75 (12.15) 25 (8.7) 1 2.46 0.12<br />

10. My family would not agree. 67 (10.85) 32 (11.2) 1 0.01 0.92<br />

Note. Items ordered by frequency of American student resp<strong>on</strong>ses; df degrees of freedom; *p .05<br />

Friendships / 31


quently than Iranian students. Compared to<br />

Iranians, American adolescents more often<br />

indicate that they would be nervous or uncomfortable,<br />

do not want to be teased, think the<br />

student with a disability would not fit in with<br />

their friends, <strong>and</strong> that they might be physically<br />

hurt. Approximately 10% of both groups feel<br />

that the student with a disability is “not as<br />

smart or intelligent as me” <strong>and</strong> that their<br />

families would not agree with having such a<br />

friendship.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

This investigati<strong>on</strong> of the attitudes <strong>and</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

students pertains to forming friendships with<br />

students with severe disabilities <strong>and</strong> offers a<br />

preliminary comparis<strong>on</strong> of western <strong>and</strong> Middle<br />

Eastern adolescent perspectives <strong>and</strong> values.<br />

Striking similarities <strong>and</strong> differences emerge<br />

that in part may be explained in relati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

experience in inclusive settings (Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. 2006), friendship functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

developmental progressi<strong>on</strong> (Bierman, 2004),<br />

the educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> training of teachers (Camer<strong>on</strong><br />

& Cook, 2007; Hamre-Nietupski et al.<br />

1994) <strong>and</strong> paraeducators (Malgren & Caust<strong>on</strong>-<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>oharis , 2006) as well as cultural factors<br />

(Bui & Turnbull, 2003; Edeh, 2007; Koh et al.,<br />

2003). As in past research, the current study<br />

supports the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that forming friendships,<br />

in this particular case, friendships with<br />

students with disabilities, is a complex process,<br />

that outcomes should not be casually generalized<br />

across populati<strong>on</strong>s (Way & Chen, 2000)<br />

<strong>and</strong> that heterogeneity within cultures must<br />

not be overlooked (French, Bae, Pidada, &<br />

Lee, 2006). Since most friendship <strong>and</strong> disability<br />

research focuses <strong>on</strong> preschool through<br />

middle school students, this investigati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

adolescent views extends the literature <strong>and</strong><br />

has implicati<strong>on</strong>s for the post-school lives of<br />

individuals with severe disabilities whose pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ships often are limited to family<br />

<strong>and</strong> caregivers (Pottie & Sumarah, 2004).<br />

Are friendships with students with severe disabilities<br />

possible? Sec<strong>on</strong>dary students in America<br />

<strong>and</strong> Iran view friendships with students with<br />

severe disabilities to have pros <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>s, similar<br />

to previous research results with American<br />

students (Bunch & Valeo, 2004; Hendricks<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 1996; Siperstein et al., 1997). Both<br />

American <strong>and</strong> Iranian sec<strong>on</strong>dary students in<br />

inclusive high schools agree that it is possible<br />

to have friendships with students with severe<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> that there are benefits to students<br />

with disabilities. (To a lesser extent both<br />

groups perceive benefits to students without<br />

disabilities.) Neither group agrees with the<br />

statement that friendships are easy to form.<br />

American students feel significantly more<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly than Iranians that such friendships<br />

are not easy. This percepti<strong>on</strong> of the increased<br />

difficulty in forming friendships may be an<br />

outcome diverging educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> societal<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s. Propelled by IDEA (1999, 2004)<br />

many preschool- <strong>and</strong> primary-aged American<br />

students are likely to have social c<strong>on</strong>tact, become<br />

acquainted with, <strong>and</strong>/or develop friendships<br />

with students with disabilities within inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al settings (Fryxell & Kennedy,<br />

1995; Lee, Yoo, & Bak, 2003). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> rate of<br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong>, however, is rather abruptly<br />

diminished in the school life of adolescents<br />

regardless of the nature of the inclusive practices.<br />

Such factors as the increasing difficulty<br />

of subject matter, the assignment of individual<br />

paraeducators to students with severe disabilities<br />

(Malgren & Caust<strong>on</strong>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>oharis, 2006),<br />

<strong>and</strong> societal pressures to achieve (e.g., district<br />

wide assessments in the U.S., preparati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

college entrance exams in Iran) create distinct<br />

curricular tracks for students with disparate<br />

intellectual capabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se realities<br />

in turn mitigate against the development of<br />

friendships.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al setting. American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students<br />

indicate that the regular class for the<br />

entire day is their highest rated choice of an<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al setting for facilitating friendship.<br />

Similar to the earlier Siperstein <strong>and</strong> colleagues’<br />

(1997) study of middle school students,<br />

American sec<strong>on</strong>dary students do not<br />

appear to str<strong>on</strong>gly support full inclusi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

American students indicate significantly less<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cordance with the policy of students with<br />

severe disabilities being placed in the regular<br />

class for the entire day (M am 2.79 vs. P ir <br />

3.28). Both groups, however, express relatively<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g disagreement with the placement of<br />

students with severe disabilities in special<br />

schools. Iranian students view special schools<br />

significantly less favorably than American students.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se data suggest that Iranian adolescents<br />

may envisi<strong>on</strong> a more idealized service<br />

32 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


model, perhaps with adults orchestrating interpers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

outcomes. Overall, sec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

in the U.S. where inclusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the least<br />

restrictive envir<strong>on</strong>ment (LRE) principle are<br />

more aggressively implemented, appear to be<br />

less c<strong>on</strong>fident of the effect of school placement<br />

<strong>on</strong> social outcomes for students with<br />

severe disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> roles of schools <strong>and</strong> families in facilitating<br />

friendships. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> argument for the interc<strong>on</strong>nectedness<br />

of adolescent socializati<strong>on</strong>, friendships,<br />

family relati<strong>on</strong>ships, <strong>and</strong> peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

is supported in the literature (Shams,<br />

2001). We know that Iranian families are similar<br />

to Asian families (see Bui & Turnbull, 2003)<br />

in that a vertical, hierarchical family structure<br />

is the general cultural norm; the father <strong>and</strong><br />

adult members of the family are shown respect<br />

<strong>and</strong> perceived as authority figures. Furthermore,<br />

there are fewer professi<strong>on</strong>als (other<br />

than regular educati<strong>on</strong> teachers) in Iranian<br />

schools, <strong>and</strong> the specialists (e.g., school psychologists)<br />

who are associated with any school<br />

are highly regarded. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore it is not surprising<br />

to find that Iranian youths in the current<br />

study are significantly more likely than<br />

American youths to perceive adults in professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., social workers, school<br />

psychologists, school counselors) as playing<br />

critical roles in the facilitati<strong>on</strong> of friendships.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, Americans are significantly more<br />

likely to expect youth clubs--venues that are<br />

not comm<strong>on</strong>ly available in Iranian communities,<br />

to facilitate friendships.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> majority of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students<br />

recommend the same two top strategies<br />

for teachers <strong>and</strong> schools, that is, use ways of<br />

teaching that have students work together <strong>and</strong> arrange<br />

social activities in which all students may<br />

participate. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se two strategies c<strong>on</strong>ceptually<br />

align with research that suggests that opportunity<br />

for interacti<strong>on</strong> is an important variable<br />

in relati<strong>on</strong>ship building (Han & Chadsey,<br />

2004) <strong>and</strong> that inclusive settings can positively<br />

affect outcomes for students with disabilities<br />

(Bunch & Valeo, 2004; Dym<strong>on</strong>d, Renzaglia, &<br />

Chun, 2008). Iranian students (compared to<br />

Americans) are significantly more likely to<br />

perceive the role of educators as c<strong>on</strong>veyors of<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> disabilities <strong>and</strong> as instrumental<br />

in encouraging parents to facilitate friendships.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, American students are<br />

more likely to recommend tutoring, a com-<br />

m<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> successful practice across populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> grade levels in U.S. schools (Turnbull<br />

et al., 2007) <strong>and</strong> an uncomm<strong>on</strong> practice<br />

in Iran. We also want to point out that approximately<br />

30% of both American <strong>and</strong> Iranian<br />

adolescents agree that it is important for educators<br />

to (a) teach students how to interact,<br />

(b) teach the importance of such friendships,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) organize a circle of friends <strong>on</strong> behalf<br />

of the student with a severe disability. That is,<br />

a significant minority of students view educators<br />

as proactive transmitters of knowledge,<br />

values, <strong>and</strong> skills. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y perceive social domain<br />

objectives as legitimate c<strong>on</strong>tent for sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

educators to teach.<br />

With regard to the role of parents <strong>and</strong> families,<br />

over 55 percent of both American <strong>and</strong><br />

Iranian students report that a positive attitude<br />

at home is essential to friendship formati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This adolescent majority viewpoint echos the<br />

opini<strong>on</strong>s of educators <strong>and</strong> related service pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

who are l<strong>on</strong>g-time advocates of the role<br />

of parents <strong>and</strong> the importance of communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> collaborati<strong>on</strong> between home <strong>and</strong><br />

school (Turnbull et al., 2007). American adolescents<br />

are significantly more likely to indicate<br />

that parents should take leadership roles<br />

in the community (e.g., form support groups,<br />

work in youth organizati<strong>on</strong>s), acti<strong>on</strong>s wellestablished<br />

<strong>and</strong> frequently advocated by schools<br />

(e.g., Parent-Teacher Associati<strong>on</strong>), community<br />

agencies (e.g., United Way, Boys <strong>and</strong> Girls<br />

Clubs of America), <strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>al organizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Arc). C<strong>on</strong>sistent with traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Iranian cultural values (e.g., authority<br />

of adults, high esteem for pers<strong>on</strong>s with professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

credentials), Iranian adolescents are<br />

significantly more likely to recommend that<br />

parents work with <strong>and</strong> through schools to design<br />

strategies for developing friendships.<br />

Adolescent Willingness to Act <strong>and</strong> Opini<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Regarding Benefits <strong>and</strong> Challenges of Friendships<br />

Willingness to take acti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results of this<br />

study show significant differences <strong>on</strong> 10 of 12<br />

items related to student willingness to actively<br />

participate in forming friendships with students<br />

with severe disabilities. Exclusive focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> these statistical differences, however, could<br />

be misleading, so we first wish to underscore<br />

the fact that over 75% of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian<br />

students indicate that they themselves<br />

Friendships / 33


should initiate the friendships, <strong>and</strong> 50% of<br />

the adolescents are willing to be a peer tutor,<br />

work <strong>on</strong> a team with a student with a severe<br />

disability, learn <strong>and</strong> share informati<strong>on</strong> about<br />

disabilities, <strong>and</strong> be part of a group that spends<br />

time with students with severe disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se results suggest an open attitude <strong>on</strong> the<br />

part of adolescents to spending time with<br />

peers with severe disabilities, particularly during<br />

the school day. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the four acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

chosen least by both Americans <strong>and</strong> Iranians<br />

are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with prior research suggesting<br />

students without disabilities do not want to<br />

socialize <strong>and</strong> engage in extra-curricular activities<br />

with students with disabilities after school<br />

(Siperstein et al., 1997). In our study, adolescents<br />

least frequently indicated a willingness<br />

to stay overnight, visit the other student’s<br />

home, invite the student to their home, or<br />

have the student stay overnight at their home.<br />

However, the statistical differences related to<br />

home visit items may in large part be cultural<br />

differences in that it is very uncomm<strong>on</strong> for<br />

Iranian friends to stay overnight at <strong>on</strong>e another’s<br />

homes while it is not unusual for<br />

American friends to do so. Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

10 items showing statistically significant differences,<br />

shows American students to be more<br />

likely to be willing to engage in a range of<br />

specific acti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., tutor, work <strong>on</strong> a team<br />

regularly, visit outside of school, stay overnight)<br />

than Iranian students. This acti<strong>on</strong> orientati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the part of U.S. adolescents may<br />

be explained in part by experiential (e.g., a<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger history of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

U.S., more youth services in the community)<br />

<strong>and</strong> cultural factors (e.g., increasing dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

of independence expected as the<br />

American student ages; reliance <strong>and</strong> trust in<br />

family authority <strong>and</strong> family members c<strong>on</strong>tinues<br />

in the Iranian adolescent’s life). Other<br />

cultural factors also appear to c<strong>on</strong>tribute to<br />

some of the differences found. In a secular<br />

nati<strong>on</strong> such as the U.S., student motivati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

based <strong>on</strong> a survey c<strong>on</strong>ducted at school may or<br />

may not reflect religious values, whereas in an<br />

Islamic country such as Iran, religious principles<br />

<strong>and</strong> practices have a high profile <strong>and</strong> are<br />

seamlessly interwoven with school activities.<br />

Reas<strong>on</strong>s for friendships. A large majority<br />

(i.e., 74%) of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian students<br />

perceive two main reas<strong>on</strong>s for developing<br />

friendships with peers with severe disabil-<br />

ities: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y need friends, too, <strong>and</strong> I like to help<br />

people. Both of these resp<strong>on</strong>ses seem to indicate<br />

an altruistic motive for friendships formati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

as well as the underlying assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

of reciprocity (i.e., equal give <strong>and</strong> take) in<br />

friendships. A relatively high percentage of<br />

both student groups (i.e., 48% to 72%) also<br />

note that the student with a disability would<br />

feel better with a friend, that students without<br />

disabilities would feel good about themselves,<br />

that students with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities<br />

might have comm<strong>on</strong> interests, <strong>and</strong> that students<br />

without disabilities could learn about<br />

disabilities. Despite similarities in terms of<br />

the distributi<strong>on</strong> of American <strong>and</strong> Iranian student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses, a significant difference is evident<br />

in seven of 10 reas<strong>on</strong>s for being a friend<br />

with the largest c<strong>on</strong>trasts seen in “It could be<br />

fun,” followed by “Because I have a good attitude,”<br />

“Because I am underst<strong>and</strong>ing,” <strong>and</strong> “I<br />

would have another friend.” Iranian social c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

do not endorse adolescent, or adult,<br />

acti<strong>on</strong>s merely because the acti<strong>on</strong> would benefit<br />

the individual. In the more collectivistic<br />

society of Iran, it is likely that Iranian students<br />

do not value or did not feel comfortable thinking<br />

about friendships in relati<strong>on</strong> to what they<br />

as individuals would gain. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> more solemn<br />

<strong>and</strong> serious Iranian society (Haghighatgou &<br />

Peters<strong>on</strong>, 1995) may discount “fun” as frivolous<br />

or even c<strong>on</strong>sider “fun” a negative goal.<br />

Barriers to friendships. Statistically significant<br />

differences are seen <strong>on</strong> 8 of 10 items<br />

related to the difficulty of forming friendships.<br />

Generally speaking, Iranian students do<br />

not voice their objecti<strong>on</strong>s or potentially negative<br />

thoughts about the difficulties of befriending<br />

a peer with a severe disability. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

highest percent of Iranians, approximately<br />

42% <strong>and</strong> 34%, respectively, did report that<br />

they wouldn’t know what to say or do or that they<br />

believe that the student with a disability could not<br />

do the things that they like to do (compared to<br />

American resp<strong>on</strong>ses of 67% <strong>and</strong> 55%, respectively).<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, American adolescents<br />

appear to be more comfortable expressing<br />

their feelings (e.g., I would feel awkward or<br />

embarrassed), outspoken about limitati<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

the activities in which they could engage (e.g.,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y couldn’t do the things I like to do), <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

more self-oriented (e.g., I have too many other<br />

commitments) in relati<strong>on</strong> to the challenges of<br />

developing friendships with peers with severe<br />

34 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se differences in particular<br />

seem to be linked to cultural expectati<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

the American individualistic, self-oriented society<br />

versus the Iranian collectivistic, grouporiented<br />

society.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> future research. Cauti<strong>on</strong> must<br />

be employed interpreting <strong>and</strong> generalizing<br />

the results of this preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

American <strong>and</strong> Iranian sec<strong>on</strong>dary students’<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s about forming friendships with<br />

students with severe disabilities. As noted earlier,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e cannot automatically assume homogeneity<br />

of the populati<strong>on</strong>s of different countries<br />

or cultures. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current survey involves<br />

a n<strong>on</strong>-r<strong>and</strong>om sample of students which limits<br />

our ability to generalize the results. Survey<br />

data have an inherent weakness, that is, the<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>dence between the opini<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> behavior<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>dents is not necessarily <strong>on</strong>eto-<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

Acti<strong>on</strong>s in which the adolescent might<br />

be willing to engage to befriend peers with<br />

severe disabilities are likely to vary depending<br />

<strong>on</strong> the immediate c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> open-ended questi<strong>on</strong>s asked of<br />

students in the U.S., Hendricks<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(1996) created-closed resp<strong>on</strong>se items<br />

pertaining to acti<strong>on</strong>s students would be willing<br />

to take, reas<strong>on</strong>s for friendships, <strong>and</strong> difficulties<br />

of friendships. If Iranian students were<br />

to be asked open-ended questi<strong>on</strong>s, it is likely<br />

that they would discuss the importance of the<br />

role of religious leaders <strong>and</strong> instituti<strong>on</strong>s associated<br />

with religi<strong>on</strong> in the supporting of friendships.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> U.S. has a traditi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stituti<strong>on</strong><br />

that separates “church <strong>and</strong> state” whereas<br />

Iran’s government <strong>and</strong> social order is closely<br />

tied to the Muslim faith. Although religious<br />

affiliati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> principles affect government<br />

policy in the U.S., in Iran the clergy <strong>and</strong> the<br />

mosque are interwoven in the daily lives of Iranians.<br />

A modified versi<strong>on</strong> of the SFPS (Hendricks<strong>on</strong><br />

et al.) that includes resp<strong>on</strong>se items<br />

based <strong>on</strong> Iranian student answers to openended<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s would allow a more robust<br />

examinati<strong>on</strong> of the data.<br />

A fuller underst<strong>and</strong>ing of a complex phenomen<strong>on</strong><br />

such as friendship, especially the<br />

development of friendships between adolescents<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without severe disabilities, is<br />

best approached with a multimodal methodology<br />

<strong>and</strong> data triangulati<strong>on</strong>. In this regard,<br />

structured <strong>and</strong> semi-structured interviews with<br />

students, including students with disabilities<br />

(Day & Harry, 1999) are recommended. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

viewpoints of adults, especially parents, professi<strong>on</strong>als,<br />

<strong>and</strong> community leaders (e.g., clergy,<br />

youth organizati<strong>on</strong> leaders) would add perspective<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text to investigative efforts.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> benefits of direct observati<strong>on</strong> must be<br />

noted in that such data (quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative)<br />

can allow examinati<strong>on</strong> of behavioral<br />

sequences, exchanges, <strong>and</strong> nuances that cannot<br />

be achieved vis á vis questi<strong>on</strong>naire data.<br />

Ultimately, developmentally <strong>and</strong> culturally appropriate<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s must be designed <strong>and</strong><br />

evaluated if evidence-based practices are to be<br />

identified <strong>and</strong> validated.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>. Interpers<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ships arguably<br />

are the bedrock of society <strong>and</strong> impact<br />

the quality of life of individuals, families, <strong>and</strong><br />

communities. Although inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

advocated internati<strong>on</strong>ally, comparative study<br />

of friendship, disability, <strong>and</strong> adolescence is in<br />

its infancy. Our preliminary data indicate that<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students in the U.S. <strong>and</strong> Iran are<br />

receptive to the possibility of developing relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

with students with severe disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y believe that the attitudes <strong>and</strong> stewardship<br />

of schools is critical to friendship development,<br />

<strong>and</strong> they appear to be hopeful that<br />

challenges to such friendships can be overcome.<br />

Addressing social domain challenges in<br />

the lives of sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with severe<br />

disabilities is an objective worthy of internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

References<br />

Asher, S. R., Parkhurst, J. T., Hymel, S., & Williams,<br />

G. A. (1990). Peer rejecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> l<strong>on</strong>eliness in<br />

childhood. In S. R. Asher, & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer<br />

rejecti<strong>on</strong> in childhood (pp. 253–273). Cambridge,<br />

UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Bagwell, C. L., Schmidt, M. E., Newcomb, A. F., &<br />

Bukowski, W. M. (2001). Friendship <strong>and</strong> peer rejecti<strong>on</strong><br />

as predictors of adult adjustment. New Directi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Development, 91, 25–49.<br />

Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejecti<strong>on</strong>: Developmental<br />

processes <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> strategies. New York, NY:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Guilford Press.<br />

Boyd, B. A., C<strong>on</strong>roy, M. A., Asmus, J. M., McKenney,<br />

E. L. W., & Mancil, G. H. (2008). Descriptive<br />

analysis of classroom setting events <strong>on</strong> the social<br />

behaviors of children with autism spectrum disorder.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

43, 186–197.<br />

Bui, Y., & Turnbull, A. P. (2003). East meets west:<br />

Friendships / 35


Analysis of pers<strong>on</strong>-centered planning in the c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

of Asian American values. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

28, 18–31.<br />

Bunch, G., & Valeo, A. (2004). Student attitudes<br />

toward peers with disabilities in inclusive <strong>and</strong> special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> schools. Disability <strong>and</strong> Society, 19, 61–76.<br />

Buysse, V., Goldman, B. D., & Skinner, M. L. (2003).<br />

Friendship formati<strong>on</strong> in inclusive early childhood<br />

classrooms: What is the teacher’s role? Early Childhood<br />

Research Quarterly, 18, 485–501.<br />

Buyesse, V., Nabors, L., Skinner, D., & Keyes, L.<br />

(1997). Playmate preferences <strong>and</strong> percepti<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

individual differences am<strong>on</strong>g typically developing<br />

preschoolers. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care,<br />

131, 1–18.<br />

Camer<strong>on</strong>, D. L., & Cook, B. G. (2007). Attitudes of<br />

preservice teachers enrolled in an infusi<strong>on</strong> preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

program regarding planning <strong>and</strong> accommodati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for included students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 42, 353–363.<br />

Day, M., & Harry, B. (1999). “Best friends”: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> of a teenage friendship, Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

37, 221–231.<br />

Dym<strong>on</strong>d, S. K., Renzaglia, A., & Chun, E. J. (2008).<br />

Inclusive high school service learning programs:<br />

Methods for <strong>and</strong> barriers to including students<br />

with disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 43, 20–36.<br />

Edeh, O. M. (2007). Cross-cultural investigati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

interest-based training <strong>and</strong> social interpers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

problem solving in students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

41, 163–176.<br />

Emers<strong>on</strong>, E., & McVilly, K. (2004). Friendship activities<br />

of adults with intellectual disabilities in<br />

supported accommodati<strong>on</strong> in northern Engl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,<br />

17, 191–197.<br />

French, D. C., Bae, A., Pidada, S., & Lee, O. (2006).<br />

Friendships of Ind<strong>on</strong>esian, South Korean, <strong>and</strong><br />

U.S. College Students. Pers<strong>on</strong>al Relati<strong>on</strong>ships, 13(1),<br />

69–81.<br />

Fryxell, D., & Kennedy, C. H. (1995). Placement<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g the c<strong>on</strong>tinuum of services <strong>and</strong> its impact <strong>on</strong><br />

students’ social relati<strong>on</strong>ships. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 20, 259–269.<br />

Furman, W., & Bierman, K. L. (1984). Children’s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s of friendship: A multimethod study<br />

of developmental changes. Developmental Psychology,<br />

20, 925–931.<br />

Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Social<br />

Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American<br />

Guidance Services.<br />

Haghighatgou, H., & Peters<strong>on</strong>, C. (1995). Coping<br />

<strong>and</strong> depressive symptoms am<strong>on</strong>g Iranian stu-<br />

dents. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Social Psychology, 135, 175–<br />

180.<br />

Hamm, J. V. (2000). Do birds of a feather flock<br />

together? <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> variable bases fro African American,<br />

Asian American, <strong>and</strong> European American<br />

Adolescents’ Selecti<strong>on</strong> of Similar Friends. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 36, 209–219.<br />

Hamre-Nietupski, S., Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J., Nietupski, J.,<br />

& Shokoohi-Yekta, M. (1994). Regular educators’<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s of facilitating friendships of students<br />

with moderate, severe, or profound disabilities<br />

with n<strong>on</strong>disabled peers. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

29, 102–117.<br />

Hamre-Nietupski, S., Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J. M., Nietupski,<br />

J., & Sasso, G. (1993). Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of teachers of<br />

students with moderate, severe, or profound disabilities<br />

<strong>on</strong> facilitating friendships with n<strong>on</strong>disabled<br />

peers. Educati<strong>on</strong> & Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

28, 111–127.<br />

Han, K. G., & Chadsey, J. G. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> influence<br />

of gender patterns <strong>and</strong> grade level <strong>on</strong> friendship<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s of middle school students toward<br />

peers with severe disabilities. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 205–214.<br />

Harper, D. C. (1997). Children’s attitudes toward<br />

physical disability in Nepal. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Cross-<br />

Cultural Psychology, 28, 710–729.<br />

Hartup, W. W. (1992). Friendships <strong>and</strong> their developmental<br />

significance. In H. McGurk (Ed.),<br />

Child social development: C<strong>on</strong>temporary perspectives<br />

(pp. 175–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Hartup, W. W. (1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> company they keep:<br />

Friendships <strong>and</strong> their developmental significance.<br />

Child Development, 67, 1–13.<br />

Heiman, T. (2000). Friendship quality am<strong>on</strong>g children<br />

in three educati<strong>on</strong>al settings. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Intellectual<br />

& Developmental Disability, 25, 1–12.<br />

Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J. M., Shokoohi-Yekta, M., Hamre-<br />

Nietupski, S., & Gable, R. A. (1996). Middle <strong>and</strong><br />

high school students’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> being friends<br />

with peers with severe disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

63, 19–28.<br />

IDEA’97 Amendments, Final Regulati<strong>on</strong>s. (1999).<br />

Retrieved 20 October 2007 from http:www.<br />

edgov//legislati<strong>on</strong>/FedRegister/finrule/199-2/<br />

062499a.pdf.<br />

Individuals with Disabilities Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement Act<br />

of 2004, Public Law 108-446. (2004). Washingt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.<br />

Joibari, A. A. K. (2003). Perspectives <strong>on</strong> inclusive educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in the Islamic Republic of Iran. Retrieved<br />

22 October 2007 from http://www.eenet.org.uk/<br />

theory_practice/iran.shtml.<br />

Kennedy, C. H., Shukla, S., & Fryxell, D. (1997).<br />

Junior high schools: H<strong>and</strong>icapped students. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 64, 31–47.<br />

Koh, J., Mendels<strong>on</strong>, M. J., & Rhee, U. (2003).<br />

36 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Friendship satisfacti<strong>on</strong> in Korean <strong>and</strong> Canadian<br />

university students. Canadian <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavioural<br />

Science, 35, 239–253.<br />

Laws, G., & Kelly, E. (2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> attitudes <strong>and</strong><br />

friendship intenti<strong>on</strong>s of children in United Kingdom<br />

mainstream schools towards peers with physical<br />

or intellectual disabilities. Internati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of Disability, Development <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 52, 79–99.<br />

Lee, S. H., Yoo, S. Y., & Bak, S. H. (2003). Characteristics<br />

of friendships between children with <strong>and</strong><br />

without mild disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Developmental Disabilities, 38, 157–166.<br />

Malgren, K. W., & Caust<strong>on</strong>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>oharis, J. N. (2006).<br />

Boy in the bubble: Effects of paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

proximity <strong>and</strong> other pedagogical decisi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong><br />

the interacti<strong>on</strong>s of a student with behavioral disorders.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Research in Childhood Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

20, 201–312.<br />

Margalit, M., & Efrati, M. (1996). L<strong>on</strong>eliness, coherence<br />

<strong>and</strong> compani<strong>on</strong>ship am<strong>on</strong>g children with<br />

learning disorders. Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 16, 69–<br />

79.<br />

Melchiorre, A. (2004). At what age are schoolchildren<br />

employed, married <strong>and</strong> taken to court?<br />

Retrieved 22 October 2000 from http://www.<br />

right-to-educati<strong>on</strong>.org/c<strong>on</strong>tent/age/iran/html.<br />

McVilly, K. R., Stancliffe, R. J., Parmenter, T. R., &<br />

Burt<strong>on</strong>-Smith, R. M. (2006). Self-advocates have<br />

the last say <strong>on</strong> friendship. Disability & Society, 21,<br />

693–708.<br />

Nasrabadi, A. N., Parsayekta, Z., & Emami, A.<br />

(2004). Smoking as a symbol of friendship:<br />

Qualitative study of smoking behavior <strong>and</strong> initiati<strong>on</strong><br />

of a group of male nurse students in Iran.<br />

Nursing Health Sciences, 6, 2009–215.<br />

O’Brien, S. F., & Bierman, K. L. (1988). C<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> perceived influence of peer groups:<br />

Interviews with preadolescents <strong>and</strong> adolescents.<br />

Child Development, 59, 1360–1365.<br />

Odom, S. L., Zercher, C., Li, S., Marquart, J. M.,<br />

S<strong>and</strong>all, S., & Brown, W. H. (2006). Social acceptance<br />

<strong>and</strong> rejecti<strong>on</strong> of preschool children with<br />

disabilities: A mixed-method analysis. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 98, 807–823.<br />

Overt<strong>on</strong>, S., & Rausch, J. L. (2002). Peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

as support for children with disabilities: An<br />

analysis of mothers’ goals <strong>and</strong> indicators of<br />

friendship. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 17, 11–29.<br />

Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship <strong>and</strong><br />

friendship quality in middle childhood: Links<br />

with peer group acceptance <strong>and</strong> feelings of l<strong>on</strong>eliness<br />

<strong>and</strong> social dissatisfacti<strong>on</strong>. Developmental Psychology,<br />

29, 611–621.<br />

Pottie, C., & Sumarah, J. (2004). Friendships between<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>and</strong> without developmental<br />

disabilities. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 42, 55–66.<br />

Rosenblum, L. P. (1998). Best friendships of adoles-<br />

cents with visual impairments: A descriptive study.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 92, 593–<br />

608.<br />

Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult<br />

life. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 30,<br />

23–51.<br />

Sasso, G. M., Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J. M., Mundschenk,<br />

N. A., Shokoohi-Yekta, M., Kelly, L., & Plagmann,<br />

L. A. (1992). Social behavior identified through<br />

an informant-based assessment procedure: Normative<br />

data across age <strong>and</strong> gender. In R. Rutherford,<br />

Jr., S. A. DiGangi, & S. R. Mathur (Eds.),<br />

Severe Behavior Disorders of Children <strong>and</strong> Youth<br />

(Vol. II) (pp. 44–56). Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Council for<br />

Children with Behavior Disorders.<br />

Shahim, S. (1999). Correlati<strong>on</strong>s between parents’<br />

<strong>and</strong> teachers’ ratings of social skills for a group of<br />

developmentally disabled children in Iran. Psychological<br />

Reports, 85, 863–866.<br />

Shams, M. (2001). Social support, l<strong>on</strong>eliness <strong>and</strong><br />

friendship preference am<strong>on</strong>g British Asian <strong>and</strong><br />

n<strong>on</strong>-Asian adolescents. Social Behavior <strong>and</strong> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

29, 399–404.<br />

Shogren, K. A., Wehmeyer, M. L., Palmer, S. B.,<br />

Soukup, J. H., Little, T. D., Garner, N., et al.<br />

(2007). Examining individual <strong>and</strong> ecological predictors<br />

of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> of students with disabilities.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 73, 488–509.<br />

Shokouhi-Behnam, S., Chambliss, C. A., & Caruso,<br />

K. A. (1997). Cross-cultural applicability of c<strong>on</strong>textual<br />

family therapy: Iranian <strong>and</strong> American college<br />

students’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s of familial <strong>and</strong> peer<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ships, Psychological Reports, 80, 691–694.<br />

Siperstein, G. N., Leffert, J. S., & Wenz-Gross, M.<br />

(1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> quality of friendships between children<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without learning problems. American<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 102, 111–125.<br />

Turnbull, A. P., Blue-Banning, M., & Pereira, L.<br />

(2000). Successful friendships of Hispanic children<br />

<strong>and</strong> youth with disabilities: An exploratory<br />

study. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 38, 138–153.<br />

Turnbull, A., Turnbull, R., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2007). Excepti<strong>on</strong>al lives: Special educati<strong>on</strong> in today’s<br />

schools (5 th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey:<br />

Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, Inc.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Munk, D. D., Lynch, K., Wyl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

N. D., Zurita, L., Bosma, J., et al. (2006). Project<br />

ACCEPT: Preparing pre-service special <strong>and</strong> general<br />

educators for inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>. Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 29, 209–212.<br />

Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). Close <strong>and</strong> general friendships<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g African American, Latino, <strong>and</strong> Asian<br />

American adolescents from low-income families.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Adolescent Research, 15, 274–301.<br />

Received: 4 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 15 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 10 February 2009<br />

Friendships / 37


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 38–53<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

L<strong>on</strong>g-term Outcomes of Services in Inclusive <strong>and</strong><br />

Self-C<strong>on</strong>tained Settings for Siblings with Comparable<br />

Significant Disabilities<br />

Diane Ryndak<br />

University of Florida<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ra Alper<br />

University of Northern Iowa<br />

Terri Ward<br />

College of St. Rose<br />

Jill F. Storch <strong>and</strong><br />

Jennifer Wils<strong>on</strong> M<strong>on</strong>tgomery<br />

University of Florida<br />

Abstract: A major goal of special educati<strong>on</strong> services is to prepare graduates for productive <strong>and</strong> rewarding lives<br />

as adults in the community. Follow-up studies, however, c<strong>on</strong>sistently have indicated very poor post-school<br />

outcomes for special educati<strong>on</strong> graduates. While there has been a wealth of research related to the effectiveness<br />

of services in inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong> settings for students of school age, few studies have investigated the<br />

impact of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>al services <strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes for students with significant disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

purpose of this study was to describe how two brothers with similar diagnoses involving significant disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> who had a 10-year age difference functi<strong>on</strong>ed in their natural settings as young adults after receiving<br />

services in the same rural <strong>on</strong>e-building district. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> older brother received special educati<strong>on</strong> services in<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained settings throughout his school career, while the younger brother received special educati<strong>on</strong> services<br />

in inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong> settings. Qualitative data were collected through records, interviews, <strong>and</strong> field<br />

notes of observati<strong>on</strong>s. Findings suggest that the brother who received special educati<strong>on</strong> services in the school’s<br />

inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong> settings achieved more positive l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes. Implicati<strong>on</strong>s are discussed for<br />

future research <strong>and</strong> provisi<strong>on</strong> of educati<strong>on</strong>al services.<br />

Many benefits of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with significant disabilities have been<br />

documented. A comprehensive list of the research<br />

in support of those benefits would be<br />

lengthy <strong>and</strong> reveal increases in appropriate<br />

social behaviors, increased interacti<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

others, more positive affect, increased friendships,<br />

<strong>and</strong> improved communicati<strong>on</strong> skills, as<br />

well as improved acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> use of academic<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> an increased likelihood<br />

of participati<strong>on</strong> in other inclusive settings<br />

(McLaughlin, Ryndak, & Alper, 2008). In c<strong>on</strong>trast,<br />

although we have found literature that<br />

presents critiques of various studies about inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> arguments that suggest<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Diane Ryndak, University of Florida,<br />

Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 1403 Norman<br />

Hall, PO Box 117050, Gainesville, FL 32611-<br />

7050. E-mail: dryndak@ufl.edu<br />

that inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> may have a negative<br />

impact up<strong>on</strong> learners (e.g., S<strong>and</strong>ler, 1999), we<br />

have found no research related to inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for students with significant disabilities<br />

that provide learner performance data in<br />

support of those arguments. (For a summary<br />

of research regarding inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> see<br />

Fisher <strong>and</strong> Ryndak (2001); McGregor <strong>and</strong> Vogelsberg<br />

(1998); Ryndak <strong>and</strong> Fisher (2003).)<br />

Fisher, Sax, <strong>and</strong> Jorgensen (1998) noted<br />

that in the United States the educati<strong>on</strong> system<br />

is expected to c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the preparati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

children for the dem<strong>and</strong>s of adult life (p. 30)<br />

(see also Lipsky & Gartner, 1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> expectati<strong>on</strong><br />

is that when students with disabilities<br />

exit school they will be prepared for successful<br />

<strong>and</strong> rewarding lives as adults, as c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

has been reflected in the literature (Alper,<br />

2003; Fisher et al.; Karagiannis, Stainback, &<br />

Stainback, 1996; Kliewer, 1998; Ryndak, Morris<strong>on</strong>,<br />

& Sommerstein, 1999).<br />

Unfortunately, the results of follow-up stud-<br />

38 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


ies of graduates of special educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

reveal dismal outcomes. For example,<br />

following graduati<strong>on</strong>, students with significant<br />

disabilities typically: (a) are isolated socially,<br />

with little c<strong>on</strong>tact with pers<strong>on</strong>s who do not<br />

have disabilities; (b) experience l<strong>on</strong>g periods<br />

of inactivity; (c) experience a low level of employment<br />

<strong>and</strong>, even when employed, seldom<br />

work full time <strong>and</strong> earn very low wages; (d)<br />

live with a parent, guardian, or relative; <strong>and</strong><br />

(e) rarely are involved in activities outside of<br />

the home (e.g., Edgar, 1987; Haring & Lovett,<br />

1990; Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se results are<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent with findings from a comprehensive<br />

l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal report issued by the Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Center <strong>on</strong> Disability <strong>and</strong> Social Security Administrati<strong>on</strong><br />

in 2000.<br />

With an emphasis <strong>on</strong> access to general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

settings <strong>and</strong> curricula, accountability,<br />

valued membership in peer groups, <strong>and</strong> facilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

of friendships that may lead to natural<br />

support networks, inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> has<br />

been c<strong>on</strong>sidered a practice that not <strong>on</strong>ly is<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent with civil rights, but also is a way to<br />

alleviate the discouraging outcomes for adults<br />

with significant disabilities. Although existing<br />

research indicates that inclusive educati<strong>on</strong><br />

can benefit students with significant disabilities<br />

<strong>on</strong> a short term basis (e.g., Fisher &<br />

Meyer, 2002; Ryndak et al., 1999), there have<br />

been no follow-up investigati<strong>on</strong>s of the lives of<br />

adults with significant disabilities who experienced<br />

inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> for lengthy periods<br />

of time. Given the lack of l<strong>on</strong>g-term follow-up<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>s, it is difficult to determine<br />

whether or not pers<strong>on</strong>s lead more satisfying<br />

lives as a functi<strong>on</strong> of educati<strong>on</strong>al services in<br />

inclusive or segregated settings. This investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

addresses this questi<strong>on</strong> by studying how<br />

two brothers with similar early diagnoses of<br />

significant disabilities functi<strong>on</strong>ed through age<br />

17, although they received educati<strong>on</strong>al services<br />

in different types of settings in the same<br />

rural <strong>on</strong>e-building school district.<br />

Method<br />

Two brothers with significant disabilities participated<br />

in this qualitative study. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se individuals<br />

<strong>and</strong> the methods used to describe<br />

their experiences, services, <strong>and</strong> outcomes<br />

across time are described below.<br />

Participants<br />

Prior to this study, the first author had an<br />

<strong>on</strong>going relati<strong>on</strong>ship with the brothers <strong>and</strong><br />

their family for approximately 11 years<br />

through the parents’ advocacy for the development<br />

<strong>and</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong> of effective services<br />

for their s<strong>on</strong>s in inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

settings. Mark <strong>and</strong> Jim were part of a<br />

family of seven (i.e., five children <strong>and</strong> two<br />

parents) who resided in a small town in a rural<br />

area of a northeastern state. All five children<br />

attended the same <strong>on</strong>e-building public school<br />

district in their home town. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> brothers had<br />

a 10 year age difference <strong>and</strong> received special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> services in different types of settings<br />

-- the older brother received services in selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> settings until age<br />

17, while the younger brother received services<br />

in inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong> settings<br />

until age 17. Because of their family relati<strong>on</strong>ship,<br />

initial diagnoses, <strong>and</strong> residence in a rural<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-building school district, these brothers<br />

presented a naturally-occurring opportunity<br />

to study the l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes of special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> services provided in different types<br />

of settings within the same family, community,<br />

<strong>and</strong> district c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first brother, Mark, was the eldest of<br />

five siblings. At age 3 Mark was c<strong>on</strong>sidered to<br />

be at-risk of having disabilities <strong>and</strong> began to<br />

receive preschool services. At age 4 he was<br />

diagnosed as having significant disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> received services in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> settings through age 16. Throughout<br />

this time Mark’s records indicated minimal<br />

to no access to peers without disabilities.<br />

However, while in high school at age 17 his<br />

access to high school students without disabilities<br />

increased to part of the school day. This<br />

access c<strong>on</strong>tinued until the age of 21 when he<br />

exited school. Throughout his educati<strong>on</strong>al career,<br />

Mark was labeled as having mild to moderate<br />

cognitive disabilities. At the end of this<br />

study, Mark was 28 years old, labeled as having<br />

significant disabilities, <strong>and</strong> receiving jobrelated<br />

<strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al support through the<br />

Medicaid Waiver Program.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> younger brother, Jim, was the fourth of<br />

the five siblings. He was identified at birth as<br />

being at-risk of having disabilities <strong>and</strong> thus<br />

received early interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> preschool services<br />

starting at four m<strong>on</strong>ths of age. During<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 39


his years in preschool he spent part of the<br />

week in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

class <strong>and</strong> part of the week in a regular preschool<br />

class. From kindergarten through age<br />

17 he c<strong>on</strong>tinuously was included in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> settings. Throughout his educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

career Jim was labeled as having multiple<br />

disabilities or moderate cognitive disabilities.<br />

Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

When Jim was 17 <strong>and</strong> Mark was 28, four of the<br />

co-authors used three qualitative methods to<br />

collect informati<strong>on</strong> related to their educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

experiences <strong>and</strong> Mark’s current performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> support as an adult. First, the coauthors<br />

worked with the brothers <strong>and</strong> their<br />

parents to obtain archival data relevant to<br />

their educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> adult services, as well as<br />

descripti<strong>on</strong>s of the brothers’ performance levels<br />

over time (Hammersley & Atkins<strong>on</strong>, 1995;<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>, 1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se data included records of<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s of both brothers periodically<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted by the first author over eleven<br />

years, school records, health records, <strong>and</strong> anecdotal<br />

records of numerous school pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

<strong>and</strong> adult services providers. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the<br />

co-authors used semi-structured interview protocols<br />

to interview the brothers, their parents<br />

<strong>and</strong>, when possible, their current service providers<br />

(Kvale, 1996; Mas<strong>on</strong>; Rubin & Rubin,<br />

1995; Strauss & Corbin, 1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> current<br />

service providers for Mark included a paid<br />

adult services caregiver, while for Jim this included<br />

members of his special educati<strong>on</strong> team<br />

at school. Third, the co-authors c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s of the brothers in their naturallyoccurring<br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts (Bogdan & Taylor, 1975;<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>; Patt<strong>on</strong>, 1990; Strauss & Corbin). Observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted of Mark with his<br />

adult services caregiver in the community, of<br />

Jim across school c<strong>on</strong>texts, <strong>and</strong> of both brothers<br />

with their parents at home.<br />

Data Analysis<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> records for each brother were organized<br />

chr<strong>on</strong>ologically. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> set of records for each<br />

brother then was read several times by two<br />

of the co-authors, <strong>and</strong> codes were developed<br />

related to the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the records (Kvale,<br />

1996; Rubin & Rubin, 1995; Strauss & Corbin,<br />

1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se two co-authors independently<br />

coded the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the records, <strong>and</strong> then<br />

met to compare their codes. When differences<br />

were found in the manner in which specific<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent was coded, the two co-authors discussed<br />

the differences <strong>and</strong> agreed up<strong>on</strong> how<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tent would be coded (Kvale; Rubin &<br />

Rubin; Strauss & Corbin). This sometimes resulted<br />

in the additi<strong>on</strong> of a new code, or in<br />

clarificati<strong>on</strong> of the meaning of an existing<br />

code. Once these two co-authors agreed <strong>on</strong><br />

how the c<strong>on</strong>tent was coded, the c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong><br />

codes were shared with two of the other coauthors.<br />

If questi<strong>on</strong>s arose about any of the<br />

codes, these four co-authors met to discuss the<br />

issue <strong>and</strong> reach c<strong>on</strong>sensus <strong>on</strong> the relevant<br />

codes for the c<strong>on</strong>tent. When all of the records<br />

were coded, secti<strong>on</strong>s with similar codes were<br />

grouped <strong>and</strong> analyzed for meaning (Kvale;<br />

Rubin & Rubin; Strauss & Corbin).<br />

Interviews were c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>and</strong> audiotaped<br />

by four of the researchers. While some interviews<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with <strong>on</strong>e individual<br />

(e.g., a brother or a service provider), other<br />

interviews were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with more than<br />

<strong>on</strong>e pers<strong>on</strong> present. For instance, the brothers’<br />

parents participated in a joint interview.<br />

Some interviews took two to five hours to complete<br />

<strong>and</strong> were c<strong>on</strong>ducted over two to three<br />

days. Other interviews (e.g., interviews with<br />

the brothers) took <strong>on</strong>e to two hours to complete.<br />

Interviews were c<strong>on</strong>ducted using guiding<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s established in an initial protocol<br />

(Creswell, 2003; Kvale, 1996; Rubin & Rubin,<br />

1995; Strauss & Corbin, 1998), but with several<br />

probing points per questi<strong>on</strong> to encourage the<br />

interviewees to give complete answers with<br />

meanings that were delineated clearly. Whenever<br />

possible, the interviewees were encouraged<br />

to exp<strong>and</strong> their answers, give examples<br />

that illustrated a point, <strong>and</strong> reiterate answers<br />

in another way in order to clarify their points<br />

(Kvale; Rubin & Rubin; Silverman, 1993). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

audiotapes then were transcribed <strong>and</strong> submitted<br />

to the interviewees for verificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

edits of the c<strong>on</strong>tent. Changes then were made<br />

to the initial transcripts, reflecting the interviewees’<br />

feedback. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tent of the final<br />

transcripts then was coded (Kvale; Rubin &<br />

Rubin; Strauss & Corbin) for analysis, using<br />

the same coding procedure used with the<br />

records.<br />

Field notes were taken during <strong>and</strong> after<br />

40 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


observati<strong>on</strong>s of the brothers by <strong>on</strong>e to three<br />

of the co-authors (Hammersley & Atkins<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1995). When observati<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with multiple observers present, each independently<br />

took notes. Current observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> multiple days, across multiple<br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts, across two weeks. After these<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s, the observers finished their independent<br />

notes <strong>and</strong> then discussed what<br />

they had observed. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> observers then returned<br />

to their independent notes <strong>and</strong> made<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al comments when appropriate<br />

(Hammersley & Atkins<strong>on</strong>). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> same two coauthors<br />

who coded the records <strong>and</strong> interviews<br />

also coded the field notes using the same coding<br />

procedure used with the records <strong>and</strong> interviews<br />

(Hammersley & Atkins<strong>on</strong>; Kvale,<br />

1996; Silverman, 1993).<br />

Once the coding procedures <strong>and</strong> analyses<br />

were completed for the c<strong>on</strong>tent of each set of<br />

data (i.e., records, interviews, field notes of<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s), the co-authors used triangulati<strong>on</strong><br />

strategies across the data sets to increase<br />

the trustworthiness of the data analysis methods<br />

(Hammersley & Atkins<strong>on</strong>, 1995; Kvale,<br />

1996; Mas<strong>on</strong>, 1996; Silverman, 1993) <strong>and</strong><br />

overall findings were articulated. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings<br />

were sent to the brothers’ parents for<br />

review (Mertens, 2005) <strong>and</strong> the parents were<br />

provided the opti<strong>on</strong> of reviewing the findings<br />

with the brothers. In either case, the parents<br />

were encouraged to make suggesti<strong>on</strong>s about<br />

edits, additi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> deleti<strong>on</strong>s that would either<br />

ensure that the findings were accurate or<br />

clarify any c<strong>on</strong>fusing points. This feedback was<br />

used to modify the findings.<br />

Findings<br />

School <strong>and</strong> district pers<strong>on</strong>nel in the brothers’<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-building school district were stable over<br />

the years, resulting in many of the same professi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

working in the school during the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al careers of both young men. In<br />

fact, some of the general <strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teachers in the high school had taught<br />

both brothers <strong>and</strong> the director of special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

had held that positi<strong>on</strong> throughout<br />

each brother’s attendance. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

parents were very active in the educati<strong>on</strong> programs<br />

for all of their children <strong>and</strong>, therefore,<br />

had <strong>on</strong>going interacti<strong>on</strong>s with many of the<br />

school <strong>and</strong> district pers<strong>on</strong>nel.<br />

In the following secti<strong>on</strong>s Mark’s <strong>and</strong> Jim’s<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al experiences <strong>and</strong> progress are described<br />

in age-related categories, including<br />

the early years, the kindergarten <strong>and</strong> elementary<br />

years, the middle school years, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

high school <strong>and</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> years. Tables 1 <strong>and</strong><br />

2 provide descriptors of the brothers across<br />

these age-related categories. Note that since<br />

Mark, the older brother, progressed through<br />

school 10 years ahead of his brother, Jim,<br />

occasi<strong>on</strong>ally his chr<strong>on</strong>ological age does not<br />

match the typical age at which most students<br />

have experiences in elementary, middle or<br />

high school. Where this occurs, clarificati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

have been provided in the secti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

below.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Early Years: Birth Through Five Years of Age<br />

Mark birth through five years of age. Mark was<br />

the eldest child in the family <strong>and</strong> was born<br />

after a normal nine m<strong>on</strong>th pregnancy <strong>and</strong><br />

with no noted birth abnormalities. Although<br />

his mother stated that she was not aware of all<br />

aspects of child development when Mark was<br />

born, she did not notice a problem with<br />

Mark’s overall development until he was three<br />

years old. She described Mark as an easy baby<br />

with some minor medical issues (e.g., ear infecti<strong>on</strong>s)<br />

during his first year. Mark often was<br />

described as a tense toddler who cried frequently.<br />

In retrospect, after raising five children<br />

<strong>and</strong> having the knowledge learned from<br />

that, his mother stated that she should have<br />

noticed that Mark developed skills later than<br />

his peer group. For example, he started walking<br />

when he was 15 m<strong>on</strong>ths old, <strong>and</strong> began<br />

toilet training after the age of three years.<br />

Given her later underst<strong>and</strong>ing of child development,<br />

his mother indicated that if Mark<br />

had not been her first born child she may have<br />

requested that her pediatrician test him earlier<br />

for developmental delay. However, since<br />

Mark showed no serious medical signs of disability,<br />

she believed he was a normally developing<br />

child. At the age of three years, Mark<br />

was labeled as being at-risk for developmental<br />

delays <strong>and</strong> was enrolled in a private preschool<br />

program that included children with disabilities.<br />

Though records during this time were<br />

incomplete, Mark’s mother stated that he received<br />

occupati<strong>on</strong>al, physical, <strong>and</strong> speech<br />

therapy in a daycare setting. As Mark prepared<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 41


TABLE 1<br />

Summary Descriptors of Mark Across Years<br />

High School <strong>and</strong> Transiti<strong>on</strong> Years:<br />

15–21 Years Adult Life: 28 Years<br />

Middle School Years:<br />

11–14 Years<br />

Kind & El Yrs:<br />

5–11 Years<br />

Early Years:<br />

Birth to 5 Years<br />

Lived with parents <strong>and</strong><br />

siblings<br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special ed classes outside<br />

of school district for 2 years<br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes outside of school<br />

district annually<br />

Counseling for developing<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ships, coping with<br />

frustrati<strong>on</strong>, adjusting to<br />

changes in routines,<br />

decreasing self-injurious<br />

behaviors<br />

OT, PT, Speech services<br />

provided primarily in<br />

segregated isolated settings<br />

Academic skills remain at K to<br />

1st grade level<br />

Anxiety documented annually;<br />

1:1 counseling annually for<br />

coping skills<br />

Easily frustrated with tasks;<br />

cried easily; difficulty<br />

changing routine<br />

Normal pregnancy; no<br />

birth abnormalities<br />

Worked less than 15<br />

hours per week<br />

At 17 accessed peers without disabilities<br />

in homeroom, lunch, <strong>and</strong> physical<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>; remaining time in selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

class<br />

Described as a “tense<br />

toddler”<br />

Relati<strong>on</strong>ships with family<br />

<strong>and</strong> paid caregivers –<br />

female <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

Remained anxious, wary<br />

of others, protective of<br />

bel<strong>on</strong>gings<br />

At 18 began attending general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classes; remaining time<br />

in community-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Counseling <strong>and</strong> SLP to develop<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ships (esp. with males) <strong>and</strong><br />

decrease anxiety<br />

Slow academic progress; some<br />

IEP objectives repeated 5<br />

c<strong>on</strong>secutive years<br />

Walked at 15 m<strong>on</strong>ths;<br />

toilet training<br />

begun after age 3<br />

Began OT, PT,<br />

Speech at age 3<br />

2 nd grade reading level; IQ score of 61;<br />

8 years 1 m<strong>on</strong>th <strong>on</strong> PPVT<br />

Described vocati<strong>on</strong>ally as “not able to<br />

work independently;” “requires<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant supervisi<strong>on</strong> to complete<br />

tasks”<br />

IEP shifted toward acquiring skills in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts after<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> began; c<strong>on</strong>tinued focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> job skills<br />

Vocati<strong>on</strong>al assessment at age 13<br />

with IEP shift to prevocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills; projecti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>gregate care “community<br />

residence” as adult<br />

Age 3 labeled at-risk<br />

of developmental<br />

delays<br />

Initial IQ score of 46;<br />

labeled with<br />

cognitive disability<br />

42 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010<br />

Needed calendar to<br />

structure daily<br />

activities; completi<strong>on</strong><br />

of tasks <strong>on</strong>ly if <strong>on</strong><br />

calendar<br />

Stated willingness to<br />

engage in social<br />

activities but was “too<br />

busy”


TABLE 2<br />

Summary Descriptors of Jim Across Years<br />

Early Years:<br />

Birth to 5 Years<br />

Normal pregnancy with<br />

numerous birth<br />

abnormalities – cleft<br />

palate, microcephaly,<br />

possible cortical<br />

blindness, gastrointestinal<br />

tube<br />

feeding<br />

Labeled as multiply<br />

impaired; Early<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> at 4<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths with OT, PT,<br />

Speech interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained pre-K in<br />

AM; day care with<br />

n<strong>on</strong>disabled peers in<br />

PM<br />

A few m<strong>on</strong>ths growth<br />

per year<br />

Age 3 <strong>and</strong> 5 IQ scores<br />

of 40<br />

Significant delay at age<br />

4 assessment of<br />

coping skills (2.6<br />

year level)<br />

Kind & El Yrs:<br />

5–11 Years<br />

1 st year in Kindergarten<br />

– no special ed<br />

support; 7 m<strong>on</strong>ths<br />

growth in receptive<br />

language (most<br />

growth to date)<br />

2 nd year in Kindergarten<br />

– coordinated OT,<br />

PT, Speech, special<br />

educator support; a<br />

1.9 year increase in<br />

receptive <strong>and</strong> 1.1<br />

years in expressive<br />

language; reduced<br />

anxiety<br />

Labeled with cognitive<br />

disability<br />

2 nd grade – stopped<br />

services for anxiety<br />

IEP goals focused <strong>on</strong><br />

participati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

science, social studies,<br />

reading, <strong>and</strong> math<br />

to leave preschool services he was evaluated<br />

for disabilities <strong>and</strong> records indicate Mark had<br />

an intelligent quotient (IQ) score of 46. No<br />

specific reference, however, was made to the<br />

tests used to determine this score. Subsequently<br />

he was labeled as having a cognitive<br />

disability. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> school district recommended a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong> Kindergarten<br />

class that was directed by a regi<strong>on</strong>al special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> cooperative <strong>and</strong> was located in a<br />

classroom rented in <strong>on</strong>e of the surrounding<br />

school districts. His mother questi<strong>on</strong>ed the<br />

appropriateness of a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained class that<br />

was located 90 minutes away from his home.<br />

Middle School Years:<br />

11–14 Years<br />

Described as flexible;<br />

exhibited few anxietyrelated<br />

behaviors<br />

Tolerated mild changes<br />

in routine without<br />

disrupti<strong>on</strong>; preferred<br />

set routines<br />

OT, PT, Speech in both<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

pull-out settings<br />

IEP goals focused <strong>on</strong><br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al academics<br />

<strong>and</strong> keyboarding<br />

Participated in extracurricular<br />

activities, with<br />

support when needed<br />

High School & Transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Years: 15–21 Years<br />

Navigated school<br />

independently; attended<br />

to tasks in classes;<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated curiosity<br />

of new tasks <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

“Hung-out” with classmates<br />

before, after, <strong>and</strong> during<br />

school; mirrored social<br />

cues; dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

flexible demeanor;<br />

comfortable interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with numerous peers<br />

<strong>and</strong> adults<br />

IEP focused <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

academics <strong>and</strong><br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> with<br />

peers<br />

Played drums in school<br />

b<strong>and</strong>; participated <strong>on</strong><br />

track & field team;<br />

played bells in church<br />

bell choir<br />

9 th grade – dismissed from<br />

OT <strong>and</strong> PT<br />

Academic skills remained<br />

at K–2 nd grade level; IQ<br />

score of 46<br />

Alternatively she requested that Mark attend<br />

Kindergarten in his <strong>on</strong>e-building home school<br />

district, believing it would be more appropriate.<br />

Her request was denied by Mark’s school<br />

district <strong>and</strong> he was placed in the self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> Kindergarten originally<br />

recommended.<br />

Jim birth through 5 years of age. Ten years<br />

later Jim was born after a normal <strong>and</strong> uneventful<br />

nine m<strong>on</strong>th pregnancy <strong>and</strong> was the fourth<br />

of five children. He was diagnosed with a cleft<br />

palate, failure to thrive, microcephaly, <strong>and</strong><br />

possible cortical blindness. In additi<strong>on</strong>, his<br />

records indicated early c<strong>on</strong>cerns regarding<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 43


possible signs of Pierre Robin syndrome. Medical<br />

appointments <strong>and</strong> therapy sessi<strong>on</strong>s became<br />

part of Jim’s daily life. He was labeled as<br />

having multiple impairments <strong>and</strong> began to<br />

receive early interventi<strong>on</strong> services at home at<br />

four m<strong>on</strong>ths of age, focusing <strong>on</strong> language <strong>and</strong><br />

physical development. In additi<strong>on</strong>, Jim was fed<br />

through a gastrointestinal tube prior to the<br />

repair of his cleft palate when he was two years<br />

old.<br />

When he was 3 years old, Jim was labeled as<br />

having multiple disabilities <strong>and</strong> placed in a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained preschool program run by the<br />

regi<strong>on</strong>al special educati<strong>on</strong> cooperative. During<br />

his sec<strong>on</strong>d year of preschool services Jim<br />

attended two programs — mornings he attended<br />

a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained language-based preschool<br />

program through the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

cooperative; afterno<strong>on</strong>s he attended a<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> preschool in his home<br />

town. When he was three <strong>and</strong> five years old<br />

Jim was formally evaluated for disabilities, resulting<br />

in a reported overall IQ score of 40 for<br />

both evaluati<strong>on</strong>s. Similar to his brother Mark,<br />

no specific tests were referenced in the<br />

records to indicate the original source for this<br />

intellectual score. In additi<strong>on</strong>, Jim was assessed<br />

for anxiety with his coping skills evaluated<br />

to be at a 2 year 6 m<strong>on</strong>th level.<br />

When he was exiting preschool services,<br />

Jim’s mother again requested that her s<strong>on</strong> be<br />

placed in a general educati<strong>on</strong> Kindergarten in<br />

his <strong>on</strong>e-building home school district. As 10<br />

years earlier with Mark, this request was denied<br />

by the school district. At this time the<br />

district stated that (a) Jim’s intellectual abilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> coping skills were too low for him to<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate any benefit from being in an<br />

inclusive general educati<strong>on</strong> Kindergarten,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (b) his need for total communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

strategies could not be met in a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Kindergarten. Jim’s parents invoked<br />

their due process rights, <strong>and</strong> refused to sign<br />

the IEP developed by the district, ensuring<br />

that he would be placed in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes until the disagreement was settled<br />

through a due process hearing.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Kindergarten <strong>and</strong> Elementary Years<br />

Mark 5–11 years of age. Throughout his<br />

Kindergarten <strong>and</strong> elementary school years<br />

Mark received special educati<strong>on</strong> services in<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong> classes administered<br />

by the regi<strong>on</strong>al special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

cooperative. His psychologists, therapists, <strong>and</strong><br />

special educators reported that Mark was an<br />

anxious child, especially in new situati<strong>on</strong>s. Because<br />

of this he performed best during set<br />

routines. Mark was known to cry easily, seek<br />

adult reinforcement c<strong>on</strong>tinuously, <strong>and</strong> become<br />

frustrated easily during dem<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

tasks. Records also indicated that Mark<br />

needed an adjustment period at the beginning<br />

of each school year before he spoke at<br />

school. By 7 years of age (i.e., 2 nd grade) he<br />

was evaluated in relati<strong>on</strong> to his anxiety <strong>and</strong><br />

each year he received either <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e or<br />

small group counseling to facilitate the development<br />

of coping skills.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to addressing anxiety <strong>and</strong> coping<br />

skills, Mark’s early IEPs focused <strong>on</strong> the<br />

development of academic <strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

skills within his special educati<strong>on</strong> classes.<br />

Although some IEP objectives were repeated<br />

over 5 c<strong>on</strong>secutive years, the most c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cern over time appeared to be Mark’s level<br />

of anxiety, especially with males. He also remained<br />

nervous, cried frequently, <strong>and</strong> obsessed<br />

about changes in his routine. Most of<br />

his service providers, however, reported they<br />

were pleased with his incremental progress<br />

<strong>and</strong> repeatedly recommended that he c<strong>on</strong>tinue<br />

in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classes.<br />

Jim 5–11 years of age. Ten years later, when<br />

Jim was in Kindergarten <strong>and</strong> Mark was in middle<br />

school, Jim’s parents filed two due process<br />

complaints against the school district requesting<br />

that both Mark <strong>and</strong> Jim receive services in<br />

their <strong>on</strong>e-building home school district al<strong>on</strong>gside<br />

their neighborhood peers who did not<br />

have disabilities. While the impartial hearing<br />

for Jim was underway, the school district<br />

moved him from preschool to a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

half-day Kindergarten. His time in the<br />

Kindergarten was limited, however, because<br />

he was removed from the class for occupati<strong>on</strong>al,<br />

physical, <strong>and</strong> speech/language therapy,<br />

as well as resource room support. Records<br />

<strong>and</strong> interview transcripts verified an overall<br />

lack of coordinated support for Jim <strong>and</strong> no<br />

integrati<strong>on</strong> of related services in the Kindergarten<br />

class or activities. Jim’s parents w<strong>on</strong><br />

their impartial hearing <strong>and</strong> he attended general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> Kindergarten for a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

year, but for both the morning <strong>and</strong> afterno<strong>on</strong><br />

44 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During this sec<strong>on</strong>d year his services<br />

were slightly more coordinated <strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development activities were provided<br />

for his educati<strong>on</strong>al team by an outside c<strong>on</strong>sultant.<br />

This was the <strong>on</strong>ly time in his educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

experience that Jim spent more than 1 year at<br />

a grade level.<br />

During his sec<strong>on</strong>d year in Kindergarten,<br />

Jim’s IEP focused <strong>on</strong> independent functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

<strong>and</strong> success in the general educati<strong>on</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ment,<br />

as well as <strong>on</strong> reducing his anxiety.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to support from a special educator,<br />

therapists, <strong>and</strong> a paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al, Jim received<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e counseling weekly to address<br />

his anxiety. Although he was identified<br />

as having higher than normal levels of anxiety,<br />

Jim’s educati<strong>on</strong> team believed the anxiety was<br />

due partially to his learning style. Interviews<br />

<strong>and</strong> reports indicated that Jim often would<br />

watch his classmates participating in a new<br />

activity before he began to participate, especially<br />

when the activities required motor involvement.<br />

Progress reports from his early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> preschool programs indicated<br />

that Jim c<strong>on</strong>sistently dem<strong>on</strong>strated growth of<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly a few m<strong>on</strong>ths per year. Despite the lack of<br />

coordinated special educati<strong>on</strong> services during<br />

his first year of Kindergarten, records revealed<br />

that Jim made 9 m<strong>on</strong>ths of progress in receptive<br />

language as measured by the Preschool<br />

Language Scale Profile (Zimmerman, Steiner,<br />

& P<strong>on</strong>d, 1969), while his expressive language<br />

level remained relatively unchanged. It was<br />

during his sec<strong>on</strong>d year in Kindergarten that<br />

Jim made his greatest growth. Specifically, his<br />

receptive language level increased by 1.9 years<br />

<strong>and</strong> his expressive language level increased by<br />

1.1 years. His end-of-year speech-language report<br />

stated that Jim no l<strong>on</strong>ger required the<br />

use of total communicati<strong>on</strong> strategies, as he<br />

was communicating effectively without those<br />

accommodati<strong>on</strong>s. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, the end-ofyear<br />

counseling report indicated that his level<br />

of anxiety had reduced significantly across all<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Following his sec<strong>on</strong>d year in Kindergarten,<br />

Jim c<strong>on</strong>tinued his elementary educati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classes with 1 year per<br />

grade. His disability label changed from multiple<br />

disabilities to cognitive disability. Beginning<br />

in third grade, his IEPs focused <strong>on</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of academic c<strong>on</strong>tent across all areas<br />

of the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum. Al-<br />

though all curriculum areas were modified for<br />

him, the focus of his program became <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> to the greatest extent possible<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> science, social studies,<br />

math, <strong>and</strong> reading. Similar to his older<br />

brother Mark, Jim c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive<br />

speech <strong>and</strong> language services, occupati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> physical therapy, <strong>and</strong> counseling. Many of<br />

these services, however, were delivered in<br />

Jim’s general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom, <strong>and</strong> counseling<br />

was removed from his IEP by sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

grade.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Middle School Years<br />

Mark 11–14 years of age. Mark c<strong>on</strong>tinued to<br />

receive special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related services<br />

in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classes outside of his home<br />

district through the regi<strong>on</strong>al special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

cooperative. Anxiety c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cern for Mark <strong>and</strong> he c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive<br />

counseling in <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> small group sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

throughout his late elementary <strong>and</strong> middle<br />

school years. His annual IEP goals related<br />

to counseling focused <strong>on</strong> increasing meaningful<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s with peers (i.e., both male<br />

<strong>and</strong> female peers, although interacti<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

males were of more c<strong>on</strong>cern), sharing experiences<br />

with peers, improving communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills when faced with frustrating tasks, accepting<br />

changes in routine, <strong>and</strong> identifying<br />

<strong>and</strong> coping with social situati<strong>on</strong>s that caused<br />

him discomfort. Though IEP updates indicated<br />

that Mark made progress in these areas,<br />

subsequent IEPs c<strong>on</strong>tinued to focus <strong>on</strong> them.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, Mark’s IEPs included goals for<br />

decreasing unspecified self-injurious behavior,<br />

although records never indicated that he<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated such behaviors.<br />

At the age of 13, when he was chr<strong>on</strong>ologically<br />

a 7th or 8 th grader, Mark’s reading <strong>and</strong><br />

math scores remained at the Kindergarten to<br />

1 st grade level. Though his previous IEPs had<br />

focused primarily <strong>on</strong> acquiring basic math,<br />

literacy, social/behavioral, <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills, Mark’s IEP for this year dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

a significant change in the focus of his overall<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> program. Although functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> math were still part of Mark’s<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al program, his IEPs now had a very<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g emphasis <strong>on</strong> the development of specific<br />

work skills, communicati<strong>on</strong> skills within<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments, <strong>and</strong> independent<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 45


living skills. When evaluated for vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

potential Mark was determined to be unemployable<br />

at an independent level <strong>and</strong> to have<br />

numerous deficits, including fine motor skills<br />

(e.g., pulling tabs off cans), sorting, matching,<br />

<strong>and</strong> collating. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> vocati<strong>on</strong>al evaluati<strong>on</strong> indicated<br />

that Mark would require extensive practice<br />

to acquire new vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills. Other<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current reports predicted that Mark would<br />

require supported living services as an adult.<br />

His IEPs incorporated c<strong>on</strong>tent such as: (a)<br />

mixing soil for plants in a greenhouse, (b)<br />

developing knowledge of floral designs, (c)<br />

using a ph<strong>on</strong>e in a vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting, (d)<br />

developing work-related behaviors, (e) reading<br />

help wanted advertisements in a newspaper,<br />

(f) counting 10 objects, (g) making simple<br />

s<strong>and</strong>wiches, <strong>and</strong> (h) cutting vegetables. It<br />

was also during this year that Mark began to<br />

attend a highly supervised segregated special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting, where he remained<br />

for 3 years.<br />

Throughout his middle school years Mark<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive speech, occupati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> physical therapy, <strong>and</strong> counseling. Over<br />

several years his speech <strong>and</strong> occupati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

therapy services increasingly were delivered in<br />

the segregated vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting, while his<br />

physical therapy c<strong>on</strong>tinued <strong>on</strong> a pull-out basis,<br />

focusing <strong>on</strong> isolated activities to develop<br />

strength <strong>and</strong> coordinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Jim 11–14 years of age. Throughout his middle<br />

school experiences ten years later, Jim<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive related services in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts. Overall, his IEPs focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al academic skills, including<br />

the use of keyboarding skills <strong>and</strong> the development<br />

of 1 st grade skills in reading, writing, <strong>and</strong><br />

math. In additi<strong>on</strong>, his IEPs addressed participati<strong>on</strong><br />

across school c<strong>on</strong>texts, speech articulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> overall length of meaningful communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

with peers without disabilities. Jim<br />

also c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive occupati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong><br />

physical therapy. Though some therapy sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were held in pull-out situati<strong>on</strong>s, all of his<br />

services were designed to assist Jim in being<br />

successful in the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts. For example, his work <strong>on</strong><br />

the development of keyboarding skills was related<br />

to Jim’s meaningful writing in classes,<br />

<strong>and</strong> his work <strong>on</strong> fine motor skills related to his<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of class projects that required manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

of materials, including writing im-<br />

plements. Jim was described as a flexible<br />

young man who exhibited few, if any, anxious<br />

behaviors. He tolerated mild changes in his<br />

routine, although he worked best under, <strong>and</strong><br />

preferred, a set routine. Essentially Jim dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

steady progress through his middle<br />

school years <strong>and</strong> no specific academic or behavioral<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerns were identified by his educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

teams.<br />

When Jim was in 5 th grade his parents requested<br />

<strong>and</strong> were provided informati<strong>on</strong> regarding<br />

school-sp<strong>on</strong>sored extra-curricular activities.<br />

Jim began to participate c<strong>on</strong>sistently in<br />

those activities. All classes <strong>and</strong> extra-curricular<br />

activities in which Jim participated were regularly<br />

scheduled for his same-age classmates<br />

who did not have disabilities; supports, however,<br />

were provided for him during those<br />

activities. Throughout middle school Jim developed<br />

many positive relati<strong>on</strong>ships with his<br />

classmates <strong>and</strong> other peers who did not have<br />

disabilities. He did not, however, develop any<br />

close pers<strong>on</strong>al friendships that extended bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

school c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> High School <strong>and</strong> Transiti<strong>on</strong> Years<br />

Mark 15–21 years of age. After four years<br />

with a str<strong>on</strong>g IEP emphasis <strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

training in the segregated setting, Mark<br />

turned 17 years of age. At that point his parents<br />

were dissatisfied with his level of educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

progress <strong>and</strong> with the lack of c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong><br />

between the c<strong>on</strong>tent of his educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

program <strong>and</strong> the life they envisi<strong>on</strong>ed for him<br />

after school. At that point they requested that<br />

Mark attend general educati<strong>on</strong> high school<br />

classes for part of the school day in his <strong>on</strong>ebuilding<br />

home school district. Mark began to<br />

have access to high school students who did<br />

not have disabilities <strong>on</strong> the bus, during lunch,<br />

in 10th grade homeroom, <strong>and</strong> in 10 th grade<br />

physical educati<strong>on</strong>. For the remainder of the<br />

school day Mark received 1:1 support either in<br />

a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong> class in the<br />

same school or during community-based instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following year his parents then<br />

requested that Mark attend <strong>on</strong>ly general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes <strong>and</strong> community-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the district agreed. When he was 18<br />

years of age Mark attended 11 th grade general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classes, including biology, com-<br />

46 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


puter applicati<strong>on</strong>s, health, study hall, <strong>and</strong><br />

physical educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

During this time there appeared to be c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong><br />

about the outcomes desired for Mark<br />

<strong>and</strong>, therefore, about his learning priorities.<br />

For instance, c<strong>on</strong>sistent with his placement in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classes the c<strong>on</strong>tent of his<br />

IEPs shifted dramatically toward the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum c<strong>on</strong>tent.<br />

Unfortunately, however, his special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

support in general educati<strong>on</strong> classes was inc<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

in its effectiveness to facilitate<br />

meaningful access to the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

curriculum <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al activities. In additi<strong>on</strong><br />

anecdotal records <strong>and</strong> reports from<br />

outside providers still focused <strong>on</strong> preparing<br />

Mark for n<strong>on</strong>competitive employment in a<br />

sheltered workshop <strong>and</strong> group home services<br />

up<strong>on</strong> exiting school services. C<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

this, Mark c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive communitybased<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al training for two hours per<br />

day. His speech <strong>and</strong> language therapy <strong>and</strong><br />

counseling services c<strong>on</strong>tinued to focus <strong>on</strong> developing<br />

positive social relati<strong>on</strong>ships with<br />

both male <strong>and</strong> female classmates who did not<br />

have disabilities. While a counseling report<br />

emphasized Mark’s increased ability to interact<br />

with female classmates, it reiterated c<strong>on</strong>cern<br />

about his c<strong>on</strong>tinued nervousness around,<br />

<strong>and</strong> reluctance to interact with, all males.<br />

Two years later, when Mark was 19 years of<br />

age, his IEP focused <strong>on</strong> developing language,<br />

social, <strong>and</strong> academic skills within general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

courses, including English 12, social<br />

studies 12, c<strong>on</strong>sumer math, physical educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> driver’s educati<strong>on</strong>. While in driver’s<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> Mark did not actually learn to drive,<br />

but participated in all other aspects of the<br />

course. An IEP was developed for each general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> subject area, delineating the<br />

level of participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

expected of Mark in each unit per subject<br />

area. Mark also c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

training from a job coach in community-based<br />

work sites (i.e., a local grocery store,<br />

video store). In spite of this support records<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to indicate that Mark was unable<br />

to work independently, <strong>and</strong> that he required<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant supervisi<strong>on</strong> from the job coach or<br />

another employee. Mark’s vocati<strong>on</strong>al goals<br />

<strong>and</strong> objectives also focused <strong>on</strong> his social interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the workplace <strong>and</strong> his ability to<br />

work with a variety of supervisors. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

anxiety c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be addressed throughout<br />

Mark’s educati<strong>on</strong>al experience <strong>and</strong> he<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e counseling<br />

for his anxiety through his high school years.<br />

It is noteworthy that the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the<br />

quarterly comments <strong>and</strong> yearly updates during<br />

Mark’s high school years reflected a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

major shift. For the first time school pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> Mark’s participati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> courses <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts, curriculum<br />

modificati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> level of engagement<br />

throughout school c<strong>on</strong>texts with peers<br />

who did not have disabilities. Prior to that<br />

time no comments related to these variables<br />

were evident in Mark’s records.<br />

As Mark exited the educati<strong>on</strong> system, his<br />

formal evaluati<strong>on</strong>s indicated that he dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

an overall IQ of 61, an age equivalent<br />

score of 8 years 1 m<strong>on</strong>th <strong>on</strong> the Peabody<br />

Picture Vocabulary Test, <strong>and</strong> a 2nd grade<br />

reading level. His transiti<strong>on</strong> from school to<br />

adult life c<strong>on</strong>sisted of c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to live with<br />

his parents <strong>and</strong> four younger siblings, <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to be unemployed. With the assistance<br />

of a job coach, he eventually was employed<br />

at a grocery store chain but was released<br />

from the positi<strong>on</strong> after two years,<br />

without indicati<strong>on</strong> as to cause.<br />

Jim 15–21 years of age. Ten years later,<br />

when Jim was 15 years old <strong>and</strong> Mark had<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>ed to adult services, Jim spent his<br />

first three years of high school included in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> courses with special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

support for modificati<strong>on</strong>s in curriculum<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent, materials, instructi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> assessment.<br />

His IEPs c<strong>on</strong>tinued to focus <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

academics <strong>and</strong> communicating effectively<br />

with peers. Of particular interest to him<br />

were classes <strong>and</strong> experiences related to music<br />

<strong>and</strong> chorus. This interest extended to after<br />

school hours when he played the drums in the<br />

high school b<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> participated in a bell<br />

choir at his church. Outside observers who<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted ecological assessments remarked<br />

<strong>on</strong> Jim’s ability to navigate the school, freely<br />

interact with peers, focus <strong>on</strong> class assignments,<br />

<strong>and</strong> act up<strong>on</strong> his curiosity about new experiences.<br />

By the end of his 9 th grade year he no<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger received either occupati<strong>on</strong>al or physical<br />

therapy.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> IEP for his junior year reflected approximately<br />

1.5 hours a day of special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

support in his general educati<strong>on</strong> classes.<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 47


<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was, however, a clear c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong> between<br />

<strong>on</strong>-site observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> the IEP. Although<br />

Jim had been served in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in past years <strong>and</strong> the current IEP stated<br />

that special educati<strong>on</strong> services were to be provided<br />

within general educati<strong>on</strong> classes, his<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher stated that she was<br />

not in favor of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> had<br />

decided to provide instructi<strong>on</strong> for Jim in the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom during the 1.5<br />

hours per day he was to receive support. During<br />

an interview with <strong>on</strong>e of the co-authors<br />

the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher stated that while<br />

she had limited knowledge of Jim’s brother<br />

<strong>and</strong> his outcomes up<strong>on</strong> exiting school, she<br />

believed that Jim should receive services in a<br />

segregated classroom, just like his older<br />

brother.<br />

In spite of this discrepancy, Jim participated<br />

in school-sp<strong>on</strong>sored extra-curricular activities,<br />

including marching b<strong>and</strong>, track <strong>and</strong> field, <strong>and</strong><br />

student clubs (e.g., Students Against Driving<br />

Drunk). Except for the undocumented time<br />

that the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher removed<br />

Jim from his general educati<strong>on</strong> classes during<br />

his junior year, Jim’s high school experience<br />

had focused <strong>on</strong> participating in meaningful<br />

ways with chr<strong>on</strong>ological age peers who did not<br />

have disabilities in general educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

He received no separate instructi<strong>on</strong> related<br />

to vocati<strong>on</strong>al preparati<strong>on</strong> or transiti<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

to adult life in the community. Data<br />

collecti<strong>on</strong> for Jim stopped with his junior year<br />

in high school, when he was 17 years old. No<br />

data, therefore, is provided for Jim from the<br />

ages of 18–21 years.<br />

Final Observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Mark in adult life. At the age of 28, Mark<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to live with his parents <strong>and</strong> 4 siblings.<br />

He later was employed less than 15<br />

hours per week in the kitchen of a pizzeria.<br />

His mother had found this positi<strong>on</strong> for Mark,<br />

as well as procured support from a job coach.<br />

He was described as anxious in most situati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated this anxiety during<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s by three of the co-authors in naturally-occurring<br />

settings, even with family<br />

members present. For instance, Mark: (a)<br />

shifted his focus between people <strong>and</strong> objects<br />

quickly; (b) watched others in the setting in a<br />

surreptitious manner, as if wary of what they<br />

were going to do; (c) removed <strong>and</strong> hid his<br />

possessi<strong>on</strong>s that initially were evident in the<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment; <strong>and</strong> (d) frequently questi<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

what people in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment were doing,<br />

especially in relati<strong>on</strong> to him <strong>and</strong> his possessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Mark engaged in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

when they were initiated by others, <strong>and</strong> he<br />

limited his resp<strong>on</strong>ses to a sentence or two. At<br />

numerous times Mark commented that he did<br />

not enjoy working or interacting with men.<br />

While Mark had received counseling for this<br />

throughout his educati<strong>on</strong>al experiences, it<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be of c<strong>on</strong>cern both to him <strong>and</strong><br />

to those with whom he interacted. When at<br />

home, Mark preferred either to (a) be al<strong>on</strong>e<br />

in his room where he listened to music or<br />

watched his own videos, or (b) work al<strong>on</strong>e<br />

outside with his family’s chickens <strong>and</strong> ducks.<br />

Although Mark had a part time job, he had<br />

very few other activities in which he participated<br />

outside of the home. Support from<br />

adult services provided him with a paid female<br />

caregiver for six hours a week. She facilitated<br />

Mark’s participati<strong>on</strong> in activities both in the<br />

home <strong>and</strong> in the community. With her support<br />

Mark shopped for groceries, cooked<br />

meals, <strong>and</strong> went bowling weekly. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> fact that<br />

the paid caregiver was female was an important<br />

factor, since Mark c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be more<br />

wary of, <strong>and</strong> less resp<strong>on</strong>sive to, males. When<br />

asked if he would enjoy more social activities,<br />

Mark stated that he was very busy <strong>and</strong> could<br />

never find the time to do things, in spite of<br />

needing <strong>on</strong>ly 2 hours per week for his job <strong>and</strong><br />

time with his paid female caregiver. Mark<br />

completed <strong>on</strong>ly tasks written <strong>on</strong> a calendar of<br />

daily tasks.<br />

At the time of this study, the state agencies<br />

were erecting an apartment building for a<br />

supported living program for approximately<br />

10 adults with disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> advocacy for<br />

this program had been initiated by Mark’s<br />

parents when he was of middle school age.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir advocacy resulted in Mark being <strong>on</strong> the<br />

waiting list for this program <strong>and</strong> it was expected<br />

that he would move into this program<br />

as so<strong>on</strong> as the building was completed. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, Mark’s parents anticipated that he<br />

would c<strong>on</strong>tinue to work part-time in the community<br />

throughout his adult life.<br />

Jim in 11 th grade. As indicated in the previous<br />

secti<strong>on</strong>, during 11 th grade Jim officially<br />

was included in general educati<strong>on</strong> courses<br />

48 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


with special educati<strong>on</strong> support for modificati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

although his special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher provided that support in a segregated<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> class for 1.5 hours<br />

per day. In additi<strong>on</strong> Jim participated in numerous<br />

school-sp<strong>on</strong>sored extra-curricular<br />

activities. Speech-language therapy services<br />

had been decreased to twice a week for 30<br />

minutes. His last reported IQ score was 46<br />

<strong>on</strong> the WISC III (Wechsler, 1991) <strong>and</strong> be<br />

performed academically at a K-2nd grade<br />

level. His Medicaid Waiver evaluati<strong>on</strong> during<br />

11 th grade described his involvement<br />

with numerous clubs at school <strong>and</strong> in the<br />

community, <strong>and</strong> the level of ease he dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

when interacting with peers <strong>and</strong><br />

adults throughout the school. When observed<br />

Jim spent unstructured time with his<br />

peers who did not have disabilities, joined<br />

groups of students outside the school building<br />

<strong>and</strong>, just like his peers, “hung out” until<br />

the school bell rang for students to enter the<br />

school <strong>and</strong> prepare for homeroom. During<br />

this time he joined a group of students from<br />

the school’s marching b<strong>and</strong>. Although his<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> with each peer was short, he exchanged<br />

“high-fives,” smiles, <strong>and</strong> verbal<br />

greetings with several students. When the<br />

other students were engaged in informal<br />

c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>, Jim laughed at appropriate<br />

times <strong>and</strong> mirrored the body movements of<br />

his peers. Essentially, Jim blended into the<br />

social milieu of his peers, so that an observer<br />

would not have identified him as having<br />

disabilities without prior informati<strong>on</strong>. Jim<br />

joined his peers in independently entering<br />

the school, finding <strong>and</strong> using his locker appropriately,<br />

<strong>and</strong> proceeding to homeroom.<br />

Throughout these activities Jim appropriately<br />

greeted both school pers<strong>on</strong>nel <strong>and</strong><br />

other students in the hallways, blending into<br />

the social c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Up<strong>on</strong> entering classrooms, Jim c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to dem<strong>on</strong>strate an awareness of <strong>and</strong> appropriate<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se to the expectati<strong>on</strong>s within each<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text. He prepared for <strong>and</strong> participated in<br />

class activities in ways that matched his peers<br />

who did not have disabilities (e.g., gathering<br />

materials, copying informati<strong>on</strong> from the<br />

blackboard, following verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s), although<br />

his instructi<strong>on</strong>al goals <strong>and</strong> materials<br />

were modified.<br />

General Findings<br />

For the findings discussed above to be meaningful,<br />

the educati<strong>on</strong>al experiences <strong>and</strong> performance<br />

of Mark <strong>and</strong> Jim need to be reviewed<br />

<strong>and</strong> analyzed over time. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se reviews<br />

<strong>and</strong> analyses are briefly discussed for each<br />

brother in the following secti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Mark’s experiences <strong>and</strong> performance over time.<br />

Unfortunately, Mark’s overall performance <strong>on</strong><br />

academic, social, <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills did not<br />

appear to change significantly over time. Initially,<br />

he was described as an irritable baby<br />

who cried often. While the crying behavior<br />

diminished with age, other related behaviors<br />

(i.e., anxiety; nervousness) were manifested<br />

during his middle school, high school, <strong>and</strong><br />

adult years. Though counseling to address his<br />

anxiety occurred throughout his educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

career, Mark c<strong>on</strong>tinued to display uneasiness<br />

in social situati<strong>on</strong>s into his adult life. He c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to live with his family, though he was<br />

making plans to live in an apartment of a<br />

supported living program in his home community.<br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d his activities with family members,<br />

his social or networking activities were<br />

with <strong>on</strong>e female caregiver who was paid<br />

through the Medicaid Waiver.<br />

Although his early st<strong>and</strong>ardized testing indicated<br />

a moderate level of cognitive disability<br />

with an overall IQ of 46 (i.e., six points higher<br />

than that of his brother), Mark’s last set of<br />

tests reported an overall IQ of 61, which was<br />

15 points higher than that of his brother.<br />

While his reported IQ score increased over<br />

time, Mark’s assessed reading <strong>and</strong> math levels<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued at or below a 2 nd grade equivalency<br />

level.<br />

Although his IEPs began to address vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> work-related behaviors at the age of<br />

13, vocati<strong>on</strong>al evaluati<strong>on</strong>s reflected a clear expectati<strong>on</strong><br />

that Mark would not develop skills<br />

that would make him competitively employable.<br />

At 28 years of age Mark was employed for<br />

less than 15 hours a week at a local pizzeria<br />

with the support of a job coach twice a m<strong>on</strong>th.<br />

Jim’s experiences <strong>and</strong> performance over time.<br />

Jim’s performance changed significantly over<br />

time. He dem<strong>on</strong>strated an increase in independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong> in daily life at his<br />

school, in his community, <strong>and</strong> at home. Although<br />

when compared with his older<br />

brother, Mark, more physical complicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 49


were evident at Jim’s birth (i.e., a cleft palate,<br />

a gastrointestinal tube for feeding, dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

of failure to thrive, microcephaly, possible<br />

cortical blindness), these challenges were<br />

overcome <strong>and</strong> did not appear to have l<strong>on</strong>gterm<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> his involvement in the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum <strong>and</strong> extra-curricular activities<br />

at school. Though his overall st<strong>and</strong>ardized<br />

academic scores remained far lower than<br />

those of his older brother, Jim was more able<br />

to follow large group routines, use envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

supports <strong>and</strong> peers to seek answers to<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> engage in a meaningful way in<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al activities related to the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum.<br />

Although both Jim <strong>and</strong> his older brother<br />

were identified early in life as having high<br />

levels of anxiety, Jim’s services <strong>and</strong> goals related<br />

to anxiety completely faded from his<br />

IEPs <strong>and</strong> records by 3 rd grade. Though when<br />

in 11 th grade Jim still preferred c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

routines, he was able to tolerate schedule<br />

changes. In additi<strong>on</strong>, he attended to the cues<br />

of others regarding social behavior, <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

social competence when moving<br />

about his school, community, <strong>and</strong> home. This<br />

was a marked c<strong>on</strong>trast from his older brother’s<br />

behavior across envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Jim’s educati<strong>on</strong>al files did not reflect any<br />

specific vocati<strong>on</strong>al goals or objectives. Nor<br />

were there any predicti<strong>on</strong>s as to Jim’s ability or<br />

inability to be competitively employed in the<br />

future. Though it is too early to study Jim’s<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al outcomes related to vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills, employability, independent living skills,<br />

or social networks in the community as an<br />

adult, the progress he dem<strong>on</strong>strated by the<br />

age of 17 was significantly more substantial<br />

than his brother’s progress.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

One argument that has been provided for<br />

developing inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> practices for<br />

students with significant disabilities has been<br />

that inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> practices are c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with the students’ civil rights, focusing <strong>on</strong><br />

equal opportunity for access to general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

curriculum, instructi<strong>on</strong>, materials, <strong>and</strong><br />

activities. This access has led to the inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with significant disabilities in state<br />

<strong>and</strong> district assessments <strong>and</strong> accountability sys-<br />

tems, <strong>and</strong> has facilitated development of<br />

friendships with same-aged peers who do not<br />

have disabilities, leading to equal membership<br />

in peer groups <strong>and</strong> more extensive natural<br />

support networks. Finally, it has been hoped<br />

that through access to general educati<strong>on</strong>, accountability<br />

systems, <strong>and</strong> membership with<br />

peers in natural support networks the l<strong>on</strong>gterm<br />

outcomes for students with significant<br />

disabilities would be more positive than has<br />

been indicated in past follow-up studies.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study was to compare<br />

the l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes of services for two<br />

siblings with significant disabilities who had<br />

similar early diagnoses. While the brother who<br />

had attended inclusive school settings seemed<br />

poised for more positive post-school outcomes,<br />

the results must be viewed in light of<br />

the following limitati<strong>on</strong>s. First, descriptive<br />

data were collected <strong>and</strong> analyzed <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly two<br />

individuals using case-study methodology.<br />

This small set of participants <strong>and</strong> methodology<br />

do not allow for generalizati<strong>on</strong> of findings<br />

to other individuals with significant disabilities.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, while the authors collected data<br />

over time from multiple sources, no causal<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ships between school placements <strong>and</strong><br />

l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes are implied. We are<br />

aware that there are a myriad of individual<br />

student characteristics, setting characteristics,<br />

attitudes, <strong>and</strong> expectati<strong>on</strong>s of family members<br />

that surround any individual that may interact<br />

to influence post-school outcomes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> comparis<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of outcomes addressed in this study<br />

occurred naturally in the lives of the two<br />

brothers. No effort was made to c<strong>on</strong>trol for<br />

other variables that might account for the differences<br />

found. For instance, limited informati<strong>on</strong><br />

was gathered in relati<strong>on</strong> to the services<br />

provided in either the self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classes or the inclusive settings. No<br />

attempts were made during this study to influence<br />

the quality, type, or amount of those<br />

services, nor the availability or quality of the<br />

adult services.<br />

In spite of these limitati<strong>on</strong>s, the brother<br />

who received inclusive services in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts dem<strong>on</strong>strated more skills<br />

that were critical both to interacting with<br />

peers <strong>and</strong> adults who did not have disabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to functi<strong>on</strong>ing independently across c<strong>on</strong>texts,<br />

including at school, at home, <strong>and</strong> in the<br />

community. In additi<strong>on</strong>, this brother acquired<br />

50 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


<strong>and</strong> used knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills in meaningful<br />

naturally-occurring c<strong>on</strong>texts. As he became a<br />

young adult, he developed <strong>and</strong> maintained a<br />

life that more closely approximated that of his<br />

same-age general educati<strong>on</strong> classmates, even<br />

though his IQ <strong>and</strong> achievement test scores<br />

were lower than those of his older brother<br />

who received special educati<strong>on</strong> services in selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classes until he was 17 years of age.<br />

It could be argued that the presence of a<br />

deeply involved parent advocate who over<br />

time had learned about child development,<br />

inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the rights of students<br />

with disabilities could have accounted for differences<br />

between the educati<strong>on</strong>al experiences<br />

<strong>and</strong> progress made by her two s<strong>on</strong>s. In his<br />

classic studies of adults with disabilities who<br />

had moved from instituti<strong>on</strong>al to community<br />

living envir<strong>on</strong>ments, Edgert<strong>on</strong> (1967, 1978,<br />

1984) discussed the influence of a “benefactor”<br />

<strong>on</strong> the lives of those individuals. This<br />

study seems to support that c<strong>on</strong>cept. Undoubtedly,<br />

the fact that the brothers’ parents<br />

increasingly called for inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

services over the years <strong>and</strong> exercised their due<br />

process rights when not satisfied with their<br />

s<strong>on</strong>s’ educati<strong>on</strong>al services impacted the nature<br />

<strong>and</strong> quality of services received by their<br />

younger s<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir role as advocates argues<br />

for the <strong>on</strong>going involvement of parents <strong>and</strong><br />

others as benefactors who ensure that special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related services, as well as adult<br />

services, provided for individuals with significant<br />

disabilities reflect each individual’s needs<br />

<strong>and</strong> assist each individual in acquiring <strong>and</strong><br />

maintaining a high quality of life. While most<br />

often advocates are students’ parents, other<br />

family members, or guardians, any interested<br />

individual (e.g., teacher, paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al,<br />

counselor, family friend) might serve in this<br />

role (see, for example, Alper, Schloss, &<br />

Schloss, 1995). In additi<strong>on</strong>, the expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of adult service providers, including vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> home living pers<strong>on</strong>nel, appear to<br />

have become more positive. At <strong>on</strong>e time expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for adults with significant disabilities<br />

remained restricted to c<strong>on</strong>gregate care<br />

residential placement, adult day care, <strong>and</strong><br />

sheltered, if any, employment. Today, it is<br />

more the norm for adult service providers to<br />

strive for the goals of independent or semiindependent<br />

living in the community <strong>and</strong> supported<br />

employment that leads to competitive<br />

employment, as we have learned about the<br />

capabilities of pers<strong>on</strong>s with significant disabilities<br />

when appropriate training <strong>and</strong> supports<br />

are provided (Rizzo, 2002; Wehman 2006;<br />

Wehman, Brooke, Green, Hewett, & Tipt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2008; White, 2004). Additi<strong>on</strong>al research is required,<br />

however, to underst<strong>and</strong> the influence<br />

of the presence or absence of a “benefactor”<br />

in the lives of individuals with significant disabilities.<br />

Another variable that might have influenced<br />

the outcomes achieved by the younger<br />

brother in this study was access to <strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in activities that fostered the development<br />

of self-advocacy <strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

While not specifically c<strong>on</strong>sidered in this study,<br />

Jim, who was included in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts, had access to role models without<br />

disabilities who were developing <strong>and</strong> using<br />

self-advocacy <strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong> skills in<br />

their daily lives. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mere access to these role<br />

models may have facilitated development of<br />

these skills. Additi<strong>on</strong>al research, however, is<br />

needed to assist in our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the<br />

influence of such models in inclusive settings.<br />

It would be simplistic to argue that placement<br />

in inclusive settings in <strong>and</strong> of itself leads<br />

to more positive outcomes. Post-school adjustment<br />

is undoubtedly determined by a complex<br />

set of interrelated factors. It seems likely<br />

that best practices in inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

presence of <strong>on</strong>-going benefactors/advocates<br />

who hold high expectati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in self-advocacy <strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

all work in harm<strong>on</strong>y to yield positive outcomes.<br />

While the findings of this case study suggest<br />

that, when compared with services in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> settings, providing<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related services in inclusive<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> settings may lead to<br />

better outcomes for students with significant<br />

disabilities, these results must be viewed with<br />

cauti<strong>on</strong>. C<strong>on</strong>siderably more research that involves<br />

many more individuals with significant<br />

disabilities over multiple years is needed before<br />

widespread c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s can be derived<br />

about the efficacy of services in these settings.<br />

Until such research is c<strong>on</strong>ducted, however,<br />

this study offers encouragement that the current<br />

trend to include students with significant<br />

disabilities in general educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>texts can<br />

lead to positive l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcomes.<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 51


References<br />

Alper, S. (2003). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> relati<strong>on</strong>ship between inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> other trends in educati<strong>on</strong>. In D. L.<br />

Ryndak & S. Alper (Eds.), Curriculum <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with significant disabilities in inclusive<br />

settings (2 nd ed; pp. 13–30). Bost<strong>on</strong>: Allyn <strong>and</strong><br />

Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Alper, S., Schloss, P. J., & Schloss, C. N. (1995).<br />

Students with disabilities: C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> advocacy.<br />

Bost<strong>on</strong>: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bogdan, R., & Taylor, S. (1975). Introducti<strong>on</strong> to qualitative<br />

research methods. New York: John Wiley.<br />

Creswell, J. W. (2003). Research design: Qualitative,<br />

quantitative, <strong>and</strong> mixed methods approaches (2 nd ed).<br />

Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.<br />

Edgar, E. (1987). Sec<strong>on</strong>dary programs in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>: Are many of them justifiable? Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 53, 555–561.<br />

Edgert<strong>on</strong>, R. B. (1967). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> cloak of competence: Stigma<br />

in the lives of the mentally retarded. Berkeley, CA:<br />

University of California Press.<br />

Edgert<strong>on</strong>, R. B., & Bercovici, S. M. (1978). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

cloak of competence: Years later. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of Mental Deficiency, 80, 485–497.<br />

Edgert<strong>on</strong>, R. B., Bollinger, M., & Herr, B. (1984).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> cloak of competence: After two decades.<br />

American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Mental Deficiency 88, 345–351.<br />

Fisher, M., & Meyer, L. H. (2002). Development <strong>and</strong><br />

social competence after two years for students<br />

enrolled in inclusive <strong>and</strong> self-c<strong>on</strong>tained educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

programs. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Severe Disabilities, 27, 165–174.<br />

Fisher, D., & Ryndak, D. L. (Eds.) (2001). Foundati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>: A compendium of articles<br />

<strong>on</strong> effective strategies to achieve inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Fisher, D., Sax, C, & Jorgensen, C. M. (1998). Philosophical<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong>s of inclusive, restructuring<br />

schools. In C. M. Jorgensen (Ed.), Restructuring<br />

high schools for all students: Taking inclusi<strong>on</strong> to the<br />

next level (pp. 29–47). Baltimore: Paul H.<br />

Brookes.<br />

Hammersley, M., & Atkins<strong>on</strong>, P. (1995). Ethnography:<br />

Principles in practice (2 nd ed). L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Routledge.<br />

Haring, K. A., & Lovett, D. L. (1990). A follow-up<br />

study of special educati<strong>on</strong> graduates. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 463–477.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, D. R., Bloomberg, L., Lin, H. C., McGrew,<br />

K., Bruininks, R. H., & Kim, C. (1995). A sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

analysis of the findings from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

Study: An examinati<strong>on</strong> of the postschool outcomes of<br />

youth with severe disabilities. Minneapolis, MN: University<br />

of Minnesota, Institute <strong>on</strong> Community Integrati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Karagiannis, A., Stainback, W., & Stainback, S.<br />

(1996). In S. Stainback & W. Stainback (Eds.),<br />

Inclusi<strong>on</strong>: A guide for educators (pp. 3–28). Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Kliewer, C. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> meaning of inclusi<strong>on</strong>. Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 36, 317–321.<br />

Kvale, S. (1996). Interviews: An introducti<strong>on</strong> to qualitative<br />

research interviewing. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA:<br />

Sage Publishing.<br />

Lipsky, D. K., & Gartner, A. (1997). Inclusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

school reform: Transforming America’s classrooms. Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>, J. (1996). Qualitative researching. Thous<strong>and</strong><br />

Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.<br />

McGregor, G., & Vogelsberg, T. (1998). Inclusive<br />

schooling practices: Pedagogical <strong>and</strong> research foundati<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

A synthesis of the literature that informs best<br />

practices about inclusive schooling. Baltimore: Paul<br />

H. Brookes.<br />

McLaughlin, T., Ryndak, D. L., & Alper, S. (2008).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> beneficial outcomes of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A research summary. C<strong>on</strong>ference proceedings.<br />

Paris, France.<br />

Mertens, D. M. (2005). Research <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> psychology: Integrating diversity with<br />

quantitative, qualitative, <strong>and</strong> mixed methods (2 nd ed).<br />

Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Center <strong>on</strong> Disability <strong>and</strong> Social Security<br />

Administrati<strong>on</strong> (2000, November). Transiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary outcomes for youth with disabilities:<br />

Closing the gap to post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> employment.<br />

Retrieved from http://www.ncd.gov/<br />

newsroom/publicati<strong>on</strong>s/2000/publicati<strong>on</strong>s.htm<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, M. (1990). Qualitative evaluati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> research<br />

methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publishing.<br />

Rizzo, D. (2002). With a little help from my friends:<br />

Supported self-employment for people with severe<br />

disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

17, 179–205.<br />

Rubin, H. J., & Rubin, I. S. (1995). Qualitative interviewing:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> art of hearing data. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks,<br />

CA: Sage Publishing.<br />

Ryndak, D. L., & Fisher, D. (Eds.) (2003). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> foundati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>: A compendium of articles<br />

<strong>on</strong> effective strategies to achieve inclusive educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(2 nd ed). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Ryndak, D. L., Morris<strong>on</strong>, A. P., & Sommerstein, L.<br />

(1999). Literacy prior to <strong>and</strong> after inclusi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> settings. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 24, 5–22.<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ler, A. G. (1999). Short-changed in the name<br />

of socializati<strong>on</strong>? Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al skills by<br />

students with severe disabilities. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

37, 148–150.<br />

Silverman, D. (1993). Interpreting qualitative data:<br />

Methods for analysing talk, text <strong>and</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>. Thous<strong>and</strong><br />

Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.<br />

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative<br />

research: Techniques <strong>and</strong> procedures for developing<br />

52 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


grounded theory (2 nd ed). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA:<br />

Sage Publishing.<br />

Weschler, D. (1991). Weschler Intelligence Scale<br />

for Children (3 rd ed). San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Psychological<br />

Corporati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Wehman, P. (2006). Integrated employment: If not<br />

now, when? If not us, who? Research & Practice for<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe Disabilities 31, 122–126.<br />

Wehman, P., Brooke, V., Green, H., Hewett, M., &<br />

Tipt<strong>on</strong>, M. (2008). Public/private partnerships<br />

<strong>and</strong> employment of people with disabilities: Preliminary<br />

evidence from a pilot project. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> 28, 53–66.<br />

White, J. (2004). Influences of least restrictive envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

<strong>and</strong> community based training <strong>on</strong> integrated<br />

employment outcomes for transiti<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

students with severe disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> 2, 149–156.<br />

Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & P<strong>on</strong>d, R. E.<br />

(1969). Preschool Language Scale. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io:<br />

Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.<br />

Received: 3 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 8 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 5 February 2009<br />

Sibling Outcomes / 53


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 54–68<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Mild Intellectual Disabilities: Legacies <strong>and</strong> Trends in<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Practices<br />

Edward A. Polloway<br />

Lynchburg College<br />

J. David Smith<br />

University of North Carolina at Greensboro<br />

Jacqueline Lubin<br />

St. Lucia Ministry of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Culture<br />

James R. Patt<strong>on</strong><br />

University of Texas at Austin<br />

Abstract: Intellectual disability has been c<strong>on</strong>sidered a high incidence disability in special educati<strong>on</strong> since the<br />

incepti<strong>on</strong> of the field in the United States. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this article is to evaluate current educati<strong>on</strong>al programs<br />

<strong>and</strong> practices for students who historically <strong>and</strong> comm<strong>on</strong>ly have been referred to as having mild mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> article examines mild intellectual disabilities within the c<strong>on</strong>text of historical trends, current<br />

developments, <strong>and</strong> future directi<strong>on</strong>s in terminology, definiti<strong>on</strong>, prevalence, educati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments, <strong>and</strong><br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>al services. Recommendati<strong>on</strong>s for educati<strong>on</strong>al practices <strong>and</strong> future research directi<strong>on</strong>s are discussed.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> field of mild intellectual disabilities (ID)<br />

has provided a foundati<strong>on</strong> for the broader field<br />

of special educati<strong>on</strong>. A significant amount of<br />

the initial research <strong>and</strong> program development<br />

d<strong>on</strong>e in the field was c<strong>on</strong>cerned with individuals<br />

who were labeled at the upper end of the<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong> or intellectual disabilities<br />

spectrum. Further, many of the leaders who<br />

shaped the field of special educati<strong>on</strong> initiated<br />

their work in this area (Polloway, 2000).<br />

Forty-two years ago, Dunn (1968) published<br />

his seminal work challenging educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

practices for this populati<strong>on</strong>. He focused his<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the practices that had enabled a<br />

segregated service delivery system to develop<br />

in which students with intellectual disabilities<br />

were educated in isolati<strong>on</strong> from their peers<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong> to drawing<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> to practices related to individuals<br />

termed at that time educable mentally retarded,<br />

his article also served as the touching off point<br />

for subsequent discussi<strong>on</strong>s of mainstreaming,<br />

the least restrictive envir<strong>on</strong>ment, <strong>and</strong> ultimately<br />

school inclusi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Fifteen years after the publicati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Edward A. Polloway, Office of<br />

Graduate Studies <strong>and</strong> Community Advancement,<br />

Lynchburg College, Lynchburg, VA 24501. E-mail:<br />

polloway@lynchburg.edu<br />

Dunn (1968) article, Polloway <strong>and</strong> Smith<br />

(1983) further assessed the state of the field by<br />

addressing c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s related to populati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

programs, <strong>and</strong> perspectives. In their article,<br />

these authors relied <strong>on</strong> federal data that<br />

provided a nati<strong>on</strong>al perspective <strong>on</strong> the prevalence<br />

of intellectual disabilities within public<br />

schools, <strong>and</strong> served as a basis for an examinati<strong>on</strong><br />

of placement <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al programs.<br />

Since Polloway <strong>and</strong> Smith (1983) presented<br />

their analysis of the field, significant changes<br />

have occurred. This period of time has seen<br />

the implementati<strong>on</strong> of numerous educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

initiatives impacting students with disabilities<br />

in general, such as those related to: the development<br />

of inclusive classroom settings; the<br />

creati<strong>on</strong> of a multi-tiered interventi<strong>on</strong> model<br />

for rec<strong>on</strong>sidering educati<strong>on</strong> for all students;<br />

the emphasis <strong>on</strong> providing access to the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum; <strong>and</strong> the tenets of<br />

No Child Left Behind <strong>and</strong> successive reauthorizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the Individuals with Disabilities<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Act (IDEA). Nevertheless, in spite<br />

of these advances <strong>and</strong> initiatives impacting<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> in general, Tymchuk, Lakin,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Luckass<strong>on</strong>’s (2001) use of the term “the<br />

forgotten generati<strong>on</strong>” c<strong>on</strong>tinues to have<br />

merit for c<strong>on</strong>sidering in particular the populati<strong>on</strong><br />

of individuals who have mild intellectual<br />

disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this article is to assess cer-<br />

54 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


tain aspects of educati<strong>on</strong>al programs <strong>and</strong><br />

practices for students with mild ID. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> paper<br />

focuses <strong>on</strong> terminology, definiti<strong>on</strong>, prevalence,<br />

school placement, transiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> school exit<br />

patterns by providing an historical basis, an<br />

analysis of current data, <strong>and</strong> a discussi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the implicati<strong>on</strong>s for specific future patterns.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> article relies in part <strong>on</strong> federal data that<br />

address questi<strong>on</strong>s related to the prevalence<br />

of individuals served within categorical programs,<br />

the educati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments in which<br />

services are delivered, <strong>and</strong> several key transiti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerns including exit from school<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or from special educati<strong>on</strong>. In doing so,<br />

we use populati<strong>on</strong> data for ‘mental retardati<strong>on</strong>’<br />

in order to generalize about specific<br />

trends <strong>and</strong> practices related to ‘mild mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>’. Because of the use of the generic<br />

“mental retardati<strong>on</strong>” in certain databases, we<br />

endeavor to separate out when possible the<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> of individuals c<strong>on</strong>sidered mildly<br />

intellectually disabled from this broader populati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

We c<strong>on</strong>clude with a general discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

of recommendati<strong>on</strong>s for educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

practice <strong>and</strong> further research.<br />

Terminology <strong>and</strong> Definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Historical C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

For over 50 years, the most frequently used<br />

term to refer to individuals with intellectual<br />

disabilities was mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. This term<br />

reflected widespread <strong>and</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term use in<br />

the wake of the eliminati<strong>on</strong> of the labels of<br />

feeblemindedness, mor<strong>on</strong>, imbecile, <strong>and</strong> idiot<br />

(Polloway & Lubin, 2009). S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong> (1998)<br />

reported in his survey that, of the 66 terms<br />

found in the internati<strong>on</strong>al literature of the<br />

field, mental retardati<strong>on</strong> was the most frequently<br />

used. Despite the negative c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong><br />

associated with the term, it was the most<br />

widely used descriptor am<strong>on</strong>g educators <strong>and</strong><br />

parents as well as researchers in the field. For<br />

example, Denning, Chamberlain, <strong>and</strong> Polloway<br />

(2000) reported that 26 state departments<br />

of educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued to use the term mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Some of the terms that have been used to<br />

refer to individuals with ID have caused some<br />

c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong>. In the United Kingdom, “learning<br />

disabilities” is the term of choice for mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. This same term was promoted by<br />

Dunn (1973) as well when he proposed terminology<br />

that used these designati<strong>on</strong>s: general<br />

learning disabilities (referring to intellectual<br />

disabilities) <strong>and</strong> specific learning disabilities<br />

(referring to those to whom are referred to in<br />

the U.S. as having a learning disability). This<br />

system never became widely accepted in the<br />

United States due in large part to the prolific<br />

growth of the field of learning disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

the widespread use of this term in its more<br />

specific applicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

A related c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> has to do with the<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>s used in the field <strong>and</strong> their implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for people with mild retardati<strong>on</strong>. (See<br />

Greenspan & Switzky, 2006, for a comprehensive<br />

discussi<strong>on</strong> of the evoluti<strong>on</strong> of terminology<br />

<strong>and</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>s). In the Denning et al. study<br />

(2000), 44 states reported using the definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

from the American Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

(AAMR) (Grossman, 1983) while 4<br />

states used the adapted versi<strong>on</strong> of that definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

from the AAMR manual. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> AAMR 1992<br />

manual (Luckass<strong>on</strong> et al., 1992) had not significantly<br />

impacted the state guidelines in<br />

general. Many states still were using the definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in the regulati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the Individuals with Disabilities Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Act (IDEA) that was generally derived from<br />

Grossman (1983) <strong>and</strong> did not make modificati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

reflective of changes in the field, as<br />

recommended by the AAMR.<br />

Current Data<br />

Polloway, Patt<strong>on</strong>, Smith, Antoine, <strong>and</strong> Lubin<br />

(2009) replicated the studies of Frankenberger<br />

(1984) <strong>and</strong> Denning <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(2000) c<strong>on</strong>cerning state terminology <strong>and</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se researchers reported that 27<br />

states use the term mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, which<br />

actually represented a slight increase from the<br />

Denning et al. (2000) study. Berger<strong>on</strong>, Floyd,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sh<strong>and</strong>s (2008) c<strong>on</strong>firmed those findings<br />

as they reported that the term mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

was used by 53% of the states. Thus, while<br />

the professi<strong>on</strong>al field of intellectual disabilities<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinues to explore terminology <strong>and</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

changes in the field, it is apparent<br />

that the implementati<strong>on</strong> of these changes<br />

varies dramatically across the states. Some of<br />

the other terms used by states, as reported by<br />

Polloway et al., included: cognitively disabled,<br />

ID Practices / 55


cognitively impaired, intellectually disabled,<br />

intellectually impaired, <strong>and</strong> mentally disabled.<br />

Smith (2006) noted that “the 2002 AAMR<br />

manual authors acknowledge the problems<br />

with the term mental retardati<strong>on</strong> but c<strong>on</strong>cluded<br />

that there was no acceptable alternative<br />

term, despite its technical shortcomings”<br />

(p. 58). However, since 2002, the term intellectual<br />

disabilities is being used much more<br />

often professi<strong>on</strong>ally in the field (previously<br />

internati<strong>on</strong>ally <strong>and</strong> now to a greater extent in<br />

the United States). Nevertheless, according to<br />

Polloway et al. (2009), it had not yet taken<br />

root in state departments of educati<strong>on</strong> guidelines<br />

<strong>and</strong> was the accepted term in <strong>on</strong>ly four<br />

states.<br />

In terms of definiti<strong>on</strong>, Polloway et al. (2009)<br />

noted that 34 states c<strong>on</strong>tinued to depend <strong>on</strong><br />

the Grossman (1983) mental retardati<strong>on</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In its stead, they noted evidence of<br />

increased use of the most recent AAMR (now<br />

the American Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Intellectual <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities, AAIDD) definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

(Luckass<strong>on</strong> et al., 2002) as well as alternative<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>al approaches. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y further<br />

reported that in terms of the intellectual assessment<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the definiti<strong>on</strong>, 34<br />

states required a cut-off score or range; most<br />

comm<strong>on</strong>ly, the ceiling level for mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

was stated as either approximately IQ<br />

70 or two st<strong>and</strong>ard deviati<strong>on</strong>s below the mean.<br />

Berger<strong>on</strong> et al. (2008) substantiated that fact<br />

as they noted that “the majority of states use<br />

IQ cutoff at least two SDs below the normative<br />

mean” (p. 125).<br />

In the sec<strong>on</strong>d key definiti<strong>on</strong>al domain,<br />

adaptive behavior (AB), Polloway et al. (2009)<br />

reported that all states noted that they required<br />

assessment in this area. Twenty-three<br />

states highlighted that specific AB practices<br />

were to be followed. In terms of developmental<br />

period, 33 states (64.7%) did not specify<br />

any specific ceiling age for the developmental<br />

period although few students are initially<br />

identified as mentally retarded later in their<br />

school years.<br />

Future Patterns<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of the term “intellectual disabilities”<br />

to describe the populati<strong>on</strong>—whether schoolage<br />

or adult—to whom the professi<strong>on</strong> typically<br />

referred as having “mental retardati<strong>on</strong>” is<br />

clearly increasing (Schalock et al., 2007). This<br />

change is reflected in organizati<strong>on</strong>al names<br />

(AAIDD), journal titles (e.g., Intellectual <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities), <strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

book titles. Although some governmental<br />

agencies are moving slowly to change terminology<br />

(i.e., terms used in state educati<strong>on</strong><br />

codes), the professi<strong>on</strong>al community is embracing<br />

this term at a quick pace.<br />

MacMillan, Siperstein, <strong>and</strong> Leffert (2006)<br />

underscored the need for a new term <strong>and</strong><br />

supported the use of intellectual disability<br />

when they stated:<br />

the precipitous decline in the applicati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the term mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in school settings<br />

is, to a significant extent, attributable<br />

to its pejorative c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>. ...if mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong> is to serve as a unifying c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

that includes [mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>], a<br />

new term needs to be adopted that is less<br />

stigmatizing <strong>and</strong> more palatable to educators,<br />

parents, <strong>and</strong> the individuals themselves. In<br />

our view, a term such as intellectual disability,<br />

which is increasingly becoming a<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard term outside the United States,<br />

cognitive impairment, or general learning<br />

disability would be more acceptable (p. 215).<br />

Changes in terminology can impact <strong>on</strong> a<br />

number of areas. In school settings, such a<br />

name change may not be as important as how<br />

it is defined <strong>and</strong> which criteria are used to<br />

determine eligibility. However, term change<br />

may very well have important implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

individuals within the c<strong>on</strong>text of n<strong>on</strong>-school<br />

settings, such as in the legal arena. In this<br />

domain, being diagnosed as having “mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>” is just beginning to be understood<br />

to some degree, particularly in the light<br />

of the attenti<strong>on</strong> given to this diagnostic label<br />

in the wake of the Supreme Court decisi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the Daryl Atkins death penalty case (see Patt<strong>on</strong><br />

& Keyes, 2006). Changing terminology<br />

will undoubtedly create some c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sternati<strong>on</strong> in the criminal justice system.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> name change could also have an impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> other adult service areas (e.g., Social Security<br />

eligibility) until such time as state codes<br />

are revised to reflect the use of intellectual<br />

disability. Finally, as Polloway <strong>and</strong> Lubin<br />

(2009) have noted, terminology c<strong>on</strong>tinues to<br />

evolve, as the term feebleminded gave way to<br />

56 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


the c<strong>on</strong>cept “mental retardati<strong>on</strong>”, which then<br />

yielded to “intellectual disabilities”.<br />

School Prevalence<br />

Historical C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

For many years a comm<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> was<br />

that approximately 3% of the school populati<strong>on</strong><br />

would likely be identified as mentally retarded<br />

(MacMillan, 2007). While this figure<br />

was often cited, <strong>and</strong> became a basis for public<br />

policy initiatives <strong>and</strong> governmental support,<br />

this prevalence rate was not generally c<strong>on</strong>firmed<br />

in research. Rather, the best historical<br />

predictor of nati<strong>on</strong>al prevalence in the 1960s<br />

<strong>and</strong> 1970s was closer to 2%.<br />

With the advent of the annual reports to<br />

C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the Individuals with Disabilities<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Act, a nati<strong>on</strong>al database was established,<br />

which has subsequently provided a<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong> for tracking prevalence trends in<br />

the public schools. Of particular note is that,<br />

subsequent to the passage of the Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for All H<strong>and</strong>icapped Children Act in 1975,<br />

there was a steady <strong>and</strong> significant decline for<br />

about 20 years in the number of individuals<br />

identified as mentally retarded. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se data<br />

indicated that neither the 3% nor the 2%<br />

figures were accurate portrayals of the school<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> rather the actual numbers<br />

were substantially lower. This impact resulted<br />

primarily from decreases in the number of<br />

individuals who earlier would have been c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

to have mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Polloway <strong>and</strong> Smith (1983) discussed the<br />

diminishing number of individuals who were<br />

being identified as mentally retarded <strong>and</strong><br />

stressed that further changes were likely to<br />

occur regarding those individuals identified<br />

with mild ID. Specific c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> hypotheses<br />

included changes in definiti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards (e.g., increased emphasis <strong>on</strong> adaptive<br />

behavior), the impact of key litigati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

minority over-representati<strong>on</strong> within this populati<strong>on</strong><br />

(especially in court cases in California),<br />

professi<strong>on</strong>al reluctance to use the label<br />

“mentally retarded,” <strong>and</strong> the possible beneficial<br />

effects of early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs <strong>on</strong><br />

preventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Current Data<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> 27th Annual Report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> of Individuals with Disabilities Ed-<br />

ucati<strong>on</strong> Act (USDOE, 2007) provides a database<br />

for determining current practices <strong>on</strong><br />

both a nati<strong>on</strong>al level as well as an analysis at<br />

the individual state level. While the data are<br />

always delayed in publicati<strong>on</strong> for several years<br />

(for example, the 2007 document includes<br />

data from the 2003–04 academic year), this<br />

document nevertheless provides a comprehensive<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong> for analyzing data trends<br />

within <strong>and</strong> across years.<br />

Table 1 provides percentage data <strong>on</strong> prevalence<br />

figures for the overall populati<strong>on</strong>, ages<br />

6–21, across disability categories. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se trend<br />

data provide a 10-year perspective <strong>on</strong> special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>. As can be noted in the table, the<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al figures for mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (as<br />

rounded within the table) have shown virtually<br />

no variance <strong>on</strong> an annual basis during<br />

this timeframe <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently reflect a trend<br />

that slightly less than <strong>on</strong>e in a hundred schoolage<br />

children nati<strong>on</strong>ally might be identified as<br />

mentally retarded. Further, the data reveal<br />

that four times as many school-age individuals<br />

are identified as having a learning disability.<br />

Nevertheless, by comparis<strong>on</strong> with other disability<br />

areas, <strong>on</strong>e would still c<strong>on</strong>clude that ID<br />

remains a high incidence disability.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most compelling finding related to<br />

prevalence in the field of intellectual disabilities<br />

is the significant variance across states. A<br />

total of 13 states report prevalence rates in<br />

excess of 1.2%, including two states that report<br />

prevalence figures above 2% (West Virginia:<br />

2.47% <strong>and</strong> Wyoming: 2.25). On the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, there are 19 states that report prevalence<br />

rates that are below 0.6%; these include<br />

New Jersey, New Hampshire, <strong>and</strong> Maine, each<br />

of which report a prevalence figure of 0.34%<br />

(USDOE, 2007).<br />

For the states that report high prevalence<br />

figures, it might be c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the populati<strong>on</strong><br />

being served is not dissimilar to that<br />

which was comm<strong>on</strong>ly identified 30 or more<br />

years ago under the label mental retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> would likely include a sizable number of<br />

individuals with mild disabilities. On the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, for those states who reported serving<br />

approximately 0.4% or less of their populati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

it could probably be c<strong>on</strong>cluded that this populati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

so identified, would include <strong>on</strong>ly individuals<br />

with more significant disabilities,<br />

given the fact that this prevalence figure is<br />

comm<strong>on</strong>ly cited in epidemiology studies as<br />

ID Practices / 57


TABLE 1<br />

Percentage of the Populati<strong>on</strong> Ages 6 Through 21 Receiving Special Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Related Services, by<br />

Disability Category: Fall 1993 Through Fall 2003<br />

Disability<br />

the most likely predictor of the number of<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with severe disabilities within a given<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., Abramowicz & Richards<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1975; MacMillan, 2007). It remains an important<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> to determine if states with lower<br />

prevalence rates are serving any students that<br />

might be c<strong>on</strong>sidered mildly intellectually disabled<br />

under this category, if these individuals<br />

receive no special educati<strong>on</strong> services, or if they<br />

are simply labeled differently, therefore skewing<br />

federal report data.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> data <strong>on</strong> ID can be placed in broader<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text by also c<strong>on</strong>sidering individuals identified<br />

as “developmentally delayed”—a categorical<br />

designati<strong>on</strong> under IDEA that states may<br />

choose to use for students between 3 <strong>and</strong> 9<br />

years of age. Although the state variance for<br />

this category is again noteworthy (California<br />

<strong>and</strong> New Jersey, for example, reported no<br />

school-age individuals so identified while a<br />

significant number of states report in excess of<br />

0.20), it is instructive to note that the number<br />

of individuals identified nati<strong>on</strong>ally as developmentally<br />

delayed, ages 6–9, increased dramatically<br />

between 1997 <strong>and</strong> 2003. Figure 1 provides<br />

a graphic representati<strong>on</strong> of this increase<br />

to above a prevalence rate of 0.40 (USDOE,<br />

2007). It is reas<strong>on</strong>able to c<strong>on</strong>sider that some<br />

of the children identified as developmentally<br />

delayed will meet the identificati<strong>on</strong> criteria for<br />

intellectual disability <strong>on</strong>ce the age of nine is<br />

1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003<br />

Percent<br />

Specific learning disabilities 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.3 4.4 4.4 4.4 4.4 4.3 4.3 4.3<br />

Speech or language<br />

impairments 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.7 1.6 1.7 1.7<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9 0.9<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7 0.7<br />

Multiple Disabilities 0.2 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2 0.2<br />

Other health impairments 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.3 0.3 0.3 0.4 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> » » » 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.2 0.2<br />

Developmental delay » » » » 0.1 0.1 0.1<br />

All disabilities 8.1 8.2 8.4 8.5 8.5 8.6 8.7 8.7 8.8 8.9 9.1<br />

Source: Adapted from U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2007). 27 th Report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

IDEA (p. 31). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

reached <strong>and</strong> the term developmental delay is<br />

no l<strong>on</strong>ger acceptable for usage under IDEA.<br />

One interesting data set that speaks to the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> of prevalence is the US Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong> (2007) summary of the percentage<br />

of elementary students who are declassified<br />

from special educati<strong>on</strong>. Providing data<br />

from 1999–2002, this federal report indicated<br />

that <strong>on</strong>ly 2% of students identified as<br />

mentally retarded were declassified during<br />

this period, compared with 9% for learning<br />

Figure 1. Percentage of children ages 6 through 9<br />

receiving special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related<br />

services due to developmental delay: Fall<br />

1997 through fall 2003. Source: US Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong> (2007). 27 th Report<br />

to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

IDEA (p. 39). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

58 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


disabilities <strong>and</strong> 10% for emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> these data, it might be<br />

cautiously c<strong>on</strong>cluded that <strong>on</strong>ce the label of<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (or intellectual disabilities)<br />

is given, it is more likely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be<br />

applied, no doubt influenced in part because<br />

of the presence of those with more significant<br />

disabilities within this category.<br />

Future Patterns<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> implicati<strong>on</strong>s of trends in terms of the<br />

prevalence of intellectual disabilities within<br />

the schools, as well as an extrapolati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

likely trends in terms of mild ID, suggest that<br />

this category will not increase in numbers.<br />

Rather, ten years of c<strong>on</strong>sistent data represent<br />

a str<strong>on</strong>g case for the fact that the number of<br />

students identified will remain relatively static.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that approximately<br />

0.4% of individuals in a given populati<strong>on</strong> may<br />

be identified as having severe or significant<br />

disabilities, it seems reas<strong>on</strong>able to c<strong>on</strong>clude<br />

that approximately 0.5% of the school populati<strong>on</strong><br />

nati<strong>on</strong>wide could be identified as mildly<br />

intellectually disabled. Again, the variance<br />

across states would lead <strong>on</strong>e to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to<br />

assume that for some states with low prevalence<br />

rates virtually no individuals would be so<br />

identified while in other states approximately<br />

1.5–2.0% would likely be c<strong>on</strong>sidered mildly<br />

intellectually disabled. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of the category<br />

“developmentally delayed” may c<strong>on</strong>tinue to<br />

increase in usage, as it provides a less stigmatizing<br />

way to describe individuals who may<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate delays such as in cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

linguistic functi<strong>on</strong>ing.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Envir<strong>on</strong>ments/Placement<br />

Historical C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> historical image of special educati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the 1960s <strong>and</strong> 1970s was often based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

profile of programs for students with intellectual<br />

disabilities. Much of the attenti<strong>on</strong> given<br />

to special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> school placement<br />

reflected the comm<strong>on</strong> finding that the majority<br />

of these students were in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms <strong>and</strong> spent limited<br />

time in integrated settings with peers<br />

who were n<strong>on</strong>-disabled (Polloway, 1984). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the reports<br />

by Dunn (1968) <strong>and</strong> Polloway <strong>and</strong> Smith<br />

(1983) who c<strong>on</strong>firmed that such placements<br />

were most often the reality in the schools.<br />

Two major types of classes were comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

found in schools throughout the nati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

1960s <strong>and</strong> most of the 1970s: classes for students<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be educable mentally retarded<br />

(EMR)—i.e., mild intellectual disabilities—<strong>and</strong><br />

for those c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be trainable<br />

mentally retarded (TMR)—i.e., moderatesevere<br />

intellectual disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> so-called<br />

efficacy studies of the 1950s <strong>and</strong> 1960s sought<br />

to research the relative effectiveness of selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

vs. inclusive school envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

for the former populati<strong>on</strong>; the research can<br />

best be summarized as equivocal because of<br />

the numerous c<strong>on</strong>founds within the research<br />

methodology as well as the questi<strong>on</strong> of determining<br />

what should be studied as the key<br />

indicators of effectiveness (Stant<strong>on</strong> & Cassidy,<br />

1964).<br />

Current Data<br />

In spite of the fact that the trend toward increased<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> in schools has moved forward<br />

for three decades, the data <strong>on</strong> inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with intellectual disabilities<br />

(USDOE, 2007) indicate that placement of<br />

those students into programs outside of general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> is still predominant, both <strong>on</strong> a<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al level <strong>and</strong> in most states. Table 2 presents<br />

data <strong>on</strong> selected categories of disabilities<br />

per the relative amount of time spent outside<br />

of general educati<strong>on</strong> settings. As can be noted<br />

in the table, mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (11.7%) is<br />

substantially lower than most other disabilities<br />

in terms of the likelihood of being placed<br />

within general educati<strong>on</strong> for more than 21%<br />

of the school day. In other words, this group<br />

was in more segregated settings for 88.3% of<br />

the time. It must be noted of course that the<br />

“mental retardati<strong>on</strong>” category also includes a<br />

large number of individuals who have more<br />

significant challenges in additi<strong>on</strong> to those<br />

with mild ID, thus likely biasing the data toward<br />

more restrictive settings.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> data are most significant when compared<br />

to data for students across all disabilities,<br />

students with specific learning disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> for those with emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance (see<br />

Table 2). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se federal data indicate that a<br />

total of 58.3% of students with intellectual<br />

ID Practices / 59


TABLE 2<br />

Percentage of Students Ages 6 Through 21 With Disabilities Receiving Special Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Related<br />

Services in Different Envir<strong>on</strong>ments, by Disability Category: Fall 2003<br />

Disabilities<br />

21 percent<br />

Of the day<br />

disabilities are in envir<strong>on</strong>ments that are removed<br />

from the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom<br />

at least 60% of the day (51.8%) or are in<br />

separate envir<strong>on</strong>ments such as special day<br />

schools <strong>and</strong> residential programs (6.3%).<br />

Placed in a broader c<strong>on</strong>text of time, Williams<strong>on</strong>,<br />

McLeskey, Hoppey, <strong>and</strong> Lenz (2006)<br />

noted that placement of this group of students<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong>-based programs increased<br />

from 27.3% to 44.7% in the 1990s but<br />

that this increase essentially reached a plateau<br />

after 1997–1998. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> data from the USDOE<br />

(2007) essentially c<strong>on</strong>firm this plateau effect<br />

for students with intellectual disabilities (i.e.<br />

11.7% outside the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom<br />

less than 21% of the day plus 30.2%<br />

outside for 21–60% of time, for a total of<br />

41.9%). Smith (2007) noted that the percentage<br />

of students with intellectual disabilities<br />

served in the most inclusive setting (i.e.,<br />

21% outside of regular class) declined between<br />

1997–98 <strong>and</strong> 2002–03 in 34 states.<br />

As with prevalence data, the data <strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments reflect a high degree of<br />

interstate variati<strong>on</strong>. Thus, for example, the<br />

state of Verm<strong>on</strong>t reports that 59.1% of students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> are served in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classes greater than 79% of<br />

the day while data for Utah indicates that <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

Time outside the regular class<br />

21–60 percent<br />

Of the day<br />

Percent<br />

60 percent<br />

Of the day<br />

Separate<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

Specific learning disabilities 48.8 37.3 13.0 0.9<br />

Speech/language impairments 88.2 6.8 4.6 0.4<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> 11.7 30.2 51.8 6.3<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance 30.3 22.6 30.2 16.9<br />

Multiple disabilities 12.1 17.2 45.8 24.9<br />

Other health impairments 51.1 30.5 15.0 3.5<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> 26.8 17.7 43.9 11.6<br />

Developmental delay 51.2 28.2 18.6 2.0<br />

All disabilities 49.9 27.7 18.5 3.9<br />

Source: Adapted from U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2007). 27 th Report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

IDEA (p. 46). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

2.55% of students are so placed (USDOE,<br />

2007).<br />

When c<strong>on</strong>sidering the interacti<strong>on</strong> of prevalence<br />

rates <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al placements, it is<br />

intuitively attractive to presume that states<br />

with low prevalence rates are very likely serving<br />

a populati<strong>on</strong> that is more significantly<br />

disabled <strong>and</strong> that c<strong>on</strong>sequently <strong>on</strong>e would<br />

expect to find that those students are more<br />

likely to be educated in more restricted settings.<br />

In a parallel sense, it might also be<br />

assumed that states serving a larger percentage<br />

of students under the label intellectual<br />

disabilities are likely to be educating more<br />

individuals with mild disabilities <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequently<br />

may be more likely to provide services<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong>-based settings. However,<br />

the US Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> (2007) data<br />

reflect the fact that there is significant variance<br />

in the utilizati<strong>on</strong> of general educati<strong>on</strong>based<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments by states, in a<br />

number of cases irrespective of state prevalence<br />

figures. For example, Utah reflects what<br />

might be c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be a comm<strong>on</strong> pattern<br />

in that it serves a very low prevalence of individuals<br />

with ID (0.48%) <strong>and</strong> educates <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

2.55% of these students in general educati<strong>on</strong>based<br />

programs more than 79% of the time.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> other predicted pattern is seen in several<br />

60 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


high prevalence states emphasizing general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>-based programs. This is illustrated<br />

by Iowa <strong>and</strong> Kentucky with high prevalence<br />

rates of individuals served with intellectual disabilities<br />

(1.80; 1.98) educating 35.9% <strong>and</strong><br />

26.3%, respectively, of these students in the<br />

most inclusive programs.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, several states reflect<br />

different <strong>and</strong> perhaps less expected patterns.<br />

For example, Verm<strong>on</strong>t reports a prevalence of<br />

0.90 for intellectual disabilities (that is, at the<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al average) while serving 59.11% of<br />

these students in general educati<strong>on</strong> for 79%<br />

or more of the school day, a figure that is<br />

more than five times greater than the nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

average. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> District of Columbia, with a relatively<br />

high prevalence rate of 1.63, reports<br />

that <strong>on</strong>ly 2.58% of the students are served in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> programs (greater than<br />

79% of the time). In these two examples, there<br />

is <strong>on</strong> the <strong>on</strong>e h<strong>and</strong> a str<strong>on</strong>g commitment in<br />

Verm<strong>on</strong>t to the apparent inclusi<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with more significant disabilities in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> in juxtapositi<strong>on</strong> to DC, which would<br />

appear to identify more students with mild<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> to serve them more often in<br />

pull-out programs.<br />

Future Patterns<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> nati<strong>on</strong>al data indicate that a plateau was<br />

essentially reached in late 1990s in terms of<br />

the trend toward increased inclusi<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Data through<br />

the 2003–04 academic year c<strong>on</strong>firmed that<br />

approximately 42% of students with intellectual<br />

disabilities were being served in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> programs, inclusive of those individuals<br />

who were outside of the regular classroom<br />

less than 20% of the time as well as those<br />

outside between 21–60% of the time. Nevertheless,<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tinuing increased emphasis <strong>on</strong><br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> access to the general curriculum<br />

may yet be reflected in subsequent federal<br />

reports.<br />

Any trend toward inclusi<strong>on</strong> is inescapably<br />

linked to how well the general educati<strong>on</strong> setting<br />

is prepared to h<strong>and</strong>le students who will<br />

require differentiated strategies in terms of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent, instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials, instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

assignments, testing, products, setting, <strong>and</strong><br />

management (Hoover & Patt<strong>on</strong>, 2005, 2008).<br />

In a multi-tiered model of providing interven-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> that also has implicati<strong>on</strong>s for eligibility<br />

determinati<strong>on</strong>, the need for general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teachers to be equipped to address the<br />

needs of a wide range of students, including<br />

those with mild intellectual disabilities, is imperative.<br />

Thus, efforts c<strong>on</strong>tinue to provide skills<br />

to general educati<strong>on</strong> teachers to differentiate<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> implement educati<strong>on</strong>al programs<br />

supported by collaborative arrangements<br />

with special educators <strong>and</strong> other schoolbased<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>nel.<br />

Schools that operate within a multi-tiered<br />

system also are creating a change of roles for<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>nel as well (Hoover &<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, 2008). Teachers who have resp<strong>on</strong>sibility<br />

for individuals with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

must become more than primarily<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for direct instructi<strong>on</strong> in pull-out<br />

programs <strong>and</strong> rather also must be expected<br />

to work collaboratively in preventive efforts<br />

typically associated with Tier I instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(enhanced instructi<strong>on</strong>al delivery in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> programs) as well as supportive efforts<br />

often associated with Tier II instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(<strong>on</strong>going collaborati<strong>on</strong> in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

programs).<br />

At the same time, a number of issues remain<br />

in terms of ensuring that educati<strong>on</strong>al practices<br />

result in quality programs for students<br />

with ID. Bouck (2007) has used the term “lost<br />

in translati<strong>on</strong>” to refer to the challenges faced<br />

by teachers in addressing federal laws while<br />

c<strong>on</strong>structing effective programs for this populati<strong>on</strong><br />

that “promote the greatest academic<br />

<strong>and</strong> social experiences for each individual student”<br />

(p. 85). Wehmeyer (2006) provided a<br />

blueprint for c<strong>on</strong>sidering effective inclusive<br />

efforts by identifying aspects c<strong>on</strong>sistent with a<br />

universal design for learning approach to<br />

making general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms more<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sive to the learning needs of this group<br />

of students.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re also remains the questi<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of relevant <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>temporary research<br />

to educati<strong>on</strong>al programs for students<br />

with mild intellectual disabilities. Several individuals<br />

(e.g. Polloway, 2005; Bouck, 2007) have<br />

highlighted the relative absence of recent research<br />

<strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al interventi<strong>on</strong>s for this<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>. While changes have been occurring<br />

over the last 20 years in the students who<br />

are being identified as intellectually disabled,<br />

the field has not received the level of attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

ID Practices / 61


that has been accorded to other areas of excepti<strong>on</strong>ality<br />

(e.g., learning disabilities, severe/<br />

significant disabilities, autism, emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioral disorders). As a c<strong>on</strong>sequence, many<br />

of the judgments about this group have been<br />

generalized from much older research or research<br />

d<strong>on</strong>e with other populati<strong>on</strong>s (most comm<strong>on</strong>ly,<br />

students with learning disabilities). In<br />

this regard, it is worth c<strong>on</strong>sidering the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the President’s Committee for<br />

People with Intellectual Disabilities (2007)<br />

who indicated that an important aspect of<br />

policy in the future would be: “strengthening<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> bridges from the Federal Government<br />

to state <strong>and</strong> local departments of educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

schools, teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents by providing<br />

current, research-based strategies <strong>and</strong> best<br />

practices related to students with intellectual<br />

disabilities” (p. 14).<br />

School Exit Patterns <strong>and</strong> Transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Historical C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

If there is <strong>on</strong>e group that has benefited from<br />

activities related to transiti<strong>on</strong> planning <strong>and</strong><br />

services for the l<strong>on</strong>gest period of time, it is<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Work-study programs have been recommended<br />

for, <strong>and</strong> implemented with, this group since<br />

the 1950s <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerns about what would<br />

occur when school ended were <strong>on</strong> the minds<br />

of sec<strong>on</strong>dary-level staff well before the <strong>on</strong>set of<br />

more formal transiti<strong>on</strong> planning requirements<br />

<strong>and</strong> services in the 1980s. In Kolstoe<br />

<strong>and</strong> Frey’s (1965) descripti<strong>on</strong> of high school<br />

work-study programs, it is clear that recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the importance to future outcomes was<br />

given not <strong>on</strong>ly to job performance skills but<br />

also to academic, pers<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> social skills.<br />

Over the years, most students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities stayed in school through<br />

to the completi<strong>on</strong> of high school, although<br />

frequently they left without a high school diploma,<br />

receiving a certificate of some type (i.e.,<br />

attendance, completi<strong>on</strong> of program). Some of<br />

these students remained until they “aged-out”<br />

at 22 or later, dependent <strong>on</strong> state guidelines<br />

(Edgar, 1987, 1988).<br />

A series of critical events have had an impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> the transiti<strong>on</strong> services for students<br />

with intellectual disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first was the<br />

funding provided in 1984 by the federal government<br />

of projects that focused <strong>on</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of youth with disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d was the<br />

reauthorizati<strong>on</strong> of IDEA in 1990 when transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

services were m<strong>and</strong>ated for all students<br />

who were receiving special educati<strong>on</strong> services.<br />

Subsequent reauthorizati<strong>on</strong>s of IDEA<br />

have modified some of the elements of transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

services; however, the main intent has<br />

endured.<br />

Current Data<br />

Two key sources of data provide a c<strong>on</strong>temporary<br />

perspective <strong>on</strong> the nature of the transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

process for students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first source is USDOE (2007)<br />

previously cited in this paper while the other is<br />

the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study–2<br />

(NLTS-2) (Wagner, Newman, Cameto, Garza,<br />

& Levine, 2005). This latter resource is the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d study that has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted to follow<br />

up after school with students who were<br />

previously receiving special educati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

NLTS-2 report provides the most comprehensive<br />

picture of what happens to students after<br />

high school.<br />

In Table 3, we present federal data <strong>on</strong> student<br />

dropout rates across areas of disability<br />

that provide a 10-year retrospective. As can be<br />

noted, the dropout rates for mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

(or intellectual disabilities) are generally<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent but show somewhat of a decline, to<br />

28.6%. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se numbers are comparable to<br />

data for learning disabilities <strong>and</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

disturbance although twice the rate for students<br />

with autism. By any interpretati<strong>on</strong>, it is<br />

clear that a substantial number of students are<br />

not completing school as defined within the<br />

federal data base.<br />

Complementary data reflect graduati<strong>on</strong><br />

rates with a regular diploma (see Table 4). As<br />

can be noted, <strong>on</strong>ly 36.9% of the individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (or intellectual disabilities)<br />

receive regular diplomas; those numbers<br />

have been generally stable with <strong>on</strong>ly a<br />

small increase noted since 1993–94. Across<br />

this 10-year period, students identified as<br />

having intellectual disabilities were c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

less likely to graduate than were students<br />

with learning disabilities, speech <strong>and</strong><br />

language impairments, other health impairments,<br />

autism, <strong>and</strong> notably multiple disabili-<br />

62 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 3<br />

Students Ages 14 <strong>and</strong> Older With Disabilities Who Dropped Out of School: 1993–94<br />

Disability<br />

1993–<br />

94<br />

1994–<br />

95<br />

1995–<br />

96<br />

ties. Only students with emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance<br />

were less likely to graduate am<strong>on</strong>g these selected<br />

categories of excepti<strong>on</strong>ality.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> federal data <strong>on</strong> school exit patterns are<br />

not easily interpreted because of the multiple<br />

categories reflected in possible school exit.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, individuals who are intellectually<br />

1996–<br />

97<br />

1997–<br />

98<br />

Percent<br />

1998–<br />

99<br />

1999–<br />

2000<br />

2000–<br />

01<br />

2001–<br />

02<br />

2002–<br />

03<br />

Specific learning<br />

disabilities 43.1 44.7 44.4 43.4 41.3 40.2 39.9 38.6 35.4 31.6<br />

Speech/language<br />

impairments 49.3 51.4 50.4 48.0 44.5 40.9 39.4 39.3 35.8 31.2<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> 35.4 37.9 38.0 38.2 36.3 34.9 35.7 34.2 31.2 28.6<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance 67.8 69.2 69.9 69.2 67.2 65.5 65.2 64.9 61.2 55.92<br />

Multiple disabilities 24.6 35.1 27.4 27.7 26.3 28.1 25.8 26.7 25.9 24.2<br />

Other health<br />

impairments 37.4 38.1 36.8 37.8 34.9 36.3 35.1 36.1 32.7 29.5<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> 25.9 29.5 23.8 24.0 19.2 22.8 23.7 20.6 17.6 15.5<br />

All disabilities 45.1 47 46.8 45.9 43.7 42.3 42.1 40.9 37.6 33.6<br />

Source: Adapted from U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2007). 27 th Report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

IDEA (p. 52). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

TABLE 4<br />

disabled are also represented significantly in<br />

the populati<strong>on</strong> of those who do not complete<br />

diplomas but complete school through some<br />

alternative means of school exit (such as<br />

through receipt of a certificate) as well as<br />

those who age out of school. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

federal data include the category “moved, not<br />

Students Ages 14 <strong>and</strong> Older With Disabilities Who Graduated With a Regular Diploma: 1993–94 a Through<br />

2002–03<br />

Disability<br />

1993–<br />

94<br />

1994–<br />

95<br />

1995–<br />

96<br />

1996–<br />

97<br />

1997–<br />

98<br />

Percent<br />

1998–<br />

99<br />

1999–<br />

2000<br />

2000–<br />

01<br />

2001–<br />

02<br />

2002–<br />

03<br />

Specific learning<br />

disabilities 49.1 47.7 48.2 48.8 51.0 51.9 51.6 53.7 56.9 57.4<br />

Speech/language<br />

impairments 42.9 41.7 42.2 44.8 48.1 51.2 53.2 52.7 55.7 59.2<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> 35.0 33.8 34.0 33.0 34.3 36.0 34.4 35.0 37.8 36.9<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance 27.0 26.0 25.1 25.9 27.4 29.2 28.6 29.1 32.1 35.4<br />

Multiple disabilities 36.1 31.4 35.3 35.4 39.0 41.0 42.3 41.5 45.2 45.3<br />

Other health<br />

impairments 54.6 52.6 53.0 53.1 56.8 55.0 56.5 56.2 59.2 59.0<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> 33.4 35.3 36.4 35.9 38.7 40.5 40.8 42.0 51.1 50.5<br />

All disabilities 43.5 42.1 42.4 43.0 45.3 46.5 46.1 47.7 51.1 51.9<br />

Source: Adapted from U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2007). 27 th Report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

IDEA (p. 51). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

ID Practices / 63


TABLE 5<br />

Summary of NLTS-2 Findings: Students with Intellectual Disabilities<br />

Area Findings<br />

Transiti<strong>on</strong> Services • Many students (60%) had transiti<strong>on</strong> planning begun by age 14<br />

• 48.7% of the students did not provide input in discussing their transiti<strong>on</strong> plans<br />

• Very few individuals (3.3%) took a leadership role during the transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

process<br />

• General educators’ involvement in transiti<strong>on</strong> process was minimal<br />

• Few individuals (9.8%) chose postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> as a transiti<strong>on</strong> goal<br />

• 51.4% of the students chose living independently as a transiti<strong>on</strong> goal<br />

• 75.8% received instructi<strong>on</strong> specifically focused <strong>on</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong>-related topics<br />

• <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> school program provided for most students (79.5%) was identified as<br />

“very or fairly well suited” for preparing them to achieve their transiti<strong>on</strong> goals<br />

Outcomes Area: Percentage<br />

Postsec<strong>on</strong>dary School Enrollment:<br />

any postsec<strong>on</strong>dary school 15.4<br />

postsec<strong>on</strong>dary vocati<strong>on</strong>al, business, or technical school 11.0<br />

2-year/community college 5.1<br />

Current Employment 24.8<br />

Living Arrangements<br />

with parents 72.2<br />

<strong>on</strong> own, with spouse, roommate, or in college dorm 16.3<br />

Post-school Independence: Have a...<br />

driver’s license/permit 20.7<br />

checking account 10.1<br />

charge account or credit card 6.9<br />

Leisure Time:<br />

watching TV/videos 41.0<br />

using a computer 13.5<br />

listening to music 18.7<br />

doing hobbies, reading for pleasure 4.7<br />

talking <strong>on</strong> the ph<strong>on</strong>e with friends 9.2<br />

playing sports 16.3<br />

Friendship Interacti<strong>on</strong>s: in past year,<br />

saw friends outside of school or work at least weekly 46.3<br />

percentage communicating by computer at least daily 21.9<br />

Participati<strong>on</strong> in Community Activities: in the past year, took part in ...<br />

community group (e.g. sports team, club, religious group) 23.8<br />

volunteer or community service activity 22.7<br />

Source: Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Garza, N., & Levine, P. (2005). After high school: A first look at<br />

the post-school experiences of youth with disabilities: A report from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study-2 (NTLS-2).<br />

Menlo Park, CA: SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

known to c<strong>on</strong>tinue”, certainly suggestive of<br />

school dropout.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NLTS-2 report (Wagner et al., 2005)<br />

provides an extended picture of the transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

process for students with ID. Wagner et al.<br />

gathered data at different points in time from<br />

students, parents/guardians, <strong>and</strong> school pers<strong>on</strong>nel.<br />

Table 5 summarizes some of the major<br />

findings of the study in relati<strong>on</strong> to students<br />

with intellectual disabilities (referred to as<br />

having mental retardati<strong>on</strong>). It should again be<br />

noted that students classified as mentally retarded<br />

represent a range of students meeting<br />

the definiti<strong>on</strong> of “mental retardati<strong>on</strong>,”<br />

One area of the NLTS-2 (Wagner et al.,<br />

2005) that is of particular interest is parental<br />

64 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 6<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Skills of Out-of-School Youth, by Selected Disability Category<br />

Learning<br />

Disability<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong> of their child’s functi<strong>on</strong>al skills.<br />

Table 6 lists these percepti<strong>on</strong>s of parents<br />

across disability categories. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> table provides<br />

a picture of five important areas: self-help<br />

skills, functi<strong>on</strong>al cognitive skills (e.g., making<br />

change), social skills, everyday communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> health. From examining these data,<br />

it is evident that young adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities c<strong>on</strong>tinue to face major challenges<br />

in dealing with the dem<strong>and</strong>s of everyday life in<br />

the community. Particularly notable are the<br />

fact that a relatively low percentage of individuals<br />

were rated “high” <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al cognitive<br />

skills that are essential to everyday life, few<br />

individuals were given a rating of “high” <strong>and</strong> a<br />

much larger number of individuals were rated<br />

“low” <strong>on</strong> social skills, <strong>and</strong> a sizable percentage<br />

were noted as experiencing difficulties in<br />

communicating effectively with others.<br />

Current transiti<strong>on</strong> practices have resulted<br />

in a more systematic approach to assessing<br />

the needs of students. Increased attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

also has been given to improving the selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills of students with intellectual<br />

disabilities in recent years <strong>and</strong> instilling<br />

the value of student-centered planning. IDEA<br />

2004 introduced the requirement that students<br />

be provided a summary of performance<br />

when they exit school. When used to advantage,<br />

this document has become a valuable<br />

resource for students <strong>and</strong> families as they take<br />

<strong>on</strong> the challenges of a postsec<strong>on</strong>dary world.<br />

More programs are also being developed for<br />

Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

students with intellectual disabilities in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>, often as dual enrollment<br />

programs (Hart, Zimbrich, & Parker,<br />

2005).<br />

Future Patterns<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Disturbance <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Multiple<br />

Disabilities<br />

Percentage rated by parents “high” <strong>on</strong>:<br />

Self-care skills 98.5 84.2 97.5 66.0 56.3<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al cognitive skills 80.4 42.3 83.6 72.2 44.5<br />

Percentage with social skills rated:<br />

High 19.1 7.3 5.4 3.1 17.7<br />

Low 17.4 26.0 45.8 28.1 25.9<br />

Percentage with health reported to be:<br />

Excellent 41.2 30.4 36.0 47.0 27.3<br />

Fair or poor 8.1 12.1 13.5 5.9 18.5<br />

Source: Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Garza, N., & Levine, P. (2005). After high school: A first look at<br />

the postschool experiences of youth with disabilities: A report from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study-2 (NTLS-2)<br />

(Exhibit 2-4). Menlo Park, CA: SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> IDEA m<strong>and</strong>ate for transiti<strong>on</strong> planning<br />

certainly is likely to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be emphasized<br />

in the future. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most important issue<br />

will be the quality of the transiti<strong>on</strong> services<br />

that students <strong>and</strong> their families receive. Wagner<br />

et al. (2005) highlighted these key areas<br />

related to transiti<strong>on</strong>:<br />

● Identifying <strong>and</strong> implementing evidencebased<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> practices<br />

● Instituting techniques for starting the transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

process earlier in a student’s school<br />

career, such as through a number of informal<br />

procedures<br />

● Empowering students so that they are more<br />

involved in their transiti<strong>on</strong> process <strong>and</strong> are<br />

able to advocate for themselves when school<br />

is completed<br />

● Increasing efforts to empower parents/<br />

guardians by providing them with critical<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> about the transiti<strong>on</strong> process <strong>and</strong><br />

various aspects what to expect when school<br />

is finished for their s<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> daughters<br />

● Being vigilant to cultural <strong>and</strong> family values<br />

● Increasing opportunities for students with<br />

intellectual disabilities in terms of further<br />

ID Practices / 65


educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> training, especially in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

instituti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

As Katsiyannis, Zhang, Woodruff, <strong>and</strong> Dix<strong>on</strong><br />

(2005) noted, research is needed “to examine<br />

the link between the transiti<strong>on</strong> process <strong>and</strong><br />

specific post-school outcomes” (p. 115). Moreover,<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>als need to validate<br />

many of the transiti<strong>on</strong> practices that are used<br />

in schools today <strong>and</strong> develop sound practices<br />

that school pers<strong>on</strong>nel can use effectively <strong>and</strong><br />

efficiently.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this article has been to provide<br />

an analysis of trends related to the selected<br />

aspects of the educati<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with intellectual disabilities, with a focus <strong>on</strong><br />

mild disabilities. Certain summative observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

appear warranted. First, there is a modest<br />

trend away from the term mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> toward intellectual disabilities, which<br />

we anticipate will accelerate in the coming<br />

years. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, there is inc<strong>on</strong>sistent use of<br />

more recent definiti<strong>on</strong>s of mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

by state departments of educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

reliance <strong>on</strong> the 25 year old definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

developed by Grossman <strong>and</strong> his colleagues<br />

(1983). If extrapolati<strong>on</strong> can be made from<br />

Polloway et al. (2009), there does seem to be a<br />

limited trend toward the adopti<strong>on</strong> of more<br />

c<strong>on</strong>temporary definiti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Third, there remains tremendous variance<br />

across states in terms of students identified as<br />

mentally retarded. In general, it can be c<strong>on</strong>cluded<br />

that a number of states appear to be<br />

serving virtually no students who might be<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidered mildly intellectually disabled (based<br />

<strong>on</strong> these prevalence data) while a number of<br />

other states serve a substantial percentage of<br />

their school populati<strong>on</strong> (1.5% or greater) under<br />

this label <strong>and</strong> therefore likely a substantial<br />

number of students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities.<br />

Fourth, there is also significant variance in<br />

terms of the educati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments in<br />

which these students are educated. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> practices<br />

across states are widely variant in terms of<br />

the apparent commitment to inclusive vs. pullout<br />

services for students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. Fifth, the data <strong>on</strong> school dropout<br />

rates, graduati<strong>on</strong> rates, <strong>and</strong> adult outcomes<br />

provide limited reas<strong>on</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>fidence that<br />

these students are well-positi<strong>on</strong>ed for successful<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d these summative statements, there<br />

are a number of issues that emerge directly<br />

<strong>and</strong> indirectly from the existing data <strong>on</strong> mild<br />

ID. First, a reading of trends in terms of state<br />

practices certainly underscores the fact that<br />

changes in the philosophies of professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> resulting revisi<strong>on</strong>s in terminology<br />

<strong>and</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong>al manuals do not automatically<br />

result in rapid changes within state<br />

policies. Thus, there c<strong>on</strong>tinues to be a significant<br />

gap between c<strong>on</strong>temporary thinking<br />

within the field of intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> state legislati<strong>on</strong> governing educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

programs. As a c<strong>on</strong>sequence, it becomes<br />

difficult to assess the impact of such<br />

recommended changes <strong>on</strong> practices until a<br />

significant number of years have elapsed. Nevertheless,<br />

it can be posited that advocacy for<br />

such changes cannot be limited to professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>al documents <strong>and</strong> rather<br />

must be accompanied by c<strong>on</strong>certed efforts at<br />

the nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> state level if changes are to<br />

occur. At the same time, it remains to be dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

if key changes, such as in terminology,<br />

will have a positive impact <strong>on</strong> the educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> lives of students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities.<br />

A sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> has to do with public<br />

school outcomes for individuals who have<br />

substantial learning challenges in the schools<br />

but are not identified within the populati<strong>on</strong><br />

of 0.90% of students nati<strong>on</strong>ally identified as<br />

intellectual disabled. If we can accept the assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

that the additi<strong>on</strong>al perhaps 1% of<br />

the school populati<strong>on</strong> might have, at other<br />

times in history, been identified as mild ID, we<br />

might then c<strong>on</strong>clude that there remain a substantial<br />

number of students who are likely to<br />

be what MacMillan <strong>and</strong> Borthwick (1980)<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce referred to as “marginal achievers” in the<br />

schools, including those that Shaw (2008)<br />

identified as “students with diverse learning<br />

needs” or “borderline intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing”.<br />

Advances in general educati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

strategies within inclusive settings will<br />

be critical for these students as well as those<br />

formally identified.<br />

Third, the apparent plateau in the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments in which students with<br />

intellectual disabilities are being served should<br />

66 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


cause professi<strong>on</strong>als to look more carefully at<br />

the challenges associated with successful inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al practices. While the higher<br />

incidence of learning disabilities has generated<br />

significant attenti<strong>on</strong> to such questi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

there has been far less attenti<strong>on</strong> given to this<br />

unique populati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> particularly to individuals<br />

with mild ID <strong>on</strong>ce they reach the sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

school level (Bouck, 2004, 2007). A<br />

key aspect of this resoluti<strong>on</strong> will be the effectiveness<br />

with which school programs are able<br />

to address the state st<strong>and</strong>ards c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum while also<br />

providing for functi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong> that prepares<br />

individuals for successful adult lives<br />

(Hoover & Patt<strong>on</strong>, 2005).<br />

Fourth, with graduati<strong>on</strong> rates low for individuals<br />

with ID <strong>and</strong> with dropout rates relatively<br />

high, there remains need for an increased<br />

commitment to retenti<strong>on</strong> in school<br />

<strong>and</strong> successful completi<strong>on</strong>. Benz, Lindstrom,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Yobanoff (2000) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the key<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents that result in improved school<br />

outcomes (<strong>and</strong>, thus presumably, in enhanced<br />

adult outcomes) include direct support within<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> programs, paid work experiences<br />

that relate to specific career interests,<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in functi<strong>on</strong>al academic areas al<strong>on</strong>g<br />

with vocati<strong>on</strong>al educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent, <strong>and</strong> the completi<strong>on</strong> of transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

goals that are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with student preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> choices.<br />

While pers<strong>on</strong>s with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

have been described as members of a<br />

“forgotten generati<strong>on</strong>” (Tymchuk et al., 2001),<br />

they warrant the c<strong>on</strong>tinued attenti<strong>on</strong> of educators.<br />

An emphasis <strong>on</strong> research related to<br />

this populati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the further development<br />

of validated, successful educati<strong>on</strong>al practices<br />

remains paramount.<br />

References<br />

Abramowicz, H. K., & Richards<strong>on</strong>, S. A. (1975).<br />

Epidemiology of severe mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

children: Community studies. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Mental Deficiency, 80, 18–39.<br />

Benz, M. R., Lindstrom, L., & Yovanoff, P. (2000).<br />

Improving graduati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> employment outcomes<br />

of students with disabilities: Predictive factors<br />

<strong>and</strong> student perspectives. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 66,<br />

509–529.<br />

Berger<strong>on</strong>, R., Floyd, R. G., & Sh<strong>and</strong>s, E. I. (2008).<br />

States’ eligibility guidelines for mental retarda-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong>: An update <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of part scores<br />

<strong>and</strong> unreliability of IQs. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Developmental Disabilities, 43, 123–131.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2004). Exploring sec<strong>on</strong>dary special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for mild mental impairment: A program<br />

in search of its place. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 25, 367–382.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2007). Lost in translati<strong>on</strong>?: Educating<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental impairment.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Disabilities Policies Studies, 18, 79–87.<br />

Denning, C. B., Chamberlain, J. A., & Polloway,<br />

E. A. (2000). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of state guidelines for<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Focus <strong>on</strong> definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> classificati<strong>on</strong><br />

practices. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 35,<br />

226–232.<br />

Dunn, L. M. (1968). Special educati<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

mildly retarded: Is much of it justifiable? Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 35, 5–22.<br />

Dunn, L. M. (1973). Children with mild general<br />

learning disabilities. In L. M. Dunn (Ed.), Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

children in the schools: Special educati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> (2nd ed., pp. 124–188). New York: Holt,<br />

Rinehart & Winst<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Edgar, E. (1987). Sec<strong>on</strong>dary programs in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>: Are many of them justifiable? Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 53, 555–561.<br />

Edgar, E. (1988). Employment as an outcome for<br />

mildly h<strong>and</strong>icapped students: Current status <strong>and</strong><br />

future directi<strong>on</strong>s. Focus <strong>on</strong> Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 21,<br />

1–8.<br />

Frankenberger, W. K. (1984). A survey of state<br />

guidelines for the identificati<strong>on</strong> of mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 22, 17–20.<br />

Greenspan, S., & Switzky, H. N. (2006). Forty-four<br />

years of AAMR manuals. In Switzky, H. N., &<br />

Greenspan, S. (Eds.). What is mental retardati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Ideas for an evolving disability in the 21 st century<br />

(pp. 3–28). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: AAMR.<br />

Grossman, H. J. (1983). Classificati<strong>on</strong> in mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong> DC: American Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

Mental Deficiency.<br />

Hart, D., Zimbrich, K., & Parker, D. R. (2005). Dual<br />

enrollment as a postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> opti<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with intellectual disabilities. In E. E.<br />

Getzel & P. Wehman (Eds.), Going to college: Exp<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

opportunities for people with disabilities<br />

(pp. 253–267). Baltimore: Paul Brookes.<br />

Hoover, J. J., & Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2005). Curriculum<br />

adaptati<strong>on</strong>s for students with learning <strong>and</strong> behavior<br />

problems. Austin, TX: PRO.ED.<br />

Hoover, J. J., & Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2008). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> role of<br />

special educators in a multitiered instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

system. Interventi<strong>on</strong> in School <strong>and</strong> Clinic, 43, 195–<br />

202.<br />

Katsiyannis, A., Zhang, D., Woodruff, N., & Dix<strong>on</strong>,<br />

A. (2005). Transiti<strong>on</strong> supports to students with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An examinati<strong>on</strong> of data from<br />

ID Practices / 67


the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study 2. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

40, 109–116.<br />

Kolstoe, O. P., & Frey, R. M. (1965). A high school<br />

work study program for mentally subnormal students.<br />

Carb<strong>on</strong>dale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.<br />

Luckass<strong>on</strong>, R., Borthwick-Duffy, S., Buntinx, W. H. E.,<br />

Coulter, D. L., Ellis, M., Reeve, A., et al. (2002).<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Definiti<strong>on</strong>, classificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> systems<br />

of support (10 th ed). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: AAMR.<br />

Luckass<strong>on</strong>, R., Coulter, D. L., Polloway, E. A., Reiss,<br />

S., Schalock, R. L., Snell, M. E. et al. (1992).<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Definiti<strong>on</strong>, classificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> systems<br />

of supports (9 th ed.). Washingt<strong>on</strong> DC: AAMR.<br />

MacMillan, D. L. (2007, December). Mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

over five decades: Less<strong>on</strong>s we might have<br />

learned. Presentati<strong>on</strong> at the Next Generati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

Research in Intellectual Disabilities: Charting the<br />

Course (A Special Olympics working c<strong>on</strong>ference).<br />

Miami, FL.<br />

MacMillan, D. L., & Borthwick, S. (1980). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> new<br />

educable mentally retarded populati<strong>on</strong>: Can they<br />

be mainstreamed? Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 155–158.<br />

MacMillan, D. L., Siperstein, G. N., & Leffert, J. S.<br />

(2006). Children with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

challenge for classificati<strong>on</strong> practices–revised. In<br />

H. N. Switzky, & S. Greenspan (Eds.). What is<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>? Ideas for an evolving disability in<br />

the 21 st century (pp. 197–220). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC:<br />

AAMR.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., & Keyes, D. (2006). Death penalty<br />

issues following Atkins. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 14, 237–255.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (1984). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> integrati<strong>on</strong> of mildly<br />

retarded students in the schools: A historical review.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 5, 18–28.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2000). Influential pers<strong>on</strong>s in the<br />

development of the field of special educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 21, 322–324.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2005). Perspectives <strong>on</strong> mild mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> status of a category of excepti<strong>on</strong>ality.<br />

In J. J. Hoover (Ed.). Current issues in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>: Meeting diverse needs in the twenty-first century<br />

(pp. 36–46). Colorado: BUENO Center for<br />

Multicultural Educati<strong>on</strong>, University of Colorado.<br />

Polloway, E. A., & Lubin, J. (2009). Feeblemindedness.<br />

Encyclopedia of American Disability<br />

History.<br />

Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Smith, J. D.., Antoine,<br />

K., & Lubin, J. (2009). State guidelines for mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> intellectual disabilities: A revisitati<strong>on</strong><br />

of previous analyses in light of changes<br />

in the field. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 44, 14–24.<br />

Polloway, E. A., & Smith, J. D. (1983). Mild mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>: Populati<strong>on</strong>, programs, <strong>and</strong> perspectives.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 50, 149–159.<br />

President’s Committee for People with Intellectual<br />

Disabilities (2007). Holding truths to be self-evident:<br />

Affirming the value of people with intellectual disabilities.<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong>, R. (1998). A survey <strong>on</strong> terminology that<br />

refers to people with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>/developmental<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

33, 290–295.<br />

Schalock, R., Luckass<strong>on</strong>, R. A., Shogren, K. A.,<br />

Borthwick-Duffy, S., Bradley, V., Buntinx, W. H. E.<br />

et al. (2007). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> renaming of mental retardati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong>ing the change to the term intellectual<br />

disability. Intellectual <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 45, 116–124.<br />

Shaw, S. (2008). An educati<strong>on</strong>al programming<br />

framework for students with diverse learning<br />

needs: Borderline intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing. Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

in School <strong>and</strong> Clinic, 43, 291–299.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2006). Quo vadis mental retardati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Definiti<strong>on</strong> by aggregati<strong>on</strong> versus the hope for<br />

individual futures. In H. N. Switzky, & S. Greenspan<br />

(Eds.). What is mental retardati<strong>on</strong>? Ideas for an<br />

evolving disability in the 21 st century (pp. 51–59).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: AAMR.<br />

Smith, P. (2007). Have we made any progress? Including<br />

students with intellectual disabilities in<br />

regular educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms. Intellectual <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 45, 297–309.<br />

Stant<strong>on</strong>, J. E., & Cassidy, V. M. (1964). Effectiveness<br />

of special classes for educable mentally retarded.<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 2, 8–13.<br />

Tymchuk, A. J., Lakin, K. C., & Luckass<strong>on</strong>, R.<br />

(2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> forgotten generati<strong>on</strong>. Baltimore: Paul H.<br />

Brookes.<br />

US Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> (2007). 27th annual<br />

report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of the individuals<br />

with disabilities educati<strong>on</strong> act. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC:<br />

Author.<br />

Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Garza, N., &<br />

Levine, P. (2005). After high school: A first look at the<br />

post-school experiences of youth with disabilities: A report<br />

from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study-2<br />

(NTLS-2). Menlo Park, CA: SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2006). Universal design for learning,<br />

access to the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum,<br />

<strong>and</strong> students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

14, 225–235.<br />

Williams<strong>on</strong>, P., McLeskey, J., Hoppey, D., & Rentz,<br />

T. (2006). Educating students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 72, 347–361.<br />

Received: 19 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 18 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 5 February 2009<br />

68 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 69–80<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

An Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the Effects of a Social Communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the Play Behaviors of Children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Spectrum Disorder<br />

Jennifer L<strong>on</strong>cola Walberg<br />

DePaul University<br />

Lesley A. Craig-Unkefer<br />

Middle Tennessee State University<br />

Abstract: This study examined the effect of a social-communicati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the play behaviors of<br />

children with autism. Six children between the ages of five <strong>and</strong> eight participated in this study that employed<br />

a single-subject, multiple baseline design. Children recruited from an urban Midwestern public school system<br />

participated in an interventi<strong>on</strong> that utilized a plan-play-report format. A post hoc analysis was c<strong>on</strong>ducted to<br />

determine the effect of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> peer communicati<strong>on</strong>, play behaviors <strong>and</strong> joint attenti<strong>on</strong> to determine<br />

if these behaviors were impacted by the interventi<strong>on</strong> even though they were not specifically targeted. Results<br />

indicated that children dem<strong>on</strong>strate increases in their rates of the above behaviors. Discussi<strong>on</strong> of results in light<br />

of known research in autism is highlighted.<br />

Play is a medium through which children first<br />

begin to explore their envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Babies<br />

explore by grabbing <strong>and</strong> mouthing toys <strong>and</strong><br />

as children grow <strong>and</strong> develop, they begin to<br />

imitate what they see around them (Berk,<br />

2000). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se early skills lead children to the<br />

all important development of play. Play is<br />

means through which children develop language,<br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> even literacy<br />

skills (Bodrova & Le<strong>on</strong>g, 2003; Guralnick &<br />

Neville, 1997). It is a means for children to<br />

learn about the customs <strong>and</strong> cultures that surround<br />

them (G<strong>on</strong>cu, 1999) <strong>and</strong> it helps them<br />

to make sense of what they see in their envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

<strong>and</strong> allows them to reenact scenes<br />

from their everyday life that may be c<strong>on</strong>fusing<br />

(Alvarez & Philips, 1998). Most importantly,<br />

play provides a c<strong>on</strong>text through which children<br />

can practice interacti<strong>on</strong> skills.<br />

Children with autism have difficulty with<br />

play behaviors. Lack of make-believe or socialimitative<br />

play is <strong>on</strong>e of the diagnostic criteria<br />

listed in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric<br />

Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1994). Characteristically, children<br />

with autism play in unusual ways. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Jennifer L<strong>on</strong>cola Walberg, DePaul<br />

University, 2320 N. Kenmore Ave., Chicago, IL<br />

60614. E-mail: jwalberg@depaul.edu.<br />

may prefer to play in isolati<strong>on</strong> or to use objects<br />

in a manner in which they were not intended.<br />

For example, a child with autism may take a<br />

plastic dinosaur <strong>and</strong> flip it back in forth in<br />

fr<strong>on</strong>t of his face, manipulating the tail rather<br />

than having the dinosaur chase other animals<br />

or eat vegetati<strong>on</strong> from a plastic tree. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re are<br />

several theories as to why play <strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

are difficult for children with autism<br />

including problems with theory of mind,<br />

interfering stereotypies, <strong>and</strong> low language<br />

skills (Baker, Koegel, & Koegel, 1998; Bar<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Choen, Baldwin, & Crows<strong>on</strong>, 1997; Lord &<br />

Pickles, 1996).<br />

Although children with autism do engage in<br />

play, sp<strong>on</strong>taneous pretend play seems to be<br />

lacking <strong>and</strong> children instead may engage what<br />

is referred to as sensorimotor play (Libby,<br />

Powell, Messer & Jordan, 1998). This type of<br />

play, as opposed to play where children use<br />

objects as they are intended (functi<strong>on</strong>al play),<br />

is seen when children use toys in a way that<br />

disregards their functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> is often repetitive<br />

in nature (Roger, Cook, & Meryl, 2005).<br />

For example, a child may take building blocks<br />

or cars <strong>and</strong> bang them together or line them<br />

up end to end instead of building things or<br />

racing the cars around the floor. Libby et al.<br />

(1998) suggest that the reas<strong>on</strong> children with<br />

autism produce less functi<strong>on</strong>al play than typically<br />

developing children may be because they<br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 69


lack the underlying categorical <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

knowledge that makes these toys have a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> in the first place. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, it would<br />

make sense that their immature play would<br />

mirror that of an infant who has yet to build<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts with which to interact with the toys<br />

in a more sophisticated way.<br />

Play <strong>and</strong> language are not isolated skills.<br />

Language levels <strong>and</strong> social communicative behaviors<br />

in children with autism are linked in<br />

several ways. Lord <strong>and</strong> Pickles (1996) found<br />

that children with autism who dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

very little language usage also had more pr<strong>on</strong>ounced<br />

deficits in play <strong>and</strong> social behaviors<br />

than children with autism who used phrases<br />

(here defined as three word utterances) or<br />

had a greater comm<strong>and</strong> of language use.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se authors also found children with autism<br />

frequently displayed some of the hallmarks of<br />

the disorder regardless of language use. That<br />

is, problems with eye gaze, facial expressi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> attending were visible in children regardless<br />

of language level or use. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings<br />

are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with other studies of this type.<br />

Another reas<strong>on</strong> children with autism may<br />

have difficulty with social interacti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> play<br />

could result from the difficulty involved in<br />

simultaneously dem<strong>on</strong>strating two novel skills.<br />

In a study comparing the play behaviors of<br />

preschool children with <strong>and</strong> with out pervasive<br />

developmental disorders, Pierce-Jordan<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lifter (2005) found that when children<br />

were engaged in emerging play or trying out<br />

new play skills that were difficult for them,<br />

they were less likely to be socially interactive<br />

with their peers. When the children were engaged<br />

in social interacti<strong>on</strong> in these situati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the interacti<strong>on</strong>s were not likely to be coordinated.<br />

This finding was evident for all children<br />

in the study supporting the idea that<br />

combining social interacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> coordinating<br />

play may be too taxing when both are<br />

areas of need. Children may need to become<br />

competent in <strong>on</strong>e are prior to development in<br />

the other.<br />

Despite the aforementi<strong>on</strong>ed c<strong>on</strong>cerns, children<br />

with autism can learn play skills. Social<br />

<strong>and</strong> play skills can be taught to children with<br />

autism while they are at school, home, <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

in clinic based situati<strong>on</strong>s (Jahr, Eldevik, &<br />

Eikseth, 2000; Nikopolous, & Keenan, 2004;<br />

Terpstra, Higgins, & Pierce, 2002; Sawyer, Luiselli,<br />

Ricciardi, & Gower, 2005). Children with<br />

autism can learn needed social <strong>and</strong> play skills<br />

through peer-mediated sessi<strong>on</strong>s (Sawyer, Luiselli,<br />

Ricciardi, & Gower, 2005), typical peer<br />

models (Garfinkle & Schwartz, 2002), adult<br />

priming (Zanoli & Daggett, 1998), video modeling,<br />

<strong>and</strong> script fading (Sarokoff, Taylor, &<br />

Pouls<strong>on</strong>, 2001) techniques.<br />

Given the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between play <strong>and</strong><br />

language in children with autism, this study<br />

examined whether an interventi<strong>on</strong> which successfully<br />

increased peer-directed commenting,<br />

language complexity <strong>and</strong> diversity in dyads<br />

of children with autism (L<strong>on</strong>cola & Craig-<br />

Unkefer, 2005) also had an effect <strong>on</strong> play<br />

behaviors; specifically joint attenti<strong>on</strong>, talk with<br />

their play partner, <strong>and</strong> different developmental<br />

levels of play. This study is unique because<br />

two children with autism were paired together<br />

<strong>and</strong> provided the interventi<strong>on</strong> at the same<br />

time. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following research questi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

addressed in this inquiry: 1) What effect does<br />

a social communicati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> have <strong>on</strong><br />

joint attenti<strong>on</strong>? 2) Were there changes in the<br />

amount of peer talk from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

across participants? 3) What types of play<br />

behaviors did the participants engage in <strong>and</strong><br />

were more interactive play behaviors observed<br />

from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Six children between the ages of five <strong>and</strong> eight<br />

years-old participated in this study. To determine<br />

eligibility for participati<strong>on</strong>, an initial<br />

screening assessment was completed by the<br />

classroom teachers. In order to participate in<br />

the study, a child needed to be between the<br />

ages of five- <strong>and</strong> eight-years-old, have a diagnosis<br />

of mild/moderate autism, <strong>and</strong> passed<br />

the Chicago Public Schools annual hearing<br />

assessment. Once a child met the initial criteria,<br />

they were further assessed <strong>on</strong> language<br />

<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior functi<strong>on</strong>ing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> three<br />

criteria relative to language <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior<br />

included: 1) a score of at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard deviati<strong>on</strong> (SD) below the mean <strong>on</strong><br />

measures of expressive <strong>and</strong> receptive vocabulary<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the Peabody Picture Vocabulary<br />

Test–III [PPVT-III] (Dunn & Dunn, 1997)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Expressive Vocabulary Test [EVT]<br />

(Williams, 1997); 2) a score within two stan-<br />

70 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


dard deviati<strong>on</strong>s of the mean inn<strong>on</strong>verbal cognitive<br />

ability as measured by the Leiter Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Performance Scale-Revised (Roid &<br />

Miller, 1995); <strong>and</strong> 3) a deficit in at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

subscale measured by the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive<br />

Behavior Scales: Classroom Editi<strong>on</strong> (Sparrow,<br />

Balla, & Cicchetti, 1985).<br />

Children were excluded from the study if<br />

they met <strong>on</strong>e or more of the following exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

criteria: 1) a diagnosed hearing impairment,<br />

2) a diagnosis of severe/profound autism,<br />

3) a primary diagnosis other than<br />

autism, 4) a sec<strong>on</strong>dary diagnosis of mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>, serious emoti<strong>on</strong>al disturbance,<br />

hearing loss, or low visi<strong>on</strong>, 5) were under the<br />

age of five years, <strong>on</strong>e m<strong>on</strong>th or over the age of<br />

eight years, 1 m<strong>on</strong>th, or 6) English was not<br />

their primary language. No participants were<br />

excluded based solely <strong>on</strong> sex, race, or ethnic<br />

group. Characteristics of the participants are<br />

described below.<br />

Child A1 was a 6-year-old Hispanic girl with<br />

very low language <strong>and</strong> social skills <strong>and</strong> an<br />

average IQ. On the PPVT, her age equivalent<br />

was below 1-9 <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> the EVT she scored an<br />

age equivalent of 2-1. She scored 2 SDs below<br />

the mean <strong>on</strong> the communicati<strong>on</strong>, daily living,<br />

<strong>and</strong> socializati<strong>on</strong> secti<strong>on</strong>s of the Vinel<strong>and</strong><br />

Adaptive Behavior Scale. Her brief IQ as measured<br />

by the Leiter yielded a score of 85. Both<br />

Spanish <strong>and</strong> English were spoken in the<br />

child’s home <strong>and</strong> the teacher indicated that<br />

Child A1 understood <strong>and</strong> spoke both languages.<br />

During classroom activities, Child A1<br />

needed frequent redirecti<strong>on</strong> to attend to<br />

tasks. In free play activities she would approach<br />

other children <strong>and</strong> take desired toys,<br />

but did not interact with other children or use<br />

spoken language despite a large, single word<br />

vocabulary. Teacher reports indicated that the<br />

child <strong>on</strong>ly used spoken language when<br />

prompted or very upset. Child A1 regularly<br />

attempted to run from the classroom <strong>and</strong> the<br />

child’s classroom door was gated at all times to<br />

prevent her from running from the room. She<br />

was included with the regular educati<strong>on</strong> kindergarten<br />

for physical educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> library.<br />

Child A2 was a 6-year-old Caucasian male<br />

with low language skills, an average IQ, <strong>and</strong><br />

deficits in adaptive behavior. In tests of Expressive<br />

<strong>and</strong> Receptive vocabulary, Child A2<br />

received age equivalent scores of below 1-9<br />

<strong>and</strong> 2-8 respectively. According to the Vine-<br />

l<strong>and</strong>, adaptive behavior deficits were noted in<br />

areas of communicati<strong>on</strong>, daily living, <strong>and</strong> socializati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> brief IQ was measured at 80.<br />

English was the <strong>on</strong>ly language Child A1 utilized.<br />

One <strong>and</strong> two word utterances were used<br />

to get his needs met; however, he did not use<br />

language in social situati<strong>on</strong>s with children<br />

other than as an initial greeting. Child A2’s<br />

spent the morning in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained class for<br />

students with autism <strong>and</strong> the afterno<strong>on</strong> he was<br />

included in a regular educati<strong>on</strong> kindergarten.<br />

He preferred to play with adults <strong>and</strong> his communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills with adults were perceived as<br />

better than with children.<br />

Child B1 was a 6-year-old Arabic male with<br />

below average language scores, deficits in<br />

daily living skills, <strong>and</strong> an average IQ. His<br />

scores <strong>on</strong> the PPVT <strong>and</strong> the EVT were the<br />

highest of all study participants with age equivalent<br />

of 3-3 <strong>and</strong> 5-1 respectively. Results of the<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> assessment indicated that he had a<br />

score that was 2 SDs below the mean in <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e area; daily living skills. Child B1’s brief IQ<br />

was measured at 77. Of the six children included<br />

in the study, Child B1 was the most<br />

competent communicator. He was fluent in<br />

both English <strong>and</strong> Arabic <strong>and</strong> despite low language<br />

scores, communicated well in both English<br />

<strong>and</strong> Arabic. Child B2 spent the morning<br />

in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained class for students with autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> was included in the regular educati<strong>on</strong><br />

kindergarten in the afterno<strong>on</strong>. He c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

used language <strong>and</strong> frequently kept a<br />

running commentary <strong>on</strong> what was going <strong>on</strong><br />

around him, often talking over others <strong>and</strong><br />

ignoring their resp<strong>on</strong>ses to questi<strong>on</strong>s. He had<br />

problems with syntax <strong>and</strong> made most statements<br />

into questi<strong>on</strong>s by adding, “okay?” at the<br />

end.<br />

Child B2 was an English speaking 8-year-old<br />

African American male who had an undisclosed<br />

medical c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that affected his<br />

gross <strong>and</strong> fine motor skills <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>fined him<br />

to a wheelchair. Testing revealed age equivalents<br />

of 3-1 <strong>and</strong> 4-5 <strong>on</strong> the PPVT <strong>and</strong> EVT.<br />

Scores <strong>on</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong> were 2 SDs below the<br />

mean in communicati<strong>on</strong>, daily living skills,<br />

<strong>and</strong> socializati<strong>on</strong> while his brief IQ as measure<br />

by the Leiter was 73. Child B2 was educated in<br />

a full day, self-c<strong>on</strong>tained class for students with<br />

autism <strong>and</strong> was included with his first grade<br />

classmates for physical educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> music.<br />

He would have been included for Library <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 71


Computer, but these classes were located <strong>on</strong><br />

the sec<strong>on</strong>d floor of his school building which<br />

was not wheelchair accessible. Despite low language<br />

scores, Child B2 had many words <strong>and</strong><br />

could communicate in full sentences when<br />

prompted. He would use as few words as he<br />

could, unless prompted for more, <strong>and</strong><br />

phrased most sp<strong>on</strong>taneous statements in the<br />

form of a questi<strong>on</strong>. For example, at snack time<br />

he would say “more cookies” <strong>and</strong> when he the<br />

teacher did not resp<strong>on</strong>d, he would say “I want<br />

more cookies, please.” When referring to a<br />

picture he drew, Child B2 would ask questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

such as, “Is this a drag<strong>on</strong>?” rather than stating<br />

“This is a drag<strong>on</strong>.” Finally, Child B2 had many<br />

unintelligible verbal stereotypes <strong>and</strong> would often<br />

revert to clicking <strong>and</strong> squealing noises.<br />

Child C1 was a 7-year-old Caucasian male.<br />

He spoke English at school <strong>and</strong> Polish at<br />

home <strong>and</strong> both of his parents were fluent in<br />

English. Child C1 had low language scores<br />

with age equivalents of 3-0 <strong>and</strong> 2-7 <strong>on</strong> the<br />

PPVT <strong>and</strong> the EVT, deficits in all areas of<br />

adaptive behavior, <strong>and</strong> an average IQ measured<br />

at 87. He was educated in a full day,<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained autism classroom <strong>and</strong> was included<br />

with the first grade for physical educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> music. Child C2 was heavily dependant<br />

<strong>on</strong> visual schedules for working <strong>and</strong><br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> although Child C1 used language<br />

infrequently in social exchanges, he<br />

talked his way through all of the steps <strong>on</strong> his<br />

visual schedules. He also gave himself verbal<br />

reminders such as, “snack is finished, time for<br />

reading.” Child C1’s sp<strong>on</strong>taneous language<br />

was very repetitive <strong>and</strong> repeatedly used a few<br />

scripted phrases. He seemed very interested in<br />

playing with other children, but did not seem<br />

to know what to communicate to peers.<br />

Child C2 was a 7-year-old boy of Hispanic<br />

descent who spoke <strong>on</strong>ly English. He had low<br />

language scores, deficits in adaptive behavior,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a low-average IQ. On the PPVT, Child C2<br />

had an age equivalent of 2-5 <strong>and</strong> the EVT his<br />

age-equivalent was 3-7. He displayed deficits<br />

in areas of communicati<strong>on</strong>, daily living, <strong>and</strong><br />

socializati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> his brief IQ<br />

was 70. He was placed in a full day, selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

autism classroom <strong>and</strong> was included<br />

with the regular educati<strong>on</strong> first grade for Library,<br />

Physical Educati<strong>on</strong>, Music, <strong>and</strong> Computer.<br />

Child C2 had a full time assistant assigned<br />

to him <strong>and</strong>, prior to the beginning of<br />

this study, Child C2 had been removed from<br />

school for two weeks while his parents <strong>and</strong><br />

doctor attempted to get his aggressive behavior<br />

under c<strong>on</strong>trol. Child C2 used language<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly when prompted <strong>and</strong> typically used <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e word utterances. He had little interest in<br />

socializing with peers or adults <strong>and</strong> if left unattended,<br />

would sit quietly <strong>and</strong> look at books<br />

for hours. All children in the study received<br />

related services for Speech <strong>and</strong> Language <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Work.<br />

Children participants were paired into<br />

three dyads; <strong>on</strong>e mixed gender dyad <strong>and</strong> two<br />

same gender dyads. As autism affects more<br />

boys then girls, it was not possible to recruit<br />

enough girls for three mixed gender dyads<br />

<strong>and</strong> no child was excluded <strong>on</strong> the basis of<br />

gender. Children were paired according to<br />

scores <strong>on</strong> the PPVT <strong>and</strong> EVT <strong>and</strong> according<br />

to schedule availability. An attempt was made<br />

to pair a child with lower language scores <strong>and</strong><br />

a child with slightly higher language scores,<br />

although all study participants showed significant<br />

delays in expressive <strong>and</strong> receptive vocabulary.<br />

Setting<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted at a public elementary<br />

school located in a large Midwestern city.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> school’s populati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted of preschool<br />

through 8 th grade children <strong>and</strong> included<br />

programs for both general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong> students. Special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

programs at the school served students<br />

with visual impairments, children with physical<br />

disabilities, <strong>and</strong> housed self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

programs for students with autism. Demographic<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> for the school indicates<br />

the racial/ethnic breakdown of the school<br />

was 53.4% Caucasian, 27% Hispanic, 10.5%<br />

Asian/Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong>er, 5.4% African American,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 3.6% Native American. Forty-six percent<br />

of the students enrolled at the school at<br />

the time of the study had low-income status<br />

<strong>and</strong> 15.3% were c<strong>on</strong>sidered Limited English<br />

Proficient (LEP) <strong>and</strong> qualified for transiti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

bilingual programs.<br />

Baseline <strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred<br />

in a secti<strong>on</strong>ed off area of a large hallway in the<br />

school. Two accordi<strong>on</strong> style dividers were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>structed each measuring 8 feet l<strong>on</strong>g by 4<br />

feet high. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se dividers were placed in an<br />

72 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


L-shape against a wall secti<strong>on</strong>ing off a “U”<br />

shaped space that measured 8 feet by 8 feet<br />

enclosed <strong>on</strong> three sides with the camera <strong>and</strong><br />

tripod at the open end of the U. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> area<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained a table <strong>and</strong> two chairs.<br />

Materials<br />

Materials used in the baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s were representative of play materials<br />

comm<strong>on</strong>ly found in classrooms of<br />

young children <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted of dramatic play<br />

items including themes (e.g., grocery store,<br />

kitchen) <strong>and</strong> role playing materials (e.g.,<br />

doctor, veterinarian). Manipulative items<br />

such as blocks <strong>and</strong> cars were also used. Materials<br />

were grouped into seven different play<br />

themes: Doctor, Vet/Zoo, C<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>, Grocery<br />

Store, Farm, Housekeeping/Kitchen, <strong>and</strong><br />

Airport. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> doctor play theme c<strong>on</strong>sisted of<br />

white shirts, play doctor kits, an x-ray machine,<br />

ace b<strong>and</strong>ages, <strong>and</strong> teleph<strong>on</strong>es. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

vet/zoo theme had stuffed animals, blocks for<br />

building the zoo walls, a veterinarian kit, <strong>and</strong><br />

assorted vegetables for feeding the animals.<br />

When playing with the c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> theme,<br />

children had access to plastic c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong><br />

hats, a plastic tool kit, plastic blocks, screws,<br />

bolts, dump trucks <strong>and</strong> assorted dolls. Grocery<br />

store items were comprised of a cash register,<br />

wallets, pretend m<strong>on</strong>ey, plastic <strong>and</strong> boxed<br />

food, grocery cart, <strong>and</strong> McD<strong>on</strong>ald’s® play<br />

food. In the Farm theme, children could play<br />

with a Fischer Price® barn, plastic vegetables,<br />

straw hats <strong>and</strong> b<strong>and</strong>anas. Housekeeping/<br />

kitchen, the most popular theme with the children,<br />

had a plastic sink <strong>and</strong> stove, plastic<br />

dishes, utensils, <strong>and</strong> cups, a microwave, assorted<br />

foods, a vacuum, feather dusters, <strong>and</strong><br />

ph<strong>on</strong>es. Finally, the airport theme c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

an airport block set, airplanes, helicopters,<br />

trucks, <strong>and</strong> emergency vehicles. All themes<br />

except the farm <strong>and</strong> veterinarian c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

assorted dolls.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

This study utilized a multiple baseline design<br />

to determine the result of an interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

designed to promote social communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills in young children with autism (Kazdin,<br />

1982). Each dyad in the study remained in<br />

baseline until a stable or decreasing baseline<br />

was dem<strong>on</strong>strated for at least five sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first dyad with a stable baseline was identified<br />

as Dyad One <strong>and</strong> the other two dyads<br />

remained in baseline until a clear treatment<br />

effect was visible for the sec<strong>on</strong>d dyad, identified<br />

as Dyad 2, <strong>and</strong> so <strong>on</strong>. Following the multiple<br />

baseline design discussed above, the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was then introduced to Dyad Two<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ce effects were evident in the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

dyad, the interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced to the<br />

third. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> two experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

this study were baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>. All<br />

children received at least five baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> between 10 <strong>and</strong> 12 interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Baseline. Baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

three times per week. During the baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, 10 minutes of play were videotaped<br />

<strong>and</strong> subsequently coded. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

brought the two children in each dyad to the<br />

area designated for project use. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

told the children to play with the toys<br />

arranged <strong>on</strong> the carpeted floor or table. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

toys c<strong>on</strong>sisted of a play theme r<strong>and</strong>omly selected<br />

from those discussed above <strong>and</strong> rotated<br />

throughout the baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. One play<br />

theme was r<strong>and</strong>omly chosen to start the baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> then taken out of the next<br />

r<strong>and</strong>om selecti<strong>on</strong>. Selecti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued in this<br />

manner until all play themes had been used<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce. All play themes had at least two rotati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the selected order. During Interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

some children began to request certain<br />

play themes <strong>and</strong> the order was dropped. In<br />

the baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist did<br />

not prompt language or prohibit any behaviors<br />

except those that were harmful to the<br />

peers or materials (e.g. mouthing materials,<br />

hitting lockers).<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>. An interventi<strong>on</strong> that incorporated<br />

the cognitive-social learning model<br />

(Ladd & Mize, 1983) was used as a means to<br />

promote social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills for children<br />

with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> had three<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents: 1) a play organizer sessi<strong>on</strong>, 2) a<br />

ten minute play sessi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> 3) a review sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first comp<strong>on</strong>ent, the advanced play<br />

organizer, lasted approximately five minutes.<br />

During this comp<strong>on</strong>ent, the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

described the toys, how to play with toys, how<br />

to share toys, <strong>and</strong> how to get your partner’s<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> specific to a play theme designated<br />

for the sessi<strong>on</strong>. Children were encouraged to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tribute to this discussi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> same play<br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 73


themes used in the baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s were used<br />

in the interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d comp<strong>on</strong>ent was a 10-minute<br />

play sessi<strong>on</strong>. During this comp<strong>on</strong>ent, the children<br />

played with the toys <strong>and</strong> materials provided.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist sat away from the<br />

play area, watching the children, <strong>and</strong> used<br />

verbal redirects <strong>and</strong> reflective statements to<br />

sustain <strong>and</strong> maintain the children’s play interacti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist did not prompt<br />

or comment while the dyad was engaged in<br />

an interacti<strong>on</strong>, but redirected children when<br />

they were engaged in inappropriate or isolating<br />

behaviors. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist determined<br />

the types of redirects to use based <strong>on</strong><br />

the degree of support needed by the child <strong>and</strong><br />

the particular play situati<strong>on</strong>. During this<br />

phase, the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist also made reflective<br />

statements <strong>and</strong> comments about the toys, the<br />

themes, or the children’s play. Reflective statements<br />

did not specifically prompt child acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

or model language. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of reflective<br />

statements was to keep the children<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> the play theme by suggesting an<br />

appropriate activity within the play theme<br />

(e.g. “<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> baby wants to be fed.”).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> third comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the review sessi<strong>on</strong>, took place immediately following<br />

the play sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> lasted approximately<br />

5 minutes. In this phase of the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist reentered the play<br />

area <strong>and</strong> sat near the children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

<strong>and</strong> the children discussed the play<br />

that occurred in the preceding comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

including what the children had played with<br />

<strong>and</strong> what scenarios they had enacted. If the<br />

children were not forthcoming with specifics,<br />

the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist reviewed what was d<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

thanked the children for participati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

gave verbal praise for language <strong>and</strong> play skills.<br />

A complete descripti<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong> is<br />

provided L<strong>on</strong>cola <strong>and</strong> Craig-Unkefer (2005).<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist. One Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist collected<br />

all baseline, <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> data. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

was a doctoral c<strong>and</strong>idate in Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>. She had a master’s degree in Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> six years experience teaching<br />

young children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist trained for this study<br />

according to the Peer Language <strong>and</strong> Behavior<br />

Code: Manual <strong>and</strong> Coding Protocol (Craig-<br />

Unkefer, Williams, & Kaiser, 1998). First the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>ist transcribed at least eight min-<br />

utes of previously videotaped interacti<strong>on</strong> using<br />

the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts<br />

(SALT: Miller & Chapman, 2000) as<br />

the transcripti<strong>on</strong> program. Next all transcripts<br />

were verified for accuracy. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> transcripts<br />

were then coded <strong>and</strong> at least <strong>on</strong>e code was<br />

assigned to the end of each line of a word or<br />

phrase. Finally a summary of the coded data<br />

was provided for each speaker. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

also reviewed tapes of a previous study<br />

(Craig-Unkefer & Kaiser, 2002) <strong>and</strong> discussed<br />

technique with the first author.<br />

Data collecti<strong>on</strong>. Baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s were videotaped by the Interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

using a S<strong>on</strong>y DCR-TRV17 digital camcorder.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se sessi<strong>on</strong>s were then transferred<br />

to DVD for coding. In order to determine if<br />

there were changes across measures as a result<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong>, the last three baseline <strong>and</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s were coded utilizing the<br />

criteria below. This procedure is c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with those established by Craig-Unkefer <strong>and</strong><br />

Kaiser (2003).<br />

Measures<br />

Different types of children’s play were coded<br />

according to the Peer Play Code (Craig-<br />

Unkefer et al., 1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Play Code was developed<br />

from Parten (1932) <strong>and</strong> Rubin (1989).<br />

Six categories of play were observed: aggressi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

solitary, <strong>on</strong>looker, parallel play, associative<br />

play, <strong>and</strong> cooperative play. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> coding<br />

categories can be seen in Table 1. A graduate<br />

student in Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology completed<br />

all of the coding. Coding was completed with<br />

pen <strong>and</strong> paper <strong>and</strong> the coder watched 15-s of<br />

video <strong>and</strong> then recorded for 5-s. Categories<br />

were assigned when the coder noted 8-s of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuous play behavior thus excluding the<br />

possibility that more than <strong>on</strong>e type of play<br />

could be observed in any <strong>on</strong>e interval. Each<br />

interval was also coded for peer talk if the<br />

child talked to their peer for at least 10-s of the<br />

interval. Given the nature of autism, codes<br />

were modified to take into account both echolalia,<br />

stimming <strong>and</strong> singing. Echolalia <strong>and</strong><br />

singing were coded as no peer talk <strong>and</strong> stimming<br />

behaviors were coded as solitary play.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, the intervals that children attended<br />

to their play partner were also coded.<br />

Joint attenti<strong>on</strong> was coded when both peers<br />

were observing or attenti<strong>on</strong> is focused in same<br />

74 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 1<br />

Coding definiti<strong>on</strong>s for Play Behaviors<br />

directi<strong>on</strong> for at least 5 sec<strong>on</strong>ds. Commencement<br />

of joint attenti<strong>on</strong> starts <strong>on</strong>ce a peer has<br />

requested the attenti<strong>on</strong> of the other peer using<br />

behaviors such as pointing, showing or<br />

verbal comments (e.g. “Look at the ball”).<br />

Other behaviors indicating joint attenti<strong>on</strong> include<br />

self-referential. Joint attending is a dyadic<br />

code <strong>and</strong> both peers need to be engaged<br />

in the acti<strong>on</strong> in order for the behavior to be<br />

coded as occurring.<br />

Results<br />

Code Definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Aggressi<strong>on</strong> Aggressi<strong>on</strong> refers to n<strong>on</strong> playful physical with another child. It is almost antag<strong>on</strong>istic<br />

in nature. Acts of aggressi<strong>on</strong> include hitting, kicking, <strong>and</strong> grabbing.<br />

Solitary <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child is within the physical vicinity of peer, the child does not interact with the<br />

peer for at least 8 sec<strong>on</strong>ds. Solitary play is coded when the child is isolated from<br />

the play area or w<strong>and</strong>ers without noticing the peer. Code solitary when the child<br />

is obtaining visual or auditory informati<strong>on</strong> form an object (physical manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

of comp<strong>on</strong>ent of a toy—rolling a wheel <strong>on</strong> a car).<br />

Onlooker <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child is watching the peer for at least 10sec<strong>on</strong>ds but does not engage in play or<br />

interactive activity. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child may talk to the peer (offer suggesti<strong>on</strong>s or asking<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s) but does not overtly enter into the play activity of the peer. In this play<br />

category the child is definitely observing the peer.<br />

Parallel Play <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child plays independently but with toys that will bring him or her closer to the<br />

peer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child plays beside but not with the peer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child plays with toys as<br />

he/she sees fit, <strong>and</strong> does not seem to influence or modify the activity of the peer.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is no attempt by the child to c<strong>on</strong>trol the activity of the peer.<br />

Associate Play <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child plays with the peer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is an exchange of materials. A c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al;<br />

exchange occurs about comm<strong>on</strong> activity which has the potential to bring the<br />

other peer into the activity, but each child engages as he/she wishes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is no<br />

divisi<strong>on</strong> of labor <strong>and</strong> no organizati<strong>on</strong> of the activity towards the development or<br />

creati<strong>on</strong> of a mutually c<strong>on</strong>trived project or play theme.<br />

Cooperative Play Both children engage in an organized <strong>and</strong> purposeful activity. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child directs the<br />

activities of other peers the play interacti<strong>on</strong> for some purpose necessitating a<br />

divisi<strong>on</strong> of labor <strong>and</strong> an organizati<strong>on</strong> of roles. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> play is goal driven so that<br />

efforts of <strong>on</strong>e peer are supplemented by those of the other peer.<br />

Peer Talk Child is talking for at least 10 sec<strong>on</strong>ds of the interval to their peer. Peer talk does<br />

not include echolalic behavior or singing.<br />

Joint Attenti<strong>on</strong> Both peers are observing the same object or attenti<strong>on</strong> is focused in the same<br />

directi<strong>on</strong> for at least 5 sec<strong>on</strong>ds. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> commencement of joint attenti<strong>on</strong> starts after<br />

a peer has requested the attenti<strong>on</strong> of the other peer either by pointing, showing<br />

or a verbal comment.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of the study was to determine the<br />

effects of an interventi<strong>on</strong> to promote social<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the play behaviors of six<br />

children diagnosed with <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorder.<br />

Behaviors associated with play were<br />

measured to determine if there were changes<br />

as a result of participating in the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> describe those changes that are specific to<br />

appropriate play interacti<strong>on</strong>s. Those behaviors<br />

that were measured included: types of<br />

play children engaged in, amount of joint attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> amount of peer talk. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interobserver<br />

agreement of the play behaviors is<br />

also reported.<br />

Interobserver Agreement of Play Behaviors<br />

Reliability data were collected <strong>on</strong> 20% of the<br />

play sessi<strong>on</strong>s using the Peer Play Code<br />

(Parten, 1932; Rubin, 1989). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were independently viewed <strong>and</strong> coded by the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d author. Interobserver reliability was<br />

assessed using an exact agreement formula in<br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 75


Figure 1. Mean Percentage of Intervals of Joint<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

which the total number of agreements was<br />

divided by the total number of agreements<br />

plus disagreements <strong>and</strong> multiplied by 100. Reliability<br />

was assessed separately for play <strong>and</strong><br />

joint attenti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> overall IOA for peer play<br />

was 90%. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> range for all sessi<strong>on</strong>s was 70–<br />

98%. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> overall reliability for joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

was 84% with a range of 65–97%.<br />

Amount of Joint Attenti<strong>on</strong> between Peers<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean percentage of intervals of joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

displayed by dyads is presented in<br />

Figure 1. Dyad Two attended to each other<br />

more than their peers in the other dyads having<br />

the greatest number of intervals of joint<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> in the baseline with approximately<br />

5% increase in the average number of intervals<br />

from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Dyad Three<br />

also dem<strong>on</strong>strated gains from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

with an average 10% of the intervals<br />

during baseline to 15% of the intervals<br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong>. Dyad One remained stable<br />

across phases with a minimal increase of<br />

1% <strong>on</strong> average of joint attending from baseline<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Intervals of Peer Talk<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean percentage of peer talk is displayed<br />

in Figure 2. All dyads had increases from baseline<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Dyad Two had the greatest<br />

gains with an average of 50% of the intervals<br />

where the children engaged in talk during<br />

baseline to 75% of the intervals in the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase. Dyad Two averaged 7% of the<br />

intervals in peer talk with increases to an average<br />

of 24% during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase.<br />

Figure 2. Mean Percentage of Intervals with Peer<br />

Talk<br />

Dyad One averaged 20% of their sessi<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

peer talk during the baseline phase. During<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase, they average 39% of<br />

intervals of peer talk.<br />

Peer Play<br />

Individual play behaviors as well as categories<br />

of play behaviors are reported. Four of the six<br />

children (A1, A2, C1, C2) increased the percentage<br />

of intervals they spent in solitary or<br />

<strong>on</strong>looker play from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Only the two children in Dyad Two decreased<br />

solitary <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>looker behaviors from baseline<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Given the increases in solitary<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>looker behaviors for the four children,<br />

there were also decreases in parallel <strong>and</strong> associative<br />

play behaviors from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Only the children in Dyad Two (B1<br />

<strong>and</strong> B2) had a mean increase of 3.8 mean<br />

intervals to 6.6 mean intervals engaged in<br />

more complex play.<br />

To characterize the play of the dyads bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

discrete behaviors, the six individual<br />

play behaviors were grouped into three play<br />

classes. Isolate play is the combinati<strong>on</strong> of aggressive<br />

<strong>and</strong> solitary play. Independent play is<br />

a combinati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>looker <strong>and</strong> parallel play.<br />

Interactive play is a combinati<strong>on</strong> of cooperative<br />

<strong>and</strong> associative play. Table 2 displays the<br />

mean percentage of intervals of these play<br />

categories for the three dyads. Dyad Two decreased<br />

in isolate play <strong>and</strong> increased in independent<br />

play primarily with minimal increases<br />

in interactive play from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Dyad Three decreased in the average<br />

number of intervals of isolate play <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>versely<br />

increased in independent play with no<br />

76 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 2<br />

Mean Percentage of Intervals of Isolate,<br />

Independent, <strong>and</strong> Interactive Play Behaviors<br />

During Pre-Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Sessi<strong>on</strong>s By Dyad<br />

occurrence of interactive play in either phase.<br />

Dyad One increased in their isolate play behaviors<br />

<strong>and</strong> decreased in their independent<br />

play behaviors <strong>on</strong> average from baseline to<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was no change in interactive<br />

play across the phases.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Pre-Interventi<strong>on</strong> Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Mean Range Mean Range<br />

Dyad 1 Isolate 89.9 93.9<br />

85.4 – 93.7 91.1 – 96.6<br />

Independent 9.4 6.1<br />

5.5 – 13.6 3.3 – 8.8<br />

Interactive 1 0<br />

0–1 0<br />

Dyad 2 Isolate 83.9 75.6<br />

78 – 89.5 68.8 – 82.2<br />

Independent 13.3 21.1<br />

6.6 – 18 14.4 – 27.7<br />

Interactive 2.9 3.3<br />

0 – 2.8 0 – 3.3<br />

Dyad 3 Isolate 95.5 91.2<br />

93.7 – 96.8 87.9 – 94.5<br />

Independent 4.7 8.8<br />

3 – 5.2 5.5 – 12.0<br />

Interactive 0 0<br />

0 0<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study was to determine<br />

how an interventi<strong>on</strong> to promote social com-<br />

TABLE 3<br />

Mean Percentage of Intervals of Peer Talk During<br />

Pre-Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong> Sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

By Dyad<br />

Pre-Interventi<strong>on</strong> Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Mean Mean<br />

Dyad 1 20.7 38.9<br />

Dyad 2 50.6 75.4<br />

Dyad 3 7.2 24.2<br />

municati<strong>on</strong> skills with children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Spectrum Disorder changed the play skills of<br />

the participants. In order to determine if the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> did affect the play of the participants,<br />

the amount of joint attenti<strong>on</strong>, talk, <strong>and</strong><br />

specific play behaviors were measured. Results<br />

from this study indicated that a social communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> can increase joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

in children with autism. Children increased<br />

the amount of intervals they engaged<br />

in peer talk. Increases in interactive play were<br />

also seen from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

One of the primary characteristics of this<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> is the absence of joint attending to<br />

others. Given the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between joint<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> expressive language development<br />

(Mundy et al., 1990), the need to promote<br />

this skill increases the likelihood that<br />

language development may occur. For the<br />

children in this study there were increases in<br />

joint attending, particularly for Dyads Two<br />

<strong>and</strong> Three. C<strong>on</strong>versely, there were increases<br />

in peer talk for all three dyads. Changes in<br />

play behaviors were also apparent.<br />

Several c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s can be drawn from the<br />

results of this investigati<strong>on</strong>. First, the children<br />

in this study all engaged in joint attending.<br />

Two of the dyads (Dyads Two <strong>and</strong> Three)<br />

increased their levels of joint attending from<br />

baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong> while <strong>on</strong>e remained<br />

stable (Dyad One). Joint attenti<strong>on</strong> has been<br />

defined as the ability of two children to coordinate<br />

social activity <strong>and</strong> engage with another<br />

partner, <strong>and</strong> is often measured via resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

to shifts in eye gaze <strong>and</strong> pointing (Daws<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al., 2002; Mundy et al., 1996). In this study,<br />

joint attenti<strong>on</strong> was coded when both children<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> the same object or activity for at<br />

least 5-s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> fact that all children displayed<br />

joint attending behaviors during baseline <strong>and</strong><br />

increases in joint attending for two of the<br />

dyads occurred is notable, especially given<br />

that both children in the dyad were diagnosed<br />

with autism. J<strong>on</strong>es <strong>and</strong> Carr (2004), in an<br />

extensive review of the literature <strong>on</strong> joint attending,<br />

suggested that changes in joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

will <strong>on</strong>ly be “modestly improved by relatively<br />

simple manipulati<strong>on</strong>s of social <strong>and</strong> play<br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts.” (p. 17). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results of this study do<br />

support previous findings, but with an added<br />

dimensi<strong>on</strong>. While most changes in joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

have been seen in adult directed interacti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the findings of the present study suggest<br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 77


that these changes may also be observed in<br />

child-directed interacti<strong>on</strong>s even when both<br />

children have autism.<br />

Changes in the amount of time children<br />

spent talking to each other were evident in all<br />

three dyads. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se increases occurred across<br />

dyads <strong>and</strong> indicate that all children were more<br />

engaged in social c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

period. While types of language use<br />

<strong>and</strong> skill level am<strong>on</strong>g the different children is<br />

not analyzed in this study, a previous study<br />

(L<strong>on</strong>cola & Craig-Unkefer, 2005) indicated<br />

that not all children c<strong>on</strong>versed at the same<br />

level. Children in Dyads One <strong>and</strong> Three dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

very basic levels of c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong><br />

while children in Dyad Two had more advanced<br />

levels of c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the greatest<br />

diversity in word use. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se differences may<br />

be helpful in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the data related<br />

to play behaviors <strong>and</strong> will be discussed momentarily.<br />

All dyads displayed changes in the types of<br />

play they engaged in from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

though <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of the dyads (Dyad<br />

Two) displayed increases in more advanced<br />

<strong>and</strong> interactive play behaviors. Dyads One <strong>and</strong><br />

Three displayed increases in more isolate play<br />

during the interventi<strong>on</strong>. This informati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

best interpreted in light of the changes in<br />

language for the children as initially discussed<br />

in L<strong>on</strong>cola <strong>and</strong> Craig-Unkefer (2005).<br />

Results from L<strong>on</strong>cola <strong>and</strong> Craig-Unkefer<br />

(2005) indicated that the interventi<strong>on</strong> was successful<br />

in increasing social communicative behaviors<br />

such as commenting in all children.<br />

Further, increases were also seen in language<br />

complexity <strong>and</strong> diversity. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> children in<br />

Dyads One <strong>and</strong> Three initially used the least<br />

amount of language, while the children in<br />

Dyad Two had more variati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> used more<br />

complex language. A comparis<strong>on</strong> of the results<br />

of both analyses revealed that during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the children in Dyads<br />

One <strong>and</strong> Three made gains in language, but<br />

increased the amount of isolated play. That is,<br />

they were talking to each other, but not playing<br />

together. Children in Dyad Two both increased<br />

the amount of language they used <strong>and</strong><br />

were able to engage in more interactive play.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> differences between the dyads may be<br />

explained by their language levels at the <strong>on</strong>set<br />

of the study. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> children in Dyad Two were<br />

already relatively comfortable using language<br />

in social settings, thus they were able to simultaneously<br />

use language <strong>and</strong> engage in play.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> children in Dyads One <strong>and</strong> Three may<br />

have c<strong>on</strong>centrated <strong>on</strong> improving language<br />

<strong>and</strong> were not able to simultaneously use new<br />

language <strong>and</strong> engage in more interactive<br />

forms of play.<br />

Results of a study c<strong>on</strong>ducted by Peirce-<br />

Jordan <strong>and</strong> Lifter (2005) substantiates this<br />

finding indicating that children with autism<br />

may have difficulty engaging in social <strong>and</strong><br />

communicative acti<strong>on</strong>s at the same time because<br />

they compete for the same cognitive<br />

resources. A child who is learning language<br />

may not be able to also utilize higher levels of<br />

play. Likewise, a child engaging in more sophisticated<br />

play may not be able to use more<br />

sophisticated language during that play sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Given these findings, it is underst<strong>and</strong>able<br />

that the children in Dyad Two might<br />

increase the amount of interactive play as they<br />

were learning new language. Play was not a<br />

drain <strong>on</strong> cognitive resources because they already<br />

had some skill in that area. On the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, the children in Dyads One <strong>and</strong> Three<br />

were not able to sustain more advanced levels<br />

of play because there cognitive resources may<br />

have been dedicated to absorbing <strong>and</strong> practicing<br />

new attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> language skills. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se<br />

children reverted to a more comfortable level<br />

of isolate play as they used new language.<br />

Another possible explanati<strong>on</strong> for the increases<br />

in isolate play observed in Dyads One<br />

<strong>and</strong> Three is related to the coding scheme.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> coding method may not have been sensitive<br />

enough to changes given the requirement<br />

during 15-s intervals of 8-s of c<strong>on</strong>tinuous behavior<br />

within that interval to c<strong>on</strong>stitute the<br />

executi<strong>on</strong> of the target behavior. Koegel,<br />

Werner, Vismara, <strong>and</strong> Koegel (2005) used 30<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d intervals for analyzing play interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between children with autism <strong>and</strong> typical<br />

peers. Extending the intervals from 15-s to<br />

30-s may have produced different results. Additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

time may have been needed to allow<br />

for the participants to gain momentum <strong>and</strong><br />

sustain the play behavior.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

This study had several limitati<strong>on</strong>s related to<br />

time c<strong>on</strong>straints, external validity <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of behavior change. Because the in-<br />

78 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


terventi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted near the end of the<br />

academic year, there was not time to c<strong>on</strong>duct<br />

a maintenance or generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe. Further<br />

<strong>on</strong>e interventi<strong>on</strong>ist c<strong>on</strong>ducted all the sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> though fidelity of treatment was<br />

performed, it is possible that there was something<br />

outside of the protocol <strong>and</strong> in the interventi<strong>on</strong>ist’s<br />

repertoire that c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the<br />

results. Finally, for the purposes of coding,<br />

joint attenti<strong>on</strong> was operati<strong>on</strong>ally defined as<br />

two children focused <strong>on</strong> the same object or<br />

activity for at least 5-s. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s in camera<br />

angle precluded the use of eye gaze or direct<br />

eye c<strong>on</strong>tact as a measure of joint attending<br />

which would have allowed for a more comprehensive<br />

look at this area. Additi<strong>on</strong>al detail <strong>on</strong><br />

the limitati<strong>on</strong>s of this study, can be found in<br />

L<strong>on</strong>cola <strong>and</strong> Unkefer (2005).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

This study c<strong>on</strong>tributes to the field in several<br />

unique ways. First, it establishes that, when<br />

paired together, children with autism can engage<br />

in social behaviors with minimal prompting.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, this research indicates that promoti<strong>on</strong><br />

of social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills can also<br />

result in positive changes in other areas such<br />

as joint attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> play complexity. Third,<br />

the authors have identified an effective interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

technique that has multiple opportunities<br />

for children to practice skills, is peer<br />

mediated with intermittent adult support <strong>and</strong><br />

occurs in a naturalistic envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Finally,<br />

this study adheres to the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> for Early<br />

Childhood of the Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children (DEC) recommended practices. Specifically,<br />

by employing single subject methodology,<br />

there was the opportunity to systematically<br />

individualize the interventi<strong>on</strong> for each of<br />

the participants; taking into c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> a<br />

child’s current behavior <strong>and</strong> individualizing<br />

<strong>and</strong> adapting practices to meet the child’s<br />

changing needs (Wolery, 2000).<br />

References<br />

Alvarez, A. & Philips, A. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> importance of<br />

play: A child psychotherapist’s view. Child Psychology<br />

<strong>and</strong> Psychiatry Review, 3, 99–103.<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> (1994). Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed.).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC; Author.<br />

Baker, M., Koegel, R., & Koegel, L. (1998). Increasing<br />

the social behavior of children with autism<br />

using their obsessive behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the<br />

Associati<strong>on</strong> for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 23(4).<br />

300–8.<br />

Bar<strong>on</strong>-Choen, S., Baldwin, D. A, & Crowsen, M.<br />

(1997). Do children with autism use speakers directi<strong>on</strong><br />

of gaze to crack the code of language?<br />

Child Development, 68, 48–57.<br />

Berk, L. (2000). Child Development (5 th ed.). Allyn<br />

<strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>: Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA.<br />

Bodrova, E, & Le<strong>on</strong>g, J. (2003). Building language<br />

<strong>and</strong> literacy through play. Scholastic Early Childhood<br />

Today, 18(2), 34–8, 40–3.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. A., Williams, C., & Kaiser, A. P.<br />

(1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> peer language <strong>and</strong> behavior code: Manual<br />

<strong>and</strong> coding protocol. Unpublished manuscript.<br />

V<strong>and</strong>erbilt University.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. A. (adapted from Parten 1932;<br />

Rubin, 1989) (1998). Peer Play Code. Unpublished<br />

manuscript.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. A., & Kaiser, A. P.(2002). Improving<br />

the social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills of at risk<br />

preschool children in a play c<strong>on</strong>text. Topics in<br />

Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 22(1), 3–13.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. A., & Kaiser, A. P. (2003). Increasing<br />

peer directed social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills of<br />

children enrolled in Head Start. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 25, 229–247.<br />

Daws<strong>on</strong>, G., Muns<strong>on</strong>, J., Estes, A., Osterling, J.,<br />

MacPartl<strong>and</strong>, J., Toth, K. et al. (2002). Neurocognitive<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> joint attenti<strong>on</strong> ability in<br />

young children with autism spectrum disorder<br />

versus developmental delay. Child Development, 73,<br />

345–358.<br />

Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary<br />

Test Third Editi<strong>on</strong>. Circle Pines, NM: American<br />

Guidance Service.<br />

Garfinkle, A. N., & Schwartz, I. S. (2002). Peer<br />

imitati<strong>on</strong>: Increasing social interacti<strong>on</strong>s in children<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities<br />

in inclusive preschool classrooms. Topics<br />

in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 22(1),<br />

26–38.<br />

G<strong>on</strong>cu, A. (1999). Children’s <strong>and</strong> researcher’s engagement<br />

in the world. In A. G<strong>on</strong>cu (Ed.), Children’s<br />

engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives.<br />

New York: Cambridge.<br />

Guralnick, M. J., & Neville, B. (1997). Designing<br />

early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs to promote children’s<br />

social competence. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

effectiveness of early interventi<strong>on</strong>. (pp. 579–610). Baltimore:<br />

Paul H Brookes.<br />

Jahn, E., Eldevik, S., & Eikseth, S. (2000). Teaching<br />

children with autism to initiate <strong>and</strong> sustain cooperative<br />

play. Research in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

21, 151–169.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>es, E. A., & Carr, E. G. (2004). Joint attenti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Social Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Play Behaviors / 79


children with autism: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory <strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental Disabilities,<br />

19, 13–26.<br />

Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single case research designs. New<br />

York: Oxford.<br />

Koegel, R. L., Werner, G. A., Vismara, L. A., &<br />

Koegel, L. K. (2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness of c<strong>on</strong>textually<br />

supported play date interacti<strong>on</strong>s between<br />

children with autism <strong>and</strong> typically developing<br />

peers. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe<br />

Disabilities, 30, 93–102.<br />

Libby, S., Pwell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1998).<br />

Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous play in children with autism: A reappraisal.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,<br />

28, 487–97.<br />

Ladd, G. W., & Mize, J. (1983). A cognitive-social<br />

learning model of social-skill training. Psychological<br />

Review, 90, 127–157.<br />

L<strong>on</strong>cola, J. A., & Craig-Unkefer, L. A., (2005).<br />

Teaching social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills to young<br />

urban children with autism. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 243–263.<br />

Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (1996). Language level <strong>and</strong><br />

n<strong>on</strong>verbal social-communicative behaviors in autistic<br />

<strong>and</strong> language delayed children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the<br />

American Academy of Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry,<br />

35, 1542–1550.<br />

Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (1985). Systematic analysis<br />

of language transcripts. Madis<strong>on</strong>, WI: Language<br />

Analysis Laboratory.<br />

Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

study of joint attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> language<br />

development in autistic children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.<br />

Mundy, P., & Willoughby, J. (1996). N<strong>on</strong>verbal communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

joint attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> early socioemoti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development. In M. Lewis & M.W. Sullivan<br />

(Eds.), Emoti<strong>on</strong>al development in atypical<br />

children (pp. 65–87). Hillsdale: Earlbaum.<br />

Nikopoulos, C., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of<br />

video modeling <strong>on</strong> social initiati<strong>on</strong>s by children<br />

with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,<br />

93–96.<br />

Parten, M. B. (1932). Social participati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

pre-school children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Abnormal <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology, 27, 243–269.<br />

Pierce-Jordan, S., & Lifter, K. (2005). Interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

of social <strong>and</strong> play behaviors in preschoolers with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without pervasive developmental disorders.<br />

Topics in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 25(1),<br />

34–47.<br />

Rogers, S., Cook, I., & Meryl, A. (2005), Interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> play in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. P. A. Klin,<br />

<strong>and</strong> D. Cohen (Eds.) H<strong>and</strong>book of autism <strong>and</strong> pervasive<br />

developmental disorders (3 rd ed.). Hoboken:<br />

Wiley.<br />

Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Performance Scale-Revised. Wood Dale: Stoelting Co.<br />

Rubin, K. (1989). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> play observati<strong>on</strong> scale. Unpublished<br />

manuscript.<br />

Sarokoff, R. A., Taylor, B. A., & Pouls<strong>on</strong>, C. L.<br />

(2001). Teaching children with autism to engage<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al exchanges: script fading with<br />

embedded textual stimuli. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 34, 81–84.<br />

Sawyer, L., Luiselli, J., Ricciardi, J., & Gower, J.<br />

(2005). Teaching a child with autism to share<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g peers in an integrated preschool classroom:<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, maintenance <strong>and</strong> social validati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of Children, 28,<br />

1–10.<br />

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1985).<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales: Classroom Editi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Circle Pines: MN. American Guidance Services.<br />

Terpstra, J., Higgins, K., & Pierce, T. (2002). Can I<br />

play? Classroom based interventi<strong>on</strong>s for teaching<br />

play skills to children with autism. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 17, 119–129.<br />

Williams, K. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle<br />

Pines, MN; American Guidance Service.<br />

Wolery, M. (2000). Recommended practices in<br />

child-focused interventi<strong>on</strong>s. In S. S<strong>and</strong>all, M. E.<br />

McLean, & B. J. Smith (Eds.), DEC Recommended<br />

practices in early interventi<strong>on</strong>/early childhood special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> (pp. 29–37). L<strong>on</strong>gm<strong>on</strong>t, CO: Sopris<br />

West.<br />

Zanoli, K., & Daggett, J. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of reinforcement<br />

rate <strong>on</strong> the sp<strong>on</strong>taneous social initiati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of socially withdrawn preschoolers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 117–125.<br />

Received: 24 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 20 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 19 September 2009<br />

80 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 81–93<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Characteristics <strong>and</strong> Hypothesized Functi<strong>on</strong>s of Challenging<br />

Behavior in a Community-Based Sample<br />

Anna Ingeborg Petursdottir<br />

Texas Christian University<br />

John W. Esch<br />

Esch Behavior C<strong>on</strong>sultants, Inc.<br />

Rachael A. Sautter <strong>and</strong> Kelise K. Stewart<br />

Esch Behavior C<strong>on</strong>sultants, Inc., <strong>and</strong> Western Michigan University<br />

Abstract: An archival study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted to document (a) types of challenging behavior, <strong>and</strong> (b) functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment outcomes, for a sample of pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities who were referred to communitypracticing<br />

behavior analysts for assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of challenging behavior. Functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

reports, prepared by 17 behavior analysts, were obtained <strong>on</strong> 174 individuals who presented with total of 536<br />

challenging behaviors that were assessed separately. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most frequent topographical class of behavior was<br />

physical aggressi<strong>on</strong>, followed by verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong>, n<strong>on</strong>compliance, property destructi<strong>on</strong>, inappropriate verbal<br />

<strong>and</strong> social behavior, <strong>and</strong> self-injury. Functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment yielded a single hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong> for 53.2%<br />

of all behaviors, but multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s for 41.0%. Attenti<strong>on</strong> was the most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

most topographies of behavior. This finding may be attributable to sample characteristics, or to the use of<br />

informant-based functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment methods for a majority of sample cases.<br />

Many people with developmental disabilities<br />

engage in <strong>on</strong>e or more forms of challenging<br />

behavior, which has been defined as “destructive,<br />

harmful, disruptive or otherwise unacceptable<br />

behaviors that occur with sufficient<br />

frequency <strong>and</strong>/or severity to be of major c<strong>on</strong>cern”<br />

(Sigafoos, Arthur, & O’Reilly, 2003,<br />

p. 7). Prevalence estimates have varied widely<br />

(Bruininks, Ols<strong>on</strong>, Lars<strong>on</strong>, & Lakin, 1994).<br />

However, recent data from Engl<strong>and</strong> (Emers<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 2001) <strong>and</strong> Norway (Holden & Gitlesen,<br />

2006) indicate that the overall prevalence of<br />

challenging behavior may be 10–15% am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities who<br />

We thank James E. Carr, Linda LeBlanc, <strong>and</strong><br />

other members of the Esch Behavior C<strong>on</strong>sultants<br />

lab for helpful suggesti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> coding <strong>and</strong> data analysis.<br />

We are also indebted to Catrina Litzenburg,<br />

Er<strong>on</strong>ia Melesse, <strong>and</strong> Stephanie Sheridan for their<br />

invaluable assistance with coding. Rachael A. Sautter<br />

is now affiliated with the Y.A.L.E. School Southeast,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kelise K. Stewart is now affiliated with the<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Help Center in Melville, NY. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be addressed<br />

to Anna Ingeborg Petursdottir, Department of Psychology,<br />

TCU Box 298920, Fort Worth, TX 76129.<br />

E-mail: a.petursdottir@tcu.edu<br />

reside primarily in community settings, as does<br />

the majority of this populati<strong>on</strong> in the United<br />

States (Prouty, Smith, & Lakin, 2006).<br />

Behavioral techniques have l<strong>on</strong>g been applied<br />

successfully to the treatment of challenging<br />

behavior. A behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

is typically designed by a behavior analyst or<br />

other professi<strong>on</strong>al based <strong>on</strong> the outcome of<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> goal of functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment is to identify envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

variables that c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the occurrence of<br />

challenging behavior, such that their influence<br />

may be minimized or removed. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> primary<br />

focus of the assessment is generally <strong>on</strong><br />

immediate antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences of<br />

behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> outcome of the assessment thus<br />

typically includes an identificati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e or<br />

more potential maintaining behavioral c<strong>on</strong>tingencies,<br />

which may be roughly classified as<br />

social-positive reinforcement, social-negative<br />

reinforcement, <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>social c<strong>on</strong>tingencies.<br />

A number of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment techniques<br />

are available. Experimental functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis<br />

(Carr & Dur<strong>and</strong>, 1985; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer,<br />

Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994) involves the<br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong> of antecedents or c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

of a particular target behavior. Descriptive as-<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 81


sessment involves direct observati<strong>on</strong> of naturally<br />

occurring antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

of the target behavior (e.g., Freeman, Anders<strong>on</strong>,<br />

& Scotti, 2000; O’Neill et al., 1997), <strong>and</strong><br />

informant-based methods can take the form<br />

of interviews (e.g., O’Neill et al., 1997) or rating<br />

scales (e.g., Dur<strong>and</strong> & Crimmins, 1988).<br />

Several researchers have published summaries<br />

of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment outcomes across<br />

large numbers of clinical cases (Asmus et al.,<br />

2004; Derby et al., 1992; Iwata, Pace, et al.,<br />

1994; Reese, Richman, Zarc<strong>on</strong>e, & Zarc<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

2003). One purpose of these analyses has<br />

been to provide data <strong>on</strong> the prevalence of<br />

different behavioral functi<strong>on</strong>s. Such data may<br />

provide informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the variables that<br />

most comm<strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the development<br />

of challenging behavior, which in turn<br />

may have important implicati<strong>on</strong>s for preventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> treatment strategies (see Iwata, Pace,<br />

et al.; Reese et al.). In additi<strong>on</strong>, the availability<br />

of large-scale functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment data can<br />

benefit practiti<strong>on</strong>ers who might need to defend<br />

the need for c<strong>on</strong>ducting functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

before prescribing an interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> influence decisi<strong>on</strong>-making regarding the<br />

training of professi<strong>on</strong>als.<br />

Iwata, Pace, et al. (1994) summarized the<br />

outcomes of 152 experimental functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analyses of self-injury. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> participants, more<br />

than half of whom resided in instituti<strong>on</strong>s, had<br />

been referred to the authors for assessment,<br />

which took place either in a pediatric hospital<br />

or in a state residential facility. A single behavioral<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> was identified in 90.1% of<br />

all functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses. Social-negative reinforcement,<br />

such as escape from task dem<strong>and</strong>,<br />

was found to be the most comm<strong>on</strong> maintaining<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence (38.1%), followed by socialpositive<br />

reinforcement (attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> tangibles;<br />

26.3%) <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>social c<strong>on</strong>tingencies (25.7%).<br />

Two other studies (Asmus et al., 2004; Derby<br />

et al., 1992) have presented outcomes of large<br />

numbers of experimental analyses. Participants<br />

in both studies presented with various<br />

topographical classes of challenging behaviors,<br />

including physical aggressi<strong>on</strong>, self-injury,<br />

stereotypy, property destructi<strong>on</strong>, disrupti<strong>on</strong><br />

(defined by Asmus et al. as at least two of<br />

“crying, screaming, stomping feet, flopping to<br />

floor” [p. 287]), <strong>and</strong> other behavior. Derby et<br />

al. summarized data from 79 individuals who<br />

had been evaluated at a university-based out-<br />

patient clinic, whereas Asmus et al. summarized<br />

data from 138 individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities who were admitted to an<br />

inpatient treatment program (the sample additi<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

included individuals without disabilities,<br />

but data from those individuals were<br />

analyzed separately). In both studies, as in<br />

Iwata, Pace, et al., social-negative reinforcement<br />

emerged as the most comm<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of challenging behavior, <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as the least comm<strong>on</strong>. However, a single<br />

behavioral functi<strong>on</strong> was identified in <strong>on</strong>ly 74%<br />

(Derby et al.) <strong>and</strong> 48% (Asmus et al.) of all<br />

cases in these studies.<br />

A major strength of these case summaries is<br />

the use of rigorous experimental procedures<br />

to provide an unambiguous dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

behavioral functi<strong>on</strong>. However, Reese et al.<br />

(2003) obtained similar results using the informant<br />

interview developed by O’Neill et al.<br />

(1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interview was administered to<br />

caregivers of 100 young children diagnosed<br />

with autism, who engaged in disruptive behavior<br />

“defined as yelling, hitting, kicking,<br />

biting, scratching, falling to the floor, <strong>and</strong><br />

self-injury” (p. 88). As with the experimental<br />

analyses, social-negative reinforcement was<br />

the most frequently hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>;<br />

however, a single functi<strong>on</strong> was identified for<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly 37% of the children. N<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were not reported. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors additi<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

identified specific types of social-negative <strong>and</strong><br />

social-positive reinforcement that were comm<strong>on</strong><br />

am<strong>on</strong>g this sample, <strong>and</strong> a follow-up study<br />

verified that these functi<strong>on</strong>s were more likely<br />

to be observed am<strong>on</strong>g children with autism<br />

than am<strong>on</strong>g matched c<strong>on</strong>trols (Reese, Richman,<br />

Belm<strong>on</strong>t, & Morse, 2005).<br />

Hanley, Iwata, <strong>and</strong> McCord (2003) summarized<br />

the results of all published experimental<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses identified in an extensive<br />

review of the literature to date. A large majority<br />

were analyses of self-injury <strong>and</strong> aggressi<strong>on</strong><br />

(presumably physical). In spite of potential<br />

bias resulting from the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of published<br />

data <strong>on</strong>ly, their analysis of functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis<br />

outcomes yielded results that were c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with the other studies (Asmus et al., 2004;<br />

Derby et al., 1992; Iwata, Pace, et al., 1994;<br />

Reese et al., 2003). That is, a single reinforcement<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> was identified in 85.4% of all<br />

cases, <strong>and</strong> social-negative reinforcement was<br />

overall the most frequently identified func-<br />

82 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


ti<strong>on</strong>, although this was not the case for all<br />

topographies of behavior.<br />

Overall, these studies str<strong>on</strong>gly suggest that<br />

challenging behavior tends to be maintained<br />

by identifiable reinforcement c<strong>on</strong>tingencies,<br />

<strong>and</strong> that the maintaining c<strong>on</strong>tingency cannot<br />

be predicted <strong>on</strong> the type of challenging behavior<br />

(e.g., self-injury, aggressi<strong>on</strong>), underscoring<br />

the need for individualized functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y also suggest that socially mediated<br />

reinforcement is implicated in the majority<br />

of cases of challenging behavior of pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities, <strong>and</strong> that<br />

this populati<strong>on</strong> may be particularly susceptible<br />

to developing challenging behavior maintained<br />

by social-negative reinforcement c<strong>on</strong>tingencies<br />

(e.g., escape from dem<strong>and</strong>). It is<br />

important to note, however, that the samples<br />

employed in those studies may not necessarily<br />

reflect the broader populati<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

developmental disabilities who require treatment<br />

for challenging behavior. For example,<br />

93.4% of participants in the Iwata, Pace, et al.<br />

(1994) study, <strong>and</strong> 52% of participants in the<br />

Derby et al. (1992) study were functi<strong>on</strong>ing in<br />

the severe to profound range of mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

By c<strong>on</strong>trast, pers<strong>on</strong>s with mild mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stitute by far the largest group<br />

of all pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities<br />

(American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2000). Although<br />

epidemiological studies have found a<br />

positive correlati<strong>on</strong> between degree of impairment<br />

<strong>and</strong> the presence of challenging behavior,<br />

challenging behavior has been found to<br />

be quite prevalent am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

in the mild to moderate range of mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., Emers<strong>on</strong> et al., 2001; Holden<br />

& Gitlesen, 2006).<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, epidemiological studies have<br />

identified forms of challenging behavior that<br />

may have been underrepresented in the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment literature. For example,<br />

Emers<strong>on</strong> et al. (2001) reported generalized<br />

n<strong>on</strong>compliance, repetitive pestering, running<br />

away, stealing, <strong>and</strong> inappropriate sexual behavior<br />

as frequently occurring types of challenging<br />

behavior. Those forms of behavior<br />

have rarely been reported in the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis literature (Hanley et al., 2003) <strong>and</strong><br />

did not appear in any of the aforementi<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

case summaries (Asmus et al., 2004; Derby et<br />

al., 1992; Iwata, Pace et al., 1994; Reese et al.,<br />

2003). It is possible that pers<strong>on</strong>s exhibiting<br />

those behaviors are less likely to be referred<br />

for functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

treatment than pers<strong>on</strong>s exhibiting behaviors<br />

that may be perceived as more physically<br />

harmful. However, it may be noted that each<br />

study described cases seen at universityaffiliated<br />

clinics or inpatient programs, in<br />

which evaluati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> treatment were overseen<br />

by Ph.D-level behavior analysts. By c<strong>on</strong>trast,<br />

many professi<strong>on</strong>als who provide functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment practice in<br />

community settings. It is c<strong>on</strong>ceivable that the<br />

characteristics <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s of challenging<br />

behavior encountered in those settings differ<br />

from those encountered in university-based<br />

settings, as the latter may offer a greater degree<br />

of expertise in h<strong>and</strong>ling treatmentresilient<br />

cases or cases of extreme behavior<br />

severity. If such differences were found, they<br />

might underscore a need for extending research<br />

<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>based<br />

treatment to additi<strong>on</strong>al types of challenging<br />

behavior.<br />

In the present paper, we report the results<br />

of an archival study of 174 cases that were<br />

referred to community-practicing behavior<br />

analysts for functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment<br />

of challenging behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of<br />

the analysis was twofold. First, it was intended<br />

to document the topographical classes of challenging<br />

behavior that occasi<strong>on</strong> referral for<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment by professi<strong>on</strong>als practicing<br />

in the community. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d purpose<br />

was to document the hypothesized behavioral<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s of challenging behavior observed,<br />

<strong>and</strong> compare this informati<strong>on</strong> with results of<br />

previous studies involving pers<strong>on</strong>s referred to<br />

university-affiliated programs.<br />

Method<br />

Database<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> database c<strong>on</strong>sisted of all functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

reports that were submitted to a local<br />

ethics review committee in a southeastern<br />

state over a 54-m<strong>on</strong>th period (from 1/28/2001<br />

to 7/27/2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> committee was charged<br />

with reviewing behavior plans for all pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities who received<br />

behavior analytic services to address potentially<br />

harmful behavior, <strong>and</strong> resided in the<br />

area served by a particular district office. All<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 83


ehavior plans submitted to the committee<br />

were accompanied or preceded by a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment report. A total of 200 functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment reports were submitted during<br />

the review period, by 17 behavior analysts<br />

who gave permissi<strong>on</strong> to use the reports for<br />

research purposes. Twelve were Master’s level<br />

Board Certified Behavior Analysts (BCBAs);<br />

two were Bachelor’s level Board Certified Associate<br />

Behavior Analysts working under the<br />

supervisi<strong>on</strong> of BCBAs, <strong>and</strong> three had other<br />

professi<strong>on</strong>al certificati<strong>on</strong> or licensure.<br />

Three reports were unavailable for analysis<br />

due to electr<strong>on</strong>ic storage failure, <strong>and</strong> four<br />

reports were excluded from analysis because<br />

no challenging behavior was identified during<br />

initial c<strong>on</strong>tact with c<strong>on</strong>sumer or caregiver(s).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> remaining 193 reports described functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment outcomes for 174 individual<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers of services. Two assessment reports<br />

from different time periods were available for<br />

18 c<strong>on</strong>sumers, <strong>and</strong> three reports were available<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>sumer. When two or more<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment reports were available<br />

for the same c<strong>on</strong>sumer, <strong>on</strong>ly the first report<br />

was included in the data analysis; thus, a total<br />

of 174 reports were included. Of this total,<br />

158 reports (90.8%) were prepared by the 12<br />

BCBAs.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> reports were written in narrative format,<br />

<strong>and</strong> sometimes included numerical data<br />

<strong>and</strong> graphs, but structure <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent varied<br />

across behavior analysts <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers. All<br />

reports c<strong>on</strong>tained some basic demographic informati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>sumers. In most cases,<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> was provided about the c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

living arrangements <strong>and</strong> daily activities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> most reports included a list of current<br />

medicati<strong>on</strong>s, including vitamins <strong>and</strong> supplements.<br />

Many reports included diagnostic informati<strong>on</strong><br />

that had been obtained by the behavior<br />

analyst via interview or record review,<br />

but the level of detail with which this informati<strong>on</strong><br />

was reported varied.<br />

All reports c<strong>on</strong>tained a detailed descripti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the presenting behavior, a descripti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the methods employed to assess the functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of each behavior, <strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> assessment<br />

outcome, including statements about<br />

the hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong> of each topography<br />

of challenging behavior.<br />

Procedure <strong>and</strong> Coding System<br />

From each report, data were collected <strong>on</strong> demographic<br />

<strong>and</strong> other sample descriptors,<br />

current medicati<strong>on</strong>s, type <strong>and</strong> severity of the<br />

presenting behavior, type of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted, <strong>and</strong> assessment outcome.<br />

All identifying informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

<strong>and</strong> service providers was removed from the<br />

reports before they were given to raters for<br />

coding.<br />

Sample informati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> age <strong>and</strong> sex of<br />

each c<strong>on</strong>sumer were recorded, <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s<br />

living situati<strong>on</strong> was coded as family<br />

home; group home; supported living; independent<br />

living; or other. Other demographic<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>, such as race or marital status, was<br />

rarely included in the reports. For each c<strong>on</strong>sumer,<br />

it was noted whether he or she was<br />

reported to attend any of the following: school<br />

or preschool; adult training program; supported<br />

employment; or employment without<br />

support.<br />

Diagnostic informati<strong>on</strong> was recorded, <strong>and</strong> it<br />

was noted whether any of the following were<br />

listed: (a) <strong>Autism</strong> or other pervasive developmental<br />

disorders, (b) <strong>on</strong>e or more DSM-IV<br />

Axis I diagnosis (American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2000), (c) seizures or seizure disorders,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (d) sensory impairment. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

level of mental retardati<strong>on</strong> was rarely specified<br />

<strong>and</strong> was therefore not coded.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ communicati<strong>on</strong> abilities<br />

were reported inc<strong>on</strong>sistently, <strong>and</strong> results of<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ardized assessment of intellectual or language<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing were rarely included in the<br />

reports. Based <strong>on</strong> the limited informati<strong>on</strong><br />

available, each c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s mode of communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

was classified as (a) vocal language<br />

(if the report stated or clearly implied that the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumer primarily communicated in vocal<br />

language), (b) alternative or augmentative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

systems (based <strong>on</strong> reported use of<br />

e.g., PECS; sign language), or (c) unclear.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> names of all medicati<strong>on</strong>s were recorded<br />

<strong>and</strong> classified with the aid of the<br />

MedlinePlus <strong>on</strong>line database as an antipsychotic,<br />

antidepressant, antic<strong>on</strong>vulsant, anxiolytic,<br />

mood stabilizer, stimulant, or other. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

number of medicati<strong>on</strong>s in each category was<br />

then tallied for each c<strong>on</strong>sumer.<br />

Presenting behavior. Each report listed <strong>on</strong>e<br />

or more topographical classes of behavior that<br />

84 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 1<br />

Categories of Challenging Behavior <strong>and</strong> Coding Definiti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Behavior Definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Elopement Behavior labeled elopement; other label/descripti<strong>on</strong> implies that the<br />

behavior involves leaving an area without permissi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Inappropriate sexual behavior Behavior labeled inappropriate sexual behavior; behavior labeled or<br />

described as public masturbati<strong>on</strong>; other label/descripti<strong>on</strong> implies<br />

behavior that may be interpreted as sexually motivated (e.g.,<br />

touching others in private areas).<br />

Inappropriate social or verbal<br />

behavior<br />

were assessed separately. For each c<strong>on</strong>sumer,<br />

the number of behaviors assessed was noted. If<br />

more than five separate behaviors were assessed<br />

(which was the case for 10 c<strong>on</strong>sumers), <strong>on</strong>ly the<br />

first five behaviors listed were analyzed. Each<br />

behavior was coded into <strong>on</strong>e of 13 topographical<br />

categories, based <strong>on</strong> the behavior analyst’s<br />

label <strong>and</strong> descripti<strong>on</strong> of the behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavior<br />

categories <strong>and</strong> coding definiti<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

listed in Table 1. Behaviors that did not fall into<br />

any of the categories were coded as other, <strong>and</strong><br />

the behavior analyst’s label written down.<br />

Most reports provided some informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

Behavior labeled inappropriate social or verbal behavior; behavior<br />

described as c<strong>on</strong>sisting of bizarre or inappropriate but n<strong>on</strong>threatening<br />

verbal statements; behavior described as c<strong>on</strong>sisting of<br />

inappropriate but n<strong>on</strong>-aggressive behavior towards others (e.g.,<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ing too close to others).<br />

N<strong>on</strong>compliance Behavior labeled n<strong>on</strong>compliance; behavior labeled or described as<br />

refusal or failure to follow instructi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Physical aggressi<strong>on</strong> Behavior labeled physical aggressi<strong>on</strong>; behavior labeled aggressi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> descripti<strong>on</strong> makes clear that aggressi<strong>on</strong> is physical; other<br />

label/descripti<strong>on</strong> that clearly implies potential for physical injury<br />

to others (for example, hitting others).<br />

Pica Behavior labeled pica; behavior labeled or described as c<strong>on</strong>suming<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-nutritive items.<br />

Property destructi<strong>on</strong> Behavior labeled property destructi<strong>on</strong>, or other label/descripti<strong>on</strong><br />

clearly implies potential for damage to physical envir<strong>on</strong>ment (for<br />

example, throwing objects).<br />

Screaming or disruptive<br />

vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Behavior labeled screaming; behavior described as disruptive or<br />

inappropriate vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s, but no verbal c<strong>on</strong>tent of vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

reported.<br />

Self-injury Behavior labeled self-injury, or other label/descripti<strong>on</strong> clearly<br />

implies potential for physical injury to the c<strong>on</strong>sumer (for example,<br />

head-banging or biting self).<br />

Stealing Behavior labeled stealing; behavior described as taking the<br />

bel<strong>on</strong>gings of others without permissi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Suicide threats or attempts Behavior labeled or described as attempts to commit suicide; threats<br />

to commit suicide or suicidal talk.<br />

Tantrums Behavior labeled tantrum; coded if different resp<strong>on</strong>se topographies<br />

comprising tantrum behavior were not assessed separately.<br />

Verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong> Behavior labeled verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong>; behavior labeled or described as<br />

cursing; behavior labeled or described as threatening others.<br />

the frequency of each class of challenging<br />

behavior; however, the method by which frequency<br />

data were obtained (caregiver report,<br />

existing incident reports, direct observati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

varied across reports <strong>and</strong> across behaviors<br />

within reports. For each c<strong>on</strong>sumer, it was<br />

noted whether at least <strong>on</strong>e topography of<br />

challenging behavior was reported to occur at<br />

least <strong>on</strong>ce daily. Although more detailed frequency<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> was available in some<br />

cases, the format in which it was reported was<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>sistent, preventing more fine-grained<br />

coding.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 85


TABLE 2<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Comp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>and</strong> Coding Definiti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Assessment Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Definiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Interview Report c<strong>on</strong>tains informati<strong>on</strong> about behavior, antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

obtained via verbal communicati<strong>on</strong> with the c<strong>on</strong>sumer, caregivers, or<br />

other informants.<br />

Rating Scales Report c<strong>on</strong>tains data from <strong>on</strong>e or more functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment informant<br />

rating scales.<br />

Structured descriptive<br />

(ABC) assessment<br />

Unstructured descriptive<br />

(ABC) assessment<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it was noted whether any of<br />

the following adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences of challenging<br />

behavior were reported to have occurred in<br />

the past: (a) tissue damage, including bruising,<br />

redness, <strong>and</strong> callusing; (b) physician care or<br />

hospitalizati<strong>on</strong>, (c) involuntary admissi<strong>on</strong> to a<br />

psychiatric facility, or (d) arrest.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> assessment<br />

methodology employed by the behavior<br />

analyst was coded by noting whether<br />

the report c<strong>on</strong>tained data obtained with the<br />

use of the assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents listed in<br />

Table 2. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents were<br />

coded separately for each behavior if multiple<br />

behaviors of the same c<strong>on</strong>sumer were assessed.<br />

If the behavior analyst had attempted<br />

to use an assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ent that involved<br />

direct observati<strong>on</strong>, but less than two instances<br />

of the behavior were actually observed, the use<br />

of that comp<strong>on</strong>ent was not scored.<br />

Assessment outcome. For each behavior, the<br />

reported hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>(s) were coded<br />

as <strong>on</strong>e or more of the following: (a) attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

(b) socially mediated access to tangible rein-<br />

Report c<strong>on</strong>tains data obtained via direct observati<strong>on</strong>, during which the<br />

observer (the behavior analyst or another pers<strong>on</strong>) recorded behavior,<br />

antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences by checking pre-defined opti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> a data<br />

sheet immediately when the behavior occurred.<br />

Report c<strong>on</strong>tains data obtained via direct observati<strong>on</strong>, during which the<br />

observer (the behavior analyst or another pers<strong>on</strong>) recorded a narrative<br />

descripti<strong>on</strong> of behavior, antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences immediately when<br />

the behavior occurred.<br />

Informal direct observati<strong>on</strong> Report c<strong>on</strong>tains some informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the behavior obtained via direct<br />

observati<strong>on</strong> by the behavior analyst, but the data are not presented as the<br />

outcome of either a structured or an unstructured descriptive assessment.<br />

Scatter plot <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> report c<strong>on</strong>tains a scatter plot that displays the occurrence of the<br />

behavior as a functi<strong>on</strong> of the time of the day across multiple days or<br />

weeks.<br />

Experimental analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> report c<strong>on</strong>tains data from an experimental analysis that includes, at<br />

minimum, a test for <strong>on</strong>e behavioral functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>trol c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Unclear <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> report c<strong>on</strong>tains data <strong>on</strong> the antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences of behavior,<br />

but it is unclear how these data were obtained.<br />

forcers, (c) socially mediated escape, <strong>and</strong><br />

(d) n<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>. It should be noted<br />

that the n<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong> category sometimes<br />

included tangible reinforcers (e.g., in<br />

the case of stealing). No attempt was made to<br />

separate n<strong>on</strong>-socially mediated access to tangible<br />

reinforcers from hypothesized sensory<br />

or other n<strong>on</strong>-tangible c<strong>on</strong>sequences due to<br />

inc<strong>on</strong>sistencies in the reporting of these functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., access to tangibles as a result of<br />

theft was often described in terms of the sensory<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences that interacti<strong>on</strong> with the<br />

item was hypothesized to produce).<br />

Interrater agreement. A sec<strong>on</strong>d rater independently<br />

coded 18.7% of all reports. Pointby-point<br />

agreement was assessed by comparing<br />

the two raters’ coding sheets for the same<br />

report, variable by variable. For variables that<br />

permitted the rater to code <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e category<br />

(sex; living situati<strong>on</strong>; mode of communicati<strong>on</strong>;<br />

behavior frequency), or required the<br />

rater to type in informati<strong>on</strong> (age), an agreement<br />

was scored if both observers coded the<br />

variable the same way, or typed in the same<br />

86 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


informati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> a disagreement was scored<br />

if their records differed. For variables that<br />

permitted the rater to code multiple categories<br />

for each c<strong>on</strong>sumer (diagnoses; daily activities;<br />

medicati<strong>on</strong>s; categories of presenting<br />

behavior; adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences) or each behavior<br />

(functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents;<br />

assessment outcome), an occurrence agreement<br />

was scored each time both raters checked<br />

the same category, <strong>and</strong> a disagreement was<br />

scored if <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e observer checked a particular<br />

category, whereas categories that were<br />

left unchecked by both observers were not<br />

included the agreement analysis. Percentage<br />

agreement was calculated by summing the<br />

number of agreements across all reports, dividing<br />

the result by the number of agreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> disagreements, <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>verting this ratio<br />

to a percentage. Agreement percentages were<br />

as follows: 100.0% for age <strong>and</strong> sex; 92.9% for<br />

diagnosis; 88.9% for mode of communicati<strong>on</strong>;<br />

90.3% for daily activities; 93.8% for medicati<strong>on</strong>s;<br />

85.6% for categories of presenting behavior;<br />

86.1% for behavior frequency; 89.5%<br />

for adverse c<strong>on</strong>sequences; 89.5% for functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents; <strong>and</strong> 85.3% for<br />

assessment outcome.<br />

Results<br />

Sample Descripti<strong>on</strong><br />

Informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> age (M 27.0 years; range: 4–<br />

53 years) <strong>and</strong> sex was available for all c<strong>on</strong>sumers,<br />

<strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> living arrangement<br />

at the time of first assessment was available<br />

for 169 c<strong>on</strong>sumers. This informati<strong>on</strong> is summarized<br />

in Table 3. At the time of their first<br />

assessment, 16.1% of the c<strong>on</strong>sumers were reported<br />

to have received diagnoses of autism or<br />

other pervasive developmental disorders, <strong>and</strong><br />

26.7% were reported to have Axis I diagnoses<br />

according to the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric<br />

Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2000). Seizures were reportedly<br />

experienced by 18.9% of the c<strong>on</strong>sumers,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 5.7% were reported to have hearing<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or visi<strong>on</strong> impairments. It should be noted<br />

that diagnostic informati<strong>on</strong> may have been<br />

underreported, as the behavior analysts who<br />

wrote the report may not always have had<br />

access to accurate informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It was reported or could clearly be inferred<br />

from the reports that 71.8% of the c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

TABLE 3<br />

Sample Characteristics<br />

communicated primarily in vocal language. It<br />

was reported that 8.6% used alternative or<br />

augmentative communicati<strong>on</strong> systems (e.g.,<br />

sign language; PECS), but in the remaining<br />

instances, mode of communicati<strong>on</strong> was not<br />

clear.<br />

Of children under 18, 91.3% were reported<br />

to attend school or preschool programs. Of<br />

adults 18 <strong>and</strong> older, 61.7% were reported to<br />

regularly attend adult training programs,<br />

8.7% were reported to attend school, <strong>and</strong><br />

12.5% were reported to be employed full- or<br />

part-time, with or without support.<br />

At the time of assessment, 127 c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

(73.0%) were reported to be using at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

form of psychotropic medicati<strong>on</strong>. One or more<br />

antipsychotic medicati<strong>on</strong>s were used by 54.0%<br />

of the sample; 37.9% used antic<strong>on</strong>vulsants;<br />

32.2% antidepressants, 14.4% anxiolytic medicati<strong>on</strong>s;<br />

4.6% stimulant medicati<strong>on</strong>s; <strong>and</strong> .6%<br />

mood stabilizers.<br />

Topography of Presenting Behavior<br />

Frequency %<br />

Age in Years (N 174)<br />

10 or younger 18 10.3<br />

11–20 45 25.9<br />

21–30 41 23.6<br />

31–40 37 21.3<br />

41–50 27 15.5<br />

51 or older 6 3.4<br />

children under 18 46 26.4<br />

adults 128 73.6<br />

Sex (N 174)<br />

female 70 40.2<br />

male 104 59.8<br />

Living Arrangement (N 169)<br />

family home 78 46.2<br />

group home 66 39.1<br />

supported living 11 6.5<br />

independent living 3 1.8<br />

other 11 6.5<br />

For each c<strong>on</strong>sumer, the number of behaviors<br />

assessed ranged from <strong>on</strong>e to eight (M 3.17,<br />

SD 1.49; mode 2). For the 174 c<strong>on</strong>sumers,<br />

a total of 536 behaviors were identified for<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> in the present analysis (a total of 549<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 87


TABLE 4<br />

Frequency of Behavior in Each Category <strong>and</strong><br />

Proporti<strong>on</strong> of Sample Presenting with Each<br />

Frequency<br />

% of Sample<br />

(N 174)<br />

Physical aggressi<strong>on</strong> 88 50.6<br />

Verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong> 64 36.8<br />

N<strong>on</strong>compliance 59 33.9<br />

Property destructi<strong>on</strong> 58 33.3<br />

Inappropriate social or<br />

verbal behavior 44 22.4 a<br />

Self-injury 42 24.1<br />

Elopement 33 19.0<br />

Screaming or disruptive<br />

vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s 28 16.1<br />

Inappropriate sexual<br />

behavior 19 10.9<br />

Stealing 15 8.6<br />

Tantrums 9 5.2<br />

Suicide threats or attempts 5 2.9<br />

Pica 4 2.3<br />

Other behavior 68 28.2 b<br />

a Percentage based <strong>on</strong> 39 c<strong>on</strong>sumers who presented<br />

with behavior in this category (three c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

presented with more than <strong>on</strong>e for a total of<br />

44 behaviors).<br />

b Percentage based <strong>on</strong> 49 c<strong>on</strong>sumers who presented<br />

with behavior in this category (twelve c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

presented with more than <strong>on</strong>e for a total of<br />

68 behaviors).<br />

behaviors were assessed, but in the 10 cases<br />

in which six or more behaviors were assessed<br />

for the same c<strong>on</strong>sumer, <strong>on</strong>ly the first five<br />

listed were included in the analysis). Table 4<br />

shows the frequency of each behavior category,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the percentage of c<strong>on</strong>sumers who<br />

exhibited behavior in each category. Physical<br />

aggressi<strong>on</strong> was by far the most prevalent topographical<br />

class, assessed in more than half of<br />

the reports. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d most frequent class<br />

was verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong>, followed by n<strong>on</strong>compliance,<br />

property destructi<strong>on</strong>, inappropriate verbal<br />

or social behavior, <strong>and</strong> self-injury. A large<br />

number of behaviors fell into the “other” category.<br />

Descripti<strong>on</strong>s of those behaviors varied<br />

widely <strong>and</strong> included, am<strong>on</strong>g others, feedingrelated<br />

problems such as excessive c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

of food items or fluids, vomiting, inappropriate<br />

toileting (e.g., in public places),<br />

inappropriate use of items, disrobing, <strong>and</strong><br />

stereotypy (<strong>on</strong>e case).<br />

Frequency <strong>and</strong> Adverse C<strong>on</strong>sequences of<br />

Challenging Behavior<br />

In the case of 71 c<strong>on</strong>sumers (41.6%), the<br />

assessment report described at least <strong>on</strong>e adverse<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence of the c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s challenging<br />

behavior that had occurred in the<br />

past. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most frequently reported c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

was tissue damage, which was reported<br />

in the case of 62 c<strong>on</strong>sumers (34.8%). Am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers who displayed self-injury, tissue<br />

damage was reported as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence in<br />

78.6% of all cases, <strong>and</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>sumers who<br />

displayed physical aggressi<strong>on</strong>, it was reported<br />

in 51% of all cases. Other c<strong>on</strong>sequences were<br />

reported less frequently, but overall, nine c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

(5.1%) had required physician care or<br />

hospitalizati<strong>on</strong> as a c<strong>on</strong>sequence of their behavior,<br />

nine c<strong>on</strong>sumers (5.1%) had been involuntarily<br />

committed to a psychiatric facility,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 13 c<strong>on</strong>sumers (7.3%) had been arrested.<br />

Informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the frequency of challenging<br />

behavior was available for 167 c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

(96.0%), <strong>and</strong> 109 of those (65.3%) were reported<br />

to engage in at least <strong>on</strong>e form of challenging<br />

behavior at least daily.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Methods <strong>and</strong> Outcomes<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> analysis of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

revealed that informant-based assessment<br />

was in many cases the <strong>on</strong>ly assessment<br />

strategy used to arrive at a hypothesis of behavioral<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>. Specifically, the functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of 396 behaviors (73.9%) was assessed either<br />

via interview al<strong>on</strong>e (71.5%), or with the use<br />

of informant rating scales in additi<strong>on</strong> to interviews<br />

(2.4%), but without the use of descriptive<br />

or experimental methods. In the remaining<br />

140 cases (26.1%), the strategies<br />

employed in additi<strong>on</strong> to informant-based<br />

methods included informal direct observati<strong>on</strong><br />

(14.0%), unstructured descriptive assessment<br />

(7.3%), structured descriptive assessment<br />

(.2%), scatter plot (.2%), experimental analysis<br />

(.2%), <strong>and</strong> unclear methods (5.4%).<br />

Table 5 shows data <strong>on</strong> the outcome of functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment. No hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong><br />

was reported for 5.8% of all behaviors. A single<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> was hypothesized for 53.2% of<br />

the behaviors, most often an attenti<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

followed by escape, n<strong>on</strong>social, <strong>and</strong> finally<br />

tangible functi<strong>on</strong>s. Multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

88 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 5<br />

Summary of Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes<br />

Frequency<br />

(N 536) %<br />

Single functi<strong>on</strong> hypothesized 285 53.2<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong> 144 26.9<br />

Escape 62 11.6<br />

Tangible 28 5.2<br />

N<strong>on</strong>social 51 9.5<br />

Multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s hypothesized 220 41.0 a<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> escape 70 13.1<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> tangible 49 9.1<br />

Escape <strong>and</strong> tangible 22 4.1<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong>, tangible, <strong>and</strong><br />

escape 54 10.1<br />

Automatic <strong>and</strong> social 25 4.7<br />

No functi<strong>on</strong> hypothesized 31 5.8<br />

Total<br />

Attenti<strong>on</strong> 337 62.9<br />

Escape 218 40.7<br />

Tangible 160 29.9<br />

Automatic 76 14.2<br />

a<br />

Differs from sum of sub-categories due to<br />

rounding.<br />

hypothesized for 41.0% of all behaviors. In a<br />

large majority of those cases, attenti<strong>on</strong> was<br />

<strong>on</strong>e of the hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>s, whereas a<br />

n<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong> was rarely hypothesized in<br />

c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with other functi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

As shown in the bottom four rows of Table<br />

5, attenti<strong>on</strong> was overall the most frequently<br />

observed functi<strong>on</strong>, hypothesized to be a maintaining<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence of 62.9% of all behaviors,<br />

either al<strong>on</strong>e (26.9%) or in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong><br />

with other c<strong>on</strong>sequences (36.0%). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

<strong>and</strong> third most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s were escape <strong>and</strong> tangible functi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

respectively. Both of these functi<strong>on</strong>s were reported<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with other functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

much more often than they were hypothesized<br />

to be the sole maintaining variables.<br />

N<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>s were hypothesized to be a<br />

maintaining variable in the case of <strong>on</strong>ly 11.6%<br />

of all behaviors; however, this functi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

reported as the sole maintaining c<strong>on</strong>tingency<br />

more often than it was reported as <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Figure 1 shows the overall percentage distributi<strong>on</strong><br />

of hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>s of the six<br />

most frequent categories of challenging be-<br />

havior. Attenti<strong>on</strong> was the most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> for all categories except<br />

n<strong>on</strong>compliance. In the case of inappropriate<br />

verbal <strong>and</strong> social behavior, attenti<strong>on</strong> was most<br />

often hypothesized as the sole maintaining<br />

variable, whereas for other classes of behavior,<br />

it was typically hypothesized to be <strong>on</strong>e of multiple<br />

maintaining variables. Socially mediated<br />

escape was the most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>compliance, <strong>and</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

or third most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of other behaviors. Tangible reinforcers<br />

were hypothesized to be the sec<strong>on</strong>d most comm<strong>on</strong><br />

maintaining c<strong>on</strong>sequence of self-injury,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the third most comm<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> of other<br />

behaviors. N<strong>on</strong>social reinforcement was hypothesized<br />

to maintain self-injury in a third of<br />

all cases (tied with escape), <strong>and</strong> was hypothesized<br />

to be the sole functi<strong>on</strong> of the behavior in<br />

14.3% of all self-injury cases. N<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were rarely hypothesized for the other<br />

behaviors shown in Figure 1.<br />

As for the less frequently observed behavior<br />

categories, attenti<strong>on</strong> was the most frequently<br />

hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong> of suicide threats <strong>and</strong><br />

attempts (100%), screaming <strong>and</strong> disruptive<br />

vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s (82.1%), inappropriate sexual<br />

behavior (73.7%), elopement (63.6%), <strong>and</strong><br />

other behavior (39.7%). For tantrums, escape<br />

was the most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong><br />

(77.8%) <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>social functi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

most comm<strong>on</strong>ly hypothesized for pica<br />

(100.0%) <strong>and</strong> stealing (93.3%).<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

In the literature <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis, aggressi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> self-injury have emerged as the most<br />

comm<strong>on</strong>ly investigated topographical classes<br />

of challenging behavior (Hanley et al., 2003).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> present results indicate that physical aggressi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> self-injury indeed comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

occasi<strong>on</strong> referral to behavior analysts in community<br />

settings. However, other comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

observed topographical classes include verbal<br />

aggressi<strong>on</strong>, n<strong>on</strong>compliance 1 , inappropriate<br />

verbal <strong>and</strong> social behavior, <strong>and</strong> inappropriate<br />

sexual behavior; all of which have received less<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> in the functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis literature.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is probably no reas<strong>on</strong> to expect that<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>-based treatment<br />

strategies do not apply to those behaviors<br />

as well as they apply to others; however,<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 89


Figure 1. Hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>s of the six most frequent categories of challenging behavior. Each bar shows<br />

the percentage of behaviors for which a particular functi<strong>on</strong> was hypothesized. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> black porti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

each bar shows cases in which the functi<strong>on</strong> was hypothesized as the sole maintaining c<strong>on</strong>tingency; the<br />

light gray porti<strong>on</strong> shows cases in which it was hypothesized as <strong>on</strong>e of multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al research <strong>on</strong> these types of behavior<br />

seems warranted. In additi<strong>on</strong>, it is possible<br />

that the presence of some of these topographies<br />

is correlated with client characteristics<br />

that warrant special c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>. For example,<br />

it is not unlikely that behaviors such as<br />

verbal aggressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> inappropriate verbal behavior<br />

are more prevalent am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

who have a higher level of functi<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong><br />

greater verbal ability than the majority of<br />

those in previous case summaries (Asmus et<br />

al., 2004; Derby et al., 1992; Iwata, Pace, et al.,<br />

1994; Reese et al., 2003). Unfortunately, informati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the level of mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, or<br />

other informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> language or intellectual<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing, was limited in the present database,<br />

which prevents a firm c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that<br />

members of the sample were, <strong>on</strong> the average,<br />

higher-functi<strong>on</strong>ing than those in previous<br />

studies. Anecdotally, however, a substantial<br />

number of reports either explicitly described<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>sumers as high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing, included<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> their daily activities that implied<br />

that they functi<strong>on</strong>ed relatively independently,<br />

or described relatively sophisticated<br />

verbal behavior that was observed during the<br />

assessment.<br />

Regardless of the topography of challenging<br />

behavior, functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment results<br />

typically suggested individual differences in<br />

behavioral functi<strong>on</strong>, as would be expected<br />

based <strong>on</strong> previous research (Asmus et al.,<br />

2004; Derby et al., 1992; Hanley et al., 2003;<br />

Iwata, Pace, et al., 1994; Reese et al., 2003)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis literature in general.<br />

Although there appeared to be an asso-<br />

90 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


ciati<strong>on</strong> between topography <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> (see Figure 1), the hypothesized<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s clearly could not be predicted solely<br />

<strong>on</strong> the basis of topography. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> data <strong>on</strong> verbal<br />

aggressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> inappropriate verbal <strong>and</strong> social<br />

behavior seem to suggest that, as in the<br />

case of more comm<strong>on</strong>ly studied topographies,<br />

challenging verbal behavior may be a functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of different types of socially mediated reinforcement<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tingencies (although perhaps<br />

rarely of n<strong>on</strong>social c<strong>on</strong>tingencies). This c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

must, however, be tempered by the<br />

fact that in a large majority of all cases, functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment was completed almost exclusively<br />

via informant-based methods, mostly interviews<br />

without the reported use of rating<br />

scales. Evidence exists that at least under some<br />

circumstances, structured or semi-structured<br />

interviews with informants may be valid predictors<br />

of behavioral functi<strong>on</strong> (Arndorfer &<br />

Miltenberger, 1994; Cunningham & O’Neill,<br />

2000; Borgmeier & Horner, 2006; Yarborough<br />

& Carr, 2000). However, little is known about<br />

the extent to which the same is true of unstructured<br />

interviews. Although our coding<br />

system did not distinguish between unstructured<br />

interviews <strong>and</strong> structured interview formats<br />

(e.g., the Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Interview<br />

by O’Neill et al., 1997), we did not<br />

frequently see menti<strong>on</strong> of the latter.<br />

Two notable differences exist between the<br />

overall outcomes of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment in<br />

the present study <strong>and</strong> in previous reviews<br />

(Asmus et al., 2004; Derby et al., 1992; Hanley<br />

et al., 2003; Iwata, Pace, et al., 1994; Reese et<br />

al., 2005). First, multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s were hypothesized<br />

in a substantially larger proporti<strong>on</strong><br />

of cases than by Derby et al., <strong>and</strong> Iwata,<br />

Pace, et al., <strong>and</strong> overall in the published functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis literature (Hanley et al.). Sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> was overall the most frequently<br />

hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>, while previous studies<br />

have been in agreement that social-negative<br />

reinforcement is the most prevalent maintaining<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tingency for a variety of behavior topographies.<br />

When multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

hypothesized, <strong>on</strong>e of these functi<strong>on</strong>s was usually<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> thus it appears that the<br />

additi<strong>on</strong> of a hypothesized attenti<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong><br />

accounted for a large proporti<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

multiple-functi<strong>on</strong> outcomes.<br />

It is possible that some characteristics of the<br />

sample were resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the large number<br />

of cases in which an attenti<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

hypothesized. For example, it is c<strong>on</strong>ceivable<br />

that pers<strong>on</strong>s with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong> are<br />

more susceptible to various forms of social<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> as a reinforcer than are pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

more severe disability, <strong>and</strong> that the present<br />

database included a large proporti<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the former category. A possible alternative<br />

explanati<strong>on</strong> is that the functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

methods employed c<strong>on</strong>tributed to<br />

this finding. In the natural envir<strong>on</strong>ment, it is<br />

likely that challenging behavior is often followed<br />

by some form of attenti<strong>on</strong>, such as reprim<strong>and</strong>s<br />

or statements of c<strong>on</strong>cern. In the absence<br />

of experimental analysis, it may be<br />

difficult to rule out potential reinforcing effects<br />

of such attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> in fact, evidence<br />

exists that descriptive functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

methods may tend to overidentify attenti<strong>on</strong> as<br />

a maintaining c<strong>on</strong>tingency (Hall, 2005; Lerman<br />

& Iwata, 1993; Thomps<strong>on</strong> & Iwata, 2007).<br />

Similar findings have not been obtained in<br />

studies that have compared the outcome of<br />

informant-based methods with experimental<br />

analysis (Arndorfer & Miltenberger, 1994;<br />

Borgmeier & Horner, 2006; Cunningham &<br />

O’Neill, 2000; Hall, 2005; Paclawskyj, Mats<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Rush, Smalls, & Vollmer, 2001; Yarborough &<br />

Carr, 2000), <strong>and</strong> in additi<strong>on</strong>, Reese et al.<br />

(2003) did not identify attenti<strong>on</strong> as a very<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong> using the O’Neill et al.<br />

(1997) interview. Nevertheless, the possibility<br />

that unstructured interviews are more susceptible<br />

to overidentifying attenti<strong>on</strong> cannot be<br />

dismissed.<br />

It is unknown why functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

methods other than interviews were rarely employed,<br />

but c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> time <strong>and</strong> labor resources<br />

are a likely reas<strong>on</strong>. An interview can<br />

be completed in a relatively short amount of<br />

time <strong>and</strong> does not require additi<strong>on</strong>al staff besides<br />

the pers<strong>on</strong> who completes the interview.<br />

A thorough descriptive assessment, by c<strong>on</strong>trast,<br />

requires l<strong>on</strong>g periods of observati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> typically requires training <strong>and</strong> cooperati<strong>on</strong><br />

of caregivers or others who are in a positi<strong>on</strong><br />

to collect data in the c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s natural<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Evidence exists that brief experimental<br />

analyses may be completed in as little<br />

as 90 minutes (Derby et al., 1992); however,<br />

experimental analyses require highly trained<br />

staff, <strong>and</strong> may therefore be difficult to complete<br />

by a privately practicing professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 91


who may not have access to trained staff to<br />

assist with assessment.<br />

It is also possible that experimental analysis<br />

was sometimes excluded as an opti<strong>on</strong> because<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s challenging behavior did not<br />

occur at sufficiently high rates. Although a<br />

majority of the sample was reported to exhibit<br />

at least <strong>on</strong>e topography of challenging behavior<br />

at least <strong>on</strong>ce a day, in some of those<br />

cases it still may not have occurred at sufficient<br />

rates to c<strong>on</strong>sider experimental analysis.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses of low-rate behavior have<br />

been reported in the literature (Kahng, Apt,<br />

& Schoenbachler, 2001; Tarbox, Wallace, Tarbox,<br />

L<strong>and</strong>aburu, & Williams, 2004); however,<br />

the methods employed in those cases either<br />

required c<strong>on</strong>tinuous or st<strong>and</strong>-by availability<br />

of trained staff throughout the day, <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

appear impractical for privately practicing<br />

behavior analysts. Low-rate behavior,<br />

however, cannot be the sole reas<strong>on</strong> why experimental<br />

analyses were not c<strong>on</strong>ducted more<br />

often, because numerous reports described<br />

behavior as occurring as often as multiple<br />

times per hour, <strong>and</strong> yet no experimental analysis<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

In spite of potential limitati<strong>on</strong>s of the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessments methodology by which the<br />

present data were generated, we believe that<br />

the data c<strong>on</strong>tribute several pieces of informati<strong>on</strong><br />

to the literature <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

of challenging behavior am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

developmental disabilities. First, they suggest<br />

types of challenging behavior that have received<br />

scarce attenti<strong>on</strong> in the experimental<br />

literature often occasi<strong>on</strong> referral for functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment in the community.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the data suggest the possibility<br />

that am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s who are referred to<br />

community-based services, attenti<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

may be more prevalent than social-negative<br />

reinforcement functi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

methods employed prevent a firm<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that this was the case, but future<br />

research might explore this possibility further,<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with its implicati<strong>on</strong>s for preventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

treatment strategies. Finally, the prevalent use<br />

of informant-based assessment methods appear<br />

to highlight a need for developing <strong>and</strong><br />

effectively disseminating validated functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment methods that can be implemented<br />

with relative ease by practiti<strong>on</strong>ers in the community<br />

whose resources are limited.<br />

References<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> (2000). Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed.,<br />

text revisi<strong>on</strong>). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

Arndorfer, R. E., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1994).<br />

Home-based descriptive <strong>and</strong> experimental analysis<br />

of problem behaviors in children. School Psychology<br />

Quarterly, 14, 64–87.<br />

Asmus, J. M., Ringdahl, J. E., Sellers, J. A., Call,<br />

N. A., Andelman, M. S., & Wacker, D. P. (2004).<br />

Use of a short-term inpatient model to evaluate<br />

aberrant behavior: Outcome data summaries<br />

from 1996 to 2001. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 37, 283–304.<br />

Borgmeier, C., & Horner, R. H. (2006). An evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the predictive validity of c<strong>on</strong>fidence ratings<br />

in identifying functi<strong>on</strong>al behavioral assessment<br />

hypothesis statements. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive<br />

Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 8, 100–105.<br />

Bruininks, R. H., Ols<strong>on</strong>, K. M., Lars<strong>on</strong>, S. A., &<br />

Lakin, K. C. (1994). Challenging behaviors<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in residential<br />

settings. In T. T. Thomps<strong>on</strong> & D. B. Gray<br />

(Eds.), Destructive Behavior in Developmental Disabilities<br />

(pp. 24–48). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Call, N. A., Wacker, D. P., Ringdahl, J. E., Cooper-<br />

Brown, L. J., & Boelter, E. W. (2004). An assessment<br />

of antecedent events influencing n<strong>on</strong>compliance<br />

in an outpatient clinic. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 37, 145–157.<br />

Carr, E. G., & Dur<strong>and</strong>, V. M. (1985). Reducing<br />

behavior problems through functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

training. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis,<br />

18, 111–126.<br />

Cunningham, E., & O’Neill, R. E. (2000). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of results of functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment <strong>and</strong> analysis<br />

methods with young children with autism.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disabilities, 35, 406–414.<br />

Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M.,<br />

Northup, J., Cigr<strong>and</strong>, K., et al. (1992). Brief functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment techniques to evaluate aberrant<br />

behavior in an outpatient setting: A summary of<br />

79 cases. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25,<br />

713–721.<br />

Dur<strong>and</strong>, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1998). Identifying<br />

the variables maintaining self-injurious behavior.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,<br />

17, 99–117.<br />

Emers<strong>on</strong>, E., Kiernan, C., Alborz, A, Reeves, D.,<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>, H., Swarbrick, R., et al. (2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> prevalence<br />

of challenging behaviors: A total populati<strong>on</strong><br />

study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22,<br />

77–93.<br />

Freeman, K. A., Anders<strong>on</strong>, C. M., & Scotti, J. R.<br />

(2000). A structured descriptive methodology: Increasing<br />

agreement between descriptive <strong>and</strong> ex-<br />

92 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


perimental analyses. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

31, 55–66.<br />

Hall, S. S. (2005). Comparing descriptive, experimental,<br />

<strong>and</strong> informant-based assessments of problem<br />

behaviors. Research in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

26, 514–526.<br />

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. (2003).<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis of problem behavior: A review.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–<br />

185.<br />

Holden, B., & Gitlesen, J. P. (2006). A total populati<strong>on</strong><br />

study of challenging behaviour in the<br />

county of Hedmark, Norway: Prevalence, <strong>and</strong> risk<br />

markers. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27,<br />

456–465.<br />

Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K.,<br />

& Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis of self-injury. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from Analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> in Developmental Disabilities, 2,<br />

3–20, 1982).<br />

Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarc<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

J. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> functi<strong>on</strong>s of self-injurious behavior: An<br />

experimental-epidemiological analysis. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240.<br />

Kahng, S., Apt, K. A., & Sch<strong>on</strong>bachler, H. E. (2001).<br />

Assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of low-rate highintensity<br />

problem behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 34, 225–228.<br />

Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive<br />

<strong>and</strong> experimental analyses of variables maintaining<br />

self-injurious behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 26, 293–319.<br />

Mace, F. C., Hock, M. L., Lalli, J. S., West, B. J.,<br />

Belfiore, P., Pinter, E., et al. (1988). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 123–141.<br />

O’Neill, R. E., Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Sprague,<br />

J. R., Storey, K., & Newt<strong>on</strong>, J. S. (1997). Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> program development for problem behavior:<br />

A practical h<strong>and</strong>book. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks<br />

Cole.<br />

Paclawskyj, T. R., Mats<strong>on</strong>, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls,<br />

Y., & Vollmer, T. R. (2001). Assessment of the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>vergent validity of Questi<strong>on</strong>s of Behavioral<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>s Scale with analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Motivati<strong>on</strong> Assessment Scale. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 484–494.<br />

Prouty, R. W., Smith, G., & Lakin, K. C. (Eds.)<br />

(2006). Residential services for pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities: Status <strong>and</strong> trends through 2005.<br />

Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota, Research<br />

<strong>and</strong> Training Center <strong>on</strong> Community Living<br />

<strong>and</strong> Institute <strong>on</strong> Community Integrati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Reese, R. M., Richman, D. M., Belm<strong>on</strong>t, J. M., &<br />

Morse, P. (2005). Functi<strong>on</strong>al characteristics of<br />

disruptive behavior in developmentally disabled<br />

children with <strong>and</strong> without autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 35, 419–428.<br />

Reese, R. M., Richman, D. M., Zarc<strong>on</strong>e, J., & Zarc<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

T. (2003). Individualizing functi<strong>on</strong>al assessments<br />

for children with autism: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

of perseverative behavior <strong>and</strong> sensory disturbances.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 18, 87–92.<br />

Sigafoos, J., Arthur, M., & O’Reilly, M. (2003). Challenging<br />

behavior & developmental disability. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Whurr.<br />

Tarbox, J., Wallace, M. D., Tarbox, R. S. F., L<strong>and</strong>aburu,<br />

H. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis <strong>and</strong> treatment of low rate problem behavior<br />

in individuals with developmental disabilities.<br />

Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 19, 187–204.<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, R. H., & Iwata, B. A. (2007). A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of outcomes from descriptive <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analyses of problem behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 40, 333–338.<br />

Wilder, D. A., Harris, C., Reagan, R., & Rasey, A.<br />

(2007). Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>and</strong> treatment of n<strong>on</strong>compliance<br />

by preschool children. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 40, 173–177.<br />

Yarbrough, S. C., & Carr, E. G. (2000). Some relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

between informant assessment <strong>and</strong><br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis of problem behavior. American<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 105, 130–151.<br />

Received: 19 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 20 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 20 March 2009<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Assessment Outcomes / 93


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 94–106<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

A Collaborative Approach to Implement Positive Behavior<br />

Support Plans for Children with Problem Behaviors:<br />

A Comparis<strong>on</strong> of C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> versus C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Feedback Approach<br />

Dilek Erbas<br />

Erciyes University<br />

Abstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study is to compare the effectiveness of c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus<br />

feedback <strong>on</strong> the proper use of positive behavior support strategies (PBS) <strong>on</strong> behaviors of three mothers with<br />

children with developmental disabilities. Results indicated that c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback was more effective<br />

than c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the mothers’ behavior. Also, results showed that when mothers implemented<br />

PBS procedures correctly, children’s problem behaviors decreased <strong>and</strong> remained at low rate.<br />

Students with disabilities frequently display<br />

problem behaviors (Carr, Taylor, & Robins<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1991; Koegel & Koegel, 1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> presence<br />

of problem behaviors in school settings is often<br />

associated with negative outcomes for students<br />

(e.g., segregati<strong>on</strong>, punitive discipline,<br />

poor academic outcomes) (e.g., McLeskey,<br />

Henry, & Hodges, 1999; Sprague & Walker,<br />

2000; Sugai et al., 2000). Problem behaviors<br />

can cause teacher stress <strong>and</strong> have adverse effects<br />

<strong>on</strong> peers as well (Emmer & Strought,<br />

2001; Sterlin-Turner, Robins<strong>on</strong>, & Wilczynski,<br />

2001).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> negative impact of problem behaviors<br />

Funding for this study was provided by the Scientific<br />

<strong>and</strong> Technical Research Council of Turkey<br />

(Grant number: SBB-104K061). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> opini<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

other c<strong>on</strong>tent c<strong>on</strong>tained in this material do not<br />

necessarily reflect the opini<strong>on</strong> of the agency <strong>and</strong> no<br />

official endorsement should be inferred. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> author<br />

would like to thank the parents that participated<br />

in this study. Also, I would like to express<br />

appreciati<strong>on</strong> to Hatice Sahin for her role in collecting<br />

data for interobserver agreement. Dr. W. James<br />

Halle, my professor, is ultimately the <strong>on</strong>e who deserve<br />

the most credit because he encouraged <strong>and</strong><br />

supported me to work <strong>on</strong> this topic in my graduate<br />

studies. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article<br />

should be addressed to Dilek Erbas, Erciyes University,<br />

College of Educati<strong>on</strong>, Department of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, Kayseri, TURKEY, 38080. E–mail:<br />

derbas@erciyes.edu.tr<br />

<strong>on</strong> the parents of children with developmental<br />

disabilities has also been reported (Fox,<br />

Vaughn, Wyatte, & Dunlap, 2002). Dem<strong>and</strong>s<br />

for caring a child with problems has been<br />

reported as a significant source of stress for<br />

the parents (Dunlap & Fox, 1999; Turnbull &<br />

Ruef, 1996). Furthermore, problem behaviors<br />

can cause disrupti<strong>on</strong>s in family routines <strong>and</strong><br />

activities (Fox, Vaughn, Dunalp, & Bucy,<br />

1997). Given the negative c<strong>on</strong>sequences of<br />

problem behaviors, problem behaviors should<br />

be prevented <strong>and</strong> reduced.<br />

Positive behavioral support program (PBS)<br />

has been found as being <strong>on</strong>e of the most<br />

effective methods to reduce problem behaviors.<br />

Positive behavior support (PBS) is a “collaborative<br />

assessment based <strong>on</strong> an approach<br />

to developing effective individulized interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for people with problem behavior”<br />

(Lucyshyn, Horner, Dunlap, Albin, & Ben,<br />

2002, p. 7). Principles of applied behavior<br />

analysis greatly c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the development<br />

of PBS. PBS evolved in the mid-1980s as<br />

an alternative to punitive approaches for the<br />

individuals with severe disabilities who exhibit<br />

severe problem behaviors (Hieneman, Dunlap,<br />

& Kincaid, 2005). It originally was defined<br />

as a n<strong>on</strong>- aversive approach to replace the use<br />

of painful <strong>and</strong> stigmatizing c<strong>on</strong>sequent approaches<br />

(Hieneman et al.). PBS emphasizes<br />

the value of multi-comp<strong>on</strong>ent interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that include teaching new skills, modifying the<br />

94 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


envir<strong>on</strong>ment, <strong>and</strong> providing c<strong>on</strong>sequences for<br />

positive <strong>and</strong> negative behaviors (Carr & Carls<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1993). Over the past two decades, the use<br />

of PBS approach has exp<strong>and</strong>ed from being an<br />

individual student focused interventi<strong>on</strong>, to<br />

<strong>on</strong>e that meets the needs of whole classrooms<br />

<strong>and</strong> entire schools (Sugai & Horner, 2002).<br />

Furthermore its use has been adapted by<br />

many disciplines including mental health,<br />

school psychology, <strong>and</strong> general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., Clark & Hieneman, 1999; Cr<strong>on</strong>e & Horner,<br />

2003; Scott & Eber, 2003). Although the<br />

existing literature <strong>on</strong> PBS has c<strong>on</strong>tributed<br />

greatly to reducing the problem behaviors of<br />

individuals with developmental delays, the majority<br />

of studies examining effectiveness of<br />

PBS have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted in clinical settings<br />

by researchers (Carr et al., 1999; Nels<strong>on</strong>, Roberts,<br />

Mathur, & Rutherford, 1999; Reid & Nels<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2002). To increase ecological validity of<br />

PBS, it is critical to implement PBS in natural<br />

settings by using those individuals who are<br />

indigenous to these envir<strong>on</strong>ments (e.g., teachers<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents) (Carr et al.; Reid & Nels<strong>on</strong>).<br />

Parent involvement is critical in planning<br />

<strong>and</strong> implementing PBS strategies for children<br />

with problem behaviors (Vaughn, Dunlap,<br />

Fox, Clarke, & Bucy, 1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> rati<strong>on</strong>ale for<br />

including parents in PBS is that parents typically<br />

spend more time with their children than<br />

any others <strong>and</strong> have more informati<strong>on</strong> about<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>texts in which problem behaviors occur<br />

(Dunlap & Fox 1999). Also, PBS is both<br />

child <strong>and</strong> family centered (Lucyshyn et al.,<br />

2002). Goals in PBS are defined in partnership<br />

with families <strong>and</strong> reflect their preferences<br />

based <strong>on</strong> their time <strong>and</strong> resources that<br />

are available for them (Lucyshyn & Albin,<br />

1993). Thus, effective <strong>and</strong> efficient parent<br />

training methods are needed to train parents<br />

to manage their children’s problem behaviors.<br />

A variety of training methods, including verbal<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>, modeling, rehearsal, <strong>and</strong> feedback<br />

have been shown to be effective in training<br />

parents as interventi<strong>on</strong>ist to implement<br />

behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>s successfully (Dunlap<br />

& Fox, 1999; Dunlap, Newt<strong>on</strong>, Fox, Benito, &<br />

Vaughn, 2001; Fox, Dunlap, & Philbrick,<br />

1997; Lucyshyn et al., 2002, Vaugh et al., 1997;<br />

Symes, Remingt<strong>on</strong>, Brown, Hasting, 2006).<br />

However, few studies have compared the effectiveness<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g different comp<strong>on</strong>ents of a<br />

training package (Feldman et al., 1992) <strong>and</strong><br />

many of interventi<strong>on</strong> studies did not use parents<br />

as interventi<strong>on</strong>ists (Iwata et al., 2000;<br />

Moore et al., 2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, the extent to<br />

which the training techniques will produce<br />

same results with parents is not clear.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of the current study was to<br />

replicate <strong>and</strong> extend this literature by examining<br />

the effectiveness of two interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

programs (C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting vs. c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

meeting plus feedback) <strong>on</strong> the parents’<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedures of PBS. Also, outcomes<br />

related to children’s problem behaviors are<br />

presented in both training package in this<br />

study.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Prior to the study, an approval from Ministry<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> an informed parental c<strong>on</strong>sent<br />

were received. Three parent-children<br />

dyad participated in the study. Participants<br />

were selected based <strong>on</strong> three criteria: (a) having<br />

a child with a developmental disability<br />

exhibiting problem behavior (s), (b) no prior<br />

experience <strong>and</strong> training in PBS, <strong>and</strong> (c) willingness<br />

to participate in this study. All of the<br />

children were toilet trained, <strong>and</strong> could perform<br />

self-help skills independently (e.g.,<br />

grooming <strong>and</strong> dressing).<br />

Participant 1. Davut is a 4-year 6 m<strong>on</strong>th<br />

old boy diagnosed with developmental disabilities.<br />

Davut is reportedly taking Risperdal, but<br />

his mother is unable to provide specific informati<strong>on</strong><br />

about the dosage prescribed. He receives<br />

20 hr per week of <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e behavior<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> in his school. He is n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

although he can repeat a few sounds following<br />

an adult model. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> child answers questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with yes/no resp<strong>on</strong>ses; maintains eye c<strong>on</strong>tact<br />

in social interacti<strong>on</strong>s. His parents have BA in<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> work as primary teachers in a<br />

public school. Davut’s mother is c<strong>on</strong>cerned<br />

about his aggressive behavior. His aggressive<br />

behaviors include hitting, kicking <strong>and</strong> spiting<br />

at other children. She also reported that these<br />

behaviors result in peer exclusi<strong>on</strong> during play.<br />

He engages in challenging behaviors when<br />

asked to participate in activities in playground.<br />

He frequently requests playing with<br />

Parent Training / 95


s<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> independently engages for short<br />

periods of time, but so<strong>on</strong> begins to spill s<strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> children playing in the playground. Davut’s<br />

mother was c<strong>on</strong>cerned that these behaviors<br />

kept him excluded from peers.<br />

Participant 2. Alp was 5 years 2 m<strong>on</strong>ths at<br />

the beginning of the study <strong>and</strong> lived at home<br />

with his mother, father <strong>and</strong> older <strong>and</strong> younger<br />

brothers. He was diagnosed with developmental<br />

disabilities. He communicates verbally<br />

through <strong>on</strong>e to two-word phrases <strong>and</strong> gestures.<br />

Alp receives part time special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

services in a resource room for functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills instructi<strong>on</strong>. Alp’s father works as a medical<br />

doctor in a state hospital. Alp’s mother<br />

has Ph.D. in Medicine <strong>and</strong> works as an assistant<br />

professor in College of Medicine. Alp’s<br />

parents were c<strong>on</strong>cerned about Alp’s challenging<br />

behaviors during play time with his peers.<br />

When he plays with his peers, he hits, kicks, or<br />

pushes them. He also forcefully takes play materials<br />

from his peers. When given n<strong>on</strong> preferred<br />

tasks or activities, he refuses to complete<br />

the activity.<br />

Participant 3. Yakup is a 5-year, 4-m<strong>on</strong>thold<br />

boy who lives with his mother, father, <strong>and</strong><br />

older brother. Yakup’s mother has a high<br />

school degree <strong>and</strong> does not have a job. His<br />

father has Bachelor’s degree <strong>and</strong> works as a<br />

faculty secretary in the university. Yakup does<br />

not attend preschool program but he receives<br />

8 hr of <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e interventi<strong>on</strong> in special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

school weekly. Yakup primarily communicates<br />

his needs verbally using short<br />

phrases <strong>and</strong> completes short sentences. Yakup’s<br />

mother is c<strong>on</strong>cerned about Yakup’s problem<br />

behaviors during shopping at stores.<br />

Yakup always wants to buy multiple items requested<br />

without paying the cashier. When he<br />

is asked to put items back or buy <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e item<br />

instead of multiple <strong>on</strong>es, he screams, hits <strong>and</strong><br />

refuses to follow directi<strong>on</strong>s. His parents also<br />

reported that they avoid doing shopping because<br />

of his problem behaviors.<br />

Setting<br />

This study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the settings in<br />

which children show problem behaviors most<br />

frequently. For Alp <strong>and</strong> Davut, the setting was<br />

a neighborhood playground. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were typically<br />

four to six typically developing children<br />

<strong>and</strong> their parents in the playground. For<br />

Yakup, the setting was the shopping center in<br />

which Yakup’s parents regularly shop. Other<br />

customers <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>sible people present in<br />

the envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Shopping center had lots of<br />

checkout lanes <strong>and</strong> wide aisles.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> author served as primary interventi<strong>on</strong>ist<br />

throughout the study, c<strong>on</strong>ducting or coordinating<br />

research activities <strong>and</strong> collaborating<br />

with the family training <strong>and</strong> support activities.<br />

She has a doctoral degree in special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> extensive training <strong>and</strong> experience in the<br />

theory <strong>and</strong> practice of applied behavior analysis.<br />

Training sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred in the park<br />

<strong>and</strong> community <strong>on</strong>e to three times per week<br />

(M 1.3 times per week) <strong>and</strong> lasted 15 to 25<br />

min.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

An adapted within-subject alternating treatments<br />

design was used to examine which<br />

training package resulted in high accuracy of<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedures by mothers of children<br />

with developmental disabilities. Also, to evaluate<br />

whether mother applicati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures resulted in<br />

progress by target children’s problem behaviors,<br />

the following analyses were c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

First, level of children’s problem behaviors<br />

was compared between c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e) <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus<br />

feedback).<br />

An adapted within-subject alternating treatments<br />

design for instructi<strong>on</strong>al research was<br />

used to dem<strong>on</strong>strate experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

(O’Reilly et al., 1992). Each mother was received<br />

<strong>on</strong>e training package <strong>on</strong> antecedent<br />

procedure (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e or c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

plus feedback), <strong>and</strong> the other training<br />

package that she was not exposed to (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e or c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback) <strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences procedure. In other words, Davut’s<br />

mother received c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e) <strong>on</strong> antecedent procedure <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback) <strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures, whereas Alp’s<br />

mother received c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e) <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences procedure <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

2 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback) <strong>on</strong> antecedent<br />

procedures. Yakup’s mother received<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e) <strong>on</strong><br />

antecedent procedure <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 (c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

96 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 1<br />

Alternating Treatments Design Pattern Across C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback) <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures<br />

(see Tables 1 <strong>and</strong> 2). Training procedures<br />

(c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting vs. c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

meeting plus performance feedback) were<br />

counterbalanced across c<strong>on</strong>sequences <strong>and</strong> antecedent<br />

procedures across mothers. Once a<br />

differential effect between two training package<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 <strong>and</strong> 2) occurred for a<br />

mother in the alternating treatment phase,<br />

the more effective of the two training package<br />

was implemented for both antecedent <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures.<br />

Prior to Alternating Treatment Phase<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> Al<strong>on</strong>e)<br />

Prior to the start of the alternating treatment<br />

phase, mothers were given theoretical <strong>and</strong><br />

practical informati<strong>on</strong> about PBS by using the<br />

Individualized Intensive Interventi<strong>on</strong>s: Determining<br />

the Meaning of Challenging Behavior<br />

Module <strong>and</strong> Individualized Intensive Interventi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

Developing a Behavior Support Plan<br />

Module (http://www.v<strong>and</strong>erbilt.edu/csefel/<br />

modules.html) in a two day workshop as a<br />

group. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se modules were translated into<br />

Turkish by two special educati<strong>on</strong> faculty who<br />

are fluent in both English <strong>and</strong> Turkish. In the<br />

workshop, the case studied was used to pro-<br />

vide experience of applying their knowledge<br />

of PBS strategies to an actual case study child<br />

in this phase.<br />

Maintenance Phase<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus Feedback)<br />

Davut’s mother antecedent procedures c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures<br />

Alp’s mother c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures antecedent procedures<br />

Yakup’s mother antecedent procedures c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures<br />

TABLE 2<br />

Independent Variable (Parent Training) C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> Pattern Across Sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s were employed 2, 4, <strong>and</strong><br />

6 m<strong>on</strong>ths to examine the effect of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

after terminating sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Following each<br />

maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>, feedback was provided<br />

for correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of target procedures.<br />

Feedback was delivered immediately after<br />

each maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Target Behaviors for Mothers-Child Dyads <strong>and</strong><br />

reliability<br />

Dependent variables: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following two target<br />

behaviors (dependent variable) were recorded<br />

in this study: Percentage of steps implemented<br />

as written by mothers in both<br />

antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures <strong>and</strong><br />

percentage of child problem behaviors.<br />

To collect data <strong>on</strong> mothers’ behaviors a<br />

data collecti<strong>on</strong> sheet was adapted from the<br />

<strong>on</strong>e used in Codding, Feinberg, Dunn, & Pace<br />

(2005). Data sheet included (a) the type of<br />

procedure (i.e. antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence)<br />

Sessi<strong>on</strong>s 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19<br />

Davut’s mother C1 C2 C1 C1 C2 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2<br />

Alp’s mother c2 c1 c2 c1 c2 c1 c2 c2 c1 c1 c2 c2 c1 c2 c1 c2 c1 c2 c1<br />

Yakup’s mother C1 C2 C1 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C1 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C2 C’ C2<br />

Note. C1 c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> with antecedent procedures, C2 c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback with c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedures, c1 c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> with c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures, c2 c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback antecedent<br />

procedures<br />

Parent Training / 97


that was prescribed for the target child, (b) an<br />

operati<strong>on</strong>al definiti<strong>on</strong> of each comp<strong>on</strong>ent of<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> (c) observer ratings the<br />

mothers implementati<strong>on</strong> of each comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

using of the following levels, <strong>and</strong> (d) space for<br />

the observer to record comments or examples<br />

(Codding et al.). One of three levels of implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

was scored under the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

rating secti<strong>on</strong> of the data sheet (a) implemented<br />

as written (i.e., the entire comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

was implemented every time the target behavior<br />

occurred or when the situati<strong>on</strong> (b) not<br />

implemented as written, (i.e., sometimes implemented<br />

the entire comp<strong>on</strong>ent as written,<br />

implemented the entire comp<strong>on</strong>ent as written<br />

or did not employ the comp<strong>on</strong>ent as written),<br />

(c) no opportunity to observe (i.e., the target<br />

behavior did not occur, or the antecedent was<br />

not present) (Codding et al.). Percentage of<br />

correct implementati<strong>on</strong> was calculated by dividing<br />

the number of mother behaviors implemented<br />

correctly by the total number of items<br />

in the data sheet.<br />

Data <strong>on</strong> child target behaviors were recorded<br />

using a 15-s partial interval recording<br />

system <strong>and</strong> were reported as percentage of<br />

intervals in which problem behaviors occurred.<br />

A stopwatch was used to code behaviors<br />

from the videotapes. Problem behaviors<br />

were disruptive or destructive behaviors,<br />

which included throwing objects, <strong>and</strong> kicking<br />

or hitting peers.<br />

Reliability data for dependent variables<br />

(correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures for parent’s behaviors,<br />

<strong>and</strong> children’s target behaviors) were collected<br />

during 20% of all experimental sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se sessi<strong>on</strong>s were selected r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

<strong>and</strong> recorded by a sec<strong>on</strong>d observer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

observer was a PhD c<strong>and</strong>idate in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> was naïve to the purpose of the<br />

study. Assigned sessi<strong>on</strong>s were observed independently<br />

by both researcher <strong>and</strong> the observer;<br />

then, agreement between researcher <strong>and</strong><br />

the observers was calculated. Dependent variable<br />

reliability was calculated by using the<br />

point-by-point method with a formula of the<br />

number of agreements divided by the number<br />

of agreements plus disagreements multiplied<br />

by 100. Interobserver agreement scores for<br />

mothers behaviors were 94% for c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1,<br />

ranging from 70% to %100, <strong>and</strong> 98 % for<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2, ranging from 88% to 100%. In-<br />

terobserver agreement scores for children’s<br />

problem behaviors were 92% for c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1,<br />

ranging from 78% to 100%, <strong>and</strong> 96% for c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

2, ranging from 86% to 100%.<br />

Independent Variables<br />

Two types of training procedures were implemented:<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

meeting plus feedback.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting al<strong>on</strong>e. Four 15 to 30<br />

minute c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meetings were provided<br />

individually to mothers. In the first c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

meeting, definiti<strong>on</strong>s of problem behaviors<br />

of his children were discussed by using<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment interview form. For the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting, mothers were<br />

asked to observe their children in home using<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Home Observati<strong>on</strong> Card (http://csefel.uiuc.<br />

edu/modules/module3a/h<strong>and</strong>out3.pdf) until<br />

the third c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting. In the third<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting, mothers were expected<br />

to generate a problem behavior statement. At<br />

minimum, acceptable behavior statements<br />

had to be c<strong>on</strong>tained in <strong>on</strong>e sentence <strong>and</strong> included<br />

at least two different comp<strong>on</strong>ents in<br />

the correct antecedent, behavior, c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

sequence. Based <strong>on</strong> these criteria,<br />

each Home Observati<strong>on</strong> Card was carefully<br />

scanned <strong>and</strong> examined. Target problem behaviors<br />

were videotaped while parents were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducting direct observati<strong>on</strong> in their home.<br />

Before fourth c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meting, detailed<br />

written descripti<strong>on</strong>s of the tapes were prepared<br />

at each viewing to indicate the target<br />

student’s behaviors, antecedents <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

of problem behaviors. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se descripti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were later compared <strong>and</strong> rechecked until<br />

agreement between researcher developed<br />

<strong>and</strong> mother-developed hypotheses were examined.<br />

After agreement between these hypotheses<br />

was settled, the fourth c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

At the fourth c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting, mothers<br />

were asked to identify potential c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

<strong>and</strong> antecedent procedures for problem<br />

behavior of their children based <strong>on</strong><br />

informati<strong>on</strong> they learned for pre-interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

workshop. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>n, the individualized behavior<br />

support plans were created with mother-researcher<br />

collaborati<strong>on</strong> (see Table 3, 4, 5, <strong>and</strong><br />

6). Mothers were expected to employ these<br />

behaviors based <strong>on</strong> the support plan under<br />

98 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 3<br />

Yakup’s Positive Behavior Support Plan<br />

Antecedent Strategies C<strong>on</strong>sequence Strategies<br />

Choice<br />

1. Given choice of 2 preferred items<br />

during shopping.<br />

2. Never finish a shopping without<br />

getting preferred item.<br />

Script story for shopping (see Table 5)<br />

1. Before going to shopping, review<br />

the scripts story with child.<br />

2. Make eye c<strong>on</strong>tact with child.<br />

3. Read a story with showing pictures<br />

to the child.<br />

Using cue card<br />

1. Gain child’s attenti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., look at<br />

me <strong>and</strong> I am going to show a<br />

picture).<br />

2. Show him the picture cue card<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with simple verbal cue (e.g.,<br />

walk through aisles, choice <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e<br />

preferred item, stop, if your mother<br />

stop the secti<strong>on</strong> to find preferred<br />

items from the list, <strong>and</strong> wait until<br />

your mother pay cashier).<br />

3. Wait 5 sec. for child to perform the<br />

correct behavior showed cue card.<br />

specific c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e or c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

plus feedback) without further training.<br />

But mothers had written guidelines to<br />

follow about behavior support plan.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meeting plus feedback. During<br />

this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> meetings were<br />

implemented as stated above. Also, feedback<br />

was provided for each mother. Feedback was<br />

TABLE 4<br />

Script Story<br />

Going to Shopping<br />

I like to go shopping. It is fun to go store. It is<br />

fun to buy what I want. Sometimes I feel like to<br />

buy everything. But I can choice <strong>on</strong>e preferred<br />

item form shopping list. Sometimes I feel like<br />

running. But running inside of store could hurt<br />

me or other people. I will try to walk through<br />

the aisles <strong>and</strong> get <strong>on</strong>e item. I will wait for<br />

mother to pay cashier<br />

Give the preferred item.<br />

Give the verbal praise (good boy, you listen the story,<br />

<strong>and</strong> then we can go the shopping).<br />

If he has difficulty during shopping, anticipate the<br />

difficulty, <strong>and</strong> cue him to do what it is appropriate.<br />

If he engages in appropriate shopping behaviors,<br />

mother will provides specific praise as an attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> (a) praise for correct implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of target procedures, (b) corrective feedback<br />

<strong>on</strong> mothers’ performance, (c) addressing<br />

mothers’ questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> comments. All<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s were videotaped by the mothers. Before<br />

feedback sessi<strong>on</strong>, the researcher took<br />

careful notes while observing the sessi<strong>on</strong> to be<br />

able to give feedback about correct or incorrect<br />

use of the procedures. Feedback was delivered<br />

immediately prior to the start of the<br />

next sessi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researcher played the video<br />

tape <strong>and</strong> to discuss performance <strong>on</strong> the target<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for that sessi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se feedback sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in a room in the Yakup’s<br />

<strong>and</strong> Davut’s home, <strong>and</strong> Alp’s mother’s office,<br />

<strong>and</strong> lasted approximately 10–15 min. When<br />

the mother showed an incorrect behavior<br />

while implementing the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the researcher<br />

stopped the video, indicated the error,<br />

<strong>and</strong> asked how to correct the behavior<br />

(e.g., “Zeynep, let’s review what you have<br />

d<strong>on</strong>e. What else we could use in this situati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

When the mother gave a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se to<br />

Parent Training / 99


TABLE 5<br />

Alp’s Positive Behavior Support Plan<br />

Antecedent Strategies C<strong>on</strong>sequence Strategies<br />

Use of safety signal (say “We will be g<strong>on</strong>e<br />

in 5 minutes.”).<br />

Using cue card<br />

1. Gain child’s attenti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., look at<br />

me <strong>and</strong> I am going to show a<br />

picture).<br />

2. Show him the picture cue card<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with simple verbal cue (e.g.,<br />

get permissi<strong>on</strong> from friends to use<br />

their toys, when I reminded you to<br />

go home, you finish playing).<br />

this questi<strong>on</strong>, the sessi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>tinued to<br />

watch. When the mother gave incorrect or no<br />

TABLE 6<br />

Davut’s Positive Behavior Support Plan<br />

If he has difficulty during playing into playground,<br />

<strong>and</strong> cue him to do what it is appropriate.<br />

If he engages in appropriate playing behaviors,<br />

mother will provides specific as praise <strong>and</strong> give<br />

token for appropriate playing behavior. Tokens<br />

exchanges for preferred activities (i.e., go to<br />

McD<strong>on</strong>ald’s).<br />

Redirecti<strong>on</strong> strategies (Guiding a student toward a<br />

positive interacti<strong>on</strong> with peer) H<strong>and</strong>s are for<br />

playing, eating, <strong>and</strong> hugging; use your words.<br />

Logical, natural c<strong>on</strong>sequences when problem<br />

behavior occurred (If he grasps a toy from other<br />

children without permissi<strong>on</strong>, the toy is<br />

unavailable for his use for 10 minutes).<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se to this questi<strong>on</strong>, the researcher explained<br />

the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se verbally (e.g., the<br />

Antecedent Strategies C<strong>on</strong>sequence Strategies<br />

Using cue card<br />

1. Gain child’s attenti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., look<br />

at me <strong>and</strong> I am going to show a<br />

picture)<br />

2. Show him the picture cue card<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g with simple verbal cue<br />

(e.g., get permissi<strong>on</strong> from<br />

friends to use their toys, when I<br />

reminded you to go home, you<br />

finish playing).<br />

Use of safety signal (say “We will be<br />

g<strong>on</strong>e in 5 minutes.”).<br />

Differential reinforcement<br />

If he engages in appropriate social interacti<strong>on</strong> a play<br />

behavior with peer provide specific social praise.<br />

Gives social praise if engaged in other appropriate<br />

behavior.<br />

If he has difficulty during playing into playground, <strong>and</strong><br />

cue him to do what it is appropriate.<br />

If he engages in appropriate playing behaviors, mother<br />

will provides specific as praise <strong>and</strong> give token for<br />

appropriate playing behavior.<br />

Logical, natural c<strong>on</strong>sequences when problem behavior<br />

occurred (If he grasps a toy from other children<br />

without permissi<strong>on</strong>, the toy is unavailable for his use<br />

for 5 minutes).<br />

100 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


esearcher told the mother that “As so<strong>on</strong> as<br />

the child exhibited appropriate behavior, you<br />

should have g<strong>on</strong>e to next to him”. At the end<br />

of the feedback sessi<strong>on</strong>, the researcher delivered<br />

specific verbal praise for the mother’s<br />

performance <strong>on</strong> the target c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

asked to mother whether she had any questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

or comment about that sessi<strong>on</strong> or not.<br />

Results<br />

Figure 1 shows mothers’ percentage of correct<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequent<br />

procedures during c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e) <strong>and</strong> 2 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback).<br />

Figure 2 shows percentage of intervals<br />

in which children showed problem behaviors<br />

during c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 <strong>and</strong> 2. Table 7 represents<br />

means <strong>and</strong> ranges of child <strong>and</strong> mother behaviors<br />

in each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Parent Behavior<br />

During c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e), percentage<br />

of the correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures were<br />

low for all parents: Davut’s mother’s percentage<br />

of the correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent<br />

procedures (M 20,58 with a range of<br />

12% <strong>and</strong> 20%), Alp’s mother’s percentage of<br />

the correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedures (M 22,40 with range of 15% <strong>and</strong><br />

30%); <strong>and</strong> Yakup’s mother’s percentage of<br />

the correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent<br />

procedures (M 5 with a range of 0% <strong>and</strong><br />

10%). Unlike other two parents, Yakup’s<br />

mother showed delay for 2 sessi<strong>on</strong>s to use<br />

antecedent procedures. Yakup’s mother had<br />

several sessi<strong>on</strong>s in which they performed all<br />

the antecedent procedures (9 of 12 sessi<strong>on</strong>s).<br />

During c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback),<br />

percentage of the correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

increased dramatically for all parents: Davut’s<br />

mother (M 73,3% for c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures<br />

with a range of 45% <strong>and</strong> 90%), Alp’s<br />

mother (M 77% for antecedent procedures<br />

with a range of 40% <strong>and</strong> 100%); <strong>and</strong> Yakup’s<br />

mother (M 67,50% for c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures<br />

with a range, 35% to 90%).<br />

Child Behavior<br />

During c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1, level of problem behaviors<br />

was moderate for Yakup (M 28, 18% of<br />

intervals; range, 5% to 52%), Davut (M <br />

18,78% of intervals; range, 0% to 35%), <strong>and</strong><br />

Alp (M 17,09% of intervals; range, 0% to<br />

30%). During c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2, the level of problem<br />

behavior decreased for Yakup (M 23,<br />

20% of intervals; range 0% to 48%) <strong>and</strong> (M <br />

Davut 17, 10% of intervals; range, 0% to<br />

32%), <strong>and</strong> Alp (M 15, 81% of intervals;<br />

range, 0% to 32%).<br />

Maintenance<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mothers’ percentage of correct implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

remained high across the three<br />

maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s. All mothers implemented<br />

all procedures during the three maintenance<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

All children’s level of problem behaviors<br />

during the maintenance phase reflected levels<br />

close to those observed during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase. Alp’s behaviors (M 5,5; range,<br />

0% to 9.5%) remained low <strong>and</strong> under 10% of<br />

intervals for all three maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Davut’s behaviors (M 19,5; range, 0% to<br />

24,3%) <strong>and</strong> Yakup’s behaviors remained<br />

lower.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results of the study indicate that c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

plus feedback is effective in teaching<br />

mothers of children with developmental disabilities<br />

to use c<strong>on</strong>sequence <strong>and</strong> antecedent<br />

procedures. Many of studies have shown the<br />

effectiveness of feedback increasing participants’<br />

behaviors (Roscoe, Fisher, Glover, &<br />

Volkert, 2006; Scheeler & Lee, 2002, Codding<br />

et al., 2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> findings from the present<br />

study are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with previous study dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

that feedback is an important comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

of the parent training sessi<strong>on</strong>s (Dunlap<br />

& Fox, 1999; Dunlap, et al., 2001; Fox,<br />

Vaughn, Dunlap, & Bucy, 1997; Lucyshyn et<br />

al., 2002, Vaugh et al., 1997; Symes, et al.,<br />

2006). Children problem behaviors are inversely<br />

related to their mothers’ implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence <strong>and</strong> antecedent procedures.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results showed that when mothers<br />

implemented PBS procedures correctly, chil-<br />

Parent Training / 101


Figure 1. Mothers’ percent of correct implementati<strong>on</strong> of antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures.<br />

102 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Figure 2. Child participants’ percent of intervals problem behavior each sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

dren’s problem behaviors decreased <strong>and</strong> remained<br />

low throughout the study.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback was more effective<br />

than c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e in teaching the<br />

Parent Training / 103


TABLE 7<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> Ranges for Participants’ Behaviors<br />

Participants<br />

(# Sessi<strong>on</strong>s per<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>)<br />

proper use of antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedures. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings support the results<br />

of previous studies that have been d<strong>on</strong>e<br />

with teachers, <strong>and</strong> student teachers (e.g., Codding<br />

et al., 2005; Coulter & Grossen, 1997,<br />

O’Reilly et al., 1992; O’ Reilly et al., 1994,<br />

Scheeler & Lee, 2002). Correct implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of c<strong>on</strong>sequence <strong>and</strong> antecedent procedures<br />

did not improve rapidly during c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1). One<br />

explanati<strong>on</strong> for why the parents did not dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

high level of correct performance<br />

during c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> might be<br />

because they were given informati<strong>on</strong> written<br />

<strong>and</strong> verbally about interventi<strong>on</strong> procedures<br />

but not necessarily feedback <strong>on</strong> their performance.<br />

While parents were provided with verbal<br />

feedback <strong>and</strong> their errors were corrected<br />

by using video segments of their actual performance,<br />

they made resulted in an immediate<br />

improvement in performance during c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

plus feedback c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In other<br />

words, they watched themselves perform target<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>on</strong> videotape <strong>and</strong> were given<br />

clear verbal descripti<strong>on</strong> of what their errors<br />

were <strong>and</strong> how to correct them from researchers.<br />

In general, mothers resp<strong>on</strong>d correctly in<br />

both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

plus feedback). One explanati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

this might be due to parents’ high motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

to obtain necessary informati<strong>on</strong> to help <strong>and</strong><br />

cope with their child’s problem behavior.<br />

Another interesting finding was that Alp’s<br />

mother showed more rapid acquisiti<strong>on</strong> than<br />

% Intervals of<br />

Challenging Behaviors<br />

[Mean (Range)]<br />

% of Correct<br />

Implementati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

Procedures [Mean (Range)]<br />

Yakup Mother<br />

Yakup’s mother C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 28,18 5 (0–10)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 23,20 67,50 (35–90)<br />

Alp Mother<br />

Alp’s mother C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 17,09 22,40 (15–30)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 15,81 77 (40–100)<br />

Davut Mother<br />

Davut’s mother C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 1 18,78 20,58 (12–20)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> 2 17,10 73,33 (45–90)<br />

other parents during c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This differential effect may<br />

have been due to Alp’s mother educati<strong>on</strong><br />

level.<br />

A limitati<strong>on</strong> of the study was that mothers of<br />

children with disabilities were selected based<br />

<strong>on</strong> willingness to participate. Thus, participants<br />

may represent a highly motivated group<br />

that may not be representative of mothers of<br />

children with disabilities in general. Also, this<br />

study compared the effect of c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus feedback <strong>on</strong> the<br />

proper use of a few specific antecedent <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence strategies (e.g., using cue card,<br />

giving a choice, differential reinforcement),<br />

but not necessarily <strong>on</strong> all of the comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

skills necessary to implement PBS strategies.<br />

For example, actual implementati<strong>on</strong> of PBS<br />

(both antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequence procedures)<br />

requires additi<strong>on</strong>al skills such as the<br />

ability to develop interventi<strong>on</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

to functi<strong>on</strong> of problem behaviors <strong>and</strong> data<br />

analysis. Moreover, a comp<strong>on</strong>ent analysis was<br />

not d<strong>on</strong>e to assess which antecedent or c<strong>on</strong>sequence<br />

procedure is implemented more accurately.<br />

Future research is needed to address<br />

these c<strong>on</strong>cerns.<br />

In summary, the present results c<strong>on</strong>tributed<br />

to the literature in two ways. First, when mothers<br />

of children with disabilities learned how to<br />

use antecedent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequences procedures<br />

in an <strong>on</strong>going PBS with c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> plus<br />

feedback, their children’s problem behaviors<br />

were reduced to low level. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the study<br />

represents a replicati<strong>on</strong> of parent training in<br />

104 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


PBS, in that a new group of researchers applied<br />

the procedures in a new country. Replicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

across investigative teams <strong>and</strong> countries<br />

are needed to further advance <strong>and</strong><br />

strengthen the use of PBS.<br />

References<br />

Carr, E. G., & Carls<strong>on</strong>, J. I. (1993). Reducti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

severe behavior problems in the community using<br />

a multicomp<strong>on</strong>ent treatment approach. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 157–172.<br />

Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Turnbull, A. P., Marquis,<br />

J. G., Magito-McLaughlan, D., McAtee, M. L., et<br />

al. (1999). Positive behavior support for people with<br />

developmental disabilities: A research synthesis. Washingt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

DC: American Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Metal Retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Carr, E. G., Taylor, J. C., & Robins<strong>on</strong>, S. (1991). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

effects of severe behavior problems <strong>on</strong> the teaching<br />

behavior of adults. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 24, 523–535.<br />

Clark, H. B., & Hieneman, M. (1999).Comparing<br />

the wraparound process with features of positive<br />

behavioral support. What we can learn. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Positive Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 1, 183–186.<br />

Codding, R. S., Feinberg, A. B., Dunn, E. K., & Pace,<br />

G. M. (2005). Effects of immediate performance<br />

feedback <strong>on</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong> of behavior support<br />

plans. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 205–<br />

219.<br />

Coulter, G. A., & Grossen, B. (1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness<br />

of in-class instructive feedback versus after<br />

class instructive feedback for teachers learning<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> teaching behaviors. Effective School<br />

Practices, 16, 21–35.<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>e, D.A., & Horner, R.H. (2003). Building positive<br />

behavior support systems in schools: Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

behavioral assessment. New York: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

Dunlap, G., & Fox, L. (1999). A dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

behavioral support for young children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 1(2),<br />

77–87.<br />

Dunlap, G., Newt<strong>on</strong>, J. S., Fox, L., Benito, N., &<br />

Vaughn, B. (2001). Family Involvement in Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Assessment <strong>and</strong> Positive Behavior Support.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental Disabilities,<br />

16, 215–221.<br />

Emmer, E. T., & Strought, L. M. (2001). Classroom<br />

management: A critical part of educati<strong>on</strong>al psychology,<br />

with implicati<strong>on</strong>s for teacher educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychologist, 36, 103–112.<br />

Feldman, M. A., Case, L., Garrick, M., MacIntryre-<br />

Gr<strong>and</strong>e, W., Carnwell, J., & Sparks, B. (1992).<br />

Teaching child-care skills to mothers with devel-<br />

opmental disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 25, 205–215.<br />

Fox, L., Dunlap, G., & Philbrick, L. A. (1997). Providing<br />

individualized supports to young children<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> their families. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 21, 1–14.<br />

Fox, L., Vaughn, B. J., Dunlap, G., & Bucy, M.<br />

(1997). Parent-professi<strong>on</strong>al partnership in behavioral<br />

support: A qualitative analysis of <strong>on</strong>e family’s<br />

experience. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Associati<strong>on</strong> for Pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 22,198–2007.<br />

Fox, L., Vaughn, B. J., Wyatte, M. L., & Dunlap, G.<br />

(2002). “We can’t expect other people to underst<strong>and</strong>”:<br />

Family perspectives <strong>on</strong> problem behavior.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 68, 437–450.<br />

Hieneman, M., Dunlap, G., & Kincaid, D. (2005).<br />

Positive support strategies for students with behavior<br />

disorders in general educati<strong>on</strong> settings.<br />

Psychology in the Schools, 42, 779–794.<br />

Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1996). Teaching<br />

children with autism: Strategies for initiating positive<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> improving learning opportunities. Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Iwata, B. A., Wallece, M. D., Kahng, S., Lindberg,<br />

J. S., Roscoe, E. M., C<strong>on</strong>ners, J., et al. (2000). Skill<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in the implementati<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis methodology. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 33, 181–194.<br />

Lucyshyn, J. M., & Albin, R. W. (1993). Comprehensive<br />

support to families of children with disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> problem behaviors: Keeping it “friendly”.<br />

In G. H. S. Singer & L. E. Powers (Eds.), Families,<br />

disability, <strong>and</strong> empowerment: Active coping skills <strong>and</strong><br />

strategies for family interventi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 365–407). Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Lucyshyn, J. M., Horner, R. H., Dunlap, G., Albin<br />

R. W., & Ben, K. (2002). Positive behavior support<br />

with families. In J.M. Lucyshyn, G. Dunlap, &<br />

R. W. Ablin (Eds.), Families <strong>and</strong> positive behavior<br />

support (pp. 3–44). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Moore, J. W., Edwards, R. P., Sterling-Turner, H. E.,<br />

Riley, J., DuBard, M., & McGeorge, A. (2002).<br />

Teacher acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis methodology.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35,<br />

73–77.<br />

Nels<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Roberts, M. L., Mathur, S. R., & Rutherford,<br />

R. B. (1999). Has public policy exceeded<br />

our knowledge base? A review of the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

behavioral assessment literature. Behavioral Disorders,<br />

24, 169–179.<br />

O’ Reilly, M. F., Renzaglia, A., Hutchins, M., Koterba-Bass,<br />

L. Clayt<strong>on</strong>, M., Halle, J., et al. (1992).<br />

Teaching systemic instructi<strong>on</strong> competencies to<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> student teachers. An applied<br />

behavioral supervisi<strong>on</strong> model. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 17, 104–<br />

111.<br />

O’ Reilly, M. F., Renzaglia, A., & Lee, S. (1994). An<br />

Parent Training / 105


analysis of acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

of systemic instructi<strong>on</strong> competencies by<br />

preservice teachers using behavioral supervisi<strong>on</strong><br />

techniques. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental disabilities, 29, 22–33.<br />

Scheeler, M. C., & Lee, D. L. (2002). Using Technology<br />

to deliver immediate corrective feedback<br />

to preservice teachers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

11, 231–241.<br />

Scott, T., & Eber, L. (2003) Functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

<strong>and</strong> wraparound as systemic school processes: Primary,<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary, <strong>and</strong> tertiary systems examples.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive Behavior Supports, 5, 131–143.<br />

Sugai, G., & Horner, R.H. (2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> evoluti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

discipline practices: School-wide positive behavior<br />

supports. Child <strong>and</strong> Family Behavior <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>rapy, 24,<br />

23–50.<br />

Sugai, G., Horner, R. H., Dunlap, G., Hieneman,<br />

M., Lewis, T. J., Nels<strong>on</strong>, C. M., et al. (2000).<br />

Applying positive behavior support <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

assessment in schools. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive<br />

Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 2, 131–143.<br />

Suprague, J., & Walker, H. (2000). Early identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> for youth with antisocial<br />

<strong>and</strong> violent behavior. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 66,<br />

367–379.<br />

Sterling-Turner, H. E., Robins<strong>on</strong>, S. L., & Wilczynski,<br />

S. M. (2001) Functi<strong>on</strong>al assessments of dis-<br />

tracting <strong>and</strong> disruptive behaviors in the school<br />

setting. School Psychology Review, 3, 211–226.<br />

Symes, M. D., Remingt<strong>on</strong>, B., Brown, T., & Hastings,<br />

R. P. (2006). Early intensive behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for children with autism: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>rapists’ perspectives<br />

<strong>on</strong> achieving procedural fidelity. Research in<br />

Developmental Disabilities, 27, 30–42.<br />

Turnbull, A. P., & Ruef, M. B. (1996). Family perspectives<br />

<strong>on</strong> problem behavior. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

34, 280–293.<br />

Reid, R., & Nels<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> utility, acceptability,<br />

<strong>and</strong> practicality of functi<strong>on</strong>al behavioral<br />

assessment for students with high-incidence problem<br />

behaviors. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23,<br />

15–23.<br />

Roscoe, E. M., Fisher, W. W., Glover, A. C., & Volkert,<br />

V. M. (2006). Evaluating the relative effects of<br />

feedback <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tingent m<strong>on</strong>ey for staff training<br />

of stimulus preference assessments. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 39, 63–77.<br />

Vaughn, B. J., Dunlap, G., Fox, L., Clarke, S., &<br />

Bucy, M. (1997). Parent-professi<strong>on</strong>al partnership<br />

in behavioral support: A case study of community<br />

based interventi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 22, 186–197.<br />

Received: 24 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 28 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 10 April 2009<br />

106 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 107–115<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Jie Zhang<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> College at Brockport, SUNY<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong>: A Review<br />

John J. Wheeler<br />

Tennessee Technological University<br />

Abstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> prevalence of autism has increased approximately four times in children in nearly <strong>on</strong>e decade<br />

(California Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services Agency, 2003). It has been reported that explanati<strong>on</strong>s such as<br />

immigrati<strong>on</strong>, shifts in the interpretati<strong>on</strong> of diagnostic criteria, improved identificati<strong>on</strong>, or diagnostic accuracies<br />

cannot explain the observed increase (Geier & Geier, 2005). One potential cause that has alarmed many has<br />

been the presence of thimersol, the mercury-based preservative found am<strong>on</strong>g immunizati<strong>on</strong>s. Although many<br />

refute this, c<strong>on</strong>cern has been leveled by many families <strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>als c<strong>on</strong>cerning the potential impact of<br />

mercury pois<strong>on</strong>ing as a causal factor. Researchers have proposed that autism may be in part caused by mercury,<br />

because there was cumulative mercury exposure through dental amalgam, fish c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

polluti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>ally, through increased thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines for both mothers <strong>and</strong> newborns<br />

(Mutter, Naumann, Schneider, Walach, & Haley, 2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study is to review the<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> from studies c<strong>on</strong>cerning the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between mercury exposure <strong>and</strong> autism.<br />

It has been estimated that prevalence of autism<br />

has increased approximately four times<br />

in childhood autism in about <strong>on</strong>e decade,<br />

from 1 in 1,333 children (7.5 per 10,000<br />

children) am<strong>on</strong>g children born in the mid-<br />

1980s to 1 in 323 children (31.2 per 10,000<br />

children) am<strong>on</strong>g children born in the late-<br />

1990s (California Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services<br />

Agency, 2003). In 2004, the Department of<br />

Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services <strong>and</strong> the American<br />

Academy of Pediatrics issued an <strong>Autism</strong><br />

ALARM claiming that presently 1 in 166 children<br />

(60 per 10,000 children) has autism. In<br />

California, the autism rate increased by 634%<br />

between 1987 <strong>and</strong> 2002 (California Health<br />

<strong>and</strong> Human Services Agency). Once a rare<br />

disorder, autism has now been found to be<br />

more prevalent than childhood cancer, diabetes<br />

<strong>and</strong> Down Syndrome (California Health<br />

<strong>and</strong> Human Services Agency). It has been reported<br />

that explanati<strong>on</strong>s such as immigrati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

shifts in the interpretati<strong>on</strong> of diagnostic criteria,<br />

improved identificati<strong>on</strong>, or diagnostic accuracies<br />

cannot explain the observed increase<br />

(Geier & Geier, 2005). Researchers have pro-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Jie Zhang, Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Human<br />

Development, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> College at Brockport, State University<br />

of New York, 350 New Campus Drive, Brockport,<br />

NY 14420. E-mail: jzhang@brockport.edu<br />

posed that autism may be in part caused by<br />

mercury, because there was cumulative mercury<br />

exposure through fish <strong>and</strong> industrial<br />

sources, amalgam <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>ally, through<br />

increased thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines for<br />

both mothers <strong>and</strong> newborns (Mutter, Naumann,<br />

Schneider, et al., 2005).<br />

Similarities Between <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Mercury Pois<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> harmful effects of mercury include impaired<br />

motor planning, decreased facial recogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

blurred visi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stricted visual<br />

fields, insomnia, irritability, tantrums, excitability,<br />

social withdrawal, anxiety, difficulty<br />

verbalizing, altered taste, impaired short-term<br />

memory, slowed reacti<strong>on</strong> time <strong>and</strong> difficulty<br />

with c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> (Jeps<strong>on</strong>, 2004). Bernard, a<br />

parent of a child with autism, <strong>and</strong> several<br />

other investigators compared signs <strong>and</strong> symptoms<br />

of mercury pois<strong>on</strong>ing with those of autism<br />

(Bernard, Enayati, et al., 2001). Distinct<br />

similarities were found between autism <strong>and</strong><br />

mercury exposure in their effects up<strong>on</strong> immune,<br />

sensory, neurological, motor, <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> these similarities<br />

extend to neuroanatomy, neurotransmitters,<br />

<strong>and</strong> biochemistry (Geier & Geier, 2005).<br />

Thus, Bernard, Enayati, et al. suggest that the<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> / 107


egressive form of autism represents a form of<br />

mercury pois<strong>on</strong>ing (Bernard, Enayati, et al.).<br />

Historically, there were such illnesses caused<br />

by envir<strong>on</strong>mental exposure to mercury as<br />

Acrodynia or Pink’s disease <strong>and</strong> Minamata’s<br />

disease. Like autism, these diseases also presented<br />

with large range of variability <strong>and</strong> susceptibility<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g individuals in the populati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> were eradicated when the source of<br />

the exposure was eliminated (Jeps<strong>on</strong>, 2004).<br />

In the early 20th century, Acrodynia (Pink’s<br />

Disease) affected up to 1 of 500 infants in<br />

some industrial countries. Acrodynia disappeared<br />

after a frequently used teething powder,<br />

which c<strong>on</strong>tained mercury as calomel<br />

(Hg 2Cl 2), was removed from the market. Calomel<br />

works by pois<strong>on</strong>ing the nerves in the<br />

baby’s gums. When given orally, it is <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the less toxic forms of mercury <strong>and</strong> is about<br />

100 times less toxic than ethyl mercury to<br />

neur<strong>on</strong>es in vitro (Deth, 2004). From 1932 to<br />

1968, 27 t<strong>on</strong>s of organic mercury compounds<br />

were dumped into Minamata Bay, Japan by<br />

the Chisso Corporati<strong>on</strong> (Stoller, 2006). Over<br />

3,000 victims whose normal diet included fish<br />

from the bay unexpectedly developed symptoms<br />

of methyl mercury pois<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> have<br />

been recognized as having Minamata disease,<br />

a myriad of neurological <strong>and</strong> neurodevelopmental<br />

symptoms (Stoller). After the outbreaks<br />

in Minamata, c<strong>on</strong>genital Minamata disease<br />

was documented. Pregnant women in<br />

Minamata who c<strong>on</strong>sumed the c<strong>on</strong>taminated<br />

fish displayed mild or no symptoms but gave<br />

birth to infants with severe developmental<br />

disabilities such as cerebral palsy, mental retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> seizures. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>genital Minamata<br />

disease indicated that the fetal brain may<br />

be highly sensitive to MeHg exposure (Davids<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Myers, & Weiss, 2004).<br />

Biochemical <strong>and</strong> Genomic Basis<br />

Studies point to the possibility that testoster<strong>on</strong>e<br />

significantly enhances mercury toxicity,<br />

whereas estrogen decreases the toxic effects.<br />

Manning, Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, Wheelwright, <strong>and</strong><br />

S<strong>and</strong>ers (2001) examined 72 children with<br />

autism, including 23 children with Asperger<br />

syndrome, 34 siblings, 88 fathers, 88 mothers,<br />

<strong>and</strong> sex <strong>and</strong> age-matched c<strong>on</strong>trols to investigate<br />

prenatal testoster<strong>on</strong>e levels in children<br />

with autistic spectrum disorders (Lutchmaya,<br />

Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, Raggatt, Knickmeyer, & Manning,<br />

2004; Manning et al.). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> authors dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that the more severely autism the<br />

higher the levels of prenatal testoster<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

Tordjman, Ferrari, Sulm<strong>on</strong>t, Duyme, <strong>and</strong><br />

Roubertoux (1997) measured plasma testoster<strong>on</strong>e<br />

<strong>and</strong> adrenal <strong>and</strong>rogen in nine drugfree<br />

prepubertal children (6–10 years old)<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> 62 neurotypical c<strong>on</strong>trol children<br />

of same age, sex, weight (within 2 kg),<br />

<strong>and</strong> stage of puberty. Results showed that<br />

three of the nine children with autism had<br />

an abnormally high plasma testoster<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Tordjman et al.). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se studies<br />

may explain that autism affects males four to<br />

five times as often as females (Bertr<strong>and</strong>, Mars,<br />

et al., 2001; Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003). In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, closer observati<strong>on</strong> indicates that the<br />

more severe the autism the higher the male to<br />

female ratio. For example, in very severe cases<br />

males may outnumber females by 15 to 1 or<br />

even more (Holmes, Blaxill, & Haley, 2003).<br />

Boris, Goldblatt, Galanko, <strong>and</strong> James (2004)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted genomic studies of children with<br />

autism in comparis<strong>on</strong> to normal c<strong>on</strong>trol populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to examine genes in pathways that are<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the synthesis of key biochemical<br />

molecules, which are functi<strong>on</strong>ally relevant<br />

in the excreti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong>/or oxidative stress protecti<strong>on</strong><br />

of mercury from the body (Boris et<br />

al.). Results dem<strong>on</strong>strated that there was approximately<br />

a twice statistically significant increase<br />

in the homozygous methylenetetrahydrofolate<br />

reductase (MTHFR) 677TT gene<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g children with autism compared to c<strong>on</strong>trols.<br />

MTHFR 677TT is <strong>on</strong>e of the key genes in<br />

the biochemical pathway resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the<br />

synthesis of glutathi<strong>on</strong>e, a key molecule in the<br />

body’s natural defenses against mercury, <strong>and</strong><br />

those with the MTHFR 677TT gene have been<br />

found to have an enzyme with <strong>on</strong>ly 32% of the<br />

activity of normal (Chango, Boiss<strong>on</strong>, & Barbe,<br />

2000). James, Cutler, <strong>and</strong> Melnyk (2004) evaluated<br />

the methi<strong>on</strong>ine cycle <strong>and</strong> transsulfurati<strong>on</strong><br />

metabolites in children with autism in<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> to age- <strong>and</strong> sex-matched c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

children (James, 2004; James et al.). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> process<br />

of cysteine <strong>and</strong> glutathi<strong>on</strong>e synthesis,<br />

both of which are crucial for natural mercury<br />

excreti<strong>on</strong>, are reduced in children with autism<br />

108 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


(Deth, 2004; Waly et al., 2004). Results showed<br />

that there were significant decreases in the<br />

plasma c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> of both cysteine (19%<br />

lower) <strong>and</strong> glutathi<strong>on</strong>e (46% lower) in children<br />

with autism (James; James et al.). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> of plasma level of cysteine <strong>and</strong> glutathi<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g others, adversely affect the<br />

ability to detoxify <strong>and</strong> excrete metals like<br />

mercury (Deth; James et al.), which may lead<br />

to higher Hg c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s in tissues like<br />

the nervous system <strong>and</strong> also lead to a l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

half-life of mercury (Deth; Mutter, Naumann,<br />

Sadaghiani, Walach, & Drasch, 2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Working Group (EWG) investigated<br />

the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between mercury<br />

exposure, especially mercury exposure from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines, <strong>and</strong><br />

autistic disorders (Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Working<br />

Group, 2004). It is found that there is a severe<br />

metabolic imbalance in the ratio of active to<br />

inactive glutathi<strong>on</strong>e, the body’s most important<br />

tool for detoxifying <strong>and</strong> excreting metals,<br />

in children with autism. As a result, these children<br />

would be susceptible to the harmful effects<br />

of mercury <strong>and</strong> other toxic chemical exposures.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> EWG drew the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that<br />

by identifying a metabolic comm<strong>on</strong> to nearly<br />

all children with autism that would make these<br />

children poorly equipped to mount a defense<br />

against a number of neurotoxic compounds,<br />

including mercury (Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Working<br />

Group). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se findings significantly strengthen<br />

the possibility that mercury could cause or<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tribute to autism <strong>and</strong> other neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se are some of the biochemical<br />

<strong>and</strong> genomic basis for the increased<br />

body-burden of mercury in children with<br />

autism.<br />

Higher Body-Burdens of Mercury<br />

Several clinical studies evaluated the bodyburden<br />

of heavy metals present in children<br />

with autism in comparis<strong>on</strong> to typically developing<br />

children (Geier & Geier, 2005b).<br />

Holmes et al. (2003) <strong>and</strong> Hu, Bernard, <strong>and</strong><br />

Che (2003) found that despite significantly<br />

higher mercury levels exposure through maternal<br />

dental amalgam <strong>and</strong> thimerosal c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

immunoglobulins during pregnancy<br />

(Holmes et al.; Hu et al.), mercury levels in<br />

the first babies’ haircut of 94 children with<br />

autism were about 8-fold lower than 49 c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

trol children. Interestingly, Gr<strong>and</strong>jean, Weihe,<br />

White, <strong>and</strong> Debes (1998) found that infants<br />

who reached milest<strong>on</strong>e criteria early had significantly<br />

higher mercury c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

the hair at 12 m<strong>on</strong>ths of age (Gr<strong>and</strong>jean et al.;<br />

Mutter, Naumann, Sadaghiani, Schneider, &<br />

Walach, 2004). It was also observed that the<br />

more severe the autism the lower mercury<br />

levels were present in the first baby haircut,<br />

which leads to the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that the mercury<br />

levels in the first baby haircuts were inversely<br />

related to the severity of the autism<br />

(Geier & Geier). Further studies showed elevated<br />

levels of mercury, lead <strong>and</strong> uranium in<br />

haircuts from 40 older children with autism<br />

in comparis<strong>on</strong> to 40 normal c<strong>on</strong>trols, while<br />

there was no difference for other toxic metals<br />

like aluminium, arsenic, cadmium or beryllium<br />

(Fido & Al-Saad, 2005). Bradstreet et al.<br />

evaluated the c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> of heavy metals in<br />

the urine am<strong>on</strong>g 221 vaccinated children with<br />

autistic spectrum disorders in comparis<strong>on</strong> to a<br />

neurotypical c<strong>on</strong>trol populati<strong>on</strong> (Bradstreet,<br />

Geier, & Kartzinel, 2003). Based <strong>on</strong> excreti<strong>on</strong><br />

following an identical three-day oral provocati<strong>on</strong><br />

with meso 2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid<br />

(DMSA), there were approximately six times<br />

significantly greater urinary mercury c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g vaccinated children with autism<br />

matched to vaccinated neurotyptical c<strong>on</strong>trols,<br />

whereas the two groups of children had<br />

similar urinary cadmium <strong>and</strong> lead c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Following DMSA treatment, similar<br />

urinary mercury c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong> levels were observed<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g matched vaccinated <strong>and</strong> unvaccinated<br />

neurotypical children. Results suggest<br />

that after exposure to mercury, children with<br />

autism are unable to properly eliminate mercury<br />

<strong>and</strong> thus have significantly higher bodyburdens<br />

of mercury than neurotypical children<br />

(Bradstreet et al.).<br />

Slikker from the FDA c<strong>on</strong>firmed that thimerosal<br />

crosses the bloodbrain barrier <strong>and</strong><br />

placental barriers, resulting in appreciable<br />

mercury c<strong>on</strong>tent in tissues including the brain<br />

(Slikker, 2000). In additi<strong>on</strong>, Sager reported<br />

the half-life of mercury in the brain of infant<br />

primates was approximately 28 days following<br />

administrati<strong>on</strong> of soluti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccine<br />

comparable c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s of thimerosal<br />

(Institute of Medicine, 2004).<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> / 109


Mercury Exposure during Pregnancy<br />

Even before they are born, it is speculated that<br />

children with autism may have had a higher<br />

mercury exposure during pregnancy due to maternal<br />

dental amalgam, thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

immunoglobulin shots, <strong>and</strong> fish c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Vahter et al. (2000) examined the different<br />

species of mercury in the blood of pregnant<br />

women. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y found high correlati<strong>on</strong>s between<br />

inorganic mercury levels in blood <strong>and</strong> urine<br />

during early pregnancy, a significant correlati<strong>on</strong><br />

between cord <strong>and</strong> maternal blood, <strong>and</strong><br />

decreased mercury levels during lactati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

presumably due to excreti<strong>on</strong> in milk (Vahter<br />

et al.).<br />

Dental amalgam c<strong>on</strong>sists of about 50% of the<br />

most toxic n<strong>on</strong>radioactive element (Mutter,<br />

Naumann, Walach, & Daschner, 2005), <strong>and</strong><br />

of other heavy metals as tin, copper, silver,<br />

zinc additi<strong>on</strong>ally. Amalgam fillings were first<br />

used in 1830 in the United States (Stoller,<br />

2006). By 1840, organized dentistry denounced<br />

the use of amalgam as a poor filling material<br />

because of c<strong>on</strong>cerns about mercury pois<strong>on</strong>ing,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the American Society of Dental Surge<strong>on</strong>s<br />

was formed <strong>and</strong> required members to<br />

sign a pledge promising not to use mercury<br />

fillings. In 1926, Dr. Alfred Stock, a chemist,<br />

noted that mercury amalgam fillings in the<br />

mouth were a source of mercury vapor<br />

(Stoller). Mercury vapor escapes during the<br />

preparati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> placement of amalgam restorati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Some of the vapor may be inhaled<br />

(Davids<strong>on</strong> et al., 2004). Research indicates<br />

that mercury levels in human placentas correlate<br />

with the number of maternal amalgam<br />

fillings <strong>and</strong> a substantial amount of mercury<br />

from amalgam reaches the fetus (Stoiber et al.<br />

2004; <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir et al., 2003).<br />

Rh-negative women were routinely given<br />

Rhogam injecti<strong>on</strong>s several times during their<br />

pregnancies until recently. Those Rhogam injecti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained significant amounts of thimerosal,<br />

which has been removed <strong>on</strong>ly recently<br />

(Geier & Geier, 2003). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

there are other prescripti<strong>on</strong> drugs <strong>and</strong> overthe-counter<br />

medicati<strong>on</strong>s that c<strong>on</strong>tain significant<br />

amounts of thimerosal (Geier & Geier).<br />

As Davids<strong>on</strong> et al. (2004) pointed out, the<br />

principal source of organic mercury that humans<br />

are exposed to in the aquatic envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

is atmospheric mercury deposited <strong>on</strong><br />

the surfaces of bodies of water that is then<br />

biomethylated by microorganisms <strong>and</strong> subsequently<br />

biomagnifies as it ascends the food<br />

chain. Some older, larger carnivorous fish at<br />

the top of the food chain can c<strong>on</strong>tain more<br />

than 1 ppm. Infants <strong>and</strong> children may be exposed<br />

postnatally to mercury from breast milk<br />

if their mother c<strong>on</strong>sumes foods that c<strong>on</strong>tain<br />

high levels or if they c<strong>on</strong>sume fish or foodstaff<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>tain fish products (Davids<strong>on</strong> et al.).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> FDA currently recommends that pregnant<br />

women <strong>and</strong> those women who may become<br />

pregnant avoid species with the highest<br />

average amounts of methylmercury <strong>and</strong> that a<br />

balanced diet of seafood c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong> should<br />

be followed to keep methylmercury levels low<br />

(Geier & Geier, 2003).<br />

Mercury Exposure after Birth<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regressive form of autism is rising at a<br />

much higher rate than the form of autism<br />

evident from birth. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regressive form now is<br />

felt to make up close to 75% of the new cases<br />

(Jeps<strong>on</strong>, 2004). Jeps<strong>on</strong> uses the rise in envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

exposure to explain that more<br />

children nowadays have less severe deficits<br />

genetically.<br />

Elemental mercury is the form of mercury<br />

represented in the hazardous air pollutant<br />

(HAP) database (Windham, Zhang, Gunier,<br />

Croen, & Grether, 2006). Every year, there are<br />

approximately 3,400 metric t<strong>on</strong>s of elemental<br />

mercury released into the envir<strong>on</strong>ment, 95%<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g which resides in terrestrial soils, 3% in<br />

ocean surface waters, <strong>and</strong> the remaining 2%<br />

in the atmosphere. Approximately 70% of the<br />

mercury in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment comes from anthropogenic<br />

sources such as coal-fired electric<br />

power generati<strong>on</strong> facilities <strong>and</strong> industrial<br />

waste. Natural sources, however, also deposits<br />

it in the envir<strong>on</strong>ment from volcanos, mines,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the erosi<strong>on</strong> of ores (Davids<strong>on</strong> et al.,<br />

2004).<br />

Windham et al. (2006) linked the California<br />

autism surveillance system to estimated hazardous<br />

air pollutant (HAP) c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

compiled by the U.S. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Protecti<strong>on</strong><br />

Agency (EPA) to explore possible associati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between autism spectrum disorders<br />

(ASD) <strong>and</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>mental exposures. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y included<br />

284 children with ASD <strong>and</strong> 657 c<strong>on</strong>trols,<br />

born in 1994 in the San Francisco Bay<br />

110 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


area. Results suggest a potential associati<strong>on</strong><br />

between autism <strong>and</strong> estimated metal c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Windham et al.). Palmer, Blanchard,<br />

Stein, M<strong>and</strong>ell, <strong>and</strong> Miller (2006) did an epidemiologic<br />

study, linking Toxic Release Inventory<br />

(TRI) data <strong>on</strong> mercury to special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

data in Texas. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study reported a<br />

61% increase in autism prevalence rates (or<br />

17% adjusted) per 1,000 pounds of mercury<br />

released (Palmer et al.).<br />

Thimerosal is an ethylmercury-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

compound (49.6% mercury by weight) (Geier<br />

& Geier, 2006). It is a preservative that is<br />

added to many vaccines at 0.005% to 0.01%<br />

level to prevent bacterial c<strong>on</strong>taminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

thus, prol<strong>on</strong>ging shelf-life <strong>and</strong> facilitating multiuse<br />

vials. It has become a major source of<br />

mercury am<strong>on</strong>g children in the United States<br />

since within their first two years of life, who<br />

may have received a quantity of mercury that<br />

exceeded Federal Safety Guidelines (Ball, Ball,<br />

& Pratt, 2001; Redwood, Bernard, & Brown,<br />

2001). Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier pointed out that the<br />

routine childhood immunizati<strong>on</strong> schedule used<br />

to include five doses of Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

Diptheria-Tetanus-whole-cell-Pertussis (DTP) vaccine<br />

with the first dose being administered at<br />

two m<strong>on</strong>ths of age. From the late 1980s through<br />

the 1990s, the Centers for Disease C<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong><br />

Preventi<strong>on</strong> (CDC) exp<strong>and</strong>ed the number of<br />

doses of Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines to be<br />

administered to US infants. Eventually, the<br />

immunizati<strong>on</strong> schedule came to be composed<br />

of three doses of Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining hepatitis<br />

B vaccine with the first dose administered<br />

as early as the day of birth, <strong>and</strong> four<br />

doses of Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining Haemophilus<br />

Influenzae type b (Hib) vaccine with the first<br />

dose administered at two m<strong>on</strong>ths of age. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

the CDC also began recommended<br />

that three doses of Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining influenza<br />

vaccine be administered to certain infant<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> with the first dose administered<br />

by the sixth m<strong>on</strong>th of age (Geier & Geier).<br />

As a result, if all thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines<br />

were administered according to the immunizati<strong>on</strong><br />

schedule recommended by the<br />

CDC, infants in the United States may have<br />

been exposed to 12.5 micrograms (g) of ethylmercury<br />

at birth, 62.5 g at two m<strong>on</strong>ths,<br />

50 g at four m<strong>on</strong>ths, 62.5 g at six m<strong>on</strong>ths,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 50 g at 18 m<strong>on</strong>ths, for a total of 237.5 g<br />

of ethylmercury during the first 18 m<strong>on</strong>ths of<br />

life (Ball et al., 2001; Redwood et al., 2001). In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, if three thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining influenza<br />

vaccines were administered during the<br />

first 18 m<strong>on</strong>ths of life as they were suggested<br />

to certain infant populati<strong>on</strong>, the total mercury<br />

exposure could have been as high as 275 gof<br />

mercury (Ball et al.; Redwood et al.).<br />

Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier (2004) reviewed the 2003<br />

US Physician’s Desk Reference (PDR) <strong>and</strong><br />

found that some childhood vaccines still c<strong>on</strong>tain<br />

thimerosal. For example, DTaP manufactured<br />

by Aventis Pasteur c<strong>on</strong>tains 25 g of<br />

mercury, b (Hib) vaccine manufactured by<br />

Wyeth c<strong>on</strong>tains 25 g of mercury, <strong>and</strong> pediatric<br />

hepatitis B vaccine manufactured by Merck<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tains 12.5 g of mercury. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, influenza<br />

vaccine recommended for an increasing<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> also c<strong>on</strong>tains 25 g of mercury.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, possible total childhood mercury<br />

in the United States in 2003 is more than<br />

300 g, am<strong>on</strong>g which levels of mercury from<br />

thimerosal c<strong>on</strong>tained childhood vaccinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

are higher than at any time in the past (Geier<br />

& Geier). Furthermore, injecting thimerosal<br />

into infants muscle also bypasses the GI tract,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e of the body’s first-line defense mechanisms<br />

(Geier & Geier).<br />

In 1982, an expert panel at the Food <strong>and</strong><br />

Drug Administrati<strong>on</strong> (FDA) reviewed thimerosal,<br />

reported it as toxic, causing cell damage,<br />

ineffective in killing bacteria or halting their<br />

replicati<strong>on</strong>, not as safe or effective as generally<br />

recognized (Federal Register, 1982), <strong>and</strong><br />

thus called for its removal in over-the-counter<br />

products (Stoller, 2006). In 1988, the FDA<br />

ruled that thimerosal should be removed from<br />

OTC products, but gave the industry another<br />

16 years to phase out its presence. In 1999,<br />

the FDA announced that under the recommended<br />

childhood immunizati<strong>on</strong> schedule,<br />

infants might be exposed to cumulative doses<br />

of ethylmercury from vaccines that exceed<br />

federal safety guidelines established for the<br />

oral ingesti<strong>on</strong> of methylmercury (Geier &<br />

Geier, 2006). In the same year of 1999, the<br />

Public Health Service (PHS) <strong>and</strong> the American<br />

Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) issued a<br />

joint statement calling for the removal of<br />

thimerosal from all childhood vaccines as<br />

so<strong>on</strong> as possible due to any potential risk<br />

(Stoller). In 2001, the FDA required that<br />

vaccine manufacturers remove thimerosal<br />

from their vaccines. In the same year of 2001,<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> / 111


the Institute of Medicine (IOM) stated that it<br />

was biologically plausible for mercury from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines to<br />

cause childhood neurodevelopmental disorders<br />

(Geier & Geier, 2004). Interestingly,<br />

thimerosal was taken out of animal vaccines a<br />

decade ago because of the safety c<strong>on</strong>cern<br />

(Stoller).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Protecti<strong>on</strong> Agency<br />

(EPA)’s safety guideline of vicinal thimerosal<br />

is 1 part-per-milli<strong>on</strong> (ppm) for <strong>on</strong>e-year old.<br />

Redwood et al. (2001) utilized a <strong>on</strong>e compartment<br />

pharmacokinetic model to estimate<br />

hair mercury c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s expected to result<br />

from the recommended CDC childhood<br />

immunizati<strong>on</strong> schedule during the 1990s, <strong>and</strong><br />

the results indicated that modeled hair mercury<br />

c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s in infants exposed to vicinal<br />

thimerosal were in excess of the EPA’s<br />

safety guidelines for up to the first 365 days,<br />

with several peak c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s within this<br />

period (Geier & Geier, 2005b; Redwood et<br />

al.). Nowadays in the United States, thimerosal<br />

has been removed from many vaccines<br />

administered to infants, but there is no recall<br />

of the existing thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining product.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, other thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

vaccines are still being developed <strong>and</strong> marketed<br />

such as influenza, Tetanus-diphtheria,<br />

Rhogam, <strong>and</strong> m<strong>on</strong>ovalent tetanus. Thimerosal<br />

has also been present in other commercial<br />

products such as c<strong>on</strong>tact lens soluti<strong>on</strong>, which<br />

was removed in 1998, eardrops <strong>and</strong> various<br />

nasal preparati<strong>on</strong>s (Jeps<strong>on</strong>, 2004).<br />

Thimerosal has been recognized as a developmental<br />

toxin by the California Envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

Protecti<strong>on</strong> Agency, Office of Envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

Health Hazard Assessment. Maternal<br />

exposure during pregnancy can disrupt the<br />

development, cause birth defects, low birth<br />

weight, biological dysfuncti<strong>on</strong>s, or even the<br />

death of the fetus, <strong>and</strong> exposure to it can also<br />

cause psychological or behavior deficits that<br />

become evident as the child grows (Geier &<br />

Geier, 2005). In vitro, mercury <strong>and</strong> thimerosal<br />

in levels reached eight days after vaccinati<strong>on</strong><br />

inhibit methi<strong>on</strong>ine synthetase (MS) by 50%.<br />

MS is crucial in biochemical steps for brain<br />

development, attenti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> producti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

glutathi<strong>on</strong>e, an important antioxidative <strong>and</strong><br />

detoxifying agent. Subsequently, children with<br />

autism have significantly decreased level of<br />

reduced glutathi<strong>on</strong>e (Geier & Geier). In ad-<br />

diti<strong>on</strong>, a study reported that thimerosal was a<br />

potent inhibitor of phagocytosis, which is the<br />

first step of the innate immune system (Rampersad<br />

et al., 2005). While the acquired innate<br />

immune system can be <strong>on</strong>ly built up by<br />

aging, it seems likely that injecti<strong>on</strong> of thimerosal<br />

would inhibit an infant’s immune system.<br />

Fagan, Pritchard, Clarks<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Greenwood<br />

(1977) reported that 10 children out of 13<br />

treated with a thimerosal c<strong>on</strong>taining topological<br />

“antiseptic” for umbilical cord infecti<strong>on</strong><br />

died, even though this antiseptic was used<br />

world-wide <strong>on</strong> adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>and</strong> very<br />

little negative effects were reported. This<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly implied that infants were much more<br />

susceptible to thimerosal toxicity than others<br />

(Mutter, Naumann, Schneider, et al., 2005).<br />

An ecological study evaluated birth cohorts<br />

from the mid-1980s through the late 1990s to<br />

investigate the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between the average<br />

mercury doses children received from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines in comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

to the prevalence of autism. Results showed<br />

that there was an increasing linear correlati<strong>on</strong><br />

between the amount of mercury children received<br />

from thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines<br />

<strong>and</strong> the cohort prevalence of autism (Geier &<br />

Geier, 2004).<br />

Epidemiological studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the<br />

United States using various databases, including<br />

the California Department of Developmental<br />

Services (CDDS), Vaccine Adverse<br />

Event Reporting System (VAERS), US Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the Vaccine Safety<br />

Datalink (VSD) to examine the relati<strong>on</strong>ship<br />

between thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines<br />

<strong>and</strong> neurodevelopmental disorders (Geier<br />

& Geier, 2006). Results indicate significant links<br />

between exposure to Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

vaccines <strong>and</strong> neurodevelopmental disorders,<br />

showing that in comparis<strong>on</strong> to children receiving<br />

thimerosal-free childhood vaccines,<br />

children receiving thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood<br />

vaccines were two- to six-fold statistically<br />

significantly increased risks to develop neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders, depending up<strong>on</strong> the<br />

specific c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s or symptoms examined<br />

(Geier & Geier, 2005b).<br />

Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier (2003) evaluated doses of<br />

mercury from thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood<br />

immunizati<strong>on</strong>s in comparis<strong>on</strong> to U.S.<br />

Federal Safety Guidelines. Results showed that<br />

children received mercury from thimerosal-<br />

112 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines, as part of the routine US<br />

childhood immunizati<strong>on</strong> schedule, in excess<br />

of the Federal Safety Guidelines for the oral<br />

ingesti<strong>on</strong> of methylmercury. Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, based<br />

up<strong>on</strong> the Vaccine Adverse Events Reporting<br />

System (VAERS) database, Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier<br />

investigated the effects of increasing doses of<br />

mercury <strong>on</strong> the incidence rates of neurodevelopment<br />

disorders reported following<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines in comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

to thimerosal-free vaccines. Outcomes showed<br />

increasing relative risk of neurodevelopment<br />

disorders with increasing doses of mercury.<br />

Finally, the authors used data from the US<br />

Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> to analyze the prevalence<br />

of school children at various ages with<br />

various types of disabilities in comparis<strong>on</strong> to<br />

the mercury dose that children received from<br />

thimerosal in their childhood vaccines. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

analyses showed autism <strong>and</strong> speech disorders<br />

were correlated with increasing mercury from<br />

childhood vaccines (Geier & Geier).<br />

Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier (2004) used the Biological<br />

Surveillance Summaries of the Centers for<br />

Disease C<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong> Preventi<strong>on</strong> (CDC), the<br />

U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> datasets, <strong>and</strong><br />

the CDC’s yearly live birth estimates to evaluate<br />

the effects of mercury from thimerosalc<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

childhood vaccines <strong>on</strong> the prevalence<br />

of autism. Results indicated that there was<br />

a close correlati<strong>on</strong> between mercury doses from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines <strong>and</strong><br />

the prevalence of autism from the mid-to-late<br />

1980s through the mid-1990s. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it<br />

was found that there were statistically significant<br />

odds ratios for the prevalence of autism<br />

following increasing doses of mercury from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines with birth cohorts<br />

of 1985 <strong>and</strong> from 1990 to 1995 in comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

to a baseline measurement with birth<br />

cohort of 1984 (Geier & Geier).<br />

Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier (2005) did a two-phased<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>-based epidemiological study. Phase<br />

<strong>on</strong>e examined reported neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders (NDs) to the Vaccine Adverse Event<br />

Reporting System (VAERS) following thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

Diphtheria-Tetanus-acellular-<br />

Pertussis (DTaP) vaccines in comparis<strong>on</strong> to<br />

thimerosal-free DTaP vaccines administered<br />

from 1997 through 2001. Phase two evaluated<br />

the automated Vaccine Safety Datalink (VSD)<br />

for cumulative exposures to mercury from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines at 1-, 2-, 3-, <strong>and</strong><br />

6-m<strong>on</strong>ths of age for infants born from 1992<br />

through 1997 <strong>and</strong> the eventual risk of developing<br />

NDs to determine whether the affect<br />

from thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines<br />

<strong>on</strong> neurodevelopmental disorders observed<br />

in the VSD database was c<strong>on</strong>sistent with observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

made in the VAERS database. Results<br />

showed that exposure to mercury from<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines administered<br />

in the United States was a c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

significant risk factor for the development<br />

of NDs (Geier & Geier).<br />

Geier <strong>and</strong> Geier (2006) also c<strong>on</strong>ducted a<br />

meta-analysis epidemiological assessment of<br />

the Vaccine Adverse Event Reporting System<br />

(VAERS) for neurodevelopment disorders<br />

(NDs) reported following Diphtheria-<br />

Tetanus-whole-cell-Pertussis (DTP) vaccines<br />

in comparis<strong>on</strong> to Diphtheria-Tetanus-wholecell-Pertussis-Haemophilus<br />

Influenzae Type b<br />

(DTPH) vaccines administered from 1994 to<br />

1997 <strong>and</strong> following Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

Diphtheria-Tetanus-acellular-Pertussis (DTaP)<br />

vaccines in comparis<strong>on</strong> to Thimerosal-free<br />

DTaP vaccines administered from 1997 to<br />

2000. Results dem<strong>on</strong>strated that a significant<br />

risk factor for the development of neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders was the amount of mercury<br />

children received from Thimerosalc<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

childhood immunizati<strong>on</strong>s, thus<br />

there is a significant relati<strong>on</strong>ship between the<br />

Thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining childhood vaccines<br />

evaluated <strong>and</strong> childhood neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders (Geier & Geier).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

To sum up, there has been a great deal of<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> from different studies that seems<br />

to indicate that repetitive mercury exposure<br />

during pregnancy, through thimerosal, dental<br />

amalgam, <strong>and</strong> fish c<strong>on</strong>sumpti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> after<br />

birth, through thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> polluti<strong>on</strong>, in genetically susceptible<br />

individuals is <strong>on</strong>e potential factor in autism.<br />

Certainly this questi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinues to stir debate<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g professi<strong>on</strong>als across the medical<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavioral sciences. It serves as a grey area<br />

for many families as they seek to quell their<br />

anxiety invoked by this debate by discovering<br />

the facts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this article was to<br />

synthesize the findings relative to this questi<strong>on</strong><br />

to hopefully serve as a resource to educa-<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> / 113


tors as we seek to become more well-informed<br />

<strong>on</strong> this timely issue. As the prevalence rate for<br />

autism in children c<strong>on</strong>tinues to rise, more<br />

research is needed to better underst<strong>and</strong><br />

causal factors. It is also crucial that quality<br />

reviews be c<strong>on</strong>ducted to synthesize a body of<br />

knowledge pertaining to these questi<strong>on</strong>s if the<br />

puzzle is to be solved pertaining to the link<br />

between mercury exposure <strong>and</strong> autism.<br />

References<br />

Ball, L. K., Ball, R., & Pratt, R. D. (2001). An assessment<br />

of thimerosal use in childhood vaccines.<br />

Pediatrics, 107, 1147–1154.<br />

Bernard, S., Enayati, A., Redwood, L., Roger, H., &<br />

Binstock, T. (2001). <strong>Autism</strong>: A novel form of mercury<br />

pois<strong>on</strong>ing. Medical Hypotheses, 56, 462–471.<br />

Bertr<strong>and</strong>, J., Mars, A., Boyle, C., Bove, F., Yeargin-<br />

Allsopp, M., & Decoufle, P. (2001). Prevalence of<br />

autism in a United States populati<strong>on</strong>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Brick<br />

Township, New Jersey, investigati<strong>on</strong>. Pediatrics,<br />

108, 1155–1161.<br />

Boris, M., Goldblatt, A., Galanko, J., & James, S. J.<br />

(2004). Associati<strong>on</strong> of MTHFR gene variants with<br />

autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of American Physicians <strong>and</strong> Surge<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

9, 106–108.<br />

Bradstreet, J., Geier, D. A., & Kartzinel, J. J. (2003).<br />

A case-c<strong>on</strong>trol study of mercury burden in children<br />

with autistic spectrum disorders. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

American Physicians <strong>and</strong> Surge<strong>on</strong>s, 8, 76–79.<br />

California Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services Agency.<br />

(2003). California Department of Developmental Services:<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> spectrum disorders: Changes in the California<br />

caseload <strong>and</strong> update: 1999 through 2002. Sacramento,<br />

CA.<br />

Chango, A., Boiss<strong>on</strong>, F., & Barbe, F. (2000). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

effect of 677CfiT <strong>and</strong> 1298AfiC mutati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong><br />

plasma homocysteine <strong>and</strong> 5,10-methylenetetrahydrofolate<br />

reductase activity in healthy subjects.<br />

British <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Nutriti<strong>on</strong>, 83, 593–596.<br />

Davids<strong>on</strong>, P. W., Myers, G. J., & Weiss, B. (2004).<br />

Mercury exposure <strong>and</strong> child development outcomes.<br />

Pediatrics, 113, 1023–1029.<br />

Deth, R. C. (2004). Truth revealed: New scientific discoveries<br />

regarding mercury in medicine <strong>and</strong> autism.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>gressi<strong>on</strong>al Testim<strong>on</strong>y before the U.S. House of Representatives.<br />

Subcommittee <strong>on</strong> human rights <strong>and</strong> wellness,<br />

Sept. 8. Retrieved Nov. 13, 2007, from http://<br />

reform.house.gov/WHR/Hearings/EventSingle.<br />

aspx?EventID18156<br />

Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Working Group. (2004). Overloaded?<br />

New science, new insights about mercury <strong>and</strong> autism in<br />

susceptible children. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: EWG Acti<strong>on</strong><br />

Fund.<br />

Fagan, D. G., Pritchard, J. S., Clarks<strong>on</strong>, T. W., &<br />

Greenwood, M. R. (1977). Organ mercury levels<br />

in infants with omphaloceles treated with organic<br />

mercurial antiseptic. Archives of Disease in Childhood,<br />

52, 962–964.<br />

Federal Register (1982). FDA panel report: Mercury<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining drug products for topical antimicrobial overthe-counter<br />

human use; Establishment of a m<strong>on</strong>ograph.<br />

47, 436–442.<br />

Fido, A., & Al-Saad, S. (2005). Toxic trace elements<br />

in the hair of children with autism. <strong>Autism</strong>, 9,<br />

290–298.<br />

Geier, D. A., & Geier, M. R. (2004). A comparative<br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the effects of MMR immunizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> mercury doses from thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

childhood vaccines <strong>on</strong> the populati<strong>on</strong> prevalence<br />

of autism. Medical Science M<strong>on</strong>itor, 10, 133–139.<br />

Geier, D. A., & Geier, M. R. (2005). A two-phased<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> epidemiological study of the safety of<br />

thimerosal-c<strong>on</strong>taining vaccines: A follow-up analysis.<br />

Medical Science M<strong>on</strong>itor, 11, 160–170.<br />

Geier, D. A., & Geier, M. R. (2006). A meta-analysis<br />

epidemiological assessment of neurodevelopmental<br />

disorders following vaccines administered<br />

from 1994 through 2000 in the United States.<br />

Neuroendocrinology Letters, 27, 401–413.<br />

Geier, M. R., & Geier, D. A. (2003). Thimerosal<br />

in childhood vaccines, neurodevelopment disorders,<br />

<strong>and</strong> heart disease in the United States. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of American Physicians <strong>and</strong> Surge<strong>on</strong>s, 8, 6–11.<br />

Geier, M. R., & Geier, D. A. (2005b). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> potential<br />

importance of steroids in the treatment of autistic<br />

spectrum disorders <strong>and</strong> other disorders involving<br />

mercury toxicity. Medical Hypotheses, 64, 946–954.<br />

Gr<strong>and</strong>jean, P., Weihe, P., White, R. F., & Debes, F.<br />

(1998). Cognitive performance of children prenatally<br />

exposed to “safe” levels of methylmercury.<br />

Envir<strong>on</strong>mental Research, 77, 165–172.<br />

Holmes, A. S., Blaxill, M. F., Haley, B. E. (2003).<br />

Reduced levels of mercury in first baby haircuts of<br />

autistic children. Internati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Toxicology,<br />

22, 277–285.<br />

Hu, L. W., Bernard, J., & Che, J. (2003). Neutr<strong>on</strong><br />

Activati<strong>on</strong> analysis of Hair Samples for the Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of <strong>Autism</strong>. Transacti<strong>on</strong>s of the American<br />

Nuclear Society, 89, 681–682.<br />

Institute of Medicine (US). (2004). Immunizati<strong>on</strong><br />

safety review: Vaccines <strong>and</strong> autism. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC:<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy Press.<br />

James, S. J. (2004). Increased oxidative stress <strong>and</strong><br />

impaired methylati<strong>on</strong> capacity in children with<br />

autism: Metabolic biomarkers <strong>and</strong> genetic predispositi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Fall DAN! 2004 C<strong>on</strong>ference, Los Angeles,<br />

CA, 1–3, 143–159.<br />

James, S. J., Cutler, P., & Melnyk, S. (2004). Metabolic<br />

biomarkers of increased oxidative stress <strong>and</strong><br />

impaired methylati<strong>on</strong> capacity in children with<br />

autism. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Clinical Nutriti<strong>on</strong>, 80,<br />

1611–1617.<br />

Jeps<strong>on</strong>, B. (2004). Underst<strong>and</strong>ing autism: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> physio-<br />

114 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


logical basis <strong>and</strong> biomedical interventi<strong>on</strong> opti<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

autism spectrum disorders. Retrieved Nov. 14, 2007<br />

from www.cbcutah.org.<br />

Lutchmaya, S., Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Knickmeyer,<br />

R., & Manning, J. T. (2004). 2nd to 4th<br />

digit ratios, fetal testoster<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> estradiol. Early<br />

Human Development, 77, 23–28.<br />

Manning, J. T., Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., &<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ers, G. (2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> 2nd to 4th digit ratio <strong>and</strong><br />

autism. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,<br />

43, 160–164.<br />

Mutter, J., Naumann, J., Sadaghiani, C., Schneider,<br />

R., & Walach, H. (2004). Alzheimer Disease: Mercury<br />

as pathogenetic factor <strong>and</strong> apolipoprotein E<br />

as a moderator. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 25, 275–<br />

283.<br />

Mutter, J., Naumann, J., Sadaghiani, C., Walach, H.,<br />

& Drasch, G. (2005). Mercury <strong>and</strong> autism: Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

to the letter of K. E. v. Muhlendahl. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Hygiene <strong>and</strong> Envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

Health, 208, 435–438.<br />

Mutter, J., Naumann, J., Schneider, R., Walach, H.,<br />

& Haley, B. (2005). Mercury <strong>and</strong> autism: Accelerating<br />

evidence? Neuroendocrinology Letters, 26,<br />

439–446.<br />

Mutter, J., Naumann, J., Walach, H., & Daschner,<br />

F. D. (2005). Amalgam: Eine Risikobewertung<br />

unter Berucksichtigung der neuen Literatur bis<br />

2005. [Amalgam risk assessment with coverage<br />

of references up to 2005]. Gesundheitswesen, 67,<br />

204–216.<br />

Palmer, R. F., Blanchard, S., Stein, Z., M<strong>and</strong>ell, D.,<br />

& Miller, C. (2006). Envir<strong>on</strong>mental mercury release,<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> rates, <strong>and</strong> autism disorder:<br />

An ecological study of Texas. Health Place, 12,<br />

203–209.<br />

Rampersad, G. C., Suck, G., Sakac, D., Fahim, S.,<br />

Fop, A., & Denomme, G. A., et al. (2005). Chemical<br />

compounds that target thioldisulfide groups<br />

<strong>on</strong> m<strong>on</strong><strong>on</strong>uclear phagocytes inhibit immune mediated<br />

phgocytosis of red blood cells. Transfusi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

45, 384–93.<br />

Redwood, L., Bernard, S., & Brown, D. (2001).<br />

Predicted mercury c<strong>on</strong>centrati<strong>on</strong>s in hair from<br />

infant immunizati<strong>on</strong>s: Cause for c<strong>on</strong>cern. Neurotoxicology,<br />

22, 691–697.<br />

Slikker, W. (2000). Developmental neurotoxicology<br />

of therapeutics: Survey of novel recent findings.<br />

Neurotoxicology, 21, 250.<br />

Stoiber, T., B<strong>on</strong>acker, D., Bohm, K. J., Bolt, H. M.,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir, R., & Degen, G. H., et al. (2004). Disturbed<br />

microtubule functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> inducti<strong>on</strong> of micr<strong>on</strong>uclei<br />

by chelate complexes of mercury (II).<br />

Mutati<strong>on</strong> Research, 563, 97–106.<br />

Stoller, K. P. (2006). <strong>Autism</strong> as a Minamata disease<br />

variant: Analysis of a pernicious legacy. Medical<br />

Veritas, 3, 772–780.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir, R., B<strong>on</strong>acker, D., Stoiber, T., Bohm, K. J.,<br />

Wang, M., & Unger, E., et al. (2003). Interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

of metal salts with cytoskeletal motor protein systems.<br />

Toxicology Letters, 140–141, 75–81.<br />

Tordjman, S., Ferrari, P., Sulm<strong>on</strong>t, V., Duyme, M.,<br />

& Roubertoux, P. (1997). Androgenic activity in<br />

autism. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Psychiatry, 154, 1626–<br />

1627.<br />

Vahter, M., Akess<strong>on</strong>, A., Lind, B., Bjors, U., Schutz,<br />

A., & Berglund, M., et al. (2000). L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

study of methylmercury <strong>and</strong> inorganic mercury<br />

in blood <strong>and</strong> urine of pregnant <strong>and</strong> lactating<br />

women, as well as in umbilical cord blood. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

Research, 81, 186–194.<br />

Waly, M., Olteanu, H., Banerjee, R., Choi, S. W.,<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>, J. B., Parker, B. S., et al. (2004). Activati<strong>on</strong><br />

of methi<strong>on</strong>ine synthase by insulin-like growth<br />

factor-1 <strong>and</strong> dopamine: A target for neurodevelopmental<br />

toxins <strong>and</strong> thimerosal. Molecular Psychiatry,<br />

9, 358–70.<br />

Windham, G. C., Zhang, L., Gunier, R., Croen,<br />

L. A., & Grether, J. K. (2006). <strong>Autism</strong> spectrum<br />

disorders in relati<strong>on</strong> to distributi<strong>on</strong> of hazardous<br />

air pollutants in the San Francisco bay area. Envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

Health Perspectives, 114, 1438–1444.<br />

Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Rice, C., Karapurkar, T.,<br />

Doernberg, N., Boyle, C., & Murphy, C. (2003).<br />

Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the American Medical Associati<strong>on</strong>, 289,<br />

49–55.<br />

Received: 24 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 21 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 6 February 2009<br />

Mercury <strong>and</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> / 115


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 116–123<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards: An Effective Interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

Students with Disabilities<br />

Chann<strong>on</strong> Horn<br />

University of Kentucky<br />

Abstract: In this review of literature, the author analyzed published research to determine the effectiveness of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se card strategies <strong>on</strong> students with disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> author determined that resp<strong>on</strong>se cards are effective in<br />

increasing the number of opportunities that students resp<strong>on</strong>d, increasing the number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

increasing <strong>on</strong>-task behavior, <strong>and</strong> decreasing inappropriate behavior for the majority of the studies reviewed.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for additi<strong>on</strong>al research are also noted.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies that enable students to<br />

engage in a high rate of active participati<strong>on</strong><br />

offer several educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> behavioral benefits<br />

to a variety of learners. Active participati<strong>on</strong><br />

has been described as “the planned <strong>and</strong><br />

cognizant comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the instructor’s less<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which enables students to participate<br />

overtly in the less<strong>on</strong>” (Pratt<strong>on</strong> & Hales, 1986,<br />

p. 211). Active participati<strong>on</strong> involves students<br />

performing a specific behavior, such as calculating<br />

mathematical equati<strong>on</strong>s or imitating a<br />

desired skill. Although active participati<strong>on</strong> allows<br />

students the opportunity to practice the<br />

skill during the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> fluency phases<br />

of learning, it also allows teachers the opportunity<br />

to m<strong>on</strong>itor student progress <strong>and</strong> provide<br />

feedback while educati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>cepts are<br />

being acquired.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> traditi<strong>on</strong>al form of active participati<strong>on</strong><br />

involves students raising their h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> verbally<br />

stating the answer; however, this method<br />

has numerous drawbacks. First, active participati<strong>on</strong><br />

through the use of h<strong>and</strong> raising <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

actively engages <strong>on</strong>e student while the remainder<br />

of the class is passively engaged in the task.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the enactment of No Child Left Behind<br />

<strong>and</strong> the “highly qualified” teacher requirement<br />

has led to a dramatic increase in<br />

co-teaching which has inundated general educators<br />

with students with a variety of disabil-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Chann<strong>on</strong> Horn, Department of<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 229 Taylor Educati<strong>on</strong> Building,<br />

University of Kentucky, Lexingt<strong>on</strong>, KY 40506-0001.<br />

ities in their classrooms. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the No<br />

Child Left Behind act requires that students<br />

with disabilities receive access to core c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

which has led to an increase of inclusi<strong>on</strong> practices<br />

for students who have more significant<br />

disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher must be providing students with a<br />

variety of disabilities the opportunity to actively<br />

participate in their natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Although h<strong>and</strong> raising may be an<br />

acceptable technique to passively engage students<br />

without disabilities, it may not be a suitable<br />

technique for students who have physical<br />

disabilities or who are n<strong>on</strong>verbal. Furthermore,<br />

Heward’s (1994) review of active student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se strategies revealed that instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

strategies that engage students in high<br />

rates of active participati<strong>on</strong> promote the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of skill development (Fischer & Berliner,<br />

1985; Greenwood, Dequadri, & Hall,<br />

1984) <strong>and</strong> increase students’ rates of <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior (Miller, Hall, & Heward, 1995; Sainato,<br />

Strain, & Ly<strong>on</strong>, 1987), as well provide<br />

instructors with immediate feedback <strong>on</strong> student<br />

performance (Narayan, Heward, Gardner,<br />

Cours<strong>on</strong>, & Omness, 1990). Simply put,<br />

the more opportunities that students have to<br />

actively engage in learning, the more time<br />

they spend learning instead of engaging in<br />

off-task behaviors.<br />

R<strong>and</strong>olph (2007) c<strong>on</strong>tended that the underlying<br />

premise of active participati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the principle asserting that the<br />

learning trial is a fundamental comp<strong>on</strong>ent of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> learning trial, which is found<br />

116 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


in both the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

stages of learning, is derived from three essential<br />

elements. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first element is formulated<br />

by the teacher posing a questi<strong>on</strong> to the<br />

entire class or a specific student. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

element c<strong>on</strong>sists of a resp<strong>on</strong>se to the teacher<br />

directed questi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> final element is the<br />

delivery of teacher feedback. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> time<br />

elapsed between the three trial elements <strong>and</strong><br />

the time elapsed between each trial presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

correlates to the number of opportunities<br />

for students to actively participate in a teaching<br />

sequence. Researchers also have stated<br />

that the pace <strong>and</strong> frequency of learning trials<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributes to the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of student learning<br />

(Heward, 1994). Instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies<br />

that promote active student resp<strong>on</strong>ding, such<br />

as resp<strong>on</strong>se cards, guided notes, <strong>and</strong> choral<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding, assist in capitalizing <strong>on</strong> the number<br />

of complete learning trials that can be<br />

delivered during whole group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(R<strong>and</strong>olph), while maximizing the number of<br />

opportunities that students have to actively<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>d to the stimuli presented.<br />

One active resp<strong>on</strong>ding method that repeatedly<br />

has been found to be an effective <strong>and</strong><br />

efficient means to actively engage students<br />

in instructi<strong>on</strong>al practices is resp<strong>on</strong>se cards.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards, which offer educators a low<br />

cost, low tech tool to increase active student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding during instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s, have<br />

been described as “cards, signs, or other c<strong>on</strong>veyances<br />

simultaneously held up by all students<br />

in the class to display their resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

a teacher presented questi<strong>on</strong> or problem – to<br />

be an effective way of increasing student participati<strong>on</strong>”<br />

(Cavanaugh, Heward, & D<strong>on</strong>els<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1996, p. 403). Berr<strong>on</strong>g, Schuster, Collins, <strong>and</strong><br />

Morse (2007) c<strong>on</strong>cluded that all learners, especially<br />

those with disabilities, can benefit<br />

from the systematic implementati<strong>on</strong> of active<br />

student resp<strong>on</strong>ding techniques (2007). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore,<br />

the purpose of this paper is to review <strong>and</strong><br />

synthesize the published, empirical literature<br />

that has focused <strong>on</strong> using resp<strong>on</strong>se cards as a<br />

means of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding for students who<br />

have an identified physical or cognitive disability<br />

without regard to the severity of the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>, it is believed that substantial<br />

evidence exists to establish resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards as an evidenced-based practice for students<br />

who have been identified as having a<br />

disability.<br />

Method<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> author completed an ERIC search <strong>and</strong><br />

reviewed the Psychological Abstracts that were<br />

identified. Furthermore, she c<strong>on</strong>ducted a<br />

h<strong>and</strong> search of approximately 20 educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> psychological, peer-reviewed journals<br />

<strong>and</strong> reviewed the reference lists located in<br />

cited manuscripts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> procedures described<br />

yielded a total of six relevant studies. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> six<br />

studies all used resp<strong>on</strong>se cards as a means of<br />

engaging students in active resp<strong>on</strong>ding, <strong>and</strong><br />

most discussed the impact that the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards had <strong>on</strong> student participati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior, <strong>and</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

desired skill. Based <strong>on</strong> the criteria outlined in<br />

Horner, Carr, Ahlle, McGee, Odom, <strong>and</strong> Wolery<br />

(2005), resp<strong>on</strong>ses cards should be c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

an evidenced-based practice because the<br />

following guidelines have been met: (a) a minimum<br />

of five studies have adequately documented<br />

the use of experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol in<br />

peer-reviewed journals, (b) the investigati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted by a variety of researchers in<br />

a variety of settings, <strong>and</strong> (c) the investigati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with a minimum of 20 total<br />

participants.<br />

Results<br />

Table 1 provides informati<strong>on</strong> pertaining to<br />

the studies reviewed. Studies illustrating the<br />

effectiveness of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with students in preschool through the<br />

ninth grade, in both inclusive <strong>and</strong> self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments. Participants involved in<br />

the investigati<strong>on</strong>al studies have had varying<br />

degrees of disabilities, with participants having<br />

no identifiable disabilities to students with<br />

severe cognitive disabilities. An ABAB design<br />

was used to exhibit experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol in<br />

five of the studies reviewed. Overall in each of<br />

the six studies, students exhibited an increased<br />

rate of accurate resp<strong>on</strong>ses, while half<br />

of the studies reported an increase in <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> a decrease in the occurrence of<br />

inappropriate behavior.<br />

Although the literature has been exp<strong>and</strong>ed<br />

in recent years pertaining to students with<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards, past<br />

research has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classrooms with students with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

disabilities. In <strong>on</strong>e such study, Narayan,<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards / 117


TABLE 1<br />

Overview of Resp<strong>on</strong>se Card Studies<br />

Study Participants Method Results<br />

Narayan et al. (1990) 4th grade social studies<br />

students with <strong>and</strong><br />

without disabilities<br />

Cavanaugh et al. (1996). 9 th grade science<br />

students with <strong>and</strong><br />

without disabilities<br />

Godfrey et al. (2003) Preschool students with<br />

attending difficulties<br />

Davis et al. (2004) 4 th grade English<br />

students identified<br />

with learning <strong>and</strong><br />

behavior disorders<br />

Horn et al. (2006) Middle school students<br />

identified with<br />

moderate to severe<br />

disabilities<br />

Berr<strong>on</strong>g et al. (2007) Elementary students<br />

identified with<br />

moderate to severe<br />

disabilities<br />

Heward, Gardner, Cours<strong>on</strong>, & Omness (1990)<br />

used resp<strong>on</strong>se cards in a fourth grade social<br />

studies classroom. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers collected<br />

data <strong>on</strong> four dependent variables, including<br />

the rate at which the teacher presented instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the number of student resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

the accuracy of the student resp<strong>on</strong>ses, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

students’ daily quiz scores. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y divided each<br />

class period into three segments. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first<br />

segment c<strong>on</strong>sisted of the instructor delivering<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with the use of an overhead projector<br />

<strong>and</strong> verbally questi<strong>on</strong>ing the learners<br />

after each c<strong>on</strong>cept had been presented. During<br />

the sec<strong>on</strong>d segment, the instructor reviewed<br />

material presented during either the<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se card or h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

remainder of the class was spent taking a quiz.<br />

In the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the instructor<br />

verbally stated a questi<strong>on</strong>; if the students<br />

wished to resp<strong>on</strong>d, they raised their h<strong>and</strong>s. In<br />

the resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> the instructor<br />

first presented a questi<strong>on</strong> to the whole class.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructor then asked students to “write”<br />

their answer <strong>on</strong> their resp<strong>on</strong>se cards. Last, the<br />

ABAB design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards resulted in increased<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se rate, correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

<strong>and</strong> daily quiz scores<br />

ABAB design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards yielded higher rates<br />

of accuracy <strong>on</strong> next day <strong>and</strong><br />

weekly quiz scores<br />

ATD design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards resulted in increased<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior <strong>and</strong> greater rates<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>ding, as well as<br />

diminished occurrences of<br />

inappropriate behavior<br />

ABAB design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards resulted in higher<br />

rates of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>and</strong><br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

ABAB design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards resulted in greater<br />

occurrence of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding,<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior <strong>and</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the targeted skill occurred at<br />

an increased rate<br />

ABAB design Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards resulted in higher<br />

rates of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding, <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> lower occurrence of<br />

inappropriate behavior.<br />

instructor requested that the students hold up<br />

their resp<strong>on</strong>se cards. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructor then<br />

swiftly scanned the students’ resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong><br />

provided feedback. If every student in the<br />

class resp<strong>on</strong>ded accurately to the teacherposed<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>, the instructor provided descriptive<br />

verbal praise in a statement similar to<br />

“Great job, the capitol of Kentucky is Frankfort.”<br />

However, if incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses were<br />

indicated, the teacher provided whole class<br />

corrective feedback, such as “I see many of you<br />

indicated Frankfort is the capitol of Kentucky;<br />

that is correct.” If no student in the class resp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

correctly, the teacher said, “I see no<br />

<strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>ded correctly, the capitol of Kentucky<br />

is Frankfort.” <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of descriptive<br />

feedback allowed the students to hear the correct<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> multiple occasi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Through the use of an ABAB design, the<br />

researchers found that (a) a greater number<br />

of students offered resp<strong>on</strong>ses during the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, (b) correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

occurred at a higher rate during the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> (c) daily quiz scores were<br />

118 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


higher during the resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> as<br />

opposed to the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Furthermore,<br />

the results indicated that, during a<br />

20-min instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>, students resp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

fewer than two times during the<br />

h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. During the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, students resp<strong>on</strong>ded an estimated<br />

30 times per 20-min instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Cumulatively, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards could offer<br />

students an additi<strong>on</strong>al 5,000 opportunities to<br />

actively participate during an entire academic<br />

year.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards also has<br />

been investigated by Cavanaugh et al. (1996)<br />

through the use of review sessi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

at the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of a ninth grade general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> science class. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers collected<br />

data from 23 ninth grade students with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without disabilities during next-day <strong>and</strong><br />

weekly science tests. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> next-day science tests<br />

included 12 fill-in-the-blank questi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

the weekly tests involved 42 fill-in-the-blank<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>sisted of a<br />

30-min science less<strong>on</strong> that was divided into<br />

three parts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first part involved the teacher<br />

lecturing <strong>on</strong> science material. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d segment<br />

involved an experiment or dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

in which the class was actively participating.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> final segment entailed the teacher<br />

reviewing the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the less<strong>on</strong>. During<br />

the passive review c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of the experiment,<br />

the teacher read each key point of her<br />

science less<strong>on</strong> while disclosing it to her students<br />

<strong>on</strong> an overhead projector. After providing<br />

students with the key term definiti<strong>on</strong> expressed<br />

in a complete sentence, the instructor<br />

provided an example or additi<strong>on</strong>al explanati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as necessary before proceeding to the<br />

next point. During the resp<strong>on</strong>se card review<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the teacher c<strong>on</strong>tinued to disclose<br />

key points <strong>on</strong> the overhead projector while<br />

simultaneously reading the informati<strong>on</strong><br />

aloud. However, during the resp<strong>on</strong>se card review,<br />

each key point c<strong>on</strong>tained a blank in<br />

place of the key term or c<strong>on</strong>cept. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructor<br />

prompted students to write the word in<br />

order to complete the point. Once the teacher’s<br />

verbal cue had been given, students were<br />

required to hold their resp<strong>on</strong>se card above<br />

their head. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructor immediately surveyed<br />

the results <strong>and</strong> then provided the class<br />

with the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se before proceeding<br />

to the next questi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results obtained through an ABAB design<br />

indicate that students performed with a<br />

higher degree of accuracy <strong>on</strong> next day <strong>and</strong><br />

weekly fill in the blank quizzes when resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were in effect. Although the<br />

data indicate that resp<strong>on</strong>se cards were effective<br />

in teaching science material to students<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities, the research indicates<br />

that the resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was<br />

also effective in providing the instructor with<br />

immediate feedback <strong>on</strong> the effectiveness of<br />

the less<strong>on</strong> taught. On numerous occasi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during the resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the instructor<br />

was able to identify that teaching was<br />

ineffective when no students were able to provide<br />

an accurate resp<strong>on</strong>se to material previously<br />

taught. Although end of the unit tests<br />

may provide instructors with informati<strong>on</strong> pertaining<br />

to the effectiveness of their teaching<br />

strategy, the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards provides<br />

immediate feedback that can assist instructors<br />

in planning subsequent less<strong>on</strong>s with a higher<br />

rate of efficiency.<br />

In a more recent study, Godfrey, Grisham-<br />

Brown, <strong>and</strong> Schuster (2003) used an alternating<br />

treatments design to assess the effectiveness<br />

of active student resp<strong>on</strong>ding techniques<br />

with five preschool students who previously<br />

had been identified as having attending difficulties<br />

during large group instructi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

researchers compared the effectiveness of a<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al h<strong>and</strong> raising technique to choral<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se cards. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y collected<br />

data to determine which technique was<br />

most efficient in (a) increasing <strong>on</strong>-task behavior,<br />

(b) increasing student participati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

(c) decreasing the occurrence of inappropriate<br />

behavior during a morning calendar<br />

group activity. Each calendar sessi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

of eight teacher-directed questi<strong>on</strong>s pertaining<br />

to the daily calendar or current weather c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Students were required to raise their<br />

h<strong>and</strong>s, resp<strong>on</strong>d in unis<strong>on</strong>, or use their resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards to answer the teacher-directed<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>, depending <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> being<br />

implemented in the study. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se card<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> involved students attaching their resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

to a laminated resp<strong>on</strong>se card. Each<br />

participant was encouraged to make an independent<br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> from four predetermined<br />

choices given to each student following each<br />

teacher-posed questi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> data indicated<br />

that, when resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were in<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards / 119


effect, students resp<strong>on</strong>ded to a greater number<br />

of teacher-directed questi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se cards also was more effective in promoting<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior for all students than<br />

choral resp<strong>on</strong>ding or h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, when resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were in effect, students participated in fewer<br />

occurrences of inappropriate behavior as<br />

compared to the choral resp<strong>on</strong>ding or h<strong>and</strong><br />

raising techniques.<br />

Furthermore, the researchers noted that<br />

the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards allowed the students<br />

to participate in kinesthetic, auditory, <strong>and</strong> visual<br />

modes of learning that can be beneficial<br />

when dealing with more active learners. Allowing<br />

students to manipulate items during instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

may have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the increase<br />

in <strong>on</strong>-task behavior for the students participating<br />

in the study as well as the decrease in<br />

inappropriate behavior, c<strong>on</strong>sidering students’<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> remained focused the materials involved<br />

with their resp<strong>on</strong>se cards.<br />

In the next study, Davis <strong>and</strong> O’Neill (2004)<br />

compared resp<strong>on</strong>se cards to h<strong>and</strong> raising during<br />

a writing skills class with four middle<br />

school students who were identified as having<br />

learning disabilities <strong>and</strong> who were receiving<br />

English as a Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language (ESL) instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers collected data within an<br />

ABAB reversal design during an English class<br />

in which instructi<strong>on</strong> focused <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of writing skills. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers collected<br />

data <strong>on</strong> the rate of student academic resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

percentage of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses, percentage<br />

of trials in which student’s resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

occurred after raising their h<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

occurrence of off task behavior. In additi<strong>on</strong>al,<br />

the researchers collected data <strong>on</strong> fill in the<br />

blank quizzes pertaining to material that had<br />

been taught during instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s that<br />

had occurred the preceding week. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers<br />

also obtained social validity informati<strong>on</strong><br />

pertaining to which resp<strong>on</strong>se technique<br />

was more effective in increasing student participati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> learning. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results of the<br />

study indicated that students had higher rates<br />

of academic resp<strong>on</strong>ses, as well as higher rates<br />

of correct academic resp<strong>on</strong>ses, during resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Substantially higher<br />

quiz scores also were evidenced during the<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se card phases of the study. However,<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se card usage had limited effects <strong>on</strong> the<br />

occurrence of inappropriate behavior for students<br />

participating in this study.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, the social validity data indicated<br />

that students did not prefer to use resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards as a learning tool. However,<br />

when students were asked to elaborate <strong>on</strong><br />

their preference for the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

all comments provided had a negative<br />

c<strong>on</strong>notati<strong>on</strong>. Students indicated that, when<br />

the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was in effect, the<br />

classroom was too noisy, students’ performance<br />

was lower, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e student was<br />

able to resp<strong>on</strong>d to the instructor’s questi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Students indicated they did not like having to<br />

provide a written resp<strong>on</strong>se during the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> social validity data<br />

provided by this study is in direct c<strong>on</strong>tradicti<strong>on</strong><br />

to the literature reviewed. It should be<br />

noted that students participating in this study<br />

were receiving instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> writing skills,<br />

which may have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to their negative<br />

reacti<strong>on</strong>s regarding resp<strong>on</strong>se card usage that<br />

required writing.<br />

In yet another study, Horn, Schuster, <strong>and</strong><br />

Collins (2006) used an ABAB design to determine<br />

the effectiveness of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards in a<br />

middle school special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom to<br />

teach three students with moderate to severe<br />

disabilities how to tell time. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> researchers<br />

collected data <strong>on</strong> four dependent variables<br />

that included the rate of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding,<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior, occurrence of inappropriate<br />

behavior, <strong>and</strong> the accuracy of student resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

During each training sessi<strong>on</strong>, the instructor<br />

asked students 10 telling time questi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

students were required to raise their h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

wait to be called up<strong>on</strong>. During the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, each student had a resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

card that could be manipulated to provide an<br />

answer that would resemble the face of a digital<br />

clock. Results indicated that students’ active<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding rates were c<strong>on</strong>sistently higher<br />

when resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were in effect.<br />

Students had higher percentages of <strong>on</strong> task<br />

behavior during resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with lower occurrences of inappropriate behavior.<br />

Data collected also indicated that students<br />

acquired the telling time skill with substantially<br />

higher rates during all resp<strong>on</strong>se card<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s as compared to the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Social validity data indicated that all stu-<br />

120 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


dents preferred the resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

when compared to the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, when resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were in effect, students provided each<br />

other with immediate feedback <strong>on</strong> their performances.<br />

Students provided feedback <strong>on</strong><br />

their counterpart’s time to resp<strong>on</strong>d, encouraged<br />

them to “focus” after an incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>se,<br />

<strong>and</strong> provided <strong>on</strong>e another with “high<br />

fives” when correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses were recorded<br />

for the group.<br />

Berr<strong>on</strong>g et al. (2007) also used resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards with eight elementary students who were<br />

identified as having a low incidence disability.<br />

One individual participating in this study<br />

did not have the ability to raise his h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

was allowed to tap <strong>on</strong> the table as a means<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, a sec<strong>on</strong>d student<br />

had a hearing impairment <strong>and</strong> did not verbally<br />

communicate. Instructi<strong>on</strong> occurred in a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom during calendar<br />

group. During the h<strong>and</strong> raising c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, students<br />

were required to raise their h<strong>and</strong>s or<br />

independently tap <strong>on</strong> the table in order to<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>d. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> required<br />

students to grasp their resp<strong>on</strong>se cards<br />

in their h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> place then in the appropriate<br />

positi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> their resp<strong>on</strong>se boards<br />

within 10 s of the instructor’s questi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

instructor collected data <strong>on</strong> the percentage of<br />

active resp<strong>on</strong>ding, the percentage of <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior, <strong>and</strong> the occurrence of inappropriate<br />

behavior. During resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

students had higher rates of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

as well as higher rates of <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> occurrence of inappropriate<br />

behavior was lower during resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for the majority of the students participating<br />

in this study.<br />

Although Berr<strong>on</strong>g’s research failed to look<br />

at the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of an academic task, it provided<br />

clear evidence that resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can<br />

be used successfully to increase <strong>on</strong>-task behavior,<br />

decrease inappropriate behavior, <strong>and</strong> elevate<br />

the number of active resp<strong>on</strong>ses expressed<br />

by students during group instructi<strong>on</strong>al activities.<br />

Furthermore, Berr<strong>on</strong>g’s study illustrates<br />

that resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can be used effectively<br />

with students who are physically impaired, as<br />

well as students who have limited verbal communicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards provide access to<br />

the curriculum, which may have been previously<br />

unobtainable in the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards provide<br />

a lost cost, low-tech soluti<strong>on</strong> when compared<br />

to the majority of augmentative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

devices.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Although the published research <strong>on</strong> the effectiveness<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards is limited, the research<br />

available clearly illustrates that resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards can be a beneficial tool in the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al process of students with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> research available provides<br />

clear evidence that resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can<br />

be a low cost, high power instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool. In<br />

general, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards have been shown to<br />

increase student rates of active resp<strong>on</strong>ding,<br />

decrease inappropriate behavior, promote<br />

skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> enhance the occurrence<br />

of <strong>on</strong> task behavior exhibited by students.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> recent enactment of No Child Left Behind<br />

has inundated general educati<strong>on</strong> teachers<br />

with students with an array of disabilities.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards may be the instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool<br />

necessary to promote increased rates of academic<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding am<strong>on</strong>g the diverse populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

inhabiting our classrooms. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> research<br />

provides substantial evidence that resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards are an effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool that<br />

does not require additi<strong>on</strong>al planning time or<br />

reduce the m<strong>on</strong>etary resources that are currently<br />

in short supply. Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

enhance <strong>on</strong>-task behavior <strong>and</strong> skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

but they also provide educators with immediate<br />

feedback <strong>on</strong> their instructi<strong>on</strong>al practices,<br />

which can be used to guide subsequent<br />

less<strong>on</strong> formati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

One of the most important features of resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards is the opportunity they provide<br />

to students. Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards allow students to<br />

actively engage in the acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, fluency,<br />

maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> phases of<br />

learning. Although the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the skill<br />

may be the most beneficial feature to learners,<br />

incorporating resp<strong>on</strong>se cards into the fluency,<br />

maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> phases of<br />

learning are also extremely beneficial because<br />

they afford students the opportunity to resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

while allowing educators to c<strong>on</strong>tinually<br />

assess student learning.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> benefits that the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards<br />

provides to educators also are immense. First,<br />

incorporating resp<strong>on</strong>se cards into a whole<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards / 121


group instructi<strong>on</strong>al sequence has been proven<br />

to reduce the occurrence of inappropriate behavior<br />

exhibited by students with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

disabilities. Although the research does not<br />

indicate a specific reas<strong>on</strong> for this reducti<strong>on</strong>, a<br />

variety of ideas can be hypothesized. One hypothesis<br />

for the reducti<strong>on</strong> in inappropriate<br />

behavior may be attributed to the noti<strong>on</strong> that,<br />

when resp<strong>on</strong>se card c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s are in effect,<br />

students have items to manipulate. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> kinestitics<br />

involved in the most minute of movements<br />

required by resp<strong>on</strong>se cards may assist<br />

students in remaining focused <strong>on</strong> the appropriate<br />

task.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can provide<br />

students with additi<strong>on</strong>al opportunities<br />

to practice the knowledge being taught. Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards allow students to resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s throughout the instructi<strong>on</strong>al less<strong>on</strong><br />

while instructive feedback affords students<br />

with an additi<strong>on</strong>al opportunity to acquire the<br />

knowledge being taught. Furthermore, the research<br />

also indicates that teacher-posed questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

occur at a substantially increased rate<br />

over more traditi<strong>on</strong>al resp<strong>on</strong>ding techniques.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, c<strong>on</strong>trary to the traditi<strong>on</strong>al h<strong>and</strong><br />

raising technique, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards enable the<br />

whole class to answer the teacher-directed<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s instead of a select few who volunteer<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d.<br />

Third, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards provide educators<br />

with immediate feedback <strong>on</strong> the effectiveness<br />

of the instructi<strong>on</strong> being delivered to their students.<br />

Most educators do not systematically<br />

evaluate their performance <strong>on</strong> a daily basis.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards enables instructors<br />

to present material, pose a questi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> immediately<br />

evaluate if students successfully acquired<br />

the necessary informati<strong>on</strong>. Although,<br />

seatwork <strong>and</strong> homework activities allow educators<br />

to evaluate student performance, they<br />

do not provide a clear link to the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

practices being administered in the classroom.<br />

However, if an instructor is introducing<br />

a new topic, providing definiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> simultaneously<br />

delivering questi<strong>on</strong>s to the class as a<br />

whole, judgments can be made about the effectiveness<br />

of the less<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> adjustments can<br />

be implemented immediately.<br />

Last, resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can be a low cost, lowtech<br />

tool to accommodate students with a variety<br />

of disabilities. Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can assist<br />

students who have limited verbal skills in the<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> process as well as assist students<br />

who have limited fine motor abilities by<br />

c<strong>on</strong>structing their resp<strong>on</strong>se cards with high<br />

frequency selecti<strong>on</strong>s that relate to the topic<br />

being discussed.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

In c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>, the available research <strong>on</strong> the<br />

use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards with children with disabilities<br />

was summarized. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> research provides<br />

clear evidence to support the use of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se cards with students who have a variety<br />

of disabilities. Although the author’s<br />

search of relevant material <strong>on</strong>ly produced six<br />

studies, the results are clear. Resp<strong>on</strong>se card<br />

usage can positively affect the number of<br />

opportunities students have to actively resp<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

as well as increase the students’ performance<br />

<strong>on</strong> assessment material. Furthermore,<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can be effective in increasing<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behavior as well as diminishing the<br />

occurrence of inappropriate behavior. Additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research is warranted to determine if<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se cards can be effectively implemented<br />

into various collaborative settings involving<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent with the c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

success of previous studies.<br />

References<br />

Berr<strong>on</strong>g, A., Schuster, J., Collins, B. C., & Morse, T.<br />

(2007). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards <strong>on</strong> active<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> social behavior of students with<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al mental disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Physical<br />

<strong>and</strong> Development Disabilities, 19, 187–199.<br />

Cavanaugh, R., Heward, W., & D<strong>on</strong>els<strong>on</strong>, F. (1996).<br />

Effects <strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se cards during less<strong>on</strong> closure<br />

<strong>on</strong> the academic performance of sec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

in an earth science course. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 29, 403–406.<br />

Davis, L., & O’Neill, R. (2004). Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards with a group of students with learning disabilities<br />

including those for whom English is a<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d language. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis,<br />

37, 219–222.<br />

Fisher, C. W., & Berliner, D. C. (1985). Perspectives <strong>on</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al time. New York: L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Godfrey, S. W., Grisham-Brown, J., & Schuster, J. W.<br />

(2003). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects if three active resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

techniques <strong>on</strong> student participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> social<br />

behavior with preschool children who have special<br />

needs. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of Children, 26,<br />

355–373.<br />

Greenwood, C. R., Delquadri, J., & Hall, R. V.<br />

122 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


(1984). Opportunity to resp<strong>on</strong>d <strong>and</strong> academic<br />

achievement. In W. L. Heward, T. E. Her<strong>on</strong>, D. S.<br />

Hill, & J. Trap-Porter, Focus <strong>on</strong> behavioral analysis<br />

in educati<strong>on</strong> (pp. 58–88). Columbus, OH: Merrill.<br />

Heward, W. L. (1994). Three “low tech” strategies<br />

for increasing the frequency of active student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se during group instructi<strong>on</strong>. In R. Gardener,<br />

III, D. M. Sainato, J. O. Cooper, & T. E.<br />

Her<strong>on</strong> (Eds.), Behavior analysis in educati<strong>on</strong>: Focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> measurably superior instructi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 283–320).<br />

M<strong>on</strong>terey, CA: Brooks/Cole<br />

Horn, C., Schuster, J. W., & Collins, B. C. (2006).<br />

Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards to teach telling time to<br />

students with moderate to severe disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

41, 382–391.<br />

Horner, R., Carr, E., Halle, L., McGee, G., Odom,<br />

S., & Wolery, M. (2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of single subject<br />

research to identify evidenced-based practice in<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71, 165–<br />

179.<br />

Miller, A. D., Hall, S. W., & Heward, W. L. (1995).<br />

Effects of sequential 1-minute time trials with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without inter-trial feedback <strong>and</strong> self-correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> general educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

students’ fluency with math facts. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavioral<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 3, 319–345.<br />

Narayan, J. S., Heward, W. L., Gardner, R., Cours<strong>on</strong>,<br />

F. H., & Omness, C. K. (1990). Using resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards to increase student participati<strong>on</strong> in an elementary<br />

classroom. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 23, 483–490.<br />

Pratt<strong>on</strong>, J., & Hales, L. W. (1986). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of<br />

active participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> student learning. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research, 4, 210–215.<br />

R<strong>and</strong>olph, J. (2007). Meta-analysis of the research<br />

<strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se cards: Effects <strong>on</strong> test achievement,<br />

quiz achievement, participati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> off-task behavior.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 9,<br />

113–128.<br />

Sainato, D. M., Strain, P. S., & Ly<strong>on</strong>, S. R. (1987).<br />

Increasing academic resp<strong>on</strong>ding of h<strong>and</strong>icapped<br />

preschool children during group instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> for Early Childhood, 12(1),<br />

23–30.<br />

Received: 19 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 17 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 12 February 2009<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Cards / 123


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 124–135<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Effects of C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay Procedure <strong>on</strong> the<br />

Halliwick’s Method of Swimming Rotati<strong>on</strong> Skills for<br />

Children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

I˙lker Yılmaz<br />

Anadolu University<br />

Binyamin Birkan<br />

Tohum <strong>Autism</strong> Foundati<strong>on</strong>, Turkey<br />

Ferman K<strong>on</strong>ukman<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> College at Brockport, SUNY<br />

Arzu Özen <strong>and</strong> Mehmet Yanardagù<br />

Anadolu University<br />

I˙lhan C amursoy<br />

Adult MR Womens’ Care <strong>and</strong> Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> Center, Turkey<br />

Abstract: Effects of a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills (i.e.,<br />

vertical <strong>and</strong> lateral rotati<strong>on</strong>) for children with autism were investigated. A single subject multiple baseline model<br />

across behaviors with probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s was used. Participants were three boys, 8–9 years old. Data were collected<br />

over a 10-week period using the single opportunity method as an interventi<strong>on</strong>. Results revealed that all subjects<br />

increased their correct rotati<strong>on</strong> skills significantly during interventi<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>, subjects maintained their<br />

successful skills during first, sec<strong>on</strong>d <strong>and</strong> fourth week of generalizati<strong>on</strong> phases. Results showed that c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay was an effective way of increasing <strong>and</strong> maintaining Halliwick’s method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills of<br />

children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> therapeutic use of water activities or<br />

swimming can facilitate language, self c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

<strong>and</strong> improve adaptive behaviors for children<br />

with disabilities. In additi<strong>on</strong>, these activities<br />

provide an appropriate educati<strong>on</strong>al setting<br />

for many early interventi<strong>on</strong>s (Killian, Joyce-<br />

Petrovic, Menna, & Arena, 1984; Yılmaz, Birkan,<br />

K<strong>on</strong>ukman, & Erkan, 2005a). For example,<br />

Best <strong>and</strong> J<strong>on</strong>es (1974) used swimming as<br />

a main comp<strong>on</strong>ent of their movement therapy<br />

<strong>on</strong> three children with autism. Subjects participated<br />

in an individualized 15-week swimming<br />

program which utilized manipulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

imitati<strong>on</strong>, comm<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> self motivati<strong>on</strong> as a<br />

progress. Researchers reported that the subjects<br />

improved their c<strong>on</strong>fidence <strong>and</strong> body<br />

This study was presented as a poster sessi<strong>on</strong> at<br />

the Annual C<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong> for American Alliance of<br />

Health Physical Educati<strong>on</strong> Recreati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Dance<br />

in Philadelphia, US in April 2003. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be addressed to<br />

I˙lker Yılmaz, Anadolu University, School of Physical<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> & Sports, Eskisehir, 26470, TURKEY.<br />

E-mail: ilkery@anadolu.edu.tr<br />

awareness as well as skills of fr<strong>on</strong>t, back float,<br />

<strong>and</strong> kicking.<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> is a lifel<strong>on</strong>g developmental disability<br />

that causes delays in verbal <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong> as well<br />

as exhibiti<strong>on</strong> of ritualistic <strong>and</strong> compulsive behaviors<br />

(Loovis & Ersing, 1979). Children with<br />

autism have severe communicati<strong>on</strong>, language,<br />

<strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong> problems compared to<br />

their n<strong>on</strong>disabled peers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, teaching<br />

games <strong>and</strong> physical activity is an important<br />

need to improve vital social skills of children<br />

with autism (Leaf & McEaching, 1999; Maurice,<br />

Green, & Fox, 2001).<br />

Autistic children have several stereotypical<br />

motor behaviors (e.g. swinging their bodies<br />

backward <strong>and</strong> forward, playing with their fingers,<br />

moving their head in a circular moti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> jumping). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se behaviors cause communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> learning problems for children<br />

with autism. However, it is possible to reduce<br />

these behaviors via teaching physical activity<br />

<strong>and</strong> games (Leaf & McEaching, 1999; Smith,<br />

2001). Sherrill (1986) stated that some of<br />

these stereotypical behaviors can be used to<br />

124 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


teach skills similar in behavior such as swimming<br />

(e.g., swinging their bodies backward<br />

<strong>and</strong> forward, moving arms up <strong>and</strong> down).<br />

Research indicated that children with autism<br />

have an interest in aquatic activities <strong>and</strong><br />

these activities reported as enjoyable <strong>and</strong><br />

helpful to improve motor skills. Moreover,<br />

these studies reviewed aquatic <strong>and</strong> swimming<br />

activities as popular activities am<strong>on</strong>g the children<br />

with autism (Campi<strong>on</strong>, 1985; Dewey,<br />

1973; Killian et al., 1984; Mosher, 1975; Oppenheim,<br />

1977; Wing, 1976; Yılmaz, Birkan,<br />

Yanardagù , & K<strong>on</strong>ukman, 2005b)<br />

Although past research showed that children<br />

with autism have normal motor development<br />

patterns, a recent study found that autistic<br />

children have very low performance in<br />

motor skills. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, it is recommended<br />

that autistic children be encouraged to participate<br />

in games <strong>and</strong> other physical activities for<br />

motor skill development (Smith, 2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re<br />

are several studies that showed the possibility<br />

of teaching individuals with autism or moderate<br />

to severe intellectual disabilities play skills<br />

such as playing darts (Schleien, Kiernan, &<br />

Wehman, 1981), pinball (Hill, Wehman, &<br />

Horst, 1993), frisbee (Horst, Wehman, Hill, &<br />

Bailey, 1981), Also, Camer<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Capello<br />

(1983) taught specific sport skills to individuals<br />

with autism or severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Moreover, a recent study revealed that<br />

most to least prompting was an effective<br />

method in teaching leisure skills to an adult<br />

with autism (Vuran, 2008).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is an effective method<br />

in teaching children with severe mental disabilities<br />

(Bozkurt & Gürsel, 2005; Schuster,<br />

Gast, Wolery, & Guiltinan, 1988; Stevens &<br />

Schuster, 1987; Tekin & Kırcaali-I˙ftar, 2001;<br />

Wolery, Ault, & Doyle, 1992). In several studies,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay was utilized to teach<br />

play skills such as playing darts, pool, pin<br />

knocking, hotshot basketball, golf, <strong>and</strong> frisbee<br />

to children with autism or intellectual disabilities<br />

(Tekin et al., 2001). C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

was also used to teach lifetime sport skills to<br />

adolescents with severe to profound intellectual<br />

disabilities (Zhang, Gast, Horvat, & Datillo,<br />

1995), playing UNO, croquet, horse shoe<br />

to adolescents or young adult with moderate<br />

to severe intellectual disabilities (Wall & Gast,<br />

1997), <strong>and</strong> teaching bowling skill to an adult<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (Jiabei, Bridget, Shihui,<br />

& John, 2004).<br />

Parallel to these findings in the literature,<br />

research has shown that c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is<br />

an effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedure in teaching<br />

students with autism (Alig-Cybriwsky &<br />

Schuster, 1990; Ault, Wolery, Gast, Doyle, &<br />

Elizenstat, 1988; Browder, Morris, & Snell,<br />

1981; Kleinert & Gast, 1982; McIlvane, Withst<strong>and</strong>ley,<br />

& Stoddard, 1984; Schoen & Sivil<br />

1989; Schuster et al., 1988; Stevens & Schuster,<br />

1987; Tekin et al., 2001; Yılmaz et al.,<br />

2005b).<br />

Although there have been studies about effects<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedures <strong>on</strong><br />

different disabilities in the literature, there<br />

was no research <strong>on</strong> the effects of c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay procedures <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s method<br />

of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills (i.e., vertical <strong>and</strong><br />

lateral rotati<strong>on</strong>) for children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, the purpose of the current investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

was to examine the effectiveness of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedures <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s<br />

swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills of children<br />

with autism who acquired mental adjustment<br />

to the water. Also, maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

effects of the procedure were assessed.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Halliwick’s method of teaching swimming<br />

skills was designed by James McMillan<br />

who taught at Halliwick School for Girls in<br />

Southgate, L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>. This teaching method<br />

based <strong>on</strong> scientific principles of hydrodynamics<br />

<strong>and</strong> body mechanics. It has been found<br />

very safe for people of all ages, <strong>and</strong> individual<br />

with disabilities as well as for the able bodied<br />

(Martin, 1981). Swimmers trained <strong>on</strong> a <strong>on</strong>eto-<strong>on</strong>e<br />

ratio of instructor until complete independence<br />

achieved. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> swimmer-instructor<br />

pair becomes a unit within a group activity<br />

so that the swimmer gains the advantages of<br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong> with his peers while at the<br />

same time enjoying the unobtrusive but c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> of an individual instructor.<br />

Groups became aware of properties <strong>and</strong> behavior<br />

of water <strong>and</strong> how to c<strong>on</strong>trol their own<br />

specific balance problems. Swimmers disengaged<br />

from instructor when they learned initial<br />

mental adjustment to the water <strong>and</strong> balance<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol principles learned. In this<br />

method, games <strong>and</strong> activities were performed<br />

in a vertical positi<strong>on</strong> before the swimmer is<br />

subjected to the massive effect of buoyancy<br />

<strong>and</strong> willing to be in activities with horiz<strong>on</strong>tal<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 125


TABLE 1<br />

Stages of <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Halliwick’s Swimming Method<br />

Phases Skills Purpose<br />

Phase 1 Mental adjustment Adjustment to the water<br />

Disengagement<br />

Phase 2 Vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> Rotati<strong>on</strong>s in the water<br />

Lateral rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

Combined rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

Phase 3 Up thrust C<strong>on</strong>trol of movement in the water<br />

Balance<br />

Turbulent gliding<br />

Phase 4 Simple progressi<strong>on</strong> Movement in the water<br />

Basic progressi<strong>on</strong><br />

positi<strong>on</strong> where the instructor’s h<strong>and</strong>s used to<br />

facilitate balance. Lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> is a movement<br />

of the body around the spine <strong>and</strong> the<br />

swimmers are encouraged to move an arm or<br />

leg or both across the body. Thus, the body<br />

tips over <strong>and</strong> the swimmer faces down in the<br />

water. Finally, the 360 degree roll has to be<br />

practiced until it can be performed easily. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Halliwick’s method has ten stages <strong>and</strong> divided<br />

into four phases. Table 1 shows phases of the<br />

Halliwick’s method of teaching swimming rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Three boys with autism, ages 8-9 years, participated.<br />

Six prerequisite c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were established<br />

for participants before the study:<br />

1) Ability to resp<strong>on</strong>se to visual <strong>and</strong> audio stimuli<br />

for at least 7–10 minutes, 2) Ability to<br />

imitate gross muscle skills, 3) Have regular<br />

restroom habits, 4) Have no open wound <strong>on</strong><br />

the body, 5) Ability to keep swimming suit <strong>and</strong><br />

cap <strong>on</strong> the body <strong>and</strong>, 6) Ability to have mental<br />

adaptati<strong>on</strong> to the water such as jumping,<br />

waiting, sitting, kicking, <strong>and</strong> splashing in the<br />

water. All participants met these criteria.<br />

Osman was an 8 year old boy with autism.<br />

He participated in an early special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

program when he was 3-5 years old. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

he had an individual special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

service twice a week when he was 6 years old.<br />

At the time of the study he was a mainstream<br />

student at a public school for two years. Os-<br />

man had reading, writing, <strong>and</strong> simple mathematical<br />

skills. However, he had difficulty in<br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong>, communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills. Osman did not have any experience<br />

or systematic interventi<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay procedure.<br />

Cemil was an 8 years old boy with autism.<br />

He also participated in an early special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

program when he was 4–5 years old. He<br />

also is mainstream student at a public school<br />

for a year, <strong>and</strong> he had reading, writing, <strong>and</strong><br />

simple mathematical skills. However, he had<br />

problems in communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills. Moreover, he did not get any prior systematic<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

procedure.<br />

Uzan was a 9 year old boy with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

he is the twin brother of Cemil. He participated<br />

in an early special educati<strong>on</strong> program<br />

when he was 4-5 years old. In additi<strong>on</strong>, he had<br />

an individual special educati<strong>on</strong> service four<br />

times a week when he was 6 year old. At the<br />

time of the study, he had been a mainstream<br />

student at a public school for a year. Uzan had<br />

reading, writing, <strong>and</strong> all simple mathematical<br />

skills. However, similar to other participants<br />

he had problems in social interacti<strong>on</strong>, communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> language skills. Uzan did not<br />

have any systematic interventi<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay procedure prior to study.<br />

Trainers<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> phase was applied by four<br />

researchers. All researchers had degrees in<br />

126 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


TABLE 2<br />

Task Analyses for Making Vertical <strong>and</strong> Lateral Rotati<strong>on</strong> Skills<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> prior research experience in<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Settings<br />

All instructi<strong>on</strong>al, probe, maintenance <strong>and</strong><br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred at the Anadolu<br />

University indoor swimming pool. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

swimming pool was divided into two parts with<br />

a rope. At the beginning, all students participated<br />

in fun water activities (e.g. jumping,<br />

splashing water, <strong>and</strong> walking h<strong>and</strong> by h<strong>and</strong>)<br />

with instructors <strong>on</strong> the right side of the pool.<br />

Each student then was transferred individually<br />

to the left side of the pool for instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>, all sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred<br />

in a <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e format for 10 weeks, three<br />

times a week, between 7:30 am <strong>and</strong> 8:30 am.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was also a writing board at the swimming<br />

pool.<br />

Materials<br />

Skills Steps in the task analyses<br />

Vertical Rotati<strong>on</strong> 1. Participant turns his back to the trainer<br />

2. Participant slightly bends his head to the trainer<br />

3. Participant lays back (Trainer supports him behind the shoulders)<br />

4. Participant bends his head <strong>and</strong> tucks his legs to the chest<br />

5. Participant st<strong>and</strong>s still<br />

Lateral Rotati<strong>on</strong> 1. Participant lays back (Trainer st<strong>and</strong>s behind him)<br />

2. Participant places both h<strong>and</strong>s to his abs<br />

3. Participant holds from trainer’s h<strong>and</strong> with his other h<strong>and</strong><br />

4. Participant takes a deep breath <strong>and</strong> close his mouth<br />

5. Participant rotates <strong>on</strong>ce with commend of trainer <strong>and</strong> lays back<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was no special equipment used during<br />

the study. However, a video recorder, video<br />

tapes, data collecti<strong>on</strong> forms, a writing board<br />

<strong>and</strong> pencil were used to collect data. Social<br />

reinforcements were used for motivati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

purposes (e.g., free time game activities, jumping<br />

in the water up ward).<br />

Screening Procedure for Target Behaviors<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> main purpose of this study was to teach<br />

Halliwick’s method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills for children with autism (Martin, 1981).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, these skills were selected from this<br />

method. Each child trained <strong>on</strong> water adjustment<br />

skills before the implicati<strong>on</strong> of study.<br />

Tasks<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> task analyses were developed by all authors<br />

by performing the skills. Later, three of<br />

the authors got together <strong>and</strong> reviewed the task<br />

analyses by performing these skills again.<br />

Some modificati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> revisi<strong>on</strong>s were d<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se task analyses are presented in Table 2.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study was designed as a multiple probe<br />

model to implicate target behaviors efficiently.<br />

However, target behaviors must be selected<br />

according to two important characteristics:<br />

1) Target behaviors functi<strong>on</strong>ally should<br />

be similar to each other; <strong>and</strong> 2) Target behaviors<br />

should be independent from each other<br />

(Alberto & Troutman, 1990). At this point, all<br />

of these selected behaviors are functi<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

similar gross motor tasks that can be taught<br />

easily via c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay method. Besides,<br />

these behaviors are independent from each<br />

other so learning a selected skill does not have<br />

a negative effect <strong>on</strong> the other target skills.<br />

Selected target behaviors are functi<strong>on</strong>ally independent<br />

from each other in the study.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is an errorless teaching<br />

procedure in which the stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol is<br />

transferred from a given stimulus c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g. teacher) to other stimulus c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g. target stimulus). This is a promising al-<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 127


ternative for instructing individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities because the instructor<br />

presents a target stimulus, waits the specific<br />

fixed amount of delay interval, <strong>and</strong> finally presents<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt. This prompt is<br />

then faded by systematically inserting a fixed<br />

amount of time between presenting the target<br />

stimulus <strong>and</strong> providing a c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt<br />

that will ensure the student does the task correctly<br />

(Tekin et al., 2001; Wolery et al., 1992).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is an effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedure because it reduces the probability<br />

of the learner making mistakes. Especially,<br />

0-s c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is a good way of<br />

eliminating the number of mistakes made by<br />

learner. This procedure also provides reinforcement<br />

to the learner, <strong>and</strong> it could be a<br />

more fun <strong>and</strong> positive experience for both the<br />

learner <strong>and</strong> teacher (Tekin et al., 2001)<br />

Experimental Procedures<br />

A 1 to 1 instructi<strong>on</strong>al format was used during<br />

all experimental sessi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were probe,<br />

probe, maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the study. Teacher <strong>and</strong> participants<br />

were face to face in all sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> all participants<br />

were ready in the pool before the start<br />

of the study.<br />

Baseline (A) C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

A probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was implemented before<br />

the training of each target behavior, <strong>and</strong> after<br />

the criteri<strong>on</strong> was reached in training of that<br />

target behavior for a minimum of three probe<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred prior to<br />

training each target behavior <strong>and</strong> after the<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> was met in that target behavior. Each<br />

probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> had a minimum of three c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s. A single opportunity<br />

procedure was used during probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> teacher presented the task directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

recorded the subject’s resp<strong>on</strong>se to steps of the<br />

task analysis. When the subject initiated an<br />

incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>se, performed an incorrect<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se or no resp<strong>on</strong>se, he was interrupted<br />

by the teacher <strong>and</strong> the subject’s resp<strong>on</strong>se was<br />

recorded as a minus () <strong>and</strong> the rest of the<br />

steps in the task analysis were recorded as<br />

incorrect. When a subject performed a correct<br />

step he got a plus () (Brown & Snell, 2000).<br />

For example, the trainer took his/her place in<br />

the pool <strong>and</strong> said, “Cemil, are you ready to<br />

perform lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> movement in the<br />

water?” to get attenti<strong>on</strong> of subject. Once an<br />

affirmative verbal or physical resp<strong>on</strong>se was received,<br />

the trainer delivered the task directi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

“Do the lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> movement in the<br />

water.” <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>n the trainer waited 4-s for the<br />

subject to initiate a resp<strong>on</strong>se. Subject was reinforced<br />

with a descriptive verbal phrase when<br />

he initiated the correct steps in 4-s <strong>and</strong> kept it<br />

15-s. Incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses were defined as not<br />

initiating a step in 4 s, initiating but not completing<br />

in 15-s, <strong>and</strong> initiating an incorrect step<br />

of the task analysis is not c<strong>on</strong>sidered. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

if the first resp<strong>on</strong>se was incorrect then<br />

the rest of steps in the task analysis were recorded<br />

as incorrect (Wolery et al., 1992).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay Instructi<strong>on</strong>al (B) C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Halliwick’s method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills were taught by using a 4-s c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay procedure. Two types of delay intervals<br />

were used: 0-s <strong>and</strong> 4-s delay intervals. During<br />

0-s trials the trainer secured subject’s attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> said, “Osman are you ready to perform<br />

the lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> moti<strong>on</strong> in the water?”<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>n the trainer said, “Osman do lateral rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

moti<strong>on</strong>”, then presented the c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

prompt immediately <strong>and</strong> said, “Osman lay<br />

back.” <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt was determined<br />

according to the performance <strong>and</strong><br />

characteristics of each subject <strong>and</strong> target behavior<br />

individually. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> 4-s delay, trials were<br />

implemented in the same way as 0-s delay<br />

except the trainer waited 4-s before providing<br />

the subjects a c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were six types of subject resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

during instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s: correct resp<strong>on</strong>se;<br />

anticipati<strong>on</strong>s, errors, n<strong>on</strong>wait, wait,<br />

<strong>and</strong> no resp<strong>on</strong>se. Correct waits were defined<br />

as completing a step of the task analysis correctly<br />

within 15-s after the prompt. Anticipati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were defined as initiating a step of the<br />

task analysis before the prompt <strong>and</strong> completing<br />

the resp<strong>on</strong>se correctly within 15-s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re<br />

were three types of n<strong>on</strong>wait errors: initiating a<br />

step of the task analysis before the prompt but<br />

performing it correctly as a topographical error,<br />

initiating a step of the task analysis before<br />

the prompt but not completing it within 15-s<br />

durati<strong>on</strong> as a durati<strong>on</strong> error, performing a<br />

different step of task analysis as a sequence<br />

128 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


error. Wait errors were completing the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

incorrectly or completing the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

within 15-s of the prompting. No resp<strong>on</strong>se was<br />

not initiating a resp<strong>on</strong>se within 4-s of the<br />

prompt. All types of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses, correct<br />

anticipati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> correct waits resulted in descriptive<br />

verbal praise. Finally, all incorrect<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses were ignored <strong>and</strong> the task directi<strong>on</strong><br />

for the next step was provided (Tekin & Kırcaali-I˙ftar,<br />

2001).<br />

Maintenance <strong>and</strong> Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Maintenance data were collected <strong>on</strong>e, two,<br />

<strong>and</strong> four weeks after the instructi<strong>on</strong> had<br />

stopped. Maintenance data showed that the<br />

subjects maintained the rotati<strong>on</strong> skills taught<br />

to them at criteri<strong>on</strong> level <strong>on</strong>e, two, <strong>and</strong> four<br />

weeks after the instructi<strong>on</strong>. Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

across pers<strong>on</strong>s was examined by a pre-post test<br />

design. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred before training<br />

<strong>and</strong> at the end of teaching for each targeted<br />

leisure skill. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted exactly the same but with another<br />

trainer. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> data showed that<br />

all subjects generalized the rotati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

taught to them across people to a certain extent<br />

100%.<br />

Reliability<br />

Reliability data were collected during at least<br />

35% of all experimental sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Interobserver<br />

reliability was calculated by using<br />

point by point method with a formula of the<br />

number of agreements divided by the number<br />

of agreements plus disagreements multiplied<br />

by 100 (Tawney & Gast, 1984).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean percent of the inter-observer<br />

agreement for the vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> skill during<br />

baseline was 98% (90% to 100%); during<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> was 92% (80% to 100%); during<br />

maintenance was 100% <strong>and</strong> during generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

was 97% (90% to 100%). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean<br />

percent of the inter-observer agreement for<br />

the lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> skill baseline during was<br />

100%; during instructi<strong>on</strong> was 96% (80% to<br />

100%); during maintenance was 100%; <strong>and</strong><br />

during generalizati<strong>on</strong> was 100%.<br />

Independent variable reliability (procedural<br />

reliability) was calculated by dividing the<br />

number of teacher behaviors observed by the<br />

number of teacher behaviors planned <strong>and</strong><br />

multiplied by 100 (Billingsley, White, & Muns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1980). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following teacher behaviors<br />

were observed for procedural reliability during<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong>: (a) having the materials<br />

ready, (b) securing the subject’s attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

(c) delivering the task directi<strong>on</strong>, (d) delivering<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt in time (if appropriate),<br />

(e) waiting for the resp<strong>on</strong>se interval,<br />

(f) delivering the correct behavioral c<strong>on</strong>sequences,<br />

(g) waiting for the inter-trail interval.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> same steps were observed during probe,<br />

maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s except<br />

delivering the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompts in<br />

time.<br />

Procedural reliability measures resulted in<br />

an overall percentage of 100% during baseline<br />

for the vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> skill. Procedural<br />

reliability measures resulted in an overall percentage<br />

of 92% (85% to 100%) during instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for the vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> skill. This<br />

teacher implemented maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s with 100% accuracy for<br />

the vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> skill. Procedural reliability<br />

measures resulted in an overall percentage<br />

of 100% during baseline for the lateral rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skill. Procedural reliability measures resulted<br />

in an overall percentage of 96% (80%<br />

to 100%) during instructi<strong>on</strong> for the lateral<br />

rotati<strong>on</strong> skill. This teacher implemented<br />

maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

100% accuracy for the lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> skill.<br />

Results<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Data<br />

Probe <strong>and</strong> training data for Osman, Cemil,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Uzan are shown in Figure 1 through 2<br />

respectively. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> open circles represent the<br />

percentage of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding during<br />

full probe <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s, maintenance<br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>. As seen<br />

in Figures 1 through 2, all subjects met the<br />

criteria after the introducti<strong>on</strong> of 4-s c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se data revealed that c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay was effective <strong>on</strong> teaching<br />

rotati<strong>on</strong> skills of Halliwick method to the<br />

children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong>al data each subject, the<br />

number of training sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the number <strong>and</strong><br />

percentage of training errors, amount of<br />

training time are in Table 3. A total of 32<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 129


Figure 1. Percentage of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses after the prompt for vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> skill during full probe,<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al, maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Closed circles represent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses during full probe, instructi<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Open circles represent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses during generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> approximately 3 hours<br />

<strong>and</strong> 16 minutes training time were required<br />

for all students to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> two rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills. Table 3 shows instructi<strong>on</strong>al data<br />

for each student through the criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Osman needed 9 training sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. A total of 45 minutes<br />

training time was required to reach criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. Cemil needed 14 training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills. A total of <strong>on</strong>e hour <strong>and</strong> 42 minutes<br />

training time was required to reach criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

130 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Figure 2. Percentage of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses after the prompt for lateral rotati<strong>on</strong> skill during full probe,<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al, maintenance, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Closed circles represent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses during full probe, instructi<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Open circles represent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses during generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. Uzan needed 8 training sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. A<br />

total of 51 minutes training time was required<br />

to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. Percentage<br />

of training errors which occurred during<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong>s was between 0% <strong>and</strong> 6%.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 131


TABLE 3<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Data for Each Student Through Criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Students Behaviors<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> main aim of this study was to determine<br />

the effects of a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure<br />

<strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills for children with autism. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow up data was<br />

collected. Results of the study were analyzed<br />

using graphic illustrati<strong>on</strong>s. Results showed<br />

that all subjects increased their correct target<br />

skills in lateral <strong>and</strong> vertical rotati<strong>on</strong> with a<br />

significant amount during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase. Moreover, subjects maintained their<br />

successful rotati<strong>on</strong> skills during the first, sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

<strong>and</strong> fourth weeks of generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

phases.<br />

Literature review shows that c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay is an effective method to teach chained<br />

tasks to individuals with disabilities in using<br />

copy <strong>and</strong> soda machines (Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, Schuster,<br />

& Stevens, 1993); using napkins, spo<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

cup (Collins, Gast, Wolery, Holcombe, &<br />

Leatherby, 1991); reacting positively to newly<br />

introduced people (Collins, Schuster, & Nels<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1992); applying first aid skills for small<br />

wounds, burns <strong>and</strong> bites (Gast, Winterling,<br />

Wolery, & Farmer, 1992) learning kitchen<br />

skills (Griffen, Wolery, & Schuster, 1992; Hall,<br />

Schuster, Wolery, Gast, & Doyle, 1992); life<br />

time sport skills (Zhang, Gast, Horvat, & Datillo,<br />

1995); <strong>and</strong> teaching drink <strong>and</strong> snack<br />

preparati<strong>on</strong> (Bozkurt & Gürsel, 2005). Parallel<br />

to the these findings, this study dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay was an effective<br />

method to teach <strong>and</strong> maintain Halliwick’s<br />

of sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

thru criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Errors thru<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Training time<br />

thru criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Osman Vertical Rotati<strong>on</strong> 4 0% 00:20:35<br />

Lateral Rotati<strong>on</strong> 5 0% 00:25:08<br />

Total 9 00:45:43<br />

Cemil Vertical Rotati<strong>on</strong> 7 6% 00:42:15<br />

Lateral Rotati<strong>on</strong> 7 2% 00:51:27<br />

Total 14 01:33:42<br />

Uzan Vertical Rotati<strong>on</strong> 4 0% 00:23:32<br />

Lateral Rotati<strong>on</strong> 4 0% 00:27:53<br />

Total 8 00:51:25<br />

Gr<strong>and</strong> total 32 03:16:08<br />

lateral <strong>and</strong> vertical swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills to<br />

children with autism.<br />

According to the graphic illustrati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

data, it can be c<strong>on</strong>cluded that all participants<br />

received the same amount of sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> first<br />

<strong>and</strong> third subject did not have any error in<br />

their performance. Only sec<strong>on</strong>d subject performed<br />

with minimal percentage error. C<strong>on</strong>sidering<br />

the difficulties subjects have with attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> skills, this study<br />

revealed that c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay was an effective<br />

method to teach the Halliwick’s swimming<br />

rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. In additi<strong>on</strong>, procedural<br />

reliability measures showed that all teachers<br />

applied c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

between 92%-100%. In the literature it<br />

is recommended that procedural reliability<br />

which is minimally 80% <strong>and</strong> above 90% is<br />

high regarded (Wolery, Bailey, & Sugai,<br />

1988). This study showed that procedural reliability<br />

was highly for teachers during the<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. It can be c<strong>on</strong>cluded that all teachers<br />

efficiently applied the procedures of c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay to teach <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Halliwick’s swimming<br />

rotati<strong>on</strong> skills for children with autism. Also all<br />

participants performed rotati<strong>on</strong> skills very well<br />

in early sessi<strong>on</strong>s with this interventi<strong>on</strong> in a<br />

limited time. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, this procedure is<br />

highly recommended for further studies.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study has two important c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to literature: 1) support of the literature that<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay was an effective method to<br />

teach chain <strong>and</strong> maintained tasks to individuals<br />

with disabilities: 2) first research attempt to<br />

132 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


determine the effects of c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

procedure <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s swimming rotati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills for children with autism. Results of<br />

this study provide several recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for future research. First, 1 to 1 teaching arrangement<br />

<strong>and</strong> single opportunity method to<br />

teach swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills was used. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

results of this study can be replicated using<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al group arrangements <strong>and</strong> other<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods such as direct instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

peer tutoring. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, lateral <strong>and</strong> horiz<strong>on</strong>tal<br />

swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills were selected<br />

from the Halliwick’s swimming educati<strong>on</strong> program<br />

(Martin, 1981). Thus, all children became<br />

ready to participate <strong>and</strong> learn actual<br />

swimming skill patterns. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, it is recommended<br />

to teach different swimming skills<br />

to children with autism for future experimental<br />

research. Third, trainers reported that all<br />

children enjoyed these aquatic drills during<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> children improved their social<br />

<strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> skills with peers compared<br />

to their out of pool behaviors. Fourth,<br />

most important, trainers observed autistic<br />

children had less stereotypical motor behaviors<br />

(e.g., swinging their bodies backward <strong>and</strong><br />

forward, playing fingers, moving head in a<br />

circular moti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong>, jumping) in the water<br />

during training sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sequently, findings of this study indicated<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay is an effective<br />

method of increasing <strong>and</strong> maintaining the<br />

Halliwick’s swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills for children<br />

with autism. Also, it can be c<strong>on</strong>cluded<br />

that teachers can teach many different activities<br />

via these swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills. However,<br />

this study has several limitati<strong>on</strong>s, such as<br />

the characteristics of participants, tests, <strong>and</strong><br />

the measurements that were applied. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore,<br />

further studies should address effects of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure <strong>on</strong> different<br />

sport skills, disabilities, gender, <strong>and</strong> ages of<br />

children with autism.<br />

References<br />

Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (1990). Applied<br />

behavior analysis for teachers. New York: McMillan.<br />

Alig-Cybriwsky, C. A., & Schuster, J. W. (1990). Using<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedures to teach multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 11,<br />

54–59.<br />

Ault, M. J., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., Doyle, P. M., &<br />

Elizenstat, V. (1988). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting procedures in teaching numerical<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> to autistic subjects. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 18, 627–636.<br />

Best, J. F., & J<strong>on</strong>es, J. G. (1974). Movement therapy<br />

in the treatment of autistic children. Australian<br />

Occupati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>rapy <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 21, 72–86.<br />

Billingsley, F., White, O. R., & Muns<strong>on</strong>, R. (1980).<br />

Procedural reliability: A rati<strong>on</strong>ale <strong>and</strong> an example.<br />

Behavioral Assessment, 2, 229–241.<br />

Bozkurt, F., & Gürsel, O. (2005) Effectiveness of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay <strong>on</strong> teaching snack <strong>and</strong> drink<br />

preparati<strong>on</strong> skills to children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 40, 390–400.<br />

Browder, D. M., Morris, W. W., & Snell, M. E.<br />

(1981). Using time delay to teach manual signs to<br />

a severely retarded student. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

of the Mentally Retarded, 16, 252–258.<br />

Brown, F., & Snell, M. E. (2000). Measurement,<br />

analysis, <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong>. In M. E. Snell & F. Brown<br />

(Eds.), Instructi<strong>on</strong> of students with severe disabilities<br />

(5 th ed., pp. 173–206). Columbus, OH: Merrill.<br />

Camer<strong>on</strong>, M. J., & Capello, M. J. (1993). We’ll cross<br />

that hurdle when we get to it. Teaching athletic<br />

performance within adaptive physical educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 17, 136–147.<br />

Campi<strong>on</strong>, R. M. (1985). Hydrotherapy in pediatrics.<br />

L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: William Heinemann Medical Books,<br />

Inc.<br />

Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, W., Schuster, J. W., & Stevens, K. B.<br />

(1993). Teaching employment skills to adolescents<br />

with mild to moderate disabilities using a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 28, 155–168.<br />

Collins, B. C., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Holcombe, A.,<br />

& Leatherby, J. G. (1991). Using c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay to teach self-feeding to young students with<br />

severe/profound h<strong>and</strong>icaps. Evidence of limited<br />

effectiveness. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Developmental <strong>and</strong> Physical<br />

Disabilities, 3, 157–179.<br />

Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Nels<strong>on</strong>, C. M.<br />

(1992). Teaching a generalized resp<strong>on</strong>se to the<br />

lures of strangers to adults with severe h<strong>and</strong>icaps,<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 3, 67–80.<br />

Dewey, M. (1973). Recreati<strong>on</strong> for autistic <strong>and</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

disturbed children. Rockville, MD: Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Institute of Mental Health (ERIC Document<br />

Reproducti<strong>on</strong> Service No. ED 0940495).<br />

Gast, D. L., Winterling, W., Wolery, M., & Farmer,<br />

J. L. (1992). Teaching first aid skills to students<br />

with moderate h<strong>and</strong>icaps in small group instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of Children, 14, 1–18.<br />

Griffen, A. K., Wolery, M., & Schuster, J. W. (1992).<br />

Triadic instructi<strong>on</strong> of chained food preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses; Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 25, 193–<br />

204.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 133


Hall, M. G., Schuster, J. W., Wolery, M., Gast, D. L.,<br />

& Doyle, P. M. (1992). Teaching chained skills in<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-school setting using a divided half instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

format. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2,<br />

257–274.<br />

Hill, J. W., Wehman, P., & Horst, G. (1982). Toward<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> of appropriate leisure <strong>and</strong> social<br />

behavior in severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped youth: Pinball<br />

machine use. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

Severely H<strong>and</strong>icapped, 6, 38–44.<br />

Horst, G., Wehman, P., Hill, J. W., & Bailey, C.<br />

(1981). Developing age appropriate leisure<br />

skills in severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped adolescents. Teaching<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 14, 11–16.<br />

Jiabei, Z., Bridget, C., Shihui, C., & John, L. (2004).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effect of a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure <strong>on</strong><br />

teaching an adult with severe mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

a recreati<strong>on</strong> bowling skill. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Physical Educator,<br />

61, 63–74.<br />

Killian, Joyce-Petrovich R. A., Menna, L., & Arena, S.<br />

(1984). Measuring water orientati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> beginner<br />

swim skills of autistic individuals. Adapted Physical<br />

Activity Quarterly, 1, 287–295.<br />

Kleinert, H. L., & Gast, D. L. (1982). Teaching a<br />

multiply h<strong>and</strong>icapped adult manual signs using a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for the Severely H<strong>and</strong>icapped, 6, 25–32.<br />

Leaf, R., & McEaching, J. (1999). A work in progress:<br />

Behavior management strategies <strong>and</strong> a curriculum for<br />

intensive behaviors treatment of autism. New York:<br />

DRL Books.<br />

Loovis, E. M., & Ersing, W. F. (1979). Assessing <strong>and</strong><br />

programming gross motor development for children.<br />

Clevel<strong>and</strong> Heights, OH: Ohio Motor Assessment<br />

Maurice, C., Green, G., & Fox, R. M. (2001). Making<br />

a difference: Behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong> for autism. Austin,<br />

TX: ProEd.<br />

Martin, J. (1981). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Halliwick Method. Physiotherapy,<br />

67, 288–291.<br />

McIlvane, W. J., Withst<strong>and</strong>ley, J. K., & Stoddard,<br />

L. T. (1984). Positive <strong>and</strong> negative stimulus relati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in severely retarded individual’s c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

discriminati<strong>on</strong>. Analysis <strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 4, 235–251.<br />

Mosher, R. (1975). Perceptual-motor training <strong>and</strong><br />

the autistic child. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Leisurability, 2, 29–35.<br />

Oppenheim, R. C. (1977). Effective teaching methods<br />

for autistic children. Springfield, II: Thomas.<br />

Schleien, S. J., Kiernan, J., & Wehman, P. (1981).<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of an age-appropriate leisure skills<br />

program for moderately retarded adults. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training of the Mentally Retarded, 16, 13–<br />

19.<br />

Schoen, S. F., & Sivil, E. O. (1989). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

procedures in teaching self–help skills: Increasing<br />

assistance, time delay <strong>and</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,<br />

19, 57–72.<br />

Schuster, J. W., Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., & Guiltinan,<br />

S. (1988). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness of c<strong>on</strong>stant timedelay<br />

procedure to teach chained resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

adolescents with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Applied Behavioral Analysis, 21, 169–178.<br />

Sherill, C. (1986). Adapted physical educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> recreati<strong>on</strong><br />

(3 rd ed.). Dubuque, Iowa: Brown Publishers.<br />

Smith, M. J. (2001). Teaching play skills to children with<br />

autistic spectrum disorder. New York: DRL Books.<br />

Stevens, K., & Schuster, J. W. (1987). Effects of a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure <strong>on</strong> the written<br />

performance of a learning disabled student.<br />

Learning Disability Quarterly, 10, 9–16.<br />

Tawney, J. W., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Single subject<br />

research in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Columbus, Ohio: Bell<br />

<strong>and</strong> Howell.<br />

Tekin, E., Kırcaali-I˙ftar, G., Birkan, B., Uysal, A.,<br />

Yıldırım, S., & Kurt, O. (2001). Using c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay to teach leisure skills to children with<br />

developmental disabilities. Mexican <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavior<br />

Analysis, 27, 337–362.<br />

Tekin, E., & Kırcaali-I˙ftar, G. (2001). Özel egùitimde<br />

yanlissiz ögùretim yöntemleri (Errorless learning procedures<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>). Ankara: Nobel Yayınevi.<br />

Vuran, S. (2008). Empowering leisure skills with in<br />

adult with autism: An experimental investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

through the most to least prompting procedure.<br />

Internati<strong>on</strong>al <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 174–<br />

181.<br />

Wall, M. E., & Gast, D. L. (1997). Caregivers’ use of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay to teach leisure skills to adolescent<br />

or young adults with moderate or severe<br />

intellectual disabilities, Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

32, 340–356.<br />

Wing, L. (1976). Diagnosis, clinical descripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

prognosis. In L. Wing (Ed.), Early childhood autism.<br />

New York: Pergam<strong>on</strong> Press.<br />

Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992).<br />

Teaching students with moderate to severe disabilities:<br />

Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies. New York: L<strong>on</strong>gman<br />

Publishing Group.<br />

Wolery, M., Bailey, D. B., & Sugai, G. M. (1988).<br />

Effective teaching: Principles <strong>and</strong> procedures of applied<br />

behavioral analysis with excepti<strong>on</strong>al students. Bost<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Zhang, J., Gast, D., Horvat, M., & Dattilo, J. (1995).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness of a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching lifetime sport skills to adolescents<br />

with severe to profound intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 30, 51–64.<br />

Yılmaz, I˙., Birkan, B., K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F., & Erkan, M.<br />

(2005a). Using a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure<br />

to teach aquatic play skills to children with au-<br />

134 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


tism. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

40, 171–182.<br />

Yılmaz, I˙., Birkan, B., Yanardag, M., & K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F.<br />

(2005b). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effects of progressive time delay<br />

procedure <strong>on</strong> teaching basic progressi<strong>on</strong> swimming<br />

skills for children with autism. Research<br />

Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 76 (1), (Supplement),<br />

A-119.<br />

Received: 19 August 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 26 October 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 9 January 2009<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 135


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 136–149<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Use of Self-Modeling Static-Picture Prompts via a H<strong>and</strong>held<br />

Computer to Facilitate Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring in the General<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Classroom<br />

David F. Cihak <strong>and</strong> Rachel Wright<br />

University of Tennessee<br />

Kevin M. Ayres<br />

University of Georgia<br />

Abstract: This study was designed to evaluate the effects of a combined self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring <strong>and</strong> static self-model<br />

prompts procedure <strong>on</strong> the academic engagement of three students with autism served in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classrooms. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> percentage of intervals engaged academically <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher prompts was analyzed<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>text of a multiple probe across settings design with an embedded A-B-A-B. Results indicated that<br />

students all benefitted from use of the h<strong>and</strong>held computer depicting self-model static-picture prompts. Students<br />

also were able to successfully self-m<strong>on</strong>itor <strong>and</strong> regulate their behavior in multiple settings. Results are discussed<br />

relative to the use of self-model prompts, h<strong>and</strong>held computer, <strong>and</strong> independence provided by self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring.<br />

If students with disabilities are to access the<br />

benefits available in general educati<strong>on</strong> settings,<br />

appropriate supports that maximize participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> classes must be<br />

provided (Hunt & Goetz, 1997). Task engagement<br />

was identified as an essential ingredient<br />

in programs for students with autism (Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Research Council, 2001). One of the<br />

salient features often displayed by students<br />

with autism spectrum disorders is an absence<br />

of, or a poorly developed set of self-management<br />

skills, such as difficulty directing, c<strong>on</strong>trolling,<br />

inhibiting, or maintaining <strong>and</strong> generalizing<br />

behaviors required for adjustment<br />

both in <strong>and</strong> outside of the classroom without<br />

external support <strong>and</strong> structure from others<br />

(Adre<strong>on</strong> & Stella, 2001; Myles & Simps<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2002; Oz<strong>on</strong>off, Daws<strong>on</strong>, & McPartl<strong>and</strong>, 2002;<br />

Tantam, 2003). Despite these difficulties, students<br />

with autism display significant strengths<br />

that when identified appropriately <strong>and</strong> reliably<br />

can lead to enhanced lifestyle outcomes.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se strengths include rote memory, c<strong>on</strong>crete<br />

thinking, <strong>and</strong> the ability to efficiently<br />

process visuospatial informati<strong>on</strong> (Quill,<br />

1995). One method that has been dem<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to David F. Cihak, University of Tennessee,<br />

A412 Bailey Educati<strong>on</strong> Complex, Knoxville,<br />

TN 37996-3442. E-mail: dcihak@utk.edu<br />

strated to be highly effective in teaching students<br />

with autism is the use of visual cues as a<br />

primary form of instructi<strong>on</strong> (Mesibov & Shea,<br />

1996). Visual cues can be represented in the<br />

form of individualized activity schedules comprised<br />

of photographs, pictures, <strong>and</strong>/or symbols.<br />

Researchers have indicated increased academic<br />

<strong>and</strong> task engagement with the use of<br />

photographic activity schedules (Cars<strong>on</strong>, Gast,<br />

& Ayres, 2008; Hall, McClannahan, & Krantz,<br />

1995; Krantz, MacDuff, & McClannahan,<br />

1993; MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan,<br />

1993; Massey & Wheeler, 2000; Spriggs, Gast,<br />

& Ayres, 2007), cooperative group learning<br />

(Dugan, Kamps, Le<strong>on</strong>ard, & Watkins, 1995;<br />

Kamps, Le<strong>on</strong>ard, Potucek, & Garris<strong>on</strong>-Harrell,<br />

1995), choral resp<strong>on</strong>ding (Kamps, Dugan,<br />

Le<strong>on</strong>ard, & Daoust, 1994), <strong>and</strong> self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

(Shearer, 1996). In particular, selfmanagement<br />

strategies have gained<br />

popularity as an alternative to teacher-managed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tingency procedures for students<br />

with disabilities (Cole & Bambara, 2000; Mc-<br />

Dougall, 1998; Rock, 2005; Shapiro & Cole,<br />

1994). Self-management strategies were implemented<br />

successfully for students with autism<br />

spectrum disorders (e.g., Callahan &<br />

Rademacher, 1999; Koegel, Koegel, & Carter,<br />

1999; Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992;<br />

Odom et al., 2003; Wilkins<strong>on</strong>, 2005). In most<br />

studies, self-management programs typically<br />

136 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


involved two or more instructi<strong>on</strong>al comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining (a) self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring including<br />

self-assessment <strong>and</strong> self-recording, (b) selfevaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

including decisi<strong>on</strong>-making <strong>and</strong><br />

goal setting, <strong>and</strong> (c) self-reinforcement for<br />

goal attainment.<br />

An important benefit of self-management is<br />

the focus <strong>on</strong> skill building to teach students to<br />

be more independent, self-reliant, <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>sible<br />

for their own classroom behavior.<br />

By learning self-management techniques, students<br />

can become more self-directed <strong>and</strong> less<br />

dependent <strong>on</strong> external c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuous<br />

supervisi<strong>on</strong>. Further, by teaching students<br />

to engage in a positive behavior in place of an<br />

undesirable behavior, students can have c<strong>on</strong>current<br />

benefits of improving academic performance.<br />

Self-management also provided students<br />

with an opportunity to participate in the<br />

development <strong>and</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong> of their behavior<br />

management programs, an important<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> for high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing students<br />

with autism spectrum disorders (Myles &<br />

Simps<strong>on</strong>, 2003). Shifting the resp<strong>on</strong>sibility for<br />

managing behavior from teachers <strong>and</strong> other<br />

external sources is well suited for students<br />

with autism who value locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong><br />

structure (Klin & Volkmar, 2000). Self-management<br />

has been c<strong>on</strong>sidered a pivotal skill<br />

that can help generalize adaptive behavior,<br />

promote aut<strong>on</strong>omy, <strong>and</strong> produce broad behavioral<br />

improvements across various c<strong>on</strong>texts<br />

for many children with autism spectrum disorders<br />

(Koegel, Harrower, & Koegel, 1999;<br />

Lee, Simps<strong>on</strong>, & Shogren, 2007). In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

researchers have suggested that self-management<br />

can be useful in facilitating the inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with autism in the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom (McDougall, 1998).<br />

However, in spite of its potential usefulness<br />

<strong>and</strong> current trends in ensuring that students<br />

with disabilities have increased access to the<br />

general curriculum, there is a lack of research<br />

<strong>on</strong> the effects of self-management in students<br />

with autism who are being educated in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms (McDougall, 1998).<br />

Lee et al.’s (2007) meta-analysis regarding the<br />

effects of self-management for students with<br />

autism, more than half of the studies reviewed<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in school settings <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the studies occurred in a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom. Moreover, no studies used selfmanagement<br />

methods to increase academic<br />

performance in general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms.<br />

Accordingly, there is an obvious need for further<br />

research in these areas.<br />

Lee et al. (2007) also noted that self-management<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> materials had included<br />

tangible, visual, token systems, <strong>and</strong> paper recording<br />

materials to successfully teach students<br />

with autism self-management skills.<br />

However, <strong>on</strong>ly four studies from 1992 through<br />

2001 incorporated the use of tangibles or visual<br />

materials. Picture prompts have been<br />

used extensively for students with moderate<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities for task acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., Robins<strong>on</strong>-Wils<strong>on</strong>, 1977), generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., Wacker, Berg, Berrie, &<br />

Swatta, 1985) <strong>and</strong> maintenance (e.g., Cuvo,<br />

Jacobi, & Sipko, 1981). Picture prompts also<br />

were used to teach students functi<strong>on</strong>al (e.g.,<br />

Thinesen & Bryan, 1981), vocati<strong>on</strong>al (e.g.,<br />

Sowers, Verdi, Bourbeau, & Sheehan, 1985;<br />

Wacker, & Berg, 1983), community (e.g.,<br />

Cihak, Alberto, Taber-Doughty, & Gama,<br />

2006) <strong>and</strong> domestic skills (e.g., Bates, Cuvo,<br />

Miner, & Korbeck, 2001; S<strong>and</strong>ers & Parr,<br />

1989). However, much of the literature <strong>on</strong><br />

picture prompts for students with autism have<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> schedules, transiti<strong>on</strong>ing, <strong>and</strong> helping<br />

students learn to transiti<strong>on</strong> independently<br />

between tasks (e.g. Bryan & Gast, 2000; Mac-<br />

Duff et al., 1993; Spriggs et al., 2007). Few<br />

studies have examined the use of picture<br />

prompts or visual materials to improve behavioral<br />

regulati<strong>on</strong> in more academic envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

for students with autism.<br />

Adapting some of the successful features of<br />

picture activity schedules <strong>and</strong> self-management<br />

techniques to improve student independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> work completi<strong>on</strong>, the current investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

focused <strong>on</strong> academic engagement<br />

in a general educati<strong>on</strong> setting. In general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

settings there are several key behaviors<br />

to which students need to attend in order<br />

to be successful. Because of the complexity of<br />

multiple c<strong>on</strong>current behavioral requirements<br />

in an academic setting (e.g. sitting quietly,<br />

reading, writing, following instructi<strong>on</strong>s), the<br />

current investigati<strong>on</strong> incorporated the use of<br />

self-modeled picture-prompts, similar to those<br />

proposed by MacDuff et al. (1993).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>temporary self-modeling literature<br />

has primarily focused <strong>on</strong> video self-modeling.<br />

Video self-modeling is a specific applicati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

video modeling that allows the student to ob-<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 137


serve him or herself performing a behavior<br />

successfully (Dowrick, 1999). Some researchers<br />

have suggested the utility of a variati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

video modeling called video prompting which<br />

involves the individual watching a video dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

of a model performing specific<br />

steps of a skill or behavior (Cihak et al.,<br />

2006).Unlike video modeling, video prompting<br />

involves the student observing <strong>on</strong>ly separate<br />

steps of a task or behavior rather than<br />

watching the dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of the entire task.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, video prompting incorporates<br />

both c<strong>on</strong>tinuous dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s of a behavioral<br />

sequence <strong>and</strong> static-pictures (individual<br />

frames of the video) which show close-up images<br />

(Alberto, Cihak, & Gama, 2005). Alberto<br />

et al. noted that static-pictures allow for a<br />

stati<strong>on</strong>ary focus of relevant features <strong>and</strong> the<br />

absence of attenti<strong>on</strong> diverting moti<strong>on</strong>s, which<br />

may benefit students who have difficulty maintaining<br />

task engagement.<br />

While video self-modeling has proven effective<br />

in a variety of c<strong>on</strong>texts, <strong>on</strong>ly two studies<br />

have specifically evaluated video modeling to<br />

improve task engagement (Coyle & Cole,<br />

2004; Hagiwara & Myles, 1999). Coyle <strong>and</strong><br />

Cole examined the effects of a video self-modeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring interventi<strong>on</strong> program<br />

<strong>on</strong> the off-task classroom behavior of<br />

three students with autism. Students watched<br />

a video of themselves “working very well” while<br />

the teacher paired a picture communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

card depicting “working” <strong>on</strong> a classroom task<br />

(p. 8). Hagiwara <strong>and</strong> Myles also used video<br />

modeling to target <strong>on</strong>-task behavior for <strong>on</strong>e<br />

student with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> interventi<strong>on</strong> incorporated<br />

video self-modeling with an interactive<br />

computer-based social story. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> student<br />

read <strong>and</strong> listened to the social story <strong>and</strong> then<br />

watched a brief video of them performing<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behaviors prior to entering the lunchroom<br />

<strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

results indicated the interventi<strong>on</strong> had minimal<br />

impact <strong>on</strong> the student’s <strong>on</strong>-task performance.<br />

It may be possible that the student did<br />

not acquire adequately the informati<strong>on</strong> from<br />

the model through watching <strong>and</strong> attending to<br />

the model. While these previous results are<br />

mixed, they suggest a possible way for incorporating<br />

self-modeling into behavioral regulati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s in the form of static picture<br />

prompts.<br />

Cihak, Kessler, <strong>and</strong> Alberto (2007, 2008)<br />

pi<strong>on</strong>eered the use of a h<strong>and</strong>held computer<br />

(e.g., pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant) to display selfmodeled<br />

static-pictures to prompt students<br />

with severe disabilities to complete complex<br />

tasks <strong>and</strong> to transiti<strong>on</strong> independently from<br />

task-to-task <strong>and</strong> place-to-place. Because h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computers have the capacity to display<br />

sequences of images, they provide a unique,<br />

unobtrusive means for displaying picture<br />

prompts to students. Further, since they are<br />

about the size of a large cell ph<strong>on</strong>e, students<br />

can take the h<strong>and</strong>held computer with them<br />

anywhere, which permits a student to view<br />

their models as often as needed to facilitate<br />

learning.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study was to examine<br />

the use of a h<strong>and</strong>held computer to deliver<br />

self-model static-picture prompts to facilitate<br />

the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring for students<br />

with high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism in a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom. Specifically, what were the<br />

effects self-modeling static-picture prompts via<br />

a h<strong>and</strong>held computer to increase task engagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> reduce teacher prompts for students<br />

with high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism in the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classroom? In additi<strong>on</strong>, what<br />

was the social validity of using a h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer to facilitate self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring in the<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom?<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Setting<br />

Three middle-school students diagnosed with<br />

high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism participated in this<br />

study. Students were diagnosed by their physician.<br />

All students dem<strong>on</strong>strated (a) difficulties<br />

initiating <strong>and</strong> attending to task, (b) participated<br />

in all general educati<strong>on</strong> classes, (c)<br />

no hearing or visi<strong>on</strong> impairments that impeded<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>, (d) agreeing participate in<br />

the study, <strong>and</strong> (e) parental permissi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Adam was an 11-year-old student in the<br />

sixth grade. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> administrati<strong>on</strong> of the Childhood<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Rating Scale (CARS; Schopler,<br />

Reichler, & Renner, 1988) placed Adam<br />

within the high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autistic range.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children<br />

(WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991) indicated a fullscale<br />

IQ equivalence score of 108. Jordan also<br />

was a 11-years-old in the sixth grade. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Gilliam<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Rating Scale (GARS; Gilliam,<br />

138 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


1995) placed Jordan within the high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

autistic range <strong>and</strong> the WISC-III (Wechsler)<br />

indicated a full-scale IQ equivalence<br />

score of 72. Richard was 13-years-old in the<br />

seventh grade. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> administrati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

GARS (Gilliam) placed Richard within the<br />

high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autistic range <strong>and</strong> the WISC-<br />

III (Wechsler) indicated a full-scale IQ equivalence<br />

score of 105.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, nine general educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

two special educati<strong>on</strong> teachers participated.<br />

Although all students were in the same school<br />

system, Adam <strong>and</strong> Richard both attend the<br />

same middle school, whereas Jordan attended<br />

a different middle school. All students were<br />

fully included <strong>and</strong> participated in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes throughout the entire school<br />

day. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> average class size was 27 students. All<br />

phases of this study occurred in the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. For each student three<br />

courses targeted. Adam’s classes included language<br />

arts, reading <strong>and</strong> math. For Jordan, his<br />

three classes were social studies, language arts<br />

<strong>and</strong> math, <strong>and</strong> Richard’s classes were language<br />

arts, science <strong>and</strong> math.<br />

Materials<br />

Three colored-photos were taken of each student<br />

using a Can<strong>on</strong> PowerShot A580 ® digital<br />

camera. Pictures showed the student self-modeling<br />

task engagement. Photos included the<br />

student writing, reading, <strong>and</strong> watching <strong>and</strong><br />

listening to their teacher. Photos then were<br />

downloaded <strong>and</strong> inserted into a PowerPoint ®<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong>. Only <strong>on</strong>e photo was used per<br />

slide. A different photo showing the student<br />

modeling task engagement was displayed every<br />

30 s. Lastly, the presentati<strong>on</strong> was downloaded<br />

into the HP iPAQ Mobile Media Compani<strong>on</strong><br />

® h<strong>and</strong>held computer. A plastic<br />

business card holder was used to hold the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held device upright <strong>on</strong> the student’s<br />

desk to be viewed easily. In additi<strong>on</strong>, a selfrecording<br />

3 5 in. index card was provided,<br />

which included 10 numbered “yes” or “no”<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses for students to circle if they were<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating task engagement.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Measurement <strong>and</strong> Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> target behavior for all students was task<br />

engagement. Task engagement was defined as<br />

(a) being in <strong>on</strong>e’s seat, (b) looking at the<br />

materials or teacher as requested, (c) writing<br />

numbers or words related to the assigned task,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (d) complying with instructi<strong>on</strong>s with 4 s.<br />

Task engagement data were collected during<br />

the first 15 min of class time. For all sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the teacher recorded the occurrences of the<br />

student’s task engagement via paper <strong>and</strong> pencil<br />

using a c<strong>on</strong>tinuous 15 s partial-interval recording<br />

technique. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> number of task engagement<br />

occurrence intervals was then<br />

divided by the total number of intervals possible<br />

(i.e., 60 intervals) <strong>and</strong> then multiplied by<br />

100 to calculate a percentage of task enragement<br />

occurrence intervals. In additi<strong>on</strong>, event<br />

recording was used to record the number of<br />

times the teacher either verbally redirected<br />

the student <strong>on</strong>-task or restated an instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

directive throughout the entire class<br />

period (50 min).<br />

Experimental Design<br />

A multiple probe across settings with an embedded<br />

A-B-A-B (Barlow & Hersen, 1984) design<br />

was used to examine the efficacy of the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held device <strong>on</strong> a student’s task engagement<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher<br />

directed prompts. This design allowed sequential<br />

applicati<strong>on</strong>, comparis<strong>on</strong> effectiveness,<br />

<strong>and</strong> an opportunity to replicate the effects<br />

of the h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture<br />

prompts within students. In the student’s first<br />

classroom setting, the study included four<br />

phases: baseline, h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture<br />

prompts interventi<strong>on</strong>, no h<strong>and</strong>held interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a reimplementati<strong>on</strong> of the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. In subsequent classroom<br />

settings, the two phases were baseline <strong>and</strong> the<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> of the h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling<br />

picture prompts interventi<strong>on</strong>. Prior to<br />

baseline, a pretraining period occurred during<br />

which the students were taught to operate<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held computer, to press the hardware<br />

butt<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> to view the pictorial Power-<br />

Point ® presentati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Experimental Procedures<br />

Pretraining. Prior to baseline, students participated<br />

in a pretraining phase. Students<br />

were instructed <strong>on</strong> how to operate the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y were instructed to phys-<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 139


ically turn <strong>on</strong> the device <strong>and</strong> to select the<br />

hardware butt<strong>on</strong> to start the pictorial Power-<br />

Point ® presentati<strong>on</strong>. A Model-Lead-Test procedure<br />

(Adams & Engelmann, 1996) was used<br />

to instruct the students <strong>on</strong> the use of the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> teacher modeled<br />

the necessary steps for accessing the presentati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

led the students as they accessed <strong>and</strong><br />

viewed the pictorial presentati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> tested<br />

the student’s performance <strong>on</strong> using the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer <strong>and</strong> accessing the picture presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

independently. Students were c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

trained if they could independently<br />

access the pictorial presentati<strong>on</strong> for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

trials.<br />

Baseline. During baseline, students were<br />

presented with the typical class milieu. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

number of task engagement occurrences during<br />

the first 15 min of class <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

teacher prompts throughout the entire class<br />

were recorded. During the first 15 min of<br />

class, students were expected to complete two<br />

tasks: writing less<strong>on</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., less<strong>on</strong><br />

objective, homework assignments) in their<br />

agenda <strong>and</strong> complete a warm-up activity. Data<br />

were collected until a stable baseline was<br />

achieved for a minimum of five sessi<strong>on</strong>s. No<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al feedback, prompting, or cueing occurred<br />

during the first 15 min of class time.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture prompts. At<br />

the beginning of each class period, students<br />

were provided with the h<strong>and</strong>held computer<br />

including the self-modeling static-picture<br />

prompt presentati<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> device was placed<br />

in a plastic business card holder in the corner<br />

of the student’s desk. Students were prompted<br />

to activate the device. After the students activated<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held computer, the pictorial<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong> showed photos of the student<br />

self-modeling task engagement behavior. Photos<br />

depicted a different self-modeling staticpicture<br />

every 30 s <strong>and</strong> the presentati<strong>on</strong> repeated<br />

automatically throughout the entire<br />

class period. When the self-model picture was<br />

displayed, the student self-recorded by circling<br />

either “yes” or “no” <strong>on</strong> a 3 5 in. index<br />

card if they were dem<strong>on</strong>strating task engagement.<br />

This process c<strong>on</strong>tinued until the student<br />

reached a criteri<strong>on</strong> of 90% occurrence<br />

intervals of task engagement for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

No h<strong>and</strong>held pictorial prompting. Similar to<br />

the baseline phase, the number of task en-<br />

gagement occurrences <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

teacher prompts were recorded for each student.<br />

Data were collected for a minimum of<br />

three sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Moreover, no additi<strong>on</strong>al feedback,<br />

prompting, or cueing occurred during<br />

the first 15 min of class time.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture prompts reinstated.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> criteri<strong>on</strong> to reinstate the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase occurred when the mean of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> withdrawal phase returned to<br />

within close proximity of the mean of the<br />

baseline phase <strong>and</strong> trended in a c<strong>on</strong>tra-therapeutic<br />

directi<strong>on</strong>. Similar to the previous h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

self-modeling picture prompts phase, students<br />

were provided with the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer, instructed to turn <strong>on</strong> the device at<br />

the beginning of class <strong>and</strong> self-record. Student<br />

then pressed the butt<strong>on</strong> to play <strong>and</strong> watch the<br />

pictorial PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong> of themselves<br />

modeling task engagement behaviors,<br />

as well as, self-recoding every 30 s.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> procedures. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> criteria to<br />

generalize the interventi<strong>on</strong> across settings occurred<br />

when the student reached of 90% occurrence<br />

intervals of task engagement for<br />

three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s in the first classroom<br />

during the reinstatement of the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

self-modeling picture prompts (B 2). Similar<br />

to h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture<br />

prompts phase, the student was provided the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer <strong>and</strong> prompted to activate<br />

the device <strong>and</strong> to watch the pictorial Power-<br />

Point presentati<strong>on</strong> of them self-modeling task<br />

engagement in the sec<strong>on</strong>d classroom. After<br />

the student reach criteri<strong>on</strong>, the h<strong>and</strong>held selfmodeling<br />

picture prompts interventi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

then introduced in the third classroom.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Following the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the study, all participating<br />

teachers completed the Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Rating Profile (IRP)-15 (Martens, Witt,<br />

Elliot, & Darveaux, 1985) to assess the social<br />

validity <strong>and</strong> acceptability of the use of h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

self-modeling picture prompts to increase<br />

task engagement <strong>and</strong> reduce teacher<br />

directed prompts for students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> IRP-15 is a<br />

15-item Likert-type scale that assesses general<br />

acceptability of interventi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Likert scale<br />

ranges from <strong>on</strong>e-str<strong>on</strong>gly disagrees to sixstr<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

agrees. Scores generated by the<br />

IRP-15 range from 15 to 90. Higher scores<br />

140 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


indicate better acceptance of interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> ratings above 52.5 were c<strong>on</strong>sidered to<br />

reflect acceptability by the rater (Brock & Elliott,<br />

1987). In additi<strong>on</strong>, the participating students’<br />

social acceptability was assessed using<br />

the Student Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong> Acceptability<br />

<strong>and</strong> Importance of Effects Survey for grades<br />

four through six (Lane & Beebe-Frankenberger,<br />

2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> students’ survey c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

of a 14-item Likert-type scale that assesses general<br />

acceptability of interventi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Likert<br />

scale ranges from <strong>on</strong>e-str<strong>on</strong>gly disagrees to<br />

seven-str<strong>on</strong>gly agrees. Following the 14-items,<br />

an open-ended questi<strong>on</strong> asked students,<br />

“What else do you think?” <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Likert scale<br />

ranges from <strong>on</strong>e-str<strong>on</strong>gly disagrees to sevenstr<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

agrees. Scores generated by the student<br />

survey range from 7 to 98. Higher scores<br />

indicate better acceptance of interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> ratings above 70 were c<strong>on</strong>sidered to reflect<br />

acceptability by the rater.<br />

Reliability<br />

Interobserver reliability data <strong>and</strong> procedural<br />

reliability data were collected simultaneously<br />

by the primary investigator <strong>and</strong> the classroom<br />

teacher. Interobserver <strong>and</strong> procedural reliability<br />

data were collected during 33% of baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> each c<strong>on</strong>current phase. Observers<br />

independently <strong>and</strong> simultaneously recorded<br />

task engagement. Interobserver agreement<br />

was calculated by dividing the number of intervals<br />

of agreements by the number of intervals<br />

of agreements plus disagreements <strong>and</strong><br />

multiplying by 100. Interobserver reliability<br />

ranged from 95 to 100%, with a mean of 97%<br />

agreement. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> mean interobserver reliability<br />

agreement for each student across c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

was Adam, 100%; Jordan, 97%; <strong>and</strong> Richard,<br />

95%.<br />

Procedural reliability measures verified the<br />

teachers’ performance of providing the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

self-model picture prompting device to<br />

the student <strong>and</strong> prompting the student to activate<br />

the device. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> procedural agreement<br />

level was calculated by dividing the number of<br />

observed teacher behaviors by the number of<br />

planned teacher behaviors <strong>and</strong> multiplying by<br />

100 (Billingsley, White, & Muns<strong>on</strong>, 1980).<br />

Procedural reliability was 100%.<br />

Results<br />

Figures 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 3 display the students’ percentage<br />

of task engagement <strong>and</strong> number of<br />

teacher-directed verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong><br />

without the h<strong>and</strong>held computer <strong>and</strong> across<br />

classroom settings. Overall, the students’<br />

mean percentage of task engagement during<br />

baseline was 29% <strong>and</strong> increased to a mean of<br />

94% intervals of task engagement during the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompting phase. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

mean number of teacher-directed verbal<br />

prompts during baseline was 26.7 <strong>and</strong> decreased<br />

to a mean of 3.3 during the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

picture prompting phase.<br />

Figure 1 shows Adam’s percentage of task<br />

engagement <strong>and</strong> number of teacher-directed<br />

verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong> without the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

self-modeled picture prompt <strong>and</strong> across<br />

classroom settings. During baseline, Adam<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated a mean of 39% occurrence intervals<br />

of task engagement <strong>and</strong> his teacher<br />

verbally prompted him a mean of 33 times in<br />

the language arts classroom. During the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

picture prompting phase, Adam increased<br />

task engagement to a mean of 91%<br />

(range 77–100%) <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

teacher prompts decreased to a mean of 5<br />

occurrences (range 1–15). When the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was withdrawn, Adam’s task<br />

engagement decreased to a mean of 29%<br />

(range 23–40%) <strong>and</strong> teacher prompts increased<br />

to a mean of 26 (range 22–34).<br />

However, when the h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompting<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was reintroduced, Adam’s<br />

task engagement increased to a mean of 97%<br />

(range 95–98%) <strong>and</strong> the mean number of<br />

teacher prompts decreased to 3 (range <br />

1–4). During reading, Adam’s task engagement<br />

was a mean of 35% occurrence intervals<br />

<strong>and</strong> the mean number of teacher prompts was<br />

23 during baseline. When the h<strong>and</strong>held picture<br />

prompt was implemented, Adam’s task<br />

engagement increased to a mean of 95%<br />

(range 90–100%) occurrence intervals <strong>and</strong><br />

the number of teacher prompts decreased to a<br />

mean of 2.6 (range 1–4) occurrences. During<br />

math, Adam’s task engagement was a<br />

mean of 27% occurrence intervals <strong>and</strong> the<br />

mean number of teacher prompts was 29 during<br />

baseline. When the h<strong>and</strong>held picture<br />

prompt was implemented, Adam’s task engagement<br />

increased to a mean of 94%<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 141


Figure 1. Adam’s percentage of task engagement <strong>and</strong> number of teacher-directed verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong><br />

without the h<strong>and</strong>held computer across classroom settings.<br />

(range 87–100%) occurrence intervals <strong>and</strong><br />

the number of teacher prompts decreased to a<br />

mean of 3.6 (range 1–7) occurrences.<br />

Figure 2 shows Jordan’s percentage of task<br />

engagement <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher-di-<br />

rected verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong> without the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeled picture prompt <strong>and</strong><br />

across classroom settings. During baseline,<br />

Jordan dem<strong>on</strong>strated a mean of 38% occurrence<br />

intervals of task engagement <strong>and</strong> his<br />

142 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Figure 2. Jordan’s percentage of task engagement <strong>and</strong> number of teacher-directed verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong><br />

without the h<strong>and</strong>held computer across classroom settings.<br />

teacher verbally prompted him a mean of 26<br />

times in the social studies classroom. During<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompting phase, Jordan<br />

increased task engagement to a mean of<br />

95% (range 88–98%) <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

teacher prompts decreased to a mean of three<br />

occurrences (range 1–12). When the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was withdrawn, Jordan’s<br />

task engagement decreased to a mean of 26%<br />

(range 20–33%) <strong>and</strong> teacher prompts increased<br />

to a mean of 18 (range 17–19).<br />

However, when the h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompt-<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 143


ing interventi<strong>on</strong> was reimplemented, Jordan’s<br />

task engagement increased to a mean of 98%<br />

(range 95–100%) <strong>and</strong> the mean number of<br />

teacher prompts decreased to two (range <br />

1–3). During language arts, Jordan’s task engagement<br />

was a mean of 36% occurrence intervals<br />

<strong>and</strong> the mean number of teacher<br />

prompts was 23 during baseline. When the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompt was implemented,<br />

Jordan’s task engagement increased to a mean<br />

of 94% (range 83–100%) occurrence intervals<br />

<strong>and</strong> the number of teacher prompts decreased<br />

to a mean of 3.6 (range 1–9) occurrences.<br />

During math, Jordan’s task engagement<br />

was a mean of 34% occurrence intervals <strong>and</strong> the<br />

mean number of teacher prompts was 24.5 during<br />

baseline. When the h<strong>and</strong>held picture<br />

prompt was implemented, Jordan’s task engagement<br />

increased to a mean of 92% (range <br />

83–100%) occurrence intervals <strong>and</strong> the number<br />

of teacher prompts decreased to a mean of 6<br />

(range 1–13) occurrences.<br />

Figure 3 shows Richard’s percentage of task<br />

engagement <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher-directed<br />

verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong> without the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeled picture prompt <strong>and</strong><br />

across classroom settings. During baseline,<br />

Richard dem<strong>on</strong>strated a mean of 19% occurrence<br />

intervals of task engagement <strong>and</strong> his<br />

teacher verbally prompted him a mean of 30<br />

times in the language arts classroom. During<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompting phase, Richard<br />

increased task engagement to a mean of<br />

84% (range 67–98%) <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

teacher prompts decreased to a mean of 6.5<br />

occurrences (range 1–12). When the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was withdrawn, Richard’s<br />

task engagement decreased to a mean of 24%<br />

(range 20–28%) <strong>and</strong> teacher prompts increased<br />

to a mean of 19 (range 16–22).<br />

However, when the h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompting<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was reimplemented, Richard’s<br />

task engagement increased to a mean of<br />

98% (range 97–100%) <strong>and</strong> the mean number<br />

of teacher prompts decreased to two<br />

(range 1–3). During science, Richard’s task<br />

engagement was a mean of 18% occurrence<br />

intervals <strong>and</strong> the mean number of teacher<br />

prompts was 27 during baseline. When the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompt was implemented,<br />

Richard’s task engagement increased to a<br />

mean of 89% (range 77–100%) occurrence<br />

intervals <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher prompts<br />

decreased to a mean of 3.6 (range 1–9)<br />

occurrences. During math, Richard’s task engagement<br />

was a mean of 16% occurrence intervals<br />

<strong>and</strong> the mean number of teacher<br />

prompts was 25 during baseline. When the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held picture prompt was implemented,<br />

Richard’s task engagement increased to a<br />

mean of 94% (range 83–98%) occurrence<br />

intervals <strong>and</strong> the number of teacher prompts<br />

decreased to a mean of three (range 1–8)<br />

occurrences.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the IRP-15 (Martens et al., 1985),<br />

the nine general educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> two special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teachers indicated that the use of<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held self-modeling picture prompts<br />

to increase task engagement <strong>and</strong> reduce<br />

teacher directed prompts in the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom was socially acceptably. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

teachers’ mean IRP-15 score was 80 (range <br />

75–90). All teachers indicated that agreed or<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly agreed across all items, specifically<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> was (a) an acceptable interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for the student’s problem behavior,<br />

(b) most teachers would find the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

appropriate, (c) I would suggest this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to other teachers; (d) this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

would be appropriate for a variety of students;<br />

<strong>and</strong> (e) this interventi<strong>on</strong> was beneficial to the<br />

student. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re was no difference in rating<br />

scores between special <strong>and</strong> general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teachers; both found the interventi<strong>on</strong> socially<br />

valid.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, the participating students’ also<br />

indicated that the use of the h<strong>and</strong>held selfmodeling<br />

picture prompts was socially acceptably<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the Student Post-Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Acceptability <strong>and</strong> Importance of Effects Survey<br />

for grades four through six (Lane &<br />

Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> students’<br />

mean score was 83 (range 79–91); Jamie’s<br />

survey score was 91, Jordan’s was 80, <strong>and</strong> Richard’s<br />

was 79. All students rated all items similarly,<br />

agreeing to str<strong>on</strong>gly agreeing that the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was (a) easy for me to stick with,<br />

(b) helped me change in important ways; (c)<br />

quickly improved my skills; (d) made a difference<br />

in my grades, <strong>and</strong> (e) is <strong>on</strong>e I would tell<br />

other kids about. However, all students disagreed<br />

or str<strong>on</strong>gly disagreed that the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

“helped me make more friend.”<br />

144 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Figure 3. Richard’s percentage of task engagement <strong>and</strong> number of teacher-directed verbal prompts with <strong>and</strong><br />

without the h<strong>and</strong>held computer across classroom settings.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose of this study was to examine the<br />

use of self-modeling static-picture prompts via<br />

a h<strong>and</strong>held computer <strong>and</strong> self-modeling to<br />

increase task engagement <strong>and</strong> reduce teacher<br />

prompts for students with high-functi<strong>on</strong> autism<br />

in the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. Prior<br />

to this study all students performed high levels<br />

of off-task behaviors <strong>and</strong> required high levels<br />

of teacher prompts to initiate <strong>and</strong> maintain<br />

task engagement. When the interventi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 145


implemented, all students dem<strong>on</strong>strated an<br />

increase in task engagement <strong>and</strong> decrease in<br />

teacher directed prompts. Moreover, all students<br />

generalized the use of the self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

<strong>and</strong> prompting strategy across other general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms. A functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

relati<strong>on</strong> was established since experimental<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol occurred by dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of a covariati<strong>on</strong><br />

between change in behavior patterns<br />

<strong>and</strong> introducti<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong> within, at<br />

least, three different series at three different<br />

points in time (Horner et al., 2005).<br />

This study supports previous self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

studies, which dem<strong>on</strong>strated improved<br />

task engagement (Callahan & Rademacher,<br />

1999; Koegel, Koegel et al., 1999; Koegel et al.,<br />

1992; Lee et al., 2007; Odom et al., 2003;<br />

Wilkins<strong>on</strong>, 2005). This study also supports<br />

previous studies that have used self-modeling<br />

to increase task engagement (Coyle & Cole,<br />

2004; Hagiwara & Myles, 1999). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

this study supports the use of h<strong>and</strong>held computers<br />

to enhance student independent functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

(Cihak et al., 2007, 2008).<br />

This study extends the literature in several<br />

ways. First, this study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms. In spite of self-management<br />

effectiveness <strong>and</strong> usefulness, previous<br />

self-management studies in school settings<br />

have not dem<strong>on</strong>strated its applicati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms. With current<br />

trends of educating students with disabilities<br />

in the general classrooms, this study exp<strong>and</strong>s<br />

the versatility of self-management techniques<br />

for inclusive settings. By learning self-management<br />

techniques, students were more self-directed<br />

<strong>and</strong> less dependent <strong>on</strong> teacher supports<br />

<strong>and</strong> supervisi<strong>on</strong>. Moreover, learning to<br />

self-manage their task engagement, the students<br />

generalize adaptive behavior skill, aut<strong>on</strong>omy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated behavioral improvements<br />

across various classroom<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, this study exp<strong>and</strong>ed the use of<br />

video prompting procedures by incorporating<br />

the use of self-modeling static-picture<br />

prompts. One of the advantages of using static-picture<br />

prompts is a stati<strong>on</strong>ary display of<br />

relevant features <strong>and</strong> a lack of attenti<strong>on</strong> diverting<br />

moti<strong>on</strong>s, which may occur with video modeling.<br />

Since all the students in this study had<br />

difficulty initiating <strong>and</strong> maintaining attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the students benefited from a prompt that<br />

provided extended time for students to focus<br />

<strong>and</strong> obtain the essential informati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

pictorial prompt. Moreover, the static-picture<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly showed relevant stimulus informati<strong>on</strong>, so<br />

the potential for students to overlook relevant<br />

or attend to irrelevant features of a prompt<br />

were minimized.<br />

Thirdly, the use of h<strong>and</strong>held computers was<br />

applied in the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom to<br />

facilitate positive behavior change. When<br />

modeling is used for priming a behavior<br />

change (Coyle & Cole, 2004; Hagiwara &<br />

Myles, 1999) or watching the model in-vivo,<br />

the observer may have <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e or limited<br />

opportunities to acquire the behavior through<br />

observati<strong>on</strong> learning. Since the static pictures<br />

were presented <strong>on</strong> a h<strong>and</strong>held computer, it<br />

increased the opportunities for the learner to<br />

retrieve informati<strong>on</strong> anytime to guide behavior.<br />

With increased opportunities for an observer<br />

to watch a model, the opportunity for<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>al learning is enhanced.<br />

Fourth, this study exp<strong>and</strong>ed the social validity<br />

of using self-modeling static-picture<br />

prompts <strong>and</strong> a h<strong>and</strong>held computer as a positive<br />

behavioral support in the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom. Both students <strong>and</strong> teachers<br />

reported social acceptability for the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

use. Moreover, both special <strong>and</strong> general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teacher suggested that they would<br />

recommend its use to other teachers <strong>and</strong> use<br />

self-modeling static-picture prompts <strong>and</strong> a<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer again for students who<br />

experience similar behavioral challenges. Although<br />

the students noted that the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

did not help make more friends, they did<br />

report that it was easy to use, it helped them<br />

quickly <strong>and</strong> made a difference in their grades,<br />

which are critical comp<strong>on</strong>ents in the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

programming for students with autism<br />

(Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council, 2001).<br />

However, several limitati<strong>on</strong>s may have affected<br />

the overall interpretati<strong>on</strong>s of this study<br />

<strong>and</strong> future research is needed. First, photos of<br />

the students reading, writing, or watching <strong>and</strong><br />

listening to their teachers did not always corresp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

to the task at h<strong>and</strong>. That is, a photo<br />

of the student writing may have appeared<br />

when actually they were expected to be reading.<br />

Although the students were instructed to<br />

self-record “yes” if they were engaged in the<br />

task at h<strong>and</strong>, future studies are required to<br />

examine the displaying of specified picture-<br />

146 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


prompts with specified behaviors. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, as<br />

with other single-subject designs a small sample<br />

size was examined. In this study, <strong>on</strong>ly three<br />

students participated. With this mind, c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

must be interpreted within the c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

of this study. Future research is needed to<br />

verify these results <strong>and</strong> its external validity<br />

across larger samples. Third, a functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment<br />

or analysis was not c<strong>on</strong>ducted prior<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong>, so the student’s<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> of behavior was not determined.<br />

Fourth, the experimental design does<br />

not allow evaluati<strong>on</strong> of separate effects resulting<br />

from self-management or self-prompting<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e. Future investigati<strong>on</strong>s could employ<br />

multielement or multiple treatment designs in<br />

an attempt to distill how each of these comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributes to behavioral change.<br />

Moreover, future self-management studies are<br />

needed to examine the impact of this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> specific functi<strong>on</strong>s of behavior (e.g.,<br />

escape, attenti<strong>on</strong>, sensory, multiple functi<strong>on</strong>s).<br />

Thirdly, all students’ were highly motivated<br />

<strong>and</strong> enjoyed using the h<strong>and</strong>held computer<br />

device, which may have produced<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> novelty effects. Evaluati<strong>on</strong>s over<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger periods of time are required. Nevertheless,<br />

all students acquired the skills necessary<br />

to transiti<strong>on</strong> independently to different envir<strong>on</strong>ments,<br />

<strong>and</strong> teachers reported favorable<br />

opini<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>cerning video modeling procedure<br />

<strong>and</strong> outcomes to improve behavioral<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing.<br />

References<br />

Adams, G., & Englemann, S. (1996). Research <strong>on</strong><br />

direct instructi<strong>on</strong>: 25 years bey<strong>on</strong>d DISTAR. Seattle,<br />

WA: Educati<strong>on</strong>al Achievement Systems.<br />

Alberto, P. A., Cihak, D. F., & Gama, R. I. (2005).<br />

Use of static picture prompts versus video modeling<br />

during simulati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>. Research in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 26, 327–339.<br />

Adre<strong>on</strong>, D., & Stella, J. (2001). Transiti<strong>on</strong> to middle<br />

<strong>and</strong> high school: Increasing the success of students<br />

with Asperger syndrome. Interventi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

School <strong>and</strong> Clinic, 36, 266–271.<br />

Barlow, D. H., & Hersen, M. (1984). Single case<br />

experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior<br />

change (2 nd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn &<br />

Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bates, P. E., Cuvo, T., Miner, C. A., & Korbeck, C. A.<br />

(2001). Simulated <strong>and</strong> community-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

involving pers<strong>on</strong>s with mild <strong>and</strong> moderate<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Research in Developmental Disabilities,<br />

22, 95–115.<br />

Billingsley, F. F., White, O. R., & Muns<strong>on</strong>, R. (1980).<br />

Procedure reliability: A rati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> an example.<br />

Behavioral Assessments, 2, 229–241.<br />

Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>-schedule behaviors to high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

children with autism via picture activity schedules.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders,<br />

30, 553–567.<br />

Callahan, K., & Rademacher, J. A. (1999). Using<br />

self-management strategies to increase the <strong>on</strong>task<br />

behavior of a student with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 1, 117–122.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Alberto, P. A., Taber-Doughty, T., &<br />

Gama, R. I. (2006). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of static picture<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> video prompting simulati<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

using group instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures. Focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental Disabilities, 2,<br />

89–99.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. A. (2008). Use<br />

of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system to transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

independently through vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks for students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 43, 102–110.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. A. (2007).<br />

Generalized use of h<strong>and</strong>held prompting systems.<br />

Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, 397–408.<br />

Cars<strong>on</strong>, K. D., Gast, D. L., & Ayres, K. M. (2008).<br />

Effects of a photo activity schedule book <strong>on</strong> independent<br />

task changes by students with intellectual<br />

disabilities in community job sites. European<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Special Needs, 23, 269–279.<br />

Cole, C. L., & Bambara, L. M. (2000). Self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory <strong>and</strong> practice. In E. S. Shapiro & T. R.<br />

Kratochwill (Eds.), Behavioral assessment in schools:<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory, research, <strong>and</strong> clinical foundati<strong>on</strong>s (2nd ed.,<br />

pp. 202–232). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Coyle, C., & Cole, P. (2004). A videotaped selfmodeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring treatment program<br />

to decrease off-task behavior in children with autism.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Intellectual <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disability,<br />

29, 3–15.<br />

Cuvo, A. J., Jacobi, L., & Sipko, R. (1981). Teaching<br />

laundry skills to mentally retarded students. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training of the Mentally Retarded, 16,<br />

54–64.<br />

Dowrick, P. W. (1999). A review of self modeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> related interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Applied <strong>and</strong> Preventative<br />

Psychology, 8, 23–39.<br />

Dugan, E., Kamps, D., Le<strong>on</strong>ard, B., & Watkins, N.<br />

(1995). Effects of cooperative learning groups<br />

during social studies for students with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

fourth-grade peers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 28, 175–188.<br />

Gilliam, J. (1995). Gilliam autism rating scale. Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-ED.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 147


Hagiwara, T., & Myles, B. S. (1999). A multimedia<br />

social story interventi<strong>on</strong>: Teaching skills to children<br />

with autism. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 14, 82–95.<br />

Hall, L. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J.<br />

(1995). Promoting independence in integrated<br />

classrooms by teaching aides to use activity schedules<br />

<strong>and</strong> decreased prompts. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities,<br />

30, 208–217.<br />

Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G.,<br />

Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of singlesubject<br />

research to identify evidence-based practice<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

71,165–179.<br />

Hunt, P., & Goetz, L. (1997). Research <strong>on</strong> inclusive<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al programs, practices, <strong>and</strong> outcomes<br />

for students with severe disabilities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 31, 3–29.<br />

Kamps, D. M., Dugan, E. P., Le<strong>on</strong>ard, B. R., &<br />

Daoust, P. M. (1994). Enhanced small group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

using choral resp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>and</strong> student<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> for children with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities. American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

99, 60–73.<br />

Kamps, D. M., Le<strong>on</strong>ard, B., Potucek, J., & Garris<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Harrell, L. (1995). Cooperative Learning Groups<br />

in reading: An integrati<strong>on</strong> strategy for students<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> general classroom peers. Behavioral<br />

Disorders, 21, 89–109.<br />

Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2000). Treatment <strong>and</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> guidelines for individuals with Asperger<br />

syndrome. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, &<br />

S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 340–<br />

366). New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Koegel, L. K., Harrower, J. K., & Koegel, R. L.<br />

(1999). Support for children with developmental<br />

disabilities in full inclusi<strong>on</strong> classrooms through<br />

self-management. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

1, 26–34.<br />

Koegel, L. K., Keogel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea,<br />

W. D. (1992). Improving social skills <strong>and</strong> disruptive<br />

behavior in children with autism through<br />

self-management. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis,<br />

25, 341–353.<br />

Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999).<br />

Pivotal teaching interacti<strong>on</strong>s for children with autism.<br />

School Psychology Review, 28, 576–594.<br />

Krantz, P. J., MacDuff, M. T., & McClannahan, L. E.<br />

(1993). Programming participati<strong>on</strong> in family activities<br />

for children with autism: Parents’ use of<br />

photographic activity schedules. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 26, 137–138.<br />

Lane, K. L., & Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004).<br />

School-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> tools you need to succeed.<br />

Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong><br />

Lee, S., Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. L., & Shogren, K. A. (2007).<br />

Effects <strong>and</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s of self-management for<br />

students with autism: A meta-analysis. Focus <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental Disabilities, 22,<br />

2–13.<br />

MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E.<br />

(1993). Teaching children with autism to use<br />

photographic activity schedules: Maintenance<br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of complex resp<strong>on</strong>se chains.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 89–97.<br />

Massey, N. G., & Wheeler, J. J. (2000). Acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of activity schedules <strong>and</strong> their<br />

effects <strong>on</strong> task engagement in a young child with<br />

autism in an inclusive preschool classroom. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 35, 326–335.<br />

Martens, B. K., Witt, J. C. Elliott, S. N., & Darveaux,<br />

D. (1985). Teacher judgments c<strong>on</strong>cerning the<br />

acceptability of school-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Psychology: Research <strong>and</strong> Practice, 16, 191–198.<br />

McDougall, D. (1998). Research <strong>on</strong> self-management<br />

techniques used by students with disabilities<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> settings. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 19, 310–320.<br />

Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (1996). Full inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> students with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Developmental Disorders, 26, 337–346.<br />

Myles, B. S., & Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. L. (2003). Asperger syndrome:<br />

A guide for educators <strong>and</strong> parents (2nd ed.).<br />

Austin, TX: PRO-ED.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council. (2001). Educating children<br />

with autism. Washingt<strong>on</strong>: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy<br />

Press.<br />

Odom, S. L., Brown, W. H., Frey, T., Karasu, N.,<br />

Smith-Canter, L. L., & Strain, P. S. (2003). Evidence-based<br />

practices for young children with<br />

autism: C<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s from single-subject design<br />

research. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 18, 166–175.<br />

Oz<strong>on</strong>off, S., Daws<strong>on</strong>, G., & McPartl<strong>and</strong>, J. (2002). A<br />

parent’s guide to Asperger syndrome <strong>and</strong> high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

autism: How to meet the challenges <strong>and</strong> help your<br />

child to thrive. New York: Guilford Press.<br />

Quill, K. A. (1995). Visually cued instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children with autism <strong>and</strong> pervasive developmental<br />

disorders, Focus <strong>on</strong> Autistic Behavior, 10(3), 10–<br />

20.<br />

Rock, M. L. (2005). Use of strategic self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

to enhance academic engagement, productivity,<br />

<strong>and</strong> accuracy of students with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 7,<br />

3–17.<br />

Robins<strong>on</strong>-Wils<strong>on</strong>, M. A. (1977). Picture recipe cards<br />

as an approach to teaching severely <strong>and</strong> profoundly<br />

retarded adults to cook. Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Training of the Mentally Retarded, 2, 69–73.<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ers, M. R., & Parr, J. M. (1989). Training developmentally<br />

disabled adults in independent<br />

meal preparati<strong>on</strong>. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 168–<br />

191.<br />

148 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988).<br />

Childhood autism rating scale. Los Angeles: Western<br />

Psychological Services.<br />

Shapiro, E. S., & Cole, C. L. (1994). Behavior change<br />

in the classroom: Self-management interventi<strong>on</strong>s. New<br />

York: Guilford Press.<br />

Shearer, D. D. (1996). Promoting independent interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between preschoolers with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

their n<strong>on</strong>disabled peers: An analysis of self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring.<br />

Early Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Development, 7, 205–<br />

220.<br />

Spriggs, A. D., Gast, D. L., & Ayres, K. M. (2007).<br />

Using picture activity schedule books to increase<br />

<strong>on</strong>-schedule <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>-task behaviors. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Developmental Disabilities, 42, 209–223.<br />

Sowers, J., Verdi, M., Bourbeau, P., & Sheehan, M.<br />

(1985). Teaching job independence <strong>and</strong> flexibility<br />

to mentally retarded students through the use<br />

of a self-c<strong>on</strong>trol package. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 18, 81–85.<br />

Thinesen, P. J., & Bryan, A. J. (1981). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> use of<br />

sequential pictorial cues in the initiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

maintenance of grooming behaviors with mentally<br />

retarded adults. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 19, 246–<br />

250.<br />

Tantam, D. (2003). Assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of<br />

comorbid emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> behavior problems. In<br />

M. Prior (Ed.), Learning <strong>and</strong> behavior problems in<br />

Asperger syndrome (pp. 148–174). New York: Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

V<strong>on</strong>Brock, M. B., & Elliott, S. N. (1987). Influences<br />

of treatment effectiveness informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the acceptability<br />

of classroom interventi<strong>on</strong>s. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

School Psychology, 25, 131–144.<br />

Wacker, D. P., & Berg, W. K. (1983). Effects of<br />

picture prompts <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of complex<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks by mentally retarded adolescents.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 417–433.<br />

Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Berrie, P., & Swatta, P.<br />

(1985). Generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance of complex<br />

skills by severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped adolescents<br />

following picture prompt training. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 18, 329–336.<br />

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler, manual for the Wechsler<br />

intelligence scale for children fourth editi<strong>on</strong> manual.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Psychological Corporati<strong>on</strong>, San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX:<br />

Psychological Corporati<strong>on</strong> Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.<br />

Wilkins<strong>on</strong>, L. A. (2005). Supporting the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

a student with Asperger syndrome: A case study<br />

using c<strong>on</strong>joint behavioural c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> selfmanagement.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology in Practice,<br />

21,307–332.<br />

Received: 3 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 10 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 2 February 2009<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Self-M<strong>on</strong>itoring / 149


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities, 2010, 45(1), 150–159<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> for Children with <strong>Autism</strong>: A<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> with the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council<br />

Recommended Practices<br />

Robyn C<strong>on</strong>ley Downs<br />

Central Washingt<strong>on</strong> University<br />

Andrew Downs<br />

University of Portl<strong>and</strong><br />

Abstract: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council (2001) report was reviewed to identify <strong>and</strong> document recommended<br />

practices for programs serving young children with autism spectrum disorder. Twenty seven surveys inquiring<br />

about program practices were sent to educati<strong>on</strong>al service districts, school districts, <strong>and</strong> neurodevelopmental<br />

centers in Oreg<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Washingt<strong>on</strong> that had a program specifically designed to serve young children with autism<br />

spectrum disorder. Survey results indicated that the practices currently utilized within autism early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

programs are in many ways inc<strong>on</strong>sistent with the recommended practices identified in the literature. Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> future research needs are discussed.<br />

Currently, an estimated 1 in 150 (66.7 in<br />

10,000) children have an autism spectrum disorder<br />

(ASD; Center for Disease C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

[CDC], 2007). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se prevalence estimates are<br />

of particular c<strong>on</strong>cern because they have increased<br />

dramatically from the early 1990s,<br />

when the estimated rate was 4 to 8 in 10,000<br />

(Rogers, 1999). In additi<strong>on</strong> to the rise in prevalence,<br />

children with ASD are now being identified<br />

at an earlier age. Recent research has<br />

indicated that ASD can be reliably diagnosed<br />

in children by the age of 2 years (Cox, et al.,<br />

1999; Osterling & Daws<strong>on</strong>, 1994; St<strong>on</strong>e, et al.,<br />

1999). More recently, researchers suggest that<br />

ASD might be diagnosed as early as 15 to 18<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths (Hurth, Shaw, Izeman, Whaley, &<br />

Rogers, 1999), <strong>and</strong> possibly even by 8 to 12<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths (Mars, Mauk, & Dowrick, 1998; Osterling<br />

& Daws<strong>on</strong>).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> recent changes in prevalence rates <strong>and</strong><br />

diagnostic capabilities combined with the regulati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the Individuals with Disabilities Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Improvement Act (IDEIA; US Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2006), the federal law that<br />

Robyn C<strong>on</strong>ley Downs is now at Beavert<strong>on</strong> School<br />

District, Beavert<strong>on</strong>, OR. Robyn would like to acknowledge<br />

Debbi Prigge, C<strong>on</strong>nie Lambert, <strong>and</strong><br />

Christian Curran. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this<br />

article should be addressed to Robyn C<strong>on</strong>ley<br />

Downs, 2543 NW 118th Ter, Portl<strong>and</strong> OR 97229 or<br />

robync<strong>on</strong>leydowns@gmail.com<br />

provides special educati<strong>on</strong> services to children<br />

with disabilties, has led to a significant rise in<br />

publicly funded early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs<br />

serving infants, toddlers, <strong>and</strong> preschoolers<br />

identified with ASD (Heflin & Simps<strong>on</strong>, 1998;<br />

Hurth et al., 1999). Though the proliferati<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> programs is promising, the<br />

accelerated rate at which these programs have<br />

come into existence has increased possibility<br />

that some may not adhere to best practices<br />

(H<strong>and</strong>leman & Harris, 2001). Thus, the potential<br />

of such programs to improve the outcomes<br />

of children with ASD may be compromised.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> present study was designed to<br />

address the following research questi<strong>on</strong>: How<br />

do current practices utilized by publicly<br />

funded early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs compare<br />

to recommended practices?<br />

Historically it has been difficult to analyze<br />

areas of agreement regarding best practices<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g existing interventi<strong>on</strong> programs because<br />

few, if any, studies compare different programs<br />

of equal intensity (Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research<br />

Council, 2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> challenge of evaluating<br />

existing programs was further compounded<br />

because little st<strong>and</strong>ardizati<strong>on</strong> of program evidentiary<br />

bases (e.g., r<strong>and</strong>omizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> matching<br />

procedures) existed. Similarly, informati<strong>on</strong><br />

regarding sample size, ethnicity, family<br />

characteristics, reporting measures, <strong>and</strong> characteristics<br />

of students involved in each program<br />

has often been lacking or insufficient<br />

150 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


(Kasari, 2002; Wolery & Garfinkle, 2002).<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, l<strong>on</strong>g-term follow-up <strong>and</strong> costbenefit<br />

analysis informati<strong>on</strong> has rarely been<br />

offered (Daws<strong>on</strong> & Osterling, 1997; Lovaas,<br />

1987).<br />

In part to address the relative lack of c<strong>on</strong>sensus<br />

regarding best practices in autism interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council<br />

(NRC) was commissi<strong>on</strong>ed by the U.S. Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>’s Office of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Programs to review the autism early interventi<strong>on</strong>-related<br />

literature. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NRC (2001)<br />

was composed of a group of nati<strong>on</strong>al autism<br />

experts who c<strong>on</strong>sidered the following comprehensive<br />

programs in their review: Children’s<br />

Unit; Denver Model; Developmental Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Model; Douglass Developmental Center;<br />

Individualized Support Program; Learning<br />

Experiences, an Alternative Program for Preschoolers<br />

<strong>and</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir Parents Preschool; Pivotal<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Model; Treatment <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

of Autistic <strong>and</strong> Communicati<strong>on</strong>-H<strong>and</strong>icapped<br />

Children; Young <strong>Autism</strong> Project; <strong>and</strong> Walden<br />

Early Childhood Programs. Taken together<br />

these represent a sample of nati<strong>on</strong>ally recognized,<br />

well-established, data-based programs<br />

serving children with ASD.<br />

Following their review of existing programs<br />

the NRC (2001) developed <strong>and</strong> disseminated<br />

recommendati<strong>on</strong>s in the following areas: diagnosis;<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> prevalence; role of<br />

families; goals for educati<strong>on</strong>al services; characteristics<br />

of effective interventi<strong>on</strong>s; public<br />

policies; pers<strong>on</strong>nel preparati<strong>on</strong>s; <strong>and</strong> needed<br />

research. Most relevant to the current study<br />

are the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s pertaining to goals<br />

for educati<strong>on</strong>al services <strong>and</strong> characteristics of<br />

effective interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> recommendati<strong>on</strong> related to educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

services made by the NRC (2001) specified<br />

the need for a specialized curriculum.<br />

Specifically, the report indicated eight skill<br />

areas that should be carefully targeted within<br />

curriculum <strong>and</strong> programming. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y are social<br />

skills; expressive, receptive <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>; functi<strong>on</strong>al symbolic communicati<strong>on</strong>;<br />

attending to the envir<strong>on</strong>ment <strong>and</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding to appropriate motivati<strong>on</strong>al systems;<br />

fine <strong>and</strong> gross motor skills; cognitive<br />

skills (including symbolic play, basic c<strong>on</strong>cepts,<br />

academic skills); replacement of problem behaviors<br />

with appropriate behaviors; <strong>and</strong> skills<br />

needed to functi<strong>on</strong> in a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom.<br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d educati<strong>on</strong>al service recommendati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the NRC (2001) released additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

guidelines regarding characteristics of effective<br />

early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y are<br />

summarized as follows. First, interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

should begin as so<strong>on</strong> as a child is suspected of<br />

having ASD. This implies that interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

begins before, or while waiting for, a formal<br />

diagnosis to be made, assuming there is reas<strong>on</strong><br />

to suspect that the child will later qualify<br />

for services. Next, interventi<strong>on</strong>s should be individualized<br />

to the student’s <strong>and</strong> family’s<br />

strengths <strong>and</strong> areas of need. Regarding intensity<br />

of services, the NRC suggested that programs<br />

serving young children with ASD<br />

should provide a minimum of 25 hr of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

per week, 12 m<strong>on</strong>ths per year. Further,<br />

student-to-staff ratios should be as low as<br />

possible, allowing each child to receive individualized<br />

therapy <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Other recommended program characteristics<br />

included systematic, planned teaching <strong>and</strong><br />

m<strong>on</strong>itoring <strong>and</strong> quantifying child progress <strong>on</strong><br />

an <strong>on</strong>going basis. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NRC (2001) indicated<br />

that this data should be used to assess Individual<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Program (IEP) or Individual<br />

Family Service Plan (IFSP) goals <strong>and</strong> objectives<br />

as well as to determine the effectiveness<br />

of the program for each child <strong>and</strong> to decide if<br />

program alterati<strong>on</strong>s are necessary. Further, inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

with typically developing peers was recommended,<br />

with the stipulati<strong>on</strong> that it leads to<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al goals c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

recommendati<strong>on</strong>s previously stated.<br />

Although the NRC (2001) recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

have the potential to improve the early<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al services that are available for children<br />

with ASD, this likely will not be the case<br />

if existing interventi<strong>on</strong> programs are not c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with the recommended practices <strong>and</strong><br />

characteristics. Thus, it is necessary to examine<br />

the extent to which current ASD early<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> programs are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the<br />

NRC recommendati<strong>on</strong>s. This is particularly<br />

important to do for publicly funded programs<br />

which provide services to the majority of children<br />

with developmental disabilities (Bride &<br />

Schwartz, 2003) without regard to their family<br />

income or resources.<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 151


Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants were directors of ASD early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

programs in the states of Oreg<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>. Early interventi<strong>on</strong> services were<br />

defined as those serving children birth to 5<br />

years of age. Furthermore, the programs were<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly selected if they were specifically designed<br />

to serve young children with ASD (i.e., not<br />

developmental preschools). Finally, programs<br />

were limited to publicly funded classrooms<br />

providing services in affiliati<strong>on</strong> with a school<br />

district, educati<strong>on</strong>al service district (ESD), or<br />

neurodevelopmental facility. In other words,<br />

private programs were not included in this<br />

study.<br />

Once these parameters were determined,<br />

the offices of the superintendent of public<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> of each state were c<strong>on</strong>tacted to<br />

obtain informati<strong>on</strong> regarding ASD programs.<br />

Up<strong>on</strong> initial c<strong>on</strong>tact it was discovered that<br />

such informati<strong>on</strong> is not available directly from<br />

either state superintendent’s office. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> difficulty<br />

encountered while attempting to generate<br />

a list of program c<strong>on</strong>tacts is necessary to<br />

note for two reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, it illustrates the<br />

current disc<strong>on</strong>nect am<strong>on</strong>g programs in both<br />

states <strong>and</strong> the state educati<strong>on</strong> agencies. This<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strates <strong>on</strong>e of the obstacles facing organizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents wishing to locate effective<br />

programs or obtain informati<strong>on</strong> about<br />

programs generally. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the lack of a central<br />

database of programs potentially limits<br />

the scope of this study. While exhaustive efforts<br />

were made to send a survey to every<br />

program in both states, it is quite possible that<br />

some were overlooked.<br />

After c<strong>on</strong>tacting both state superintendents’<br />

offices, the following strategies were<br />

employed to gain informati<strong>on</strong> about district<br />

<strong>and</strong> ESD affiliated ASD programs. First, the<br />

director for the <strong>Autism</strong> Outreach Project in<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong> State was c<strong>on</strong>tacted, as were individual<br />

ESD directors <strong>and</strong> early childhood program<br />

directors in both states. From these communicati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

a list of possible programs was<br />

generated. Surveys were sent to all programs<br />

<strong>on</strong> the list, which included all Oreg<strong>on</strong> early<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>/early childhood special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(EI/ECSE) areas <strong>and</strong> all Washingt<strong>on</strong><br />

ESDs with possible programs. Finally, individ-<br />

ual programs were added to the list <strong>and</strong> sent<br />

surveys based <strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

with program directors as well as with other<br />

autism professi<strong>on</strong>als in both states. It should<br />

be noted that in some cases surveys were sent<br />

to jurisdicti<strong>on</strong>s that were not <strong>on</strong> the generated<br />

list just in case a program might exist. As a<br />

result, the overall return rate may understate<br />

the actual return rate.<br />

Neurodevelopmental providers were also<br />

included in the scope of this project. As with<br />

the educati<strong>on</strong>al programs, informati<strong>on</strong> regarding<br />

neurodevelopmental programs providing<br />

autism specific services was difficult to<br />

obtain. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore, each neurodevelopmental<br />

center in Oreg<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Washingt<strong>on</strong> was initially<br />

emailed, then called. Surveys were then sent<br />

to programs that affirmatively resp<strong>on</strong>ded to<br />

having an ASD early interventi<strong>on</strong> program.<br />

Procedure<br />

Once the list of potential participants was established,<br />

a cover letter <strong>and</strong> survey were sent<br />

to a total of 26 programs. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> survey was<br />

addressed to the director of each program<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that as the pers<strong>on</strong> in<br />

charge of administering the day-to-day workings<br />

of the program the directors would be<br />

most likely to be able to accurately resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

the survey questi<strong>on</strong>s. A sec<strong>on</strong>d mailing was<br />

made two m<strong>on</strong>ths following the initial mailing.<br />

In total, 17 out of 26 surveys were returned,<br />

indicating a 65.4% resp<strong>on</strong>se rate. Of<br />

the 17 returned, ESDs <strong>and</strong> school districts<br />

represented 66%, <strong>and</strong> neurodevelopmental<br />

centers represented 34%.<br />

Measure<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> items for the 25-questi<strong>on</strong> survey were generated<br />

from a c<strong>on</strong>tent analysis of several published<br />

studies that reviewed comp<strong>on</strong>ents of<br />

existing effective ASD programs (Daws<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Osterling, 1997; Hurth et al., 1999; Iovann<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

Dunlap, Huber, & Kincaid, 2003; NRC, 2001;<br />

Powers, 1992). A bank of questi<strong>on</strong>s was generated,<br />

<strong>and</strong> from this bank 25 closed questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were selected to create the survey. This type<br />

of questi<strong>on</strong>ing method was selected because it<br />

is relatively easy to complete, thus increasing<br />

the chance of return (Fowler, 2002) <strong>and</strong> so<br />

the data could be analyzed using percentages<br />

152 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


(Alreck & Settle, 2004). Many of the questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

also included an “other” opti<strong>on</strong>, allowing participants<br />

to provide an answer different than<br />

the opti<strong>on</strong>s provided. Of these 25 questi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

some were selected because they made inquiries<br />

regarding practices recommended by the<br />

NRC. Others were selected to gain additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> regarding various program characteristics<br />

(e.g., logistical informati<strong>on</strong>, staffing,<br />

teaching methods, etc.).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> survey was field-tested with the following<br />

professi<strong>on</strong>als: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> director, medical director,<br />

<strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al director of a local neurodevelopmental<br />

facility, the director of an<br />

autism program in the state of California, a<br />

professor with specializati<strong>on</strong> in the field of<br />

autism, <strong>and</strong> a professor with expertise in the<br />

area of survey research. As a result of the<br />

field-testing process numerous changes were<br />

made to the survey before the final versi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

sent out.<br />

It should be noted that the original purpose<br />

of the survey was twofold. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> first aim<br />

was to compare actual practices used by programs<br />

with the NRC (2001) recommendati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> other was to obtain informati<strong>on</strong> not<br />

included in the literature that was to be used<br />

to help guide the development of a new autism<br />

early interventi<strong>on</strong> program. This paper<br />

will focus <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> the results of the survey<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s pertaining to the comparis<strong>on</strong> between<br />

the NRC recommended practices <strong>and</strong><br />

actual practices.<br />

Data Analysis<br />

For each survey questi<strong>on</strong> the percentage of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>dents selecting each opti<strong>on</strong> was calculated.<br />

Because there were 17 resp<strong>on</strong>dents,<br />

each resp<strong>on</strong>se represented 5.88% of the sample.<br />

However, for clarity <strong>and</strong> ease of presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

this percentage was rounded up to 6%.<br />

Participants had the opti<strong>on</strong> to mark more<br />

than <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se for each questi<strong>on</strong>; therefore<br />

it is possible for the total percentage of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses for each questi<strong>on</strong> to add up to more<br />

than 100%.<br />

Results<br />

Earliest Start to Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Regarding wait time, a high number of program<br />

directors reported students typically<br />

experience no wait to begin attending the<br />

program (72%). Only <strong>on</strong>e director indicated<br />

the program waiting list was no more than 1<br />

m<strong>on</strong>th (6%). Several directors specified a wait<br />

time of no more than 6 m<strong>on</strong>ths (24%), <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e indicated 1 year or more (6%). A few<br />

directors added that wait time varied by the<br />

service requested (18%). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

indicate that students typically experience<br />

limited wait time when admitted to most ASD<br />

programs.<br />

Intensity<br />

Days per week. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong> regarding<br />

number of days per week proved somewhat<br />

difficult to answer for many directors, as 48%<br />

indicated that the number of days per week<br />

depends <strong>on</strong> the student. A lower percentage<br />

indicated student attendance at 5 days per<br />

week (24%), followed by 4 days per week<br />

(18%), then 3 days per week (12%), with 2<br />

days or less per week cited <strong>on</strong>ly by <strong>on</strong>e director<br />

(6%).<br />

Hours per week. Regarding the number of<br />

hours per week most students receive, resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

reflect the wide range of hour opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

currently being provided for students with<br />

ASD. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> highest percentage of resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

program directors indicated students receive<br />

7 to 9 hr per week (36%), followed by 4 to 6 hr<br />

per week (24%). Programs providing 3 hr or<br />

less (12%), 10–13 hr (12%), 14–16 hr (12%),<br />

17–19 hr (12%) <strong>and</strong> 23–26 hr (12%) were all<br />

equal in number. Finally, <strong>on</strong>e director marked<br />

the “other” opti<strong>on</strong> (6%) but did not specify<br />

hours.<br />

Student-to-staff ratio. Directors also had difficulty<br />

providing informati<strong>on</strong> regarding student-to-staff<br />

ratio, with many marking more<br />

than <strong>on</strong>e answer. A high percentage gave the<br />

student-to-staff ratio of 1:1–3 (90%). Next,<br />

directors indicated a ratio of 1:3–6 students<br />

(42%), while a smaller number said the ratio<br />

was 1:6–9 students (24%). No directors reported<br />

a ratio at or higher than 1 staff member<br />

to every 10 or more students.<br />

Access to Typically Developing Peers<br />

A large majority of directors indicated that<br />

students with ASD have access to typically developing<br />

peers while attending the program<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 153


TABLE 1<br />

Summary of Director Resp<strong>on</strong>ses Regarding Teaching Approaches Followed Most Closely (N 9)<br />

Approaches <strong>and</strong> percentages<br />

(90%). Of the program directors providing<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> opportunities, most indicated that<br />

students with ASD have access to typical peers<br />

50% or less of the time. In reviewing the additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

written comments made by directors,<br />

it also was made clear that within programs<br />

some students may have access to these peers,<br />

while others do not.<br />

General Interventi<strong>on</strong> Approaches<br />

Directors were provided with five recognized<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> approaches within the field of<br />

ASD research noted by the NRC (2001).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se approaches were applied behavior<br />

analysis (ABA), neobehavioral (Lifeskills <strong>and</strong><br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> for Students with <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> other<br />

Pervasive Behavioral Challenges [LEAP]),<br />

Treatment <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> of Autistic <strong>and</strong> Related<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>-H<strong>and</strong>icapped Children<br />

(TEACCH), interactive approaches (Floortime),<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental theory (Denver<br />

Model). As expected, the directors surveyed<br />

employ an array of different teaching approaches.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> most comm<strong>on</strong> approach indicated<br />

was applied behavior analysis (54%). A<br />

few directors also indicated approaches not<br />

listed as resp<strong>on</strong>ses (12%), but did not specify<br />

the respective approaches. Many directors<br />

checked more than <strong>on</strong>e approach, writing<br />

that their program is completely integrated.<br />

More than half of the programs reported utilizing<br />

a blended approach (54%). Directors<br />

selecting the blended approach were asked to<br />

provide the percentage of time each approach<br />

is used. Table 1 outlines the teaching approaches<br />

<strong>and</strong> percentages provided by these<br />

program directors.<br />

Curriculum <strong>and</strong> Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Number of<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

Completely integrated so cannot provide percentage 2<br />

Percentage depends <strong>on</strong> the child 2<br />

Interactive 66, ABA 36 1<br />

Interactive 50, TEACCH 25, ABA 15, Developmental 10 1<br />

Interactive 75, TEACCH 15, ABA 10 1<br />

Interactive 25, ABA 25, TEACCH 25, PECS 25 1<br />

Interactive <strong>and</strong> ABA, percentage depends <strong>on</strong> the child 1<br />

Note. ABA Applied Behavior Analysis; TEACCH Treatment <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> of Autistic <strong>and</strong> Communicati<strong>on</strong>-<br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped Children; PECS Picture Exchange Communicati<strong>on</strong> System.<br />

Directors were to indicate the specific instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

methods <strong>and</strong> curriculums utilized<br />

in their programs. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y were given a choice<br />

of <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong>, group instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

discrete trial teaching (DTT), incidental teaching,<br />

peer mediated/naturalistic, Strategies<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> Research (STAR), structured<br />

teaching, pivotal resp<strong>on</strong>se training (PRT), functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

routines, Playschool curriculum, positive<br />

behavior support, <strong>and</strong> Floortime. Most<br />

program directors reported employing several<br />

different methods. All of the directors<br />

surveyed checked <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong> as a<br />

method used within their program (100%).<br />

Other instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods <strong>and</strong> curricula<br />

used by a high percentage of programs included<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al routines (84%), group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(78%), incidental teaching (78%),<br />

<strong>and</strong> structured teaching (78%). Other methods<br />

reported were discrete trial teaching<br />

(66%), positive behavior support (66%), Developmental,<br />

Individual Differences, Relati<strong>on</strong>ship-Based<br />

Approach/Floortime (54%), peer<br />

mediated/naturalistic methods (48%), <strong>and</strong><br />

pivotal resp<strong>on</strong>se training (48%). Finally, the<br />

Strategies Based <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> Research Program<br />

(42%), Playschool curriculum (24%), <strong>and</strong><br />

other methods (12%) were all noted as in-<br />

154 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


structi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> curricular methods employed<br />

by programs.<br />

Problem Behavior Preventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Most directors surveyed reported a variety of<br />

strategies to prevent problem behaviors. All of<br />

the programs use predictability <strong>and</strong> routine<br />

(100%). Most reported using direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in basic behavioral skills (90%), a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

approach to problem behaviors (84%),<br />

<strong>and</strong> positive behavior interventi<strong>on</strong> (84%). A<br />

slightly smaller percentage utilized a highly<br />

structured envir<strong>on</strong>ment (78%), functi<strong>on</strong>al behavior<br />

assessment (78%), <strong>and</strong> principles of<br />

applied behavior analysis (72%). A lower number<br />

of directors reported using a c<strong>on</strong>sequencebased<br />

approach (30%), <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e reported<br />

using physically intrusive or aversive<br />

methods (6%).<br />

M<strong>on</strong>itoring Student Progress <strong>and</strong> Success<br />

To measure student progress <strong>and</strong> success, a<br />

high percentage of programs reported evaluating<br />

whether IEP or IFSP goals <strong>and</strong> objectives<br />

have been met (88%). Others reported<br />

using structured assessments (71%), parent<br />

<strong>and</strong> teacher ratings (65%), <strong>and</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcome<br />

studies (24%). Thirty-five percent of directors<br />

reported c<strong>on</strong>sidering students <strong>and</strong><br />

staff having fun as a measure of student <strong>and</strong><br />

program success.<br />

Individualizati<strong>on</strong> for Families<br />

In terms of family individualizati<strong>on</strong>, all directors<br />

indicated that parents receive specific<br />

training regarding strategies to teach <strong>and</strong> support<br />

their children as a part of their program<br />

(100%). Many also noted that support for<br />

families (60%) <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> services for<br />

children (54%) are provided in the home. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, a number of programs use parents<br />

as volunteers (42%). A number of directors<br />

marked the “other” opti<strong>on</strong> for this questi<strong>on</strong><br />

(30%) <strong>and</strong> explained that their program provides<br />

parent organizati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong><br />

programs (30%).<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Because it is clear that an early start to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

is highly correlated with positive outcomes<br />

for children with ASD (Rogers, 1996),<br />

the NRC (2001) has recommended an early<br />

start to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Most directors surveyed<br />

allow children to enter their programs immediately<br />

up<strong>on</strong> referral, though 24% of directors<br />

indicated a wait time of approximately 6<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths <strong>and</strong> 6% a wait time of over a year. It<br />

appears that most of the programs surveyed<br />

are able to provide services to children with<br />

minimal wait time, however, it is c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

that there are many programs that are not<br />

able to provide services as so<strong>on</strong> as a referral is<br />

made. It will be important for existing <strong>and</strong><br />

new interventi<strong>on</strong> programs to take the necessary<br />

steps to reduce wait-time for services as<br />

much as possible.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to the need for an early start to<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, the NRC (2001) stressed the importance<br />

of intensity of engagement, measured<br />

in days per week, hours peer week, <strong>and</strong><br />

student-to-staff ratio. Regarding days per<br />

week, the NRC recommended that children<br />

receive interventi<strong>on</strong> 5 days per week. Comparing<br />

this recommendati<strong>on</strong> to the number of<br />

hours per week indicated by the directors surveyed<br />

we found significant variability. For example,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly 24% indicated student attendance<br />

of 5 days per week. It should be noted<br />

that many directors indicated that student attendance<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s are made <strong>on</strong> an individual<br />

basis <strong>and</strong>, therefore, did not provide exact<br />

numbers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se numbers are still c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

as the majority of children served by the programs<br />

surveyed appear to be receiving services<br />

less than 5 days per week.<br />

Similarly c<strong>on</strong>cerning was the number of reported<br />

hours per week in which services were<br />

provided. Particularly troubling was the wide<br />

variati<strong>on</strong> in the number of hours across programs.<br />

Only 12% of programs reported providing<br />

23-26 hours per week, numbers within<br />

the range recommended by the NRC (2001),<br />

whereas 72% of programs provide less than 9<br />

hours per week. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> effectiveness of providing<br />

a high number of interventi<strong>on</strong> hours is<br />

well documented in the research literature<br />

(McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993; NRC), yet<br />

our results indicate that most of the programs<br />

surveyed are not providing anywhere near the<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 155


number of recommended hours. Taken together,<br />

our results regarding the number of<br />

days <strong>and</strong> hours per week provided suggest that<br />

the outcomes of children with ASD enrolled<br />

in many public early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs<br />

likely will be less than optimal in both the<br />

short- <strong>and</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> third factor related to intensity of engagement<br />

is student-to staff ratio, with the<br />

NRC (2001) recommending a low classroom<br />

ratio. Survey results related to this issue<br />

showed that most programs provide 1 staff<br />

member to every 1–3 students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se results<br />

are encouraging, however, many directors<br />

also reported a ratio of 1:3–6 students. Because<br />

directors were allowed to check more<br />

than <strong>on</strong>e survey resp<strong>on</strong>se it appears that ratios<br />

may vary within programs depending <strong>on</strong> the<br />

needs of each student. It is important to note<br />

that the staff ratios reported include certified<br />

teachers <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als, as well as specialists<br />

(e.g., speech <strong>and</strong> language pathologists).<br />

This is significant because many early<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>als do not receive adequate<br />

formal training in assessment <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

methods that they can use in the<br />

classroom to facilitate the development of<br />

young children with ASD <strong>and</strong> related developmental<br />

disabilities (Bricker, 1995; Schepis,<br />

Reid, Ownby, & Pars<strong>on</strong>s, 2001). This may be<br />

especially true for the increasing number of<br />

paraeducators working in early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

settings (Giangreco & Doyle, 2002; Giangreco,<br />

Edelman, Broer, & Doyle, 2001) who have<br />

lower levels of educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> training than do<br />

certified teachers (Riggs & Mueller, 2001). As<br />

ASD programs c<strong>on</strong>tinue to exp<strong>and</strong> it will be<br />

important to ensure that all staff in such settings<br />

are adequately trained <strong>and</strong> capable of<br />

positively c<strong>on</strong>tributing to the development of<br />

the children in their care.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NRC (2001) also recommended that<br />

programs provide access to typically developing<br />

peers, though it did not specify the<br />

amount of time within each day that should be<br />

spent in c<strong>on</strong>tact with such peers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> majority<br />

of programs surveyed provide access to peers,<br />

though the amount of time varies greatly<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g programs, with most directors indicating<br />

that students with ASD have access to typical<br />

peers 50% or less of their total time in<br />

program. It may be helpful for future recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to include specific informati<strong>on</strong><br />

regarding the amount of time students should<br />

have access to typical peers.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NRC (2001) did not endorse <strong>on</strong>e type<br />

of teaching approach over another. Perhaps<br />

not surprisingly, our survey results indicated<br />

that programs are utilizing a wide variety of<br />

teaching approaches. Over 50% of the directors<br />

reported using applied behavior analysis,<br />

<strong>and</strong> over 50% also reported using a blended<br />

approach. Whereas the effectiveness of applied<br />

behavior analytic approaches have been documented<br />

in the research literature (Lovaas,<br />

1987; McEachin et al., 1993), the research is<br />

still inc<strong>on</strong>clusive regarding the effectiveness<br />

of using a blended approach. With the current<br />

use of blended methods there appears to<br />

be a risk of drift from the types of systematic,<br />

planned instructi<strong>on</strong> that have been found to<br />

be so effective with young children who have<br />

ASD. Future research should focus <strong>on</strong> the<br />

effectiveness of blended approaches, as well as<br />

the ability of programs to provide systematic,<br />

planned instructi<strong>on</strong> through the use of a<br />

blended approach.<br />

Providing a specialized curriculum clearly<br />

targeting deficit skill areas is a clear recommendati<strong>on</strong><br />

made by the NRC (2001). Our<br />

results suggest that most programs are using a<br />

specialized curriculum. As with types of teaching<br />

approaches, it appears that programs are<br />

utilizing a wide variety of curriculums <strong>and</strong><br />

often blending many different curriculums.<br />

Future research is warranted to examine the<br />

effectiveness of blending multiple curriculums<br />

<strong>and</strong> strategies. Further, research should<br />

search for the factors involved in selecting the<br />

different types of curriculums <strong>and</strong> strategies<br />

within each program. For example, are strategies<br />

selected based <strong>on</strong> the preferences of<br />

program pers<strong>on</strong>nel or in resp<strong>on</strong>se to the effectiveness<br />

of the strategies when used with<br />

the particular child? In additi<strong>on</strong>, it will be<br />

important to examine the extent to which the<br />

curriculums <strong>and</strong> strategies employed address<br />

the developmental skill areas outlined in the<br />

NRC recommendati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

A related issue is how programs measure<br />

student progress <strong>and</strong> success. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> NRC (2001)<br />

stressed the importance of m<strong>on</strong>itoring <strong>and</strong><br />

quantifying child progress <strong>on</strong> an <strong>on</strong>going basis.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sistent with the NRC recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

the large majority of program directors<br />

reported assessing child progress toward IEP/<br />

156 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


IFSP goals. Most directors also reported using<br />

a mutli-method, multi-informant assessment<br />

approach including structured assessments, as<br />

well as parent <strong>and</strong> teacher ratings. Thus, it<br />

appears that most of the programs surveyed<br />

are m<strong>on</strong>itoring child progress as recommended<br />

by the NRC. What is less clear is<br />

whether the programs are using such data to<br />

make alterati<strong>on</strong>s to each child’s program as<br />

necessary. As noted by Downs <strong>and</strong> Str<strong>and</strong><br />

(2006) this is a critical issue that is often neglected<br />

<strong>and</strong> which needs to be effectively addressed<br />

by all early interventi<strong>on</strong> programs.<br />

Employing strategies to prevent problem<br />

behaviors is another practice recommended<br />

by the NRC (2001). All directors surveyed reported<br />

the use of such strategies. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> majority<br />

reported predictability <strong>and</strong> routine, direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in basic behavioral skills, a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

approach to problem behaviors, <strong>and</strong><br />

positive behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong> as the primary<br />

strategies utilized. This is a recommendati<strong>on</strong><br />

that most of the programs surveyed<br />

appear to be following.<br />

Similarly, little debate exists in the research<br />

literature regarding the importance of family<br />

involvement <strong>and</strong> individualizati<strong>on</strong> in an ASD<br />

program, as noted by the NRC (2001). This<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sensus is echoed in the survey results, as all<br />

programs provide specific training to enable<br />

parents to teach <strong>and</strong> support their child. This<br />

is the recommendati<strong>on</strong> that the highest percentage<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>ders reported following,<br />

though the way in which families are included<br />

varies from program to program. It is important<br />

to note that the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s that can be<br />

drawn from this study are limited somewhat by<br />

the methodology employed. Specifically, this<br />

study, as is the case with all survey research,<br />

relied <strong>on</strong> self-report rather than actual observati<strong>on</strong><br />

of programs. Thus, it is not possible to<br />

be certain that the practices reported by program<br />

directors are actually in place. In fact,<br />

because self-report biases typically serve to cast<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>dents in a positive light it is possible<br />

that the programs surveyed may be providing<br />

services that are even less c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the<br />

NRC recommendati<strong>on</strong>s than those reported.<br />

A related issue is the importance of assessing<br />

whether specific teaching approaches are being<br />

implemented with appropriate fidelity. Future<br />

studies should seek to compare selfreports<br />

with observati<strong>on</strong>al data to provide a<br />

potentially more accurate assessment of program<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>and</strong> fidelity.<br />

This study also was limited somewhat by the<br />

possibility of sampling bias. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re is no central<br />

registry of ASD programs serving young<br />

children in Oreg<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Washingt<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>refore,<br />

obtaining name <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tact informati<strong>on</strong><br />

proved to be a daunting task <strong>and</strong> may have<br />

limited the number of ASD programs included<br />

in the survey process, as at least a few<br />

potentially eligible programs may not have<br />

received a survey. This study was also limited<br />

by surveying <strong>on</strong>ly two states. Further, though<br />

the resp<strong>on</strong>se rate was quite high (65.4%), it is<br />

possible that resp<strong>on</strong>ders <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-resp<strong>on</strong>ders<br />

may have differed in some way. However, it is<br />

important to note that surveys were sent to<br />

some jurisdicti<strong>on</strong>s where there was not c<strong>on</strong>firmati<strong>on</strong><br />

that an ASD program even existed.<br />

As a result, it is quite likely that the actual rate<br />

of return was higher than indicated. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the resp<strong>on</strong>se rate am<strong>on</strong>g programs in<br />

rural areas (i.e., those with a populati<strong>on</strong> under<br />

50,000) was 57% <strong>and</strong> the resp<strong>on</strong>se rate<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g programs in urban <strong>and</strong> suburban<br />

areas was 68%, suggesting a fairly good geographic<br />

distributi<strong>on</strong> in the final sample. Still,<br />

the potential sampling biases noted underscore<br />

the need to examine this issue <strong>on</strong> a<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al level.<br />

Despite the limitati<strong>on</strong>s noted, this study<br />

provides some useful informati<strong>on</strong> regarding<br />

the practices <strong>and</strong> characteristics of early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

programs for young children with<br />

ASD.<br />

As the number of children diagnosed with<br />

ASD c<strong>on</strong>tinues to rise, it is likely that the<br />

number of publicly funded program designed<br />

to meet the needs of these children <strong>and</strong> their<br />

families will also c<strong>on</strong>tinue to rise. As this occurs<br />

it will be important for programs to carefully<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sider the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s put forth<br />

by the NRC (2001) regarding best practices<br />

for providing services for these young citizens.<br />

Although they should be interpreted cautiously,<br />

the results of the present study suggest<br />

that many programs may not currently meet<br />

many of these recommendati<strong>on</strong>s. Further, it is<br />

clear that the strategies utilized from program<br />

to program likely vary widely, <strong>and</strong> it is unclear<br />

how each program is making individual programming<br />

determinati<strong>on</strong>s. As programs c<strong>on</strong>tinue<br />

to proliferate to meet the needs of the<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 157


increasing number of children identified with<br />

ASD, it will be necessary to m<strong>on</strong>itor the c<strong>on</strong>sistency<br />

with which such programs are aligned<br />

with the best practices <strong>and</strong> recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

outlined in the literature. Future research<br />

should also focus <strong>on</strong> how to best support programs<br />

in following the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

more closely, so that children with ASD may<br />

receive the most effective interventi<strong>on</strong> possible.<br />

References<br />

Alreck, P. L., & Settle, R. B. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> survey<br />

research h<strong>and</strong>book (3rd ed.). Bost<strong>on</strong>: McGraw-Hill.<br />

Bricker, D. (1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> challenge of inclusi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 19, 179–194.<br />

Centers for Disease C<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong> Preventi<strong>on</strong>. (2007).<br />

Frequently asked questi<strong>on</strong>s-Prevalence. Retrieved<br />

July 19, 2007, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/<br />

autism/faq_prevalence.htm#whatisprevalence<br />

Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Bar<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., et al. (1999). <strong>Autism</strong><br />

spectrum disorders at 20 <strong>and</strong> 42 m<strong>on</strong>ths of age:<br />

Stability of clinical <strong>and</strong> ADI-R diagnosis. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of<br />

Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 40, 719–732.<br />

Daws<strong>on</strong>, G., & Osterling, J. (1997). Early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

in autism. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

effectiveness of early interventi<strong>on</strong> (pp. 307–326). Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Downs, A., & Str<strong>and</strong>, P. S. (2006). Using assessment<br />

to increase the effectiveness of early interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Child <strong>and</strong> Family Studies, 15, 671–680.<br />

Fowler, F. J. (2002). Survey research methods (3rd ed.).<br />

Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Giangreco, M. F., & Doyle, M. B. (2002). Students<br />

with disabilities <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al supports:<br />

Benefits, balance, <strong>and</strong> b<strong>and</strong>-aids [Electr<strong>on</strong>ic versi<strong>on</strong>].<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 34(7), 1–12.<br />

Giangreco, M. F., Edelman, S. W., Broer, S. M., &<br />

Doyle, M. B. (2001). Paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al support of<br />

students with disabilities: Literature from the past<br />

decade [Electr<strong>on</strong>ic versi<strong>on</strong>]. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

68, 45–63.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>leman, J. S., & Harris, S. L. (2001). Preschool<br />

programs for children with autism. In J. S.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>leman & S. L. Harris (Eds.), Preschool educati<strong>on</strong><br />

programs for children with autism (pp. 1–11).<br />

Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Heflin, L. J., & Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. (1998). Interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children <strong>and</strong> youth with autism: Prudent choices<br />

in a world of exaggerated claims <strong>and</strong> empty promises.<br />

Part II: Legal/Policy analysis <strong>and</strong> recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for selecting interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> treatments.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental<br />

Disabilities, 13, 212–220.<br />

Hurth, J., Shaw, E., Izeman, S. G., Whaley, K., &<br />

Rogers, S. J. (1999). Areas of agreement about<br />

effective practices am<strong>on</strong>g programs serving young<br />

children with autism spectrum disorders. Infants<br />

<strong>and</strong> Young Children, 12(2), 17–26.<br />

Iovann<strong>on</strong>e, R., Dunlap, G., Huber, H., & Kincaid, D.<br />

(2003). Effective educati<strong>on</strong>al practices for students<br />

with autism spectrum disorders. Focus <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other Developmental Disabilities, 18,<br />

150–165.<br />

Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

programs with children with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 32, 447–<br />

461.<br />

Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment <strong>and</strong> normal<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing in young<br />

children with autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of C<strong>on</strong>sulting <strong>and</strong><br />

Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3–9.<br />

Mars, A. E., Mauk, J. E., & Dowrick, P. (1998).<br />

Symptoms of pervasive developmental disorders<br />

as observed in prediagnostic home videos of infants<br />

<strong>and</strong> toddlers. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Pediatrics, 132, 500–<br />

504.<br />

McBride, B. J., & Schwartz, I. S. (2003). Effects of<br />

teaching early interventi<strong>on</strong>ists to use discrete<br />

trials during <strong>on</strong>going classroom activities [Electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

Versi<strong>on</strong>]. Topics in Early Childhood Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 5–17.<br />

McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993).<br />

L<strong>on</strong>g-term outcome for children with autism<br />

who received early intensive behavioral treatment.<br />

American <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 97,<br />

359–372.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council. (2001). Educating children<br />

with autism. C. Lord, & J. P. McGee (Eds.).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Academy Press.<br />

Osterling, J., & Daws<strong>on</strong>, G. (1994). Early recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

of children with autism: A study of first birthday<br />

home video tapes. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disorders, 24, 247–257.<br />

Powers, M. D. (1992). Early interventi<strong>on</strong> for children<br />

with autism. In D. E. Berkell (Ed.), <strong>Autism</strong>:<br />

Identificati<strong>on</strong>, educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> treatment (pp. 225–<br />

252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Riggs, C. G., & Mueller, P. H. (2001). Employment<br />

<strong>and</strong> utilizati<strong>on</strong> of paraeducators in inclusive settings<br />

[Electr<strong>on</strong>ic versi<strong>on</strong>]. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 35, 54–62.<br />

Rogers, S. J. (1996). Brief report, early interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

in autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disorders, 26, 243–246.<br />

Rogers, S. J. (1999). Interventi<strong>on</strong> for young children<br />

with autism: From research to practice. Infants<br />

<strong>and</strong> Young Children, 12(2), 1–16.<br />

Schepis, M. N., Reid, D. H., Ownbey, J., & Pars<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

M. B. (2001). Training support staff to embed<br />

teaching within natural routines of young children<br />

with disabilities in an inclusive preschool.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 313–327.<br />

158 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities-March 2010


St<strong>on</strong>e, W. I., Lee, E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J.,<br />

Hepburn, S. L., Co<strong>on</strong>rod, E. E., et al. (1999). Can<br />

autism be diagnosed accurately in children under<br />

three years? <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry,<br />

40, 219–226.<br />

US Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2006). Regulati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Retrieved July 19, 2007 from http://idea.ed.gov/<br />

explore/view/p/%2Croot%2Cregs%2C<br />

Wolery, M., & Garfinkle, A. N. (2002). Measures in<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> research with young children who<br />

have autism. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Journal</str<strong>on</strong>g> of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Disorders, 32, 463–478.<br />

Received: 24 September 2008<br />

Initial Acceptance: 30 November 2008<br />

Final Acceptance: 8 March 2009<br />

Practices in Early Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 159


Look! I'm in College! DVD<br />

Look, I’m in College! Is a half-hour documentary that follows four students through an<br />

extraordinary time in their lives. Terence, Benny, Rayquan, <strong>and</strong> D<strong>on</strong>ald are New York<br />

City public school students from high-need communities. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y all have autism <strong>and</strong><br />

intellectual disabilities, <strong>and</strong> they are the charter class in a college-based inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

program. Through collaborative efforts of the New York City District 75 <strong>and</strong> Pace<br />

University, these four young men from challenging socio-ec<strong>on</strong>omic backgrounds met with<br />

success as they participated in a college community am<strong>on</strong>g their age-appropriate peers.<br />

By the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disabilities (DADD). 2008. 31 minutes.<br />

Member Price: $ 34.95<br />

N<strong>on</strong>-Member Price: $ 39.95<br />

http://www.cec.sped.org/ScriptC<strong>on</strong>tent/orders/ProductDetail.cfm?pc=D5890

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!