01.08.2013 Views

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training<br />

in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> Number 3<br />

Focusing <strong>on</strong> individuals with<br />

autism, intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities<br />

DAD<br />

D<br />

September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

The Journal of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sulting Editors<br />

Martin Agran<br />

Reuben Altman<br />

Phillip J. Belfiore<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> Borthwick-Duffy<br />

Michael P. Brady<br />

Fredda Brown<br />

Mary Lynne Calhoun<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> F. Cramer<br />

Caroline Dunn<br />

Lise Fox<br />

David L. Gast<br />

Herbert Goldstein<br />

Juliet E. Hart<br />

Carolyn Hughes<br />

Larry K. Irvin<br />

James V. Kahn<br />

H. Earle Knowlt<strong>on</strong><br />

Barry W. Lavay<br />

Rena Lewis<br />

Kathleen J. Marshall<br />

Editorial Assistant: Kathleen M. Corley<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College<br />

John McD<strong>on</strong>nell<br />

Gale M. Morris<strong>on</strong><br />

Gabriel A. Nardi<br />

John Nietupski<br />

James R. Patt<strong>on</strong><br />

Edward A. Polloway<br />

Thomas G. Roberts<br />

Robert S. Rueda<br />

Diane L. Ryndak<br />

Edward J. Sabornie<br />

Laurence R. Sargent<br />

Gary M. Sasso<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Scott Sparks<br />

Fred Spo<strong>on</strong>er<br />

Robert Stodden<br />

Keith Storey<br />

David L. Westling<br />

John J. Wheeler<br />

Mark Wolery<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> is sent to all members of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> of The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children. All <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> members must first be members of The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> membership dues are $30.00 for regular members <strong>and</strong> $15.00 for full time students. Membership is <strong>on</strong> a yearly basis. All inquiries<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning membership, subscripti<strong>on</strong>, advertising, etc. should be sent to the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2900 Crystal<br />

Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, VA 22202-3557. Advertising rates are available up<strong>on</strong> request.<br />

Manuscripts should be typed, double spaced, <strong>and</strong> sent (five copies) to the Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, Box<br />

871811, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1811. Each manuscript should have a cover sheet that gives the names, affiliati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

complete addresses of all authors.<br />

Editing policies are based <strong>on</strong> the Publicati<strong>on</strong> Manual, the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2009 revisi<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

provided <strong>on</strong> the inside back cover. Any signed article is the pers<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> of the author; likewise, any advertisement is the resp<strong>on</strong>sibility<br />

of the advertiser. Neither necessarily carries <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> endorsement unless specifically set forth by adopted resoluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> is abstracted <strong>and</strong> indexed in Psychological Abstracts, PsycINFO, e-psyche,<br />

Abstracts for Social Workers, Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> Research, Current C<strong>on</strong>tents/Social <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Sciences, Excerpta<br />

Medica, Social Sciences Citati<strong>on</strong> Index, Adolescent Mental Health Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Administrati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research<br />

Abstracts, <strong>and</strong> Language <strong>and</strong> Language Behavior Abstracts. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it is annotated <strong>and</strong> indexed by the ERIC Clearinghouse <strong>on</strong><br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> Gifted Children for publicati<strong>on</strong> in the m<strong>on</strong>thly print index Current Index to Journals in Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the quarterly index,<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Child Educati<strong>on</strong> Resources.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> Vol. <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>, No. 3, September 2012, Copyright 2012 by the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Austim<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Board of Directors<br />

Officers<br />

Past President Emily Bouck<br />

President Teresa Taber-Doughty<br />

President-Elect Richard Gargiulo<br />

Vice President Nikki Murdick<br />

Secretary T<strong>on</strong>i Merfeld<br />

Treasurer Gardner Umbarger<br />

Members<br />

Debra Cote<br />

Mark Francis<br />

Robert S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong><br />

Jordan Shurr (Student Governor)<br />

Debora Wichmanowski<br />

Dianne Zager<br />

Executive Director<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Publicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Michael Wehmeyer<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Emily C. Bouck<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference Coordinator<br />

Cindy Perras<br />

The purposes of this organizati<strong>on</strong> shall be to advance the educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, research<br />

in the educati<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, competency of educators in this field, public underst<strong>and</strong>ing of autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong> needed to help accomplish these goals. The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> shall encourage <strong>and</strong> promote professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

growth, research, <strong>and</strong> the disseminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> utilizati<strong>on</strong> of research findings.<br />

EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES (ISSN 2154-16<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>) (USPS 0168-5000) is published<br />

quarterly in March, June, September, <strong>and</strong> December, by The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557. Members’ dues to The Council for<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> include $8.00 for subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN<br />

AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES. Subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMEN-<br />

TAL DISABILITIES is available without membership; Individual—U.S. $60.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $44.00;<br />

Instituti<strong>on</strong>s—U.S. $195.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $199.50; single copy price is $30.00. U.S. Periodicals postage<br />

is paid at Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22204 <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al mailing offices.<br />

POSTMASTERS: Send address changes to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES,<br />

2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

VOLUME <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> NUMBER 3 SEPTEMBER 2012<br />

Preparing Children with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> for Life in the<br />

Community: A Tanzanian Perspective 255<br />

ANGI STONE-MACDONALD<br />

Two Approaches to Ph<strong>on</strong>ics Instructi<strong>on</strong>: Comparis<strong>on</strong> of Effects with Children<br />

with Significant Cognitive Disability 269<br />

ELIZABETH GRACE FINNEGAN<br />

Comparing Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay for<br />

Teaching Functi<strong>on</strong>al Sight Words to Students with Moderate Intellectual Disability 280<br />

MARI BETH COLEMAN, KEVIN J. HURLEY, <strong>and</strong> DAVID F. CIHAK<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s for Children with<br />

Intellectual Disability 293<br />

DANA KEETER <strong>and</strong> JESSICA L. BUCHOLZ<br />

Using Video Modeling to Teach Young Children with <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong>ly<br />

Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech 302<br />

SARAH CLIFFORD SCHEFLEN, STEPHANNY F. N. FREEMAN, <strong>and</strong> TANYA PAPARELLA<br />

Effects of a Video Model to Teach Students with Moderate Intellectual<br />

Disability to Use Key Features of an iPh<strong>on</strong>e 319<br />

KATHRYN WALSER, KEVIN AYRES, <strong>and</strong> ERIKA FOOTE<br />

Comparing the Effects of Video Prompting with <strong>and</strong> without Error Correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> Skill Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for Students with Intellectual Disability 332<br />

HELEN I. CANNELLA-MALONE, JOE E. WHEATON, PEI-FANG WU,<br />

CHRISTOPHER A. TULLIS, <strong>and</strong> JU HEE PARK<br />

Cognitive Strategy Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill: Effects for<br />

Young Adults with Intellectual Disability 345<br />

YOUJIA HUA, BENJAMIN S. T. MORGAN, ERICA R. KALDENBERG, <strong>and</strong> MINKOWAN GOO<br />

Increasing Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> for Middle School Students with Moderate<br />

Intellectual Disability <strong>on</strong> Age-Appropriate Texts 359<br />

JORDAN SHURR <strong>and</strong> TERESA TABER-DOUGHTY<br />

Grade-Aligned Math Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Students with Moderate<br />

Intellectual Disability 373<br />

DIANE M. BROWDER, BREE A. JIMENEZ, <strong>and</strong> KATHERINE TRELA<br />

Review of Academic Mathematics Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Students with Mild<br />

Intellectual Disability 389<br />

CASEY HORD <strong>and</strong> EMILY C. BOUCK<br />

Manuscripts Accepted for Future Publicati<strong>on</strong> in Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> 254<br />

The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> retains literary property rights <strong>on</strong> copyrighted articles. Up<br />

to 100 copies of the articles in this journal may be reproduced for n<strong>on</strong>profit distributi<strong>on</strong> without permissi<strong>on</strong> from<br />

the publisher. All other forms of reproducti<strong>on</strong> require permissi<strong>on</strong> from the publisher.


Manuscripts Accepted for Future Publicati<strong>on</strong> in Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

December 2012<br />

Promoting independence through assistive technology: Evaluating audio recorders to support<br />

grocery shopping. Emily Bouck, Rajiv Satsangi, Whitney Bartlett, <strong>and</strong> Pei-Lin Weng, Purdue<br />

University, 5146 BRNG Hall, 100 N. University Street, West Lafayette, IN <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>907.<br />

Teaching café waiter skills to adults with intellectual disability: A real setting study. Atilla Cavkaytar,<br />

Anadolu University, Faculty of Educati<strong>on</strong>, Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, Eskisehir, 26<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>0,<br />

TURKEY.<br />

Data-based decisi<strong>on</strong>s guidelines for teachers of students with severe intellectual <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities. Bree Jimenez, Pamela Mims, <strong>and</strong> Diane M. Browder, University of North Carolina at<br />

Greensboro, 421 School of Educati<strong>on</strong> Building, PO Box 26170, Greensboro, NC 27402-6170.<br />

Using video self-modeling via iPads to increase academic resp<strong>on</strong>ding of an adolescent with autism<br />

spectrum disorder <strong>and</strong> intellectual disability. Juliet Hart <strong>and</strong> Kelly Whal<strong>on</strong>, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University,<br />

Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, PO Box 37100, M/C 3151, Phoenix, AZ 85069-7100.<br />

Parents’ participati<strong>on</strong> in special educati<strong>on</strong> in the c<strong>on</strong>text of implicit ideologies <strong>and</strong> socioec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

status. Priya Lalvani, Dept. of Early Childhood, M<strong>on</strong>tclair State University, 1 Normal Avenue,<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tclair, NJ 07043.<br />

Efficacy of individualized clinical coaching in a virtual reality classroom for increasing teachers’<br />

fidelity of implementati<strong>on</strong> of discrete trial teaching. Krista Vince Garl<strong>and</strong>, Eleazar Vasquez III, <strong>and</strong><br />

Cynthia Pearl, Dept. of Child, Family, <strong>and</strong> Community Sciences, University of Central Florida, 4000<br />

Central Florida Blvd., Orl<strong>and</strong>o, FL 32816-0165.<br />

Increasing literacy skills for students with developmental <strong>and</strong> intellectual disability: Effects of<br />

integrating comprehensive reading instructi<strong>on</strong> with sign language. Larissa Beecher <strong>and</strong> Amy<br />

Childre, 200 Charlotte Drive, B<strong>on</strong>aire, GA 31005.<br />

Effects of a treatment package to facilitate English/Language Arts learning for middle school<br />

students with moderate to severe disabilities. Pamela J. Mims, Angel Lee, Diane M. Browder,<br />

Tracie-Lynn Zakas, <strong>and</strong> Susan Flynn, Dept. of Human Development <strong>and</strong> Learning, East Tennessee<br />

State University, 303 Warf Pickel, Johns<strong>on</strong> City, TN 37614-1707.<br />

Maintaining vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills of individuals with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities through<br />

video modeling. T<strong>on</strong>i Van Laarhoven, Lauren Winiarski, Erika Blood, <strong>and</strong> Jeffrey M. Chan, Dept.<br />

of Special <strong>and</strong> Early Learning, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115.<br />

Research to practice in autism, intellectual disability, <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities. Stanley H.<br />

Zucker, Cindy Perras, Darlene E. Perner, <strong>and</strong> Richard Gargiulo, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Mary Lou<br />

Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, PO Box 871811, Tempe, AZ 85287-1811.<br />

Address is supplied for author in boldface type.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 255–268<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Preparing Children with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> for Life<br />

in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective<br />

Angi St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald<br />

University of Massachusetts Bost<strong>on</strong><br />

Abstract: Special educati<strong>on</strong> is relatively new in Tanzania. The Irente Rainbow School (IRS) in Lushoto,<br />

Tanzania, where this ethnographic case study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted, is the first school for children with developmental<br />

disabilities in the area. Their curriculum stresses skills important in family life <strong>and</strong> the rural ec<strong>on</strong>omy of<br />

Lushoto. The purpose of the study was to explore how local c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>and</strong> beliefs about disability influenced how<br />

participants understood their roles at the school <strong>and</strong> how they implemented curriculum. The ethnographic case<br />

study employed qualitative research techniques to ensure credibility <strong>and</strong> triangulati<strong>on</strong> of data <strong>and</strong> research was<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted over a ten-m<strong>on</strong>th period.<br />

The Rainbow staff created a natural setting to teach <strong>and</strong> practice daily living <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills to prepare<br />

students for home <strong>and</strong> work. The curriculum was based <strong>on</strong> the local community funds of knowledge, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

pedagogy <strong>on</strong> practices that were supported by the local culture. As special educati<strong>on</strong> evolves, programs will<br />

change to meet the needs of local populati<strong>on</strong>s. Knowledge of local c<strong>on</strong>text is critical to give children with<br />

disabilities the best opportunity for an educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> meaningful participati<strong>on</strong> in their community.<br />

Students with disabilities around the world<br />

learn first from their families <strong>and</strong> their envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Curriculum should be both culturally<br />

<strong>and</strong> socially relevant, providing individuals<br />

with the knowledge relevant to their local<br />

community <strong>and</strong> skills that they will be able to<br />

use after completing school. Evidence-based<br />

practice is a w<strong>on</strong>derful starting point for curriculum<br />

development, but in unique locati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

like rural Tanzania, it is also essential to<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> the needs of the local community.<br />

Since the 1980s, the focus <strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

people with disabilities intensified through<br />

the work of the United Nati<strong>on</strong>s. At the same<br />

time, a shift toward inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with disabilities worldwide occurred<br />

in 1994 after the acceptance of the Salamanca<br />

Statement <strong>on</strong> Principles, Policy, <strong>and</strong> Practices<br />

in Special Educati<strong>on</strong> by ninety-two governments<br />

<strong>and</strong> twenty-five internati<strong>on</strong>al organizati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Given that all children have a right to a<br />

This research was supported in part by a grant<br />

from Fulbright IIE. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

this article should be addressed to Angi St<strong>on</strong>e-Mac-<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ald, Department of Curriculum <strong>and</strong> Instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

University of Massachusetts Bost<strong>on</strong>, 100 Morrissey<br />

Blvd., Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA 02125.<br />

quality educati<strong>on</strong>, the issue of effective educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

practice becomes important.<br />

In the United States <strong>and</strong> the United Kingdom,<br />

the use of evidence-based practices to<br />

improve the quality of educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> to<br />

strengthen practices that are proven to increase<br />

learning for students has guided the<br />

development of curriculum (Brusling, 2005).<br />

Davies (1999) characterized evidence-based<br />

practices as practices supported by a research<br />

base. Evidence-based educati<strong>on</strong> describes systematic<br />

guidelines for research to establish<br />

evidence-based practices (Hargreaves, 1996).<br />

The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Center for Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Research (NCSER) in the United States, as<br />

part of the Institute for Educati<strong>on</strong> Sciences<br />

(IES), funds research to establish evidencebased<br />

practices that have been validated as<br />

effective for students with disabilities (“The<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Center for Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Research<br />

(NCSER) Home Page,” 2008). The<br />

What Works Clearinghouse program <strong>and</strong> website<br />

were developed by IES to disseminate<br />

studies in educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> catalog evidencebased<br />

practices. Universities educate future<br />

teachers about evidence-based practices for<br />

students with developmental disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

school districts expect teachers to teach the<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 255


curriculum <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards using evidencebased<br />

practices.<br />

Curricula exist in schools around the world,<br />

but the c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> pedagogy of those curricula<br />

differ. Curricula is defined by curriculum<br />

theorists as “planned learning experiences” or<br />

a “structured series of intended learning outcomes”<br />

(Johns<strong>on</strong> Jr., 2007, p. 130). These two<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>s look at the process of learning <strong>and</strong><br />

the outcomes. Other scholars look at curricula<br />

in terms of the specific c<strong>on</strong>tent that is prescribed<br />

for learning (Eisner, 1965; Gagné,<br />

1966). Curricula are often prescribed <strong>and</strong><br />

distributed by nati<strong>on</strong>al, state, or local educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

authorities to use in schools. Schools<br />

are where curricula are used <strong>and</strong> curricula are<br />

designed for these specific formal settings<br />

(Marsh, 1997).<br />

Since P.L. 94–142, the Educati<strong>on</strong> for All<br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped Children Act, curricula in the<br />

United States for students with disabilities<br />

have been guided by the Individual Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Plans (IEPs) required for each student with a<br />

disability. In recent years, IEPs have been written<br />

so that students with disabilities access<br />

the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum. Since the<br />

passage of the No Child Left Behind Law<br />

(NCLB), educators in the United States have<br />

been urged to use evidence-based practices<br />

that have been validated through rigorous scientific<br />

studies. Scholars <strong>and</strong> policymakers now<br />

seek to identify evidence-based practices <strong>and</strong><br />

the st<strong>and</strong>ards for determining an evidencebased<br />

practice (Odom, et al., 2005).<br />

While efforts to establish evidence-based<br />

practices may have benefited educati<strong>on</strong>al programs<br />

for students with disabilities in the<br />

United States, it is unclear how evidencebased<br />

practices used in the United States<br />

might translate to other countries <strong>and</strong> cultures<br />

<strong>and</strong> what might be c<strong>on</strong>sidered best practice<br />

in Tanzania. Evidence-based practices can<br />

be validated to work with specific populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with certain types of disabilities, but the practices<br />

may not be viable when the local c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

<strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>cept of disability in a particular<br />

culture differ.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Curricula<br />

Traditi<strong>on</strong>ally, students with more significant<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> some level of cognitive impairment<br />

are provided with curricula that focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al academics, functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills,<br />

<strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills to prepare them for life<br />

after school, rather than a strictly academic<br />

curriculum that might be in place if students<br />

are preparing to attend college (Browder &<br />

Cooper-Duffy, 2003). The emphasis in the<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum is <strong>on</strong> students’ learning<br />

skills to improve their quality of life. In this<br />

case, functi<strong>on</strong>al is a term used to describe activities<br />

in which people without disabilities<br />

would engage independently in natural settings.<br />

Such activities as shopping at a grocery<br />

store or riding a bus are c<strong>on</strong>sidered functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

(Dym<strong>on</strong>d & Orelove, 2001). The c<strong>on</strong>tent of<br />

the functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum reflects existing academic<br />

curricula as well as skills <strong>and</strong> knowledge<br />

necessary for life <strong>and</strong> work in an inclusive<br />

community (Brown, et al., 1979; Schmalle<br />

& Retish, 1989). The rati<strong>on</strong>ale for the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum is that students with developmental<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe disabilities need explicit<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in life skills <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al academics,<br />

because they do not typically acquire<br />

them through daily interacti<strong>on</strong> with peers <strong>and</strong><br />

adults (Halpern & Benz, 1987; Snell, 1997).<br />

Several researchers have emphasized the need<br />

to use functi<strong>on</strong>al curricula in recogniti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

students with disabilities are not well prepared<br />

for adult life (Bouck, 2004; Cr<strong>on</strong>in, 1996;<br />

Dever & Knapezyk, 1997; Polloway, Patt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Smith, & Roderique, 1991). Nevertheless,<br />

there have been few studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong><br />

effective curricula <strong>and</strong> strategies to help students<br />

with significant disabilities access the<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum <strong>and</strong> there is<br />

limited knowledge about evidence-based practices<br />

that address general educati<strong>on</strong> access<br />

(Agran, Cavin, Wehmeyer, & Palmer, 2006;<br />

Nietupski, Hamre-Nietupski, Curtin, & Shirkanth,<br />

1997).<br />

In developing countries, students with disabilities<br />

are often taught from a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum to provide them with the skills <strong>and</strong><br />

knowledge to participate in society (Kisanji,<br />

1995b). While functi<strong>on</strong>al skills are important,<br />

schools in many developing countries also focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> basic academic skills.<br />

In Tanzania, as in other developing countries,<br />

the n<strong>on</strong>-governmental organizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(NGOs) <strong>and</strong> d<strong>on</strong>or agencies have coordinated<br />

programs <strong>and</strong> provided the resources to<br />

help local communities <strong>and</strong> regi<strong>on</strong>s set up<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al programs for people with disabil-<br />

256 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ities. Scholars have argued that d<strong>on</strong>or agencies<br />

frequently did not take local culture <strong>and</strong><br />

interests into account in the past, even when<br />

implementing small-scale local programs<br />

(Chaudhry & Owen, 2005; Kalyanpur, 1996).<br />

D<strong>on</strong>or agency-sp<strong>on</strong>sored programs have often<br />

come directly from the West, <strong>and</strong> have been<br />

transplanted to developing countries. They<br />

have been implemented by a mixture of local<br />

staff <strong>and</strong> imported c<strong>on</strong>sultants. Because the<br />

d<strong>on</strong>ors have provided the funding, they have<br />

had the ability to influence the types of programs<br />

used (Kisanji, 1998). In many cases, the<br />

programs implemented combined curricula<br />

<strong>and</strong> pedagogy used in the schools for typically<br />

developing students <strong>and</strong> were based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

needs of typically developing students <strong>and</strong><br />

pedagogical practices comm<strong>on</strong> to the school<br />

systems from developed countries. These curricula<br />

<strong>and</strong> pedagogy did not address local c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

or specifically look at what was needed for<br />

students to be successful in their daily lives,<br />

such as instructi<strong>on</strong> in a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum.<br />

The curricula tended to focus <strong>on</strong> rote academic<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, 2010).<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong> in Tanzania<br />

In many African countries, people with disabilities<br />

receive few of the many services provided<br />

to other society members due to a variety<br />

of cultural beliefs about disability, lack of<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey <strong>and</strong> resources, <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omic priorities<br />

that marginalize people with disabilities in favor<br />

of those believed to be more able to c<strong>on</strong>tribute<br />

to the ec<strong>on</strong>omic growth of the countries<br />

(Deku, 2002; Ingstad, 1995; Kristensen,<br />

Omagor-Loican, & Onen, 2003). In Tanzania,<br />

this is especially true. People with disabilities<br />

have fewer opportunities <strong>and</strong> opti<strong>on</strong>s for life<br />

in their communities (St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald,<br />

2010). However, the spirit of Ujamaa, the system<br />

of socialism aimed at enhancing communities<br />

<strong>and</strong> communal cooperati<strong>on</strong> in Tanzania,<br />

has given some people with disabilities<br />

more opportunities than people with disabilities<br />

in other African countries.<br />

Many communities in Tanzania do not have<br />

the support pers<strong>on</strong>nel or the services available<br />

to meet the needs of all individuals with disabilities.<br />

Children with disabilities lack both<br />

adequate medical <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al services<br />

(Ihenacho, 1985). Nevertheless, the govern-<br />

ment of Tanzania has committed <strong>on</strong> paper to<br />

provide health <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al services to<br />

people with disabilities (Ministry of Labour<br />

Youth Development <strong>and</strong> Sports, 2004).<br />

Each year progress has been made to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

the issues faced by people with disabilities<br />

in Tanzanian society <strong>and</strong> to address<br />

those needs, starting by educating more children<br />

with disabilities <strong>and</strong> training teachers to<br />

be special educators. There are educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

opportunities <strong>and</strong> schools for Tanzanian children<br />

with disabilities, but those opti<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

limited <strong>and</strong> may not be preparing them for<br />

the transiti<strong>on</strong> to adult life <strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

their communities. According to the 2002<br />

census, there are approximately 34,569,232<br />

people in Tanzania. Of those people, about 10<br />

percent have disabilities, similar to the estimate<br />

by the World Health Organizati<strong>on</strong><br />

(WHO) (United Republic of Tanzania, 2003).<br />

Nevertheless, in 2005 <strong>on</strong>ly 1% of students with<br />

any kind of disability attended school (Karakoski<br />

& Stroem, 2005). In a study of special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> services in Tanzania in 2005, researchers<br />

found that <strong>on</strong>ly 821 teachers had<br />

credentials to teach students with special<br />

needs <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>e of those teachers held bachelors’<br />

degrees. Since that time, two higher<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> programs in Tanzania have started<br />

offering bachelor’s degrees in special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the first class of students will graduate<br />

in October 2011.<br />

This ethnographic case study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

at the Irente Rainbow School (IRS) in<br />

Lushoto, Tanzania during 2008–2009. The<br />

purpose of the study was to explore how local<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>and</strong> beliefs about disability played a<br />

role in how participants understood their<br />

roles at the school <strong>and</strong> how the curriculum<br />

was implemented. By analyzing this particular<br />

case of how disability <strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

are understood within a local c<strong>on</strong>text, a<br />

deeper underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the role of local c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

in the United States <strong>and</strong> other countries<br />

can be achieved. The following research questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

focused my study: (a) What are the local<br />

funds of knowledge about disability <strong>and</strong> the<br />

role of people with disabilities in the community<br />

in Lushoto, Tanzania? (b) How are these<br />

funds of knowledge manifested in curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> daily teaching practices at the Irente Rainbow<br />

School? (c) How do the interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

between the Irente Rainbow School <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 257


Lushoto community illustrate beliefs about<br />

disability in this community?<br />

Method<br />

Research Design<br />

The ethnographic case study aimed to explain<br />

how the school activities <strong>and</strong> curriculum reflected<br />

local culture <strong>and</strong> needs in this rural<br />

Tanzanian community. To develop this ethnography,<br />

I observed <strong>and</strong> participated in the<br />

daily activities of the school <strong>and</strong> community<br />

for a total of 13 m<strong>on</strong>ths over two research<br />

periods; I c<strong>on</strong>ducted interviews using a representative<br />

sampling of parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong><br />

community members; I collected documents<br />

relevant to daily work at the school, life in the<br />

community, <strong>and</strong> the development of the local<br />

<strong>and</strong> nati<strong>on</strong>al curricula; <strong>and</strong> I used video <strong>and</strong><br />

feedback interviews to record additi<strong>on</strong>al data<br />

at the school <strong>and</strong> check my underst<strong>and</strong>ing. I<br />

also employed several techniques to ensure<br />

credibility of my findings, which will be discussed<br />

below. C<strong>on</strong>structivist grounded theory<br />

methodology was determined to be most appropriate<br />

for capturing the experiences <strong>and</strong><br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s of the teachers, students, family<br />

members, <strong>and</strong> community members in order<br />

to gather a richer underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the total<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text (Charmaz, 2006). This methodology<br />

allows a researcher to identify a process or<br />

phenomen<strong>on</strong> to study <strong>and</strong> focus <strong>on</strong> a few key<br />

local c<strong>on</strong>cepts or features (Glaser & Strauss,<br />

1967).<br />

Grounded theory allowed me to accurately<br />

develop categories <strong>and</strong> explain the experiences<br />

of the particular case under investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> then to make general statements that<br />

may be useful in similar situati<strong>on</strong>s. Using this<br />

methodology, prec<strong>on</strong>ceived categories were<br />

avoided initially. Initial data analyses lead to<br />

emerging categories <strong>and</strong> themes. The categories<br />

rose out of the data <strong>and</strong> were recorded<br />

using in vivo codes. In vivo codes are terms<br />

used as codes that are taken directly from<br />

participants’ words <strong>and</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s (Glaser &<br />

Strauss, 1967; Strauss & Corbin, 1998). Coding<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued until clear themes emerged<br />

<strong>and</strong> categories became saturated. Follow-up<br />

interviews <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuous observati<strong>on</strong> were<br />

used to verify the hypothesized themes <strong>and</strong><br />

data to support the themes. I used triangula-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>firm themes as well as to validate<br />

my data between sources, using interviews, observati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong> document analysis.<br />

This approach allowed me to capture the<br />

uniqueness of the situati<strong>on</strong>, gather a richer<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the total c<strong>on</strong>text, <strong>and</strong> make<br />

asserti<strong>on</strong>s that may be applicable in other settings.<br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>text, <strong>on</strong>e must<br />

explore how the participants’ world is c<strong>on</strong>structed<br />

<strong>and</strong> the processes therein (Charmaz,<br />

2005b).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>text of the Study<br />

Founded in 2005, Irente Rainbow is unique in<br />

Tanzania as a special school for children with<br />

developmental disabilities because the students<br />

live with their families, as opposed to<br />

attending a boarding school. The skills that<br />

the students learn at school are immediately<br />

transferred to life at home. The school curriculum<br />

is purposefully designed to prepare students<br />

to become productive members of the<br />

local rural community through an emphasis<br />

<strong>on</strong> the local envir<strong>on</strong>ment <strong>and</strong> culture.<br />

Irente Rainbow School in Lushoto, Tanzania,<br />

a rural town of approximately 10,000 people,<br />

currently serves 28 children, aged 6–25,<br />

with developmental disabilities including<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, autism, hydrocephalus<br />

<strong>and</strong> cerebral palsy. Although the local Lutheran<br />

Church founded the school, approximately<br />

half the children are Christian, while<br />

the other half are Muslim. The school aims to<br />

provide students with the academic, vocati<strong>on</strong>al,<br />

social, <strong>and</strong> cultural knowledge <strong>and</strong><br />

skills to be active members of the community.<br />

Therefore, local c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>and</strong> cultural beliefs<br />

about disability are vitally important to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

regarding the design <strong>and</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the curriculum.<br />

Formalized special educati<strong>on</strong> is relatively<br />

new in Tanzania <strong>and</strong> this is the first school for<br />

children with developmental disabilities in the<br />

area. The school started in 2005. Most students<br />

at IRS did not attend school before<br />

2005, because public primary schools would<br />

not accept them.<br />

Data Collecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Analysis<br />

Interviews were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with 15 families, 13<br />

school staff, <strong>and</strong> four local leaders. Informal<br />

258 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


discussi<strong>on</strong>s were held with 12 additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

teachers <strong>and</strong> families. I used a representative<br />

sampling of teachers, parents, <strong>and</strong> community<br />

members to document experiences across socioec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

status <strong>and</strong> age <strong>and</strong> severity of a<br />

child’s disability. Observati<strong>on</strong>, interviews, <strong>and</strong><br />

document collecti<strong>on</strong> occurred over my entire<br />

research period.<br />

Data analysis occurred throughout the<br />

study using the c<strong>on</strong>stant comparis<strong>on</strong> method<br />

from the traditi<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>structivist grounded<br />

theory for data analysis (Charmaz, 2005a,<br />

2006; Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Strauss &<br />

Corbin, 1990). In this case study, the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

process for students at the Irente Rainbow<br />

School was the focus. Key c<strong>on</strong>cepts included<br />

cultural beliefs about disability, various<br />

influences <strong>on</strong> the curriculum, <strong>and</strong> the participati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the students in the local community.<br />

Initial decisi<strong>on</strong>s about data collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

were guided by my knowledge of the phenomen<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> further decisi<strong>on</strong>s about data collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

were made during the process based <strong>on</strong><br />

the analysis of data gathered (Glaser &<br />

Strauss, 1967).<br />

To ensure credibility of my findings, I utilized<br />

prol<strong>on</strong>ged engagement in the field including<br />

daily visits to the school over ten<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths, participant observati<strong>on</strong>, interviews,<br />

<strong>and</strong> document collecti<strong>on</strong>. I also used a research<br />

assistant to help with transcripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

translati<strong>on</strong> to check for the accuracy of meanings.<br />

Finally, I used five peer debriefers, who<br />

had knowledge of special educati<strong>on</strong>, research<br />

methodology, <strong>and</strong> the school <strong>and</strong> local community,<br />

to review the data <strong>and</strong> coding. To<br />

ensure the validity of the data I used five strategies<br />

outlined by McMillan <strong>and</strong> Schumacher<br />

(2006): prol<strong>on</strong>ged time in the field; in-depth<br />

interviews; triangulati<strong>on</strong> of interviews; observati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> documents; member checks; <strong>and</strong><br />

peer debriefing.<br />

Results<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Framework<br />

Using grounded theory, a unique c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

framework that combines the work of three<br />

groups of scholars explaining the global <strong>and</strong><br />

local forces impacting the school, community,<br />

the curriculum, <strong>and</strong> the pedagogy emerged.<br />

Br<strong>on</strong>fenbrenner’s bioecological model (1992)<br />

was employed to explain how community<br />

characteristics <strong>on</strong> a global level influence the<br />

views of disability <strong>and</strong> community life. Within<br />

the local c<strong>on</strong>text, the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the curriculum<br />

at IRS is based <strong>on</strong> local funds of knowledge<br />

important to family life <strong>and</strong> local culture.<br />

Funds of knowledge illuminate the<br />

critical skills necessary for survival <strong>and</strong> success<br />

as members of the Lushoto community<br />

(G<strong>on</strong>zález, Moll, & Amanti, 2005). Figure 1<br />

represents the overall model of the study<br />

showing the relati<strong>on</strong>ships am<strong>on</strong>g Br<strong>on</strong>fenbrenner,<br />

funds of knowledge, <strong>and</strong> Rogoff’s<br />

cultural traditi<strong>on</strong>s of instructi<strong>on</strong> (St<strong>on</strong>e-Mac-<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ald, 2010).<br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> the pedagogy of the school,<br />

I examined the instructi<strong>on</strong>al traditi<strong>on</strong>s described<br />

by Rogoff <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2007). At<br />

the school, they use the instructi<strong>on</strong>al model of<br />

intent community participati<strong>on</strong> to teach social<br />

<strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills. In this model, adults <strong>and</strong><br />

students work together <strong>on</strong> daily activities in<br />

their community <strong>and</strong> learning occurs through<br />

feedback, modeling, <strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong>. On<br />

the other h<strong>and</strong>, math <strong>and</strong> literacy were taught<br />

using rote methods based <strong>on</strong> assembly-line<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (which is the comm<strong>on</strong> form of<br />

teaching employed in most Tanzanian public<br />

schools). This model views the teacher as the<br />

expert, pencil <strong>and</strong> paper activities are comm<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> students dem<strong>on</strong>strate their knowledge<br />

through questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> answers.<br />

At the Irente Rainbow School, the teachers<br />

utilized <strong>and</strong> augmented the “funds of knowledge”<br />

the students gain from family <strong>and</strong> the<br />

community. G<strong>on</strong>zalez et al. (2005) define<br />

“funds of knowledge” as “historically accumulated<br />

<strong>and</strong> culturally developed bodies of<br />

knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills essential for household<br />

or individual functi<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> well-being” (p.<br />

72). In order to be successful participants in<br />

the community, Irente Rainbow School teachers<br />

“must look bey<strong>on</strong>d the school itself to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> local meanings <strong>and</strong> the impact of<br />

schooling” (G<strong>on</strong>zalez et al., p. 40). While students<br />

with disabilities may be seen as lacking<br />

basic skills <strong>and</strong> “viewed with a lens of deficiencies<br />

[<strong>and</strong> as] subst<strong>and</strong>ard in their socializati<strong>on</strong><br />

practices, language practices, <strong>and</strong> orientati<strong>on</strong><br />

toward scholastic achievement,” the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong> of these funds of knowledge<br />

may help them to be more successful<br />

members of society (G<strong>on</strong>zalez et al., p. 34). At<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 259


Irente Rainbow School, funds of knowledge<br />

inform teaching practices to provide locally<br />

<strong>and</strong> culturally relevant less<strong>on</strong>s. The underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of community funds of knowledge<br />

informs the preparati<strong>on</strong> of students with disabilities<br />

for integrati<strong>on</strong> into their communities<br />

after schooling.<br />

Each community has a different set of skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> knowledge that need to be acquired to<br />

participate socially <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omically in daily<br />

life. Table 1 shows an extensive list of the<br />

community funds of knowledge important in<br />

Lushoto that informed teaching <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

in the school <strong>and</strong> community.<br />

This list illustrates the knowledge that is<br />

most important for all individuals to have to<br />

participate as culturally, socially, <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omically<br />

active members of the community. In<br />

Lushoto, self-care <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills are<br />

the most important skills to have for daily<br />

life. Academic skills are also important, but it<br />

is possible to survive <strong>and</strong> work with minimal<br />

skills–functi<strong>on</strong>al literacy <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Figure 1. Visual Representati<strong>on</strong> of the Study.<br />

knowledge of m<strong>on</strong>ey. One student’s mother<br />

made <strong>and</strong> sold charcoal to support her five<br />

children <strong>and</strong> two gr<strong>and</strong>children without a<br />

husb<strong>and</strong>. Her children helped her, but she<br />

was unable to read or write. A member of the<br />

school staff helped me to explain the intricacies<br />

of informed c<strong>on</strong>sent to this mother in her<br />

local language. She could recognize m<strong>on</strong>ey<br />

<strong>and</strong> exchange it in the denominati<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

which she comm<strong>on</strong>ly worked in her business.<br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d her work, she bartered for some of<br />

her goods <strong>and</strong> grew her own food at home <strong>on</strong><br />

her farm. While she is living in poverty, she is<br />

surviving <strong>and</strong> her children have their basic<br />

needs met <strong>on</strong> a daily basis. To this mother,<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skills are more critical to survival<br />

than academic skills. In her experiences in<br />

this local community, she has not seen how<br />

academic skills have helped the people she<br />

knows to do any better than she does in life.<br />

Opportunities are simply limited <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills are most important to be success-<br />

260 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 1<br />

A Sample of Community Funds of Knowledge in Lushoto<br />

ful socially <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omically in this community<br />

(St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, 2010).<br />

Themes<br />

Household<br />

Skills<br />

Sewing<br />

Cooking<br />

Sweeping<br />

Washing dishes<br />

Washing the floor<br />

Setting the table<br />

Fetching water<br />

Caring for children<br />

or elderly<br />

Fixing broken<br />

utensils/tools<br />

Washing clothes<br />

Agriculture<br />

Skills<br />

Feeding a goat<br />

Cleaning animal<br />

areas<br />

Using a<br />

machete<br />

Using a hoe<br />

Planting seeds<br />

Preparing a<br />

garden<br />

Harvesting<br />

produce<br />

Shucking corn<br />

Carrying leaves<br />

<strong>and</strong> produce<br />

Beliefs about Disability. The local beliefs <strong>and</strong><br />

values about disability in Lushoto are complex<br />

<strong>and</strong> center around beliefs about God’s role in<br />

peoples’ lives. In Lushoto, most people underst<strong>and</strong><br />

the role of medical <strong>and</strong> health issues in<br />

the cause of disabilities, but people also believe<br />

that there is a God or another force that<br />

influences the medical problems. In this culture,<br />

people believe that God has a plan for<br />

everything <strong>and</strong> that having a child with a disability<br />

is mpango wa Mungu (God’s plan).<br />

While it is not easy to have a child with a<br />

disability, this is part of the role God designated<br />

in the society for the parents of children<br />

with disabilities, by giving them the children,<br />

<strong>and</strong> such parents should feel blessed. One<br />

father said:<br />

For me pers<strong>on</strong>ally, I believe that causes of<br />

disability are part of God’s plan, because<br />

children with disabilities are not <strong>on</strong>ly our<br />

children. We Tanzanians, even in Europe,<br />

at another time you were surprised that<br />

even livestock were born with disabilities,<br />

but this is part of God’s plan.<br />

In an interview, a teacher stated that in a<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Care Skills Social Skills<br />

Bathing<br />

Dressing<br />

Using the toilet<br />

Brushing teeth<br />

Washing h<strong>and</strong>s<br />

with a<br />

pitcher<br />

Hair care<br />

Shining shoes<br />

Greeting people<br />

Receiving guests<br />

Washing h<strong>and</strong>s<br />

for guests<br />

Helping neighbors<br />

Riddles <strong>and</strong> myths<br />

Singing<br />

Cell ph<strong>on</strong>e use<br />

Material<br />

Knowledge<br />

Carpentry<br />

Mas<strong>on</strong>ry<br />

Shoemaking<br />

Repairing tools<br />

<strong>and</strong> machines<br />

Brick making<br />

Plumbing<br />

Making charcoal<br />

Cutting trees<br />

Academic<br />

Skills<br />

Counting<br />

Measuring<br />

Writing<br />

Recognizing name<br />

Identifying/using<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey<br />

Reading<br />

Knowing days of<br />

week/time of day<br />

Knowing historical<br />

<strong>and</strong> current<br />

political figures<br />

Listening to stories<br />

passage in the book of John, Jesus comm<strong>and</strong>s<br />

his people to take care of all society <strong>and</strong> to<br />

support the weak <strong>and</strong> raise them up. She explained<br />

that the Bible tells God’s people that<br />

it is their resp<strong>on</strong>sibility to care for children<br />

with disabilities <strong>and</strong> that God has a plan. The<br />

Bible passage justifies their work.<br />

The children had disabilities because they<br />

were fulfilling a role for God. Based <strong>on</strong> str<strong>on</strong>g<br />

religious beliefs, both Muslims <strong>and</strong> Christians<br />

see children with disabilities as part of God’s<br />

plan. Individuals talk little about curses or<br />

witchcraft <strong>and</strong> see these beliefs as part of their<br />

past. Christian <strong>and</strong> Muslim beliefs about foster<br />

care <strong>and</strong> support for families permeate local<br />

culture, but do not necessarily involve active<br />

membership in the community. People still<br />

turn to traditi<strong>on</strong>al healers <strong>and</strong> medical doctors<br />

to treat the symptoms of a child’s disability.<br />

School staff teaches community members<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents about the difference between an<br />

illness that can be cured <strong>and</strong> a disability that is<br />

permanent, but can be treated <strong>and</strong> the symptoms<br />

reduced after interventi<strong>on</strong>, therapy, <strong>and</strong><br />

educati<strong>on</strong>. People comm<strong>on</strong>ly hold both traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> religious beliefs about the causes<br />

of disability, <strong>and</strong> these beliefs were not necessarily<br />

mutually exclusive in the traditi<strong>on</strong>al culture.<br />

These findings are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the<br />

existing literature (Devlieger, 1994, 1999;<br />

Kisanji, 1995a; Kiyaga & Moores, 2003). Islam<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 261


or Christianity had been interc<strong>on</strong>nected with<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al beliefs for several decades in the<br />

local culture. While local beliefs systems <strong>and</strong><br />

Christian belief systems required that children<br />

with disabilities be cared for in the families,<br />

this did not always happen due to shame.<br />

Shemweta (2008) found:<br />

In the [Wasambaa] culture disability is,<br />

therefore, held as unpleasant situati<strong>on</strong> that<br />

exists <strong>and</strong> that must be accepted. Parents<br />

<strong>and</strong> the community feel compelled to take<br />

care of their disabled children <strong>and</strong> adults.<br />

. . . Due to the communal life practiced by<br />

the [Wasambaa], the problem <strong>and</strong> effects<br />

of intellectual impairment to the children<br />

were h<strong>and</strong>led by the community, not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

by the family or parents. If parents were not<br />

able to cultivate or plant seeds, due to the<br />

problem of the defected child, the people<br />

around filled the gap <strong>and</strong> brought even<br />

food to the particular family.<br />

Children are to be cared for by their families<br />

<strong>and</strong> protected based <strong>on</strong> cultural <strong>and</strong> religious<br />

beliefs, but local practice does not require<br />

that children with disabilities be given an educati<strong>on</strong><br />

or active role in the community<br />

(St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, 2010).<br />

Models of Disability. In the study, I c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

four models of disability to explain how<br />

disability is understood in the Lushoto community.<br />

The medical model represented the<br />

most positivist view that disability is caused by<br />

a medical diagnosis <strong>and</strong> that problems can be<br />

addressed through rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> services <strong>and</strong><br />

medical care (Kaplan, 1999). The pluralistic<br />

model presented by Susan Peters looks at disability<br />

as a social c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong> which is both<br />

viewed <strong>and</strong> addressed with the local c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

<strong>and</strong> set of beliefs in that culture. This model,<br />

she argues, empowers the individual <strong>and</strong> his<br />

or her family to take c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong> make their<br />

own decisi<strong>on</strong> about how the disability will impact<br />

their future (Peters, 1993).<br />

In Lushoto, the medical model, the rehabilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

model, <strong>and</strong> the pluralistic model were<br />

all apparent in the beliefs of local stakeholders.<br />

People recognized that medical causes<br />

existed for people’s disabilities <strong>and</strong> that those<br />

disabilities could hinder their ability to participate<br />

in their community or do work.<br />

Despite these widely stated beliefs, the out-<br />

reach staff <strong>and</strong> others at the Irente Rainbow<br />

School believe more work is needed to persuade<br />

the general public in Lushoto that supporting<br />

individuals with disabilities to participate<br />

in the community is the correct path. In<br />

the outreach program, the staff works with<br />

parents <strong>and</strong> district villages to educate them<br />

about the rights of people with disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

how these individuals can be part of the community.<br />

Teachers <strong>and</strong> parents saw the school<br />

as a path to rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> for their children.<br />

One church leader explained:<br />

The opportunity of the school or the role of<br />

the school in society is to show that it is<br />

completely possible that these children go<br />

to school like other children <strong>and</strong> the school<br />

has helped also in the educati<strong>on</strong> [of the<br />

community] through words <strong>and</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

see that “it is possible, it can be d<strong>on</strong>e” <strong>and</strong><br />

indeed the meaning of the school is to push<br />

other schools <strong>and</strong> teachers to show respect<br />

for those who do this work <strong>and</strong> to recognize<br />

that in our district there is a school that<br />

provides this service. Therefore, I can say<br />

that in our community or in society the<br />

school dem<strong>on</strong>strates to the society that children<br />

have rights to receive an educati<strong>on</strong><br />

like all other children. It is a school like any<br />

other school, therefore [Rainbow] is indeed<br />

showing that [to society].<br />

Through educating the children at the school<br />

<strong>and</strong> through its work in the outreach program,<br />

the diocese dem<strong>on</strong>strates that it cares<br />

about these children <strong>and</strong> their future in the<br />

community. Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> would provide<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> knowledge to do work <strong>and</strong> participate<br />

socially in the community. Finally, some<br />

teachers <strong>and</strong> parents recognized the importance<br />

of their beliefs <strong>and</strong> how their local culture<br />

embraced these children <strong>and</strong> helped<br />

them find their place through educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

knowledge of cultural practices. Several models<br />

of disability are present in Lushoto because<br />

community members themselves have different<br />

beliefs about disability <strong>and</strong> different underst<strong>and</strong>ings<br />

of the role of people with disabilities<br />

in the community.<br />

The Irente Curriculum. The school’s pedagogy<br />

can be explained through two traditi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of instructi<strong>on</strong> described as didactic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> intent community participati<strong>on</strong> (Rogoff,<br />

262 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


et al., 2007). At the school, students are taught<br />

academic subjects such as math <strong>and</strong> literacy<br />

using didactic instructi<strong>on</strong> or rote methods of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> learn this knowledge through<br />

lecture <strong>and</strong> direct instructi<strong>on</strong> with the teacher<br />

as the expert. A Rainbow teacher explained<br />

how the students at Rainbow learned their<br />

numbers very slowly over a few years, when she<br />

taught the child the number <strong>on</strong>e first, <strong>and</strong><br />

then moved to two <strong>and</strong> then three. One<br />

teacher said:<br />

In class <strong>on</strong>e, students begin to count 1–10,<br />

this is core of kindergarten, but here a child<br />

is doing 1–10 <strong>and</strong> this is his third year in the<br />

class. Therefore, you must first begin with<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> then go to two, <strong>and</strong> then three.<br />

But, in St<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>on</strong>e [in the primary<br />

school], it is totally different. You are able to<br />

teach a student <strong>on</strong>e, two, three <strong>and</strong> he/she<br />

will underst<strong>and</strong> after you do exercises <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e, two, three for a few days.<br />

The following secti<strong>on</strong> from field notes illustrates<br />

the difficulty some students have with<br />

counting:<br />

Using blocks, [the teacher] asked Sebastian<br />

to count 3 blocks <strong>and</strong> show what 3 is. Willy<br />

was supposed to show two blocks <strong>and</strong> Theo<br />

was supposed to show 4 blocks. When Sebastian<br />

was wr<strong>on</strong>g, she asked the class if he<br />

was right, but he had shown 2. He finally got<br />

it <strong>on</strong> the 3rd try with help. Vincent modeled<br />

how to count the blocks <strong>and</strong> showed him<br />

how to move them to the side after he<br />

counted them. Willy <strong>and</strong> Jally also needed<br />

more than <strong>on</strong>e try <strong>and</strong> scaffolding to show<br />

their numbers.<br />

In these less<strong>on</strong>s, students have fewer opportunities<br />

to work together <strong>and</strong> help each other in<br />

the learning process. In this excerpt, “sema”<br />

means say, so she was telling the students, “Say<br />

<strong>on</strong>e, say two, say three,” <strong>and</strong> so <strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> they<br />

then said the number after the comm<strong>and</strong>. In<br />

the next example, a teacher showed Rose a<br />

picture of the letter a.<br />

Teacher: Herefu huu ni nini? (Which vowel<br />

is this?)<br />

Rose: No resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Teacher: Sema “a” (Say “a”)<br />

Rose: A<br />

Teacher: Tena (again)<br />

Rose: A<br />

Teacher: Tena (again)<br />

Rose: A<br />

Teacher: Tena (again)<br />

Rose: A<br />

Teacher: Wote (everybody)<br />

Whole class: A<br />

Teacher: Tena (again)<br />

Whole class: A<br />

This type of call <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se for an individual<br />

student <strong>and</strong> the whole class was very<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> occurred daily in different subjects.<br />

The rigidity of communicati<strong>on</strong> is evident,<br />

but it is also important to note that the<br />

language is in isolati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> not c<strong>on</strong>nected to<br />

real tasks or how <strong>on</strong>e would typically use language.<br />

The students can chant vowels <strong>and</strong><br />

syllable patterns, but do not underst<strong>and</strong> how<br />

those exercises c<strong>on</strong>nect to reading stories or<br />

signs in the community.<br />

Vocati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> social skills are taught<br />

through intent community participati<strong>on</strong>. This<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong> supports the collective nature of the<br />

culture <strong>and</strong> encourages adults <strong>and</strong> students to<br />

learn from <strong>and</strong> support each other. These<br />

children can learn better through seeing <strong>and</strong><br />

doing, rather than being told. Students learn<br />

the tasks most important to daily life, participating<br />

in them using the same tools <strong>and</strong> procedures<br />

as adults. A Rainbow teacher explained<br />

their participati<strong>on</strong> structure:<br />

Smaller children learn from the bigger <strong>on</strong>es<br />

because we have shown the bigger <strong>on</strong>es how<br />

to begin <strong>and</strong> the smaller children like this<br />

very much. So, the students strive to go to<br />

the garden <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinue to do the work <strong>on</strong><br />

their own. Even cleaning tasks, the younger<br />

<strong>on</strong>es watch the older students sweep <strong>and</strong><br />

mop <strong>and</strong> do it themselves. The teachers<br />

have stopped doing this work. The younger<br />

students learn a lot from the older students<br />

<strong>and</strong> if a younger student sees an older student<br />

doing something independently, they<br />

like to do it too.<br />

Students work side by side with teachers doing<br />

the work. Students are never forced to participate,<br />

but are allowed to watch. But frequently<br />

students want to try <strong>and</strong> help, as well as practice<br />

the task because their teacher <strong>and</strong> peers<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 263


are doing the task. Furthermore, the work the<br />

students do at school supports the daily operati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the school <strong>and</strong> is not solely for practice,<br />

but for a purpose. Students are gaining<br />

<strong>and</strong> practicing skills that they use at home <strong>on</strong><br />

a regular basis to c<strong>on</strong>tribute to their family<br />

socially <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omically.<br />

The curriculum was based <strong>on</strong> the nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curricula, the M<strong>on</strong>tessori principles, <strong>and</strong><br />

knowledge of local skills that were needed in<br />

the community. The curriculum was originally<br />

enacted without the nati<strong>on</strong>al curriculum for<br />

students with intellectual disability, <strong>and</strong><br />

school administrati<strong>on</strong> utilized their knowledge<br />

of the primary school curriculum <strong>and</strong><br />

the local knowledge about farming, animal<br />

husb<strong>and</strong>ry, cleaning, carpentry, self-care skills,<br />

<strong>and</strong> social skills students would need to be<br />

accepted in the community as active members.<br />

This collecti<strong>on</strong> of knowledge <strong>and</strong> skills<br />

was determined through an informal process<br />

by stakeholders to find the community funds<br />

of knowledge (G<strong>on</strong>zález, et al., 2005; G<strong>on</strong>zález,<br />

et al., 1995). Community funds of<br />

knowledge were then utilized in the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

process at the Irente Rainbow School. A<br />

parent explained what his child has learned<br />

since he came to Rainbow <strong>and</strong> his c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

to the family:<br />

He works, like when his sister is not here, if<br />

you tell him, he washes the dishes well <strong>and</strong><br />

they are very clean. If he got work in a hotel,<br />

he could go there <strong>and</strong> wash dishes. He goes<br />

to the Mosque <strong>on</strong> Fridays <strong>and</strong> he can go by<br />

himself without any problems. We send him<br />

to the store. We can give him a piece of<br />

paper with the name of the item if he<br />

doesn’t underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> he can give it to<br />

the storekeeper <strong>and</strong> he will get whatever he<br />

needs.<br />

This student can support his family, but the<br />

family also supports his learning needs by using<br />

a piece of paper to write down the item.<br />

Together, they are both learning <strong>and</strong> participating<br />

in daily life.<br />

The School is Educating the People. The<br />

Irente Rainbow School has been dynamic in<br />

changing the beliefs of people in the local<br />

Irente <strong>and</strong> Lushoto community to view people<br />

with disabilities positively <strong>and</strong> to help support<br />

children with disabilities <strong>and</strong> their families.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al services <strong>and</strong> support services<br />

through the community-based outreach rehabilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

program have occurred through the<br />

work of the church, rather than the government.<br />

In Tanzania, students with disabilities<br />

have been slow to receive their rights to an<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> have fewer opportunities to<br />

participate in public primary schools if able.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the local village government<br />

leaders in Irente <strong>and</strong> some areas of the<br />

outreach program have been quite supportive<br />

of the Rainbow School, <strong>and</strong> the outreach<br />

classrooms, giving more children with disabilities<br />

the opportunity to have an educati<strong>on</strong><br />

even if they cannot come to Lushoto. The<br />

presence of the students as community members<br />

increases the likelihood that the community<br />

as a whole will accept people with disabilities<br />

as full members.<br />

Nevertheless, there are counterexamples of<br />

how Lushoto <strong>and</strong> Irente face difficulties in<br />

finding acceptance <strong>and</strong> community membership<br />

for students with disabilities. One local<br />

researcher stated,<br />

During the research, the researcher faced<br />

several problems. Some of the interviewees,<br />

especially in the rural areas, hesitated to<br />

give relevant informati<strong>on</strong> as they feared to<br />

be punished by the government, especially<br />

those who have children with intellectual<br />

impairment. Hence it was time c<strong>on</strong>suming<br />

job to c<strong>on</strong>vince them that they were safe<br />

enough, <strong>and</strong> their informati<strong>on</strong> would be<br />

helpful to the society (Shemweta, 2008, p.<br />

12).<br />

There are church leaders <strong>and</strong> diocese employees<br />

who do not show respect for or value<br />

individuals with disabilities in their community.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> disability awareness training<br />

needs to occur for all diocese employees if<br />

students are going to successfully take diocese<br />

jobs <strong>and</strong> participate in the diocese as active<br />

members.<br />

Is the Community Ready to Include People with<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>? The stakeholders in the school<br />

believe that the students can participate in the<br />

community <strong>and</strong> are learning the skills needed<br />

to participate actively in the community <strong>and</strong><br />

in their families. The parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong><br />

community members also believe that the<br />

community as a whole is not ready to give the<br />

264 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


students opportunities to work <strong>and</strong> be active<br />

adults in the community. As l<strong>on</strong>g as the students<br />

stay in their families <strong>and</strong> work or participate<br />

in the local areas or neighborhoods<br />

where they live, they will be accepted. The<br />

community is unsure about greater visibility<br />

for these students <strong>and</strong> needs more educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> assurance about their skills <strong>and</strong> abilities.<br />

One church leader explained:<br />

The opportunity of the school or the role of<br />

the school in society is to show that it is<br />

completely possible that these children go<br />

to school like other children <strong>and</strong> the school<br />

has helped also in the educati<strong>on</strong> [of the<br />

community] through words <strong>and</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

see that “it is possible, it can be d<strong>on</strong>e” <strong>and</strong><br />

indeed the meaning of the school is to push<br />

other schools <strong>and</strong> teachers to show respect<br />

for those who do this work <strong>and</strong> to recognize<br />

that in our district there is a school that<br />

provides this service. Therefore, I can say<br />

that in our community or in society the<br />

school dem<strong>on</strong>strates to the society that children<br />

have rights to receive an educati<strong>on</strong><br />

like all other children. It is a school like any<br />

other school, therefore [Rainbow] is indeed<br />

showing that [to society].<br />

There is still a disc<strong>on</strong>nect am<strong>on</strong>g community<br />

members between having skills <strong>and</strong> being able<br />

to do the job. Students with disabilities from<br />

the school can dem<strong>on</strong>strate their skills to help<br />

a carpenter, clean a shop, or wash dishes, but<br />

thus far when school administrators try to find<br />

jobs for these students both in church instituti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> in community instituti<strong>on</strong>s, people<br />

are not welcoming <strong>and</strong> worry about the risks<br />

of employing a pers<strong>on</strong> with a disability, such as<br />

poor behavior or reduced productivity. The<br />

lack of formal employment opti<strong>on</strong>s in the<br />

country, particularly in rural areas, exacerbates<br />

this problem.<br />

Students can stay at home with their families<br />

in the extended household after school,<br />

but will need some way to earn m<strong>on</strong>ey for<br />

basic supplies <strong>and</strong> to reduce their burden <strong>on</strong><br />

their family. In this community, the needs are<br />

very basic, but the chances for employment<br />

outside the home are few <strong>and</strong> the possible pay<br />

is also small. Students have the best chance to<br />

earn m<strong>on</strong>ey by making or growing things <strong>and</strong><br />

selling them in the market. Families are sup-<br />

portive of their children <strong>and</strong> believe that they<br />

can participate in the family <strong>and</strong> community,<br />

but it has taken time <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>vince<br />

the parents <strong>and</strong> the community members.<br />

The children have the skills to facilitate<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> acceptance, but must be<br />

given the chance.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Despite the current emphasis <strong>on</strong> evidencebased<br />

practice in special educati<strong>on</strong> (Arnove,<br />

2003), there is a great need to educate students<br />

with disabilities to be members of the<br />

community <strong>and</strong> to provide them with the skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> knowledge to be active in the community.<br />

Given these circumstances, curriculum must<br />

be resp<strong>on</strong>sive to the local needs of the students<br />

<strong>and</strong> their community, as well as individual<br />

needs of the students. The curriculum at<br />

the school, as it is enacted by the teachers,<br />

should reflect the community’s funds of<br />

knowledge.<br />

This research can serve both as a study to<br />

support the work at the Irente Rainbow<br />

School <strong>and</strong> a case study for underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

how local beliefs <strong>and</strong> practices influence curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> community integrati<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with disabilities in other rural communities<br />

especially in Tanzania, throughout<br />

Africa <strong>and</strong> in developing countries. This study<br />

can help to explain the unique situati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Lushoto, Tanzania <strong>and</strong> be used in the development<br />

of a protocol for underst<strong>and</strong>ing the<br />

local c<strong>on</strong>text, which can be useful in other<br />

settings. The findings from the study also provide<br />

an opportunity to critically examine how<br />

to provide opportunities for individuals with<br />

disabilities in local communities for integrati<strong>on</strong><br />

in order to support them, their families<br />

<strong>and</strong> their community. This study has implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for practice in Tanzania <strong>and</strong> the United<br />

States, but it also has global implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

teacher educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> training around the<br />

world. The c<strong>on</strong>ceptual framework utilizing<br />

the adaptati<strong>on</strong>s of the three theories could be<br />

used to better underst<strong>and</strong> the community, the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent, <strong>and</strong> the pedagogy that could be most<br />

useful for particular schools, communities, or<br />

cultural groups. Funds of knowledge have<br />

been applied in several settings, but it c<strong>on</strong>tinues<br />

to be an important model for learning<br />

about <strong>and</strong> applying relevant knowledge im-<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 265


portant for educating students in a culturally<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sive way. Funds of knowledge should be<br />

applied to more settings to underst<strong>and</strong> how<br />

students with disabilities <strong>and</strong> their families<br />

who are also culturally diverse can most benefit<br />

from the curriculum they are learning.<br />

This study can also provide less<strong>on</strong>s about<br />

how to improve teacher training outside the<br />

United States by American-based professors<br />

<strong>and</strong> educators. When c<strong>on</strong>ducting training<br />

seminars in schools outside <strong>on</strong>e’s language<br />

<strong>and</strong> culture, teacher trainers should attempt<br />

to work with the pers<strong>on</strong> translating before<br />

the presentati<strong>on</strong>. The translator needs to be<br />

aware of what will happen in the presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the goals for the teachers. In the ideal<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>, the trainer would work with some<strong>on</strong>e<br />

familiar with the teachers, school, <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent to provide feedback <strong>on</strong> the training<br />

<strong>and</strong> its cultural relevancy.<br />

Natural settings are important for learning<br />

skills used in daily life. Primary schools with<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classrooms in Tanzania can use<br />

the findings from this study to examine how<br />

their classroom settings <strong>and</strong> curricula could<br />

be modified to meet the needs of their students.<br />

Planting a garden or building a goat<br />

enclosure for students to practice would be<br />

relatively simple in a typical primary school.<br />

Furthermore, helping to serve tea <strong>and</strong> clean<br />

up would be natural activities that already occur<br />

<strong>and</strong> students could participate in these<br />

activities. Implementing the less<strong>on</strong>s learned in<br />

this study in a Tanzanian c<strong>on</strong>text would be<br />

feasible because several pieces are already<br />

available. The critical variable would be the<br />

buy-in of families <strong>and</strong> teachers to this mode of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, 2010).<br />

This study has applicability in the United<br />

States as well. First, the results show that local<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text is important <strong>and</strong> knowledge of local<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text in designing functi<strong>on</strong>al curricula for<br />

students with developmental disabilities is essential.<br />

A curriculum must address not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

the various domains in functi<strong>on</strong>al academics,<br />

life skills, social skills, <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills<br />

(Patt<strong>on</strong>, Cr<strong>on</strong>in, & Jairrels, 1997), but the<br />

manner in which these domains are addressed<br />

needs to reflect the local c<strong>on</strong>text. To accomplish<br />

these goals, teachers need to know the<br />

important community funds of knowledge<br />

that impact their students <strong>and</strong> their families.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, classrooms should represent natu-<br />

ral settings for that community. Students acquire<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintain skills better when they<br />

learn <strong>and</strong> practice in community-based settings<br />

(Westling & Fox, 2000). In some special<br />

school in Tanzania, students practice cooking<br />

at school <strong>on</strong> wood or charcoal stoves because<br />

these are the two most comm<strong>on</strong> (<strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omical)<br />

ways to cook food in Lushoto. If<br />

most students in a city in the United States<br />

would do their laundry at a laundromat, the<br />

school should provide machines that resemble<br />

laundromat machines, rather than washboards<br />

or a set of stacked machines with different<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trols or machines that do not use<br />

coins. In additi<strong>on</strong>, students can benefit from<br />

participating in authentic work at the school<br />

that has purpose for them <strong>and</strong> the productivity<br />

of the school, as well as teaching the students<br />

particular work skills like answering the<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e or filing papers.<br />

The current st<strong>and</strong>ards-based reform movement<br />

is pushing for the use of evidence-based<br />

practices for all students regardless of their<br />

individual characteristics, needs, culture, or<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text. While evidence-based practices may<br />

be important in special educati<strong>on</strong>, the decisi<strong>on</strong><br />

about which specific practice to use<br />

should be based <strong>on</strong> the student, teacher, <strong>and</strong><br />

the c<strong>on</strong>text. One-size-fits-all educati<strong>on</strong> does not<br />

work without c<strong>on</strong>text. Instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> curriculum<br />

must have meaning <strong>and</strong> purpose for<br />

students, teachers, <strong>and</strong> the community.<br />

References<br />

Agran, M., Cavin, M., Wehmeyer, M. L., & Palmer,<br />

S. (2006). Participati<strong>on</strong> of students with moderate<br />

to severe disabilities in the general curriculum:<br />

The effects of the self-determined learning model<br />

of instructi<strong>on</strong>. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 31, 230–241.<br />

Arnove, R. F. (2003). Introducti<strong>on</strong>: Reframing comparative<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>: The dialectic of the global<br />

<strong>and</strong> the local. In R. F. Arnove & C. A. Torres<br />

(Eds.), Comparative educati<strong>on</strong>: The dialectic of the<br />

global <strong>and</strong> the local (2nd ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman<br />

<strong>and</strong> Littlefield Publishers, Inc.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2004). How size <strong>and</strong> setting impact<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> in rural schools. The Rural Educator, 25,<br />

38–42.<br />

Br<strong>on</strong>fenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory.<br />

In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development:<br />

Revised formulati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> current issues (pp.<br />

187–249). L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: Jessica Kingsley.<br />

266 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Browder, D. M., & Cooper-Duffy, K. (2003). Evidence-based<br />

practices for students with severe disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> the requirement for accountability in<br />

“No Child Left Behind.” Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

37, 157–163.<br />

Brown, L., McLean, M. B., Hamre-Nietupski, S.,<br />

Pumpian, I., Creto, N., & Grunewald, L. (1979).<br />

A strategy for developing chr<strong>on</strong>ological ageappropriate<br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al curricular c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

for severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped adolescents <strong>and</strong> young<br />

adults. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 81–90.<br />

Brusling, C. (2005). Evidence-based practice in teaching<br />

<strong>and</strong> teacher educati<strong>on</strong>. Paper presented at the Professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Development of Teachers in a Lifel<strong>on</strong>g<br />

Perspective: Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong>, Knowledge Producti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Instituti<strong>on</strong>al Reform. Retrieved from<br />

www.cvustork.dk/agora<br />

Charmaz, K. (2005). Grounded theory in the 21st<br />

century: Applicati<strong>on</strong>s for advancing social justice<br />

studies. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.),<br />

The Sage h<strong>and</strong>book of qualitative research (3rd ed.,<br />

pp. xix, 1210). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Charmaz, K. (2005). Grounded theory in the 21st<br />

century. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.),<br />

The Sage h<strong>and</strong>book of qualitative research (3rd ed.).<br />

Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Charmaz, K. (2006). C<strong>on</strong>structing grounded theory: a<br />

practical guide through qualitative analysis. Thous<strong>and</strong><br />

Oaks, CA: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Chaudhry, V., & Owen, D. (2005). Examining inclusi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Disability <strong>and</strong> community driven development.<br />

Social Development Notes, (100), 1–4.<br />

Retrieved from siteresources.worldbank.org/<br />

DISABILITY/Resources/280658-1172671461088/<br />

ExaminingInclusi<strong>on</strong>.pdf<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E. (1996). Life skills curriculum for<br />

students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 53–68.<br />

Davies, P. (1999). What is evidence-based educati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

British Journal of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Studies, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 108–<br />

121.<br />

Deku, P. (2002). Effects of training in communitybased<br />

rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> attitudinal change of people<br />

toward people with disabilities. I F E PsychologIA,<br />

10, 175–184.<br />

Devlieger, P. J. (1994). Disability <strong>and</strong> community<br />

acti<strong>on</strong> in a Zimbabwean community: priorities<br />

based <strong>on</strong> a biocultural approach. Journal of the<br />

Steward Anthropological Society, 22, 41–57.<br />

Devlieger, P. J. (1999). Frames of reference in African<br />

proverbs <strong>on</strong> disability. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of<br />

Disability, Development <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 46, 439–451.<br />

Dym<strong>on</strong>d, S. K., & Orelove, F. P. (2001). What c<strong>on</strong>stitutes<br />

effective curricula for students with severe<br />

disabilities? Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 9, 109–122.<br />

Eisner, E. (1965). Levels of curriculum <strong>and</strong> curric-<br />

ulum research. Elementary School Journal, 66, 155–<br />

162.<br />

Gagné, R. M. (1966). Curriculum Research <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Promoti<strong>on</strong> of Learning. Paper presented at the<br />

AERA Meeting.<br />

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of<br />

grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research.<br />

New York: Aldine De Gruyter.<br />

G<strong>on</strong>zález, N., Moll, L. C., & Amanti, C. (Eds.).<br />

(2005). Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in<br />

households, communities, <strong>and</strong> classrooms (Vol. ix–<br />

307). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum<br />

Associates, Inc.<br />

G<strong>on</strong>zález, N., Moll, L. C., Tenery, M. F., Rivera, A.,<br />

Rendón, P., G<strong>on</strong>zales, R., <strong>and</strong> Amanti, C. (1995).<br />

Funds of knowledge for teaching in latino households.<br />

Urban Educati<strong>on</strong>, 29, 444–<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>1.<br />

Halpern, A. S., & Benz, M. R. (1987). A statewide<br />

examinati<strong>on</strong> of sec<strong>on</strong>dary special educati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with mild disabilities: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

the high school curriculum. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

54, 122–129.<br />

Hargreaves, D. (1996). Teaching as a research-based<br />

professi<strong>on</strong>: possibilities <strong>and</strong> prospects. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Teacher Training Agency.<br />

Ihenacho, I. J. (1985). Introducti<strong>on</strong> to special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in Nigeria: historical <strong>and</strong> sociological perspectives. Plateau<br />

State, Nigeria: 3 <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> Printing Press Limited.<br />

Ingstad, B. (1995). Public discourse <strong>on</strong> rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

from Norway to Botswana. In B. Ingstad &<br />

S. R. Whyte (Eds.), Disability <strong>and</strong> culture. Berkeley,<br />

CA: University of California Press.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong> Jr., M. (2007). Definiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> models in<br />

curriculum theory. Educati<strong>on</strong>al Theory, 17, 127–140.<br />

Kalyanpur, M. (1996). The influence of western<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> community based services in<br />

India. Disability <strong>and</strong> Society, 11, 249–270.<br />

Kaplan, D. (1999). The Definiti<strong>on</strong> of Disability. Retrieved<br />

from http://www.accessiblesociety.org/<br />

topics/demographics-identity/dkaplanpaper.htm<br />

Karakoski, J., & Stroem, K. (2005). Special needs educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in Tanzania: A fact-finding missi<strong>on</strong> final report:<br />

Ministry of Foreign Affairs of Finl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Kisanji, J. (1995). Attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefs about disability<br />

in Tanzania. In B. O’Toole & R. McC<strong>on</strong>key<br />

(Eds.), Innovati<strong>on</strong>s in developing countries for people<br />

with disabilities (pp. 51–70). Chorley, Engl<strong>and</strong>: Lisieux<br />

Hall Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Kisanji, J. (1995). The relevance of indigenous customary<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> principles in the formulati<strong>on</strong> of special needs<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> policy. Paper presented at the Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong> C<strong>on</strong>gress.<br />

Kisanji, J. (1998). The march towards inclusive educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in n<strong>on</strong>-Western countries: Retracing the<br />

steps. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Inclusive Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2,<br />

55–72.<br />

Kiyaga, N. B., & Moores, D. F. (2003). Deafness in<br />

Life in the Community: A Tanzanian Perspective / 267


Sub-Saharan Africa. American Annals of the Deaf,<br />

148, 18–24.<br />

Kristensen, K., Omagor-Loican, M., & Onen, N.<br />

(2003). The inclusi<strong>on</strong> of learners with barriers to<br />

learning <strong>and</strong> development into ordinary school<br />

settings: A challenge for Ug<strong>and</strong>a. British Journal of<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 30, 194–201.<br />

Marsh, C. J. (1997). Key c<strong>on</strong>epts for underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

curriculum: Planning, management, <strong>and</strong> ideology<br />

(Vol. 2). L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>: The Falmer Press.<br />

McMillan, J. H., & Schumacher, S. (2006). Research<br />

in educati<strong>on</strong>: Evidence-based inquiry (6th ed.). Bost<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Pears<strong>on</strong>/Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ministry of Labour Youth Development <strong>and</strong> Sports.<br />

(2004). Nati<strong>on</strong>al policy <strong>on</strong> disability. Dar es Salaam,<br />

Tanzania: United Republic of Tanzania.<br />

The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Center for Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Research<br />

(NCSER) Home Page. (2008). Retrieved<br />

from http://ies.ed.gov/ncser/<br />

Nietupski, J., Hamre-Nietupski, S., Curtin, S., &<br />

Shirkanth, K. (1997). A review of curricular research<br />

in severe disabilities from 1976 to 1995 in<br />

six selected journals. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

31, 36–55.<br />

Odom, S. L., Brantlinger, E., Gersten, R., Horner,<br />

R. H., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, B., & Harris, K. R. (2005).<br />

Research in special educati<strong>on</strong>: Scientific methods<br />

<strong>and</strong> evidence-based practice. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

71, 137–148.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E., & Jairrels, V. (1997).<br />

Curricular implicati<strong>on</strong>s of transiti<strong>on</strong>: Life skills as<br />

an integral part of transiti<strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>. Remedial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 294–306.<br />

Peters, S. J. (1993). An ideological-cultural framework<br />

for the study of disability. In S. J. Peters<br />

(Ed.), Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> disability in cross-cultural perspective<br />

(pp. 19–35). New York: Garl<strong>and</strong> Publishing,<br />

Inc.<br />

Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Smith, J. D., & Roderique,<br />

T. W. (1991). Issues in program design for<br />

elementary students with mild retardati<strong>on</strong>: Emphasis<br />

<strong>on</strong> curriculum development. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 26, 142–150.<br />

Rogoff, B., Moore, L., Najafi, B., Dexter, A., Correa-<br />

Chavez, M., & Solis, J. (2007). Children’s development<br />

of cultural repetroires through participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in everyday routines <strong>and</strong> practices. In J. E.<br />

Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of socializati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Theory <strong>and</strong> research. New York: The Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

Schmalle, B., & Retish, P. M. (1989). Job-task reporting<br />

system. Teaching Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 21, 14–<br />

18.<br />

Shemweta, M. (2008). Master’s Thesis. Makumira<br />

University, Usa River, Tanzania.<br />

Snell, M. E. (1997). Teaching young children <strong>and</strong><br />

young adults with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in school<br />

programs: Current research. Behavior Change, 14,<br />

73–105.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>e-MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, A. (2010). From Goats to Gardens:<br />

Preparing Children with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> for<br />

Community Integrati<strong>on</strong> in Rural Tanzania. Dissertati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Indiana University, Bloomingt<strong>on</strong>, Indiana.<br />

Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. M. (1990). Basics of<br />

qualitative research: Grounded theory procedures <strong>and</strong><br />

techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. M. (1998). Basics of<br />

qualitative research: Techniques <strong>and</strong> procedures for developing<br />

grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thous<strong>and</strong><br />

Oaks, CA: Sage Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

United Republic of Tanzania. (2003). 2002 Populati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> housing census. Retrieved from http://<br />

www.tanzania.go.tz/census/census/reports.htm<br />

Westling, D. L., & Fox, L. (2000). Teaching students<br />

with severe disabilities. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Received: 9 August 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 12 October 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 14 December 2011<br />

268 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 269–279<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Two Approaches to Ph<strong>on</strong>ics Instructi<strong>on</strong>: Comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

Effects with Children with Significant Cognitive Disability<br />

Elizabeth Grace Finnegan<br />

St. Thomas Aquinas College<br />

Abstract: The effects of two systematic methods of ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> for children with significant cognitive<br />

disability were compared. Fifty-two participants, aged 5–12 years were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned to <strong>on</strong>e of three<br />

treatment groups: (i) a synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>, (ii) an analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> group, <strong>and</strong> (iii) a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group. Participants in the synthetic <strong>and</strong> analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics groups received twelve sessi<strong>on</strong>s of individual<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. Findings suggest that for many students with significant cognitive disability systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> is beneficial. Further research should focus <strong>on</strong> the maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of ph<strong>on</strong>ics skills<br />

acquired by children with significant cognitive disability.<br />

Under the Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Act<br />

(IDEA, 2004), a small percentage of children<br />

with disabilities take alternate assessments instead<br />

of the general statewide assessment.<br />

These students are defined as students with<br />

significant cognitive disability (IDEA, 2004).<br />

Research <strong>on</strong> how best to teach core academic<br />

subjects to this populati<strong>on</strong> is sparse. Existing<br />

research shows that students with significant<br />

cognitive disability can learn <strong>and</strong> acquire new<br />

skills (Browder, Fallin, Davis, & Karv<strong>on</strong>en,<br />

2003). However, research is still needed to<br />

substantiate which methods of instructi<strong>on</strong> are<br />

effective in enabling students with significant<br />

cognitive disability to meet st<strong>and</strong>ards in academic<br />

subjects such as reading <strong>and</strong> mathematics.<br />

This study compared the effects of two<br />

approaches to ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> in teaching<br />

children with significant cognitive disability to<br />

read.<br />

This research was c<strong>on</strong>ducted while Elizabeth<br />

Grace Finnegan was a student in Department of<br />

Health <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Studies, Teachers College,<br />

Columbia University, New York as part of her dissertati<strong>on</strong><br />

requirements. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

this article should be addressed to Elizabeth<br />

Grace Finnegan, St. Thomas Aquinas College, 125<br />

Route 340, Sparkill, NY10976. Email: efinnegan@<br />

stac.edu<br />

Effectiveness of Ph<strong>on</strong>ics Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Explicit, systematic instructi<strong>on</strong> in ph<strong>on</strong>ics has<br />

been established as being beneficial for students<br />

in the beginning stages of reading <strong>and</strong><br />

for students with difficulties learning to read<br />

(Chall, 1996; Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading Panel, NRP,<br />

2000). Hoover <strong>and</strong> Gough (1990) proposed<br />

that “decoding” skills <strong>and</strong> linguistic comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

need to be dissociated in order for<br />

children to gain substantial skills in either, but<br />

for reading to be effective they need to be<br />

combined. Ehri, Nunes, Stahl <strong>and</strong> Willows<br />

(2001) found all methods of ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to be effective but systematic methods<br />

have been found to be more effective than<br />

methods than were not, with synthetic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

having the largest effect (d 0.45).<br />

There are many reas<strong>on</strong>s why ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

has not been an integral part of<br />

reading instructi<strong>on</strong> for children with significant<br />

cognitive disability. Three of those reas<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

instituti<strong>on</strong>al beliefs, historical exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

from research studies, <strong>and</strong> strength of literature<br />

supporting sight word instructi<strong>on</strong>, will be<br />

discussed.<br />

Instituti<strong>on</strong>al Beliefs<br />

Within the educati<strong>on</strong>al community there are a<br />

number of instituti<strong>on</strong>al beliefs that have<br />

shaped reading instructi<strong>on</strong> for children with<br />

significant cognitive disability. A belief that<br />

children with cognitive disability were not ca-<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 269


pable of learning to read, a belief that children<br />

needed prerequisite skills in order to<br />

learn to read <strong>and</strong> a belief that other skills were<br />

more important to learn. These beliefs originated<br />

in the historical treatment of individuals<br />

with severe disabilities (Winzer, 1993) <strong>and</strong><br />

in the subsequent writings of influential researchers.<br />

Dolch <strong>and</strong> Bloomster (1937), for<br />

example, c<strong>on</strong>cluded that a child must have a<br />

mental age of seven or above to benefit from<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>. Kliewer <strong>and</strong> Biklen<br />

(2001) pointed to developmental research as<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for creating a curriculum founded<br />

up<strong>on</strong> a hierarchy of sub-skills, for example<br />

attending skills must be secure before more<br />

advanced skills such as reading are taught.<br />

These beliefs effectively reduced the opportunities<br />

for children with significant cognitive<br />

disability to acquire literacy skills.<br />

Recent surveys indicate that parents <strong>and</strong><br />

teachers rate academic skills like reading, below<br />

skills such as communicati<strong>on</strong> skills,<br />

grooming skills <strong>and</strong> social skills (Agran, Alper,<br />

& Wehmeyer 2002; Flowers, Ahlgrim-Delzell,<br />

Browder, & Spo<strong>on</strong>er, 2005) for children with<br />

significant disabilities. Opportunities for students<br />

with significant cognitive disability to<br />

develop literacy skills have c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be<br />

replaced with less<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al skills, therapy<br />

skills, or avoided altogether (Kliewer &<br />

L<strong>and</strong>is, 1999).<br />

Historical Exclusi<strong>on</strong> from Research Studies<br />

In past studies <strong>on</strong> reading instructi<strong>on</strong> children<br />

with significant cognitive disability were not<br />

included. Chall’s (1996) major synthesis of<br />

research regarding effective reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that first <strong>and</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d graders,<br />

<strong>and</strong> students with a low IQ who received training<br />

in ph<strong>on</strong>ics, were ahead of their peers who<br />

used basal readers <strong>on</strong> word recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

oral reading. In c<strong>on</strong>sidering this synthesis retrospectively,<br />

it is not clear whether this includes<br />

students with significant cognitive disability<br />

or not. Low IQ could mean lowaverage,<br />

below average, or slow learner.<br />

Students with significant cognitive disability<br />

were not necessarily receiving schooling at the<br />

time this synthesis was c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>and</strong> probably<br />

not included in the studies.<br />

Children with significant cognitive disability<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be excluded from major reviews<br />

of the literature. The NRP (2000) did not<br />

examine students whose IQs fell outside of<br />

average range because the studies involving<br />

these groups did not meet other criteria for<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> in their analysis. Such exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

makes it difficult to substantiate which methods<br />

of reading instructi<strong>on</strong> meet the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

needs of children with significant cognitive<br />

disability.<br />

Literature Supporting Sight Word Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

There are more published studies <strong>on</strong> sight<br />

word instructi<strong>on</strong> for children with significant<br />

disabilities than ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>. In a<br />

meta-analysis of 32 single-subject studies <strong>on</strong><br />

the instructi<strong>on</strong> of sight words, Browder <strong>and</strong><br />

Xin (1998) found that there were c<strong>on</strong>siderable<br />

effects for sight word training. Most of<br />

these studies measured the number of words<br />

children could read correctly, <strong>and</strong> did not<br />

ascertain whether or not participants could<br />

use the words in a generalized way. In comparis<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Joseph <strong>and</strong> Seery (2004) found <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

seven studies published over a period of<br />

twelve years <strong>on</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics-based instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with significant cognitive disability.<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>ics Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Children with Significant<br />

Cognitive Disability<br />

Although research <strong>on</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children with significant cognitive disability<br />

has been sparse, studies have shown that it can<br />

be effective. Four single-subject design studies<br />

examined the effectiveness of direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

with children with moderate intellectual<br />

disability. Participants in the studies had IQ<br />

scores of between 38–76 (Barbetta, Heward, &<br />

Bradley, 1993; Bradford, Shippen, Alberto,<br />

Houchins, & Flores, 2006; Flores, Shippen,<br />

Alberto, & Crowe, 2004; Waugh, Fredrick, &<br />

Alberto, 2009). The samples in these studies<br />

were small: six participants (Flores et al.,<br />

2004), three participants (Bradford et al.,<br />

2006), five participants (Barbetta et al., 1993)<br />

<strong>and</strong> three participants (Waugh et al., 2009).<br />

Notably, Waugh et al. (2009) c<strong>on</strong>ducted a<br />

study to determine if letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

was taught systematically would the participants<br />

be able to sound out new words using<br />

those letter sounds? The researchers found<br />

that participants were able to master some sets<br />

270 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


of words, <strong>and</strong> in the short term were able to<br />

read previously unseen words, but had difficulty<br />

recalling them over time. Students were<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly able to read a few novel words after<br />

twelve weeks of interventi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> it was discussed<br />

whether teaching letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

in isolati<strong>on</strong> with no reference to<br />

the meaning of the words was a factor in the<br />

participants’ limited success. Two other studies<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated that ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

could be effective (Bradford et al., 2006;<br />

Flores et al., 2004), while the fourth (Barbetta<br />

et al., 1993) was inc<strong>on</strong>clusive.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, C<strong>on</strong>ners, Atwell, Rosenquist<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sligh (2001) investigated the cognitive<br />

processes involved in ph<strong>on</strong>ological decoding.<br />

The study included children with IQ scores of<br />

at least 40 but less than 70, <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cluded that<br />

the ability to rehearse ph<strong>on</strong>ological informati<strong>on</strong><br />

in the working memory seemed to offer a<br />

better advantage in word decoding than overall<br />

IQ.<br />

A comprehensive ph<strong>on</strong>ics-based direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

reading program was found to be<br />

effective in improving early reading <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills for participants with significant<br />

cognitive disability (Allor, Mathes, Roberts,<br />

J<strong>on</strong>es, & Champlin, 2010). Twenty-eight students<br />

in grades 1–4 (mean age of 9.46 years)<br />

with IQ scores ranging from 40–55 were r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

assigned into either a treatment or a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group. The treatment group received<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> based <strong>on</strong> Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

Reading, Level 1 (Mathes & Torgesen, 2005)<br />

for two years. Results were significant for<br />

blending words <strong>and</strong> segmenting words <strong>on</strong> The<br />

Comprehensive Test of Ph<strong>on</strong>ological Processing<br />

(Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999), ph<strong>on</strong>emic<br />

decoding <strong>on</strong> the Test of Word Reading<br />

Efficiency (Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte,<br />

1999) <strong>and</strong> word attack skills <strong>on</strong> The Woodcock<br />

Language Proficiency Battery (Woodcock, 1991),<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating that teaching ph<strong>on</strong>ics using direct<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> was effective in increasing participants’<br />

word attack <strong>and</strong> other reading skills.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Fifty two participants, aged 5–12 years of age<br />

from five schools in the New York metropolitan<br />

area, all eligible for alternate assessment as<br />

determined by their Individual Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Plan (IEP) team <strong>and</strong> thereby c<strong>on</strong>sidered to<br />

have a significant cognitive disability were included.<br />

Schools were selected <strong>on</strong> their willingness<br />

to be involved with the study. Sixty-eight<br />

students were invited to participate in the<br />

study <strong>and</strong> informed c<strong>on</strong>sent was secured from<br />

fifty-four participants’ legal guardians. Permissi<strong>on</strong><br />

was also given for the school district to<br />

release IQ scores where available. Two participants<br />

were not included in the data analysis,<br />

as the researcher was not available to complete<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> with them.<br />

Demographic informati<strong>on</strong> was collected<br />

through a survey sent home to participants’<br />

parents. The mean age of the participants was<br />

8.661 years (SD 2.560). IQ scores were collected<br />

<strong>on</strong> 29 participants (M 55.96, SD <br />

12.65). Seven of the participants were c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

to be n<strong>on</strong>-verbal. All participants c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to receive reading instructi<strong>on</strong>, which varied<br />

widely from their teacher (see Table 1).<br />

Participants were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned to <strong>on</strong>e<br />

of three treatment groups: synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

treatment group, analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group, or a c<strong>on</strong>trol group. One-way analyses<br />

of variances (ANOVAs) showed no significant<br />

differences <strong>on</strong> age or IQ between the three<br />

treatment groups. Chi-square tests were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

to compare the treatment groups <strong>on</strong><br />

the categorical variables of gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity,<br />

<strong>and</strong> showed no significant differences. Demographic<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> is summarized in<br />

Table 2.<br />

Design<br />

An experimental pretest/posttest <strong>on</strong>e-way<br />

ANOVA design with multiple performance<br />

measures was used.<br />

Independent variable. The independent<br />

variable was the treatment variable or instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. In the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

treatment c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> participants learned individual<br />

letter sounds <strong>and</strong> how to blend them to<br />

make a word (e.g., /b/, /a/, <strong>and</strong> /t/ make<br />

bat). In the analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

participants learned the sounds of comm<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ants e.g., /b/ <strong>and</strong> comm<strong>on</strong><br />

“rimes” e.g., /at/. By combining a visual<br />

“rime” with comm<strong>on</strong> letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dences<br />

participants learned to read words<br />

with similar patterns (e.g., bat, mat, cat). The<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 271


TABLE 1<br />

Reading Programs Used to Teach Reading to Participants<br />

Reading Program/Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group c<strong>on</strong>tinued with their regular<br />

reading program with no additi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Dependent variables. Measures of reading<br />

achievement directly related to word identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> decoding skills were used. Two<br />

subtests <strong>on</strong> Woodcock-Johns<strong>on</strong> III Diagnostic<br />

Reading Battery (WJIII DRB, Schrank, et al.,<br />

2004) were used: (i) Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

measures the ability of the resp<strong>on</strong>dent to<br />

identify letters <strong>and</strong> words, (ii) Word Attack<br />

measures the ability of the resp<strong>on</strong>dent to apply<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ic skills to decode n<strong>on</strong>-words. Together<br />

the two tests form a basic reading skills<br />

Synthetic<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

Number of Participants using Program<br />

Analogy<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>ics C<strong>on</strong>trol Total<br />

Letter-sound recogniti<strong>on</strong> using flashcards 2 3 1 5<br />

Letter-sound recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> sight-words 5 5 5 15<br />

Dolch Sight Words (Dolch, 1948) 0 2 1 3<br />

Edmark Reading Series (Edmark, 1992) 2 3 4 9<br />

Reading Milest<strong>on</strong>es (Quigley, McNally,<br />

Rose, & King, 2001)<br />

0 1 3 4<br />

Merrill Reading Series (Mercer,<br />

Rudolph, & Wils<strong>on</strong>, 1998)<br />

1 1 1 3<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong> Reading Program (Wils<strong>on</strong>, n.d.) 0 2 0 2<br />

My Sidewalks-modified by teacher (Juel,<br />

Patatore, & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, 2008)<br />

7 4 2 13<br />

Authentic Children’s Literature with<br />

teacher-made questi<strong>on</strong>naires<br />

1 1 1 3<br />

TABLE 2<br />

Demographic Characteristics of Participants<br />

Characteristic<br />

Synthetic<br />

(n 17)<br />

Treatment Group<br />

Analogy<br />

(n 18)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

(n 19)<br />

Female 41% 39% 41%<br />

Ethnicity<br />

Caucasian 69% 55% 70%<br />

African American 0% 11% 0%<br />

Hispanic 19% 12% 12%<br />

Asian 12% 0% 18%<br />

Other 0% 22% 0%<br />

cluster with a mean reliability of 0.93 in the<br />

age range of 5–19 years. The cluster has a<br />

str<strong>on</strong>g correlati<strong>on</strong> with the Kaufman Test of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Achievement (KTEA) (Kaufman<br />

& Kaufman, 1985), <strong>and</strong> the Wechsler Individual<br />

Achievement Test (Wechsler, 1992), evidence<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>current validity.<br />

The initial items <strong>on</strong> the WJIII DRB tests did<br />

not require resp<strong>on</strong>dents to give verbal answers<br />

but required them to point to answers. Four of<br />

the n<strong>on</strong>-verbal participants did not progress<br />

bey<strong>on</strong>d these items. For items in which resp<strong>on</strong>dents<br />

were required to read a word, items<br />

were presented r<strong>and</strong>omly in groups of three<br />

words, <strong>and</strong> read aloud by the researcher. N<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

participants were asked to point to the<br />

word read aloud.<br />

Two measures created by the researcher<br />

were also used. Training word identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

measured the number of words participants<br />

read correctly after those words had been explicitly<br />

taught to <strong>and</strong> practiced by participants.<br />

Transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong> measured<br />

the number of words participants read correctly<br />

which had a similar ph<strong>on</strong>etic structure<br />

to the training words but had not been practiced<br />

by participants during the study. Forty<br />

training words <strong>and</strong> forty transfer words were<br />

selected using the following procedure. The<br />

Dolch High Frequency Reading List (Dolch,<br />

272 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 3<br />

Word Lists<br />

List No. Training Words Transfer Words<br />

1 at, cat, mat, pat, rat, bat, hat, sat, vat, fat<br />

2 fan, pan, can, tan, van an, ban, man, ran, dan<br />

3 it, hit, sit, fit, lit pit, kit, bit, quit, wit<br />

4 got, pot, hot, lot, not dot, rot, tot, cot, jot<br />

5 old, cold, hold, told, scold bold, gold, mold, fold, sold<br />

6 bank, blank, plank, sank, thank clank, rank, spank, tank, yank<br />

7 behind, blind, find, kind, wind bind, grind, hind, mind, rind<br />

8 art, artist, chart, dart, start depart, martin, part, smart, tart<br />

1948) <strong>and</strong> Fry’s 300 Instant Sight Words (Fry<br />

& Kress, 2006) were compared. Words comm<strong>on</strong><br />

to both lists were categorized according<br />

to their ph<strong>on</strong>etic structure, <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

words with the same ph<strong>on</strong>etic structure were<br />

added to each group. Words with a comm<strong>on</strong><br />

ph<strong>on</strong>etic structure were grouped into eight<br />

lists <strong>and</strong> sequenced to reflect other methods<br />

of ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> in which words are sequences<br />

in an order that facilitates systematic<br />

<strong>and</strong> cumulative learning. Words with short<br />

vowels were introduced first, followed by<br />

blends <strong>and</strong> vowel combinati<strong>on</strong>s in the order<br />

shown in Table 3. N<strong>on</strong>-verbal participants<br />

were presented with three words <strong>and</strong> then<br />

asked to point or indicate which word was<br />

being spoken by the researcher.<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s alpha was used to calculate internal-c<strong>on</strong>sistency<br />

for all four measures used<br />

in the study. For the first 24 items in Letter-<br />

Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> p xx’ .948, for the first<br />

ten items in Word Attack p xx’ .761, for the<br />

first ten items in training word identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

p xx’ .924 <strong>and</strong> for the first ten items in transfer<br />

word identificati<strong>on</strong> p xx’ .924.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

To determine the starting point of the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

scores <strong>on</strong> the pretest for training<br />

words were used. If a participant read 60% or<br />

less of the words in a list correctly then interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

started with that list. If a participant<br />

read 80% or more of the words in a list correctly<br />

then pretesting c<strong>on</strong>tinued. The interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

took place in the participant’s classroom,<br />

<strong>and</strong> was administered individually to<br />

participants, by the researcher who was<br />

trained <strong>and</strong> experienced in ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

using sound-symbol associati<strong>on</strong>. Participants<br />

in the synthetic <strong>and</strong> analogy treatment<br />

groups received twelve sessi<strong>on</strong>s of ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

lasting 15–20 minutes, a time commensurate<br />

with instructi<strong>on</strong>al time recommended<br />

for typically developing children in<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics (NRP, 2000). The instructi<strong>on</strong> in both<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s followed the same format. Participants<br />

were asked to imitate <strong>and</strong> practice the<br />

letter sounds <strong>and</strong> words presented <strong>on</strong> 5”x7”<br />

flashcards in bold Ariel f<strong>on</strong>t size 96 <strong>and</strong> read<br />

aloud to the participant. After practicing the<br />

letter sounds <strong>and</strong> words, participants were<br />

given a matching exercise in which they<br />

matched the words they had learned to pictures.<br />

Participants were praised for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

For incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses the sound or<br />

word was modeled again.<br />

Results<br />

Main Analysis<br />

One-way Analyses of the Variance (ANOVAs)<br />

were performed to compare pretest scores <strong>on</strong><br />

all four dependent variables. No significant<br />

differences were found between the three<br />

treatment groups. Means in all the posttest<br />

measures were higher than the means in the<br />

pretest for all three groups (Table 4). Pears<strong>on</strong><br />

r correlati<strong>on</strong>s for pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest measures<br />

of the dependent variables (Table 5)<br />

showed that the scores were highly correlated<br />

<strong>on</strong> all four measures. The pattern of pretest<br />

<strong>and</strong> posttest correlati<strong>on</strong>s for the total sample<br />

<strong>and</strong> each of the treatment groups support the<br />

assumpti<strong>on</strong> of linearity <strong>and</strong> homogeneity of<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 273


TABLE 4<br />

Means <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> Dependent Variables<br />

Measures<br />

within-group regressi<strong>on</strong>. Analyses of the covariance<br />

(ANCOVAs) were used to compare the<br />

posttest scores of the three treatment groups<br />

<strong>on</strong> all four dependent variables. One-way AN-<br />

COVA showed no significant difference between<br />

the treatment groups in posttest scores<br />

after c<strong>on</strong>trolling for pretest scores <strong>on</strong> either<br />

Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> or the Word Attack.<br />

One-way ANCOVA for training word<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> showed a significant difference<br />

between treatment groups in posttest scores<br />

after c<strong>on</strong>trolling for pretest scores, F(2,48) <br />

16.353, p .01. Pairwise comparis<strong>on</strong>s using<br />

B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i’s corrected values for the means<br />

(synthetic M 6.36, analogy M 5.29, c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

M 3.32) showed a significant difference<br />

between the adjusted means of the synthetic<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group. The difference between the adjusted<br />

Synthetic (n 17) Analogy (n 18) C<strong>on</strong>trol (n 17)<br />

Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post<br />

Letter-Word ID 14.59 16.24 15.28 16.78 13.82 14.06<br />

(11.59) (11.50) (7.29) (7.75) (9.44) (9.06)<br />

Word Attack 1.62 4.12 1.67 3.61 1.78 2.29<br />

(3.11) (5.26) (1.61) (2.33) (1.24) (2.73)<br />

Training Word 4.95 11.18 3.94 9.78 3.41 3.94<br />

(10.69) (11.24) (8.19) (9.74) (4.96) (5.73)<br />

Transfer Word 4.00 7.35 3.00 5.19 2.06 3.23<br />

(9.75) (11.04) (7.75) (7.94) (3.83) (4.08)<br />

TABLE 5<br />

Correlati<strong>on</strong>s of Pretest <strong>and</strong> Posttest Variables<br />

Synthetic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

(n 17)<br />

mean scores was 5.639, (ES 0.542). There<br />

was also a significant difference between the<br />

adjusted means of the analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trol group. The difference<br />

between the adjusted mean scores was<br />

5.281, (ES0.508). In both the treatment<br />

groups (synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics <strong>and</strong> analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics)<br />

the means were higher than the mean of<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>trol group. There was not a significant<br />

difference between the adjusted mean of the<br />

synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics group <strong>and</strong> the analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

groups. Pairwise comparis<strong>on</strong>s are reported<br />

in Table 6.<br />

One-way ANCOVA for transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

showed that there was a significant<br />

difference between treatment groups <strong>on</strong> posttest<br />

scores after c<strong>on</strong>trolling for pretest scores,<br />

F(2,48) 4.293, p .05. Pairwise comparis<strong>on</strong>s<br />

using B<strong>on</strong>ferr<strong>on</strong>i’s corrected values for<br />

Analogy Ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

(n 18)<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

(n 17)<br />

Total Sample<br />

(N 52)<br />

Pre/Post .97** .95** .98** .96**<br />

Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

Pre/Post .97** .82** .72** .90**<br />

Word Attack<br />

Pre/Post .93** .94** .96** .90**<br />

training words<br />

Pre/Post .96** .88** .91** .92**<br />

Transfer words<br />

** Correlati<strong>on</strong> is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).<br />

274 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 6<br />

Pairwise Comparis<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> Posttest Scores for Training <strong>and</strong> Transfer Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

(I) Group (J) Group<br />

the means showed a significant difference between<br />

the adjusted means of the synthetic<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group. The difference between the adjusted<br />

mean scores was 3.039, (ES 0.330). Posttest<br />

scores were significantly higher in training<br />

word identificati<strong>on</strong> in children with significant<br />

cognitive disability for both the synthetic<br />

<strong>and</strong> analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment groups than<br />

that of the c<strong>on</strong>trol group, with the posttest<br />

scores of the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group being significantly higher than those of<br />

the analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group. The<br />

scores of the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group were also significantly higher than the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group in transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>al Analyses<br />

Two further questi<strong>on</strong>s arose in the course of<br />

the study that warranted further investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

First, even though the experimental groups<br />

did not differ significantly from the c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong><br />

either Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> or Word Attack,<br />

did participants’ performance <strong>on</strong> those<br />

measures improve over the course of the<br />

study? Paired-samples t tests were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

to pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest scores. Significant differences<br />

were found between the mean of the<br />

pretest <strong>and</strong> the mean of the posttest for the<br />

synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group t(16)2.384,<br />

p .030, .05 <strong>and</strong> the analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

treatment group t(17) 2.519, p .022, .05<br />

<strong>on</strong> Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong>. Significant differences<br />

were found between the mean of the<br />

Mean Error<br />

Difference (I-J) Std. Sig.<br />

Training Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol Synthetic 5.639* 1.110 .000<br />

Analogy 5.281* 1.091 .000<br />

Synthetic Analogy .359 1.092 1.000<br />

Transfer Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol Synthetic 3.039* 1.046 .017<br />

Analogy 1.176 1.027 .774<br />

Synthetic Analogy 1.863 1.027 .228<br />

* The mean is significant at the .05 level.<br />

pretest <strong>and</strong> the mean of the posttest for all<br />

three treatment c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> Word Attack;<br />

synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group t(16)3.955,<br />

p .001, .01, analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group t(17) 6.115, p .000, .05 <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol t(16)2.273, p .037, .05. Using<br />

estimated values for age <strong>and</strong> grade equivalency<br />

provided by the WJIII DRB (Table 7) it<br />

can be seen that <strong>on</strong> Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

improvement was commensurate with the<br />

time spent <strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>, about 12 m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

However, <strong>on</strong> Word Attack age <strong>and</strong> grade<br />

equivalencies for the synthetic <strong>and</strong> analogy<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment groups went up a full year.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, were there individual differences<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g participants with significant cognitive<br />

disability in their resp<strong>on</strong>se to systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>? Nine participants in the<br />

study scored zero <strong>on</strong> the pretests <strong>and</strong> posttests<br />

<strong>on</strong> both training word identificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong>. ANOVAs were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted to compare these nine participants<br />

with the other forty-three participants in the<br />

study <strong>on</strong> age, IQ <strong>and</strong> pretest scores <strong>on</strong> the<br />

four dependent variables. There were significant<br />

differences <strong>on</strong> the Letter-Word identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

pretest F(1,51) 13.264, p .05 <strong>and</strong><br />

the Word Attack pretest F(1,51) 4.679, p <br />

.05. It was also detected that five participants<br />

in the study had scores greater than 20 words<br />

correct <strong>on</strong> the pretest for training word identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

ANOVAs showed there were significant<br />

differences between these five participants<br />

<strong>and</strong> all the other participants <strong>on</strong> all four<br />

pretests, but no significant differences were<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 275


TABLE 7<br />

Means for WJIII DRB Pretest <strong>and</strong> Posttest Scores with Estimated Age <strong>and</strong> Grade Equivalencies<br />

Measures Synthetic (n 17) Analogy (n 18) C<strong>on</strong>trol (n 17)<br />

Mean Age 9.42 8.06 8.52<br />

Letter-Word Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

Pretest 14.59 15.28 13.824<br />

(6-1/K.8) (6-1/K.8) (6-0/K.7)<br />

Postest 16.24 16.78 14.06<br />

(6-2/K.8) (6-3/K.9) (6-0/K.7)<br />

Word Attack<br />

Pretest 1.83 1.67 1.78<br />

(5-8/K.2) (5-8/K.2) (5-1/K.0)<br />

Postest 4.12 3.61 2.29<br />

(6-9/1.4) (6-9/1.4) (5-8/K.2)<br />

detected <strong>on</strong> the variables of age. There was<br />

insufficient data to account for these differences.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Significant effects were found <strong>on</strong> the measure<br />

of training word identificati<strong>on</strong> for both the<br />

synthetic <strong>and</strong> analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

groups compared to the c<strong>on</strong>trol group, indicating<br />

that participants who received a systematic<br />

approach to ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> outperformed<br />

those students who did not. No<br />

significant effects were found between the adjusted<br />

means of the two groups which received<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> suggesting that neither<br />

approach was superior in increasing the number<br />

of words the participants could read. Significant<br />

effects were also found <strong>on</strong> transfer<br />

word identificati<strong>on</strong> for the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

treatment groups compared to the c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group. No significant effects were found <strong>on</strong><br />

transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong> for the analogy<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment group compared to the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group. These results imply that participants<br />

in the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group were better able to apply the decoding<br />

skills they had learned during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to words they were unfamiliar with, <strong>and</strong> support<br />

the findings of Waugh et al. (2009) where<br />

participants with intellectual disability were<br />

able to read novel words with a similar structure<br />

to those taught in the course of the study.<br />

However, no significant effects were found<br />

between the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

group <strong>and</strong> the analogy treatment group which<br />

suggests that either there are no additive effects<br />

to synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> or that<br />

differences between the groups could not be<br />

detected due to either small sample size or the<br />

relatively brief length of the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics approach may have<br />

been more effective in teaching generalized<br />

decoding skills for a number of reas<strong>on</strong>s. The<br />

approach emphasized the alphabetic code, introducing<br />

<strong>on</strong>e letter at a time rather than a<br />

segment of a word. Participants could track<br />

each letter in a word <strong>and</strong> sound it out rather<br />

than having to recognize segments of word by<br />

sight. Slowly introducing <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>cept at a<br />

time may have been a beneficial aspect of this<br />

approach especially to children with significant<br />

cognitive disability who learn at a slower<br />

rate than typically developing children. Participants<br />

in this study were <strong>on</strong>ly required to read<br />

short <strong>on</strong>e-syllable words, three to five letters<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g. The brevity of the words could also be a<br />

factor in participants’ success.<br />

For many years sight word instructi<strong>on</strong> has<br />

been the favored method of reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for children with significant cognitive disability.<br />

This study suggests that children with<br />

significant cognitive disability are able to benefit<br />

from systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics approach which more closely<br />

resembles sight word instructi<strong>on</strong> was not<br />

shown to be as effective as the synthetic ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

approach.<br />

No significant differences were found between<br />

groups for either the measure of Letter-<br />

276 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Word Identificati<strong>on</strong> or Word Attack <strong>on</strong> the<br />

WJIII DRB. Comparis<strong>on</strong>s of pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest<br />

means revealed that participants’ scores in<br />

the synthetic <strong>and</strong> analogy ph<strong>on</strong>ics treatment<br />

groups did improve significantly. On Word<br />

Attack all three treatment groups showed significant<br />

gains in the means of their pretest<br />

<strong>and</strong> posttest scores. This would suggest that<br />

the classroom instructi<strong>on</strong> the participants in<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>trol group received allowed them to<br />

make adequate progress. This is inc<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with findings in the main analysis which suggest<br />

that systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

more effective in teaching children to read<br />

new words. It is possible that the gains could<br />

have been made due to internal threats such<br />

as practice effects of testing or maturati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the participants. The sudden increases in age<br />

<strong>and</strong> grade level equivalences <strong>on</strong> Word Attack<br />

may have been due to the fact that participants<br />

were receiving explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> in a<br />

skill they had not been taught before, <strong>and</strong><br />

they were applying this skill to their existing<br />

knowledge of letters <strong>and</strong> words.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s of the Study<br />

Although the results of this study suggest the<br />

effectiveness of systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

homogeneity of the sample cannot be<br />

assumed. There may be factors related to the<br />

schools in this study that may differentiate<br />

them from other schools. It was not practical<br />

to analyze data to determine if differences<br />

existing between classes, age groups or reading<br />

programs c<strong>on</strong>currently used all of which<br />

could have influenced the results.<br />

A l<strong>on</strong>ger interventi<strong>on</strong> period may have<br />

yielded clearer differences between the treatments<br />

groups if in fact they did exist, reducing<br />

the likelihood of a Type II error. Allor et al.<br />

(2010) showed more significant results in the<br />

effectiveness of their ph<strong>on</strong>ic based reading<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, which was c<strong>on</strong>ducted over a two<br />

year period. The WJIII DRB may not have<br />

been sensitive enough to detect variati<strong>on</strong> between<br />

the participants’ performance <strong>and</strong><br />

progress resulting in an analysis that found no<br />

significant differences between treatment<br />

groups <strong>on</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized measures.<br />

Finally, this study used an experimental design<br />

to compare differences between groups.<br />

Experimental designs are valuable in that they<br />

allow us to find the most effective interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for a group. Their limitati<strong>on</strong> lies in that<br />

their results focus <strong>on</strong> the overall group benefits.<br />

With low-incidence populati<strong>on</strong>s, excepti<strong>on</strong>ality<br />

is not unusual <strong>and</strong> this study showed<br />

that some participants were unresp<strong>on</strong>sive to<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> in that they did not make<br />

gains <strong>on</strong> either training word identificati<strong>on</strong> or<br />

transfer word identificati<strong>on</strong>. Parsing out these<br />

data is necessary to underst<strong>and</strong> individual differences<br />

within the sample <strong>and</strong> to guide decisi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> designing educati<strong>on</strong>al programs that<br />

are individualized <strong>and</strong> fit the needs of each<br />

child.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for Practice<br />

Evidence that systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics design is<br />

beneficial to children with significant cognitive<br />

disability has implicati<strong>on</strong>s for curricular<br />

design <strong>and</strong> for teacher training. The researcher<br />

found that participants maintained<br />

their interest in the learning activities during<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong>. The participants received<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> were rewarded<br />

with a sticker for their participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> effort.<br />

The sessi<strong>on</strong>s were short in length <strong>and</strong><br />

there were a high number of opportunities for<br />

participants to interact with <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

the learning materials. The materials were designed<br />

so that they were easy to read, easy to<br />

manipulate, <strong>and</strong> appealing to look at. These<br />

factors may have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the success of<br />

the study.<br />

The training <strong>and</strong> transfer words for the<br />

study were chosen because they corresp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

to the high frequency words found in the<br />

Dolch (1948) sight word list. It is important to<br />

acknowledge that children with significant<br />

cognitive disability may have a limited capacity<br />

to store <strong>and</strong> retrieve words, <strong>and</strong> care must be<br />

taken when designing curricula to ensure that<br />

students’ unique needs are met. The participants<br />

also received <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This has implicati<strong>on</strong>s for policy <strong>on</strong> the ratio of<br />

teachers to students in classrooms where children<br />

with significant cognitive disability are<br />

placed.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for Further Research<br />

There are many questi<strong>on</strong>s that still need to be<br />

answered in addressing how best to teach chil-<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 277


dren with significant cognitive disability to<br />

read. There is a need to replicate existing<br />

studies with larger sample sizes. It is also important<br />

to isolate questi<strong>on</strong>s in regard to reading<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> so that a better picture can be<br />

formed <strong>on</strong> how children with significant cognitive<br />

disability learn to read. There may be<br />

factors apart from IQ, e.g. memory <strong>and</strong> attending<br />

skills, which can predict reading<br />

achievement. This may have implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

determining which students should receive instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in ph<strong>on</strong>ics <strong>and</strong> other isolated reading<br />

skills. In regards to children with significant<br />

cognitive disability, the pressure to<br />

change how reading instructi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

came from legal m<strong>and</strong>ates to include<br />

all children in statewide assessments. Further<br />

research must examine whether children with<br />

significant cognitive disability are reaching<br />

meaningful goals. L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal studies are<br />

needed to examine whether educati<strong>on</strong>al reforms<br />

are in the best interests of children with<br />

significant cognitive disability. It is an issue of<br />

insuring “academic skills have meaningful applicati<strong>on</strong><br />

to students’ lives” (Nietupski, Nietupski,<br />

Curtin, & Shrikanth, 1997, p.50).<br />

References<br />

Agran, M., Alper, S., & Wehmeyer, M. (2002). Access<br />

to general curriculum for students with significant<br />

disabilities: What it means to teachers?<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37, 123–133.<br />

Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., Roberts, J. K., J<strong>on</strong>es, F. G.,<br />

& Champlin, T. M. (2010). Teaching students<br />

with moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An<br />

experimental examinati<strong>on</strong> of a comprehensive<br />

reading interventi<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 3–22.<br />

Barbetta, P. M., Heward, W. L., & Bradley, D. M.<br />

(1993). Relative effects of whole-word <strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>etic-prompt<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance of sight words by students with<br />

developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 26, 99–100.<br />

Bradford, S., Shippen, M. E., Alberto, P., Houchins,<br />

D. E., & Flores, M. (2006). Using systematic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to teach decoding skills to middle<br />

school students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 333–343.<br />

Browder, D. M., Fallin, K., Davis, S., & Karv<strong>on</strong>en, M.<br />

(2003). C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of what may influence student<br />

outcomes <strong>on</strong> alternate assessment. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 38, 255–<br />

270.<br />

Browder, D., & Xin, Y. (1998). A meta-analysis <strong>and</strong><br />

review of sight word research <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al reading to individuals<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. The Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 32, 130–153.<br />

Chall, J. (1996). Learning to read. The great debate.<br />

Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt, Brace & Company.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ners, F. A., Atwell, J. A., Rosenquist, C. J., &<br />

Sligh, A. C. (2001). Abilities underlying decoding<br />

differences in children with intellectual disability.<br />

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 292–<br />

299.<br />

Dolch, E. (1948). Problems in reading. Gerrard Press.<br />

Dolch, E. W., & Bloomster, M. (1937). Ph<strong>on</strong>ic readiness.<br />

Elementary School Journal, 38, 201–205.<br />

Edmark, (1992). Edmark reading program: Word recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

(2 nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Edmark Corporati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S.A., & Willows,<br />

D. M. (2001). Systematic ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

helps students learn to read: Evidence from the<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Review of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research, 71, 393–4<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Flores, M. M., Shippen, M. E., Alberto, P., & Crowe,<br />

L. (2004). Teaching letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

to students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Journal of Direct Instructi<strong>on</strong> 4, 173–188.<br />

Flowers, C., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., Browder, D., &<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2005). Teacher’s percepti<strong>on</strong>s of alternate<br />

assessment. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 81–92.<br />

Fry, E. B., & Kress, J. E. (2006). The reading teacher’s<br />

book of lists (5 th ed). San Francisco: John Wiley &<br />

S<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Hoover, W., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view<br />

of reading. Reading <strong>and</strong> Writing: An Interdisciplinary<br />

Journal, 2, 127–160.<br />

Individual with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act of 2004,<br />

20 U.S.C.A. § 1400 et seq.<br />

Joseph, L. M., & Seery, M. E. (2004). Where is the<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics? A review of the literature <strong>on</strong> the use of<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>etic analysis with students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 25, 88–94.<br />

Juel, C., Paratore, J. R., & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, D. (2008). My<br />

sidewalks. Leban<strong>on</strong>, IN: Pears<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1985). Kaufman<br />

test of educati<strong>on</strong>al achievement. Circle Pines, MN:<br />

American Guidance Service.<br />

Kliewer, C., & Biklen, D. (2001). “School’s not really<br />

a place for reading”: A research synthesis of the<br />

literate lives of students with severe disabilities.<br />

Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong> for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps,<br />

26, 1–12.<br />

Kliewer, C., & L<strong>and</strong>is, D. (1999). Individualizing<br />

literacy instructi<strong>on</strong> for young children with mod-<br />

278 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


erate to severe disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 66,<br />

85–100.<br />

Mathes, P. G., & Torgesen, J. K. (2005). Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in Reading Level 1. Columbus, OH: SRA-<br />

McGraw-Hill.<br />

Mercer, C., Rudolph, M., & Wils<strong>on</strong>, R. (1998). Merrill<br />

Reading Series. Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading Panel. Nati<strong>on</strong>al Institute of Child<br />

Health <strong>and</strong> Human Development. (2000). Report<br />

of the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Reading Panel. Teaching children to<br />

read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research<br />

literature <strong>on</strong> reading <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

reading instructi<strong>on</strong> (NIH Publicati<strong>on</strong> No. 00–<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>69). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: U.S. Government Printing<br />

Office.<br />

Nietupski, J., Nietupski, S., Curtin, S., & Shrikanth,<br />

K. (1997). A review of curricular research in severe<br />

disabilities from 1976 to 1995 in six selected<br />

journals. The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 33(1),<br />

36–55.<br />

Quigley, S. P., McAnally, P. L., Rose, S., & King,<br />

C. M. (2001). Reading milest<strong>on</strong>es. Austin, TX: PRO-<br />

Ed.<br />

Schrank. F. A., Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. W.<br />

(2004). Woodcock-Johns<strong>on</strong> III diagnosticreading battery.<br />

Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing.<br />

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A.<br />

(1999). The test of word reading efficiency. Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-Ed.<br />

Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999).<br />

Comprehensive test of ph<strong>on</strong>ological processing. Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-Ed.<br />

Waugh, R. E., Fredrick, L. D., & Alberto, P. (2009).<br />

Using simultaneous prompting to teach sounds<br />

<strong>and</strong> blending skills to students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 1435–14<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler individual achievement<br />

test. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Psychological Corporati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong>, B. (n.d.) Wils<strong>on</strong> reading system. Oxford, MA:<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong> Language Training<br />

Winzer, M. A. (1993). The history of special educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

From isolati<strong>on</strong> to integrati<strong>on</strong>. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Gallaudet<br />

University Press.<br />

Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock language proficiency<br />

battery-revised. Itsaca, IL: Riverside.<br />

Received: 6 July 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 19 September 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 16 December 2011<br />

Effects of Systematic Ph<strong>on</strong>ics / 279


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 280–292<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Comparing Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay for Teaching Functi<strong>on</strong>al Sight Words to<br />

Students with Moderate Intellectual Disability<br />

Mari Beth Coleman<br />

University of Tennessee<br />

David F. Cihak<br />

University of Tennessee<br />

Kevin J. Hurley<br />

Knox County Schools<br />

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency of teacher-directed <strong>and</strong><br />

computer-assisted c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay strategies for teaching three students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

to read functi<strong>on</strong>al sight words. Target words were those found in recipes <strong>and</strong> were taught via teacher-delivered<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay or through a PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong> set up with a delay interval followed by a c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

prompt. These c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were compared using an alternating treatments design. For the purposes of generalizati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

students were given the task of following recipes for snacks c<strong>on</strong>taining previously targeted sight words.<br />

Results indicated both strategies were effective; however, the teacher-directed strategy was slightly more efficient<br />

in terms of trials to criteri<strong>on</strong>. The findings are encouraging given that students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability often depend <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> may benefit from instructi<strong>on</strong> with PowerPoint software.<br />

Literacy may open doors to independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> employment not available through other<br />

modalities for individuals with intellectual disability.<br />

Often, however, individuals with moderate<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disability are<br />

faced with disadvantages with regard to acquiring<br />

literacy skills (Snell & Brown, 2006).<br />

Often, students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

have difficulty with attenti<strong>on</strong> including<br />

shorter attenti<strong>on</strong> span, loss of attenti<strong>on</strong>, distractibility<br />

<strong>and</strong> difficulty in attending to relevant<br />

stimuli (Westling & Fox, 2004). Problems<br />

with memory may be present including difficulties<br />

with storage <strong>and</strong> retrieval of informati<strong>on</strong><br />

stored in short <strong>and</strong> l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory.<br />

This results in an individual’s inability to generalize<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintain skills due to a lack of<br />

adequate opportunity to practice a new skill<br />

(Westling & Fox). These learning characteristics<br />

often lend themselves to the student hav-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Mari Beth Coleman, A416 Jane <strong>and</strong><br />

David Bailey Educati<strong>on</strong> Complex, 1122 Volunteer<br />

Boulevard, Knoxville, TN 37996-3442. Email:<br />

mbc@utk.edu<br />

ing higher rates of success with functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

rather than traditi<strong>on</strong>al literacy skills.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al literacy, or the ability to perform<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> communicative tasks necessary to<br />

perform daily routines in various envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

(Alberto, Frederick, Hughes, McIntosh,<br />

& Cihak, 2007), may provide an individual<br />

with an intellectual disability the ability to<br />

have c<strong>on</strong>trol over choices in his life. Teaching<br />

literacy skills that are part of a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum allows students to participate in<br />

learning skills that can be used to enhance<br />

independence in their home, school <strong>and</strong> community<br />

(Brown et al., 1979).<br />

With increasing focus <strong>on</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards-based<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> being set forth by the No Child<br />

Left Behind (NCLB) Act <strong>and</strong> the Individuals<br />

With <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement Act<br />

(IDEA), individualized educati<strong>on</strong> programs<br />

(IEP) are emphasizing goals <strong>and</strong> objectives<br />

directed towards teaching individuals with disabilities<br />

to perform chr<strong>on</strong>ological-age-appropriate<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skills in natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

(Brown et al., 1980). Therefore, special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teachers require a need for instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

approaches that ensure students with<br />

disabilities dem<strong>on</strong>strate annual yearly prog-<br />

280 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ess in a variety of subjects including reading.<br />

By teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al literacy, teachers <strong>and</strong><br />

students can meet both objectives set forth by<br />

NCLB <strong>and</strong> IDEA.<br />

A variety of resp<strong>on</strong>se-based prompting strategies<br />

have been shown to successfully increase<br />

sight word reading abilities with students who<br />

have moderate intellectual disability. These<br />

strategies include the system of least prompts<br />

(SLP; increasing assistance), most-to-least<br />

prompting (MLP; decreasing assistance), <strong>and</strong><br />

antecedent prompting <strong>and</strong> fading procedures<br />

(Billingsley & Romer, 1983). One prompting<br />

procedure dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective in increasing<br />

sight word recogniti<strong>on</strong> is c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay (CTD). The CTD procedure was<br />

established as a means of shifting stimulus<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol from a prompt to the target stimulus<br />

through the inserti<strong>on</strong> of a fixed amount of<br />

time between the presentati<strong>on</strong> of the stimulus<br />

<strong>and</strong> the delivery of a c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt that<br />

ensures the student completes the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

correctly (Touchette, 1971). Numerous studies<br />

have dem<strong>on</strong>strated CTD to be an effective<br />

<strong>and</strong> efficient strategy for teaching sight words<br />

to individuals with disabilities (Gast, Ault, Wolery,<br />

Doyle & Beringer, 1988; Gast, Wolery,<br />

Morris, Doyle & Meyers, 1990).<br />

Another strategy dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective<br />

for teaching sight words is computerassisted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (CAI). Lee <strong>and</strong> Vail<br />

(2005) taught sight words to elementary-aged<br />

students with intellectual disability or developmental<br />

delays using a specialized software<br />

program, Word Wizard. This multimedia program<br />

delivered instructi<strong>on</strong> with a 5-sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

time delay procedure. Instructi<strong>on</strong> was successful<br />

in teaching sight words <strong>and</strong> incidental informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

While this type of software offers<br />

many benefits, it is not readily available to<br />

most teachers. Another opti<strong>on</strong> for CAI is the<br />

use of PowerPoint software.<br />

Coleman-Martin, Heller, Cihak, <strong>and</strong> Irvine<br />

(2005) dem<strong>on</strong>strated the effectiveness of using<br />

PowerPoint software to teach reading decoding<br />

using the N<strong>on</strong>verbal Reading Approach<br />

with three students who had severe<br />

speech impairments <strong>and</strong> either a physical disability<br />

or autism <strong>and</strong> an intellectual disability.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted across three c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

teacher <strong>on</strong>ly, teacher <strong>and</strong> CAI, <strong>and</strong> CAI<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly. Results indicated that PowerPoint software<br />

can be used as an instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool for<br />

teaching reading to students with disabilities.<br />

Because PowerPoint software is widely available<br />

<strong>and</strong> easy to use, it offers many advantages over<br />

specialized software programs. PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

can increase a student’s opportunities<br />

to practice reading skills in multiple settings,<br />

including home for students who have a<br />

computer with PowerPoint or the free Power-<br />

Point player software (Coleman, 2009). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

research in a small number of studies<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strates that PowerPoint can be combined<br />

with CTD for sight word instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Yaw et al. (2011) used PowerPoint software<br />

with a 2-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay to teach Dolch words to<br />

a sixth-grade student with <strong>Autism</strong>. During assessment<br />

trials, the student interacted with<br />

PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s that were visually<br />

identical to teaching PowerPoint’s but lacking<br />

auditory presentati<strong>on</strong> of the words. He was<br />

instructed to read each word before the 2-sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

delay expired while another software<br />

program, GarageB<strong>and</strong>, recorded his resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

This interventi<strong>on</strong> was effective in increasing<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong> at a rapid rate. Anecdotally,<br />

the authors noted that the student enjoyed<br />

interacting with the PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> this study, the authors proposed that<br />

PowerPoint is an effective <strong>and</strong> efficient method<br />

to provide increased opportunities for students<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d <strong>and</strong> receive feedback.<br />

While Yaw et al. (2011) taught traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

reading words via the use of PowerPoint with<br />

CTD, Mechling, Gast, <strong>and</strong> Krups (2007) used<br />

PowerPoint with 3-sec<strong>on</strong>d CTD presented <strong>on</strong> a<br />

SMART Board to teach sight word reading of<br />

grocery words to a small group of high school<br />

students with moderate intellectual disability.<br />

This strategy was effective for teaching students<br />

to read target words <strong>and</strong> match grocery<br />

item photos to the target grocery word. Since<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> occurred in a group format,<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>al learning was assessed <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that students were able to learn<br />

each other’s words <strong>and</strong> to acquire incidental<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> about target words.<br />

Results from these studies indicate that<br />

PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s with built-in c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay may improve word reading for students<br />

with disabilities. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

other research that indicates the effectiveness<br />

of CAI. One of the benefits of CAI is that it<br />

can provide structured independent practice<br />

in the classroom without requiring extensive<br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 281


amounts of teacher supervisi<strong>on</strong> time. The purpose<br />

of this study was to compare the effectiveness<br />

<strong>and</strong> efficiency of two c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay interventi<strong>on</strong>s: teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al sight words to students<br />

with moderate intellectual disability.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Three elementary-aged students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability participated in this<br />

study. All students were enrolled in a rural<br />

pre-K through fifth-grade public school in the<br />

Southeastern United States. The school c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

of 524 students, the majority of whom<br />

were Caucasian. Approximately 66.8% of the<br />

total school populati<strong>on</strong> received free/reduced<br />

lunch including all three of the participants<br />

in the study. Selecti<strong>on</strong> criteria for participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in the study included: (a) receiving<br />

25 hours of special educati<strong>on</strong> services in a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom weekly, (b) having<br />

an IEP goal that targeted functi<strong>on</strong>al literacy,<br />

(c) having no prior experience learning<br />

through the use of a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure,<br />

(d) scoring a low percentage of accuracy<br />

<strong>on</strong> a pretest of 40 functi<strong>on</strong>al words, <strong>and</strong><br />

(e) receiving a recommendati<strong>on</strong> from the<br />

classroom teacher.<br />

At the time of the study, Joe was a 10-yearold<br />

male identified as having a moderate intellectual<br />

disability with a sec<strong>on</strong>dary disability<br />

of language impairment. Using Wechsler Intelligence<br />

Scale for Children, Fourth Editi<strong>on</strong><br />

(WISC-IV), Joe had a full-scale IQ scored of<br />

48. He received small-group instructi<strong>on</strong> due<br />

to high level of support needed for acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of academic skills. Joe received <strong>on</strong>e hour of<br />

speech/language services each week. No<br />

adaptive behavior scores were available for<br />

this participant.<br />

When the study began, Kyle was a 12-yearold<br />

male diagnosed with multiple disabilities<br />

including autism, seizure disorder, speech/<br />

language impairment, <strong>and</strong> intellectual disability.<br />

Using the WISC-IV, Kyle received a fullscale<br />

score of 46 with a verbal comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

score of 59. Using the Wechsler Individual<br />

Achievement Test, Sec<strong>on</strong>d Editi<strong>on</strong> (WIAT-II)<br />

Kyle received a word reading st<strong>and</strong>ard score<br />

of 40. Kyle’s st<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>on</strong> the Adaptive<br />

Behavior Assessment System-Sec<strong>on</strong>d Editi<strong>on</strong><br />

(ABAS-II) were a general adaptive composite<br />

score of 56, a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual score of 50, a social<br />

score of 61, <strong>and</strong> a practical score of 70. Kyle<br />

received intensive small-group instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

meet his academic <strong>and</strong> social needs. Kyle also<br />

received <strong>on</strong>e hour of occupati<strong>on</strong>al therapy<br />

<strong>and</strong> speech/language services each week.<br />

Jake was 10-years-old at the time of the<br />

study. Jake is diagnosed with a primary disability<br />

of intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> a sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

disability of speech impairment. Using the<br />

WISC-IV, Jake had a full-scale IQ score of 44.<br />

When tested using the Woodcock Johns<strong>on</strong><br />

Test of Cognitive Abilities (WJ-III), Jake’s basic<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> broad reading skills scored 37<br />

<strong>and</strong> 31 respectively. ABAS-II scores indicated a<br />

general adaptive composite score of 70, c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

score of 65, social score of 81, <strong>and</strong><br />

practical score of 72. Jake also received <strong>on</strong>e<br />

hour of speech/language services each week.<br />

Setting<br />

All phases of the study occurred in a selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom for students with moderate<br />

to severe disabilities c<strong>on</strong>sisting of 11 students<br />

with varying intellectual disability, an<br />

interning teacher, a teaching assistant, <strong>and</strong> a<br />

classroom teacher. The interning teacher<br />

served as the primary researcher for all interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Teacher-directed CTD interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

occurred in a <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e teacher to<br />

student format at a kidney-shaped table located<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e side of the classroom. Computer-assisted<br />

CTD interventi<strong>on</strong> occurred at <strong>on</strong>e<br />

of two classroom computers located in the<br />

corner of the classroom. Dividers were placed<br />

between computers to limit any observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning between participants.<br />

Materials<br />

Materials used during teacher-directed CTD<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 11 functi<strong>on</strong>al cooking words for<br />

each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> printed at 96-point f<strong>on</strong>t <strong>on</strong> a<br />

4 6 inch note card. Words for <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

targeted making an English muffin pizza <strong>and</strong><br />

words for the other c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> targeted making<br />

instant pudding. Each card c<strong>on</strong>tained a<br />

picture of the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding word which was<br />

faded after the student reached criteri<strong>on</strong> us-<br />

282 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 1<br />

Target Vocabulary Words by C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for Each Student<br />

Student<br />

ing the preferred CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Computerdirected<br />

CTD used a Macintosh desktop computer<br />

to present a PowerPoint slideshow<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisting of the same 11 words used during<br />

teacher-directed CTD for each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Words for both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were presented in<br />

lower case since those letters occur more comm<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

in books <strong>and</strong> other reading materials.<br />

The researcher used an audio recorder to<br />

verify student resp<strong>on</strong>ses, record the durati<strong>on</strong><br />

of each sessi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> for the purposes of collecting<br />

data for interobserver reliability <strong>and</strong><br />

procedural integrity.<br />

Design<br />

Two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, teacher-directed CTD <strong>and</strong><br />

computer-assisted CTD, for teaching recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

of sight words were compared using an<br />

alternating treatments design. This design allows<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of the effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency<br />

of more than <strong>on</strong>e interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the<br />

dependent variable (Alberto & Troutman,<br />

2006). The experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for this<br />

study were (a) baseline, (b) interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisting<br />

of comparis<strong>on</strong> of teacher-directed CTD<br />

<strong>and</strong> computer-assisted CTD, (c) preferred<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> where sight words from the<br />

least efficient instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> were<br />

taught using procedures from the more efficient<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, (d) faded picture prompts<br />

where picture stimuli were removed, <strong>and</strong><br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s Target Words<br />

TD CAI Pudding-Pizza<br />

Joe Pudding Pizza pour - spo<strong>on</strong><br />

whisk - sauce<br />

Kyle Pudding Pizza bowl - tomato<br />

pudding - pepper<strong>on</strong>i<br />

Jake Pizza Pudding refrigerate - microwave<br />

add - heat<br />

serve - spread<br />

minute - slices<br />

stir - cheese<br />

cold - half<br />

milk - bread<br />

Note. CAI computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>; TD teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

(e) generalizati<strong>on</strong> during which students were<br />

asked to read words to complete a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

task.<br />

Baseline. Students were assessed <strong>on</strong> recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

of 40 functi<strong>on</strong>al words using flashcards.<br />

Words targeted skills for making an English<br />

pizza <strong>and</strong> instant pudding (see Table 1 for a<br />

list of target words). Each word was presented<br />

<strong>and</strong> the student was asked to read the word. If<br />

the student could not read the word correctly,<br />

the word was placed in an unknown word pile.<br />

The researcher did not provide any feedback<br />

or assistance. Students were tested <strong>on</strong> the unknown<br />

words two more times to ensure that<br />

the words were unknown to him or her. Of<br />

the unknown words, 11 words <strong>on</strong> which the<br />

student received 0% correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses were<br />

chosen for each task (pudding or pizza) to<br />

be taught during each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (computerassisted<br />

or teacher-directed). To decrease the<br />

possibility of practice effects, <strong>and</strong> given that<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly words with 0% accuracy were selected for<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, the three sessi<strong>on</strong>s of preassesment<br />

were used as baseline data.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>. Students participated in two<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s which were counterbalanced<br />

to reduce possible carryover effects.<br />

A total of 11 target words were presented for<br />

each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

taught functi<strong>on</strong>al word sets that could be used<br />

to complete two food preparati<strong>on</strong> tasks. One<br />

task included learning words that would allow<br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 283


the student to successfully make instant pudding,<br />

while the other targeted words allowed<br />

the student to create an English muffin pizza.<br />

During the teacher-directed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, Kyle<br />

<strong>and</strong> Joe learned functi<strong>on</strong>al words for completing<br />

the pudding task while Jake learned the<br />

words for completing the pizza task. During<br />

the computer-assisted c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, Kyle <strong>and</strong> Joe<br />

learned the words for completing the pizza<br />

task whereas Jake learned the words for completing<br />

the pudding task. Table 1 lists students<br />

by c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Students participated in both<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s until 90% or higher<br />

accuracy was achieved for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s in each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> that resulted in the student reaching<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> in fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s is referred to as<br />

the preferred CTD strategy. Data were collected<br />

using event recording <strong>and</strong> permanent<br />

product recoding procedures. All sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were audio taped for the purposes of resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

verificati<strong>on</strong>, interobserver reliability <strong>and</strong> procedural<br />

integrity. During the teacher-directed<br />

CTD, the interning teacher recorded the<br />

number of functi<strong>on</strong>al sight words read correctly<br />

<strong>and</strong> number of words read incorrectly.<br />

During the computer-assisted CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

student resp<strong>on</strong>ses were audio taped <strong>and</strong><br />

scored for correctness later. The number of<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al sight words read correctly was divided<br />

by the total number of words presented<br />

to calculate the percentage of accuracy.<br />

Teacher-directed CTD. During teacher-directed<br />

CTD, each student was presented 11<br />

words per sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> flashcards that c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

the word al<strong>on</strong>g with a picture representing the<br />

word (e.g., “pour” c<strong>on</strong>tained a picture of a<br />

h<strong>and</strong> pouring from a pitcher). Each teacherdirected<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> began with the teacher saying,<br />

“Let’s practice reading,” followed by the<br />

teacher reminding students, “Remember if<br />

you do not know what the answer is, wait <strong>and</strong> I<br />

will tell you.” During the first teacher-directed<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, a 0-sec delay was implemented in<br />

which the teacher presented the card, ensured<br />

a look resp<strong>on</strong>se, <strong>and</strong> said “What’s the<br />

word?” The correct resp<strong>on</strong>se was modeled immediately<br />

followed by, “What’s the word?’ to<br />

solicit student practice. During subsequent<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the teacher began the sessi<strong>on</strong> as<br />

described, but silently counted to 4-sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

between saying, “What’s the word?” <strong>and</strong> modeling<br />

the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. To keep the c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

diti<strong>on</strong>s similar, all student resp<strong>on</strong>ses resulted<br />

in modeling of the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>and</strong> time<br />

for student to practice the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

After every two words were presented, the<br />

teacher told the students “Remember if you<br />

do not know what the answer is, wait <strong>and</strong> I will<br />

tell you.” Verbal praise for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

was not provided. Instead, at the completi<strong>on</strong><br />

of each sessi<strong>on</strong>, students received verbal recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

for their participati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Computer-assisted CTD. During the computer-assisted<br />

CTD, the students participated in<br />

completing a teacher-developed CTD Power-<br />

Point presentati<strong>on</strong>. Figure 1 displays an example<br />

of the PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>. Procedures<br />

were similar to the teacher-directed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

except audio <strong>on</strong> all slides was prerecorded<br />

using the researcher’s voice. Each PowerPoint<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> began with a slide saying, “Let’s practice<br />

reading,” followed by a slide that reminded<br />

students, “Remember, if you do not<br />

know what the answer is, wait <strong>and</strong> I will tell<br />

you.” During the first sessi<strong>on</strong>, a 0-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay<br />

was used in which the computer presented the<br />

target word al<strong>on</strong>g with a representative picture,<br />

presented the task request, “What’s the<br />

word?” <strong>and</strong> immediately presented the word<br />

again while modeling the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

This was followed by, “What’s the word?” to<br />

solicit a student resp<strong>on</strong>se. During subsequent<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the computer began the sessi<strong>on</strong> as<br />

described but a 4-sec wait occurred between<br />

the task request, “What’s the word?” <strong>and</strong> the<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong> of the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. Multiple<br />

PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s were created during<br />

which word order was r<strong>and</strong>omized to prevent<br />

students learning the word order. All student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses resulted in modeling of correct resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

with a prompt to practice the correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se. After two sight words were presented<br />

<strong>and</strong> correctly modeled, the PowerPoint<br />

told students, “Remember if you do not know<br />

what the answer is, wait <strong>and</strong> I will tell you.”<br />

Student resp<strong>on</strong>ses were recorded using a digital<br />

voice recorder. At the completi<strong>on</strong> of each<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, students received verbal recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

for their participati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Preferred CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

which students reached criteri<strong>on</strong> with the fewest<br />

trials was determined to be the preferred<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Once criteri<strong>on</strong> of 90% accuracy<br />

for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s was reached in<br />

<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the word list from the n<strong>on</strong>pre-<br />

284 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 1. Example of PowerPoint Slides used during CAI 3-Sec<strong>on</strong>d Delay C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

ferred c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (i.e., c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> in which criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

was not met) was combined with the<br />

word list from the preferred c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued until students reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> of 90% accuracy for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Faded Picture Prompt c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. After reaching<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> using the preferred CTD procedure,<br />

the picture stimulus was removed from<br />

all flashcards or PowerPoint slides. The preferred<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> minus the pictures c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to be implemented similar to procedures<br />

described above. This phase c<strong>on</strong>tinued until<br />

the students read 90% of words correctly for<br />

three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong>. During the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

phase, students read the words in order to<br />

prepare a snack. A task analysis (see Table 2)<br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 285


TABLE 2<br />

Steps for Completing Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Tasks<br />

was created c<strong>on</strong>sisting of steps c<strong>on</strong>taining<br />

the functi<strong>on</strong>al words taught to the student<br />

during both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Both reading the<br />

word <strong>and</strong> performing the steps of the task<br />

analysis were observed <strong>and</strong> recorded. The<br />

interning-teacher recorded the number of<br />

words used correctly to make the particular<br />

snack. If the student correctly read <strong>and</strong> used<br />

the word, the teacher provided verbal praise.<br />

However, if the student did not read or<br />

use the word correctly, then the teacher<br />

prompted the student by reading the word.<br />

Prompting occurred after 5s of wait time. If<br />

student could not complete the step within<br />

the 5s, the step was recorded as incorrect <strong>and</strong><br />

the student was prompted regarding that specific<br />

word.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Task Student C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> Steps<br />

Instant Pudding Kyle CAI 1. pour the pudding in a bowl<br />

2. add a cup of cold milk<br />

3. use a whisk to stir<br />

4. stir the pudding for a minute<br />

5. refrigerate for 5 minutes<br />

6. serve the pudding<br />

English Muffin Pizza Joe TD 1. put tomato sauce <strong>on</strong> half the bread<br />

Jake 2. spread sauce with a spo<strong>on</strong><br />

3. put cheese <strong>on</strong> top<br />

4. add 4 slices of pepper<strong>on</strong>i<br />

5. heat in microwave<br />

6. eat the pizza<br />

Note. CAI computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>; TD teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

At the end of the study, participants completed<br />

a Likert scale survey describing their<br />

experiences <strong>and</strong> preference for each of the<br />

CTD methods. The classroom teacher <strong>and</strong><br />

teacher’s assistant completed a Likert scale<br />

survey describing their attitudes for each of<br />

the CTD methods al<strong>on</strong>g with their feelings <strong>on</strong><br />

the efficiency <strong>and</strong> effectiveness for using each<br />

of the methods. Student resp<strong>on</strong>ses were recorded<br />

<strong>and</strong> shared with both the teacher <strong>and</strong><br />

teaching assistant to ensure reliability.<br />

Results<br />

During baseline, students did not read any of<br />

the target words correctly. During the teacherdirected<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the mean percentage<br />

of words ready correctly increased to<br />

78.11% <strong>and</strong> students reached criteri<strong>on</strong> with a<br />

mean of 19 sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the computerassisted<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the mean percentage<br />

of words read correctly increased to 77% <strong>and</strong><br />

students reached criteri<strong>on</strong> with a mean of 24<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Although the percentage of words<br />

read correctly was similar, students acquired<br />

the targeted words quicker during the teacher-directed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. However, individual<br />

differences did occur. Table 3 displays the<br />

participants’ mean percentages of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

during each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Joe. During baseline, Joe did not read any<br />

of the target words correctly. During the<br />

teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the mean<br />

percentage of words read correctly increased<br />

to 82% <strong>and</strong> Joe reached criteria after five<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the computer-assisted CTD<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, his mean percentage of words read<br />

correctly increased to 82% <strong>and</strong> he reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> after seven sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Although the<br />

percentage of words read correctly was the<br />

same, Joe acquired the targeted words more<br />

efficiently during the teacher-directed CTD<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, the teacher-directed<br />

CTD procedures were re-implemented to<br />

286 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 3<br />

Mean Percentage of Correct Resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

Joe Kyle Jake<br />

TD<br />

% correct 82% 77.38% 75.83%<br />

# of sessi<strong>on</strong>s 5 8 6<br />

CAI<br />

% correct 82% 78% 72.8<br />

# of sessi<strong>on</strong>s 7 7 10<br />

Preferred TD<br />

% correct 91% – 88.75%<br />

# of sessi<strong>on</strong>s 4 – 4<br />

CAI<br />

% correct – 90.75% –<br />

# of sessi<strong>on</strong>s – 4 –<br />

Faded Picture Prompt<br />

% correct 94% 79% 94%<br />

# of sessi<strong>on</strong>s 3 6 3<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

% correct 100% 91% 100%<br />

Note. CAI computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>; TD <br />

teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

teach the words related to making a pizza.<br />

Joe’s mean percentage of words read correctly<br />

was 91% <strong>and</strong> he required four sessi<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. When the picture prompt was<br />

removed, Joe’s mean performance increased<br />

to 94% following three sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> phase, Joe read <strong>and</strong> completed<br />

100% of the task analysis correctly to make a<br />

pizza. See Figure 2 for Joe’s results.<br />

Kyle. During baseline, Kyle did not read<br />

any of the target words correctly. During the<br />

teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the mean<br />

percentage of words read correctly increased<br />

to 77.38% <strong>and</strong> Kyle reached criteria after<br />

eight sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the computer-assisted<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, his mean percentage of words<br />

read correctly increased to 78% <strong>and</strong> he<br />

reached criteri<strong>on</strong> after seven sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Although<br />

the percentage of words read correctly<br />

was nearly the same, Kyle acquired the targeted<br />

words more efficiently during the computer-assisted<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, the<br />

computer-assisted CTD procedures were reimplemented<br />

to teach the words related to<br />

making a pudding. Kyle’s mean percentage of<br />

words read correctly was 90.75% <strong>and</strong> he required<br />

four sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. When<br />

the picture prompt was removed, Kyle’s mean<br />

performance decreased to 79% but reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> in six sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

phase, Kyle read <strong>and</strong> completed 91%<br />

of the task analysis correctly to make instant<br />

pudding. See Figure 3 for Kyle’s results.<br />

Jake. During baseline, Jake did not read<br />

any of the target words correctly. During the<br />

Figure 2. Joe’s percentages correct of targeted vocabulary with computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> (CAI) <strong>and</strong><br />

teacher-directed (TD) c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 287


Figure 3. Kyle’s percentages correct of targeted vocabulary with computer-assisted <strong>and</strong> teacher directed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the mean<br />

percentage of words read correctly increased<br />

to 75.83% <strong>and</strong> Jake reached criteria after six<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the computer-assisted CTD<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, his mean percentage of words read<br />

correctly increased to 72.8% <strong>and</strong> he reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> after seven sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Although the<br />

percentage of words read correctly was similar,<br />

Jake attained the targeted words more<br />

efficiently during the teacher-directed CTD<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, the teacher-directed<br />

CTD procedures were re-implemented to<br />

teach the words related to making a pizza.<br />

Jake’s mean percentage of words read correctly<br />

was 88.75% <strong>and</strong> he required four sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. When the picture<br />

prompt was removed, Jake’s mean performance<br />

increased to 94% following three sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During the generalizati<strong>on</strong> phase, Jake<br />

read <strong>and</strong> completed 100% of the task analysis<br />

to make a pizza. See Figure 4 for Jake’s results.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Students completed a <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e survey with<br />

the researcher to measure the social validity<br />

<strong>and</strong> student approval of the CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

All students agreed that they enjoyed using<br />

the computer to practice reading, thought the<br />

computer helped them improve their reading,<br />

<strong>and</strong> enjoyed making the snack. Students all<br />

expressed interest in using the computer<br />

again for reading instructi<strong>on</strong>. Two out of the<br />

three students agreed that they enjoyed working<br />

individually with the teacher during teacher-directed<br />

CTD sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Kyle was the <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

participant that was unsure how he felt toward<br />

the teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. He also<br />

was the <strong>on</strong>ly student who reached criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

more efficiently during the computer-assisted<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. All participants expressed enjoyment<br />

in their participati<strong>on</strong> of making the<br />

snack at the completi<strong>on</strong> of the study.<br />

A teacher survey was designed to assess the<br />

classroom teacher <strong>and</strong> teaching assistant’s approval<br />

<strong>and</strong> social validity of the strategies<br />

used. Both teachers str<strong>on</strong>gly agreed with each<br />

of the questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the survey. The classroom<br />

teacher additi<strong>on</strong>ally resp<strong>on</strong>ded that she noticed<br />

the participants’ increase in completi<strong>on</strong><br />

of other classroom resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities in order to<br />

participate in their sessi<strong>on</strong>s for the study.<br />

Interobserver Reliability <strong>and</strong> Procedural Integrity<br />

All sessi<strong>on</strong>s were recorded using a digital audio<br />

recorder for the purposes of interobserver<br />

reliability <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity. Interobserver<br />

reliability <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity data<br />

were collected <strong>on</strong> a minimum <strong>on</strong> 33% of sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for each student. To determine the percentage<br />

of interobserver reliability, the num-<br />

288 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 4. Jake’s percentages correct of targeted vocabulary with computer-assisted (CAI) <strong>and</strong> teacher directed<br />

(TD) c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

ber of agreements was divided by the number<br />

of agreements plus disagreements. Procedural<br />

integrity was determined by rating the researcher’s<br />

adherence via a checklist of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

procedures. Means for interobserver<br />

reliability <strong>and</strong> procedural integrity were 100%.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

The purpose of this study was to compare the<br />

effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency of teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sight words to students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability using teacher-directed<br />

<strong>and</strong> computer-assisted c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay.<br />

Results indicated both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were effective<br />

in teaching sight word reading to all three<br />

students. However, the results showed two of<br />

the three students learned words more efficiently<br />

during the teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

in terms of trials to criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Of the three participants, the student with<br />

autism, Kyle, was the <strong>on</strong>ly student to reach<br />

criteria more quickly during the computerassisted<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This difference could<br />

be due to Kyle’s difficulty in maintaining<br />

higher teacher dem<strong>and</strong>s during the teacherdirected<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> compared to the<br />

computer-assisted CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This is c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with Coleman-Martin, et al. (2005)<br />

which dem<strong>on</strong>strated preference for learning<br />

via PowerPoint for a student with autism. Fur-<br />

ther research may be necessary to c<strong>on</strong>firm this<br />

finding. Alternatively, Joe <strong>and</strong> Jake both displayed<br />

a preference for the teacher-directed<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Joe’s data indicated that both<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were equally effective for teaching<br />

sight words. He achieved 91% during the<br />

third sessi<strong>on</strong> for each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. However, in<br />

the teacher-directed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, he c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

at 91% for two more sessi<strong>on</strong>s, thus reaching<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>, while he fell back to 82% accuracy<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, teacher-directed<br />

CTD was <strong>on</strong>ly slightly more efficient for Joe.<br />

In terms of effectiveness, Joe reached 100%<br />

accuracy <strong>on</strong> his last sessi<strong>on</strong> of computerassisted<br />

CTD whereas he did not reach 100%<br />

during teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong>. Jake’s<br />

data displayed a clear fracti<strong>on</strong>ati<strong>on</strong> between<br />

the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s after the third sessi<strong>on</strong>. His<br />

data indicate that computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

may not be the best strategy for him. This<br />

could be due to many factors including increased<br />

adult attenti<strong>on</strong> or difficulty attending<br />

to computerized stimuli. Jake seemed to have<br />

difficulty focusing <strong>on</strong> the computer presentati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

rather than other classroom activities,<br />

whereas he was more attentive during teacherdirected<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. N<strong>on</strong>etheless, he was able<br />

to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> with computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In terms of time efficiency, teacher-directed<br />

CTD sessi<strong>on</strong>s were shorter in average durati<strong>on</strong><br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 289


(average of 1.9 minutes during teacherdirected<br />

CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> 2.5 minutes during<br />

computer-assisted CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>). This is<br />

due to the structured pace of the PowerPoint<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong>s compared to the researcher being<br />

able to eliminate pauses up<strong>on</strong> correct student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding or pauses between the end<br />

of <strong>on</strong>e trial (word) <strong>and</strong> the next trial in the<br />

teacher-directed c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, the<br />

PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s took more time to<br />

prepare than the flashcards. However, <strong>on</strong>ce<br />

slides for a word were created in PowerPoint, it<br />

was easy for the researcher to copy <strong>and</strong> paste<br />

them into new presentati<strong>on</strong>s. For teachers,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce a PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong> is created, it<br />

can be used for multiple students from year to<br />

year for the time investment to have a larger<br />

payoff. The bigger time factor was direct<br />

teacher instructi<strong>on</strong>al time. During computerassisted<br />

CTD, the teacher did not have to<br />

instruct each student individually. The Power-<br />

Point presentati<strong>on</strong>s were used to instruct multiple<br />

students in a <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e format without<br />

direct teacher <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e instructi<strong>on</strong>. Thus,<br />

average teacher time during computer-assisted<br />

CTD instructi<strong>on</strong> was 0 minutes as compared<br />

to 1.9 minutes multiplied by 3 students<br />

for an average of 5.7 minutes each day for<br />

teacher-directed CTD.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

There are several limitati<strong>on</strong>s of this study. The<br />

first limitati<strong>on</strong> is the lack of ability to generalize<br />

findings given the small number of participants<br />

<strong>and</strong> absence of female participants. A<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d limitati<strong>on</strong> stemmed from difficulties<br />

associated with technology. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred<br />

while other students were engaged in other<br />

activities. Jake was easily distracted by activities<br />

occurring in the classroom while working <strong>on</strong><br />

the computer. The time required to make the<br />

PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s was also a factor.<br />

Slides were created by an interning teacher.<br />

He reported that slides initially were timec<strong>on</strong>suming<br />

but required less time to complete<br />

after his proficiency with PowerPoint software<br />

increased. A final limitati<strong>on</strong> was student absences<br />

during the study, which could have<br />

possibly affected word acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. During the<br />

study, Jake received two days of out of school<br />

suspensi<strong>on</strong> between the collecti<strong>on</strong> of baseline<br />

data <strong>and</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong> of the interven-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> procedures. Joe also was absent quite frequently<br />

throughout the interventi<strong>on</strong> procedures.<br />

Kyle’s participati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

were affected due to his behavior during other<br />

classroom activities.<br />

Future Research<br />

Further research is needed to verify the results<br />

of this study <strong>and</strong> to examine a larger number<br />

of participants, especially females <strong>and</strong> students<br />

with a variety of ability levels. Future<br />

research should focus <strong>on</strong> words that can be<br />

applied in a larger variety of activities to increase<br />

the utility of computer-assisted CTD in<br />

a variety of c<strong>on</strong>texts. Future studies could include<br />

the use of computer-assisted CTD to<br />

teach chained tasks or to learn other functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

academic skills such as m<strong>on</strong>ey recogniti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

community survival words, etc. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

further studies should be c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

comparing the effectiveness of teacher-directed<br />

CTD <strong>and</strong> computer-assisted CTD to<br />

delineate which c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> is most appropriate<br />

for students with autism.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

This study extended current research by using<br />

individually-presented PowerPoint’s with embedded<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay for the instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

of recipe words for students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability. Previous research has<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated computer-assisted CTD to be an<br />

effective strategy for teaching multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts (Wils<strong>on</strong>, Majsterek, & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, 1996),<br />

Dolch words (Yaw et al., 2011) or grocery<br />

words presented <strong>on</strong> a SMART Board to a small<br />

group of students (Mechling et al., 2007). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

this study is unique in that it compared<br />

computer-assisted <strong>and</strong> teacher-assisted<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay. Finally, this study extends<br />

the literature by dem<strong>on</strong>strating generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of recipe words taught through a combinati<strong>on</strong><br />

of c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay strategies to the task of<br />

food preparati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This study presents many possibilities for<br />

effective teaching strategies in a classroom setting.<br />

Although the teacher-directed CTD c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

was more efficient, results indicated<br />

that both teacher-directed CTD <strong>and</strong> computer-assisted<br />

CTD strategies can be used to teach<br />

sight words effectively. This supports Cole-<br />

290 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


man-Martin et al. (2005) by dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

that, for most students, initial teaching should<br />

begin with teacher-directed instructi<strong>on</strong> with<br />

computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> used for further<br />

practice <strong>and</strong> reinforcement of skill development.<br />

When used in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with other<br />

forms of instructi<strong>on</strong>, technology, specifically<br />

PowerPoint software, can strengthen a student’s<br />

ability to learn sight words. CTD is an effective<br />

<strong>and</strong> efficient method for teaching sight words<br />

due to the reduced amount of student guessing<br />

(Wolery, Ault, & Doyle, 1992). Because<br />

computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> allows the student<br />

to work independently, presentati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

CTD using PowerPoint can allow instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

occur while freeing the teacher to work with<br />

other students. This is especially beneficial<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidering that finding meaningful learning<br />

tasks which can be completed individually by<br />

students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

often poses a challenge to teachers. Furthermore,<br />

students who have computers at home<br />

could be afforded the opportunity to practice<br />

skills at home if PowerPoint presentati<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

sent home <strong>on</strong> a CD-ROM or flash drive.<br />

The effectiveness of computer-assisted CTD,<br />

student engagement during CAI sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

specified interest during assessment of social<br />

validity suggest that technology can be used<br />

effectively with students who have moderate<br />

intellectual disability. As technology becomes<br />

more sophisticated, greater possibilities of enhancing<br />

student learning will become possible.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, as people use technology in<br />

everyday life, students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability need to be able to do the same.<br />

Using PowerPoint software, a form of technology<br />

not generally c<strong>on</strong>sidered for meeting the<br />

needs of students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability, may provide these students with<br />

unique learning opportunities that may enhance<br />

skill development <strong>and</strong>, thus, increase<br />

quality of life.<br />

References<br />

Alberto, P. A., Fredrick, L., Hughes, M., McIntosh,<br />

L., & Cihak, D. F. (2007). Comp<strong>on</strong>ents of visual<br />

literacy: Teaching logos. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 22, 234–243.<br />

Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (2006). Applied<br />

behavior analysis for teachers (7th ed.). Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Billingsley, F. F., & Romer, L. T. (1983). Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> the transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol:<br />

Methods, research, <strong>and</strong> a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual framework.<br />

Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong> for the Severely H<strong>and</strong>icapped,<br />

8, 3–12.<br />

Browder, D. M., & Xin, Y. P. (1998). A meta-analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> review of sight word research <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong><br />

for teaching reading to individuals with<br />

moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 32, 130–153.<br />

Brown, L., Branst<strong>on</strong>, M. B., Hamre-Nietupski, S.,<br />

Pumpian, I., Certo, N., & Gruenewald, L. (1979).<br />

A strategy for developing chr<strong>on</strong>ological-ageappropriate<br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al curricular c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

for severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped adolescents <strong>and</strong> young<br />

adults. The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 81–90.<br />

Brown, L., Falvey, M., Vincent, L., Kaye, N., Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

F., Ferrara-Parrish, P., & Gruenewald, L.<br />

(1980). Strategies for generating comprehensive,<br />

l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal, <strong>and</strong> chr<strong>on</strong>ological-age-appropriate<br />

individualized educati<strong>on</strong> programs for adolescent<br />

<strong>and</strong> young-adult severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped students.<br />

The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 14, 199–215.<br />

Coleman, M. B. (2009). PowerPoint is not just for<br />

business presentati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> college lectures! Using<br />

PowerPoint to enhance instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with disabilities. Teaching Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Plus, 6(1), Retrieved from http://escholarship.<br />

bc.edu/educati<strong>on</strong>/tecplus/vol6/iss1/art5.<br />

Coleman-Martin, M. B., Heller, K. W., Cihak, D. F.,<br />

& Irvine, K. L. (2005). Using computer-assisted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the n<strong>on</strong>verbal reading approach<br />

to teach word identificati<strong>on</strong>. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 20, 80–90.<br />

Cooper-Duffy, K., Szedia, P., & Hyer, G. (2010).<br />

Teaching literacy to students with significant cognitive<br />

disabilities. TEACHING Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

42, 30–39.<br />

Gast, D. L., Ault, M. J., Wolery, M., Doyle, P. M., &<br />

Belanger, S. (1988). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>stant time<br />

delay <strong>and</strong> the system of least prompts in teaching<br />

sight word reading to students with moderate<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

96, 117–128.<br />

Gast, D. L., Wolery, M., Morris, L. L., Doyle, P. M.,<br />

& Meyers, S. (1990). Teaching sight words reading<br />

in a group instructi<strong>on</strong>al arrangement using<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 1, 81–96.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, G., Gersten, R., & Carnine, D. (1987).<br />

Effects of instructi<strong>on</strong>al design variables <strong>on</strong> vocabulary<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of LD students: A study of computer-assisted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

20, 206–213.<br />

Lee, Y., & Vail, C. O. (2005). Computer-based reading<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for young children with disabilities.<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 20,<br />

5–18.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Krupa, K. (2007).<br />

Teacher-Directed <strong>and</strong> Computer-Assisted C<strong>on</strong>stant Time Delay / 291


Impact of SMART Board technology: An investitgati<strong>on</strong><br />

of sight word reading <strong>and</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders,<br />

37, 1869–1882.<br />

Snell, M. E., & Brown, F. (2006). Instructi<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with severe disabilities. Upper Saddle River, NJ:<br />

Pears<strong>on</strong>/Merrill/Prentice Hall.<br />

Touchette, P. (1971). Transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol:<br />

Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the<br />

Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 223–232.<br />

Westling, D., & Fox, L. (2004). Teaching students with<br />

severe disabilities. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pears<strong>on</strong>/Merrill/Prentice<br />

Hall.<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong>, R., Majsterek, D., & Simm<strong>on</strong>s, D. (1996).<br />

The effects of computer-assisted versus teacherdirected<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the multiplicati<strong>on</strong> performance<br />

of elementary students with learning dis-<br />

abilities. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 382–<br />

390.<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong>, R., & Wess<strong>on</strong>, C. (1986). Making every minute<br />

count: Academic learning time in LD classrooms.<br />

Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong> Focus, 2, 13–19.<br />

Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992).<br />

Teaching students with moderate to severe disabilities.<br />

White Plains, NY: L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Yaw, J. S., Skinner, C. H., Parkhurst, J., Taylor, C. M.,<br />

Booher, J., & Chambers, K. (2011). Extending<br />

research <strong>on</strong> a computer-based sight-word reading<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> to a student with autism. Journal of<br />

Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 20, 44–54.<br />

Received: 21 September 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 17 November 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 15 January 2012<br />

292 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 293–301<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

Children with Intellectual Disability<br />

Dana Keeter<br />

Scoggins Middle School,<br />

Paulding County School District<br />

Jessica L. Bucholz<br />

University of West Georgia<br />

Abstract: This study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted to examine the effects literacy-based behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>s have <strong>on</strong><br />

improving the behavior of students with intellectual disability. A sec<strong>on</strong>d purpose of this study was to determine<br />

if literacy-based behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>s could be an effective interventi<strong>on</strong> strategy when used simultaneously<br />

with a group of students targeting the same behavior. A multiple baseline design across small groups was used<br />

to assess whether the group delivered story interventi<strong>on</strong> would decrease the negative behaviors of the five<br />

participants in this study. Results showed that the behaviors decreased for all five students.<br />

In the early 1990s, Gray <strong>and</strong> Gar<strong>and</strong> developed<br />

social stories to be used specifically as<br />

an interventi<strong>on</strong> to help children with autism<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>, correctly participate in, <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

to various social situati<strong>on</strong>s. Students<br />

with autism are not the <strong>on</strong>ly children who<br />

struggle with social skills deficits. Children<br />

with emoti<strong>on</strong>al behavioral disorders, attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), intellectual<br />

disability, <strong>and</strong> learning disabilities also<br />

struggle with underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> appropriately<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding in social situati<strong>on</strong>s (Brown,<br />

2001; Hall, Peters<strong>on</strong>, Webster, Bolen, &<br />

Brown, 1999; Vaughn, Haager, Hogan, &<br />

Kouzehanani, 1992). Students with different<br />

disabilities have difficulties functi<strong>on</strong>ing in social<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> may benefit from a social<br />

story interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Gray <strong>and</strong> Gar<strong>and</strong> (1993) used social stories<br />

that were presented to <strong>on</strong>e student targeting<br />

<strong>on</strong>e specific behavior. They developed<br />

suggested guidelines for writing a social<br />

story. According to Gray (1995), a social<br />

story should be an individually written story<br />

that describes a specific situati<strong>on</strong> in which the<br />

child is having difficulty, social cues that are<br />

relevant to the situati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> expected re-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Jessica L. Bucholz, University of<br />

West Georgia, Department of Collaborative Support<br />

<strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 1601 Maple Street, Carrollt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

GA 30118.<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>ses to the situati<strong>on</strong>. Gray also suggests<br />

specific types of sentences to be used when<br />

writing a social story al<strong>on</strong>g with a ratio of<br />

each type of sentence. Gray describes the sentence<br />

types as descriptive, perspective, directive,<br />

<strong>and</strong> affirmative. Social stories are a relatively<br />

easy strategy to implement with a<br />

student. They are not time intensive in their<br />

development or use <strong>and</strong> can be used for a<br />

wide variety of situati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> behaviors (Reynhout<br />

& Carter, 2006). Social stories can also<br />

be used with individuals with a wide range of<br />

ages. Finally, social stories can be developed<br />

for children in the mild to moderate range of<br />

cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> who have basic language<br />

skills (Gray & Gar<strong>and</strong>).<br />

Gray <strong>and</strong> Gar<strong>and</strong> (1993) developed social<br />

stories to help children with autism correctly<br />

use social skills to participate in social<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s. Social skills are described as “the<br />

cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> specific verbal <strong>and</strong><br />

n<strong>on</strong>verbal behaviors that an individual engages<br />

in when interacting with others” (Gut &<br />

Safran, 2002, p. 88). Difficulty in underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

<strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ding in social situati<strong>on</strong>s can<br />

occur in a number of ways, including, sharing,<br />

taking turns, c<strong>on</strong>flict resoluti<strong>on</strong>, adapting to<br />

routines, making choices, underst<strong>and</strong>ing body<br />

language, facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> gestures<br />

(Gut & Safran). Gut <strong>and</strong> Safran (2002) list<br />

other social skills which are a critical comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

to the school setting, such as, “coping<br />

skills (i.e., expressing anger appropriately);<br />

work habits (i.e., using class time efficiently);<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s / 293


<strong>and</strong> peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships (i.e., interacting appropriately<br />

with a variety of children <strong>on</strong> a regular<br />

basis)” (p. 88).<br />

In the literature reviewed, <strong>on</strong>ly seven studies<br />

were found that examined the use of social<br />

stories with individuals who had been identified<br />

as having disabilities other than autism.<br />

Of these seven studies, <strong>on</strong>e of the studies (Staley,<br />

2001) included participants with a diagnosis<br />

of autism. The other participants in the<br />

study however, had <strong>on</strong>e or more additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

disabilities, such as learning disorders or intellectual<br />

disability. Moore (2004) worked with a<br />

student who had a learning disability but was<br />

also identified as having autism spectrum disorder.<br />

Finally, <strong>on</strong>e study (Soenksen & Alper,<br />

2006) used social stories with a student they<br />

labeled as having hyperlexia, but received special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> services under the category of<br />

autism spectrum disorder. Because these studies<br />

involved students who were identified as<br />

having other disabilities in additi<strong>on</strong> to autism<br />

they were included in the following summary.<br />

Staley (2001) used social stories with five<br />

male students who were educated in a selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. Two<br />

of the participants were identified as having<br />

Down syndrome, <strong>on</strong>e had a pervasive developmental<br />

disorder, <strong>on</strong>e had autism, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e was<br />

diagnosed as having Fragile X syndrome. The<br />

goal was to improve the eating behaviors of<br />

the five participants. Specifically, the goal was<br />

to teach the participants to use a napkin <strong>and</strong><br />

to chew food with a closed mouth. The results<br />

of this study found that the social story<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e was not effective for getting the participants<br />

to change their eating behaviors. When<br />

the social story was combined with the use<br />

of a primary reinforcer (e.g., cereal or potato<br />

chips) there was an immediate positive<br />

change in behavior. In this case, social stories<br />

were not shown to be effective for individuals<br />

with disabilities other than autism nor<br />

were they effective for participants who were<br />

<strong>on</strong> the autism spectrum.<br />

Moore (2004) used a social story with a<br />

4-year-old boy with a learning disability, autism<br />

spectrum disorder, <strong>and</strong> receptive speech <strong>and</strong><br />

language delays. The participant in this case<br />

study had difficulty with his bedtime routine.<br />

The boy pleaded to sleep with his mother,<br />

would wake in the middle of the night to dem<strong>and</strong><br />

milk, <strong>and</strong> would throw a tantrum if his<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>s were not met. A story was written to<br />

teach the young boy a new bedtime routine.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it reminded him of the rewards he<br />

could earn if he followed the new routine without<br />

throwing a tantrum. Moore c<strong>on</strong>cluded the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> as a whole appeared to be effective<br />

at changing the participant’s sleep routine.<br />

However, the impact of the social story <strong>on</strong> the<br />

behavior is unclear due to lack of c<strong>on</strong>crete data<br />

<strong>and</strong> the use of other reinforcers as part of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Toplis <strong>and</strong> Hadwin (2006) used a social<br />

story to address the lunchtime behavior of five<br />

students who were identified as having challenging<br />

behavior within a school setting. Three boys<br />

<strong>and</strong> two girls with an average age of 7 years 5<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths participated in the study. The researchers<br />

used an ABAB design to evaluate the effectiveness<br />

of a social story <strong>on</strong> the target behavior<br />

of independently walking to the school dining<br />

area <strong>and</strong> sitting within two minutes of being<br />

dismissed from class. The stories in this study<br />

followed Gray’s (1995) basic sentence ratio. Toplis<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hadwin found the social story interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to be effective for three of the five children<br />

who participated in the study. Two of the<br />

children c<strong>on</strong>tinued to require prompts to enter<br />

the dining room <strong>and</strong> sit as directed.<br />

Soenksen <strong>and</strong> Alper (2006) used a multiple<br />

baseline design across settings to evaluate the<br />

effectiveness of a social story to teach a<br />

5-year-old boy identified as having hyperlexia,<br />

who was served under the category of autism<br />

spectrum disorders, to appropriately gain the<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> of his peers. The interventi<strong>on</strong> was a<br />

combinati<strong>on</strong> of written <strong>and</strong> verbal cues embedded<br />

into a social story which was written<br />

to follow Gray’s (1995) guidelines. The target<br />

behavior was defined as verbally saying a<br />

peer’s name <strong>and</strong>/or looking at a peer’s face<br />

while talking to him or her. Results of the<br />

study indicated an increase in frequency with<br />

which the student would appropriately gain a<br />

peer’s attenti<strong>on</strong> across multiple settings (e.g.,<br />

recess, choice time, <strong>and</strong> math). The appropriate<br />

behavior was maintained 49 days after the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance phases ended.<br />

Bucholz, Brady, Duffy, Scott, <strong>and</strong> K<strong>on</strong>tosh<br />

(2008) used social stories <strong>and</strong> literacy-based<br />

behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>s (LBBI) with three<br />

adults with cognitive disability in the workplace<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Bucholz <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

used the term LBBI for the stories that were<br />

294 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


used in the study that did not follow Gray’s<br />

(1995) guidelines in regard to sentence type<br />

or ratio <strong>and</strong> therefore were not technically<br />

social stories. One participant was a 57-yearold<br />

male with Down syndrome <strong>and</strong> an IQ of 30<br />

who worked in a job training facility. The<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d two participants worked at the same<br />

sheltered workshop. The sec<strong>on</strong>d participant<br />

was a 48-year-old female who had severe intellectual<br />

disability. The third participant was a<br />

26-year-old female with severe intellectual disability.<br />

The goal of their research was to determine<br />

if social stories <strong>and</strong> LBBI were an<br />

effective strategy for individuals with intellectual<br />

disability in a work envir<strong>on</strong>ment. The<br />

target behaviors included returning to work<br />

<strong>on</strong> time from break <strong>and</strong> asking for more work or<br />

assistance when necessary. The authors c<strong>on</strong>cluded<br />

that the social stories <strong>and</strong> LBBI were<br />

effective at changing the target behaviors for<br />

individuals with intellectual disability. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

the change in behavior was maintained for<br />

the two female participants who worked at the<br />

same sheltered workshop setting.<br />

Kalyva <strong>and</strong> Agaliotis (2009) used a group design<br />

to evaluate the effectiveness of a recorded<br />

social story interventi<strong>on</strong> for teaching students<br />

with learning disabilities to successfully resolve<br />

interpers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>flicts. Sixty-three children with<br />

a mean age of 10 years 7 m<strong>on</strong>ths participated in<br />

this study. Forty-<strong>on</strong>e of the participants were<br />

male <strong>and</strong> 22 were females. The students were<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned to the experimental group<br />

or the c<strong>on</strong>trol group. The experimental group<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 19 boys <strong>and</strong> 12 girls while the c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 22 boys <strong>and</strong> 10 girls. The<br />

researchers read <strong>on</strong>e of three different stories to<br />

the experimental group at each phase of the<br />

study. The stories were written to describe an<br />

interpers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>flict <strong>and</strong> participants were<br />

asked what strategies they might use to h<strong>and</strong>le<br />

the situati<strong>on</strong>. The teachers of the participants<br />

were asked to complete the Mats<strong>on</strong> Evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

of Social Skills with Youngsters Teacher Form in<br />

order to gain informati<strong>on</strong> about each child’s<br />

appropriate <strong>and</strong> inappropriate social behaviors.<br />

Results of this study found that the social stories<br />

were effective for helping the students in<br />

the experimental group use more positive c<strong>on</strong>flict<br />

resoluti<strong>on</strong> strategies when compared to the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group. Furthermore, the students in the<br />

experimental group c<strong>on</strong>tinued to use more appropriate<br />

strategies two m<strong>on</strong>ths after the inter-<br />

venti<strong>on</strong> was ended while the c<strong>on</strong>trol group c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to select less appropriate strategies.<br />

Schneider <strong>and</strong> Goldstein (2009) c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

<strong>on</strong>e study not involving students with autism.<br />

Their research included three male participants<br />

in grades first through third. Each of the<br />

participants were diagnosed with language impairments,<br />

had impaired verbal <strong>and</strong> social<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>, but could communicate verbally.<br />

These students displayed problem behaviors<br />

in the classroom which included<br />

aggressi<strong>on</strong>, n<strong>on</strong>compliance <strong>and</strong> impulse c<strong>on</strong>trol.<br />

The students received speech <strong>and</strong> language<br />

therapy, but they did not receive services<br />

to address their problem behaviors. The<br />

primary target behavior addressed in this<br />

study was appropriately participating in the<br />

target activity which included following directi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

completing work, making eye c<strong>on</strong>tact,<br />

<strong>and</strong> raising a h<strong>and</strong>. A multiple baseline design<br />

across participants was used to determine the<br />

effectiveness of the story interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Schneider <strong>and</strong> Goldstein c<strong>on</strong>cluded that the<br />

social story interventi<strong>on</strong> was effective in increasing<br />

<strong>on</strong>-task behaviors for children with<br />

language impairments although the change in<br />

behavior did vary for each student. Positive<br />

changes were maintained after interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

ceased <strong>and</strong> the students generalized the <strong>on</strong>task<br />

behavior to different activities <strong>and</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

throughout the day.<br />

Almost all of these seven research studies<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that social stories were an effective<br />

strategy to be used with participants with a<br />

variety of disabilities, not <strong>on</strong>ly those with autism<br />

spectrum disorders. This research indicates<br />

that social stories or LLBI may not<br />

need to be restricted to students with autism<br />

to be effective. Based <strong>on</strong> the limited research<br />

available studying the use of social stories<br />

with participants with disabilities other than<br />

autism, the need for additi<strong>on</strong>al research is<br />

warranted (Bucholz et al., 2008; Reynhout &<br />

Carter, 2006).<br />

Gray (1995) recommends for social stories<br />

to be specific to <strong>on</strong>e individual participant, however,<br />

if more than <strong>on</strong>e student has the same or<br />

similar social difficulties, it could be beneficial<br />

for educators to develop <strong>on</strong>e story or LBBI that<br />

could be used by multiple children at the same<br />

time. Various strategies such as modeling, roleplaying<br />

(Stickel, 1990), video modeling (Graetz,<br />

Mastropieri, & Scruggs, 2006), social skills<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s / 295


games (Williams White, Ke<strong>on</strong>ig, & Scahill,<br />

2007), <strong>and</strong> social skills classes (Roy, 1993) have<br />

been successfully implemented to increase social<br />

skills in a group setting. The literature reviewed<br />

revealed <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e research study (Kalyva<br />

& Agaliotis, 2009) that explored the use of a<br />

social story or a LBBI with more than <strong>on</strong>e<br />

individual at the same time.<br />

The first purpose of this study was to determine<br />

if LBBI are an effective interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy when used with students with intellectual<br />

disability. A sec<strong>on</strong>d purpose of this study<br />

was to determine if a LBBI is an effective<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> strategy when used simultaneously<br />

with a group of students targeting the<br />

same or similar social situati<strong>on</strong> or behavior. In<br />

this study the term LBBI is used because the<br />

stories do not follow Gray’s (1998) specific<br />

guidelines as they were not individually written<br />

for <strong>on</strong>ly participant at <strong>on</strong>e time.<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Setting<br />

Five middle school students participated in<br />

this study. All students attended a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom for all academic subjects. All<br />

students in this class have been identified as<br />

having mild/moderate intellectual disability.<br />

Students attended c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s classes (art,<br />

PE, home ec<strong>on</strong>omics, etc.) in a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

setting with paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al support.<br />

Students were placed in two groups. One<br />

group c<strong>on</strong>sisted of two students <strong>and</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

group c<strong>on</strong>tained three students. The students<br />

in these groups exhibited similar behaviors<br />

to be targeted with the LBBI.<br />

Group A included two students who “called<br />

out” during class. Carol was a 14-year-old, female<br />

in the eighth grade. Her cognitive level<br />

was in the mild intellectual disability range as<br />

determined by the school psychologist. Carol<br />

read <strong>on</strong> a fourth grade level, while her reading<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> skills were at a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

grade level. Carol’s math skills were at a third<br />

grade level. Brittany, the sec<strong>on</strong>d member of<br />

Group A, was a 14-year-old girl in the seventh<br />

grade. Her cognitive level was in the moderate<br />

intellectual disability range as determined by<br />

the school psychologist. Brittany read at the<br />

primer level <strong>and</strong> her math skills were at a first<br />

grade level. Brittany received occupati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

therapy for 45 minutes per m<strong>on</strong>th.<br />

Group B c<strong>on</strong>sisted of three participants who<br />

exhibited “off task” behaviors. Bryan was a<br />

16-year-old male in the eighth grade. His cognitive<br />

level was in the moderate intellectual<br />

disability range as determined by the school<br />

psychologist. This student was n<strong>on</strong>verbal <strong>and</strong><br />

used an augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

(AAC) device for communicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

He also received 45 minutes of speech services<br />

each week. Although he had an AAC device,<br />

he rarely used it, preferring to point <strong>and</strong> make<br />

unintelligible verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Bryan was working<br />

<strong>on</strong> matching letters <strong>and</strong> numbers, counting<br />

objects, <strong>and</strong> matching functi<strong>on</strong>al symbols<br />

<strong>and</strong> signs. C<strong>on</strong>nor, the sec<strong>on</strong>d member of<br />

Group B, was a 12-year-old male in the sixth<br />

grade. His cognitive level was in the moderate<br />

intellectual disability range as determined by<br />

the school psychologist. He also received 45<br />

minutes of speech services each week. C<strong>on</strong>nor<br />

read at a primer level <strong>and</strong> his math skills were<br />

at a first grade level. Zeke, the third member<br />

of Group B, was a 12 year-old male in the sixth<br />

grade. His cognitive level was in the mild intellectual<br />

disability range as determined by<br />

the school psychologist. Zeke read at a third to<br />

fourth grade level. His reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> math skills were at a third grade level.<br />

Behavioral Measures<br />

The stories written for these students followed<br />

Gray’s (1998) guidelines for sentence type <strong>and</strong><br />

ratio. However, these stories differed from<br />

Gray’s guidelines because they were not individualized<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e specific student; rather they covered<br />

a few examples of the targeted behavior in<br />

more generalized terms for a small group of<br />

students. Therefore, the interventi<strong>on</strong>s in this<br />

study are referred to as LBBI <strong>and</strong> not social<br />

stories. Stories were individualized in that they<br />

stated the name of the school, the names of the<br />

classroom teachers, <strong>and</strong> targeted <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e specific<br />

behavior. The reading <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

level was kept at a basic level in order for the<br />

students in the group to underst<strong>and</strong> the story. A<br />

mixture of real pictures <strong>and</strong> drawings were used<br />

in each story. The classroom teacher or classroom<br />

paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al read the LBBI to the<br />

target group <strong>on</strong>ce per day in the morning prior<br />

to the time period where data were to be col-<br />

296 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


lected. Stories were read in a quiet locati<strong>on</strong> away<br />

from the other students.<br />

Behaviors targeted in this research study<br />

were blurting out behaviors <strong>and</strong> off-task behaviors.<br />

Blurting out behaviors were defined<br />

as talking without being called <strong>on</strong> by the<br />

teacher or paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al, talking when the<br />

teacher or paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al was talking or instructing,<br />

or talking while another student was<br />

speaking. Off-task was defined as not being engaged<br />

in the assignment, playing with objects<br />

during instructi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong>/or staring at the desk or<br />

looking around the room during work time.<br />

The independent variable in this study was the<br />

use of a literacy-based behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

A multiple baseline across groups research<br />

design was used to evaluate the effectiveness<br />

of a literacy-based behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

written for a small group of students. Two<br />

separate groups of participants targeting different<br />

behaviors were studied. The target behavior<br />

for Group A was blurting out. The target<br />

behavior for Group B was being <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

during assigned work times. Baseline data<br />

were collected <strong>on</strong> both groups before the<br />

group LBBI was initially implemented with<br />

either group. The data collecti<strong>on</strong> method utilized<br />

for both groups was a whole interval<br />

recording method. The interval period was<br />

10 s for observati<strong>on</strong> with 5 s allowed for recording<br />

data for a 15 minute observati<strong>on</strong> period<br />

each day. Data were collected three days<br />

per week <strong>on</strong> M<strong>on</strong>day, Wednesday, <strong>and</strong> Friday.<br />

In the event of a school holiday or closure, the<br />

data were collected the following day to maintain<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistency in data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The first group LBBI was presented to<br />

Group A in the special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom.<br />

Data were collected three times per week by<br />

the researcher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>on</strong> the<br />

effectiveness of the LBBI <strong>on</strong> each individual<br />

student’s behaviors in the group. Once a positive<br />

change was observed with the participants<br />

in Group A, the sec<strong>on</strong>d LBBI was presented<br />

to Group B in the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom <strong>and</strong> data were collected <strong>on</strong> the associated<br />

behaviors for the individual students.<br />

The LBBI c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be read to Group A<br />

<strong>and</strong> data were still collected <strong>on</strong> Group A participants’<br />

behaviors.<br />

Two weeks after Group A’s interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase ended <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e week after Group B’s<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> phase ended, maintenance data<br />

were collected to determine if the appropriate<br />

behaviors for each participant had increased<br />

or decreased with the removal of the LBBI.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, generalizati<strong>on</strong> data were collected<br />

by the researcher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

in settings other than the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom, such as the cafeteria, art, <strong>and</strong> during<br />

physical educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Interobserver Agreement<br />

The paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al in the mild/moderate<br />

intellectual disability (MI/MO) classroom was<br />

trained to collect data al<strong>on</strong>g with the classroom<br />

teacher in order to provide interobserver<br />

agreement for the study. The classroom<br />

teacher/researcher instructed the paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>on</strong> the targeted behaviors, data collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

procedures, <strong>and</strong> interval recording procedures.<br />

The paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al was required to<br />

record a positive mark if a behavior was exhibited<br />

at any time during the interval. The classroom<br />

teacher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al practiced<br />

several times prior to collecting baseline data<br />

to maintain accuracy <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistency am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

raters. In additi<strong>on</strong>, interobserver agreement<br />

data c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be collected throughout the<br />

research study. Data were collected <strong>on</strong>e day<br />

per week by both the classroom teacher <strong>and</strong><br />

paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al during the same 15 minute<br />

time frame to assess interobserver agreement<br />

for the study.<br />

Interobserver agreement data were collected<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce per week for 9 weeks, representing<br />

33% of the observati<strong>on</strong>s. Interobserver<br />

agreement was calculated by totaling the number<br />

of intervals which both observers recorded<br />

each targeted behavior, dividing that total by<br />

disagreements plus agreements, <strong>and</strong> multiplying<br />

that result by 100 to arrive at a percentage<br />

(Kennedy, 2005). For Brittany, the average<br />

interobserver agreement was 99.33%, with a<br />

range of 96%–100%. For Carol, the average<br />

interobserver agreement was 100%. For Zeke,<br />

the average interobserver agreement was<br />

100%. For C<strong>on</strong>nor, the average interobserver<br />

agreement was 100%. For Bryan, the average<br />

interobserver agreement was 99.33% with a<br />

range of 96%–100%.<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s / 297


Results<br />

Figure 1 depicts the rate of behavior for the<br />

five participants. All five participants (both<br />

groups A <strong>and</strong> B) dem<strong>on</strong>strated a reducti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Figure 1. Blurting out <strong>and</strong> off task behaviors.<br />

their respective disruptive behaviors after the<br />

LBBI was introduced, although the level of<br />

improvement varied for all five participants.<br />

The average daily percentages were calculated<br />

298 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


y determining the number of intervals the<br />

target behavior was observed during each data<br />

collecti<strong>on</strong> period divided by the total number<br />

of intervals <strong>and</strong> multiplying by 100. The total<br />

average for each phase of the study was calculated<br />

by totaling the data collecti<strong>on</strong> period<br />

averages over <strong>on</strong>e particular phase (baseline,<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, maintenance, or generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

phase) <strong>and</strong> dividing by the number of collecti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

within the phase. For Brittany, blurting<br />

out was observed <strong>on</strong> average of 39.2% of the<br />

intervals (range 23%–52%) during the baseline<br />

phase. Throughout the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase of the study, blurting out behaviors decreased<br />

to an average of 17% of the intervals<br />

(range 10–30%). This is an average decrease<br />

of 22.2%. For Carol, blurting out was observed<br />

an average of 18.3% of the intervals (range<br />

18%–25%) during the baseline phase.<br />

Throughout the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase of the<br />

study, blurting out was observed an average of<br />

11.4% of the intervals (range 5–16%), an average<br />

decrease of 6.9%.<br />

The LBBI also improved the behaviors for<br />

the students in the sec<strong>on</strong>d group, group B.<br />

For Zeke, off-task behaviors were observed an<br />

average of 58.6% of the intervals (range 55%–<br />

63%) during the baseline phase. Throughout<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase of the study, off-task<br />

behaviors were observed an average of 35% of<br />

the intervals (range 27–50%), a decrease of<br />

23.6%. For C<strong>on</strong>nor, off-task behaviors were<br />

observed an average of 45.7% of the intervals<br />

(range 42%–50%) during the baseline phase.<br />

Throughout the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase of the<br />

study, off-task behaviors were observed an average<br />

23% of the intervals (range 13–45%).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>nor’s behavior decreased an average of<br />

22.7% during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase. For<br />

Bryan, off-task behaviors occurred during an<br />

average of 83% of the intervals (range 78%–<br />

87%) during baseline. Throughout the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase of the study, off task behaviors<br />

were observed during an average of 55% of<br />

the intervals (range <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>–67%), a decrease of<br />

28%.<br />

Maintenance<br />

Maintenance data were collected two weeks<br />

after the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase ended for Group<br />

A <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e week after the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase<br />

ended for Group B. Maintenance data were<br />

collected <strong>on</strong> a M<strong>on</strong>day, Wednesday, <strong>and</strong> Friday.<br />

For Brittany, blurting out behaviors were<br />

observed during an average of 10% of the<br />

intervals (range 10–12%). This average is 7%<br />

less than her average during the inteventi<strong>on</strong><br />

data collecti<strong>on</strong> period. For Carol, blurting out<br />

behaviors were observed during an average of<br />

5% of the intervals (no range). This average is<br />

6.4% less than her average (11.4%) during<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> period. For Zeke, off-task behaviors<br />

were observed during an average of<br />

28% of the intervals (range 27–30%). This<br />

average is 7% less than his average during the<br />

inteventi<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> period. For C<strong>on</strong>nor,<br />

off-task behaviors were observed during an<br />

average of 14% of the intervals (range 13–<br />

16%). This average is 9% less than his average<br />

during the interventi<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al period.<br />

For Bryan, off-task behaviors were observed<br />

during an average of <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>% of the intervals (no<br />

range). This is 8% less than the average rate of<br />

behavior observed during the interventi<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong><br />

period.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> data were collected <strong>on</strong>ce a day<br />

for 4 days during c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> classes by the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom teacher. The generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

results were c<strong>on</strong>sistent with the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

data for each student. For Brittany,<br />

blurting out behaviors ranged from 10%–23%<br />

of the generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> intervals (average<br />

of 16.2%). Her rate ranged from 10% to<br />

30% during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase. For Carol,<br />

blurting out behaviors ranged from 5%–13%<br />

of the generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> intervals (average<br />

of 9.4%). Carol’s rate of behavior<br />

ranged from 5%–16% during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase. For Zeke, off-task behaviors ranged<br />

from 27%–50% of generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

intervals (average of 36.5%). During<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase his behavior ranged<br />

also ranged from 27%–50% of the intervals.<br />

For C<strong>on</strong>nor, off-task behaviors ranged from<br />

13%–33% of generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al intervals<br />

(average of 21.5%). During the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase off-task behavior were observed<br />

from a low of 13% to a high of 45% of the<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>al intervals. For Bryan, off-task behaviors<br />

during generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

ranged from <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>%–63% of intervals (average<br />

of 56.66%). This is similar to his rate during<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s / 299


the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase when his off-task behavior<br />

ranged from <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>% to 67% of the observati<strong>on</strong><br />

intervals. Bryan was absent for <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the four generalizati<strong>on</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> dates.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

The purpose of this study was to examine the<br />

effects of LBBI <strong>on</strong> the behaviors of small<br />

groups of students with intellectual disability.<br />

While these stories used the sentence types<br />

suggested by Gray (1995) they are not true<br />

social stories because they were not individually<br />

written for <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e partipant. Research<br />

has indicated that social stories or LBBI are<br />

useful for teaching new routines (Moore,<br />

2004), addressing work related behavior (Bucholz<br />

et al., 2008), or teaching c<strong>on</strong>flict resoluti<strong>on</strong><br />

skills (Kalyva & Agaliotis, 2009).<br />

In this study LBBI led to all participants<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating a reducati<strong>on</strong> in their target behaviors.<br />

The greatest reducti<strong>on</strong> of target behavior<br />

occurred for Brittany as she decreased<br />

her calling out from an average of 39.2% to<br />

18.7%. Brittany’s target behaviors c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to decrease even after the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase<br />

ended (maintenance phase–average 10%).<br />

Also, the target behaviors c<strong>on</strong>tinued to decrease<br />

in her c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s classes throughout<br />

the study (generalizati<strong>on</strong> data–average<br />

16.2%). Bryan’s target behavior, being off<br />

task, reduced from a high of 83% to 60%.<br />

After the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase ended, Bryan<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued to decrease his target behaviors<br />

(maintenance phase–average <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>%). In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

he c<strong>on</strong>tinued to decrease the instances<br />

of the target behavior during his c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

classes (generalizati<strong>on</strong> data–average 56.7%).<br />

It is believed that his results would have been<br />

reduced even further if he did not have frequent<br />

absences. Bryan was absent for 13 out of<br />

20 data collecti<strong>on</strong> dates during the baseline<br />

phase, two out of eight data collecti<strong>on</strong> dates<br />

during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase. Bryan was absent<br />

a total of 17 out of 36 data collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

dates for the entire research study (<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>%).<br />

While the research <strong>on</strong> social stories has<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> students with autism, this study was<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted to exp<strong>and</strong> the research <strong>on</strong> this type<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>, specifically literacy-based behavioral<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s, to middle school students<br />

with mild/moderate intellectual disability.<br />

The stories were written to follow the<br />

sentence type guidelines suggested by Gray<br />

(1998), but they differ in that they are written<br />

to target more than <strong>on</strong>e student at a time.<br />

One impact of these students’ intellectual disability<br />

was a lower level of comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

difficulty in retaining informati<strong>on</strong> that is read<br />

or heard. One recommendati<strong>on</strong> would be to<br />

include comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s after the<br />

story is read to ensure the participants’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of the story.<br />

The positive outcomes of this research are<br />

especially important due to the lack of research<br />

that has been d<strong>on</strong>e <strong>on</strong> LBBI <strong>and</strong> social<br />

stories with students who have disabilities<br />

other than autism. These results indicate that<br />

using LBBI with students with intellectual disability<br />

can result in positive changes in behavior.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, these positive outcomes are<br />

important because it is the first research study<br />

that utilized <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e LBBI with a group of<br />

students targeting <strong>on</strong>e behavior, the <strong>on</strong>e<br />

other group study (Kalyva & Agaliotis, 2009)<br />

used three different stories to target social<br />

skill behaviors. Because of the relative ease to<br />

develop <strong>and</strong> implement the story strategy, the<br />

results of this research can exp<strong>and</strong> the type of<br />

behaviors that can be positively impacted by<br />

using LBBI.<br />

As this is the <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of two research studies<br />

that has targeted a group of students, additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research is needed to validate the<br />

effectiveness of using LBBI with small groups<br />

of students. Additi<strong>on</strong>al research is also needed<br />

to study the effectiveness of using LBBI with<br />

students having disabilities other than autism.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

A limitati<strong>on</strong> to this study is that all students are<br />

in the same classroom for all academic classes.<br />

It is unknown if results would have been different<br />

had the students been in different classrooms<br />

with different teachers. An attempt to<br />

remedy this limitati<strong>on</strong> was to utilize the paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

in reading the stories in a small,<br />

separate locati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Another limitati<strong>on</strong> was the length of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> phase. The target behaviors may<br />

have c<strong>on</strong>tinued to decrease if the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase had c<strong>on</strong>tinued. Also, the length of time<br />

between the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase <strong>and</strong> the maintenance<br />

phase could have been l<strong>on</strong>ger to ensure<br />

that the behaviors had c<strong>on</strong>tinued to decrease<br />

300 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


with the removal of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to be maintained at a lower rate.<br />

The present study investigated whether<br />

LBBI would improve the behaviors for five<br />

students with intellectual disability. The results<br />

are encouraging <strong>and</strong> provide empirical<br />

support for the use of LBBIs for individuals<br />

with inteellectual disability. However, additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research is needed to further evaluate<br />

the effectiveness for LBBI for individuals with<br />

various types of disabilities.<br />

References<br />

Brown, W. H. (2001). An interventi<strong>on</strong> hierarchy for<br />

promoting you children’s peer interacti<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments. Topics in Early Childhood<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 21, 162–176.<br />

Bucholz, J. L., Brady, M. P., Duffy, M. L., Scott, J., &<br />

K<strong>on</strong>tosh, L. G. (2008). Using literacy-based behavioral<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> social stories to improve<br />

work behavior in employees with developmental<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 486–501.<br />

Gray, C. A., & Gar<strong>and</strong>, J. D. (1993). Social stories:<br />

Improving resp<strong>on</strong>ses of students with autism with<br />

accurate social informati<strong>on</strong>. Focus <strong>on</strong> Autistic Behavior,<br />

8, 1–10.<br />

Gray, C. A. (1995). Teaching children with autism<br />

to read social situati<strong>on</strong>s. In: K. A. Quill, (Ed.),<br />

Teaching children with autism: strategies to enhance<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> socializati<strong>on</strong> (pp. 219–241). Albany,<br />

NY: Delmar.<br />

Gray, C. A. (1998). Social Stories <strong>and</strong> comic strip<br />

c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s with students with Asperger syndrome<br />

<strong>and</strong> high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism. In E.<br />

Schopler, G. B. Mesibov, & L. J. Kunce (Eds.),<br />

Asperger syndrome or high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism? (pp.<br />

167–198). New York: Plenum.<br />

Graetz, J. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E.<br />

(2006). Show time: Using video self-modeling to<br />

decrease inappropriate behavior. Teaching Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 38(5), 43–48.<br />

Gut, D. M., & Safran, S. P. (2002). Cooperative<br />

learning <strong>and</strong> social stories: Effective social skills<br />

strategies for reading teachers. Reading & Writing<br />

Quarterly, 18, 87–91.<br />

Hall, C. W., Peters<strong>on</strong>, A. D., Webster, R. E., Bolen,<br />

L. M., & Brown, M. B. (1999). Percepti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

n<strong>on</strong>verbal social cues by regular educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

ADHD <strong>and</strong> ADHD/LD students. Psychology in the<br />

Schools, 36, 505–514.<br />

Kalyva, E., & Agaliotis, I. (2009). Can social stories<br />

enhance the interpers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>flict resoluti<strong>on</strong><br />

skills of children with LD? Research in Developmen-<br />

tal <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 192–202. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.<br />

2008.02.005<br />

Kennedy, C. (2005). Single-case designs for educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research. Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Moore, P. S. (2004). The use of social stories in a<br />

psychology service for children with learning disabilities:<br />

A case study of a sleep problem. British<br />

Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 32, 133–138. http://<br />

<strong>on</strong>linelibrary.wiley.com/journal/10.1111/ (ISSN)<br />

1468-3156<br />

Reynhout, G., & Carter, M. (2006). Social stories for<br />

children with disabilities. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 36, 445–469. doi:<br />

10.1007/s10803-006-0086-1<br />

Roy, G. W. (1993). The development <strong>and</strong> implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of a group social skills program for<br />

emoti<strong>on</strong>ally disturbed children in a special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

school. (Report No. RIEJAN1994). Master’s<br />

Practicum Report, Nova University. (ERIC<br />

Document Reporducti<strong>on</strong> Service No. ED360802)<br />

Schneider, N., & Goldstein, H. (2009). Social Stories<br />

improve the <strong>on</strong>-task behavior of children<br />

with language impairment. Journal of Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 31, 250–264. doi: 10.1177/<br />

1053815109339564<br />

Soenksen. D., & Alper, S. (2006). Teaching a young<br />

child to appropriately gain attenti<strong>on</strong> of peers using<br />

a social story interventi<strong>on</strong>. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21, 36-44.<br />

Staley, M. J. (2001). An investigati<strong>on</strong> of social-story<br />

effectiveness by using reversal <strong>and</strong> multiple baseline<br />

designs. Unpublished doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>, University<br />

of Kansas.<br />

Stickel, S. (1990). Using multimodal social-skills<br />

groups with kindergarten children. Elementary<br />

School Guidance <strong>and</strong> Counseling, 24, 281–288.<br />

Toplis, R., & Hadwin, J. A. (2006). Using social<br />

stories to change problematic lunchtime behavior<br />

in school. Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology in Practice, 22,<br />

53–67.<br />

Vaughn, S., Haager, D., Hogan, A., & Kouzehanani,<br />

K. (1992). Self-c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>and</strong> peer acceptance in<br />

students with learning disabilities: A four- to fiveyear<br />

prospective study. Journal of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology,<br />

84, 43–50.<br />

Williams White, S., Ke<strong>on</strong>ig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007).<br />

Social skills development in children with autism<br />

spectrum disorders: A review of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

research. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders,<br />

37, 1858–1868. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-<br />

0320-x<br />

Received: 29 June 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 30 August 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 11 October 2011<br />

Group Delivered Literacy-Based Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s / 301


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 302–318<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Using Video Modeling to Teach Young Children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong>ly Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech<br />

Sarah Clifford Scheflen, Stephanny F. N. Freeman, <strong>and</strong> Tanya Paparella<br />

University of California, Los Angeles<br />

Abstract: Four children with autism were taught play skills through the use of video modeling. Video instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

was used to model play <strong>and</strong> appropriate language through a developmental sequence of play levels integrated<br />

with language techniques. Results showed that children with autism could successfully use video modeling to<br />

learn how to play appropriately with toys in both structured <strong>and</strong> generalized situati<strong>on</strong>s, although the speed with<br />

which the progressi<strong>on</strong> was made was not uniform. In additi<strong>on</strong>, some children showed an increase in the<br />

frequency <strong>and</strong> complexity of their language used when playing.<br />

Lack of symbolic play is an important defining<br />

characteristic of children with autism spectrum<br />

disorders regardless of cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

levels (APA 1994; Thorp, Stahmer, &<br />

Schreibman, 1995). Play in children with autism<br />

has been described as delayed in rate of<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, different in complexity <strong>and</strong> form,<br />

repetitive <strong>and</strong> stereotypical, <strong>and</strong> lacking in<br />

diversity from the play of typical children<br />

<strong>and</strong> from children with other developmental<br />

disabilities (Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, 1987; Jarrold,<br />

Boucher, & Smith, 1993; Stanley & K<strong>on</strong>stantareas,<br />

2007; Ungerer & Sigman, 1981; Whyte<br />

& Owens, 1989; Wullf, 1985). Some comm<strong>on</strong><br />

behavioral observati<strong>on</strong>s include obsessively arranging<br />

toys according to physical characteristics,<br />

repeating scripts, engaging in repetitive<br />

The authors gratefully acknowledge the assistance<br />

of Karla Kirsh<strong>on</strong> for her tireless observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> coding. We thank Nisha Bansal, Kelly Goods,<br />

Jenny Lee, <strong>and</strong> Crystal Lee for their classroom <strong>and</strong><br />

data assistance. We thank Dr. Steven Forness <strong>and</strong><br />

Joanne Kim, M.Ed. for their comments <strong>on</strong> earlier<br />

drafts of this paper. In particular, we wish to thank<br />

the children <strong>and</strong> families who participated in the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>. The play <strong>and</strong> language training videos<br />

used in this study were produced professi<strong>on</strong>ally by<br />

Teach2Talk, LLC (www.teach2talk.com). Video<br />

transcripts can be accessed by c<strong>on</strong>tacting the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

author. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this<br />

article should be addressed to Sarah Clifford<br />

Scheflen, 77-4<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> Semel Institute for Neuroscience &<br />

Human Behavior, UCLA, 760 Westwood Plaza, Los<br />

Angeles, CA 90024-1759.<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al acts <strong>on</strong> a toy (door opening, butt<strong>on</strong><br />

pushing), or showing extreme fixati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e<br />

toy in particular, <strong>on</strong>e part of a toy, or a small<br />

set of toys (Paters<strong>on</strong> & Arco, 2007).<br />

Symbolic play serves many developmental<br />

roles. Of particular interest however, is the<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship between play skills <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills. Language in autism is also delayed in<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, different in complexity <strong>and</strong> form,<br />

repetitive <strong>and</strong> stereotypical, <strong>and</strong> lacking in diversity.<br />

These c<strong>on</strong>cerns include general delays<br />

in expressive <strong>and</strong> receptive language, echolalia,<br />

use of scripted speech <strong>and</strong> jarg<strong>on</strong> in place<br />

of novel <strong>and</strong> natural language, <strong>and</strong> difficulties<br />

with abstract language. Children with autism<br />

also display social-cognitive deficits in their<br />

linguistic <strong>and</strong> communicative development, i.e.,<br />

social <strong>and</strong> communicative gestures, pragmatics,<br />

initiating <strong>and</strong> maintaining reciprocal c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

eye c<strong>on</strong>tact <strong>and</strong> exhibiting poor listening<br />

skills (Lord, 1985; Rutter & Garmezy, 1983;<br />

Sigman, Mundy, Sherman, & Ungerer, 1986).<br />

The c<strong>on</strong>current correlati<strong>on</strong> between language<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> symbolic play skills is supported<br />

by numerous studies. Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen (1987)<br />

found that children with autism who engaged<br />

in symbolic play had significantly higher verbal<br />

mental ages than those who did not. Sigman<br />

<strong>and</strong> Ungerer (1984) found that both<br />

receptive <strong>and</strong> expressive language skills were<br />

related to play in children with autism (even<br />

though in typical children <strong>and</strong> children with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ly receptive language<br />

was related to play).<br />

302 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Accordingly, recent research has investigated<br />

the possibility of teaching play skills to<br />

children with autism given the potential benefits<br />

of improved language development. For<br />

example, <strong>on</strong>e study with a r<strong>and</strong>omized c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group design used a developmental sequence<br />

to teach play through scaffolding <strong>and</strong> adultdirected<br />

prompting with language as an outcome.<br />

This method required the instructor<br />

to identify the child’s current level of functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

within identified developmental sequences<br />

<strong>and</strong> then target interventi<strong>on</strong> in a developmentally<br />

appropriate sequence (Kasari,<br />

Freeman, & Paparella, 2006). The child’s play<br />

repertoires were assessed <strong>on</strong> an adaptati<strong>on</strong><br />

of Lifter’s <strong>Developmental</strong> Play Assessment<br />

which is based <strong>on</strong> l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal descriptive<br />

studies of unstructured <strong>and</strong> structured play<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g children without disabilities (Lifter,<br />

Sulzer-Azaroff, Anders<strong>on</strong>, & Cowdery, 1993;<br />

Lifter, 2000) <strong>and</strong> was modified by Kasari et al.<br />

(2006) for children with autism. Significant<br />

improvements in both play <strong>and</strong> language<br />

skills were observed. Clearly, teaching play<br />

has potential for improving not <strong>on</strong>ly play development<br />

but for language development as<br />

well.<br />

Bart<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Wolery (2008) recently reviewed<br />

the literature <strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s for promoting<br />

pretend play in children with disabilities.<br />

Only 16 studies were found (ten studies<br />

with a focus <strong>on</strong> autism), the methodologies<br />

were varied, <strong>and</strong> samples sizes were all below<br />

ten with the excepti<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e study (Kasari et<br />

al., 2006). The theoretical viewpoint of each<br />

study was not identified in the review <strong>and</strong> the<br />

review authors state that the studies did not<br />

describe the teaching procedure with replicable<br />

precisi<strong>on</strong>. All 16 studies, however, appeared<br />

to use some combinati<strong>on</strong> of physical,<br />

modeling, or verbal prompting of pretense<br />

behaviors. They also used prompting hierarchies,<br />

scripts, <strong>and</strong> typical models, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e study used video modeling.<br />

Video modeling has been shown to be effective<br />

in teaching a wide variety of skills to<br />

children with autism. Video modeling typically<br />

involves the child being shown a videotape<br />

of a model engaging in the targeted behavior<br />

that the child is assisted <strong>and</strong> prompted<br />

to subsequently imitate. The videotape is usually<br />

edited to focus <strong>on</strong> specific behaviors, <strong>and</strong><br />

repeated clips of the same model or multiple<br />

exemplars of the behavior are shown. Discrete<br />

trial sessi<strong>on</strong>s are used, followed by generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

probes <strong>and</strong> periodic reviews of the models<br />

as necessary (Hine & Wolery, 2006). Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

using video modeling has been<br />

shown in <strong>on</strong>e case to be more effective than<br />

live, or in vivo, modeling (Charlop & Milstein,<br />

1989). Researchers have advanced a number<br />

of theories for the efficacy of video modeling<br />

as an instructi<strong>on</strong>al technique for children with<br />

autism. Some reas<strong>on</strong>s may be that video modeling<br />

plays to typical strengths of children with<br />

autism (including their str<strong>on</strong>g memory <strong>and</strong><br />

visual skills) (Charlop & Milstein). It has also<br />

been theorized that eliminating the social<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text associated with in vivo modeling (<strong>and</strong>,<br />

as suggested by Stahmer, Ingersoll, <strong>and</strong><br />

Carter, 2003, avoiding failure by children who<br />

do not learn from social interacti<strong>on</strong>s) allows<br />

children with autism who have sensory <strong>and</strong><br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> issues <strong>and</strong> are easily distracted, to<br />

filter out extraneous stimuli (Zihni & Zihni,<br />

2005). Many children with autism like audiovisual<br />

displays <strong>and</strong> readily attend to them, so<br />

simply watching the video models may also<br />

result in intrinsic reinforcement (Paters<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Arco, 2007).<br />

Video modeling may also have some structural<br />

advantages in that it allows the exact<br />

same, carefully selected model to be systematically<br />

repeated, avoiding accidental deviati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in an in vivo envir<strong>on</strong>ment. This increases the<br />

predictability <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trollability of the model.<br />

Further, a wide range of models (peers, siblings<br />

<strong>and</strong> adults) can be used repeatedly <strong>and</strong>,<br />

if needed (as in the case of self-modeling),<br />

filtering for the most correct models can occur<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneously <strong>and</strong> immediately. McCoy<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hermansen’s (2007) literature review of<br />

the type of model used in videos for teaching<br />

children with autism c<strong>on</strong>cluded that adults,<br />

peers, <strong>and</strong> self modeling all could be effective<br />

in producing positive results. Video modeling<br />

also allows for a wide range of naturalistic<br />

settings <strong>and</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ments that would be impossible<br />

or impractical to reproduce in a<br />

clinic or school setting. Video modeling also<br />

enhances generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the targeted skill<br />

through the use of multiple models featuring<br />

the same stimuli across a variety of naturalized<br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts, pers<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ments (Charlop-<br />

Christy & Daneshvar, 2003; Haring, Kennedy,<br />

Adams, & Pitts-C<strong>on</strong>way, 1987). Finally, video<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 303


modeling may be both cost <strong>and</strong> time effective,<br />

since the same video models can be reused<br />

with <strong>on</strong>e child as well as with multiple children.<br />

Video modeling has also been shown in<br />

numerous studies to be an effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

methodology for developing play, but a<br />

number of factors still remain unexplored.<br />

Charlop <strong>and</strong> Milstein (1989) used video modeling<br />

to increase use of scripted c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong><br />

about toys <strong>and</strong> found generalizati<strong>on</strong> across<br />

novel c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s, toys <strong>and</strong> partners, but<br />

neither scripted nor novel motor play was<br />

studied. Taylor, Levin, <strong>and</strong> Jasper (1999)<br />

taught two children with autism to engage in<br />

play-related statements with siblings, using a<br />

forward chaining procedure to teach a l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

series of comments, but focused <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> playrelated<br />

statements <strong>and</strong> not motor play skills<br />

themselves. D’Ateno, Mangiapanello, <strong>and</strong><br />

Taylor (2003) were able to increase a child’s<br />

use of complex play sequences, including<br />

both verbal <strong>and</strong> motor play; but no data was<br />

presented <strong>on</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the learned<br />

sequences to different toys or settings. Nikopoulus<br />

<strong>and</strong> Keenan (2003, 2004), were able to<br />

successfully decrease latency in initiating social<br />

play <strong>and</strong> increase appropriate play using<br />

video modeling instructi<strong>on</strong> in the majority<br />

of their subjects, but generalizati<strong>on</strong> tended to<br />

be limited <strong>on</strong>ly to toys depicted in the video<br />

models. MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, Clark, Garrigan, <strong>and</strong><br />

Vangala (2005) used video modeling to teach<br />

thematic pretend play skills to two preschool<br />

children with autism but <strong>on</strong>ly scripted, rather<br />

than unscripted play, increased significantly.<br />

Reag<strong>on</strong>, Higbee, <strong>and</strong> Endicott (2006) extended<br />

the use of video modeling to teach<br />

pretend play skills to <strong>on</strong>e participant using a<br />

sibling as a video model but findings were<br />

limited by a quasi-experimental, A-B research<br />

design. Hine <strong>and</strong> Wolery (2006) extended<br />

the findings of Charlop <strong>and</strong> Milstein (1989)<br />

to show that point-of-view modeling could<br />

also be used to teach play skills without experimenter-implemented<br />

reinforcement, but<br />

both subjects in the study were highly verbal,<br />

<strong>and</strong> readily imitated in vivo acti<strong>on</strong>s of adults<br />

with materials prior to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Furthermore,<br />

the play taught was limited to two scenarios<br />

(gardening <strong>and</strong> cooking), <strong>and</strong> did not<br />

address the developmental sequence of play<br />

by teaching skills in a progressi<strong>on</strong> from more<br />

c<strong>on</strong>crete to more abstract <strong>and</strong> imaginary acts.<br />

Paters<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Arco (2007) showed that video<br />

modeling led to increased frequency of independent<br />

motor <strong>and</strong> verbal play acti<strong>on</strong>s in two<br />

boys with autism using both related toys <strong>and</strong><br />

unrelated toys, but both subjects showed prior<br />

proficiency with the toys, <strong>and</strong> the study did<br />

not address the developmental sequence of<br />

play.<br />

The primary aims of this investigati<strong>on</strong> were<br />

threefold. The first aim was to extend the<br />

work of Kasari et al. (2006) who have shown<br />

that adhering to a developmental sequence is<br />

effective when teaching play skills to children<br />

with autism which in turn relates to language<br />

improvements. The sec<strong>on</strong>d aim was to integrate<br />

the work of D’Ateno et al. (2003), Mac-<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ald et al. (2005), <strong>and</strong> Paters<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Arco<br />

(2007), who have dem<strong>on</strong>strated that video<br />

modeling is an effective methodology to teach<br />

play skills to children with autism, including<br />

symbolic <strong>and</strong> imaginative play. The final aim<br />

was to integrate a language-based approach<br />

that incorporates specific language instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> outcome into the methodology. We<br />

attempted to achieve these aims by using a<br />

multiple-baseline single-subject design <strong>on</strong><br />

four children, teaching language <strong>and</strong> play<br />

skills through video modeling using a hierarchal<br />

developmental sequence <strong>and</strong> a language<br />

model that incorporated play-c<strong>on</strong>nected,<br />

developmentally–appropriate language. Play<br />

outcomes included immediate <strong>and</strong> generalized<br />

performance of the learned play skills<br />

across other envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> toys than those<br />

presented in the video models. Language outcomes<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> increased complexity of language<br />

used during the children’s unstructured<br />

play as well as st<strong>and</strong>ardized language<br />

assessments.<br />

Specifically, we sought to (a) teach play<br />

skills to children with autism through video<br />

modeling using a developmental sequence,<br />

(b) to assess generalizati<strong>on</strong> of play skills<br />

learned through this video modeling instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

across envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> materials, <strong>and</strong><br />

(c) to assess whether <strong>and</strong> to what extent incorporating<br />

language in the video modeling<br />

could teach language skills to those children<br />

which would generalize across envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

<strong>and</strong> materials.<br />

304 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Method<br />

Participants<br />

Four male children participated in this investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The investigati<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted at a<br />

day treatment program for children with autism.<br />

The program features an integrative <strong>and</strong><br />

cross-disciplinary team approach to treatment.<br />

All four children were enrolled five days a<br />

week for six hours per day throughout the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>. Each child received approximately<br />

the same treatment during the course<br />

of investigati<strong>on</strong>, which was based <strong>on</strong> applied<br />

behavior analysis principles utilizing a staff<br />

ratio of nearly 1:1 <strong>and</strong> incorporating behavior,<br />

speech, recreati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> occupati<strong>on</strong>al therapy<br />

(individualized). Thus, “treatment” as a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>founding variable was held c<strong>on</strong>stant. Most<br />

importantly, n<strong>on</strong>e of the children received<br />

separate instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> play skills during the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>. The program staff was blind to<br />

the hypotheses of the investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Participants were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected from<br />

the program; two were selected from a classroom<br />

comprised of 2–3 year olds recently diagnosed<br />

with autism, <strong>and</strong> two were selected<br />

from a classroom comprised of children with<br />

increased cognitive challenges <strong>and</strong> poor language<br />

skills. We stratified the selecti<strong>on</strong> to get<br />

a wider range of play skills, cognitive skills <strong>and</strong><br />

language skills. Informed c<strong>on</strong>sent was obtained<br />

from each of the child’s parents or<br />

guardians prior to admissi<strong>on</strong> to the investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

See Table 1 for diagnosis, demographic,<br />

<strong>and</strong> initial developmental characteristics.<br />

The Preschool Language Scale, 4th Editi<strong>on</strong><br />

(PLS-4) was also carried out at the end of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Video<br />

A video model was created for each of the<br />

developmental levels of play <strong>on</strong> the Kasari et<br />

al. (2006) scale (Table 2), except levels 3 <strong>and</strong><br />

4 were combined into <strong>on</strong>e level <strong>and</strong> model,<br />

levels 7 <strong>and</strong> 9 were combined into <strong>on</strong>e level<br />

<strong>and</strong> model, <strong>and</strong> levels 15 <strong>and</strong> 16 were combined<br />

into <strong>on</strong>e level <strong>and</strong> model. These levels<br />

were combined due to difficulties in accurately<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently assigning play acts between<br />

levels. Each video model depicted three<br />

play acts or sequences at that level of play,<br />

each using a different toy or set of toys, accompanied<br />

by scripted language which not<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly related to the modeled play act or sequence<br />

(e.g., “put in” when inserting a puzzle<br />

piece) but also c<strong>on</strong>sisted of a length of utterance,<br />

syntax <strong>and</strong> morphology appropriate for<br />

a typically–developing child who had<br />

achieved the level of play being modeled.<br />

All of the video models were developed<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the authors’ experience <strong>and</strong> were<br />

validated by observing the play <strong>and</strong> accompanying<br />

language of typically–developing children<br />

of various developmental levels who were<br />

provided with the selected toys <strong>and</strong> prompted<br />

to “play.” The primary author is a licensed<br />

clinical speech therapist with over 10 years<br />

experience with program development <strong>and</strong><br />

speech therapy with children with autism. The<br />

video models were acted by the sec<strong>on</strong>d author<br />

seated in a child–sized chair at the child-sized<br />

table in a speech therapy room. The acting<br />

author has over 10 years of experience engaging<br />

in play interventi<strong>on</strong> with children with<br />

autism. The camcorder was focused primarily<br />

<strong>on</strong> the toys modeled rather than the adult<br />

actor (e.g., tight shots in which the toys were<br />

the primary focus <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly the adult actor’s<br />

h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> arms were shown), except for the<br />

level 6 (Pretend Self) play video models, in<br />

which toys are extended towards self, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

combined levels 15 (Sociodramatic) <strong>and</strong> 16<br />

(Fantasy) play, where the entire body of the<br />

adult actor was included in the model.<br />

Experimental Design <strong>and</strong> Procedural Overview<br />

We used a single subject multiple baseline<br />

design across the four children.<br />

Baseline. A 15 minute free play sessi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

observed to assess the child’s mastery <strong>and</strong><br />

emergence level of play in a generalized setting<br />

(their classroom in the program) with a<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard set of toys, including a peg board,<br />

puzzles, shape sorters, ring stackers, baskets,<br />

building blocks, a doctor kit, dress–up<br />

clothes, doll house with furniture <strong>and</strong> figures,<br />

play kitchen with food, plates <strong>and</strong> utensils,<br />

play farm with animals <strong>and</strong> assorted vehicles.<br />

The third baseline sessi<strong>on</strong> (regardless of the<br />

number of baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s) was in the<br />

speech therapy room for every child to ensure<br />

that their play was not specific to the envir<strong>on</strong>ment.<br />

The observer used the developmental<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 305


TABLE 1<br />

Demographic Characteristics<br />

play scale <strong>and</strong> a Mean Length of Utterance<br />

(MLU) was calculated.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> phase. Children were brought<br />

into a therapeutic envir<strong>on</strong>ment/room twice<br />

weekly for 15 minutes (unless absent from the<br />

program). All training sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in the speech therapy room. The training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> involved video model viewing <strong>and</strong><br />

structured toy play time at the table. Video<br />

models represented play skills at each level of<br />

the developmental play scale, with each video<br />

model also incorporating language appropri-<br />

Ian Jeremy Ryan J<strong>on</strong>ah<br />

Age in m<strong>on</strong>ths 59 69 37 37<br />

First Diagnosis pediatrician parent c<strong>on</strong>cern psychiatrist psychiatrist<br />

Regi<strong>on</strong>al Center* <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong><br />

School**<br />

PEP-R***<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong> 21 m 38 m 48 m 34 m<br />

Imitati<strong>on</strong> 22 m 30 m 41 m 37 m<br />

Cognitive Verbal 36 m 33 m 35 m 41 m<br />

Cognitive Perform<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> II<br />

39 m 31 m 29 m 48 m<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong> 76 SS 54 SS 94 SS 76 SS<br />

Daily Living Skills 55 SS 53 SS 91 SS 69 SS<br />

Social Skills 63 SS 55 SS 82 SS 74 SS<br />

Motor Skills 70 SS 70 SS 93 SS 88 SS<br />

Composite<br />

PLS-4****<br />

63 SS 55 SS 87 SS 73 SS<br />

Overall 50 SS/20 m 50 SS/25 m 92 SS/34 m 94 SS/34 m<br />

Auditory Comp 50 SS/22 m 50 SS/25 m 109 SS/44 m 96 SS/37 m<br />

Expressive Comm 50 SS/19 m 50 SS/25 m 76 SS/27 m 93 SS/35 m<br />

MLU between 0–1 between 0–1 between 0–1 2–3 words<br />

Play Levels Mastered 1 (Indisc Act) 4 (Pres. Co.) 6 (Pret. Self) 8 (Child Ag)<br />

2 (Discr Act) 5 (Gen. Co.)<br />

No initiating No initiating<br />

Qualitative Descrip. communicati<strong>on</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> Sev. apraxia Requesting<br />

Maximum prompts No c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>/<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e word pragmatics Basic requests No comments<br />

No word<br />

Stereotyped<br />

combinati<strong>on</strong>s Poor verbal<br />

utterances No questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Stereotyped<br />

Echolalic Imitati<strong>on</strong> Echolalic<br />

utterances<br />

Echolalic No comments Echolalic<br />

* The State Regi<strong>on</strong>al Center system uses a licensed clinical psychologist to determine diagnosis <strong>and</strong><br />

eligibility. <strong>Autism</strong> is 299.0 DSM Category.<br />

** The State Educati<strong>on</strong> Code eligibility category is “<strong>Autism</strong> or Autistic-Like Characteristics”<br />

*** Psychoeducati<strong>on</strong>al Profile Revised (PEP-R), (Schopler et al., 1990)<br />

**** Preschool Language Scale, 4th Editi<strong>on</strong> (PLS-4) (Zimmerman, Steiner, & P<strong>on</strong>d, 2002).<br />

ate for the developmental level of play modeled.<br />

For example, for <strong>on</strong>e of the toy presentati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

at the level 8 (Child as Agent), an<br />

actress had a figure <strong>and</strong> sat the figure in a<br />

chair. The language used was, “Sit down boy.”<br />

Each skill was modeled with three different<br />

sets of toys. Each child watched a video at the<br />

next most advanced level, after the level in<br />

which the child had dem<strong>on</strong>strated mastery in<br />

a generalized setting. The video was shown <strong>on</strong><br />

a desktop computer with a m<strong>on</strong>itor measuring<br />

19” diag<strong>on</strong>ally at a viewing distance of approx-<br />

306 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 2<br />

Play Scale – <strong>Developmental</strong> Levels of Play – Kasari et al. (2006)<br />

Level Categories Definiti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

1 Indiscriminate Acti<strong>on</strong>s All objects are treated alike (e.g., objects are mouthed)<br />

Differentiates am<strong>on</strong>g c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al objects, preserving their<br />

physical characteristics, or single objects (e.g., rolls round<br />

2 Discriminative Acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

beads, squeezes stuffed animal)<br />

Separates c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>s of objects (e.g., takes all pieces out<br />

3 Takes Apart Combinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of puzzle)<br />

Re-creates combinati<strong>on</strong>s of objects according to their<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., puts puzzle pieces into<br />

4 Presentati<strong>on</strong> Combinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

puzzle; nests the nesting cups)<br />

Creates combinati<strong>on</strong>s of objects that result in simple, n<strong>on</strong>specific<br />

c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong>s such as c<strong>on</strong>tainer/c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

5 General Combinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., puts beads & puzzle pieces in the cup)<br />

Relates objects to self, indicating a pretend quality to the<br />

6 Pretend Self<br />

acti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., brings empty cup to mouth to drink)<br />

Preserves unique physical characteristics of objects in the<br />

Specific Combinati<strong>on</strong>s –<br />

(physical attributes) c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., stacks nesting<br />

7<br />

Physical Attributes<br />

cups, strings beads)<br />

Extends familiar acti<strong>on</strong>s to doll figures, with child as agent<br />

8 Child as Agent<br />

of the activity (e.g. extends cup to doll’s mouth)<br />

Preserves unique c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al characteristics of object in<br />

Specific Combinati<strong>on</strong>s –<br />

the (c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al attributes) c<strong>on</strong>figurati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., places<br />

9<br />

C<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al Attributes<br />

cup <strong>on</strong> saucer; places string of beads <strong>on</strong> self)<br />

Extends same familiar acti<strong>on</strong> to two or more figures (e.g.,<br />

10 Single Scheme Sequences<br />

extends cup to baby doll, to stuffed lamb, to interactant)<br />

Uses <strong>on</strong>e object to st<strong>and</strong> in place for another (e.g., puts<br />

11 Substituti<strong>on</strong>s With Object<br />

bowl <strong>on</strong> head for a hat)<br />

Pretends to use something that is not there (e.g., shakes an<br />

12 Substituti<strong>on</strong>s Without Object imaginary salt shaker)<br />

Moves doll figures as if they are capable of acti<strong>on</strong> (e.g.,<br />

moves figure to load blocks in a truck; puts mirror into<br />

13 Doll as Agent<br />

doll’s h<strong>and</strong> as if to see itself)<br />

Extends different acti<strong>on</strong>s to same figure (e.g., feeds doll<br />

14 Multischeme Sequences<br />

with spo<strong>on</strong>, wipes it with cloth, then puts to bed)<br />

Adopts various familiar roles in play theme (e.g., plays<br />

15 Sociodramatic Play<br />

house, assigning the various roles<br />

Adopts roles of fantasy characters (e.g., plays ‘‘Superman’’<br />

16 Thematic Fantasy Play<br />

assigning the various roles)<br />

imately <strong>on</strong>e meter in fr<strong>on</strong>t of the computer.<br />

The child watched three different toy models,<br />

two times each. Each toy model was approximately<br />

30 sec<strong>on</strong>ds l<strong>on</strong>g. Thus, the procedure<br />

was as follows. First, the child watched a specific<br />

toy being modeled by the video twice <strong>and</strong><br />

then, immediately afterward, the child was led<br />

to a child-sized seat at a child–sized table <strong>on</strong><br />

which the exact toys, or toys nearly identical to<br />

those modeled in the video models, were<br />

placed within easy reach. The child was given<br />

approximately two minutes to play. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the<br />

child was shown the video model again twice<br />

(next toy set) <strong>and</strong> then the child was brought<br />

to the table <strong>and</strong> those new toys were presented<br />

<strong>and</strong> the child was given approximately two<br />

minutes to play. Third, the child was shown the<br />

video model the third time (third set of toy<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong>s) <strong>and</strong> the child was brought back<br />

to the table with the third toy set <strong>and</strong> the child<br />

was given about two minutes to play. During<br />

the toy play at the table, the child was<br />

prompted with, “Time to play!” <strong>and</strong> the clinician<br />

provided no other prompts, but did pro-<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 307


vide limited verbal reinforcement (e.g., “great<br />

job” or “you did it”) when the child correctly<br />

imitated a play act. The entirety of each training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> was videotaped <strong>and</strong> subsequently<br />

reviewed by either <strong>on</strong>e or two research staff<br />

members.<br />

Maintenance. Maintenance observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were carried out in the same therapeutic<br />

room (speech therapy room) where the instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted (<strong>on</strong> the<br />

same day schedule as the instructi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were held). Any <strong>and</strong> all of the toys that had<br />

been presented were made available for the<br />

child. The child was permitted to play for 10<br />

minutes without prompts.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong>. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were carried out twice weekly during the training<br />

phase in each child’s program classroom<br />

for 10 minutes during the same free play time<br />

as the baseline observati<strong>on</strong>s (the classrooms<br />

have a specific set time during the day when<br />

the children can engage freely with a toy–<br />

approximately 10–15 minutes). This generally<br />

occurred <strong>on</strong> different days than the training<br />

days.<br />

Mastery. Mastery was achieved if a child<br />

was observed to carry out the targeted mastered<br />

level of play with three different toys not<br />

featured in the video models, both in the<br />

structured setting <strong>and</strong> in the generalized setting.<br />

All training sessi<strong>on</strong>s (<strong>and</strong> the third baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>) were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in a medium-sized<br />

speech <strong>and</strong> language therapy room. The video<br />

models were shown using a desktop computer.<br />

A <strong>on</strong>e–way mirror allowed for observati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

All baseline (except the third baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>) <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in the participants’ classrooms which<br />

were typically sized classrooms with <strong>on</strong>e–way<br />

mirrors allowing for observati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Participants generally attended the program<br />

for 10 to 12 weeks. Thus, the participants<br />

had generally two training sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

two generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s per week of the<br />

treatment phase. We began with Ian. Ian’s<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al level was level 3 <strong>and</strong> he was thus<br />

taught using a level 3/4 video model. Ian<br />

mastered his first level at level 4 (week 3.5)<br />

<strong>and</strong> then Jeremy began interventi<strong>on</strong>. We c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

mastery at level 3 enough to start<br />

Jeremy. Jeremy mastered his first level at instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

trial 4 (week 5.5) <strong>and</strong> then Ryan<br />

began interventi<strong>on</strong>. Ryan, at trial 2 (week 6),<br />

mastered his level <strong>and</strong> then we began J<strong>on</strong>ah.<br />

Ryan had a slight dip/loss, but we had already<br />

began J<strong>on</strong>ah <strong>and</strong> thus c<strong>on</strong>tinued with J<strong>on</strong>ah<br />

while we instructed Ryan at the higher level.<br />

Procedural Fidelity<br />

We assessed the procedural fidelity of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

in two ways. First, we formally assessed<br />

whether the interventi<strong>on</strong> procedures<br />

were administered with precisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistency<br />

across children <strong>and</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s (Detrich,<br />

1999). We did so by creating a checklist that<br />

was used by the third author to score every<br />

fifth training sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> every fifth generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

probe or maintenance probe. A single<br />

checklist was used, which c<strong>on</strong>sisted of six questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

designed to identify whether the clinician<br />

followed the protocol in a training sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> five questi<strong>on</strong>s designed to identify<br />

whether the clinician followed the protocol in<br />

a generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe or maintenance probe.<br />

All the checklists logged indicated that the<br />

protocol had been adhered to in each training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

probe with 100% accuracy. This is not<br />

surprising given the nature of the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which required a set of very simple procedures<br />

to be implemented (which were implemented<br />

primarily by the first author).<br />

We also informally assessed the design of<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Each member of the research<br />

staff participated in training sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

prior to commencement of the investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

to review experiment design <strong>and</strong> procedures.<br />

The video models utilized were not independently<br />

validated, but were created as a collaborative<br />

process in which the video models<br />

were rerecorded until the authors were satisfied<br />

that the models adhered to the scripted<br />

play acts <strong>and</strong> verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s. Finally, the video<br />

models were based not <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> the authors’<br />

experience but also <strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> of typically<br />

developing children playing with the selected<br />

toys at various developmental levels.<br />

Dependent Variables<br />

Two target variables were selected for measurement:<br />

(a) play acti<strong>on</strong>s, used to derive the<br />

highest <strong>and</strong> the average level of play achieved<br />

in each sessi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> (b) appropriate play–<br />

308 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


elated utterances, which were used to derive<br />

the mean length of utterance (MLU) for each<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Play: Coding Measures<br />

The investigati<strong>on</strong> used the Kasari et al. (2006)<br />

play scale composed of sixteen distinct developmental<br />

levels of play. This play scale was<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the developmental play scale developed<br />

by Karin Lifter <strong>and</strong> used in her <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Play Assessment (Lifter, 2000). Brief<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>al summaries of the developmental<br />

play levels are in Table 2. It is to be noted that<br />

<strong>on</strong>e child needed instructi<strong>on</strong> at level 3 to<br />

begin, <strong>and</strong> that no child was instructed at level<br />

15/16. For level 7, the child had to master<br />

both level 7 <strong>and</strong> level 9 to be moved to level 8.<br />

Therefore, after mastering level 8, the children<br />

were then instructed at level 10. To<br />

establish reliability, prior to the start of the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>e undergraduate research<br />

student <strong>and</strong> the first author participated in<br />

group training sessi<strong>on</strong>s led by the sec<strong>on</strong>d author,<br />

who has been deemed reliable <strong>on</strong> the<br />

coding system in a number of previous studies.<br />

Once training was complete <strong>and</strong> reliability<br />

was established by c<strong>on</strong>sensus, the undergraduate<br />

research student <strong>and</strong> the first author<br />

then coded multiple play sessi<strong>on</strong>s. First, they<br />

coded prior tapes of children who had been<br />

involved in previous studies <strong>on</strong> play <strong>and</strong> had<br />

participated in free play sessi<strong>on</strong>s (names removed,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sent provided from prior studies).<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, they coded children in the program<br />

who were not participating in this investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

by observing them during free-play sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the classroom. The undergraduate<br />

research student coded all of the children.<br />

The first author served as a reliability st<strong>and</strong>ard.<br />

Single measure intraclass correlati<strong>on</strong> coefficients<br />

were used to calculate reliability <strong>on</strong><br />

25% of the sample (38 sessi<strong>on</strong>s r<strong>and</strong>omly chosen<br />

from baseline, training, <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

Each coefficient was calculated for play<br />

category using the types (not frequency) of<br />

play displayed during that sessi<strong>on</strong>. Coefficients<br />

were 1.00 for the following: Takes<br />

Apart/ Presentati<strong>on</strong> Combinati<strong>on</strong>s, Substituti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Object, Substituti<strong>on</strong>s without Object,<br />

Doll as Agent, <strong>and</strong> Mutischeme Sequences.<br />

General Combinati<strong>on</strong>s .98;<br />

Pretend Self .97; Physical/ C<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Combinati<strong>on</strong>s .74; Child as Agent .99;<br />

<strong>and</strong> Single Scheme Sequences .84.<br />

An appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong> was defined as<br />

the child’s performing <strong>on</strong>e or more c<strong>on</strong>nected<br />

play acti<strong>on</strong>s using the toys provided,<br />

other than play at level 12 (Substituti<strong>on</strong> Without<br />

Object), where play acti<strong>on</strong>s did not need<br />

to incorporate a provided toy if appropriate<br />

(e.g., pretending to play guitar). An appropriate<br />

play acti<strong>on</strong> was coded by assigning a level<br />

of developmental play to any play acti<strong>on</strong>. As a<br />

result, a single play acti<strong>on</strong> could both c<strong>on</strong>tain<br />

more than <strong>on</strong>e related discrete acti<strong>on</strong> (e.g.,<br />

sequences, which c<strong>on</strong>sisted of a series of c<strong>on</strong>nected<br />

discrete play acti<strong>on</strong>s) <strong>and</strong> be coded at<br />

more than <strong>on</strong>e play level. For example, if a<br />

child placed a doll <strong>on</strong> a block pretending that<br />

the block was a bed for a doll while verbalizing<br />

“go to sleep,” it would be coded as two appropriate<br />

play acti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>on</strong>e play acti<strong>on</strong> at level 8<br />

(Child as Agent) for placing the doll <strong>on</strong> the<br />

bed, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e play acti<strong>on</strong> at level 11 (Substituti<strong>on</strong><br />

With Object) for substituting the block<br />

for the bed. Both the toy utilized <strong>and</strong> the play<br />

acti<strong>on</strong>(s) performed were recorded (as well as<br />

the utterance, whether or not appropriate, as<br />

discussed below). No distincti<strong>on</strong> was made between<br />

play acti<strong>on</strong>s modeled in the video models<br />

(i.e., scripted play acts) <strong>and</strong> novel play<br />

acti<strong>on</strong>s (i.e., unscripted play acts), as the<br />

coder was blind to the instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the<br />

video models. The highest level of play<br />

achieved was derived for each sessi<strong>on</strong> by examining<br />

all appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong>s for the<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> recording the appropriate play<br />

acti<strong>on</strong> scored at the highest level of play. The<br />

average level of play achieved for each sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

was calculated by summing the play levels recorded<br />

for each play related acti<strong>on</strong> by a child<br />

during a sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> then dividing by the total<br />

number of play related acti<strong>on</strong>s exhibited by a<br />

child during the sessi<strong>on</strong>. Appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

at level 3/4 were assigned a value of 3.5,<br />

appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong>s at level 7/9 were assigned<br />

a value of 8, <strong>and</strong> appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of 15/16 were assigned a value of 15.5.<br />

For example, the average level of play<br />

achieved in which the child exhibited four<br />

appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong>s for the sessi<strong>on</strong>, two at<br />

level 3/4, <strong>on</strong>e at level 6, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e at level 10, is<br />

5.75 (((2*3.5)(1*6)(1*10))/4).<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 309


Language Coding Measures<br />

An appropriate play–related utterance was defined<br />

as an intelligible verbalizati<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e or<br />

more English words (or an intelligible approximati<strong>on</strong><br />

thereof) by the participants immediately<br />

before, during or immediately after an<br />

appropriate play act, <strong>and</strong> the verbalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

had to be related to or in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with the<br />

play acti<strong>on</strong>. Verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s made immediately<br />

before, during or immediately after any acti<strong>on</strong><br />

or sequence by the participant which was not<br />

coded as an appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong> were recorded<br />

but not coded as an appropriate play–<br />

related verbalizati<strong>on</strong>, even if they related<br />

to <strong>and</strong> were in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with the (n<strong>on</strong>–<br />

appropriate play) acti<strong>on</strong>. Verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s that<br />

were related to an appropriate play acti<strong>on</strong> but<br />

made more than three sec<strong>on</strong>ds prior to, or<br />

three sec<strong>on</strong>ds after, the play acti<strong>on</strong> were recorded<br />

but not scored (the incidence of such<br />

verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s was extremely low). All verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during a sessi<strong>on</strong> were logged, regardless<br />

of whether they c<strong>on</strong>stituted an appropriate<br />

play–related utterance. Due to the<br />

difficulty in definitively categorizing utterances<br />

<strong>and</strong> the coder being blind to the instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the video models, no distincti<strong>on</strong><br />

was made between play–related utterances<br />

modeled in the video models (i.e., scripted<br />

utterances) <strong>and</strong> novel or different utterances<br />

(i.e., unscripted utterances).<br />

Average MLU was calculated for each sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

by dividing the total number of morphemes<br />

uttered during all appropriate play–<br />

related utterances made during the sessi<strong>on</strong> by<br />

the total number of all appropriate play–<br />

related utterances made during the sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The total number of morphemes spoken during<br />

any particular utterance was determined<br />

in accordance with the procedures set forth in<br />

the Guide to Analysis of Language Transcripts,<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d Editi<strong>on</strong>, (Retherford, 1993)<br />

which represents the st<strong>and</strong>ard methodology<br />

employed by speech language pathologists<br />

when measuring mean length of utterance<br />

(e.g., counting the utterance of <strong>on</strong>e plural<br />

noun forms as two morphemes, <strong>and</strong> counting<br />

the utterance of <strong>on</strong>e verb in predicate form as<br />

two morphemes).<br />

The first author (MS, CCC-SLP) trained the<br />

undergraduate student to measure MLU. The<br />

undergraduate student transcribed <strong>and</strong> calcu-<br />

lated MLU for all sessi<strong>on</strong>s. The first author<br />

checked every transcripti<strong>on</strong> (100%) <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>firmed<br />

any discrepancies with the undergraduate.<br />

Results<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> Outcomes<br />

Average play skills are presented graphically<br />

within video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong> (instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

baseline, <strong>and</strong> maintenance) <strong>and</strong> during<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> (no baseline or maintenance).<br />

Average MLU is presented graphically within<br />

video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong> (instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

baseline, <strong>and</strong> maintenance) <strong>and</strong> during generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

(no baseline or maintenance). Figure<br />

1 shows Ian’s progress (play observed in<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>, play in generalized observati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

MLU observed in instructi<strong>on</strong>, MLU in generalized<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>), Figures 2, 3, <strong>and</strong> 4 show<br />

Jeremy’s, Ryan’s, <strong>and</strong> J<strong>on</strong>ah’s progress respectively.<br />

During the interventi<strong>on</strong>, all four children<br />

were able to progress through multiple<br />

levels of play (Table 2) <strong>and</strong> all generally maintained<br />

those gains in the limited maintenance<br />

phase of the investigati<strong>on</strong>. Please keep in<br />

mind that the figures present ‘average’ level of<br />

play <strong>and</strong> thus, a child might have an average<br />

level lower than their mastered level (e.g., a<br />

child has the 3 higher level acts with 3 novel<br />

toys but other play acts are at other levels <strong>and</strong><br />

thus, an average is obtained; see Jeremy’s initial<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al point). Recall that n<strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the four children received level 7 instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(as it was included with the video model for<br />

level 9).<br />

Figure 1 displays Ian’s steady progressi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

both the instructi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> generalized situati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

based <strong>on</strong> average level of play achieved.<br />

Prior to the interventi<strong>on</strong>, his average level of<br />

play was 1.8 (range of 1–2). Ian was initially<br />

instructed at level 3/4 <strong>and</strong> progressed<br />

through each level until emergence at level 8<br />

where the interventi<strong>on</strong> ended. In the maintenance<br />

phase, the average level of play Ian<br />

displayed was 6.8 (the range of play acts<br />

through maintenance observati<strong>on</strong> went as low<br />

as level 5 <strong>and</strong> as high as level 10). Further, as<br />

noted, the highest level of play observed during<br />

any play sessi<strong>on</strong> (trial, generalizati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

maintenance) was level 10. Ian had a slight<br />

drop in his average play level during the first<br />

310 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 1. Ian’s Progress in Play Skills <strong>and</strong> MLU. Ian’s baseline was 3 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (1.5 weeks), instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> were 17 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (8.5 weeks), <strong>and</strong> maintenance was 3 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (1.5 weeks) with a total<br />

of 40 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (11.5 weeks). In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #2, Ian mastered play levels 3/4. In training sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

#7, Ian mastered play level 5. In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #22, Ian mastered play level 6. At training sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

#30, Ian was at level 8 (not mastered/left at this level). The highest level of play observed in any<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> was level 10.<br />

maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>, but advanced his play<br />

without instructi<strong>on</strong> back to his average level of<br />

play during instructi<strong>on</strong>. In terms of generalized<br />

play, Ian clearly generalized his skills in<br />

the classroom envir<strong>on</strong>ment (without prompting)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent with his play instructi<strong>on</strong>. Ian’s<br />

language also significantly increased in<br />

amount during the instructi<strong>on</strong> phases of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. In examinati<strong>on</strong> of all 3 baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, he had an MLU of 0.0. Across all the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s, his average MLU was<br />

about 1.8, <strong>and</strong> then about 1.9 during the three<br />

maintenance phase observati<strong>on</strong>s. During generalized<br />

free play, Ian c<strong>on</strong>tinued to use language<br />

fairly c<strong>on</strong>sistently even without an<br />

adult. Ian’s PLS-4 receptive scores improved<br />

to <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>ths <strong>and</strong> expressive scores to 33<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Figure 2 shows Jeremy’s play skills during<br />

baseline, instructi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

phases. Jeremy’s baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s yielded an<br />

average level of play at 4.9 (range of 1–5).<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Jeremy began at level 6 (play<br />

acts at level 5 was sufficient for ‘mastery’). He<br />

progressed to emergence in level 10 by the<br />

end of interventi<strong>on</strong>. His average play level<br />

progressed across the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. It was noted<br />

that Jeremy’s highest level of play observed<br />

during any play sessi<strong>on</strong> was level 13. Jeremy<br />

advanced his play slightly during baseline<br />

prior to instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> again during the first<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>on</strong>ly little instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Clearly, however, Jeremy advanced significantly<br />

as instructi<strong>on</strong> was presented. Similar<br />

to Ian, in the maintenance phase, <strong>on</strong> the first<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, Jeremy’s play overall was not as complex<br />

but at the sec<strong>on</strong>d sessi<strong>on</strong>, his average play<br />

level returned to the interventi<strong>on</strong> average<br />

(range from 2–13 in those two sessi<strong>on</strong>s). Figure<br />

2 displays Jeremy’s progress in play during<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 311


Figure 2. Jeremy’s Progress in Play Skills <strong>and</strong> MLU. Jeremy’s baseline was 7 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (3.5 weeks), instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> were 13 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (6.5 weeks), <strong>and</strong> maintenance was two sessi<strong>on</strong>s (1 week) with a<br />

total of 35 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (11 weeks). In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #4, Jeremy mastered play level 5. In training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> #7, Jeremy mastered play level 6. In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #12, Jeremy mastered play level 8. At<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong> #13, Jeremy was at level 10 (not mastered/left at this level). The highest level of play<br />

observed in any sessi<strong>on</strong> was level 13.<br />

the generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s as well. Although<br />

his play skills in generalized sessi<strong>on</strong>s were not<br />

as developmentally high as in the instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, he clearly showed progress in his play<br />

skills. In terms of MLU, Jeremy was primarily<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-verbal (MLU 0) throughout baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> for the first 5 weeks of the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

after which his average MLU increased from<br />

week 6 <strong>and</strong> through the end of the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Jeremy’s average MLU was 1.7 (sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

range of 0–2.7) during interventi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> his<br />

average MLU in the maintenance phase was<br />

1.6. In generalized sessi<strong>on</strong>s, Jeremy began to<br />

use some language even before he showed<br />

that language in the instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

This was positive <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued in the generalized<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s fairly c<strong>on</strong>sistently. Jeremy’s<br />

PLS-4 receptive scores improved to 39 m<strong>on</strong>ths<br />

<strong>and</strong> expressive scores improved to 41 m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Ryan showed a progressi<strong>on</strong> during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase which was notably main-<br />

tained in the generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong><br />

average level of play achieved (Figure 3). It is<br />

to be noted that during Ryan’s baseline, he<br />

exhibited single acts at level 7/9, but his average<br />

level of play was 4.9 (range 2–7/9). Thus,<br />

we began instructi<strong>on</strong> at level 6. He immediately<br />

showed mastery of this level in generalized<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s after <strong>on</strong>e training sessi<strong>on</strong>, so we<br />

moved to instructi<strong>on</strong> at level 7/9 <strong>and</strong> he progressed<br />

through emergence at level 10. In the<br />

maintenance phase, the highest level of play<br />

exhibited by Ryan was level 14, <strong>and</strong> his average<br />

level of play was 9.8 (range of 2–14). Ryan was<br />

primarily n<strong>on</strong>-verbal (MLU 0) throughout<br />

baseline, but made rapid initial gains in his<br />

MLU during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase (MLU <br />

1.7, with sessi<strong>on</strong> range of 0–3.0) <strong>and</strong> in the<br />

generalized sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Those gains which were<br />

maintained in the maintenance phase<br />

(MLU 2.9) with variati<strong>on</strong> in his MLU from<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> to sessi<strong>on</strong>. His PLS-4 scores improved<br />

312 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 3. Ryan’s Progress in Play Skills <strong>and</strong> MLU. Ryan’s baseline was 10 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (5 weeks), instructi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

10 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (5 weeks), generalizati<strong>on</strong> was nine sessi<strong>on</strong>s (absent <strong>on</strong>e), <strong>and</strong> maintenance was two<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s (1 week) with a total of 31 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (11 weeks). In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #1, Ryan mastered play<br />

level 6. In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #8, Ryan mastered play level 8. At training sessi<strong>on</strong> #10, Ryan was at level<br />

10 (not mastered/left at this level). The highest level of play observed in any sessi<strong>on</strong> was level 14.<br />

to 50 m<strong>on</strong>ths receptively <strong>and</strong> 37 m<strong>on</strong>ths expressively.<br />

Figure 4 shows that J<strong>on</strong>ah had the most<br />

advanced play skills of the participants prior<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong>. His baseline play skills were<br />

mastered at level 8 <strong>and</strong> his average level of<br />

play was 6.7. Thus, we started instructi<strong>on</strong> at<br />

level 10. Again, note that although instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

targeted level 10, J<strong>on</strong>ah’s average level of play<br />

in any sessi<strong>on</strong> may have been lower. Over the<br />

course of the interventi<strong>on</strong>, J<strong>on</strong>ah made gains<br />

in his average level of play, <strong>and</strong> he carried<br />

those gains over into his maintenance phase<br />

<strong>and</strong> generalized sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

J<strong>on</strong>ah progressed from level 8 through<br />

mastery of level 13 <strong>on</strong> his last sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> thus,<br />

emergence at level 14. At the highest point<br />

(during maintenance phase), he exhibited<br />

play at level 15/16. His average level of play <strong>on</strong><br />

the last maintenance observati<strong>on</strong> was 13.5.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>ah’s MLU prior to interventi<strong>on</strong> was 1.0; he<br />

made rapid gains during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase (MLU 2.5, with sessi<strong>on</strong> range of 1.5–<br />

2.9), maintained MLU usage in the generalized<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> maintained gains in the<br />

maintenance phase (MLU 2.2). Both his<br />

receptive <strong>and</strong> expressive PLS-4 scores improved<br />

to 44 m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Social Validity<br />

The purpose of a social validity assessment is<br />

to determine if the interventi<strong>on</strong> was socially<br />

relevant. Social relevance includes whether an<br />

outcome is significant, appropriate, <strong>and</strong> important<br />

(Wolf, 1973). We did not formally<br />

assess the social significance of the goals, as<br />

play <strong>and</strong> language skills are unequivocally fundamental<br />

to the development of children. We<br />

also did not formally assess the social appropriateness<br />

of the procedures used in this investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

as all of the participant’s families<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 313


Figure 4. J<strong>on</strong>ah’s Progress in Play Skills <strong>and</strong> MLU. J<strong>on</strong>ah’s baseline was 11 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (5.5 weeks), instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

was nine sessi<strong>on</strong>s (4.5 weeks), generalizati<strong>on</strong> was eight sessi<strong>on</strong>s (absent <strong>on</strong>e), <strong>and</strong> maintenance was<br />

two sessi<strong>on</strong>s (1 week) with a total of 30 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (11 weeks). In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #3, J<strong>on</strong>ah mastered<br />

play level 8. In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #6, Ryan mastered play level 10. In training sessi<strong>on</strong> #7, Ryan<br />

mastered play level 12. At training sessi<strong>on</strong> #8, Ryan mastered play level 13 <strong>on</strong> the last sessi<strong>on</strong>. Ryan<br />

left at play level 14. The highest level of play observed in any sessi<strong>on</strong> was level 15/16.<br />

indicated <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>sent form that the investigati<strong>on</strong>’s<br />

procedures were acceptable to them<br />

<strong>and</strong> video modeling has been established as<br />

both time <strong>and</strong> cost effective. (Bellini & Akullian,<br />

2007; Charlop-Christy, Le, & Freeman,<br />

2000; Hine & Wolery, 2006).<br />

We did, however, ask adults at an autism<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ference to judge the interventi<strong>on</strong> to formally<br />

establish social validity. The regi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ference focused <strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

for children with autism <strong>and</strong> other developmental<br />

disorders. Of the audience, 124 of 136<br />

adults (parents, educators, <strong>and</strong> therapists) resp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

to our request. The individuals were<br />

asked to code a r<strong>and</strong>omly–chosen selecti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

clips of the four participants, <strong>on</strong>e each during<br />

baseline, interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

phases. The individuals were asked to qualitatively<br />

judge the overall frequency <strong>and</strong> appropriateness<br />

of the participants’ play <strong>and</strong> play–<br />

related language. All resp<strong>on</strong>dents were blind<br />

to the investigati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> its purpose. A <strong>on</strong>e–<br />

minute clip was systematically selected from<br />

each of the four participants (e.g., the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

minute of each participant’s sec<strong>on</strong>d baseline<br />

trial was used for that participant’s baseline<br />

clip). The resulting twelve clips (three from<br />

each of the four participants) were placed in a<br />

r<strong>and</strong>om order, <strong>and</strong> shown to the coders in a<br />

single sessi<strong>on</strong>. The coders were asked to code<br />

each clip using a five-point Likert scale for<br />

each of the following questi<strong>on</strong>s: Was the child<br />

engaged with toys? Was the child playing with<br />

the toys appropriately? How much language is<br />

the child using appropriately? Did the child<br />

look like he was having fun? See Figure 5. The<br />

average ratings by the naïve adults were examined<br />

using paired sample t-tests between baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> treatment <strong>and</strong> between baseline <strong>and</strong><br />

maintenance. Significant differences were<br />

identified across all four children <strong>and</strong> rating<br />

categories (engagement, play, language, <strong>and</strong><br />

314 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 5. Social Validity: Average ratings across the four participants at baseline, during instructi<strong>on</strong> phases,<br />

<strong>and</strong> during maintenance by naïve observers <strong>on</strong> engagement, play, use of language, <strong>and</strong> if the child<br />

looks like he is having fun.<br />

4 Always/A Lot<br />

3 Almost Always/Quite a Bit<br />

2 Sometimes/Medium Amount<br />

1 Almost Never/Very Little<br />

0 Never/N<strong>on</strong>e<br />

From baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>* showed significant change at p < .05 (J<strong>on</strong>ah play), p < .01 (Ian<br />

engagement <strong>and</strong> play), <strong>and</strong> p < .001 (for all other participants <strong>on</strong> four ratings) with the excepti<strong>on</strong><br />

of <strong>on</strong>e n<strong>on</strong>-significant finding for Ian in his language. From baseline to maintenance**, significant<br />

changes were observed at p < .05 (J<strong>on</strong>ah play), p < .01 (Ian language) <strong>and</strong> p < .001 (for all other<br />

participants <strong>on</strong> four ratings).<br />

fun), except for language ratings <strong>on</strong> Ian’s<br />

baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>. The naïve adults<br />

rated the children as having significantly better<br />

play skills <strong>and</strong> engagement during the<br />

maintenance phase. Although language improvement<br />

was rated as having the least qualitative<br />

change, the observer ratings increased<br />

by an average of <strong>on</strong>e Likert scale point.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

The purpose of this study was to examine if a<br />

video modeling methodology could be effective<br />

in teaching both play <strong>and</strong> language skills<br />

to young children with autism. In an area that<br />

is termed a “core deficit,” a deficit that defines<br />

the disorder <strong>and</strong> is naturally difficult for such<br />

children, the four children in this study made<br />

significant gains. In additi<strong>on</strong>, their mean<br />

length of utterance <strong>and</strong> language scores also<br />

improved significantly over the course of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. These findings were both socially<br />

validated by unbiased <strong>and</strong> un-informed<br />

observers who clearly noted a qualitative<br />

change in the children’s play <strong>and</strong> language<br />

use in general.<br />

As reported by Paparella <strong>and</strong> Kasari (2004)<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kasari, Freeman, <strong>and</strong> Paparella (2001),<br />

children can be taught play skills <strong>and</strong> language<br />

can c<strong>on</strong>currently develop <strong>and</strong> be main-<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 315


tained; however, these types of targeted, discrete<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s are not easily available<br />

<strong>and</strong> the developmental knowledge of play sequences<br />

is not clinically mainstreamed.<br />

Clearly, best results would be obtained primarily<br />

in a clinical setting such as ours, from<br />

professi<strong>on</strong>als with training <strong>and</strong> experience <strong>on</strong><br />

play <strong>and</strong> language in the instructi<strong>on</strong> of children<br />

<strong>on</strong> the autism spectrum or with other<br />

developmental disabilities. If this best case is<br />

not available or attainable, video modeling<br />

might be effectively implemented by parents<br />

or lesser–trained caregivers of children with<br />

autism when given proper written instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Furthermore, video modeling could serve as a<br />

useful adjunct to traditi<strong>on</strong>al therapies <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which are in limited supply – <strong>and</strong><br />

the costs of which are significant for families<br />

already struggling with the financial burden of<br />

a child with a developmental disability.<br />

As noted by other researchers, video modeling<br />

can be scaled to instruct multiple children<br />

at the same developmental level <strong>and</strong> with<br />

the same approximate existing skill sets. Further,<br />

the methodology seems to be particularly<br />

efficacious for pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism. Although<br />

the initial investment in properly<br />

preparing video models <strong>and</strong> developing accompanying<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al methodology could<br />

be high, <strong>on</strong>ce prepared, the video models are<br />

relatively easy to reuse to address the need for<br />

repetitive presentati<strong>on</strong>. Further, video models<br />

offer a restricted field of focus <strong>and</strong> high motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Charlop & Milstein, 1989; Corbett &<br />

Abdullah, 2005; Dorwick & Jesdale, 1991;<br />

Stahmer et al., 2003; Paters<strong>on</strong> & Arco, 2007;<br />

Zihni & Zihni, 2005).<br />

A primary weakness of the investigati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

the sample size; however, the extensive data<br />

collected over the 10 week period allows for<br />

the examinati<strong>on</strong> of change over time. The<br />

data collecti<strong>on</strong>, detailed coding, <strong>and</strong> social<br />

validity were relative strengths of this investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Another relative strength was that<br />

each child participated in the same therapeutic<br />

program, receiving the same treatment,<br />

throughout the examinati<strong>on</strong>, meaning that<br />

no child received play interventi<strong>on</strong> through<br />

the program (although language development<br />

was a str<strong>on</strong>g focus of the program), <strong>and</strong><br />

that no child received unknown, other services<br />

that might have affected the child’s play<br />

<strong>and</strong> language skills. Play benefits could thus<br />

be attributed primarily to the treatment, <strong>and</strong><br />

language benefits could be at least partially<br />

attributed to the treatment. Because each participant<br />

was also receiving intensive speech<br />

therapy two times per week for 30 minutes per<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> individually or in a group, the results<br />

<strong>on</strong> appropriate play–related utterances <strong>and</strong><br />

mean length of utterance <strong>and</strong> overall language<br />

should be interpreted with cauti<strong>on</strong>. Another<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong> of the investigati<strong>on</strong> was that<br />

no follow–up probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted to measure<br />

whether the participants retained the<br />

skills generalized during the course of the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Although numerous studies have investigated<br />

video modeling as a methodology to<br />

teach play <strong>and</strong> language skills in a variety of<br />

children, including the generalizati<strong>on</strong> of acquired<br />

skills, n<strong>on</strong>e have combined the three<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents that this study employed: video<br />

modeling, a developmentally sequenced approach<br />

to teaching play, <strong>and</strong> a MLU outcome.<br />

Further research into the efficacy of video<br />

modeling to teach play <strong>and</strong> language skills is<br />

warranted in order to determine the most<br />

effective way to ensure generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> retenti<strong>on</strong><br />

of learned skills. Of particular interest<br />

would be studies to determine if additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

benefits might be gained through even more<br />

fine-grained instructi<strong>on</strong> matched to more specific<br />

developmental levels.<br />

References<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>. (1994). Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>: American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bar<strong>on</strong>-Cohen, S. (1987). <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> symbolic play.<br />

British Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong> Psychology, 5, 139–<br />

148.<br />

Bart<strong>on</strong>, E. E., & Wolery, M. (2008). Teaching pretend<br />

play to children with disabilites: A review of<br />

the literature. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

28, 109–125.<br />

Bellini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of<br />

video modeling <strong>and</strong> video self-modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with autism<br />

spectrum disorders. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 73, 264–<br />

287.<br />

Charlop, M., & Milstein, J. (1989). Teaching autistic<br />

children c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al speech using video modeling.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 275–<br />

285.<br />

Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A.<br />

316 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


(2000). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of video modeling with in<br />

vivo modeling for teaching children with autism.<br />

Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 30,<br />

537–552.<br />

Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Daneshvar, S. (2003).<br />

Using video modeling to teach perspective taking<br />

to children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 5, 12–21.<br />

Corbett, C. A., & Abdullah, M. (2005). Video modeling:<br />

Why does it work for children with autism?<br />

Journal of Early <strong>and</strong> Intensive Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2, 2–8.<br />

D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B. (2003).<br />

Using video modeling to teach complex play sequences<br />

to a preschooler with autism. Journal of<br />

Positive Behavior Interacti<strong>on</strong>s, 5, 5–11.<br />

Detrich, R. (1999). Increasing treatment fidelity by<br />

matching interventi<strong>on</strong>s to c<strong>on</strong>textual variables<br />

within the educati<strong>on</strong>al setting. School Psychology<br />

Review, 28, 608–620.<br />

Dorwick, P. W., & Jesdale, (1991). Practical Guide to<br />

Using Video in the Behavioral Sciences. New York:<br />

John Wiley & S<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Haring, T., Kennedy, C., Adams, M., & Pitts-C<strong>on</strong>way,<br />

V. (1987). Teaching generalizati<strong>on</strong> of purchasing<br />

skills across community settings to autistic youth<br />

using video modeling. Journal of Applied Behavioral<br />

Analysis, 20, 89–96.<br />

Hine, J., & Wolery, M. (2006). Using point-of-view<br />

video modeling to teach play to preschoolers with<br />

autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

26, 83–93.<br />

Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1993). Symbolic<br />

play in autism: A review. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 23, 281–307.<br />

Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2001). Early<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> in autism: joint attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> symbolic<br />

play. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Review of Research in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 207–237.<br />

Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> symbolic play in young children<br />

with autism: a r<strong>and</strong>omized c<strong>on</strong>trolled interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

study. Journal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 611–620.<br />

Lifter, K. (2000). Linking assessment to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for children with developmental disabilities<br />

or at-risk for developmental delay: The <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Play Assessment (DPA) instrument. In<br />

K. Gitlin-Weiner, A. S<strong>and</strong>grund, & C. E. Schaefer<br />

(Eds.), Play diagnosis <strong>and</strong> assessment (2 nd Ed.,<br />

pp. 228–261). New York: Wiley.<br />

Lifter, K., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., Anders<strong>on</strong>, S., &<br />

Cowdery, G. E. (1993). Teaching play activities to<br />

preschool children with disabilities: The importance<br />

of developmental c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Journal of<br />

Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 17, 139 –159.<br />

Lord, C. (1985). <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

language. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesivbov (Eds.),<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong> problems in autism (pp. 257–281).<br />

New York: Plenum Press.<br />

MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, R., Clark, M., Garrigan, E., & Vangala,<br />

M. (2005). Using video modeling to teach pretend<br />

play to children with autism. Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

20, 225–238.<br />

McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling<br />

for individuals with autism: A review of model<br />

types <strong>and</strong> effects. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of Children,<br />

30, 183–213.<br />

Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2003). Promoting<br />

social initiati<strong>on</strong> in children with autism using<br />

video modeling. Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 18, 87–<br />

108.<br />

Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of<br />

video modeling <strong>on</strong> social initiati<strong>on</strong>s by children<br />

with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,<br />

93–96.<br />

Paparella. T., & Kasari, C. (2004). Joint attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

skills <strong>and</strong> language development in special needs<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>s: Translating research to practice. Infants<br />

& Young Children, 17, 269–280.<br />

Paters<strong>on</strong>, C., & Arco, L. (2007). Using video modeling<br />

for generalizing toy play in children with<br />

autism. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 31, 660–681.<br />

Reag<strong>on</strong>, K. A., Higbee, T. S., & Endicott, K. (2006).<br />

Teaching pretend play skills to a student with<br />

autism using video modeling with a sibling as<br />

model <strong>and</strong> play partner. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment<br />

of Children, 29, 517–528.<br />

Retherford, K. S. (1993). Guide to analysis of language<br />

transcripts, Sec<strong>on</strong>d Editi<strong>on</strong>. Eau Claire, Wisc<strong>on</strong>sin:<br />

Thinking Publicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

psychopathology. In P. Mussen (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

child psychology (pp. 775–911). New York: Wiley.<br />

Schopler, E., Reichler, R.J., Bashford, A., Lansing,<br />

M., & Marcus, L., (1990). Psychoeducati<strong>on</strong>al Profile<br />

Revised [PEP-R]. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J.<br />

(1986). Social interacti<strong>on</strong>s of autistic, mentally<br />

retarded, <strong>and</strong> normal children <strong>and</strong> their caregivers.<br />

Journal of Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 27,<br />

6<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>–656.<br />

Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

language skills in autistic, mentally retarded, <strong>and</strong><br />

normal children. <strong>Developmental</strong> Psychology, 20,<br />

293–302.<br />

Stahmer, A., Ingersoll, B. & Carter, C. (2003). Behavioral<br />

approaches to promoting play. <strong>Autism</strong>, 7,<br />

401–413.<br />

Stanley, G., & K<strong>on</strong>stantareas, M. (2007). Symbolic<br />

play in children with autism spectrum disorder.<br />

Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 37,<br />

1215–1223.<br />

Taylor, B. A., Levin, L., & Jasper, S. (1999). Increasing<br />

play-related statements in children with autism<br />

toward their siblings: Effects of video model-<br />

Video Modeling for Appropriate Play <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>nected Speech / 317


ing. Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>and</strong> Physical<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 11, 253–264.<br />

Thorpe, J., Stahmer, A., & Schreibman, L. (1995).<br />

Effects of sociodramatic play training <strong>on</strong> children<br />

with autism. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disorders, 25, 265–282.<br />

Ungerer, J. A., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play<br />

<strong>and</strong> language comprehensi<strong>on</strong> in autistic children.<br />

Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry,<br />

20, 318–337.<br />

Whyte, J., & Owens, A. (1989). Language <strong>and</strong> symbolic<br />

play: Some findings from a study of autistic<br />

children. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 10, 317–<br />

332.<br />

Wolf, M. (1973). Social validity: The case for subjective<br />

measurement or applied behavior analysis is<br />

finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,<br />

11, 203–214.<br />

Wullf, S. B. (1985). The symbolic <strong>and</strong> object play of<br />

children with autism: a review. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 15, 139–148.<br />

Zihni, F., & Zihni, F. (2005). AZ method: The use of<br />

video techniques to develop language skills in<br />

autistic children. Retrieved 3/10/05, from<br />

http://www.autismuk.com/index13.htm.<br />

Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & P<strong>on</strong>d, R. E.<br />

(2002). Preschool Language Scale, 4th Editi<strong>on</strong>. San<br />

Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Pears<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Received: 26 July 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 1 October 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 21 November 2011<br />

318 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 319–331<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Effects of a Video Model to Teach Students with Moderate<br />

Intellectual Disability to Use Key Features of an iPh<strong>on</strong>e<br />

Kathryn Walser, Kevin Ayres, <strong>and</strong> Erika Foote<br />

University of Georgia<br />

Abstract This study evaluated the effects of video modeling <strong>on</strong> teaching three high school students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability to perform three activities <strong>on</strong> an iPh<strong>on</strong>e 3GS. This study is a replicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> extensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of the Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, Whatley, Ayres, <strong>and</strong> Gast (2010) study in which researchers taught this same set of skills<br />

using a slightly different format of instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> a less complex h<strong>and</strong>-held device. In the current study, a<br />

multiple probe design across three behaviors, replicated across three participants, was used to evaluate the effects<br />

of video modeling <strong>on</strong> participants’ capacities to (a) take a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>, (b) look at photographs by<br />

starting a slideshow, <strong>and</strong> (c) access <strong>and</strong> view a video. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> to a more complex home screen<br />

arrangement featuring multiple unused butt<strong>on</strong>s not present during interventi<strong>on</strong> was also measured following<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. Results of the present study indicate that video modeling was effective in teaching target behaviors<br />

in a near-errorless fashi<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, during generalizati<strong>on</strong>, students were able to navigate to each of the<br />

three tasks despite the additi<strong>on</strong> of 14 other distracter butt<strong>on</strong>s, not previously present without additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

training.<br />

The Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Improvement<br />

Act defines assistive technology (AT) devices<br />

as those tools that help students with disabilities<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> in their envir<strong>on</strong>ment (Wright,<br />

2004). The range in what qualifies as a “device”<br />

is broad <strong>and</strong> can include things from<br />

specialized eating utensils to computer software<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>verts speech to text. Some of<br />

these are everyday items that simply require a<br />

low tech modificati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., fattening a pencil<br />

with a rubber grip) while others are more<br />

complex <strong>and</strong> specialized (e.g., Dynavox augmentative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> systems). In many<br />

cases the technology is specialized in some<br />

way, either by design or modificati<strong>on</strong>, to promote<br />

independence.<br />

Depending <strong>on</strong> how dynamic an individual’s<br />

The research reported here was supported by the<br />

Institute of Educati<strong>on</strong> Sciences, U.S. Department of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, through Grant R324A100094 to the University<br />

of Georgia. The opini<strong>on</strong>s expressed are those<br />

of the authors <strong>and</strong> do not represent views of the<br />

Institute or the U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be addressed<br />

to Kevin Ayres, Department of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, The University of Georgia, 516 Aderhold<br />

Hall, Athens, GA 30602-7153. Email:<br />

kayres@uga.edu<br />

needs are, the life-span of an AT device may<br />

be limited. Many years ago, Phillips <strong>and</strong> Zhao<br />

(1993) noted shifting user needs as <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the primary reas<strong>on</strong>s individuals stopped using<br />

an AT device. Further, Brookes (1998) suggested<br />

that some families may decide not to<br />

utilize AT for their children because of the<br />

increased focus <strong>on</strong> the child <strong>and</strong> disability.<br />

Much of the research cited by Parette <strong>and</strong><br />

Scherer (2004) focused <strong>on</strong> the stigma associated<br />

with AT <strong>and</strong> the influence <strong>on</strong> the user.<br />

The fear that AT may magnify the visual percepti<strong>on</strong><br />

of an individual’s disability weighs<br />

heavily in the decisi<strong>on</strong> to use or forgo AT.<br />

However, soluti<strong>on</strong>s exist in more typical formats.<br />

Applicati<strong>on</strong>s currently exist for the iPod<br />

<strong>and</strong> iPh<strong>on</strong>e that put communicati<strong>on</strong> opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., speech-to-text) in a “normal” device that<br />

does not look atypical across multiple envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Within today’s technology-dependent<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>, the ubiquity of people using an<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e or listening to an iPod in public is<br />

striking.<br />

Shifting AT emphasis toward device opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

used more frequently by the general public<br />

may reduce the stigma typically associated<br />

with AT <strong>and</strong> increase user adopti<strong>on</strong>. Abner<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lahm (2002) noted teacher reluctance to<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 319


implement AT with which they themselves<br />

have little familiarity. Therefore, adapting a<br />

device like an iPod or iPh<strong>on</strong>e that has widespread<br />

usage within the general populati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

a logical step. Thus, beginning to train individuals<br />

to use a h<strong>and</strong>held device (e.g., an iPod<br />

or iPh<strong>on</strong>e) to take advantage of the flexible<br />

range of software supports becomes the next<br />

challenge.<br />

Video modeling <strong>on</strong> computers, h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

devices, <strong>and</strong> other tools has been used across<br />

disciplines <strong>and</strong> across settings to teach a variety<br />

of skills. In 2002, Davies, Stock, <strong>and</strong> Wehmeyer<br />

examined the use of a h<strong>and</strong>held, selfdirected<br />

visual <strong>and</strong> auditory prompting system<br />

to improve the independent performance of<br />

community-based vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the researchers tried to determine if the<br />

use of the h<strong>and</strong>held systems would also reduce<br />

the amount of support needed from<br />

teachers or job coaches. They reported that<br />

the h<strong>and</strong>held prompting systems resulted in<br />

increases for each student’s independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing in community-based tasks, but<br />

more importantly, the study dem<strong>on</strong>strated the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held systems reduced the amount of support<br />

time required by the teacher or job coach<br />

for each student.<br />

In two studies, <strong>on</strong>e with middle school aged<br />

students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

(Hamm<strong>on</strong>d et al., 2010) <strong>and</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d with<br />

elementary-aged students with autism (Hamm<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

Muething, Ayres, & Gast, in review),<br />

researchers examined the use of video modeling<br />

to teach students to access activities <strong>on</strong><br />

iPods. The intenti<strong>on</strong> was, in part, to evaluate<br />

the use of video modeling with fine motor<br />

tasks that may be more difficult for users to<br />

discriminate. In both cases, all students<br />

learned the target tasks, but important limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were identified in the initial study (Hamm<strong>on</strong>d<br />

et al., 2010). These researchers used<br />

single opportunity probes (Cooper, Her<strong>on</strong>, &<br />

Heward, 2007) to evaluate student performance<br />

of the steps of their task analyses (e.g.,<br />

accessing music, accessing videos, <strong>and</strong> accessing<br />

photographs). This may have artificially<br />

suppressed baseline resp<strong>on</strong>ding because if a<br />

student failed at the first step, they would not<br />

have had a chance to dem<strong>on</strong>strate whether<br />

they could perform subsequent steps. While<br />

the students’ mastered the skills, their performance<br />

deteriorated over time. During main-<br />

tenance probes, the researchers noted the<br />

lower levels of resp<strong>on</strong>ding <strong>and</strong> implemented<br />

“booster” sessi<strong>on</strong>s of video modeling to help<br />

the students regain criteri<strong>on</strong> level performance.<br />

In a subsequent study, Hamm<strong>on</strong>d et al. (in<br />

review) used the same instructi<strong>on</strong>al methodology<br />

<strong>and</strong> incorporated multiple opportunity<br />

probes rather than single opportunity probes.<br />

This allowed participants to make an error <strong>on</strong><br />

a step <strong>and</strong> have a chance to resp<strong>on</strong>d to the<br />

later steps in the task analysis (TA). The effects<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> were evaluated with a<br />

multiple baseline design across behaviors replicated<br />

across participants. After learning <strong>on</strong>e<br />

of the target skills, two of the participants<br />

generalized those steps to the other tasks that<br />

shared many of the same procedural steps. As<br />

a result of the design’s structure, this generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

did not weaken Hamm<strong>on</strong>d et al.’s<br />

(2010) experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol. However, it did<br />

suggest that the trio of behaviors being taught<br />

may have shared too many critical features to<br />

serve as target behaviors in this research c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

where ideally you would want skills that<br />

are functi<strong>on</strong>ally independent to safeguard<br />

against carry-over threats (Wolery, Gast, &<br />

Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, 2010).<br />

The current study is a replicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> extensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of Hamm<strong>on</strong>d et al. (in review). Capitalizing<br />

<strong>on</strong> the foundati<strong>on</strong> laid with the structure<br />

of the research design, this study used the<br />

same multiple baseline design across behaviors<br />

replicated across participants to evaluate<br />

the effects of video modeling <strong>on</strong> student acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of three skills <strong>on</strong> an iPh<strong>on</strong>e 3GS.<br />

The purpose of the replicati<strong>on</strong> is to apply a<br />

similar procedure to a more complex device<br />

<strong>and</strong> evaluate generalizati<strong>on</strong> to untrained interfaces.<br />

The research questi<strong>on</strong>s addressed<br />

here include: (1) What effect does video modeling<br />

have <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of taking a<br />

photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>, viewing photographs<br />

in a slideshow, <strong>and</strong> viewing a video in iTunes?;<br />

<strong>and</strong> (2) If students acquire the skills via video<br />

modeling in an envir<strong>on</strong>ment of reduced distracters<br />

(e.g., <strong>on</strong>ly the minimum number of<br />

butt<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> the screen) will the student generalize<br />

this without training to an envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

filled with distracters (e.g., screen filled with<br />

ic<strong>on</strong>s)?<br />

320 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Method<br />

Participants<br />

Four high school students, all identified as<br />

having moderate intellectual disability<br />

(MOID), initially agreed to participate in the<br />

study. However, due to health c<strong>on</strong>cerns <strong>and</strong><br />

multiple absences, the fourth participant had<br />

to leave the study. All participants were selected<br />

because they had IEP goals related to<br />

recreati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> leisure that fit with the target<br />

objectives of the project (e.g., visual discriminati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> following instructi<strong>on</strong>s). Participants<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>ally possessed the following prerequisite<br />

skills: (a) visual acuity <strong>and</strong> manual<br />

dexterity sufficient to place the tip of a finger<br />

<strong>on</strong>a1cmby1cmtarget <strong>on</strong> the face of an<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e; <strong>and</strong> (b) ability to sit for at least 15<br />

minutes.<br />

The first participant, Holly, was a loquacious<br />

21 year <strong>and</strong> 5 m<strong>on</strong>th old female at the<br />

<strong>on</strong>set of the study with a diagnosis of Down’s<br />

Syndrome <strong>and</strong> MOID. Holly displayed deficits<br />

in reading for informati<strong>on</strong>al purposes <strong>and</strong><br />

counting mixed dollar bills. However, Holly<br />

possessed strengths in learning to expressively<br />

state sight words, sound out unfamiliar words,<br />

<strong>and</strong> her ability to manage pers<strong>on</strong>al items (e.g.,<br />

purse, insulin materials, <strong>and</strong> cell ph<strong>on</strong>e).<br />

Prior to the study, Holly had experience using<br />

a family member’s iPh<strong>on</strong>e 3GS to send <strong>and</strong><br />

receive text messages. However, Holly’s performance<br />

was dependent up<strong>on</strong> assistance with<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> typing messages <strong>on</strong> the iPh<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

The Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales<br />

(VABS; Sparrow, Balla, Cicchetti, & Doll,<br />

1984) indicated that she showed st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

scores of 63 for daily living, 86 for socializati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> 62 for communicati<strong>on</strong> with a composite<br />

of 68. The Differential Ability Scales<br />

(DAS; Elliott, 2005) indicated that Holly<br />

scored a <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> in the verbal cluster, a 57 in the<br />

n<strong>on</strong>verbal reas<strong>on</strong>ing cluster, 50 in the spatial<br />

cluster, a <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> in the general cognitive ability,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a 51 in the special n<strong>on</strong>verbal composite.<br />

Jake, the sec<strong>on</strong>d participant, was 17 years<br />

<strong>and</strong> 9 m<strong>on</strong>ths old at the introducti<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

study with a diagnosis of Fragile X <strong>and</strong> MOID.<br />

Jake displayed anxiety <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>-seeking<br />

behaviors (e.g., h<strong>and</strong>-biting, using inappropriate<br />

language). This behavior was avoided<br />

throughout the study via verbal reassurances<br />

<strong>and</strong> redirecti<strong>on</strong> al<strong>on</strong>g with sustained involvement.<br />

Jake displayed deficits in attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

impulse c<strong>on</strong>trol, oftentimes negatively impacting<br />

his ability to acquire new informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

skills. Jake possessed strengths in computer<br />

<strong>and</strong> calculator proficiency. The DAS (Elliott,<br />

2005) indicated that Jake scored a 76 in the<br />

verbal cluster, a 53 in the n<strong>on</strong>verbal reas<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

cluster, <strong>and</strong> a 61 for the overall general cognitive<br />

ability score.<br />

The final participant, Norman, was 18 years<br />

old at the beginning of the study. He displayed<br />

deficits in adaptive behavior that impacted<br />

his educati<strong>on</strong>al progress as he tended<br />

to be easily distracted. Norman’s strengths included<br />

his ability to recall key parts of stories<br />

that were read to him <strong>and</strong> his ability to learn<br />

<strong>and</strong> recall sight words. Norman also enjoyed<br />

cooking with his class. On the VABS (Elliott,<br />

2005) he scored st<strong>and</strong>ard scores of 62 for daily<br />

living, 66 for socializati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g> for communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

with a composite of 56. Results from<br />

the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children –<br />

Third Editi<strong>on</strong> (Wechsler, 1991) indicated a<br />

verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) of 54, performance<br />

IQ of 46, <strong>and</strong> a full scale IQ of 46.<br />

Materials <strong>and</strong> Settings<br />

Students viewed video models <strong>on</strong> a 15” Mac-<br />

Book Pro laptop. The videos were viewed in<br />

QuickTime (QuickTime, 2011) <strong>and</strong> were<br />

cued for the student prior to the sessi<strong>on</strong>’s<br />

commencement. Videos depicted the h<strong>and</strong>s<br />

of an actor using an 8 GB iPh<strong>on</strong>e 3GS running<br />

iOS 4. The durati<strong>on</strong> of the three video models<br />

varied: 28 s for watching a video, 18 s for<br />

taking a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> 26 s for<br />

looking at photographs. During baseline <strong>and</strong><br />

initial instructi<strong>on</strong>, the iPh<strong>on</strong>e screen <strong>on</strong>ly displayed<br />

four soft butt<strong>on</strong>s at the bottom of the<br />

screen that included <strong>on</strong>e of the target butt<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the orange iPod butt<strong>on</strong>. Soft butt<strong>on</strong>s are those<br />

that could not be changed <strong>and</strong> were located<br />

<strong>on</strong> the bottommost horiz<strong>on</strong>tal toolbar. The<br />

rest of the screen c<strong>on</strong>tained <strong>on</strong>ly two ic<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

<strong>on</strong>e to initiate the iPh<strong>on</strong>e’s camera <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e to<br />

view the photograph album. Five high interest<br />

photographs were included in the photograph<br />

album <strong>and</strong>, similarly, for watching a<br />

video, five high interest videos were imported<br />

into iTunes. Due to <strong>on</strong>e participant, Jake’s,<br />

changing preference throughout the study, a<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 321


sixth <strong>and</strong> seventh video were added to match<br />

his changing taste <strong>and</strong> therefore motivati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

seek the videos.<br />

This study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in a rural public<br />

high school in the South. All sessi<strong>on</strong>s took<br />

place in an empty classroom located next to<br />

the school’s gymnasium approximately 20 ft<br />

by 20 ft in dimensi<strong>on</strong>. Only the participating<br />

student <strong>and</strong> data collectors were in the room<br />

during each sessi<strong>on</strong>. The researcher providing<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> always stood to the side of the<br />

student so as to be readily available to interrupt<br />

<strong>and</strong> correct errors during the multiple<br />

opportunity probe trials <strong>and</strong> to collect data.<br />

The researcher collecting interobserver agreement<br />

(IOA) <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity data always<br />

stood to the other side of the participant<br />

in order to see their physical movements <strong>and</strong><br />

the screen of the iPh<strong>on</strong>e. During all instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, students sat at a desk <strong>and</strong> the<br />

researcher h<strong>and</strong>ed them the ph<strong>on</strong>e with a<br />

black screen prior to the beginning of the<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Definiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

The primary dependent measure was independent<br />

performance of each step of the target<br />

skills (See Table 1 for task analyses). A step<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>sidered independent if the student<br />

initiated the step within 5softhetask directi<strong>on</strong><br />

or completi<strong>on</strong> of the previous step <strong>and</strong><br />

completed that step in a topographically correct<br />

manner within 5 s. Failure to initiate a<br />

step within 5 s resulted in that step being<br />

scored as a latency error. If a student initiated<br />

the step but failed to complete it within5sthe<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se was scored as a durati<strong>on</strong> error. If the<br />

student initiated an incorrect topography<br />

within the latency time frame, that step was<br />

scored as a topographical error. Keeping with<br />

protocol for multiple opportunity probes, in<br />

the case of all errors across all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, the<br />

student was interrupted, their line of visi<strong>on</strong><br />

was redirected away from the device, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

device was advanced to the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of that<br />

step prior to having their attenti<strong>on</strong> redirected<br />

back to the iPh<strong>on</strong>e. This ensured that for each<br />

step, the student was presented with correct<br />

discriminative stimulus <strong>and</strong> had the opportunity<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d. Interobserver agreement<br />

(IOA) <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity were collected<br />

by a sec<strong>on</strong>d data collector during at least 20%<br />

of all sessi<strong>on</strong>s, for each participant, in each<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. IOA was calculated using the pointby-point<br />

method (number of agreements divided<br />

by the number of agreements plus disagreements,<br />

multiplied by 100) <strong>and</strong> a<br />

minimum acceptability for reliability levels<br />

was 85%. Procedural fidelity data were calculated<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the number of behaviors correctly<br />

performed by the investigator, divided<br />

by the number of planned investigator behaviors,<br />

multiplied by 100 (Billingsley, White, &<br />

Muns<strong>on</strong>, 1980). Investigator behaviors which<br />

were assessed during baseline, Pre-Video Modeling,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Post-Video Modeling sessi<strong>on</strong>s included<br />

the following: delivery of the general<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>al cue, delivery of the task directi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

correct issuance of the iPh<strong>on</strong>e with a black<br />

screen, <strong>and</strong> the investigator’s attendance to<br />

latency, allotted resp<strong>on</strong>se durati<strong>on</strong>, error interrupti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

error restart, <strong>and</strong> issuance of corrective<br />

feedback.<br />

During a minimum of 20% of both baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s for all students, a<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d observer collected interobserver<br />

agreement (IOA) <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity. Procedural<br />

fidelity was based <strong>on</strong> a checklist of<br />

expected researcher behaviors for each sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The number of correctly performed researcher<br />

behaviors was divided by the number<br />

of expected behaviors <strong>and</strong> multiplied by 100.<br />

Procedure<br />

General procedure. This study is composed<br />

of two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (baseline <strong>and</strong> video modeling<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>). All sessi<strong>on</strong>s in both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tained an initial probe trial to measure<br />

performance <strong>on</strong> the target skill<br />

(sometimes referred to as a “cold” probe).<br />

During Video Modeling, students had the<br />

cold probe followed by Video Modeling Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> a Post-Video Modeling Practice<br />

trial. In all cases students participated individually<br />

in sessi<strong>on</strong>s lasting 5 to10 minutes 3 to 4<br />

times per week. Once the materials for a task<br />

had been arranged, participants were given<br />

the task directi<strong>on</strong> for <strong>on</strong>e of three target skills<br />

(e.g., “Take a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>,” “Look<br />

at photographs,” or “Watch a video”) while<br />

simultaneously being h<strong>and</strong>ed the iPh<strong>on</strong>e with<br />

a black screen indicating the need to initiate<br />

the target task. The ph<strong>on</strong>e was in a st<strong>and</strong>-by<br />

positi<strong>on</strong> similar to how it would be in if some-<br />

322 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 1<br />

Task Analyses for all Tasks<br />

Watch a Video<br />

Screen Appearance Task Analysis<br />

Screen Black 1. Push HOME butt<strong>on</strong><br />

2. Slide LOCK to the<br />

Lock Screen<br />

right<br />

Orange iPod Bottom<br />

Right 3. Hit iPod butt<strong>on</strong><br />

Varies 4. Hit VIDEO butt<strong>on</strong><br />

Video List 5. Select target video<br />

Target Video Begins<br />

Look at Photographs<br />

Screen Black 1. Push HOME butt<strong>on</strong><br />

2. Slide LOCK to the<br />

Lock Screen<br />

right<br />

Home Screen 3. Hit FLOWER butt<strong>on</strong><br />

Photograph List 4. Click a photograph<br />

Photograph<br />

Enlarged 5. Click the PLAY butt<strong>on</strong><br />

Slide Show Begins<br />

Take a Photograph<br />

of a Pers<strong>on</strong><br />

Screen black 1. Push HOME butt<strong>on</strong><br />

2. Slide LOCK to the<br />

Lock screen<br />

right<br />

Home Screen 3. Hit CAMERA butt<strong>on</strong><br />

4. Point camera at a<br />

Display of Camera pers<strong>on</strong><br />

Pers<strong>on</strong> in “View” 5. Click CAMERA butt<strong>on</strong><br />

* Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D. L., Whatley, A. D., Ayres, K. A., &<br />

Gast, D. L. (2010). Effectiveness of video modeling<br />

to teach iPod use to students with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45(4), 525–<br />

538.<br />

<strong>on</strong>e had just pulled it from his or her pocket.<br />

Once students began interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> received<br />

Video Modeling instructi<strong>on</strong>, they were<br />

asked to engage in a single opportunity practice<br />

trial (Post Video Modeling Practice) to<br />

allow the student to rehearse the task <strong>and</strong><br />

measure their ability to resp<strong>on</strong>d so<strong>on</strong> after<br />

viewing a model. Any error which occurred<br />

during single opportunity probes immediately<br />

terminated the sessi<strong>on</strong>. Data from the practice<br />

trials were recorded, but did not count toward<br />

skill mastery.<br />

Baseline <strong>and</strong> Pre-Video Modeling Probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Pre-Video Modeling Probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in a multiple opportunity format (Cooper,<br />

Her<strong>on</strong>, & Heward, 2007). Thus, if an error<br />

occurred, the observer m<strong>on</strong>itoring student<br />

performance would fix the error <strong>and</strong> prepare<br />

the iPh<strong>on</strong>e for the next step without the student<br />

seeing. The observer would then tell the<br />

student to “keep working.” Students were initially<br />

presented with the iPh<strong>on</strong>e with a blank<br />

screen <strong>and</strong> given the task directi<strong>on</strong> (e.g. “Let’s<br />

take a photograph.”) to indicate which target<br />

task to initiate. Data were collected after the<br />

delivery of the task directi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> each student<br />

to measure independent <strong>and</strong> accurate performance<br />

of each step of the target task. If a<br />

durati<strong>on</strong> error occurred, the student’s attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

was directed away from the ph<strong>on</strong>e as the<br />

instructor simultaneously completed the current<br />

step in the TA, preparing the ph<strong>on</strong>e for<br />

the student to complete the next step in the<br />

TA. The student’s attenti<strong>on</strong> was then verbally<br />

redirected back to the task. If a sequential or<br />

topographical error occurred, the instructor<br />

would undo the error in additi<strong>on</strong> to preparing<br />

the ph<strong>on</strong>e for the student to complete the<br />

next step in the TA as the student looked<br />

away. Corrective feedback was not provided<br />

during probe trials; however, descriptive verbal<br />

praise was given for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

(during Baseline Pre-Video Modeling as well<br />

as during Pre-Video Modeling c<strong>on</strong>ducted during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. Praise was delivered <strong>on</strong> a<br />

variable ratio schedule after the average of<br />

every third correct resp<strong>on</strong>se (VR-3). Once students<br />

reached criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a task, the reinforcement<br />

schedule was thinned to every fifth<br />

step for all target tasks (FR-5). Under the FR-5<br />

schedule of reinforcement, a student received<br />

descriptive verbal praise <strong>on</strong>ce they completed<br />

the entire target task. After three stable data<br />

points had been collected across three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s across a minimum of two days<br />

for the first task for each participant, Video<br />

Modeling Instructi<strong>on</strong> began. For the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

<strong>and</strong> third tasks, instructi<strong>on</strong> began <strong>on</strong>ly after<br />

the student dem<strong>on</strong>strated 100% independence<br />

<strong>on</strong> the previous task for at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>on</strong>ce a student<br />

completed the Pre-Video Modeling Probe, he<br />

or she moved into Video Modeling for that<br />

skill.<br />

Video Modeling. Video Modeling sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of three parts (Pre-Video Modeling<br />

probe trials, Video-Modeling, <strong>and</strong> Post-Video<br />

Modeling Practice), implemented within <strong>on</strong>e<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 323


sessi<strong>on</strong>. These Pre-Video Modeling trials were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted identical to those c<strong>on</strong>ducted during<br />

the baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> described previously.<br />

However, Post-Video Modeling Practice<br />

trials were c<strong>on</strong>ducted differently. Video<br />

Modeling Instructi<strong>on</strong> began if a student did<br />

not complete the target task with 100% accuracy<br />

during Pre-Video Modeling Probes. After<br />

the probe trial, if students were to receive<br />

video instructi<strong>on</strong>, the computer was placed in<br />

fr<strong>on</strong>t of them. Once their attenti<strong>on</strong> was secured,<br />

they were then instructed to “Watch<br />

this,” by the instructor, as the video was<br />

started. Each of the 3 videos depicted navigati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the iPh<strong>on</strong>e, in first pers<strong>on</strong> perspective,<br />

to take a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>, look at<br />

photographs, or watch a video. Post-Video<br />

Modeling trials Practice took place immediately<br />

following Video-Modeling instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

trials <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted of a single opportunity<br />

practice trial.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

A multiple probe across behaviors, replicated<br />

across participants was used to evaluate a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

relati<strong>on</strong> between the dependent variable<br />

<strong>and</strong> the video-based interventi<strong>on</strong> (Gast &<br />

Ledford, 2010). Experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol was<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated through the replicati<strong>on</strong> of effects<br />

of the video modeling procedure as the<br />

introducti<strong>on</strong> of the independent variable was<br />

staggered based <strong>on</strong> a predetermined criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

across three tasks for each of the three participants<br />

in the study.<br />

Results<br />

Reliability<br />

IOA <strong>and</strong> procedural fidelity data were collected<br />

during 22% of baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

29% of Video Modeling sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During all<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s in which IOA were collected, the<br />

mean percent agreement was 100%. The<br />

mean procedural fidelity was 98.92% (range:<br />

88.9–100%) for all investigator behaviors<br />

across all experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Instances<br />

in which procedural fidelity data were below<br />

100% included the instructor’s lack of adherence<br />

to the pre-determined latency established<br />

between the issuance of the task directi<strong>on</strong><br />

or naturally occurring discriminative<br />

stimulus within the task itself <strong>and</strong> the instructor’s<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> statement.<br />

Effectiveness of a Video Model<br />

Figures 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 3 present data <strong>on</strong> the percentage<br />

of steps completed accurately <strong>and</strong> independently<br />

for Holly, Jake, <strong>and</strong> Norman, respectively,<br />

across all three skills. Norman <strong>and</strong><br />

Jake dem<strong>on</strong>strated 0% independent resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

during baseline probes for all target behaviors<br />

whereas Holly dem<strong>on</strong>strated some<br />

skill with the iPh<strong>on</strong>e based <strong>on</strong> prior experience.<br />

All students ultimately mastered the<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated an ability to generalize<br />

to the more complex discriminati<strong>on</strong>s required<br />

when more ic<strong>on</strong>s were added to the<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e screen. Table 2 shows the types of<br />

errors students made across trials. Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Probes were single opportunity, thus, as<br />

so<strong>on</strong> as an error was emitted, the trial was<br />

finished. An analysis of the types of errors<br />

made, latency, durati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> topographic, was<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted. Types of error emitted by participants<br />

were variable across c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> participants.<br />

Latency <strong>and</strong> topographical errors<br />

were the most frequently made type of mistakes<br />

across all participants <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Holly. Holly’s baseline performance <strong>on</strong><br />

taking a photograph accelerated across the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The decisi<strong>on</strong> was made to begin<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> after the sixth sessi<strong>on</strong> because it<br />

did not appear likely that she was going to<br />

complete step 3 (hitting the camera butt<strong>on</strong>)<br />

or 5 (clicking the camera butt<strong>on</strong>) without instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Her performance <strong>on</strong> the other skills<br />

also accelerated but leveled out at 60% until<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was provided for those skills.<br />

Up<strong>on</strong> introducti<strong>on</strong> of video modeling for taking<br />

a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>, Holly c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to make the error of holding down a<br />

butt<strong>on</strong> for too l<strong>on</strong>g for the first <strong>and</strong> third step<br />

of the TA. As a result of doing this <strong>on</strong> the first<br />

step, the iPh<strong>on</strong>e’s voice comm<strong>and</strong> would initiate.<br />

When doing this with the third step, the<br />

screen of the iPh<strong>on</strong>e would go into edit mode<br />

<strong>and</strong> all of the butt<strong>on</strong>s would become inactive.<br />

As a result, a verbal prompt (e.g., “D<strong>on</strong>’t hold<br />

so l<strong>on</strong>g.”) was added after Holly made the<br />

error during step 1 of the fourth Post-Video<br />

Modeling trial. Following this additi<strong>on</strong>al verbal<br />

model, a mass trial then occurred for step<br />

1 <strong>and</strong> the proper way to touch the butt<strong>on</strong>.<br />

324 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 1. Holly multiple baseline graph. Dotted line throughout graphs indicates a change in c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, from<br />

baseline to video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>. The circle around the data point <strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong> X indicates the<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> of the aforementi<strong>on</strong>ed verbal prompt (e.g., “D<strong>on</strong>’t hold so l<strong>on</strong>g.”), added after Holly<br />

made the error during step 1.<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 325


Figure 2. Jake multiple baseline graph. Dotted line throughout graphs indicates a change in c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, from<br />

baseline to video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

326 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 3. Norman multiple baseline graph. Dotted line throughout graphs indicates a change in c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

from baseline to video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 327


TABLE 2<br />

Number <strong>and</strong> Percentages of Errors across C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

This same verbal prompt was used for the<br />

same error made in step 3. Following the fifth<br />

trial, step 3 was then mass trialed in the same<br />

manner. Holly correctly completed all steps in<br />

the sixth interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong> but then her<br />

performance deteriorated before rebounding<br />

to mastery level performance. Once she<br />

achieved mastery with taking a photograph<br />

<strong>and</strong> her baseline data were stable for the other<br />

two behaviors, interventi<strong>on</strong> began <strong>on</strong> accessing<br />

<strong>and</strong> looking at photographs. Immediately<br />

following interventi<strong>on</strong>, Holly met criteri<strong>on</strong>level<br />

performance. Additi<strong>on</strong>al baseline data<br />

were collected <strong>on</strong> watching videos to c<strong>on</strong>firm<br />

stable performance <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> was then<br />

begun. Similar to her performance <strong>on</strong> looking<br />

at photographs, Holly immediately dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

mastery performance after interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

began.<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> to evaluate performance<br />

with an iPh<strong>on</strong>e screen filled with distracters,<br />

Holly dem<strong>on</strong>strated 100% correct<br />

Baseline Probe Pre-Video Modeling Probe Post-Video Modeling Probe Total<br />

L D T L D T L D T Errors<br />

Holly<br />

Look 0 12 18 0 0 2 0 0 0 32<br />

Take 1 4 8 0 4 8 0 3 4 32<br />

Watch 0 7 23 1 1 5 0 0 2 39<br />

Total Errors 1 23 49 1 5 15 0 3 6 103<br />

Jake<br />

Look 11 0 9 0 0 2 0 0 0 22<br />

Take 28 2 2 4 0 1 0 0 1 38<br />

Watch 18 0 2 15 0 3 3 0 1 42<br />

Total Errors 57 2 13 19 0 6 3 0 2 102<br />

Norman<br />

Look 12 1 2 24 2 3 5 0 0 49<br />

Take 14 3 7 0 0 4 0 0 0 28<br />

Watch 14 1 3 11 2 8 1 0 8 48<br />

Total Errors 40 5 12 35 4 15 6 0 8 125<br />

Total Errors* 98 30 74 55 9 36 9 3 16 330<br />

Mean Percentage**<br />

Look 23.5% 43.3% 39.2% 43.6% 22.2% 19.4% 55.6% 0% 0% 31.2%<br />

Take 43.8% 30% 23% 7.3% 44.4% 36.1% 0% 100% 31.3% 29.7%<br />

Watch 32.7% 26.7% 37.8% 49.1% 33.4% 44.5% 44.4% 0% 68.7% 39.1%<br />

Key: (L) Latency Errors; (D) Durati<strong>on</strong> Errors; (T) Topographical Errors; * Represents total<br />

number of errors across all participants for each error type <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>; ** Represents percentage of error<br />

type across all participants for each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for each target behavior.<br />

performance for taking a photograph, 80%<br />

for looking at photographs, <strong>and</strong> 80% for<br />

watching a video. Her error in the latter two<br />

skills occurred <strong>on</strong> steps unrelated to the additi<strong>on</strong><br />

of the distracter butt<strong>on</strong>s, but instead were<br />

topographical errors that occurred as a result<br />

of holding the butt<strong>on</strong> down for too l<strong>on</strong>g <strong>on</strong><br />

the home screen, resulting in the iPh<strong>on</strong>e going<br />

into edit mode.<br />

Jake. Following a stable baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

across all skills, Jake began video modeling for<br />

watching a video. Up<strong>on</strong> introducti<strong>on</strong> of video<br />

modeling to the behavior of watching a video,<br />

Jake required two interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s before<br />

he began to show improvement. After four<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong>s he achieved mastery <strong>and</strong> during<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> to the iPh<strong>on</strong>e screen with<br />

multiple distracters, he c<strong>on</strong>tinued to show<br />

100% independent performance.<br />

After mastery <strong>on</strong> watching a video <strong>and</strong> a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued stable baseline was reached with<br />

the other skills, interventi<strong>on</strong> began for look-<br />

328 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ing at photographs. Jake <strong>on</strong>ly required a single<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong> before showing criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

level performance. During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

looking at photographs, Jake completed 100%<br />

of the task independently. With mastery met<br />

<strong>on</strong> watching a video <strong>and</strong> a stable baseline for<br />

taking a photograph, video modeling was introduced<br />

<strong>on</strong> this last skill. He quickly dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

100% performance for taking a photograph<br />

but his resp<strong>on</strong>ding deteriorated<br />

markedly <strong>and</strong> required an additi<strong>on</strong>al seven<br />

training sessi<strong>on</strong>s to meet <strong>and</strong> hold criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

level performance. Jake’s decrease in performance<br />

occurred simultaneously with an increase<br />

in agitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> anxious behaviors<br />

(e.g., h<strong>and</strong>-biting), seen across sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

classroom c<strong>on</strong>texts with his special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher. He correctly resp<strong>on</strong>ded with 100%<br />

accuracy during measurement of his generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

to the screen with multiple distracters.<br />

Norman. Norman displayed zero to low<br />

levels of accurate resp<strong>on</strong>ding across skills in<br />

baseline. Interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced first to<br />

looking at photographs. However, in observing<br />

Norman, it became apparent that he had<br />

minimal experience with any piece of h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

technology (e.g., iPod, iPh<strong>on</strong>e, camera,<br />

etc.). As a result, all errors initially made during<br />

baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> trials may have<br />

resulted from his hesitati<strong>on</strong> to touch the<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e (e.g., when <strong>and</strong> if he held it, he held it<br />

very gingerly out of fear of breaking the device).<br />

Up<strong>on</strong> introducti<strong>on</strong> of interventi<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

first task, it was apparent that Norman attended<br />

to the screen when the video models<br />

were playing, yet his performance both prior<br />

to <strong>and</strong> after the video model remained at 0%.<br />

In resp<strong>on</strong>se, a different ph<strong>on</strong>e was taken out<br />

when in the presence of Norman after the<br />

third sessi<strong>on</strong> at 0% <strong>and</strong> it was c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

described to him that he would not break the<br />

cell ph<strong>on</strong>e when he touched it. Norman was<br />

then allowed to punch the butt<strong>on</strong>s when told,<br />

“Do this,” as the instructor hit the butt<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Norman was reminded of this c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong><br />

prior to the beginning of the next instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>. The c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> was repeated<br />

again after the errors c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be durati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> he appeared to still be afraid of<br />

breaking the iPh<strong>on</strong>e while maintaining a 0%<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding level. The sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> occurred<br />

before the seventh instructi<strong>on</strong>al trial.<br />

Results of the seventh trial, 100% independent<br />

during the Pre-Video Modeling, indicated<br />

that he had been attending to the video<br />

model, but had been fearful to break the unknown<br />

piece of technology. He was never instructed<br />

specifically what to do, <strong>on</strong>ly that he<br />

should not be afraid to touch the screen <strong>and</strong><br />

the butt<strong>on</strong>s. Criteri<strong>on</strong> was mastered in 19 sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Norman. During the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> for looking at photographs, Norman<br />

completed 100% of the task independently.<br />

Subsequently, during the sixth video modeling<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> for looking at photographs, the<br />

additi<strong>on</strong> of a verbal prompt (“Go <strong>on</strong>”) was<br />

used to encourage Norman to touch <strong>and</strong> interact<br />

with the ph<strong>on</strong>e. This persisted with his<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d skill, watching a video, but was not<br />

required for the final skill of taking a photograph.<br />

Once criteri<strong>on</strong> was met for looking at<br />

videos, he began interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> watching videos.<br />

The same hesitance was encountered <strong>and</strong><br />

he required the verbal prompt to engage with<br />

the ph<strong>on</strong>e. Following this prompt he required<br />

three more sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach mastery. With a<br />

stable baseline still evident for taking photographs,<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced. He immediately<br />

increased his independent performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> reached 100% independence after<br />

two interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Norman dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

100% independent performance during<br />

the generalizati<strong>on</strong> probe across all tasks.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

All participants mastered all target skills.<br />

Video modeling appeared to be a c<strong>on</strong>tributing<br />

factor to their learning <strong>and</strong> replicated the<br />

findings of Hamm<strong>on</strong>d et al. (2010) with a<br />

more complicated device. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

practice <strong>and</strong> training procedures may have<br />

been resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the students learning to<br />

make discriminati<strong>on</strong>s of target butt<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

more complex arrays. However, these findings<br />

still need to be c<strong>on</strong>sidered in light of certain<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s. While all students acquired<br />

the tasks <strong>and</strong> generalized the skills to more<br />

complex displays these results need to be interpreted<br />

with cauti<strong>on</strong> given the l<strong>on</strong>g latency<br />

to change in participant behavior. Prior to the<br />

commencement of this study, <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e participant,<br />

Holly, had previous experience with an<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e or a pers<strong>on</strong>al cell ph<strong>on</strong>e of any kind.<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 329


Holly’s previous experience with the device,<br />

though limited, left her at an advantage for<br />

accurately guessing which step could be next<br />

during the baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. For example,<br />

<strong>on</strong> the final step of the first task (taking a<br />

photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>), <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e butt<strong>on</strong> was<br />

left <strong>on</strong> the screen which would correctly complete<br />

the task. As a result of limited choices,<br />

Holly would oftentimes look at the ph<strong>on</strong>e for<br />

a couple of sec<strong>on</strong>ds prior to tilting her head<br />

<strong>and</strong> saying a phrase such as, “maybe this <strong>on</strong>e,”<br />

before hitting the correct butt<strong>on</strong>. In essence,<br />

Holly’s guesses <strong>and</strong> successes resulted in her<br />

learning steps of the skill during the baseline<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. These likely occurred as a result of<br />

her prior experience <strong>and</strong> familiarity with the<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e’s mechanisms which the other two<br />

participants lacked. The sec<strong>on</strong>d observed limitati<strong>on</strong><br />

with Holly involved the appearance of<br />

slow effects of the video model for the first<br />

task, taking a photograph of a pers<strong>on</strong>. Despite<br />

the <strong>on</strong>set of interventi<strong>on</strong>, Holly took six sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to hit criteri<strong>on</strong> level <strong>and</strong> 14 to reach<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> as a result of the iPh<strong>on</strong>e’s sensitivity<br />

to a user’s finger pressure for an extended<br />

durati<strong>on</strong>. Holly c<strong>on</strong>sistently made topographical<br />

errors related to holding a butt<strong>on</strong> for too<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g across three steps which resulted in an<br />

error al<strong>on</strong>g with variable rates of data. Oftentimes,<br />

Holly was hitting the correct butt<strong>on</strong> to<br />

complete the target task correctly; however,<br />

her extended pressure <strong>on</strong> the butt<strong>on</strong> resulted<br />

in a topographical error up<strong>on</strong> the iPh<strong>on</strong>e going<br />

into edit or voice comm<strong>and</strong> mode.<br />

Neither Norman nor Jake had ever used a<br />

cell ph<strong>on</strong>e or similar device (e.g., iPod).<br />

Therefore, Norman <strong>and</strong> Jake dem<strong>on</strong>strated a<br />

period of adaptati<strong>on</strong> that should have been<br />

addressed prior to baseline. Their reluctance<br />

to do anything except hold the device may<br />

have artificially suppressed baseline resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

However, <strong>on</strong>ce students began interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> their first target behaviors, behaviors<br />

in baseline remained relatively low (even after<br />

they has been de-sensitized to the device) thus<br />

possibly moderating an adaptati<strong>on</strong> effect suppressing<br />

baseline resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Norman’s performance<br />

climbed slightly as he was now doing<br />

the first step of the task analysis. Similarly,<br />

Jake’s remained at low levels until he was<br />

given interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> those skills. Despite having<br />

to verbally prompt Norman during interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

that it was “okay to use the ph<strong>on</strong>e,”<br />

the sustained low level resp<strong>on</strong>ding during<br />

baseline indicates that any suppressi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

likely minimal <strong>and</strong> should not completely discount<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong>s of interventi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

changes in the dependent variable. This is<br />

especially apparent with the last skill for Norman<br />

where he immediately achieve criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

level performance <strong>on</strong>ce provided interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> did not require any verbal prompting.<br />

Further, the low baseline <strong>and</strong> immediate increases<br />

for Jake’s sec<strong>on</strong>d two skills point to the<br />

impact of video modeling.<br />

While video modeling has been used to<br />

teach a wide range of skills to students with<br />

developmental disabilities (Mechling, 2005),<br />

there are certain aspects of behavior that are<br />

difficult to c<strong>on</strong>vey to a learner via video. In the<br />

case of Holly, she could observe the general<br />

topography of the target resp<strong>on</strong>se (pressing a<br />

butt<strong>on</strong>), she could not see how hard the<br />

model pressed the butt<strong>on</strong>. She may have been<br />

able to count how l<strong>on</strong>g the butt<strong>on</strong> was held<br />

(<strong>on</strong>e of the factors that causes the iPh<strong>on</strong>e to<br />

move to voice c<strong>on</strong>trol) but this would still not<br />

have helped her identify how hard the butt<strong>on</strong><br />

was pressed. Therefore the decisi<strong>on</strong> was made<br />

to incorporate other instructi<strong>on</strong> to help her<br />

learn the correct pressure. Following this instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

at sessi<strong>on</strong>s 13 <strong>and</strong> 14, no further<br />

assistance was required. The small increases in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding for all untrained tasks prior to<br />

video modeling instructi<strong>on</strong> were attributed to<br />

the overlap in steps across the three task analyses.<br />

Aside from these threats to internal validity,<br />

all of history <strong>and</strong> maturati<strong>on</strong> threats<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>trolled for via the time-lagged introducti<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance of<br />

stable, low baselines prior to interventi<strong>on</strong>. Procedural<br />

fidelity <strong>and</strong> IOA were high.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for Practice. The current investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

adds to the extant literature <strong>on</strong> video<br />

modeling as an instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool <strong>and</strong> further<br />

provides evidence related to video modeling<br />

for fine motor tasks with multi-step discriminati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

These practices can be reas<strong>on</strong>ably<br />

adapted to other fine motor recreati<strong>on</strong>al leisure<br />

type skills as well as vocati<strong>on</strong>al (e.g., operating<br />

office equipment) to independent<br />

home living tasks (e.g., operating microwaves,<br />

computers, etc.). The findings also suggest<br />

that some limitati<strong>on</strong>s exist for the types of<br />

skills that educators may use video modeling<br />

to teach. In the case of the current examina-<br />

330 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ti<strong>on</strong>, some aspects of the target behavior could<br />

not adequately be captured in a video to allow<br />

a student to imitate (e.g., pressure <strong>on</strong> a butt<strong>on</strong>).<br />

In these instances, educators will need to<br />

recognize the limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> incorporate<br />

other sound instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods.<br />

References<br />

Abner, G. H. & Lahm, E. A. (2002). Implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of assistive technology with students who are visually<br />

impaired: Teachers’ readiness. Journal of Visual<br />

Impairments & Blindness, 96, 98–105.<br />

Billingsley, F. F., White, O. R., & Muns<strong>on</strong>, R. (1980).<br />

Procedural reliability: A rati<strong>on</strong>ale <strong>and</strong> an example.<br />

Behavioral Assessment, 2, 229–241.<br />

Brookes, N. A. (1998). Models for underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> assistive technology. In D. B.<br />

Gray, I. A. Quatrano, & M. L. Lieberman (Eds.),<br />

Designing <strong>and</strong> using assistive technology. The human<br />

perspective (pp. 3–11). Baltimore: Brookes.<br />

Cooper, J. O., Her<strong>on</strong>, T. E., & Heward, W. L.<br />

(2007). Single- <strong>and</strong> multiple-opportunity method.<br />

In Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed., pp. 440–<br />

441). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Pears<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2002). Enhancing independent task performance<br />

for individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

through use of a h<strong>and</strong>held self-directed visual<br />

<strong>and</strong> audio prompting system. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

37, 209–218.<br />

Elliott, C. D. (2005). The Differential Ability Scales.<br />

In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harris<strong>on</strong> (Eds.), C<strong>on</strong>temporary<br />

intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, <strong>and</strong><br />

issues (2nd ed., pp. 402–424). New York, NY: Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

Gast, D. L., & Ledford, J. (2010). Multiple baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiple probe designs. In Single subject re-<br />

search methodology in behavioral sciences (pp.<br />

276–328). New York, NY: Routledge.<br />

Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D. L., Muething, C. M., Ayres, K. M., &<br />

Gast, D. L. (in review). A systematic replicati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Effectiveness of video modeling to teach students<br />

with ASD to use an iPod.<br />

Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D. L., Whatley, A. D., Ayres, K. M., &<br />

Gast, D. L. (2010). Effectiveness of video modeling<br />

to teach iPod use to students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 525–538.<br />

Mechling, L. (2005). The effect of instructor-created<br />

video programs to teach students with disabilities:<br />

A literature review. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 20, 25–36.<br />

Parette, P., & Scherer, M. J. (2004). Assistive technology<br />

use <strong>and</strong> stigma. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 39, 217–226.<br />

Phillips, B., & Zhao, H. (1993). Predictors of assistive<br />

technology ab<strong>and</strong><strong>on</strong>ment. Assistive Technology,<br />

5, 36–45.<br />

QuickTime. (2011). QuickTime 7.<br />

Sparrow, S., Balla, D., Cicchetti, D., & Doll, E.<br />

(1984). Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales: Interview<br />

editi<strong>on</strong>, survey form manual. Circle Pines, MN:<br />

American Guidance Service.<br />

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for<br />

Children (3rd ed.). San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Pears<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., & Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D. (2010).<br />

Comparative interventi<strong>on</strong> designs. In D.L. Gast<br />

(Ed.), Single subject research methodology in behavioral<br />

sciences (pp. 329–381). New York, NY: Routledge.<br />

Wright, P. W. (2004). The Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Improvement Act of 2004: Overview, explanati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> comparis<strong>on</strong>. Retrieved January 22,<br />

2011, from http://www.wrightslaw.com/idea/<br />

idea.2004.all.pdf<br />

Received: 29 June 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 30 August 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 1 November 2011<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e Usage <strong>and</strong> Students with MOID / 331


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 332–344<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Comparing the Effects of Video Prompting with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Skill Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for Students with<br />

Intellectual Disability<br />

Helen I. Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Joe E. Wheat<strong>on</strong>, Pei-Fang Wu,<br />

Christopher A. Tullis, <strong>and</strong> Ju Hee Park<br />

The Ohio State University<br />

Abstract: This study used an iPod Touch to compare the effects of video prompting with <strong>and</strong> without error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of two daily living skills across three students with moderate to profound<br />

intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> an extremely limited daily living skills repertoire. An adapted alternating treatments<br />

design within a multiple probe across participants design was used to dem<strong>on</strong>strate that the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> from the outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> increased the efficiency of skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for at least <strong>on</strong>e task for two<br />

students. For the third student, some skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> was observed using video prompting both with <strong>and</strong><br />

without error correcti<strong>on</strong>, but more stable resp<strong>on</strong>ding was achieved using in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Many individuals with intellectual disability<br />

experience deficits in daily living skills (e.g.,<br />

Jacobs<strong>on</strong> & Ackerman, 1990; Kraijer, 2000),<br />

which may have negative effects <strong>on</strong> their quality<br />

of life (Parmenter, 1994) <strong>and</strong> limit independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing in their natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

The ability for individuals with<br />

intellectual disability to acquire desired living<br />

<strong>and</strong> working opportunities may be unattainable<br />

until they can independently complete a<br />

variety of daily living skills (Heller, Bigge, &<br />

Allgood, 2005).<br />

One method of teaching daily living skills to<br />

individuals with intellectual disability is the<br />

use of video prompting, which has been used<br />

to teach such skills as cooking (Graves, Collins,<br />

Schuster, & Kleinhart, 2005), shopping<br />

(Van Laarhoven, Johns<strong>on</strong>, Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, Grider, & Grider, 2009), <strong>and</strong> household<br />

chores (e.g., Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al.,<br />

2006; Van Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-<br />

Meyers, 2007). When teaching with video<br />

prompting, a participant watches <strong>on</strong>e step of a<br />

task <strong>and</strong> then has the opportunity to complete<br />

that step before the next task step is shown.<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Helen I. Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, A348<br />

PAES Building, 305 W 17 th Avenue, Columbus, OH<br />

43210. Email: mal<strong>on</strong>e.175@osu.edu<br />

For example, Sigafoos et al. (2005) used video<br />

prompting to teach three adults with developmental<br />

disabilities to use a microwave oven to<br />

make popcorn. Following interventi<strong>on</strong>, two of<br />

the three participants were able to use a microwave<br />

to make popcorn without the video<br />

prompt <strong>and</strong> maintained the skill for at least 10<br />

weeks.<br />

Although video prompting appears to be a<br />

promising instructi<strong>on</strong>al technology, much of<br />

the research <strong>on</strong> video prompting indicates<br />

desktop or laptop computers are used as the<br />

prompting device. A few studies have successfully<br />

investigated the effects of using h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

devices to teach individuals with intellectual<br />

disability, but the small number of such studies<br />

limits the generalizati<strong>on</strong> of their results,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the authors noted that further investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

is required (e.g., Cihak, Kessler, & Alberto,<br />

2008; Davies, Stock, & Wehmeyer,<br />

2002a, 2002b, 2004). For example, Mechling,<br />

Gast, <strong>and</strong> Seid (2009) used a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital<br />

assistant (PDA) with picture, auditory, <strong>and</strong><br />

video prompts to teach cooking skills to students<br />

with autism spectrum disorders. Portable<br />

technologies (e.g., PDAs, iPads, iPod<br />

Touches) have the potential to provide individuals<br />

with a device they can use across their<br />

daily envir<strong>on</strong>ments <strong>and</strong> with a variety of skills.<br />

The increased accessibility provided with por-<br />

332 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


table video aids may lead to greater competence<br />

<strong>and</strong> independence.<br />

In the last several years, researchers in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> have started to investigate using<br />

the iPod Touch as a means of presenting<br />

video prompts. For example, Van Laarhoven<br />

<strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2009) successfully taught<br />

a young man with moderate developmental<br />

disabilities to use an iPod Touch to independently<br />

navigate video prompts in order to<br />

complete a vocati<strong>on</strong>al task at his job site using<br />

video prompting paired with error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

One advantage of using an iPod Touch over<br />

other devices is that they can be loaded with a<br />

wide variety of applicati<strong>on</strong>s across a range of<br />

topics (e.g., educati<strong>on</strong>al, communicati<strong>on</strong>, entertainment),<br />

so individuals with disabilities<br />

could be taught to use the device for numerous<br />

purposes. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, they are portable<br />

<strong>and</strong> are ubiquitous in our society, which keeps<br />

their cost reas<strong>on</strong>able <strong>and</strong> may make them a<br />

feasible purchase for schools.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to exploring the use of portable<br />

devices such as the iPod Touch, <strong>on</strong>e area requiring<br />

further research is the use of error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> during instructi<strong>on</strong> using video<br />

prompting. The purpose of error correcti<strong>on</strong> is<br />

to provide individuals with direct <strong>and</strong> explicit<br />

feedback to prevent future errors from occurring.<br />

Within the video prompting literature,<br />

two error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedures have been<br />

employed (Goods<strong>on</strong>, Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Cannella,<br />

& Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, 2007; Van Laarhoven et al.,<br />

2009). In <strong>on</strong>e procedure, video prompting is<br />

used al<strong>on</strong>e—without error correcti<strong>on</strong>—until<br />

a participant dem<strong>on</strong>strates a plateau in skill<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, at which point error correcti<strong>on</strong> is<br />

introduced. For example, Goods<strong>on</strong> et al.<br />

(2007) attempted to teach four adults with<br />

developmental disabilities to set a table using<br />

video prompting. When <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e adult<br />

learned the skill with video prompting al<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

an error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure was implemented<br />

with the remaining three adults, resulting<br />

in skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. With this procedure,<br />

if an adult made an error, they were told<br />

it was incorrect <strong>and</strong> watched the video a sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

time. If the adult resp<strong>on</strong>ded incorrectly a<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d time, the experimenter completed the<br />

step <strong>and</strong> instructed the adult to watch the<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of the step. This procedure was<br />

repeated with all incorrectly completed steps.<br />

In the sec<strong>on</strong>d error-correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure,<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> is provided from the outset of<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, rather than following a plateau<br />

in skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. For example, Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. (2009) successfully used video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> to teach jobrelated<br />

tasks to a young man with developmental<br />

disabilities. They used similar error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> procedures as those of Goods<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al. (2007), but error correcti<strong>on</strong> was provided<br />

with the participant’s first error after interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was started, rather than waiting for his<br />

performance to plateau.<br />

Although providing error correcti<strong>on</strong> following<br />

a plateau in skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> (Goods<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 2007) <strong>and</strong> providing error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

from the outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> (Van Laarhoven<br />

et al., 2009) both appear to be effective,<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong> remains whether <strong>on</strong>e method<br />

will lead to more efficient skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, using a h<strong>and</strong>held device such as<br />

an iPod Touch to present the video prompts<br />

requires further explorati<strong>on</strong>. As such, the purpose<br />

of this study was to examine the following<br />

research questi<strong>on</strong>s: (a) Can individuals<br />

with moderate to profound intellectual disability<br />

acquire daily living skills using video<br />

prompting presented <strong>on</strong> an iPod Touch? <strong>and</strong><br />

(b) Is video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

provided from the outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> more<br />

effective or efficient at teaching daily living<br />

skills than video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

added when skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> plateaus?<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Three adolescent students with moderate to<br />

profound intellectual disability were selected<br />

for participati<strong>on</strong> because they (a) could perform<br />

few daily living skills (see Vinel<strong>and</strong> ageequivalents<br />

in Table 1), (b) had specific educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

goals related to daily living skills, <strong>and</strong><br />

(c) were recommended by their educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

teams. Participants’ visi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> hearing were<br />

within the normal range.<br />

Matt was a 15-year-old male with a profound<br />

intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> No<strong>on</strong>an syndrome.<br />

He had no systematic form of communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> his teacher reported that he was very passive<br />

(i.e., requiring high levels of physical<br />

prompting to complete tasks), did not attend<br />

well to instructi<strong>on</strong>s or activities, rarely initi-<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 333


TABLE 1<br />

Student Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales<br />

(Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984): Age<br />

Equivalents (presented in years-m<strong>on</strong>ths)<br />

Name<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> Subdomains<br />

Daily<br />

Living<br />

Skills Social<br />

Adaptive<br />

Behavior<br />

Composite<br />

Matt Below 0–1 1–10 1–7 1–9<br />

Ann 1–4 2–7 3–3 2–3<br />

Mark 1–8 3–2 3–6 2–9<br />

ated interacti<strong>on</strong>s with others in his envir<strong>on</strong>ment,<br />

<strong>and</strong> would throw or push items away<br />

with which he did not wish to engage. Matt<br />

could follow <strong>on</strong>e-step instructi<strong>on</strong>s, select between<br />

“more” <strong>and</strong> “finished” when presented<br />

with a two butt<strong>on</strong> switch, hold writing utensils,<br />

throw things in the garbage, feed himself <strong>and</strong><br />

wipe his mouth when finished, <strong>and</strong> request to<br />

use the restroom. In additi<strong>on</strong> to daily living<br />

skills, his Individualized Educati<strong>on</strong> Program<br />

(IEP) goals included navigating to <strong>and</strong> entering<br />

a predetermined locati<strong>on</strong>, selecting the<br />

correct item from an array when instructed by<br />

his teacher, assisting with setting up an activity,<br />

<strong>and</strong> participating in an activity for at least<br />

10 min.<br />

Ann was a 15-year-old female with a moderate<br />

intellectual disability, Trisomy X syndrome,<br />

<strong>and</strong> mild cerebral palsy. She spoke in<br />

two- to three-word utterances <strong>and</strong> engaged in<br />

echolalia. She had poor balance as a result of<br />

the cerebral palsy, but could complete various<br />

gross motor skills. Her teacher reported that<br />

she could follow <strong>on</strong>e-step directi<strong>on</strong>s, select<br />

between “more” <strong>and</strong> “finished” when presented<br />

with a two butt<strong>on</strong> switch, sort <strong>and</strong> verbally<br />

label colors, remain <strong>on</strong> task for more<br />

than 5 min, greet her peers <strong>and</strong> teachers, <strong>and</strong><br />

feed herself. In additi<strong>on</strong> to daily living skills,<br />

her IEP goals included shredding paper, following<br />

simple directi<strong>on</strong>s, sorting at least 24<br />

items, presenting an identificati<strong>on</strong> card when<br />

asked, coloring within the lines of a drawing,<br />

<strong>and</strong> discriminating between two colors.<br />

Mark was a 15-year-old male with a moderate<br />

intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> autism. He spoke<br />

in two- to three-word utterances, which were<br />

often echolalic in nature. His teacher reported<br />

that Mark required several verbal<br />

prompts to remain <strong>on</strong> task <strong>and</strong> would engage<br />

in challenging behavior (e.g., dropping to the<br />

floor, grabbing, pinching, eloping) to gain<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> or access a tangible item. Mark<br />

could turn the pages in a book when it was<br />

read to him, follow <strong>on</strong>e-step instructi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

build with blocks, match colors, feed himself,<br />

wash his h<strong>and</strong>s, dress himself, <strong>and</strong> request to<br />

use the restroom. In additi<strong>on</strong> to daily living<br />

skills, his IEP goals included creating patterns,<br />

following a pattern, <strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong>ing the length<br />

of his verbal utterances to four to five words.<br />

Setting<br />

This study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in an urban selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

school serving students with moderate<br />

to profound intellectual <strong>and</strong> physical<br />

disabilities between the ages of 5 <strong>and</strong> 22. All<br />

three students were served in the same classroom<br />

with five other students. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

table washing were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the school<br />

cafeteria, which c<strong>on</strong>tained large rectangular<br />

tables <strong>and</strong> chairs. Students cleaned the table<br />

closest to the entrance of the cafeteria. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for sweeping were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the students’<br />

classroom, which c<strong>on</strong>tained a large table<br />

with chairs (sitting <strong>on</strong> linoleum) <strong>and</strong> a<br />

rectangular carpet.<br />

Materials<br />

Each student completed two tasks: sweeping<br />

with a manual sweeper (sweeping) <strong>and</strong> table<br />

washing. For the sweeping task, a Bissell manual<br />

sweeper was used, which “swept” debris<br />

into two dust compartments when moved forward<br />

<strong>and</strong> backward. Because this manual<br />

sweeper was used by the classroom staff <strong>on</strong>ly to<br />

clean visible debris—rather than for general<br />

cleaning purposes—small strips of paper (approximately<br />

10 pieces) were placed <strong>on</strong> the<br />

carpet for the students to clean. For table<br />

washing, each student used <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>tainer of<br />

soapy water, <strong>on</strong>e of clean water, <strong>on</strong>e wash<br />

cloth, a roll of paper towels, <strong>and</strong> a garbage<br />

can. All materials for this task—except the<br />

garbage can—were placed <strong>on</strong> a rolling cart<br />

placed next to the table. The garbage can was<br />

placed next to the cart.<br />

A sec<strong>on</strong>d generati<strong>on</strong> Apple iPod Touch was<br />

334 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 2<br />

Task Analyses Including Video Durati<strong>on</strong><br />

Sweeping<br />

used as the prompting device in this study. An<br />

auxiliary speaker (iMainGo 2 H<strong>and</strong>held<br />

Speaker) was c<strong>on</strong>nected to the iPod Touch in<br />

order to increase the volume of the verbal<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Task Selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Video Development<br />

The target tasks were identified by the students’<br />

teachers <strong>and</strong> addressed specific daily<br />

living skill goals in their IEPs. Table washing<br />

<strong>and</strong> sweeping, were judged to be equivalent<br />

because the (a) number of steps needed to<br />

complete each task, (b) average durati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

each clip, <strong>and</strong> (c) gross <strong>and</strong> fine motor movements<br />

in each task were either equivalent or<br />

very nearly so. The tasks did not include any<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> steps.<br />

Because the students needed <strong>on</strong>e-step, simple<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>s, a task analysis was created<br />

Time<br />

(secs) Washing the<br />

Pick up sweeper from corner of room 6 Pick up wash cloth off of cart 6<br />

Bring sweeper to the secti<strong>on</strong> of floor to be swept 9 Place wash cloth in c<strong>on</strong>tainer of soapy<br />

water<br />

7<br />

Put the sweeper <strong>on</strong> the floor in fr<strong>on</strong>t of the pile 9 Take wash cloth out of soapy water 5<br />

Lean h<strong>and</strong>le backward <strong>and</strong> rotate h<strong>and</strong>s 6 Squeeze wash cloth over c<strong>on</strong>tainer 11<br />

Move sweeper backward <strong>and</strong> forward until the<br />

floor is clean<br />

16 Bring wash cloth to the table 4<br />

Walk to garbage can with sweeper 13 Wash table by moving wash cloth back<br />

<strong>and</strong> forth across entire table top<br />

26<br />

Turn sweeper upside down, holding over<br />

garbage can<br />

10 Bring wash cloth back to cart 5<br />

Open bottom tray door 7 Place wash cloth in c<strong>on</strong>tainer of clean<br />

water<br />

4<br />

Shake garbage into can 6 Take wash cloth out of clean water 4<br />

Close bottom tray door 3 Squeeze wash cloth over c<strong>on</strong>tainer 9<br />

Rotate sweeper 7 Place wash cloth back <strong>on</strong> cart 7<br />

Open bottom tray door 7 Go to roll of paper towels 4<br />

Shake garbage into can 6 Rip off two paper towels 13<br />

Close bottom tray door 3 Bring paper towels to table 4<br />

Turn sweeper right side up 7 Dry table by moving paper towels<br />

back <strong>and</strong> forth across entire table<br />

top<br />

15<br />

Put sweeper back in corner 12 Throw away paper towels in garbage<br />

can<br />

7<br />

Time<br />

(secs)<br />

Note: Average clip length was: table washing, 8.2 s (range 4.4–26 s, sum 127 s); sweeping, 8.3 s (range<br />

3.8–16.5 s, sum 131 s).<br />

with the aid of the teachers. For example, the<br />

task of wetting <strong>and</strong> wringing out a washcloth<br />

was broken down into picking up the washcloth,<br />

dipping it into the water, taking it out of<br />

the water, <strong>and</strong> wringing it out. Each of the<br />

final task analyses c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 16 steps (see<br />

Table 2).<br />

The individual steps were filmed using a<br />

digital video camera <strong>and</strong> edited by the first<br />

author using iMovie (Apple Corp., 2009).<br />

Each step was videotaped from the perspective<br />

of a spectator, meaning that the students saw<br />

another pers<strong>on</strong> (i.e., an adult female) completing<br />

each step. At the beginning of each<br />

video clip, a verbal prompt was included<br />

that stated what the student was to do. For<br />

example, the video for the first step of<br />

sweeping started with the directi<strong>on</strong>, “First,<br />

pick up the sweeper from the corner of the<br />

room.” This instructi<strong>on</strong> was immediately fol-<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 335


lowed by the adult female model completing<br />

that step.<br />

Once the videos were created <strong>and</strong> edited,<br />

they were uploaded to our developmental<br />

server so they could be added to the iPod<br />

Touch using the sync feature in iTunes. The<br />

video clips were then accessible using the<br />

video applicati<strong>on</strong> already in the iPod Touch.<br />

Dependent Variables<br />

There were three dependent measures. The<br />

primary dependent measure was the percentage<br />

of steps completed correctly for each task.<br />

During baseline, a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se was defined<br />

as completi<strong>on</strong> of each step within 30 s of<br />

the initial instructi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., “Wash the table.”)<br />

or within 30 s of the completi<strong>on</strong> of the previous<br />

step. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

was defined as completi<strong>on</strong> of a step<br />

within 30 s (or 60 s) of the end of the video<br />

prompt. Two steps (i.e., “wash the entire table”<br />

<strong>and</strong> “sweep the carpet until clean”) took<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger than 30 s to complete, so a correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se was scored if those steps were completed<br />

within 60 s.<br />

Two sec<strong>on</strong>dary dependent measures included<br />

(a) the percentage of steps requiring<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> (b) the number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

required to reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. The percentage<br />

of steps requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

was defined as the number of steps per sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong> divided by the total<br />

number of steps, then multiplied by 100. Mastery<br />

criteria were set at three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with 100% correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding for each<br />

task, <strong>and</strong> the number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach<br />

mastery was counted.<br />

Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

Data were collected <strong>on</strong> the percentage of steps<br />

completed correctly <strong>and</strong> the percentage of<br />

steps requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a sessi<strong>on</strong>by-sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

basis. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted individually<br />

with each student <strong>and</strong> schedules were<br />

determined by their daily activities. Each student<br />

received instructi<strong>on</strong> at least three, with a<br />

maximum of eight, times per week. Typically,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted per day.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

This study used an adapted alternating treatments<br />

design (Wolery, Bailey, & Sugai, 1988)<br />

within a multiple-probe across participants design<br />

(Horner & Baer, 1978) to dem<strong>on</strong>strate a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong> between the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> subsequent changes in behavior. The<br />

adapted alternating treatments design was<br />

used to examine the effects of the two independent<br />

variables (i.e., video prompting with<br />

or without error correcti<strong>on</strong>) <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the two skills. The multiple-probe<br />

across participants design was used to replicate<br />

the findings across multiple participants.<br />

Interobserver Agreement <strong>and</strong> Procedural Integrity<br />

Interobserver agreement (IOA) data were collected<br />

by independent observers during 30%,<br />

25%, <strong>and</strong> 32% of all phases for Matt, Ann, <strong>and</strong><br />

Mark, respectively. Independent observers<br />

were trained by the first author, who explained<br />

the task analysis for each task, provided<br />

the observer with data sheets <strong>and</strong> explained<br />

how to complete them, <strong>and</strong> provided<br />

examples of correct <strong>and</strong> incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

IOA was calculated <strong>on</strong> a sessi<strong>on</strong>-by-sessi<strong>on</strong> basis<br />

by dividing the number of agreements by<br />

the number of agreements plus disagreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiplying by 100. IOA was 99% (range<br />

94–100%) for Matt, 99% (range 90–100%)<br />

for Ann, <strong>and</strong> 99% (range 94–100%) for Mark.<br />

Procedural integrity data were collected by<br />

independent observers during an average of<br />

29% (range 25–32%) of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s for all<br />

students across all phases. The procedures for<br />

each sessi<strong>on</strong> were listed sequentially <strong>on</strong> a<br />

checklist <strong>and</strong> a sec<strong>on</strong>dary observer marked off<br />

which steps were completed correctly or incorrectly.<br />

Procedural integrity was calculated by<br />

dividing the number of steps completed correctly<br />

by the total number of steps <strong>and</strong> multiplying<br />

by 100. Mean procedural integrity was<br />

99% (range 90–100%) for all students.<br />

Procedure<br />

Baseline. During baseline, students were<br />

individually brought to the area where the<br />

task was to be performed <strong>and</strong> told to complete<br />

the task. For example, a student was taken to<br />

the table to be washed <strong>and</strong> told to “Wash the<br />

336 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


table.” During the sessi<strong>on</strong>, the trainer recorded<br />

the number of steps the student completed<br />

correctly using single opportunity resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

probes (Snell & Brown, 2006). If the<br />

student did not initiate the first step of the<br />

task within 30 s or complete subsequent steps<br />

within 30 s of a previous step, the sessi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

terminated. At the end of each sessi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

student was given n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent access to a<br />

choice of reinforcer.<br />

Video prompting without error correcti<strong>on</strong> (sweeping).<br />

Students completed the sweeping task<br />

using video prompting without error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

When using video prompting, the iPod<br />

Touch was held by the trainer so as to be both<br />

easily operated by the trainer <strong>and</strong> easily<br />

viewed by the student. The students did not<br />

operate the iPod Touch. Once the student was<br />

attending to the screen, the trainer said:<br />

“Watch this.” The trainer then started the first<br />

video clip. When the video clip ended, the<br />

trainer said: “Now you do it.” If the student<br />

failed to correctly complete the step within 30<br />

(or 60) s, the trainer completed—or corrected—the<br />

step as unobtrusively as possible <strong>and</strong><br />

proceeded to show the next clip using the<br />

same procedures as the first step. Students<br />

were <strong>on</strong>ly expected to complete <strong>on</strong>e step at a<br />

time. No additi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong>s, feedback, or<br />

prompts were delivered. At the completi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

each sessi<strong>on</strong>, the students were given n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent<br />

access to a choice of reinforcer.<br />

Video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> (table washing).<br />

Students completed the table washing<br />

task using video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, procedures were identical<br />

to the video prompting without error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> except error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

procedures were implemented if the student<br />

began a step incorrectly or did not complete a<br />

step correctly within 30 (or 60) s of watching<br />

the video. Error correcti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted, of interrupting<br />

the student if the step was attempted<br />

incorrectly saying: “Sorry, [name], that’s not<br />

quite right. Watch this.” The video clip was<br />

then shown a sec<strong>on</strong>d time. When the video<br />

clip ended, the trainer said: “Now you do it.”<br />

The student was then given another 30 (or 60)<br />

s to complete the step.<br />

If the sec<strong>on</strong>d viewing of the video clip did<br />

not evoke a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se, three hierarchical<br />

least-to-most prompts were provided. The<br />

first prompt was a model, in which the trainer<br />

instructed the student to: “Watch me,” then<br />

completed the step correctly. If the student<br />

failed to show progress after three sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

using modeling, verbal directi<strong>on</strong> was added,<br />

in which the trainer repeated the auditory<br />

prompt given in the video while modeling the<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. Finally, if the student still<br />

was not performing a particular step after<br />

three sessi<strong>on</strong>s, full physical prompting was<br />

added. When using physical prompting, the<br />

experimenter repeated the auditory prompt<br />

given in the video while physically guiding the<br />

student to complete the step. All error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s were made <strong>on</strong> a step-by-step<br />

basis. In other words, if a student was unsuccessful<br />

with <strong>on</strong>e step in the entire task, <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

that step received the tiered error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> for both<br />

tasks. Once video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

was shown to be more effective, it was<br />

added to the sweeping task. The procedures<br />

used were identical to those in the video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

used for table washing.<br />

In vivo instructi<strong>on</strong>. If the data for video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> plateaued<br />

below mastery levels, in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

implemented. A most-to-least prompting procedure<br />

was used in which a verbal directi<strong>on</strong><br />

was provided for each step, <strong>and</strong> the student<br />

was given 10 s to initiate a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. If<br />

the student did not initiate a resp<strong>on</strong>se or began<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d incorrectly, a physical prompt<br />

was used to complete the step. After three<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s with the physical prompt, this was<br />

faded to a gestural prompt. Finally, the verbal<br />

prompt given to initiate each step was faded as<br />

the student began to chain steps of the task<br />

together.<br />

Results<br />

Matt<br />

Percent correct data for Matt are presented in<br />

the top panel of Figure 1 <strong>and</strong> the percent of<br />

steps with error correcti<strong>on</strong> in the top panel of<br />

Figure 2. During baseline, Matt did not initiate<br />

any steps for either sweeping or table washing.<br />

When video prompting with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> were implemented, performance<br />

with both tasks steadily increased <strong>and</strong><br />

the error correcti<strong>on</strong> prompts used with table<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 337


Figure 1. Percent correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding across sweeping (video prompting <strong>on</strong>ly) <strong>and</strong> table washing (video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong>) for Matt, Ann, <strong>and</strong> Mark.<br />

338 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 2. Percent of steps with error correcti<strong>on</strong> for Matt, Ann, <strong>and</strong> Mark.<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 339


TABLE 3<br />

Number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s needed to reach criteri<strong>on</strong> (i.e., three sessi<strong>on</strong>s with 100% correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding) <strong>and</strong> the<br />

percent of steps completed correctly in the final sessi<strong>on</strong> for each phase.<br />

Student Task<br />

Matt Criteri<strong>on</strong> met (Number<br />

of Sessi<strong>on</strong>s)<br />

Percent Correct in Last<br />

Sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

Ann Criteri<strong>on</strong> met (Number<br />

of Sessi<strong>on</strong>s)<br />

Percent Correct in Last<br />

Sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

Mark Criteri<strong>on</strong> met (Number<br />

of Sessi<strong>on</strong>s)<br />

Percent Correct in Last<br />

Sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

Sweeping Without<br />

Error Correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

washing steadily decreased. In sessi<strong>on</strong> 91, performance<br />

of table washing (using video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong>) c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

to increase, but performance of the sweeping<br />

task (using video prompting without error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong>) plateaued. When comparing<br />

video prompting without error correcti<strong>on</strong> to<br />

video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong>, Matt<br />

performed an average of 41.5% (range 13–<br />

56%) of the sweeping steps correctly with<br />

video prompting al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> 41.4% (range<br />

0–69%) of the table washing steps correctly<br />

with video prompting plus error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

When error correcti<strong>on</strong> was added to the<br />

sweeping task, his performance decreased<br />

slightly, completing an average of 34% (range<br />

19–44%) steps correctly, <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> prompts needed for the table<br />

washing task also increased with this changed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. When both the table washing <strong>and</strong><br />

sweeping tasks were moved to in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

his performance <strong>on</strong> both tasks increased;<br />

he completed an average of 55.6% (range<br />

38–75%) of the sweeping steps correctly <strong>and</strong><br />

an average of 65.9% (range 56–75%) of the<br />

table-washing steps correctly. Matt did not<br />

meet mastery for either task prior to the terminati<strong>on</strong><br />

of this study, as noted in Table 3.<br />

Sweeping With<br />

Error Correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

(additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s)<br />

Ann<br />

Table Washing<br />

With Error<br />

Correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

In Vivo (additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, task)<br />

Not met (46) Not met (17) Not met (61) Not met<br />

(28, Sweeping)<br />

Not met<br />

(28, Washing)<br />

44% 38% 69% Sweeping: 75%<br />

Not met (20) Not met (24) Met (33)<br />

Table Washing: 63%<br />

n/a<br />

56% 75% 100% n/a<br />

Not met (15) Not met (30) Met (32) Met (21, Sweeping)<br />

25% 69% 100% 100%<br />

Percent correct data for Ann are presented in<br />

the sec<strong>on</strong>d panel of Figure 1 <strong>and</strong> the percent<br />

of steps with error correcti<strong>on</strong> in the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

panel of Figure 2. During baseline, of the<br />

steps initiated, Ann completed 25% correctly<br />

for sweeping <strong>and</strong> an average of 16% (range<br />

6–19%) correctly for table washing. When<br />

video prompting was implemented (either<br />

with or without error correcti<strong>on</strong>), both tasks<br />

showed a stable increase, but table washing<br />

(using video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong>)<br />

was completed more successfully than sweeping<br />

(using video prompting without error correcti<strong>on</strong>)<br />

beginning in sessi<strong>on</strong> 15. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

the percentage of steps requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

for table washing steadily decreased over<br />

the course of the interventi<strong>on</strong>. When video<br />

prompting was used without error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

for the sweeping task, Ann performed an average<br />

of 51.8% (range 31–63%) of the steps<br />

correctly. When error correcti<strong>on</strong> was added to<br />

sweeping, she performed an average of 66.2%<br />

(range 50–81%) of the steps correctly <strong>and</strong><br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated a stable decrease in the percentage<br />

of steps requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the first 15 sessi<strong>on</strong>s, though this stabilized af-<br />

340 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ter the 15 th sessi<strong>on</strong>. With table washing (which<br />

always used error correcti<strong>on</strong>), she performed<br />

an average of 81.7% (range 31–100%) of steps<br />

correctly, with the final five sessi<strong>on</strong>s maintained<br />

at 100% correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding. The number<br />

of sessi<strong>on</strong>s Ann needed to reach mastery is<br />

presented in Table 3. Ann did not meet criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

with the sweeping task (though ended<br />

her last sessi<strong>on</strong> completing 75% of the steps<br />

correctly) <strong>and</strong> met mastery with table washing<br />

in 33 sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Mark<br />

Overall percent correct data for Mark are presented<br />

in the bottom panel of Figure 1 <strong>and</strong><br />

the percent of steps with error correcti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the bottom panel of Figure 2. During baseline,<br />

of the steps he initiated, Mark completed an<br />

average of 0.6% (range 0–6) correctly for<br />

sweeping <strong>and</strong> an average of 1.4% (range<br />

0–13%) correctly for table washing. When the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced, there was a clear<br />

differentiati<strong>on</strong> between video prompting with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without error correcti<strong>on</strong>, with video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> showing a<br />

steeper increase. When video prompting with<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> was introduced with table<br />

washing, Mark completed an average of 66.1%<br />

(range 6–100%) of the steps correctly, <strong>and</strong><br />

completed the final three sessi<strong>on</strong>s with 100%<br />

accuracy. He also had a stable decrease in the<br />

percentage of steps requiring error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>versely, when video prompting without<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> was introduced with<br />

sweeping, Mark completed an average of<br />

17.6% (range 6–25%) of the steps correctly.<br />

When error correcti<strong>on</strong> was added, his performance<br />

improved, but plateaued between 69<br />

<strong>and</strong> 75% (M 57.8%, range 25–75%). The<br />

decrease in the percentage of steps requiring<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> also decreased at a fairly slow<br />

rate. Finally, when in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong> was implemented<br />

with sweeping, he reached mastery<br />

<strong>and</strong> ended this phase with three sessi<strong>on</strong>s at<br />

100% correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding (M 87.8%, range<br />

75–100%).<br />

The number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s Mark needed to<br />

reach mastery is presented in Table 3. Mark<br />

did not meet criteri<strong>on</strong> with the sweeping task<br />

using video prompting with or without error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> across 45 sessi<strong>on</strong>s (ending the<br />

video prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>di-<br />

ti<strong>on</strong> completing 69% of sweeping steps correctly),<br />

but did master the skill with in vivo<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> after an additi<strong>on</strong>al 21 sessi<strong>on</strong>s. He<br />

met mastery with table washing in 32 sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

In this study, an iPod Touch was used to compare<br />

the effects of video prompting with <strong>and</strong><br />

without error correcti<strong>on</strong> across two tasks <strong>and</strong><br />

three individuals with moderate to profound<br />

intellectual disability. All students improved<br />

their performance over baseline levels, but<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly Ann <strong>and</strong> Mark met mastery using video<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> for table<br />

washing. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> was<br />

slightly more efficient when error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

was included with the video prompts from the<br />

outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> for two of the three<br />

students. For Matt, the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

with video prompting had a minimal<br />

effect, <strong>and</strong> in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong> was used to increase<br />

his correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding with both skills.<br />

All three participants attended to the iPod<br />

Touch <strong>and</strong> did not appear to have any trouble<br />

viewing the video prompts <strong>on</strong> the small<br />

screen.<br />

One possible reas<strong>on</strong> that video prompting<br />

without error correcti<strong>on</strong> was less effective for<br />

all three participants was the lack of feedback<br />

<strong>on</strong> the accuracy of their performance. For<br />

example, both Matt <strong>and</strong> Ann c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

made the same error for certain steps in the<br />

sweeping task, yet during the video prompting<br />

without error correcti<strong>on</strong> phase, neither received<br />

any corrective feedback <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuously<br />

practiced this error. It is possible that<br />

the use of the error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedures<br />

would have remedied this problem, by providing<br />

opportunities for students to emit the correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se following the viewing of the<br />

video clip, thus enhancing the stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

the video prompt has <strong>on</strong> correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

(Wordsell et al., 2005).<br />

Video prompting (with <strong>and</strong> without error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong>) was the least effective for Matt.<br />

Anecdotally, it was noted that several of the<br />

steps in each task were physically challenging<br />

for him to complete. For example, he c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

struggled with washing <strong>and</strong> drying the<br />

entire table, squeezing the cloth, sweeping the<br />

carpet until it was clean, turning the sweeper<br />

over, shaking the dirt out of the sweeper, <strong>and</strong><br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 341


opening the tray door <strong>on</strong> the sweeper. All of<br />

the tasks he struggled with were more physically<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> required either high levels<br />

of gross (e.g., wash entire table) or fine<br />

(e.g., open tray door) motor movements. Of<br />

the three students, Matt had the most significant<br />

physical disabilities <strong>and</strong> had an extremely<br />

limited daily living skills repertoire. For individuals<br />

who have intellectual disability, the<br />

types of skills being taught <strong>and</strong> the requirements<br />

of the resp<strong>on</strong>ses affect the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of new skills (Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, 2009).<br />

Thus, it is likely that the skill requirements of<br />

the two tasks influenced his results. On the<br />

other h<strong>and</strong>, although he did not master the<br />

tasks, he made substantial progress.<br />

Although all students made progress with<br />

the sweeping task using video prompting without<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong>, the fact that n<strong>on</strong>e of<br />

them reached criteri<strong>on</strong> with this task, even<br />

after the error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedures were<br />

implemented, suggests that including error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> from the outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> is<br />

more effective. Sigafoos, O’Reilly, <strong>and</strong> Lanci<strong>on</strong>i<br />

(2010) indicate that individuals with significant<br />

disabilities generally take l<strong>on</strong>ger to<br />

learn new skills, suggesting that educators<br />

should focus their energy <strong>on</strong> teaching skills<br />

correctly from the beginning of instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Because error correcti<strong>on</strong> with sweeping was<br />

not added until performance of the target<br />

behavior plateaued, the students were not<br />

able to practice the correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses for the<br />

steps, <strong>and</strong>, in some cases, actually practiced<br />

incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses. One could argue that unlearning<br />

incorrect behavior decreases learning<br />

efficiency, resulting in the need for a more<br />

intensive teaching strategy (i.e., in vivo instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which both Matt <strong>and</strong> Mark needed).<br />

Therefore, using error correcti<strong>on</strong> from the<br />

outset is likely to limit the errors a student<br />

might make, thereby decreasing the likelihood<br />

of practicing incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong><br />

potentially increasing the efficiency of instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Even though results of this study were positive,<br />

there are several limitati<strong>on</strong>s to c<strong>on</strong>sider<br />

when interpreting these data. First, although<br />

efforts were made to identify equivalent tasks,<br />

it appears from the data that sweeping was<br />

more difficult than table washing. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

because we did not counterbalance the<br />

two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., two participants complet-<br />

ing table washing with video prompting with<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> sweeping with video<br />

prompting al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> the third participant<br />

completing table washing with video prompting<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> sweeping with video prompting<br />

with error correcti<strong>on</strong>), there was no additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol in place to address potential<br />

differences in task difficulty. It is possible that<br />

video prompting al<strong>on</strong>e could have led to mastery<br />

with the table washing task. Future research<br />

could address this issue by c<strong>on</strong>ducting<br />

a more thorough equivalency analysis <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

by counterbalancing the c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Related to the first limitati<strong>on</strong>, it was noted<br />

after the study was underway that several of<br />

the steps in the task analyses had been broken<br />

down into comp<strong>on</strong>ents that were too small.<br />

For example, in the table washing task, putting<br />

the cloth into the water, taking the cloth<br />

out of the water, <strong>and</strong> squeezing the cloth were<br />

presented as three separate steps. As we observed<br />

our students completing these steps, it<br />

became clear that these steps would have been<br />

better presented as <strong>on</strong>e step, which would<br />

have been more natural <strong>and</strong> practical. Future<br />

researchers should test their task analyses for<br />

both completeness <strong>and</strong> practicality as this may<br />

impact skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> task equivalence.<br />

A third limitati<strong>on</strong> is the manner in which<br />

the baseline probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted. In this<br />

study, participants were <strong>on</strong>ly given a single<br />

opportunity to resp<strong>on</strong>d correctly (Snell &<br />

Brown, 2006). In other words, as so<strong>on</strong> as they<br />

missed a step or stopped resp<strong>on</strong>ding, that<br />

baseline sessi<strong>on</strong> was terminated. In other studies<br />

examining video prompting, a multiple<br />

opportunity method (Snell & Brown, 2006)<br />

has been used in which steps completed incorrectly<br />

were corrected <strong>and</strong> steps not attempted<br />

were completed by the researcher<br />

while the participant’s view was blocked. Although<br />

using the single opportunity method<br />

may dem<strong>on</strong>strate depressed baseline levels,<br />

when all three students began the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

they engaged in fairly low levels of resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

that increased gradually, suggesting<br />

that even if low, the baseline measures were<br />

not unreas<strong>on</strong>able.<br />

As future researchers explore the use of<br />

video prompting with portable technology, it<br />

will be important to compare the effects<br />

across various devices. For example, although<br />

visi<strong>on</strong> problems were not reported for any of<br />

342 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


our students, it may have been difficult for<br />

them to discern the salient features of the<br />

video prompts <strong>on</strong> the iPod Touch screen. It is<br />

possible that the students would have d<strong>on</strong>e<br />

better if they had been able to watch a larger<br />

video. It might also be possible for individuals<br />

with physical disabilities to manipulate the<br />

slightly larger screen of an iPad rather than<br />

the smaller screen <strong>on</strong> the iPod Touch. As<br />

technology progresses, it will be possible to<br />

use devices that are in between laptop computers<br />

<strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held devices in size, such as<br />

the iPad. Future researchers may want to compare<br />

skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> using an iPod Touch<br />

versus an iPad to determine if acquisiti<strong>on</strong> is<br />

more efficient with <strong>on</strong>e device over the other.<br />

There are several additi<strong>on</strong>al lines of future<br />

research that should be pursued. Although<br />

this study did provide further investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

into the use of video prompting with error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong>, replicating this study with different<br />

tasks <strong>and</strong> students would be useful. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

a comp<strong>on</strong>ent analysis of which aspects<br />

of the error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure were<br />

the most salient will be important to investigate<br />

so that the most efficient procedures may<br />

be implemented. Another line of research<br />

would be to determine if individuals with intellectual<br />

disability could be taught to manipulate<br />

the small computer interfaces, such as<br />

the iPod Touch, independently.<br />

In summary, this study compared the use of<br />

video prompting with <strong>and</strong> without error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

using the iPod Touch <strong>and</strong> found that<br />

the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of error correcti<strong>on</strong> from the<br />

outset of interventi<strong>on</strong> was beneficial for two of<br />

the three students. The data also present another<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> that video prompting is<br />

an effective technology for teaching new skills<br />

to individuals with moderate to profound intellectual<br />

disability.<br />

References<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, H. I., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M.,<br />

de la Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.<br />

(2006). Comparing video prompting to video<br />

modeling for teaching daily living skills to six<br />

adults with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 344–<br />

356.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. A. (2008). Use<br />

of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system to transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

independently through vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks for students<br />

with mdoerate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 102–110.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2002a).<br />

Enhancing independent task performance for individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> through use of<br />

a h<strong>and</strong>held self-directed visual <strong>and</strong> audoo<br />

prompting system. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37,<br />

209–218.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2002b).<br />

Enhancing independent time-management skills<br />

of individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> using a<br />

palmtop pers<strong>on</strong>al computer. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

40, 358–365.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2004).<br />

A palmtop computer-based intelligent aid for individuals<br />

with intellectual disabilities to increase<br />

independent decisi<strong>on</strong> making. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice<br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 28, 182–193.<br />

Goods<strong>on</strong>, J., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H.,<br />

& Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2007). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a videobased<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure for teaching<br />

table setting to four adults with developmental<br />

disabilities. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

28, 458–467.<br />

Graves, T. B., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., &<br />

Kleinert, H. (2005). Using video prompting to<br />

teach cooking skills to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with<br />

moderate disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 34–46.<br />

Heller, K. W., Bigge, J. L., & Allgood, P. (2005).<br />

Adaptati<strong>on</strong>s for pers<strong>on</strong>al independence. In S. J.<br />

Best, K. W. Heller, & J. L. Bigge, Teaching individuals<br />

with physical or multiple disabilities (pp. 309–<br />

338). Columbus, OH: Prentice Hall.<br />

Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe<br />

technique: A variati<strong>on</strong> of the multiple baseline.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189–196.<br />

iMovie (2009) [Computer software]. Cupertino,<br />

CA: Apple Corp.<br />

Jacobs<strong>on</strong>, J. W., & Ackerman, L. J. (1990). Differences<br />

in adaptive functi<strong>on</strong>ing am<strong>on</strong>g people with<br />

autism or mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 20, 205–219.<br />

Kraijer, D. (2000). Review of adaptive behavior studies<br />

in mentally retarded pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism/<br />

pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 30, 39–<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Seid, N. H. (2009).<br />

Using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant to increase independent<br />

task completi<strong>on</strong> by students with autism<br />

spectrum disorder. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disorders, 39, 1420–1434.<br />

Mechling, L. C., & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, M. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of the effects of static picture <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting <strong>on</strong> completi<strong>on</strong> of cooking related<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Error Correcti<strong>on</strong> / 343


tasks by students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 17, 103–116.<br />

Parmenter, T. R. (1994). Quality of life as a c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

<strong>and</strong> measurable entity. Social Indicators Research,<br />

33, 9–46.<br />

Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H., Upadhyaya,<br />

M., Edrisinha, C., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., Young, D.<br />

(2005). Computer-presented video prompting for<br />

teaching microwave oven use to three adults with<br />

developmental disabilities. Journal of Behavioral<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 14, 189–201.<br />

Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. (2010).<br />

Mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. In J. Thomas & M. Hersen<br />

(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of clinical psychological competencies<br />

(HCPC): Volume III: Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> treatment for<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents (pp. 1245–1274). New<br />

York: Springer.<br />

Snell, M. E., & Brown, F. (2006). Instructi<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with severe disabilities (6 th ed.). Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Prentice Hall.<br />

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales: Interview Editi<strong>on</strong><br />

Survey Form. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance<br />

Service.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, T., Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009).<br />

The effectiveness of using a video iPod as a<br />

prompting device in employment settings. Journal<br />

of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 119–141.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., & Van Laarhoven-Myers, T.<br />

(2007). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of three video-based instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedures for teaching daily living skills to<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 365–<br />

381.<br />

Wolery, M., Bailey, D. B., & Sugai, G. M. (1988).<br />

Effective teaching: Principles <strong>and</strong> procedures of applied<br />

behavior analysis with excepti<strong>on</strong>al learners. Needham,<br />

MA: Allyn & Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Worsdell, A. S., Iwata, B. A., Dozier, C. L., Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

A. D., Neidert, P. L., & Thomas<strong>on</strong>, J. L. (2005).<br />

Analysis of resp<strong>on</strong>se repetiti<strong>on</strong> as an error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy during sight-word reading. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 511–527.<br />

Received: 23 August 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 28 October 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 18 December 2011<br />

344 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 345–358<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Cognitive Strategy Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical<br />

Skill: Effects for Young Adults with Intellectual Disability<br />

Youjia Hua, Benjamin S. T. Morgan, Erica R. Kaldenberg, <strong>and</strong> Minkowan Goo<br />

The University of Iowa<br />

Abstract: This study assessed the effectiveness of a three-step cognitive strategy (TIP) for calculating tip <strong>and</strong><br />

total bill for young adults with intellectual disability. In the c<strong>on</strong>text of pre- <strong>and</strong> posttest n<strong>on</strong>equivalent-groups<br />

design, 10 students from a postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> program for individuals with disabilities participated in the<br />

study. A teacher delivered six less<strong>on</strong>s to students in the experimental group using the working instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

model for teaching learning strategies. Results indicate that the experimental group outperformed the comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

group <strong>on</strong> items that assessed the ability to calculate tip <strong>and</strong> total bill. Students from the experimental<br />

group also generalized the procedural knowledge to tasks that required using percent values in different c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

Four of the students from the experimental group maintained the use of the strategy 8 weeks after the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, the ability to c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>on</strong>e’s<br />

own life without relying <strong>on</strong> caregivers, is an<br />

important educati<strong>on</strong>al outcome for individuals<br />

with disabilities as they transiti<strong>on</strong> to adult<br />

living (Field & Hoffman, 2002; Wehmeyer,<br />

Agran, & Hughes, 1998). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

often associated with the success achieved in<br />

the areas of living, employment, <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Halpern, 1993). One set of skills that is<br />

necessary to fulfill the expectati<strong>on</strong> of independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing in these areas is financial<br />

aut<strong>on</strong>omy including the ability to earn, budget,<br />

<strong>and</strong> spend m<strong>on</strong>ey (Browder & Grasso,<br />

1999). Unfortunately, individuals with disabilities<br />

often have difficulties with m<strong>on</strong>ey management<br />

skills (Yan, Grasso, Dipipi-Hoy, & Jitendra,<br />

2005). These difficulties, in turn,<br />

affect all aspects of adult life. For example, the<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study (Wagner<br />

et al., 1991) examined the financial management<br />

activities performed by young adults<br />

with learning disabilities who had been out of<br />

the schools for up to two years <strong>and</strong> found that<br />

very few participants had checking accounts<br />

(8.1%), a credit card of their own (8.1%), or<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Youjia Hua, Department of Teaching<br />

<strong>and</strong> Learning, N256 Lindquist Center, The University<br />

of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242. Email: youjiahua@uiowa.edu<br />

other investments (0.4%). The results of the<br />

study indicate that young adults with learning<br />

disabilities are not engaged in some of the<br />

essential m<strong>on</strong>ey management activities that<br />

most adults will need.<br />

Researchers suggest that <strong>on</strong>e important reas<strong>on</strong><br />

why individuals with disabilities have limited<br />

opportunities to reach financial aut<strong>on</strong>omy<br />

is that they lack essential functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mathematical skills (Patt<strong>on</strong>, Cr<strong>on</strong>in, & Bassett,<br />

1998). Mathematical deficiencies are as<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> as difficulties in reading for individuals<br />

with disabilities (Deshler, Ellis, & Lenz,<br />

1996). Researchers found that students with<br />

learning difficulties tend to acquire mathematical<br />

skills at a slower rate <strong>and</strong> have difficulties<br />

with skill retenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Miller & Mercer, 1997). Limited mathematic<br />

literacy for students with learning difficulties<br />

appears in elementary school <strong>and</strong> persists<br />

throughout their adulthood (e.g., Kirby &<br />

Becker, 1988). As a result, social <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

independence of adults with disabilities<br />

is negatively affected (Lerner, 1993). In<br />

order to prepare these individuals for the dem<strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> challenges of adult life, postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

mathematical curriculum should focus <strong>on</strong><br />

the skills that will be used <strong>on</strong> the job, at home,<br />

<strong>and</strong> in the community (Deshler et al., 1996;<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong> et al., 1998).<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to a functi<strong>on</strong>al skills oriented<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 345


mathematics curriculum, postsec<strong>on</strong>dary learners<br />

also need evidence-based instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Analysis of reviews of literature suggests that<br />

young adults with learning difficulties benefit<br />

from teacher-directed explicit instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(Deshler et al., 1996). Explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> is<br />

characterized by beginning the teaching sequence<br />

with a review of the prerequisite skills<br />

followed by teacher modeling <strong>and</strong> guided <strong>and</strong><br />

independent practice (Archer & Hughes,<br />

2010). During instructi<strong>on</strong>, the teacher presents<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> in small steps, uses full range<br />

of examples <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>examples, elicits frequent<br />

student resp<strong>on</strong>ses, <strong>and</strong> provides immediate<br />

feedback (e.g., Miller & Huds<strong>on</strong>, 2007).<br />

The effectiveness of explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with learning difficulties has been<br />

well documented. For example, Swans<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Hoskyn (2001) c<strong>on</strong>ducted a meta-analysis of<br />

180 published interventi<strong>on</strong> studies <strong>and</strong> found<br />

that interventi<strong>on</strong>s that included comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> resulted in larger effect<br />

sizes than those in the comparing c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Likewise, the research synthesis by Baker, Gersten,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lee (2002) indicates that interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

using teacher directed explicit instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

had a positive, moderately str<strong>on</strong>g effect<br />

<strong>on</strong> the mathematics performance of lowachieving<br />

students.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, researchers found that<br />

using explicit step-by-step structured instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

is not sufficient for students with disabilities<br />

when teaching mathematical skills to<br />

solve real-life problems as successful problem<br />

solving requires learners to use effective strategies<br />

that utilize both cognitive <strong>and</strong> metacognitive<br />

process (M<strong>on</strong>tague, 1997; Reid & Lienemann,<br />

2006). Strategies are often defined as a<br />

series of sequenced procedures that allow an<br />

individual to complete a task using the awareness<br />

<strong>and</strong> acti<strong>on</strong> of planning, implementing,<br />

<strong>and</strong> evaluating the process <strong>and</strong> outcome<br />

(Reid & Lienemann, 2006). Unfortunately,<br />

students with disabilities have difficulties selecting<br />

<strong>and</strong> applying appropriate strategies to<br />

solve problems because they have a limited<br />

awareness of the potential usefulness of the<br />

strategies for a given task. Even when they<br />

have an idea of the appropriate strategy to<br />

use, they use it ineffectively because they often<br />

take too much effort to retrieve procedures,<br />

operati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> basic mathematic facts, leav-<br />

TABLE 1<br />

Three-Step TIP Strategy<br />

T: Take a look at the total bill <strong>and</strong> enter it <strong>on</strong> the<br />

calculator.<br />

I: Identify the tip by multiplying the total by 15%.<br />

P: Plus the total <strong>and</strong> find out how much to pay.<br />

ing little cognitive resources to process new<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> (Maccini & Hughes, 1997).<br />

Research in the area of cognitive strategy<br />

suggest that students with learning difficulties<br />

can benefit from strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> focusing<br />

<strong>on</strong> mathematical word problems (e.g., Case,<br />

Harris, & Graham, 1992; M<strong>on</strong>tague, 1992)<br />

<strong>and</strong> computati<strong>on</strong> problems (e.g., Brown &<br />

Frank, 1990; Rivera & Smith, 1988). However,<br />

research in the area of functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical<br />

skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> for postsec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

with disabilities is sparse <strong>and</strong> unable to<br />

guide practice. Recent reviews (Browder &<br />

Grasso, 1999; Yan et al., 2005) <strong>on</strong> teaching<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey skills for students with disabilities highlighted<br />

the needs for additi<strong>on</strong>al research that<br />

include (a) learners at postsec<strong>on</strong>dary level<br />

with academic skills <strong>and</strong> (b) interventi<strong>on</strong>s that<br />

address more complex m<strong>on</strong>ey management<br />

skills that require using mathematical computati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills. While the utility of strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

has yet to be proven in the area of<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> it<br />

seems to be a logical interventi<strong>on</strong> in this domain<br />

for young adults with intellectual disability.<br />

The purpose of this study was to assess the<br />

effectiveness of a three-step strategy (TIP) for<br />

calculating tip <strong>and</strong> total bill for learners with<br />

disabilities in a postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> program.<br />

The TIP strategy includes three essential<br />

steps that can be used to calculate tip <strong>and</strong><br />

total bill in a variety of c<strong>on</strong>texts (e.g., restaurant,<br />

hotel, cab). Table 1 presents the three<br />

steps of the TIP strategy. The mnem<strong>on</strong>ic device<br />

(TIP) may serve as a natural cue to<br />

prompt learners to use the strategy in the<br />

applied setting. The study extends the literature<br />

by examining the utility of strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

as a means for teaching a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mathematical skill to students with disabilities<br />

at postsec<strong>on</strong>dary level. Specifically, we sought<br />

to answer the following research questi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

346 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 2<br />

Participants Demographic Data by Experimental C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

1. Can young adults with disabilities acquire<br />

<strong>and</strong> apply the TIP strategy to calculate tip<br />

<strong>and</strong> total bill?<br />

2. Will the learners generalize the procedures<br />

of the TIP strategy to solve functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mathematical problems involving<br />

percentage values?<br />

Method<br />

Variable Gender Age Ethnicity Disability IQ<br />

Experimental Group<br />

Student One Female 21 Caucasian Severe LD, ADHD 82<br />

Student Two Female 22 Caucasian MR 65<br />

Student Three Male 22 Caucasian ASD 74<br />

Student Four Female 18 Caucasian ASD 77<br />

Student Five Female 18 Caucasian ASD 80<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> Group<br />

Student One Female 24 Caucasian ASD, ADHD 65<br />

Student Two Male 22 Caucasian ASD 72<br />

Student Three Female 20 Caucasian MR, ADHD 63<br />

Student Four Female 21 Caucasian Severe LD 92<br />

Student Five Male 23 Caucasian ASD 69<br />

Note. IQ full scale intelligence quotient scores reported for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children.<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Setting<br />

Participants were students enrolled in two<br />

classes from a post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> program<br />

for young adults with learning <strong>and</strong><br />

intellectual disability at a Midwestern university.<br />

The program provided an integrated<br />

collegiate experience including academic<br />

coursework, career development, student life,<br />

<strong>and</strong> community life. The program academic<br />

coordinator suggested that some students<br />

needed additi<strong>on</strong>al help with functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical<br />

skills in the area of calculating tips<br />

<strong>and</strong> total bill. At the time of the study, all<br />

participants took a life skill class as part of the<br />

program curriculum. The class covered a variety<br />

of skills including social interacti<strong>on</strong>s, domestic<br />

skills, pre-vocati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills, as well as daily living skills. Students did<br />

not receive any instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> how to calculate<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> total bill from the courses offered<br />

by the program before the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

We did not get permissi<strong>on</strong> from the pro-<br />

gram to r<strong>and</strong>omly assign the students to<br />

groups as the two classes had different schedules<br />

<strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al placement throughout the<br />

day. Instead we used a pre- <strong>and</strong> posttest n<strong>on</strong>equivalent-groups<br />

design to examine the effects<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> bill calculati<strong>on</strong> skill of the participants.<br />

To ensure that the participants possessed the<br />

prerequisite skills <strong>and</strong> had difficulties with<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> total bill calculati<strong>on</strong>, we required the<br />

students to meet the following inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria.<br />

First, students were able to complete the<br />

mathematical computati<strong>on</strong> tasks accurately<br />

using a calculator. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, students must<br />

have completed the word problems requiring<br />

them to calculate the tip <strong>and</strong> the total bill<br />

<strong>on</strong> the pretest with accuracy lower than<br />

50%. Five students from <strong>on</strong>e class met the<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria <strong>and</strong> participated in the study<br />

as an experimental group. We also enrolled<br />

five students who met the inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria<br />

from the other class as a comparis<strong>on</strong> group.<br />

Table 2 presents a detailed descripti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

each student, including age, gender, ethnicity,<br />

IQ score, <strong>and</strong> disability category. We c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

an analysis of variance (ANOVA) to<br />

evaluate comparability of the two groups before<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> as this research design<br />

does not c<strong>on</strong>trol for interacti<strong>on</strong>s between subject<br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> other factors including<br />

school history <strong>and</strong> maturati<strong>on</strong> (Campbell &<br />

Stanley, 1966). The ANOVA tests showed that<br />

there were no significant differences between<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 3<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>


TABLE 3<br />

Examples of Types of Problems Used <strong>on</strong> Pretest, Posttest, <strong>and</strong> Maintenance Check<br />

the two groups of students with regard to their<br />

pretest (F [1, 8] .20, p .67), IQ scores (F<br />

[1, 8] .32, p .58), or age (F [1, 8] 2.42,<br />

p .16).<br />

A program instructor who had two years<br />

of experience teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills<br />

delivered the strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> to the<br />

experimental group three times a week<br />

(i.e., M<strong>on</strong>day, Wednesday, <strong>and</strong> Friday) during<br />

the regularly scheduled class time. Each<br />

less<strong>on</strong> lasted for approximately 30–35 minutes.<br />

The total durati<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was two weeks (i.e., six less<strong>on</strong>s). During the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, students in the comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

group attended their regularly scheduled<br />

classes.<br />

Materials<br />

Problem type Example<br />

Target item Mike had a dinner at a restaurant <strong>and</strong> the bill was $35.63. He paid the bill with<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al 15% for the tip.<br />

(a) How much was the tip?<br />

(b) How much did he pay in total?<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Item<br />

Different c<strong>on</strong>text The price for a can of soda is $2.99 <strong>and</strong> the sales tax is 6%.<br />

(a) How much is tax?<br />

(b) How much total will you pay for the soda?<br />

Different operati<strong>on</strong> The book is <strong>on</strong> sale for 20% off. The price for the book you want to buy is $20.00.<br />

(a) How much m<strong>on</strong>ey will you save from the sales?<br />

(b) How much is the book now?<br />

Different value Your bill for your dinner is $127.56 <strong>and</strong> you really enjoyed the service. You want to<br />

give the waiter a 20% tip.<br />

(a) How much is the tip?<br />

(b) How much is the total you will pay?<br />

We developed three equivalent probes for the<br />

pretest, posttest, <strong>and</strong> maintenance check.<br />

Each probe c<strong>on</strong>tained 16 items including<br />

(a) 10 target items that assessed students’<br />

ability to calculate tip <strong>and</strong> total bill in a variety<br />

of situati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., restaurant, delivery, taxi<br />

fare) <strong>and</strong> (b) six generalizati<strong>on</strong> items that<br />

assessed students’ ability to solve problems<br />

using percentages in a different c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

(e.g., calculate sales tax), requiring different<br />

operati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., calculate sales price), or using<br />

different percentage values. Table 3 presents<br />

examples of each type of problem <strong>on</strong> the<br />

three tests. Each target <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

item <strong>on</strong> the probes c<strong>on</strong>tained two questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that were related to the two steps of the problem<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong>. Therefore, the total available<br />

number of problems from the target items was<br />

20 <strong>and</strong> 12 from the generalizati<strong>on</strong> items. The<br />

order of the different types of items <strong>on</strong> the<br />

tests was r<strong>and</strong>omized (Case et al., 1992). We<br />

also developed additi<strong>on</strong>al worksheets that<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly c<strong>on</strong>tained target items for guided <strong>and</strong><br />

independent practice during the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

General Procedure<br />

The instructor of the course delivered the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> to the experimental group using<br />

the scripted less<strong>on</strong> plans developed by the<br />

researcher. Each scripted less<strong>on</strong> plan included<br />

a step-by-step protocol, teacher wording,<br />

examples, student worksheets, <strong>and</strong> overhead<br />

transparencies. Before each less<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

researcher <strong>and</strong> the teacher went over the less<strong>on</strong><br />

plan together <strong>and</strong> simulated the teaching<br />

process using the script.<br />

Each less<strong>on</strong> followed the structure of an<br />

explicit instructi<strong>on</strong> less<strong>on</strong>. For example, the<br />

teacher began each less<strong>on</strong> with a review of the<br />

previous less<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> a descripti<strong>on</strong> of the learning<br />

objectives <strong>and</strong> expectati<strong>on</strong>s. Following the<br />

348 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


opening of the less<strong>on</strong>, the teacher modeled<br />

skills using clear, c<strong>on</strong>sistent <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cise language.<br />

Then the students practiced the skill<br />

with teacher prompts. After the students successfully<br />

completed the tasks, the teacher<br />

gradually faded prompts <strong>and</strong> the students<br />

practiced the skills independently. Before the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the less<strong>on</strong>, the teacher reviewed<br />

the skills <strong>and</strong> gave a preview of the upcoming<br />

less<strong>on</strong>s. The teacher also embedded some critical<br />

presentati<strong>on</strong> techniques throughout the<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> including eliciting frequent unis<strong>on</strong><br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses, m<strong>on</strong>itoring student resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

providing feedback, <strong>and</strong> maintaining appropriate<br />

pace of the less<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

TIP Strategy Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Procedure<br />

The development of the TIP strategy interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was based <strong>on</strong> the features of the working<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al model for teaching learning<br />

strategies described by Deshler <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(1996). We chose this model because it addresses<br />

the learning needs of students with<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> its effectiveness has been well<br />

documented (M<strong>on</strong>tague & Dietz, 2009). The<br />

TIP strategy c<strong>on</strong>tained six instructi<strong>on</strong>al stages<br />

including pretest <strong>and</strong> making commitments,<br />

describing the strategy, modeling the strategy,<br />

verbal elaborati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> rehearsal, c<strong>on</strong>trolled<br />

practice <strong>and</strong> feedback, <strong>and</strong> advanced practice<br />

<strong>and</strong> feedback.<br />

Stage 1. The purpose of this stage was to<br />

(a) review the pretest results, (b) provide a<br />

rati<strong>on</strong>ale for learning the strategy, <strong>and</strong> (c)<br />

obtain commitment from the students to<br />

learn the new strategy. At the beginning of the<br />

less<strong>on</strong>, the teacher communicated the pretest<br />

results to the students <strong>and</strong> compared students’<br />

performances to the mastery criteria.<br />

The teacher also asked the students to identify<br />

the examples <strong>and</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>, in which they<br />

used ineffective tip calculati<strong>on</strong>. The teacher<br />

<strong>and</strong> the students then discussed the purpose<br />

<strong>and</strong> importance of learning a new strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

its relevance to their ability to live independently.<br />

At the end of the first less<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

teacher made a commitment to the students<br />

to teach the strategy effectively <strong>and</strong> prompted<br />

the students to commit to learning the strategy<br />

with effort <strong>and</strong> time.<br />

Stage 2. The purpose of the less<strong>on</strong> at this<br />

stage was to describe the TIP strategy. First,<br />

the teacher described the utility of the strategy.<br />

For example, the teacher <strong>and</strong> the students<br />

discussed where <strong>and</strong> when the TIP strategy<br />

should be used. The teacher also<br />

described situati<strong>on</strong>s in which TIP strategy was<br />

not appropriate. After the initial overview of<br />

the strategy, the teacher described why <strong>and</strong><br />

how each step of the strategy was used while<br />

emphasizing the importance of using selfinstructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to regulate the use of the strategy<br />

(e.g., “What we say to ourselves as we take<br />

each step will help us use the strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

solve the problem.”). At the end of the less<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the teacher discussed how to remember each<br />

step of the strategy by using the acr<strong>on</strong>ym<br />

TIP <strong>and</strong> asked the students to make their own<br />

cue cards c<strong>on</strong>taining each step of the TIP<br />

strategy.<br />

Stage 3. The purpose of this less<strong>on</strong> was for<br />

the teacher to model the use of TIP strategy.<br />

The teacher first dem<strong>on</strong>strated the cognitive<br />

processes <strong>and</strong> acts required to carry out each<br />

step of the strategy while “thinking aloud”<br />

using the following types of statements: (a)<br />

problem definiti<strong>on</strong> (“The problem asks me<br />

how much I tip <strong>and</strong> how much I will pay in<br />

total.”); (b) strategy use (“It says tip. It means<br />

I can use TIP strategy to calculate the tip <strong>and</strong><br />

bill. How do I spell TIP?”); (c) self instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(“Where do I find the total of the bill? Find<br />

the number that says ‘total’.”); <strong>and</strong> (d) self<br />

m<strong>on</strong>itoring (“I am not d<strong>on</strong>e yet. I will need to<br />

figure out how much in total I have to pay.”).<br />

Following initial modeling, the teacher involved<br />

the students to perform each step of<br />

the strategy by eliciting frequent choral resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

The students were prompted to (a)<br />

self-talk using the actual words they would say<br />

to use the strategy themselves (e.g., “The sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

step is the sec<strong>on</strong>d letter of the tip. Every<strong>on</strong>e<br />

all together, what is the sec<strong>on</strong>d letter of<br />

tip?”) <strong>and</strong> (b) carry out the physical act of<br />

each step (e.g., “Yes. The total is ___ <strong>and</strong> you<br />

enter it <strong>on</strong> the calculator. Now look at your<br />

calculator. All together, tell me the number<br />

<strong>on</strong> your calculator”). At the end of the less<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the teacher told the students that in order to<br />

make the strategy work they need to practice<br />

the new strategy <strong>and</strong> complete each step<br />

much faster.<br />

Stage 4. The purpose of the less<strong>on</strong> at this<br />

stage was to ensure that each student understood<br />

<strong>and</strong> memorized each step of the TIP<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 349


strategy. The teacher asked the students to<br />

explain the purpose of the TIP strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

provided examples of when the strategy can<br />

be used. The teacher then checked student<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> by asking them to verbally describe<br />

in their own words what they would do<br />

during each step. Following verbal elaborati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the teacher used “rapid-fire practice” to<br />

promote student memorizati<strong>on</strong> of each step<br />

of the strategy (Deshler et al., 1996). Using<br />

this activity, the teacher asked the students<br />

to name the steps of the TIP strategy in successi<strong>on</strong><br />

individually. As students became<br />

more fluent with the verbalizati<strong>on</strong> of each<br />

step, the teacher gradually increased the difficulty<br />

of the activity by increasing the speed<br />

<strong>and</strong> calling <strong>on</strong> students r<strong>and</strong>omly. The<br />

teacher also gradually erased words from each<br />

step <strong>on</strong> the board until all three steps were<br />

invisible. The teacher reminded the students<br />

to use the letters in TIP to help them remember<br />

the steps. After the majority of the students<br />

memorized the steps, the teacher asked<br />

the students to rehearse the steps with a partner.<br />

The teacher c<strong>on</strong>ducted an individual oral<br />

quiz at the end of the class. To reach the<br />

mastery criteri<strong>on</strong>, the student must have (a)<br />

described the purpose of the TIP strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

(b) named <strong>and</strong> explained each of the three<br />

steps in order without referring to a cue card<br />

or receiving additi<strong>on</strong>al prompts. If a student<br />

did not reach the 100% accuracy criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

the oral quiz, the teacher provided additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

feedback <strong>and</strong> asked the student to return to<br />

the seat <strong>and</strong> practice it with a partner. The<br />

teacher used a verbal practice checklist to<br />

track students’ attempts at passing the oral<br />

quiz.<br />

Stage 5. The purpose of the less<strong>on</strong> was to<br />

ensure that students mastered the use of the<br />

TIP strategy with guided practice. During the<br />

guided practice, the teacher <strong>and</strong> the students<br />

collaboratively completed two problems. Initially,<br />

the teacher told the students to use the<br />

TIP strategy by saying “You see the word tip in<br />

the questi<strong>on</strong>. That will help you remember<br />

the TIP strategy <strong>and</strong> its three steps. Each letter<br />

represents <strong>on</strong>e step. You need to tell yourselves<br />

each step so that you will know how to<br />

find the answers.” At the beginning of the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d problem the teacher <strong>on</strong>ly reminded<br />

the students of the strategy by asking “What<br />

strategy will you use <strong>and</strong> tell yourself what the<br />

first step is <strong>and</strong> complete it.” The teacher also<br />

frequently checked the accuracy of each step<br />

by eliciting choral resp<strong>on</strong>ses (e.g., “Tell me,<br />

what’s the number <strong>on</strong> your calculator?”). After<br />

successful completi<strong>on</strong> of the two problems<br />

collaboratively, the teacher provided the students<br />

with an opportunity to practice the skill<br />

independently. As the students worked <strong>on</strong> the<br />

four problems independently, the teacher<br />

m<strong>on</strong>itored student performance by walking<br />

around the room, provided feedback to the<br />

students, <strong>and</strong> prompted the students to use<br />

think-aloud process. At the end of the class,<br />

the teacher collected <strong>and</strong> graded students’<br />

independent work. All students completed<br />

the four problems <strong>on</strong> the worksheets independently<br />

with a minimum accuracy of 95% or<br />

above.<br />

Stage 6. The purpose of the learning stage<br />

was to provide students with additi<strong>on</strong>al opportunities<br />

to practice the strategy independently.<br />

As the students worked <strong>on</strong> the independent<br />

worksheets c<strong>on</strong>taining 10 problems<br />

requiring calculating tip <strong>and</strong> total bill the<br />

teacher c<strong>on</strong>tinued to m<strong>on</strong>itor student performances<br />

of the tasks. At the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

less<strong>on</strong>, the teacher collected their work <strong>and</strong><br />

praised their performance.<br />

Dependent Variables<br />

The dependent variables were total number of<br />

problems answered correctly <strong>on</strong> the target<br />

items (i.e., items that required calculating<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> total bill) <strong>and</strong> the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

items. We also analyzed the total number of<br />

procedural errors (i.e., no answer, guessing,<br />

<strong>and</strong> wr<strong>on</strong>g operati<strong>on</strong>) students made as identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of mathematical error patterns can<br />

help educators improve instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> student<br />

learning (Case et al., 1992; M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

1992).<br />

Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

Students in both experimental <strong>and</strong> comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

groups took the test before the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(pretest) <strong>and</strong> immediately following the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> (posttest). Students in the experimental<br />

group also took a maintenance test 8<br />

weeks after completi<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

To ensure assessment integrity, the researcher<br />

developed a scripted protocol to administer<br />

350 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 4<br />

Total Number of Items Answered Correctly by Students in the Comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Experimental Group<br />

Variable<br />

Pretest<br />

M(SD)<br />

the tests. At the beginning of each test, the<br />

teacher asked the students to complete the<br />

tasks with the following statement: “This is not<br />

a test or an assignment. It will not count towards<br />

your grades. Try your best to answer<br />

these questi<strong>on</strong>s. If you d<strong>on</strong>’t know how to<br />

answer the questi<strong>on</strong>, make an ‘X’ <strong>on</strong> it <strong>and</strong> go<br />

to the next <strong>on</strong>e. You have 15 minutes to work<br />

<strong>on</strong> these problems. It is okay if you d<strong>on</strong>’t<br />

finish all the questi<strong>on</strong>s in 15 minutes. If you<br />

finish early, raise your h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> I will collect<br />

your worksheets. If you need a calculator,<br />

please raise your h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> I will give you<br />

<strong>on</strong>e.” At the end of the 15 minutes, the<br />

teacher stopped the test <strong>and</strong> collected students’<br />

answer sheets. A graduate student<br />

graded the probes.<br />

Reliability <strong>and</strong> Procedural Integrity<br />

The researcher developed procedural checklists<br />

for individual less<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

scripted less<strong>on</strong> plans. The researcher trained<br />

a graduate student to observe the less<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

check the treatment integrity data using the<br />

procedural checklists. The procedural integrity<br />

was calculated by the total number of steps<br />

completed by the teacher divided by the total<br />

number of available steps <strong>on</strong> the procedural<br />

checklist. We collected treatment integrity<br />

data for all of the interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> it<br />

was 100% across the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. One graduate<br />

student who was not involved in the data collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> was blind to the participants in<br />

both comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> treatment groups c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

the interobserver reliability checks for<br />

all of the probes independently. Interobserver<br />

agreement was calculated by total number of<br />

agreements divided by agreements plus disagreements<br />

multiplied by 100%. Given the<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> Experimental<br />

Posttest<br />

M(SD)<br />

Pretest<br />

M(SD)<br />

objective nature of the answers, the mean<br />

agreement for total number of questi<strong>on</strong>s answered<br />

correctly <strong>on</strong> the target items were 99%<br />

(range between 92% to 100%) <strong>and</strong> 100% <strong>on</strong><br />

the generalizati<strong>on</strong> items.<br />

Results<br />

Posttest<br />

M(SD)<br />

Maintenance<br />

M(SD)<br />

Target .00 (.00) .20 (.45) .40 (.89) 19.60 (.89) 16.80 (6.61)<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> .20 (.45) .40 (.89) .00 (.00) 10.20 (1.30) 7.60 (5.32)<br />

Table 4 presents the means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of total number of questi<strong>on</strong>s answered<br />

correctly by the students <strong>on</strong> the pretest,<br />

posttest, <strong>and</strong> maintenance probe.<br />

Students in the comparis<strong>on</strong> group scored an<br />

average of 0 <strong>on</strong> the target items <strong>on</strong> the pretest<br />

<strong>and</strong> .20 <strong>on</strong> the posttest with a gain score of .20.<br />

With regard to the generalizati<strong>on</strong> items (Table<br />

5), students in the comparis<strong>on</strong> group<br />

scored an average of .20 <strong>on</strong> the pretest <strong>and</strong> .40<br />

<strong>on</strong> the posttest with a gain score of .20. Students<br />

in the experimental group scored an<br />

average of .40 <strong>on</strong> the target items <strong>on</strong> the<br />

pretest <strong>and</strong> 19.60 <strong>on</strong> the posttest with a gain<br />

score of 19.20. With regard to the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

items, students in the experimental<br />

TABLE 5<br />

Analysis of Gain Scores <strong>on</strong> the Target <strong>and</strong><br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Items between the Comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Experimental Group<br />

Variable<br />

Target Items<br />

Gain Score<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

Items Gain<br />

Score<br />

* p .001<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

M (SD)<br />

Experimental<br />

M (SD)<br />

Mean<br />

Difference<br />

.20 (.45) 19.20 (.45) 19.00*<br />

.20 (1.10) 10.20 (1.30) 10.00*<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 351


Figure 1. Total number of procedural errors (represented by bars) <strong>on</strong> the target items committed by students<br />

(number above bars) from experimental <strong>and</strong> comparis<strong>on</strong> group.<br />

group scored an average of 0 <strong>on</strong> the pretest<br />

<strong>and</strong> 10.20 <strong>on</strong> the posttest with a gain score of<br />

10.20. A comparis<strong>on</strong> of the gain scores between<br />

the comparis<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> experimental<br />

group using an ANOVA indicated that the<br />

differences <strong>on</strong> both target (F [1, 8] 1289.29,<br />

p .001) <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> items (F [1, 8] <br />

172.41, p .001) were statistically significant.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong>, students from the experimental<br />

group answered an average of 16.80 target<br />

items <strong>and</strong> 7.60 generalizati<strong>on</strong> items correctly<br />

<strong>on</strong> the maintenance probe.<br />

We also analyzed the types of errors participants<br />

made <strong>on</strong> both target <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

items (see Figure 1 <strong>and</strong> 2). All five students in<br />

the comparis<strong>on</strong> group made procedural errors<br />

<strong>on</strong> the target <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> items,<br />

accounting for 100% of the total errors <strong>on</strong> the<br />

pre- <strong>and</strong> posttest. Similar error patterns occurred<br />

<strong>on</strong> both types of items <strong>on</strong> the pretest<br />

by all five students in the experimental group,<br />

accounting for 98% <strong>and</strong> 100% of the total<br />

errors <strong>on</strong> the target <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> items.<br />

During posttest, students in the experimental<br />

group did not have any procedural errors <strong>on</strong><br />

the target items <strong>and</strong> four of them had a procedural<br />

error <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e of the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

items. In additi<strong>on</strong>, four students from the<br />

experimental group completed the target<br />

items <strong>on</strong> the maintenance probe without mak-<br />

352 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 2. Total number of procedural errors (represented by bars) <strong>on</strong> the generalizati<strong>on</strong> items committed by<br />

students (number above bars) from experimental <strong>and</strong> comparis<strong>on</strong> group.<br />

ing any procedural errors. With regard to<br />

their completi<strong>on</strong> of the generalizati<strong>on</strong> item<br />

<strong>on</strong> the maintenance probe, three students<br />

made errors due to lack of procedural knowledge.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Learning how to calculate tip is a useful skill<br />

for young adults with disabilities. The TIP<br />

strategy addressed a comm<strong>on</strong> but important<br />

problem that young adults with disabilities<br />

were frequently encountering in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

settings. At the time of the study, all of the<br />

participants were living <strong>on</strong> a university campus<br />

independently for the first time. Greater independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> change in their support network<br />

may have presented significant challenges<br />

for these students who lacked necessary<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al m<strong>on</strong>ey management skills in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

settings (Hughes & Smith, 1990).<br />

For example, two of the most frequent ineffective<br />

tipping approaches described by the<br />

students at the beginning stages of strategy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> were guessing <strong>and</strong> giving fixed<br />

amount of m<strong>on</strong>ey as tip regardless of the total<br />

bill (e.g., always gave two dollars as a tip). All<br />

of the students acknowledged that the ineffec-<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 353


tive strategies were impediments to their inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ing in the community<br />

<strong>and</strong> workplace.<br />

Participants’ lack of effective strategies to<br />

solve this type of functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical<br />

problem was verified by the results of the pretest.<br />

Initially, all of the errors committed by<br />

the participants were due to lack of procedural<br />

knowledge; in other words they did not<br />

know the steps involved in calculating tip <strong>and</strong><br />

total bill. The utility of the TIP strategy became<br />

evident as learning the strategy resulted<br />

in improved performance <strong>and</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> in the number of errors due to lack<br />

of an effective strategy (Case et al., 1992; M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

2001). Four of the students from the<br />

experimental group also maintained the use<br />

of strategy 8 weeks after the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

More impressively, students in the experimental<br />

group generalized the procedural knowledge<br />

they gained from the TIP strategy to<br />

tasks that required using percentage values in<br />

different c<strong>on</strong>texts, evidenced by significant reducti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the procedural errors <strong>on</strong> the test<br />

immediately following the interventi<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

three of the students maintained the<br />

skill generalizati<strong>on</strong> after the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Therefore, this study extended the utility of<br />

strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> in the area of functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mathematical skill with postsec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

who had intellectual disability.<br />

Several features of the TIP strategy <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the effectiveness of<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> in the study. First, the c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

<strong>and</strong> the design of the TIP strategy<br />

bridged the gap between the dem<strong>and</strong>s of the<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical skills in the applied<br />

settings <strong>and</strong> the skill deficit of the students<br />

with disabilities. Successful tipping requires<br />

an individual to use an effective strategy that<br />

utilizes both cognitive <strong>and</strong> metacognitive<br />

process. For example, the individual will first<br />

recognize the situati<strong>on</strong> when a tip calculati<strong>on</strong><br />

is necessary, then determine the steps <strong>and</strong><br />

operati<strong>on</strong>s involved in the computati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

complete the procedures while retrieving<br />

<strong>and</strong> applying the knowledge of basic math<br />

facts. Simply teaching young adults with intellectual<br />

disability the three steps involved<br />

in the tip <strong>and</strong> total bill calculati<strong>on</strong> may not<br />

result in acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al use of<br />

the procedures as participants in the study<br />

lacked an effective strategy to guide the exe-<br />

cuti<strong>on</strong> of this process that is highly metacognitive.<br />

For example, error analysis of the pretest<br />

indicated that all of the errors <strong>on</strong> the<br />

pretest were due to lack of procedural knowledge,<br />

suggesting that the participants had difficulties<br />

with selecting task-appropriate strategies<br />

<strong>and</strong> coordinating the procedures. In<br />

order to meet the dem<strong>and</strong>s of the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills, we developed the TIP strategy that addressed<br />

both cognitive <strong>and</strong> metacognitive<br />

learning needs of the learners in the study.<br />

The mnem<strong>on</strong>ic device (TIP) served as a natural<br />

cue to activate learners’ awareness of the<br />

strategy <strong>and</strong> helped the learners store <strong>and</strong><br />

retrieve informati<strong>on</strong> from the l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory.<br />

Each step of the strategy then prompted<br />

learners to take both overt acti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> use<br />

cognitive processes (e.g., problem definiti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

self-instructi<strong>on</strong>, self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring) to facilitate<br />

the executi<strong>on</strong> of the procedures to calculate<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> total bill.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the strategy development model<br />

<strong>and</strong> the instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures we used in<br />

the study also c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the student<br />

gains. We delivered the strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

following the working instructi<strong>on</strong>al model for<br />

teaching learning strategies developed by<br />

Deshler <strong>and</strong> colleagues (1996). This strategy<br />

development model addressed both motivati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> cognitive characteristics of students<br />

with learning difficulties. Given the l<strong>on</strong>g history<br />

of difficulties in the area of mathematics,<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong> is an important factor that c<strong>on</strong>tributes<br />

to the successful acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of new skills<br />

for adults with disabilities (Deshler, Schumaker,<br />

& Lenz, 1984). In order to encourage<br />

the learners to be involved in <strong>and</strong> take ownership<br />

of the strategy, we made learning more<br />

relevant to their pers<strong>on</strong>al goals. At the beginning<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong>, the teacher discussed<br />

the utility <strong>and</strong> benefits of the TIP strategy with<br />

the students. Throughout instructi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

teacher also emphasized that independent<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey management skill was a result of pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

effort in learning <strong>and</strong> the use of the<br />

strategy. In additi<strong>on</strong> to motivati<strong>on</strong>, modeling<br />

was the other critical comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the strategy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (M<strong>on</strong>tague & Dietz, 2009).<br />

During modeling, we emphasized the covert<br />

processes involved in the successful applicati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the TIP strategy using “think aloud.”<br />

Teacher’s verbalizati<strong>on</strong> of the thought processes<br />

gave learners the opportunities to ob-<br />

354 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


serve both cognitive (e.g., self instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

paraphrase) <strong>and</strong> metacognitive processes<br />

(e.g., analyze the task, develop a plan, evaluate<br />

the outcome) required to carry out each step<br />

of the strategy, thus improving learners’<br />

knowledge of the procedure <strong>and</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> for<br />

each individual step of the strategy (Reid &<br />

Lienemann, 2006).<br />

Similar to findings from previous research,<br />

all the students in the experimental group<br />

generalized the procedural knowledge they<br />

gained from the TIP strategy to solve functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

mathematical problems that involved<br />

using percentage values in a different c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

immediately following the interventi<strong>on</strong> (Case<br />

et al., 1992; Hutchins<strong>on</strong>, 1993; Jitendra &<br />

Hoff, 1996). Researchers found that requiring<br />

students to perform the strategy fluently increased<br />

the likelihood that the learners will<br />

successfully generalize the skill to the untaught<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text (Schmidt, Deshler, Schumaker,<br />

& Alley, 1989). When a learner can<br />

perform a strategy with automaticity the individual<br />

will be able to decrease the cognitive<br />

requirement with regard to the details of the<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> instead allocate more cognitive<br />

resources to identify the similarity between<br />

trained <strong>and</strong> novel tasks, thus fostering generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Fuchs & Fuchs, 2003). In the current<br />

study, we provided students with frequent<br />

opportunities to resp<strong>on</strong>d to the tasks that facilitated<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> memorizati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

individual steps <strong>and</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong> of the strategy.<br />

Moreover, the teacher m<strong>on</strong>itored learners’<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> provided feedback to ensure<br />

that learners performed the skills with<br />

speed <strong>and</strong> accuracy before proceeding to the<br />

next stage of learning. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the<br />

results of the study pointed to the necessity of<br />

programming for generalizati<strong>on</strong> in order for<br />

young adults with disabilities to transfer <strong>and</strong><br />

adapt the strategy to solve for problems that<br />

require similar soluti<strong>on</strong>s. For example, four of<br />

the participants from the experimental group<br />

had a procedural error <strong>on</strong> the posttest that<br />

assessed their ability to solve the problem using<br />

similar procedures with different operati<strong>on</strong><br />

(i.e., calculate sales price based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

discount rate). Instead of using subtracti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the participants added the discounted value to<br />

the original price of the product. Their rigid<br />

executi<strong>on</strong> of the strategy without modificati<strong>on</strong><br />

supported research findings that learners with<br />

learning difficulties are less likely to use the<br />

cognitive mechanism appropriately to generalize<br />

<strong>and</strong> adapt the strategies to solve for tasks<br />

that vary in complexity <strong>and</strong> purpose (M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

2008).<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

Results of this study must be interpreted<br />

within the c<strong>on</strong>text of its limitati<strong>on</strong>s. First, we<br />

used a quasi-experimental design in which we<br />

chose students from an intact class as the experimental<br />

group <strong>and</strong> a relatively comparable<br />

group of students from the other class as a<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> sample. Although the pretest differences<br />

<strong>and</strong> participants’ demographics between<br />

the two groups were below the recommended<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-half st<strong>and</strong>ard deviati<strong>on</strong>, it is still<br />

possible that some unknown variables that differentiate<br />

the two groups other than the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

were resp<strong>on</strong>sible for the observed<br />

effects (Gersten, Baker, & Lloyd, 2000). Researchers<br />

also need to determine whether<br />

other individuals with intellectual disability<br />

would benefit from this cognitive strategy as<br />

young adults with intellectual disability have<br />

large discrepancies of needs <strong>and</strong> proficiency<br />

levels in mathematical <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al skills<br />

(Patt<strong>on</strong> et al., 1998). In additi<strong>on</strong>, replicating<br />

studies with small samples allows researchers<br />

to discover the causal relati<strong>on</strong>ship between<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the dependent variables<br />

(Gersten et al., 2000; Gersten, Fuchs, Coyne,<br />

Greenwood, & Innocenti, 2005).<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the results of the study point to the<br />

need of programming for maintenance as<br />

part of the strategy instructi<strong>on</strong>. Although all of<br />

the participants in the experimental group<br />

improved their accuracy of all of the tasks <strong>on</strong><br />

the posttest, the accuracy of target (student 1)<br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> items (student 1 <strong>and</strong> 3) <strong>on</strong><br />

the maintenance probe was much lower than<br />

the posttest, suggesting that the learners did<br />

not maintain the skill of using TIP strategy<br />

over time. Therefore, providing <strong>on</strong>going practice<br />

opportunities that facilitate maintenance<br />

of the skills for learners with disabilities may<br />

be a critical comp<strong>on</strong>ent of the strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to promote l<strong>on</strong>g-term use of the strategy<br />

(Deshler et al., 1996).<br />

Third, we did not assess learners’ ability to<br />

perform the task in natural setting (e.g., cal-<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 355


culate tip <strong>and</strong> total bill after a meal in a restaurant).<br />

Researchers recommend that providing<br />

opportunities to use the strategy in real<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text is a key comp<strong>on</strong>ent to ensure that<br />

learners have mastered functi<strong>on</strong>al use of<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey (Browder, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-Delzell,<br />

Harris, & Wakeman, 2008). Future researchers<br />

need to include both training <strong>and</strong> probing<br />

of functi<strong>on</strong>al skills for generalizati<strong>on</strong> to real<br />

life situati<strong>on</strong>s using simulati<strong>on</strong>s, role play, <strong>and</strong><br />

training in multiple settings. On the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, although we did not directly assess<br />

learners’ ability to perform the task in real life<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s three students from the experimental<br />

group reported that they applied the TIP<br />

strategy in the restaurant <strong>and</strong> calculated the<br />

tip <strong>and</strong> total bill using the calculator from<br />

their cell ph<strong>on</strong>es. As an assistive technology to<br />

compensate for the difficulties with mathematic<br />

computati<strong>on</strong>, the use of cell ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

calculator may be more appropriate for young<br />

adults with disabilities because of its portability,<br />

compatibility, <strong>and</strong> social appropriateness<br />

(Raskind, 1998). With an increasing availability<br />

<strong>and</strong> use of cell ph<strong>on</strong>e, teaching learners to<br />

use a cell ph<strong>on</strong>e calculator may be a logical<br />

extensi<strong>on</strong> of the TIP strategy we implemented<br />

in the study.<br />

Practical Implicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al living skills will prepare<br />

young adults with disabilities to be successful<br />

in daily living, places of employment, <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Alwell & Cobb, 2009). Such instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

will improve their quality of life <strong>and</strong> opportunities<br />

that may result in self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Carter, Lane, Piers<strong>on</strong>, & Stang, 2008). Compared<br />

to the typical durati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> intensity level<br />

of teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills for young adults<br />

with disabilities (i.e., three to four m<strong>on</strong>ths at an<br />

intensity of a few times per week; Alwell & Cobb,<br />

2009), the TIP strategy required <strong>on</strong>ly six sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with a total durati<strong>on</strong> of approximately three<br />

hours. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the teacher expressed satisfacti<strong>on</strong><br />

with the strategy <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures.<br />

He indicated that he wanted to use the<br />

TIP strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> procedures in the upcoming<br />

year.<br />

The results of the study also indicate that<br />

the TIP strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> may be a promising<br />

strategy to teach learners with disabilities<br />

to solve for problems that required using per-<br />

centage values in a variety of c<strong>on</strong>texts. It is<br />

possible that educators can enhance the utility<br />

<strong>and</strong> impact of the TIP strategy by including<br />

procedures that promote the use of strategy<br />

bey<strong>on</strong>d the c<strong>on</strong>text in which it was taught <strong>and</strong><br />

teach learners how to modify the comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of the strategy to meet the new <strong>and</strong> different<br />

task dem<strong>and</strong> (Deshler et al., 1996; Fuchs &<br />

Fuchs, 2003; Maccini, McNaught<strong>on</strong>, & Ruhl,<br />

1999; M<strong>on</strong>tague, 1997; M<strong>on</strong>tague & Dietz,<br />

2009).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />

Previous research has dem<strong>on</strong>strated the effectiveness<br />

of cognitive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

academic areas for students with learning<br />

disabilities. This study extended the research<br />

by utilizing the strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

area of functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical <strong>and</strong> problem<br />

solving skills with postsec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

with severe learning disabilities <strong>and</strong> mild<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Researchers should c<strong>on</strong>tinue<br />

this line of research for young adults<br />

with intellectual disability with a focus <strong>on</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> a more flexible use of the<br />

strategy.<br />

References<br />

Alwell, M., & Cobb, B. (2009). Functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills<br />

curricular interventi<strong>on</strong>s for youth with disabilities:<br />

A systematic review. Career Development for<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Individuals, 32, 82.<br />

Archer, A. L., & Hughes, C. (2010). Explicit instructi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Effective <strong>and</strong> efficient teaching. New York: Guildford<br />

Press.<br />

Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Lee, D. (2002). A synthesis<br />

of empirical research <strong>on</strong> teaching mathematics to<br />

low-achieving students. Elementary School Journal,<br />

103, 51.<br />

Browder, D. M., & Grasso, E. (1999). Teaching<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey skills to individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A research review with practical applicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Remedial & Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 20, 297.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L.,<br />

Harris, A. A., & Wakeman, S. Y. (2008). A metaanalysis<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching matehmatics to students with<br />

significant cognitive disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

74, 407–432.<br />

Brown, D., & Frank, A. R. (1990). Let me do it! --<br />

Self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring in solving arithmetic problems.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> & Treatment of Children, 13, 239–248.<br />

Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimental<br />

356 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


<strong>and</strong> quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago:<br />

R<strong>and</strong> McNally.<br />

Carter, E. W., Lane, K., Piers<strong>on</strong>, M. R., & Stang, K. K.<br />

(2008). Promoting self-determinati<strong>on</strong> for transiti<strong>on</strong>-age<br />

youth: Views of high school general <strong>and</strong><br />

special educators. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 75, 55–70.<br />

Case, L. P., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1992).<br />

Improving the mathematical problem-solving<br />

skills of students with learning disabilities. Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 26, 1–19.<br />

Deshler, D. D., Ellis, A. K., & Lenz, B. K. (1996).<br />

Teaching adolescents with learning disabilities: Strategies<br />

<strong>and</strong> methods. Denver, CO: Love Publishing.<br />

Deshler, D. D., Schumaker, J. B., & Lenz, B. K.<br />

(1984). Academic <strong>and</strong> cognitive interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

LD adolescents: Part I. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

17, 108–117.<br />

Field, S., & Hoffman, A. (2002). Preparing youth to<br />

exercise self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Disability<br />

Policy Studies Special Issue: Self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, 13,<br />

113–118.<br />

Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (2003). Enhancing the<br />

mathematical problem solving of students with<br />

mathematics disabilities. In H. L. Swans<strong>on</strong> (Ed.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of learning disabilities. New York: Guildford<br />

Press.<br />

Gersten, R., Baker, S., & Lloyd, J. W. (2000). Designing<br />

high-quality research in special educati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Group experimental design. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 34, 2–18.<br />

Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Coyne, M., Greenwood, C.,<br />

& Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality indicators for<br />

group experimental <strong>and</strong> quasi-experimental research<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

71, 149.<br />

Halpern, A. S. (1993). Quality of life as a c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

framework for evaluating transiti<strong>on</strong> outcomes. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 59, 486–498.<br />

Hughes, C. A., & Smith, J. O. (1990). Cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

academic performance of college students with<br />

learning disabilities: A synthesis of literature.<br />

Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 66–79.<br />

Hutchins<strong>on</strong>, N. L. (1993). Effects of cognitive strategy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> algebra problems solving of<br />

adolescents with learning disabilities. Learning<br />

Disability Quarterly, 16, 34–63.<br />

Jitendra, A. K., & Hoff, K. (1996). The effects of<br />

schema-based instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the mathematical<br />

word-problem-solving performance of students<br />

with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

29, 422–431.<br />

Kirby, J. R., & Becker, L. D. (1988). Cognitive comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of learning problems in arithmetic. Remedial<br />

& Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 95, 7–16.<br />

Lerner, J. (1993). Learning disabilities: Theories, diagnosis,<br />

<strong>and</strong> teaching strategies (6th ed.). Bost<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifflin.<br />

Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (1997). Mathematics<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for adolescents with learning disabilities.<br />

Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong> Research & Practice,<br />

12, 168–176.<br />

Maccini, P., McNaught<strong>on</strong>, D., & Ruhl, K. L. (1999).<br />

Algebra instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with learning disabilities:<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s from a research review.<br />

Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 113–126.<br />

Miller, S. P., & Huds<strong>on</strong>, P. J. (2007). Using evidence-based<br />

practices to build mathematics competence<br />

related to c<strong>on</strong>ceptual, procedural, <strong>and</strong><br />

declarative knowledge. Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong> Research<br />

& Practice, 22, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>–57.<br />

Miller, S. P., & Mercer, C. D. (1997). Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

aspects of mathematics disabilities. Journal of<br />

Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>–56.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (1992). The effects of cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

metacognitive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the mathematical<br />

problem solving of middle school students<br />

with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 25, 230–248.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (1997). Cognitive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in mathematics for students with learning disabilities.<br />

Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 164–177.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (2001). The effects of cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

metacognitive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the mathematical<br />

problem solving of middle school students<br />

with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 25, 230–248.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (2008). Self-regulati<strong>on</strong> strategies to<br />

improve mathematical problem solving for students<br />

with learning disabilities. Learning Disability<br />

Quarterly, 31, 37–44.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tague, M., & Dietz, S. (2009). Evaluating the<br />

evidence base for cognitive strategy instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> mathematical problem solving. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 75, 285–302.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. P., Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E., & Bassett, D. S. (1998).<br />

Math literacy: The real-life math dem<strong>and</strong>s of<br />

adulthood. In S. A. Vogel & S. Reder (Eds.),<br />

Learning disabilities, literacy, <strong>and</strong> adult educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.<br />

Raskind, M. H. (1998). Literacy for adults with<br />

learning disabilities through assistive technology.<br />

In S. A. Vogel & S. Reder (Eds.), Learning disabilities,<br />

literacy, <strong>and</strong> adult educati<strong>on</strong>. Baltimore, MD:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Reid, R. C., & Lienemann, T. O. (Eds.). (2006).<br />

Strategy instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with learning disabilities.<br />

New York: Guildford Press.<br />

Rivera, D., & Smith, D. D. (1988). Using a dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy to teach midschool students with<br />

learning disabilities how to compute l<strong>on</strong>g divisi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21, 77–81.<br />

Schmidt, J. L., Deshler, D. D., Schumaker, J. B., &<br />

Alley, G. R. (1989). Effects of generalizati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the written language performance of<br />

adolescents with learning disabilities in the main-<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al Mathematical Skill / 357


stream classroom. Journal of Reading, Writing, <strong>and</strong><br />

Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 4, 291–311.<br />

Swans<strong>on</strong>, H. L., & Hoskyn, M. (2001). Instructing<br />

adolescents with learning disabilities: A comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

<strong>and</strong> composite analysis. Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Research & Practice, 16, 109–119.<br />

Wagner, M., Newman, L., D’Amico, R., Jay, E. D.,<br />

Butler-Nalin, P., Marder, C., & Cox, R. (1991).<br />

Youth with disabilities: How are they doing? Menlo<br />

Park, CA: SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (1998).<br />

Teaching self-determinati<strong>on</strong> to students with disabilities.<br />

Baltimore: Brookes.<br />

Yan, P. X., Grasso, E., Dipipi-Hoy, C., & Jitendra, A.<br />

(2005). The effects of purchasing skill Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for individuals with developmental disabilities:<br />

A meta-analysis. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71,<br />

379–400.<br />

Received: 18 May 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 21 July 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 28 September 2011<br />

358 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 359–372<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Increasing Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> for Middle School Students with<br />

Moderate Intellectual Disability <strong>on</strong> Age-Appropriate Texts<br />

Jordan Shurr<br />

Central Michigan University<br />

Teresa Taber-Doughty<br />

Purdue University<br />

Abstract: Students with moderate intellectual disability experience a lack of comparable access to literature as<br />

compared to their n<strong>on</strong>disabled peers (Browder et al., 2009; Kliewer, 1998). Problems in access for many of these<br />

students may be attributed to low expectati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> inadequate support <strong>on</strong> behalf of students as well as a lack<br />

of sufficient literacy skills instructi<strong>on</strong>. Given these issues, the literature students are able to access often is not<br />

representative of their chr<strong>on</strong>ological age. Literacy interventi<strong>on</strong>s such as read-alouds have been successfully used<br />

in special <strong>and</strong> general educati<strong>on</strong> alike to provide students access to literature bey<strong>on</strong>d their present skill level.<br />

Using a multiple-probe design, investigators read typical age-appropriate texts <strong>and</strong> examined the effectiveness of<br />

pairing texts with the picture symbols <strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> in improving student comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Discussi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

implicati<strong>on</strong>s of the findings within this study are included.<br />

Literacy is traditi<strong>on</strong>ally described as the act of<br />

reading, decoding, <strong>and</strong> comprehending language<br />

(Hoover & Gough, 1990). Literacy affords<br />

several social <strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al benefits<br />

which impact an individuals overall quality of<br />

life including perceived competence, c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

over life choices, pers<strong>on</strong>al independence,<br />

tools for organizati<strong>on</strong>, as well as access to c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

for learning, informati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> leisure<br />

(Downing, 2005), <strong>and</strong> has a profound impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> the day-to-day activities of individuals<br />

throughout the life span (Downing, 2005).<br />

Literacy can also serve as a primary means to<br />

access <strong>and</strong> participate in <strong>on</strong>e’s own culture<br />

through activities such as reading a newspaper<br />

to underst<strong>and</strong>ing the issues of importance to a<br />

particular community or reading a popular<br />

novel series (e.g. Harry Potter) to recognize<br />

references <strong>and</strong> commentary made by others<br />

who have read it (Browder et al., 2009; Janks,<br />

2010).<br />

Students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

experience limited access to literature<br />

(Downing, 2005; Ericks<strong>on</strong> & Koppenhaver,<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Jordan Shurr, Department of Counseling<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 321 Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Human Services Building, Central Michigan University,<br />

Mt. Pleasant, MI 48859. Email: shurr.jordan@<br />

gmail.com<br />

1995; Kliewer, Biklen, & Kasa-Hendricks<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2006). Major obstacles for accessing literature<br />

include inadequate literacy instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

materials, low expectati<strong>on</strong>s for success, <strong>and</strong><br />

negative social attitudes toward disability<br />

(Browder et al., 2009; Downing, 2005; Kliewer,<br />

1998; Kliewer et al., 2006). Insufficient access<br />

to literacy supports, such as assistive technology<br />

or reading instructi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> inadequate<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al time have also been cited as access<br />

hindrances. A lack of adequate supports<br />

<strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> to reading instructi<strong>on</strong> result in a<br />

limited pers<strong>on</strong>al interacti<strong>on</strong> with literature for<br />

these students (Downing, 2005). In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disability often experience<br />

difficulties with memory, generalizati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior frequently<br />

make literacy challenging (Browder,<br />

Trela, & Jimenez, 2007; Browder et al., 2009;<br />

Downing, 2005; Kliewer, 1998; Kliewer & L<strong>and</strong>is,<br />

1999; Turnbull, Turnbull, & Wehmeyer,<br />

2010). For older students, difficulty accessing<br />

literature may occur due to the discrepancy<br />

between an individual’s needed supports <strong>and</strong><br />

his or her age. Older students with intellectual<br />

disability, for example, often have support<br />

needs (e.g., representative pictures accompanying<br />

text, simplified text, text repetiti<strong>on</strong>)<br />

more similar to young early readers without<br />

intellectual disability than the support needs<br />

of their same aged peers without disabilities.<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 359


This needs-age discrepancy may limit the availability<br />

of such supports <strong>and</strong> therefore access<br />

to literature (Browder et al., 2009; Downing,<br />

2005). Due to the importance of literacy as it<br />

relates to quality of life (e.g. perceived competence,<br />

independence) in general <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> a<br />

day-to-day basis, students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability should have increased opportunities<br />

for access to literature.<br />

Literacy access for students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability can be described simply<br />

in two parts: (a) access to literature through<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> (b) access to literature through<br />

methods other than reading (Browder et al.,<br />

2009). A substantial <strong>and</strong> growing body of special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> research focuses <strong>on</strong> helping<br />

students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

improve their access to literature through increased<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> reading skills. This includes<br />

research <strong>on</strong> using ph<strong>on</strong>ics to increase<br />

abilities in word decoding <strong>and</strong> fluency (Joseph<br />

& McCachran, 2003; Waugh, Fredrick, &<br />

Alberto, 2009), a meta-analysis of research <strong>on</strong><br />

the effectiveness of sight word instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(Browder & Xin, 1998), as well as use of a<br />

mixed approach to reading instructi<strong>on</strong> (Allor,<br />

Mathes, Roberts, J<strong>on</strong>es, & Champlin, 2010;<br />

Otaiba & Hosp, 2004). Overall, research indicates<br />

that students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability are able to learn reading skills, which<br />

in turn increases their access to literature.<br />

However, especially for middle <strong>and</strong> high<br />

school students with moderate intellectual disability,<br />

reading skills al<strong>on</strong>e often are not sufficient<br />

to access age or grade-level texts. As<br />

students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

age <strong>and</strong> the literature appropriate for their<br />

age increases in complexity <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent, access<br />

to this literature via reading skills often<br />

declines (Browder et al., 2009). Access to typical<br />

age-appropriate texts, the type of literature<br />

accessed by same-aged peers without disabilities,<br />

is important for students with<br />

moderate intellectual disability. In their work<br />

<strong>on</strong> general educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tent, including typical<br />

age-appropriate literature <strong>and</strong> students<br />

with significant intellectual disability, Browder<br />

et al. (2007) detailed four reas<strong>on</strong>s for promoting<br />

access. These include (a) to foster competence<br />

<strong>and</strong> improve the quality of life, (b) promote<br />

high expectati<strong>on</strong>s for students, (c)<br />

provide equitable access to instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (d) to increase opportunities to<br />

exercise self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. When age-appropriate<br />

literature is available for these students,<br />

the benefits include access to the cultural<br />

ic<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> ideas relevant to their age group<br />

(e.g. superhero’s, movie stars), appropriately<br />

mature c<strong>on</strong>tent, (Browder et al., 2009) rich<br />

vocabulary, <strong>and</strong> exposure to advanced literacy<br />

skills (Kluth & Darm<strong>on</strong>dy-Latham, 2003).<br />

Access to literature through methods other<br />

than reading includes listening to stories read<br />

aloud either by way of technology (e.g. book<br />

<strong>on</strong> tape, computer screen reader) or by an<br />

adult or peer, also called a read-aloud. Hearing<br />

text read aloud allows students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability to access the c<strong>on</strong>tent of<br />

literature irrespective of their reading skills.<br />

This allows students to access general ageappropriate<br />

texts that are otherwise out of<br />

reach. Wehmeyer (2006) describes the successful<br />

use of technology to provide access to<br />

the novel Moby Dick to an adult with a significant<br />

intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> limited reading<br />

skills. The participant in Wehmeyer’s research<br />

indicated that this applicati<strong>on</strong> allowed him to<br />

enjoy previously inaccessible literature for leisure.<br />

Several studies successfully used readalouds<br />

to increase access to literature for students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual<br />

disability (Bell<strong>on</strong>, Ogletree, & Harn, 2000;<br />

Browder, Trela, & Jimenez, 2007; Lawh<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Cobb, 2002; Skotko, Koppenhaver, & Ericks<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2004). Browder et al. (2007) found when<br />

teachers read age-appropriate adapted stories<br />

out loud, they were able to cultivate a leisure<br />

interest in literature, improve communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills, <strong>and</strong> increase the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> abilities<br />

of older students with intellectual disability.<br />

In a study of the read-aloud behaviors of<br />

mothers to their daughters with Rett Syndrome,<br />

researchers found an effective <strong>and</strong> appropriate<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text was created to practice communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills as well as comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> skills (Skotko et al., 2004).<br />

Similarly, Bell<strong>on</strong> et al. (2000) found readalouds<br />

increased the sp<strong>on</strong>taneous communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

of a young child with autism. In additi<strong>on</strong><br />

to creating a c<strong>on</strong>text for communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills, read-alouds were also found to provide<br />

opportunities for children to observe <strong>and</strong> interact<br />

with an effective model of literate behavior.<br />

Modeling literate behaviors such as<br />

this was found to be a powerful approach to<br />

teaching reading to young children (Lawh<strong>on</strong><br />

360 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


& Cobb, 2002). Generalizati<strong>on</strong>, the ability to<br />

make sense of or complete a task under unfamiliar<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, was also associated with<br />

read-alouds as text <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts can be presented<br />

in multiple <strong>and</strong> unique ways (Browder<br />

et al., 2007; Bell<strong>on</strong> et al., 2000). In these studies,<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al supports (e.g. picture symbols<br />

<strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong>) were provided to increase student<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> engagement with<br />

the text.<br />

Visual supports such as picture symbols or<br />

photographs accompanying text proved to be<br />

beneficial in increasing access to the c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

of literature for students with intellectual disability<br />

(Browder, Mims, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-<br />

Delzell, & Lee, 2008; Browder et al., 2009;<br />

Light, Roberts, Dimarco, & Greiner, 1998;<br />

Sevcik, Romski, & Wilkins<strong>on</strong>, 1991). Slater<br />

(2002) reported the advantages of using picture<br />

symbols added to texts for both comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> word identificati<strong>on</strong>. Light, Roberts,<br />

Dimarco, Greiner (1998) also reported<br />

positive effects of visual supports for increasing<br />

the receptive communicati<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with autism. In a study <strong>on</strong> low achieving English<br />

language learners, Liu (2004) found carto<strong>on</strong><br />

images added to high-level texts to be a<br />

successful in increasing comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

positive relati<strong>on</strong>ship between using images to<br />

increase literacy comprehensi<strong>on</strong> is echoed in<br />

the general educati<strong>on</strong> literature (Hibbing &<br />

Rankin-Ericks<strong>on</strong>, 2003). A preliminary study<br />

<strong>on</strong> students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

suggested that visual symbols al<strong>on</strong>e are not<br />

sufficient to support student comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of high-level texts read aloud. An additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, text discussi<strong>on</strong>, which involves a<br />

dialogue about certain key elements of a text,<br />

was found to successfully increase comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

(Kucan & Beck, 1997; Moats, 2002). This<br />

may include clarifying questi<strong>on</strong>s, summary of<br />

main events <strong>and</strong> ideas, <strong>and</strong> situati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

text to familiar c<strong>on</strong>cepts for the student. This<br />

present study looks to combine visual supports<br />

<strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> to read-alouds, as an interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to enhance the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> abilities<br />

of students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

<strong>on</strong> typical age-appropriate texts. This research<br />

inquiry attempts to increase the research<br />

base in n<strong>on</strong>-reading access supports for<br />

this populati<strong>on</strong> to typical age-appropriate<br />

texts.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Participants in this study included four middle<br />

school students ranging in age from 12–15<br />

who were served in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom<br />

(less than 60% of their instructi<strong>on</strong>al day) setting<br />

in a suburban junior high school in a<br />

Midwestern state. Participants were nominated<br />

by their teacher based <strong>on</strong> the following<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>: (a) presence of a moderate intellectual<br />

disability, (b) ability to verbalize or physically<br />

identify a choice, (c) a lack of success<br />

with any <strong>on</strong>e specific reading strategy, <strong>and</strong> (d)<br />

a willingness to participate. Five students were<br />

initially identified for participati<strong>on</strong>. Following<br />

nominati<strong>on</strong>, students were assessed <strong>on</strong> their<br />

ability to answer five basic three-opti<strong>on</strong> multiple-choice<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Due to the emphasis <strong>on</strong><br />

multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s as a measure of<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> in this study, 80% accuracy <strong>on</strong><br />

this assessment was required for inclusi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

this study. Four students met the criteria for<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> expressed interest in participating.<br />

Table 1 provides a summary for each<br />

participant.<br />

Sarah. Sarah was a 14-year old, 8 th grade<br />

female identified as having a moderate intellectual<br />

disability <strong>and</strong> a language impairment.<br />

Sarah’s IQ score was 54. Her most recent Individual<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Program (IEP) indicated<br />

she was able to use a large vocabulary of sight<br />

words <strong>and</strong> complete simple reading activities<br />

independently but was typically not able to<br />

read new or unfamiliar words. Sarah spent the<br />

majority of her school day with the classroom<br />

staff <strong>and</strong> students. Weekly activities included<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al academic work in the classroom,<br />

prevocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks within the school <strong>and</strong><br />

community, as well as instructi<strong>on</strong>al outings to<br />

various sites within the community (e.g. grocery<br />

store, library, <strong>and</strong> restaurants). She participated<br />

in some general educati<strong>on</strong> classes<br />

throughout the school week such as choir <strong>and</strong><br />

home ec<strong>on</strong>omics classes. In terms of literacy,<br />

Sarah was exposed to age-appropriate materials<br />

such as magazines <strong>and</strong> an adapted newspaper<br />

(News-2-You ® ), literacy supports such as<br />

books <strong>on</strong> tape <strong>and</strong> picture communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

symbols, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials such as<br />

simple language-oriented reading <strong>and</strong> writing<br />

worksheets. Up<strong>on</strong> arrival to the classroom<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 361


TABLE 1<br />

Participant Characteristics<br />

Student Age Ethnicity IQ Primary Disability Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Disability<br />

Sarah 14 Caucasian 54 a<br />

Moderate ID Language Impairment<br />

Ellen 14 Caucasian 52 b<br />

Moderate ID Language Impairment<br />

William 15 Caucasian 42 c<br />

Deaf, Language Impairment, Moderate ID,<br />

Multiple<br />

Speech Impairment<br />

Louis 12 Caucasian * Moderate ID Language Impairment<br />

a WISC-IV b SB5 c UNIT<br />

* IQ score not available<br />

each day, Sarah was observed working independently<br />

<strong>on</strong> academic worksheets or actively<br />

participating in group activities. Her pers<strong>on</strong>ality<br />

could be described as bubbly as she spoke<br />

freely through her comments <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Ellen. Ellen was a 14-year old, 7 th grade<br />

female identified with a moderate intellectual<br />

disability (IQ 52) <strong>and</strong> a language impairment.<br />

Ellen’s most recent IEP noted she was<br />

able to read some basic elementary level sight<br />

words but experienced difficulty identifying<br />

words she had not memorized. Most of Ellen’s<br />

school day was spent with the classroom staff<br />

<strong>and</strong> students. She engaged in functi<strong>on</strong>al activities<br />

<strong>on</strong> a weekly basis including math <strong>and</strong><br />

reading in the classroom, prevocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks<br />

within the school <strong>and</strong> community, as well as<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al outings to various sites within the<br />

community (e.g. grocery store, library, <strong>and</strong><br />

restaurants). Ellen participated weekly in<br />

some general educati<strong>on</strong> classes such as choir.<br />

In terms of literacy, Ellen was exposed to ageappropriate<br />

materials such as magazines <strong>and</strong><br />

an adapted newspaper (News-2-You ® ), literacy<br />

supports such as books <strong>on</strong> tape <strong>and</strong> picture<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> symbols, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials such as simple language-oriented<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> writing worksheets. Ellen was a<br />

relatively quiet <strong>and</strong> shy young lady but was<br />

observed to be both willing <strong>and</strong> eager to work<br />

when asked.<br />

William. William was a 15-year old male in<br />

the eighth grade. He received special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

services for students with multiple disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> was identified as having a moderate<br />

intellectual disability (Stanford-Binet IQ <br />

42), experienced a hearing disability, <strong>and</strong><br />

speech <strong>and</strong> language disabilities. William’s<br />

most recent IEP indicated he was able to in-<br />

dependently read some elementary level sight<br />

words. Verbal communicati<strong>on</strong> was very limited<br />

for William. He used a speech-generating device<br />

(Dynavox), as well as gestures <strong>and</strong> a limited<br />

verbal vocabulary (yes/no) to communicate.<br />

The majority of William’s day was spent<br />

with the classroom staff <strong>and</strong> students engaged<br />

in functi<strong>on</strong>al activities such as academics in<br />

the classroom, prevocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks within the<br />

school <strong>and</strong> community, as well as instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

outings to various sites within the community<br />

(e.g. grocery store, library, <strong>and</strong> restaurants).<br />

In terms of literacy, William was exposed to<br />

age-appropriate materials such as magazines<br />

<strong>and</strong> an adapted newspaper (News-2-You ® ), literacy<br />

supports such as books <strong>on</strong> tape <strong>and</strong><br />

picture communicati<strong>on</strong> symbols, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials such as simple language-oriented<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> writing worksheets. Although<br />

not always clear, William was very<br />

reliant <strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> fairly efficient in navigating<br />

<strong>and</strong> using his speech generating device to<br />

communicate in single word utterances typically<br />

to answer a questi<strong>on</strong> or make a comment.<br />

Although relatively quiet, William dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

a good sense of humor <strong>and</strong> was willing<br />

<strong>and</strong> eager to participate in school activities<br />

when requested.<br />

Louis. Louis was a 12-year old 7th grade<br />

male diagnosed with Down syndrome. His disability<br />

labels included a moderate intellectual<br />

disability <strong>and</strong> language impairment. Louis has<br />

recently lived in three different states over the<br />

past four years thus his official IQ score <strong>and</strong><br />

school records were unavailable. His most recent<br />

IEP indicated that he was able to read<br />

<strong>and</strong> comprehend basic elementary sight<br />

words. Louis spent most of his school day with<br />

the classroom staff <strong>and</strong> students in functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

362 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


academic activities in the classroom, prevocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

tasks within the school <strong>and</strong> community,<br />

as well as instructi<strong>on</strong>al outings to various sites<br />

within the community (e.g. grocery store, library,<br />

<strong>and</strong> restaurants). Louis enjoyed participating<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> classes <strong>on</strong> a<br />

weekly basis including choir. In terms of literacy,<br />

Louis was exposed to age-appropriate materials<br />

such as magazines <strong>and</strong> an adapted<br />

newspaper (News-2-You ® ), literacy supports<br />

such as books <strong>on</strong> tape <strong>and</strong> picture communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

symbols, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials<br />

such as simple language-oriented reading <strong>and</strong><br />

writing worksheets. Despite his recent move to<br />

this school from out of state, Louis was very<br />

outgoing <strong>and</strong> amiable with the adults <strong>and</strong> his<br />

peers in the school. Although sometimes difficult<br />

to underst<strong>and</strong>, he readily spoke with<br />

classroom visitors <strong>and</strong> friends. Overall, Louis<br />

was observed to be willing <strong>and</strong> eager to comply<br />

with work requests, he occasi<strong>on</strong>ally needed<br />

to be reminded or redirected in order to complete<br />

tasks.<br />

Setting<br />

Study activities were c<strong>on</strong>ducted at a large table<br />

in the students’ self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom.<br />

within the local seventh <strong>and</strong> eighth grade junior<br />

high school. Participants sat across from<br />

the first author at a table located in the back<br />

of the classroom. The classroom included a<br />

small kitchen near the entrance, a bathroom,<br />

ten student desks arranged in rows facing an<br />

interactive whiteboard, a teacher desk, a small<br />

computer lab with three computers <strong>on</strong> a side<br />

wall, <strong>and</strong> a table surrounded by five chairs in<br />

the back of the room. Approximately eight<br />

students <strong>and</strong> three classroom staff members<br />

were present in the room <strong>and</strong> engaged in<br />

various group (e.g., calendar, cleaning tasks)<br />

<strong>and</strong> independent activities (e.g., independent<br />

worksheets, simple reading tasks) throughout<br />

the study. Despite the various activities occurring<br />

simultaneously, the classroom was relatively<br />

calm <strong>and</strong> free from major distracti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Independent <strong>and</strong> Dependent Variables<br />

The independent variable was a combined interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

with visual support as well as discussi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The visual support, a picture symbol strip<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining five photos representative of key<br />

elements within the text (e.g. character, setting)<br />

was presented to the student at the beginning<br />

of each sessi<strong>on</strong>. A brief discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

about text c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> the pictures c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

within the picture strip occurred prior to <strong>and</strong><br />

immediately following the text read-aloud.<br />

The dependent variable was the student’s resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

to a series of four multiple-choice<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s about the text c<strong>on</strong>tent. Questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were asked individually <strong>and</strong> students were able<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d verbally, by pointing to the correct<br />

answer, or through a combinati<strong>on</strong> of the two<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses. If a student failed to resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

within ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds, the questi<strong>on</strong> was asked<br />

again.<br />

Materials<br />

Texts. Texts for the read-alouds were selected<br />

from the SRA Specific Skills Series: Getting<br />

the Main Idea (B<strong>on</strong>ing, 1997), Book G.<br />

This SRA instructi<strong>on</strong>al series c<strong>on</strong>tained short<br />

expository <strong>and</strong> narrative high-interest texts designed<br />

by c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> readability for seventh<br />

grade students (McGraw Hill, n.d). All stories<br />

were comparable in difficulty <strong>and</strong> relative<br />

length. The SRA booklet was chosen due to<br />

the age targeted in both c<strong>on</strong>tent interest <strong>and</strong><br />

ability level. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, this text was used<br />

due to the similarity in difficulty <strong>and</strong> length<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g texts. Fifteen stories were r<strong>and</strong>omly<br />

selected from the booklet. The selected stories<br />

ranged in length from 78–108 words with a<br />

mean of 97 <strong>and</strong> median of 95 words per story.<br />

Topics were varied <strong>and</strong> included a biography<br />

of Astrid Lindgren, the author of Pippi L<strong>on</strong>gstocking,<br />

to facts about Neptune’s largest<br />

mo<strong>on</strong> Trit<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> Questi<strong>on</strong>s. Each text was accompanied<br />

by five, three-opti<strong>on</strong> multiplechoice<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Each questi<strong>on</strong> was literal in<br />

nature, based <strong>on</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ing the facts in<br />

the story, <strong>and</strong> framed as <strong>on</strong>e of six wh- questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(who, what, when, where, why, <strong>and</strong><br />

how). Table 2 provides a sample of questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> answers used for <strong>on</strong>e of the texts. The<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s were created based <strong>on</strong> Pi<strong>on</strong>tek’s<br />

(2008) nine criteria for creating valid <strong>and</strong> reliable<br />

multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s (see Table 3).<br />

To ensure adherence to the criteria, each<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> was independently checked against<br />

the criteria. Suggested adjustments were made<br />

to the questi<strong>on</strong>s, which were followed by an<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 363


TABLE 2<br />

Sample questi<strong>on</strong>s for a story about San<br />

Francisco’s cable cars<br />

1) When were the cable cars first installed?<br />

A. 1999<br />

B. 1815<br />

C. 1873*<br />

2) How are the cable cars powered?<br />

A. by a cable that runs underneath the<br />

streets*<br />

B. by a diesel gas engine<br />

C. by electric power lines above the cars<br />

3) Where were the cable cars first installed?<br />

A. near the large beach in H<strong>on</strong>g K<strong>on</strong>g<br />

B. in the dense forests of New Zeal<strong>and</strong><br />

C. <strong>on</strong> the steep hills of San Francisco*<br />

4) Who has parties, weddings, <strong>and</strong> political<br />

rallies <strong>on</strong> the cable cars?<br />

A. Canadians<br />

B. San Franciscans*<br />

C. Soldiers<br />

* correct answer<br />

independent review by another rater to assure<br />

that the adjustments matched the criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

To ensure c<strong>on</strong>sistency in readability between<br />

the stories <strong>and</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong>s in-text or textrelated<br />

words <strong>and</strong> phrases were used for the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> opti<strong>on</strong>s. A basic review of the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s was also c<strong>on</strong>ducted using the Flesch-<br />

Kincaid grade level equivalence in Microsoft<br />

Word. Each set of questi<strong>on</strong>s registered below a<br />

seventh grade reading level.<br />

TABLE 3<br />

Criteria for Multiple Choice Questi<strong>on</strong>s*<br />

1 Stem is a clearly described questi<strong>on</strong>, problem,<br />

or task.<br />

2 Stem is c<strong>on</strong>cise as possible.<br />

3 Only relevant informati<strong>on</strong> included in stem.<br />

4 No negatives used in stem.<br />

5 Only <strong>on</strong>e correct answer.<br />

6 No irrelevant clues with correct resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

7 Distractors are plausible but wr<strong>on</strong>g.<br />

8 No “all of above” opti<strong>on</strong><br />

9 Correct answer letter alternated am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Note. Stem refers to the questi<strong>on</strong> statement. Distractors<br />

refers to the incorrect answer choices.<br />

* (Pi<strong>on</strong>tek, 2008)<br />

Physical Selecti<strong>on</strong> Sheet. An 8½-by-11 inch<br />

paper with 100-f<strong>on</strong>t printed letter opti<strong>on</strong>s (A,<br />

B, & C) were presented to students allowing<br />

them to physically point to their answer following<br />

the presentati<strong>on</strong> of a multiple-choice<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>. Students could answer questi<strong>on</strong>s by<br />

physically pointing to a letter <strong>on</strong> this paper,<br />

verbally answering the questi<strong>on</strong>, or both. If<br />

students failed to resp<strong>on</strong>d within ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds<br />

of hearing the questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> multiple-choice<br />

answers, the researcher repeated the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> answers. N<strong>on</strong>e of the students required<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al prompting to resp<strong>on</strong>d.<br />

Picture Symbol Strip. A visual support of five,<br />

4-by-4 inch color photos secured to a 4-by-20<br />

inch strip of poster board accompanied each<br />

story. Each picture represented <strong>on</strong>e of the key<br />

elements of the text. The key elements <strong>and</strong><br />

representative photos were selected through a<br />

search of Google images by the first author<br />

<strong>and</strong> reviewed by two independent reviewers<br />

for corresp<strong>on</strong>dence with the story. Each reviewer<br />

was asked to match each picture symbol<br />

strip with the story it represented while presented<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omly. Both reviews resulted in<br />

100% agreement of the representati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

picture supports for each of the individual<br />

stories.<br />

Research Design<br />

A multi-probe across participants design (Kennedy,<br />

2005) was used to assess the effectiveness<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> student’s listening<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. This design was selected because<br />

it enabled replicati<strong>on</strong>s across students<br />

while c<strong>on</strong>trolling for any possible treatment<br />

effect. In additi<strong>on</strong>, this design negated the<br />

need for a lengthy c<strong>on</strong>secutive baseline that<br />

could be overly frustrating for students due to<br />

the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> difficulty of the material<br />

prior to interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

Event recording (Kennedy, 2005) was used to<br />

record each resp<strong>on</strong>se to the comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Immediately following a student resp<strong>on</strong>se,<br />

investigators recorded the letter chosen<br />

by the student. Event recording was selected<br />

due to the quick <strong>and</strong> easily identifiable<br />

nature of the dependent variable.<br />

364 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Procedure<br />

Baseline. In this multiple probe design, students<br />

received from three to six sessi<strong>on</strong>s of the<br />

baseline treatment. During baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e story was read aloud to each student.<br />

Students were then presented with four related<br />

multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s. Students<br />

were provided up to ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds to resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

the first questi<strong>on</strong> without prompting. After<br />

ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds without a resp<strong>on</strong>se, the questi<strong>on</strong><br />

was repeated with an additi<strong>on</strong>al ten-sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

pause. Student resp<strong>on</strong>ses included pointing to<br />

the letter <strong>on</strong> the letter choice sheet or saying<br />

the letter out loud. Following a resp<strong>on</strong>se to<br />

the first questi<strong>on</strong>, the next questi<strong>on</strong> was asked<br />

<strong>and</strong> the same procedures were followed.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, students<br />

were presented with the picture symbol strip<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>ding to the text presented during<br />

that sessi<strong>on</strong>. Up<strong>on</strong> presentati<strong>on</strong> of the picture<br />

strip, students were asked to verbally describe<br />

each photo. An investigator provided verbal<br />

feedback by agreeing with or clarifying student<br />

comments depending <strong>on</strong> each resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

Next, the investigator pointed to each picture<br />

<strong>and</strong> described what it specifically represented.<br />

For example, when presented with a photo of<br />

Jupiter’s mo<strong>on</strong> Trit<strong>on</strong>, the investigator commented,<br />

“Here is Jupiter. Do you see how it<br />

looks rough? Scientists nicknamed it the cantaloupe<br />

mo<strong>on</strong> because it looks rough like a<br />

cantaloupe. Do you see that?” After each<br />

photo was described, the text was read aloud<br />

to the student. Next, each photo was reviewed<br />

as it related to the text. The descripti<strong>on</strong> process<br />

was interactive as students often commented<br />

or asked questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> received resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

Following the descripti<strong>on</strong>, students<br />

were presented with the first of five comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s followed by a ten-sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

pause for a resp<strong>on</strong>se before asking the next<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>. If a student failed to resp<strong>on</strong>d within<br />

ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds, the questi<strong>on</strong> was repeated <strong>and</strong> an<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds provided for a resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Prior to the first baseline sessi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> following<br />

the final interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>, each student<br />

<strong>and</strong> the teacher was interviewed by the<br />

first author to determine his or her perceived<br />

TABLE 4<br />

Social Validity Questi<strong>on</strong>s for Students<br />

Pre Is it sometimes hard for you to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> what you hear?<br />

Pre Is it sometimes hard for you to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> stories that you hear?<br />

Pre Does anything help you underst<strong>and</strong> what<br />

you hear?<br />

Pre Do picture symbols help you underst<strong>and</strong><br />

things that you hear?<br />

Pre Do you like to listen to stories read aloud?<br />

Pre What kinds of stories do you like?<br />

Post Did looking at the pictures help you<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> the story?<br />

Post Did talking about the pictures help you<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> the story better?<br />

Post Did you like looking at <strong>and</strong> talking about<br />

the pictures?<br />

Post Would you like to use this interventi<strong>on</strong> at<br />

school?<br />

Post Did you like the stories?<br />

Post Which was your favorite story? Why?<br />

Post Which was your least favorite story? Why?<br />

Note. Pre questi<strong>on</strong>s asked prior to the study,<br />

Post questi<strong>on</strong>s asked following the study<br />

value of text comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, to assess<br />

whether or not each liked the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to gain insight <strong>on</strong> the practicality for the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>’s use in the classroom (Horner<br />

et al., 2005). Informal interview questi<strong>on</strong>s (see<br />

Tables 4 <strong>and</strong> 5) were used to assess the students’<br />

<strong>and</strong> the teacher’s perspectives regarding<br />

the independent <strong>and</strong> dependent variables.<br />

Interobserver Agreement <strong>and</strong> Treatment Fidelity<br />

To ensure data validity, the first author <strong>and</strong> a<br />

trained classroom paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al simultaneously<br />

yet independently documented each student’s<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses to comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Interobserver agreement was c<strong>on</strong>ducted for<br />

25–60% of baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s with an average<br />

of 38% of sessi<strong>on</strong>s covered. For the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase, interobserver agreement was c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

for 33–44% of sessi<strong>on</strong>s with an average<br />

of 36% of all baseline interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

covered. Resp<strong>on</strong>ses were compared to calculate<br />

reliability. Matching resp<strong>on</strong>ses were labeled<br />

agreements <strong>and</strong> those not matching<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 365


TABLE 5<br />

Social Validity Questi<strong>on</strong>s for the Teacher<br />

Pre Is auditory comprehensi<strong>on</strong> difficult for your<br />

students?<br />

Pre What types of interventi<strong>on</strong>s have you used to<br />

help your students comprehend what they<br />

hear?<br />

Pre What seems to work best?<br />

Pre Do picture symbols help your students<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> things they hear?<br />

Pre How often is auditory comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

important for your students throughout<br />

their day?<br />

Pre What kind of stories do your students like?<br />

Post Did the picture symbols <strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> seem<br />

to help your students underst<strong>and</strong> things<br />

that they heard?<br />

Post What do you think of this as an<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Post Is it effective?<br />

Post Is it practical?<br />

Post Would you use it in the classroom?<br />

Post What modificati<strong>on</strong>s would you make?<br />

Post Where the stories appropriate for your<br />

students? Why?<br />

Post What did you think about the use of<br />

multiple choice as a test of<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Note. Pre questi<strong>on</strong>s asked prior to the study,<br />

Post questi<strong>on</strong>s asked following the study<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>sidered disagreements. Reliability<br />

was calculated by the sum of agreements divided<br />

by the sum of the agreements plus disagreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiplied by 100. Interobserver<br />

agreement for all students in both the<br />

baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> phase was 100%.<br />

Due to the simplicity of observati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

dependent variable no disagreements were recorded.<br />

Treatment fidelity was measured during the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s through the observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of a classroom paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al to c<strong>on</strong>firm<br />

the correct use of the interventi<strong>on</strong> procedures.<br />

As the researcher c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al recorded<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of each step <strong>on</strong> a procedural<br />

checklist (see Table 6). An average of<br />

36% of the interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s with a range<br />

of 33–44% am<strong>on</strong>g students were m<strong>on</strong>itored<br />

for treatment fidelity. Treatment fidelity was<br />

calculated by the sum of completed steps di-<br />

TABLE 6<br />

Procedural Fidelity Checklist<br />

1 Present Picture strip<br />

2 Ask student what he/she sees<br />

3 Comment/discuss each picture<br />

4 Read story<br />

5 Comment/discuss each picture<br />

6 Ask comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> 1<br />

7 Point to letter choice while naming answer<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

8 Allow student 10 sec<strong>on</strong>ds to resp<strong>on</strong>d; If no<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se, repeat questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> wait 10<br />

more sec<strong>on</strong>ds. If still no resp<strong>on</strong>se, ask if<br />

student wants to c<strong>on</strong>tinue (student may<br />

verbally say or point to answer).<br />

9 Ask comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> 2<br />

10 Point to letter choice while naming answer<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

11 Allow student 10 sec<strong>on</strong>ds to resp<strong>on</strong>d; If no<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se, repeat questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> wait 10<br />

more sec<strong>on</strong>ds. If still no resp<strong>on</strong>se, ask if<br />

student wants to c<strong>on</strong>tinue (student may<br />

verbally say or point to answer).<br />

12 Ask comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> 3<br />

13 Point to letter choice while naming answer<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

14 Allow student 10 sec<strong>on</strong>ds to resp<strong>on</strong>d; If no<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se, repeat questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> wait 10<br />

more sec<strong>on</strong>ds. If still no resp<strong>on</strong>se, ask if<br />

student wants to c<strong>on</strong>tinue (student may<br />

verbally say or point to answer).<br />

15 Ask comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> 4<br />

16 Point to letter choice while naming answer<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

17 Allow student 10 sec<strong>on</strong>ds to resp<strong>on</strong>d; If no<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se, repeat questi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> wait 10<br />

more sec<strong>on</strong>ds. If still no resp<strong>on</strong>se, ask if<br />

student wants to c<strong>on</strong>tinue (student may<br />

verbally say or point to answer).<br />

vided by the sum of the completed steps plus<br />

incomplete steps multiplied by 100. For all<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s documented, treatment fidelity was<br />

100%. Due to the simplicity of the procedure,<br />

no disagreements were recorded.<br />

Results<br />

Figure 1 illustrates the overall effectiveness of<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of the typical<br />

age-appropriate stories. Visual analysis revealed<br />

repeated gains in reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

from the baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

366 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for each student. With the excepti<strong>on</strong><br />

of a few sessi<strong>on</strong>s, baseline remained stable<br />

or exhibited downward trending. Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

indicated stability or upward trending<br />

across all students.<br />

Figure 1. Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> Accuracy across Participants.<br />

Louis. Louis’s baseline remained at <strong>and</strong><br />

below 50% accuracy with a downward trend<br />

<strong>and</strong> an overall average of 42% accuracy <strong>on</strong><br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s. With the introducti<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>, this level rapidly in-<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 367


creased to 85% representing a 43% rise over<br />

the baseline mean. Visual analysis showed c<strong>on</strong>siderable<br />

<strong>and</strong> stable progress during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase ranging from 50% to 100%<br />

accuracy.<br />

Sarah. Sarah’s baseline accuracy ranged<br />

from 25%–75% with an average <strong>and</strong> median<br />

score of 50% over four baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. With<br />

the introducti<strong>on</strong> of interventi<strong>on</strong>, this level<br />

rapidly increased to 89% representing a 39%<br />

increase over the baseline mean. Visual analysis<br />

showed noteworthy <strong>and</strong> stable comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

accuracy during interventi<strong>on</strong> ranging<br />

from 75%–100%.<br />

Ellen. Ellen’s baseline scores indicated her<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> accuracy ranged from 25%–<br />

75% with an average of 45%. With the introducti<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>, this level rapidly increased<br />

to 75% representing a 30% increase<br />

over the baseline mean. Visual analysis revealed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderable <strong>and</strong> stable comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

accuracy during interventi<strong>on</strong> ranging<br />

from 50–100%.<br />

William. William participated in six baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated a relatively stable<br />

<strong>and</strong> low accuracy <strong>on</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

His average baseline accuracy was 17%<br />

with a median of 0% <strong>and</strong> a range of 0%–75%.<br />

With the introducti<strong>on</strong> of interventi<strong>on</strong>, this<br />

level rapidly increased to 75% representing a<br />

58% increase over the baseline mean. Visual<br />

analysis indicated c<strong>on</strong>siderable <strong>and</strong> stable<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> accuracy during interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

ranging from 50–100%.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Social validity interviews c<strong>on</strong>firmed the functi<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

social acceptance, <strong>and</strong> the practicality<br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong> used during this study.<br />

In the pre-study interview, the teacher indicated<br />

the importance of her student’s comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

abilities <strong>on</strong> a daily basis. This sentiment<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>firmed by two of the four<br />

students. When asked about supports used to<br />

aid in auditory comprehensi<strong>on</strong>, the teacher<br />

menti<strong>on</strong>ed that multiple supports including<br />

picture symbols, gestures, <strong>and</strong> object use were<br />

necessary. One student, Ellen, also menti<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

she would “ask the teacher to help” when she<br />

did not underst<strong>and</strong> something. Prior to the<br />

study, the teacher <strong>and</strong> students agreed that<br />

pictures or other visual symbols help them<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> what they heard. Following the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, students were asked to identify<br />

their favorite stories. Three students chose<br />

various favorites (e.g. the stories about dolphins,<br />

cable cars, <strong>and</strong> horse shoes) while William<br />

menti<strong>on</strong>ed he liked them all. Two students<br />

expressed a specific story they liked the<br />

least, <strong>on</strong>e did could not think of a least favorite,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sarah menti<strong>on</strong>ed that she “just liked<br />

them all.” The teacher echoed her satisfacti<strong>on</strong><br />

with the stories used. The use of age appropriate<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> new, informative topics for<br />

students were cited as reas<strong>on</strong>s. Students all<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded they would like to use the picture<br />

symbol plus discussi<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> support<br />

more at school. The teacher also favored the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> saw it as a realistic interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for the classroom. She added that she<br />

would likely use the interventi<strong>on</strong> with groups<br />

of students simultaneously rather than in individualized<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al settings so as to<br />

maximize classroom support staff, while still<br />

providing the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

In light of the multiple access barriers for<br />

middle school students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability to typical age-appropriate<br />

texts, interventi<strong>on</strong> research is essential<br />

(Browder et al., 2009; Downing, 2005; Kliewer,<br />

1998; Kliewer et al., 2006). Access to age appropriate<br />

literature is essential for these students<br />

to experience the vocabulary-rich, sufficiently<br />

mature, <strong>and</strong> socially acceptable texts<br />

with increased comprehensi<strong>on</strong> (Browder<br />

et al., 2009; Kluth & Darm<strong>on</strong>dy-Latham,<br />

2003). Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, access to these texts foster<br />

an improved quality of life, promote high expectati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

provide equitable access to instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials, <strong>and</strong> increase self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

opportunities (Browder et al., 2007)<br />

Unfortunately, the use of effective strategies<br />

<strong>and</strong> appropriate materials are often not available<br />

to these students (Browder et al., 2009;<br />

Downing, 2005; Kliewer, 1998; Kliewer et al.,<br />

2006).<br />

This study investigated the effect of a combined<br />

visual- <strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> abilities of middle<br />

school students with moderate intellectual disability<br />

when they were read typical age-appropriate<br />

texts. The interventi<strong>on</strong>’s positive effect<br />

368 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


was repeated across all participants in the<br />

study. Social validity interviews c<strong>on</strong>firmed the<br />

importance of a comprehensi<strong>on</strong>-related interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to the students <strong>and</strong> their teacher. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

the interviews indicated the ease of<br />

use <strong>and</strong> practicality of this interventi<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

classroom. Results indicate the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was successful in enhancing the comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

abilities of students when read typical<br />

age-appropriate texts.<br />

Data also point to the usefulness of this<br />

particular interventi<strong>on</strong> for increasing the<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> abilities of students with moderate<br />

intellectual disability when read texts<br />

appropriate for their age. While all students<br />

performed more accurately during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase, Sarah displayed the most c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

<strong>and</strong> stable change in performance<br />

from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong>. During the<br />

baseline phase she was generally less than 80%<br />

accurate. But during interventi<strong>on</strong>, her resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>sistently between 80% <strong>and</strong><br />

100%. She had the highest interventi<strong>on</strong> average<br />

scores of the students with the third highest<br />

average growth. During all sessi<strong>on</strong>s, Sarah<br />

appeared interested <strong>and</strong> was very cooperative.<br />

She appeared to enjoy looking at the pictures<br />

<strong>and</strong> discussing the c<strong>on</strong>tent. Frequently, she<br />

made comments about the pictures regarding<br />

what she saw, or how they related to the story.<br />

Later, Sarah indicated she enjoyed hearing all<br />

of the stories read out loud. On most occasi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

Sarah answered the questi<strong>on</strong>s by stating<br />

the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding letter.<br />

Louis, the student with the most interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s also experienced a rapid <strong>and</strong><br />

relatively stable increase in comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

scores between baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phases (43% increase). Louis was typically willing<br />

to begin the sessi<strong>on</strong>s, but preferred, <strong>and</strong><br />

was allowed, to finish any seatwork (worksheet,<br />

reading activity) before coming to the<br />

back table to work with the researcher. Louis<br />

listened intently as the stories were read <strong>and</strong><br />

answered the questi<strong>on</strong>s quickly <strong>and</strong> with c<strong>on</strong>fidence.<br />

He rarely needed to hear the questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

or opti<strong>on</strong>s twice before resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

Louis answered questi<strong>on</strong>s by pointing to the<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>ding letter <strong>on</strong> the answer selecti<strong>on</strong><br />

sheet.<br />

Ellen also performed more accurately during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> than in the baseline phase<br />

(39% increase). Ellen seemed to enjoy the<br />

individual sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> occasi<strong>on</strong> spent the<br />

first few minutes prior to beginning talking<br />

about what she did or was planning to do in<br />

the current week. On a few occasi<strong>on</strong>s, Ellen<br />

did not answer the questi<strong>on</strong>s within ten sec<strong>on</strong>ds<br />

<strong>and</strong> needed to hear them again. She<br />

frequently answered by verbalizing the correct<br />

answer by name, often in shorth<strong>and</strong>, speaking<br />

just the key words of the answer choice. For<br />

example, in a questi<strong>on</strong> about why plants <strong>and</strong><br />

animals need to adapt to the desert, she correctly<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded, “so little water” for the answer<br />

choice (c) there is so little water.<br />

William exhibited the most growth (58%<br />

increase in the mean accuracy from baseline<br />

to interventi<strong>on</strong>) of all participants in the<br />

study. Four out of his six baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

resulted in 0% accuracy in his comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of the texts. He was typically quiet but came<br />

willingly <strong>and</strong> usually paused for a few sec<strong>on</strong>ds<br />

before answering the comprehensi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

On a few occasi<strong>on</strong>s, during the discussi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

William resp<strong>on</strong>ded to the questi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

“What do you see?” with a <strong>on</strong>e word utterance<br />

through his speech generating device. It was<br />

sometimes difficult for the researcher to see<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> between his selected word <strong>and</strong><br />

the c<strong>on</strong>tent of the photo; however, William’s<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> accuracy during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase suggests he did have a good underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of the stories <strong>and</strong> was making a c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which he c<strong>on</strong>sidered valid <strong>and</strong> or<br />

logical, although unfamiliar to the investigator.<br />

William answered the comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s typically by pointing to the letter <strong>on</strong><br />

the physical selecti<strong>on</strong> sheet.<br />

Hibbing <strong>and</strong> Rankin-Ericks<strong>on</strong> (2003) suggest<br />

that students benefit from visual representati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

accompanying unfamiliar texts. In<br />

the current investigati<strong>on</strong>, images appeared to<br />

help students attend to the key c<strong>on</strong>tent within<br />

the stories. In additi<strong>on</strong>, discussing the pictures<br />

with students helped attach c<strong>on</strong>textual meaning<br />

from the story to the pictures. These findings<br />

are c<strong>on</strong>sistent with Kucan <strong>and</strong> Beck’s<br />

(1997) review of articles <strong>on</strong> the effects of discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. These authors<br />

found that teacher supported discussi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

such as that used in the study, was<br />

effective in enhancing the comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

abilities of students without disabilities. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

in regards to discussi<strong>on</strong>, Moats (2002)<br />

described the benefits of modeling <strong>and</strong> prac-<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 369


ticing discussi<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong> to texts to increase<br />

<strong>and</strong> support the development of comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

for older students who struggle<br />

with reading.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

One limitati<strong>on</strong> of this study was the exclusive<br />

use of multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s as a measure<br />

of student comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. While the students<br />

in this study did prove their ability to accurately<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>d in a multiple-choice format<br />

prior to participati<strong>on</strong>, the complexities involved<br />

in using multiple-choice resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

express comprehensi<strong>on</strong> could exclude other<br />

students with moderate or severe intellectual<br />

disability from replicating the effects of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. For instance, <strong>on</strong>e student, who<br />

may have benefited from this interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

was specifically excluded from this study due<br />

to her repeated inaccuracy in answering the<br />

novel multiple-choice questi<strong>on</strong>s. And several<br />

other students in the classroom were not recommended<br />

for participati<strong>on</strong> by the teacher<br />

due to their inc<strong>on</strong>sistency in resp<strong>on</strong>ding to<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Future research in this area should<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> an exp<strong>and</strong>ed approach for assessing<br />

student comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of read aloud texts.<br />

While two individuals separately reviewed<br />

the photo sets to ensure the accuracy of their<br />

representative c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> to the texts, this<br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong> did not sufficiently address the<br />

need for c<strong>on</strong>sistency in identifying the critical<br />

representative c<strong>on</strong>cepts of a text or selecting<br />

accurate representati<strong>on</strong>s from photos. In this<br />

study, main ideas <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts were selected<br />

from each story by the first author. These<br />

main ideas <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cepts were then used as<br />

the basis for selecting representative photos.<br />

While this process did prove reliably representative<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g the raters <strong>and</strong> popular am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

the students for the short texts used, it may<br />

not have the same effect <strong>on</strong> more elaborate<br />

texts (e.g. novels, biographies). Future research<br />

should include more precise <strong>and</strong> formulaic<br />

approaches to the process of selecting<br />

key themes <strong>and</strong> representative images for text.<br />

The discussi<strong>on</strong> porti<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

was loosely based. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s began by asking<br />

each student to comment <strong>on</strong> the photos <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued with an unscripted descripti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

each photo followed by a c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> with<br />

the student regarding how the photos repre-<br />

sented the texts. While the discussi<strong>on</strong> appeared<br />

to have a beneficial effect <strong>on</strong> the student’s<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> abilities of the texts<br />

read aloud, the lack of structure utilized is<br />

deemed a limitati<strong>on</strong> in terms of research replicati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> applicati<strong>on</strong> in the classroom. Unstructured<br />

discussi<strong>on</strong>s have the potential to<br />

not <strong>on</strong>ly vary in methodology <strong>and</strong> results, but<br />

also to detract from the main purpose of the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, which is to help students comprehend<br />

text read out loud. Future research<br />

in the combined picture symbol plus discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> should examine the use of<br />

structure interviewing strategies to provide increased<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistency in the discussi<strong>on</strong> porti<strong>on</strong><br />

of this interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Fifteen short expository <strong>and</strong> narrative high<br />

interest stories were used as the age-appropriate<br />

literature in this study. While the c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

appeared to hold students’ interest <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

using l<strong>on</strong>ger texts such as age-appropriate<br />

novels, magazine articles, or newspapers<br />

may provide more credibility to the robustness<br />

of the present interventi<strong>on</strong>. Future studies using<br />

this interventi<strong>on</strong> should explore its effectiveness<br />

with l<strong>on</strong>ger, more comm<strong>on</strong>ly accessed<br />

texts such as popular adolescent<br />

novels, academic textbooks, or newspapers.<br />

Finally, cauti<strong>on</strong> should be used when interpreting<br />

the results of this study due to the<br />

small number of participants involved. While<br />

this study c<strong>on</strong>tributes to the literature <strong>on</strong> access<br />

for students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability to age-appropriate texts, additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research <strong>on</strong> this particular interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> as<br />

well as variati<strong>on</strong>s of it, should be c<strong>on</strong>ducted to<br />

promote generalizability of these results.<br />

References<br />

Al Otaiba, S., & Hosp, M. K. (2004). Providing<br />

effective literacy instructi<strong>on</strong> to students with<br />

down syndrome. Teaching Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

36(4), 28–35.<br />

Allor, J. H., Mathes, P. G., Roberts, K., J<strong>on</strong>es, F. G.,<br />

& Champlin, T. M. (2010). Teaching students<br />

with moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An<br />

experimental examinati<strong>on</strong> of a comprehensive<br />

reading interventi<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 3–22.<br />

Bell<strong>on</strong>, M. L., Ogletree, B. T., & Harn, W. E. (2000).<br />

Repeated storybook reading as a language interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for children with autism. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 15, 52–58.<br />

370 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


B<strong>on</strong>ing, R. A. (1997). SRA Specific Skills Series: Getting<br />

the Main Idea. Book G (5th ed.). Columbus, OH:<br />

SRA/McGraw Hill.<br />

Browder, D. M., Mims, P. J., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-<br />

Delzell, L., & Lee, A. (2008). Teaching elementary<br />

students with multiple disabilities to participate<br />

in shared stories. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 33, 3–12.<br />

Browder, D. M., Trela, K., & Jimenez, B. (2007).<br />

Training teachers to follow a task analysis to engage<br />

middle school students with moderate <strong>and</strong><br />

severe developmental disabilities in grade-appropriate<br />

literature. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 22, 206–219.<br />

Browder, D. M., Wakeman, S. Y., Flowers, C., Rickelman,<br />

R. J., Pugalee, D., & Karv<strong>on</strong>en, M. (2007).<br />

Creating access to the general curriculum with<br />

links to grade-level c<strong>on</strong>tent for students with significant<br />

cognitive disabilities. The Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 41, 2.<br />

Browder, D. M., & Xin, Y. P. (1998). A meta-analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> review of sight word research <strong>and</strong> its implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al reading to individuals<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. The<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 32, 130–153.<br />

Browder, D., Gibbs, S., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., Courtade,<br />

G. R., Mraz, M., & Flowers, C. (2009). Literacy<br />

for students with severe developmental disabilities.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 30, 269–<br />

282.<br />

Downing, J. (2005). Teaching literacy to students with<br />

significant disabilities: Strategies for the K-12 inclusive<br />

classroom. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Corwin.<br />

Ericks<strong>on</strong>, K. A., & Koppenhaver, D. A. (1995). Developing<br />

a literacy program for children with severe<br />

disabilities. The Reading Teacher, 48, 676–684.<br />

Hibbing, A. N., & Rankin-Ericks<strong>on</strong>, J. L. (2003). A<br />

picture is worth a thous<strong>and</strong> words: Using visual<br />

images to improve comprehensi<strong>on</strong> for middle<br />

school struggling readers. The Reading Teacher, 56,<br />

758–770.<br />

Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple<br />

view of reading. Reading <strong>and</strong> Writing, 2, 127–160.<br />

Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G.,<br />

Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of singlesubject<br />

research to identify evidence-based practice<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71,<br />

165–179.<br />

Janks, H. (2010). Literacy <strong>and</strong> power. New York, NY:<br />

Routledge.<br />

Joseph, L. M., & McCachran, M. (2003). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of a word study ph<strong>on</strong>ics technique between<br />

students with moderate to mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> struggling readers without disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

38, 192–199.<br />

Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research. Bost<strong>on</strong>. MA: Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Kliewer, C. (1998). Citizenship in the literate community:<br />

An ethnography of children with Down<br />

syndrome <strong>and</strong> the written word. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

64, 167–180.<br />

Kliewer, C., Biklen, D., & Kasa-Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, C.<br />

(2006). Who may be literate? Disability <strong>and</strong> resistance<br />

to the cultural denial of competence. American<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research Journal, 43, 163–192.<br />

Kliewer, C., & L<strong>and</strong>is, D. (1999). Individualizing<br />

literacy instructi<strong>on</strong> for young children with moderate<br />

to severe disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 66,<br />

85–100.<br />

Kluth, P., & Darm<strong>on</strong>dy-Latham, J. (2003). Bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

sight words: Literacy opportunities for students<br />

with autism. The Reading Teacher, 56, 532–535.<br />

Kucan, L., & Beck, I. L. (1997). Thinking aloud <strong>and</strong><br />

reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong> research: Inquiry, instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>. Review of Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Research, 67, 271–299.<br />

Lawh<strong>on</strong>, T., & Cobb, J. B. (2002). Routines that<br />

build emergent literacy skills in infants, toddlers,<br />

<strong>and</strong> preschoolers. Early Childhood Educati<strong>on</strong> Journal,<br />

30, 113–118.<br />

Light, J. C., Roberts, B., Dimarco, R., & Greiner, N.<br />

(1998). Augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

to support receptive <strong>and</strong> expressive communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

for people with autism. Journal of Communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Disorders, 31, 153–180.<br />

Liu, J. (2004). Effects of comic strips <strong>on</strong> L2 learners<br />

reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. TESOL Quarterly, 38,<br />

225–243.<br />

McGraw Hill. (n.d). SRA Skills Series Brochure.<br />

McGraw Hill. Retrieved from https://www.mhe<br />

<strong>on</strong>line.com/assets/sra_download/MultipleSkills<br />

Series/MoreInfo/SRASkillsSeries_Brochure.pdf<br />

Moats, L. (2002). When older students can’t read.<br />

L.D. Online. Retrieved from http://www.ld<strong>on</strong>line.<br />

org/article/8025?themeprint<br />

Pi<strong>on</strong>tek, M. E. (2008). Best practices for designing<br />

<strong>and</strong> grading exams. University of Michigan. Retrieved<br />

from http://faculty.ksu.edu.sa/elbitar/<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al%20research/BEST%20PRACTICES<br />

%20FOR%20DESIGNING%20AND%20GRADING<br />

%20EXAMS.pdf<br />

Skotko, B. G., Koppenhaver, D. A., & Ericks<strong>on</strong>, K. A.<br />

(2004). Parent reading behaviors <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

outcomes in girls with Rhett syndrome.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 70, 145–166.<br />

Sevcik, R., Romski, M. A., & Wilkins<strong>on</strong>, K. (1991).<br />

Roles of graphic symbols in the language acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

process for pers<strong>on</strong>s with severe cognitive disabilities.<br />

Augmentative <strong>and</strong> Alternative Communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

7, 161–170.<br />

Slater, J. M. (2002). A pictorial approach for improving<br />

literacy skills in students with disabilities:<br />

An exploratory research study. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 17(3), 58–62.<br />

Comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of Typical Texts / 371


Turnbull, A. P., Turnbull, H. R., & Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L. (2010). Excepti<strong>on</strong>al lives: Special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in today’s schools (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River,<br />

NJ: Merrill.<br />

Waugh, R. E., Fredrick, L. D., & Alberto, P. A.<br />

(2009). Using simultaneous prompting to teach<br />

sounds <strong>and</strong> blending skills to students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities. Research in developmental<br />

disabilities, 30, 1435–14<str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2006). Bey<strong>on</strong>d access: Ensuring<br />

progress in the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum for<br />

students with severe disabilities. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice<br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 31, 322–326.<br />

Received: 23 September 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 28 November 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 6 January 2012<br />

372 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 373–388<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Grade-Aligned Math Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Students with<br />

Moderate Intellectual Disability<br />

Diane M. Browder<br />

University of North Carolina at Charlotte<br />

Katherine Trela<br />

Marist College<br />

Bree A. Jimenez<br />

University of North Carolina at Greensboro<br />

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of grade-aligned math instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> math skill<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of four middle schools with moderate intellectual disability. Teachers were trained to follow a task<br />

analysis to teach grade-aligned math to middle school students using adapted math problem stories <strong>and</strong> graphic<br />

organizers. The teacher implemented four math units representing four of the five Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers<br />

of Mathematics recommended math st<strong>and</strong>ards (i.e., algebra, geometry, measurement, <strong>and</strong> data analysis/<br />

probability; NCTM, 2002). A multiple probe across unit design was used to examine the effects of the math<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the number of steps completed <strong>on</strong> each math st<strong>and</strong>ard task analysis. Results indicated a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ship between math instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> student behavior with an overall increase in independent<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for practice <strong>and</strong> future research are discussed. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

future research <strong>and</strong> practice are discussed.<br />

One of the key c<strong>on</strong>cepts introduced in guidance<br />

for including students with disabilities in<br />

No Child Left Behind (2002) is that the target<br />

for some students with significant cognitive<br />

disability might be “alternate achievement”<br />

that is different in scope or complexity, but<br />

still aligned with grade level st<strong>and</strong>ards. Many<br />

states provide curricular frameworks or extensi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for each grade’s state st<strong>and</strong>ards to indicate<br />

how to access c<strong>on</strong>tent like mathematics<br />

<strong>and</strong> English/language arts. To teach to the<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards, instructi<strong>on</strong>al teams must still deter-<br />

We thank the teachers <strong>and</strong> leaders of the Charlotte<br />

Mecklenburg School System <strong>and</strong> to the parents<br />

<strong>and</strong> students for their partnership in this research.<br />

Support for this research was provided in part by<br />

Grant No. H324M03003 of the U.S. Department of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, Office of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Programs,<br />

awarded to the University of North Carolina at<br />

Charlotte. The opini<strong>on</strong>s expressed do not necessarily<br />

reflect the positi<strong>on</strong> or policy of the Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> no official endorsement should<br />

be inferred. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article<br />

should be addressed to Bree A. Jimenez, University<br />

of North Carolina at Greensboro, 421 School of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Building, PO Box 26170, Greensboro,<br />

NC 27402-6170. Email: bajimene@uncg.edu<br />

mine what the student will learn <strong>and</strong> how to<br />

teach it.<br />

Although teachers have been required to<br />

help all students make adequate yearly progress,<br />

there have been few models for c<strong>on</strong>ducting<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards-based instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with moderate/severe intellectual disability especially<br />

in the area of mathematics. Textbooks<br />

<strong>on</strong> educating students with severe disabilities<br />

provide minimal informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> teaching<br />

mathematics besides m<strong>on</strong>ey <strong>and</strong> measurement<br />

(Ryndak & Alper, 1996, 2003; Snell &<br />

Brown, 2000, 2006; Westling & Fox, 2000,<br />

2004). There also have been few research<br />

studies to guide these interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Browder,<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-Delzell, Wakeman, <strong>and</strong><br />

Harris (2008) used guidelines from Horner<br />

et al. (2005) <strong>and</strong> Gersten et al. (2005) to<br />

identify high quality evidence-based mathematics<br />

research with students with a moderate/severe<br />

intellectual disability published between<br />

1975 <strong>and</strong> 2005. Sixty-five articles yielded<br />

54 single-case <strong>and</strong> 14 group studies (some<br />

articles had 1 study). Although limited in<br />

scope, these studies provide evidence that this<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> can learn mathematics. A total of<br />

493 individuals with disabilities participated in<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 373


these studies including 336 individuals with<br />

moderate intellectual disability (mental retardati<strong>on</strong>),<br />

64 individuals with severe intellectual<br />

disability, 24 individuals with autism, 13 individuals<br />

with unspecified developmental disability,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e individual with multiple disabilities.<br />

These studies also indicate that<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s derived from principles of applied<br />

behavior analysis, such as systematic<br />

prompting with feedback, can be highly effective<br />

for teaching math c<strong>on</strong>tent.<br />

Browder et al. (2008) also found that most<br />

studies have focused <strong>on</strong> numbers <strong>and</strong> operati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

or m<strong>on</strong>ey skills. These c<strong>on</strong>tent areas are<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly a small sample of the recommended c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

for mathematics. In practice, teachers<br />

also typically focus <strong>on</strong> repetitive practice of<br />

computati<strong>on</strong>al skills, based <strong>on</strong> the belief that<br />

students master readiness skills before engaging<br />

in higher order math less<strong>on</strong>s (Woodward<br />

& M<strong>on</strong>tague, 2002). With this focus, many<br />

students will not have access to the st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

that will be included in states’ alternate assessments<br />

based <strong>on</strong> academic achievement st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

which must align to the c<strong>on</strong>tent st<strong>and</strong>ards.<br />

Most states organize their st<strong>and</strong>ards by<br />

major str<strong>and</strong>s of academic learning similar to,<br />

or the same as, those identified by the Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Council of Teachers of Mathematics<br />

(NCTM). In 1989 <strong>and</strong> again in 2000, NCTM<br />

identified five main comp<strong>on</strong>ents of math instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

including (a) numbers <strong>and</strong> operati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

(b) measurement, (c) data analysis <strong>and</strong><br />

probability, (d) geometry, <strong>and</strong> (e) algebra.<br />

Most recently, the Comm<strong>on</strong> Core St<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

in mathematics have defined a set of outcomes<br />

in these areas that are being adopted<br />

by most states in the United States.<br />

One opti<strong>on</strong> for promoting learning across<br />

more c<strong>on</strong>tent areas is to apply the practices<br />

used effectively in mathematics for students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disability<br />

across more state st<strong>and</strong>ards. Browder et al.<br />

(2008) identified <strong>on</strong>e of these practices to be<br />

task analytic instructi<strong>on</strong> with systematic<br />

prompting. In this review <strong>on</strong> mathematics,<br />

task analyses primarily were used to teach<br />

students to make a purchase from a store or<br />

vending machine (Aeschleman & Schladenauffen,<br />

1984; Browder, Snell, & Wild<strong>on</strong>ger,<br />

1988; Haring, Kennedy, Adams, & Pitts-C<strong>on</strong>way,<br />

1987). Two studies have applied task analytic<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach grade-aligned state<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards in mathematics. Jimenez, Browder,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Courtade (2008), used a multiple probe<br />

across participants design to dem<strong>on</strong>strate that<br />

three high school students with moderate intellectual<br />

disability could learn to solve an<br />

algebraic equati<strong>on</strong>. In additi<strong>on</strong>, students were<br />

able to complete their problem solving in an<br />

inclusive high school general educati<strong>on</strong> setting<br />

beside peers who were working <strong>on</strong> similar<br />

equati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> to generalize them across materials<br />

(i.e., job tasks). Browder et al. (2010)<br />

used a similar strategy to teach students<br />

multiple st<strong>and</strong>ards selected from the middle/<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary level. Students were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned<br />

to receive the st<strong>and</strong>ards-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> in either mathematics or<br />

science in a pretest/posttest c<strong>on</strong>trol group<br />

design. Students who received the mathematics<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> made higher gains <strong>on</strong> the<br />

curriculum-based math measure.<br />

Although both of these studies provide<br />

promise for teaching middle or high school<br />

mathematics st<strong>and</strong>ards, Jimenez et al. (2008)<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly addressed <strong>on</strong>e skill within <strong>on</strong>e st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

(solving a simple algebraic equati<strong>on</strong>) <strong>and</strong><br />

Browder et al. (2010) used a r<strong>and</strong>omized trials<br />

design which provided evidence of group differences,<br />

but did not reveal whether all students<br />

mastered the c<strong>on</strong>tent. One c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

that the Browder et al. study offered was that<br />

the mathematics problems were presented as<br />

real life problems that were read aloud to the<br />

students.<br />

Literature <strong>on</strong> general mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for middle school students suggests that<br />

skill development may be promoted by linking<br />

math <strong>and</strong> language arts (Zambo, 2005). Specifically,<br />

stories that are written within a c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

familiar to the student may provide a<br />

framework, or schema, up<strong>on</strong> which the student<br />

may naturally organize informati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

order to solve the problem (Anders<strong>on</strong>, Spiro,<br />

& Anders<strong>on</strong>, 1978). Pugalee (2005) developed<br />

a strategy for teaching mathematics with<br />

stories that build <strong>on</strong> research-based recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for teaching this c<strong>on</strong>tent to students<br />

with learning disabilities. This approach<br />

includes (a) an advance organizer linking new<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> with prior learning, (b) walking<br />

through the story to model thinking about the<br />

math c<strong>on</strong>cept, (c) building skills by allowing<br />

the student the opportunity to practice applying<br />

new informati<strong>on</strong>, (d) generalizati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

374 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 1<br />

Student demographic informati<strong>on</strong><br />

Student Age Sex IQ Score<br />

which students develop stories or scenarios<br />

that embed this new informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> (e) assessing<br />

students’ performance.<br />

Since students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

intellectual disability may not read, teachers<br />

may need to follow a protocol for an interactive<br />

read-aloud. Browder, Trela, <strong>and</strong> Jimenez<br />

(2007) dem<strong>on</strong>strated how to promote active<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> in literature adapted from middle<br />

<strong>and</strong> high school novels for students with<br />

moderate/severe intellectual disability or autism.<br />

In this approach, the story is introduced<br />

with some attenti<strong>on</strong> grabber (e.g., students<br />

may listen to whale calls in a story about the<br />

ocean). Then the teacher involves the student<br />

in the read-aloud, for example, by having the<br />

student complete repeated story lines or answer<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s. Similarly, in a math story problem,<br />

the teacher can engage the student with<br />

the theme (e.g., sample d<strong>on</strong>uts for a problem<br />

about how many d<strong>on</strong>uts were purchased),<br />

have the students engage with the key math<br />

facts (e.g., finding each number), <strong>and</strong> then<br />

work together to find the soluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The purpose of the current study was to<br />

extend the work <strong>on</strong> teaching upper level<br />

mathematics st<strong>and</strong>ards to students with moderate<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disability through<br />

using read-alouds of math problems with task<br />

analytic instructi<strong>on</strong> to find the soluti<strong>on</strong>. To<br />

promote generalizati<strong>on</strong> across math problems,<br />

graphic organizers were introduced to<br />

help students perform the steps of the problem<br />

solving. Although not often used in research<br />

with students with moderate/severe intellectual<br />

disability, graphic organizers have<br />

been found to promote comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

expository text for students with learning disabilities<br />

(Garjria, Jitendra, Sood, & Sacks,<br />

2007.) We theorize that the combinati<strong>on</strong> of a<br />

Test<br />

administered Classificati<strong>on</strong> Communicati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

Claire 13 Female 40 WISC III Moderate intellectual disability Verbal<br />

Kiernan 13 Male 40 WISC III Moderate intellectual disability N<strong>on</strong>-verbal, uses AC<br />

Everett 13 Male 30-40 DAS Severe intellectual disability N<strong>on</strong>-verbal, learning<br />

to use AC<br />

Todd 11 Male 41 UNIT Moderate intellectual disability Verbal<br />

read-aloud of a math word problem, a graphic<br />

organizer, <strong>and</strong> task analytic instructi<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

steps to solve the problem will be effective in<br />

promoting math learning for different types<br />

of st<strong>and</strong>ards for students with moderate <strong>and</strong><br />

severe intellectual disability.<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Setting<br />

The study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in a large urban<br />

school system in the southeastern United<br />

States. The interventi<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted by the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

middle school classroom for students with<br />

moderate/severe intellectual disability. Participants<br />

were identified by recruiting a middle<br />

school special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher <strong>and</strong> asking<br />

her to nominate four students who met the<br />

following eligibility criteria (a) full scale IQ <br />

55, (b) adequate visi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> hearing to interact<br />

with the materials, (c) an ability to communicate<br />

verbally or with an augmentative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> system, <strong>and</strong> (d) c<strong>on</strong>sistent attendance<br />

(absent less than two times per<br />

m<strong>on</strong>th). All students in the study participated<br />

in the state’s large scale assessments by taking<br />

the alternate assessment based <strong>on</strong> alternate<br />

achievement st<strong>and</strong>ards for all c<strong>on</strong>tent areas.<br />

As shown in Table 1, students ranged in age<br />

from 11 to 13 <strong>and</strong> had IQs from 30–41. All IQ<br />

scores were obtained from students’ most recent<br />

psychological evaluati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

All assessments were c<strong>on</strong>ducted by a graduate<br />

level member of the research team in the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom with the four target<br />

students. The teacher included other students<br />

in the instructi<strong>on</strong>al group besides the<br />

target students <strong>and</strong> typically implemented the<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 375


less<strong>on</strong> with the entire class (8–10 students).<br />

Teacher trainings occurred in a university<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ference room.<br />

Materials<br />

Figure 1. Sample math story <strong>and</strong> graphic organizer for Geometry.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials. Middle school mathematics<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards were selected using the<br />

state’s mathematics st<strong>and</strong>ards. These stan-<br />

dards were simplified for instructi<strong>on</strong> with the<br />

target populati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> reviewed by a university<br />

level mathematics c<strong>on</strong>tent expert to ensure<br />

that each target skill was aligned with the target<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard. The research staff then created<br />

sets of word problems for each st<strong>and</strong>ard using<br />

the same problem solving method (e.g., solving<br />

an equati<strong>on</strong>; comparing graphed data)<br />

but with different applicati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., shop-<br />

376 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


ping, dining out, voting) <strong>and</strong> differing numbers.<br />

Because of the students limited numeracy<br />

skills, the problems used numbers<br />

from 1–10. Each word problem was typed with<br />

key vocabulary (e.g., character in the word<br />

problem story) paired with pictures using a<br />

picture symbol software program (i.e., Writing<br />

with Symbols©). Eight problems per math<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard (i.e., algebra, data analysis, geometry,<br />

<strong>and</strong> measurement) were given to the<br />

teacher, with a total of 32 math stories provided.<br />

These adapted word problem stories<br />

were printed in color <strong>and</strong> placed in page protectors<br />

for durability. The teacher was provided<br />

a binder divided by each st<strong>and</strong>ard which<br />

was called a unit (e.g., Geometry Unit).<br />

Within each divided secti<strong>on</strong> of the binder, the<br />

teacher was provided the graphic organizer<br />

for that unit (st<strong>and</strong>ard) <strong>and</strong> the adapted stories<br />

that corresp<strong>on</strong>ded to the unit. The<br />

graphic organizers were printed in color <strong>and</strong><br />

laminated for durability. Velcro was used to<br />

manipulate numbers <strong>on</strong> the graphic organizer<br />

if the student was not able to use a visa-vi<br />

marker to write numbers. Additi<strong>on</strong>al manipulatives<br />

were provided as necessary to complete<br />

the math less<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., paper m<strong>on</strong>ey for measurement,<br />

green <strong>and</strong> red chips for the algebra<br />

prompt, n<strong>on</strong>permanent markers for geometry).<br />

Teachers were also provided a poster size<br />

graphic organizer for each of the units to use<br />

for group instructi<strong>on</strong>. Figure 1 shows a sample<br />

adapted story <strong>and</strong> graphic organizer from the<br />

geometry unit for which the st<strong>and</strong>ard focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> finding points, line segments, <strong>and</strong> points<br />

<strong>on</strong> a plane.<br />

Dependent Variable<br />

The dependent variable was the number correct<br />

math resp<strong>on</strong>ses made by the student during<br />

the unit (e.g., algebra) assessment probe.<br />

To investigate changes in student behavior in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se to teacher’s use of the story-based<br />

math problems, four assessments of student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses were used (see Figure 2). The graduate<br />

assistant c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong>e to two assessment<br />

probes with each student per week. A<br />

task analysis was created for the steps for each<br />

math st<strong>and</strong>ard (see Table 2). For each of the<br />

four math tasks, task analytic assessments were<br />

developed (Browder, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, & Jimenez,<br />

2011). During each of the assessments, the<br />

researcher displayed the needed materials<br />

<strong>and</strong> posed a questi<strong>on</strong> for the student to solve<br />

(e.g., “Show me how to ___ (find point A).”<br />

The student was given five sec<strong>on</strong>ds to begin<br />

each step of the task analysis. If the student<br />

did not complete a step, the researcher completed<br />

the step <strong>and</strong> said, “Keep going.” Students<br />

received praise for paying attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

working <strong>on</strong> the tasks. No task specific prompts<br />

or feedback were given. Each step of the task<br />

analysis was scored as correct () or incorrect<br />

(). For generalizati<strong>on</strong>, students were probed<br />

<strong>on</strong> math problem stories that had not been<br />

used during instructi<strong>on</strong>, but required the<br />

same mathematical problem solving skill (e.g.,<br />

steps to solve an equati<strong>on</strong>.)<br />

Experimental Design <strong>and</strong> Analysis<br />

A single subject design was used to dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>ship between the<br />

mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the dependent<br />

variable which was acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of math resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

Specifically, the design was a multiple<br />

probe across four math units (st<strong>and</strong>ards) with<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current between participant replicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for the four target participants who received<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> as a group (Gast, 2010; Horner &<br />

Baer, 1978). During baseline, the math resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

were probed for each student at minimum<br />

of three sessi<strong>on</strong>s or until data was c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

for three sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Following baseline,<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> began <strong>on</strong> Unit 1 (i.e., Geometry<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard) for all four students. Students received<br />

task-analytic math less<strong>on</strong>s by the special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teacher. Prior to the students moving<br />

from Unit 1 to Unit 2 (i.e., Algebra), all<br />

individual students’ data had to show a<br />

change or trend after receiving instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

a minimum of 5 weeks instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> that<br />

unit. Once the students were ready to move to<br />

Unit 2, the students were each probed <strong>on</strong> Unit<br />

2, Unit 3, <strong>and</strong> Unit 4 resp<strong>on</strong>ses. After students’<br />

data for Unit 2 showed a change in<br />

level or trend after receiving a minimum of<br />

five weeks instructi<strong>on</strong>, Unit 3 (i.e., Data-Analysis)<br />

<strong>and</strong> Unit 4 (i.e., Measurement) were<br />

probed. Unit 3 was then taught following the<br />

same guidelines for Units 1 <strong>and</strong> 2, before Unit<br />

4 was introduced. Prior to Unit 4 being<br />

taught, Unit 4 was probed. Instructi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tin-<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 377


ued for a minimum of five weeks <strong>and</strong> until all<br />

student data showed a change in level <strong>and</strong><br />

trend. Maintenance probes (units 1–3) of previous<br />

units were c<strong>on</strong>ducted every two to three<br />

weeks after interventi<strong>on</strong> throughout the durati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the study. No maintenance of Unit 4<br />

was taken due to the ending of the school<br />

year. Only independent correct student resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

were graphed <strong>and</strong> used for visual analysis<br />

of the data.<br />

Figure 2. Sample Student Assessment in Geometry.<br />

Procedure<br />

Baseline <strong>and</strong> Ongoing Probes. During baseline,<br />

the graduate research assistant served as<br />

the primary data collector. Inter-observer<br />

agreement was taken <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e of the three baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s by a sec<strong>on</strong>d member of the research<br />

team. Students were individually assessed<br />

for each of the four units of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

during each baseline probe. All baseline probes<br />

378 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 2<br />

C<strong>on</strong>tent St<strong>and</strong>ards, Alternate Achievement, <strong>and</strong> Task Analyses Used in Math<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al St<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

(NCTM) based <strong>on</strong> 6-8 th<br />

grade b<strong>and</strong>s<br />

Algebra: Represent <strong>and</strong><br />

analyze mathematical<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

structures using<br />

algebraic symbols.<br />

Geometry: Specify<br />

locati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> describe<br />

spatial relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

using coordinate<br />

geometry <strong>and</strong> other<br />

representati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

systems. Use<br />

visualizati<strong>on</strong>, spatial<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>ing, <strong>and</strong><br />

geometric modeling to<br />

solve problems.<br />

Measurement: Apply<br />

appropriate techniques,<br />

tools, <strong>and</strong> formulas to<br />

determine<br />

measurements.<br />

Data Analysis <strong>and</strong><br />

Probability: Formulate<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s that can be<br />

addressed with data<br />

<strong>and</strong> collect, organize,<br />

<strong>and</strong> display relevant<br />

data to answer them.<br />

Competency Goal(s) from<br />

state st<strong>and</strong>ard course of<br />

study st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

Use <strong>and</strong> evaluate algebraic<br />

expressi<strong>on</strong>s. Solve simple<br />

(<strong>on</strong>e- <strong>and</strong> two-step)<br />

equati<strong>on</strong>s or inequalities.<br />

Represent problem<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s with geometric<br />

models. Identify, predict,<br />

<strong>and</strong> describe dilati<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

the coordinate plane.<br />

Develop flexibility in solving<br />

problems by selecting<br />

strategies <strong>and</strong> using<br />

mental computati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

estimati<strong>on</strong>, calculators or<br />

computers, <strong>and</strong> paper <strong>and</strong><br />

pencil.<br />

Collect, organize, analyze,<br />

<strong>and</strong> display data<br />

(including box plots <strong>and</strong><br />

histograms) to solve<br />

problems.<br />

followed the same guidelines described under<br />

the descripti<strong>on</strong> of the dependent variable. No<br />

feedback was given to students during baseline<br />

probes. Data was graphed <strong>and</strong> visually<br />

inspected after each sessi<strong>on</strong>. After baseline,<br />

the same procedures were followed to c<strong>on</strong>tinue<br />

to probe whatever unit was receiving<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. That is, <strong>on</strong>e or two times a week<br />

prior to the less<strong>on</strong>, the graduate student c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Alternate Achievement<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards addressed in<br />

this study Task Analysis<br />

Solve simple <strong>on</strong>e-step<br />

equati<strong>on</strong>s that relate<br />

to stories about daily<br />

events.<br />

Identify <strong>and</strong> describe<br />

the intersecti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

figures in a plane.<br />

Draw line segments<br />

<strong>and</strong> a coordinate<br />

plane to dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

spatial sense for<br />

familiar c<strong>on</strong>texts like<br />

grocery store.<br />

Develop numbers sense<br />

for real numbers.<br />

Develop flexibility in<br />

solving mathematical<br />

problems by selecting<br />

strategies <strong>and</strong> using<br />

appropriate<br />

technology. Use next<br />

dollar strategy to<br />

solve problems<br />

related to everyday<br />

transacti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Collect, organize <strong>and</strong><br />

display data to solve<br />

problems from<br />

familiar events.<br />

ducted the task analytic assessment for that<br />

unit. At the end of a unit, all units were reprobed<br />

before the next was introduced.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Identify problem statement. Identify<br />

first, sec<strong>on</strong>d, <strong>and</strong> last fact in story.<br />

Name unknown quantity “x”. Place<br />

facts in correct sequence <strong>on</strong><br />

Equati<strong>on</strong> Prompt. Identify<br />

operati<strong>on</strong> needed to solve problem.<br />

State soluti<strong>on</strong> to problem. State<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong> in story c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Identify problem statement. Identify<br />

points <strong>on</strong> map using facts from<br />

story. Draw line segments formed<br />

from identified points. Identify<br />

plane formed from line segments.<br />

State soluti<strong>on</strong> to problem. State<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong> in story c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Identify problem statement. Identify<br />

dollar amount from fact in story.<br />

Count number of <strong>on</strong>e dollar bills<br />

equal to given dollar amount.<br />

Count out <strong>on</strong>e more dollar, (if<br />

verbal, may say “<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e more”<br />

while counting <strong>on</strong>e more). State<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong> to problem. State soluti<strong>on</strong><br />

in story c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Identify problem statement. Record<br />

data from story <strong>on</strong> graph. Identify<br />

choice with “more”. State soluti<strong>on</strong><br />

to problem. State soluti<strong>on</strong> in story<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

The mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong> included (a)<br />

mathematics word problem stories based <strong>on</strong><br />

familiar activities, (b) a graphic organizer <strong>and</strong><br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 379


manipulatives for the mathematics c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

(e.g., a template for solving the linear equati<strong>on</strong>),<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) step by step training in the task<br />

analysis to identify <strong>and</strong> organize key facts <strong>and</strong><br />

solve the problem stated in the written story.<br />

The special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher participated in<br />

four professi<strong>on</strong>al development workshops. A<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> mathematics teacher from<br />

the teacher’s middle school also attended the<br />

training <strong>and</strong> served as an <strong>on</strong>going resource<br />

pers<strong>on</strong> for underst<strong>and</strong>ing the mathematics<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent. Three of the four workshops involved<br />

training <strong>on</strong> the mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

the fourth served as a debriefing for the study.<br />

In the first two workshops, the teacher received<br />

training in the first two units (algebra<br />

<strong>and</strong> geometry); in the third workshop two<br />

units were trained (data analysis <strong>and</strong> measurement.)<br />

During each workshop, the research<br />

team provided an introducti<strong>on</strong> to the “big<br />

idea” of the unit (e.g., geometry addressed<br />

spatial organizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> vocabulary related to<br />

coordinate planes) <strong>and</strong> a review of current<br />

research in teaching mathematics to support<br />

special educators’ underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the mathematics<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard. The general mathematics<br />

teacher was asked to provide examples of how<br />

this st<strong>and</strong>ard was typically taught. The researcher<br />

modeled <strong>on</strong>e less<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> then the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher was given an opportunity<br />

to practice implementing the less<strong>on</strong><br />

with feedback until the read-aloud, use of the<br />

graphic organizer, <strong>and</strong> task-analytic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

could be presented without error. Although<br />

the math <strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong> teachers<br />

also practiced planning inclusive co-taught<br />

math less<strong>on</strong>s, these were not implemented<br />

during the course of the study due to logistics<br />

of the setting.<br />

Following each of the first three professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development workshops, the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher implemented the less<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

the special educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms with the target<br />

students in a group format. The teacher<br />

adapted the materials for any students’ individual<br />

needs (e.g., poster size versi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

number line in algebra, use of popsicle sticks<br />

to draw line segments in geometry). During<br />

each less<strong>on</strong>, the teacher read the word problem<br />

story aloud as students followed using<br />

their copies of the story. Then, each student<br />

was given the opportunity to perform each<br />

step of the task analysis while the other stu-<br />

dents in the group watched. The teacher used<br />

least intrusive prompting as needed for the<br />

student to make each target resp<strong>on</strong>se (n<strong>on</strong><br />

specific verbal directi<strong>on</strong>, specific verbal directi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

model, <strong>and</strong> physical guidance) <strong>and</strong> provided<br />

praise for each correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. The<br />

teacher varied the order of student resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

each day. Materials c<strong>on</strong>tained a variety of<br />

stories so that the problem to be solved <strong>and</strong><br />

specific numbers to compute varied while<br />

keeping the basic math strategy (e.g., use of a<br />

bar graph) c<strong>on</strong>stant. The teacher c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>, adding her own stories as needed,<br />

until the next unit was introduced. The researcher<br />

observed the teacher to assess procedural<br />

fidelity by observing whether each step<br />

of the task analysis was presented correctly. A<br />

procedural fidelity checklist was also used for<br />

the teacher training days to be sure the researchers<br />

included the overview, model, <strong>and</strong><br />

teacher practice. A graduate assistant who was<br />

not an author recorded these data.<br />

Results<br />

Procedural Fidelity<br />

Procedural fidelity was recorded for all four<br />

math workshops <strong>and</strong> found to be 100%. The<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher was observed eleven<br />

times (i.e., two to three times for each unit) to<br />

assess fidelity of teaching the task analyses.<br />

Procedural fidelity was computed as percentage<br />

of steps taught correctly. The teacher implemented<br />

the less<strong>on</strong> plans with 100% fidelity<br />

for all less<strong>on</strong>s observed. Two researchers c<strong>on</strong>currently<br />

scored fidelity of the less<strong>on</strong>s for 36%<br />

(four) of the observati<strong>on</strong>s. Agreement between<br />

the observers was 100%.<br />

Inter-rater Reliability<br />

The primary data collector was a graduate<br />

research assistant who was a special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

doctoral student. Independent scoring by two<br />

observers was performed <strong>on</strong> 40% of all assessment<br />

probes administered. Interobserver<br />

agreement was computed as agreements divided<br />

by agreements plus disagreements. The<br />

percent agreement was 99% <strong>and</strong> adherence to<br />

the task analytic assessment protocol was<br />

100% for all sessi<strong>on</strong>s observed.<br />

380 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Student Achievement<br />

Figures 3–6 provide the total number of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses across each of the four math<br />

units. Within each unit of instructi<strong>on</strong> skill<br />

maintenance is reported.<br />

Claire. During unit 1: geometry, Claire increased<br />

the number of independent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M 1.3, range from<br />

1 to 2) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 5.1, range from 3<br />

to 7). During unit 2: algebra, Claire increased<br />

in the total number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from<br />

baseline (M 3, range from 0 to 5) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 3.9, range from 0 to 7). During<br />

unit 3: data-analysis, Claire increased in the<br />

total number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline<br />

(M .6, range from 0 to 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 4.3, range from 2 to 7). Finally,<br />

during unit 4: measurement, Claire increased<br />

in the total number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from<br />

baseline (M .33, range from 0 to 1) to<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 2, range from 0 to 6, see<br />

Figure 3).<br />

Kiernan. During unit 1: geometry, Kiernan<br />

increased the number of independent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 6, range from 3 to 9). During unit<br />

2: algebra, Kiernan increased in the total<br />

number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline<br />

(M 1.7, range from 1 to 3) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 6.3, range from 1 to 10). During unit 3:<br />

data-analysis, Kiernan increased in the total<br />

number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline<br />

(M .6, range from 0 to 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 4.3, range from 2 to 7). Finally, during<br />

unit 4: measurement, Kiernan increased in<br />

the total number the number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

from baseline (M .5, range from 0<br />

to 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 2.6, range from 0 to<br />

4, see Figure 4).<br />

Everett. During unit 1: geometry, Everett<br />

increased the number of independent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M .33, range from<br />

0 to 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 1.7, range from 0<br />

to 3). During unit 2: algebra, Everett increased<br />

in the total number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

from baseline (M 1, range from 0 to<br />

3) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 4.7, range from 1 to<br />

7). During unit 3: data-analysis, Everett increased<br />

in the total number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

from baseline (M .2, range from 0<br />

to 1) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 2.7, range from 0 to<br />

5). Finally, during unit 4: measurement, Ever-<br />

ett increased in the total number of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M 0) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 1.4, range from 0 to 2, see Figure<br />

5).<br />

Todd. During unit 1: geometry, Todd increased<br />

the number of independent correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M 4) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 7.4, range from 3 to 9). During unit<br />

2: algebra, Todd increased in the total number<br />

of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M <br />

4, range from 2 to 5) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 7.6,<br />

range from 4 to 10). During unit 3: dataanalysis,<br />

Todd increased in the total number<br />

of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline (M .8,<br />

range from 0 to 2) to interventi<strong>on</strong> (M 7.3,<br />

range from 5 to 9). Finally, during unit 4:<br />

measurement, Todd increased in the total<br />

number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses from baseline<br />

(M .6, range from 0 to 2) to interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 9.3, range from 7 to 10, see Figure 6).<br />

Table 3 indicates the mean number of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses students had from baseline to<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> across each unit of math instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Data for generalizati<strong>on</strong> of math skills are<br />

also reported in Table 3. All students had<br />

higher mean resp<strong>on</strong>ses during interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

maintained most steps of the math task analysis<br />

over time (e.g., geometry–18 weeks), <strong>and</strong><br />

generalized the skills to untaught problems.<br />

Social Validity<br />

At the final workshop, the teacher was asked<br />

to complete an adapted interventi<strong>on</strong> rating<br />

profile (Snyder, 2002) to indicate level of satisfacti<strong>on</strong><br />

with the training <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials. The teacher resp<strong>on</strong>ded to seven<br />

items about the interventi<strong>on</strong> using a six-point<br />

Likert scale (i.e., 1 str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree; 6 <br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly agree). The teacher agreed or<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly agreed with all items (mean of 5.75)<br />

that the math less<strong>on</strong> plan trainings were helpful<br />

<strong>on</strong> clarifying how to write less<strong>on</strong> plans that<br />

access the general curriculum in sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

grades. She felt that the less<strong>on</strong> plans were<br />

practical <strong>and</strong> strengthened her skills as a<br />

teacher.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

This study dem<strong>on</strong>strated that not <strong>on</strong>ly can<br />

students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual<br />

disability learn new math skills aligned to<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 381


grade-level c<strong>on</strong>tent, they can learn new math<br />

skills across the math str<strong>and</strong>ards (e.g., algebra,<br />

geometry). This adds to the work of Browder<br />

Figure 3. Student data across math units for Claire.<br />

et al. (2010) showing that a method of st<strong>and</strong>ards-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> that can be applied<br />

across different st<strong>and</strong>ards. Like Browder et al.<br />

382 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 4. Student data across math units for Everett.<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 383


Figure 5. Student data across math units for Kiernan.<br />

384 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Figure 6. Student data across math units for Todd.<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 385


TABLE 3<br />

Student mean scores across phase of interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> unit of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

this study used read-alouds of word problems,<br />

a graphic organizer, <strong>and</strong> task analytic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in how to solve the problem. This study<br />

adds to the earlier study by dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

that each of four students made gains <strong>on</strong> each<br />

mathematical st<strong>and</strong>ard.<br />

Although st<strong>and</strong>ards-based instructi<strong>on</strong> is required<br />

for students to meet state expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> alternate assessments, there are few research<br />

models for this type of instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Since the Browder et al. 2008 review, researchers<br />

have c<strong>on</strong>tinued to focus teaching purchasing<br />

<strong>and</strong> computati<strong>on</strong>s. Collins, Hager, <strong>and</strong><br />

Galloway (2011) focused <strong>on</strong> computati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

sales-tax, but within general educati<strong>on</strong> mathematical<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent. Skibo, Mims, <strong>and</strong> Spo<strong>on</strong>er<br />

(2011) used student resp<strong>on</strong>se cards <strong>and</strong> least<br />

intrusive prompting to teach number identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

to students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

intellectual disability. Zisimopoulos (2010)<br />

used a picture fading technique to teach students<br />

with moderate intellectual disability to<br />

recall multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts. While each of these<br />

studies provides an important c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing how to teach mathematics to<br />

this populati<strong>on</strong>, the current study provides<br />

evidence of a method to teach skills that align<br />

with grade-level c<strong>on</strong>tent st<strong>and</strong>ards.<br />

This study taught students how to resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

to word problems. The NCTM promotes a<br />

problem-solving approach to mathematics<br />

(2000). Van de Walle (2004) proposes that<br />

learning to solve story problems in mathematics<br />

is the basis for learning to solve more real-<br />

Kiernan Reese Everett Claire<br />

Geometry Baseline 1.3 1 .33 4<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> 5.1 6 1.7 7.4<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> 4.6 7.6 3.8 9<br />

Algebra Baseline 3 1.7 1 4<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> 7.1 6.3 4.7 7.6<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> 7 9 6 10<br />

Data Analysis Baseline .6 .6 .2 .8<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> 4.3 4.3 2.7 7.3<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> 5 5 3 8.7<br />

Measurement Baseline .33 .5 0 .6<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> 2 2.6 1.4 9.3<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> 4.5 n/a n/a n/a<br />

world problems. Fuchs, Fuchs, Finelli, Courey,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hamlett (2004) note that mathematical<br />

problem solving involves students applying<br />

skills to novel situati<strong>on</strong>s. Teaching word problems<br />

can teach students the “when” <strong>and</strong> “why”<br />

to apply mathematical skills.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, we did not teach students how<br />

to identify the type of problem to be solved<br />

which is typically the focus of research <strong>on</strong><br />

teaching word problems. Instead, the teacher<br />

presented the graphic organizer to cue the<br />

student what type of problem this was (e.g.,<br />

data comparis<strong>on</strong> versus algebraic equati<strong>on</strong>.)<br />

Browder et al.’s (2008) review revealed <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e study that focused <strong>on</strong> teaching students a<br />

problem-solving schema. Neef, Nelles, Iwata,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Page (2003) taught math problem solving<br />

to <strong>on</strong>e student with a moderate intellectual<br />

disability (i.e., a sec<strong>on</strong>d participant had mild<br />

intellectual disability). Neef et al. taught students<br />

“precurrent operati<strong>on</strong>s” to facilitate<br />

problem solving. Specifically, the students<br />

learned to identify five comp<strong>on</strong>ents of word<br />

problems: the initial set, the change set, the<br />

operati<strong>on</strong>, the result set, <strong>and</strong> the soluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Students used a graphic organizer worksheet<br />

to enter known informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> find the soluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The interventi<strong>on</strong> included massed<br />

practice trials with a teacher model. Both students<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated generalized problem solving.<br />

Much more research is needed to determine<br />

how best to teach this populati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

recognize the type of problem presented in<br />

the math story.<br />

386 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


A sec<strong>on</strong>d limitati<strong>on</strong> of the current study is<br />

that while all students made gains, the gains in<br />

the measurement unit were minimal for three<br />

of the four students. This may have been an<br />

artifact of the specific task analysis <strong>on</strong> counting<br />

the next dollar amount. While the other<br />

task analyses <strong>on</strong>ly required the students to<br />

solve <strong>on</strong>e problem, because the next dollar<br />

task analysis was short, students solved three<br />

problems <strong>and</strong> the data were added together. If<br />

the student could not perform some steps, this<br />

would occur all three times. Students may also<br />

have been less motivated to repeat these resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

three times with no reinforcement for<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

A third limitati<strong>on</strong> is that while the stories<br />

were focused <strong>on</strong> real life math applicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., going to the movies, shopping), the<br />

teacher did not assess generalizati<strong>on</strong> to these<br />

c<strong>on</strong>texts. The students did show generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

to untrained story problems. It is unknown<br />

whether they also would have generalized<br />

these to community c<strong>on</strong>texts. While the<br />

teacher did use some generalizati<strong>on</strong> activities<br />

(e.g., voting to practice data compilati<strong>on</strong>), no<br />

data were collected.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong> for Practice <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

This study provided evidence to support that<br />

students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual<br />

disability can learn middle school mathematics<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards with a read-aloud of word problems,<br />

task analytic instructi<strong>on</strong> to solve the<br />

problem, <strong>and</strong> graphic organizer. The stories<br />

used helped focus the instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> real life<br />

applicati<strong>on</strong>s that are important to make the<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards-based instructi<strong>on</strong> meaningful (e.g.,<br />

going to the movies.) In replicating these less<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with students, educators should c<strong>on</strong>sider<br />

stories that apply to students’ local envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

(e.g., story <strong>on</strong> Charlotte Speedway<br />

would not be relevant in some c<strong>on</strong>texts.) The<br />

graphic organizers may also need to be modified<br />

for students’ visual or physical limitati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

For example, the teacher found some<br />

students resp<strong>on</strong>ded better if the graphic organizer<br />

was enlarged to poster size.<br />

Future research is needed to determine if<br />

this strategy may be applicable to st<strong>and</strong>ards in<br />

other grade levels (e.g., elementary or high<br />

school), to students with other types of disabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to other state st<strong>and</strong>ards. Research<br />

also is needed to determine if this method is<br />

the most effective for repetitive skills like<br />

counting m<strong>on</strong>ey since this produced the lowest<br />

gains. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, research evidence is<br />

needed to determine if this read-aloud problem<br />

solving strategy could be embedded in a<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text. For example,<br />

could peers c<strong>on</strong>duct the read-aloud? Finally,<br />

research also is needed <strong>on</strong> how students generalize<br />

the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of mathematics st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

to everyday activities.<br />

References<br />

Aeschleman, S. R., & Schladenauffen, A. F. (1984).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cept learning by retarded children: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of three discriminati<strong>on</strong> learning procedures.<br />

Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 26,<br />

229–238.<br />

Anders<strong>on</strong>, R. C., Spiro, R. J., & Anders<strong>on</strong>, M. C.<br />

(1978). Schemata as scaffolding for the representati<strong>on</strong><br />

of informati<strong>on</strong> in c<strong>on</strong>nected discourse.<br />

American Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research Journal, 15, 433–<br />

440.<br />

Browder, D. M., Snell, M. E., & Wild<strong>on</strong>ger, B.<br />

(1988). Simulati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> community-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

of vending machines with time delay. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 175–<br />

185.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L.,<br />

Wakeman, S. Y., & Harris, A. (2008). A metaanalysis<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching mathematics to students with<br />

significant cognitive disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

74, 407–432.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., & Jimenez, K. (2011).<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ards-based individualized educati<strong>on</strong> plans <strong>and</strong><br />

progress m<strong>on</strong>itoring. In D. Browder & F. Spo<strong>on</strong>er<br />

(Eds.), Teaching students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

disabilities (pp. 168–200). New York, NY: Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

Browder, D. M., Trela, K., Courtade, G. R., Jimenez,<br />

B. A., Knight, V., & Flowers, C. (2010). Teaching<br />

mathematics <strong>and</strong> science st<strong>and</strong>ards to students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe developmental disabilities.<br />

The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>. Advance <strong>on</strong>line<br />

publicati<strong>on</strong>. doi:10.1177/0022466910369942<br />

Browder, D. M., Trela, K., & Jimenez, B. (2007).<br />

Training teachers to follow a task analysis to engage<br />

middle school students with moderate <strong>and</strong><br />

severe developmental disabilities in grade-appropriate<br />

literature. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 22, 206–219.<br />

Collins, B. C., Hager, K. L., & Galloway, C. C.<br />

(2011). Additi<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent during<br />

core c<strong>on</strong>tent instructi<strong>on</strong> with students with mod-<br />

Grade-Aligned Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Math / 387


erate disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 46, 22–39.<br />

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Finelli, R., Courey, S. J., &<br />

Hamlett, C. L. (2004). Exp<strong>and</strong>ing schema-based<br />

transfer instructi<strong>on</strong> to help third graders solve<br />

real-life mathematical problems. American Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Research Journal, 41, 419–445. doi: 10.3102/<br />

00028312041002419<br />

Garjria, M., Jitendra, A. K., Sood, S., & Sacks, G.<br />

(2007). Improving comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of expository<br />

text in students with LD: A research synthesis.<br />

Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 210–225.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2010). Single-subject research in behavioral<br />

sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.<br />

Gersten, R., Fuchs, L., Compt<strong>on</strong>, D., Coyne, M.,<br />

Greenwood, C., & Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality<br />

indicators for group experimental <strong>and</strong> quasiexperimental<br />

research in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 71, 149–164.<br />

Haring, T. G., Kennedy, G. H., Adams, M. J., &<br />

Pitts-C<strong>on</strong>way, V. (1987). Teaching generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of purchasing skills across community settings to<br />

autistic youth using videotape modeling. Journal<br />

of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 89–96.<br />

Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe<br />

technique: A variati<strong>on</strong> of the multiple baseline.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189–196.<br />

Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G.,<br />

Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of singlesubject<br />

research to identify evidence-based practice<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71,<br />

165–180.<br />

Jimenez, B. A., Browder, D. M., & Courtade, G.<br />

(2008). Teaching an algebraic equati<strong>on</strong> to high<br />

school students with moderate developmental disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 266–274.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers of Mathematics.<br />

(1989). Curriculum <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards school<br />

mathematics. Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Author.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers of Mathematics<br />

(NCTM; 2000). Principles <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards for school<br />

mathematics. Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Author.<br />

Neef, N. A., Nelles, D. E., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. P.<br />

(2003). Analysis of precurrent skills in solving<br />

mathematics story problems. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 36, 21–33.<br />

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No.<br />

107–110, 115, § 1425, 20 U.S.C. §§ 6301 et seq.<br />

(2002).<br />

Pugalee, D. K. (2005). Writing for mathematical under-<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ing. Norwood, MA: Christopher Gord<strong>on</strong><br />

Publishers.<br />

Ryndak, D. L., & Alper, S. (1996). Curriculum c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

for students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities in<br />

inclusive settings. Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: Allyn & Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ryndak, D., & Alper, S. (2003). The curriculum<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>and</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> process: Rati<strong>on</strong>ale <strong>and</strong><br />

overview. Curriculum <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with significant disabilities in inclusive settings (pp.<br />

1–112). Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: Allyn & Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Skibo, H., Mims, P., & Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2011). Teaching<br />

number identificati<strong>on</strong> to students with severe disabilities<br />

using resp<strong>on</strong>se cards. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

46, 124–133.<br />

Snell, M. E., & Brown, F. (Eds.). (2000). Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with severe disabilities (5 th ed.). Upper<br />

Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Snell, M. E., & Brown, F. (Eds.). (2006). Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with severe disabilities (6 th ed.). Columbus,<br />

OH: Merrill.<br />

Snyder, E. P. (2002). Teaching students with combined<br />

behavioral disorders <strong>and</strong> mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

to lead their own IEP meetings. Behavioral<br />

Disorders, 27, 340–357.<br />

Van de Walle, J. A. (2004). Elementary <strong>and</strong> middle<br />

school mathematics: Teaching developmentally (5 th<br />

ed.). Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: Allyn & Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Westling, D. L., & Fox, L. (2000). Teaching students<br />

with severe disabilities (2 nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs,<br />

NJ: Merrill.<br />

Westling, D. L., & Fox, L. (2004). Teaching students<br />

with severe disabilities (3 rd ed.). Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Woodward, J., & M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (2002). Meeting the<br />

challenge of mathematics reform for students<br />

with LD. The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 36, 89–<br />

101.<br />

Zambo, R. (2005). The power of two: Linking mathematics<br />

<strong>and</strong> literature. Mathematics Teaching in<br />

Middle School, 10, 394–399.<br />

Zisimopoulos, D. A. (2010). Enhancing multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

performance in students with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities using pegword mnem<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

paired with a picture fading technique. Journal of<br />

Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 19, 117–133.<br />

Received: 6 July 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 18 September 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 15 December 2011<br />

388 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2012, <str<strong>on</strong>g>47</str<strong>on</strong>g>(3), 389–400<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Review of Academic Mathematics Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Students<br />

with Mild Intellectual Disability<br />

Casey Hord<br />

University of Cincinnati<br />

Emily C. Bouck<br />

Purdue University<br />

Abstract: Mathematics educati<strong>on</strong>–like all educati<strong>on</strong>–faced changes in recent years including increasing<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> these expectati<strong>on</strong>s have impacted all students, including students with mild intellectual<br />

disability. To explore the impact of the changes <strong>on</strong> mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> students with mild intellectual<br />

disability, the authors reviewed the literature from 1999–2010 <strong>on</strong> academic mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

students with mild intellectual disability. Of the seven articles found, the majority focused <strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

designed to improve knowledge of mathematical facts <strong>and</strong> computati<strong>on</strong>al procedures. One study included an<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> involving metacogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> diagramming of mathematical relati<strong>on</strong>ships to help students solve<br />

word problems. Results suggest a diss<strong>on</strong>ance between the mathematics focus of mathematics professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> federal <strong>and</strong> state policies regarding higher order mathematics <strong>and</strong> perhaps current practice<br />

for students with mild intellectual disability, as reflected in the research literature.<br />

Mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> is a core c<strong>on</strong>tent area<br />

with direct benefits for advanced educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> employment for all students (Algozzine,<br />

O’Shea, Crews, & Stoddard, 1987; Maccini,<br />

McNaught<strong>on</strong>, & Ruhl, 1999; Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council<br />

of Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 2000;<br />

No Child Left Behind [NCLB], 2002; Xin,<br />

Jitendra, & Deatline-Buchman, 2005). Although<br />

mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> has always<br />

been important, its stake in educati<strong>on</strong> has<br />

risen over the past few decades. Recent federal<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> laws mixed with rising expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of nati<strong>on</strong>al professi<strong>on</strong>al organizati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

changes in educati<strong>on</strong>al policy altered what is<br />

expected of all students, including students<br />

with disabilities (NCTM; NCLB; Teuscher,<br />

Dingman, Nevels, & Reys, 2008; Woodward,<br />

2004).<br />

The latest reauthorizati<strong>on</strong> of the Elementary<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sec<strong>on</strong>dary Educati<strong>on</strong> Act (ESEA)–<br />

called No Child Left Behind (NCLB, 2002)–<br />

required all students to participate in each<br />

state’s accountability system, meaning students<br />

were to be tested annually in grades 3–8<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ce again in high school in reading,<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Casey Hord, PO Box 210002,<br />

Teachers/Dyer Hall, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati,<br />

OH 45221. Email: casey.hord@uc.edu<br />

mathematics, <strong>and</strong> science. Further, schools<br />

were to show Adequately Yearly Progress<br />

(AYP) to account for all students’ improvement<br />

as well as that of particular subgroups,<br />

such as students with disabilities, <strong>on</strong> their<br />

state’s accountability system (i.e., typically the<br />

large-scale general assessment in the reading,<br />

mathematics, <strong>and</strong> science) (Yell & Drasgow,<br />

2005). Not <strong>on</strong>ly were students with disabilities<br />

included in the general educati<strong>on</strong> law but also<br />

the latest reauthorizati<strong>on</strong> of Individuals with<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act (IDEA, 2004)<br />

aligned itself with NCLB regarding students<br />

with disabilities participating in the accountability<br />

system as well promoting access to the<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum (Turnbull,<br />

Huerta, & Stowe, 2006).<br />

Also c<strong>on</strong>tributing to rising expectati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

students with disabilities in mathematics educati<strong>on</strong><br />

were changes in high school graduati<strong>on</strong><br />

requirements, including exit exams<br />

(Ysseldyke et al., 2004) <strong>and</strong> m<strong>and</strong>ated upper<br />

level mathematics classes (Teuscher et al.,<br />

2008), as well as increasingly rigorous c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

area st<strong>and</strong>ards proposed by professi<strong>on</strong>al organizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., NCTM, 2000) <strong>and</strong> the recent<br />

Comm<strong>on</strong> Core St<strong>and</strong>ards (Council of Chief<br />

State School Officers <strong>and</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al Governors<br />

Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2010). Exit exams, affecting 28<br />

states as of 2010, require a student to pass a<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 389


test to graduate from high school; failure can<br />

result in a certificate of attendance rather<br />

than a diploma (Center <strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Policy<br />

[CEP], 2010; Reard<strong>on</strong>, Arshan, Atteberry, &<br />

Kurlaender, 2010). Regarding courses in high<br />

school, some states now require students to<br />

pass high school algebra <strong>and</strong>/or geometry to<br />

earn a diploma (Mack 2010; Teuscher et al.).<br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d the NCTM st<strong>and</strong>ards, typically adopted<br />

by states for their grade level mathematics<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards, 2010 resulted in a majority of<br />

states adopting the Comm<strong>on</strong> Core st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

for all students (CCSSO <strong>and</strong> NGA Center,<br />

2010). The Comm<strong>on</strong> Core addresses st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

for students in grades K-12 in exp<strong>and</strong>ed<br />

literacy <strong>and</strong> mathematics <strong>and</strong> focus <strong>on</strong> college<br />

<strong>and</strong> career readiness (CCSSO & NGA).<br />

Students with Mild Intellectual Disability<br />

Given the increased emphasis <strong>on</strong> not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> but also rigorous<br />

mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> for all students, <strong>on</strong>e<br />

must questi<strong>on</strong> how much access to the general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum <strong>and</strong> what type of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

are students with disabilities receiving.<br />

Increasing expectati<strong>on</strong>s for higher st<strong>and</strong>ardized<br />

test scores for students with<br />

disabilities, including students with mild intellectual<br />

disability (MID), not <strong>on</strong>ly led to intense<br />

pressure for teachers to help students<br />

raise test scores, but also a need for researchers<br />

to develop interventi<strong>on</strong>s for helping students<br />

with MID <strong>and</strong> their teachers resp<strong>on</strong>d to<br />

the pressure for high test scores (Maccini,<br />

Mulcahy, & Wils<strong>on</strong>, 2007; Popham, 2001;<br />

Woodward, 2004). More importantly, it is necessary<br />

to underst<strong>and</strong> the access students with<br />

MID are being given to the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

curriculum to determine if these students are<br />

being given an opportunity to leave K-12 educati<strong>on</strong><br />

with the procedural <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of mathematics they need be<br />

successful in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary (i.e., academic,<br />

occupati<strong>on</strong>al, or daily living) settings <strong>and</strong> as<br />

participating citizens in a democracy (Cohen,<br />

1999; Lesh, Post, & Behr, 1988; NCTM, 2000;<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, Cr<strong>on</strong>in, Bassett, & Koppel, 1997; Vergnaud,<br />

1983).<br />

In the United States, a student with MID is<br />

an individual who has “significantly subaverage<br />

general intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing, existing<br />

c<strong>on</strong>currently with deficits in adaptive behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> manifested during the developmental period,<br />

that adversely affects a child’s educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

performance” (IDEA, 2004). According<br />

to Polloway, Patt<strong>on</strong>, Smith, <strong>and</strong> Buck (1997)<br />

students with MID experience “related limitati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in two or more of the following applicable<br />

adaptive skill areas: communicati<strong>on</strong>, selfcare,<br />

home living, social skills, community use,<br />

self directi<strong>on</strong>, health <strong>and</strong> safety, functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

academics, leisure, <strong>and</strong> work” (p. 298). In<br />

schools, students with MID typically have IQs<br />

between 55 <strong>and</strong> 70, although the American<br />

Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Intellectual <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> (2011)–a major organizati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the United States that advocates for individuals<br />

with intellectual disability–classifies individuals<br />

with IQs between 50 to 75 range as<br />

having MID (Bouck, in press).<br />

Researchers identified general characteristics<br />

of students with MID including slow academic<br />

growth, low academic performance,<br />

<strong>and</strong> poor post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary outcomes (Parmar,<br />

Cawley, & Miller, 1994; Sabornie, Evans, &<br />

Cullinan, 2006). Researchers hypothesized<br />

some of these difficulties are likely due to<br />

challenges students with MID face in working<br />

memory, particularly with storing informati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2006; Baddeley,<br />

2003; Schuchardt, Gebhard & Mäehler, 2010).<br />

While some informati<strong>on</strong> is available for underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

students with MID, often in research<br />

students with MID are aggregated with students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual<br />

disability or with students with other high<br />

incidence disabilities such as learning disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> emoti<strong>on</strong>al/behavior disorders (Polloway,<br />

Lubin, Smith, & Patt<strong>on</strong>, 2010). Practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

<strong>and</strong> researchers would benefit from<br />

more disaggregated data <strong>on</strong> students with<br />

MID regarding characteristics, interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong> outcomes specific to this populati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Mathematics Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Students with MID<br />

Historically, mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with MID has been divided into two<br />

main areas, not unlike the general dichotomy<br />

of curriculum for this populati<strong>on</strong> of students:<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum <strong>and</strong> an academicallyoriented<br />

approach (Alwell & Cobb, 2009;<br />

Browder, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Wakeman, Trela, & Baker,<br />

2006). Although a recent review exists <strong>on</strong><br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al mathematical curriculum received<br />

390 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


y students with intellectual disability (note, not<br />

disaggregated for students with MID) (Browder,<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-Delzell, Harris, & Wakeman,<br />

2008), the last thorough review <strong>on</strong> academic<br />

mathematical instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with mildto-moderate<br />

intellectual disability predates the<br />

current decade <strong>and</strong> hence current reforms<br />

(Butler, Miller, Lee, & Pierce, 2001).<br />

Butler <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2001) reviewed the<br />

literature <strong>on</strong> mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with mild-to-moderate intellectual disability<br />

published between 1989 <strong>and</strong> 1998. In<br />

the 16 articles found for these two populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during the time frame, Butler et al. found a<br />

shift in instructi<strong>on</strong>al focus from a curriculum<br />

based <strong>on</strong> basic skills (e.g., numeracy, mathematical<br />

symbols, equality, etc.) to a focus <strong>on</strong><br />

computati<strong>on</strong>al fluency <strong>and</strong> mathematical<br />

problem solving (e.g., tasks involving organizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> analysis of informati<strong>on</strong>). Further,<br />

Butler et al. noted the increased attenti<strong>on</strong> to<br />

developing students’ procedural <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing, such as research supporting<br />

teaching students with mild-to-moderate<br />

intellectual disability through use of strategy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for problem solving, self-regulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>crete-semic<strong>on</strong>crete-abstract<br />

teaching sequence. The shift in instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

methods coincided with recommendati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

shifting emphasis in mathematics educati<strong>on</strong><br />

in general, as supported by the Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989) as<br />

well as supported the positi<strong>on</strong> of researchers<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong> who called for greater<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> to problem-solving <strong>and</strong> the development<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing (e.g., Jitendra<br />

& Xin, 1997; Woodward & Howard, 1994).<br />

The shifting emphasis of instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

methods for students with intellectual disability<br />

also aligned with research suggesting students<br />

with MID may possess strengths in mathematics<br />

regarding higher level thinking skills,<br />

such as the ability to create <strong>and</strong> maintain strategies,<br />

utilize metacognitive skills, <strong>and</strong> develop<br />

at least some levels of c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of mathematical relati<strong>on</strong>ships <strong>on</strong> an abstract<br />

level (Baroody, 1996; Erez & Peled,<br />

2001). In a group study including twenty-four<br />

elementary, middle, <strong>and</strong> high school students<br />

with mild-to-moderate intellectual disability,<br />

some students were able to independently create<br />

more efficient strategies (e.g., short-cuts<br />

such as “counting <strong>on</strong>”) after receiving explicit<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for less efficient, more time-c<strong>on</strong>suming<br />

strategies (e.g., “counting all”) when<br />

working <strong>on</strong> single digit additi<strong>on</strong> problems<br />

(Baroody). Erez <strong>and</strong> Peled also found some<br />

middle <strong>and</strong> high school students with mild-tomoderate<br />

intellectual disability used metacognitive<br />

skills, such as reexamining choices<br />

made during problem solving processes <strong>and</strong><br />

basing future acti<strong>on</strong>s up<strong>on</strong> these reflecti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

to solve additi<strong>on</strong> word problems. Some of<br />

these students also independently developed<br />

an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the structure of these<br />

problems <strong>and</strong> the abstract mathematical relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

within the problems (e.g., part-partwhole)<br />

(Erez & Peled).<br />

While schools are expected to prepare students<br />

with MID for daily resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities <strong>on</strong>ce<br />

they leave K-12 educati<strong>on</strong>, students with MID<br />

also need a variety mathematical skills to meet<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>s placed up<strong>on</strong> them by educati<strong>on</strong>al laws<br />

(i.e., IDEA, 2004; NCLB, 2002) <strong>and</strong> high school<br />

graduati<strong>on</strong> requirements (Teuscher et al.,<br />

2008). Students with disabilities, including students<br />

with mild intellectual disability, need access<br />

to opportunities to develop c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

for success with more complex<br />

mathematics in middle school, high school, <strong>and</strong><br />

post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> (Lesh et al., 1988;<br />

Vergnaud, 1983; Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

2002). While Butler et al. (2001) indicated students<br />

with MID were receiving instructi<strong>on</strong> more<br />

rooted in mathematical c<strong>on</strong>cepts than in previous<br />

years, the questi<strong>on</strong> remains regarding<br />

whether students with MID c<strong>on</strong>tinued to receive<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> needed to develop the foundati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

mathematics for success when c<strong>on</strong>sidering both<br />

procedural <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing. The<br />

specific research questi<strong>on</strong> for this study is: What<br />

is the nature of the academic instructi<strong>on</strong> the<br />

students with MID have received in mathematics<br />

in the past 11 years?<br />

Method<br />

A systematic review of the literature was completed<br />

of academic mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for students with MID from 1999 to 2010. The<br />

researchers chose this period of time as Butler<br />

et al. (2001) reviewed research <strong>on</strong> this topic<br />

through 1998 <strong>and</strong> multiple reforms impacting<br />

mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> occurred in the last<br />

decade (e.g., Comm<strong>on</strong> Core St<strong>and</strong>ards, 2010;<br />

IDEA, 2004; NCTM, 2000). Studies were in-<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 391


cluded if (a) the sample included at least <strong>on</strong>e<br />

student with MID enrolled in school (i.e., a<br />

student between the ages of 3 <strong>and</strong> graduati<strong>on</strong><br />

or exiting school <strong>and</strong> was referred to as a<br />

student with MID by the authors of the study<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or the IQ reported in the study was between<br />

50 <strong>and</strong> 75); (b) the study focused <strong>on</strong> an<br />

academic mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>, operati<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

defined as an interventi<strong>on</strong> targeting a<br />

skill area for advancement in school mathematics<br />

rather than daily living skills such purchasing<br />

groceries or counting m<strong>on</strong>ey; (c) the<br />

article was published in a peer-reviewed journal<br />

in English between 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2010; <strong>and</strong><br />

(d) the research occurred in the United<br />

States. Comm<strong>on</strong> reas<strong>on</strong>s articles were excluded<br />

included (a) the descripti<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

participants was unspecified; (b) the data for<br />

participants with MID were aggregated with<br />

data for other disabilities <strong>and</strong> could not be<br />

isolated; (c) the term MID (or something<br />

equivalent) was used by the authors, but all<br />

participants had IQs outside of the 50–75<br />

range; <strong>and</strong>/or (d) the studies were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in foreign countries where MID is defined<br />

differently (often with IQs up to 85) (e.g.,<br />

Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2005). Articles focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> assessment (e.g., identificati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

students for special educati<strong>on</strong> services); descripti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of characteristics of students with<br />

disabilities; <strong>and</strong> positi<strong>on</strong> papers were also excluded.<br />

Multiple methods were used to locate articles<br />

meeting the criteria. First, a keyword<br />

search was completed utilizing four electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

databases: PsychInfo, ERIC, Wils<strong>on</strong> Select,<br />

<strong>and</strong> ProQuest. Due to the number of terms<br />

used syn<strong>on</strong>ymously with MID, articles that<br />

used the terms mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, developmental<br />

disability, cognitive disability,<br />

mild intellectual disability, <strong>and</strong> mild cognitive<br />

impairment were included in the study (S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1998). For the keyword search, math*<br />

was combined with the following terms: mental<br />

impairment, mild cognitive, mild intellectual,<br />

mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> mild developmental.<br />

The researchers also used the<br />

search terms, intellectual <strong>and</strong> cognitive<br />

disab*, <strong>and</strong> evaluated the IQ of the participants<br />

within these studies.<br />

Next, the researchers reviewed both general<br />

<strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong> journals for articles<br />

meeting the criteria published between the<br />

years 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2010. A h<strong>and</strong> search was completed<br />

by looking for relevant articles in the<br />

following journals: Journal for Research in Mathematics<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, The Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>. During the<br />

h<strong>and</strong> search, the researchers evaluated articles<br />

by looking for the search terms described<br />

above as well as any terms that might be relevant<br />

to this study (e.g., students with mild<br />

disabilities, math difficulties, etc.), but not<br />

specifically included in the search terms in the<br />

computer database search. Finally, the researchers<br />

completed an ancestral search using<br />

the references of the articles meeting criteria<br />

for inclusi<strong>on</strong> in this study. All of the articles<br />

were located in electr<strong>on</strong>ic databases using keyword<br />

searches except for <strong>on</strong>e article which was<br />

located by h<strong>and</strong>-searching the selected journals.<br />

The ancestral search did not result in<br />

finding any articles that met inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria.<br />

Results<br />

Seven studies were found addressing academic<br />

mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>s for students<br />

with MID published between the years 1999<br />

<strong>and</strong> 2010 (see Table 1). The participants<br />

ranged in ages from children in elementary<br />

school to a 23-year-old adult in an after high<br />

school program. The majority of the studies<br />

<strong>on</strong> mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> students with<br />

MID (i.e., six of seven) were focused <strong>on</strong> procedural<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>; in other words, mathematics<br />

facts or computati<strong>on</strong> involving basic<br />

arithmetic (e.g., additi<strong>on</strong>, subtracti<strong>on</strong>, multiplicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> divisi<strong>on</strong> facts <strong>and</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

algorithmic procedures). The other study–<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual instructi<strong>on</strong>–involved<br />

an interventi<strong>on</strong> based <strong>on</strong> the use of mathematical<br />

models, prompts for cogniti<strong>on</strong>/metacogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

related to problem structure <strong>and</strong><br />

algebraic procedures for solving word problems.<br />

Procedural Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Flashcards. Flashcards were used in four of<br />

the six studies focused <strong>on</strong> procedural instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

each designed to improve the mathematical<br />

fact knowledge of students with MID (Dihoff,<br />

Brosvic, Epstein, & Cook, 2005; Hayter,<br />

392 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


TABLE 1<br />

Studies of Mathematics Interventi<strong>on</strong>s for Students with MID: 1999–2010<br />

Citati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Sante,<br />

Laughlin,<br />

& Weber<br />

(2001)<br />

Neef, Nelles,<br />

Iwata, &<br />

Page<br />

(2003)<br />

Dihoff,<br />

Brosvic,<br />

Epstein, &<br />

Cook<br />

(2005)<br />

Hayter, Scott,<br />

McLaughlin,<br />

& Weber<br />

(2007)<br />

Bouck,<br />

Bassette,<br />

Taber-<br />

Doughty,<br />

Flanagan,<br />

& Szwed<br />

(2009)<br />

Rao & Mallow<br />

(2009)<br />

Rao & Kane<br />

(2009)<br />

Flashcards for learning<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

Meta-cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

model-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

additi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> word<br />

problems<br />

Students received<br />

feedback from the<br />

teacher while<br />

completing multiple<br />

choice questi<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se to<br />

flashcards<br />

Flashcards for leaning<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

Pentop computer for<br />

teaching<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

computati<strong>on</strong><br />

Simultaneous<br />

prompting system<br />

for automatic recall<br />

of multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts<br />

Simultaneous<br />

prompting system<br />

for regrouping<br />

during decimal<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

Scott, McLaughlin, & Weber, 2007; Rao &<br />

Mallow, 2009; Sante, McLaughlin, & Weber,<br />

2001). Sante <strong>and</strong> colleagues c<strong>on</strong>ducted a sin-<br />

#of<br />

Subjects<br />

with<br />

MID Setting Design Results<br />

2 Middle school<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom<br />

1 After high school<br />

program for<br />

students with<br />

intellectual<br />

disabilities<br />

55 Elementary<br />

school<br />

2 High school selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom<br />

3 Middle school<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom<br />

1 Middle school<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom<br />

2 Middle school<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom<br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

participants<br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

behaviors<br />

Group<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

participants<br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

participants<br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

participants<br />

Multiple baseline<br />

across<br />

participants<br />

Students’ rate of<br />

accuracy with<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

improved.<br />

Student’s word problem<br />

solving performance<br />

improved.<br />

Students’ retenti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

arithmetic facts<br />

(additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> &<br />

divisi<strong>on</strong>) improved.<br />

The skills learned<br />

were maintained by<br />

the participants for<br />

about <strong>on</strong>e-to-two<br />

weeks.<br />

Student showed higher<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

improved accuracy<br />

<strong>on</strong> multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts.<br />

Students’ performance<br />

with multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

computati<strong>on</strong><br />

improved <strong>and</strong> was<br />

maintained by the<br />

participants.<br />

Students’ accuracy <strong>and</strong><br />

speed for recalling<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

improved <strong>and</strong> was<br />

maintained <strong>and</strong><br />

generalized.<br />

Students maintained<br />

<strong>and</strong> generalized the<br />

skill of successful<br />

decimal subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

procedures.<br />

gle subject design study focused <strong>on</strong> improving<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts that included two sixthgrade<br />

participants with MID <strong>and</strong> ADHD. Both<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 393


participants performed at a third-grade mathematics<br />

level <strong>and</strong> were educated in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> mathematics class.<br />

The researchers found sixteen-to-seventeen<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s of presenting flashcards with <strong>on</strong>e<br />

digit factors <strong>and</strong> providing feedback regarding<br />

correctness of answers improved the multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

factual knowledge of the students,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the researchers recommended using this<br />

approach to enhance mathematical performance.<br />

Hayter <strong>and</strong> colleagues also used a single<br />

subject research design to study the impact of<br />

flashcards (c<strong>on</strong>taining factors ranging from<br />

four to ten) followed by informing the student<br />

of the answer <strong>on</strong> helping high school students<br />

with MID learn multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts. After<br />

eleven sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the researchers reported an<br />

increased accuracy with multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

<strong>and</strong>, after the participants experienced success,<br />

their motivati<strong>on</strong> to solve the tasks improved.<br />

After the participants were encouraged<br />

by their success, they made their own<br />

flashcards for studying for another class.<br />

Also using a single-subject research design,<br />

Rao <strong>and</strong> Mallow (2009) evaluated flashcards<br />

as an interventi<strong>on</strong> to help a middle school<br />

student with MID learn multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts,<br />

but combined the visual aspect of flashcards<br />

with teacher prompts (i.e., the teacher provided<br />

the answer al<strong>on</strong>g with the presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the flashcard). The interventi<strong>on</strong> supported<br />

growth in the participant’s factual knowledge<br />

of multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts with factors ranging<br />

from zero to twelve, <strong>and</strong> improved his speed<br />

<strong>and</strong> accuracy. The participant was able to<br />

maintain his knowledge of multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

for over a three-m<strong>on</strong>th period <strong>and</strong> made generalizati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to situati<strong>on</strong>s when the student<br />

needed to recall multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts while<br />

solving l<strong>on</strong>g divisi<strong>on</strong> problems using a traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

algorithm as well as from the special to<br />

the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom.<br />

The final flashcard study involved a group<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of 55 elementary school students<br />

with MID (Dihoff et al., 2005). Dihoff et al.<br />

used flashcards with multiple choice answers<br />

listed <strong>on</strong> the flashcards to fact questi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, subtracti<strong>on</strong>, multiplicati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> divisi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The researchers prompted elementary<br />

school students with MID when incorrect to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sider the alternative choices <strong>on</strong> the flashcards.<br />

In some situati<strong>on</strong>s, the researchers pro-<br />

vided similar prompts using a bubbled answer<br />

sheet with wax covers over each answer<br />

choice. After students removed the wax coating<br />

of their answer choice, a star under the<br />

wax coating indicated correct answer while a<br />

lack of a star prompted the participants they<br />

were incorrect. In both cases, the participants<br />

were able to keep making choices until they<br />

found the correct answer. The participants<br />

were able to retain the knowledge they gained<br />

from the practice al<strong>on</strong>g with the prompts <strong>and</strong><br />

feedback for time periods ranging from five<br />

days to two weeks.<br />

Computati<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong>. In <strong>on</strong>e of two<br />

studies focused <strong>on</strong> computati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Rao <strong>and</strong> Kane (2009) used a single subject<br />

design to study teacher prompts for two middle<br />

school students with MID to follow procedures<br />

for using traditi<strong>on</strong>al algorithms for subtracting<br />

decimals. The participants were given<br />

step-by-step instructi<strong>on</strong>s for each procedure<br />

involved (e.g., recognizing the need for regrouping<br />

as well as how to complete the regrouping<br />

procedures). The skills for completing<br />

the traditi<strong>on</strong>al algorithm for subtracting<br />

decimals were maintained for the remainder<br />

of the school year <strong>and</strong> generalized to subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems with different regrouping requirements<br />

<strong>and</strong> to real-world situati<strong>on</strong>s such<br />

as using a spreadsheet for solving m<strong>on</strong>eyrelated<br />

problems associated with pers<strong>on</strong>al finances<br />

<strong>and</strong> business budgets.<br />

In another single subject study, Bouck, Bassette,<br />

Taber-Doughty, Flanagan, & Szwed<br />

(2009) used technology as an interventi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

improve participants’ computati<strong>on</strong>al knowledge.<br />

Bouck <strong>and</strong> colleagues evaluated the<br />

FLYPen TM –a pentop computer produced by<br />

LeapFrog–as a tool to improve the multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

fluency of three middle school students<br />

with MID. The technology benefited students<br />

with both learning multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts as well<br />

as multiplicati<strong>on</strong> computati<strong>on</strong> skills with involving<br />

a single-digit numbers <strong>and</strong> two-digit<br />

numbers (e.g., 35 7). The immediate feedback<br />

students received from the technology<br />

when they made a mistake was noted as helping<br />

students.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

In the <strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>ceptual instructi<strong>on</strong> study, Neef,<br />

Nelles, Iwata, & Page (2003) used a single<br />

394 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


subject design to provide an interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

two students–<strong>on</strong>e student with MID–<strong>on</strong> how<br />

to solve additi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> subtracti<strong>on</strong> word problems.<br />

Neef <strong>and</strong> colleagues described their interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

as teaching precurrent skills (i.e.,<br />

“resp<strong>on</strong>ses that increase the effectiveness of a<br />

subsequent or ‘current’ behavior in obtaining<br />

a reinforcer,” p. 21). The participants placed<br />

mathematical signs <strong>and</strong> problem elements in<br />

a mathematical model that included a box for<br />

each number or variable in the problem, the<br />

mathematical operati<strong>on</strong>s sign, <strong>and</strong> the equal<br />

sign. After placing the known problem elements<br />

<strong>and</strong> correct mathematical signs in the<br />

model, the participants were instructed to use<br />

basic algebraic procedures to solve for the<br />

unknown in simple equati<strong>on</strong>s such as “A ?<br />

C” (p. 25). Students were encouraged to<br />

think about the structure of the problem via<br />

prompting from the teacher such as “How<br />

many objects did (name) start out with, end<br />

up with, get, lose, etc.” (p. 27). The participants’<br />

word problem solving accuracy improved<br />

after receiving the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Maintenance<br />

of skills was not tested by the<br />

researchers due to time c<strong>on</strong>straints nor was<br />

the generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the skills to other problem<br />

types.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Given the current emphasis <strong>on</strong> mathematics<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for all students, including students<br />

with MID, it is imperative to determine the<br />

nature of existing research <strong>on</strong> mathematical<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for these students <strong>and</strong> use this<br />

knowledge to inform further practice <strong>and</strong> research.<br />

This study focused <strong>on</strong> research in the<br />

past decade involving academic mathematical<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for students with MID–a populati<strong>on</strong><br />

lacking in attenti<strong>on</strong> (Polloway, 2006;<br />

Polloway et al., 2010)–from age three to exiting<br />

school. The results suggest two main findings:<br />

(a) little attenti<strong>on</strong> in research to academically-based<br />

mathematical interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

students with MID with no current studies<br />

focusing <strong>on</strong> mathematics st<strong>and</strong>ards at or<br />

above middle school levels of general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

curriculum, <strong>and</strong> (b) of the limited existing<br />

research, most studies are primarily focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> procedural instructi<strong>on</strong> (e.g.,<br />

mathematical facts <strong>and</strong> computati<strong>on</strong>al flu-<br />

ency) rather than c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

(e.g., problem-solving).<br />

The limited research <strong>on</strong> mathematics educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with MID is discouraging<br />

although perhaps not surprising. Even though<br />

mathematics received increased attenti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

the past decade due the educati<strong>on</strong>al climate<br />

(e.g., NCLB, 2002), historically less research<br />

has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> students with disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> mathematics than literacy (Fuchs &<br />

Fuchs, 2005; Fuchs et al., 2007). The lack of<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> in the last decade to mathematics<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> students with MID is problematic<br />

for multiple reas<strong>on</strong>s, including difficulty<br />

in articulating evidence-based mathematical<br />

practices for this populati<strong>on</strong> without a research<br />

base as well as the dissociati<strong>on</strong> with<br />

current practice <strong>and</strong> policy regarding the importance<br />

of mathematics educati<strong>on</strong>, especially<br />

rigorous mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> for all students<br />

(IDEA, 2004; NCLB, 2002; NCTM, 2000;<br />

Woodward, 2004). Clearly, this systematic review<br />

highlights the need for more research in<br />

academic mathematical interventi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with MID, although from the existing<br />

recent research we can gleam the prioritized<br />

pedagogical approaches.<br />

Procedural <strong>and</strong> C<strong>on</strong>ceptual Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Within the limited existing research <strong>on</strong> mathematics<br />

for students with MID, the majority<br />

(n6) focused <strong>on</strong> procedural instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

rather than c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing. In<br />

other words, current research prioritized the<br />

memorizati<strong>on</strong> of mathematical facts or steps<br />

taken to complete mathematics problems<br />

(e.g., using a traditi<strong>on</strong>al algorithm) rather<br />

than underst<strong>and</strong>ing mathematical c<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

<strong>and</strong> using reas<strong>on</strong>ing skills to make <strong>and</strong> explain<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s made during problem solving<br />

processes (NCTM, 2000). The lack of attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

in research to c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

for this populati<strong>on</strong> of students is also at odds<br />

with the current mathematics policy, educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

reforms, <strong>and</strong> recommendati<strong>on</strong>s by general<br />

<strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong> researchers (Lesh<br />

et al., 1988; Nati<strong>on</strong>al Math Advisory Panel Report,<br />

2008; NCTM, 2000; Ostad, 1998; van<br />

Garderen, 2007; Vergnaud, 1983; Woodward,<br />

2004; Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague, 2002). Special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> researchers have recommended<br />

greater emphasis <strong>on</strong> developing critical think-<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 395


ing skills about mathematics <strong>and</strong> deeper c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of mathematical ideas<br />

to empower students with knowledge that is<br />

transferable to various situati<strong>on</strong>s rather than<br />

knowledge of procedures specific to certain<br />

mathematical situati<strong>on</strong>s (van Garderen, 2007;<br />

Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague, 2002). Mathematical<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing can<br />

help students achieve higher levels in mathematics<br />

<strong>and</strong> face increased expectati<strong>on</strong>s for academic<br />

performance in K-12 educati<strong>on</strong><br />

(IDEA, 2004; NCLB, 2002; NCTM, 2000;<br />

Woodward, 2004).<br />

While some might questi<strong>on</strong> the need for<br />

more rigorous focused mathematics educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with MID, it is imperative for<br />

multiple reas<strong>on</strong>s: (a) these students are capable<br />

of being successful with these pedagogical<br />

approaches if given a chance (Baroody, 1996;<br />

Jimenez, Browder, & Courtade, 2008; Erez &<br />

Peled, 2001; Neef et al., 2003), (b) a decreased<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> procedures or rote recall is<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistent with research suggesting students<br />

with MID struggle with working memory<br />

(Schuchardt et al., 2010), <strong>and</strong> (c) helping<br />

these students to be successful <strong>on</strong> high-stakes<br />

assessments (NCLB, 2002; Woodward, 2004).<br />

First, although limited in quantity, the little<br />

research <strong>on</strong> more c<strong>on</strong>ceptual instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with intellectual disability dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

success. Neef et al. taught students<br />

with MID how to use basic algebraic procedures<br />

(e.g., solving for the unknown) while<br />

solving word problems. Related, in a study by<br />

Jimenez et al., students with IQs below 50 were<br />

successful with solving for an unknown in an<br />

algebraic equati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., 3 a 5). These<br />

findings, when c<strong>on</strong>sidered al<strong>on</strong>g with those of<br />

Baroody (1996) <strong>and</strong> Erez <strong>and</strong> Peled (2001),<br />

suggest students with MID possess a number<br />

of skills related to higher order mathematical<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> the field may need to raise<br />

its expectati<strong>on</strong>s for what these students can–<br />

<strong>and</strong> should be expected–to do.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, students with MID struggle with<br />

working memory (Schuchardt et al., 2010).<br />

Rather than focus <strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al approaches<br />

steeped in storage <strong>and</strong> recall, practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

can focus <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>ceptual underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

<strong>and</strong> use technology to circumvent student<br />

struggles (NCTM, 2000; Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

2002). Students with MID experience<br />

success using calculators (Bouck et al., 2009;<br />

Hort<strong>on</strong>, Lovitt, & White, 1992). Calculators<br />

can be used as a cognitive prosthesis (Edyburn,<br />

2006) <strong>and</strong> teachers can then potentially<br />

devote more instructi<strong>on</strong>al time to developing<br />

a thorough <strong>and</strong> deep underst<strong>and</strong>ing of mathematical<br />

ideas <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between those<br />

ideas–instructi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistent with recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

by NCTM (2000) <strong>and</strong> even special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> researchers (Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague).<br />

Last, higher level mathematical ability <strong>and</strong><br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing is needed by students with<br />

MID if they are expected to take general largescale<br />

assessment (Bouck, 2007). Given that<br />

students with MID across all grade levels typically<br />

take the same assessment as their peers<br />

without disabilities (Bouck; Yell & Drasgow,<br />

2005; Ysseldyke et al., 2004), instructi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

mathematics for this populati<strong>on</strong> needs to focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> the areas that can promote student<br />

success. If these students are expected to take<br />

the general large-scale assessment, they<br />

should be given general educati<strong>on</strong> academic<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> to increase the opportunity for<br />

success (Bouck, 2007). While assessment<br />

should not drive instructi<strong>on</strong>al practice<br />

(Faulkner & Cook, 2006; Shepard, 2010), students<br />

with MID have to be given a fighting<br />

chance to be successful by covering the c<strong>on</strong>tent.<br />

Hence, research should focus <strong>on</strong> how to<br />

help students with MID be successful in mathematics<br />

classes if the field of educati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinues<br />

the practice of high stakes testing.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for Practice<br />

The results of this review of the literature<br />

suggest practices exist that assist students with<br />

MID in ascertaining basic facts as well as c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of mathematical ideas,<br />

although more research is needed <strong>on</strong> the latter.<br />

Multiple studies in the review suggest<br />

teachers can rely <strong>on</strong> flashcards as an educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

tool for teaching mathematical facts to<br />

students with MID (Dihoff et al., 2005; Hayter<br />

et al., 2007; Rao & Mallow, 2009; Sante et al.,<br />

2001). However, teachers need to make careful<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s about how much time is devoted<br />

to memorizati<strong>on</strong> of facts as compared to critical<br />

thinking about mathematical ideas <strong>and</strong><br />

the c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s between these ideas (NCTM,<br />

2000; van Garderen, 2007; Woodward & M<strong>on</strong>tague,<br />

2002).<br />

396 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Neef <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2003) dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

some of the benefits of diagramming informati<strong>on</strong><br />

in word problems. Diagramming <strong>and</strong><br />

other forms of visual representati<strong>on</strong> may also<br />

be included in instructi<strong>on</strong> to give students<br />

with MID opportunities for access to more<br />

challenging mathematics (van Garderen,<br />

2006). By teaching students with MID methods<br />

for storing informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> paper while<br />

working through challenging, multistep problems,<br />

teachers give these students an important<br />

tool for overcoming deficits in working<br />

memory which may have previously c<strong>on</strong>tributed<br />

to the struggles of some students with<br />

MID in mathematics (Allen et al., 2006; Baddeley,<br />

2003; Barrouillet, Bernardin, Portrat,<br />

Vergauwe, & Camos, 2007; Schuchardt et al.,<br />

2010). Instructi<strong>on</strong>al principles applied by<br />

Neef <strong>and</strong> colleagues may be also applicable to<br />

various situati<strong>on</strong>s in which students with MID<br />

are likely to need help with storing <strong>and</strong> organizing<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> using metacogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

to complete proper analysis of the mathematical<br />

problem <strong>and</strong> the steps taken to solve it.<br />

Teachers can apply these principles to multiple<br />

mathematical situati<strong>on</strong>s as well as in other<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent areas when students with MID feel<br />

overwhelmed with the amount <strong>and</strong> complexity<br />

of informati<strong>on</strong> they need to store, organize,<br />

<strong>and</strong> process.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Future Directi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

A challenge exists when studying students with<br />

MID due to variati<strong>on</strong> in ways of defining <strong>and</strong><br />

labeling intellectual disability, <strong>and</strong> thus researchers<br />

face dilemmas when determining<br />

criteria for participant inclusi<strong>on</strong> (S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1998). In this study, the criteria was based <strong>on</strong><br />

IQ scores which can be problematic due to<br />

variati<strong>on</strong> of student performance that often<br />

occurs within the 50–75 IQ range (Cawley &<br />

Parmar, 1995; Fletcher, Blair, Scott, & Bolger,<br />

2004). However, the researchers chose IQ for<br />

determinati<strong>on</strong> of inclusi<strong>on</strong> of participants due<br />

to the ambiguity of the MID label especially<br />

when c<strong>on</strong>sidering studies d<strong>on</strong>e outside the<br />

United States where a student can be labeled<br />

as having MID due to having an IQ of up to 85<br />

in countries (e.g., the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s). While it<br />

is necessary to be specific about participant<br />

characteristics when reporting findings, the<br />

drawback of excluding studies d<strong>on</strong>e in foreign<br />

countries, as well as following strict guidelines<br />

for participant IQs, is the exclusi<strong>on</strong> of some<br />

important studies in mathematics for students<br />

with disabilities completed recently by researchers<br />

in other nati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Chung & Tam, 2005;<br />

Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2003; Kroesbergen &<br />

Van Luit, 2005). Therefore, some important<br />

studies to the field may have been excluded due<br />

to the strict, yet necessary, inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria set<br />

by the researchers in this study.<br />

Aside from difficulties with defining <strong>and</strong><br />

labeling students with MID, the lack of informati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> characteristics of students with<br />

MID also presents a challenge. To better study<br />

students with MID, it may be necessary to<br />

more thoroughly investigate the characteristics<br />

of students with MID by specifically studying<br />

this populati<strong>on</strong> (as in Schuchardt et al.,<br />

2010) rather than aggregating data with other<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>s which is a comm<strong>on</strong> problem <strong>and</strong><br />

has made it difficult to determine the characteristics<br />

<strong>and</strong> outcomes for students with MID<br />

(Polloway et al., 2010). Also, due to the lack of<br />

research <strong>on</strong> mathematics <strong>and</strong> students with<br />

MID, it may be important for researchers to<br />

examine the performance of these students<br />

using qualitative research designs to gain insight<br />

into the specifics of how students with<br />

MID underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> reas<strong>on</strong> about mathematics.<br />

After a foundati<strong>on</strong> of research regarding<br />

mathematical reas<strong>on</strong>ing as well as the characteristics<br />

(e.g., memory <strong>and</strong> processing abilities)<br />

of students with MID has been established,<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s may be more effectively<br />

developed. Eventually, c<strong>on</strong>sidering that teachers<br />

are expected to use evidence-based practices<br />

to guide their instructi<strong>on</strong>, researchers<br />

need to provide a more thorough research<br />

base regarding effective interventi<strong>on</strong>s for students<br />

with MID so teachers can have a better<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong> up<strong>on</strong> which to build their instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

practices.<br />

References<br />

* Indicates articles used in this review of the literature<br />

Allen, R. J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). Is<br />

the binding of visual features in working memory<br />

resource-dem<strong>and</strong>ing? Journal of Experimental Psychology,<br />

135, 298–313.<br />

Algozzine, B., O’Shea, D. J., Crews, W. B., & Stoddard,<br />

K. (1987). Analysis of mathematics compe-<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 397


tence of learning disabled adolescents. The Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 21, 97–107.<br />

Alwell, M., & Cobb, B. (2009). Functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills<br />

curricular interventi<strong>on</strong>s for youth with disabilities:<br />

A systematic review. Career Development for<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Individuals, 32, 82–93.<br />

American Associati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Intellectual <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>. (2011). Definiti<strong>on</strong> of Intellectual<br />

Disability. Retrieved from http://aamr.org/<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent_100.cfm?navID21<br />

Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory <strong>and</strong> language:<br />

An overview. Journal of Communicati<strong>on</strong> Disorders,<br />

36, 189–208.<br />

Baroody, A. J. (1996). Self-invented additi<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

by children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. American<br />

Journal of Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 101, 72–89.<br />

Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe,<br />

E., & Camos, V. (2007). Time <strong>and</strong> cognitive load<br />

in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:<br />

Learning, Memory, <strong>and</strong> Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 33, 570–585.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (in press). Intellectual disability/mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. In J. A. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of<br />

Diversity in Educati<strong>on</strong>. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2007). Lost in Translati<strong>on</strong>?: Educating<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental impairment.<br />

Journal of Disability Policy Studies, 18, 79–87.<br />

*Bouck, E. C., Bassette, L., Taber-Doughty, T.,<br />

Flanagan, S. M., & Szwed, K. (2009). Pentop computers<br />

as tools for teaching multiplicati<strong>on</strong> to students<br />

with MID. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 367–380.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L.,<br />

Harris, A. A., & Wakeman, S. (2008) A metaanalysis<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching mathematics to students with<br />

significant cognitive disabilities, Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

74, 407–432.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Wakeman, S., Trela,<br />

K., & Baker, J. N. (2006). Aligning instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

with academic c<strong>on</strong>tent st<strong>and</strong>ards: Finding the<br />

link. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 31, 309–321.<br />

Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Lee, K., & Pierce, T.<br />

(2001). Teaching mathematics to students with<br />

mild-to-moderate mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A review of<br />

the literature. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 39, 20–31.<br />

Cawley J. F., & Parmar, R. (1995). Comparis<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

reading <strong>and</strong> reading-related tasks am<strong>on</strong>g students<br />

with average intellectual ability <strong>and</strong> students with<br />

mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>.<br />

30, 118–129.<br />

Center <strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Policy. (2010). State high<br />

school tests: Exit exams <strong>and</strong> other assessments.<br />

Retrieved from http://www.cep-dc.org/cfc<strong>on</strong>tent_<br />

file.cfm?AttachmentCEP%5FHSEE10report%5<br />

F01%2E05%2E11%2Epdf<br />

Chung, K. K. H., & Tam, Y. H. (2005). Effects of<br />

cognitive-based instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> mathematical<br />

problem solving by learners with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. Journal of Intellectual & <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disability, 30, 207–216.<br />

Cohen, P. (1999). A calculating people: The spread of<br />

numeracy in early America. New York: Routledge.<br />

Council of Chief State School Officers <strong>and</strong> Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Governors Associati<strong>on</strong>. (2010). Comm<strong>on</strong> Core<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ards. Retrieved from http://www.core<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ards.org/about-the-st<strong>and</strong>ards/key-points-inmathematics.<br />

*Dihoff, R. E., Brosvic, G. M., Epstein, M. L., &<br />

Cook, M. J. (2005). Adjunctive role for immediate<br />

feedback in the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> retenti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mathematical fact series by elementary school students<br />

classified with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

Psychological Record, 55, 39–66.<br />

Edyburn, D. L. (2006). Cognitive prostheses for students<br />

with mild disabilities: Is this what assistive<br />

technology looks like? Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 21(4), 62–65.<br />

Erez, G., & Peled, I., (2001). Cogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> metacogniti<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Evidence of higher thinking in problem-solving<br />

of adolescents with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 36, 83–93.<br />

Faulkner, S. A., & Cook, C. (2006). Testing vs. teaching:<br />

The perceived impact of assessment dem<strong>and</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> middle grades instructi<strong>on</strong>al practices. Research<br />

in Middle Level Educati<strong>on</strong> Online, 29(7), 1–13.<br />

Fletcher, K. L., Blair, C., Scott, M. S., & Bolger, K. E.<br />

(2004). Specific patterns of cognitive abilities in<br />

young children with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

39, 270–278.<br />

Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (2005). Enhancing mathematical<br />

problem solving for students with disabilities.<br />

The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 39, 45–57.<br />

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Compt<strong>on</strong>, D. L., Bryant,<br />

J. D., Hamlett, C. L., & Seethaler, P. M. (2007).<br />

Mathematics screening with progress m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

at first grade: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for resp<strong>on</strong>siveness to<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 73, 311–330.<br />

*Hayter, S., Scott, E., McLaughlin, T. F., & Weber,<br />

K. P. (2007). The use of a modified direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

flashcard system with two high school students<br />

with developmental disabilities. Journal of<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> & Physical <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 19, 409–415.<br />

Hort<strong>on</strong>, S. V., Lovitt, T. C., & White, O. R. (1992).<br />

Teaching mathematics to adolescents classified as<br />

educable mentally h<strong>and</strong>icapped: Using calculators<br />

to remove the computati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>on</strong>us. Remedial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 36–60.<br />

Jimenez, B. A., Browder, D. M., & Courtade, G. R.<br />

(2008). Teaching an algebraic equati<strong>on</strong> to high<br />

school students with moderate developmental dis-<br />

398 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


abilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 266–274.<br />

Jitendra, A. K., & Xin, Y. P. (1997). Mathematical<br />

word-problem-solving instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with mild disabilities <strong>and</strong> students at risk for math<br />

failure: A research synthesis. The Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 30, 412–438.<br />

Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2003).<br />

Mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>s for children with special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al needs: A meta-analysis. Remedial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 24(2), 97–114.<br />

Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2005).<br />

C<strong>on</strong>structivist mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. European Journal<br />

of Special Needs Educati<strong>on</strong>, 20, 107–116.<br />

LeapFrog© (2008). Pers<strong>on</strong>al learning tools. Retrieved<br />

from http://www.leapfrog.com/en/pages/<br />

support/top_product_list/fly_fusi<strong>on</strong>.html<br />

Lesh, R., Post, T., & Behr, M. (1988). Proporti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>ing. In J. Hiebert & M. Behr (Eds.), Number<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong> operati<strong>on</strong>s in the middle grades (pp. 93–<br />

118). Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers<br />

of Mathematics.<br />

Maccini, P., McNaught<strong>on</strong>, D., & Ruhl, K. L. (1999).<br />

Algebra instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with learning disabilities:<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s from a research review.<br />

Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 113–126.<br />

Maccini, P., Mulcahy, C. A., & Wils<strong>on</strong>, M. G. (2007).<br />

A follow-up of mathematics interventi<strong>on</strong>s for sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

students with learning disabilities. Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> Research & Practice, 22, 58–74.<br />

Mack, J. (2010, September 7). Higher expectati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Class of 2011: Most rise to meet new st<strong>and</strong>ards,<br />

but Algebra II a c<strong>on</strong>cern, Michigan educators<br />

say. Kalamazoo Gazette. Retrieved from http://<br />

www.mlive.com/news/kalamazoo/index.ssf/2010/<br />

09/higher_expectati<strong>on</strong>s_for_class.html<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers of Mathematics.<br />

(1989). Curriculum <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards for<br />

school mathematics. Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Author.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers of Mathematics.<br />

(2000). Principles <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards for school mathematics.<br />

Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: Author.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). The<br />

final report of the nati<strong>on</strong>al mathematics advisory panel.<br />

Retrieved June 10, 2008, from http://www.ed.<br />

gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/index.html<br />

*Neef, N. A., Nelles, D. E., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. J.<br />

(2003). Analysis of precurrent skills in solving<br />

mathematics story problems. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 36, 21–33.<br />

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No.<br />

107–110, 115 Stat. 1425 (2002).<br />

Ostad, S. A., (1998). <strong>Developmental</strong> differences in<br />

solving simple arithmetic word problems <strong>and</strong> simple<br />

number-fact problems: A comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mathematically normal <strong>and</strong> mathematically disabled<br />

children. Mathematical Cogniti<strong>on</strong>, 4, 1–19.<br />

Parmar, R. S., Cawley, J. F., & Miller, J. H. (1994).<br />

Differences in mathematics performance between<br />

students with learning disabilities <strong>and</strong> students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

60, 549–564.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E., Bassett, D. S., & Koppel,<br />

A. E. (1997). A life skills approach to mathematics<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> preparing students with learning disabilities<br />

for the real-life math dem<strong>and</strong>s of adulthood.<br />

Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 178–187.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2006). Mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept in search of clarity, a populati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

search of appropriate educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> supports, a<br />

professi<strong>on</strong> in search of advocacy. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

14, 183–190.<br />

Polloway, E. A., Lubin, J., Smith, J. D., & Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R.<br />

(2010). MID: Legacies <strong>and</strong> trends in c<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

<strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al practices. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 54–68.<br />

Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Smith, T. E. C., & Buck,<br />

G. H. (1997). Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

disabilities: C<strong>on</strong>ceptual <strong>and</strong> applied issues. Journal<br />

of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>. 30, 297–308.<br />

Popham, W. J. (2001). The truth about testing: An<br />

educator’s call to acti<strong>on</strong>. Alex<strong>and</strong>ria, VA: Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Supervisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Curriculum Development.<br />

*Rao, S., & Mallow, L. (2009). Using simultaneous<br />

prompting procedure to promote recall of multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts by middle school students with<br />

cognitive impairment. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 80–90.<br />

*Rao, S., & Kane, M. T. (2009). Teaching students<br />

with cognitive impairment chained mathematical<br />

task of decimal subtracti<strong>on</strong> using simultaneous<br />

prompting. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 244–256.<br />

Reard<strong>on</strong>, S. F., Arshan, N., Atteberry, A., & Kurlaender,<br />

M. (2010). Effects of failing a high<br />

school exit exam <strong>on</strong> course taking, achievement,<br />

persistence, <strong>and</strong> graduati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong>al Evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Policy Analysis. 32, 498–520.<br />

Sabornie, E. J., Evans, C., & Cullinan, D. (2006).<br />

Comparing characteristics of high-incidence disability<br />

groups: A descriptive review. Remedial <strong>and</strong><br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 27(2), 95–104.<br />

S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong>, R. (1998). A survey <strong>on</strong> terminology that<br />

refers to people with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>/developmental<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

33, 290–295.<br />

*Sante, A. D., McLaughlin, T. F., & Weber, K. P.<br />

(2001). The use <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

flash card strategy <strong>on</strong> multiplicati<strong>on</strong> math<br />

facts mastery with two students with developmental<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> deficit hyperactivity<br />

disorder. Journal of Precisi<strong>on</strong> Teaching <strong>and</strong> Celerati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

17(2), 68–75.<br />

Mathematics <strong>and</strong> Students with MID / 399


Schuchardt, K., Gebhardt, M., & Mäehler, C.<br />

(2010). Working memory functi<strong>on</strong>s in children<br />

with different degrees of intellectual disability.<br />

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 346–<br />

353.<br />

Shepard, L. A. (2010). What the marketplace has<br />

brought us: Item-by-item teaching with little instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

insight. Peabody Journal of Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

85, 246–257.<br />

Teuscher, D., Dingman, S., Nevels, N., & Reys, B.<br />

(2008). Curriculum st<strong>and</strong>ards, course requirements,<br />

<strong>and</strong> m<strong>and</strong>ated assessments for high school<br />

mathematics. Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Supervisors of<br />

Mathematics Journal of Mathematics Educati<strong>on</strong> Leadership,<br />

10(2), 49–54.<br />

Turnbull, R., Huerta, N., & Stowe, M. (2006). The<br />

Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act as<br />

Amended in 2004. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice<br />

Hall.<br />

United States Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2004).<br />

Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement<br />

Act of 2004. Retrieved from http://idea.ed.<br />

gov/explore/view/p/%2Croot%2Cdynamic%2CT<br />

opicalBrief%2C3%2C<br />

van Garderen, D. (2006). Spatial visualizati<strong>on</strong>, visual<br />

imagery, <strong>and</strong> mathematical problem solving of<br />

students with varying abilities. Journal of Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 39, 496–506.<br />

van Garderen, D. (2007). Teaching students with<br />

LD to use diagrams to solve mathematical word<br />

problems. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 540–<br />

553.<br />

Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures. In R.<br />

Lesh & M. L<strong>and</strong>au (Eds.), Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of mathematics<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong> processes (pp. 127–174). New York:<br />

Academic Press.<br />

Woodward, J. (2004). Mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

United States: Past <strong>and</strong> present. Journal of Learning<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37, 16–31.<br />

Woodward, J., & Howard, L. (1994). The misc<strong>on</strong>cepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of youth: Errors <strong>and</strong> their mathematical<br />

meaning. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 61, 126–136.<br />

Woodward, J., & M<strong>on</strong>tague, M. (2002). Meeting the<br />

challenge of mathematics reform for students<br />

with LD. The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 36, 89–<br />

101.<br />

Xin, Y. P., Jitendra, A. K., & Deatline-Buchman, A.<br />

(2005). Effects of mathematical word problemsolving<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> middle school students<br />

with learning problems. The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

39, 181–192.<br />

Yell, M. L., & Drasgow, E. (2005). No Child Left<br />

Behind: A guide for professi<strong>on</strong>als. Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Prentice Hall.<br />

Ysseldyke, J., Nels<strong>on</strong>, R., Christens<strong>on</strong>, S., Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

D. R., Dennis<strong>on</strong>, A., Triezenberg, H., . . . Hawes,<br />

M. (2004). What we know <strong>and</strong> need to know<br />

about the c<strong>on</strong>sequences of high-stakes testing for<br />

students with disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71,<br />

75–95.<br />

Received: 20 September 2011<br />

Initial Acceptance: 17 November 2011<br />

Final Acceptance: 10 January 2012<br />

400 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Editorial Policy<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> focuses <strong>on</strong> the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

ETADD invites research <strong>and</strong> expository manuscripts <strong>and</strong> critical review of the<br />

literature. Major emphasis is <strong>on</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> assessment, educati<strong>on</strong>al programming,<br />

characteristics, training of instructi<strong>on</strong>al pers<strong>on</strong>nel, habilitati<strong>on</strong>, preventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

community underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> provisi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Each manuscript is evaluated an<strong>on</strong>ymously by three reviewers. Criteria for acceptance<br />

include the following: relevance, reader interest, quality, applicability,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the field, <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omy <strong>and</strong> smoothness of expressi<strong>on</strong>. The review<br />

process requires two to four m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Viewpoints expressed are those of the authors <strong>and</strong> do not necessarily c<strong>on</strong>form to<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s of the editors or of the officers of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Submissi<strong>on</strong> of Manuscripts<br />

1. Manuscript submissi<strong>on</strong> is a representati<strong>on</strong> that the manuscript is the author’s<br />

own work, has not been published, <strong>and</strong> is not currently under c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

publicati<strong>on</strong> elsewhere.<br />

2. Manuscripts must be prepared according to the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s in the Publicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Manual of the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong> (Sixth Editi<strong>on</strong>, 2009).<br />

3. Each manuscript must have a cover sheet giving the names <strong>and</strong> affiliati<strong>on</strong>s of all<br />

authors <strong>and</strong> the address of the principal author.<br />

4. Graphs <strong>and</strong> figures should be originals or sharp, high quality photographic<br />

prints suitable, if necessary, for a 50% reducti<strong>on</strong> in size.<br />

5. Five copies of the manuscript al<strong>on</strong>g with a transmittal letter should be sent to the<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, Box 871811,<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1811.<br />

6. Up<strong>on</strong> receipt, each manuscript will be screened by the editor. Appropriate<br />

manuscripts will then be sent to c<strong>on</strong>sulting editors. Principal authors will receive<br />

notificati<strong>on</strong> of receipt of manuscript.<br />

7. The Editor reserves the right to make minor editorial changes which do not<br />

materially affect the meaning of the text.<br />

8. Manuscripts are the property of ETADD for a minimum period of six m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

All articles accepted for publicati<strong>on</strong> are copyrighted in the name of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>.<br />

9. Please describe subjects (or any other references to pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities)<br />

with a people first orientati<strong>on</strong>. Also, use the term intellectual disability (singular)<br />

to replace any previous term used to describe the populati<strong>on</strong> of students<br />

with significant limitati<strong>on</strong>s in intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior as<br />

manifested in the developmental period.


14th Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong>,<br />

Intellectual Disability &<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Research to Practice<br />

Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> &<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

On behalf of the Board of Directors for CEC‛s <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, may<br />

I extend an invitati<strong>on</strong> to join us in K<strong>on</strong>a, Hawaii, January 23-25, 2013, for this premier professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning opportunity!<br />

<br />

The 14 th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong>, Intellectual Disability <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

will integrate research <strong>and</strong> practice, reflecting the need for evidence-based strategies <strong>and</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s within this diverse field.<br />

The program features more than 100 lecture <strong>and</strong> poster presentati<strong>on</strong>s. Noted speaker sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

include:<br />

Helping Children with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> Learn: A New Model for Integrating<br />

Technology in 21st Century Classrooms<br />

Evidence-Based Practices for Students with Intellectual Disability <strong>and</strong> ASD<br />

Sense <strong>and</strong> Sensabilities: An Inside View <strong>on</strong> Sensory Issues, What They Look Like, Avoiding<br />

Them, <strong>and</strong> Working Through Ones That Occur<br />

Good Blood, Bad Blood: Science, Nature, <strong>and</strong> the Myth of the Kallikaks<br />

A Model for Social Skills Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Students with ASD<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference delegates may also attend an in-depth pre-c<strong>on</strong>ference training institute <strong>on</strong> ASD, led by<br />

Dr. Brenda Smith Myles. C<strong>on</strong>tinuing Educati<strong>on</strong> Units (CEU‛s) will be available for c<strong>on</strong>ference<br />

delegates.<br />

Our C<strong>on</strong>ference will be held at the beautiful Sherat<strong>on</strong> K<strong>on</strong>a, Resort & Spa at Keauhou Bay <strong>on</strong> the<br />

big isl<strong>and</strong> of Hawaii.<br />

For further informati<strong>on</strong>, please c<strong>on</strong>tact:<br />

Cindy Perras<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference Co-ordinator<br />

CEC-DADD<br />

cindy.perras@cogeco.ca<br />

www.daddcec.org

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!