01.08.2013 Views

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_47(3) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

pable of learning to read, a belief that children<br />

needed prerequisite skills in order to<br />

learn to read <strong>and</strong> a belief that other skills were<br />

more important to learn. These beliefs originated<br />

in the historical treatment of individuals<br />

with severe disabilities (Winzer, 1993) <strong>and</strong><br />

in the subsequent writings of influential researchers.<br />

Dolch <strong>and</strong> Bloomster (1937), for<br />

example, c<strong>on</strong>cluded that a child must have a<br />

mental age of seven or above to benefit from<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>. Kliewer <strong>and</strong> Biklen<br />

(2001) pointed to developmental research as<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>sible for creating a curriculum founded<br />

up<strong>on</strong> a hierarchy of sub-skills, for example<br />

attending skills must be secure before more<br />

advanced skills such as reading are taught.<br />

These beliefs effectively reduced the opportunities<br />

for children with significant cognitive<br />

disability to acquire literacy skills.<br />

Recent surveys indicate that parents <strong>and</strong><br />

teachers rate academic skills like reading, below<br />

skills such as communicati<strong>on</strong> skills,<br />

grooming skills <strong>and</strong> social skills (Agran, Alper,<br />

& Wehmeyer 2002; Flowers, Ahlgrim-Delzell,<br />

Browder, & Spo<strong>on</strong>er, 2005) for children with<br />

significant disabilities. Opportunities for students<br />

with significant cognitive disability to<br />

develop literacy skills have c<strong>on</strong>tinued to be<br />

replaced with less<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al skills, therapy<br />

skills, or avoided altogether (Kliewer &<br />

L<strong>and</strong>is, 1999).<br />

Historical Exclusi<strong>on</strong> from Research Studies<br />

In past studies <strong>on</strong> reading instructi<strong>on</strong> children<br />

with significant cognitive disability were not<br />

included. Chall’s (1996) major synthesis of<br />

research regarding effective reading instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that first <strong>and</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d graders,<br />

<strong>and</strong> students with a low IQ who received training<br />

in ph<strong>on</strong>ics, were ahead of their peers who<br />

used basal readers <strong>on</strong> word recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

oral reading. In c<strong>on</strong>sidering this synthesis retrospectively,<br />

it is not clear whether this includes<br />

students with significant cognitive disability<br />

or not. Low IQ could mean lowaverage,<br />

below average, or slow learner.<br />

Students with significant cognitive disability<br />

were not necessarily receiving schooling at the<br />

time this synthesis was c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>and</strong> probably<br />

not included in the studies.<br />

Children with significant cognitive disability<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be excluded from major reviews<br />

of the literature. The NRP (2000) did not<br />

examine students whose IQs fell outside of<br />

average range because the studies involving<br />

these groups did not meet other criteria for<br />

inclusi<strong>on</strong> in their analysis. Such exclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

makes it difficult to substantiate which methods<br />

of reading instructi<strong>on</strong> meet the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

needs of children with significant cognitive<br />

disability.<br />

Literature Supporting Sight Word Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

There are more published studies <strong>on</strong> sight<br />

word instructi<strong>on</strong> for children with significant<br />

disabilities than ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong>. In a<br />

meta-analysis of 32 single-subject studies <strong>on</strong><br />

the instructi<strong>on</strong> of sight words, Browder <strong>and</strong><br />

Xin (1998) found that there were c<strong>on</strong>siderable<br />

effects for sight word training. Most of<br />

these studies measured the number of words<br />

children could read correctly, <strong>and</strong> did not<br />

ascertain whether or not participants could<br />

use the words in a generalized way. In comparis<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Joseph <strong>and</strong> Seery (2004) found <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

seven studies published over a period of<br />

twelve years <strong>on</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics-based instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with significant cognitive disability.<br />

Ph<strong>on</strong>ics Instructi<strong>on</strong> for Children with Significant<br />

Cognitive Disability<br />

Although research <strong>on</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children with significant cognitive disability<br />

has been sparse, studies have shown that it can<br />

be effective. Four single-subject design studies<br />

examined the effectiveness of direct instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

with children with moderate intellectual<br />

disability. Participants in the studies had IQ<br />

scores of between 38–76 (Barbetta, Heward, &<br />

Bradley, 1993; Bradford, Shippen, Alberto,<br />

Houchins, & Flores, 2006; Flores, Shippen,<br />

Alberto, & Crowe, 2004; Waugh, Fredrick, &<br />

Alberto, 2009). The samples in these studies<br />

were small: six participants (Flores et al.,<br />

2004), three participants (Bradford et al.,<br />

2006), five participants (Barbetta et al., 1993)<br />

<strong>and</strong> three participants (Waugh et al., 2009).<br />

Notably, Waugh et al. (2009) c<strong>on</strong>ducted a<br />

study to determine if letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

was taught systematically would the participants<br />

be able to sound out new words using<br />

those letter sounds? The researchers found<br />

that participants were able to master some sets<br />

270 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-September 2012

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!