13.08.2013 Views

Ben-Elia, E. and Ettema, D. (2011) Changing commuters' behav- ior ...

Ben-Elia, E. and Ettema, D. (2011) Changing commuters' behav- ior ...

Ben-Elia, E. and Ettema, D. (2011) Changing commuters' behav- ior ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong>, E. <strong>and</strong> <strong>Ettema</strong>, D. (<strong>2011</strong>) <strong>Changing</strong> commuters’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

using rewards: A study of rush-hour avoidance. Transportation<br />

Research Part F Traffic Psychology <strong>and</strong> Behaviour, 14 (5). pp. 354-<br />

368. ISSN 1369-8478<br />

We recommend you cite the published version.<br />

The publisher’s URL is http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.<strong>2011</strong>.04.003<br />

Refereed: Yes<br />

(no note)<br />

Disclaimer<br />

UWE has obtained warranties from all depositors as to their title in the material<br />

deposited <strong>and</strong> as to their right to deposit such material.<br />

UWE makes no representation or warranties of commercial utility, title, or fitness<br />

for a particular purpose or any other warranty, express or implied in respect<br />

of any material deposited.<br />

UWE makes no representation that the use of the materials will not infringe<br />

any patent, copyright, trademark or other property or proprietary rights.<br />

UWE accepts no liability for any infringement of intellectual property rights<br />

in any material deposited but will remove such material from public view pending<br />

investigation in the event of an allegation of any such infringement.<br />

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR TEXT.


<strong>Changing</strong> commuters’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> using rewards: A study of<br />

rush-hour avoidance<br />

* (corresponding author)<br />

Eran <strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> * ,<br />

Centre for Transport <strong>and</strong> Society<br />

Faculty of Environment <strong>and</strong> Technology<br />

University of the West of Engl<strong>and</strong><br />

Frenchay Campus, Bristol, BS16 1QY, United Kingdom<br />

eran.ben-elia@uwe.ac.uk<br />

Dick <strong>Ettema</strong><br />

Urban <strong>and</strong> Regional research centre Utrecht<br />

Faculty of Geosciences<br />

Utrecht University<br />

P.O. Box 80115<br />

3508 TC, Utrecht, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s<br />

d.ettema@geo.uu.nl<br />

Key Words<br />

Attitudes, <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change, congestion, habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, information, motivation, reward.<br />

Abstract<br />

In a 13-week field study conducted in The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s, participants were provided with daily<br />

rewards – monetary <strong>and</strong> non-monetary, in order to encourage them to avoid driving during<br />

the morning rush-hour. Participants could earn a reward (money or credits to keep a<br />

Smartphone h<strong>and</strong>set), by driving to work earlier or later, by switching to another mode or by<br />

teleworking. The collected data, complemented with pre <strong>and</strong> post measurement surveys,<br />

were analyzed using longitudinal techniques <strong>and</strong> mixed logistic regression. The results<br />

assert that the reward is the main extrinsic motivation for discouraging rush-hour driving. The<br />

monetary reward exhibits diminishing sensitivity, whereas the Smartphone has endowment<br />

qualities. Although the reward influences the motivation to avoid the rush-hour, the choice<br />

how to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is influenced by additional factors including gender <strong>and</strong> education,<br />

scheduling considerations, habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, <strong>and</strong> cognitive factors regarding attitudes <strong>and</strong><br />

perceptions, as well as travel information availability factors.<br />

1


1. Introduction<br />

Congestion on urban roads throughout the European Union is increasing <strong>and</strong> is expected to<br />

worsen as the dem<strong>and</strong> for trip making increases <strong>and</strong> supply of road infrastructure remains<br />

limited (European Commission, 2006a, 2006b). Loading of excess dem<strong>and</strong> on the<br />

transportation system has considerable external costs such as pollution, noise <strong>and</strong> road user<br />

safety (Mayeres et al., 1996). Road overloading disrupts vehicle flow, increases the<br />

frequency of incidents <strong>and</strong> magnifies the uncertainty of travel schedules (Lomax & Schrank,<br />

2003). Congestion is a collective, synchronic phenomenon: massive commuting at a more or<br />

less common time-frame (e.g. the morning rush-hour). Thus, shifting of commuters’<br />

departure times to less congested times, before or after the rush-hour, change of transport<br />

mode (from car to public transport) or change of work mode (working from home), should, in<br />

theory, lead to considerable time savings, greater travel certainty <strong>and</strong> lower external costs of<br />

congestion.<br />

Transportation dem<strong>and</strong>-based solutions (e.g. road pricing, promoting modal alternatives,<br />

parking policy <strong>and</strong> l<strong>and</strong> use planning policy) have been suggested to reduce congestion<br />

(Shiftan & Golani, 2005). In this respect, transport economists have been arguing for the<br />

implementation of road pricing as a first-best solution to efficiently alleviate congestion<br />

externalities (Nijkamp & Shefer, 1998; Rouwendal & Verhoef, 2006; Small & Verhoef, 2007).<br />

However, road pricing is controversial <strong>and</strong> its <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>al implications are not well<br />

understood. As suggested initially by Vickrey (1969), optimal pricing requires the design of<br />

variable tolls, making them quite complex for drivers’ comprehension (Bonsall et al., 2007;<br />

Verhoef, 2008). In addition, road pricing raises questions regarding social equity (Giuliano,<br />

1994), fairness <strong>and</strong> public acceptability (Eriksson et al., 2006) as well as economic efficiency<br />

(Banister, 1994; Viegas, 2001).<br />

Second-best schemes have been suggested to circumvent the difficulties in implementing<br />

first-best pricing solutions (Small & Verhoef, 2007). In The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s the notion of using<br />

rewards to achieve desired outcomes in travelers’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> has been recently implemented<br />

in the context of the Spitsmijden 1 program (<strong>Ettema</strong> et al., 2010; Knockaert et al., 2007), thus<br />

far, the largest systematic effort to analyze the potential of rewards in the field as a policy<br />

mean for changing commuter <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. A pilot study (see section 3 for further details),<br />

involving 340 participants <strong>and</strong> lasting over 13 weeks, was organized in the second half of<br />

2006. Its objective was to investigate, in an empirical field study, the potential impacts of<br />

rewards on commuters’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> during the morning rush-hour. Participants could earn a<br />

reward (money or credits to keep a Smartphone h<strong>and</strong>set which also provided real-time traffic<br />

information), by driving to work earlier or later, by switching to another travel mode or by<br />

teleworking. Initial results provided evidence of substantial <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change in response to<br />

the rewards, with commuters shifting to earlier <strong>and</strong> later departure times <strong>and</strong> more use of<br />

public transport <strong>and</strong> alternative modes or working from home (<strong>Ettema</strong> et al., 2010).<br />

The effectiveness of rewards to reinforce a desirable <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> (e.g. identification <strong>and</strong> loyalty,<br />

work effort) is supported by a large volume of empirical evidence (Kreps, 1997; Berridge,<br />

2001). However, in the context of travel <strong>and</strong> traffic <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, rewards are poorly represented.<br />

Punishments <strong>and</strong> enforcement (such as policing, felony detectors, fines etc.), have been<br />

more widely documented than rewards (e.g. Rothengatter, 1992; Perry et al., 2002;<br />

Schuitema, 2003). The relative salience of negative motivational means reflects, to a large<br />

extent, a disciplinary bias. Given that travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> has been to the most part subjected<br />

<strong>and</strong> influenced by microeconomic theories (McFadden, 2007), it is not surprising that the<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>al rationale of many dem<strong>and</strong> based strategies to manage traffic congestion is based<br />

on negative incentives that associate, through learning, the act of driving with punishments<br />

(such as tolls or increased parking costs).<br />

1 translated literally as peak avoidance<br />

2


The few examples where rewards have been applied in a travel context are short term<br />

studies involving the use of a temporary free bus ticket as an incentive to reduce car driving.<br />

To most parts, the results of these studies are inconclusive. For example, (Fujii et al., 2001;<br />

Fujii & Kitamura, 2003) found that an incentive did encourage a change towards reducing<br />

car driving; however the level of car driving returned to previous levels once the incentive<br />

was stopped. In contrast, (Bamberg et al., 2002; Bamberg et al., 2003), found that habitual<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> prevented substantial reductions in car use. It is not the scope of this paper to<br />

debate which policy (pricing or rewards) is more effective. However there is substantial<br />

evidence that people respond more favorably <strong>and</strong> are more motivated when rewarded rather<br />

than punished (Kahneman & Tversky, 1984; Geller, 1989). Thus, the potential of rewards as<br />

a base for traffic management policy is well worth considering if based on robust <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>al<br />

foundations.<br />

The main aim of this paper is to comprehensively analyze <strong>and</strong> explore the changes in<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> during the course of the aforementioned pilot study <strong>and</strong> identify key factors that<br />

influenced the response to the rewards. The rest of the paper is organized as follows:<br />

Section 2 sets a number of theoretically driven research questions <strong>and</strong> hypotheses. Section<br />

3 describes the experimental setup <strong>and</strong> methods. Results, based on a mixed logistic<br />

regression analysis are presented in section 4. A discussion is presented in section 5,<br />

followed by summary <strong>and</strong> conclusions in section 6.<br />

2. Research questions & hypotheses<br />

Several key questions are postulated: First, how effective are rewards as a means for<br />

motivating travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change? The literature does not provide a clear indication. One<br />

view suggests that satisfying rewards contribute to higher rates of motivation (Cameron et<br />

al., 2001; 1994). The other view propounds that rewards interfere <strong>and</strong> undermine intrinsic<br />

motivation, deflecting motivation from internal to external causes <strong>and</strong> reducing the amount of<br />

effort devoted to participate in activities (Deci, 1971; 1975; Lepper & Green, 1978). Theory of<br />

Cognitive Evaluation (TCE) further asserts that the effect of reward will depend on how it<br />

affects perceived self-determination <strong>and</strong> competence (Deci & Ryan, 1985).<br />

Second, does the nature of the reward (monetary, in-kind) affect the willingness to change<br />

travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> its tenacity? People seem more receptive to large monetary rewards<br />

compared to small ones (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000; Gneezy, 2003). Moreover, a monetary<br />

reward might be framed as a prospective gain. According to Prospect Theory (Kahneman &<br />

Tversky, 1979), diminishing sensitivity to money can affect the perseverance of change.<br />

Participants’ apparently have greater satisfaction <strong>and</strong> motivation is higher with gifts<br />

compared to monetary rewards; however when asked, most people prefer receiving money<br />

(Shaffer & Arkes, in press). In-kind rewards may therefore encourage <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change<br />

through a different cognitive path: the endowment effect. A Smartphone h<strong>and</strong>set granted to<br />

some participants may be regarded as an uncertain endowment. An endowment is not easily<br />

relinquished, once given (Kahneman et al., 1991). The endowment effect may well motivate<br />

to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> just in order to avoid the loss associated with the possibility to give up a<br />

valued object. In this respect, the in-kind reward, unlike the monetary one may have affective<br />

as well as motivational properties.<br />

Third, to what extent do personal <strong>and</strong> social characteristics (e.g. gender, education level,<br />

personal income, or household composition) sustain or diminish the potential impact of<br />

rewards? The connection between socio-economic characteristics <strong>and</strong> travel choices is well<br />

documented (e.g. Harris & Tanner, 1974; <strong>Ben</strong>-Akiva & Lerman, 1985; Axhausen & Gärling,<br />

1992) In this respect income may well affect motivation in the case of the monetary reward.<br />

Diminishing sensitivity could suggest that participants with higher incomes might be less<br />

motivated to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> for a rather marginal monetary gain.<br />

3


Fourth, do participants’ beliefs attitudes <strong>and</strong> norms influence their responsiveness to change<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>? Several studies (e.g. Gärling et al., 1998; Gärling et al., 2001) suggest attitudes<br />

towards travel alternatives, affect the choice of travel modes. The Theory of Planned<br />

Behav<strong>ior</strong> (TPB) (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen, 1991) suggests a positive attitude towards a<br />

certain <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> will influence a person’s intention to consciously engage in it. Rewards<br />

which create a positive attitude with a certain <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, will contribute to this <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> being<br />

repeated. Another issue is that of personal norms that are self expectations or specific<br />

actions in specific situations (Schwartz, 1977). They refer to feelings of moral obligations to<br />

<strong>behav</strong>e in a certain way (e.g. environmental friendly <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>). If a reward scheme is<br />

regarded as congruent with the personal norms <strong>and</strong> expectations, it is more likely to<br />

encourage <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change.<br />

Fifth, are there situational factors (home <strong>and</strong> work-related) that affect the relative salience of<br />

rewards as means for travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change (here, rush-hour avoidance)? TBP stresses<br />

the role of others’ attitudes, <strong>and</strong> the perceived situational control on influencing intentional<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change. If a person perceives <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> changes as difficult, the probability of<br />

repeating this action is relatively low. Scheduling constraints such as household obligations<br />

(e.g. child care, children chauffeuring) <strong>and</strong> work organization have been found to influence<br />

individuals’ responses to pricing schemes <strong>and</strong> limit their perceived effectiveness (Gärling &<br />

Fujii, 2006). Participants with child care or children chauffeuring responsibilities on one h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

or participants with inflexible working times, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, might have a limited ability to<br />

change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> even when motivated by the reward. Conversely, the support a person gets<br />

from the household, workplace <strong>and</strong> from colleagues or friends that are also participating in a<br />

reward based scheme may well contribute to one’s own participation.<br />

Sixth, to what extent options chosen to avoid the peak are determined by habits? In the long<br />

run habitual travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, as asserted by Gärling et al. (2001) <strong>and</strong> Gärling & Axhausen<br />

(2003), is quite relevant for promoting or discouraging a <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change different from the<br />

usual travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Theory of Interpersonal Behav<strong>ior</strong> (TIB) (Tri<strong>and</strong>is, 1977, 1980) stresses<br />

the role of habit in <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. With habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, decisions are made with a lesser degree<br />

of consciousness which decreases the likelihood <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> will change in response to a<br />

change in context. Habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is less intentional more automated <strong>and</strong> script based<br />

(Ronis et al., 1989; Gärling & Garvill, 1993). Travel decisions (e.g. the drive to work) are an<br />

example of habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> as repeated decisions which loose intention <strong>and</strong> become<br />

gradually routinized (Verplanken et al., 1997; Gärling et al., 1998).<br />

Last, what is the role travel information plays in changing commuters’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>? Several<br />

studies point out that availability of information has significant effects on travelers’ <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

in the lab (Avineri & Prashker, 2006; <strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> et al., 2008). For example in the case of routechoice,<br />

<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> & Shiftan, (2010) found real-time travel information expedites learning in<br />

unfamiliar environments <strong>and</strong> reduces initial exploration. At the same time, exposure to<br />

information is also associated with more heterogeneity in choice <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> in risk<br />

attitudes. In this respect the Smartphone reward could well have instrumental value as it also<br />

provides access to real-time traffic information. Information might motivate change of<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> by facilitating the travel decision process <strong>and</strong> by reducing subjective effort <strong>and</strong><br />

difficulty increasing the perceived situational control.<br />

3. Method<br />

3.1 Participants<br />

Using license plate recognition cameras, 2,300 cars, both privately owned <strong>and</strong> leased<br />

company vehicles <strong>and</strong> traveling at least three times a week during the morning rush hour on<br />

the busy stretch of the A12 motorway (about 15 km connecting Zoetermeer to The Hague).<br />

The Dutch Department of Road Transport provided the names <strong>and</strong> addresses of the car<br />

owners <strong>and</strong> they were approached by mail with an invitation to participate in the experiment.<br />

4


A total of 341 commuters - 221 men <strong>and</strong> 120 women – chose to participate in the<br />

experiment. Upon registration, the participants self selected one out of two types of reward.<br />

The first type of reward was an amount of money (3-7 Euros <strong>and</strong> see next subsection) for<br />

each day that the participant avoided driving during the morning rush-hour. In this case,<br />

participants were provided with a realistic estimate of how much they could earn in the<br />

course of the study. The second type comprised credits towards ultimately keeping a<br />

Smartphone (called Yeti) at the end of the experiment. 232 participants (60% men),<br />

selected a monetary reward (‘money’) <strong>and</strong> 109 (74% men) the Yeti reward. The Yeti’s<br />

market value was around € 500 at the time. All the participants were inhabitants of the town<br />

of Zoetermeer <strong>and</strong> the vast majority was working at the time in The Hague or its vicinities.<br />

They are characterized by relatively high percentage of higher education, moderate to high<br />

incomes <strong>and</strong> mostly families with children. Table 1 presents the descriptives of the<br />

participants by group.<br />

3.2 Procedure<br />

***Table 1 about here***<br />

The task <strong>and</strong> rules were communicated to the participants through the project's back office:<br />

Participation had to be voluntary. The participants were to commute at least three times a<br />

week from home to work. They had to have access to e-mail <strong>and</strong> the Internet. They were<br />

requested to complete surveys completely <strong>and</strong> timely. They were made aware that their<br />

movements by car would be recorded <strong>and</strong> had to agree to the installation of an on-board<br />

transponder in their car. In addition it was explained that only the car in which a transponder<br />

had been previously installed could be eligible for the reward. A travel log (i.e. logbook) was<br />

to be filled in daily on a personal webpage on the projects’ internet site. Participants that<br />

opted for the Yeti reward were also instructed to switch on the Smartphone during every car<br />

trip, in order to get full <strong>and</strong> easy access to real-time travel information. All communication<br />

was to be conducted via the project’s back-office which dealt with complaints or operational<br />

problems. A weekly newsletter was also sent to participants’ homes providing further<br />

information <strong>and</strong> clarifications. Participants’ earnings were shown on their personal webpage.<br />

The earnings were updated once a week according to the relevant treatment schemes. The<br />

monetary rewards were directly paid to participants’ bank accounts at the end of the working<br />

week by bank transfer.<br />

3.3 Design<br />

Participants were instructed that they could avoid commuting during the morning rush-hour<br />

(defined between 7:30-9:30 AM) either by shifting their departure times to earlier or later<br />

times of travel, or by choosing other modes of travel (cycling, carpool, public transport), or by<br />

working from home (teleworking). The experiment ran for a period of 13 weeks. The first two<br />

weeks were without reward (pre-test). The data collected during the pre-test was used to<br />

determine participants’ reference travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> subsequent assignment to reward<br />

classes. The final week (post-test) was also without rewards.<br />

Those participants who opted for money were the subject of three consecutive reward<br />

treatments lasting 10 weeks in total: a reward of 3€ (lasting three weeks), a reward of 7€<br />

(lasting four weeks) <strong>and</strong> a mixed reward (lasting three weeks) of up to 7€ - of which 3€ for<br />

avoiding the high peak (8:00-9:00) <strong>and</strong> an additional 4€ for avoiding also the lower peak<br />

shoulders (7:30-8:00, 9:00-9:30). A counterbalanced (blocked r<strong>and</strong>omization) design was<br />

used to allocate participants r<strong>and</strong>omly to 6 (that is 3! blocks) possible treatment orders<br />

(referred to as scheme). A few exceptions were applied to couples using the same vehicle.<br />

The scheme of treatments was communicated to the participants through their personal web<br />

5


pages. Participants in possession of the Yeti could acquire credit during a period of five<br />

consecutive weeks. If they earned enough credit relative to a known threshold they could<br />

keep the Smartphone. This threshold was determined by their reward class (see below). The<br />

other five weeks were without credits but participants could still have access to traffic<br />

information. Participants were r<strong>and</strong>omly divided between two schemes in relation to which of<br />

the first or second set of 5 weeks credits could be awarded. They were also made aware of<br />

their respective schemes.<br />

Participants in possession of a Yeti also had 24 hour access to travel information via the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>set during 11 weeks: the credit treatment, the no-credit treatment as well as the posttest.<br />

This information consisted of real-time travel times on the A12 motorway on the<br />

Zoetermeer – The Hague corridor <strong>and</strong> an online map showing congestion levels on other<br />

roads in the area. Information availability was not dependent on the reward itself. In contrast,<br />

participants in the money group had access to information available to all other drivers: pretrip<br />

through internet <strong>and</strong> media <strong>and</strong> en-route from variable message signs along the<br />

motorway.<br />

In addition to the treatments, each participant was also assigned to a reward class which<br />

determined his/her maximum eligible reward. In essence, a participant could only earn the<br />

reward as often as he/she was observed to drive in the morning peak during the pre-test.<br />

Thus, a participant who would drive in the peak three times per week in the pre-test, could<br />

only receive a reward for the third, fourth <strong>and</strong> fifth day in a week he/she avoided the peak,<br />

whereas one who drove in the peak five times per week was eligible for any working day<br />

he/she avoided the peak. This reward could be either the daily monetary reward or the<br />

threshold number of credits needed to keep the Yeti. It should be noted that retrospectively<br />

very few participants failed to meet their threshold. In order to avoid regret, it was also<br />

decided at the end of the study to allow all the participants to keep their Yeti’s. Accordingly,<br />

each participant was allocated into one of four possible reward classes. Once determined<br />

these classes were fixed throughout the rest of the experiment. The majority of participants<br />

belonged to classes A <strong>and</strong> B <strong>and</strong> the minority to classes C <strong>and</strong> D. Table 2 presents the<br />

number of participants (by gender) in each class. In both groups women are more prevalent<br />

in the classes with lower traveling frequencies. For a more detailed description of the<br />

experiment’s design see the report (in English) of Knockaert et al., (2007) also available from<br />

the authors by request.<br />

Self selection of reward types by participants suggests by definition a quasi-experimental<br />

design. Like r<strong>and</strong>om experiments, quasi-experiments share the same basic principles of<br />

manipulation (cause precedes effect) <strong>and</strong> measurable associations (covariation). In contrast,<br />

causation requires more effort as compared to r<strong>and</strong>om assignment there are more threats to<br />

internal validity. In this we will follow the recommendations of Shadish et al., (2002) noting<br />

possible threats. An analysis of threats to internal validity is described in section 4. Lack of a<br />

control group also can contribute to validity problems. Several features in the design allow<br />

improved control <strong>and</strong> reduce possible threats. First, the pre-test / post-test design is fostered<br />

by additional measurements of stated <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> through the two surveys. Specifically threats<br />

resulting from history <strong>and</strong> novelty can be assessed by comparing between the preliminary<br />

survey <strong>and</strong> the pre-test. In addition, the measured factors from the surveys such as usual<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, constraints <strong>and</strong> support measures, can provide relevant mediators to the observed<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> verify if selection is a problematic issue. This is dealt in detail in Section 4.<br />

Second, norm comparisons with traffic counts on the main A12 trajectory suggest other<br />

drivers did not change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> during this period. The sample is small enough not to have<br />

any real impact on traffic flow. Since no significant change in traffic occurred during the 13<br />

week observed period we can assert that any difference between observed <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>s with<br />

treatments <strong>and</strong> without is likely to be related to the intervention. In retrospect, it is<br />

acknowledged that r<strong>and</strong>om assignment <strong>and</strong> group control would have been the preferred<br />

solution.<br />

6


3.4 Measurements<br />

***Table 2 – about here***<br />

Data was collected during the study in several stages. In the first stage, after volunteering<br />

(April-August, 2006), participants completed a web-based preliminary survey. This survey<br />

gathered data regarding several important pre-test factors including home to work daily<br />

travel routines, individual <strong>and</strong> household characteristics (gender, age, education level,<br />

income, family composition); work schedules (i.e. flexibility in departure from home <strong>and</strong> in<br />

starting work early/late, or ability to telework), family obligations (e.g. childcare or child<br />

chauffeuring duties), availability <strong>and</strong> use of alternative means of transport, attitudes towards<br />

alternative travel modes <strong>and</strong> regular use of travel information. The survey results can be<br />

requested from the authors.<br />

The second stage was the actual experiment, lasting 13 weeks (of which weeks 3-12 were<br />

with rewards). It consisted of tracking participant’s observed <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Detection equipment<br />

using in-vehicle installed transponders <strong>and</strong> electronic vehicle identification (EVI) as well as<br />

backup road-side cameras was installed at the exits from Zoetermeer to the A12 motorway<br />

<strong>and</strong> on other routes leaving the city. This equipment allowed detecting each <strong>and</strong> every car<br />

passage during the course of the day, minimizing the ability of participants to cheat by trying<br />

to access alternative routes. In addition, participants were instructed to fill in their daily webbased<br />

logbook. They recorded whether or not they had commuted to work (<strong>and</strong> if not, why<br />

not), which means of transport they used <strong>and</strong> at what slot time they made their trip. This<br />

information was used to gain insight into situations in which the participant was not detected<br />

by the EVI.<br />

In this paper we decided to focus on the logbook data. The main reasons were the<br />

completeness of the data which included not only car travel but also non-car travel. In<br />

addition, the logs provide a unique description of each days travel choice whereas<br />

detections could appear several times a day. Furthermore, the logbooks <strong>and</strong> detections were<br />

checked by the project’s back-office for consistency to avoid complaints <strong>and</strong> disagreements<br />

with participants regarding their eligibility for a reward. The logbook contained several<br />

entries: normal entries on working days about the choice of travel <strong>and</strong> abnormal entries<br />

(including situations like use of another car, holiday, illness, problems with the equipment<br />

etc). Only normal entries relating to working days were included in the analysis. Detection<br />

data is left for future research on dynamics of departure time choice.<br />

The third stage of the study was a poster<strong>ior</strong> evaluation survey. In this survey questions were<br />

asked about the participant’s subjective experience during the course of the experiment.<br />

This dealt with their retrospective assessment of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> adjustment (was it easy / difficult<br />

to adjust travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> how much effort was involved in changing one's <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>).<br />

Other questions focused on support measures such as discussions with one's employer,<br />

colleagues <strong>and</strong> household members about flexible working times <strong>and</strong> household routines,<br />

practicing with <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change during the pre-test <strong>and</strong> purchasing of certain items.<br />

Questions were also asked regarding the use of travel information enabling a pre/post-test<br />

comparison that indicated a significant increase in usage of both traffic <strong>and</strong> public transport<br />

information.. Retrospective motivations to participate in the program were also inquired. One<br />

fact to be noted is that during the experiment disruptions occurred with the regional rail<br />

service <strong>and</strong> bus service replacements were not always adequately provided. In retrospect<br />

this was mentioned as causing participants some difficulty for using the public transport.<br />

At the same time that data was collected about the participants, a survey of non-participants<br />

was also carried out. It was based on a representative sample of Zoetermeer residents,<br />

regularly commuting to The Hague during the morning rush-hour, who did not participate in<br />

the experiment. The purpose was to determine whether the participants in the trial were<br />

7


epresentative of the total population of rush-hour drivers. Similar questions were put to<br />

these respondents. This analysis (see <strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> & <strong>Ettema</strong>, 2009) demonstrated that although<br />

the reward is the main motivation in potentially choosing to participate in a similar rewardbased<br />

scheme, lack of flexibility in daily schedules was the main reason to reject the<br />

scheme.<br />

4. Results<br />

Responses appearing in the logbook were sorted into four distinctive <strong>and</strong> exclusive<br />

categories: rush-hour driving (RD), driving earlier (DE) or driving later (DL) than the rushhour,<br />

<strong>and</strong> non-driving (ND) which included all non-auto modes of travel (public transport,<br />

cycling, car pool) as well as teleworking. Since the rewards were provided on a weekly<br />

basis, the number of rush-hour avoidances within the week, could well be correlated. Daily<br />

responses were therefore assembled to weekly average shares (i.e. proportions). Weekly<br />

averages were further aggregated to treatment averages for statistical testing purposes in<br />

the following way: in the money group five repeated measurements (pre-test, three<br />

treatment levels, post-test); <strong>and</strong> in the Yeti group four repeated measurements (pre-test,<br />

credit, non-credit, post-test). The data analysis itself consisted of two stages. In the first<br />

stage (available from the authors by request), each of the four response categories was<br />

analyzed separately using GLM-repeated measures. In the second stage a mixed logistic<br />

regression (MLR) model was estimated based on the significant factors found in the first<br />

stage.<br />

The rationale behind using MLR was that the four response categories (RD, DE, DL, ND)<br />

attributed to each participant are in a sense a closed set of discrete choice alternatives <strong>and</strong><br />

therefore correlated. The probability of choosing a discrete response (i.e. an alternative) is<br />

specified as the dependent variable <strong>and</strong> the independent factors explain this probability.<br />

Usually, the relationship between alternatives <strong>and</strong> explanatory factors is specified with an<br />

outcome function referred to as 'utility'. The greater the utility of an alternative is, the higher<br />

is the probability of a participant choosing it (Train, 2002). Simple logistic regression is<br />

unsuitable for analysis of repeated measurements (McFadden & Train, 2000). However the<br />

MLR model can accommodate this by specifying a panel data model (Revelt & Train 1998;<br />

Bhat, 1999;). We estimated the MLR model using the estimation program of NLOGIT 4.0<br />

(Econometric Software Inc.,) <strong>and</strong> using share-based data with 1,000 r<strong>and</strong>om draws (see<br />

Train, 2000), for further details regarding drawing methods.<br />

Formally, the utility of person n of alternative i in response t <strong>and</strong> the probability (P) of person<br />

n choosing alternative i in response t are (eq. 1, 2):<br />

where P is the conditional probability that person n chooses alternative i out of a set of J<br />

alternatives, Y, is an indicator that i is chosen at response t, X is a vector of explanatory<br />

factors, , is a vector of fixed coefficients (including a constant), is a vector of r<strong>and</strong>om<br />

parameters with a distribution f (0 mean <strong>and</strong> a variance parameter ) <strong>and</strong> is a vector of<br />

independently, identically distributed (iid) Gumbel (or extreme-value type 1) error terms.<br />

The MLR model's main purpose is to estimate the composite effects on all four (correlated)<br />

response categories accounting for the sequential structure of the data. Each category has<br />

8<br />

(1)<br />

(2)


its own utility specification which is linear. Factors were entered into the utilities in a<br />

sequential manner whereby, non significant factors are dropped out <strong>and</strong> significant ones<br />

remain. R<strong>and</strong>om effects (i.e ‘s) are specified (for statistical restrictions only for three out of<br />

four categories) as normally distributed error terms (with zero mean <strong>and</strong> unknown variance<br />

) to better capture differences between respondents (i.e. heterogeneity) across the<br />

observations. In addition we allowed covariation between the r<strong>and</strong>om effects to account for<br />

inherent correlations between the unobserved factors in the model. This is also due to the<br />

nature of the similarity between the three driving alternatives (RD, DE, DL) relative to not<br />

driving (ND).<br />

Table 3 presents the treatments’ average measurements (also illustrated in Figure 1) <strong>and</strong><br />

between group pre/post-test differences. Table 4, presents the coefficient estimates for the<br />

MLR model. As noted in Table 3, pre-measurement levels of RD were substantially higher<br />

than the pre-test <strong>and</strong> this difference is significant for both groups. Thus any significant<br />

change between the pre-test <strong>and</strong> other treatments is also expected to be significant relative<br />

to the pre-measurement level. Since around one third of the participants stated in the<br />

poster<strong>ior</strong> survey that practicing with rush-hour avoidance during the pre-test assisted them to<br />

change their <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, exploration with alternatives to rush-hour driving could be one way to<br />

explain the difference between stated <strong>and</strong> observed pre-test <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>s. However, since the<br />

pre-measurement is based on stated rather than observed <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, to remain conservative<br />

we did not include it in the MLR analysis. In addition although the between-group analysis of<br />

RD, suggests that post-test differences are significant this is not confirmed in the more<br />

robust MLR. Consequently only the reward treatments were specified in the model whereby<br />

the coefficients reflect their effects relative to the pre-test.<br />

In terms of goodness of fit the model has a final log likelihood of -1,648.12 <strong>and</strong> the rhosquare<br />

is 0.22. A simple multinomial logistic regression model (without r<strong>and</strong>om effects) had<br />

a log likelihood of -1,678.24. The log likelihood ratio test shows this difference is significant<br />

( 2 = 60.2, df=6, p


Mediators (between-subjects factors) included the design related factors (reward class <strong>and</strong><br />

treatment scheme), <strong>and</strong> factors relating to the participants’ stated <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> derived from the<br />

two surveys. First, as neither the treatment scheme nor any of its interactions are significant<br />

we can conclude that the order of treatments had no effect on <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Therefore the order<br />

effect is discarded from the final model in Table 4. Second, among socio-demographic<br />

characteristics gender has a marginally significant effect on RD (p≈0.1) suggesting men tend<br />

to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> more than women. In the case of money, higher education has a<br />

significant <strong>and</strong> negative effect on DE. A possible explanation is that education as a proxy for<br />

income could well be masking an income effect. However testing of moderation by income is<br />

not possible due to the small groups involved <strong>and</strong> consequent loss of statistical power.<br />

Third, we find that factors relating to habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> have significant results. The reward<br />

class, which relates to pre-test levels of driving at the rush-hour, has a negative association<br />

with <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change. It was found that moderating the class effect by group proved<br />

significant. Participants, in both groups, associated with classes A, B (2.5 - 5 rush-hour trips<br />

at pre-test) were more likely to continue driving during the rush-hour compared to classes C<br />

<strong>and</strong> D (0-2.5 trips). In addition, the class coefficient for money is slightly larger than that of<br />

Yeti. The usual departure time has a negative association with DE: i.e. the earlier is the<br />

usual departure time - the more probable is a change of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> by driving earlier. One may<br />

argue that similar factors that affect driving early in the non rewarded situation (such as<br />

household obligations) will still be at play during the rewarded period. The preferred start of<br />

work time, a likely proxy for the preferred arrival time, has a similar negative effect on DE but<br />

also a positive effect on DL. That means that participants driving later are those that are<br />

more accustomed to depart later in usual circumstances. Finally, the use of other modes for<br />

commuting has a positive effect on not driving. Fourth, concerning scheduling flexibility <strong>and</strong><br />

constraints, a number of factors have been found to affect change of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Child<br />

chauffeuring is positively associated with RD. Other constraints on early departure, such as<br />

childcare responsibilities, were not found significant. Conversely, participants who stated<br />

they had support from their employers with arranging flexible working times are less likely to<br />

drive during the rush-hour. These results demonstrate the relevancy of constraints <strong>and</strong><br />

support measures as important factors that determine the probability to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>.<br />

The number of days (per week) that starting work late is possible has a positive effect on DL,<br />

a finding that suggests that participants with more flexible working schedules are more likely<br />

to drive later. Similarly but with a marginally significant positive effect (p


for public transport alternatives to support their <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> are more likely to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

by not driving ( the coefficients for both these factors are positive).<br />

*** Table 3 – about here ***<br />

***Table 4 – about here ***<br />

As noted in section 3, this study is compromised of a quasi-experimental design. Based on<br />

the recommendation of Shadish et al. (2002), we describe here the plausible threats to the<br />

validity of the results. Threats to statistical inferences are not discussed here as we contend<br />

that these are likely to be low given the conservative nature of the analysis method applied<br />

which guarantees proper statistical identification of the measurable effects.<br />

The two most plausible threats in our study are selection <strong>and</strong> history. Attrition is not an issue<br />

since no dropouts occurred. Maturation is also not relevant given the short period of time<br />

that the experiment was running. The issue of selection relates to a pr<strong>ior</strong>i differences in the<br />

money <strong>and</strong> Yeti groups which could compromise the results. In Table 5 we present a<br />

statistical comparison of the differences between the self-selected groups by the factors that<br />

are associated with the response in the MLR model. Most factors have no significant<br />

difference, but gender, chauffeuring children <strong>and</strong> arrangements with employers regarding<br />

flexible working times do. The latter which has the most significant difference was measured<br />

in the poster<strong>ior</strong> survey whereas the first two factors relate to pre-measurement. To contend<br />

with the threats we estimated the effects moderated by group, specifying the MLR model<br />

with group-specific coefficients for these three factors.<br />

Regarding gender, it was barely significant in the model (p≈0.1). We tested moderating<br />

effects for money <strong>and</strong> Yeti but this was found not to be significant. We can therefore<br />

conclude that gender (woman) has a weak negative association with avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>.<br />

Chauffeuring children is significantly <strong>and</strong> negatively associated with avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

(p=0.02). We investigated if this might be moderated by gender but given the small group<br />

involved of both men <strong>and</strong> women who have this constraint we could not identify with<br />

confidence any significant moderating effect. Moderating by group also does not reveal<br />

significant group differences in the MLR model. Therefore we can suggest that the negative<br />

effect identified for chauffeuring on peak avoidance is probable.<br />

The threat attributed to pre-arrangements regarding flexible work times requires more<br />

attention (p


treatment (pre/post test) remains relatively valid. Practicing also has a weak negative effect<br />

on rush-hour driving (p=0.07) which was only found relevant for the money group.<br />

5. Discussion<br />

Effectiveness of rewarding<br />

***Table 5 – about here ***<br />

The results demonstrate, that rewarding, at least in the short run, is effective as within a<br />

short period of time of several weeks, the share of rush-hour avoidance substantially<br />

increased. Thus in concordance with motivation theories (e.g. Cameron et al., 2001) rewards<br />

do influence the motivation to avoid the rush-hour. Moreover, we also found that the decision<br />

how to exercise this change of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, whether by driving at other times or by changing<br />

transport or work modes seem to be determined by other factors unrelated to the type or<br />

level of reward.<br />

Nonetheless, it is difficult to conclude from a relatively short longitudinal study about the<br />

impacts of rewards in the long run. Motivation theories suggest that if intrinsic motivation<br />

kicks in, the change of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is more likely to be sustained. However, we observed in the<br />

post-test, once rewards ceased, avoidance shares had dropped <strong>and</strong> participants had<br />

returned more or less to their usual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> of rush-hour driving (as observed in the pretest).<br />

In this respect the results are similar to those obtained by Fujii & Kitamura (2003)<br />

regarding free bus tickets. Therefore at first glance it seems the change was not sustained<br />

for most of the participants. Notwithst<strong>and</strong>ing, in the poster<strong>ior</strong> survey less than 15% of<br />

participants stated they had returned to their previous <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Unfortunately, we do not<br />

have observations to corroborate this subjective evaluation. Further research is being carried<br />

out in this respect (see section 7). We also do not posses sufficient (post-test) data to<br />

conclude about the affective qualities of the rewards apart for the fact that the vast majority<br />

of participants (in both groups) answered affirmatively to the question 'did you like the<br />

reward' in the poster<strong>ior</strong> survey.<br />

Reward type <strong>and</strong> levels<br />

We found that both types of reward (monetary <strong>and</strong> in-kind) have a significant <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

effect on rush-hour driving. In the case of a monetary reward, diminishing sensitivity was<br />

clearly noted. The 7€ treatment has the largest overall effect on RD; however the largest<br />

marginal effect (the derivative) is associated with the 3€ treatment. Therefore, for practical<br />

purposes, a moderate monetary reward seems to be sufficient to encourage a relatively<br />

substantial change of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. In the case of the Yeti reward, the main effect is the credits<br />

which had an effect similar to the 7€ reward. In this sense an in-kind reward, likely perceived<br />

as an endowment, can be just as useful as the monetary reward. However, for practical<br />

reasons, there may be difficulty in implementing an in-kind reward over a long period of time.<br />

Though not statistically significant in comparison to pre-test levels, avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> was<br />

also apparent without valid credits. This treatment had no extrinsic reward but travel<br />

information was still accessible to Yeti users. Furthermore, it was evident that Yeti users<br />

were more likely to drive later compared to participants in the money group. Two possible<br />

explanations are possible for this different <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. On one h<strong>and</strong>, Yeti users had higher<br />

shares regarding support provided from employers. Thus, it is possible that pre-adjustments<br />

were involved in choosing to depart later (especially during the pre-test). On the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

the main advantage Yeti users had over the other group was 24 hour access to travel<br />

information. This leads us to suggest that the decision how to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is also<br />

influenced by travel information availability (discussed later on).<br />

12


Socio-demographic characteristics<br />

As noted, this is hardly a novel assumption in travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> studies. Gender (marginally)<br />

<strong>and</strong> education, were found to have an impact on the response to the rewards. It seems that<br />

men (mainly in the money group) are more likely to avoid the rush-hour compared to women.<br />

The lower motivation of women to avoid the rush-hour can be associated with many issues.<br />

One idea that has been suggested in social mobility studies (Palma et al., 2009) is that<br />

women are more constrained in time compared to men for various reasons, mainly<br />

household tasks <strong>and</strong> child raising obligations. Dutch women quite often leave work early in<br />

the afternoon to pick up children from nurseries (Schwanen, 2007). This limits their ability to<br />

change their schedule - e.g. to start work later even when extrinsically motivated by a<br />

reward. However, a larger sample is needed to clearly mark the causation between gender<br />

<strong>and</strong> time-use <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>.<br />

Education had a negative effect on <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change (driving later). Participants (in the<br />

money group) with higher education were less likely to drive later. Education is a known<br />

proxy for latent income effects. Income is regarded as a key issue determining willingness to<br />

pay for travel purposes as well as the value of travel time savings (<strong>Ben</strong>-Akiva & Lerman,<br />

1985; Axhausen & Gärling, 1992). In the context of the money group, the significance of<br />

higher education strengthens the notion of diminishing sensitivity in relation to the monetary<br />

reward: participants with higher real income are likely to be less sensitive to a marginal<br />

monetary gain compared to participants with lower incomes. As a result motivation to avoid<br />

the rush-hour would be negatively associated with real income. Education did not appear to<br />

be a relevant factor on the <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> of Yeti users, possibly because it is cognitively <strong>and</strong><br />

affectively appreciated as an endowment, rather than in monetary (how much it’s worth)<br />

value.<br />

Scheduling<br />

This is a new territory of travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> research, lately identified by Gärling <strong>and</strong> Fujii<br />

(2006). The results suggest that <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change <strong>and</strong> more so the choice of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

change is associated with the ability or disability to change daily schedules. Both home<br />

related <strong>and</strong> work related flexibilities are relevant. Family obligations, such as children<br />

chauffeuring - a constraint associate positively with rush-hour driving - make it more difficult<br />

for parents to change travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. The ability to accommodate a flexible schedule <strong>and</strong><br />

the support provided by others are also significant factors. Participants that could start<br />

working later or could telework were more likely to drive later. Participants reporting to have<br />

received support from their employer with arranging flexible working times were also less<br />

likely to drive in the rush-hour. We see these results as supporting evidence that flexibility,<br />

especially at the work place is a key issue in promoting changes in travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Contrary<br />

to our expectations, home-related support measures such as household arrangements did<br />

not have a significant effect on <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change. A possible explanation to the effects of<br />

scheduling is the extent of control over one’s actions <strong>and</strong> their outcomes. The Theory of<br />

Planned Behav<strong>ior</strong> (Ajzen, 1991) suggests perceived situational control is a key factor in<br />

encouraging a conscience <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change. Thus flexibility in time-use promotes a sense of<br />

self confidence <strong>and</strong> ability to contend with the schedule’s change.<br />

Habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, experience <strong>and</strong> attitudes<br />

As suggested by Theory of Interpersonal Behav<strong>ior</strong> (Tri<strong>and</strong>is, 1977, 1980) we found that<br />

factors relating to habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> play an important role in the choice how to change<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. This corroborates findings from other studies (e.g. Gärling et al., 2001; Gärling &<br />

Axhausen, 2003). The effect of habitual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is well manifested in the significance of the<br />

reward class, usual departure time, the preferred start of work time (in the case of shifting<br />

driving times) as well as the use of other modes for commuting purposes (in the case of<br />

switching mode). Participants with higher rush-hour commute frequencies during the pre-test<br />

(reward class A, B) were relatively less likely to avoid the rush-hour compared to participants<br />

13


with lower rush-hour frequencies (class C, D). Two potential explanations are put forward.<br />

First, in terms of effort, one could argue that a similar relative response dem<strong>and</strong>s more rushhour<br />

avoidances from frequent rush-hour drivers than from less frequent rush-hour drivers.<br />

Hence, the effort involved is higher for high frequency drivers. This is in line with Garling et<br />

al., (2004) <strong>and</strong> Cao <strong>and</strong> Mokhtarian (2005), who found that travelers prefer low effort<br />

responses over high effort responses. A second explanation is that the added value of<br />

additional rewards depends on the amount already gained, in the sense that the marginal<br />

utility of reward decreases (i.e. diminishing sensitivity). Thus, the extra rewards gained by<br />

high frequency drivers will have a lower impact on <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. This is in line with the idea of<br />

satisficing <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> described by Simon (1987). In the case of Yeti users, the effect of<br />

reward class is weaker. This might be related to the affective qualities of the Smartphone<br />

endowment i.e. avoiding the displeasure of having to give back the h<strong>and</strong>y Smartphone<br />

encouraged avoidance. In addition, real-time travel information may have been useful in<br />

reducing perceived effort <strong>and</strong> promoting self confidence in the ability to manage with rushhour<br />

avoidance.<br />

It is also evident that a relation exists between the usual schedules (usual departure <strong>and</strong><br />

arrival times) <strong>and</strong> choice of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change. The usual departure time was a decisive factor<br />

affecting the choice to depart earlier whereas the preferred start of work time, a likely proxy<br />

for the preferred arrival time, had a significant influence on both driving earlier <strong>and</strong> later.<br />

Furthermore, previous experience using other transport modes was an important contributor<br />

to the choice not to drive. That is, familiarity with an alternative seems to increase intrinsic<br />

motivation. It appears that the choice of <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change is closely related to the perceived<br />

gap between the usual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> the required change – the smaller the gap the more<br />

likely is that the change will be exercised. One may argue that alternatives that are more<br />

similar to the current <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, will better meet the travelers’ preferences with respect to<br />

characteristics of the travel mode <strong>and</strong> timing.<br />

Contrary to the usual <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, a <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>-change requires gaining of knowledge through<br />

exploration <strong>and</strong> reinforced learning about the new situation. It is suggested that exploration<br />

had an important role during the pre-test. Practicing avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> during the pre-test<br />

was reported by almost a third of the participants( in both groups) <strong>and</strong> it is one explanation<br />

for the dramatic drop in the pre-test shares of rush-hour driving compared to the usual<br />

(stated) <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> recorded in the preliminary survey. This factor was also found (albeit<br />

weakly) to increase the likelihood of decreasing rush-hour driving for the money group.<br />

Recent findings in the context of route-choice <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> suggest information expedites<br />

learning in the short-run whereas lack of information requires greater effort devoted to<br />

exploration <strong>and</strong> learning (<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> et al., 2008; <strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong> & Shiftan, 2010). It is plausible that<br />

the pre-test was devoted by participants for information acquisition.<br />

Attitudes have been recently gaining attention in travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> studies (Gärling &<br />

Axhausen, 2003). Moreover, perceptions <strong>and</strong> attitudes have been the focus of invigorating<br />

attempts to improve choice modeling (Walker, 2001; Cherchi, 2009). As suggested by TPB,<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> personal norms are significant factors in encouraging or discouraging a<br />

conscious decision to change <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Our results support this assertion in that participants’<br />

attitudes regarding alternative modes were a key factor in determining the choice of<br />

avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Positive attitudes (defined as regarding a travel mode as a realistic<br />

alternative), regarding public transport <strong>and</strong> cycling, discouraged driving (including at off-peak<br />

periods) <strong>and</strong> encouraged mode switch away from the car. Conversely, perceptions regarding<br />

the (high) effort involved in changing <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> decreased the likelihood of changing <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> were positively associated with rush-hour driving. We could not find real support for the<br />

relevance of personal norms in the decision to avoid the rush-hour.<br />

Information availability<br />

Several studies confirm the key role that availability of travel information has on promoting<br />

sensible travel choices (Mahmassani & Liu, 1999; Srinivasan & Mahamassani, 2003). Our<br />

14


esults sustain this association in three ways. First, there appear to be significant betweengroup<br />

differences in the <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change. Yeti users had relatively higher shares of driving<br />

later compared to higher shares of driving earlier in the money group. Yeti users’ main<br />

advantage was the real-time access to travel information whereas the participants in the<br />

money group had to search for the same information i.e. it involved more effort. However, as<br />

noted this result could also be confounded by pr<strong>ior</strong> arrangements with employers over<br />

flexible working times which were more dominant with the Yeti group. Hence, we cannot be<br />

certain if the different response is attributed to the Yeti treatment or related to<br />

prearrangements which facilitated driving later. However, the information explanation is<br />

strengthened by the fact that moderating this effect by group was not significant.<br />

Second, access of travel information, mainly traffic but also public transport information,<br />

intensified during the course of the experiment (pre/post-test comparison). Thus, decisionmaking<br />

in a changed environment apparently increased the need for information about the<br />

outcomes of alternatives. Third, information availability is positively associated with not<br />

driving or driving later. Participants who frequently accessed public transport information <strong>and</strong><br />

who were actively perusing information over public transport connections were more likely to<br />

avoid driving altogether. In addition, participants with higher frequency of accessing traffic<br />

information where more likely to choose driving later. It seems therefore that information<br />

acquisition <strong>and</strong> choice of avoidance <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> are clearly related. However causality here is<br />

uncertain as participants could also increase information acquisition for the alternative they<br />

found is best.<br />

6. Summary <strong>and</strong> conclusions<br />

The main conclusion regarding the use of rewards in encouraging commuters to change<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> is that it actually works. Rewards are effective extrinsic motivators for travel<br />

<strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> change - here rush-hour avoidance. The monetary reward was likely perceived as<br />

a gain with diminishing sensitivity, whereas the Yeti should be regarded as an in-kind reward<br />

which had added endowment <strong>and</strong> instrumental qualities. The rewards were able to sustain<br />

the <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>al change throughout the experiment. Nonetheless, it is still an open question<br />

whether the change would be sustained in the long run <strong>and</strong> without rewards. We do not have<br />

enough post-test observations to provide an answer apart from subjective assessments by<br />

the participants. A second conclusion that can be drawn from this research is that the reward<br />

influences the magnitude of change – an increase or decrease in rush-hour avoidance.<br />

However the choice how to avoid – driving at other times, switching to another mode of<br />

transport or working from home, is determined by external factors relating to participants’<br />

personal <strong>and</strong> social characteristics, scheduling flexibility, history <strong>and</strong> information availability,<br />

Although already of some interest to the travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> research community these issues<br />

deserve further attention in future research.<br />

As a closing remark, following the success of the current study, application of reward-based<br />

schemes is now taking place across The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s. Although some concern, based on<br />

traffic simulation models, indicated that too many people might start changing their<br />

schedules to gain a reward (Bliemer & van Amelsfort, 2008), the evidence in the field does<br />

not support this claim. Their effectiveness in mitigating congestion, especially in situations<br />

involving temporary road maintenance or lane closures has been verified (Bliemer et al.,<br />

2009). A recent survey of firms also has shown positive attitude amongst employers towards<br />

the reward scheme (Vonk Noordegraaf & Annema, 2009). So far, the majority of the Dutch<br />

public (apart for the public transport users who are ineligible <strong>and</strong> consequently grumbling)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the government are quite content with the results. However as recently published in the<br />

media the (last) government also wanted to advance a punishment policy through universal<br />

kilometer road charging – a decision that stresses the importance of well-informed,<br />

evidence-based, as well as <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>ally-sound public policy.<br />

15


Acknowledgments<br />

This study was undertaken as part of the Spitsmijden project, which was funded by<br />

Transumo, the Ministry of Transport in the Netherl<strong>and</strong>s, Bereik, RDW, NS, Rabobank, ARS<br />

T&TT, OC Mobility Coaching, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, TU Delft, Universiteit Utrecht.<br />

The modeling framework was discussed in the 5 th Discrete Choice Modeling Workshop<br />

organized at EPFL (Lausanne, Swizterl<strong>and</strong>) in August, 2009. The comprehensive comments<br />

<strong>and</strong> suggestions of two anonymous reviewers are very highly appreciated.<br />

References<br />

Ajzen, I. (1991), "The theory of planned <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>." Organizational Behav<strong>ior</strong> <strong>and</strong> Human Decision Processes,<br />

50, 179-211.<br />

Avineri, E., Prashker, J., N. (2006), "The impact of travel time information on travelers’ learning under<br />

uncertainty." Transportation, 33 (4), 393-408.<br />

Axhausen K & Gärling (1992) Activity-based approaches to travel analysis: conceptual frameworks, models <strong>and</strong><br />

research problems. Transport Reviews 12: 324–341.<br />

Bamberg, S., Ajzen, I., Schmidt, P. (2002), "Choice of travel mode in the Theory of Planned Behav<strong>ior</strong>: The<br />

Roles of past <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, habit, <strong>and</strong> reasoned action." Basic <strong>and</strong> Applied Social Psychology, 25, 175 -<br />

187.<br />

Bamberg, S., Rolle, D., Weber, C. (2003), "Does habitual car use not lead to more resistance to change of travel<br />

mode." Transportation, 30, 97-108.<br />

Banister, D. (1994), Equity <strong>and</strong> Acceptability Questions in Internalising the Social Costs of Transport,<br />

Internalising the Social Costs of Transport, OECD, Paris.<br />

<strong>Ben</strong>-Akiva, M. Lerman, S. R (1985), Discrete Choice Analysis: Theory <strong>and</strong> Application to Travel Dem<strong>and</strong>,<br />

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong>, E., Erev, I., Shiftan, Y. (2008), " The combined effect of information <strong>and</strong> experience on drivers’<br />

route-choice <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>." Transportation, 35 (2), 165-177.<br />

<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong>, E., <strong>Ettema</strong>, D. (2009), "Carrots versus sticks: Rewarding commuters for avoiding the rush-hour - a<br />

study of willingness to participate." Transport Policy, 16, 68-76.<br />

<strong>Ben</strong>-<strong>Elia</strong>, E., Shiftan, Y. (2010), "Which road do I take? A learning based model of route-choice <strong>and</strong> real-time<br />

information." Transportation Research A (44), 249-264.<br />

Berridge, K. C. (2001), "Rewarding learning: reinforcement, incentives <strong>and</strong> expectations." The Psychology of<br />

Learning <strong>and</strong> Motivation, D. L. Medin, ed., Academic Press.<br />

Bhat, C. (1999), "An analysis of evening commute stop-making <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> using repeated choice observation<br />

from a multi-day survey." Transportation Research B., 33, 495–510.<br />

Bliemer, M. C. J., van Amelsfort, D. H. "Rewarding instead of charging road users: a model case study<br />

investigating effects on traffic conditions." paper presented at 3rd Kuhmo-Nectar Summer School <strong>and</strong><br />

Conference, Amsterdam, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Bonsall, P., Shires, J., Maule, J., Matthews, B., Beale, J. (2007), "Responses to complex pricing signals: Theory,<br />

evidence <strong>and</strong> implications for road pricing." Transportation Research A, 41 (7), 672-683.<br />

Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., Pierce, W. D. (2001), "Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation:<br />

Te myth continues." The Behav<strong>ior</strong> Analyst, 24, 1-44.<br />

Cameron, J., Pierce, W. D. (1994), "Reinforcement, reward <strong>and</strong> intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis." Review<br />

of Educational Research, 64, 363-423.<br />

Cao, X., Mokhtarian, P.L. (2005), How do individuals adapt their personal travel? Objective <strong>and</strong> subjective<br />

influences on the consideration of travel-related strategies for San Francisco Bay Area commuters,<br />

Transport Policy, 12 (4), 291-302.<br />

16


Cherchi, E. "Modelling individual preferences: State of the art, recent advances <strong>and</strong> future directions." paper<br />

presented at 12th International Conference on Travel Behaviour <strong>and</strong> Research, Jaipur.<br />

Deci, E. L. (1971), "Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation." Journa of Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Psychology, 18, 105-115.<br />

Deci, E. L. (1975), Intrinsic motivation, Plenum Press, New York.<br />

Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. (1985), Intrinsic motivation <strong>and</strong> self determination in human <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>, Plenum Press, New<br />

York.<br />

Eriksson, L., Garvill, J., Nordlund, A. M. (2006), "Acceptability of travel dem<strong>and</strong> management measures: The<br />

importance of problem awareness, personal norm, freedom, <strong>and</strong> fairness." Journal of Environmental<br />

Psychology, 26, 15-26.<br />

<strong>Ettema</strong>, D., Knockaert, J., Verhoef, E. "Using Incentives as Traffic Management Tool: Empirical Results of the<br />

“Peak Avoidance” Experiment.", Transportation Letters, 2, 39-51.<br />

European Commission. (2006a), Keep Europe moving – Sustainable mobility for our continent. (Mid-term<br />

review of the European Commission’s 2001 Transport White Paper), Communication from the<br />

Commission, European Commission, COM(2006)314.<br />

European Commission. (2006b), Raising Awareness of ICT for Smarter, Safer <strong>and</strong> Cleaner Vehicles,<br />

Communication from the Commission, European Commission, COM(2006)59.<br />

Fishbein, M., Ajzen, I. (1975), Belief, Attitude, Intention <strong>and</strong> Behaviour: An Introduction to Theory <strong>and</strong><br />

Research, Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA.<br />

Fujii, S., Gärling, T., Kitamura, R. (2001), "Changes in drivers' perceptions <strong>and</strong> use of public transport during a<br />

freeway closure: Effects of temporary structural change on cooperation in a real-life social dilemma."<br />

Environment <strong>and</strong> Behaviour, 33, 796-808.<br />

Fujii, S., Kitamura, R. (2003), "What does a one-month free bus ticket do to habitual? An experimental analysis<br />

of habit <strong>and</strong> attitude change ", Transportation, 30, 81-95.<br />

Gärling, T., Axhausen, K. W. (2003), "Introduction: Habitual travel choice." Transportation, 30, 1-11.<br />

Gärling, T., Fujii, S. (2006), "Travel <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> modification: Theories, methods <strong>and</strong> programs." paper presented<br />

at 11th IATBR conference, Kyoto, Japan.<br />

Gärling , T., Fujii, S., Boe, O. (2001), "Empirical tests of a model of determinants of script-based driving<br />

choices." Tranportation Research F, 4, 89-102.<br />

Gärling, T., Garvill, J. (1993), "Psychological explanations of participants in everyday activities." Behaviour<br />

<strong>and</strong> Environment: Psychological <strong>and</strong> Geographical Approaches, T. Gärling <strong>and</strong> R. G. Golledge, eds.,<br />

Elsevier, Amsterdam.<br />

Gärling , T., Gillholm, R., Gärling , A. (1998), "Reintroducing attitude theory in travel <strong>behav</strong>iour research: The<br />

validity of an interactive interview procedure to predict car use." Transportation, 25, 147-167.<br />

Gärling, T., Jakobsson, C., Loukopoulos, P., Fujii, S. (2004), Adaptation of Private Car Use in Response to<br />

Travel Dem<strong>and</strong> Management Measures: Potential Roles of Intelligent Transportation Systems, Journal<br />

of Intelligent Transportation Systems Technology, Planning, <strong>and</strong> Operations, 8 (4), 189-194.<br />

Geller, E. (1989), "Applied <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> analysis <strong>and</strong> social marketing: An integration for environmental<br />

preservation." Journal of Social Issues, 45, 17-36.<br />

Giuliano, G. (1994), Equity <strong>and</strong> fairness considerations of congestion pricing, in Special Report 242: Curbing<br />

gridlock: Peak-period fees to relieve traffic congestion. vol 2., Transportation Research Board,<br />

Washington DC.<br />

Gneezy, U. (2003), The W effect of incentives, working paper, University of Chicago.<br />

Gneezy, U., Rustichini, A. (2000), "par enough or don't pay at all." Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115, 791-<br />

810.<br />

Harris, A. J., Tanner, J. C. (1974). Transport dem<strong>and</strong> models based on personal characteristics, in D. J. Buckley<br />

(ed.), Transportation <strong>and</strong> Traffic Theory, Elsevier, Amsterdam.<br />

17


Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1979), "Prospect theory – an analysis of decision under risk." Econometrica, 47<br />

(2), 263-291.<br />

Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1984), "Choices values <strong>and</strong> frames." American Psychologist, 39 (4), 341-350.<br />

D. Kahneman, J. Knetsch <strong>and</strong> R. H. Thaler (1991), The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, <strong>and</strong> Status Quo Bias:<br />

Anomalies, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5, 193-206.<br />

Knockaert, J., Bliemer, M., <strong>Ettema</strong>, D., Joksimovic, D., Mulder, A., Rouwendal, J., van Amelsfort, D. (2007),<br />

Experimental design <strong>and</strong> modelling Spitsmijden, Bureau Spitsmijden, The Hague.<br />

Kreps, D. (1997), "Intrinsic motivation <strong>and</strong> extrinsic incentives." American Economic Review Papers <strong>and</strong><br />

Proceedings, 87, 359-364.<br />

Lepper, M. R., Green, D. (1978), The hidden costs of reward: New perspectives in the psychology of human<br />

motivation, John Wiley <strong>and</strong> Sons, NJ.<br />

Lomax, T., Schrank, D. (2003), 2002 Urban Mobility Study, Texas Transportation Institute.<br />

Mahmassani, H. S., Liu, Y.-H. (1999), "Dynamics of commuting decisions <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> under advanced traveler<br />

information systems." Transportation Research. C, 7 (2), 91-107.<br />

Mayeres, I., Ochelen, S., Proost, S. (1996), "Marginal <strong>and</strong> external costs of urban transport." Transportation<br />

Research D, 1 (2), 111-130.<br />

McFadden, D. (2007), "The <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>al science of transportation ", Transport Policy, 14, 269-274.<br />

McFadden, D., Train, K. (2000), "Mixed MNL models of discrete response." Journal of Applied Econometrics,<br />

15, 447-470.<br />

Nijkamp, P., Shefer, D. (1998), Urban Transport Externalities <strong>and</strong> Pigouvian Taxes: A Network Approach,<br />

Edward Elgar Publishing, Cheltenham.<br />

Palma, P. A., González , C. D., Carrasco, S. L. "Gender differences in time use <strong>and</strong> mobility: Time poverty <strong>and</strong><br />

dual consumption." paper presented at Time Use Observatory, Santiago, Chile.<br />

Perry, O., I. Erev, E. Haruvy. (2002), "Frequent probabilistic punishment in law enforcement ", Economics of<br />

Governance 3,71-86.<br />

Revelt, D., Train , K. (1998), "Mixed MNL models for discrete response." Review of Economics <strong>and</strong> Statstics,<br />

80, 647-657.<br />

Ronis, D. L., Yates, J. F., Kirscht, J. P. (1989), "Attitudes, decisions <strong>and</strong> habits as determinants of repeated<br />

<strong>behav</strong>iour." Attitude Structure <strong>and</strong> Function, A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, <strong>and</strong> A. G. Greenwald, eds.,<br />

Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.<br />

Rothengatter, T. (1992), "The effects of police surveillance <strong>and</strong> law enforcement on driver <strong>behav</strong>iour ", Current<br />

Psychological Reviews, 2, 349-358.<br />

Rouwendal, J., Verhoef, E. T. (2006), "Basic economic principles of road pricing: From theory to applications."<br />

Transport Policy, 13 (2), 106-114.<br />

Schuitema, G. (2003), "Pricing policies in transport." Human decision making <strong>and</strong> environmental perception:<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> assisting human decision making in real-life settings L. Hendrickx, W. Jager, <strong>and</strong> L.<br />

Steg, eds., University of Groningen, Groningen.<br />

Schwanen, T. (2007), "Gender Differences in Chauffeuring Children among Dual-Earner Families." The<br />

Professional Geographer, 59, 447-462.<br />

Schwartz S. (1977), Normative influences on altruism, in : Berkowitz L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social<br />

Psychology, vol. 10, Academic Press, London.<br />

Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., Campbell D.T., (2002), Experimental <strong>and</strong> Quasi-Experimental Designs for<br />

Generalized Causal Inference, Wadsworth, Belmont.<br />

Shaffer, V. A., Arkes, H. R. (in press), "Preference reversals in evaluations of cash versus non-cash incentives."<br />

Journal of Economic Psychology.<br />

18


Shiftan, Y., Golani, A. (2005), "Effect of Auto Restraint on Travel Behav<strong>ior</strong> ", Transportation Research Record,<br />

1932, 156-163.<br />

Simon H. A. (1987). Satisficing. In The New Palgrave: A Dictionary of Economics, Eatwell J, Miligate M,<br />

Newman P (eds.). Vol. 4: Stockton Press: New York; 243-245.<br />

Small, K. A., Verhoef, E. T. (2007), The Economics of Urban Transportation, Routledge, London.<br />

Srinivasan, K. K., Mahamassani, H. S. (2003), "Analyzing heterogeneity <strong>and</strong> unobserved structural effects in<br />

route-switching <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong> under ATIS: a dynamic kernel logit formulation." Transportation Research<br />

B., 37, 793-814.<br />

Train, K. (2002), Discrete Choice Methods with Simulation, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA.<br />

Tri<strong>and</strong>is, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.<br />

Tri<strong>and</strong>is, H. C. (1980). Values, attitudes, <strong>and</strong> interpersonal <strong>behav</strong><strong>ior</strong>. In H. E. Howe & M. M. Page (Eds.),<br />

Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1979 (pp. 195-259). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.<br />

Verhoef, E. (2008), "Road Transport Pricing: Motivation, Objectives <strong>and</strong> Design from an Economic<br />

Perspective." Pricing In Road Transport: A Multi-Disciplinary Perspective, E. Verhoef, M. Bliemer, L.<br />

Steg, <strong>and</strong> B. van Wee, eds., Edward Elgar Publishing, London.<br />

Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., van Knippenberg, A. (1997), "Attitude versus general habit: Antecedents of travel<br />

mode choice." Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 285-300.<br />

Vickrey, W. S. (1969), "Congestion theory <strong>and</strong> transport investment." American Economic Review, 59 (251-<br />

261).<br />

Viegas, J. M. (2001), "Making urban road pricing acceptable <strong>and</strong> effective: searching for quality <strong>and</strong> equity in<br />

urban mobility." Transport Policy, 8 (4), 289-294.<br />

Vonk Noordegraaf, D. M., Annema, J. A. "Employer attitude to rush-hour avoidance." proceedings of the<br />

European Transport Conference, Noordwijkerhout<br />

Walker, J. L. (2001), "Extended Discrete Choice Models: Integrated Framework, Flexible Error Structures, <strong>and</strong><br />

Latent Variables," Ph. Dthesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.<br />

19


Table 1: Participants’ characteristics<br />

Gender<br />

Education level<br />

Income €<br />

(net person/month)<br />

Household<br />

composition<br />

Cars / Household<br />

Age (years)<br />

20<br />

Money Yeti<br />

N % N %<br />

man 140 60.3 81 74.3<br />

woman 92 39.7 28 25.7<br />

Secondary 24 10.4 9 8.3<br />

Low vocational 9 3.9 5 4.6<br />

Middle vocational 64 27.7 36 33.3<br />

Higher education 134 58.0 58 53.7<br />

4500 11 4.8 3 2.8<br />

didn't answer 53 22.9 21 19.4<br />

single 35 15.2 10 9.3<br />

partner no kids 61 26.4 20 18.5<br />

partner + kids 118 51.1 73 67.6<br />

single parent 13 5.6 3 2.8<br />

other 4 1.7 2 1.9<br />

1 120 51.9 45 41.7<br />

2 103 44.6 59 54.6<br />

3+ 8 3.5 4 3.7<br />

Mean 41.3 44.8<br />

Median 42.5 45<br />

Per.25 34 37<br />

Per.75 49 51<br />

Table 2: Reward classes* by gender <strong>and</strong> reward type (group)<br />

Money Yeti<br />

A B C D A B C D<br />

Thresholds** 5 4 2 1 15 20 23 25<br />

N<br />

Men 83 33 13 11 34 27 13 7 221<br />

62% 54% 57% 79% 72% 87% 59% 78% 65%<br />

Women 51 28 10 3 13 4 9 2 120<br />

38% 46% 44% 21% 28% 13% 41% 22% 35%<br />

Total 134 61 23 14 47 31 22 9 341<br />

* A: 3.5-5, B:2/5-3.5, C: 1-2.5, D: 0-1 trips/week.<br />

** Money: maximum number of eligible rewards per week; Yeti: number of credits at the end of 5 weeks required to keep<br />

the phone.


Table 3: Mean values of response <strong>and</strong> between-group differences<br />

Money Yeti between-group<br />

difference<br />

Resp. Measurement N Mean s.d N Mean s.d t-stat Mann-<br />

Whitney<br />

U<br />

Rush Preliminary Survey - S 232 89.9 15.44 108 89.8 13.25 0.03 12,153<br />

Hour Pre test - R1 219 48.7 38.17 107 44.0 36.69 1.06 10,395<br />

3€ - R2 229 22.4 28.66<br />

7€ - R3 231 17.7 26.70<br />

3-7€ - R4 230 17.8 26.88<br />

No Credit - R5 109 31.0 31.26<br />

Credit - R6 107 15.4 21.95<br />

Post test - R7 225 47.3 38.91 101 37.6 37.64 2.22 9,808<br />

Driving Preliminary Survey - S<br />

Early Pre test - R1 219 22.7 33.03 107 22.0 33.06 0.20 11,489<br />

3€ - R2 229 37.7 37.87<br />

7€ - R3 231 41.8 38.05<br />

3-7€ - R4 230 42.4 38.49<br />

No Credit - R5 109 24.8 33.24<br />

Credit - R6 107 33.8 37.12<br />

Post test - R7 225 24.3 35.47 101 27.7 37.57 -0.78 10,768<br />

Driving Preliminary Survey - S<br />

Late Pre test - R1 219 10.1 22.29 107 20.3 31.32 -3.37 9,740<br />

3€ - R2 229 17.7 27.64<br />

7€ - R3 231 15.9 26.20<br />

3-7€ - R4 230 15.9 27.15<br />

No Credit - R5 109 24.1 32.19<br />

Credit - R6 107 25.6 31.76<br />

Post test - R7 230 15.9 27.15 101 19.1 31.87 -1.87 10,327<br />

Not Preliminary Survey - S<br />

Driving Pre test - R1 219 18.5 30.25 107 13.8 26.00 1.39 10,816<br />

3€ - R2 229 22.2 30.70<br />

7€ - R3 231 24.6 30.72<br />

3-7€ - R4 230 23.9 32.73<br />

No Credit - R5 109 20.1 28.50<br />

Credit - R6 107 25.1 31.79<br />

Post test - R7 225 15.6 29.02 101 15.6 30.28 0.02 11,086<br />

21


Table 4: Results of mixed logistic regression model<br />

Alt.* Parameter Est. S.E t-test p<br />

RD Constant rush-hour driving 0.750 0.31 2.44 0.010<br />

3€ reward -1.440 0.46 -3.12


Table 5: Between-group differences on Response-associated factors<br />

Factor<br />

23<br />

Money<br />

(N=231)<br />

Yeti<br />

(N=108)<br />

Socio-demographics Gender (woman) 40% 26%<br />

Alternative modes<br />

Schedules<br />

Difficulties<br />

Support measures<br />

travel information<br />

High education 58% 53%<br />

Other modes used for commuting 21% 19%<br />

Public transport is realistic alternative 35% 32%<br />

Cycling is realistic alternative 20% 14%<br />

Usual departure time (hour.min) 7.52 7.57<br />

Preferred start of work time (hour.min) 8.24 8.35<br />

Start work later (days) 3.5 3.64<br />

Telework (days) 0.46 0.58<br />

Chauffeuring children duties 16% 27%<br />

High effort perceived with changing <strong>behav</strong>iour 6% 9%<br />

Problems with regional rain 33% 29%<br />

p*<br />

0.01<br />

0.43<br />

0.58<br />

0.63<br />

0.21<br />

0.27<br />

0.20<br />

0.52<br />

0.37<br />

0.02<br />

0.29<br />

0.72<br />

Arrangements with employer 34% 55%


Figure 1: Average response shares by group by treatment<br />

24

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!