29.08.2013 Views

Linking elements in compounds - LOT publications

Linking elements in compounds - LOT publications

Linking elements in compounds - LOT publications

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

INTRODUCTION<br />

differ <strong>in</strong> detail, they both consider derivational and <strong>in</strong>flectional processes of a<br />

language to be organized at different levels. A simplified schema of these<br />

theories (P<strong>in</strong>ker, 1999, p. 180) is presented <strong>in</strong> Figure 1.<br />

Memorized<br />

roots (<strong>in</strong>cl.<br />

irregulars)<br />

Figure 1. A simplified schema of morphology based on Kiparsky (P<strong>in</strong>ker, 1999,<br />

p. 180).<br />

P<strong>in</strong>ker (1999) assumes that irregular forms are stored as a whole <strong>in</strong> the mental<br />

lexicon, while regular forms are created by rules. The level-order<strong>in</strong>g hypothesis<br />

(Kiparsky, 1982) and the words-and-rules theory (e.g., Berent & P<strong>in</strong>ker, 2007,<br />

2008; P<strong>in</strong>ker, 1999) predict that irregular words like mice are available as <strong>in</strong>put<br />

for <strong>compounds</strong> (e.g., mice-eater), while regular forms like rats are not (*rats-eater,<br />

see Figure 1). This thesis will present new data based on Dutch <strong>compounds</strong><br />

that may falsify the hypothesis that word formation is constra<strong>in</strong>ed by regular<br />

<strong>in</strong>flection.<br />

Complex<br />

word<br />

formation<br />

1.2 Aims and outl<strong>in</strong>e of the thesis<br />

Regular<br />

<strong>in</strong>flection<br />

Syntax<br />

The ma<strong>in</strong> aim of this thesis is to <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether speakers of Dutch<br />

<strong>in</strong>terpret Dutch modifiers with l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g en <strong>in</strong> spoken <strong>compounds</strong> as plural forms.<br />

It elaborates on the <strong>in</strong>fluence of form variation on numerosity. A substantial<br />

amount of variation can be found <strong>in</strong> the occurrence of a l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g element <strong>in</strong><br />

Dutch <strong>compounds</strong>. Some <strong>compounds</strong> occur with and without a l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g<br />

element, like bloem+bak and bloem+en+bak (‗flower box‘). Form variation may<br />

also occur <strong>in</strong> Dutch dialects, for example appel+e+boum (‗apple tree‘) with<br />

l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g [ǝ] <strong>in</strong> the Limburg dialect, versus appel+boom (‗apple tree‘) without<br />

l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g element <strong>in</strong> standard Dutch. Perceived numerosity of a compound‘s<br />

19

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!