13.01.2014 Views

The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia: An Historical and ...

The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia: An Historical and ...

The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia: An Historical and ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>The</strong> <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong>: <strong>An</strong> <strong>Historical</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Philosophical <strong>An</strong>alysis<br />

Kenneth S. Kendler<br />

Kenneth F. Schaffner<br />

Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, Volume 18, Number 1, March<br />

2011, pp. 41-63 (Article)<br />

Published by <strong>The</strong> Johns Hopkins University Press<br />

DOI: 10.1353/ppp.2011.0005<br />

For additional information about this article<br />

http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/ppp/summary/v018/18.1.kendler.html<br />

Access Provided by Purdue University at 03/04/13 6:09PM GMT


<strong>The</strong> <strong>Dopamine</strong><br />

<strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Schizophrenia</strong>:<br />

<strong>An</strong> <strong>Historical</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Philosophical <strong>An</strong>alysis<br />

Kenneth S. Kendler <strong>and</strong><br />

Kenneth F. Schaffner<br />

Abstract: <strong>The</strong> dopamine (DA) hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

(DHS) has, since its inception over 30 years ago,<br />

been among the most prominent etiologic theories in<br />

psychiatry. This essay begins by summarizing the history<br />

<strong>of</strong> its emergence <strong>and</strong> efforts to empirically test it<br />

through the examination <strong>of</strong> (i) cerebrospinal fluid DA<br />

metabolites, (ii) neuroendocrine measures, (iii) clinical<br />

response to psychostimulants, (iv) brain levels <strong>of</strong> DA<br />

<strong>and</strong> its metabolites, (v) brain studies <strong>of</strong> DA receptors,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (vi) genetic association studies. We then examine<br />

how successful the DHS has been <strong>and</strong> by what criteria<br />

its performance should be evaluated. In this process, it<br />

is critical to distinguish the etiological DHS from the<br />

pharmacological DA hypothesis <strong>of</strong> neuroleptic action.<br />

Although the DHS stimulated much science, most efforts<br />

to empirically validate it have failed, in contrast with<br />

the well-supported pharmacological DA hypothesis <strong>of</strong><br />

neuroleptic action. Nonetheless, the DHS has held the<br />

status <strong>of</strong> a scientific paradigm defended by some with<br />

great avidity. Like other temporally extended theories,<br />

the DHS in its most general form is relatively nonspecific<br />

<strong>and</strong> protean in nature. In its evolution through<br />

successive more specific forms, <strong>of</strong>ten embodying ad hoc<br />

modifications <strong>of</strong> subsidiary hypotheses, it became very<br />

difficult to falsify. Although stimulating much research,<br />

it has not produced a progressive research program<br />

generating various novel <strong>and</strong> confirmed predictions<br />

about schizophrenia. For most <strong>of</strong> its history, the DHS<br />

has lacked a viable competing alternative theory against<br />

which it could be incisively compared. Sociological factors,<br />

especially the rise to prominence <strong>of</strong> the biological<br />

psychiatry movement, <strong>and</strong> the conflation <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

<strong>and</strong> the DA theory <strong>of</strong> antipsychotic drug action have<br />

probably played an important role in its persistence.<br />

Psychiatry needs theories with higher levels <strong>of</strong> specificity<br />

<strong>and</strong> falsifiability. As the science <strong>of</strong> psychiatry matures,<br />

the field needs to become more self-critical about the<br />

validity <strong>of</strong> its theories.<br />

Keywords: dopamine, schizophrenia, etiology, philosophy<br />

<strong>of</strong> science<br />

This essay selectively reviews, from an<br />

historical <strong>and</strong> philosophical perspective,<br />

the dopamine (DA) hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

(DHS; Table 1 lists the abbreviations<br />

used in this essay). Our goal is not to adjudicate<br />

the validity <strong>of</strong> the theory—although we arrive at<br />

a generally skeptical conclusion—but to focus on<br />

the process whereby the DHS has evolved over<br />

time <strong>and</strong> been evaluated. Since its inception, the<br />

DHS has been the most prominent etiologic theory<br />

in psychiatry <strong>and</strong> is still referred to widely in current<br />

textbooks (e.g., Buchanan <strong>and</strong> Carpenter, Jr.<br />

2005, 1336; Cohen 2003, 225; Gazzaniga 2004,<br />

© 2011 by <strong>The</strong> Johns Hopkins University Press


42 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

1257; K<strong>and</strong>el et al. 2000, 1200). Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

its origins <strong>and</strong> evolution should help to clarify the<br />

nature <strong>of</strong> modern psychiatry.<br />

This essay has two parts. First, we review<br />

how the DHS emerged, how it was subsequently<br />

tested, <strong>and</strong> how well its predictions were verified.<br />

We are necessarily selective because the DHS has<br />

generated a huge literature. We concentrate on<br />

earlier episodes in its history where the results <strong>of</strong><br />

empirical efforts at verification are relatively clear.<br />

We examine the modern areas <strong>of</strong> genetics <strong>and</strong><br />

receptor imaging, but given the great flux these<br />

fields are now in, definitive conclusions about<br />

their ability to verify predictions <strong>of</strong> the DHS are<br />

not possible. Second, we evaluate the DHS from<br />

the perspective <strong>of</strong> prominent theories <strong>of</strong> the nature<br />

<strong>of</strong> scientific progress.<br />

(For most <strong>of</strong> this essay, we are forced by<br />

historical circumstance to make the unrealistic<br />

assumption that schizophrenia is an etiologically<br />

homogenous entity. This is because, despite huge<br />

amounts <strong>of</strong> effort <strong>and</strong> many proposed typologies,<br />

no broadly validated or deeply meaningfully method<br />

for subdividing schizophrenia has yet emerged<br />

that approaches the wide acceptance <strong>of</strong> the division<br />

<strong>of</strong> diabetes mellitus into types I <strong>and</strong> II (or<br />

even the lower st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>of</strong> the division <strong>of</strong> affective<br />

illness into unipolar <strong>and</strong> bipolar). Although there<br />

are important exceptions, the large majority <strong>of</strong><br />

the literature on the etiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia that<br />

we here review assumes etiologic homogeneity.)<br />

<strong>An</strong> <strong>Historical</strong> Sketch <strong>of</strong><br />

the <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Methodology<br />

For empirical questions in this selective review,<br />

we relied, whenever possible, on the most recent<br />

meta-analyses or reviews found through PubMed.<br />

Rise <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

requires an appreciation <strong>of</strong> the scientific context<br />

in which it arose (Baumeister <strong>and</strong> Francis 2002;<br />

Valenstein 1998). It is difficult to now recreate<br />

the excitement felt by the researchers who, in the<br />

1950s <strong>and</strong> 1960s, were clarifying “the chemical<br />

language <strong>of</strong> the brain” (Carlsson 2001). Although<br />

chemical neurotransmission was demonstrated<br />

in the peripheral nervous system in the 1920s,<br />

debates about whether nerve cells in the brain<br />

communicated by chemical or electrical means<br />

raged well into the 1960s. In 1957, Montagu was<br />

the first to discover DA in brain tissue. This finding<br />

was quickly followed by the dramatic results<br />

from Ehringer <strong>and</strong> Hornykiewicz (Kopin 1993) <strong>of</strong><br />

the lowered content <strong>of</strong> DA in post-mortem brains<br />

<strong>of</strong> patients dying with Parkinson’s disease. This<br />

finding led directly to the impressive treatment<br />

effects obtained by giving Parkinson patients L-<br />

dopa (Kopin 1993).<br />

A “paradigm shift” took place between 1960<br />

<strong>and</strong> 1965 when the budding field <strong>of</strong> neuropharmacology<br />

went from skepticism to belief that the<br />

monoamines—particularly DA, norepinephrine,<br />

<strong>and</strong> serotonin—were chemical mediators between<br />

nerve cells in brain (Carlsson 2001). In the early<br />

to mid 1960s, hist<strong>of</strong>luorescence stains were developed<br />

whereby the cell bodies <strong>and</strong> the neuronal<br />

pathways for the monoamine neurotransmitters<br />

DA, norepinephrine, <strong>and</strong> serotonin could be traced<br />

(e.g., Dahlstrom et al. 1962; Dahlstrom <strong>and</strong> Fuxe<br />

1964; Falck et al. 1982; Fuxe et al. 1966).<br />

<strong>The</strong> first clear articulation <strong>of</strong> the DHS was made<br />

by van Rossum in 1967:<br />

When the hypothesis <strong>of</strong> the DA blockade by neuroleptic<br />

agents can be further substantiated it may have fargoing<br />

consequences for the pathophysiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia.<br />

Overstimulation <strong>of</strong> DA receptors could then be part <strong>of</strong><br />

the etiology. (p. 327)<br />

As this quote indicates, a key early impetus<br />

for the DHS was the discovery by Carlsson <strong>and</strong><br />

Lindqvist in 1963 that neuroleptic drugs effective<br />

in the treatment <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia increased DA<br />

turnover in rodent brain (that is, augmented the<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> DA being produced <strong>and</strong> degraded).<br />

However, research on the mechanism <strong>of</strong> action<br />

<strong>of</strong> stimulants also played an important role in<br />

van Rossum’s thinking (Baumeister <strong>and</strong> Francis<br />

2002). (van Rossum’s work was not cited in early<br />

American papers on the DHS. Two <strong>of</strong> the early<br />

theorists [S. Matthysse <strong>and</strong> S. Snyder, written<br />

communication, July 2006] were not aware <strong>of</strong> it<br />

until years later.) Although hinted at in a lengthy


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 43<br />

Table 1. Abbreviations<br />

Abbreviation<br />

CSF<br />

Meaning<br />

Cerebrospinal fluid<br />

D1, D2, D3, D4 <strong>and</strong> D5 Names for five distinct brain DA receptors<br />

DA<br />

DHADA<br />

DHS<br />

GH<br />

HVA<br />

IT<br />

IRF<br />

TET<br />

<strong>Dopamine</strong><br />

<strong>Dopamine</strong> hypothesis <strong>of</strong> antipsychotic drug action<br />

<strong>Dopamine</strong> hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

Growth hormone<br />

Homovanillic acid (main breakdown product <strong>of</strong> DA in humans)<br />

Increased turnover<br />

Increased receptor function<br />

Temporally extended theory<br />

1968 review by Faurbye <strong>and</strong> briefly discussed in a<br />

1972 symposium summary by Snyder, Aghajanian,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Matthysse, the first detailed <strong>and</strong> widely cited<br />

articulation <strong>of</strong> the DHS was by Matthysse in 1973.<br />

Matthysse reviews evidence that clinically effective<br />

neuroleptic drugs are distinguished from other<br />

similar agents ineffective in treating schizophrenia<br />

by their effect on DA turnover. He then asks<br />

Suppose we now assume that the hypothesis <strong>of</strong> specific<br />

blockade <strong>of</strong> DA transmission by neuroleptic drugs is<br />

true; . . . does the theory give us any clues to the neuropathological<br />

basis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia? (p. 203)<br />

He reviews what was then known about DA<br />

tracts in the brain, arguing that, although the<br />

nigrostriatal, retinal, or tubero-infundibular DA<br />

systems are unlikely c<strong>and</strong>idates for involvement<br />

in psychosis, the mesolimbic DA system could<br />

likely influence the emotional, perceptual, <strong>and</strong><br />

cognitive functions disturbed in schizophrenia.<br />

He concludes<br />

From the blocking action <strong>of</strong> neuroleptics on DA synapses,<br />

it is a relatively small step to postulate over-activity<br />

<strong>of</strong> dopaminergic transmission in schizophrenia, whether<br />

generalized or confined to one nuclear group. (p. 204)<br />

In a review paper published in 1975 (Matthysse<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lipinski 1975), Matthysse provides a more focused<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> the DHS: “too much dopamine<br />

is released at synapses in the central nervous system”<br />

(p. 558). He then states that, in fact, “there<br />

are several dopamine hypotheses <strong>of</strong> the etiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia,” including excessive release <strong>of</strong><br />

DA at synapses, hypersensitive DA receptors,<br />

underactive antagonistic neurochemical systems<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or defective feedback loops. Presciently,<br />

Matthysse’s comment about “several dopamine<br />

hypotheses” illustrates his early awareness that<br />

the “general” DHS fact in fact required elaboration<br />

or further specification using more precise<br />

subsidiary hypotheses (e.g., specifying whether<br />

there is excess release or whether the DA receptors<br />

are hypersensitive). This is a point to which we<br />

return in the next section.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DHS was also influenced by observations<br />

that chronic administration <strong>of</strong> high doses<br />

<strong>of</strong> amphetamines could produce a paranoid or


44 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

schizophrenia-like psychosis (Connell 1958). Two<br />

studies in 1968 (Griffiths et al. 1968) <strong>and</strong> 1974<br />

(<strong>An</strong>grist et al. 1974) showed that these effects<br />

occurred in non-psychotic individuals <strong>and</strong> did<br />

not result from sleep deprivation. This work was<br />

emphasized in Snyder’s early formulation <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS (Snyder 1976).<br />

Shortly after the initial articulations <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS, new scientific results began to appear <strong>of</strong><br />

relevance to the nascent theory. In 1974, Hökfelt<br />

et al. demonstrated DA terminals in limbic cortex.<br />

Shortly thereafter, the first DA receptors were isolated<br />

<strong>and</strong> it was demonstrated, in 1976, that the<br />

clinical potency <strong>of</strong> neuroleptic drugs correlated<br />

strongly with their ability to inhibit binding <strong>of</strong><br />

specific lig<strong>and</strong>s at the DA receptor (Creese et al.<br />

1976; Seeman et al. 1976).<br />

<strong>The</strong> early status <strong>of</strong> the DHS was summarized in<br />

a detailed 1976 review by Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl. <strong>The</strong>ir<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> the DHS was “schizophrenia may be<br />

related to a relative excess <strong>of</strong> DA-dependent neuronal<br />

activity.” In addition to the mesolimbic DA<br />

system, Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl focus extensively on the<br />

mesocortical DA system about which they write:<br />

It is tempting to assume that the disturbances <strong>of</strong> thinking<br />

<strong>and</strong> symbolic processes that are an essential feature <strong>of</strong><br />

many . . . [p]atients in the schizophrenia spectrum are<br />

based on dysfunction <strong>of</strong> these dopaminergic neurons<br />

with cerebral cortical terminals. (p. 24)<br />

As emphasized earlier by Matthysse, Meltzer<br />

<strong>and</strong> Stahl’s review makes clear that the “relative<br />

excess” <strong>of</strong> DA function could arise in many places<br />

in complex neuronal <strong>and</strong> biochemical systems.<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> further scientific advances had<br />

direct impact on the DHS. <strong>The</strong>se include the<br />

discovery in 1977 that low doses <strong>of</strong> the DA agonist<br />

drug apomorphine inhibits DA functioning<br />

(Aghajanian <strong>and</strong> Bunney 1977), which led to the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> the concept <strong>of</strong> the autoreceptor—receptors<br />

that sit on the pre-synaptic neuron<br />

<strong>and</strong> typically, when stimulated, inhibit neuronal<br />

firing. In 1979, two subtypes <strong>of</strong> DA receptors were<br />

isolated then termed D1 <strong>and</strong> D2 (Kebabian <strong>and</strong><br />

Calne 1979). Genes were identified for these <strong>and</strong><br />

other DA subtypes for D2 in 1988 (Bunzow et al.<br />

1988), D3 in 1990 (Sokol<strong>of</strong>f et al. 1990), <strong>and</strong> D4<br />

<strong>and</strong> D5 both in 1991 (Sunahara et al. 1991; Van<br />

Tol et al. 1991).<br />

Attempts at Empirical Validation<br />

<strong>The</strong> dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia is . . . supported<br />

by no direct evidence. No one has found anything<br />

conclusively abnormal about dopamine in body fluids or<br />

brains or schizophrenics. (Snyder 1976, 197–8)<br />

Although by the late 1970s, the DHS was the<br />

leading biological theory for schizophrenia, it was<br />

widely appreciated that the supporting evidence<br />

for the hypothesis was entirely indirect. <strong>The</strong>re<br />

then began a wide-spread effort, continuing up to<br />

the present day, to test empirical prediction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS—that individuals with schizophrenia should<br />

demonstrate a functional DA excess somewhere<br />

in their brains. This italicized expression represents<br />

a high-level-<strong>of</strong>-abstraction, central hypothesis<br />

that is common to a range <strong>of</strong> more specific<br />

elaborations or subforms <strong>of</strong> a theory. Usually, the<br />

specific elaborations arise over time, as different<br />

investigators provide the details <strong>of</strong> the theory<br />

that elaborate on the abstract central hypothesis.<br />

Those details typically specify the etiology <strong>and</strong><br />

mode <strong>of</strong> action <strong>of</strong> the excess DA (e.g., increased<br />

turnover [IT] or increased post-synaptic receptor<br />

function), the causal consequences <strong>of</strong> the excess<br />

DA (both chemical <strong>and</strong> symptomatic), as well as<br />

the location <strong>of</strong> action <strong>of</strong> the DA in the brain (is<br />

action global or regional <strong>and</strong> if the latter which<br />

region?). <strong>The</strong> abstract central hypothesis <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS <strong>and</strong> one possible manner <strong>of</strong> its elaboration<br />

is shown graphically in Figure 1. This way <strong>of</strong><br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing the nature <strong>of</strong> theory in psychiatry<br />

parallels the accounts developed both in Schaffner<br />

(1993, Chapters 3 <strong>and</strong> 5) <strong>and</strong> in Thagard (2000,<br />

Chapter 2, especially pages 34–5).<br />

From this immense literature, we review six<br />

research areas, in part because <strong>of</strong> the availability <strong>of</strong><br />

good quality reviews: (i) Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF)<br />

DA metabolites, (ii) neuroendocrine measures, (iii)<br />

clinical response to psychostimulants, (iv) brain<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> DA <strong>and</strong> its metabolites, (v) brain studies<br />

<strong>of</strong> DA receptors, <strong>and</strong> (vi) genetic association studies.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se areas, outlined in Table 2, are chosen<br />

to be representative <strong>of</strong> efforts to validate the DHS<br />

<strong>and</strong> do not reflect all relevant information about<br />

the DHS. Furthermore, the first four areas are no<br />

longer actively under investigation <strong>and</strong> so may<br />

be viewed with some historical detachment <strong>and</strong>


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 45<br />

Figure 1. Schematic view <strong>of</strong> alternative versions <strong>of</strong> the dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia (DHS) as articulated early in its history. This figure is meant<br />

to be illustrative rather than exhaustive.<br />

DHS-functional excess DA in brain<br />

<br />

<br />

Regional excess DA<br />

Global excess DA<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Mesolimbic<br />

DASystem<br />

Mesocortical<br />

DA System<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

postsynaptic<br />

receptor<br />

function<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

turnover<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

postsynaptic<br />

receptor<br />

function<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

turnover<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

postsynaptic<br />

receptor<br />

function<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

turnover


46 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

perspective. <strong>The</strong> last two areas are, by contrast,<br />

quite active foci <strong>of</strong> on-going research <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

cannot be definitively summarized at this time.<br />

CSF Studies<br />

<strong>The</strong> first empirical method widely used to test<br />

the DHS was an examination <strong>of</strong> the key DA metabolite<br />

in humans—homovanillic acid (HVA)—in<br />

the CSF <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic versus control patients.<br />

If, as predicted by the DHS, DA systems in the<br />

brains <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic individuals are overactive,<br />

this overactivity should be reflected in increased<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> HVA in CSF. This expectation <strong>of</strong> an effect<br />

on the CSF is a subsidiary hypothesis <strong>of</strong> this<br />

variant <strong>of</strong> the general DHS. By 1976, Meltzer<br />

<strong>and</strong> Stahl reviewed a number <strong>of</strong> studies inconsistent<br />

with this prediction. Indeed, they showed,<br />

if anything, a tendency toward reduced DA metabolites<br />

in the CSF <strong>of</strong> schizophrenic patients. A<br />

later meta-analysis reached the same conclusion<br />

as the earlier Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl review (Tuckwell<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kosiol 1993).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se negative results raise two important<br />

points about the DHS. First, it could be understood<br />

as predicting global changes in brain DA<br />

function or only changes in specific DA systems. In<br />

this article, we refer to these two subforms <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS as global <strong>and</strong> regional, respectively. Second,<br />

functional excess <strong>of</strong> DA function could arise in two<br />

broad ways: IT or increased post-synaptic receptor<br />

function (IRF for increased receptor function) that<br />

could in turn result from changes in the number or<br />

sensitivity <strong>of</strong> receptors. Only the IT subform <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS predicts that individuals with schizophrenia<br />

would have excess CSF levels <strong>of</strong> HVA. (Indeed,<br />

some version <strong>of</strong> the IRF form <strong>of</strong> the DHS would<br />

predict decreased levels <strong>of</strong> CSF HVA.)<br />

<strong>The</strong> non-confirmation <strong>of</strong> the predictions <strong>of</strong><br />

the DHS by the CSF findings had little impact<br />

on enthusiasm for the DHS. As noted by Meltzer<br />

<strong>and</strong> Stahl:<br />

this result should not be used to reject the hypothesis<br />

<strong>of</strong> increased DA turnover in schizophrenia since the<br />

functional activity <strong>of</strong> DA relevant to schizophrenia may<br />

not be measured by the method. (1976, 52)<br />

In humans, much <strong>of</strong> the HVA in CSF comes<br />

from the caudate nucleus (Sourkes 1973), the<br />

endpoint <strong>of</strong> the nigrostriatal DA system. We interpret<br />

Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl’s comment as suggesting<br />

that these results argue against a global DHS—in<br />

which excess DA activity is seen in all brain DA<br />

systems—but does not disprove a more restricted<br />

DHS in which excess DA activity is limited to<br />

certain regional DA systems not reflected in CSF,<br />

particularly those outside the striatum.<br />

This point was elaborated upon by Carlsson in<br />

a 1978 review, which enumerated a total <strong>of</strong> five<br />

possible subforms <strong>of</strong> the DHS including IT, IRF,<br />

<strong>and</strong> other possibilities we do not examine in detail<br />

in this review, including “deficient inactivation <strong>of</strong><br />

DA” <strong>and</strong> alterations in presynaptic DA systems.<br />

Hormonal Systems<br />

Because two pituitary hormones—growth<br />

hormone (GH) <strong>and</strong> prolactin—are significantly<br />

influenced by brain DA systems, early attempts<br />

were made to validate the DHS by determining<br />

whether expected abnormalities in these hormonal<br />

systems were seen in patients with schizophrenia.<br />

GH is phasically stimulated by DA in the arcuate<br />

nucleus <strong>of</strong> the hypothalamus. If schizophrenia<br />

were due to widespread IT in DA neurons or to<br />

IRF <strong>of</strong> the relevant DA receptors, then excess GH<br />

stimulation should be seen in schizophrenia. As<br />

reviewed by Marx <strong>and</strong> Lieberman (1998), studies<br />

<strong>of</strong> basal GH secretion in schizophrenic <strong>and</strong> control<br />

subjects have been inconsistent with a few finding<br />

increased levels <strong>of</strong> GH in schizophrenic subjects,<br />

but with most finding no difference. <strong>The</strong> IRF form<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DHS could be specifically tested by examining<br />

whether schizophrenia is associated with<br />

greater GH stimulation in response to DA agonist<br />

drugs. A modest number <strong>of</strong> studies have examined<br />

this question <strong>and</strong> results were quite variable. In<br />

aggregate, Marx <strong>and</strong> Lieberman conclude that<br />

“schizophrenic <strong>and</strong> control subjects do not appear<br />

to differ significantly in GH response to dopamine<br />

agonists” (1998, 415). <strong>The</strong> sensitivity <strong>of</strong> the GH<br />

system can also be tested by the administration <strong>of</strong><br />

GH-releasing hormone. In a review <strong>of</strong> three such<br />

studies, compared with controls, schizophrenic patients<br />

had a similar GH response to GH releasing<br />

hormone in two studies <strong>and</strong> a diminished response<br />

in a third (Skare et al. 1994).<br />

Prolactin is inhibited by DA secreted into the<br />

portal vein <strong>of</strong> the pituitary. Increased DA turnover


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 47<br />

Table 2. Selected Tests <strong>of</strong> the <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Test Prediction <strong>of</strong> DHS Main Subform Result<br />

<strong>of</strong> DHS Tested<br />

CSF levels <strong>of</strong> HVA Increased* Reg, IT --<br />

Basal levels <strong>of</strong> growth hormone Increased Reg, IT, or IRF --<br />

Growth hormone stimulation to DA agonists Increased Reg, IRF -<br />

Basal levels <strong>of</strong> prolactin Decreased Reg, IT, or IRF --<br />

Prolactin suppression by DA agonists Increased Reg, IRF --<br />

Levels <strong>of</strong> DA or HVA in post-mortem brain Increased* Glob or Reg, IT -<br />

Psychotic symptoms in response to Increased Glob, IRF +<br />

amphetamine administration<br />

Increase in vivo levels <strong>of</strong> brain DA receptors Increased Glob or Reg, IRF -+<br />

Association studies <strong>of</strong> DA-related genes<br />

D1 Altered Glob -<br />

D2 Altered Glob --<br />

D4 Altered Glob -+<br />

D5 Altered Glob ?<br />

COMT Altered Glob -<br />

DA transporter Altered Glob --<br />

DA decarboxylase Altered Glob -<br />

Abbreviations: --, results strongly against prediction <strong>of</strong> DHS; - results moderately against prediction <strong>of</strong> DHS, +- results equivocal<br />

regarding DHS; +, genetic changes in DA receptors would most likely produce IRF but other changes could produce IT; ++<br />

results strongly support prediction <strong>of</strong> DHS; Glob, global, reflecting DA function in general in the brain (or in unknown more<br />

specific areas); IRF, increased receptor function (due to increased number or sensitivity <strong>of</strong> DA receptors); IT, increased turnover;<br />

Reg, regional, reflecting DA function in specific brain areas.<br />

*Note that CSF levels <strong>of</strong> HVA <strong>and</strong> brain levels <strong>of</strong> DA or HVA primarily test for the IT version <strong>of</strong> the DHS. However, if the<br />

IRF version <strong>of</strong> the DHS were true, then you might expect the reverse result. That is, due to increased feedback due to increased<br />

receptor function, the levels <strong>of</strong> HVA or DA could be reduced.


48 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

or IRF in this system should produce reduced<br />

prolactin levels in schizophrenic subjects. Marx<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lieberman (1998) conclude that the available<br />

literature suggests no systematic differences between<br />

medication-free schizophrenic subjects <strong>and</strong><br />

control subjects in their basal level <strong>of</strong> prolactin.<br />

Several studies have also looked at the suppression<br />

<strong>of</strong> prolactin by DA agonist drugs—which would<br />

test the IRF version <strong>of</strong> the DHS—<strong>and</strong> found no<br />

systematic differences between schizophrenic patients<br />

<strong>and</strong> controls.<br />

<strong>The</strong> neuroendocrine hypotheses derived from<br />

DHS have not been empirically validated. How<br />

strong were these tests? Although we could find<br />

no published discussion <strong>of</strong> the broad relevance <strong>of</strong><br />

these findings for the DHS (as there were for the<br />

CSF results), the conclusions seem similar. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

results would argue against a global DHS but not<br />

more restricted hypotheses that postulated that<br />

IT or IRF versions <strong>of</strong> the DHS were still, true but<br />

resided elsewhere in the brain.<br />

Psychotic Symptoms in Response to<br />

Psychostimulant Administration<br />

As noted, an early key finding that helped lead<br />

to the original formulation <strong>of</strong> the DHS was the<br />

clinical observation <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia-like states<br />

resulting from administration <strong>of</strong> amphetaminelike<br />

drugs. A derivative empirical test for the DHS<br />

was developed that involved the administration,<br />

under controlled conditions, <strong>of</strong> modest doses <strong>of</strong><br />

amphetamine to individuals with schizophrenia<br />

<strong>and</strong> matched controls. <strong>The</strong> prediction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS was that schizophrenic individuals but not<br />

controls would develop psychotic symptoms upon<br />

amphetamine administration. This test would<br />

apply specifically to the IRF form <strong>of</strong> the DHS,<br />

which hypothesizes excess or supersensitive DA<br />

receptors.<br />

In a detailed review published in 1987,<br />

Lieberman et al. identified eleven controlled<br />

studies that compared the clinical response to<br />

psychostimulants in schizophrenic <strong>and</strong> nonschizophrenic<br />

comparison subjects. We were unable to<br />

locate any more recent controlled trials. Of these<br />

eleven studies (which administered amphetamine,<br />

methylphenidate or ephedrine), seven found the<br />

schizophrenics subjects to “worsen in substantially<br />

greater proportion” than non-schizophrenic controls,<br />

whereas four did not. This review paper also<br />

pooled the results across all studies <strong>and</strong> reports a<br />

highly significant difference in the rate <strong>of</strong> clinical<br />

worsening after psychostimulants in schizophrenic<br />

versus non-schizophrenic subjects (Lieberman et<br />

al. 1987).<br />

<strong>The</strong>se results seem broadly consistent with the<br />

IRF subform <strong>of</strong> the DHS. However, the quality<br />

<strong>of</strong> this test <strong>of</strong> the DHS is a function <strong>of</strong> the degree<br />

to which the effects <strong>of</strong> these psychostimulants<br />

are specific to the DA system. To take one wellcharacterized<br />

psychostimulant as an example,<br />

amphetamine is a “dirty drug” <strong>and</strong> has greater<br />

potency at causing release <strong>of</strong> norepinephrine<br />

than <strong>of</strong> DA <strong>and</strong> is about equipotent in blocking<br />

the reuptake <strong>of</strong> the two neurotransmitters (Koob<br />

<strong>and</strong> Le Moal 2006). If stimulation <strong>of</strong> DA receptors<br />

were the sole means by which amphetamine<br />

increased psychotic symptoms, then the purer DA<br />

agonist drugs ought to have greater psychogenic<br />

potential. This has not generally been observed<br />

(Depatie <strong>and</strong> Lal 2001). In summary, evidence<br />

from psychostimulant challenge studies provides<br />

some support for the DHS.<br />

Brain DA Metabolites<br />

Although beset with a range <strong>of</strong> methodological<br />

difficulties, a direct test <strong>of</strong> the IT subform <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS would be to compare levels <strong>of</strong> DA or HVA<br />

in the brains <strong>of</strong> individuals with schizophrenia <strong>and</strong><br />

controls. In their 1991 review, Davis et al. summarized<br />

five such studies in postmortem samples.<br />

In total, these studies examined four subcortical<br />

regions (amygdala, putamen, caudate, <strong>and</strong> accumbens)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the temporal, cingulate, <strong>and</strong> frontal<br />

cortex. Because several studies examined multiple<br />

brain regions, thirteen comparisons were summarized.<br />

Focusing where possible on schizophrenics<br />

who were neuroleptic-free at the time <strong>of</strong> death, no<br />

differences were found in ten comparisons <strong>and</strong> in<br />

three results were as predicted by the DHS. <strong>The</strong><br />

positive findings were from three different regions:<br />

amygdala, accumbens, <strong>and</strong> caudate. We are aware<br />

<strong>of</strong> an additional study not included in this review<br />

(Farley et al. 1977), which found no significant<br />

elevation in levels <strong>of</strong> DA or HVA in the brains <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenic individuals.


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 49<br />

<strong>The</strong> results from post-mortem studies <strong>of</strong> DA<br />

or HVA levels have not, in general, confirmed<br />

predictions <strong>of</strong> the DHS. <strong>The</strong> positive findings<br />

that have emerged are inconsistent with respect to<br />

anatomical location. <strong>The</strong>se studies provide a more<br />

direct test <strong>of</strong> the IT subform <strong>of</strong> the DHS than was<br />

possible using CSF or neuroendocrine approaches.<br />

In particular, post-mortem studies can examine<br />

abnormalities in DA function that are anatomically<br />

restricted in their effect. A complexity in the<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> these findings is that some <strong>of</strong> the<br />

IRF forms <strong>of</strong> the DHS would predict decreased<br />

brain levels <strong>of</strong> DA or HVA, the opposite <strong>of</strong> that<br />

predicted by the IT version.<br />

Brain DA Receptors<br />

As pointed out in 1975 by Matthysse <strong>and</strong><br />

Lipinski, perhaps the excess DA transmission is<br />

due to IRF in turn the result <strong>of</strong> greater numbers or<br />

increased sensitivity <strong>of</strong> brain DA receptors. When<br />

the original DHS was articulated, it could not<br />

have been foreseen that the DA receptor—which<br />

was then postulated but not yet isolated—would<br />

first become two <strong>and</strong> then five different receptor<br />

subtypes.<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature on post-mortem <strong>and</strong> in vivo<br />

measurement <strong>of</strong> DA receptors in schizophrenia<br />

is both large <strong>and</strong> technically complex. We rely<br />

here largely on results from the most recent metaanalysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> the literature published in 2001 (Kestler<br />

et al. 2001) <strong>and</strong> a 2005 review by Abi-Dargham<br />

(Gunderson et al. 1995). Sufficient studies were<br />

available only to examine D1 <strong>and</strong> D2 receptors.<br />

Examining both post-mortem <strong>and</strong> in vivo studies,<br />

a moderate to large effect was observed for<br />

schizophrenic versus control subjects for the D2<br />

but not the D1 receptor. Interestingly, both receptor<br />

density <strong>and</strong> affinity were elevated in patients<br />

with schizophrenia. <strong>The</strong> effects on the D2 receptor<br />

were substantially greater in patients currently or<br />

recently on neuroleptic medication. Controlling<br />

for this effect diminished but did not eliminate the<br />

observed difference. <strong>The</strong> results differed neither in<br />

post-mortem versus in vivo studies or across brain<br />

regions. A number <strong>of</strong> studies examined multiple<br />

regions. Of the thirty-six studies, thirty-four examined<br />

all or parts <strong>of</strong> the striatum (the terminal <strong>of</strong><br />

the Nigrostriatal pathway). Only four examined<br />

the nucleus accumbens (a terminal region for the<br />

mesolimbic DA pathways) <strong>and</strong> two the frontal<br />

cortex (the terminal region for the mesocortical<br />

DA pathway).<br />

Although it is clear that schizophrenic subjects<br />

have elevated D2 receptor density in their brains,<br />

the more crucial question <strong>of</strong> whether this is due to<br />

schizophrenia or neuroleptic exposure is less clear<br />

than this review (Kestler et al. 2001) indicates for<br />

at least four reasons (J. Kleinman MD, personal<br />

communication, October 2005). First, the number<br />

<strong>of</strong> drug-naïve schizophrenic subjects studied<br />

post-mortem was probably too small to provide<br />

definitive data. Second, despite evidence that the<br />

men <strong>and</strong> women might differ systematically in the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> D2 receptors, several studies included<br />

in this review were poorly matched for sex in<br />

a way that could produce artifactually positive<br />

findings. Third, two <strong>of</strong> the studies that present<br />

the strongest evidence for elevated D2 receptor<br />

numbers in drug-free subjects have a large overlap<br />

in their schizophrenic subjects (Tune et al. 1993;<br />

Wong et al. 1997). Fourth, one study showing no<br />

schizophrenia control difference in baseline D2<br />

receptor binding potential was not included in the<br />

review (Abi-Dargham et al. 2000). Abi-Dargham<br />

concludes that the findings on D2 receptors in<br />

schizophrenia post-mortem brain tissue “are<br />

related to prior neuroleptic exposure rather than<br />

to the disease process per se” (Gunderson et al.<br />

1995).<br />

Abi-Dargham reviews seventeen imaging studies<br />

<strong>of</strong> striatal D2 receptors in drug-naïve or drug-free<br />

schizophrenic patients versus controls (Gunderson<br />

et al. 1995) <strong>and</strong> finds across studies a significant<br />

but modest 12% increase. Of concern however,<br />

is a strong temporal trend in these results. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

studies can be divided into those published before<br />

1991 (n = 5), between 1991 <strong>and</strong> 1995 (n = 6), <strong>and</strong><br />

after 1995 (n = 7). <strong>The</strong> mean effect size for schizophrenia–control<br />

differences declines substantially<br />

across time: before 1991, +0.91; 1991 to 1995,<br />

+0.53; <strong>and</strong> after 1995, +0.16. <strong>The</strong>se results, along<br />

with concerns about this literature raised above,<br />

at least casts some uncertainty about the correct<br />

interpretation <strong>of</strong> the D2 in vivo studies in schizophrenia.<br />

Abi-Dargham goes on to conclude that<br />

in vivo imaging studies have consistently failed to


50 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

find evidence <strong>of</strong> altered striatal D1 receptor availability<br />

in schizophrenic patients <strong>and</strong> notes that the<br />

modest number <strong>of</strong> studies <strong>of</strong> prefrontal D1 receptors<br />

in schizophrenia have produced no consistent<br />

evidence <strong>of</strong> alterations (Gunderson et al. 1995).<br />

Even after recognition <strong>of</strong> these problems, D2<br />

receptor studies provide some <strong>of</strong> the strongest<br />

direct empirical verification <strong>of</strong> the DHS obtained<br />

to date, <strong>of</strong> particular relevance to the IRF subform<br />

<strong>of</strong> the general theory. However, contrary to<br />

prior suggestions that a regional DHS was most<br />

plausible, these results are supportive <strong>of</strong> a global<br />

DHS, because excess DA receptors have been seen<br />

across three <strong>of</strong> the major brain DA systems. In fact,<br />

most <strong>of</strong> the positive evidence coming from a DA<br />

system—the nigrostriatal—that earlier advocates<br />

<strong>of</strong> the theory had argued was not likely to be<br />

primarily involved in schizophrenia. As may be<br />

recalled, the nigrostriatal DA system contributes<br />

most strongly to CSF HVA concentration. <strong>The</strong><br />

negative results from the CSF HVA studies were<br />

discounted largely because the nigrostriatal system<br />

was thought to have little to do with the etiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia.<br />

Genetics<br />

At the inception <strong>of</strong> the DHS, it would have<br />

been impossible to foresee that tools would develop<br />

to test whether variants in genes involved<br />

in the brain DA system influence susceptibility to<br />

schizophrenia. Given that genetic factors impact<br />

strongly on liability to schizophrenia (Sullivan et<br />

al. 2003), if the DHS were true, some <strong>of</strong> this genetic<br />

risk would likely be expressed in variants that<br />

directly or indirectly resulted in increased brain<br />

DA function. This approach might be considered<br />

a further subdivision <strong>of</strong> the DHS, with specific genetic<br />

variants contributing to either the IT or IRF<br />

mechanisms, in either a global or specific regional<br />

manner (Figure 1). We do not pursue these specific<br />

issues further here.<br />

Association studies are the method <strong>of</strong> choice<br />

to determine whether specific genes are etiologically<br />

involved in a disorder. <strong>The</strong>refore, we review<br />

the large literature for those genes known to be<br />

directly involved in DA function: DA receptors,<br />

synthetic <strong>and</strong> degradative enzymes, <strong>and</strong> the DA<br />

transporter. (For a more detailed recent review <strong>of</strong><br />

this literature, see Talkowski et al. 2007).<br />

We begin with genes for which meta-analyses<br />

are available. <strong>The</strong> D2 receptor gene has been<br />

widely studied with most interest focusing on the<br />

Cys311Ser polymorphism. A recent meta-analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> 27 case-control studies reported a significant<br />

but modest association between the Cys allele <strong>and</strong><br />

schizophrenia with estimated odds ratios (ORs) <strong>of</strong><br />

1.4 (Glatt <strong>and</strong> Jonsson 2006). A meta-analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

10 studies <strong>of</strong> a different variant in the same gene<br />

(-141C insertion/deletion) found no evidence for<br />

association with schizophrenia (Glatt et al. 2004).<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature on the Ser9Gly variant in the<br />

D3 receptor gene <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia is vast, with<br />

seven published meta-analyses. <strong>The</strong> most recent <strong>of</strong><br />

these included more than 11,000 total subjects <strong>and</strong><br />

showed a very small <strong>and</strong> nonsignificant association<br />

with schizophrenia (Jonsson et al. 2004). In the D4<br />

receptor, most interest has focused on the 48-basepair<br />

repeat in exon 3. A recent meta-analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

19 studies showed no significant association with<br />

schizophrenia (Jonsson et al. 2003). Seven studies<br />

had examined a twelve base-pair repeat in exon 1<br />

<strong>and</strong> no significant association was found (Jonsson<br />

et al. 2003). However, three studies examined a<br />

promoter variant (-521C/T) <strong>and</strong> a meta-analyses<br />

<strong>of</strong> these studied indicated a modest <strong>and</strong> significant<br />

association with schizophrenia (OR = 1.22).<br />

<strong>The</strong> literature on the association between variants<br />

in the catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT)<br />

gene <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia is immense <strong>and</strong> inconsistent.<br />

Two recent meta-analyses have reached<br />

negative conclusions for the widely studied Val/<br />

Met polymorphism (Fan et al. 2005; Munafo et<br />

al. 2005) although other variants in the gene have<br />

been studied <strong>and</strong> found to be associated (e.g.,<br />

Shifman et al. 2002). A study with more than<br />

2,800 individuals looking at both the Val/Met<br />

polymorphism <strong>and</strong> a previously identified highrisk<br />

haplotype be was negative (Williams et al.<br />

2005). (A haplotype is a DNA segment so short<br />

that it tends to pass through populations intact).<br />

A recent meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> six case-control studies<br />

<strong>of</strong> the VNTR polymorphism in the 3´ untranslated<br />

region <strong>of</strong> the DA transporter showed no evidence<br />

for association with schizophrenia [Gamma et<br />

al. 2005]).<br />

<strong>The</strong> most studied variant in the DA transporter<br />

gene is a variable number t<strong>and</strong>em repeat in the<br />

3´ untranslated region <strong>of</strong> the gene. Gamma et


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 51<br />

al. (2005) performed a meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> six casecontrol<br />

studies that produced no evidence for association.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y reviewed the known family-based<br />

association studies <strong>of</strong> this polymorphism, which<br />

were also all negative.<br />

We now turn to those genes for which no<br />

meta-analyses were available. For the D1 receptor,<br />

the most recent locatable report, which contains<br />

both new results <strong>and</strong> a literature review <strong>of</strong> several<br />

polymorphisms, finds no consistent evidence for<br />

association with schizophrenia (Kojima et al.<br />

1999). For DOPA decarboxylase, we located two<br />

negative reports (Borglum et al. 2001; Zhang et<br />

al. 2004). For tyrosine hydroxylase, we found<br />

a recent negative report with a short literature<br />

review that indicated mixed prior results (Pae et<br />

al. 2003). Also noteworthy is a recent study that<br />

examined eighteen variants in twelve different<br />

key DA-related genes in 496 schizophrenic <strong>and</strong><br />

matched control subjects (Hoogendoorn et al.<br />

2005). None <strong>of</strong> these were associated.<br />

Although this review is not exhaustive, a<br />

general picture emerges. <strong>The</strong> large majority <strong>of</strong><br />

genetic studies that have attempted to validate<br />

the DHS have produced negative results. Two<br />

positive findings emerge, the best validated <strong>of</strong><br />

which is the Cys311Ser polymorphism in the D2<br />

receptor. Less well studied is a promoter variant<br />

in the D4 gene. <strong>The</strong> effect sizes <strong>of</strong> these variants<br />

are, however, modest with ORs in the range <strong>of</strong><br />

1.2 to 1.4. A monozygotic twin <strong>of</strong> an individual<br />

with schizophrenia has a risk for schizophrenia<br />

that is increased 50- to 100-fold (Sullivan et al.<br />

2003). If these gene effects are real, they account<br />

for very small proportions <strong>of</strong> the total genetic risk<br />

for schizophrenia.<br />

What do the findings from genetic association<br />

studies tell us about the DHS? <strong>The</strong> large majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> the data is inconsistent with the theory. But how<br />

strong are these tests? As pointed out by Talkowski<br />

et al. (2007), many <strong>of</strong> the association studies <strong>of</strong><br />

DA-related genes in schizophrenia have been<br />

underpowered to detect modest effect sizes that<br />

are being seen in complex diseases. Furthermore,<br />

genomic coverage has also been far from ideal for<br />

most studies. <strong>The</strong> number <strong>of</strong> genetic variants that<br />

impact on DA function in the human brain may<br />

be very large. <strong>The</strong> field has focused to date on the<br />

obvious suspects, which together may constitute a<br />

small proportion <strong>of</strong> all genes influencing brain DA<br />

systems. Individual negative findings may therefore<br />

be <strong>of</strong> limited overall significance. In the next<br />

several years, multiple genome-wide association<br />

studies <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia will be published, further<br />

clarifying the relationship between DA-related<br />

genes <strong>and</strong> risk for schizophrenia.<br />

How are we to interpret the positive findings,<br />

especially given that variants at the D2 Cys311Ser<br />

polymorphism may make a small contribution<br />

to the genetic risk for schizophrenia? Do these<br />

results confirm the DHS? Is it significant that the<br />

leading susceptibility genes for schizophrenia do<br />

not seem to have a major impact on brain DA, but<br />

rather may alter glutamate functioning (Owen et<br />

al. 2004)?<br />

Reformulations <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

In its earliest phases, the DHS focused on the<br />

full syndrome <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. However, on<br />

closer examination, the two major indirect supports<br />

for the DHS were not equally compelling<br />

for all aspects <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Neuroleptic drugs<br />

were more effective at treating positive symptoms<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia than the negative symptoms.<br />

Amphetamine-induced psychosis reflected much<br />

more the positive than the negative symptoms <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenia (Connell 1958).<br />

In the 1980s, negative symptoms became more<br />

central to the conceptualization <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

(<strong>An</strong>dreasen 1981; Crow 1980). This led to a reevaluation<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DHS, which was also prompted<br />

by studies in Japan (reviewed in Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl<br />

1976) <strong>and</strong> later in the United States (e.g., Cutler<br />

et al. 1984; Tamminga et al. 1986), showing that<br />

DA agonist drugs improved the clinical picture in<br />

schizophrenia, particularly the negative features.<br />

Although this effect could result from DA autoreceptor<br />

stimulation, they raised the question <strong>of</strong><br />

whether negative symptoms might actually be due<br />

to functional DA deficits.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se <strong>and</strong> other results lead to an effort by<br />

Davis et al. to propose, in 1991, a major revision<br />

<strong>of</strong> the theory. <strong>The</strong>ir formulation was that<br />

“schizophrenia can be characterized by hypodopaminergia<br />

in mesocortical <strong>and</strong> hyperdopaminergia<br />

in mesolimbic dopamine neurons” (Davis et al.<br />

1991). <strong>The</strong>ir proposal represents a modification


52 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

<strong>of</strong> the original DHS in two major ways. First, this<br />

proposal increased the anatomic specificity <strong>of</strong> the<br />

original hypothesis proposing specific pathways or<br />

regions for the action <strong>of</strong> DA. Second, <strong>and</strong> more<br />

radically, it reversed the direction <strong>of</strong> the original<br />

hypothesis with respect to one <strong>of</strong> these major DA<br />

tracts.<br />

Figure 2 outlines the changes in the DHS as<br />

articulated by Davis et al. Note that the apex <strong>of</strong><br />

the figure is yet more abstract <strong>and</strong> reads “DHS =<br />

dysregulation <strong>of</strong> DA in brain.” <strong>The</strong> boxes under<br />

the mesocortical DA system now read “decreased<br />

turnover” or “decreased post-synaptic receptor<br />

function.”<br />

Attempts at Empirical Evaluation <strong>of</strong><br />

the DHS—Summary<br />

We could not summarize herein all studies relevant<br />

to the DHS, reviews <strong>of</strong> which continue to<br />

appear regularly in the psychiatric literature (e.g.,<br />

Abi-Dargham 2004; Hietala <strong>and</strong> Syvalahti 1996;<br />

Willner 1997). For example, we have not examined<br />

studies <strong>of</strong> plasma levels <strong>of</strong> HVA, which may<br />

help to evaluate a “global” DHS. Surprisingly, no<br />

meta-analyses or detailed reviews <strong>of</strong> this literature<br />

were found. Several (Garcia et al. 1989; Maas et<br />

al. 1993; Zhang et al. 2001) but not all studies<br />

(Steinberg et al. 1993) report elevated levels <strong>of</strong><br />

plasma HVA in schizophrenic versus control subjects.<br />

However, a minority <strong>of</strong> plasma HVA comes<br />

from central DA neurons (Amin et al. 1995). Furthermore,<br />

plasma HVA levels can be substantially<br />

influenced by state variables (Csernansky <strong>and</strong><br />

Newcomer 1994), such as diet (Donnelly et al.<br />

1996; Kendler et al. 1983), exercise (Kendler et al.<br />

1983), <strong>and</strong> mental stress (Sumiyoshi et al. 1999).<br />

Studies <strong>of</strong> plasma HVA are unlikely to provide<br />

definitive evidence about the validity <strong>of</strong> the DHS.<br />

We also have not reviewed the empirical literature<br />

favoring the role <strong>of</strong> DA in neuroleptic action.<br />

We do not dispute the strength <strong>of</strong> this evidence,<br />

but argue (see below) that it is <strong>of</strong> limited relevance<br />

to an evaluation <strong>of</strong> the DHS.<br />

Table 2 summarizes the empirical tests <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS that we have evaluated. Two summary points<br />

are noteworthy. First, overall, the DHS has performed<br />

poorly. Few <strong>of</strong> its predictions have been<br />

empirically validated (although we do not claim<br />

that the quality <strong>of</strong> these tests has been uniformly<br />

high or that we have reviewed all the relevant<br />

information). Second, many <strong>of</strong> these tests were<br />

not evaluating the same scientific hypothesis.<br />

Some were testing the IT version <strong>of</strong> the DHS <strong>and</strong><br />

others the IRF version. Some were testing regional<br />

versions <strong>of</strong> the DHS <strong>and</strong> other global versions.<br />

In seeking a perspective on the current status<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DHS, we could find no more appropriate<br />

view than this recent quote from in a text edited by<br />

Carlsson <strong>and</strong> Lecrubier (2004, 99) from original<br />

comments by Stahl:<br />

In spite <strong>of</strong> much research effort over more than 30<br />

years, direct evidence for changes in brain dopamine<br />

concentrations or in dopamine receptor densities<br />

remained frustratingly intangible. However, in recent<br />

years a new lease <strong>of</strong> life has been given to this hypothesis.<br />

. . . Rather than seeing dopamine hyperactivity as a<br />

primary source <strong>of</strong> pathology in schizophrenia, we now<br />

see this rather as a vector <strong>of</strong> a more complex primary<br />

etiology, which allows the expression <strong>of</strong> psychotic<br />

symptomatology. In this model, the primary deficit<br />

would lie in inappropriate information processing in the<br />

prefrontal cortex, perhaps through structural anomalies<br />

in synaptic organization during development, perhaps<br />

due to plastic changes in connectivity involving<br />

anomalies in glutamatergic transmission. In addition,<br />

the abnormalities in dopaminergic neurotransmission<br />

may be better considered as dysregulation rather than<br />

hyperactivity, with certain symptoms, particularly cognitive<br />

ones being related to insufficient dopaminergic<br />

activity in the cortex.<br />

<strong>The</strong> simpler form(s) <strong>of</strong> the DHS—as originally<br />

postulated—are difficult to recognize in this formulation.<br />

Consistent with Davis’s modification,<br />

the emphasis is on DA “dysregulation” rather<br />

than hyperactivity. DA abnormalities are no longer<br />

given etiologic primacy, which in this account<br />

seems to have been shifted to glutamate. If this<br />

reflects a “new lease on life” for the DHS, it is<br />

in the form <strong>of</strong> a substantially altered theory, <strong>and</strong><br />

even more likely, as a virtual replacement <strong>of</strong> the<br />

original DHS. This is an issue we return to toward<br />

the end <strong>of</strong> the next section.<br />

Philosophical <strong>An</strong>alysis<br />

How successful has the DHS been <strong>and</strong>, by<br />

what criteria should we evaluate its performance?<br />

Trying to answer this question leads us through<br />

several major theories <strong>of</strong> scientific progress <strong>and</strong> to<br />

two further questions: (i) Would a more produc-


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 53<br />

Figure 2. Schematic <strong>of</strong> the revised DHS proposed by Davis et al (1991).<br />

DHS – dysregulation <strong>of</strong> DA in brain<br />

Down regulated<br />

Mesocortical<br />

DA System<br />

<br />

Up regulated<br />

Mesolimbic<br />

DA System<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

turnover<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

postsynaptic<br />

receptor<br />

function<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

turnover<br />

Due to<br />

increased<br />

postsynaptic<br />

receptor<br />

function<br />

<br />

<br />

tive theoretical approach toward the etiology <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenia have been possible? <strong>and</strong> (ii) Why<br />

has the DHS persisted despite its relatively poor<br />

empirical track record?<br />

Evaluation <strong>of</strong> the DHS in the<br />

Light <strong>of</strong> Four <strong>The</strong>ories About<br />

the Nature <strong>of</strong> the Scientific<br />

Progress<br />

Karl Popper<br />

In their influential review, Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl<br />

(1976) wrote<br />

If, as Karl Popper says, the value <strong>of</strong> a hypothesis lies<br />

not so much in whether it is right or wrong but in its<br />

capacity to stimulate attempts to refute it, then the DA<br />

hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia has been extraordinarily<br />

successful.<br />

A PubMed search for “schizophrenia” or “psychosis”<br />

<strong>and</strong> “dopamine” yielded 5,880 articles<br />

from 1963 to March 2008. If the criterion for success<br />

<strong>of</strong> a theory is its ability to generate research,<br />

then Meltzer <strong>and</strong> Stahl are correct <strong>and</strong> DHS has<br />

been strikingly successful. However, Popper’s work<br />

on the evaluation <strong>of</strong> scientific theory repeatedly<br />

stressed not the generativity <strong>of</strong> a theory but its<br />

falsifiability (Magee 1982). Only by examining<br />

how a theory could be disproven was it possible,<br />

according to Popper, to discriminate a truly scientific<br />

from <strong>and</strong> a pseudo-scientific theory (Popper<br />

1959, 1962). Furthermore, according to Popper,<br />

theories could vary in their degree <strong>of</strong> falsifiability.<br />

<strong>The</strong>ories <strong>of</strong> greater falsifiability are, Popper argued,<br />

<strong>of</strong> greater scientific value because they make<br />

bolder <strong>and</strong> more informative claims. Scientific<br />

progress would consist <strong>of</strong> a series <strong>of</strong> conjectures


54 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

<strong>and</strong> refutations, as newer <strong>and</strong> yet more falsifiable<br />

theories were advanced.<br />

How would the DHS fare on these Popperean<br />

grounds? At its inception, the DHS was highly abstract<br />

<strong>and</strong> nonspecific, postulating that the etiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> a complex neuropsychiatric syndrome was due<br />

to hyperactivity in one neurotransmitter system.<br />

Even at the time <strong>of</strong> the articulation <strong>of</strong> the DHS,<br />

neurotransmitter systems were known to be complex<br />

<strong>and</strong> diverse, with the same neurotransmitter<br />

being used in different pathways with distinct<br />

functional roles. Control mechanisms existed at<br />

many levels, both within <strong>and</strong> between cells. Overactivity<br />

<strong>of</strong> the system could arise in many ways.<br />

As scientific underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> brain functioning<br />

advanced, this problem became worse, making<br />

it more <strong>and</strong> more difficult to articulate a single,<br />

clear test <strong>of</strong> the DHS. Because <strong>of</strong> the complexity<br />

<strong>of</strong> the systems involved, the DHS was elastic in<br />

absorbing a wide range <strong>of</strong> results. <strong>The</strong> IT version<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DHS predicted that levels <strong>of</strong> DA <strong>and</strong> HVA<br />

should be increased in the brains <strong>of</strong> individuals<br />

with schizophrenia. However, the IRF version <strong>of</strong><br />

the DHS could produce a decrease in DA <strong>and</strong> HVA<br />

levels. If abnormalities were not found in global<br />

measures <strong>of</strong> DA function, then it was because the<br />

abnormalities in schizophrenia were found only<br />

in specific brain regions or pathways.<br />

In Popper’s terms, the DHS had a low degree<br />

<strong>of</strong> falsifiability. As Popper might have predicted,<br />

given its abstract formulation, if one prediction <strong>of</strong><br />

the DHS was not confirmed, another easily took<br />

its place. This nonspecificity allowed the DHS to<br />

take on the protean features that we described. In<br />

this process, the DHS has become nearly immune<br />

from falsification. Indeed, in its latest incarnation<br />

(see quote above), it seems to be more <strong>of</strong> a general<br />

framework than a specific theory.<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is a deep irony in the observation that the<br />

DHS has fared poorly at the Popperean criterion<br />

<strong>of</strong> falsifiability. <strong>The</strong> DHS emerged at a time when<br />

the nascent field <strong>of</strong> biological psychiatry argued<br />

strongly that psychoanalysis had to be ab<strong>and</strong>oned.<br />

One key argument was that psychoanalysis was<br />

not a science that was illustrated by the protean<br />

<strong>and</strong> non-falsifiable nature <strong>of</strong> key psychoanalytic<br />

theories. Such theories, they argued, could explain<br />

anything. At its inception, early biological psychiatrists<br />

would have insisted that the DHS was the<br />

antithesis <strong>of</strong> psychoanalytic theory. However, over<br />

time, the nature <strong>of</strong> DHS as a scientific hypothesis<br />

has come to resemble the psychodynamic theories<br />

that it was meant to displace.<br />

Scientists have frequently embraced the Popperean<br />

philosophy <strong>and</strong>, on deeper reflection, later<br />

became more critical <strong>of</strong> this approach (Katz 1994),<br />

in part because the actual flexibility <strong>and</strong> practice<br />

<strong>of</strong> science go beyond the Popperean model. It is to<br />

such alternative approaches we now turn.<br />

Thomas Kuhn<br />

<strong>The</strong> work <strong>of</strong> Kuhn in his classic “<strong>The</strong> Structure<br />

<strong>of</strong> Scientific Revolutions” (Kuhn 1996) can help<br />

us to underst<strong>and</strong> the endurance <strong>of</strong> the DHS. For<br />

Kuhn, Popperean falsification did not accord with<br />

scientific practice. Rather, Kuhn suggested that<br />

scientists work within broad conceptual frameworks<br />

or “paradigms.” Like most human beings,<br />

working scientists find it difficult to think outside<br />

the framework in which they “grew up.” Indeed,<br />

Kuhn suggested that scientists typically strongly<br />

resist purported falsifications <strong>of</strong> their paradigms,<br />

<strong>and</strong> irrationally retain belief in them in the face<br />

<strong>of</strong> increasing anomalies. However, at some critical<br />

point, two things can occur together, which unleashes<br />

what he termed a “scientific revolution.”<br />

First, the accumulation <strong>of</strong> anomalies in a particular<br />

research paradigm becomes unsustainable. Second,<br />

another paradigm becomes available that<br />

does a superior job at explaining these anomalies.<br />

<strong>The</strong>n the scientific field undergoes a crisis, <strong>and</strong> experiences<br />

a “conversion” as individual researchers<br />

(more <strong>of</strong>ten the young than the old) switch to the<br />

new paradigm.<br />

<strong>The</strong> DHS probably does not rise to the level <strong>of</strong><br />

a Kuhnian paradigm, which might, for example,<br />

be an appropriate description for the “biomedical<br />

model” for psychiatric disorders. Nonetheless,<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the insights <strong>of</strong> Kuhn on the historical process<br />

<strong>of</strong> science can be usefully applied to the DHS.<br />

In particular, no revolution has occurred in<br />

theories about the etiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia that<br />

has resulted in the wholesale ab<strong>and</strong>onment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS. Why? Certainly, many empirical findings,<br />

some outlined above, are poorly explained by<br />

the DHS. However, one key ingredient has been


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 55<br />

lacking—a viable alternative theory against which<br />

the DHS has had to compete. Thus, one potential<br />

lesson from Kuhn for the DHS is a negative one:<br />

it persisted because no sufficiently prominent <strong>and</strong><br />

more successful alternative theory ever developed,<br />

in t<strong>and</strong>em with which the burgeoning empirical<br />

anomalies <strong>of</strong> the DHS could generate a true crisis<br />

leading to a subsequent revolution. At present,<br />

there is still no consensus on an alternative theory<br />

to the DHS, although most speculation seems to<br />

focus on multi-neurotransmitter neurocircuitry,<br />

in which glutamate may play a prominent role<br />

(Harrison <strong>and</strong> Weinberger 2005).<br />

Imre Lakatos<br />

In an attempt to synergize Popper’s falsifiability<br />

approach with Kuhnian insights, Lakatos<br />

(1970) developed what he termed “a methodology<br />

<strong>of</strong> research programs.” Research programs,<br />

he suggested, typically responded to problems <strong>of</strong><br />

falsifying experiments, not with wholesale rejection<br />

<strong>of</strong> their theory, but rather with modifications<br />

<strong>of</strong> its peripheral elements, leaving intact what he<br />

called its “hard core.” Furthermore, in contrast<br />

with Kuhn, Lakatos suggested that proponents <strong>of</strong><br />

different programs could communicate with each<br />

other <strong>and</strong> assess the relative strengths <strong>of</strong> different<br />

“programs.”<br />

Lakatos was particularly interested in distinguishing<br />

two kinds <strong>of</strong> programs. Progressive<br />

research programs confronted difficult empirical<br />

facts, explained them using their theories, <strong>and</strong> generated<br />

novel predictions that were subsequently<br />

empirically confirmed. Einstein’s theory <strong>of</strong> general<br />

relativity is an exemplar <strong>of</strong> a progressive research<br />

program. It predicted the unexpected “bending”<br />

<strong>of</strong> light around the sun observed by Eddington in<br />

1919 <strong>and</strong> continues to be verified by increasingly<br />

accurate physical measures <strong>of</strong> our universe (Bennett<br />

2005).<br />

By contrast, degenerating research programs<br />

confront empirical difficulties with ad hoc alterations<br />

to their hypotheses that explain the individual<br />

anomaly, but do not account for new facts.<br />

A classical example <strong>of</strong> a degenerating research<br />

program is the Ptolemaic astronomic system<br />

that, when confronted with increasingly accurate<br />

measures <strong>of</strong> planetary motion, simply added more<br />

epicycles.<br />

Which <strong>of</strong> these systems does the DHS most<br />

closely resemble? <strong>The</strong> DHS did generate a few<br />

novel findings such as the association between<br />

schizophrenia <strong>and</strong> the Cys311Ser polymorphism<br />

in the D2 gene. However, this result must be set<br />

against the many failed predictions <strong>of</strong> the theory,<br />

many <strong>of</strong> which were accompanied by post hoc<br />

rationalizations about the source <strong>of</strong> the failure.<br />

Although the picture is mixed, in general, the DHS<br />

has more closely resembled a degenerating than a<br />

progressive research program.<br />

Bayesian Approaches<br />

One difficulty with Lakatos’s vision <strong>of</strong> scientific<br />

progress is the lack <strong>of</strong> both a general measure to<br />

assess how well a program is faring <strong>and</strong> a system<br />

by which to determine how experimental successes<br />

or failures incrementally impact on the validity<br />

<strong>of</strong> an underlying theory. Several philosophers<br />

<strong>of</strong> science have developed an approach to these<br />

problems utilizing a Bayesian model <strong>of</strong> scientific<br />

reasoning (Dorling 1979; Howson <strong>and</strong> Urbach<br />

2005; Schaffner 1993).<br />

From a Bayesian perspective, the DHS began<br />

as a theory with a reasonable prior probability <strong>of</strong><br />

being true. <strong>The</strong> relationship between DA blockade<br />

<strong>and</strong> antipsychotic drug action had been well<br />

supported <strong>and</strong> consistent evidence indicated that<br />

DA agonist drugs could produce psychotic-like<br />

pictures. A Bayesian model allows each new experiment<br />

to impact on the posterior probability<br />

that a theory is true. A verification <strong>of</strong> the predictions<br />

<strong>of</strong> the theory makes it more likely to be true.<br />

A failure <strong>of</strong> verification makes it less likely to be<br />

true. <strong>The</strong> size <strong>of</strong> the change in this probability<br />

relates to the quality <strong>of</strong> the test. <strong>The</strong> better the<br />

test, the bigger is the change in either direction.<br />

So, when Eddington showed that light from a<br />

distant star was really bent when it passed by the<br />

sun, this had a big impact on the probability that<br />

Einstein’s theory <strong>of</strong> general relatively was true.<br />

It was a powerful test <strong>and</strong> the prediction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

theory was validated.<br />

How has the DHS fared from a Bayesian viewpoint?<br />

It has had many tests, most <strong>of</strong> them <strong>of</strong> only<br />

modest quality. However, a high proportion <strong>of</strong><br />

them were disconfirming—the prediction <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHS was not verified. <strong>The</strong>refore, in aggregate,<br />

although the DHS started with a reasonable prior


56 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> being true, this probability has<br />

declined over the years as non-verifications have<br />

substantially outnumbered verifications. However,<br />

it is possible that further advances in genetics or<br />

imaging that confirm predictions <strong>of</strong> the DHS could<br />

reverse this trend.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Bayesian viewpoint—one strength <strong>of</strong><br />

which is its ability to be consistently updated as<br />

new information becomes available—can also<br />

incorporate evolution <strong>of</strong> the theory itself. That<br />

is, we could see the “revised” DHS articulated by<br />

Davis et al. (Figure 2) as a new theory that might<br />

have stronger support from a Bayesian viewpoint.<br />

Other Accounts <strong>of</strong> Scientific Progress<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Issues <strong>of</strong> <strong>The</strong>ory Identity <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong>ory Substructure<br />

Other philosophers <strong>of</strong> science have grappled<br />

with the issues <strong>of</strong> scientific progress, including<br />

Laudan (1977), Shapere <strong>and</strong> Dordrecht (1984),<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kitcher (1993). <strong>The</strong>se philosophers have additional<br />

suggestions that, although possibly useful<br />

for the DHS, cannot be reviewed in the space<br />

available. It might be <strong>of</strong> interest, however, to note<br />

yet another problem for the DHS that would be<br />

raised by Laudan’s analysis.<br />

In his Progress <strong>and</strong> Its Problems, Laudan set<br />

out a problem-solving model <strong>of</strong> progress in which<br />

quite disparate “research traditions” could compete<br />

with each other by attempting to solve their<br />

own key problems more efficiently <strong>and</strong> completely.<br />

Applying this commonsensical approach to the<br />

evaluation <strong>of</strong> scientific progress, we can simply<br />

ask—Did the DHS achieve its main aim <strong>of</strong> solving<br />

the problem <strong>of</strong> the etiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia? <strong>The</strong><br />

answer, we would suggest, is no. However, we are<br />

aware that this is a high st<strong>and</strong>ard especially for<br />

the young field <strong>of</strong> psychiatry.<br />

<strong>The</strong> history <strong>of</strong> DHS’s attempted verifications<br />

<strong>and</strong> apparent falsifications reviewed above highlights<br />

the changing character <strong>of</strong> the assumptions<br />

<strong>of</strong> the DHS over time. This is a feature <strong>of</strong> what<br />

Kuhn termed paradigms <strong>and</strong> Lakatos referred<br />

to as research programs, <strong>and</strong> an adaptation <strong>and</strong><br />

elaboration <strong>of</strong> Lakatos’ distinction between the<br />

hard core <strong>and</strong> the peripheral hypotheses <strong>of</strong> a<br />

theory might shed some additional light on this<br />

history. One <strong>of</strong> us (KFS) has developed a relevant<br />

philosophical account <strong>of</strong> scientific progress that<br />

incorporates these themes from both Kuhn <strong>and</strong><br />

Lakatos (Schaffner 1993), the elements <strong>of</strong> which<br />

were introduced above <strong>and</strong> represented graphically<br />

in an atemporal manner in Figures 1 <strong>and</strong> 2<br />

(that is, we are not showing temporal progression<br />

<strong>of</strong> the changing theories over time, but only the<br />

alternatives at a given time).<br />

That analysis, extensively applied to a research<br />

program in immunology in (Schaffner 1992), suggests<br />

that a research program is best analyzed as<br />

a temporally extended theory (TET), which is in<br />

turn partitioned into high-level central hypotheses<br />

<strong>and</strong> a temporal series <strong>of</strong> more specific mechanisms<br />

that embody the central hypothesis. If the specific<br />

mechanisms, which instantiate the central high<br />

level hypothesis <strong>and</strong> are empirically testable, fail<br />

tests <strong>of</strong> verification, <strong>and</strong> these mechanisms become<br />

patched up in ad hoc ways, then any competing<br />

account with a distinctly different central<br />

high-level hypothesis becomes significantly more<br />

attractive. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, if a TET is successful,<br />

investigators working with it will develop a<br />

series <strong>of</strong> rigorous experimental tests that cannot<br />

be accounted for by other competing mechanisms.<br />

When that situation occurs, the TET account suggests<br />

that we have attained “direct evidence” for<br />

the high-level central hypothesis. Viewed from<br />

this perspective, the DHS would have the central<br />

high-level hypothesis <strong>of</strong> excess DA activity contributing<br />

to the etiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. <strong>The</strong><br />

specific mechanisms (<strong>and</strong> their related tests <strong>of</strong><br />

CSF, endocrine measures, post-mortem DA levels)<br />

have generally faired poorly in empirical tests <strong>and</strong><br />

thereby reflected poorly on the attractiveness <strong>of</strong><br />

the central hypothesis. <strong>The</strong> search for such “direct<br />

evidence” was a “holy grail” <strong>of</strong> DHS research,<br />

<strong>and</strong>, alas, like the grail, it has not yet been found.<br />

This TET analysis also suggests that only by<br />

stretching the original central or core abstract<br />

hypothesis was the theory significantly modified<br />

as in Davis et al.’s 1991 revised DHS. It is possible<br />

that the central or core hypothesis could be<br />

still further abstracted from the original DHS<br />

formulation, namely to the even more unspecific<br />

claim that “dopamine must have something to do<br />

with schizophrenia” or the problem is with DA<br />

“dysregulation.” However, such an additional


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 57<br />

abstraction makes the hypothesis more vague<br />

<strong>and</strong> harder to test empirically. Similarly, the very<br />

broad extension <strong>of</strong> the theory summarized in the<br />

quote from Stahl suggests a violation <strong>of</strong> a core<br />

hypothesis <strong>of</strong> the original DHS, <strong>and</strong> in point <strong>of</strong><br />

fact, a replacement by a more complex theory<br />

that is actually a competitor <strong>of</strong> the original DHS.<br />

In addition, <strong>and</strong> somewhat similar to the<br />

Kuhnian point about the need for a competing<br />

paradigm as a condition for a scientific revolution,<br />

from the TET perspective, a scientific field would<br />

not be ready to ab<strong>and</strong>on a central hypothesis in<br />

the absence <strong>of</strong> an alternative. In the Stahl quote,<br />

<strong>and</strong> much <strong>of</strong> the recent literature on the DHS,<br />

this emerging multi-transmitter hypothesis may<br />

be paving the way for an ultimate rejection <strong>of</strong><br />

any simple DHS.<br />

How Might We Have Done Better?<br />

Although we can be justly criticized as passing<br />

judgment from a position <strong>of</strong> hindsight, it is nonetheless<br />

worthwhile to ask how the DHS might<br />

have done better <strong>and</strong> been made more falsifiable<br />

<strong>and</strong> progressive. Psychiatric researchers might<br />

have emulated the molecular biology that was a<br />

model for them during the period that the DHS<br />

was developed, <strong>and</strong> attempted to follow a Popperlike<br />

methodology <strong>of</strong> “strong inference.” Such an<br />

approach was articulated by Platt, who in 1964<br />

attempted to summarize the methods then used<br />

by the stunningly successful discipline <strong>of</strong> molecular<br />

genetics. Platt suggested that rapid scientific<br />

progress could be made only if well-formulated<br />

alternative hypotheses were subjected to crucial<br />

experiments designed to eliminate most <strong>of</strong> the<br />

alternatives. He characterized this approach as<br />

involving the following steps:<br />

1. Devise alternative hypotheses.<br />

2. Devise one or more crucial experiments with alternative<br />

possible outcomes, each <strong>of</strong> which will, as nearly<br />

as possible, exclude one or more <strong>of</strong> the hypotheses.<br />

3. Carrying out the experiment so as to get a clean<br />

result.<br />

1’.Recycle the procedure, making subhypotheses or<br />

sequential hypotheses to refine the possibilities that<br />

remain; <strong>and</strong> so on.<br />

Closely integrated with this attempt at strong<br />

tests whose goal was dispro<strong>of</strong> was the need to<br />

simultaneously entertain multiple hypotheses. It<br />

was critical, he argued, for the working scientist<br />

to avoid being wedded to one favorite, <strong>and</strong> thus<br />

look for ad hoc excuses to save the preferred hypothesis.<br />

In this line <strong>of</strong> argument, Platt drew on<br />

Chamberlin’s 1897 methodological caution which<br />

we quote in extenso:<br />

<strong>The</strong> moment one has <strong>of</strong>fered an original explanation<br />

for a phenomenon which seems satisfactory, that<br />

moment affection for his intellectual child springs<br />

into existence, <strong>and</strong> as the explanation grows into a<br />

definite theory his parental affections cluster about his<br />

<strong>of</strong>fspring <strong>and</strong> it grows more <strong>and</strong> more dear to him. .<br />

. . <strong>The</strong>re springs up also unwittingly a pressing <strong>of</strong> the<br />

theory to make it fit the facts <strong>and</strong> a pressing <strong>of</strong> the facts<br />

to make them fit the theory. . . . To avoid this grave<br />

danger, the method <strong>of</strong> multiple working hypotheses is<br />

urged. It differs from the simple working hypothesis<br />

in that it distributes the effort <strong>and</strong> divides the affections.<br />

. . . Each hypothesis suggests its own criteria, its<br />

own method <strong>of</strong> pro<strong>of</strong>, its own method <strong>of</strong> developing<br />

the truth, <strong>and</strong> if a group <strong>of</strong> hypotheses encompass the<br />

subject on all sides, the total outcome <strong>of</strong> means <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong> methods is full <strong>and</strong> rich.<br />

<strong>The</strong> lack <strong>of</strong> a genuine alternative to the DHS<br />

facilitated Kuhnian resistance prevented a robust<br />

competition between alternative research programs,<br />

<strong>and</strong> militated against strong tests from both<br />

a Popperean <strong>and</strong> Bayesian perspective.<br />

This paper is not the place to speculate on<br />

what genuine alternatives might have emerged<br />

or what types <strong>of</strong> alternatives might best be now<br />

developed. <strong>The</strong> currently fashionable glutamate/<br />

NMDA hypothesis may have similar defects to the<br />

DHS. We suspect that successful c<strong>and</strong>idates will be<br />

multi-systems models incorporating both multiple<br />

explanatory levels as well as incorporating aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> neurodevelopment (Kendler <strong>and</strong> Parnas 2008).<br />

Future research might also succeed in efforts begun<br />

in the days <strong>of</strong> Kraepelin <strong>and</strong> Bleuler to subdivide<br />

the syndrome <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia into more discrete<br />

etiologic subtypes.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Persistence <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

When viewed from the perspective <strong>of</strong> these four<br />

major theories <strong>of</strong> scientific progress, the DHS has<br />

performed relatively poorly. Yet it has remained a


58 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

major theory in psychiatry for over four decades<br />

<strong>and</strong> still has a prominent place in many textbooks<br />

(e.g., Buchanan <strong>and</strong> Carpenter, Jr. 2005; Cohen<br />

2003 Gazzaniga 2004; K<strong>and</strong>el et al. 2000;). How<br />

can we account for its tenacity? Addressing this<br />

question requires us to segue from philosophical<br />

analyses <strong>of</strong> science to a sketch <strong>of</strong> the sociological<br />

factors that have impacted on the development<br />

<strong>and</strong> persistence <strong>of</strong> the DHS. Of these, the most<br />

important was surely the rise <strong>of</strong> the biological<br />

psychiatry movement.<br />

Psychiatry has long sought for simple etiologic<br />

explanations <strong>of</strong> the complex clinical syndromes<br />

that it describes <strong>and</strong> treats. <strong>The</strong> dramatic successes<br />

with General Paresis <strong>of</strong> the Insane <strong>and</strong> Pellagra<br />

held out the promise that similar advances would<br />

be possible for other syndromes. Beginning in the<br />

early twentieth century, an etiological approach<br />

based on increased underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> physiology<br />

<strong>and</strong> biochemistry began to have success in general<br />

medicine (Schaffner 2002). Psychoanalysis,<br />

which held a dominant position in mid-twentieth<br />

century American psychiatry, had, however, little<br />

interest in these developments, believing that the<br />

causes <strong>of</strong> psychiatric disorder lay in the mental<br />

realm. Led by a few key individuals, especially<br />

Kety, the nascent biological psychiatry movement<br />

began in the 1950s to seek physiological explanations<br />

for major psychiatric disorders, especially<br />

schizophrenia. However, a number <strong>of</strong> their early<br />

efforts (e.g., ceruloplasman, taraxein, the “pink<br />

spot” <strong>and</strong> transmethylation theories [Kety 1959,<br />

1966; Predescu et al. 1968]) went nowhere—an<br />

embarrassing situation for a young field seeking<br />

to legitimize itself. Snyder et al. summed up this<br />

situation:<br />

Innumerable “discoveries” <strong>of</strong> the biochemical abnormality<br />

in one or another body fluid <strong>of</strong> schizophrenics<br />

have relentlessly been followed by failures <strong>of</strong> confirmation<br />

in other laboratories. (1974, 1243).<br />

We suggest that the DHS was grasped with<br />

avidity by the nascent biological psychiatry<br />

movement for three reasons. First, the DHS was<br />

grounded in two inter-related “new” basic sciences—neurochemistry<br />

<strong>and</strong> psychopharmacology.<br />

By linking etiologic theories to these high-prestige,<br />

“hard-science” disciplines, biological psychiatry<br />

could obtain legitimization <strong>and</strong> be seen as applying<br />

to psychiatry the biological etiologic approach<br />

that had proved increasingly successful in the<br />

rest <strong>of</strong> medicine. This fulfilled one central goal<br />

<strong>of</strong> the movement—to bring psychiatry back into<br />

the medical mainstream. <strong>The</strong> history <strong>of</strong> medicine<br />

provides other examples <strong>of</strong> groups <strong>of</strong> physicians<br />

attempting to ground their theories in new scientific<br />

advances, in part to bolster their pr<strong>of</strong>essional<br />

status (e.g., Brown 1970). Second, the stunning<br />

success with Parkinson’s disease, where all the<br />

features <strong>of</strong> the illness could be apparently traced<br />

to a single neurotransmitter deficit, suggested the<br />

viability <strong>of</strong> the tempting “one transmitter, one<br />

disease” model for neuropsychiatric disorders<br />

(Shorter 1997). This model fit very well with the<br />

reductive zeitgeist <strong>of</strong> the biological psychiatry<br />

movement that sought to continue in the tradition<br />

<strong>of</strong> the stories <strong>of</strong> GPI <strong>and</strong> pellagra. Third, the<br />

DHS tied the etiologic theory <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

to the increasingly unassailable evidence <strong>of</strong> the<br />

efficacy <strong>of</strong> antipsychotic drugs for the treatment<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Thus, the DHS could, at least in<br />

this indirect manner, be anchored in the practical<br />

world <strong>of</strong> psychiatric therapy. Relatedly, Shorter<br />

has suggested that a “one transmitter one-disease<br />

hypothesis” was “a perfect marketing concept for<br />

the drug companies” (1997, 267).<br />

<strong>The</strong> DHS has also been sustained over time by<br />

increasing evidence that the therapeutic effect <strong>of</strong><br />

antipsychotic drugs correlates robustly with their<br />

capacity to block DA receptors. Such results are<br />

still commonly used as support for the DHS. In<br />

our view, this argument is logically flawed <strong>and</strong><br />

results from a conflation <strong>of</strong> two distinct theories:<br />

<strong>The</strong> DHS, a theory <strong>of</strong> disease etiology, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

“dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> antipsychotic drug<br />

action” (DHADA), a theory about pharmacologic<br />

mechanism. Information in support <strong>of</strong> the<br />

DHADA was clearly useful in supporting the<br />

plausibility <strong>of</strong> DHS at the time <strong>of</strong> its proposal.<br />

However, medicine has many examples in which<br />

the mechanism <strong>of</strong> action <strong>of</strong> a therapeutic drug is<br />

far removed from disease etiology, albeit perhaps<br />

not from its downstream pathophysiology. Diuretics<br />

are common treatments for congestive heart<br />

failure, yet the mode <strong>of</strong> action <strong>of</strong> these drugs in<br />

the kidney tell us little about the etiology <strong>of</strong> heart<br />

failure. <strong>An</strong>ti-inflammatory drugs reduce symptoms<br />

in a wide variety <strong>of</strong> conditions, but typically act on


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 59<br />

physiological processes remote from those which<br />

cause the disorder. For this reason, we suggest<br />

that further evidence supporting the DHADA is<br />

<strong>of</strong> minimal relevance to the validity <strong>of</strong> the DHS.<br />

Although one can justly claim from the DHADA<br />

that “dopamine must have something to do with<br />

schizophrenia,” this is a weak argument because<br />

the mode <strong>of</strong> action <strong>of</strong> drugs can be so far removed<br />

from basic disease etiology as to be largely uninformative<br />

about etiologic processes.<br />

We suggest that, in their enthusiasm for this<br />

theory, the biological psychiatry movement was,<br />

initially <strong>and</strong> for many subsequent years, willing<br />

to overlook the substantial limitations <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

that we have reviewed above <strong>and</strong> conflate the DHS<br />

<strong>and</strong> DHADA. Although a further explication <strong>of</strong><br />

this part <strong>of</strong> the story lies outside the bounds <strong>of</strong> this<br />

review, a full historical underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

would require consideration both <strong>of</strong> scientific <strong>and</strong><br />

extra-scientific forces.<br />

Concluding Thoughts<br />

In conclusion, we want to be clear about what<br />

we are <strong>and</strong> are not saying. We do not claim that<br />

DA is unrelated to the etiology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

or that DA does not explain a lot about the action<br />

<strong>of</strong> antipsychotic drugs. Furthermore, we<br />

recognize that theories can be useful in guiding<br />

treatment even if they are etiologically incomplete<br />

or wrong (Schaffner 2002). Many hypotheses in<br />

medicine that were not based on deep etiological<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing have produced practical therapeutic<br />

benefits. This is likely the case for the DHS.<br />

By contrast, we do conclude that, as a scientific<br />

theory, the DHS has to date performed relatively<br />

poorly <strong>and</strong> has, from the perspective <strong>of</strong> leading<br />

models <strong>of</strong> science, important deficiencies. Furthermore,<br />

we suggest that the story <strong>of</strong> the DHS<br />

has at least four important lessons for our field.<br />

First, psychiatry needs theories with higher levels<br />

<strong>of</strong> specificity <strong>and</strong> falsifiability. Global, nonspecific<br />

theories like the DHS have heuristic value <strong>and</strong> can<br />

play an important role in stimulating research<br />

in the short run. However, such theories are less<br />

effective at guiding research in the long run in<br />

fruitful <strong>and</strong> progressive directions. Several <strong>of</strong> the<br />

etiologic theories for schizophrenia that compete<br />

with the DA hypothesis, including those focusing<br />

on glutamate or NMDA receptors, probably also<br />

suffer from lows levels <strong>of</strong> specificity <strong>and</strong> falsifiability.<br />

Other leading “neurochemical” theories in<br />

psychiatry, such as the serotonin hypothesis <strong>of</strong> depression,<br />

may be similar (Lacasse <strong>and</strong> Leo 2005).<br />

Second, science works best when diverse theories<br />

with distinct predictions compete with one<br />

another. This has been hard to implement in the<br />

field <strong>of</strong> mental health research for many reasons.<br />

Third, it has been common in the history <strong>of</strong> science<br />

in general <strong>and</strong> the medical <strong>and</strong> social sciences in<br />

particular for theories to be defended with a fervor<br />

that cannot be justified by the available evidence.<br />

More than we may wish to admit, this has been<br />

the case with the DHS. As our science <strong>and</strong> field<br />

matures beyond ideologically driven controversy,<br />

it would be wise <strong>and</strong> mature for all <strong>of</strong> us, regardless<br />

<strong>of</strong> whether we see ourselves as biological,<br />

social or psychodynamic, to be more self-critical<br />

about the theories we adopt <strong>and</strong> as more tolerant<br />

<strong>of</strong> diversity in theory articulation. Finally, psychiatry<br />

is probably not ready for “big” unitary<br />

theories like the DHS. Although very tempting,<br />

it will likely be more realistic <strong>and</strong> productive for<br />

us to focus on smaller questions, <strong>and</strong> to settle for<br />

“bit-by-bit” progress as we clarify, in a piecemeal<br />

manner, the immensely complex web <strong>of</strong> causes that<br />

contribute to disorders like schizophrenia (Kendler<br />

2005; Schaffner 1994).<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

This work is supported in part by the Rachael<br />

Banks Endowment Fund (KSK) <strong>and</strong> the National<br />

Science Foundation under Grant Nos. 0324367<br />

<strong>and</strong> 0628825 (KFS). <strong>An</strong>y opinions, findings,<br />

conclusions, or recommendations expressed in<br />

this material are those <strong>of</strong> the author(s) <strong>and</strong> do<br />

not necessarily reflect the views <strong>of</strong> the National<br />

Science Foundation. Helpful comments on earlier<br />

versions <strong>of</strong> this essay were kindly provided by Joel<br />

Kleinman, MD, Steven Matthysse, PhD, Solomon<br />

Snyder, MD, Carl Craver, PhD, Robert Malenka,<br />

MD, PhD, Karoly Mirnics, MD, John Bickle,<br />

PhD, James Bogen, PhD, Peter Machamer, PhD,<br />

Edouard Machery, PhD, Robert C. Olby, PhD,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Carol Tamminga, MD.


60 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

References<br />

Abi-Dargham, A. 2004. Do we still believe in the dopamine<br />

hypothesis? New data bring new evidence.<br />

International Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuropsychopharmacology<br />

7, Suppl 1:S1–S5.<br />

Abi-Dargham, A., J. Rodenhiser, D. Printz, Y. Zea-<br />

Ponce, R. et al. 2000. Increased baseline occupancy<br />

<strong>of</strong> D2 receptors by dopamine in schizophrenia.<br />

Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the National Academy <strong>of</strong> Science U<br />

S A 97:8104–9.<br />

Aghajanian, G. K., <strong>and</strong> B. S. Bunney. 1977.<br />

<strong>Dopamine</strong>”autoreceptors”: Pharmacological<br />

characterization by microiontophoretic single cell<br />

recording studies. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Archives<br />

<strong>of</strong> Pharmacology 297, no. 1:1–7.<br />

Amin, F., M. Davidson, R. S. Kahn, J. Schmeidler, et al.<br />

1995. Assessment <strong>of</strong> the central dopaminergic index<br />

<strong>of</strong> plasma HVA in schizophrenia. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> Bulletin<br />

21, no. 1:53–66.<br />

<strong>An</strong>dreasen, N. C. 1981. Scale for the assessment <strong>of</strong><br />

negative symptoms (SANS). Iowa City: <strong>The</strong> University<br />

<strong>of</strong> Iowa.<br />

<strong>An</strong>grist, B., H. K. Lee, <strong>and</strong> S. Gershon. 1974. <strong>The</strong> antagonism<br />

<strong>of</strong> amphetamine-induced symptomatology<br />

by a neuroleptic. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry<br />

131, no. 7:817–9.<br />

Baumeister, A. A., <strong>and</strong> J. L. Francis. 2002. <strong>Historical</strong><br />

development <strong>of</strong> the dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> History Neuroscience 11, no.<br />

3:265–77.<br />

Bennett, C. L. 2005. Astrophysical observations: Lensing<br />

<strong>and</strong> eclipsing Einstein’s theories. Science 307,<br />

no. 5711:879–84.<br />

Borglum, A. D., M. Hampson, T. E. Kjeldsen, W. Muir,<br />

et al. 2001. Dopa decarboxylase genotypes may<br />

influence age at onset <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Molecular<br />

Psychiatry 6, no. 6:712–7.<br />

Brown, T. M. 1970. <strong>The</strong> College <strong>of</strong> Physicians <strong>and</strong> the<br />

acceptance <strong>of</strong> iatromechanism in Engl<strong>and</strong>, 1665–<br />

1695. Bulletin <strong>of</strong> the History <strong>of</strong> Medicine 44:12–30.<br />

Buchanan, R. W., <strong>and</strong> W. T. Carpenter Jr. 2005. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> other psychotic disorders. In Kaplan &<br />

Sadock’s Comprehensive textbook <strong>of</strong> psychiatry, volume<br />

1, ed. B. J. Sadock, <strong>and</strong> V. A. Sadock, 1329–558.<br />

New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.<br />

Bunzow, J. R., H. H. Van Tol, D. K. Gr<strong>and</strong>y, P. Albert,<br />

et al. 1988. Cloning <strong>and</strong> expression <strong>of</strong> a rat D2 dopamine<br />

receptor cDNA. Nature 336, no. 6201:783–7.<br />

Carlsson, A. 1978. Does dopamine have a role in<br />

schizophrenia? Biological Psychiatry 13, no. 1:3–21.<br />

——–. 2001. A half-century <strong>of</strong> neurotransmitter research:<br />

Impact on neurology <strong>and</strong> psychiatry (Nobel<br />

lecture). Chembiochem 2, no 7–8:484–93.<br />

Carlsson, A., <strong>and</strong> Y. Lecrubier. 2004. Progress in dopamine<br />

research in schizophrenia: A guide for physicians.<br />

London: Taylor & Francis Group.<br />

Carlsson, A., <strong>and</strong> M. Lindqvist. 1963. Effect <strong>of</strong> chlorpromazine<br />

or haloperidol on formation <strong>of</strong> 3-methoxytyramine<br />

<strong>and</strong> normetanephrine in mouse brain.<br />

Acta Pharmacologica et Toxicology 20:140–4.<br />

Chamberlin, T. C. 1897. <strong>The</strong> method <strong>of</strong> multiple working<br />

hypotheses. Journal <strong>of</strong> Geology 5:837–48.<br />

Cohen, B. J. 2003. <strong>The</strong>ory <strong>and</strong> practice <strong>of</strong> psychiatry.<br />

Oxford: Oxford University Press.<br />

Connell, P. H. 1958. Amphetamine psychosis. In Maudsley<br />

monographs, No. 5, Institute <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry.<br />

London: Chapman <strong>and</strong> Hall, Ltd.<br />

Creese, I., D. R. Burt, <strong>and</strong> S. H. Snyder. 1976. <strong>Dopamine</strong><br />

receptor binding predicts clinical <strong>and</strong> pharmacological<br />

potencies <strong>of</strong> antischizophrenic drugs. Science 192,<br />

no. 4238:481–3.<br />

Crow, T. J. 1980. Molecular pathology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia:<br />

More than one disease process? British Medical<br />

Journal 280, no. 6207:66–8.<br />

Csernansky, J. G., <strong>and</strong> J. W. Newcomer. 1994. Are <strong>The</strong>re<br />

neurochemical indicators <strong>of</strong> risk for schizophrenia.<br />

<strong>Schizophrenia</strong> Bulletin 20, no. 1:75–88.<br />

Cutler, N. R., D. V. Jeste, C. A. Kaufmann, F. Karoum,<br />

et al. 1984. Low dose bromocriptine: A study <strong>of</strong><br />

acute effects in chronic mediated schizophrenics.<br />

Progress Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological<br />

Psychiatry 8, no. 2:277–83.<br />

Dahlstrom, A., <strong>and</strong> K. Fuxe. 1964. Evidence for the<br />

existence <strong>of</strong> monoamine-containing neurons in the<br />

central nervous system. I. Demonstration <strong>of</strong> monoamines<br />

in the cell bodies <strong>of</strong> brain stem neurons. Acta<br />

Physiologica Sc<strong>and</strong>inavica Suppl 232:1.<br />

Dahlstrom, A., K. Fuxe, L. Olson, <strong>and</strong> U. Ungerstedt.<br />

1962. Ascending systems <strong>of</strong> catecholamine neurons<br />

from the lower brainstem. Acta Physiologica Sc<strong>and</strong>inavica<br />

62:485–6.<br />

Davis, K. L., R. S. Kahn, G. Ko, <strong>and</strong> M. Davidson.<br />

1991. <strong>Dopamine</strong> in schizophrenia: A review <strong>and</strong><br />

reconceptualization. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry<br />

148, no. 11:1474–86.<br />

Depatie, L., <strong>and</strong> S. Lal. 2001. Apomorphine <strong>and</strong> the<br />

dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia: A dilemma?<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry & Neuroscience 26, no.<br />

3:203–20.<br />

Donnelly, C. L., J. P. McElvoy, W. H. Wilson, <strong>and</strong> N.<br />

Narasimhachari. 1996. A study <strong>of</strong> the potential<br />

confounding effects <strong>of</strong> diet, caffeine, nicotine <strong>and</strong><br />

lorazepam on the stability <strong>of</strong> plasma <strong>and</strong> urinary homovanillic<br />

acid levels in patients with schizophrenia.<br />

Biological Psychiatry 40, no. 12:1218–21.


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 61<br />

Dorling, J. 1979. Bayesian personalism, the methodology<br />

<strong>of</strong> scientific-research programs, <strong>and</strong> Duhem’s<br />

problem. Studies in History <strong>and</strong> Philosophy <strong>of</strong> Science<br />

10, no. 3:177–87.<br />

Falck, B., N. A. Hillarp, G. Thieme, <strong>and</strong> A. Torp.<br />

1982. Fluorescence <strong>of</strong> catechol amines <strong>and</strong> related<br />

compounds condensed with formaldehyde. Brain<br />

Research Bulletin 9, no. 1–6:xi–xv.<br />

Fan, J. B., C. S. Zhang, N. F. Gu, X. W. Li, et al. 2005.<br />

Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val/Met functional<br />

polymorphism <strong>and</strong> risk <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia: A<br />

large-scale association study plus meta-analysis.<br />

Biological Psychiatry 57, no. 2:139–44.<br />

Farley, I. J., K. S. Price, <strong>and</strong> O. Hornykiewicz. 1977.<br />

<strong>Dopamine</strong> in the limbic regions <strong>of</strong> the human brain:<br />

Normal <strong>and</strong> abnormal. Advances in Biochemical<br />

Psychopharmacology 16:57–64.<br />

Faurbye, A. 1968. <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> amines in the etiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry 9, no.<br />

2:155–77.<br />

Fuxe, K., T. Hokfelt, O. Nilsson, <strong>and</strong> S. Reinius. 1966.<br />

A fluorescence <strong>and</strong> electron microscopic study on<br />

central monoamine nerve cells. <strong>An</strong>atomical Record<br />

155, no. 1:33–40.<br />

Gamma, F., S. V. Faraone, S. J. Glatt, Y. C. Yeh, et al.<br />

2005. Meta-analysis shows schizophrenia is not associated<br />

with the 40-base-pair repeat polymorphism<br />

<strong>of</strong> the dopamine transporter gene. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Research 73, no. 1:55–8.<br />

Garcia, A., A. Galinowski, P. Guicheney, E. Mignot, et<br />

al. 1989. Free <strong>and</strong> conjugated plasma homovanillic<br />

acid in schizophrenic patients. Biological Psychiatry<br />

26, no. 1:87–96.<br />

Gazzaniga, M. S. 2004. <strong>The</strong> cognitive neurosciences III,<br />

3rd ed. Cambrdige, MA: <strong>The</strong> MIT Press.<br />

Glatt, S. J., S. V. Faraone, <strong>and</strong> M. T. Tsuang. 2004.<br />

DRD2-141C insertion/deletion polymorphism is not<br />

associated with schizophrenia: Results <strong>of</strong> a metaanalysis.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Medical Genetics<br />

Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics 128B, no. 1:21–3.<br />

——–. 2006. <strong>The</strong> Cys allele <strong>of</strong> the DRD2 Ser311Cys<br />

polymorphism has a dominant effect on risk for<br />

schizophrenia: Evidence from fixed- <strong>and</strong> r<strong>and</strong>omeffects<br />

meta-analyses. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Medical<br />

Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics 141,<br />

no. 2:149–54.<br />

Griffiths, J. J., J. Oates, <strong>and</strong> J. Cavanaugh. 1968.<br />

Paranoid episodes induced by drugs. Journal <strong>of</strong> the<br />

American Medical Association 205:39–46.<br />

Gunderson, J. G., M. C. Zanarini, C. L. Kisiel. 1995.<br />

Borderline personality disorder. In <strong>The</strong> DSM-IV<br />

personality disorders, ed. W. J. Livesley, 141–57.<br />

New York: <strong>The</strong> Guilford Press.<br />

Harrison, P. J., <strong>and</strong> D. R. Weinberger. 2005. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

genes, gene expression, <strong>and</strong> neuropathology:<br />

On the matter <strong>of</strong> their convergence. Molecular<br />

Psychiatry 10, no. 1:40–68.<br />

Hietala, J., <strong>and</strong> E. Syvalahti. 1996. <strong>Dopamine</strong> in schizophrenia.<br />

<strong>An</strong>nals <strong>of</strong> Medicine 28, no. 6:557–61.<br />

Hökfelt, T., A. Ljungdahl, K. Fuxe, <strong>and</strong> O. Johansson.<br />

1974. <strong>Dopamine</strong> nerve terminals in the rat limbic<br />

cortex: Aspects <strong>of</strong> the dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong><br />

schizophrenia. Science 184, no. 133:177–9.<br />

Hoogendoorn, M., S. Bakker, H. Schnack, J. Selten, et<br />

al. 2005. No association between 12 dopaminergic<br />

genes <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia in a large Dutch sample.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric<br />

Genetics) 134B:6–9.<br />

Howson, C., <strong>and</strong> P. Urbach. 2005. Scientific reasoning:<br />

<strong>The</strong> Bayesian approach, 3rd ed. Chicago: Open<br />

Court Publishing Company.<br />

Jonsson, E. G., R. Kaiser, J. Brockmoller, V. L. Nimgaonkar,<br />

et al. 2004. Meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> the dopamine<br />

D3 receptor gene (DRD3) Ser9Gly variant <strong>and</strong><br />

schizophrenia. Psychiatric Genetics 14, no. 1:9–12.<br />

Jonsson, E. G., G. C. Sedvall, M. M. Nothen, <strong>and</strong> S.<br />

Cichon. 2003. <strong>Dopamine</strong> D4 receptor gene (DRD4)<br />

variants <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia: meta-analyses. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Research 61, no. 1:111–9.<br />

K<strong>and</strong>el, E. R., J. H. Schwartz, <strong>and</strong> T. M. Jessell. 2000.<br />

Principles <strong>of</strong> neural science, 4th ed. New York:<br />

McGraw-Hill Medical.<br />

Katz, B. 1994. Planning <strong>and</strong> following the unexpected<br />

in scientific-research. Creativity Research Journal 7,<br />

no. 3–4:225–38.<br />

Kebabian, J. W., <strong>and</strong> D. B. Calne. 1979. Multiple receptors<br />

for dopamine. Nature 277, no. 5692:93–6.<br />

Kendler, K. S. 2005. Toward a philosophical structure<br />

for psychiatry. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry 163,<br />

no. 3:433–40.<br />

Kendler, K. S., R. C. Mohs, <strong>and</strong> K. L. Davis. 1983. <strong>The</strong><br />

effects <strong>of</strong> diet <strong>and</strong> physical activity on plasma homovanillic<br />

acid in normal human subjects. Psychiatry<br />

Research 8, no. 3:215–23.<br />

Kendler, K. S., <strong>and</strong> J. Parnas. 2008. Philosophical issues<br />

in psychiatry: Explanation, phenomenology<br />

<strong>and</strong> nosology. Baltimore, MD: <strong>The</strong> Johns Hopkins<br />

University Press.<br />

Kestler, L. P., E. Walker, <strong>and</strong> E. M. Vega. 2001. <strong>Dopamine</strong><br />

receptors in the brains <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia<br />

patients: A meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> the findings. Behavioural<br />

Pharmacology 12, no. 5:355–71.<br />

Kety, S. 1959. Biochemical theories <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia.<br />

Science 129:1528–32.<br />

——–. 1966. Catecholamines in neuropsychiatric states.<br />

Pharmacology Review 18, no. 1:787–98.


62 ■ PPP / Vol. 18, No. 1 / March 2011<br />

Kitcher, P. 1993. <strong>The</strong> advancement <strong>of</strong> science: Science<br />

without legend, objectivity without illusions. New<br />

York: Oxford University Press.<br />

Kojima, H., O. Ohmori, T. Shinkai, T. Terao, et al.<br />

1999. <strong>Dopamine</strong> D1 receptor gene polymorphism<br />

<strong>and</strong> schizophrenia in Japan. American Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Medical Genetics 88, no. 2:116–9.<br />

Koob, G. F., <strong>and</strong> M. Le Moal. 2006. Psychostimulants.<br />

In Neurobiology <strong>of</strong> addiction, ed. G. F. Koob, <strong>and</strong><br />

M. Le Moal, 69–120. London: Academic Press,<br />

Elsevier, Inc.<br />

Kopin, I. J. 1993. Parkinson’s disease: Past, present, <strong>and</strong><br />

future. Neuropsychopharmacology 9, no. 1:1–12.<br />

Kuhn, T. S. 1996. <strong>The</strong> structure <strong>of</strong> scientific revolutions,<br />

3rd ed. Chicago: University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press.<br />

Lacasse, J. R., <strong>and</strong> J. Leo. 2005. Serotonin <strong>and</strong> depression:<br />

A disconnect between the advertisements <strong>and</strong><br />

the scientific literature. PLoS Med 2, no. 12:e392.<br />

Lakatos, I. 1970. Falsification <strong>and</strong> the methodology <strong>of</strong><br />

scientific research programmes. In Criticism <strong>and</strong> the<br />

growth <strong>of</strong> knowledge, ed. I. Lakatos, <strong>and</strong> A. Musgrave,<br />

91–197. London: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Laudan, L. 1977. Progress <strong>and</strong> its problems. Berkeley:<br />

University <strong>of</strong> California Press.<br />

Lieberman, J. A., J. M. Kane, <strong>and</strong> J. Alvir. 1987. Provocative<br />

tests with psychostimulant drugs in schizophrenia.<br />

Psychopharmacology 91:415–33.<br />

Maas, J. W., S. A. Contreras, A. L. Miller, N. Berman,<br />

et al. 1993. Studies <strong>of</strong> catecholamine metabolism in<br />

schizophrenia/psychosis—I. Neuropsychopharmacology<br />

8, no. 2:97–109.<br />

Magee, B. 1982. Popper. London: Fontana Paperbacks.<br />

Marx, C. E., <strong>and</strong> J. A. Lieberman. 1998. Psychoneuroendocrinology<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics<br />

<strong>of</strong> North America 21, no. 2:413–34.<br />

Matthysse, S. 1973. <strong>An</strong>tipsychotic drug actions—Clue<br />

to neuropathology <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. Federation<br />

Proceedings 32, no. 2:200–5.<br />

Matthysse, S., <strong>and</strong> J. Lipinski. 1975. Biochemical aspects<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia. <strong>An</strong>nual Review <strong>of</strong> Medicine<br />

26:551–65.<br />

Meltzer, H. Y., <strong>and</strong> S. M. Stahl. 1976. <strong>The</strong> dopamine<br />

hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia: a review. <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Bulletin 2, no. 1:19–76.<br />

Montagu, K. A. 1957. Catechol compounds in rat tissues<br />

<strong>and</strong> in brains <strong>of</strong> different animals. Nature 180,<br />

no. 4579:244–5.<br />

Munafo, M. R., L. Bowes, T. G. Clark, <strong>and</strong> J. Flint.<br />

2005. Lack <strong>of</strong> association <strong>of</strong> the COMT (Val158/108<br />

Met) gene <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia: A meta-analysis <strong>of</strong><br />

case-control studies. Molecular Psychiatry 10, no.<br />

8:765–70.<br />

Owen, M. J., N. M. Williams, <strong>and</strong> M. C. O’Donovan.<br />

2004. <strong>The</strong> molecular genetics <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia: New<br />

findings promise new insights. Molecular Psychiatry<br />

9, no. 1:14–27.<br />

Pae, C. U., J. J. Kim, A. Serretti, C. U. Lee, et al. 2003.<br />

VNTR polymorphism <strong>of</strong> tyrosine hydroxylase gene<br />

<strong>and</strong> schizophrenia in the Korean population. Neuropsychobiology<br />

47, no. 3:131–6.<br />

Platt, J. R. 1964. Strong inference. Science 146, no.<br />

3642: 347–53.<br />

Popper, K. R. 1959. <strong>The</strong> logic <strong>of</strong> scientific discovery.<br />

New York: Basic Books.<br />

——–. 1962. Conjectures <strong>and</strong> refutations. New York:<br />

Basic Books.<br />

Predescu, V., D. Florescu, <strong>and</strong> C. Radulesc. 1968. Pink<br />

spot as a diagnostic test in schizophrenia. Nature<br />

217, no. 5134:1150.<br />

Schaffner, K. F. 1992. <strong>The</strong>ory change in immunology.<br />

2. <strong>The</strong> clonal selection theory. <strong>The</strong>oretical Medicine<br />

13, no. 2:175–216.<br />

——–. 1993. Discovery <strong>and</strong> explanation in biology <strong>and</strong><br />

medicine. Chicago: <strong>The</strong> University <strong>of</strong> Chicago Press.<br />

——–. 1994. Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> molecular biology:<br />

Reductionistic approaches to schizophrenia in<br />

philosophical perspectives on psychiatric diagnostic<br />

classification. Baltimore, MD: <strong>The</strong> Johns Hopkins<br />

University Press.<br />

——–. 2002. Clinical <strong>and</strong> etiological psychiatric diagnoses:<br />

Do causes count? In Descriptions <strong>and</strong> prescriptions:<br />

Values, mental disorders, <strong>and</strong> the DSMs, ed.<br />

J. Z. Sadler, 271–90. Baltimore, MD: <strong>The</strong> Johns<br />

Hopkins University Press.<br />

Seeman, P., T. Lee, M. Chauwong, <strong>and</strong> K. Wong. 1976.<br />

<strong>An</strong>tipsychotic drug doses <strong>and</strong> neuroleptic-dopamine<br />

receptors. Nature 261, no. 5562:717–9.<br />

Shapere, D., <strong>and</strong> D. R. Dordrecht. 1984. Reason <strong>and</strong><br />

the search for knowledge: Investigations in the philosophy<br />

<strong>of</strong> science (Boston studies in the philosophy<br />

<strong>of</strong> science). Boston: D. Reidel Publishing Company.<br />

Shifman, S., M. Bronstein, M. Sternfeld, A. Pisante-<br />

Shalom, et al. 2002. A highly significant association<br />

between a COMT haplotype <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Human Genetics 71, no.<br />

6:1296–302.<br />

Shorter, E. 1997. A history <strong>of</strong> psychiatry: From the era<br />

<strong>of</strong> the asylum to the age <strong>of</strong> Prozac. New York: John<br />

Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Skare, S. S., M. W. Dysken, <strong>and</strong> C. J. Billington. 1994.<br />

A review <strong>of</strong> GHRH stimulation test in psychiatry.<br />

Biological Psychiatry 36, no. 4:249–65.<br />

Snyder, S. H. 1976. <strong>The</strong> dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia:<br />

Focus on the dopamine receptor. American<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry 133, no. 2:197–202.<br />

Snyder, S. H., G. K. Aghajanian, <strong>and</strong> S. Matthysse.<br />

1972. Prospects for research on schizophrenia. V.<br />

Pharmacological observations, drug-induced psy-


Kendler <strong>and</strong> Schaffner / <strong>Dopamine</strong> <strong>Hypothesis</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Schizophrenia</strong> ■ 63<br />

choses. Neuroscience Research Program Bulletin<br />

10, no. 4:430–45.<br />

Snyder, S. H., S. P. Banerjee, H. I. Yamamura, <strong>and</strong> D.<br />

Greenberg. 1974. Drugs, neurotransmitters, <strong>and</strong><br />

schizophrenia. Science 184, no. 4143:1243–53.<br />

Sokol<strong>of</strong>f, P., B. Giros, M. P. Martres, M. L. Bouthenet,<br />

et al. 1990. Molecular cloning <strong>and</strong> characterization<br />

<strong>of</strong> a novel dopamine receptor (D3) as a target for<br />

neuroleptics. Nature 347, no. 6289:146–51.<br />

Sourkes, T. L. 1973. On the origin <strong>of</strong> homovanillic acid<br />

(HVA) in the cerebrospinal fluid. Journal <strong>of</strong> Neural<br />

Transmission 34:153–7.<br />

Steinberg, J. L., D. L. Garver, F. G. Moeller, P. J. Orsulak,<br />

et al. 1993. Serum homovanillic-acid levels in<br />

schizophrenic-patients <strong>and</strong> normal controls. Biological<br />

Psychiatry 33, no. 6A:A99.<br />

Sullivan, P. F., K. S. Kendler, <strong>and</strong> M. C. Neale. 2003.<br />

<strong>Schizophrenia</strong> as a complex trait: Evidence from a<br />

meta-analysis <strong>of</strong> twin studies. Archives <strong>of</strong> General<br />

Psychiatry 60, no. 12:1187–92.<br />

Sumiyoshi, T., O. Saitoh, T. Yotsutsuji, H. Itoh, et al.<br />

1999. Differential effects <strong>of</strong> mental stress on plasma<br />

homovanillic acid in schizophrenia <strong>and</strong> normal controls.<br />

Neuropsychopharmacology 20, no. 4:365–9.<br />

Sunahara, R. K., H. C. Guan, B. F. O’Dowd, P. Seeman,<br />

et al. 1991. Cloning <strong>of</strong> the gene for a human dopamine<br />

D5 receptor with higher affinity for dopamine<br />

than D1. Nature 350, no. 6319:614–9.<br />

Talkowski, M. E., M. Bamne, H. Mansour, <strong>and</strong> V. L.<br />

Nimgaonkar. 2007. <strong>Dopamine</strong> genes <strong>and</strong> schizophrenia:<br />

Case closed or evidence pending? <strong>Schizophrenia</strong><br />

Bulletin 33, no. 5:1071–81.<br />

Tamminga, C. A., M. D. Gotts, G. K. Thakar, L. D.<br />

Alphs, et al. 1986. <strong>Dopamine</strong> agonist treatment<br />

<strong>of</strong> schizophrenia with N-propylnorapomorphine.<br />

Archives <strong>of</strong> General Psychiatry 43, no. 4:398–402.<br />

Thagard, P. 2000. How scientists explain disease. Princeton,<br />

NJ: Princeton University Press.<br />

Tuckwell, H. C., <strong>and</strong> J. A. Kosiol. 1993. A meta-analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> homovanillic acid concentrations in schizophrenia.<br />

International Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience 73:109–14.<br />

Tune, L. E., D. F. Wong, G. Pearlson, M. Strauss, et<br />

al. 1993. <strong>Dopamine</strong> D2 receptor density estimates<br />

in schizophrenia: A positron emission tomography<br />

study with 11C-N-methylspiperone. Psychiatry<br />

Research 49, no. 3:219–37.<br />

Valenstein, E. S. 1998. Blaming the brain: <strong>The</strong> truth<br />

about drugs <strong>and</strong> mental health. New York: <strong>The</strong><br />

Free Press.<br />

van Rossum, J. M. 1967. <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> dopaminereceptor<br />

blockade for the action <strong>of</strong> neuroleptic<br />

drugs. Neuropsychopharmacology excerpta Medica<br />

Foundation, Amsterdam, 1967:321–9.<br />

Van Tol, H. H., J. R. Bunzow, H. C. Guan, R. K. Sunahara,<br />

et al. 1991. Cloning <strong>of</strong> the gene for a human<br />

dopamine D4 receptor with high affinity for the antipsychotic<br />

clozapine. Nature 350, no. 6319:610–4.<br />

Williams, H. J., B. Glaser, N. M. Williams, N. Norton,<br />

et al. 2005. No association between schizophrenia<br />

<strong>and</strong> polymorphisms in COMT in two large samples.<br />

American Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatry 162, no. 9:1736–8.<br />

Willner, P. 1997. <strong>The</strong> dopamine hypothesis <strong>of</strong> schizophrenia:<br />

current status, future prospects. International<br />

Clinical Psychopharmacology 12, no. 6:297–308.<br />

Wong, D. F., G. D. Pearlson, L. E. Tune, L. T. Young,<br />

et al. 1997. Quantification <strong>of</strong> neuroreceptors in the<br />

living human brain. 4. Effect <strong>of</strong> aging <strong>and</strong> elevations<br />

<strong>of</strong> D-2-like receptors in schizophrenia <strong>and</strong> bipolar<br />

illness. Journal <strong>of</strong> Cerebral Blood Flow <strong>and</strong> Metabolism<br />

17, no. 3:331–42.<br />

Zhang, B., Y. Jia, Y. Yuan, X. Yu, et al. 2004. No association<br />

between polymorphisms in the DDC gene<br />

<strong>and</strong> paranoid schizophrenia in a northern Chinese<br />

population. Psychiatric Genetics 14, no. 3:161–3.<br />

Zhang, Z. J., M. Peet, C. N. Ranch<strong>and</strong>, S. Shah, et al.<br />

2001. Plasma homovanillic acid in untreated schizophrenia—Relationship<br />

with symptomatology <strong>and</strong><br />

sex. Journal <strong>of</strong> Psychiatric Research 35, no. 1:23–8.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!