08.07.2021 Views

Screen Time and Learner Well-being

The Debate, the Evidence and Directions for Future

The Debate, the Evidence and Directions for Future

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future<br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by<br />

UNESCO MGIEP<br />

Anantha DURAIAPPAH<br />

Yoko MOCHIZUKI<br />

Robin SHARMA<br />

Kriti SINGH (Research Support)<br />

Introduction: Growing up in a Digital Environment<br />

In many parts of the world, children are growing up surrounded by digitally mediated<br />

communication, gaming <strong>and</strong> a plethora of information <strong>and</strong> entertainment on screens. Children<br />

today live in a digitally connected world. Irrespective of the type of digital device used, the<br />

screen is the key portal that connects the user with the digital world.<br />

The screen has become the gateway to entertainment, social interaction, information <strong>and</strong><br />

education. In the recent context of social distancing in the face of the COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic,<br />

screens have become even more embedded in our everyday lives for both adults <strong>and</strong> children.<br />

While we cannot overstate the need to address the digital divide within <strong>and</strong> across countries,<br />

the children <strong>and</strong> young people of today—the digital natives—are practically born with a screen<br />

in their h<strong>and</strong>s. For example, over 75% of 450 French parents of infants (ages 5-40 months) have<br />

reported that their young children use some touch-screen technology (Cristia <strong>and</strong> Seidl 2015),<br />

1<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


while over 66% of children aged 3-8 years have been reported to use their parents’ mobile<br />

touch-screen devices in five Southeast Asian countries (Unantenne 2014). 1<br />

Figure 1: Percentage of young people (15-24 year olds) who are internet users by region<br />

Source: Adopted from UNICEF (2017), p. 46, Figure 2.2.<br />

Unsurprisingly, the overwhelming majority of young people are internet users across the world,<br />

with a notable exception of the African continent (see Figure 1). There is no denying that the<br />

increasingly mobile <strong>and</strong> connected digital devices are becoming an integral part of the daily lives<br />

of the younger generation. Just as the book <strong>and</strong> the printed medium was an integral part of the<br />

20 th century, the digital media is the preferred choice in the 21 st century especially among the<br />

younger generation.<br />

In fact, there is a generational divide when it comes to the use of digital technology. In a recent<br />

study on the use of digital technology during the COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic, it was found that the<br />

users with the maximum time spent on digital devices were the GEN Z followed by the<br />

Millennials (see Figure 2 below).<br />

1<br />

In these studies, ‘touch-screen technology’ <strong>and</strong> ‘mobile touch-screen devices’ refer to tablets <strong>and</strong> smartphones.<br />

2<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Figure 2: Mobile <strong>and</strong> PC/laptop usage increase on lockdown. Source: Adopted from GlobalWebMiIndex<br />

(2020), p.28 2<br />

In recent years, young children’s <strong>and</strong> adolescents’ time spent on screens has attracted<br />

considerable public <strong>and</strong> policy attention, with concerns around the effects of screen time on<br />

physical <strong>and</strong> mental health <strong>and</strong> educational outcomes. What is the impact of the increased use<br />

of digital devices on the well-<strong>being</strong> of learners <strong>and</strong> in particular young children <strong>and</strong> adolescents<br />

is a key question on the minds of parents <strong>and</strong> many decision makers. However, we must<br />

acknowledge that these questions are generally posed by policymakers who belong to the Baby<br />

Boomer generation who incidentally have the lowest time usage of digital technology as<br />

indicated in Figure 2.<br />

With the COVID-19 crisis, the use of digital technology for teaching <strong>and</strong> learning has taken on a<br />

new alignment, which has a completely new meaning <strong>and</strong> imperative for policymakers,<br />

researchers <strong>and</strong> educators alike across the globe. The annual total internet usage worldwide<br />

jumped from a 2.6 % average over the past 14 years to a growth of 5.4% just in 2020 (ITU<br />

2020) 3 . The average daily time spent on the internet for all age groups (16-64) was around 7<br />

hours a day (GWI 2020).<br />

In the face of a paramount need to address continuous learning <strong>and</strong> learner well-<strong>being</strong> at the<br />

times of social distancing <strong>and</strong> uncertainty, this paper aims at providing a ‘state of the art’ review<br />

of the knowledge on the use of screen-based media devices 4 <strong>and</strong> learner well-<strong>being</strong>, drawn<br />

primarily from peer-reviewed literature on the topic.<br />

2<br />

https://blog.gwi.com/chart-of-the-week/device-trends-shaping-2020/<br />

3<br />

https://www.itu.int/en/ITU-D/Statistics/Pages/ff2020interactive.aspx<br />

4<br />

<strong>Screen</strong>-based media devices (SBMD) include mobile phone, tablet, laptop, television etc. (Mireku et al. 2019).<br />

3<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Reframing the Debate: <strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong><br />

There are always hypes <strong>and</strong> concerns surrounding new technology. While changing technologies<br />

certainly have some effects on us, these effects—both negative <strong>and</strong> positive—can often be<br />

exaggerated during the early stages of diffusion of a new technology. Even before the COVID-19<br />

p<strong>and</strong>emic hit the world, there were concerns about screen time as related to ‘learner well<strong>being</strong>’<br />

(See Box 1) that includes social skills deficits, deteriorating physical <strong>and</strong> mental health,<br />

<strong>and</strong> aggression of young children <strong>and</strong> adolescents.<br />

Box 1: Defining ‘screen time’ <strong>and</strong> ‘learner well-<strong>being</strong>’<br />

Definition of screen time: <strong>Screen</strong> time refers to time spent on screen-based media devices,<br />

including TV <strong>and</strong> both mobile <strong>and</strong> stationary digital devices of different sizes, such as smart<br />

phones, tablets <strong>and</strong> computers.<br />

Definition of learner well-<strong>being</strong>: The notion of learner well-<strong>being</strong> in this paper entails both<br />

learning outcomes (cognitive) <strong>and</strong> health outcomes (mental such as social <strong>and</strong> emotional<br />

development <strong>and</strong> physical health).<br />

The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) revised its screen time guidelines in 2016 to<br />

recommend no use of screen media except for video-chatting for children under 18 months,<br />

<strong>and</strong> ‘high-quality’ programming for children of 18-24 months of age <strong>and</strong> 1 hour daily screen<br />

limit for 2 to 5 year olds, with parental co-viewing. Similarly, regarding pre-school exposure to<br />

screens, the Canadian Paediatric Society (2017) recommends no screen time for children under<br />

2 years old, a 1-hour daily limit for 2-5-year olds <strong>and</strong> avoiding screens for an hour before bed.<br />

It has been pointed out that screen time guidelines are not always evidence-based, <strong>and</strong> that it is<br />

not possible to give limits or thresholds on screen time given that the effects of screen time<br />

depend so much on context (Viner, Davie <strong>and</strong> Firth 2019; Stiglic <strong>and</strong> Viner 2019).<br />

There are general tendencies in the media <strong>and</strong> paediatric communities to overstate the risks of<br />

prolonged screen time. It is important to bring to the fore what we know <strong>and</strong> do not know<br />

about the use of digital media devices <strong>and</strong> learner well<strong>being</strong> <strong>and</strong> the gaps in research <strong>and</strong><br />

practice we need to address.<br />

With social distancing restricting our ability to have face-to-face interaction, for many of us,<br />

screens are our lifelines <strong>and</strong> our window into the outside world. Both the negative <strong>and</strong> positive<br />

effects of using screen devices need to be considered in reframing the overgeneralized <strong>and</strong><br />

oversimplified screen-time debate <strong>and</strong> advancing public debate <strong>and</strong> evidence-informed<br />

policymaking on the use of screen-based digital media devices for young children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents.<br />

4<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Approach<br />

This paper provides a rapid scoping review of recent systematic reviews (including a review of<br />

reviews <strong>and</strong> meta-analyses) <strong>and</strong> research studies on the effects of the use of screen-based<br />

digital media devices on learner well-<strong>being</strong> (including learning gains). The purpose is to scope a<br />

body of health literature on screen time, clarify definitions <strong>and</strong> conceptual boundaries of screen<br />

time, <strong>and</strong> identify gaps in the research.<br />

This scoping review draws primarily on a core set of 22 articles on screen time recently<br />

published in peer-reviewed journals as its main source of information (see Appendix I). Aside<br />

from two articles, they were all published within the last 10 years, <strong>and</strong> the majority of them<br />

were published within the last 5 years. While the majority of these 22 studies cover North<br />

American <strong>and</strong> European countries, they include focused studies undertaken in Australia,<br />

Thail<strong>and</strong>, Turkey, Iran, <strong>and</strong> China.<br />

The core set of articles includes five systematic reviews (one Review of Reviews <strong>and</strong> two metaanalyses)<br />

which together cover 377 studies in total. The total number of participants in the 22<br />

studies covered in the present review amounts to 3.77 million (see Appendix I). Rubrics were<br />

designed <strong>and</strong> used to highlight analytical foci <strong>and</strong> key messages <strong>and</strong> draw key recommendations<br />

from these articles (see Appendix II).<br />

In addition, the paper draws on peer-reviewed literature which does not explicitly focus on<br />

screen time but on relevant topics such as learning through different media <strong>and</strong> instructional<br />

effectiveness of computer games. It also covers reports by international organizations that<br />

might not have been peer reviewed, taking into account their influence in national <strong>and</strong><br />

international policymaking.<br />

Methodological limitations of the current review<br />

Due to the rapid nature of this scoping review, this paper is not based on a comprehensive<br />

literature search but draws on a selection of studies that have explored the topic of screen time,<br />

<strong>and</strong> only studies in English were included. Although Carson’s (2016) systematic review of<br />

sedentary behaviour <strong>and</strong> health indicators covered 235 studies from 71 countries, most<br />

literature reviewed for this paper focuses on data from North America <strong>and</strong> Europe (see<br />

Appendix I), <strong>and</strong> thus generalizability of research findings <strong>and</strong> their applicability to the rest of<br />

the world might need to be cautiously evaluated.<br />

As a result of the prevalence of remote working <strong>and</strong> distance learning in the context of the<br />

COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic, it is speculated that averages of daily screen time have drastically<br />

increased. For example, a newspaper reported that screen time for children doubled after the<br />

first lockdown was introduced in India based on a survey conducted with parents of children<br />

5<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


aged 5 - 15 years. 5 Given the importance of observing social distancing measures, it may not be<br />

practical to follow the screen time guidelines developed in the pre-COVID-19 times. This paper<br />

however was not able to cover research on screen time in the context of COVID-19, as few<br />

studies have been published by the time the paper was completed.<br />

What is the current ‘state of the art’ of the knowledge on screen time?<br />

While there is an emerging body of high-quality studies, research on screen time suffers from<br />

conceptual, theoretical <strong>and</strong> methodological limitations that make the evidence collected so far<br />

inadequate in terms of guiding policy. As will be discussed below in Key Insights 2 <strong>and</strong> 3, ‘screen<br />

time’ is not a sensible construct as an object of study. Consequently, studies that take it at face<br />

value are likely misleading because of the confounding of more important variables (Key Insight<br />

4).<br />

Not only is the geographic coverage of screen-time literature largely restricted to North America<br />

<strong>and</strong> Europe, most studies are limited in terms of deriving causal relationships, as they are<br />

correlational or associational in nature. Many studies use cross-sectional data, which is helpful<br />

for hypothesis generation, but not suitable for hypothesis testing <strong>and</strong> for capturing the longterm<br />

outcomes. Furthermore, most studies control <strong>and</strong> account for a limited set of background<br />

variables, with insufficient consideration of children’s broader life context, which risks<br />

overestimating the effect of screen time (‘positive confounding’) or observing association<br />

between screen time <strong>and</strong> well-<strong>being</strong> outcomes when in fact there might be none.<br />

As UNICEF’s literature review covering research conducted between 2005 <strong>and</strong> 2017 on the<br />

impact of digital technology use on children’s well-<strong>being</strong> (Kardefelt-Winther 2017) has pointed<br />

out, research in this area tends to focus on screen harm <strong>and</strong> the hypothetical idea of addiction<br />

to technology, rather than investigating why some children spend more time on screen-based<br />

media devices than others <strong>and</strong> when more screen time under what conditions might have an<br />

effect—positively or negatively—on their well-<strong>being</strong>.<br />

Perhaps more fundamentally, not only is there insufficient evidence base to determine when<br />

screen time becomes excessive, determining ‘excessive use’ itself depends on the context in<br />

which screen-based media devices are used <strong>and</strong> is shaped by what is perceived to be ‘normal’<br />

or ‘moderate’ use of such devices. Furthermore, in addition to generational differences on what<br />

is considered ‘normal’ use, views <strong>and</strong> opinions on ‘how much screen time is too much’ <strong>and</strong><br />

‘which use is appropriate’ will change over time <strong>and</strong> according to changing contexts. In this<br />

respect, it is telling that WHO supported video games as a healthy means of physical distancing<br />

5<br />

COVID-19 Impact: <strong>Screen</strong> time up by 100% for children, 15 June 2020, The Economic <strong>Time</strong>s<br />

https://economictimes.indiatimes.com/industry/services/education/covid-19-impact-screen-time-up-by-100-forchildren/articleshow/76383951.cms?from=mdr<br />

6<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


<strong>and</strong> social connection in the COVID-19 context, while it added “gaming disorder” as a<br />

classification in alongside others related to addictive behaviour. 6<br />

“…not only is there insufficient evidence base to determine when screen time<br />

becomes excessive, determining ‘excessive use’ itself depends on the context<br />

in which screen-based media devices are used <strong>and</strong> is shaped by what is<br />

perceived to be ‘normal’ or ‘moderate’ use of such devices.<br />

Key insights <strong>and</strong> recommendations from the review<br />

This section presents key insights that we highlight from the literature review, followed by some<br />

key recommendations. The key recommendations are a combination of recommendations<br />

emerging directly from the literature <strong>and</strong> recommendations we propose based on the key<br />

insights.<br />

Key Insights<br />

1. Digital technology will continue to grow <strong>and</strong> advance <strong>and</strong> its use in education will continue<br />

to increase.<br />

The advances made in digital technology over the past two decades within the educational<br />

sector have been phenomenal, including the development <strong>and</strong> use of artificial intelligence.<br />

There is no doubt that digital technology will have significant impacts on the lives of the current<br />

<strong>and</strong> future generations <strong>and</strong> its use in education will continue to increase. The recent COVID-19<br />

p<strong>and</strong>emic has given a renewed significance to digital learning tools.<br />

6<br />

The WHO is recommending video games as an effective way to stop the spread of COVID-19, one year after adding 'gaming<br />

disorder' to its list of addictive behaviours, 2 April 2020, Business Insider Australia,.<br />

https://www.businessinsider.com.au/who-video-games-coronavirus-p<strong>and</strong>emic-mental-health-disorder-2020-4?r=US&IR=T;<br />

Gaming fosters social connection at a time of physical distance, 14 April 2020, The Conversation,<br />

https://theconversation.com/gaming-fosters-social-connection-at-a-time-of-physical-distance-135809<br />

7<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Figure 3: Increase in use of internet by school going children Source: Adopted from OECD (2018), p.6,<br />

In 2017, more than 98% of homes in the United States (US) with young children had a mobilescreen<br />

device, as compared to 52% six years earlier; similar trends have been observed in<br />

France, Australia <strong>and</strong> in many parts of Asia (Straker et al. 2018; Rideout 2017). The use of<br />

internet by school going children is rapidly increasing year by year (see Figure 3), <strong>and</strong> young<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents are using digital media at ever-increasing rates. For instance, in 2016,<br />

about 75% of teenagers in the US owned a smartphone <strong>and</strong> 76% were connected to at least one<br />

social media site (AAP 2016).<br />

Adapting to the increased use of digital devices at home, at school, at work <strong>and</strong> in society at<br />

large will certainly require changes not only in parenting but also in how education <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

are organized. The current situation is rather uncommon as children are often more<br />

comfortable in using digital devices <strong>and</strong> applications than adults. To foster constructive<br />

dialogues around screen time, especially in the current context of social distancing, it is crucial<br />

to listen to <strong>and</strong> take into account children’s voices.<br />

2. A high level of policy incoherence exists between the health <strong>and</strong> education sectors on the<br />

impacts of screen time<br />

Education authorities encourage the use of digital technology by children to prepare them for<br />

the digital world, although it is not uncontested within the education sector itself. 7 In contrast,<br />

health authorities discourage young children from using these devices based on concerns for<br />

7<br />

For example, in China, the Ministry of Education in 2018 together with seven other government departments launched an action<br />

plan to address the problem of nearsightedness in primary <strong>and</strong> secondary school students. This plan forbids students from bringing<br />

electronic devices to class, <strong>and</strong> requests teachers not to depend on electronic devices in teaching <strong>and</strong> giving homework (Ministry<br />

of Education of the People’s Republic of China 2018). The Republic of Korea announced its intention of going fully digital in school<br />

education, but in spite of a well-planned schedule put forward in the 1990s, Korea held back its plan to roll out the fully digital<br />

learning environment. Reasons for postponing the plan included the harmful effects of students’ overexposure to digital devices<br />

(Harlan 2012, as cited by Smart <strong>and</strong> Jagannathan 2018, p.59).<br />

8<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


their cognitive, emotional <strong>and</strong> social well<strong>being</strong> (Straker et al. 2018). This has led to policy<br />

incoherence between education <strong>and</strong> health guidelines <strong>and</strong> recommendations regarding<br />

children’s use of digital technology.<br />

Health officials <strong>and</strong> professionals often seek to minimize screen time with an assumption that<br />

screen time is sedentary, passive <strong>and</strong> displacing beneficial activities that are important for well<strong>being</strong><br />

outcomes. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, digital education guidelines often look at active screen time<br />

that involves interactive activities linked to learning outcomes.<br />

Given the digital imperative especially in the face of COVID-19, there is a need for cross-sector<br />

coordination <strong>and</strong> alignment of different goals <strong>and</strong> preoccupations of different sectors, with the<br />

notion of learner well-<strong>being</strong> at the heart of the screen time debate. Caution must be exercised<br />

when interpreting research on the negative effects of screen time as well as the positive effects<br />

of digital learning.<br />

More evidence disentangling prolonged screen time from other factors affecting health <strong>and</strong><br />

educational outcomes in young children <strong>and</strong> adolescents is clearly required. It is however<br />

important to note that a whole child approach emphasizing the sensible balance across good<br />

sleep, face-to-face social interaction, digital use, <strong>and</strong> physical exercises should remain a core<br />

component of recommendations for promoting learner well-<strong>being</strong>.<br />

3. Quality Content is more important than screen time. Many studies do not distinguish<br />

between the entertainment use of screen-based media devices (primarily television<br />

viewing time) <strong>and</strong> educational use of digital media devices such as computers, game<br />

consoles <strong>and</strong> smart phones.<br />

Traditionally, studies on screen time have focused on television, <strong>and</strong> many studies do not make a<br />

distinction between time spent on watching TV for pure entertainment <strong>and</strong> time spent on<br />

education related content. The same observation is valid for mobile digital screen devices for<br />

education such as computers, game consoles <strong>and</strong> smart phones versus these <strong>being</strong> used for<br />

entertainment. Given the elusive notion of ‘screen time’, one of the problems is overgeneralization<br />

of harmful effects of screen time without taking into account the enormously<br />

diverse use of screen-based media devices.<br />

The term ‘screen time’ is itself misleading <strong>and</strong> overly simplistic. Distinguishing between passively<br />

absorbing what is on the screen <strong>and</strong> actively engaging with it is critical to underst<strong>and</strong>ing the<br />

overall impacts of screen time. For example, a seminal 3-year ethnographic investigation of<br />

informal learning through digital media use following over 800 youth in the US (Ito et al. 2009)<br />

identified three major ways they engage actively with digital technology <strong>and</strong> captured them in<br />

what came to be known as HOMAGO (Hanging Out, Messing Around, Geeking Out) framework<br />

(see Box 2).<br />

Box 2: HOMAGO framework on digital media <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

HOMAGO is a concept that emerged out of the findings of the Digital Youth Project, a 3-year<br />

ethnographic investigation of informal learning of over 800 youth in the US, funded by<br />

MacArthur Foundation as part of the Digital Media <strong>and</strong> Learning Initiative (Ito et al. 2009).<br />

HOMAGO refers to:<br />

9<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


• Hanging out (friendship-driven, primarily social media);<br />

• Messing around (more intensive engagement with digital media <strong>and</strong> technology, e.g.<br />

searching for information online, gaming, digital media production); <strong>and</strong><br />

• Geeking out (interest-driven learning, learning how to navigate specialized domains of<br />

knowledge <strong>and</strong> practice <strong>and</strong> participating in online communities).<br />

In screen time research, the non-differentiation across television <strong>and</strong> digital devices screen time<br />

is problematic (Straker et al. 2018). For example, in a study focusing on 6-15 year-old children in<br />

the US, a negative association was found between physical <strong>and</strong> functional strength <strong>and</strong> television<br />

viewing but not with computer or video game use (Edelson et al. 2016; van den Heuvel et al. 2019;<br />

Christakis et al. 2009). These results demonstrate a need for a deeper <strong>and</strong> more nuanced<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of screen time.<br />

Literature on screen time <strong>and</strong> impacts has tended to focus on health outcomes <strong>and</strong> primarily with<br />

screen time associated with entertainment use. For example, a meta-analysis on screen time <strong>and</strong><br />

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms found that television viewing or<br />

gaming among children of 4–17 years of age was modestly associated with later ADHD symptoms<br />

(Nikkelen et al. 2014, Tanama et.al 2019, Christakis et.al 2004). However, there have an equal<br />

number of studies which have questioned the validity of many of these studies <strong>and</strong> have<br />

demonstrated that no correlation has been found between screen time <strong>and</strong> ADHD (McBee et.al<br />

2021, Camerer et.al 2018, Foster et.al 2010, Huber et.al 2018, Kostyrka-Allchorne et.al 2017)<br />

Relationship between screen time <strong>and</strong> cognitive outcomes has been less investigated. According<br />

to Anderson <strong>and</strong> Subrahmanyam (2017), studies show that for children less than 2 years, TV has<br />

negative impacts on language <strong>and</strong> executive functions, while for preschool children it is found to<br />

have both positive <strong>and</strong> negative outcomes. A large pool of research shows that educational TV<br />

programs could have positive impact on cognitive development, with examples such as Sesame<br />

Street. Beyond preschool, research tends to look at only entertainment program consumption<br />

<strong>and</strong> therefore cognitive outcomes have not been well explored.<br />

4. The cognitive, health <strong>and</strong> socio-emotional outcomes are influenced by the age <strong>and</strong> other<br />

individual characteristics of users as well as the content <strong>and</strong> the context of media device<br />

use.<br />

There are studies that report negative impacts of screen time on cognitive development such as<br />

language learning (see Key Insight 3), but there are also many studies that show positive or null<br />

associations (Drummond <strong>and</strong> Sauer 2014; Regondola <strong>and</strong> Barbado 2017). A recent meta-analysis<br />

of studies on the association between screen media use <strong>and</strong> academic performance among young<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents found no association between the two (Adelantado-Renau et al. 2019).<br />

This could be partially the result of several aspects of the overall screen media time measure that<br />

are not captured.<br />

Different studies give support to impact of screen time on children’s well<strong>being</strong> based on different<br />

hypotheses of the potential relationship (see Box 3).<br />

Box 3: Four hypotheses of the impact of screen time on children’s well-<strong>being</strong><br />

10<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


UNICEF’s literature review on the impact of digital technology on children’s well-<strong>being</strong><br />

identified four main hypotheses explored in research on digital technology use <strong>and</strong> user well<strong>being</strong><br />

(Kardefelt Winther 2017). This typology is useful in terms of examining research on the<br />

impact of screen time on learner well-<strong>being</strong>.<br />

1. The displacement hypothesis suggests that screen time is replacing or displacing<br />

opportunities for positive activities such as face-to-face interaction, exercise, sleep<br />

which are important for well-<strong>being</strong>.<br />

2. The rich-get-richer hypothesis suggests that those who already have strong in-person<br />

social networks <strong>and</strong> social skills will benefit more from digital media use in terms of<br />

social interaction than those who have weaker social connections.<br />

3. The social compensation hypothesis suggests that digital media use will be beneficial<br />

to those who are socially anxious <strong>and</strong> isolated as it compensates for lacked social skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> allows them to feel more at ease in interacting with others.<br />

4. The stimulation hypothesis suggests that digital media use stimulates communication<br />

with existing friends, leading to mostly positive outcomes.<br />

We must keep in mind that both positive <strong>and</strong> negative outcomes of screen time tend to be highly<br />

contextual <strong>and</strong> dependent on the design of activities children engage in through screen devices.<br />

A 2015 study on cognition <strong>and</strong> media concludes that there is no clear consensus <strong>and</strong> that effects<br />

likely depend on the age, type of content <strong>and</strong> other contextual factors (Anderson <strong>and</strong> Kirkorian<br />

2015). Focusing on screen time without adequately considering the internal <strong>and</strong> external<br />

attributes may severely limit the value of the conclusions drawn.<br />

The lack of association between screen media use <strong>and</strong> well-<strong>being</strong> outcomes, as well as the lack of<br />

agreement among studies, could be partially attributed to the measure of screen time which fail<br />

to take into account: (i) internal attributes of children that include: (a) age of children; <strong>and</strong> (b)<br />

gender; <strong>and</strong> (ii) external attributes such as (a) purpose <strong>and</strong> content of programming (e.g.<br />

educational versus entertainment); (b) the context in which young children <strong>and</strong> adolescents use<br />

screen media; <strong>and</strong> (c) active versus passive media (e.g. interactive media versus the older oneway<br />

media). As illustrated below, there is an ostensibly contradictory body of evidence on the<br />

associations between these factors <strong>and</strong> well-<strong>being</strong> outcomes.<br />

Internal Attributes:<br />

Age of children: For children less than 2 years, TV has negative impacts on language <strong>and</strong> executive<br />

functions. In preschool children it is found to have both positive <strong>and</strong> negative outcomes. A 2020<br />

meta-analysis study found positive association across screen use <strong>and</strong> stronger language skills for<br />

older children (Madigan et al. 2020). However, children (average age 12) who watch mainly<br />

entertainment programs tend to have reduced reading achievements <strong>and</strong> executive functions like<br />

focus <strong>and</strong> attention regulation (Ibid.).<br />

Gender: A meta-analysis on media use <strong>and</strong> ADHD-related behaviours in young children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents found no significant interactions between screen time <strong>and</strong> gender (Nikkelen et al.<br />

2014). According to the evidence-focused literature review by Kardefelt Winther (2017) for<br />

11<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


UNICEF, few significant gender differences were found in relation to the impact of children’s<br />

digital technology use on their mental well-<strong>being</strong>.<br />

However, more recently, studies looking at newer uses of digital screen devices such as social<br />

media showed gendered impact of screen time on mental health outcomes. One study that has<br />

received much media attention suggested that adolescents were 13% more likely to experience<br />

high levels of depression symptoms if they used social media at least once per day, <strong>and</strong> 34% more<br />

likely to have at least one “suicide-related outcome” 8 if they used an electronic device three or<br />

more hours per day, compared to two or fewer hours per day (Twenge et al. 2018). The<br />

correlation between time spent on social media <strong>and</strong> depression symptoms was only evident for<br />

female participants. Also important to note is that the time spent on social media was only related<br />

to higher depression symptoms for adolescents who did not engage much in face-to-face<br />

socializing. This would seem to be aligned with the “rich get richer” hypothesis elaborated in Box3.<br />

External Attributes<br />

Content: A recent systematic review <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis of associations between screen use <strong>and</strong><br />

child language skills found that ‘better-quality’ screen use (defined by ‘educational programs’ <strong>and</strong><br />

‘co-viewing’) was correlated with stronger child language skills (Madigan et al. 2020), which<br />

suggests that content (as well as the social context of viewing—see below) does matter in<br />

improving learning outcomes. One of the studies reviewed by Anderson <strong>and</strong> Subrahmanyam<br />

(2017), albeit a small-scale experiment involving a group of 18 children <strong>and</strong> a comparison group<br />

of 11 children (4 years of age), revealed after 3 months a literacy skill gain of 11 st<strong>and</strong>ard score<br />

points for the experimental group exposed to educational software while only 4 points for the<br />

comparison group exposed to entertainment software <strong>and</strong> a mathematics skill gain of 8 points<br />

for the education group while only 1 point for the entertainment group.<br />

“…content (as well as the social context of viewing) does matter in<br />

improving learning outcomes.<br />

There is an ongoing effort to demonstrate educational benefits of computer games, in addition<br />

to visionary work by pioneers who highlighted the potential of gaming to revolutionize education<br />

such as Paul Gee (2007) <strong>and</strong> Jane McGonical (2011). For example, a recent report highlighted that<br />

more than a third of 4,626 respondents (ages 11-16) reported that playing games made them<br />

better readers (Picton et al. 2020). A cross-sectional study based on data from the School Children<br />

Mental Health Europe project conducted in six European Union countries (youth ages 6–11, n =<br />

3,195) found video gaming beneficial for cognitive functioning (Kovess-Masfety et al. 2017). A<br />

recent review of experimental studies of instructional effectiveness of computer games found<br />

that games can increase student learning of academic material in science, mathematics <strong>and</strong><br />

second-language learning (Mayer 2019; see Table 1). These results indicate that content <strong>and</strong><br />

pedagogy play an important role in ensuring screen time be maximised for learning outcomes.<br />

8<br />

Suicide-related outcomes include a) significant sadness or hopelessness; b) seriously considering suicide; c) having a suicide<br />

plan; <strong>and</strong> d) making a suicide attempt.<br />

12<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Table 1: Effect of computer games on cognitive skills<br />

Discipline Experiments in which effects were observed Effect size<br />

Science 12 out of 16 experiments 0.7<br />

Mathematics 4 out of 6 experiments 0.5<br />

Language 4 out of 5 experiments 1.0<br />

Source: Adopted from Mayer 2019, Table 4.<br />

Note: An effect size greater than 0.4 is considered significant.<br />

Social context of viewing: A 2019 study showed how increased TV viewing was associated with<br />

language delays in early childhood (Madigan et al. 2019). However, one study observed that this<br />

could be attributed to the reduced child-parent interaction <strong>and</strong> play which is critical for early<br />

development (Christakis et al. 2009). A 2017 national study by Common Sense Media covering a<br />

representative sample of over 1,400 parents across the US revealed that on average 46% of<br />

children under 2 years of age had used a mobile media device at least once, but the average time<br />

spent watching TV or videos on a mobile device amounted only to five minutes per day (Rideout<br />

2017). In another study, with a sample size of 350 children from low-income urban population in<br />

the US, it was found that 96.6% of children were found to use mobile devices (Kabali et.al 2015).<br />

The same study reported that 70% of parents were giving children devices to keep them occupied<br />

while doing household chores while 65% used these devices to keep the children calm. These<br />

findings might suggest that increased screen time on mobile devices by young children can be<br />

linked to reduced time interacting with parents <strong>and</strong> negative well-<strong>being</strong> outcomes (see below).<br />

Active vs passive: Studies which have shown language impairments caused by increased screen<br />

time <strong>and</strong> in particular to children below the age of two have primarily used television screen<br />

time—a passive one way process—as the key explanatory variable. However, studies which<br />

looked at computers <strong>and</strong> other educational digital media—an active two way interactive<br />

process—found mixed effects on cognition.<br />

As Kardefelt-Winther (2017) summarizes in UNICEF’s literature review on the impact of digital<br />

technology use on children’s well-<strong>being</strong>, many studies show the use of digital media devices<br />

brings benefits to children, by opening new opportunities for social interaction <strong>and</strong> active creative<br />

expression. This points to the benefits of active screen time, but not all active screen time may<br />

lead to positive outcomes; content <strong>and</strong> educational outcomes are key if digital devices are to be<br />

beneficial for well<strong>being</strong>.<br />

Computer games with content developed with educational outcomes as the objective can provide<br />

promising examples of active screen time. While there is an emerging body of literature that<br />

suggests computer games can be as effective as or more effective than conventional media for<br />

particular instructional domains, more research is needed in which the control group is exposed<br />

to exactly the same academic material as the game group (Mayer 2019). It is important to keep<br />

in mind that positive education outcomes of active screen time are highly contextual <strong>and</strong> depend<br />

on the design of digital games <strong>and</strong> other activities children can engage in through screen devices<br />

(see Schrier 2018 for discussion of the use <strong>and</strong> design of game-based learning).<br />

13<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Key Recommendations<br />

1. Addressing the holistic development <strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong> of the whole child<br />

Policy should address the holistic development <strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong> of the whole child <strong>and</strong> support<br />

informed decision making by parents, caregivers <strong>and</strong> professionals. This requires a reexamination<br />

of the diverse perspectives derived from health <strong>and</strong> education literature, with a view<br />

to provide consistent, neutral <strong>and</strong> evidence-informed messages to parents <strong>and</strong> school<br />

administrators (Straker et al. 2018). Many of the apparent connections between screen time <strong>and</strong><br />

screen harm may be mediated by lost opportunities for positive activities (socialising, exercise,<br />

sleep) due to displacement by screen time. 9<br />

Recommendations addressing the holistic development <strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong> of the whole child include<br />

the following:<br />

a. <strong>Screen</strong> time guidelines should address the importance of adequate physical activity,<br />

healthy nutrition, good sleep hygiene, <strong>and</strong> nurturing social environment in promoting<br />

children’s well-<strong>being</strong> outcomes. These factors would contribute together with<br />

activities related to screen time to produce overall health effects (Ashton <strong>and</strong> Beattie<br />

2019). Positive activities such as socializing <strong>and</strong> physical exercise can also be<br />

conducted via screen devices, for example through the use of social media, playing<br />

active video games, or taking online exercise classes. 10 Careful attention also needs to<br />

be paid to areas where screen time seems to have direct negative impact on physical<br />

health. Obesity <strong>and</strong> myopia (near-sightedness), which were already major public<br />

health concerns prior to the COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic, are cases in point. For example, the<br />

increase in the consumption of high-density foods while using screen media appears<br />

to be related to <strong>being</strong> distracted from feelings of satiety or fullness (Robinson et al.<br />

2017). While current evidence is not conclusive, increased digital screen time appears<br />

to be associated with myopia onset <strong>and</strong> progression in children (Wai WONG et al.<br />

2020).<br />

b. Interventions on screen-based media use should include curtailed use within an hour<br />

before bedtime <strong>and</strong> particularly in darkness. Parents, teachers, health professionals<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents should be made aware of the associations between night-time<br />

screen-based media devise use (including TV watching) <strong>and</strong> sleep outcomes, as these<br />

may impact on cognitive function <strong>and</strong> educational attainment (Mireku et al. 2019).<br />

c. Health <strong>and</strong> education professionals as well as parents <strong>and</strong> caregivers should encourage<br />

screen-based activities that are beneficial to individual children based on their<br />

developmental stages <strong>and</strong> individual characteristics <strong>and</strong> needs, rather than<br />

categorically limiting screen time. Children can benefit from spending time in the<br />

9<br />

Kardefelt Winther’s (2017) literature review found little support for the displacement hypothesis, when it is understood as a<br />

hypothesis that screen harm is proportional to exposure (also see Box 2).<br />

10<br />

In the midst of the COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic, WHO tweeted that active video games <strong>and</strong> online exercise classes can be means to<br />

stay active <strong>and</strong> healthy at home. See https://twitter.com/WHO/status/1242738384772239360?s=20<br />

14<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


digital space. For digital natives whose identity, existence, <strong>and</strong> reality are almost<br />

unimaginable without digital devices, a forced reduction in screen time may lead to<br />

curtailment of positive activities such as interacting with friends during school closures<br />

or engaging in creative play that are important for their well-<strong>being</strong>. Instead, focus on<br />

finding a menu of different activities with children that they like to do, or guide them<br />

to make choices as to how they spend their time online or otherwise. If there are<br />

underlying problems which prompt excessive screen time, it is critical to address those<br />

problems rather than imposing a reduction in screen time.<br />

2. Improving the quality of screen-based activities<br />

Research shows that the quality of the content <strong>and</strong> for parents or caregivers to be there to help<br />

children process what is on screen are important factors that can lead to positive well-<strong>being</strong><br />

outcomes, especially for young children.<br />

a. Policy should support the development of high-quality educational content for digital<br />

devices. For example, there is evidence to support a hybrid form of learning, such as<br />

the introduction of touch-screen devices while reading traditional books in early<br />

childhood education (Anderson <strong>and</strong> Subrahmanyam 2017). However, clear<br />

information on appropriate digital technology use for educational purposes is still<br />

limited.<br />

b. Parents are encouraged to participate in screen time activities for children from early<br />

childhood to at least the early years in school. Online interaction is a valid form of<br />

interaction, <strong>and</strong> positive relationships can be formed <strong>and</strong> encouraged online.<br />

However, for children to develop the skills <strong>and</strong> resilience to cope with the world,<br />

they need regular interaction in the physical world. This is especially true for young<br />

children who need regular offline play <strong>and</strong> interaction with other people. Family<br />

involvement can play an important role in having children engage with technology in<br />

positive ways to ensure development process of children growing up in a digital age<br />

(Madigan et al. 2019). Parental involvement is also important in terms of facilitating<br />

home-based learning during school closures <strong>and</strong> also at normal times. Authorities<br />

should consider measures to enable parents to make a living <strong>and</strong> take care of <strong>and</strong><br />

educate their children during lockdowns, such as reduced work hours for the same<br />

wages <strong>and</strong> online support to parents.<br />

3. Encouraging further research <strong>and</strong> strengthening research-policy-practice nexus<br />

Given the rapid increase in the use of digital screen devices by young children <strong>and</strong> adolescents<br />

internationally, notably for new content areas such as social media, there is a need for more <strong>and</strong><br />

better research as well as for strengthening the research-policy-practice nexus. The following<br />

recommendations are suggested to strengthen the literature to guide policymaking in the use of<br />

digital media.<br />

15<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


a. Promote further research to disaggregate the notion of ‘screen time’ <strong>and</strong> deepen the<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the impact of the contexts <strong>and</strong> content of digital devices on learner<br />

well-<strong>being</strong>, particularly in relationship to mobile digital devices. With its reliance on<br />

self-reporting of the total daily or weekly time spent on screen <strong>and</strong> the ensuing low<br />

quality of measurement available in current data sources, there is limited high-quality<br />

(robust <strong>and</strong> transparent) evidence for harmful impacts of screen time. Aggregate<br />

measures of ‘screen time’ are conceptually problematic <strong>and</strong> need to be reconsidered,<br />

as they ignore the different screen devices used by children <strong>and</strong> how they engage with<br />

what is on the screen. This might lead to ending the use of the construct ‘screen time’,<br />

except in the restricted sense of its impact on vision or sleep quality.<br />

b. Conduct a long-term follow up while controlling for confounding variables, <strong>and</strong> produce<br />

longitudinal data to gain a better underst<strong>and</strong>ing of screen time <strong>and</strong> its positive <strong>and</strong><br />

negative impacts (Ashton <strong>and</strong> Beattie 2019; Straker et al. 2018; Anderson <strong>and</strong><br />

Subrahmanyam 2017). Little is known about the cumulative effects of spending much<br />

time on digital screen media from a young age, <strong>and</strong> it is particularly important to study<br />

the long-term consequences of digital media use. This requires more longitudinal<br />

studies for confirmatory (hypothesis-testing) research. It also requires including more<br />

control variables so as not to exclude variables that have established effects on well<strong>being</strong><br />

outcomes. Fully taking into consideration that children’s experiences with<br />

screen media cannot be studied in vacuum in isolation from their life contexts,<br />

researchers should tighten research design to control <strong>and</strong> account for sociodemographic<br />

<strong>and</strong> other variables <strong>and</strong> increase the likelihood of having true (unconfounded)<br />

results. 11 At the same time, it is important to be mindful that the rapid<br />

evolution of digital technology is still ongoing <strong>and</strong> even accelerating, <strong>and</strong> that there is<br />

a risk that longitudinal studies become obsolete by the time they conclude due to<br />

dramatic changes in the context of media use. One cannot easily gather evidence of<br />

long-term effects when the constructs keep changing as the research is conducted.<br />

c. Support research studies by a team of multi-disciplinary experts <strong>and</strong> where possible<br />

multi-stakeholder groups to provide a more holistic underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the multidimensional<br />

impacts that might emerge from digital media use by young children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents. Not only quantitative studies but also qualitative studies should be<br />

encouraged, as qualitative data can generate more nuanced <strong>and</strong> context-specific<br />

insights based on children’s voices, which in turn can contribute to generating<br />

hypotheses <strong>and</strong> building theories along with cross-sectional studies. Multi-disciplinary<br />

collaboration <strong>and</strong> close partnerships between health <strong>and</strong> education ministries, schools<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents are necessary to strengthen the research-policy-practice nexus.<br />

11<br />

Kardefelt-Winther (2017) also recommends strengthening reproducibility of research <strong>and</strong> the reliability of findings by<br />

encouraging researchers to register their hypotheses prior to data collection <strong>and</strong> then share the raw data <strong>and</strong> analysis code<br />

with each publication, following an ‘Open Science’ approach (for example through the Open Science framework—see<br />

https://osf.io/ ).<br />

16<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Conclusion<br />

<strong>Time</strong> spent on screens—from social media to computers <strong>and</strong> television—is a major part of<br />

everyday life <strong>and</strong> a necessary part of education today <strong>and</strong> into the foreseeable future. One of the<br />

problems is over-generalization of harmful effects of screen time without taking into account the<br />

enormously diverse use of screen-based media devices. Different uses have different effects. On<br />

the same media platform (for example on YouTube <strong>and</strong>/or Instagram), different content will have<br />

different effects. Furthermore, the same content could have different effects on different people<br />

or on the same person in different time frames or in different environments or under different<br />

conditions of usage. Technology is used in varied ways, with wide-ranging motivations, to access<br />

an enormous amount of diverse content. Certain uses affect certain people in a negative way,<br />

while certain uses affect certain people in a positive way.<br />

The risks from screen exposure should not be overstated. The evidence is relatively weak overall,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the magnitude of impact of screens is small on key health outcomes. The existing screen time<br />

guidelines tend to focus on risks, without recognizing the potential benefits of digital screen use<br />

in education. Currently the literature takes little account of the increasing dem<strong>and</strong>s for homework<br />

to be undertaken on computers nor the needs for school lessons to be delivered online, although<br />

these will likely be taken up as important research topics given the COVID-19.<br />

Collaboration across the health <strong>and</strong> education sectors, inter-disciplinary research, <strong>and</strong> schoolfamily<br />

partnerships will be the key to advancing policy <strong>and</strong> practice around screen time in a digital<br />

age. Another key point we must take into consideration is the role of the younger generation.<br />

We now know for sure that the Generation Z, Millennials <strong>and</strong> to a smaller extent Generation X<br />

are all primarily digital natives. They thrive in the digital world <strong>and</strong> therefore any policy referring<br />

to use of digital media must involve the voices of youth <strong>and</strong> their concerns <strong>and</strong> wants in the wideranging<br />

use of digital technology in their lives—for entertainment, socialising, learning <strong>and</strong> more.<br />

“Collaboration across the health <strong>and</strong> education sectors, inter-disciplinary<br />

research, <strong>and</strong> school-family partnerships will be the key to advancing policy<br />

<strong>and</strong> practice around screen time in a digital age.<br />

Acknowledgements <strong>and</strong> Disclaimer<br />

We would like to thank Karen (Kat) Schrier <strong>and</strong> John Sabatini for their insightful comments on an<br />

earlier version of this research brief. We also thank Kriti Singh for finding post-2020 data on<br />

digital device use.<br />

The analysis, conclusions, <strong>and</strong> recommendations contained in this paper are solely a product of<br />

the individual authors <strong>and</strong> are not the policy or opinions of, nor do they represent an<br />

endorsement by UNESCO or UNESCO MGIEP.<br />

17<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Bibliography<br />

AAP (American Association of Pediatrics) Council on Communications <strong>and</strong> Media (2016) ‘Media use in schoolaged<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents’, Pediatrics, 138(5), e2016259.<br />

Abramson, M.J., Benke, G.P., Dimitriadis, C., Inyang, I.O. <strong>and</strong> Sim, M.R. (2009) ‘Mobile telephone use is<br />

associated with changes in cognitive function in young adolescents’, Bioelectromagnetics, 30, pp. 678-686.<br />

Adelantado-Renau, M., Urdiales, D.M., Redonodo, I.C., Valls, M.R.B., Vizcaino, V.M. <strong>and</strong> Bueno, C.A. (2019)<br />

‘Association between screen media use <strong>and</strong> academic performance among children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: a<br />

systematic review <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis’, JAMA Pediatrics, 173(11), pp. 1058–67.<br />

Anderson, D.R. <strong>and</strong> Kirkorian, H.L. (2015) ‘Media <strong>and</strong> cognitive development’, in Liben, L.S., Müller, U. <strong>and</strong><br />

Lerner, R.M. (eds.) H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology <strong>and</strong> developmental science - Cognitive processes. John Wiley<br />

& Sons Inc., pp. 949–994.<br />

Anderson, D.R. <strong>and</strong> Subrahmanyam, K. (on behalf of the Cognitive Impacts of Digital Media Workgroup)<br />

(2017) ‘Digital screen media <strong>and</strong> cognitive development’, Pediatrics (Official Journal of the American Academy<br />

of Pediatrics), 140 (Supplement 2), pp. S57-S61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758C.<br />

Ashton, J.J. <strong>and</strong> Beattie, R.M. (2019) ‘<strong>Screen</strong> time in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: is there evidence to guide parents<br />

<strong>and</strong> policy?’, Lancet Child Adolescent Health, 3(5), pp. 292-294. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30062-8.<br />

Bowers, A.J. <strong>and</strong> Matthew, B. (2013) ‘Does recreational computer use affect high school achievement?’,<br />

Educational Technology Research & Development, 61(1), pp. 51-69. doi:10.1007/s11423-012-9274-1.<br />

Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., Kirchler, M., Nave, G., Nosek,<br />

B. A., Pfeiffer, T., Altmejd, A., Buttrick, N., Chan, T., Chen, Y., Forsell, E., Gampa, A., Heikensten, E., Hummer,<br />

L., Imai, T., . . . Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature <strong>and</strong> Science<br />

between 2010 <strong>and</strong> 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 637–644. https:// doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z<br />

Canadian Paediatric Society, Digital Health Task Force (2017) ‘<strong>Screen</strong> time <strong>and</strong> young children: promoting<br />

health <strong>and</strong> development in a digital world’, Paediatrics <strong>and</strong> Child Health, 22(8), pp. 461-468.<br />

Canadian Paediatric Society, Digital Health Task Force (2019) ‘Digital media: promoting healthy screen use in<br />

school-aged children <strong>and</strong> adolescents’, Paediatrics <strong>and</strong> Child Health, 24(6), pp. 402–408.<br />

doi: 10.1093/pch/pxz095<br />

Carson V, Hunter S, Kuzik N, et al. (2016) ‘Systematic review of sedentary behaviour <strong>and</strong> health indicators in<br />

school-aged children <strong>and</strong> youth: an update’, Applied Physiology, Nutrition, <strong>and</strong> Metabolism, 41(6 Suppl 3): pp.<br />

S240-S265. doi:10.1139/apnm-2015-0630.<br />

Carter, B., Rees, P., Hale, L., Bhattacharjee, D. <strong>and</strong> Paradkar, M.S. (2016) ‘Association between portable<br />

screen-based media device access or use <strong>and</strong> sleep outcomes: a systematic review <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis’, JAMA<br />

Pediatrics 170(12), pp. 1202-1208.<br />

18<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Christakis, D.A., Gilkerson J., Richards J.A., Zimmerman, F.J., Garrison, M.M., Xu, D., Gray, S. <strong>and</strong> Yapanel, U.<br />

(2009) ‘Audible television <strong>and</strong> decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, <strong>and</strong> conversational turns: a<br />

population-based study’, Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicin, 163(6), pp. 554-558.<br />

Cristia, A. <strong>and</strong> Seidl A. (2015) ‘Parental reports on touch screen use in early childhood’, PLoS One, 10(6),<br />

e0128338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128338.<br />

Drummond, A. <strong>and</strong> Sauer, J.D. (2014) ‘Video-games do not negatively impact adolescent academic performance<br />

in science, mathematics or reading’, PLoS ONE 9(4), e87943.<br />

Edelson, L.R., Mathias, K.C., Fulgoni, V.L. 3 rd <strong>and</strong> Karagounis, L.G. (2016) ‘<strong>Screen</strong>-based sedentary behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

associations with functional strength in 6–15 year-old children in the United States’, BMC Public Health,16,<br />

Article 116. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-2791-9.<br />

Esmaeilzadeh, S. <strong>and</strong> Kalantari, H. (2013) ‘Physical fitness, physical activity, sedentary behavior <strong>and</strong> academic<br />

performance among adolescent boys in different weight statuses’, Mediterranean Journal of Nutrition <strong>and</strong><br />

Metabolism, 6, pp. 207–216. doi: 10.1007/s12349-013-0133-3.<br />

Foerster, M. <strong>and</strong> Rsli, M. (2017) ‘A latent class analysis on adolescents media use <strong>and</strong> associations with health<br />

related quality of life’, Computers in Human Behavior, 71, pp. 266-274. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.015.<br />

Foster, E. M., & Watkins, S. (2010). The value of reanalysis: TV viewing <strong>and</strong> attention problems. Child<br />

Development, 81(1), 368–375. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624 .2009.01400.x<br />

Gee, J.P. (2007) Good video games <strong>and</strong> good learning. New York: Peter Lang.<br />

GlobalWebIndex (GWI). (2020). Coronavirus Research | March 2020 Series 3: Multi-market research. Retrieved<br />

from:https://www.globalwebindex.com/hubfs/1.%20Coronavirus%20Research%20PDFs/GWI%20coronavirus<br />

%20findings%20March%202020%20-%20Multi-Market%20data%20(Release%203).pdf<br />

Hale L. <strong>and</strong> Guan S. (2015) ‘<strong>Screen</strong> time <strong>and</strong> sleep among school-aged children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: a systematic<br />

literature review’, Sleep Medicine Reviews, 21, pp. 50-58. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2014.07.007.<br />

Huber, B., Yeates, M., Meyer, D., Fleckhammer, L., & Kaufman, J. (2018). The effects of screen media content<br />

on young children’s executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 170, 72–85. https://doi<br />

.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.006<br />

International Telecommunication Union (ITU). (2020). Measuring Digital Development: Facts <strong>and</strong> Figures 2020.<br />

Retrieved from: https://www.itu.int/en/ITU-D/Statistics/Pages/facts/default.aspx<br />

Ito, M., Baumer, S., Bittanti, M., boyd, d., Cody, R., Herr Stephenson, B., Horst, H.A., Lange, P.G., Mahendran,<br />

D., Martínez, K.Z., Pascoe, C.J., Perkel, D., Robinson, L., Sims, C. <strong>and</strong> Tripp, L. (2009) Hanging Out, Messing<br />

Around, <strong>and</strong> Geeking Out: kids living <strong>and</strong> learning with new media. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.<br />

Jackson, L.A., von Eye, A., Fitzgerald, H.E., Witt, E.A. <strong>and</strong> Zhao, Y. (2011) ‘Internet use, videogame playing <strong>and</strong><br />

cell phone use as predictors of children’s body mass index (BMI), body weight, academic performance, <strong>and</strong><br />

social <strong>and</strong> overall self-esteem’, Computers in Human Behavior, 27 (1), pp. 599-604. doi:<br />

10.1016/j.chb.2010.10.019.<br />

19<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Kabali, H.K., Irigoyen, M.M., Nunez-Davis, R., Budacki, J.G., Mohanty, S.H., Leister, K.P. <strong>and</strong> Bonner, R.L. (2015)<br />

‘Exposure <strong>and</strong> use of mobile media devices by young children’, Pediatrics, 136(6), pp. 1044–1050.<br />

doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151.<br />

Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2017) How does the time children spend using digital technology impact their mental<br />

well-<strong>being</strong>, social relationships <strong>and</strong> physical activity? An evidence-focused literature review. Innocenti<br />

Discussion Paper no. 2017-02. Florence: UNICEF Office of Research - Innocenti. Available at:<br />

https://www.unicef-irc.org/publications/925-how-does-the-time-children-spend-using-digital-technologyimpact-their-mental-well.html<br />

Kiatrungrit, K. <strong>and</strong> Hongsanguansri, S. (2014) ‘Cross-sectional study of use of electronic media by secondary<br />

school students in Bangkok, Thail<strong>and</strong>’, Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry, 26(4), pp. 216–226. doi:<br />

10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2014.04.005.<br />

Kostyrka-Allchorne, K., Cooper, N. R., & Simpson, A. (2017). The relationship between television exposure <strong>and</strong><br />

chil- dren’s cognition <strong>and</strong> behavior: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 44, 19–58. https://doi.org/10<br />

.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002<br />

Kovess-Masfety, V., Keyes, K., Hamilton, A., Hanson, G., Bitfoi, A., Golitz, D., Koç, C., Kuijpers, R., Lesinskiene,<br />

S., Mihova, Z., Otten, R., Fermanian, C. <strong>and</strong> Pez, O. (2016) ‘Is time spent playing video games associated with<br />

mental health, cognitive <strong>and</strong> social skills in young children?’, Social Psychiatry <strong>and</strong> Psychiatric Epidemiology,<br />

51(3), pp. 349–357. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1179-6.<br />

Li S, Jin X, Wu S, Jiang F, Yan C, Shen X. (2007) ‘The impact of media use on sleep patterns <strong>and</strong> sleep disorders<br />

among school-aged children in China’, Sleep, 30(3), pp. 361-367. doi:10.1093/sleep/30.3.361.<br />

Madigan, S., Browne, D., Racine, N., Mori, C. <strong>and</strong> Tough, S. (2019) ‘Association between screen time <strong>and</strong><br />

children’s performance on a developmental screening test’, JAMA Pediatrics, 173(3), pp. 244-250. doi:<br />

10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056.<br />

Madigan, S., McArthur, B.A., Anhorn, C., Eirich, R., Christakis, D.A. (2020) ‘Associations between screen use<br />

<strong>and</strong> child language skills: a systematic review <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatrics, 174(7), pp. 665-675. doi:<br />

10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327.<br />

Mayer, R. (2019) ‘Computer games in education’, Annual Review of Psychology, 70, pp 531–49.<br />

McBee,M.T., Br<strong>and</strong>,R.J. <strong>and</strong> Dixon,W.E., (2021). ‘Challenging the Link Between Early Childhood Television<br />

Exposure <strong>and</strong> Later Attention Problems: A Multiverse Approach’, Psychological Science. 1-23. DOI:<br />

10.1177/0956797620971650.<br />

McGonigal, J. (2011) Reality is broken: why games make us better <strong>and</strong> how they can change the world. New<br />

York: Penguin Press.<br />

Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China (2018) Implementation plan on prevention <strong>and</strong> control<br />

of near-sightedness in students in primary <strong>and</strong> secondary school. Beijing, China: People’s Education Press.<br />

20<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Mireku, M.O., Barker, M.M., Mutz, J., Dumontheil, I., Thomas, M.S.C., Röösli, M., Elliott, P. <strong>and</strong> Toledano, M.B.<br />

(2019) ‘Night-time screen-based media device use <strong>and</strong> adolescents' sleep <strong>and</strong> health related quality of life’,<br />

Environment International, 124, pp 66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2018.11.069.<br />

Muñoz-Miralles, R., Ortega-González, R., López-Morón, M.R., et al. (2016) ‘The problematic use of Information<br />

<strong>and</strong> Communication Technologies (ICT) in adolescents by the cross sectional JOITIC study’, BMC Pediatrics,<br />

16(1), Article 140. doi:10.1186/s12887-016-0674-y.<br />

Nikkelen, S.W.C., Valkenburg, P.M., Huizinga, M. <strong>and</strong> Bushman, B. (2014) ‘Media use <strong>and</strong> ADHD-related<br />

behaviors in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: a meta-analysis’, Developmental Psychology, 50(9), pp. 2228–2241.<br />

OECD (2018) Children <strong>and</strong> young people’s mental health in the digital age: shaping the future. Available at:<br />

https://www.oecd.org/els/health-systems/Children-<strong>and</strong>-Young-People-Mental-Health-in-the-Digital-Age.pdf<br />

Orben, A. <strong>and</strong> Przybylski, A.K. (2019a) ‘The association between adolescent well-<strong>being</strong> <strong>and</strong> digital technology<br />

use’, Nature Human Behaviour, 3, pp. 173–182. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1.<br />

Orben, A. <strong>and</strong> Przybylski, A.K. (2019b) ‘<strong>Screen</strong>s, teens, <strong>and</strong> psychological well-<strong>being</strong>: evidence from three<br />

time-use-diary studies’, Psychological Science, 30(5), pp. 682-696. doi: 10.1177/0956797619830329.<br />

Ozmert EN, Ince T, Pektas A, Ozdemir R, Uckardes Y. (2011) ‘Behavioral correlates of television viewing in<br />

young adolescents in Turkey’, Indian Pediatrics, 48(3), pp. 229-231. doi:10.1007/s13312-011-0048-3.<br />

Picton, I., Clark, C. <strong>and</strong> Judge, T. (2020) Video game playing <strong>and</strong> literacy: a survey of young people aged 11 to<br />

16. A National Literacy Trust research report. Available at:<br />

https://cdn.literacytrust.org.uk/media/documents/Video_game_playing_<strong>and</strong>_literacy_report_final_updated.<br />

pdf<br />

Przybylski, A. <strong>and</strong> Weinstein, N. (2017) ‘A large-scale test of the Goldilocks Hypothesis: quantifying the relations<br />

between digital-screen use <strong>and</strong> the mental well-<strong>being</strong> of adolescents’, Psychological Science, 28(2), pp. 204–<br />

215. doi: 10.1177/0956797616678438.<br />

Regondola, E.N. <strong>and</strong> Barbado, L.N. (2017) ‘Media habits <strong>and</strong> academic performance of elementary pupils of the<br />

Camarines Norte State College Laboratory School’, Imperial Journal of Interdisciplinary Research, 3(11), pp. 216-<br />

24.<br />

Rideout, V. (2017). The Common Sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. San Francisco, CA:<br />

Common Sense Media. Available at:<br />

https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/csm_zerotoeight_fullreport_relea<br />

se_2.pdf<br />

Robinson, T.N., B<strong>and</strong>a, J.A., Hale, L., Lu, A.S., Fleming-Milici, F., Calvert, S.L. <strong>and</strong> Wartella, E. (2017) ‘<strong>Screen</strong><br />

media exposure <strong>and</strong> obesity in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents’, Pediatrics, 140 (Suppl 2), pp. S97–S101. doi:<br />

10.1542/peds.2016-1758K.<br />

Rosen, B. (2019) How to utilize the research about children <strong>and</strong> screen time. Clinical Science Insights, January<br />

2019. The Family Institute at Northwestern University.<br />

21<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Schoeni, A., Roser, K., Röösli, M. (2015). ‘Symptoms <strong>and</strong> cognitive functions in adolescents in relation to mobile<br />

phone use during night’, PLoS One, 10(7), e0133528. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133528.<br />

Schrier, K. (2018) ‘Guiding questions for game-based learning’, in Voogt, J., Knezek, G., Christensen, R., Lai, K.W.<br />

(eds.) Second H<strong>and</strong>book of Information Technology in Primary <strong>and</strong> Secondary Education. Springer International<br />

H<strong>and</strong>books of Education. Springer, Cham. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-71054-9_59.<br />

Smart, A. <strong>and</strong> Jagannathan, S. (2018) Textbook policies in Asia: development, publishing, printing, distribution,<br />

<strong>and</strong> future implications. Manila: Asian Development Bank.<br />

Stiglic, N. <strong>and</strong> Viner, R.M. (2019). ‘Effects of screentime on the health <strong>and</strong> well-<strong>being</strong> of children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents: a systematic review of reviews’, BMJ Open, 9, e023191. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191.<br />

Straker, L., Zabatiero, J., Danby, S., Thorpe, K. <strong>and</strong> Edwards, S. (2018) ‘Conflicting guidelines on young children's<br />

screen time <strong>and</strong> use of digital technology create policy <strong>and</strong> practice dilemmas’, The Journal of Pediatrics, 202,<br />

pp. 300-303. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.07.019.<br />

Tamana, S. K., Ezeugwu, V., Chikuma, J., Lefebvre, D. L., Azad, M. B., Moraes, T. J., Subbarao, P., Becker, A. B.,<br />

Turvey, S. E., Sears, M. R., Dick, B. D., Carson, V., Rasmussen, C., CHILD study Investigators, Pei, J., & M<strong>and</strong>hane,<br />

P. J. (2019). ‘<strong>Screen</strong>-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: Results from the CHILD birth<br />

cohort study’, PloS one, 14(4), e0213995. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213995<br />

Twenge, J.M., Joiner, T.E., Rogers, M.L. <strong>and</strong> Martin, G.N. (2018). ‘Increases in depressive symptoms, suiciderelated<br />

outcomes, <strong>and</strong> suicide rates among U.S. adolescents after 2010 <strong>and</strong> links to increased new media screen<br />

time’, Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 3-27.<br />

Unantenne, N. (2014). Mobile device usage among young kids: a Southeast Asia study. Report commissioned<br />

by Samsung Kidstime. Singapore: theAsianparent Insights.<br />

Available at: https://s3-ap-southeast-1.amazonaws.com/tap-sgmedia/theAsianparent+Insights+Device+Usage+A+Southeast+Asia+Study+November+2014.pdf<br />

UNICEF (2017) Children in digital world: the state of world’s children 2017 report. Available at:<br />

https://www.unicef.org/publications/files/SOWC_2017_ENG_WEB.pdf<br />

van den Heuvel, M., Ma, J., Borkhoff, C.M., Koroshegyi, C., Dai, D.W.H., Parkin, P.C., Maguire, J.L. <strong>and</strong> Birken,<br />

C.S., TARGet Kids! Collaboration (2019) ‘Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language<br />

delay in 18-month-old children’, Journal of Developmental <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Pediatrics, 40(2), pp. 99-104. doi:<br />

10.1097/DBP.0000000000000630.<br />

Viner, R., Davie, M. <strong>and</strong> Firth, A. (2019) The health impacts of screen time: a guide for clinicians <strong>and</strong> parents.<br />

Royal College of Paediatrics <strong>and</strong> Child Health. Available at: https://www.rcpch.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2018-<br />

12/rcpch_screen_time_guide_-_final.pdf<br />

Wai WONG, C., TSAI, A., Jonas, J.B., Ohno-Matsui, K., CHEN, J., ANG, M. <strong>and</strong> Wei TING, D.S. (2020) ‘Digital<br />

screen time during COVID-19 p<strong>and</strong>emic: risk for a further myopia boom?’, American Journal of<br />

Ophthalmology, doi: 10.1016/j.ajo.2020.07.034.<br />

22<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Wouters, P., van Nimwegen, C., van Oostendorp, H. <strong>and</strong> van der Spek, E. D. (2013) ‘A meta-analysis of the<br />

cognitive <strong>and</strong> motivational effects of serious games’, Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 249.<br />

23<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> <strong>Time</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Learner</strong> <strong>Well</strong>-<strong>being</strong>:<br />

The Debate, the Evidence <strong>and</strong> Directions for Future Research <strong>and</strong> Policy<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Appendix I: Types <strong>and</strong> designs of study, geographic coverage, <strong>and</strong> participants of the studies covered in the present review<br />

Title of the Study Type Study Design Region Participants Age<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> time <strong>and</strong> sleep among school-aged children<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents: a systematic literature review (Hale<br />

<strong>and</strong> Guan 2014)<br />

Systematic review of sedentary behaviour <strong>and</strong> health<br />

indicators in school-aged children <strong>and</strong> youth: an<br />

update (Carson et al. 2016)<br />

Effects of screen time on health <strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong> of<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adolescents : a systematic review of<br />

reviews (Stiglic <strong>and</strong> Viner 2019)<br />

Associations between screen use <strong>and</strong> child language<br />

skills: A systematic review <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis<br />

(Madigan et al. 2020)<br />

A meta-analysis of the effect of media devices on<br />

sleep outcomes (Carter et al. 2016)<br />

Cross-sectional study of use of electronic media by<br />

secondary school students in Bangkok, Thail<strong>and</strong><br />

(Kiatrungrit <strong>and</strong> Hongsanguansri 2014)<br />

Mobile telephone use is associated with changes in<br />

cognitive function in young adolescents (Abramson<br />

et al. 2009)<br />

Symptoms <strong>and</strong> cognitive functions in adolescents in<br />

relation to mobile phone use during night (Schoeni et<br />

al. 2015)<br />

The problematic use of Information <strong>and</strong><br />

Communication Technologies (ICT) in adolescents by<br />

the cross sectional JOITIC study (Muñoz-Miralles et<br />

al. 2016)<br />

A latent class analysis on adolescents media use <strong>and</strong><br />

associations with health-related quality of life<br />

(Foerster <strong>and</strong> Rsli 2017)<br />

Behavioral correlates of television viewing in young<br />

adolescents in Turkey (Ozmert et al. 2011)<br />

Does recreational computer use affect high school<br />

achievement (Bowers <strong>and</strong> Matthew 2013)<br />

Systematic<br />

Review<br />

Systematic<br />

Review<br />

Systematic<br />

Review<br />

Meta Analysis;<br />

Systematic<br />

Review<br />

Meta Analysis<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Literature Review<br />

Review of Reviews (RoR)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Focussed Paper Cross sectional<br />

Studies from several<br />

countries included in<br />

review<br />

Studies from 71<br />

countries included in<br />

review<br />

Majority studies<br />

under review<br />

undertaken in United<br />

States<br />

US<br />

North America,<br />

Europe, Asia, <strong>and</strong><br />

Australasia<br />

Thail<strong>and</strong><br />

Number of<br />

Studies<br />

covered<br />

Total Number<br />

of Participants<br />

(N)<br />

5-17 year olds 67 401,869<br />

5-17 year olds 235 1,657,064<br />

0-18 year olds 13 1,046,165<br />

Average age of<br />

participants was 12<br />

years<br />

42 18905<br />

6 - 19 year olds 20 125198<br />

Adolescents<br />

(secondary grade<br />

students)<br />

1 768<br />

Focussed Paper Cross sectional Australia 7th Grade students 1 317<br />

Focussed Paper Cross Sectional Switzerl<strong>and</strong> 12-17 year olds 1 439<br />

Focussed Paper Cross sectional Spain 12 - 20 years olds 1 5,538<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Switzerl<strong>and</strong><br />

Turkey<br />

Adolescents (7th - 9th<br />

Graders)<br />

Adolescents (7th - 8th<br />

grade students)<br />

1 895<br />

1 581<br />

US 10th Grade students 1 13,960<br />

Comments<br />

Covered in Stiglic <strong>and</strong> Viner<br />

2019 but numbers not<br />

included<br />

Covered in Stiglic <strong>and</strong> Viner<br />

2019 but numbers not<br />

included<br />

N is sum of only 4 reviews<br />

where it was reported.<br />

Other 9 do not report total<br />

number of subjects.<br />

Included in Carson et al.<br />

2016<br />

Included in Carson et al.<br />

2016<br />

24<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Internet use, videogame playing <strong>and</strong> cell phone use<br />

as predictors of children’s body mass index (BMI),<br />

body weight, academic performance, <strong>and</strong> social <strong>and</strong><br />

overall self-esteem (Jackson et al. 2011)<br />

Night-time screen-based media device use <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents' sleep <strong>and</strong> health-related quality of life<br />

(Mireku et al. 2019)<br />

Physical fitness, physical activity, sedentary behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> academic performance among adolescent boys<br />

in different weight statuses (Esmaeilzadeh <strong>and</strong><br />

Kalantari 2013)<br />

The impact of media use on sleep patterns <strong>and</strong> sleep<br />

disorders among school aged children in China (Li et<br />

al. 2007)<br />

Association between screen time <strong>and</strong> children's<br />

performance on a development screening test<br />

(Madigan et al. 2019)<br />

A large-scale test of the Goldilocks Hypothesis:<br />

quantifying the relations between digital-screen use<br />

<strong>and</strong> the mental well-<strong>being</strong> of adolescents (Przybylski<br />

<strong>and</strong> Weinsten 2017)<br />

Association between adolescent well-<strong>being</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

digital tech use (Orben <strong>and</strong> Przybylski 2019)<br />

<strong>Screen</strong>s, teens <strong>and</strong> psychological well<strong>being</strong>: evidence<br />

from three time use diary studies (Orben <strong>and</strong><br />

Przybylski 2019)<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> time in children <strong>and</strong> adolescents - is there<br />

evidence to guide parents <strong>and</strong> policy (Ashton <strong>and</strong><br />

Beattie 2019)<br />

Digital screen media <strong>and</strong> cognitive development<br />

(Anderson <strong>and</strong> Subrahmanyam 2017)<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Investigatory (identify<br />

associations)<br />

Focussed Paper Longitudinal Study<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Focussed Paper<br />

Two part study: exploratory <strong>and</strong><br />

confirmatory<br />

Specification Curve Analysis<br />

(identify associations) on data<br />

from 3 large scale social datasets<br />

Two part study: exploratory <strong>and</strong><br />

confirmatory<br />

US<br />

Average age of<br />

participants was 12<br />

years<br />

UK 11-12 year olds 1 6,616<br />

1 482 Included in Carson 2016<br />

Iran 15 - 17 year olds 1 538 Included in Carson 2016<br />

China<br />

Canada<br />

Elementary Grade<br />

Children, Mean age =<br />

9 years<br />

Data collected at 2, 3<br />

<strong>and</strong> 5 years of age for<br />

each participant<br />

1 19,299<br />

1 2,441<br />

UK 15 year olds 1 120,115<br />

US <strong>and</strong> UK 12 - 18 year olds 1 355,358<br />

Irel<strong>and</strong>, United<br />

States, United<br />

Kingdom<br />

9 year olds 1 17,247<br />

Comment Comment Article NA NA NA NA<br />

Comment<br />

Comment Article/Literature<br />

Review<br />

Mainly US<br />

Mainly infants <strong>and</strong><br />

toddlers<br />

NA<br />

NA<br />

Included in Hale <strong>and</strong> Guan<br />

2014<br />

Covered in Stiglic <strong>and</strong> Viner<br />

2019 but numbers not<br />

included<br />

Total 392 3,793,795<br />

Adjusted Total after excluding double counts 385 3,772,127<br />

25<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Appendix II: Analytical foci <strong>and</strong> key conclusions of the studies covered in the present review<br />

Factor→<br />

Study↓<br />

Evidence<br />

/Data<br />

Sample<br />

Devices<br />

Timing of<br />

Usage<br />

Environment<br />

of Usage<br />

Purpose of<br />

Usage<br />

Content of<br />

Media<br />

Impact on Health<br />

Impact on Learning<br />

Conclusion/<br />

Message<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> time <strong>and</strong> sleep<br />

among school-aged<br />

children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents: a<br />

systematic literature<br />

review (Hale <strong>and</strong> Guan<br />

2014)<br />

Yes<br />

5-17 year<br />

olds<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Night time<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> time across<br />

different devices <strong>and</strong> on<br />

a consistent basis<br />

adversely associated<br />

with sleep outcomes but<br />

causal association not<br />

confirmed<br />

Not considered<br />

More studies necessary<br />

Systematic review of<br />

sedentary behaviour<br />

<strong>and</strong> health indicators in<br />

school-aged children<br />

<strong>and</strong> youth: an update<br />

(Carson et al. 2016)<br />

Yes<br />

5-17 year<br />

olds<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Higher durations of<br />

screen time associated<br />

with unfavourable body<br />

composition,<br />

cardiometabolic risk,<br />

unfavourable social<br />

behavior, low fitness<br />

<strong>and</strong> low self esteem.<br />

Higher duration of TV viewing<br />

associated with lower<br />

academic achievement but<br />

quality of evidence marked as<br />

'very low'.<br />

Overall quality of evidence in<br />

this review: low to moderate.<br />

Higher quality <strong>and</strong> strong<br />

studies required to confirm<br />

primarily observational<br />

evidence.<br />

Effects of screen time<br />

on health <strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong><br />

of children <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents : a<br />

systematic review of<br />

reviews (Stiglic <strong>and</strong><br />

Viner 2019)<br />

Yes<br />

0-18 year<br />

olds<br />

TV, Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Smartphones<br />

(but non TV<br />

data very<br />

sparse)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Strong indication of<br />

negative effects of<br />

screen time on health.<br />

Low quality/poor evidence<br />

that greater screen time was<br />

associated with poorer<br />

educational attainment.<br />

Moderate quality evidence for<br />

harmful effects of screen time<br />

on cognition among infants.<br />

Strong evidence for correlation<br />

between screen time adiposity,<br />

unhealthy diet, depressive<br />

symptoms <strong>and</strong> quality of life.<br />

Weak evidence that small<br />

amounts of daily screen time<br />

may have some benefits.<br />

Associations between<br />

screen use <strong>and</strong> child<br />

language skills: A<br />

systematic review <strong>and</strong><br />

meta-analysis (Madigan<br />

et al. 2020)<br />

Yes<br />

Average<br />

age of<br />

participant<br />

s was 12<br />

years<br />

All <strong>Screen</strong><br />

Based Devices<br />

primarily TV<br />

<strong>and</strong> Mobile<br />

Phones<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Co-viewing<br />

with<br />

Parents<br />

considered<br />

For<br />

Educational<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Entertainment<br />

Purposes<br />

Educational<br />

<strong>and</strong> Non<br />

Educational<br />

(both)<br />

Impact on Language<br />

Skills<br />

Greater Quantity of screen<br />

use associated with lower<br />

language skills but<br />

educational programs <strong>and</strong> coviewing<br />

associated with<br />

stronger language skills<br />

Limit exposure to screen time,<br />

select high quality programmes<br />

for viewing <strong>and</strong> co-view with<br />

parents when possible<br />

A meta-analysis of the<br />

effect of media devices<br />

on sleep outcomes<br />

(Carter et al. 2016)<br />

Yes<br />

6 - 19 year<br />

olds<br />

All <strong>Screen</strong><br />

Based Devices<br />

primarily TV<br />

<strong>and</strong> Mobile<br />

Phones<br />

Discussion<br />

on nighttime<br />

usage<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Bedtime media device<br />

usage associated with<br />

inadequate sleep<br />

quantity, poor sleep<br />

quality, <strong>and</strong> daytime<br />

sleepiness<br />

Not considered<br />

Age specific guidance for<br />

screen based media device<br />

usage <strong>and</strong> parent led initiatives<br />

to reduce access <strong>and</strong> use in<br />

collaboration with health<br />

providers <strong>and</strong> teachers.<br />

Cross-sectional study of<br />

use of electronic media<br />

by secondary school<br />

students in Bangkok,<br />

Yes<br />

Adolescents<br />

(secondary<br />

grade<br />

students)<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Use of devices not related to<br />

academic performance. As<br />

they age adolescents tend to<br />

spend more time engaged in<br />

Longitudinal studies with<br />

precise time logs of device<br />

usage <strong>and</strong> description of type<br />

of content required to<br />

26<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Thail<strong>and</strong> (Kiatrungrit<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hongsanguansri<br />

2014)<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

interpersonal communication<br />

through digital devices. Boys<br />

more interested in<br />

competitive games, girls more<br />

in passive recreation <strong>and</strong><br />

communication.<br />

determine the extent of effects<br />

in social <strong>and</strong> psychological<br />

development of adolescents.<br />

Mobile telephone use is<br />

associated with changes<br />

in cognitive function in<br />

young adolescents<br />

(Abramson et al. 2009)<br />

Yes<br />

7th Grade<br />

students<br />

Mobile<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Mobile phone use associated<br />

with faster but less accurate<br />

response to cognitive tasks.<br />

Greater mobile phone use<br />

related to poorer accuracy on<br />

working memory <strong>and</strong><br />

associative learning tasks.<br />

Symptoms <strong>and</strong> cognitive<br />

functions in adolescents<br />

in relation to mobile<br />

phone use during night<br />

(Schoeni et al. 2015)<br />

Yes<br />

12-17 year<br />

olds<br />

Mobile Phone<br />

Night time<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Increase in health<br />

symptom reports such<br />

as tiredness, rapid<br />

exhaustibility, headache<br />

<strong>and</strong> physical ill-<strong>being</strong><br />

No indications of impact on<br />

memory <strong>and</strong> concentration<br />

capacity<br />

More studies necessary<br />

The problematic use of<br />

Information <strong>and</strong><br />

Communication<br />

Technologies (ICT) in<br />

adolescents by the cross<br />

sectional JOITIC study<br />

(Muñoz-Miralles et al.<br />

2016)<br />

Yes<br />

12 - 20<br />

years olds<br />

All ICTs<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Problematic use of<br />

mobile phones<br />

associated with drug<br />

consumption. Video<br />

game use among male<br />

students associated with<br />

consumption of drugs,<br />

poor academic<br />

performance <strong>and</strong> poor<br />

family relationships.<br />

Problematic use of ICT <strong>and</strong><br />

Internet greatest risk for poor<br />

academic performance.<br />

Number of hours of use is a risk<br />

factor. Causality cannot be<br />

established.<br />

A latent class analysis on<br />

adolescents media use<br />

<strong>and</strong> associations with<br />

health-related quality of<br />

life (Foerster <strong>and</strong> Rsli<br />

2017)<br />

Yes<br />

Adolescents<br />

(7th - 9th<br />

Graders)<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

5 classes of<br />

usage<br />

identified:<br />

Low Use,<br />

Medium Use,<br />

Gaming, Call<br />

Preference<br />

<strong>and</strong> High<br />

Social Use<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

High Social Use class<br />

showed less well <strong>being</strong><br />

but best connection<br />

with peers; low selfperception;<br />

least<br />

satisfaction at home;<br />

less connectedness to<br />

school <strong>and</strong> teachers<br />

Not considered<br />

Direction of associations<br />

cannot be clarified but class<br />

analysis important to identify<br />

effects of media use<br />

Behavioral correlates of<br />

television viewing in<br />

young adolescents in<br />

Turkey (Ozmert et al.<br />

2011)<br />

Yes<br />

Adolescents<br />

(7th - 8th<br />

grade<br />

students)<br />

TV<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Parent reported<br />

durations were found to<br />

be correlated with<br />

aggressive behaviour.<br />

Not considered<br />

27<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


Does recreational<br />

computer use affect<br />

high school<br />

achievement (Bowers<br />

<strong>and</strong> Matthew 2013)<br />

Yes<br />

10th<br />

Grade<br />

students<br />

Computer<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Use of computers for fun <strong>and</strong><br />

gaming were positive <strong>and</strong><br />

significant on reading <strong>and</strong><br />

mathematics achievement<br />

assessments in high school.<br />

Causality is difficult to ascertain<br />

but effects of using computers<br />

for fun <strong>and</strong> games for 1-2 hours<br />

a day are +ve.<br />

Internet use, videogame<br />

playing <strong>and</strong> cell phone<br />

use as predictors of<br />

children’s body mass<br />

index (BMI), body<br />

weight, academic<br />

performance, <strong>and</strong> social<br />

<strong>and</strong> overall self-esteem<br />

(Jackson et al. 2011)<br />

Yes<br />

Average age<br />

of<br />

participants<br />

was 12<br />

years.<br />

Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong> Mobile<br />

phones<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Use of IT did not predict<br />

BMI.<br />

Videogame play predicted<br />

positive visual-spatial skills<br />

but lower GPAs. Use of<br />

internet associated with<br />

better reading scores among<br />

females. More videogame<br />

time associated with low self<br />

esteem.<br />

More research needed to<br />

investigate the causes <strong>and</strong><br />

consequences of IT use <strong>and</strong> to<br />

incorporate individual<br />

differences in effects of IT use.<br />

Night-time screen-based<br />

media device use <strong>and</strong><br />

adolescents' sleep <strong>and</strong><br />

health-related quality of<br />

life (Mireku et al. 2019)<br />

Yes<br />

11-12 year<br />

olds<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Night time<br />

(within one<br />

hour before<br />

sleep SBMD<br />

usage)<br />

Lit <strong>and</strong> dark<br />

rooms<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Night time use<br />

associated with higher<br />

odds of insufficient<br />

sleep; Magnitude of<br />

association higher when<br />

night time usage<br />

occurred in darkness.<br />

Not considered<br />

<strong>Screen</strong>-based media device<br />

(SBMD) usage should be<br />

curtailed within an hour before<br />

bedtime <strong>and</strong> particularly in<br />

darkness.<br />

Physical fitness, physical<br />

activity, sedentary<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> academic<br />

performance among<br />

adolescent boys in<br />

different weight<br />

statuses (Esmaeilzadeh<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kalantari 2013)<br />

Yes<br />

15 - 17<br />

year olds<br />

All sedentary<br />

behaviour<br />

through usage<br />

of all digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Positive relationship<br />

between sedentary<br />

behaviour <strong>and</strong> BMI.<br />

No relationship between<br />

weight classes <strong>and</strong> academic<br />

performance<br />

Longitudinal studies needed to<br />

determine the directionality.<br />

The impact of media use<br />

on sleep patterns <strong>and</strong><br />

sleep disorders among<br />

school aged children in<br />

China (Li et al. 2007)<br />

Yes<br />

Elementar<br />

y Grade<br />

Children,<br />

Mean age<br />

= 9 years<br />

TV <strong>and</strong><br />

Computer<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Device in<br />

bedroom of<br />

children<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

TV viewing of more than<br />

2 hours was a risk factor<br />

for sleep disorders.<br />

Computer use, however,<br />

had NO correlation with<br />

any sleep disorder.<br />

Not considered<br />

Presence of media in child's<br />

bedroom <strong>and</strong> media use had a<br />

negative impact on sleep<br />

patterns duration <strong>and</strong> sleep<br />

disorders.<br />

Association between<br />

screen time <strong>and</strong><br />

children's performance<br />

on a development<br />

screening test (Madigan<br />

et al. 2019)<br />

Yes<br />

Data<br />

collected<br />

at 2, 3 <strong>and</strong><br />

5 years of<br />

age for<br />

each<br />

participant<br />

TV, Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Smartphones<br />

(but non TV<br />

data very<br />

sparse)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Development <strong>Screen</strong>ing<br />

Test measured:<br />

communication, gross<br />

motor, fine-motor,<br />

problem solving <strong>and</strong><br />

personal-social. Higher<br />

levels of screen time<br />

significantly associated<br />

NA<br />

Evidence of directional<br />

association between screen<br />

time <strong>and</strong> poor performance on<br />

development screening<br />

28<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP


with poorer<br />

performance on<br />

development screening<br />

tests.<br />

A large-scale test of the<br />

Goldilocks Hypothesis:<br />

quantifying the relations<br />

between digital-screen<br />

use <strong>and</strong> the mental well<strong>being</strong><br />

of adolescents<br />

(Przybylski <strong>and</strong><br />

Weinsten 2017)<br />

Yes<br />

15 year<br />

olds<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Compares<br />

weekday vs<br />

weekend<br />

usage<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

4 classes of<br />

usage identified:<br />

Watching films,<br />

TV programs,<br />

etc.; Playing<br />

games; Using<br />

computers for<br />

internet, email,<br />

etc.; Using<br />

smartphone for<br />

social<br />

networking,<br />

chatting, etc.<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

The categories of digital<br />

activity examined in this<br />

study are unlikely to<br />

present risk to mental<br />

well-<strong>being</strong> at moderate<br />

levels.<br />

Not considered<br />

Moderate engagement in<br />

digital activities is not harmful.<br />

Association between<br />

adolescent well-<strong>being</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> digital tech use<br />

(Orben <strong>and</strong> Przybylski<br />

2019a)<br />

Yes<br />

12 - 18<br />

year olds<br />

All digital<br />

devices (TV,<br />

computer,<br />

smart phones,<br />

tablets,<br />

gaming<br />

consoles)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Association between<br />

Digital Technology use<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescent<br />

well<strong>being</strong> is negative but<br />

small<br />

Not considered<br />

Effects too small to warrant<br />

policy change<br />

<strong>Screen</strong>s, teens <strong>and</strong><br />

psychological well<strong>being</strong>:<br />

evidence from three<br />

time use diary studies<br />

(Orben <strong>and</strong> Przybylski<br />

2019b)<br />

Yes<br />

9 year olds<br />

TV, Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Smartphones<br />

(but non TV<br />

data very<br />

sparse)<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Little substantive<br />

statistically significant<br />

<strong>and</strong> negative association<br />

between screen time<br />

<strong>and</strong> well<strong>being</strong>.<br />

Not considered<br />

Highlights need for robust <strong>and</strong><br />

reliable data, analysis <strong>and</strong><br />

research to support claims.<br />

<strong>Screen</strong> time in children<br />

<strong>and</strong> adolescents - is<br />

there evidence to guide<br />

parents <strong>and</strong> policy<br />

(Ashton <strong>and</strong> Beattie<br />

2019)<br />

NA<br />

NA<br />

TV, Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

Smartphones<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Not discussed<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Overall negative but lack<br />

of evidence; correlation<br />

but no causation.<br />

Not discussed<br />

Highlights need for more<br />

evidence; caution when<br />

interpreting research on the<br />

negative effects of screen<br />

time.<br />

Digital screen media <strong>and</strong><br />

cognitive development<br />

(Anderson <strong>and</strong><br />

Subrahmanyam 2017)<br />

NA<br />

Mainly<br />

infants<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

toddlers<br />

TV, computers<br />

<strong>and</strong> touch<br />

screen devices<br />

Not<br />

discussed<br />

Co-viewing<br />

with<br />

parents<br />

considered<br />

Entertainment<br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

educational<br />

Entertainme<br />

nt <strong>and</strong><br />

educational<br />

Not discussed<br />

Impact on cognitive<br />

development is the main<br />

focus of the article.<br />

Educational television found<br />

to have positive impact on<br />

early cognitive development.<br />

Violent content found to<br />

influence antisocial <strong>and</strong><br />

aggressive behaviour.<br />

The greatest impact on<br />

cognitive development likely<br />

via content. A paucity of<br />

research on the differential<br />

effects of content as regards<br />

interactive digital media.<br />

29<br />

A Research Brief by UNESCO MGIEP

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!