26.04.2013 Views

TVR_106-3

TVR_106-3

TVR_106-3

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Volume <strong>106</strong>, Number 3<br />

ISSN 0042-8639<br />

Winter 2006<br />

The Volta Review<br />

Alexander Graham Bell Association<br />

for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing<br />

Early Hearing Detection and Intervention:<br />

Trends, Progress and Challenges<br />

Edited by<br />

Melody Harrison, Ph.D.


Volume <strong>106</strong>, Number 3<br />

ISSN 0042-8639<br />

Winter 2006<br />

The Volta Review<br />

Alexander Graham Bell Association<br />

for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing<br />

Early Hearing Detection and Intervention:<br />

Trends, Progress and Challenges<br />

Edited by<br />

Melody Harrison, Ph.D.


The Volta<br />

Review<br />

Early Hearing Detection and<br />

Intervention: Trends, Progress<br />

and Challenges<br />

233 Foreword<br />

Melody Harrison, Ph.D.<br />

237 Early Intervention for Children with Permanent Hearing Loss:<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution<br />

Karl R. White, Ph.D.<br />

259 Statewide Collaboration in the Delivery of EHDI Services<br />

Joni Alberg, Ph.D., Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT, and<br />

Jackson Roush, Ph.D., CCC-A<br />

275 Family Empowerment: Supporting Language Development in<br />

Young Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing<br />

Jean L. DesJardin, Ph.D.<br />

299 Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss<br />

Sarah McKay, Au.D.<br />

Volume <strong>106</strong>, Number 3<br />

ISSN 0042-8639<br />

Winter 2006<br />

Organized in 1890 to encourage the teaching of speech, speechreading and the use of residual hearing to<br />

people who are deaf or hard of hearing, the Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf and Hard of<br />

Hearing (AG Bell) welcomes to its membership all who are interested in improving educational, professional<br />

and vocational opportunities for people who are deaf or hard of hearing. Affiliated with AG Bell are the<br />

Parent Section, the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Section and the International Professional Section.<br />

321 Early Spanish Speech Acquisition Following Cochlear Implantation<br />

Jan Allison Moore, Ph.D., Scott Prath, M.A., and Adrianne Arrieta, M.A.<br />

343 Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children: Experiences<br />

and Considerations<br />

Andrea Bohnert, M.T.A.-F., Vera Spitzlei, Dipl.-Ing., Karl L. Lippert, Ph.D.,<br />

Dipl.-Ing., and Annerose Keilmann, M.D.


365 Early Cochlear Implant Experience and Emotional Functioning<br />

During Childhood: Loneliness in Middle and Late Childhood<br />

Efrat A. Schorr, Ph.D.<br />

381 Early Hearing Detection and Intervention in Developing<br />

Countries: Current Status and Prospects<br />

Bolajoko O. Olusanya, M.D., M.Phil.<br />

419 Beyond Early Intervention: Providing Support to Public<br />

School Personnel<br />

Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT<br />

433 Author Index for Volume <strong>106</strong><br />

Permission to Copy: The Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf and Hard of<br />

Hearing, as copyright owner of this journal, allows single copies of an article to be made for<br />

personal use. This consent does not extend to posting on Web sites or other kinds of<br />

copying, such as copying for general distribution, for advertising or promotional purposes,<br />

for creating new collective works of any type, or for resale without the express written<br />

permission of the publisher. For more information, contact AG Bell at 3417 Volta Place,<br />

NW, Washington, DC 20007, e-mail editor@agbell.org, or call 202/337-5220 (voice) or<br />

202/337-5221 (TTY).<br />

232 Table of Contents


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 233-235<br />

Foreword<br />

Melody Harrison, Ph.D.<br />

When I began working at the University of North Carolina over 25 years<br />

ago, the first child I served in our university clinic was an 11-month-old infant<br />

who had a severe-to-profound hearing loss. At that time, she was the youngest<br />

child with whom I had ever worked. I was thrilled to have the opportunity<br />

to provide speech and language services to a young mother and a baby who<br />

wasn’t even old enough to qualify as a toddler!<br />

Today in many parts of the world, including the United States, identification<br />

of hearing loss at age 10 months and fitting with hearing aids at 11<br />

months would be considered woefully late. Between 1993 and 2005, the percentage<br />

of newborns in the United States whose hearing was screened at birth<br />

rose from 3 to 93 percent and the age at which hearing loss was confirmed<br />

dropped dramatically. Digital hearing aids became available and cochlear<br />

implant technology improved. Equally significant was the development of a<br />

procedure for fitting hearing aids to the unique properties of a child’s small<br />

but growing ears.<br />

The combination of early identification and technological innovation has<br />

provided us with the tools to revolutionize long-term outcomes for children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing. Yet, we are faced with challenges few<br />

professionals could have imagined 15 years ago. Although the benefits of<br />

early intervention are gaining increasing acceptance around the world, in<br />

areas where resources are meager, the age of identification is measured in<br />

years rather than months and intervention is not at all early. For some children,<br />

it is nonexistent. In those places, we must think collectively and creatively<br />

about how to use current technology in a manner that meshes with the<br />

cultural and economic realities of a specific region.<br />

Even prosperous countries such as the United States experience the significant<br />

strains early identification has placed on early intervention services.<br />

Access to qualified providers for families and children with hearing loss<br />

continues to be a major challenge. Few professionals have been adequately<br />

prepared to work with children ages birth to 3, and the majority have not<br />

been educated in methodologies that promote access to spoken language. In<br />

fact, many professionals who work under the direction of a state’s Part C<br />

early intervention program have no educational background in childhood<br />

hearing loss at all. Further, there is little research to guide professionals to<br />

Foreword 233


serve children with a mild, moderate or unilateral hearing loss—all which can<br />

now be identified shortly after birth. As increasing numbers of children are<br />

identified with hearing loss at earlier ages, the number of children referred for<br />

services continues to steadily rise, creating further demands for qualified<br />

service providers. Although the challenges related to service coordination can<br />

appear insurmountable, the potential for making an extraordinary difference<br />

in the lives of young children with hearing loss has never been greater.<br />

This monograph on early intervention unites contributors from around the<br />

world, from Nigeria to Israel, Germany and the United States, and offers a<br />

range of perspectives and approaches to the conundrums that, to some degree<br />

or another, we all face. We begin our journey in the United States with Karl<br />

White, Ph.D., director of the National Center for Hearing Assessment and<br />

Management, who discusses potential solutions for the challenges faced by<br />

early intervention service providers across the country following wider<br />

implementation of newborn hearing screening.<br />

Next, Joni Alberg, Ph.D., executive director of BEGINNINGS for Parents of<br />

Children Who Are Deaf and Hard of Hearing, Inc., and her colleagues outline<br />

a model of collaboration among state agencies to benefit children and families<br />

in North Carolina.<br />

Jean Desjardin, Ph.D., advanced research associate in the Children’s Research<br />

and Evaluation (CARE) Center at the House Ear Institute, explores the<br />

relationship between parents’ sense of their own competence to guide their<br />

child’s early development and how it influences their child’s acquisition of<br />

spoken language.<br />

In the past, many children with unilateral hearing loss were identified only<br />

after they had begun to struggle academically during their school years.<br />

Newborn hearing screening now makes it possible to identify this population<br />

at birth. Sarah McKay, Au.D., senior audiologist at the Center for Childhood<br />

Communication at The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, focuses on the<br />

issues and challenges associated with managing unilateral hearing loss in<br />

very young children.<br />

For many years, bilingual learners in the United States were located almost<br />

solely along the southwestern border of the country. Today, none of the 48<br />

contiguous states is completely unilingual. Jan Moore, Ph.D., and colleagues<br />

present a case study of the spoken Spanish-language development of a child<br />

who received a cochlear implant at age 20 months. The case study is designed<br />

to expand on existing knowledge regarding Spanish language development<br />

among at-risk children to improve services for this population.<br />

Next, Andrea Bohnert, M.T.A.-F., and colleagues at the University Clinic<br />

Mainz, Germany, share their experiences with bilateral implantation and auditory<br />

development among 41 young children. The article also discusses several<br />

factors that must be considered when evaluating children for bilateral<br />

implants.<br />

Efrat Schorr, Ph.D., a postdoctoral fellow at the Gonda Brain Research<br />

234 Foreword


Center at Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel, investigates the relationship<br />

between early cochlear implantation and its effects on loneliness in middle<br />

and late childhood.<br />

London-based Bolajoko Olusanya, M.D., M.Phil., explores different approaches<br />

to culturally appropriate early identification and intervention in<br />

developing countries as well as key challenges and possible approaches to<br />

building service capacity.<br />

Our journey ends in the Unites States, where Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-<br />

CLP, Cert. AVT, director of the Resource Support Program at the Office of<br />

Education Services for the North Carolina Department of Health and Human<br />

Services, transitions the reader out of early intervention into the realm of<br />

public school. Wilson’s article describes a service model used in North Carolina<br />

to support public school teachers serving children who have transitioned<br />

from early intervention services.<br />

In the spirit of the unique cultural, governmental and professional frameworks<br />

within which we all work, the articles in this monograph issue are<br />

intended to prompt us to think critically about each topic and ask, “What can<br />

I implement in my setting?” “What theories and practices do I question?”<br />

“What excites me?” My hope is that each of you will read with a critical mind<br />

and open heart as we move forward in our shared quest to provide the very<br />

best to each child and family we serve.<br />

Melody Harrison, Ph.D.<br />

Foreword 235


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 237-258<br />

Early Intervention for<br />

Children with Permanent<br />

Hearing Loss: Finishing the<br />

EHDI Revolution<br />

Karl R. White, Ph.D.<br />

The value of identifying permanent hearing loss during the first few months of life<br />

and providing effective treatment to ameliorate or even eliminate the negative consequences<br />

has been recognized for many decades. Unfortunately, improvements in<br />

achieving this goal were very gradual until the early 1990s. At that time, the combination<br />

of technological advances in screening and diagnostic equipment and hearing<br />

technologies created a revolution in our ability to identify and provide<br />

intervention to children with permanent hearing loss during the first few months of<br />

life.<br />

Evidence continues to accumulate that for those children who are identified early<br />

and provided with appropriate hearing technologies and early intervention, dramatic<br />

progress is often possible. However, there are many infants and young children who<br />

are not yet benefiting from this revolution because timely and appropriate early<br />

intervention services are not available in many parts of the country. Part of the reason<br />

why so many children are missing out on these benefits is there has not been sufficient<br />

coordination between the state Early Hearing Detection and Intervention (EHDI)<br />

programs and the federally sponsored Infant and Toddler programs for which states<br />

receive partial funding under Part C of the Individuals with Disabilities Improvement<br />

Act of 2004. This article discusses how the early intervention aspects of EHDI<br />

programs could be improved by closer attention to the components required under<br />

Part C of eligibility criteria, child-find and referral systems, preservice and inservice<br />

personnel development, coverage of hearing aids and public awareness programs.<br />

Introduction<br />

At the beginning of this decade, the U.S. Department of Health and Human<br />

Services issued national health objectives (Healthy People 2010, 2000) that<br />

Karl White, Ph.D., is a professor of psychology at Utah State University and the founding<br />

director of the National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 237


identified the most significant preventable threats to the health of people in<br />

the United States and established national goals to improve people’s health<br />

and well-being. Developed by a broad array of stakeholders and using the<br />

best scientific evidence available, the objectives in Healthy People 2010 were<br />

predicated on the simple but powerful idea that broad awareness of understandable<br />

health objectives will enable various stakeholders to work together<br />

to achieve important outcomes.<br />

The fact that early identification of permanent hearing loss in children 1 was<br />

included as one of the goals in this high-visibility national plan is an important<br />

indication of how thinking about early identification and intervention<br />

with children who are deaf or hard of hearing (referred to hereafter as DHH<br />

children) has changed over the last 20 years. Goal 28-11 from Healthy People<br />

2010 states, “Increase the proportion of newborns who are screened for hearing<br />

loss by age 1 month, have audiologic evaluation by age 3 months, and are<br />

enrolled in appropriate intervention services by age 6 months.” The idea<br />

behind this goal is not new, but the belief that it could be achieved is a<br />

significant change in what people have believed for the past hundred years.<br />

Some of the first to advocate for systematic and sustained efforts to identify<br />

DHH children during the first few months of life were Ewing and Ewing<br />

(1944), when they called for the:<br />

need to study further and more critically methods of testing hearing in<br />

young children . . . during this first year the existence of deafness needs to<br />

be ascertained . . . training needs to be begun at the earliest age that the<br />

diagnosis of deafness can be established. (page 309)<br />

For more than 50 years following the Ewings’ call to action, many people<br />

agreed that it was important to identify DHH children early and provide<br />

early intervention services, but most were skeptical that it could be done<br />

effectively. For example, as recently as 1996, the prestigious United States<br />

Preventive Services Task Force, while noting that “congenital hearing loss is<br />

a serious health problem associated with developmental delay in speech and<br />

language function,” concluded that “there is little evidence to support the use<br />

of routine, universal screening for all neonates.”<br />

Skepticism about the practicality of early identification and intervention<br />

with DHH children began to change in the late 1980s when Dr. C. Everett<br />

Koop, then the Surgeon General of the United States, recognized the potential<br />

of technological advances in procedures and equipment for revolutionizing<br />

1 Many different terms are used to refer to children with permanent hearing loss (e.g., deafness,<br />

hearing impairment, auditory disorders). Recognizing that there are limitations to any single<br />

term, this article will use the term “deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) children” to refer to children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing, except in those cases where another source is being quoted.<br />

238 White


the way in which newborn hearing screening, diagnosis, and early intervention<br />

was being done.<br />

Deafness in infants is a serious concern because it interferes with the development<br />

of language–that which sets humans apart from all other living<br />

things . . . early intervention with hearing impaired children results in improved<br />

language development, increased academic success, and increased<br />

lifetime earnings . . . [and] actually saves money since hearing impaired<br />

children who receive early help require less costly special education services<br />

later . . . . I am optimistic. I foresee a time in this country . . . when no<br />

child reaches his or her first birthday with an undetected hearing impairment.<br />

(as reported in Northern & Downs, 1991, pp. 2-3)<br />

Dr. Koop’s commitment to early identification of DHH children and his<br />

optimism that it could be successfully implemented surprised many people,<br />

especially because fewer than 3% of all newborns in the United States were<br />

screened for hearing loss at that time (White, 2004). The average age of identification<br />

of DHH children was 2 1 ⁄2 to 3 years and had changed little during<br />

the previous fifty years (Toward Equality, 1988).<br />

As a result of Dr. Koop’s efforts, the Department of Health and Human<br />

Services established a goal to “reduce the average age at which children with<br />

significant hearing impairment are identified to no more than 12 months”<br />

(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1990). The rationale that<br />

accompanied this objective stated:<br />

. . . it is difficult, if not impossible, for many [children with congenital<br />

hearing loss] to acquire the fundamental language, social, and cognitive<br />

skills that provide the foundation for later schooling and success in society.<br />

When early identification and intervention occur, hearing impaired children<br />

make dramatic progress, are more successful in school, and become<br />

more productive members of society. The earlier intervention and habilitation<br />

begin, the more dramatic the benefits. (p. 460)<br />

During the next 15 years, positive changes in early identification of DHH<br />

children began happening faster and faster. Instead of the gradual evolutionary<br />

changes that had characterized the previous 100 years, revolutionary<br />

changes that began to occur improved both the process and the outcomes in<br />

many areas for DHH children. For example, as shown in Figure 1, the percentage<br />

of newborns screened for hearing loss increased from 3% in 1993 to<br />

93% in 2005 (NCHAM, 2006a). The widespread implementation of universal<br />

newborn hearing screening programs reduced the average age of identification<br />

for DHH children from 30 months to six months as noted by Harrison,<br />

Roush and Wallace (2003).<br />

Forty states have now passed legislation requiring newborn hearing<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 239


Figure 1. Percentage of newborns screened for hearing loss.<br />

screening (NCHAM, 2006b), and the number of children under age 5 receiving<br />

cochlear implants has more than quadrupled in the last five years to over<br />

2,000 children per year (NCHAM, 2006c). These changes mean that for those<br />

DHH children who are identified early, fit with appropriate hearing aids or<br />

cochlear implants and receive early intervention services from appropriately<br />

trained staff, many are able to progress at age-appropriate rates (Kennedy et<br />

al., 2006; Moeller, 2000; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl, 1998), require<br />

few, if any, special education services and result in educational cost<br />

savings of at least $400,000 per child (Honeycutt et al., 2003; Mohr et al., 2000).<br />

Although many more DHH children are receiving the benefits of early<br />

intervention and modern assistive listening devices now compared to 20<br />

years ago, there are still many DHH children and their families who are not<br />

(CDC, 2006a). In fact, a recent letter sent to all state early intervention programs<br />

from officials at the Department of Education and the Department of<br />

Health and Human Services (NCHAM, 2006d) noted that there was a “growing<br />

national crises in the provision of essential early intervention and health<br />

care services for infants and toddlers with hearing loss.” That letter stated:<br />

Studies have demonstrated that when hearing loss of any degree, including<br />

mild bilateral or unilateral hearing, is not adequately diagnosed and addressed,<br />

the hearing loss can adversely affect the speech, language, academic,<br />

emotional, and psychosocial development of young children . . .<br />

Although efforts to identify and evaluate hearing loss in young children<br />

have improved, there is still anecdotal evidence to suggest that many<br />

240 White


young children with hearing loss may not be receiving the early intervention<br />

or other services they need in a timely manner that will enable them to<br />

enter preschool and school ready to succeed.<br />

As emphasized by this letter, the importance of providing effective early<br />

intervention services to DHH children is the focus of increased emphasis, and<br />

there is widespread agreement that the families of DHH infants and young<br />

children should have access to appropriate educational services in whatever<br />

mode of communication the family chooses (OSERS, 2006). However, this is<br />

impossible in many parts of the country because of serious shortages and<br />

inadequate geographical distribution of:<br />

• educational programs that use the most up-to-date methods for teaching<br />

language in the mode of communication selected by the family,<br />

• health care and education professionals who are trained and knowledgeable<br />

about current methods for effectively educating children with hearing<br />

loss,<br />

• adequate in-service training programs for individuals already in the field<br />

of deaf education and preservice programs for individuals entering the<br />

field and<br />

• information, public awareness and family support programs for helping<br />

families make informed decisions about the educational options for<br />

DHH children.<br />

The shortages noted above have occurred in part because of the success of<br />

universal newborn hearing screening. The fact that the average age of identification<br />

had gone from 30 months to three months during the past ten years<br />

means that there are many more DHH infants and toddlers to be served.<br />

Twenty years ago, most of these children were not identified until they were<br />

too old to qualify for birth-to-three programs. As shown in Figure 2, federally<br />

funded Part C programs that serve children ages birth to three have historically<br />

served between 1.2 and 2.3 percent of this population. Newborn hearing<br />

screening programs are adding another three children per thousand, which<br />

equates to a 17% increase in the average number of infants and toddlers<br />

served during the last 15 years. Such an increase is difficult for a program to<br />

accommodate when it is already seriously underfunded (Limb, McManus, &<br />

Fox, 2006).<br />

Another factor contributing to these shortages is that in most parts of the<br />

country, early intervention systems for DHH children were developed at a<br />

time when the majority of DHH children were not identified until two to five<br />

years of age, and digital hearing aids and cochlear implants were not available;<br />

thus, the nature of services provided was very different. Very few Part<br />

C early intervention providers have the specialized training necessary to<br />

provide state-of-the-art services to DHH infants and toddlers (Marge &<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 241


Figure 2. Percentage of infants and toddlers served in Part C programs.<br />

Marge, 2005). For example, prior to universal newborn hearing screening<br />

programs, the vast majority of DHH children identified before age 3 were<br />

those with severe or profound bilateral hearing losses. Today, many infants<br />

and toddlers with mild and moderate or even unilateral hearing losses are<br />

being identified before age 3 months. These children require very different<br />

types of services than those needed by DHH children with bilateral severe/<br />

profound losses.<br />

The initiation of universal newborn hearing screening in the early 1990s<br />

started a revolution in our ability to identify DHH children at very young<br />

ages. These revolutionary changes provide great opportunities, but many<br />

DHH children and their families are not yet benefiting to the degree that they<br />

should. How can this situation be improved? Although the solutions will<br />

require many different strategies and resources from many stakeholders, part<br />

of the solution is already in place–but underutilized. In 1997, Congress passed<br />

Public Law (PL) 99-457–the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA),<br />

which provided resources and guidelines for all states to offer early intervention<br />

services to infants and toddlers with disabilities. Better coordination<br />

between this federal program and the activities of state EHDI programs<br />

would do much to strengthen the early intervention components of EHDI<br />

programs. The remainder of this article will show how the Infant and Toddler<br />

portion of IDEA (often referred to as Part C after the section of the law that<br />

outlines how states must provide services to infants and toddlers with disabilities)<br />

can be used to finish the EHDI revolution. By using the framework<br />

and infrastructure defined by Part C of IDEA, professionals serving DHH<br />

children and their families will be more effective in creating successful early<br />

intervention services for thousands of DHH infants and toddlers being identified<br />

by universal newborn hearing screening and diagnostic programs.<br />

242 White


Part C of IDEA<br />

In 1975, Congress recognized that children with disabilities often did not<br />

receive the education and related services needed to make the developmental<br />

progress of which they were capable. As a result, Congress passed landmark<br />

legislation in 1975 (PL 94-142, The Education for All Handicapped Children Act),<br />

requiring states to provide all 5- to 21-year-old children with disabilities with<br />

a free and appropriate education in the least restrictive environment. In 1986,<br />

Congress amended that Act with PL 99-457, which was reauthorized and<br />

amended in 1997 as PL 105-17, and again in 2004 as PL 108-446. In the 1986<br />

amendments, Congress called for the creation of statewide, coordinated, multidisciplinary,<br />

interagency programs for the provision of appropriate early<br />

intervention services for all infants and toddlers with disabilities. This section<br />

of the law, known as Part C (formerly known as Part H), did not actually<br />

require states to provide early intervention services but instead made partial<br />

reimbursement of the costs of services readily available to states that wished<br />

to participate. All states have chosen to participate and have established Part<br />

C programs for children ages birth to three years. Section 631 of the current<br />

law (the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act: Part C–Infants<br />

and Toddlers with Disabilities) is designed to:<br />

• enhance the development of infants and toddlers with disabilities to<br />

minimize the potential for developmental delay,<br />

• reduce the education costs to society by minimizing the need for special<br />

education and related services after infants and toddlers with disabilities<br />

reach school age,<br />

• minimize the likelihood of institutionalization and maximize the potential<br />

for independent living in society,<br />

• enhance the capacity of families to meet the needs of their children and<br />

• enhance the capacity of states and local programs to meet the needs of<br />

underrepresented populations, particularly minority, low income, inner<br />

city and rural populations.<br />

States choosing to participate in the Part C program (which all states have<br />

done) are required to meet a set of minimum requirements as outlined in<br />

Section 635 of the statute (NECTAC, 2006). Some of these requirements govern<br />

the system’s infrastructure and apply in about the same way for all<br />

children with disabilities (e.g., designating a lead agency for providing general<br />

administration and supervision, developing policies for contracting for<br />

services, providing procedures for timely reimbursement of funds, establishing<br />

procedural safeguards, compiling and reporting data about the early<br />

intervention system and creating a state interagency coordinating council).<br />

The remaining requirements call for the creation of components to be applied<br />

differently depending on the child’s disability. Better understanding of these<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 243


components will contribute to improving the quality of early intervention<br />

services for DHH children. The following components are particularly relevant<br />

for creating a strong EHDI system and will each be discussed in the<br />

following sections: (1) eligibility criteria, (2) comprehensive child-find and<br />

referral systems (3) comprehensive systems of personnel development, (4)<br />

appropriate early intervention services and (5) public awareness programs.<br />

Section 634 of the law notes that:<br />

Eligibility Criteria<br />

in order to be eligible for a grant . . . a state shall provide assurances to the<br />

secretary that the state has adopted a policy that appropriate early intervention<br />

services are available to all infants and toddlers with disabilities in<br />

the state and their families.<br />

The federal regulations accompanying the law (34 CFR Part 303.16) require<br />

states to provide appropriate early intervention services to any infant or<br />

toddler:<br />

who is experiencing developmental delays as measured by appropriate<br />

diagnostic instruments and procedures in one or more of the areas of<br />

cognitive development, physical development, communication development,<br />

and adaptive development; or [who has] a diagnosed physical or<br />

mental condition that has a high probability of resulting in developmental<br />

delay.<br />

Infants and toddlers with permanent hearing loss will almost always exhibit<br />

developmental delays in one or more of the specified developmental<br />

areas if appropriate early intervention services are not provided. However,<br />

existing measures of development are not sensitive enough to measure these<br />

delays until children are at least a year old, which is far too late for early<br />

intervention programs to begin. (Kral & Tillein, 2006; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey,<br />

Coulter, & Mehl, 1998). Thus, if they are to be eligible, most DHH children<br />

will have to qualify for Part C services under the condition that they have “a<br />

diagnosed physical or mental condition that has a high probability of resulting<br />

in developmental delay.” In fact, the federal regulations for this section of<br />

the law use “severe sensory impairments, including hearing and vision” as<br />

one of the examples that would make a child with no measured delays eligible<br />

for Part C services.<br />

How the eligibility criteria are operationalized is left to the discretion of<br />

each state, but the criteria must be described in detail in a plan the state<br />

submits to the federal government. To better understand how states are providing<br />

services to DHH children, the written state plans submitted to the<br />

244 White


federal government were obtained and analyzed (NCHAM, 2006e). As expected,<br />

all of the 55 states and territories indicated that services would be<br />

provided to a child who had a diagnosed physical or mental condition that<br />

has a high probability of resulting in development delay. In describing the<br />

conditions for which their state would provide such services, only 37 of the55<br />

(67%) listed hearing loss, auditory impairment, deafness, or something similar<br />

as one of the specific conditions that would make a child eligible for Part<br />

C services. This suggests that some Part C program officials may not recognize<br />

the negative consequences that occur when DHH children do not begin<br />

receiving early intervention during the first few months of life.<br />

Furthermore, only 7 of the 55 states and territories (13%) provided any kind<br />

of operational definition that could be used to determine if a specific DHH<br />

child would be eligible for services (e.g., any child with bilateral sensorineural<br />

hearing impairment greater than 40 dB qualifies for services). Twelve other<br />

states (22%) provided some type of operational definition for hearing loss in<br />

other documents but did not include it in the official state plan. Detailed<br />

information about the eligibility criteria in each state plan and associated state<br />

documents is available at www.infanthearing.org (NCHAM, 2006e).<br />

The fact that the official state plan for so many Part C programs does not<br />

specifically address the eligibility criteria for DHH children is concerning<br />

because it suggests that these children may not be consistently served in Part<br />

C programs. Even in those plans that include references to DHH children, it<br />

is often unclear whether children with unilateral hearing loss will be served,<br />

what degree of hearing loss is required to make a child eligible for services<br />

and what type of diagnostic testing must be done to qualify a child for<br />

services (the operational definitions in the existing state plans range from<br />

very specific criteria, which indicate degree, type and laterality of hearing<br />

loss, such as is the case in the state of Washington, to vague statements about<br />

parental concern or failure on hearing screening tests).<br />

The current situation would be significantly improved if those responsible<br />

for the state’s EHDI program and other stakeholders (e.g., parents, audiologists,<br />

primary healthcare physicians, etc.) would work with Part C program<br />

officials to define specific well-defined eligibility criteria for DHH children to<br />

be enrolled in the Part C programs. Examples of states that have done a<br />

relatively good job of this include Connecticut, Louisiana, New Hampshire<br />

and Washington (although some of these eligibility criteria are included in<br />

other documents from the state and not in the state plan submitted to the U.S.<br />

Department of Education). Being clear about which DHH children are eligible<br />

for Part C services would be relatively easy and would yield valuable benefits.<br />

Comprehensive Child-Find and Referral System<br />

Section 635(a)(5) notes that each state’s Part C program must include “a<br />

comprehensive child-find system . . . for making referrals to service providers<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 245


that includes timelines and provides for participation by primary referral<br />

sources.” The federal regulations for this part of the law found at 34 CFR Part<br />

303.321 note the following:<br />

(b) Procedures. The child find system must include the policies and procedures<br />

that the State will follow to ensure that<br />

(1) All infants and toddlers in the State who are eligible for services<br />

under this part are identified, located, and evaluated; and<br />

(2) An effective method is developed and implemented to determine<br />

which children are receiving needed early intervention services.<br />

(c) Coordination.<br />

(1) The lead agency . . . shall ensure that the child find system under<br />

this part is coordinated with all other major efforts to locate and<br />

identify children conducted by other State agencies responsible for<br />

administering the various education, health, and social service programs<br />

relevant to this part . . . .<br />

(2) The lead agency . . . shall take steps to ensure that—<br />

(i) There will not be unnecessary duplication of effort by the various<br />

agencies involved in the State’s child find system under this<br />

part; and<br />

(ii) The State will make use of the resources available through each<br />

public agency in the State to implement the child find system in<br />

an effective manner.<br />

(d) Referral procedures.<br />

(1) The child find system must include procedures for use by primary<br />

referral sources for referring a child to the appropriate public<br />

agency . . .<br />

(2) The procedures required in paragraph (b)(1) of this section must—<br />

(i) Provide for an effective method of making referrals by primary<br />

referral sources;<br />

(ii) Ensure that referrals are made no more than two working days<br />

after a child has been identified; and<br />

(e) Timelines for public agencies to act on referrals.<br />

(1) Once the public agency receives a referral, it shall appoint a service<br />

coordinator as soon as possible.<br />

(2) Within 45 days after it receives a referral, the public agency shall—<br />

(i) Complete the evaluation and assessment activities in Sec.<br />

303.322; and<br />

(ii) Hold an IFSP meeting, in accordance with Sec. 303.342.<br />

IDEA clearly specifies that state agencies responsible for administering<br />

various education, health and social service programs must work together to<br />

identify children who would benefit from and need services under IDEA.<br />

246 White


Figure 3. Per child funding from Part C of IDEA.<br />

This point is also emphasized by the Year 2000 Position Statement from the<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing (JCIH), which notes that:<br />

In accordance with the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA),<br />

referral to a public agency must take place within 2 working days after the<br />

infant has been identified as needing evaluation. Care can be facilitated by<br />

involving a public agency, as the role of this agency is to appoint a service<br />

coordinator, identify an audiologist to complete the audiologic evaluation,<br />

and identify other qualified personnel to determine the child’s level of<br />

functioning.<br />

Unfortunately, many state EHDI programs are not well coordinated with Part<br />

C programs. In a survey of state EHDI coordinators conducted by NCHAM<br />

(2006a), only 57% of state EHDI coordinators indicated that they had “good<br />

or excellent coordination and cooperation” with their state Part C program.<br />

Of state EHDI coordinators, 34% did not know or said there was not someone<br />

on the Part C Interagency Coordinating Council with expertise and experience<br />

in serving infants and toddlers with hearing loss and 16% of the state<br />

EHDI programs indicated they did not report children identified with hearing<br />

loss to the state’s Part C programs. Only 40% of the state EHDI programs<br />

reported that children enrolled in the state Part C programs for reasons other<br />

than permanent hearing loss were regularly checked for hearing status.<br />

Infants who do not pass the newborn hearing screening are clearly at risk<br />

for permanent hearing loss and should be included in the Part C child-find<br />

system. Unfortunately, given the current funding limitations, it is unlikely<br />

that Part C programs will assume responsibility for another substantial population.<br />

As shown in Figure 3, per child funding for Part C of IDEA has been<br />

declining since 1999. In terms of real dollars from initial program funding in<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 247


1991, there is only about $1,000 available per child to cover all of the Part C<br />

services, including child-find activities.<br />

Given the number of children currently served, the expectation that Part C<br />

will be responsible for conducting diagnostic evaluations of all of infants<br />

referred from newborn hearing screening programs is unrealistic. This means<br />

that Part C would be responsible for approximately 33% more children in<br />

their child-find activities, which would be very difficult given the resources<br />

Part C has available. Nonetheless, it is imperative that state EHDI programs<br />

and Part C programs find ways to collaborate more effectively on child-find<br />

activities. At the very least, better information sharing between programs and<br />

more systematic evaluation of hearing loss of children already enrolled in<br />

Part C programs should be established. Given the requirements in the IDEA<br />

regulations for completing evaluations and developing an individualized<br />

family service program for children identified with disabilities within 45 days<br />

of referral, Part C and EHDI programs must work together to eliminate<br />

waiting periods for diagnostic evaluations so that services can be provided in<br />

a timely manner.<br />

Comprehensive System of Personnel Development<br />

IDEA requires state early intervention systems to operate “a comprehensive<br />

system of personnel development [that promotes] the preparation of<br />

early intervention providers who are fully and appropriately qualified to<br />

provide early intervention services (34 CFR 303.168).” As the age of identification<br />

for DHH children has decreased and cochlear implants and digital<br />

hearing aids have become more widely available, the nature of early intervention<br />

services for DHH children has changed. Approximately 95% of DHH<br />

children are born to hearing parents (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004). Given<br />

recent evidence about the ability of DHH children who received early cochlear<br />

implants and high-quality early intervention to achieve similar levels<br />

as their hearing peers (Cheng et al., 2006; Geers, 2004), it is not surprising that<br />

increasing numbers of parents are choosing auditory/oral programs, which<br />

focus on teaching DHH children to listen and speak, rather than sign language-based<br />

programs, which, historically is how most DHH children were<br />

educated. For example, Brown (2006) compared the choices made by parents<br />

of DHH children in North Carolina in 1995 and again in 2005. North Carolina<br />

is one of the few states in the country that has a well-developed early intervention<br />

program for DHH children that includes a full range of auditory/oral<br />

and sign language-based options. In 1995, 40% of families chose auditory/<br />

oral options compared to 60% who chose sign language-based options. In<br />

2005, 85% of families chose auditory/oral options compared to 15% who<br />

chose sign language-based options. Such a dramatic change over a ten-year<br />

period has important implications for how early intervention program providers<br />

are trained.<br />

248 White


Figure 4. Primary emphasis of personnel preparation programs for teachers of the<br />

deaf or hard of hearing. Although many programs describe themselves as<br />

providing “comprehensive” services, most have a primary emphasis on a specific<br />

approach, as indicated by the curriculum offerings, the placement of graduates, the<br />

type of practicum available, etc. Classification of programs on this map considered<br />

those factors in conjunction with annual self-report survey data from the 2004 and<br />

2005 issues of the American Annals of the Deaf.<br />

Currently there are about 70 university-based programs for preparing<br />

teachers of the deaf or other professionals to work with DHH children. Based<br />

on information contained in the 2004 and 2005 issues of the American Annals<br />

of the Deaf, combined with information about faculty publications, curriculum,<br />

the placement of graduates and program Web sites, these programs<br />

were classified as to whether the primary emphasis was on sign languagebased<br />

or auditory/oral-based options. As shown in Figure 4, the vast majority<br />

of these programs focus primarily on sign language-based options. In fact, of<br />

all graduates from DHH teacher-education programs in 2004, only 8% were<br />

from programs that focused primarily on auditory/oral options. This is similar<br />

to the distribution of programs funded by the Office of Special Education<br />

and Rehabilitative Services (OSERS) for teacher preparation of professionals<br />

working with DHH children. Of the 19 OSERS-funded programs for teachers<br />

of DHH children, only three have an auditory/oral emphasis.<br />

Thus, Part C programs are in a difficult position. The vast majority of<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 249


parents, when they are given an option, choose auditory/oral-based programs.<br />

In contrast, the vast majority of teachers graduating from universitybased<br />

deaf education programs are trained primarily in sign language-based<br />

options. Adjusting the mix of options in a way that provides families with the<br />

choices they would like to have will be challenging and, yet, is essential to<br />

meet the requirements of IDEA.<br />

Appropriate Early Intervention Services<br />

Section 635 of PL 108-446 requires that DHH children be provided with<br />

“appropriate early intervention services based on scientifically based research.”<br />

According to the JCIH (2000), almost all infants and children with<br />

permanent hearing loss need some form of assistive listening device (either<br />

hearing aids or a cochlear implant) for early intervention services to be effective.<br />

If the family decides to have an assistive listening device for the child,<br />

selection and fitting should be provided as soon as possible. Because DHH<br />

children are more likely than adults to have asymmetric losses and configurations<br />

of hearing loss that are more varied than adults, DHH infants and<br />

children often require different features and capabilities in their hearing aids<br />

than those typically used by adults. Children’s hearing aids must provide a<br />

signal that makes all speech sounds audible and comfortable and ensure<br />

intensities are limited to safe levels (Pittman & Stelmachowicz, 2003; Stelmachowicz<br />

et al., 2000; Pittman, Stelmachowicz, Lewis, & Hoover, 2003; American<br />

Academy of Audiology, 2003). Recently developed digital hearing aids<br />

with features such as wide dynamic range compression, automatic feedback<br />

cancellation and multiple channels can achieve these goals in contrast to<br />

analog hearing aids, which are often unable to accommodate these demands.<br />

If DHH children are not provided with assistive listening devices during<br />

the first few months of life, there are serious negative consequences. Without<br />

auditory input and consistent early intervention programs that focus on<br />

teaching language, DHH children almost always lag behind their peers in<br />

language, cognitive and social-emotional development (JCIH, 2000). Several<br />

studies have shown that by the time they are eight years old, deaf children are<br />

already 1 1 ⁄2 years behind their hearing peers in reading comprehension scores<br />

(Schildroth & Karchmer, 1986; Gallaudet Research Institute, 1996) and half of<br />

all deaf children graduate from high school with a fourth grade reading level<br />

or less (Gallaudet Research Institute, 1996). Such deficits are not limited to<br />

children with bilateral hearing loss. Children with unilateral hearing loss are<br />

ten times as likely to be held back at least one grade, compared with children<br />

who have normal hearing (Oyler, Oyler, & Matkin, 1998). Furthermore, studies<br />

comparing children with unilateral hearing loss to matched groups of<br />

children with normal hearing find that children with unilateral hearing loss<br />

lag behind their hearing peers by one-half to two-thirds of a standard deviation<br />

by the time they reach the third or fourth grade (Blair, Peterson, &<br />

250 White


Viehweg, 1985; Brookhouser, Worthington, & Kelly, 1991; Culbertson & Gilbert,<br />

1986).<br />

When DHH children are not identified early and provided with appropriate<br />

access to auditory input and early intervention programs, there are also<br />

significant costs to society in terms of direct medical costs, special education<br />

expenditures and lost productivity. According to Chambers, Shkolnik, &<br />

Perez (2003), DHH children required more than twice the annual per student<br />

educational expenditure as children without disabilities ($15,992 versus<br />

$6,556) in the year 2000. Another study (Schroeder et al., 2006) found that the<br />

average economic costs per year for children with bilateral moderate, severe<br />

or profound hearing loss were nearly four times those for hearing children<br />

($26,207 versus $7,823). Many of the negative outcomes DHH children have<br />

historically experienced can be minimized or avoided completely if they are<br />

provided with appropriate amplification and early intervention that focuses<br />

on teaching language (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998; Moeller, 2000; Kennedy et al.,<br />

2006).<br />

In spite of the consensus among professionals about the importance of<br />

fitting hearing aids as early as possible, data from various sources suggests<br />

that it is quite difficult for many DHH children to obtain hearing aids. For<br />

example, most private health insurance plans explicitly exclude coverage for<br />

hearing aids (Nemes, 2004). A recent survey of hearing aid users found that<br />

only 16% of children had any coverage of hearing aids through their private<br />

health insurance plan (Rhoades & Powell, 2002, No. 35). Fox, McManus, &<br />

Reichman (2002) reported that only 7% of the most commonly sold private<br />

health insurance plans offered hearing aid coverage.<br />

More than half of all children in the United States are covered by Medicaid,<br />

which generally does provide coverage for hearing aids. Unfortunately, reimbursement<br />

rates are so low that most children do not obtain the type of<br />

hearing aids they need. McManus et al. (2004) found in a 15-state region that<br />

average Medicaid fees for hearing aids and other auditory audiology services<br />

were only 38% of those paid by private health insurers. Furthermore, in many<br />

states, “medical necessity” guidelines require that Medicaid provide the<br />

“least costly alternative.” Consequently, analog aids, rather than digital aids,<br />

are often provided to children despite clear evidence that digital aids would<br />

be more beneficial (AAA, 2003).<br />

At the present time, Part C programs seldom pay for hearing aids, It would<br />

seem however, that Part C should be a source of funding to provide appropriate<br />

hearing aid technology to DHH children when other sources are unavailable.<br />

According to IDEA, funds from Part C cannot be used to pay for<br />

services that would otherwise have been paid for from another public or<br />

private source (34 CFR 303.520). However, because both Medicaid and private<br />

insurance plans are unlikely to cover the type of hearing aids needed for<br />

DHH children to make optimal developmental progress, Part C should be<br />

available to fill the gap.<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 251


Historically, the federal Department of Education ruled that education<br />

agencies are not required to provide a personal device that a student would<br />

require, whether in school or not (and this would apply to hearing aids).<br />

However, in 1990, Congress added the definitions of assistive technology<br />

devices and services to the Education of the Handicapped Act by incorporating<br />

the definitions of assistive technology devices included in the Technology<br />

Related Assistance for Individuals with Disabilities Act of 1988 (PL 101-476). Those<br />

amendments make it clear that assistive technology must be provided as a<br />

part of a free and appropriate public education whenever it is necessary for<br />

the child to benefit from educational services. The same requirements apply<br />

to early intervention services under Part C. Assistive technology device is<br />

defined in that legislation as follows:<br />

The term “assistive technology device” means any item, piece of equipment,<br />

or product system, whether acquired commercially off the shelf,<br />

modified, or customized, that is used to increase, maintain, or improve<br />

functional capabilities of individuals with disabilities.<br />

In its passage of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act<br />

on December 3, 2004, Congress continued to emphasize the important role<br />

played by assistive technology devices and services in the education of students<br />

with disabilities. In the preamble to that statute, it was noted that:<br />

almost 30 years of research and experience has demonstrated that the education<br />

of children with disabilities can be made more effective by . . . supporting<br />

the development and use of technology, including assistive<br />

technology devices and services, to maximize accessibility for children with<br />

disabilities.<br />

Thus, it seems clear that Congress intended for hearing aids to be covered by<br />

Part C programs to the extent that they are not covered by other sources and<br />

to the degree that they are necessary to assist in meeting the developmental<br />

needs of DHH children. The fact that Part C programs have been hesitant to<br />

pay for hearing aids could be a function of the limited amount of funding<br />

available to these programs. Nonetheless, it seems clear that early provision<br />

of appropriate hearing aids will lead to better outcomes for students and in<br />

the long term will actually save money. Thus, state EHDI programs should<br />

continue to work with Part C programs to ensure that hearing aids are available<br />

through Part C when they are not available from other sources.<br />

Public Awareness<br />

A major obstacle in providing effective early intervention services to DHH<br />

children has been making various stakeholders aware of the progress that has<br />

252 White


Table 1. Estimates From Primary Health Care Providers Regarding When Children<br />

Can Be Fit with Hearing Aids<br />

Type of Physician<br />

Age at Which Hearing Aids Can Be Fit<br />

Less than<br />

1 month<br />

2-3<br />

mos<br />

4-6<br />

mos<br />

7-11<br />

mos<br />

15+<br />

mos<br />

Pediatrician (n = 1145) 36.3% 16.9% 29.0% 2.1% 15.6%<br />

Family Physician (n = 531) 38.2% 15.6% 23.2% 0.9% 22.0%<br />

Neonatologist (n = 52) 28.8% 21.2% 32.7% 0.0% 17.3%<br />

Ear, Nose & Throat Specialist (n = 58) 27.6% 15.5% 20.7% 1.7% 34.5%<br />

been made in the last 15 years. Many of these changes affect the way early<br />

intervention services should be provided. Unless people are aware of these<br />

changes, they will continue to make decisions based on old data, thus preventing<br />

DHH children and families from accessing the most effective services.<br />

One particularly important group who need to be educated about the<br />

progress that has been made are primary healthcare providers. The American<br />

Academy of Pediatrics has worked hard to include pediatricians in the early<br />

intervention process for all children with disabilities (AAP, 1999a, 2001). One<br />

area in particular in which the AAP has worked to educate healthcare providers<br />

is the recent advances in early identification of hearing loss and how<br />

that should impact the advice they give to parents about early intervention<br />

services for their DHH child (AAP 1999b, 2006).<br />

There is still much work to be done in this area. In a recent study reported<br />

by Moeller, White, & Shisler (2006) in which responses to a survey were<br />

collected from almost 2,000 primary care physicians from across the United<br />

States, it was found that even though pediatricians and other primary care<br />

providers recognized the benefits of early detection and intervention for<br />

DHH children, many of them were not current in their understanding of how<br />

early intervention services can best be provided to this population. For example,<br />

in response to an open-ended question in which they were asked,<br />

“What is your best estimate of the earliest age at which a child can begin<br />

wearing hearing aids,” 41.6% said this could not occur until children were six<br />

months of age or older. As shown in Table 1, the results were not much<br />

different for pediatricians, family physicians, neonatologists and ENTs. Respondents<br />

were also asked to list any specialists to whom they would “routinely<br />

refer the family of a child with a confirmed permanent hearing loss.”<br />

According to the JCIH (2000), all such children should be referred for an<br />

ophthalmological and genetic evaluation. Yet, less than 1% of the respondents<br />

indicated they would refer the child to an ophthalmologist, and only 9%<br />

indicated they would refer the child to a geneticist. Almost half of the respondents<br />

gave incorrect responses to the question asking which children<br />

would be candidates for cochlear implant surgery (indicating that children<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 253


with unilateral profound losses, bilateral mild to moderate losses, and unilateral<br />

mild to moderate losses would be candidates for a cochlear implant).<br />

These gaps in knowledge are all the more concerning given that 89% of the<br />

respondents said that they were very confident or somewhat confident in<br />

explaining the newborn hearing screening process to parents who had questions<br />

about their infant’s results.<br />

The fact that primary healthcare providers are unaware of many of the<br />

recent improvements in early intervention services for this population is not<br />

surprising given how rapidly changes have happened and the relatively<br />

small percentage of DHH children in the population. Nonetheless, effective<br />

early intervention services depend, in large part, on families obtaining upto-date,<br />

accurate information, and primary healthcare providers are a primary<br />

and trusted source of information for many families (Arnold et al.,<br />

2006). Therefore, an important part of strengthening the early intervention<br />

system will be to ensure that healthcare providers (including nurses, nurse<br />

practitioners and physician assistants) are educated about the changes that<br />

have occurred during the last 15 years.<br />

Conclusions<br />

The importance of identifying DHH children as soon as possible and providing<br />

them with appropriate early intervention services (including medical,<br />

audiological and educational services) has been recognized for many years. In<br />

the last 15 years, technological advances and the widespread implementation<br />

of universal newborn hearing screening programs have stimulated progress<br />

at an accelerated rate. Unfortunately, for many DHH infants and young children,<br />

improvements in the early intervention process have lagged behind<br />

other components of EHDI.<br />

Completing the EHDI revolution will require systematic and sustained<br />

attention to providing comprehensive, up-to-date early intervention services<br />

to children identified through newborn hearing screening programs. Part Cof<br />

IDEA provides a framework for these changes to occur. However, in the past<br />

EHDI programs and Part C programs have not worked together as comprehensively<br />

as they should. The framework for such cooperative work is present,<br />

but more attention needs to be given to ensuring that all DHH children<br />

are eligible for Part C services and that EHDI screening and diagnostic programs<br />

are better coordinated with Part C child-find procedures. Additionally,<br />

both preservice and inservice personnel preparation activities must become<br />

responsive to changes in parent preferences for the type of intervention they<br />

want for their child. Hearing aids and other assistive listening devices must<br />

be more accessible to all DHH children and, finally, various stakeholders<br />

must become educated about the progress and changes that have occurred in<br />

the last 15 years.<br />

254 White


Acknowledgments<br />

Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by the Maternal and<br />

Child Health Bureau under Grant Number 6 H61 MC 00006.<br />

References<br />

American Academy of Audiology. (2003). Pediatric Amplification Protocol.<br />

Reston, VA: American Academy of Audiology.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (1999a). The pediatrician’s role in<br />

development and implementation of an Individual Education Plan (IEP)<br />

and/or an Individual Family Service Plan (IFSP). Pediatrics, 104, 124-127.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (1999b). Newborn and infant hearing<br />

loss: Detection and intervention. Pediatrics, 103(2), 527-530.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (2001). Role of the pediatrician in<br />

family-centered early intervention services. Pediatrics, 107, 1155-1157.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics. (2006). The Medical Home Policy Statement.<br />

Pediatrics 110(1). Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://www<br />

.aap.org/policy/s060016.html.<br />

Arnold, C.L., Davis, T.C., Humiston, S.G., Bocchini, J.A., Bass, P.F., Bocchini,<br />

A., Kennen, E.M., White, K., & Forsman, I. (2006). Infant hearing screening:<br />

Stakeholder recommendations for parent-centered communication. Pediatrics,<br />

117(5), S341-S349.<br />

Blair, J., Peterson, M., & Viehweg, S. (1985). The effects of mild hearing loss<br />

on academic performance among young school age children. The Volta<br />

Review, 87, 87-94.<br />

Brookhouser, P.E., Worthington, D.W., & Kelly, W.J. (1991). Unilateral hearing<br />

loss in children. Laryngoscope, 101, 1264-1272.<br />

Brown C. (2006). Early intervention: Strategies for public and private sector collaboration.<br />

Paper presented at the 2006 Convention of the Alexander Graham<br />

Bell Association for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing, Pittsburgh PA.<br />

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, DC. (2006a). DSHPSHWA Data<br />

Summary: Reporting Year 2004. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://<br />

www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/ehdi/dips.htm.<br />

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2006b). National EHDI<br />

Goals. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/<br />

ehdi/nationalgoals.htm.<br />

Chambers, J.C., Shkolnik, J., & Perez, M. (2003). Total Expenditures for Students<br />

with Disabilities, 1999-2000: Spending Variation by Disability. Washington,<br />

DC: Center for Special Education Finance, Special Education Expenditure<br />

Project.<br />

Cheng, A.K., Rubin, H.R., Powe, N.R., Mellon, N.K., Francis, H.W., &<br />

Niparko, J.K. (2000). Cost-utility analysis of cochlear implant in children.<br />

Journal of American Medical Association, 284(7), 850-856.<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 255


Culbertson, J., & Gilbert, A.M. (1986). Children with unilateral sensorineural<br />

hearing loss: Cognitive, academic, and social development. Ear and Hearing,<br />

7, 38-42.<br />

Ewing, I.R., & Ewing, A.W.G. (1944). The ascertainment of deafness in infancy<br />

and early childhood. The Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 59, 309-333.<br />

Fox, H.B., McManus, M.A., & Reichman, M.B. (2002). The Strengths and Weaknesses<br />

of Private Health Insurance Coverage for Children with Special Health Care<br />

Needs. Washington, DC: Maternal and Child Health Policy Research Center.<br />

Gallaudet Research Institute (GRI). (1996). Stanford Achievement Test, 9 th Edition,<br />

Form S, Norms Booklet for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students; Washington,<br />

DC: Gallaudet University.<br />

Geers, A.E. (2004). Speech, language, and reading skills after early cochlear<br />

implantation. Archives of Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, 130(5), 634-<br />

638.<br />

Harrison, M., Roush, J., & Wallace, J. (2003). Trends in age of identification<br />

and intervention in Infants with hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 24, 89-95.<br />

Healthy People 2010. (2000). Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://www<br />

.healthypeople.gov/About/.<br />

Honeycutt, A., Dunlap, L., Chen, H., al Homsi, G., Grosse, S., & Schendel, D.<br />

(2003). Economic costs associated with mental retardation, cerebral palsy,<br />

hearing loss, and vision impairment–United States. MMWR Weekly, 53(03),<br />

57-59.<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing (2000). Year 2000 Position Statement: Principles<br />

and Guidelines for Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Programs.<br />

Available at: http://www.infanthearing.org/jcih/.<br />

Kennedy, C.R., McCann, D.C., Campbell, M.J., Law, C.M., Mullee, M., Petrou,<br />

S., Watkin, P., Worsfold, S., Yuen, H.M., & Stevenson, J. (2006). Language<br />

ability after early detection of permanent childhood hearing impairment.<br />

New England Journal of Medicine, 354(20), 2131-41.<br />

Kral, A., & Tillein, J. (2006). Brain plasticity under cochlear implant stimulation.<br />

Advances in Otorhinolaryngology, 64, 89-108.<br />

Limb, S.J., McManus, M.A., & Fox, H.B. (2006). Assuring financial access to<br />

hearing aids for infants and young children. A report of the Children’s Audiology<br />

Financing Work Group. Washington DC: Maternal and Child Health Policy<br />

Research Center.<br />

Marge, D.K., & Marge, M. (2005). Beyond newborn hearing screening: Meeting the<br />

educational and health care needs of infants and young children with hearing loss<br />

in America. Report of the National Consensus Conference on Effective Educational<br />

and Health Care Interventions for Infants and Young Children<br />

with Hearing Loss, September 10-12, 2004. Syracuse, New York: Department<br />

of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, SUNY Upstate Medical University.<br />

McManus, M., Levtov, R., White, K., Forsman, I., Foust, T., & Thompson, M.<br />

256 White


(2004). The adequacy of Medicaid reimbursement of hearing services for children.<br />

Washington, DC: Maternal and Child Health Policy Research Center.<br />

Mitchell, R.E., & Karchmer, M.A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent:<br />

Parental hearing status of Deaf and Hard of Hearing students in the United<br />

States. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138-163.<br />

Moeller, M.P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children<br />

who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>(3), E43.<br />

Moeller, M.P., White, K.R., & Shisler, L. (2006). Primary care physicians’<br />

knowledge, attitudes, and practices related to newborn hearing screening.<br />

Pediatrics, 118(4), 1357-1370.<br />

Mohr, P.E., Feldman, J.J., Dunbar, J.L., McConkey-Robbins, A., Niparko, J.K.,<br />

Rittenhouse, R.K., & Skinner, M.W. (2000). The societal costs of sever to<br />

profound hearing loss in the United States. International Journal of Technology<br />

Assessment in Health Care, 16(4), 1120-1135.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

(2006a). Universal newborn hearing screening: Summary statistics of<br />

UNHS in the United States. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://<br />

www.infanthearing.org/status/unhsstate.html.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

(2006b). Legislative activities. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://<br />

www.infanthearing.org/legislative/summary/index.html.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

(2006c). Cochlear Implants for infants and young children. Retrieved December<br />

15, 2006, from http://www.infanthearing.org/earlyintervention/<br />

CIs per year.pdf.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

(2006d). Joint OSERS and ODH letter to Part C Coordinators. Retrieved<br />

December 15, 2006, from http://www.infanthearing.org/earlyintervention/<br />

Hager-Giannini letter.pdf.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management (NCHAM).<br />

(2006e). Criteria for infants and toddlers with hearing loss to be eligible for<br />

early intervention services under IDEA. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from<br />

http://www.infanthearing.org/earlyintervention/eligibility.pdf.<br />

National Early Childhood Technical Assistance Center (NECTAC). (2006).<br />

Early Intervention program for infants and toddlers with disabilities (Part<br />

C of IDEA) Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://www.nectac.org/<br />

partc/partc.asp.<br />

Nemes, J. (2004). Third-party coverage of hearing aids: A report on current<br />

initiatives. The Hearing Journal, 57(7), 19-24.<br />

Northern, J.L., & Downs, M.P. (1991). Hearing in Children, Fourth Edition.<br />

Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.<br />

O’Neill, C., O’Donoghue, G.M., Archbold, S.M., & Normand, C. (2000). A cost<br />

utility analysis of pediatric cochlear implantation. Laryngoscope, 110, 156-<br />

160.<br />

Finishing the EHDI Revolution 257


Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services (OSERS). Opening<br />

doors: Technology and communication options for children with hearing<br />

loss. Retrieved December 15, 2006, from http://www.ed.gov/about/<br />

offices/list/osers/products/opening_doors/opening_doors.pdf.<br />

Oyler, R., Oyler, A., & Matkin, N. (1998). Unilateral hearing loss: demographics<br />

and educational impact. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,<br />

19, 201-209.<br />

Pittman, A.L., & Stelmachowicz, P.G. (2003). Hearing loss in children and<br />

adults: audiometric configuration, asymmetry, and progression. Ear and<br />

Hearing, 24, 198-205.<br />

Pittman, A.L., Stelmachowicz, P.G., Lewis, D.E., & Hoover, B.M. (2003). Spectral<br />

characteristics of speech at the ear: implications for amplification in<br />

children. Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research, 46, 649-657.<br />

Schildroth, A.N., & Karchmer, M.A. (1986). Deaf Children in America. San<br />

Diego, California: College Hill Press.<br />

Schroeder, L., Petrou, S., Kennedy, C., McCann, D., Law, C., Watkin, P.M.,<br />

Worsfold, S., & Yuen, H.M. (2006). The economic costs of congenital bilateral<br />

permanent childhood hearing impairment. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1101-1112.<br />

Stelmachowicz, P.G., Hoover, B.M., Lewis, D.E., Kortekaas, R.W., & Pittman,<br />

A.L. (2000). The relation between stimulus context, speech audibility, and<br />

perception for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired children. Journal of<br />

Speech and Language Hearing Research, 43, 902-914.<br />

Toward Equality (1988). A Report to the Congress of the United States: Toward<br />

equality—Commission on Education of the Deaf. Washington, DC: U.S. Government<br />

Printing Office.<br />

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1990). Healthy People 2000:<br />

National health promotion and disease prevention objectives. Washington,<br />

DC: Public Health Service.<br />

U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. (1996). Screening for hearing impairment.<br />

In U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Guide to Clinical Preventive<br />

Services, 2nd ed. (pp. 393-405), Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.<br />

White, K.R. (2004). Early hearing detection and intervention programs: Opportunities<br />

for genetic services. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 130A,<br />

29-36.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A.L., Coulter, D.K., & Mehl, A.L. (1998). Language<br />

of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics,<br />

102, 1161-1171.<br />

258 White


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 259-274<br />

Statewide Collaboration in<br />

the Delivery of EHDI Services<br />

Joni Alberg, Ph.D, Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT ® , and<br />

Jackson Roush, Ph.D., CCC-A<br />

Introduction<br />

In 1999, North Carolina enacted legislation requiring the screening of all<br />

newborn infants for hearing loss as part of a comprehensive statewide effort<br />

to achieve early hearing detection and intervention (EHDI). This law (NCAC<br />

43F.1201) requires physiologic screening prior to hospital discharge unless<br />

declined by the parents or precluded by medical complications. Results are<br />

then reported to the North Carolina State Laboratory for Public Health. Since<br />

full implementation in August 2000, more than 700,000 babies have been born<br />

in the state’s 90 birthing hospitals, and most have been screened successfully.<br />

In the first few years following this legislation, much of the time, energy and<br />

financial resources of the EHDI system were devoted to the screening itself:<br />

establishing programs, equipping nurseries and training staff. But with each<br />

passing year, there has been growing awareness of the need to focus on the<br />

services that must be provided after the screening: comprehensive audiologic<br />

assessment, otologic examination, and for infants found to have permanent<br />

hearing loss, family support and referral for early intervention services.<br />

This has resulted in opportunities as well as challenges and, not surprisingly,<br />

there has been mixed success. Most importantly, those involved with<br />

the statewide EHDI system have come to recognize that early identification<br />

and intervention require the coordination and collaboration of many programs,<br />

agencies, and institutions (JCIH 2000). The following program description<br />

examines three components of North Carolina’s statewide EHDI<br />

program that have undergone substantial growth and development as a result<br />

of newborn screening: (1) the clinical and educational programs of the<br />

Joni Alberg, Ph.D., is the executive director of BEGINNINGS for Parents of Children Who Are<br />

Deaf or Hard of Hearing, Inc. Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT ® , is the director<br />

of the Resource Support Program in the Office of Education Services for the North Carolina<br />

Department of Health and Human Services and an adjunct professor at the University of<br />

North Carolina-Chapel Hill. Jackson Roush, Ph.D., CCC-A, is professor and director of the<br />

Division of Speech and Hearing Sciences, University of North Carolina School of Medicine at<br />

Chapel Hill, and the section head for audiology at the University of North Carolina Center for<br />

the Study of Development and Learning.<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 259


University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC), which are based in the<br />

UNC School of Medicine and UNC Hospitals (UNCH); (2) services to families<br />

provided by BEGINNINGS for Parents of Children Who are Deaf or Hard of<br />

Hearing; and (3) services provided through our statewide network of early<br />

intervention programs, administered by the Office of Education Services<br />

(OES) in the North Carolina Department of Health and Human Services<br />

(DHHS), as well as strategies for building a comprehensive program. In addition<br />

to describing successful outcomes, this report examines aspects of each<br />

component that have been frustrating or problematic and the ongoing efforts<br />

to improve quality of care and access to services for all families in North<br />

Carolina.<br />

The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill<br />

The hospitals at UNC provide inpatient and outpatient clinical programs<br />

for the UNC School of Medicine, serving patients from each of North Carolina’s<br />

100 counties. Every year thousands of infants and their families are<br />

served in UNC’s Women’s and Children’s Hospitals. UNC otolaryngologists<br />

deliver specialized ear, nose and throat services to more than 30,000 patients<br />

annually. The pediatric audiology program at UNCH has grown substantially<br />

since the onset of newborn hearing screening, from two to three comprehensive<br />

infant diagnostic assessments per month in 1999 to an average of 30 per<br />

month in 2007 (P. Roush, personal communication, January 2007). The Carolina<br />

Children’s Communicative Disorders Program (CCCDP), affiliated with<br />

the Department of Otolaryngology, includes a pediatric cochlear implant<br />

program; the Center for Acquisition of Spoken Language Through Listening<br />

Enrichment (CASTLE), which offers an auditory/oral preschool for children<br />

with hearing loss; and assessment-intervention services provided by speechlanguage<br />

pathologists and teachers, some of whom are Certified Auditory-<br />

Verbal Therapists. Through CASTLE, the CCCDP offers internships, a<br />

summer institute in Auditory-Verbal therapy, an annual conference, and<br />

mentoring for professionals in the development of spoken language through<br />

audition. The close working relationship between the UNCH pediatric audiology<br />

program and the CCCDP, which together follow more than 1,300 children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing, makes it possible for the audiologists,<br />

otolaryngologists, speech-language pathologists and intervention specialists<br />

to participate in weekly team meetings to monitor children’s progress and<br />

make recommendations regarding implant candidacy (C. Brown & P. Roush,<br />

personal communication, January 2007).<br />

In addition to providing clinical services, both UNCH and CCCDP make<br />

important contributions to graduate education. Students in the UNC Doctor<br />

of Audiology (Au.D.) program, which is based in the Division of Speech and<br />

Hearing Sciences, participate in a full range of pediatric services. Audiology<br />

trainees participate in newborn and neonatal intensive care unit hearing<br />

260 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


screening, physiologic assessment of infants referred from screening using<br />

frequency-specific auditory brainstem response (ABR) and other physiologic<br />

measures, pediatric hearing aid selection and fitting and cochlear implant<br />

evaluation and mapping. In their first year, students observe and provide<br />

assistance to the pediatric audiologists. Second- and third-year students are<br />

enrolled in clinical externships involving assessment and management of<br />

hearing loss in infants and young children. Both the UNCH Pediatric Audiology<br />

program and the CCCDP provide externship placements for fourthyear<br />

students.<br />

Accomplishments and Challenges<br />

Growth in the number of families served has necessitated an increase in the<br />

number of pediatric audiologists available to provide clinical services. The<br />

large and growing caseloads have made it possible for audiologists at UNCH<br />

to acquire expertise evaluating and managing newly identified infants. Physiologic<br />

assessment including ABR, auditory steady-state responses (ASSR),<br />

otoacoustic emissions and acoustic immittance measures are performed routinely<br />

with infants in natural sleep, in conscious sedation and, when necessary,<br />

under general anesthesia.<br />

For infants referred to UNCH without health complications, the average<br />

age of diagnosis is approximately 3 months. When families concur with the<br />

recommendation for hearing aid use, the initial fitting is typically completed<br />

within two weeks. Infants are followed every four to six weeks for monitoring<br />

of earmolds, hearing aid use, and middle ear status.<br />

Once infants reach a developmental level of 6-7 months, physiologic tests<br />

are supplemented with behavioral measures using visual reinforcement audiometry<br />

(VRA). VRA can be used successfully with most infants to obtain<br />

threshold measures for each ear from 250 to 4000 Hz by 9 months of age.<br />

Initial hearing aid selection and fitting is accomplished with real-ear-tocoupler<br />

difference (RECD) measurements, a “real-ear” method appropriate<br />

for young children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Hearing aids are fitted<br />

using the Desired Sensation Level prescriptive formula that targets gain and<br />

output to ensure that sound is both audible and comfortable (Bagatto et al.,<br />

2005). If families concur, fitting can move forward with minimal delay because<br />

of an efficient approval process that allows earmold impressions to be<br />

taken and hearing aids ordered at the time of diagnosis. Once hearing threshold<br />

levels are established for both physiologic and behavioral measures, children<br />

are followed every three months until approximately 3 years of age and<br />

then every four to six months thereafter.<br />

Aided speech perception measures are completed as soon as children have<br />

the requisite language skills. Initially, these measures are accomplished<br />

through the identification of closed sets of objects and pictures. As children’s<br />

speech and language abilities increase, appropriate open set tests are used.<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 261


When children begin to walk and the distance between speaker and listener<br />

increases, personal FM systems are introduced to families for home use. The<br />

North Carolina Assistive Technologies program funds one pair of digital<br />

hearing aids, a personal FM system, and hearing aid repairs and earmolds<br />

until children reach age 3. Unfortunately, when a child turns 3 they are no<br />

longer eligible to receive funds from this program. Families must find other<br />

resources with which to purchase technology for their children. In addition to<br />

self-pay, options include the state Medicaid program and a state-funded program<br />

for families who do not qualify for Medicaid but have limited financial<br />

resources.<br />

In addition to providing early, accurate audiologic management, the UNC<br />

program provides services that support families and are sensitive to cultural<br />

differences. Although providing collaborative, family friendly services can be<br />

challenging in a large medical center, the audiologists are committed to providing<br />

family-centered care that transcends the practice setting. Children’s<br />

hearing care needs are different from those of adults and, as a result, accommodations<br />

have been made to allow for the time and staffing necessary to<br />

provide appropriate pediatric management.<br />

When hearing loss is identified in an infant, a comprehensive otologic<br />

work-up is required. At UNC and other centers where comprehensive care of<br />

infants with hearing loss is provided, the components of the otologic work-up<br />

include examination of the ears, a thorough case history and relevant laboratory<br />

studies. At UNCH, imaging studies, such as MRI are completed on all<br />

children with permanent hearing loss. Information obtained from radiologic<br />

studies permits identification of congenital ear abnormalities, such as enlarged<br />

vestibular aqueducts and cochlear nerve deficiency (small or absent<br />

eighth nerves), allowing audiologists and otolaryngologists to make informed<br />

decisions regarding appropriate habilitative options. Ophthalmology and genetics<br />

consultations also are offered to families. Children who receive a diagnosis<br />

of neural conduction disorder (auditory neuropathy/dyssynchrony)<br />

are referred for comprehensive neurological evaluation.<br />

For some children acoustic amplification is insufficient even with early<br />

identification and intervention (Davidson, 2006). The team approach at UNC<br />

allows for regular monitoring and review of children’s progress to determine<br />

those who are candidates for cochlear implantation and when implantation<br />

should occur. At weekly team meetings otolaryngologists, audiologists, and<br />

speech-language pathologists discuss children referred for cochlear implantation.<br />

Before scheduling a surgery date, speech-language pathologists communicate<br />

with the child’s parents, local therapists, and teachers to ensure that<br />

adequate services will be in place after implantation.<br />

Despite a well-integrated and collaborative team approach, many challenges<br />

remain. Appropriate management of young infants is time consuming<br />

and expensive. In addition to specialized instrumentation and clinical expertise,<br />

institutional support is needed to manage the financial burden associated<br />

262 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


with providing comprehensive care when reimbursement from third-party<br />

payers or the family is insufficient to cover all costs associated with the<br />

program. In North Carolina, as in many states, only a few centers have the<br />

expertise and institutional support needed to provide specialized care to<br />

infants. Consequently, some families must travel a considerable distance to<br />

obtain services, and inevitably, some are unable to access services because of<br />

logistical challenges or expenses associated with transportation and followup.<br />

The difficulties have been especially acute for low-income rural families<br />

and those whose first language is not English. At this time, tracking and<br />

surveillance remain difficult for these and other disadvantaged families.<br />

Many families also experience financial pressures. Although state funding<br />

for hearing aids, FM systems, repairs and earmolds adequately covers children<br />

under the age of 3, when they turn three, financial assistance for hearing<br />

aids is limited to families who meet eligibility criteria. Given the high cost of<br />

hearing instruments, even families with relatively good incomes may be burdened<br />

by the cost of replacement hearing aids.<br />

In addition to challenges that exist at a systems level certain areas of audiology<br />

management remain difficult. For example, when infants show no<br />

response to both ABR and ASSR, a period of uncertainty exists until residual<br />

hearing status can be supplemented by behavioral assessment. A more mundane<br />

but crucial issue—acoustic feedback—remains a challenge when the<br />

degree of hearing loss requires high output levels from the hearing aid.<br />

The decision of when to offer cochlear implantation also can be difficult.<br />

When little or no progress with acoustic amplification is seen, the decision<br />

generally is uncomplicated, but when progress is measurable but less than<br />

anticipated, the reasons why may be difficult to determine. Further, as the<br />

age of implantation has declined, speech perception measures appropriate for<br />

use with older children cannot be used, making it necessary to rely on threshold<br />

measures and progress reports from early intervention specialists. It is<br />

hoped that promising new applications of cortical-evoked potentials and<br />

evaluation of infant speech perception using behavioral measures will eventually<br />

lead to evidenced-based clinical procedures helpful in addressing this<br />

important issue.<br />

Other populations for whom clinical management is controversial or uncertain<br />

are infants, toddlers, and young children with unilateral hearing loss<br />

and those with neural conduction disorders (auditory neuropathy/dyssynchrony).<br />

Clinical research in these areas is under way (Rance, 2005), but<br />

evidenced-based protocols for clinical management are lacking at this time.<br />

Important challenges remain, but it has been gratifying to see growth,<br />

development and coordination of pediatric assessment procedures, intervention<br />

services, and educational experiences for graduate students. But even the<br />

most comprehensive audiology, otolaryngology and other hospital- or clinicbased<br />

services are not sufficient to provide the support families need when<br />

hearing loss is first identified. Much of the success in North Carolina in recent<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 263


years is attributable to partnerships among EHDI-related agencies within the<br />

state. BEGINNINGS has been a vital link between the hospital and early<br />

intervention services.<br />

BEGINNINGS for Parents of Children Who Are Deaf or<br />

Hard of Hearing<br />

For more than 20 years, BEGINNINGS has provided emotional support,<br />

unbiased information about all communication and education options and<br />

technical assistance to parents of children who are deaf or hard of hearing<br />

ages birth to 22. It also supports parents who are themselves deaf or hard of<br />

hearing and whose children may or may not have hearing loss. Further,<br />

information and assistance is available to professionals who work with these<br />

families.<br />

The belief that parents should be the primary decision makers for their<br />

child is a guiding principle for BEGINNINGS. When a child in North Carolina<br />

is diagnosed with a hearing loss, the parents are informed about BEGIN-<br />

NINGS and invited to talk with a parent educator. Audiologists are the<br />

primary source of family referral, though anyone can make a referral. In<br />

addition, parents of any child with hearing loss can obtain services until the<br />

child reaches age of 22. 1<br />

A parent educator is assigned to each family referred to BEGINNINGS.<br />

Identifying information about the family and child (e.g., address, phone numbers,<br />

age of identification, type of hearing loss) is entered into a database. This<br />

information is confidential, and specific procedures have been established for<br />

maintaining each family’s privacy. Parent educators obtain written permission<br />

from parents to share information with other professionals. Before contacting<br />

the family, the parent educator first contacts the referral source to gain<br />

additional information regarding the family and their needs. The parent educator<br />

then meets with the family at a convenient time and place, usually in the<br />

family’s home. Initially, home visits are to ensure that parents understand<br />

their child’s hearing loss and provide information regarding financial assistance,<br />

language development, communication options and early intervention<br />

services. Parent educators emphasize the importance of keeping all appointments<br />

with the audiologist and encourage parents to enroll their child as soon<br />

as possible in North Carolina’s early intervention program, which is described<br />

later.<br />

Although parent educators encourage parents to arrange their own appointments<br />

with various service agencies, BEGINNINGS provides assistance<br />

if needed. Parent educators work closely with the North Carolina Early<br />

1<br />

Services to adults in North Carolina are available through the NC Division of Services to the<br />

Deaf and Hard of Hearing.<br />

264 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


Intervention Program for Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing (EIP).<br />

BEGINNINGS routinely provides information about families being served<br />

whose children are under 3 and services provided by BEGINNINGS. This<br />

“service progress log” is exchanged biweekly with the EIP and the regional<br />

Child Health Audiology Consultants from the North Carolina Early Hearing<br />

Detection and Intervention program. This communication tool has been invaluable<br />

in ensuring families are served in a timely manner as they move<br />

from diagnosis to intervention. Additionally, face-to-face meetings with representatives<br />

from the EIP, BEGINNINGS and the Division of Public Health<br />

occur quarterly to discuss strategies for improving communication, follow-up<br />

with families who have been challenging to find or serve and other issues that<br />

arise. Once services are in place, frequent contact with BEGINNINGS is not<br />

needed. Parent educators check in with families periodically and at specific<br />

educational milestones, such as transition from early intervention to preschool<br />

(see the article by Wilson, this issue, page 419).<br />

Important changes occur once children turn age 3 in North Carolina and in<br />

most states. Under Part C of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act<br />

(IDEA), services from birth to 3 are set forth in an Individualized Family<br />

Service Plan (IFSP) and, in North Carolina, are administered by the Division<br />

of Public Health. After age 3, responsibility for educational services is transferred<br />

to the state Department of Public Instruction, necessitating the creation<br />

of an Individualized Education Program (IEP). Parent educators play an important<br />

role in assisting families in developing goals for their child’s IEP.<br />

They explain educational rights and accommodations, assist parents in articulating<br />

their goals for their child, and, if requested, accompany the parents<br />

to an IEP meeting to ensure that the plan is written in a way that will facilitate<br />

success in the educational setting. One of BEGINNINGS’ most important<br />

goals is to help parents develop the knowledge and skills they need to become<br />

advocates for their child and family, and parent educators focus on<br />

helping parents gain the confidence they need to accomplish this goal, even<br />

if it means encouraging them to function more independently.<br />

Parent educators also provide school-based support, such as classroom<br />

observations and teacher in-service education, to ensure children have the<br />

supports and modifications necessary for full access to classroom instruction.<br />

They advise the school about the need for assistive technology and explain<br />

the school’s responsibility to provide it. They accompany parents to audiologic<br />

appointments to support them as they learn about their child’s hearing<br />

loss and to help them understand the information provided. This service has<br />

been especially valuable to parents whose first language is not English. BE-<br />

GINNINGS has two Hispanic outreach/parent educators who are native<br />

Spanish speakers. They do not provide interpreter/translation services;<br />

rather, they support parents in their native language. Support is specific to<br />

each family but may involve helping parents understand their child’s audiogram<br />

and becoming aware of what is involved in managing their child’s<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 265


hearing loss, including audiologic management and early education services,<br />

the types of services that are available to their child and family and assistance<br />

in accessing those services. In 2006, families whose primary language in the<br />

home is Spanish comprised approximately 14 percent of 287 families served.<br />

Presentations and workshops for teachers, school nurses, related professionals<br />

and students enrolled in North Carolina’s colleges and universities are<br />

part of the BEGINNINGS mission as well. Outreach programs recently were<br />

expanded to provide practicum opportunities for students in the UNC speech<br />

and hearing programs. These experiences allow students to interact with<br />

families in their natural environment.<br />

All of the services BEGINNINGS provides to parents and professionals in<br />

North Carolina are at no charge to families. The majority of funding comes<br />

through a state appropriation from the North Carolina General Assembly.<br />

This appropriation is directed to the OES and administered through a contract<br />

with BEGINNINGS. The contract specifies services to be provided and<br />

includes accountability requirements. Additional funding comes from other<br />

contracts, grants and private donations. Most of the support BEGINNINGS<br />

provides is to parents of young, newly identified children. Referrals to serve<br />

these families have more than doubled since the implementation of statewide<br />

newborn hearing screening.<br />

Because the service delivery system for early intervention (IDEA Part C)<br />

and the public schools (IDEA Part B) is administered by two separate state<br />

agencies and the nature of early intervention and public school education is<br />

different, parent educators have found it important to stay in close contact<br />

with parents during and after the transition. When children turn 2 1 ⁄2 years<br />

old, the parent educator contacts the family to discuss the transition and<br />

changes they will encounter when their child enters the local education<br />

agency (LEA). To facilitate this process, parent educators contact the early<br />

intervention program and work closely with the service provider as they<br />

prepare families for this important transition. For example, the parent educator<br />

frequently will accompany the early intervention teacher to the family’s<br />

home to discuss the upcoming transition. Together, the EI teacher and parent<br />

educator will help the parents set goals for their child’s IEP. Close communication<br />

among BEGINNINGS, the early intervention staff and LEA representatives<br />

is key to ensuring successful transitions.<br />

Accomplishments and Challenges<br />

The services BEGINNINGS provides to parents of children with hearing<br />

loss are tailored to each family’s specific needs. Parents have reported gratitude<br />

for the support provided by their parent educator, and surveys administered<br />

anonymously to parents in 2006 overwhelmingly confirmed their<br />

satisfaction with BEGINNINGS services. Of the 49 surveys returned, 94% of<br />

parents agreed or strongly agreed with all nine satisfaction statements.<br />

266 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


Having served families for more than 20 years, BEGINNINGS has become<br />

an integral part of the EHDI programs in North Carolina. Based on growth in<br />

the number of families requesting services each year, awareness of and need<br />

for the services clearly is recognized. In fiscal year 2005, 294 families new to<br />

BEGINNINGS and 203 families returning for additional services were served.<br />

In fiscal year 2006, the number of families new to BEGINNINGS remained<br />

about the same (281) while the number of returning families increased to 250.<br />

Based on the first 7 months of fiscal year 2007, new families to BEGINNIGNS<br />

are expected again to remain about the same (296 projected) while the number<br />

of returning families is expected to increase significantly (354 projected).<br />

Support for 60 IEP meetings was provided, and staff delivered numerous<br />

in-services, workshops and presentations to a variety of professionals. A new<br />

challenge BEGINNINGS is facing is the influx of families who speak Spanish/<br />

Mexican dialects indigenous to a specific village or location in their country<br />

of origin. These families do not speak or read English or Spanish, and typically<br />

the only option for our Hispanic outreach/parent educators is to relyon<br />

a family member who knows the dialect. This is seen as an ongoing challenge<br />

as BEGINNINGS serves a growing number of families from diverse backgrounds.<br />

The BEGINNINGS model not only has grown in North Carolina, but also<br />

is being used in other states. In 2004, BEGINNINGS formed a partnership<br />

with the National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management<br />

(NCHAM) in Utah as part of a federally funded outreach project to provide<br />

technical assistance to state EHDI programs as they develop family support<br />

services. Four states are working to develop programs similar to BEGIN-<br />

NINGS, and 10 others have requested support as they explore options for<br />

family support. Within the NCHAM collaboration, BEGINNINGS staff provide<br />

training, demonstrating parent educator techniques for providing services<br />

and unbiased information to families in a supportive and culturally<br />

sensitive manner.<br />

North Carolina Early Intervention Services<br />

In North Carolina, Part C services under IDEA are administered by the<br />

Division of Public Health in the Department of Health and Human Services.<br />

A statewide system of 18 regional Children’s Developmental Services Agencies<br />

(CDSAs) provides a portal of entry into the state’s Infant-Toddler Program.<br />

At the CDSA, children receive evaluation services, eligibility<br />

determination and a service coordinator for those deemed eligible. The child’s<br />

service coordinator is responsible for developing the IFSP, which defines<br />

family and child goals and the necessary services to achieve these goals, and<br />

ensuring it is reviewed and modified as needed every six months.<br />

Once enrolled through the CDSA and assigned a service coordinator,<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 267


infants and toddlers who are deaf or hard of hearing are eligible to receive<br />

services through the OES early intervention component. This component, the<br />

Early Intervention Program for Children Who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing,<br />

provides direct, specialized services to children and their families. At the state<br />

level the program is administered by the OES superintendent. Three regional<br />

directors are in place to manage the program at the local level. Each director<br />

supervises a team of licensed teachers of the deaf who, in turn, provide direct<br />

services to children and families. Each region also employs a licensed speechlanguage<br />

pathologist with expertise in auditory/oral and Auditory-Verbal<br />

approaches, Cued Speech, Total Communication, American Sign Language<br />

and the diagnosis and treatment of a variety of coexisting speech and language<br />

disorders.<br />

In the first few weeks after a child’s enrollment in the early intervention<br />

program, service providers administer a variety of assessments to collect<br />

baseline performance measures. Teachers and therapists obtain information<br />

regarding communication and language, social-emotional, cognitive, vocabulary<br />

and motor development. These assessments are conducted in an effort to<br />

supplement rather than supplant or duplicate the information gathered by<br />

the CDSA. In addition to the baseline evaluation, all early intervention service<br />

providers follow a protocol requiring administration of a variety of assessments<br />

every six months. Test results are interpreted, and written reports are<br />

provided to parents.<br />

In accordance with Part C of IDEA, services are implemented in natural<br />

environments, and for most children this is the home environment. Services<br />

may be delivered in day care settings. Most children receive at least one home<br />

visit per week from either the teacher of the deaf or the program’s speechlanguage<br />

pathologist, and efforts are made to schedule visits at times that are<br />

most convenient for the family.<br />

A primary goal of the program is to involve parents and other family<br />

members to the greatest extent possible. Administrators, teachers and related<br />

personnel recognize the importance of a family-centered approach, one that<br />

focuses on working primarily with regular caregivers rather than with children.<br />

Although the federal law mandates that early intervention services<br />

must occur in natural environments, this provision is not exclusively about<br />

where services are provided. Walsh, Rous and Lutzer (2000) emphasized that<br />

the natural environment mandate is not being met when providers work with<br />

children without including parents and caregivers in the session. In the North<br />

Carolina program an effort is being made to actively engage family members.<br />

During parent participation sessions, teachers and therapists address questions<br />

and concerns from the family, review the child’s progress since the<br />

previous session, identify goals, model strategies and techniques and guide<br />

parents as they practice effective strategies. Teachers and therapists also assist<br />

caregivers in identifying appropriate ways to carry over goals during the<br />

course of daily routines. Through this process parents understand their role<br />

268 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


as primary teacher and the importance of daily interactions in achieving<br />

desired outcomes.<br />

Accomplishments and Challenges<br />

Every effort is made to enroll children and begin intervention immediately<br />

after referral. It is noteworthy that at no time in the referral process is a child<br />

placed on a waiting list. An additional strength of the program is the requirement<br />

that all professionals be licensed in deaf education and/or speechlanguage<br />

pathology. Thus, services are implemented by specialists rather<br />

than by generalists. Furthermore, providers must complete requirements to<br />

obtain the state’s Infant-Toddler Credential. To earn this credential, professionals<br />

must demonstrate knowledge, skills and expertise with the birth-tothree<br />

population, including parent-centered practices. The Infant-Toddler<br />

Credential, in combination with licensure in deaf education and/or speechlanguage<br />

pathology, is important to ensuring that children and families receiving<br />

early intervention services have access to appropriately trained<br />

personnel.<br />

Implementation of baseline and ongoing assessment practices provides<br />

practitioners with essential information to guide intervention, monitor progress<br />

and make changes as necessary to meet the family’s goals. Although this<br />

aspect of the program can be time consuming, routine assessment every six<br />

months for the birth-to-three population can be regarded as a program<br />

strength and a practice necessary for appropriate intervention. Because parental<br />

involvement is deemed an essential component for children with special<br />

needs to make appropriate progress, the program endeavors to empower<br />

parents as key decision makers and teachers and to actively involve them in<br />

each intervention session.<br />

With input from teachers, therapists and parents, administrators continually<br />

review program practices to determine components that are working<br />

well and to identify concerns and challenges. It is well documented that a<br />

substantial number of children who are deaf or hard of hearing also present<br />

with one or more disabilities (Picard, 2004). The Gallaudet Research Institute<br />

(2003) reported that as many as 40 percent of children who are deaf or hard<br />

of hearing have additional challenges ranging from severe cognitive impairments<br />

to mild learning differences that are not apparent until school age.<br />

Children enrolled in the early intervention program present with a variety of<br />

syndromes, developmental disabilities and medical conditions. Given the<br />

range of additional challenges, it is not always possible to identify and assign<br />

early intervention personnel who already have knowledge and expertise in<br />

the particular areas needed. In those situations early intervention personnel<br />

often find themselves in the position of working with children and families<br />

with special needs outside the scope of their existing training and expertise,<br />

at least initially. When faced with serving children with additional challenges,<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 269


early intervention program personnel collaborate with other professionals<br />

both within the early intervention program and externally to identify resources<br />

to assist them in meeting the needs of these children with multiple<br />

disabilities.<br />

North Carolina, like many other states, has experienced rapidly growing<br />

Hispanic and Latino populations (Grieco, 2003), but as reported by Johnson<br />

(2004) only 5 to 10 percent of teachers of the deaf in the United States are<br />

members of an ethnic minority. Like many states, the current workforce of<br />

early intervention providers in North Carolina is primarily white, female,<br />

hearing and English speaking. Clearly, teachers from diverse backgrounds,<br />

those who speak more than one language and those trained in practices to<br />

meet the needs of children and families of various backgrounds are needed to<br />

enhance the effectiveness of family-centered intervention. During fiscal year<br />

2005-2006, 189 children were enrolled in the early intervention program, 37<br />

percent by age 6 months (J. Dunn, M. Hice, & L. Kendall, personal communication,<br />

September 2006). Although far short of our goal to enroll all children<br />

by age 6 months, the complexities associated with multiple disabilities, non-<br />

English-speaking families, loss to follow-up and other challenges often result<br />

in later identification and diagnosis, which in turn result in later enrollment<br />

in early intervention. North Carolina is well on its way to implementing<br />

Hearing Link, a statewide electronic tracking and data system. The pilot<br />

phase of Hearing Link began in February 2005 with full implementation<br />

expected by the end of calendar year 2008. This system has the potential to<br />

move the state forward in achieving the goal of enrollment in early intervention<br />

by age 6 months.<br />

In addition to ongoing efforts aimed at earlier enrollment for intervention<br />

services, future plans include review of assessment practices, instruments,<br />

frequency of administration and how results are reported and used to make<br />

decisions about service delivery. Additionally, the population of children and<br />

families served reflects students from varied racial and ethnic backgrounds,<br />

especially Latino/Hispanic cultures. Training and supporting early intervention<br />

providers working with children who have multiple disabilities, increasing<br />

parent participation and enhancing collaboration with professionals from<br />

other disciplines are key to meeting the needs of all children.<br />

How Are We Doing? Developing a Statewide<br />

Evaluation Plan<br />

Although significant challenges remain, North Carolina’s EHDI programs<br />

continue to expand and improve. Even so, it is difficult to determine the level<br />

of success without a comprehensive evaluation plan that includes measurement<br />

of child and family outcomes. In 2005, OES and BEGINNINGS, in<br />

cooperation with UNC, embarked on an effort to address this need. Recently<br />

a one-year pilot project addressed several important questions related to<br />

270 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


statewide EHDI goals. What is the age of screening, diagnosis and intervention?<br />

If goals are not being achieved, what are the barriers to timely identification<br />

and intervention? Are children leaving early intervention with<br />

expressive and receptive language skills that are similar to those of their<br />

hearing peers? What are the children’s language outcomes as they move<br />

through preschool and at ages 4, 5 and 6? These questions are being addressed<br />

through data collection efforts involving staff from several EHDI<br />

components. Answering them will take time, but progress is under way.<br />

The OES–BEGINNINGS database now includes age of identification, age of<br />

hearing instrument fitting, age of early intervention, type and intensity of<br />

intervention and level of parent involvement. Also included is a description<br />

of the type and degree of hearing loss and, when known, the etiology. The<br />

database includes other conditions or disabilities that co-occur with hearing<br />

loss as well as specific language outcomes at age 3 based on the Preschool<br />

Language Scale-Fourth Edition (PLS-4; Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 2002).<br />

The outcomes from the OES and BEGINNINGS longitudinal study should<br />

eventually provide the information needed to better evaluate the success of<br />

the state EHDI programs and provide guidance for program review and<br />

improvement. Preliminary findings indicate that nearly half the children are<br />

scoring within the average range for total language as measured by the PLS-4.<br />

Efforts are now underway to combine language measures with developmental<br />

assessments in order to better understand and interpret each child’s progress.<br />

Other Statewide Collaboration<br />

Child Health Audiology and Speech-Language Consultants<br />

North Carolina is fortunate to have the services of regional audiology and<br />

speech-language consultants employed by the Division of Public Health. Six<br />

child-health audiology consultants cover the entire state to provide a variety<br />

of services, including tracking infants referred from newborn screening,<br />

monitoring hospital compliance with screening protocols, conducting inservice<br />

training for screening personnel and providing technical assistance<br />

regarding newborn screening. Seven child-health speech-language consultants<br />

work with the audiology team to assist with tracking, surveillance and<br />

follow-up. They also assist families with applications for assistive technology<br />

and enrollment in children’s special health services.<br />

Continuing Education for Parents and Professionals<br />

Each year EHDI programs in North Carolina collaborate to plan and deliver<br />

a statewide conference on topics relevant to early identification, assessment<br />

and intervention. On even years UNC hosts a two-day pediatric<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 271


audiology symposium that brings together approximately 100 pediatric audiologists<br />

and professionals from related disciplines. Topics include physiologic<br />

and behavioral assessment of infants, pediatric hearing aid fitting and<br />

audiologic management of special populations, such as infants with neural<br />

conduction disorders or unilateral hearing loss. During the meeting, EHDI<br />

program coordinators provide updates and information regarding statewide<br />

programs. In alternate years, a three-day statewide EHDI conference provides<br />

content to a broader audience, including parents, early intervention professionals,<br />

administrators, physicians, audiologists and speech-language pathologists.<br />

Funding from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and<br />

corporate support have made it possible to deliver these conferences at an<br />

affordable cost to attendees.<br />

Parent-to-Parent Support<br />

Hearing Impaired Toddlers and Children Have Unlimited Potential<br />

(HITCH-UP), a volunteer organization founded by parents and now active in<br />

two metropolitan areas, provides monthly support groups for families. Operated<br />

entirely by parents without public support, HITCH-UP plays an important<br />

role in providing peer support, exchange of information and adviceto<br />

professional organizations for improving and expanding services to families<br />

with children who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

Conclusion<br />

Statewide collaboration in the delivery of EHDI services is not easily<br />

achieved. Early identification, comprehensive audiologic and otologic evaluation,<br />

appropriate selection and fitting of hearing instruments and enrollment<br />

in family-centered early intervention services require a dedicated team of<br />

professionals working within a service delivery model that is accessible, well<br />

coordinated and appropriately funded. Table 1 describes some of the essential<br />

goals of a comprehensive EHDI system. Achieving them is an ongoing challenge<br />

that is never finished, but working toward full inclusion of families<br />

within a collaborative statewide model is our long-term objective.<br />

North Carolina has a longstanding commitment to families whose children<br />

are deaf or hard of hearing, but important challenges remain: a growing<br />

number of children with health conditions or disabilities that exist in addition<br />

to hearing loss, a growing number of non-English-speaking families and a<br />

shortage of early intervention providers with expertise and communication<br />

skills needed to serve these special populations. For children with neural<br />

conduction disorders or unilateral hearing loss, audiologists must make clinical<br />

decisions based on limited evidenced-based research. Decisions regarding<br />

whether and when to provide cochlear implantation also can be challenging,<br />

especially when the child is making some progress with amplification. For all<br />

272 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


Table 1. Goals for a Comprehensive Statewide EHDI System<br />

• A comprehensive statewide hearing screening program using protocols and<br />

procedures appropriate for the well baby or neonatal intensive care unit<br />

nurseries.<br />

• Audiologic and otologic expertise necessary to provide timely and accurate<br />

diagnostic evaluations for infants referred from newborn screening.<br />

• Application of hearing technologies (hearing aids, cochlear implants)<br />

appropriate for the child and consistent with family preferences.<br />

• Capacity to provide advocacy, emotional support and information to parents<br />

and children regardless of communication option or place of residence.<br />

• Accurate and culturally sensitive information to families regarding all<br />

communication and education options given as soon as possible after diagnosis.<br />

• Well-qualified professionals with the knowledge, skill and specialization needed<br />

to serve young children with hearing loss, including those with coexisting<br />

disabilities and special needs.<br />

• A statewide plan for in-service and continuing education.<br />

• A tracking system that follows children and families through each component<br />

of the EHDI system.<br />

• A surveillance system for children at risk for late-onset or progressive hearing<br />

loss.<br />

• Regular meetings of key agencies to review and evaluate the EHDI system.<br />

• Developmental assessments every six months for children ages birth to 3.<br />

• Written reports to parents providing the results of formal and informal<br />

assessments.<br />

• Knowledge and cultural competence needed to serve non-English-speaking or<br />

bilingual families.<br />

• Administration of early intervention services by a central agency or, when a<br />

single agency is not possible, careful coordination of each early intervention<br />

component to enable uniformity and consistency in the application of policies,<br />

procedures and clinical practices.<br />

• Clearly articulated procedures to ensure successful transition from early<br />

intervention (Part C) services to school-based (Part B) services.<br />

• Comprehensive statewide evaluation of child and family outcomes to determine<br />

the strengths and needs of the EHDI system.<br />

• Participation and inclusion of families to the full extent desired in each<br />

component and at each level of the EHDI system.<br />

children, the transition from early intervention at age 3 to the local school<br />

system is an important one, and for many families it is a time of stress and<br />

uncertainty. Despite these challenges, progress will continue. We are confident<br />

that advances in technology combined with parent-professional collaboration<br />

will allow each child to achieve the best possible developmental<br />

outcomes.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

We thank the following individuals for their contributions to the North<br />

Carolina EHDI programs and to the content of this program description:<br />

Statewide EHDI Collaboration 273


Cyndie Bennett, superintendent of the Office of Education Services; Carolyn<br />

Brown, director of the Carolina Children’s Communicative Disorders Clinic<br />

and CASTLE Preschool; and Patricia Roush, director of pediatric audiology,<br />

UNC Hospitals.<br />

References<br />

Bagatto, M., Moodie, S., Scollie, S., Seewald, R., Moodie, S., Pumford, J., etal.<br />

(2005). Clinical protocols for hearing instrument fitting in the Desired Sensation<br />

Level method. Trends in Amplification, 9(4), 199-226.<br />

Davidson, L. (2006). Effects of stimulus level on the speech perception abilities<br />

of children using cochlear implants or digital hearing aids. Ear and<br />

Hearing, 27(5), 493-507.<br />

Gallaudet Research Institute. (2003). Regional and national summary report of<br />

data from the 2002-2003 annual survey of deaf and hard of hearing children and<br />

youth. Washington, DC: GRI, Gallaudet University.<br />

Grieco, E. (2003). Foreign born Hispanics in the United States. Retrieved September<br />

2006 from http://www.migrationinformation.org.<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing (2000). Year 2000 position statement: Principles<br />

and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs.<br />

Johnson, H. (2004). U.S. deaf education teacher preparation programs: A look<br />

at the present and a vision for the future. American Annals of the Deaf, 149(2),<br />

75-91.<br />

Picard, M. (2004). Children with permanent hearing loss and associated disabilities:<br />

Revisiting current epidemiological data and causes of deafness.<br />

The Volta Review, 104, 221-236.<br />

Rance, G. (2005). Auditory neuropathy/dys-synchrony and its perceptual<br />

consequences. Trends in Amplification, 9(1), 1-43.<br />

Walsh, S., Rous, B., & Lutzer, C. (2000). The federal IDEA natural environments<br />

provisions: Making it work. In S. Sandall & M. Ostrosky (Eds.),<br />

Young exceptional children: Natural environments and inclusion (pp. 3-15).<br />

Longmont, CO: Sopris West.<br />

Zimmerman, I.L., Steiner, V.G., & Pond, R.E. (2002). Preschool Language Scale-<br />

4th edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.<br />

274 Alberg, Wilson, & Roush


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 275-298<br />

Family Empowerment:<br />

Supporting Language<br />

Development in Young<br />

Children Who Are Deaf or<br />

Hard of Hearing<br />

Jean L. DesJardin, Ph.D.<br />

The current model of early intervention with children who are deaf or hard of<br />

hearing emphasizes parental self-efficacy and involvement. The purpose of this study<br />

was to investigate the relationships between mothers’ self-efficacy beliefs and involvement<br />

and children’s language skills in a group of mothers of children who are deaf or<br />

hard of hearing and wear hearing aids (N = 32). Mothers completed a questionnaire<br />

(Scale of Parental Involvement and Self-Efficacy), and mother-child interactions were<br />

videotaped. Mothers’ self-efficacy beliefs related to developing their children’s speech<br />

and language were positively associated with higher level facilitative language techniques<br />

(recast and open-ended question), and one lower level technique (closed-ended<br />

question). Perceived involvement was also positively related to lower level techniques.<br />

Regression analyses indicated that the same higher level techniques were associated<br />

with children’s language skills. Findings present early intervention implications for<br />

professionals who work with families and children who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

Introduction<br />

Families of young children who are deaf or hard of hearing have many<br />

more opportunities today than in years past to develop a child’s spoken<br />

language skills. In the past, parents may have felt isolated in their search for<br />

resources related to hearing loss, with parent-professional partnerships traditionally<br />

marked by “power over” relationships that are defined by professionals<br />

presuming a higher sense of competence and greater knowledge than<br />

parents (Turnbull, Turbiville, & Turnbull, 2000). The present model of early<br />

intervention, referred to as the empowerment model (Turnbull & Turnbull,<br />

Jean DesJardin, Ph.D., is an advanced research associate in the Children’s Research and<br />

Evaluation (CARE) Center at House Ear Institute in Los Angeles, Calif.<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 275


2001), places an emphasis on parental involvement in an equal partnership<br />

with the professional. In this professional-parent partnership, both work collaboratively<br />

to learn from each other about better ways to support the child’s<br />

language development.<br />

Current recommended practices for children who are deaf or hard of hearing<br />

emphasize the importance of not only parental involvement, but also<br />

parental competence. Both the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement<br />

Act (IDEIA, 2004) and the Division of Early Childhood (DEC) Recommended<br />

Practices in Early Intervention/Early Childhood Special Education (Sandall,<br />

McLean, & Smith, 2000) specify that one desired outcome of early intervention<br />

is for parents to perceive themselves as capable of supporting their<br />

children’s growth and development—parental self-efficacy. Parental beliefs<br />

will, in turn, make an impact on the child’s learning. The role of early intervention<br />

professionals is to support parents as they seek to gain access to the<br />

information they require to become competent and confident in the skills<br />

necessary to support their children’s development (McWilliam & Scott, 2001;<br />

Turnbull et al., 2000). Parents’ beliefs about their skills and involvement in<br />

their children’s early intervention program are critical components for children’s<br />

language learning.<br />

Parental Self-Efficacy<br />

Parental self-efficacy beliefs are defined as one’s sense of knowledge and<br />

abilities to perform or accomplish daily parenting tasks and roles. According<br />

to self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997), parental self-efficacy beliefs should<br />

incorporate (1) the level of specific knowledge pertaining to the behaviors<br />

involved in child-rearing and (2) the degree of confidence in one’s own ability<br />

to carry out the specific parental role. For example, Conrad, Gross, Fogg, and<br />

Ruchala (1992) noted that mothers’ increased knowledge alone did not result<br />

in better interactions with their young children. Increased knowledge and<br />

confidence together, however, resulted in more effective interactions with<br />

their hearing toddlers.<br />

A key component of self-efficacy theory states that parents’ efficacy beliefs<br />

are linked to the goals they have for their children (Bandura, 1989). These<br />

goals may translate into parental practices that support their children’s ability<br />

and persist until the goals are attained (Brody, Flor, & Gibson, 1999; DesJardin,<br />

2004). In other words, a person who is self-efficacious both knows about<br />

and persists in attempting a given task until success is achieved, whereas<br />

self-inefficacious individuals may have the knowledge needed, but may be<br />

unable to persist because of self-doubt. Thus, parents of children who are deaf<br />

or hard of hearing need the knowledge and skills necessary to build their<br />

children’s spoken language abilities, but, to achieve confidence in those skills,<br />

they also need to use those skills daily as they interact with their children.<br />

Indeed, maternal self-efficacy beliefs may influence mother-child interactions.<br />

In a sample of mothers and their hearing infants, Teti and Gelfand<br />

276 DesJardin


(1991) found that maternal self-efficacy mediated the negative relationship<br />

between mothers’ depressive symptoms and mothers’ behaviors reflecting<br />

responsiveness (e.g., warmth and sensitivity) to their infants. Maternal selfefficacy<br />

also remained positively associated with maternal responsiveness,<br />

after controlling for all other factors (e.g., income level, perceptions of infant<br />

difficulty and social-marital support). These findings suggest a link between<br />

mothers’ belief in their own abilities and maternal behavior.<br />

The link between mothers’ self-efficacy and maternal behaviors may need<br />

to be tailored to the specific needs of children who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

As suggested by Bandura (1989), parental perceived knowledge and confidence<br />

of specific skills must be measured for a particular population. Although<br />

there are parenting scales that tap into parents’ self-efficacy for other<br />

populations of children with disabilities (Akey, Marquis, & Ross, 2000; Trivette<br />

& Dunst, 2003), only one has been specifically designed for parents of<br />

young children who are deaf or hard of hearing (Scale of Parental Involvement<br />

and Self-Efficacy [SPISE]; DesJardin, 2003). Using this particular measure,<br />

DesJardin (2004) examined a sample of 24 mothers with preschool<br />

children with cochlear implants (mean age = 3.7 years). Mothers’ perceived<br />

self-efficacy in terms of developing their children’s language at the first timepoint<br />

in data collection (Time 1) related positively to children’s receptive and<br />

expressive language skills one year later (even after controlling for their<br />

child’s language skills at Time 1).<br />

In that same study at Time 2 (DesJardin, 2004; Time 2), with the inclusion<br />

of eight more mother-child dyads (N = 32), mothers’ sense of efficacy also was<br />

related to specific facilitative language techniques employed during motherchild<br />

interactions. Mothers who perceived themselves as more knowledgeable<br />

and competent in developing their children’s language skills provided<br />

their children with higher level language techniques (recast and open-ended<br />

questions). These same techniques were associated with better child language<br />

outcomes. Conversely, mothers who perceived themselves as less knowledgeable<br />

and competent in terms of developing their children’s language<br />

used lower level language techniques (linguistic mapping, directives and<br />

imitations). Although there is evidence to suggest that lower level techniques<br />

support language learning in hearing children with severe expressive language<br />

delays (Girolametto, Weitzam, Wiigs, & Pearce, 1999), these techniques<br />

did not seem to support preschool children with cochlear implants whose<br />

language ages were approximately 2 1/2 years. Consistent with Bandura’s<br />

model, mothers’ self-efficacy beliefs appear related to the developmental language<br />

goals endorsed for their children that may link to the ways in which<br />

parents interact with their children.<br />

Parental Involvement<br />

Parental involvement is another pivotal feature in present-day early intervention<br />

programs. Over the past several years, researchers have found that<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 277


family involvement strongly influences parent and child outcomes for children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing (Calderon, Bargones, & Sidman, 1998;<br />

Calderon, 2000; Moeller, 2000). Parent involvement has been shown to relate<br />

positively to better parental communication exchanges with their children<br />

(Calderon et al., 1998), more advanced language outcomes in kindergartenaged<br />

children (Moeller, 2000) and later educational development (Calderon,<br />

2000; Calderon & Naidu, 2000; Yoshinaga-Itano, 2000). Moreover, Calderon<br />

and Naidu (2000) found mothers’ communication skills, rather than direct<br />

parental involvement in their children’s education programs, related significantly<br />

to their children’s language and early reading skills.<br />

The way in which parents are involved is also important for children’s<br />

language learning. Young children learn new words in the contexts of their<br />

daily experiences and, particularly, through interactions with their caregivers.<br />

According to the social-interactionist theory, the main emphasis is on<br />

caregiver linguistic input (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1997). Variation observed in children’s<br />

language skills may strongly be linked to the quantity (e.g., number of<br />

words) and quality (e.g., facilitative language techniques) of the caregiver’s<br />

linguistic input (Girolameto et al., 1999; Hart & Risley, 1999). For example,<br />

language techniques, parallel talk and recasting have been reported to relate<br />

positively to children’s rapid language development (Fey, Krulik, Loeb, &<br />

Proctor-Williams, 1999; Kaiser & Hancock, 2003).<br />

The Guidelines in Division of Early Childhood (DEC) Recommended Practices in<br />

Early Intervention emphasize parental responsiveness to children’s linguistic<br />

attempts for facilitating optimal language development (Cook, Tessier, Klein,<br />

& Armbruster, 2000; Sandall et al., 2000). Facilitative language techniques,<br />

such as imitation and expansion, may enhance language learning in young<br />

children at the single-word stage of language development (Girolametto et<br />

al., 1999). In contrast, language techniques such as parallel talk, recast and<br />

open-ended questions may elicit better responses from children who are at<br />

the two-to-three-word language level (Fey et al., 1999; Hulit & Howard, 1997;<br />

McNeil & Fowler, 1999). Indeed, language techniques that are “fine-tuned” to<br />

children’s language levels accelerate children’s expressive language development<br />

(Yoder & Warren, 1998).<br />

Hearing mothers of children who are deaf may intuitively adapt their<br />

language techniques to their children’s language level. Koester, Papousek, &<br />

Smith-Gray (2000) noted that “intuitive parenting is a proficient yet often<br />

overlooked communication technique used by parents when interacting with<br />

their deaf toddlers” (p. 56). For example, a mother may use a higher level<br />

technique, such as an open-ended question and, if the child does not respond,<br />

adapt her linguistic input to a lower level technique, such as a closed-ended<br />

question. In fact, hearing mothers of children who are deaf may intuitively<br />

make adaptations to their children’s linguistic abilities and demonstrate response-control<br />

behaviors (e.g., questioning) to compensate for their child’s<br />

deafness (Lederberg & Prezbindowski, 2000).<br />

278 DesJardin


Mothers’ sense of involvement may also influence language techniques<br />

while interacting with their children who are deaf. Findings from a longitudinal<br />

study of 24 mothers of children with cochlear implants (DesJardin, 2004)<br />

suggest that mothers’ perceived involvement in their children’s language<br />

development at Time 1 was related to higher level facilitative language techniques<br />

(parallel talk, recast and open-ended questions) one year later. Even<br />

after controlling for their children’s language ages at Time 1, these same<br />

techniques were also associated with their children’s language outcomes.<br />

Conversely, mothers who felt less involved in their children’s language program<br />

exhibited lower level language techniques (linguistic mapping, imitation<br />

and directive). Such lower level techniques, in turn, related negatively to<br />

their children’s language skills for this population of mothers and preschool<br />

children who are deaf and use cochlear implants. Furthermore, at Time 2, and<br />

with the inclusion of eight more mother-child dyads (N = 32), higher level<br />

language techniques, recast and open-ended questions remained predictor<br />

variables for children’s receptive and expressive language skills, respectively,<br />

even after controlling for children’s length of cochlear implant use for this<br />

group of mothers and their young children who use cochlear implants (Des-<br />

Jardin, 2006).<br />

Although there is theoretical and empirical support for self-efficacy and<br />

involvement in parents of young children and, specifically, children with<br />

cochlear implants, we do not know if the same holds true for mothers of<br />

young children who use hearing aids. The goal of the present study was to<br />

examine the relationships between mothers’ perceived self-efficacy and involvement,<br />

and children’s language outcomes, in a group of mothers and<br />

children who use hearing aids. This study addresses two research questions:<br />

(1) Are perceived maternal self-efficacy and involvement associated with<br />

facilitative language techniques during mother-child interactions? and (2)<br />

What are the maternal predictor variables for children’s language outcomes?<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

The author recruited mothers and children from the Children’s Auditory<br />

Research and Evaluation (CARE) Center, located at House Ear Institute in Los<br />

Angeles. The CARE Center serves children ages birth to 18 years and their<br />

families, providing a range of clinical services that include initial diagnosis of<br />

hearing loss and audiological follow-up services.<br />

Mothers<br />

Thirty-two mothers and children who are deaf or hard of hearing participated<br />

in this study. As shown in Table 1, mothers averaged 36 years of age<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 279


Table 1. Demographic Characteristics for Mothers (N = 32)<br />

(range = 23-53), most had some level of college experience (78.2%) and the<br />

majority was in the upper middle-income level (62.5%) or above, with an<br />

average yearly income level of $50,000. Most of the mothers were Caucasian<br />

(53.1%) and spoke English (96.9%).<br />

Children<br />

Mothers’ Characteristics<br />

Mean; (SD)<br />

Range: 23-53 years<br />

Age 36.3; (6.25)<br />

Education level Elementary: 0; (0.0%)<br />

High School: 7; (21.9%)<br />

Some College: 3; (9.4%)<br />

Bachelor’s Degree: 7; (21.9%)<br />

Postgraduate: 15; (46.9%)<br />

Household income $100,000 = 10 (31.3%)<br />

Ethnicity Caucasian: 17; (53.1%)<br />

Latino: 11; (34.4%)<br />

Asian-American: 2; (6.3%)<br />

Other: 2; (6.3%)<br />

Primary home language English 31 (96.9%)<br />

Spanish 1 (3.1%)<br />

Marital status Married 29 (87.5%)<br />

Single 2 (9.4%)<br />

Divorced 1 (3.1%)<br />

All of the children presented with bilateral sensorineural hearing loss, were<br />

hearing aid users and had been enrolled in a family-centered intervention or<br />

school-age program for at least three months. As noted in their medical<br />

charts, none of the children had an additional disability or developmental<br />

delay. As displayed in Table 2, this is a heterogeneous group of children who<br />

ranged in age from 1.1-6.0 years (M = 3.0 years). As a group, the children were<br />

identified with hearing loss and enrolled in a family-centered early intervention<br />

program at approximately 12 months and received hearing aids at approximately<br />

18 months. At the time of testing, 25 children used an auditory/<br />

oral modality of communication and seven children used auditory/oral with<br />

some sign support. As per the Reynell Developmental Language Scales, children’s<br />

mean receptive and expressive language skills were 29 months and 25<br />

months, respectively.<br />

280 DesJardin


Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Children (N = 32)<br />

Measures<br />

Children’s Characteristics<br />

Mean; (SD)<br />

(Range in months)<br />

Gender Boys: 17; (53.1%)<br />

Girls: 15; (46.9%)<br />

Age at testing 36 months (20.2)<br />

(25-72)<br />

Age at identification 11.7 months (15.9)<br />

(


In the self-efficacy section, mothers rate 10 questions on a Likert-type scale<br />

ranging from “not at all” (1) to “very much” (7). Questions in this section relate<br />

to mothers’ perceptions about their level of influence on their child’s auditory<br />

development. The questions selected were derived from using exploratory<br />

factor analysis. Two subscales emerged and Cronbach’s alpha coefficients<br />

suggested high internal consistency for each subscale: (1) Maternal Self-<br />

Efficacy of Sensory Device Use, the extent to which a mother believes that she<br />

has the knowledge and skills to following through with prescribed sensory<br />

aid maintenance; and (2) Maternal Self-Efficacy of Child’s Speech-Language<br />

Development, the extent to which a mother believes that she has the knowledge<br />

and skills to affect her child’s speech and language development (see<br />

Appendix for subscale items and Crochbach’s [1951] alpha coefficients, ranging<br />

from 0.80-0.87).<br />

The involvement section consists of 11 questions on a Likert-type scale<br />

ranging from “not at all” (1) to “very much” (7). The queries in this section<br />

include items related to a mother’s perception of her involvement in her<br />

child’s sensory device use and family-centered intervention or school-age<br />

program. Using exploratory factor analysis, two subscales emerged: (1) Maternal<br />

Involvement of Sensory Device Use, the extent to which a mother<br />

believes that she is involved in her child’s use of sensory device and listening<br />

development; and (2) Maternal Involvement of Child’s Speech-Language Development,<br />

the extent to which a mother believes that she is involved in her<br />

child’s speech-language development and family-centered intervention program<br />

or school-age program (e.g., therapy, home visits and IFSP/IEP meetings)<br />

(see Appendix for subscale items and Cronbach’s [1951] alpha<br />

coefficients ranging from 0.76-0.83).<br />

The SPISE was developed and pilot tested with a focus group that consisted<br />

of three audiologists, a teacher of the deaf, a speech-language pathologist and<br />

two parents of children with hearing loss (one child with hearing aids and<br />

one child with a cochlear implant). Thus, the SPISE offers secure content<br />

validity. Although the predictive validity of the SPISE is unknown at this<br />

time, future avenues of research will investigate the relationships between<br />

items on the SPISE and performance on a variety of observed tasks during<br />

parent-child interactions.<br />

(2) The Reynell Developmental Language Scales, Third Edition (RDLS III)<br />

(Reynell & Gruber, 1990). The RDLS-III are individually administered tests of<br />

verbal comprehension and expressive language skills for young children. The<br />

RDLS III uses toys (e.g., ball, doll), pictures of objects (e.g., window, flower)<br />

and real objects (e.g., spoon, cup) to elicit responses from the child. The two<br />

scales of the RDLS have been widely used both with hearing children and<br />

children who are deaf (Stallings, Gao, & Svirsky, 2002; Svirsky, Robbins, Kirk,<br />

Pisoni, & Miyamoto, 2000). The RDLS calculates raw scores and standard<br />

scores and their age equivalents based on normative data from children with<br />

normal hearing. The present investigation used auditory-oral raw scores in<br />

282 DesJardin


the data analyses because almost all of the children had standard scores that<br />

were below the floor of 63 or 64. There was, however, sufficient variability in<br />

the raw scores for meaningful statistical analysis.<br />

(3) Mother-Child Interactions. Mothers and children engaged in free play<br />

and storybooks together. During free play, mothers were asked to engage<br />

their children with two Mr. Potato Head (Lerner, 1952) fun activity kits (one<br />

kit designed to construct a female potato and one kit designed to construct a<br />

male potato) for seven minutes. The Mr. Potato Head activity kits came with<br />

various parts to engage the children in fantasy play (e.g., scuba goggles,<br />

dress-up shoes, various hats). Mothers were instructed to “play with your<br />

child as you would normally do at home when you have free time” and to<br />

interact with their children using whatever mode or modes of communication<br />

they use normally at home (auditory/oral or auditory/oral with sign support).<br />

Following free play, mothers were provided two storybooks, What Next,<br />

Baby Bear! by Jill Murphy (1983) and Frog, Where Are You? by Mercer Mayer<br />

(1969). What Next, Baby Bear! is a relatively short, colorfully illustrated book<br />

appropriate for preschoolers with a fantasy narrative about a little bear’s<br />

travel preparations for a trip to the moon. This book has been used in several<br />

other mother-child interaction studies with young hearing children (Weizman<br />

& Snow, 2001). Frog Where Are You? is a relatively short, wordless picture<br />

book and has been used in more than 150 studies studying children acquiring<br />

50 different languages and investigating children’s narrative language development<br />

(Berman & Slobin, 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1997). Mothers were instructed<br />

to “read with your child as you would normally do at home when<br />

you have time,” choosing either book to begin and then continuing with the<br />

other book when they were finished with the first book.<br />

Procedures<br />

Prospective participants received an invitation letter and stamped, selfaddressed<br />

postcard. Those interested in the study completed the postcard<br />

and returned it to the CARE Center to schedule appointments. Mothers who<br />

indicated interest in participating in the study received the SPISE questionnaire<br />

in the mail two weeks before their scheduled appointments and either<br />

brought the completed questionnaire to the appointment or mailed it to the<br />

House Ear Institute in a stamped, self-addressed envelope.<br />

The mother-child interaction sessions were conducted in a comfortable<br />

playroom at the CARE Center. The first seven minutes consisted of free play.<br />

Following free play, the mothers and children interacted with the two books<br />

described above for approximately 5 minutes each. Interactions were videotaped<br />

using a digital camera (Canon Optura 30) hidden behind a one-way<br />

mirror. Following the videotaping session, the children were administered<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 283


the Reynell Developmental Language Scales. All language assessments were<br />

given in a child’s primary mode of communication (auditory/oral or auditory/oral<br />

with sign support). For this study, the analyses used auditory-oral<br />

raw scores.<br />

Transcription and Reliability<br />

Data Preparation<br />

All speech, vocalizations and signs produced by the mother and child were<br />

transcribed in full by the author, using the Codes for the Human Analysis of<br />

Transcripts (CHAT) transcription system. To establish inter-rater reliability of<br />

transcription, a speech-language pathologist from the CARE Center transcribed,<br />

in full, 10% of the randomly selected videotaped data. The calculation<br />

of word-by-word correspondence yielded a high reliability between transcribers,<br />

ranging from 95-98% agreement for mothers and children’s intelligible<br />

verbal utterances and 88-93% reliability for mothers’ and children’s<br />

utterances that also contained some signs. The analyses used mother and<br />

child vocal and signed utterances.<br />

Each mother’s transcribed utterance — linguistic phrase or sentence — was<br />

coded for one higher level and lower level facilitative language technique<br />

(during both free play and storybook conditions, as defined in Tables 3 and<br />

4, respectively). Proportional scores of each facilitative language technique<br />

were calculated and used in the analyses to avoid penalizing less talkative,<br />

yet very responsive, mothers. Accordingly, proportional data were calculated<br />

by dividing the total number of each language technique by the overall number<br />

of mothers’ linguistic attempts. The CARE Center speech-language pathologist<br />

coded 25% of the transcripts, randomly selected to establish testretest<br />

reliability. A line-by-line analysis revealed high agreement between<br />

coders, ranging from 94-98% reliability.<br />

Results<br />

Relationships Between Maternal Perceived Self-Efficacy and Facilitative<br />

Language Techniques<br />

Pearson product correlations were conducted to examine the relationships<br />

between the two self-efficacy subscale items (sensory device use and speechlanguage<br />

development) and the 10 facilitative language techniques. As displayed<br />

in Table 5, statistically significant positive relationships emerged<br />

between self-efficacy in terms of developing their children’s speech-language<br />

and two higher level facilitative language techniques, recast (r = 0.48; p < 0.05)<br />

and open-ended question (r = 0.38; p < 0.05). A significant positive relationship<br />

also emerged between this same self-efficacy subscale item and one<br />

284 DesJardin


Table 3. Definitions and Examples of Higher Level Facilitative Language Techniques<br />

Higher Level Facilitative<br />

Language Techniques Definition Examples<br />

Child is playing with a toy car and mother<br />

says, “You are making the car go fast!”<br />

Child says, “Doggie go,” and mother says,<br />

“The doggie is going.”<br />

Child says, “Doggie go,” and mother says,<br />

“Did the doggie go?”<br />

Mother says, “Tell me more about where<br />

the bear is going. How do you think the<br />

bear is going to get there?”<br />

Parallel talk Providing linguistic input directly about what the<br />

mother or child is doing, as long as child is<br />

directly looking at the activity.<br />

Expansion Repeating the immediate preceding child<br />

utterance approximation or verbalization by<br />

adding one or more morphemes or words<br />

without adding new information.<br />

Recast Repeating the immediate preceding child<br />

utterance approximation or verbalization in a<br />

question format.<br />

Open-ended question Stating a phrase or question that the child can<br />

answer in a two- to three-word phrase or more.<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 285


Table 4. Definitions and Examples of Lower Level Facilitative Language Techniques<br />

286 DesJardin<br />

Lower Level Facilitative<br />

Language Techniques Definition Examples<br />

Mother says, “That is a mustache,” “There is<br />

the moon” or “I see the bear.”<br />

Child hands mother a toy dog and vocalizes;<br />

mother says, “Doggie.” Child pushes a book<br />

away and vocalizes; mother says, “All<br />

done.”<br />

“Is that the bear?” or “Do you like that book?”<br />

Child says, “Hat,” and mother says, “Yeah,<br />

hat.” Child says, “Potato has a hat,” and<br />

mother says, “Yes, a hat.”<br />

“Uh huh, you got it!” “That’s right!” “Yeah!”<br />

“Very good.” “Let’s see.” “Uh oh!” ”Wow!”<br />

“Come here.” “Read the word.” “Turn the<br />

page.” “Listen to me.” “Sit down.” “Look<br />

right here.”<br />

Label Labeling a toy or picture (child may or may<br />

not be looking directly at the object).<br />

Linguistic mapping Putting into words or interpreting the child’s<br />

intended message using the context as a clue<br />

(child uses a preceding vocalization that is<br />

not recognizable as an approximation of a<br />

word).<br />

Close-ended question Stating a question that the child can answer<br />

with only one word.<br />

Imitation Direct repeat or imitation of child’s preceding<br />

vocalization or verbalization without adding<br />

any new words. Mother may reduce the<br />

sentence by imitating fewer words.<br />

Comment Stating a comment to keep the conversation<br />

going or to positively reinforce the child.<br />

Directive Telling the child to do something or<br />

commanding a behavior.


Table 5. Relationships Between Maternal Self-Efficacy Subscale Items and Mothers’<br />

Facilitative Language Techniques<br />

Facilitative Language<br />

Techniques<br />

Maternal Self-Efficacy:<br />

Sensory Device<br />

Maternal Self-Efficacy:<br />

Speech and Language<br />

Parallel talk 0.20 0.07<br />

Expansion 0.07 0.33<br />

Recast 0.04 0.48*<br />

Open-ended question 0.33 0.38*<br />

Linguistic mapping 0.08 0.09<br />

Closed-ended question 0.24 0.46*<br />

Imitation 0.12 0.24<br />

Label –0.11 –0.11<br />

Directive –0.34 –0.57**<br />

Comment –0.32 –0.16<br />

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01<br />

lower level technique, closed-ended question (r = 0.46; p < 0.05), and a negative<br />

relationship emerged with the lower level technique, directive (r = –0.57;<br />

p < 0.01).<br />

Relationships Between Maternal Perceived Involvement and Facilitative<br />

Language Techniques<br />

Two self-efficacy subscale items (sensory device use and speech-language<br />

development) were correlated with the 10 facilitative language techniques. As<br />

Table 6 illustrates, perceived involvement in terms of the child’s sensory<br />

device related positively with one higher level technique, open-ended question<br />

(r = 0.41; p < 0.05), and had a negative association with one lower level<br />

technique, directive (r = –0.65; p < 0.01). There were no statistically significant<br />

associations between mothers’ perceived involvement of their child’s speechlanguage<br />

development and any higher level techniques; however, perceived<br />

involvement related positively to two lower level techniques, closed-ended<br />

question (r = 0.48; p < 0.05) and imitation (r = 0.38; p < 0.05).<br />

Further correlation item analyses with the questions pertaining to this particular<br />

parental involvement subscale revealed statistically significant positive<br />

and negative relationships between the subscale items and lower level<br />

techniques. Mothers’ perceived notion of difficulty to check and listen to the<br />

children’s hearing aids on a daily basis related positively to the use of the<br />

imitation technique, (r = 0.37; p < 0.05). There were also positive relationships<br />

between mothers’ perception of how much speech-language therapists or<br />

early interventionists demonstrate listening-language activities and mothers’<br />

comfort level in performing listening-language techniques in their homes<br />

without a professional present, and the use of the lower level technique,<br />

closed-ended question (r = 0.40; p < 0.05 and r = 0.51; p < 0.01, respectively).<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 287


Table 6. Relationships Between Maternal Involvement Subscale Items and<br />

Mothers’ Facilitative Language Techniques.<br />

Facilitative Language<br />

Techniques<br />

Maternal Involvement:<br />

Sensory Device<br />

Maternal Involvement:<br />

Speech and Language<br />

Parallel talk 0.25 –0.16<br />

Expansion 0.19 0.7<br />

Recast 0.25 0.19<br />

Open-ended question 0.41* 0.26<br />

Linguistic mapping 0.14 –0.12<br />

Close-ended question 0.28 0.48*<br />

Imitation 0.15 0.38*<br />

Label –0.15 –0.14<br />

Directive –0.65** –0.44*<br />

Comment –0.25 0.24<br />

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01<br />

Relationships Between Facilitative Language Techniques and Children’s Receptive and<br />

Expressive Language Skills<br />

Pearson product correlations were conducted between the 10 facilitative<br />

language techniques and children’s receptive and expressive language raw<br />

scores. Statistically significant positive relations emerged between two higher<br />

level techniques, recast (r = 0.64; p < 0.01; r = 0.63; p < 0.01) and open-ended<br />

question (r = 0.58; p < 0.01; r = 0.60; p < 0.01) for children’s receptive and<br />

expressive language respectively. Negative associations emerged between<br />

two lower level techniques, label (r = –0.60; p < 0.01; r = –0.53; p < 0.01) and<br />

directive (r = –0.54; p < 0.01; r = –0.50; p < 0.05) and children’s receptive and<br />

expressive language outcomes, respectively. Generally speaking, this pattern<br />

of correlation is consistent with the view that mothers’ use of higher level<br />

facilitative language techniques tends to be associated with more advanced<br />

development in children’s language skills.<br />

Stepwise regression analysis was performed, controlling for children’s<br />

pure-tone average and length of hearing aid use, to further explore the relationships<br />

between mothers’ facilitative language techniques and children’s<br />

receptive and expressive language skills. The higher- and lower level facilitative<br />

techniques that related positively to mothers’ self-efficacy and involvement<br />

in the previous analyses were included in the regression analyses<br />

(recast, open-ended question and closed-ended question). Children’s puretone<br />

average and length of hearing aid use was entered in block 1, and<br />

facilitative techniques were entered sequentially in block 2 and block 3. As<br />

shown in Table 7, pure-tone average and length of hearing aid use accounted<br />

for a significant amount of the variance in children’s receptive language skills<br />

(45%). Mothers’ use of recast and open-ended question were also significant<br />

predictor variables of children’s receptive language, for a total of 23.3% of the<br />

288 DesJardin


Table 7. Summary of Regression Models for Mothers’ Linguistic Input and<br />

Children’s Receptive Language Skills<br />

Regression Models R R 2<br />

variance. Similarly, children’s pure-tone average and length of hearing aid<br />

use also accounted for 44% of the variance in children’s expressive language<br />

skills, with the use of recast and open-ended question accounting for an<br />

additional 24.3% (see Table 8).<br />

Discussion<br />

Change in R 2<br />

Significance in<br />

F Change<br />

Model 1 0.674 0.454 0.000<br />

Model 2 0.771 0.594 0.140 0.005<br />

Model 3 0.829 0.687 0.093 0.010<br />

Predictors <br />

Pure tone average in better ear –0.295 0.016<br />

Length of hearing aid use 0.230 0.094<br />

Open-ended question 0.348 0.007<br />

Recast 0.370 0.010<br />

Early intervention practices for young children focus on empowering families<br />

and building their sense of self-efficacy and involvement as they support<br />

their children’s early development (DesJardin, 2005; McWilliam & Scott, 2002;<br />

Sandall et al., 2000; Turnbull & Turnbull, 2001). Despite these recommended<br />

practices, few studies have investigated parents’ sense of self-efficacy and<br />

involvement in their children’s early intervention program for families of<br />

children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Of particular interest were the<br />

relationships between maternal facilitative techniques and children’s receptive<br />

and expressive language skills. Results from this study generally confirm<br />

previous findings among young children who are deaf (DesJardin, 2004) in<br />

terms of the positive associations between maternal perceived self-efficacy<br />

and involvement, and facilitative language techniques. In essence, early intervention<br />

implications may be further generalized to a larger population of<br />

young children who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

Similar to the DesJardin (2004) study on preschool children with cochlear<br />

implants, findings from this study provide evidence that maternal perceived<br />

self-efficacy and involvement in terms of developing children’s speechlanguage<br />

development (rather than sensory device use) relates to specific<br />

higher level facilitative techniques during mother-child interactions for children<br />

with hearing aids. These same techniques are also associated with children’s<br />

language skills. Furthermore, these higher level techniques have been<br />

noted in prior research to facilitate later language skills in preschool hearing<br />

children (Baumwell, Tamis-LeMonda, & Bornstein, 1997; Kaiser & Hancock,<br />

2003).<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 289


Table 8. Summary of Regression Models for Mothers’ Linguistic Input and<br />

Children’s Expressive Language Skills<br />

Regression Models R R 2<br />

Change in R 2<br />

Significance in<br />

F Change<br />

Model 1 0.666 0.444 0.000<br />

Model 2 0.771 0.595 0.151 0.004<br />

Model 3 0.829 0.688 0.092 0.010<br />

Predictors <br />

Pure tone average in better ear –0.315 0.010<br />

Length of hearing aid use 0.197 0.149<br />

Open-ended question 0.364 0.005<br />

Recast 0.369 0.010<br />

Maternal beliefs about their children’s language learning, however, also<br />

were positively associated with one lower level technique (closed-question).<br />

It could be that mothers in this study were intuitively adapting their language<br />

techniques to their children’s linguistic attempts. As prior research has suggested<br />

(Lederberg & Prezbindowski, 2000), hearing mothers of children who<br />

are deaf may intuitively make adaptations to their children’s linguistic abilities<br />

and demonstrate response-control behaviors (e.g., questioning) to compensate<br />

for their children’s deafness. Mothers in this study who felt more<br />

knowledgeable and confident in developing their children’s language also<br />

may have been tailoring the language techniques to their children’s linguistic<br />

attempts.<br />

Contrary to the findings in the DesJardin (2004) study, no significant relationships<br />

emerged between mothers’ perceived involvement and any of the<br />

higher level facilitative language techniques for the mothers in this study. On<br />

the other hand, perceived involvement in developing their children’s speech<br />

and language skills related positively to two out of the six lower level techniques<br />

(imitation and closed-ended questions). These same techniques were<br />

not related to their children’s language abilities. These findings suggest that<br />

mothers of children with hearing aids may be receiving varied information<br />

regarding how to support their children’s language learning. Furthermore, it<br />

is plausible that early intervention professionals working with these mothers<br />

and children may be providing inaccurate information regarding language<br />

techniques to support the children’s language skills. In fact, further subscale<br />

item analyses revealed that there was a positive relationship between two<br />

items on the SPISE questionnaire; (1) mothers’ sense of comfort when conducting<br />

listening-language activities at home without the presence of the<br />

professional and (2) the frequency with which early intervention professionals<br />

demonstrated listening-language techniques to mothers and mothers’ use<br />

of a lower level technique (closed-ended question).<br />

Facilitative language techniques must be tailored to children’s language<br />

290 DesJardin


level. As children approach the two-to-three-word stage of language development,<br />

particular techniques are essential for them to develop more complex<br />

language. Facilitative language techniques such as parallel talk (Yoder, Mc-<br />

Cathren, Warren, & Watson, 2001), recast (Fey et al., 1999) and open-ended<br />

questions (Lilly & Green, 2004) encourage conversation, eliciting more complex<br />

grammar and syntactic skills. These techniques are used more frequently<br />

after children achieve a more advanced level of lexical and grammatical understanding<br />

(Hulit & Howard, 1997). Conversely, techniques such as linguistic<br />

mapping, imitation and closed-ended questions are more didactic in<br />

nature and necessary for children who are at the prelinguistic and one-word<br />

level of language development (Yoder et al., 2001).<br />

Implications for Family-Centered Early Intervention<br />

Early intervention programs emphasize the importance of supporting family<br />

strengths and collaborating with caregivers to facilitate better interactions<br />

with their children (Chidress, 2004; Roper & Dunst, 2003). By tailoring their<br />

behaviors to the strengths of individual families, interventionists hope to<br />

provide caregivers with a sense of confidence and competence — selfefficacy<br />

— about their children’s current and future learning and development<br />

(Dunst, 2000; Sonnenstrahl-Benedict & Raimondo, 2003). In turn, parental<br />

self-efficacy of certain skills (e.g., facilitative language techniques) may<br />

enhance children’s language learning (DesJardin, 2006). Thus, encouraging a<br />

mother’s sense of efficacy in developing her child’s language skills is an<br />

important goal in early intervention and should be taken into consideration<br />

when formulating an Individual Family Service Plan (IFSP) or Individual<br />

Education Plan (IEP).<br />

One way to enhance parents’ self-efficacy is to employ a mentorship approach.<br />

In a mentorship model, parents receive hands-on training and practice<br />

with constructive and encouraging feedback based on the parent’s<br />

strengths and needs. Providing caregivers continual feedback enhances their<br />

generalization of newly learned techniques across various activities and settings<br />

(Woods, Kashinath, & Goldstein, 2004). Ultimately, the goal of any<br />

mentorship relationship is for parents to achieve independent, confident use<br />

of techniques that will enhance their children’s language development.<br />

Through a mentorship model, parents would also receive ongoing demonstrations<br />

of skills. A finding of great concern from this study was the significant<br />

positive relationship between mothers’ sense of involvement in their<br />

children’s speech-language development and lower level language techniques.<br />

Further analyses of the data suggested that mothers’ perception of<br />

how much early intervention professionals or therapists demonstrate listening-language<br />

activities was related to mothers’ use of lower level language<br />

techniques. Early intervention programs highlight the importance of embedding<br />

language techniques in naturally occurring interactions with children<br />

within daily routines and activities (Dunst, 2000; Fewell & Deutscher, 2004;<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 291


Roper & Dunst, 2003). Everyday occurrences such as mealtime, hand washing,<br />

playing with toys and bedtime routines are common in most families.<br />

Parents may benefit from a visual reminder of learning opportunities and<br />

activities they can use during the week (Dunst et al., 2001). Moreover, professionals<br />

working with families of children with hearing aids must demonstrate<br />

how to embed facilitative language techniques in everyday natural<br />

language learning experiences (Woods et al., 2004).<br />

Language techniques also should be further tailored to a child’s language<br />

level. According to Vygotskian theory (1962), children develop linguistically<br />

through caregiver interactions that reflect children’s zone of proximal development<br />

(ZPD). A child’s ZPD is defined as the distance between a child’s<br />

current level of development and the level in which the child can function<br />

with adult assistance. For example, for a child who is deaf or hard of hearing<br />

whose language age is at the prelinguistic stage of development, parents’ use<br />

of lower level language techniques (e.g., imitations and expansions) that have<br />

been shown to facilitate language learning in younger hearing children reflect<br />

the child’s zone of proximal development. For preschool children who are at<br />

the two- to three-word linguistic stage, however, higher level language techniques<br />

(recast and open-ended questions) best facilitate children’s language<br />

development. When parents use this form of support, they are helping their<br />

children gain communicative competence and confidence (Vygotsky, 1978;<br />

White, 1985).<br />

Study Limitations<br />

A few limitations of the present study should be mentioned. First, the<br />

sample of mothers in this study may not be a true reflection of mothers and<br />

children in the general population of families of children who are deaf or hard<br />

of hearing. Although many families from lower socioeconomic status and<br />

diverse cultures come to the CARE Center for services, mothers in this study<br />

were all in the middle- to above-average income range with some college<br />

experience. Consequently, the results of this study must be viewed cautiously<br />

when applied to mothers of lower socioeconomic status and mothers in other<br />

geographical locations. Furthermore, the facilitative language techniques examined<br />

in this study may not be consistent with the child interactions that<br />

typify parenting styles in other cultures (van Kleeck, 1994).<br />

Second, the various kinds of family-centered early intervention programs<br />

were not investigated in this study. The families who participated in this<br />

study came from various states and public and/or private family-centered<br />

intervention programs. Within the kinds of programs offered, early intervention<br />

professionals may employ various teaching and communication methodologies<br />

(Auditory-Verbal, Total Communication, etc.). It is most probable<br />

292 DesJardin


that the mothers received different services and, possibly, different instructions,<br />

regarding intervention language techniques. Future work controlling<br />

for the different types of family-centered intervention programs will be fruitful.<br />

Third, the SPISE is a self-report measure of self-efficacy and involvementin<br />

parents. Previous research has suggested that, for many social constructs,<br />

self-report measures may not necessarily reflect what the parents actually do<br />

in their homes (Akey et al., 2000). Although self- reports of how people<br />

perceive their knowledge and competence may be reasonably valid, selfreports<br />

of participatory behavior may show less consistency (e.g., parents’<br />

involvement in the development of their children’s IFSP). Nevertheless, the<br />

SPISE offers researchers and practitioners valuable information in terms of<br />

parents’ perception of specific skills related to their children’s sensory device<br />

and speech-language development (see DesJardin, 2003, for further research<br />

on and practical use of the SPISE).<br />

Summary<br />

Both the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act (IDEIA, 2004)<br />

and the Division of Early Childhood (DEC) Recommended Practices in Early Intervention/Early<br />

Childhood Special Education state the importance of not only<br />

family involvement, but also early intervention practices implemented in a<br />

manner that strengthens parents’ sense of competence in guiding their children’s<br />

early development. Findings from this study highlight the influence of<br />

parental self-efficacy and involvement on children’s spoken language development.<br />

Families of children who are deaf or hard of hearing could benefit<br />

from early intervention programs that capitalize on parents’ sense of efficacy<br />

and facilitative language techniques that are tailored to their children’s language<br />

level.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

The author wishes to express a very special thank you to the mothers and<br />

children who participated in this study. Support for this research was provided<br />

by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders<br />

(NIDCD) of the National Institutes of Health, grant #R01DC006238.<br />

The author would like to thank Laurie Eisenberg, Ph.D. for her valuable<br />

contributions throughout this project. Gratitude is also expressed to the<br />

CARE Center’s research language consultant, Donna Thal, Ph.D., who provided<br />

expertise and guidance in videotape analyses, and Kathleen Lehnert,<br />

M.A., CCC-SLP, for transcription and videotape coding.<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 293


References<br />

Akey, T., Marquis, J., & Ross, M. (2000). Validation of scores on the Psychological<br />

Empowerment Scale: A measure of empowerment for parents of<br />

children with a disability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60,<br />

419-438.<br />

Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived selfefficacy.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 25, 729-735.<br />

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.<br />

Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Bornstein, M. (1997). Maternal verbal<br />

sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development,<br />

20, 247-258.<br />

Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic<br />

developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Brody, G.H, Flor, D.L., & Gibson, N.M. (1999). Linking maternal efficacy<br />

beliefs, developmental goals, parenting practices, and child competence in<br />

rural single-parent African-American families, Child Development, 70(5),<br />

1197-1208.<br />

Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children’s education programs<br />

as a predictor of child’s language, early reading, and socialemotional<br />

development. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 140-<br />

155.<br />

Calderon, R., Bargones, J., & Sidman, S. (1998). Characteristics of hearing<br />

families and their young deaf and hard of hearing children: Early intervention<br />

follow-up. American Annals of the Deaf, 143, 347-362.<br />

Calderon, R., & Naidu, S. (2000). Further support of the benefits of early<br />

identification and intervention with children with hearing loss. The Volta<br />

Review, 100, 53-84.<br />

Chidress, D.C. (2004). Special instruction and natural environments: Best<br />

practices in early intervention. Infants and Young Children, 17(2), 162-170.<br />

Conrad, B., Gross, D., Fogg, L., & Ruchala, P. (1992). Maternal confidence,<br />

knowledge, and quality of mother-toddler interactions: A preliminary<br />

study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 13, 353-362.<br />

Cook, R., Tessier, A., Klein, M., & Armbruster, V. (2000). Nurturing communication<br />

skills. In R.E. Cook, A. Tessier, & M.D., Klein (Eds.), Adapting early<br />

childhood curricula for children in inclusive settings (5 th ed.) (290-339). Englewood<br />

Cliffs, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.<br />

Psychometrika, 6, 297-334.<br />

DesJardin, J.L. (2003). Assessing parental perceptions of self-efficacy and involvement<br />

in families of young children with hearing loss. The Volta Review,<br />

103(4), 391-409.<br />

294 DesJardin


DesJardin, J.L. (2004). Maternal self-efficacy and involvement: Supporting language<br />

development in young deaf children with cochlear implants. Unpublished<br />

doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.<br />

DesJardin, J.L. (2005). Maternal perceptions of self-efficacy and involvement<br />

in the auditory development of young children with prelingual deafness.<br />

Journal of Early Intervention, 27, 193-209.<br />

DesJardin, J.L. (2006). Maternal contributions: Supporting language development<br />

in young children with hearing loss. Paper presented at the Universal Hearing<br />

Screening: Beyond Newborn Screening, Lake Como, Italy.<br />

Dunst, C.J. (2000). Revisiting “Rethinking Early Intervention.” Topics in Early<br />

Childhood Special Education, 20(2), 95-104.<br />

Dunst, C.J., Bruder, M.B., Trivette, C.M., Raab, M., & McLean, M. (2001).<br />

Natural learning opportunities for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers.<br />

Young Exceptional Children, 4(3), 18-25.<br />

Fewell, R.R., & Deutscher, B. (2004). Contributions of early language and<br />

maternal facilitation variables to later language and reading abilities. Journal<br />

of Early Intervention, 26(2), 132-145.<br />

Fey, M., Krulik, T., Loeb, D., & Proctor-Williams, K. (1999). Sentence recast<br />

use by parents of children with typical language and children with specific<br />

language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 273-<br />

286.<br />

Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P.S. (1999). The relationship<br />

between maternal language measures and language development in<br />

toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-<br />

Language Pathology, 8, 364-374.<br />

Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1999). Observing children and families talking. In B.<br />

Hart & T.R. Risley (Eds.), The social world of children learning to talk (pp.<br />

7-29). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.<br />

Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1997). Language development. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/<br />

Cole Publishing Company.<br />

Hulit, L., & Howard, M. (1997). Born to talk: An introduction to speech and<br />

language development. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.<br />

Kaiser, A., & Hancock, T. (2003). Teaching parents new skills to support their<br />

young children’s development. Infants and Young Children, 16, 9-21.<br />

Koester, L.S., Papousek, H., & Smith-Gray, S. (2000). Intuitive parenting, communication,<br />

and interaction with deaf infants. In P.E. Spencer, C.J. Erting,<br />

& M. Marschark (Eds.), The deaf child in the family and at school: Essays<br />

in honor of Kathryn P. Meadow-Orlans (pp. 55-71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.<br />

Lederberg, A., & Prezbindowski, A. (2000). Impact of child deafness on<br />

mother-toddler interaction: Strengths and weaknesses. In P.E. Spencer, C.J.<br />

Erting, & M. Marschark (Eds.), The deaf child in the family and at school: Essays<br />

in honor of Kathryn P. Meadow-Orlans (73-92). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 295


Lerner, G. (1952). Mr. Potato Head Fun Activity Kits. HASBRO Company.<br />

Lilly, E., & Green, C. (2004). Developing partnerships with families through children’s<br />

literature. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall.<br />

Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Books for Young<br />

Readers.<br />

McNeil, J., & Fowler, S. (1999). Let’s talk: Encouraging mother-child conversations<br />

during story reading. Journal of Early Intervention, 22, 51-69.<br />

McWilliam, R., & Scott, S. (2001). A support approach to early intervention: A<br />

three-part framework. Infants & Young Children, 13, 55-66.<br />

Moeller, M.P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children<br />

who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>(3), 1-9.<br />

Murphy, J. (1983). What next, baby bear! New York: Dial Books for Young Readers.<br />

Reynell, J., & Gruber, C. (1990). Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Los<br />

Angeles: Western Psychological Services.<br />

Roper, N., & Dunst, C.J. (2003). Communication intervention in natural learning<br />

environments: Guidelines for practice. Infants and Young Children, 16(3),<br />

215-226.<br />

Sandall, S., McLean, M.E., & Smith, B.J. (Eds.). (2000). DEC recommended practices<br />

in early intervention/early childhood special education. Longmont, CO:<br />

Sopris West.<br />

Sonnenstrahl-Benedict, B., & Raimondo, B. (2003). Family rights, legislation,<br />

and policies. In B. Bodner-Johnson, & M. Sass-Lehrer (Eds.), The young<br />

deaf or hard of hearing child: A family-centered approach to early intervention<br />

(pp. 3-33). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.<br />

Stallings, L., Gao, S., & Svirsky, M. (2002). Assessing the language abilities of<br />

pediatric cochlear implant users across a broad range of ages and performance<br />

abilities. The Volta Review, 102, 215-235.<br />

Svirsky, M.A., Robbins, A.M., Kirk, K.I., Pisoni, D.B., & Miyamoto, R.T.<br />

(2000). Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear<br />

implants. Psychological Science, 11, 153-158.<br />

Teti, D.M., & Gelfand, D.M. (1991). Behavioral competence among mothers of<br />

infants in the first year: The mediational role of maternal self-efficacy. Child<br />

Development, 62, 918-929.<br />

Trivette, C.M. & Dunst, C.J. (2000). Recommended practices in family-based<br />

practices. In S. Sandall, M.E. McLean, & B.J. Smith (Eds.), DEC recommended<br />

practices in early intervention/early childhood special education (39-46). Longmont,<br />

CO: Sopris West.<br />

Trivette, C.M. & Dunst, C.J. (2003). Parenting Experiences Scale. Morganton,<br />

NC: Orelena Hawks Puckett Institute.<br />

Turnbull, A.P., & Turnbull, H.R. (2001). Families, professionals, and exceptionality:<br />

Collaborating for empowerment (4 th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill<br />

Prentice Hall.<br />

Turnbull, A.P., Turbiville, V., & Turnbull, H.R. (2000). Evolution of familyprofessional<br />

partnerships: Collective empowerment as the model for the<br />

296 DesJardin


early twenty-first century. In J.P. Shonkoff & S.J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of<br />

early childhood intervention (2 nd ed.), (630–650). Cambridge: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

van Kleek, A. (1994). Potential cultural bias in training parents as conversational<br />

partners with their children who have delays in language development.<br />

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 3, 67-78.<br />

Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of psychological processes.<br />

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Weizman, Z., & Snow, C. (2001). Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary<br />

acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for<br />

meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37, 265-279.<br />

White, B. (1985). The first three years of life. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.<br />

Woods, J., Kashinath, S., & Goldstein, H. (2004). Effects of embedding caregiver-<br />

implemented teaching strategies in daily routines on children’s communication<br />

outcomes. Journal of Early Intervention, 26(3), 175-193.<br />

Yoder, P.J., & Warren, S.F. (1998). Maternal responsively predicts the prelinguistic<br />

communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional<br />

communication. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1207-<br />

1219.<br />

Yoder, P.J., McCathren, R.B., Warren, S.F., & Watson, A.L. (2001). Important<br />

distinctions in measuring maternal responses to communication in prelinguistic<br />

children with disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 22(3),<br />

135-147.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2000). Successful outcomes for deaf and hard of hearing<br />

children. Seminars in Hearing, 21, 309-325.<br />

Empowering Families of Young Children Who Are Deaf 297


Appendix<br />

Scale of Parent Involvement and Maternal Self-Efficacy (SPISE) Subscale Items for Maternal<br />

Self-Efficacy and Parent Involvement<br />

Maternal Self-Efficacy Parent Involvement<br />

298 DesJardin<br />

Child’s Speech and Language<br />

Internal Consistency Reliability;<br />

Cronbach’s = 0.83<br />

Child’s Sensory Aid Use<br />

Internal Consistency Reliability;<br />

Cronbach’s = 0.76<br />

Child’s Speech and Language<br />

Internal Consistency Reliability;<br />

Cronbach’s = 0.87<br />

Child’s Sensory Aid Use<br />

Internal Consistency Reliability;<br />

Cronbach’s = 0.80<br />

How often are you invited to<br />

participate in home visits/therapy<br />

with your child?<br />

How hard is it for you to<br />

check your child’s hearing<br />

aids/cochlear implant<br />

every day?<br />

How much do you feel you<br />

know how to help your<br />

child develop sounds?<br />

How comfortable are you with<br />

participating in home visits/therapy<br />

with your child?<br />

How hard is it for you to put<br />

on your child’s hearing<br />

aids/cochlear implant<br />

every day?<br />

How much do you feel you<br />

know about and are able to<br />

do speech-language<br />

activities/strategies with<br />

your child at home on a<br />

How much do you feel that you<br />

know about and are able to check<br />

and put on your child’s hearing<br />

aids/cochlear implant on a daily<br />

basis?<br />

How much do you feel you know<br />

about and are able to adjust the<br />

settings of your child’s hearing<br />

aids (volume) or cochlear implant<br />

(programs) on a daily basis?<br />

How often does the teacher or<br />

speech-language pathologist show<br />

you listening/language activities?<br />

How hard is it for you to<br />

check your child’s listening<br />

skills, using the Ling<br />

Six-Sound Test every day?<br />

How many days per week<br />

will your child wear<br />

his/her hearing aid or<br />

cochlear implant?<br />

How many hours per day<br />

will your child wear<br />

his/her hearing aid or<br />

cochlear implant?<br />

daily basis?<br />

How much do you feel you<br />

can positively affect your<br />

child’s speech<br />

development?<br />

How much do you feel you<br />

can positively affect your<br />

child’s language<br />

development?<br />

How much do you feel you<br />

can positively affect your<br />

child’s early development?<br />

How much do you feel you know<br />

about and are able to check your<br />

child’s listening skills daily, using<br />

the Ling Six-Sound Test?<br />

How much do you feel that you<br />

know how to help your child<br />

How much are you comfortable doing<br />

the listening and language activities<br />

with your child when the teacher or<br />

therapist is not there?<br />

How often do you feel that the teacher<br />

of the deaf includes you in the<br />

planning of your child’s IFSP or IEP?<br />

How comfortable are you in helping to<br />

develop your child’s IFSP or IEP?<br />

develop words?<br />

How much do you feel you can<br />

positively affect your child’s<br />

listening development?


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 299-319<br />

Management of Young<br />

Children with Unilateral<br />

Hearing Loss<br />

Sarah McKay, Au.D.<br />

Children with unilateral hearing loss (UHL) are at risk for academic, speech and<br />

language and social-emotional difficulties. To date, most of the evidence documented<br />

in the literature has been obtained from school-age children, most of whom were<br />

diagnosed with UHL after enrollment in school. Following the widespread institution<br />

of universal newborn hearing screening, most children are now screened prior to three<br />

months of age. Consequently, infants with UHL are being identified years before<br />

entering school, leaving hearing health and early intervention professionals asking<br />

how to best serve this population of children. Until evidence is obtained that will<br />

direct best practice initiatives, caregivers can take advantage of this previously nonexistent<br />

“window of opportunity” to provide optimal auditory environments for their<br />

children. It is the responsibility of audiology professionals and other professionals who<br />

provide services to children with hearing loss to be proactive and educate parents and<br />

caregivers so all children with UHL have the best chance for success.<br />

Introduction<br />

The risks associated with unilateral hearing loss (UHL) in children have<br />

been well documented in the literature. Specifically, children with UHL are at<br />

higher risk for educational, speech-language and social-emotional difficulties<br />

than their peers with normal hearing (Bess & Tharpe, 1984, 1986; English &<br />

Church, 1999; Oyler, Oyler, & Matkin, 1988). Before the advent of universal<br />

newborn hearing screening (UNHS), many children with UHL were not identified<br />

until school age (Brookhauser, Worthington, & Kelly, 1991). Most states<br />

have now implemented UNHS, and children with congenital UHL are being<br />

identified in the first weeks of life. Although this early identification of hearing<br />

loss is clearly an advantage, it leaves caregivers with questions. Roush<br />

(2000) stated that “for all families, it is imperative that they leave the diagnostic<br />

evaluation with the knowledge that much can and will be done to<br />

ameliorate the effects of hearing loss for their child” (p. 58).<br />

Sarah McKay, Au.D., is a senior audiologist at The Center for Childhood Communication at<br />

The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia.<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 299


Parents want to know why their child has UHL, what the impact will be<br />

and what they can do to help their child. Early identification allows the<br />

audiologist to connect with the parents, share relevant information about<br />

UHL, listen to parents’ fears and build trust. Most of the evidence to date has<br />

been on older children. Professionals who diagnose and treat this population<br />

of newly identified babies are faced with new questions. How do we help<br />

young children avoid or minimize some of the deleterious effects of hearing<br />

loss? Does aggressive early intervention lower children’s risk of later educational<br />

difficulties? How do we know which children are going to experience<br />

difficulties? Can we justify changing early intervention policy based on evidence<br />

seen in the school-age population? We know that language outcomes<br />

of children with bilateral hearing loss are improved if intervention is initiated<br />

within the first year of life (Moeller, 2000; Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998). Should we<br />

expect this benefit in children with UHL? Although there is evidence of the<br />

benefit of early intervention for infants with hearing loss as well as evidence<br />

that supports the difficulties school-age children with UHL may encounter, is<br />

it enough to support changes in the way we manage infants and toddlers with<br />

UHL? Gravel et al. (2005) examined whether changes in policy and practice<br />

were warranted based on the evidence presented from previous studies. With<br />

regard to mild and unilateral hearing losses, they noted:<br />

While a body of work suggests that about half of elementary and middle<br />

school-age children with mild bilateral or unilateral hearing loss have some<br />

degree of academic or social-emotional difficulties, as yet there is little<br />

direct evidence in larger sample sizes using prospective research designs<br />

that mild forms of bilateral or unilateral hearing loss are related to developmental<br />

delays in the infant and preschool-age population. There are no<br />

studies concerning which individual infants and toddlers with mild forms<br />

of hearing loss are more likely to experience school-related problems at<br />

older ages and what the influence of other intrinsic and extrinsic variables<br />

is on these children’s developmental outcome.” (p. 219)<br />

The topic of UHL is not new to the audiology profession. With regard to the<br />

profession’s response to UHL, Oyler et al. (1988) stated:<br />

Historically, the involvement of hearing health professionals in the management<br />

of children with unilateral hearing loss has been limited. The<br />

conventional approach was to identify the cause of the hearing loss and to<br />

assure the parents that there would be no handicap.” (p. 18)<br />

In 2005, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) and the<br />

Marion Downs Hearing Center cosponsored “The National Workshop on<br />

Mild and Unilateral Hearing Loss.” More than 50 professionals with expertise<br />

300 McKay


in mild and/or unilateral hearing loss convened to review and discuss current<br />

information related to the identification and appropriate intervention for<br />

children with mild and unilateral hearing losses. Areas covered were prevalence<br />

and screening; diagnosis, amplification and outcomes; and early intervention,<br />

eligibility and clinical practice. The information generated in this<br />

workshop was comprehensive and illuminated the need for future research<br />

with this population of children. This need was particularly evident when<br />

considering infants and preschool children with UHL. When children with<br />

UHL remain unidentified until they are school age, or until problems arise,<br />

intervention is often failure based. Although identifying children with UHL<br />

at birth presents some unanswered questions, it also allows professionals<br />

who care for them to be proactive in their care. Evidence gathered in the<br />

school-age population may help to guide future research endeavors in the<br />

birth-to-3 population and help to support decisions about best practices.<br />

Status of Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Programs<br />

Currently, 40 jurisdictions (38 states, the District of Columbia and Puerto<br />

Rico) have legislation in effect for early hearing detection and intervention<br />

(EHDI) programs (National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management,<br />

2006). The specific guidelines vary from state to state. Some states<br />

require that all babies be screened, whereas others require only babies born at<br />

specified hospitals and still others, a specified percentage of babies born. For<br />

a variety of reasons, it may not be feasible to screen all babies before discharge:<br />

Not all babies are born in hospitals, some babies are discharged early,<br />

prior to before screening, or parents deny newborn hearing screening. An<br />

estimated 90% of infants born in the United States, however, are being<br />

screened for hearing loss (Directors of Speech and Hearing Programs in State<br />

Health and Welfare Agencies, 2003).<br />

Eligibility criteria for early intervention services through the Individuals<br />

with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004 (IDEA 2004) Part C and Part<br />

B also vary from state to state. Although Part C allows eligibility for early<br />

intervention services if a child has a condition that places him or her at risk<br />

for developmental delay, specifics regarding hearing loss vary. Some state<br />

Part C and Part B programs define eligibility for services based on percentage<br />

of delay. For instance, in Alaska, established risk conditions are those that<br />

“have a high probability of resulting in a 50% developmental delay.” In<br />

Connecticut, children with UHL are not automatically eligible for services.<br />

Eligibility in Florida specifies sensorineural hearing loss in excess of 50 dB HL<br />

in the worst ear. (A state-specific guide to services is available at www<br />

.infanthearing.org.) Some babies are eligible to receive services immediately,<br />

whereas others are entitled to an evaluation and periodic monitoring. Once a<br />

child is three years old and eligibility is based on the criteria stated in IDEA<br />

Part B, services most often are based on existing delay.<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 301


Incidence of UHL<br />

Impact of UHL<br />

Definitions of UHL have varied over the years, but generally, a UHL is a<br />

loss in one ear of any degree (mild to profound) (American Speech-Language-<br />

Hearing Association, 2006). The incidence of UHL is approximately 0.83/<br />

1,000 (Prieve et al., 2000). If the incidence is divided into categories of babies<br />

who were in the neonatal intensive care unit and well-baby nursery, the<br />

incidence is 3.2/1,000 and 0.41/1,000, respectively. It should be noted that<br />

these numbers might not accurately reflect the number of children with mild<br />

UHL because current UNHS technologies are not sensitive to mild degrees of<br />

hearing loss. Johnson et al. (2005) found that 80% of infants with later confirmed<br />

bilateral mild hearing loss or UHL were not referred (using a passrefer<br />

criterion) when a two-step, two-technology hearing screening protocol<br />

(Otoacoustic Emissions [OAE] and automated Auditory Brainstem Response<br />

[ABR] testing) was used.<br />

Prevalence of UHL<br />

The prevalence of UHL in the school-age population is approximately 3%<br />

to5% according to Bess, Dodd-Murphy and Parker (1998) and 5.6% according<br />

to Niskar et al. (1998). These figures are alarming, particularly compared to<br />

incidence rates of UHL in the neonatal population (0.83/1,000). Although<br />

some children in the older age group have late-onset or acquired hearing loss,<br />

it is speculated that others may have had existing mild hearing loss at birth<br />

that was fluctuating or progressive in nature. This information is pertinent<br />

because children with speech and language delays may be enrolled in early<br />

intervention programs, and their early intervention providers may be the first<br />

to detect signs of hearing difficulty.<br />

Psychoacoustic Impact<br />

The difficulties that children with UHL experience have explanations<br />

rooted in psychoacoustics. Binaural advantages include localization, binaural<br />

summation, reduction in the head shadow effect and binaural release from<br />

masking. Localization difficulties on the horizontal plane have been documented<br />

(Bess & Tharpe, 1986; Morrongiello, 1989). Localization is affected<br />

because individuals with UHL do not have the benefit of interaural time and<br />

intensity cues. Typically, when sound approaches from one direction, the<br />

interaural time difference between ears allows one to determine from which<br />

direction the sound is coming. With UHL, one may not be able to hear those<br />

time differences (depending on the degree of UHL) and, thus, can have difficulty<br />

localizing.<br />

302 McKay


Children with UHL experience difficulties understanding in noise, in<br />

large part, because they do not have the benefit of binaural release from<br />

masking. Normal hearing in both ears allows one to filter out noise to better<br />

hear the speech signal. Individuals with UHL do not have this advantage. As<br />

Bess and Tharpe (1986) demonstrated, children with UHL exhibit greater<br />

difficulty understanding nonsense syllables in noise than children with normal<br />

hearing.<br />

Binaural summation is the increased perception of loudness when sound is<br />

heard by two normal hearing ears rather than by just one. For a speech signal<br />

the advantage is approximately 3 to 8 dB (Lieu, 2004). Children with UHL do<br />

not have the advantage of binaural summation. This disadvantage may contribute<br />

to their difficulty with understanding speech from a distance.<br />

The difficulties resulting from a lack of binaural advantage are magnified<br />

in children. They do not yet have the language base to fill in the gaps when<br />

speech enters from the impaired side, is present in noise or comes from a<br />

distance. Therefore, children with UHL may benefit from accommodations to<br />

counteract or preempt these psychoacoustic limitations. Such accommodations<br />

can be made at home and in a child’s preschool. These may be in the<br />

form of acoustic modifications, placement strategies and amplification or FM<br />

systems.<br />

Studies summarized by Emmer (1999) revealed that auditory deprivation<br />

was documented in children with binaural hearing loss who were fit monaurally,<br />

in adults with sensorineural UHL and in the unaided, poorer ear of<br />

those adults with asymmetrical sensorineural hearing loss. Emmer noted that<br />

auditory deprivation is more marked and has earlier onset in individuals<br />

with UHL than in those with bilateral hearing loss who were fit monoaurally.<br />

Additional evidence cited in this review indicated that auditory deprivation<br />

“involves alteration in the central nervous system during a critical period of<br />

development” (p. 26). Emmer provided an illustrative case of a 4-year-old<br />

child with bilateral hearing loss. She had relatively equal speech perception<br />

abilities in each ear but chose to wear only one hearing aid. By age 8, the<br />

child’s speech recognition abilities had declined in the unaided ear. It should<br />

be noted that the phenomenon of acclimatization (improvement following<br />

auditory deprivation) also has been observed following later introduction of<br />

amplification to the unaided ear. With regard to UHL, questions remain<br />

about whether providing stimulation to the affected ear through amplification<br />

would avoid auditory deprivation and overcome these psychoacoustic<br />

challenges.<br />

Educational Impact<br />

The difficulties children with UHL experience were first described in detail<br />

in the 1980s. As previously mentioned, this population of children typically<br />

was not identified until they were school age (Brookhauser et al., 1991).<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 303


Children with UHL are at risk for academic delays, and grade failure has been<br />

noted (Bess & Tharpe, 1986; Oyler et al., 1988). In the Bess and Tharpe (1986)<br />

study, 35% of children with UHL were found to have failed a grade. This<br />

finding was 10 times higher than the greater Nashville county average of 3.5%<br />

for grades K to 6. Oyler et al. (1988) found that 24% of children with UHL had<br />

failed a grade in school, again approximately 10 times greater than the county<br />

average (2% for grades K to 8). These findings were stunning. In addition,<br />

Bess and Tharpe found that 13.3% of children with UHL were in need of<br />

resource assistance. Oyler et al. reported 40.7% of these children were receiving<br />

special services. More recently, English and Church (1999) noted that<br />

grade failure was no longer a preferred practice; therefore, grade failure could<br />

not be compared with the 1980s studies. English and Church did find that a<br />

large percentage (54%) of children with UHL received individualized special<br />

education services. The nature of the services varied, with most (27%) in the<br />

area of fitting and monitoring of amplification; 16% in resource room support<br />

for reading and math; 6% in speech-language and listening therapy; and 5%<br />

in either physical therapy, occupational therapy, vision therapy or counseling.<br />

At the CDC “National Workshop on Mild and Unilateral Hearing Loss,”<br />

Neault (2005) provided an excellent summary of the results of five studies on<br />

a total of 190 children with UHL. Comparing bilateral to unilateral listeners<br />

across a variety of tasks, the average scores of “bilateral listeners” performed<br />

at the 50th percentile, whereas “unilateral listeners” were poorer in all areas<br />

(math, 30th percentile; language arts, 25th percentile; social skills, 32nd percentile).<br />

Children with severe to profound UHL also have been found to have<br />

a lower full-scale IQ score on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-<br />

Revised (WISC-R) than children with lesser degrees of UHL (Culbertson &<br />

Gilbert, 1986; Klee & Davis-Dansky, 1986).<br />

Speech and Language Impact<br />

Some studies have indicated that children with UHL are at risk for speech<br />

and language delays. Kiese-Himmel (2002) evaluated the emergence of oneand<br />

two-word phrases in children with UHL. She found that the average age<br />

for first words was within normal limits (12.7 months). However, the average<br />

age for first two-word utterances was found to be delayed (23.5 months). The<br />

defined norm for the emergence of two-word phrases in this study was 18<br />

months. Lieu (2004) reported on the findings of the Colorado Home Intervention<br />

Program (CHIP) that followed the language abilities of 15 children<br />

with UHL since infancy. Of the 15 children, four (27%) were found to have<br />

significant language delays, and one (7%) was found to have borderline language<br />

delays.<br />

Social-Emotional Impact<br />

Because of the psychoacoustic variables mentioned earlier, sound often is<br />

audible but not always understandable to children with UHL. This disadvantage<br />

304 McKay


can leave a child appearing inattentive, disinterested or aloof. These negative<br />

perceptions can affect school and peer interactions. When a child is not able<br />

to hear conversations in groups, he or she may feel insecure or “left out,”<br />

which can lead to difficulties with behavior and peer relationships. Bess et al.<br />

(1998) studied groups of third, sixth and ninth graders with minimal hearing<br />

loss (including UHL) and found that these children had greater dysfunction<br />

than their peers with normal hearing on domains of stress, self-esteem and<br />

behavior.<br />

Teacher Perceptions of Children With UHL<br />

Minimal hearing loss (a term that includes UHL) has been described as an<br />

“invisible acoustic filter” (Flexer, 1995). Children with UHL may not exhibit<br />

obvious difficulties. If teachers are not informed that a child has UHL, they<br />

may never know that the hearing loss exists. Hearing aids, which alert teachers<br />

to children with bilateral hearing loss, are not always recommended or<br />

chosen for children with UHL, and as mentioned previously children with<br />

UHL are at risk for academic delays (Bess & Tharpe, 1984; Brookhauser et al.,<br />

1991; English & Church, 1999; Oyler et al., 1988). Teacher perceptions about<br />

children with UHL have been studied. Dancer, Burl and Waters (1995) obtained<br />

Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk (SIFTER) questionnaires<br />

from teachers of children with UHL. They found that teachers gave<br />

lower scores to children with UHL in all five areas of the SIFTER: academics,<br />

attention, communication, participation and behavior. Culbertson and Gilbert<br />

(1986) reported that teachers rated 39% of children with UHL as “below<br />

average” compared to 5% for their peers with normal hearing. Children with<br />

UHL were described as giving up easily on new and difficult tasks and<br />

needing more direction for task completion. They also received more negative<br />

ratings than their peers with normal hearing in the areas of dependence and<br />

independence, attention to task, emotional ability, peer relations and social<br />

confidence. Teachers and care providers of children with UHL should be<br />

informed of the child’s hearing loss, or they may draw other (negative) conclusions<br />

about the child based on his or her performance, attentiveness and<br />

behavior. Bess & Tharpe (1986) illuminated the need for teachers to receive<br />

training regarding children with UHL and the everyday problems they may<br />

encounter. They also warned that teachers need to be informed about issues<br />

regarding localization, listening in noise and safety issues. Although this<br />

warning was intended for teachers of school-age children, it would be just as<br />

applicable to daycare and preschool teachers. Any knowledge that helps a<br />

teacher understand the unique challenges of his or her students should be<br />

beneficial.<br />

Recommendations<br />

To the author’s knowledge, best practice guidelines have not yet been<br />

identified for infants and toddlers with UHL, which leaves those who work<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 305


with this population of children to make recommendations and choose options<br />

based on evidence from an older population. This section delineates<br />

some of these options and possible ways to apply them to a younger population.<br />

Amplification Options<br />

Some studies have reported on the efficacy of amplification with UHL, but<br />

most have been with school-age children. Little is known about the benefit of<br />

early amplification for infants and toddlers with UHL. The Special Consideration<br />

section of the American Academy of Audiology (AAA, 2003) Pediatric<br />

Amplification Protocol addresses the fitting of amplification on children with<br />

UHL as follows:<br />

Use of hearing aid amplification is indicated for some children with unilateral<br />

hearing losses. The decision to fit a child with unilateral hearing loss<br />

should be made on an individual basis, taking into consideration the child’s<br />

or family’s preference as well as audiologic, developmental, communication,<br />

and educational factors. (p. 3)<br />

The choice of when to fit a child with amplification may depend on different<br />

factors. Evidence on the efficacy of fitting babies with UHL with hearing<br />

aids does not yet exist; thus, parents and audiologists find themselves<br />

examining other factors. The timing of initial fitting with a hearing aid may<br />

have social ramifications. If parents choose to wait to fit their child with a<br />

hearing aid until the child experiences failure (i.e., later elementary grades),<br />

the attitudes of the child’s peers toward the amplification at that time may be<br />

poorer than they would have been at a younger age (Dengerink & Porter,<br />

1984; Riensche, Peterson, & Linden, 1990). These studies show that older<br />

children perceive their peers who wear hearing aids to be less smart and less<br />

attractive. Younger (preschool) children have no negative attitudes toward<br />

their peers who wear hearing aids. It is possible, therefore, that earlier versus<br />

later amplification may allow a child to develop positive self-esteem and<br />

friendship/peer groups in which a hearing aid goes unnoticed.<br />

The use of conventional hearing aids with children who have moderately<br />

severe or better hearing in the impaired ear has met with some success, as<br />

subjective rating scales indicate (Kiese-Himmel, 2002; McKay, 2002). A questionnaire<br />

administered to parents of children with UHL who wore hearing<br />

aids and asked parents to rate how their child was doing at present versus<br />

before amplification (McKay, 2002). The results were very positive, with 72%<br />

of parents and patients reporting benefit (improved or greatly improved) on<br />

questions involving different listening environments. Additionally, all of the<br />

parents reported being happy with their decision to fit their child with a<br />

hearing aid. Fifty percent wished they had fit their child with a hearing aid<br />

306 McKay


sooner. Most of the children in this questionnaire group were school age. For<br />

the children who were under three, ratings were more neutral. These younger<br />

children had not yet encountered some of the listening difficulties that older<br />

children experience, which may account for the neutral ratings. Kiese-<br />

Himmel (2002) reported 81% acceptance from children with moderately severe<br />

or better UHL who wore a hearing aid but “very poor use of hearing aid<br />

or no use of hearing aid” (p. 59) when the hearing loss was severe or profound.<br />

With a lesser degree of hearing loss, it may be possible to achieve a<br />

more balanced sense of hearing between the ears. All available audiologic<br />

information, including electrophysiological and behavioral test results, in<br />

conjunction with verification measures, such as real ear probe measures with<br />

desired sensation level (DSL) targets, always should be used to ensure appropriate<br />

fitting.<br />

As parents seek sources to inform themselves about options for their newly<br />

diagnosed baby with UHL, it is important to give them information about all<br />

possible devices, including those that would not be recommended or are not<br />

an option at that time. Examples are contralateral routing of signal (CROS)<br />

amplification, bone-anchored hearing apparatus (BAHA) and even a cochlear<br />

implant. Although most hearing professionals know that a child is a candidate<br />

for a cochlear implant only if he or she has bilateral severe-to-profound<br />

hearing loss, parents and well-intending relatives and friends do not. It is a<br />

simple strategy to inform parents about all available technologies to avoid<br />

confusion or disappointment.<br />

The AAA (2003) Pediatric Amplification Protocol also addresses the use of<br />

CROS amplification. The CROS system has been purported to be useful in<br />

quiet situations, especially in cases where the signal originates on the side of<br />

the impaired ear. The protocol cautions audiologists that CROS amplification<br />

may not be beneficial in the classroom because of the introduction of noise to<br />

the normal hearing ear through the microphone on the impaired side (Kenworthy,<br />

Klee, & Tharpe, 1990). The CROS system, therefore, should not be<br />

considered for young children.<br />

The BAHA was originally intended for individuals with conductive or<br />

mixed hearing loss but has recently been marketed for individuals with UHL.<br />

Because of issues related to anatomical maturation, the BAHA is only Federal<br />

Drug Administration approved for use in children over 5. The premise of<br />

fitting a BAHA on a person with severe or profound sensorineural hearing<br />

loss is to allow transcranial delivery of a signal. Although there are reports of<br />

BAHA fittings on adults with UHL (Wazen, Ghossani, Kacker, & Zschommler,<br />

2001; Wazen, Ghossani, Spitzer, & Kuller, 2005; Wazen et al., 2003), to<br />

the author’s knowledge, there are no reports to date on children with severe<br />

or profound sensorineural hearing loss in one ear. It could be speculated that<br />

the introduction of noise to the impaired side, which is sent transcranially to<br />

the normal side, would have a similar impact on a child to that of a CROS aid.<br />

A great deal more evidence regarding outcomes for adults and older children<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 307


who have UHL and the BAHA implant should be obtained to determine if<br />

surgical implantation of BAHA in children with UHL is beneficial.<br />

The benefit of FM technology in this population has been well documented<br />

(Flexer, 1995; Kenworthy et al., 1990; Updike, 1994). Increasing the signal-tonoise<br />

ratio for a child who is developing language is clearly an advantage. As<br />

with the aforementioned studies, most evidence has been conducted among<br />

school-age children. In the infant and toddler population, one must think<br />

creatively about which listening situations FM use could be beneficial. These<br />

situations might include while a child is in a stroller or car seat or during<br />

instructional time in a day care or preschool environment. Another factor to<br />

consider is the best choice for routing the FM signal. If a child already has a<br />

hearing aid, the audiologist may choose to couple an FM receiver to it. Another<br />

option is to couple the FM receiver to the normal hearing ear through<br />

an ear-level FM system. Personal and sound field FM systems also may be an<br />

option. To the author’s knowledge, no evidence is available to indicate the<br />

most appropriate choice of FM for a preschool-age child with UHL. In keeping<br />

with the AAA (2003) Pediatric Amplification Protocol, decisions must be<br />

made on an individual basis. As a child approaches school age, or if he or she<br />

is already receiving early intervention services, it is recommended that the<br />

decision be coordinated with the child’s educational audiologist or hearing<br />

support teacher. They may have additional important information about the<br />

class setting, and use of FM with other students will need to be considered.<br />

This information is not typically available to the clinical audiologist.<br />

Medical Considerations<br />

Upon diagnosis of UHL, parents should be informed of additional recommended<br />

evaluations. Understanding the rationale behind these recommendations<br />

as well as the potential results and implications is important for all<br />

care providers, even those not directly involved in the evaluations, because<br />

parents may have immediate questions. Recommendations may need to be<br />

made over a series of appointments to avoid crossing the fine line between<br />

empowering and overwhelming parents. The child’s primary care physician<br />

(PCP), who should act as “gate keeper” for all subsequent medical referrals,<br />

should be informed immediately of the diagnosis and recommendations. The<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing (JCIH, 2000) Year 2000 Position Statement:<br />

Principles and Guidelines for Early Hearing Detection and Intervention Programs<br />

described the importance of the medical home model, which allows pediatricians<br />

and PCPs to be the center of all care for the child:<br />

Pediatricians and other primary care physicians, working in partnership<br />

with parents and other health-care professionals, make up the infant’s<br />

“medical home.” A medical home is defined as an approach to providing<br />

308 McKay


health care services where care is accessible, family-centered, continuous,<br />

comprehensive, coordinated, compassionate, and culturally competent.<br />

Pediatricians act in partnership with parents in a medical home to identify<br />

and access services needed in developing a global plan of appropriate and<br />

necessary health and habilitative care for infants identified with hearing<br />

loss. (p. 801)<br />

The JCIH position statement also made the following statement about the role<br />

of the PCP:<br />

The infant’s pediatrician or other primary care physician is responsible for<br />

monitoring the general health and well-being of the infant. In addition, the<br />

primary care physician in partnership with the family and other health care<br />

professionals, assures that audiologic assessment is conducted on infants<br />

who do not pass screening and initiates referrals for medical specialty<br />

evaluations. (p. 804)<br />

The position statement made specific recommendations for medical referrals<br />

following the diagnosis of sensorineural hearing loss that include evaluations<br />

by otolaryngology; genetics; ophthalmology; developmental pediatrics; neurology;<br />

and, as needed, nephrology and cardiology. These recommendations<br />

are posted on many Web sites of otolaryngology departments in major medical<br />

centers across the country. These evaluations are further described here.<br />

Otologist/Otolaryngologist<br />

Children with UHL should be referred to an otolaryngologist to rule out<br />

outer or middle ear pathology, look at inner ear structures and screen for<br />

other associated conditions. The otolaryngologist may recommend the following<br />

tests:<br />

CT scan of the temporal bone to check for evidence of middle ear and<br />

inner ear anomalies. Findings from this scan may give information about<br />

whether the hearing loss may progress in one ear or both.<br />

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to check for inner ear malformations<br />

and status of the auditory nerve and pathways.<br />

Urinalysis to screen for associated kidney abnormalities.<br />

Echocardiogram (ECG) to rule out prolonged QT (the interval between<br />

two points [Q and T] on the common electrocardiogram) associated with<br />

Jervell and Lange Neilsen syndrome. An ECG sometimes is ordered for<br />

children with UHL but often is conducted for bilateral sensorineural<br />

hearing loss.<br />

Blood work if thyroid dysfunction, autoimmune disease or a specific<br />

infection is suspected.<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 309


Progression of hearing loss is a concern in children with UHL and warrants<br />

comprehensive baseline medical and audiologic testing and close audiologic<br />

monitoring. Hearing loss not only can progress in the impaired ear, but also<br />

can become bilateral. The CHIP currently collects information about the development<br />

of children with UHL. This group reported that close to 10% of<br />

participants had UHL that progressed to bilateral hearing loss (CHIP, 2005).<br />

In a review of the existing literature on children with UHL, Lieu (2004) noted<br />

a study reporting that that five out of 35 children with UHL were found to<br />

have progressive losses, and 11 had inner ear anomalies (Bamiou, Savy,<br />

O’Mahoney, Phelps, & Sirimanna, 1999). When examining characteristics of<br />

sensorineural hearing loss in children with inner ear anomalies, Coticchia,<br />

Gokhale, Waltonen and Sumer (2006) found that children with inner ear<br />

anomalies were most likely to have UHL. Another population of children at<br />

risk for progressive hearing loss are those with cytomegalovirus. Alerting<br />

physicians about the possible causes and incidence of progressive hearing<br />

loss will help them become aware of the importance of close audiologic<br />

monitoring and convey the importance of listening to parents if they express<br />

concern that their child’s hearing has changed. It is important for educational<br />

and therapeutic care providers of a child with UHL, who may be the first to<br />

detect subtle changes in a child’s auditory or speech abilities, to be informed<br />

of this risk as well.<br />

Alerting caregivers of children with UHL that outer and middle ear conditions<br />

may cause additional temporary hearing loss also is important. The<br />

JCIH (2000) made the following statement with regard to medical management<br />

of middle ear pathology:<br />

The impact of OME (otitis media with effusion) is greater for infants with<br />

sensorineural hearing loss than those with normal cochlear function. Sensory<br />

or permanent conductive hearing loss is compounded by additional<br />

conductive hearing loss associated with OME. OME further reduces access<br />

to auditory/oral language stimulation and spoken language development<br />

for infants whose families choose an auditory-oral approach to communication<br />

development. Prompt referral to otolaryngologists for treatment of<br />

persistent or recurrent OME is indicated in infants with sensorineural hearing<br />

loss. (p. 807)<br />

Most young children experience OME during their early childhood. An<br />

established relationship with an otolaryngologist will be helpful in vigilant<br />

management of OME in children with UHL.<br />

Genetics<br />

Children with sensorineural hearing loss should be referred to a geneticist<br />

to determine possible etiology or association with a syndrome. A comprehensive<br />

genetics evaluation will likely include:<br />

310 McKay


Dysmorphology examination<br />

Complete family and medical history<br />

Audiologic testing of parents and siblings<br />

Gene testing if syndrome is suspected<br />

Ophthalmology<br />

Every child with permanent hearing loss is referred to ophthalmology to<br />

ensure optimal visual acuity. Most known disorders that have both visual<br />

and hearing problems, such as Usher’s syndrome, are usually present only in<br />

children with bilateral hearing loss.<br />

Neurology<br />

A neurological examination is recommended only if hearing loss is considered<br />

to originate past the point of the outer hair cells of the cochlea (i.e.,<br />

auditory neuropathy, neural hearing loss or central disorder). Findings of<br />

abnormal ABR results in conjunction with present OAE would lead to this<br />

recommendation.<br />

Nephrology<br />

This referral is made only when kidney function is in question.<br />

Speech-Language Pathology<br />

Any child with UHL should be referred for an initial evaluation with a<br />

speech-language pathologist. Subsequent evaluations to monitor development<br />

also are recommended.<br />

Parental Education and Advocacy<br />

Upon receiving the news that their child has a UHL, parents will have<br />

immediate questions and concerns. Roush and Harrison (2002) found that<br />

after a child has been diagnosed with hearing loss, parents wanted to know<br />

about (1) the cause of the hearing loss, (2) how to cope with the emotional<br />

aspects of the hearing loss, (3) how to understand the audiogram, (4) how<br />

their child will learn to listen and speak and (5) the ear and hearing. These<br />

prioritized concerns were those of parents of children with newly identified<br />

bilateral hearing loss. Although the answers may not be the same for a child<br />

with newly diagnosed UHL, it is important to be aware of parents’ concerns.<br />

Providing information to parents at this point may alleviate some of their<br />

fears. Knowing that they will be able to advocate for their child by having the<br />

information they need can be empowering. Even if a child is not eligible for<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 311


ongoing services because of a specific state’s Part C eligibility restrictions, an<br />

initial evaluation and scheduled monitoring allows an opportunity for these<br />

parents to be informed. Ideally, parents should receive written information<br />

after the evaluations for their reference. This information should include<br />

ways to help their child at home and in his or her day care or preschool<br />

environment. Calderon (2000) looked at the outcomes of children with hearing<br />

loss and parent involvement and found that parental communication<br />

skills had an impact on the outcomes. Parents should be given information on<br />

normal development of speech and language milestones. Additionally, they<br />

may benefit from information on how to be a good language model for their<br />

child and stimulate language as well as the importance of monitoring language.<br />

The following are examples of information to share with parents:<br />

When you are holding or feeding your baby, try to make sure your<br />

child’s normal hearing ear is facing you.<br />

Always be aware of where your child’s normal hearing ear is facing. It<br />

should always be facing you or those talking to your child. Think about<br />

this when your child is at dinner, in the car or in her stroller, for example.<br />

Try to avoid letting your child’s normal hearing ear face a noise source,<br />

such as a dishwasher.<br />

Try to make eye contact and use facial expressions.<br />

Get your child’s attention before talking to him or her.<br />

Talk about what you are doing within your daily routine.<br />

Start conversations and take turns talking.<br />

Help your child localize sound by using visual cues.<br />

Use repetition.<br />

Look for cues that your child understands what you are saying.<br />

Expand your child’s vocabulary by using other adjectives and adverbs.<br />

Play listening games (auditory version of “I Spy”).<br />

Raise your voice slightly and face him or her when you are at a greater<br />

distance (walking your baby in the stroller).<br />

Parents should be aware of the listening situations in which their child may<br />

experience the most difficulty. These situations include listening in environments<br />

that are noisy or when there is distance between the parent and the<br />

child. Parents should know that if they call their child and give him or her<br />

instructions from another room, the child might hear them but not understand<br />

what was said.<br />

Parents should be guided on how to best advocate for their child with<br />

regard to early intervention and education if their child experiences difficulties.<br />

For example, in the state of Pennsylvania, information is compiled in a<br />

booklet by the Parent Education Network (2003) that outlines much of the<br />

information parents will need to know as they navigate the early intervention<br />

312 McKay


and educational systems. Topics covered include an overview of special education<br />

laws, explanation of the evaluation process, Individualized Family<br />

Service Plans and Individualized Education Programs (what to ask for), what<br />

to look for in an early intervention program, classroom strategies and appendices<br />

with useful definitions and checklists for parents.<br />

Education of Day Care and Preschool Providers<br />

The phrase “preferential seating” is familiar to all who work with children<br />

with hearing loss and is consistently recommended for children with UHL.<br />

Although preferential seating may be helpful, the recommendation is not<br />

always straightforward and may give caregivers the impression that it is the<br />

solution. We must think beyond preferential seating and the typical classroom.<br />

Parents should be informed about how to carefully choose day care<br />

providers and preschools and how to advocate for their child’s needs once the<br />

child is enrolled in a facility. Ideally, written information should be given to<br />

a care provider (day care or preschool teacher), and a hearing care specialist<br />

should meet with this person to explain how to best meet the needs of the<br />

child in his or her environment. Information about the difficulties older children<br />

with UHL may experience should be shared with teachers of younger<br />

children to alert them to potential problems. Examples of difficulties seen in<br />

some older children with UHL are fatigue, inattentiveness, frustration and<br />

behavior and social problems. Teachers can be given strategies to help the<br />

child in the day care or preschool environment, such as the following:<br />

Teach the children (and staff) in the class good “listening etiquette.”<br />

When one person is talking, everyone listens. Although this is not practical<br />

during play or free time, it should be exercised during structured<br />

lessons.<br />

Try to get the child’s attention by calling his or her name before giving<br />

instructions.<br />

Use a visual cue, such as pointing to the child who is to speak next. This<br />

will direct the child toward the direction in which to turn.<br />

Try to face the child when speaking.<br />

Be aware of the child’s placement in the class. Do not place him or her<br />

near a noise source, such as an air conditioner. Proximity is important;<br />

therefore, place the child near you and with his or her normal hearing ear<br />

facing you.<br />

Have a nonverbal cue that can be used to regain attention (i.e., tap on the<br />

shoulder followed by eye contact) if the child seems inattentive.<br />

Try to place noisy centers off on their own.<br />

Take interest in the child’s hearing aid or FM system (if applicable). Let<br />

the child share it at “show and tell.” Acquire and read children’s books<br />

that explain hearing loss and hearing aids.<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 313


Environmental and Acoustic Modifications<br />

Educating parents regarding the importance of good acoustic environments<br />

for their child will aid them in choosing appropriate day care or preschools.<br />

Crandell and Smaldino (2001) addressed the importance of acoustic<br />

friendly classrooms. Although most applications these authors noted were<br />

made to a typical grade school class, they can be implemented in day care and<br />

preschool classes as well. Some recommendations are as follows:<br />

Avoid day care programs and preschools that practice “open class”<br />

teaching with many classes within a large area. The child with UHL<br />

needs the least amount of competing noise.<br />

Request area rugs if there is not wall-to-wall carpeting.<br />

Use window treatments (thick materials).<br />

Avoid using hard surfaces whenever possible. Use an alternative such as<br />

a corkboard.<br />

Use tennis balls or rubber tips on the bottoms of chairs.<br />

In play or leisure areas, place soft seating such as bean bag chairs.<br />

Use creative artwork such as decorated egg crates, material or rug strips<br />

and Styrofoam balls hung from the ceiling.<br />

Place movable boards at an angle in the room.<br />

Turn off noisy equipment (i.e., computers, tape recorders) when not in<br />

use.<br />

Try to keep doors and windows closed.<br />

Audiologic Monitoring<br />

The importance of close monitoring must be stressed to all involved in the<br />

care of a child with UHL. Audiologic monitoring is needed to ensure that<br />

hearing sensitivity remains stable in both ears. It is particularly important in<br />

light of the high incidence of progressive hearing loss in this population.<br />

Speech and language monitoring may help to pick up subtle difficulties that<br />

a child may have and allow for remediation before a child is school age.<br />

Parents and teachers may find functional auditory measures helpful in tracking<br />

the ongoing auditory abilities of a child with UHL. Oticon, Inc., published<br />

a brochure that recommends specific measures based on age and degree of<br />

hearing loss (Tharpe & Flynn, 2005). Specific measures to be completed by<br />

parents and teachers for the birth-to-3 age group and the 3-to-5 age group are<br />

recommended. Close monitoring allows professionals to interact with parents,<br />

answer questions and raise awareness. Such opportunities only can be<br />

beneficial.<br />

Conclusion<br />

School-age children with UHL are known to be at risk for academic and<br />

speech-language delays. With the widespread implementation of UNHS,<br />

314 McKay


many children with congenital UHL are now being identified in their first<br />

months of life. Recently this population of children has drawn interest and<br />

focus from groups such as the CDC, the AAA, and ASHA. Information<br />

should continue to evolve with regard to best practices for young children<br />

with UHL. Future evidence will shape consistent policy in the best interest of<br />

these children. At The National Workshop for Mild and Unilateral Hearing<br />

Loss (2005), participants in working groups outlined future research needs, a<br />

brief sampling of which is as follows:<br />

Increase public and professional awareness of UHL.<br />

Develop recommendations on how to best inform parents about the<br />

potential outcomes of children with UHL.<br />

Collect outcome data on amplification for UHL (early versus late, FM<br />

versus hearing aid).<br />

Compare the effects of early FM system use, hearing aid use and no<br />

amplification on communication, educational and social-emotional outcomes<br />

in children with UHL.<br />

Develop more sensitive and age-appropriate outcome measures and<br />

functional assessments for the birth-to-3 age group.<br />

Conduct descriptive studies of successful early intervention systems;<br />

successful state systems can serve as useful models.<br />

Share research findings with state Part C coordinators for use in updating<br />

their eligibility criteria.<br />

Conduct randomized trials to compare outcomes for children who receive<br />

early intervention with those who do not.<br />

Investigate long-term outcomes to determine whether children from<br />

birth-to-3 who experience delays are the same children experiencing<br />

delays in school.<br />

Identify factors that predict successful speech, language and behavior<br />

outcomes.<br />

A complete list of future needs as well as the proceedings of this workshop<br />

can be found at www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/ehdi. Although questions remain<br />

about the best practice for infants and toddlers with UHL, early identification<br />

gives hearing health and early intervention professionals a unique “window<br />

of opportunity” not previously available. As EDHI programs’ current and<br />

future research begin to gather the evidence needed to establish consensus<br />

and mandates, those who care for children with UHL will be able to use this<br />

evidence to develop and implement practices and recommendations that will<br />

optimally benefit these children. As questions are answered over the coming<br />

years, it will be critical to reinforce the need to share the results with the Part<br />

C coordinators in order to effect changes in eligibility criteria. Although it is<br />

important to acknowledge there are little data regarding outcomes for very<br />

young children with UHL, there are strategies that may mitigate potential<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 315


negative effects of unilateral hearing. We do have recommendations for parents<br />

on how to help their child now. We know that early intervention is<br />

beneficial to children with hearing loss, and we know that parental involvement<br />

positively influences outcomes. By continuing to educate parents and<br />

those who work with or care for a child with UHL, we will enable these<br />

individuals to better help the child, advocate for the child and teach the child<br />

how to advocate for him- or herself. Considering the potential deleterious<br />

effects of UHL, professionals must be responsible for proactively educating<br />

parents and caregivers so that all children with UHL have the best chance for<br />

success.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

The author wishes to thank Judith Gravel, Ph.D., Carol Knightly, Au.D.,<br />

and Anne Marie Tharpe, Ph.D., for their ongoing support and guidance in her<br />

interest in unilateral hearing loss in children.<br />

References<br />

American Academy of Audiology. (2003). Pediatric amplification protocol. Retrieved<br />

February 1, 2005, from www.audiology.org/professionals/position<br />

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2006). Access audiology.<br />

Current trends: Mild and unilateral hearing loss in children. Rockville, MD:<br />

American Speech-Language- Hearing Association.<br />

Bamiou, D.E., Savy, L., O’Mahoney, C., Phelps, P., & Sirimanna, T. (1999).<br />

Unilateral sensorineural hearing loss and its aetiology in childhood: The<br />

contribution of computerized tomography in aetiological diagnosis and<br />

management. International Journal of Pediatric Otolaryngology, 51, 91-99.<br />

Bess, F.H., & Tharpe, A.M. (1984). Unilateral hearing impairment in children.<br />

Pediatrics, 74, 206-216.<br />

Bess, F.H., & Tharpe A.M. (1986). Case history data on unilaterally hearing<br />

impaired children. Ear and Hearing, 7(1), 14-17.<br />

Bess, F.H., Dodd-Murphy, J.D., & Parker, R.A. (1998). Children with minimal<br />

sensorineural hearing loss: Prevalence, educational performance, and functional<br />

health status. Ear and Hearing, 17(1), 1-11.<br />

Brookhauser, P.E., Worthington, D.W., & Kelly, W.J. (1991, December). Unilateral<br />

hearing loss in children. Laryngoscope, 101, 1264-1272.<br />

Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children’s education programs<br />

as a predictor of child’s language, early reading, and socialemotional<br />

development. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 140-<br />

155.<br />

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005). Proceedings of the National<br />

Workshop on Mild and Unilateral Hearing Loss. Available at www.cdc.gov/<br />

ncbddd/ehdi.<br />

316 McKay


Colorado Home Intervention Program. (2005). Services to children with unilateral<br />

hearing loss. Retrieved September 10, 2005, from www.csdb.org/chip.<br />

Coticchia, J., Gokhale, A., Waltonen, J., & Sumer, B. (2006). Characteristics of<br />

sensorineural hearing loss in children with inner ear anomalies. American<br />

Journal of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Medicine and Surgery, 27, 33-38.<br />

Crandell, C.C., & Smaldino, J.J. (2001). Acoustic modifications for the classroom.<br />

Classroom acoustics: Understanding barriers to learning. The Volta<br />

Review, 101(5), 33-46.<br />

Culbertson, J.L., & Gilbert, L.E. (1986). Children with unilateral sensorineural<br />

hearing loss: Cognitive, academic and social development. Ear and Hearing,<br />

7, 38-42.<br />

Dancer, J., Burl, N., & Waters, S. (1995). Effects of unilateral hearing loss on<br />

teacher responses to the SIFTER. American Annals of the Deaf, 140(3), 291-<br />

294.<br />

Dengerink, J.E., & Porter, J.B. (1984). Children’s attitudes toward peers wearing<br />

hearing aids. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 15, 199-<br />

204.<br />

Directors of Speech and Hearing Programs in State Health and Welfare Agencies.<br />

(2003). Retrieved August 8, 2006, from Current Trends: Mild and<br />

Unilateral Hearing Loss in Children, ASHA Online, www.asha.org/<br />

members/aud/access-aud-online/AAUD0306.<br />

Emmer, M.B. (1999). Review of late-onset auditory deprivation and clinical<br />

implications. Hearing Journal, 52(11), 26-30.<br />

English, K., & Church, G. (1999). Unilateral hearing loss in children: An<br />

update for the 1990s. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 30,<br />

26-31.<br />

Flexer, C. (1995). Classroom management of children with minimal hearing<br />

loss. Hearing Journal, 48(9), 10-13.<br />

Gravel, J., White, K., Johnson, J., Widen, J., Vohr, B., & James, M. (2005). A<br />

multisite study to examine the efficacy of the Otoacoustic Emission/<br />

Automated Auditory Brainstem Response newborn hearing screening protocol:<br />

Recommendations for policy, practice, and research. American Journal<br />

of Audiology, 14, 217-228.<br />

Johnson, J.L., White, K.R. Widen, J.E., Gravel, J.S., James, M., Kennalley, T., et<br />

al. (2005). A multicenter evaluation of how many infants with permanent<br />

hearing loss pass a two-stage Otoacoustic Emissions/Automated Auditory<br />

Brainstem Response newborn hearing screening protocol. Pediatrics, 116,<br />

663-672.<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. (2000). Year 2000 position statement:<br />

Principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs.<br />

Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, 798-817.<br />

Kenworthy, O.T., Klee, T., & Tharpe, A.M. (1990). Speech recognition ability<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 317


of children with unilateral sensorineural hearing loss as a function of amplification,<br />

speech stimuli and listening condition. Ear and Hearing, 11, 264-<br />

270.<br />

Kiese-Himmel, C. (2002). Unilateral sensorineural hearing impairment in<br />

childhood: Analysis of 31 consecutive cases. International Journal of Audiology,<br />

41, 57-63.<br />

Klee, T., & Davis-Dansky, E. (1986). A comparison of unilaterally hearingimpaired<br />

children and normal-hearing children on a battery of standardized<br />

language tests. Ear and Hearing, 7, 27-37.<br />

Lieu, J.E. (2004). Speech-language and educational consequences of unilateral<br />

hearing loss in children. Archives of Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery,<br />

130, 524-530.<br />

McKay, S. (April 2002). To aid or not to aid: Children with unilateral hearing loss.<br />

Poster presented at the annual American Academy of Audiology convention,<br />

Philadelphia, PA.<br />

Moeller, M.P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children<br />

who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, 43-50.<br />

Morrongiello, B.A. (1989). Infants’ monaural localization of sounds: Effects of<br />

unilateral ear infection. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 597-<br />

602.<br />

National Center for Hearing Assessment and Management. Retrieved<br />

August 18, 2006, from www.infanthearing.org/summary/summary<br />

.html#efficiency.<br />

Neault, M. (2005, July). Unilateral hearing loss: Unilateral refers may show bilateral<br />

hearing loss. Paper presented at the Centers for Disease Control and<br />

Prevention National Workshop on Mild and Unilateral Hearing Loss,<br />

Breckenridge, CO.<br />

Niskar, A., Kieszak, S., Holmes, A., Esteban, E., Rubin, C., & Brody, D. (1998).<br />

Prevalence of hearing loss among children 6 to 19 years of age: The third<br />

National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. JAMA, 279, 1071-1075.<br />

Oyler, R.F., Oyler, A.L., & Matkin, N.D. (1988). Unilateral hearing loss: Demographics<br />

and educational impact. Language, Speech Hearing Services in<br />

Schools, 19, 201-209.<br />

Parent Education Network. (2003). . . . But what about my deaf child? A guide to<br />

special education in Pennsylvania for parents of children who are deaf or hard of<br />

hearing. York, PA: Author. (Available from pen@parentdnet.org)<br />

Prieve, B., Dalzell, L., Berg, A., Bradley, M., Cacace, A., Campbell, D., et al.<br />

(2000). The New York State Universal Newborn Hearing Screening Demonstration<br />

Project: Outpatient outcome measures. Ear and Hearing, 21, 104-<br />

117.<br />

Riensche, L., Peterson, K., & Linden, S. (1990). Young children’s attitudes<br />

toward peer hearing aid wearers. Hearing Journal, 43(10), 19-20.<br />

Roush, J. (2000). What happens after screening? Hearing Journal, 53(11), 56-60.<br />

318 McKay


Roush, J., & Harrison, M. (2002). What parents want to know at diagnosis and<br />

during the first year. Hearing Journal, 55(11), 52-54.<br />

Tharpe, A.M., & Flynn, T. (2005). Incorporating functional auditory measures into<br />

pediatric practice [Brochure]. (Available from Oticon, Inc., 29 Schoolhouse<br />

Rd. Somerset, NJ 07724)<br />

Updike, C.D. (1994). Comparison of FM auditory trainers, CROS aids and<br />

personal amplification in unilaterally hearing impaired children. Journal of<br />

the American Academy of Audiology, 5, 204-209.<br />

Wazen, J.J., Ghossani, S.N., Kacker, A., & Zschommler, A. (2001). Results of<br />

bone-anchored hearing aid in unilateral hearing loss. Laryngoscope, 111,<br />

955-958.<br />

Wazen, J.J., Ghossani, S.N., Spitzer, J.B., & Kuller, M. (2005). Localization by<br />

unilateral BAHA users. Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, 132, 928-932.<br />

Wazen, J.J., Spitzer, J.B., Ghossani, S.N., Fayad, J.N., Niparko, J.K., Cox, K., et<br />

al. (2003). Transcranial contralateral cochlear stimulation in unilateral deafness.<br />

Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, 129, 248-254.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2005, July). Language, speech and social-emotional outcomes<br />

on infants and children with mild and unilateral hearing loss and their families.<br />

Paper presented at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention National<br />

Workshop on Mild and Unilateral Hearing Loss, Breckenridge, CO.<br />

Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing Loss 319


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 321-341<br />

Early Spanish Speech<br />

Acquisition Following<br />

Cochlear Implantation<br />

Jan Allison Moore, Ph.D., Scott Prath, M.A., and<br />

Adrianne Arrieta, M.A.<br />

The purpose of this paper is to document the early Spanish speech development of<br />

a toddler, K, whose cochlear implant was activated at age 20 months. The contribution<br />

of K’s developmental pattern to research is unique in that Spanish is the sole<br />

language of the home, as well as being the language of intervention up to 16 months<br />

postimplant (3 years chronological age). Early organization of consonant-vowel<br />

structures was consistent with other cochlear implant (CI) recipients who were learning<br />

English and generally support the theoretical perspective that the oral motor<br />

system has a fundamental role in the organization of early speech production. Overall<br />

accuracy of speech of words at 12 months postimplant showed that K had substantially<br />

greater word accuracy compared to English-speaking CI peers with the same age<br />

of implantation and device. These results highlight the phonetic and phonologic<br />

differences between English and Spanish which have direct clinical application in the<br />

provision of speech intervention services to children whose first language is Spanish.<br />

Introduction<br />

Speech and hearing professionals in the United States often have limited<br />

exposure in working with families and children from multilingual homes or<br />

from monolingual homes in which English is not spoken. This exposure is<br />

even more limited in low-incidence disabilities in childhood such as hearing<br />

loss. In addition, most graduate training programs do not routinely offer<br />

Jan Allison Moore, Ph.D., is an assistant professor in the Department of Communication<br />

Sciences and Disorders at the University of Texas at Austin. Scott Prath, M.A., completed his<br />

graduate degree in communication sciences and disorders at the University of Texas at Austin.<br />

He currently works with Spanish-speaking early childhood intervention and school-age populations<br />

in central Texas. Adrianne Arrieta, M.A., received a master’s degree in communication<br />

sciences and disorders from the University of Texas at Austin. She currently works as<br />

a public school-licensed bilingual speech-language pathologist in Pflugerville, Texas.<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 321


specific information regarding differences and benchmarks of communicative<br />

development of languages other than English.<br />

This paper has two primary purposes. First, it will serve as an introductory<br />

tutorial of differences between Spanish and English in the area of early speech<br />

development. Second, this paper will discuss emerging speech patterns and<br />

accuracy in a child, K, whose implant was activated at age 20 months and for<br />

whom Spanish is his first language. Our study focuses on Spanish, but it<br />

should be noted that K also began to develop English within the educational<br />

system. It is hoped that speech-language pathologists, audiologists and teachers<br />

of children with hearing loss will have increased awareness of the process<br />

of acculturation of new immigrant families in the United States. This knowledge<br />

should positively impact the ways in which we work with families from<br />

minority backgrounds.<br />

First, an overview of typical speech development will be presented along<br />

with the theoretical background of the Frame Content Theory (MacNeilage &<br />

Davis, 2000). The Frame Content Theory (FC) is the theoretical perspective<br />

that drives our analysis of early speech. This will be followed by some general<br />

statistics regarding Spanish language use in the United States and specific<br />

information on typical development of speech in Spanish, speech development<br />

in children with hearing loss and speech development in children with<br />

cochlear implants (CI). This will be followed by the details of our case study<br />

and implications for service delivery for families from Spanish-speaking<br />

homes.<br />

Review of the Literature<br />

Typical Development of Prelinguistic Vocalization Patterns<br />

The development of the sound system leading up to the production of<br />

words has been well documented (Oller, 1980; Stark, 1980; Vihman, Macken,<br />

Miller, Simmons, & Miller, 1985; Vihman, Ferguson, & Elbert, 1986). The stage<br />

that is of particular importance to this study is the development of true<br />

consonant-vowel (CV) syllables around age 7 months. This stage, the canonical<br />

babbling stage, is significant because the vocalizations sound much more<br />

“speech-like” in that they are syllabic in nature. Canonical babbling lays the<br />

foundation for the rapid timing of syllable production, which is present in all<br />

languages, and the basis for word production. Canonical babbling is defined<br />

as rhythmic production of consonant-vowel (CV) syllables. The syllable<br />

strings in canonical babbling may be reduplicated or variegated. Reduplicated<br />

babbling (e.g., “dudududu”) was once thought to precede variegated<br />

babbling (e.g., “dadi”) but these two forms of syllable production arise during<br />

the same time period (Stark, 1980) rather than sequential stages of development.<br />

322 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


Theories on Speech Development<br />

Theories vary widely accounting for how and why the speech mechanism<br />

develops to eventually produce meaningful speech. Although it is now generally<br />

accepted that sound qualities in canonical babbling are continuous with<br />

sound qualities in the early word period (Vihman et al. 1985), there are<br />

varying theories as to why preverbal vocalizations and canonical babbling<br />

take place.<br />

The theoretical perspective of this research is one that accounts for the<br />

dependable and universal nature of babbling and the maturation of the oralmotor<br />

system. Davis and colleagues (Davis, MacNeilage, & Matyear, 2002)<br />

addressed these issues biologically in terms of the Frame Content Theory. The<br />

“frame” or structure of speech is provided by the rhythmic oscillation of the<br />

mandible. The “content” (consonants and vowels) of an utterance is placed<br />

within a frame. The FC Theory is based on knowledge of the evolution of<br />

speech and language. The movement of the frame is believed to have<br />

emerged from reflexive motions of ingestion such as eating and sucking.<br />

Vowels (V) evolve from the open aspect of mandibular movements, whereas<br />

the closing phase creates a consonant (C).<br />

At the canonical babbling stage, the tongue and lips do not operate independently<br />

of the rhythmic cycles of the mandible. “Changes in amplitude of<br />

the mandibular cycle would result in height changes for vowels (e.g., [ae]<br />

verses [i]) and manner changes for consonants (e.g., [d] versus [j])” (Davis &<br />

MacNeilage, 1995, p. 3). Further, these researchers state that alternations between<br />

oral and nasal sounds appear when control is gained over soft palate<br />

closure. The basis of babbling exists within the timing and development of the<br />

action and not in descriptions of phonological representations.<br />

Biological assertions relating physical movements to speech cannot be language<br />

specific or dependent. That is to say, speech development is universally<br />

challengeable, and similar outcomes result across an array of ambient<br />

(native) languages. MacNeilage and colleagues (MacNeilage, Davis, Kinney,<br />

& Matyear, 2000) suggest that in early development an infant babbles in<br />

similar patterns regardless of ambient language. In their study on serial organization<br />

in infants across languages, consistencies in babbling productions<br />

were explored across six populations (English, French, Swedish, Japanese,<br />

Brazilian Portuguese and Quichua). Later, Lee (2003) investigated this phenomenon<br />

in Korean. Results of these studies yielded patterns of syllable<br />

organization in words and babbling that contain some universal consistencies<br />

(MacNeilage et al., 2000). These consistencies refer to the distribution of preferred<br />

syllable shapes seen in typically developing children with normal hearing.<br />

These CV syllable shapes include the “pure frame” or labial consonant<br />

(e.g., b, m) followed by a central vowel (e.g., a), the dorsal consonant (e.g., k,<br />

g) followed by the back vowel (e.g., u, o) and the coronal consonant (e.g., t, d,<br />

s) followed by a front vowel (e.g., e, ae). The “pure frame” that occurs when<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 323


labial consonants are paired with central vowels are considered “pure” in the<br />

sense that the sound is solely the result of mandibular movement. The consonant<br />

is formed as the lips come together, and the vowel is formed while the<br />

tongue is in a resting position (MacNeilage & Davis, 2000).<br />

Syllables first appear in an infant’s repertoire as monosyllables (Vihman et<br />

al., 1985). Normally these syllables begin with a consonant and end with a<br />

vowel (MacNeilage et al., 2000). MacNeilage and his colleagues also state that<br />

the CV sequence is so important that it is “often given the status of the only<br />

universal syllable type” (MacNeilage et al., 2000, p. 154). Following the appearance<br />

of these syllables in isolation, an infant begins to reduplicate (Oller,<br />

1980).<br />

Reduplicated syllable production (babbling) is the result of repeated oscillations<br />

of the mandible. Mandibular movements appear earlier than lingual<br />

or labial movements in canonical babbling because oscillation is easiest when<br />

the articulators stay in one place (Davis & MacNeilage, 1995; Davis et al.,<br />

2002; MacNeilage & Davis, 2000; MacNeilage et al., 2000). Initially, 50% of the<br />

total syllable is reduplicated and 67% of the consonants are reduplicated<br />

(Davis & MacNeilage, 1995). As the child starts to produce his or her first<br />

words, 30% of the total syllables are reduplicated and 73% of the consonants<br />

are reduplicated (MacNeilage, Davis & Matyear, 1997). As variegated babble<br />

leads into full words, there is very little reduplication in the child’s attempts<br />

at speech (MacNeilage, et al., 2000).<br />

In summary, prelinguistic babbling is the biological result of mandibular<br />

oscillations that are accompanied by phonation (Davis & MacNeilage, 1995;<br />

MacNeilage & Davis, 2000; MacNeilage et al., 2000). Many of these syllable<br />

patterns that are created during babbling are universal, without influence of<br />

ambient language (MacNeilage et al., 2000). This being said, an explanation of<br />

speech development, even for a Spanish-speaking child, would be generic up<br />

until babbling makes the transition into first words (Oller, 1980; Locke, 1983).<br />

With the development of first words we would expect sound sequences intrinsic<br />

to Spanish to emerge.<br />

In regard to children with hearing loss, Oller and Eilers (1988) highlighted<br />

the necessity of audibility in the development of true canonical babbling<br />

defined both by the CV nature of production and also the rapid timing of the<br />

syllables and rapid transitions between consonant and vowel. A few studies<br />

have documented the development of syllables in children following cochlear<br />

implantation (McCaffrey, Davis, MacNeilage, & von Hapsburg, 2000; Moore,<br />

Davis, & Tobey, 2004; von Hapsburg, 2003). CIs provide audibility to the<br />

infant, albeit in a compromised form, in terms of temporal, frequency and<br />

loudness resolution. These preliminary studies have described in detail the<br />

emergence of speech patterns in a small number of children who have received<br />

implants. Overall, early syllable development of these children with<br />

CIs have supported the notion of frame dominance in their syllable organization;<br />

however, other preferred patterns of syllables were also observed,<br />

324 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


indicating an interaction of a normal oral-motor system, an impaired auditory<br />

system and the effect of learning speech via direct intervention.<br />

Although FC has been supported via studies of both typically developing<br />

children with normal hearing and those with hearing loss, the theory, to our<br />

knowledge, has yet to be tested on a child in which hearing loss and language<br />

difference co-occur. The universal nature that the FC Theory proposes will be<br />

further scrutinized in the discussion of our participant’s vocalization patterns<br />

in Spanish. Therefore, our first question will be: How does K’s syllable development<br />

relate to the results seen with other children with implants and<br />

children from different language groups? With this in mind we will examine<br />

highlights of typical development in Spanish.<br />

Typical Development of Speech in Spanish<br />

The population of Spanish speakers worldwide is approximately 322 million<br />

(Encarta, 2004). In the United States alone, 10% (35 million) of the population<br />

is Hispanic, and this number is projected to double by 2030 (U.S.<br />

Census Bureau, 2000). In the states that border Mexico, the percentage of<br />

speakers using Spanish as their primary language often exceeds 30% of the<br />

population. These demographic changes have increased the possibility that<br />

an interventionist or researcher will come into contact with Spanish and<br />

Spanish-speaking cultures. This becomes problematic when “most speechlanguage<br />

pathologists are not Spanish speakers, [and] may not be sufficiently<br />

aware of the important phonological differences [of] Spanish” (Goldstein &<br />

Iglesias, 1996, p. 82). The international and domestic research communities<br />

have responded with a plethora of information concerning Spanish developmental<br />

tendencies, including work by Acevado, Bedore, Fernandez, Goldstein,<br />

Iglesias, Jimenez, Quilis and Washington.<br />

Phonology<br />

The phonologies of English and Spanish have many similarities (Quilis &<br />

Fernandez, 1999). Although both languages utilize the same alphabet, the<br />

Spanish phonetic system is more concise compared to English. Spanish has 18<br />

consonant phonemes compared to 26 in English (Goldstein & Iglesias, 1996).<br />

These totals do not include allophonic or dialectal variations within either<br />

language. Spanish lacks the glottal stop, the voiced affricate (judge), the<br />

voiced /ð/ and unvoiced // (thigh, thy), the voiced /ʒ/ (azure) and unvoiced<br />

/ʃ/ (shy), the /z/, the /ŋ/ (sing), and the flap /ɾ/ (as in butter).<br />

English, on the other hand, does not employ the trilled /rr/ or the (canyon)<br />

of Spanish (Quilis & Fernandez, 1999).<br />

Spanish Vowels<br />

The Spanish vowel space is like most Latin languages, relying on five<br />

vowels /a/, /e/, /i/, /o/ and /u/ (compared to 13 in English). Diphthongs<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 325


are as prevalent in Spanish as they are in English. A Spanish diphthong is the<br />

combination of one hard vowel (/a/, /o/, /u/) and one weak vowel (/i/,<br />

/e/) or the combination of two weak vowels (Quilis & Fernandez, 1999).<br />

When vowels and consonants form syllables, speech patterns that are found<br />

in Spanish can be identified through studying the order of phoneme acquisition,<br />

syllable shape, word length and phonological processes.<br />

Defining normal phonological acquisition for Spanish speakers is difficult<br />

due to the diversity of the populations that are often studied (Goldstein &<br />

Washington, 2001). In English, dialectal variations are generally defined by<br />

vowel differences (Goldstein & Iglesias, 1996). Contrarily, Spanish dialectal<br />

changes are marked by shifts in consonantal classes such as fricatives, liquids,<br />

glides and nasals (Goldstein & Iglesias, 1996). Nevertheless, some similar<br />

trends can be generalized throughout.<br />

Spanish Consonants<br />

There is consensus among many researchers that nearly all phonemes in<br />

Spanish are acquired by four years of age (Acevedo, 1993; Jimenez, 1987;<br />

Anderson & Smith, 1987; Goldstein & Washington, 2001). In one example,<br />

Acevado (1993) found that Mexican-American children acquire all phonemes<br />

by age four except /j/, /l/, // and /s/. Mann and Hodson (1994) cite<br />

Terroro’s 1979 study of Venezuelan children in which he found that after age<br />

four, only /j/, /l/, //, /s/, /r/ and // were not mastered. The differences<br />

regarding outlying phonemes could be the result of differences between the<br />

children studied (Anderson & Smith, 1987), dialectal differences between the<br />

populations (Goldstein & Iglesias, 1996) or variations in the studies themselves<br />

(Acevedo, 1993; Jimenez, 1987). Universally, stop consonants were<br />

acquired first, followed by nasals, then fricatives and affricates, with liquids<br />

being the last sounds to appear (Goldstein & Washington, 2001). This suggests<br />

that K’s development in Spanish should resemble the progression of<br />

sound acquisition of English once auditory input begins.<br />

Syllable Structure in Spanish<br />

The syllable structures in Spanish are highly dominated by CV sequences<br />

(Goldstein & Cintron, 2001). This is a crosslinguistic phenomenon in babbling<br />

(MacNeilage et al., 2000), but Spanish retains the CV-dominated syllable type<br />

in the adult language (Quilis & Fernandez, 1999). Isolated vowels are second<br />

most regular (18%), and syllables that end with consonants (CVC, VC, CCVC)<br />

are the least common (Goldstein & Cintron, 2001). This syllable structure is<br />

vastly different from English, which is dominated by CVC syllable structure.<br />

Syllable Complexity in Spanish<br />

The average length of words in any language is directly related to syllable<br />

structure. Basically, when a language does not support final consonants, such<br />

326 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


as Spanish, automatically there are a limited number of one-syllable words.<br />

The number of one-syllable words would be restricted to the finite combination<br />

of C+V (Quilis & Fernandez, 1999). This is clearly evident in Spanish.<br />

Goldstein and Cintron (2001) used syllable count to determine word length of<br />

Spanish-speaking children and noted that 83% of the words created were two<br />

syllables in length.<br />

Development of Speech in Children with Hearing Loss<br />

A great deal of research has focused on the role of audition as it relates to<br />

the development of speech (Oller, Eilers, Bull, & Carney, 1985; Yoshinaga-<br />

Itano, Stredler-Brown, & Jancosek, 1992; Steffens, Eilers, Fishman, Oller, &<br />

Urbano, 1994; Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998a, 1998b; Oller & Eilers, 1988; Stoel-<br />

Gammon & Otomo, 1986; Ertmer et al., 2002; Tye-Murray & Kirk, 1993; Kirk,<br />

Sehgal, & Miyamoto, 1997; Tobey, Geers, & Brunner, 1994). Children receive<br />

greater access to language due to discoveries concerning early diagnosis,<br />

treatment and advances in hearing aid fitting techniques (Yoshinaga-Itano,<br />

1998a). Identifying relationships between speech and audition has hastened<br />

and improved therapy (Yoshinaga-Itano et al., 1992). Demonstrating the advantages<br />

of early exposure to the sound system validated the initial “experimental”<br />

attempts at cochlear implantation (Niparko & Wilson, 2000).<br />

It is not enough to say that audition and speech are uniquely intertwined<br />

(Wallace, 1998). Some components of speech and audition develop symbiotically<br />

where one has to be present for the other to exist. Other aspects of<br />

speech and audition develop independently and have been shown to be<br />

present when the other is absent (Yoshinaga-Itano et al., 1992). The interaction<br />

of speech and hearing systems may also differ across time (Oller, 1980),<br />

depending on severity of the hearing loss and concomitant problems (Yoshinaga-Itano<br />

et al., 1992; Wallace, 1998). Additionally, a loss of hearing affects<br />

each parameter of speech (e.g. prosody, perception) in a unique way (Oller &<br />

Eilers, 1988; Oller et al., 1985). A timeline of vocal development assists in<br />

highlighting how speech develops in a child that is hearing impaired.<br />

There has been considerable debate as to whether children who have a<br />

hearing loss vocalize in the same manner as children with normal hearing. As<br />

Oller and Eilers (1988) describe in their study on audition and babbling, this<br />

confusion is the result of treating vocalizations as one developmental period.<br />

When infant vocalizations are categorized into precanonical and canonical<br />

utterances, the relation that speech has to hearing becomes more clearly defined<br />

(Oller & Eilers, 1988). The focus of this particular study is on canonical<br />

vocalizations; therefore, the literature that highlights differences between<br />

typically developing children with normal hearing and those with hearing<br />

loss at the canonical stage will be presented.<br />

Infants with a severe-to-profound hearing loss do not develop the same<br />

canonical vocal patterns that are innate in typically developing children<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 327


(Yoshinage-Itano et al., 1992; Steffens et al., 1994; Oller et al., 1985;<br />

Stoel-Gammon & Otomo, 1986). The delay in onset of babble is the most<br />

notable difference. Steffens and colleagues (1994) reported that in infants with<br />

profound hearing loss, canonical vocalizations do not normally appear until<br />

the twelfth month and have been reported to appear as late as the 31st month<br />

(Steffens et al., 1994). In addition, the acoustic characteristics of the timing of<br />

the consonant-vowel components of the syllable are longer than occur in<br />

typically developing children (Oller & Eilers, 1988).<br />

The onset of canonical babble marks a point at which true divisions can be<br />

drawn between typically developing speech in children with normal hearing<br />

and speech produced by a child with a hearing loss. Aberrations in vocal<br />

development are present in the number of productions and the types of<br />

productions, as well as suprasegmental properties such as prosody and duration.<br />

Differences in organization of vocalizations related to lack of syllable<br />

productions in infants with profound hearing impairment can be more clearly<br />

related to a reduction or absence of hearing. This fundamental difference<br />

alters a child’s transition to speech (Wallace, 1998).<br />

Degree of Hearing Loss<br />

The degree of hearing loss is the greatest contributing factor to the impairment<br />

of speech development (Yoshinaga-Itano et al., 1992; Yoshinaga-Itano,<br />

1998b; Wallace 1998). There is a magnitude of difference in speech abilities in<br />

children who operate across the mild-moderate-severe hearing loss continuum<br />

(Stoel-Gammon & Otomo, 1986). Even a minimal amount of residual<br />

hearing greatly improves a child’s chances at producing accurate speech<br />

when compared to a child with no access to the sound system. Degree of<br />

hearing loss is the most reliable source for predicting speech deficits and<br />

intelligibility later in childhood (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998b; Wallace, 1998). Babbling<br />

in the first year of life is often the focus of studies on speech and<br />

intelligibility of children who are deaf. However, many children who babble<br />

at levels similar to typically developing children with normal hearing do not<br />

develop intelligible speech (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998b). In her study of 20 children<br />

with HI, Wallace (1998) found that degree of hearing loss was the greatest<br />

predictor of intelligibility and speech ability later in life. At 5-10 years of<br />

age, 82% of those children judged to have a mild-to-severe hearing loss were<br />

intelligible. Contrarily, no child with a severe hearing loss was intelligible.<br />

Wallace concluded that “an exclusive focus on canonical babble . . . will not<br />

provide a full understanding of the development of speech in the deaf and<br />

hard of hearing population because looking at canonical babble alone misses<br />

the issue” (Wallace, 1998, p. 131). Unfortunately, the claims of this study were<br />

confounded by poor methodology. Certainly one cannot predict overall intelligibility<br />

or speech deficits later in childhood from babbling; however,<br />

canonical babbling lays the foundation for speech timing and coarticulation<br />

that is fundamental to speech production.<br />

328 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


In summary, young children with severe-to-profound deafness exhibit<br />

speech, which is characterized by short, monosyllabic utterances that primarily<br />

consist of bilabial and nasal sounds. The speech may be arrhythmic, varying<br />

in amplitude and reliant on sounds that provide visual or tactile feedback.<br />

The degree of hearing loss greatly influences speech, and in the absence of<br />

hearing there is little to no chance of developing an intelligible speech system.<br />

Understanding this, cochlear implantation has succeeded in giving a child<br />

with profound deafness access to the speech spectrum. The advances in this<br />

field are perpetual and increasingly more accurate. Children who have never<br />

received benefit from amplification usually begin to hear, and their development<br />

is as unique as it is extraordinary. Research has begun to chronicle<br />

speech development along the path from deafness to hearing. Unfortunately,<br />

most of the existing data has been restricted to English-speaking populations.<br />

Development of Early Speech in Children with Cochlear Implants<br />

Much available research describes children on a case-by-case basis, with a<br />

few studies doing comparisons across individuals (Geers & Tobey, 1992). In<br />

comparison studies, however, it is not easy to correlate data because each<br />

child differs in age of implantation, severity of hearing loss, concomitant<br />

issues (e.g., motor impairment), preexisting speech and pre- and postlingual<br />

deafness (Kirk, 2000). However, data on children with cochlear implants can<br />

be collectively summarized to provide projections of speech development.<br />

Additionally, those individual differences that confound studies have been<br />

found to serve as predictors of later intelligibility (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998a).<br />

Speech Acquisition<br />

Cochlear implantation improves the rate at which a child with hearing loss<br />

moves through early speech developmental stages (Ertmer et al., 2002a, Ertmer,<br />

Leonard & Pachuilo, 2002b; Moore & Bass-Ringdahl, 2002). In a study on<br />

vocal development in young children with cochlear implants, Ertmer et al.<br />

(2002b) documented the speech development of two implanted children.<br />

Eighty-five percent of one participant’s vocalizations were precanonical prior<br />

to implantation and only 14% one year after. The second subject’s development<br />

was less dramatic but still showed a proportional increase in canonical<br />

sounds following implantation. On the average, children implanted at 18<br />

months of age begin canonical babble at 6.5 months postimplant (Moore &<br />

Bass-Ringdahl, 2002).<br />

Upon moving into the canonical stage, children with cochlear implants<br />

tend to acquire sounds in relatively the same manner and order as hearing<br />

children (McCaffrey et al., 1999). This is contrary to infants with profound<br />

deafness wearing hearing aids who typically exhibit delayed and reduced<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 329


vocal productions (Oller et al., 1985); however, it should be noted that advances<br />

in hearing aid technology and early identification may also lead to<br />

more normal sequences of development than the set of children reported by<br />

Oller in the mid 1980s. Ertmer and Mellon (2001) followed the vocal development<br />

of a child implanted at 20 months. Their study measured the vocal<br />

development of a child with a cochlear implant to see how her gains compared<br />

to the rate of typical development. Results showed utterance productions<br />

resembling typical developmental patterns but which began later than<br />

normal (postimplant). The fact that the child’s progression of vocal skills<br />

occurred at a rate faster than reported in typical development supports the<br />

notion that implantation during a linguistically rich period can aid in the<br />

acquisition of language (Ertmer & Mellon, 2001).<br />

One of the greatest predictors for determining later developing intelligibility<br />

is identifying the degree of hearing loss (Yoshinaga Itano, 1998b; Geers,<br />

2002; Geers & Tobey, 1992). Yoshinaga-Itano found that “children with profound<br />

hearing loss had significantly poorer speech than children with mild<br />

through severe hearing loss” (1998b, p. 220). Added to this, early onset of<br />

hearing loss has devastating effects on speaking skills (Geers & Tobey, 1992).<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano (1998b) suggests that even a minor amount of residual hearing<br />

vastly improves speech output. She states that the difference is so great<br />

that only two categories of hearing loss are needed: hearing impaired (mild<br />

through severe) and profound.<br />

Age of Onset, Age of Implantation<br />

The age of onset and the age that a child is implanted dramatically alter the<br />

time frame in which a child is learning to speak (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1998b;<br />

Ertmer et al, 2002; Blamey et al., 2001). Ertmer et al. (2002b) present data on<br />

two children who received cochlear implants. Participant D lost his hearing<br />

at age 3 years and was implanted at 7 years, 6 months. Participant B was<br />

identified with hearing loss at age 5 months and was implanted at age 3.<br />

D showed higher preimplant speech variability, probably due to three years<br />

of hearing exposure. After being implanted, he had rapid success in producing<br />

intelligible sounds with errors involving stop consonants and omitting<br />

fricatives. B’s preimplant speech was minimal. Postimplant he began producing<br />

/m/, /b/ and many vowels, but most of his utterances were nonmeaningful.<br />

His speech developed slowly through the precanonical and canonical<br />

stage before making meaningful sound. The authors note that B had other<br />

impairments that slowed his growth. However, the exposure to language that<br />

D received before becoming deaf offered him a quicker, more accurate access<br />

to the sound system.<br />

Much is known about the development of speech following cochlear implantation<br />

due to the multifaceted nature of existing literature. Case studies<br />

and group designs offer insight into the effects of age of implantation, severity<br />

of hearing loss, and situational influences, as well as intelligence. By<br />

330 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


documenting speech production patterns, tentative norms and developmental<br />

expectancies have been established. However, nearly all of the research<br />

currently available has been derived from English-speaking children. Information<br />

is needed to test the universal nature of some of the existing research<br />

on cochlear implantation. Additionally, clinicians working in the field need<br />

developmental norms in order to serve a culturally diverse population.<br />

This case study is an effort to provide such culturally and linguistically<br />

sensitive information. The following questions are posed by using a longitudinal<br />

case study method during the first 17 months postimplant of a<br />

Spanish-speaking child: (1) Will babbling and first word consonant-vowel<br />

co-occurrence patterns in Spanish differ from that of typically developing<br />

children and English-speaking children with cochlear implants? (2) Will the<br />

timeline of development of canonical babbling be consistent with other children<br />

implanted at the same age? (3) What is the impact of language use<br />

(Spanish or English) on accuracy of speech production at 1 year postimplant?<br />

(4) Will our participant’s overall accuracy in Spanish differ from CI peers who<br />

are in English-speaking families?<br />

Methods<br />

Participants<br />

The focus of this study was to follow the serial organization of early speech<br />

of a monolingual Spanish-speaking CI recipient. Our participant, K, lives with<br />

his parents and one older brother. K’s brother speaks Spanish and has learned<br />

English in school and would be considered a balanced sequential bilingual.<br />

This means that his English and Spanish skills are equivalent and that language<br />

development in his first language, Spanish, was mastered prior to the<br />

introduction of English at age six. The parents’ only language is Spanish. K’s<br />

parents have a large social network of relatives and friends who all speak<br />

Spanish. The parents reported all electronic and print media in the home was<br />

in Spanish, with the exception of K’s brother’s school texts. Spanish was also<br />

the language of all speech and audiology intervention that K and his family<br />

received up to three years of age. His early intervention utilized an oral-aural<br />

approach in Spanish. At age three he was enrolled in an English oral preschool<br />

program for children with hearing loss within the local school system.<br />

There was no oral Spanish preschool programming available to the family in<br />

the community. An oral education methodology was selected by his parents<br />

as part of his early intervention and preschool programming. Spanish was<br />

also exclusively used in the interactions of all three authors with the family<br />

during the course of this study.<br />

K was identified as having a profound sensorineural hearing loss at birth.<br />

There is a positive history of hereditary hearing loss within his extended<br />

family. He received a Nucleus 24 M device in the right ear that was activated<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 331


at 20 months of age. His surgery was unremarkable will a full insertion of the<br />

electrode array. He utilizes the ACE (Advanced Combination Encoder) strategy.<br />

His medical history is unremarkable except for the presence of rightsided<br />

motor weakness that was noted in infancy. The parents did not provide<br />

any specific information regarding the etiology of this condition. His motor<br />

asymmetry has become less apparent as he has grown and has not impacted<br />

his ability to engage in gross motor skills such as walking or throwing. He is<br />

left handed. It is not certain whether this is due to natural occurrence or due<br />

to restricted use of his right hand in infancy. Fine motor skills were not a<br />

reported concern of the parents during the study period. His preverbal communication<br />

was normal, with K engaging in turn-taking, gesturing, facial<br />

expression and appropriate eye contact, which provided further evidence of<br />

normal nonverbal communicative development.<br />

To answer the third question on the accuracy of speech production across<br />

languages, data from four other children who received Nucleus 24 implants<br />

between 18-20 months were used as a control group. Like K, the children in<br />

the control group had uneventful medical and surgical histories. Data collection<br />

and analysis for this group was identical to that of K. These children were<br />

also participants in research on longitudinal speech production following<br />

cochlear implantation. The group was composed of two boys and two girls<br />

from two-parent, middle-class, English-speaking homes. The children in the<br />

control group were being educated orally.<br />

Data Collection, Transcription, Reliability and Analysis<br />

Data were obtained in the home at monthly intervals one month prior to<br />

implantation and continued monthly postimplant for the duration of the<br />

study. The participants were videotaped in natural play contexts with their<br />

parents and all three of the authors. Sessions were 45-60 minutes in duration.<br />

Data were collected using a Canon digital video recorder and aided by a Telex<br />

wireless microphone that K wore.<br />

The second and third authors transcribed each recording. Speech-like productions<br />

including consonant- and vowel-like productions that occurred in<br />

isolation or in syllables were transcribed and classified using the International<br />

Phonetic Alphabet. The transcription included all productions that were produced<br />

independently, via imitation, or prompting. Transcriptions included<br />

all vocalizations that were babbling or speech-like in nature. The following<br />

types of vocalizations were transcribed: singleton vowel productions, singleton<br />

consonant productions (nasals, fricatives), and CV or VC syllables.<br />

Laughing, crying and other nonspeech vocalizations were not transcribed.<br />

Syllables including glottals (e.g., /h/) were excluded from the analysis because<br />

these speech sounds are not produced in the oral cavity. The data were<br />

entered into the Logical International Phonetics Programs or LIPP (Oller &<br />

Delgato, 1993) for analyses of accuracy and syllable shape. Target productions<br />

332 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


were determined by the child’s productions, knowledge of their expressive<br />

language vocabulary and the overall context of the child’s interactions with<br />

toys or with his parents. At the times, parents would indicate the target form<br />

of a word.<br />

Ten percent of the data were analyzed for reliability. A graduate student in<br />

speech-language pathology who was not directly involved with the project<br />

provided the reliability for each of the sessions. Reliability consisted of calculating<br />

point-to-point consistency of both vowels and consonants for each<br />

utterance analyzed. Reliability for vowel transcription was 79%, and subsequent<br />

analysis of the data suggested many of the transcription differences<br />

regarded height of the vowel. For example, in a few instances the reliability<br />

transcriber heard high-front (/i/), whereas the original transcriber recorded<br />

front-central vowels. Differences in regard to position (e.g., central for back)<br />

did occur but only in a few instances. Reliability for consonant transcription<br />

was 75% for sessions between 0-6 months postimplant but improved to 90%<br />

as K entered his second year of device use. With consonants, most of the<br />

sounds transcribed were congruent. Class differences were the main sourceof<br />

error (e.g., /b/ for /d/) usually confusing alveolar and bilabial sounds.<br />

CV co-occurrence patterns were classified according to the FC Theory by<br />

place of articulation, and observed-to-expected ratios calculated to determine<br />

whether each pattern occurred significantly above chance. In addition, chisquare<br />

tests of significance were performed for each age interval and group<br />

to determine whether the observed co-occurrence patterns occurred more<br />

frequently than by chance.<br />

Results<br />

Onset of Canonical Babbling<br />

The growth of K’s speech relative to the activation of his CI is commensurate<br />

with data recently reported by Moore and Bass-Ringdahl (2002). There<br />

was an increase in vocal play after CI activation while a surge in vocalizations<br />

and canonical babble occurred approximately six months postactivation.<br />

See Figure 1 for the growth in syllables produced in the first six months<br />

postimplant.<br />

CV Co-Occurrence Patterns<br />

K’s intrasyllabic CV co-occurrence patterns were examined to see whether<br />

they were consistent with the predictions of the Frame Content Theory. The<br />

chi-square tests of significance indicated that K did show a slight trend for<br />

production of the preferred CV syllable patterns seen in typically developing<br />

children during the first six months of implant use; however, the results were<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 333


Figure 1. Growth of syllable productions over the first six months postimplant.<br />

nonsignificant. Insufficient data were obtained to test the hypothesis for dorsal<br />

consonants at the 1-6 month interval. At the 8-10 month interval and the<br />

13-16 month interval, K consistently produced coronal-front syllables that<br />

would be expected under frame dominance; however, he also produced patterns<br />

which would not be expected, such as dorsal consonants paired with<br />

central vowels. The chi-square tests at the 8-10 month interval ( 2 (df=4)=<br />

19.91, significant) and the 13-16 month interval ( 2 (df = 4) = 22.29, significant)<br />

were statistically significant, indicating that the patterns observed are significantly<br />

different from those that would occur by chance. Table 1 illustrates the<br />

intrasyllabic CV co-occurrence patterns for each of the age intervals. Overall,<br />

these results are consistent with syllable development of monolingual English<br />

learning CI infants and toddlers (Moore, Davis, & Tobey, 2004).<br />

Syllable Shape<br />

The overall content of K’s speech can be described as having a high percentage<br />

of CV syllables (51%), followed in frequency by singleton vowels<br />

(21%). VC combinations made up 8% of the vocalizations, and singleton<br />

consonants, primarily /m/, were present in 6% of K’s vocal attempts. There<br />

was a consistent growth in all syllable types across the six sessions with the<br />

exception of VCV (e.g. /b/). The VCV vocalizations were initially made in<br />

isolation. As utterance length increased, this combination appeared less frequently.<br />

334 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


Table 1. Observed-to-Expected Ratios for CV Co-Occurrence Patterns From 0-6,<br />

8-10 and 13-16 Months Postimplant<br />

Group<br />

Vowel<br />

Accuracy<br />

At the 12-month postimplant interval, overall accuracy for word productions<br />

was calculated for K and four English-speaking controls who had received<br />

their CIs at the same chronological age as K. At 12 months<br />

postimplant, K’s overall accuracy level was 60% compared to an average of<br />

23% accuracy for the four English-speaking children. These results are found<br />

in Figure 2.<br />

Discussion<br />

Production<br />

Consonant<br />

Coronal Labial Dorsal<br />

0-6 m Front 1.09 0.88 —<br />

Central 0.98 1.02 —<br />

Back — — —<br />

8-10 m Front 1.22 0.99 0.36<br />

Central 1.01 0.94 1.18<br />

Back 0.60 1.31 1.17<br />

13-16 m Front 1.16 0.51 1.06<br />

Central 0.84 0.94 1.09<br />

Back 1.11 0.80 0.69<br />

Note. Values exceeding 1.0 indicate CV patterns exceeding chance; however, the data at the 0-6<br />

month interval were not statistically significant. The distribution of syllable shapes at the older<br />

age intervals was statistically significant, indicating that the distribution is different from that<br />

occurring by chance. Patterns bolded in the shaded boxes are consistent with the FC Theory.<br />

Those patterns bolded in unshaded boxes are occurring at greater-than-chance levels but are not<br />

consistent with the frame dominance theory.<br />

The purpose of this study was to document speech productions of a prelinguistically<br />

deaf, Spanish-speaking child who received a cochlear implant at<br />

20 months of age. In the process of gathering this information, other discoveries<br />

were also made regarding the uniqueness of how Spanish develops<br />

when auditory input is delayed. Many similarities do exist between Spanish<br />

and English concerning gross developmental features such as timing, content<br />

and order of sound acquisition. However, linguistic features of Spanish also<br />

uniquely influence other aspects of speech production such as accuracy.<br />

Onset of Canonical Babbling<br />

The growth in speech production across the first six months postimplantation<br />

is commensurate with data introduced by Moore and Bass-Ringdahl in<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 335


Figure 2. Mean accuracy data for English-speaking children (1-4) and K at one<br />

year postimplant. All the children were implanted at 18-19 months of age. K’s<br />

accuracy exceeded that of English speakers due to a more concise phonetic<br />

structure of the language and a preponderance of CV word shapes in Spanish.<br />

These forms would be easier to master at a younger age.<br />

2002. Equal to the nine children that were followed in their study, canonical<br />

vocalizations emerged for K at six months, three weeks. The process of activation<br />

can be characterized by an immediate increase in vocalizations, a<br />

period of development in which each individual child shows gains and regressions,<br />

a hiatus or decrease in activity, followed by an explosion in speech.<br />

Further research is needed to identify whether there are true milestones at<br />

months two through five. For instance, if all children uniformly experience a<br />

hiatus or a regression prior to incredible gains, then the increase at six months<br />

will be predictable, and therapy can be planned to take advantage of this time<br />

of growth. This data suggest that the immediate increase in speech development<br />

at six months postimplant is not dependent on the ambient language of<br />

the speaker, intervention or simply audibility.<br />

The explosion in speech production at six months postactivation is matched<br />

by an increase in syllable diversity. Large syllable strings (eight to eleven)<br />

were present in single instances throughout the first five months postactivation.<br />

By the end of the data collection period, large syllable sequences were<br />

recorded in abundance. This data suggests that the “explosion” that has been<br />

shown to occur six months postactivation is an increase in number as well as<br />

complexity.<br />

CV Co-Occurrence Patterns<br />

K’s CV co-occurrence pattern of CV syllables produced in both babbling,<br />

and word attempts were consistent with previous studies of children with CI.<br />

336 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


His Spanish language use did impact his organization of early CV syllables<br />

compared to CI peers exposed to English. Although some aspects of K’s CV<br />

pairs were consistent with that of typically developing children, other patterns<br />

were inconsistent. This would indicate that children with CIs exhibit<br />

more normal serial organization of speech, but not normal development.<br />

Longitudinal studies on this topic will help determine whether this different<br />

serial organization of early speech impacts speech intelligibility during the<br />

preschool years or later.<br />

Syllable Shape<br />

When viewing consonant and vowel co-occurrence as a whole, K’s speech<br />

was dominated by open syllables (ending in a vowel), which is congruent<br />

with the speech of children with hearing loss (Oller et al., 1985; Oller & Eilers,<br />

1988; Yoshinaga-Itano et al., 1992; Davis et al., 2002), as well as with the<br />

Spanish language (Goldstein & Cintron, 2001).<br />

Accuracy<br />

The phonemic and syllable structure of Spanish heavily influenced overall<br />

word accuracy measures at 12 months postimplant. K’s overall accuracy<br />

clearly exceeded that of peers exposed to English. A more concise vowel and<br />

consonant structure as well as a preponderance of CV syllables that would<br />

lead to vowel final consonants in Spanish likely affected this accuracy score.<br />

Therefore, clinicians working with Spanish-speaking children and families<br />

should expect higher levels of accuracy in the earlier stages of speech development.<br />

As vocabulary increases, more complex multisyllabic words will<br />

need to be mastered by Spanish speakers, and it is likely that overall accuracy<br />

may drop as children attempt these complex forms. Thus, there appears to be<br />

a phonetic and phonologic trade-off between Spanish and English. In Spanish,<br />

the concise vowel space and consonant repertoire coupled with open<br />

syllables (CV) leads to increased accuracy on two-syllable word forms typical<br />

in Spanish. In contrast, the more complex English phonology and CVC word<br />

forms lead to less accuracy at the early word stage.<br />

Conclusions<br />

Our participant, K, has made significant progress in Spanish language<br />

acquisition in his first 16 months postimplant. Clinicians working with Spanish-speaking<br />

children should emphasize the phonetic and syllable structure<br />

of the native language in early intervention programs. Like children with<br />

normal hearing, it is hoped that as the sound system and lexicon of a first<br />

language are established, children like K will develop competent English<br />

skills as well over time.<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 337


Although the knowledge that currently exists today regarding Spanish<br />

development and at-risk populations is extremely limited, it is hoped that<br />

case studies like this one will contribute to improved services to children and<br />

families from minority language backgrounds. This limited knowledge is a<br />

major concern considering the high number of Spanish-speaking clients on<br />

the caseloads of current speech-language pathologists and audiologists. Future<br />

research is needed to expand our research knowledge of how Spanish<br />

develops when hearing is absent or diminished and when audibility is restored<br />

using a cochlear implant.<br />

Future studies of one or more children similar to K are needed to compare<br />

with the findings from this study. This study is limited by having only one<br />

participant. As more Spanish-speaking children receive cochlear implants,<br />

the pool on which studies can be based will be broadened. Knowledge concerning<br />

verbal productions of Spanish-speaking CI children will greatly aid<br />

the professional populations that serve Spanish-speaking children by providing<br />

norms or guidelines for development.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

This research is supported by the National Institutes of Health research<br />

grant R-01 HD277733-03 (The University of Texas).<br />

References<br />

Acevedo, M.A. (1993). Development of Spanish consonants in preschool children.<br />

Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 15, 9-15.<br />

Anderson, R., & Smith, B. (1987). Phonological development of two-year-old<br />

monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Child Language,<br />

14, 57-78.<br />

Blamey, P., Barry, J., Bow, C., Sarant, J., Paatsch, L., & Wales, R. (2001). The<br />

development of speech production following cochlear implantation. Clinical<br />

Linguistics and Phonetics, 15(5), 363-382.<br />

Davis, B.L., & MacNeilage, P.F. (1995). The articulatory basis for babbling.<br />

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(6), 1199-1211.<br />

Davis, B.L., MacNeilage, P.F., & Matyear, C.L. (2002). Acquisition of serial<br />

complexity in speech production: A comparison of phonetic and phonological<br />

approaches to first word productions. Phonetica, 59, 75-107.<br />

Encarta: Spanish Language. (2004). Retrieved January 7, 2004, from http://<br />

encarta.msn.com/encnet/refpages/RefEdList.aspx?refid=210137508<br />

Ertmer, D.J., & Mellon, J.A. (2001). Beginning to talk at 20 months: Early vocal<br />

development in a young cochlear implant recipient. Journal of Speech, Language,<br />

and Hearing Research, 44, 192-206.<br />

Ertmer, D.J., Young, N., Grohne, K., Mellon, J.A., Johnson, C., Corbett, K, &<br />

Saindon, K. (2002a). Vocal development in young children with cochlear<br />

338 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


implants: Profiles and implications for intervention. Language, Speech, and<br />

Hearing Service in Schools, 33, 184-195.<br />

Ertmer, D.J., Leonard, J.S., & Pachuilo, M.L. (2002b). Communication intervention<br />

for children with cochlear implants: Two case studies. Language,<br />

Speech, and Hearing Service in Schools, 33, 205-215.<br />

Geers, A.E. (2002). Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and<br />

literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Language, Speech, and<br />

Hearing Services in the Schools, 33, 172-183.<br />

Geers, A.E., & Tobey, E. (1992). Effects of cochlear implants and tactile aids on<br />

the development of speech production skills in children with profound<br />

hearing impairment. The Volta Review, 94(5), 135-163.<br />

Goldstein, B., & Cintron, P. (2001). An investigation of phonological skills in<br />

Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking 2-year-olds. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,<br />

15(5), 343-361.<br />

Goldstein, B., & Iglesias, A. (1996). Phonological patterns in normally developing<br />

Spanish-speaking 3- and 4-year-olds of Puerto Rican descent. Language,<br />

Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 27, 82-90.<br />

Goldstein, B., & Washington, P.S. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological<br />

patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish-English bilingual<br />

children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 153-164.<br />

Jimenez, B.C. (1987). Acquisition of Spanish consonants in children aged 3-5<br />

years, 7 months. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 18(4),<br />

357-363.<br />

Kirk, K.I. (2000). Challenges in the clinical investigation of cochlear implant<br />

outcomes. In J. Niparko, K.I. Kirk, N.K. Mellon, A. McKonkey Robbins,<br />

D.L. Tucci, B.S. Wilson (Eds.), Cochlear Implants: Principles and Practices.<br />

Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.<br />

Kirk, K.I., Sehgal, M., & Miyamoto, R.T. (1997). Speech perception performance<br />

of Nucleus cochlear implant users with partial electrode insertions.<br />

Ear and Hearing, 18, 456-471.<br />

Lee, S.A. (2003). The phonetic basis of early speech acquisition in Korean. Unpublished<br />

doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.<br />

Locke, J.L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

MacNeilage, P.F., & Davis, B.L. (2000). On the origin of internal structure of<br />

word forms. Science, 288, 527-531.<br />

MacNeilage, P.F., Davis, B.L., & Matyear, C.L. (1997). Babbling and first<br />

words: Phonetic similarities and differences. Speech Communication, 22, 269-<br />

277.<br />

MacNeilage, P.F., Davis, B.L., Kinney, A., & Matyear, C.L. (2000). The motor<br />

core of speech: A comparison of serial organization patterns in infants and<br />

languages. Child Development, 71(1), 153-163.<br />

Mann, D., & Hodson, B. (1994). Spanish-speaking children’s phonologies:<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 339


Assessment and remediation of disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language,<br />

15, 137-147.<br />

McCaffrey, H.A., Davis, B.L., MacNeilage, P.F., & von Hapsburg, D. (1999).<br />

Multichannel cochlear implantation and the organization of early speech.<br />

The Volta Review, 101, 5-28.<br />

Moore, J.A., & Bass-Ringdahl, S. (2002). Role of infant vocal development in<br />

candidacy for efficacy of cochlear implantation. Annals of Otology, Rhinology<br />

& Laryngology, 111, 52-55.<br />

Moore, J.A., Davis, B.L., & Tobey, E. (2004). Application of Frame-Content<br />

Theory to infant speech productions following cochlear implantation. Poster presented<br />

at The International Conference on Newborn Hearing Screening<br />

Diagnosis and Intervention, Cernobbio (Como), Italy.<br />

Niparko, J.K., & Wilson, B.S. (2000). History of Cochlear Implants. In J. Niparko,<br />

K.I. Kirk, N.K. Mellon, A. McKonkey Robbins, D.L. Tucci, B.S. Wilson<br />

(Eds.), Cochlear Implants: Principles and Practices. Philadelphia: Lippincott<br />

Williams & Wilkins.<br />

Oller, D.K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G.<br />

Yeni-Komshian, J.F. Kavanagh, & C.A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child Phonology:<br />

Vol. 1. Production (pp. 93-112). New York: Academic Press.<br />

Oller, D.K., & Delgato, R.E. (1993). Logical International Phonetics Programs<br />

[Computer software]. Miami: Intelligent Hearing Services.<br />

Oller D.K., & Eilers, R.E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child<br />

Development, 59, 441-449.<br />

Oller, D.K., Eilers, R.E., Bull, D.H., & Carney, A.E. (1985). Prespeech vocalizations<br />

of a deaf infant: A comparison with normal metaphonological<br />

development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 47-63.<br />

Quilis, A., & Fernandez, J.A. (1999). Curso de fonetica y fonologia españolas.<br />

Madrid: Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC).<br />

Stark, R.E. (1980). Stages of speech development in the first year of life. In G.<br />

Yeni-Komshian, J.F. Kavanagh, & C.A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child Phonology:<br />

Vol. 1. Production (pp. 93-112). New York: Academic Press.<br />

Steffens, M.L., Eilers, R.E., Fishman, L., Oller, D.K., & Urbano, R.C. (1994).<br />

Early vocal development in tactually aided children with severe-profound<br />

hearing loss. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 700-711.<br />

Stoel-Gammon, C., & Otomo, K. (1986). Babbling development of hearingimpaired<br />

and normally hearing subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,<br />

51, 33-41.<br />

Tobey, E., Geers, A., & Brunner, C. (1994). Speech production results: Speech<br />

feature acquisition. The Volta Review, 96(5), 109-129.<br />

Tye-Murray, N., & Kirk, K.I. (1993). Vowel and diphthong production by<br />

young users of cochlear implants and the relationship between the phonetic<br />

level evaluation and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,<br />

36(3), 488-503.<br />

United States Census Bureau. Census 2000. Retrieved January 7, 2004, from<br />

340 Moore, Prath, & Arrieta


http://eire.census.gov/popest/data/national/tables/asro/NA-EST2002-<br />

ASRO-04.php.<br />

Vihman, M.M., Macken, M.A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J. (1985).<br />

From babbling to speech: A re-assessment of the continuity issue. Language,<br />

61(2), 397-445.<br />

Vihman, M.M., Ferguson, C.A., & Elbert, M. (1986). Phonological development<br />

from babbling to speech: common tendencies and individual differences.<br />

Applied Psycholinguistics, 7, 3-40.<br />

von Hapsburg, D. (2003). The development of speech in children with varying levels<br />

of hearing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.<br />

Wallace, V. (1998). Is babble the gateway to speech for all children? A longitudinal<br />

study of children who are deaf or hard of hearing. The Volta Review,<br />

100(5), 121-149.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1998a). Development of audition and speech: Implications<br />

for early intervention with infants who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

The Volta Review, 100(5), 213-235.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1998b). Early speech development in children who are<br />

deaf or hard of hearing: Interrelationships with the language and hearing.<br />

The Volta Review, 100(5), 181-212.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Stredler-Brown, A., & Jancosek, E. (1992). From phone to<br />

phoneme: What can we understand from babble. The Volta Review, 94,<br />

283-314.<br />

Early Speech Acquisition in Spanish 341


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 343-364<br />

Bilateral Cochlear<br />

Implantation in Children:<br />

Experiences and<br />

Considerations<br />

Andrea Bohnert, M.T.A.-F., Vera Spitzlei, Dipl.-Ing.,<br />

Karl L. Lippert, Ph.D., Dipl.-Ing., and Annerose Keilmann, M.D.<br />

Between 2000 and 2006, the University Clinic for Ear Nose and Throat and<br />

Communication Disorders in Mainz, Germany, performed 41 bilateral cochlear implantations<br />

in children. This article addresses some of the factors to be considered in<br />

a decision to bilaterally implant a child, including the age of the child at the first<br />

implant, the length of time between the first and second implants, the age of the child<br />

at the time of the second implant and the level of auditory learning experienced with<br />

the first implant. At the time the article was written, 33 of the 41 children were able<br />

to participate in speech discrimination testing in quiet and in noise. The children’s<br />

localization abilities were also reported. All of the tests concerning hearing, localization<br />

and speech understanding with the implants were conducted as part of the<br />

child’s regularly scheduled appointments to verify the settings of each speech processor<br />

and the performance with both implants. The information shared in this article<br />

reflects findings from clinical practice and was not obtained in a controlled research<br />

protocol. Nevertheless, the experiences and observations reported are clinically relevant<br />

to those involved in or contemplating bilateral cochlear implantation of young<br />

children.<br />

Andrea Bohnert, MTA-F, holds teaching posts at the University Clinic Mainz and the Teaching-Unit<br />

for Logopedics and is senior MTA-F in audiology and pedaudiology at the University<br />

Clinic for ENT and Communication Disorders in Mainz, Germany. Vera Spitzlei, Dipl.-Ing.,<br />

is a member of the audiology team at the University Clinic for ENT and Communication<br />

Disorders in Mainz, Germany. Karl L. Lippert, Ph.D., Dipl.-Ing., holds teaching posts in<br />

electroacoustics and audiology at the University Clinic Mainz and at the Teaching-Unit for<br />

Logopedics. Annerose Keilmann, M.D., is a senior physician at the University Clinic for<br />

Communication Disorders in Mainz, Germany, and head of the department of communication<br />

disorders at the University Clinic for ENT, also in Mainz.<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 343


Introduction<br />

Since the early 1980s, when multichannel cochlear implants (CIs) became<br />

available, cochlear implantation has become a widely accepted treatment of<br />

severe hearing disorders in patients of all ages in Europe and the United<br />

States. During the following decade and a half, unilateral cochlear implantation<br />

was routinely performed at the University Clinic for Ear, Nose, and<br />

Throat (ENT) and Communication Disorders in Mainz, Germany (Biesalski,<br />

Lippert, & Bohnert, 1985). In the early 1990s, workgroups in Australia began<br />

investigating the potential of bilateral cochlear implantation (van Hoesel,<br />

Tong, Hallow, & Clark, 1992). In 1996, an adult patient in Wuerzburg, Germany<br />

was successfully implanted with two multichannel devices. Four weeks<br />

after the initial fitting, this patient demonstrated significant improvement in<br />

speech recognition in quiet and in noise. Two years later, the Wuerzburg team<br />

performed the first bilateral implant in a child (Mueller, 1998, 1999). The<br />

success of these first two cases led to further investigations (Kuehn-Inacker,<br />

Shehata-Dieler, Mueller, & Helms, 2004) and influenced the acceptance of<br />

bilateral cochlear implants in children (Mueller, 2005).<br />

In 2000, following the success in Wuerzburg, the first pediatric bilateral<br />

cochlear implant surgery was performed at the University Clinic for ENT and<br />

Communication Disorders at Mainz, Germany. In the intervening six years,<br />

41 children have been bilaterally implanted. This article presents some of the<br />

clinical considerations and guidelines in pediatric bilateral implantation that<br />

have evolved from experiences with those young patients and reports on the<br />

children’s sound localization abilities and speech discrimination in quiet and<br />

in noise conditions.<br />

Guidelines in Bilateral Implantation<br />

In 2005, a panel convened at the Second Meeting Consensus on Auditory<br />

Implants in Valencia, Spain published a consensus statement on bilateral<br />

cochlear implants (Offeciers et al., 2005). Included in the consensus statement<br />

were recommendations for the fitting of two cochlear implants in the following<br />

types of patients: (1) those in whom the benefits obtained with one cochlear<br />

implant is poor, (2) patients with meningitis who develop cochlear<br />

ossification (with recommendations to perform implantation as soon as possible<br />

to achieve full implantation) (3) children with permanent bilateral profound<br />

hearing loss (with recommendations to pay special attention to young<br />

children who are in their speech and language acquisition years) and (4) in<br />

adults who want to restore binaural hearing or need it in order to remain in<br />

their chosen profession (Offeciers et al., 2005; Hoth, 2006).<br />

In the discussion of the staging of bilateral implants, there was consensus<br />

that one-stage bilateral implantation surgeries are more cost effective but<br />

should only be conducted by experienced surgeons and centers. Regarding<br />

344 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


the time interval between the two surgeries in children, the recommendation<br />

was that a short interval of less than 6-12 months “enables prelingually deaf<br />

children to achieve good performance with the second implant within a few<br />

weeks” (Offeciers et al., 2005, p. 918). A longer period between surgeries<br />

would increase the probability of a longer and more intense period of habilitation<br />

for the child to become accustomed to a second implant.<br />

Advantages of Binaural Hearing<br />

In the normal auditory system, binaural hearing provides advantages that<br />

are unavailable to the listener with unilateral hearing. For example, spatial<br />

hearing, localization, and signal source discrimination in quiet as well as<br />

noise are possible only with binaural hearing. During localization, individuals<br />

with binaural hearing use interaural time and level differences (headshadow<br />

effect), as well as diffraction phenomena, sonic reflections at the<br />

auricle and the so-called squelch effect (Laszig et al., 2004; Schleich, Nopp, &<br />

D’Haese, 2004) to determine where the sound source originates. Thus, binaural<br />

hearing provides significant advantages, especially in noisy environments<br />

(Blauert, 1974). These studies led to the presumption that bilateral<br />

implantation can provide advantages to children with hearing loss.<br />

With the advantages of the normally functioning binaural system serving<br />

as a model (Blauert, 1974), several goals that support bilateral implantation in<br />

children can be determined. These include: (1) to provide hearing thresholds<br />

in the non-implanted ear that could permit the development of speech discrimination<br />

ability, (2) to decrease listening effort, (3) to provide the possibility<br />

of localization for increased safety in daily life (traffic, etc.) and (4) to<br />

increase speech discrimination in noise.<br />

Parent Acceptance of Bilateral Cochlear Implantation<br />

The decision to pursue a cochlear implant is a challenging process for many<br />

parents. When the option of bilateral cochlear implantation has been presented<br />

to parents at our center, almost all of them have ultimately elected a<br />

two-stage implantation process rather than simultaneous surgeries. There are<br />

several factors that guide families in the decision to pursue unilateral implantation<br />

before proceeding to a second device. Some parents are concerned that<br />

the implant will not provide sufficient auditory benefit to their child and<br />

want evidence of success before proceeding. Or, a family may elect to “save”<br />

one ear for the possibility of medical or technological advances that provide<br />

a different treatment option. The situation in which parents seem to be most<br />

comfortable pursuing bilateral implantation occurs when a child becomes<br />

deaf following meningitis. The risk of rapid cochlear ossification following<br />

meningitis reduces the likelihood of optimal implant success if the surgery is<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 345


delayed, thereby increasing parental acceptance of simultaneous bilateral implantation.<br />

It is essential to discuss parents’ expectations regarding their child’s auditory<br />

development when he or she receives the first implant and again upon<br />

receiving the second implant. It has been our experience that fitting a child<br />

with a cochlear implant often creates high expectations in parents. When a<br />

second implant is being pursued, expectations can be even higher. Most<br />

parents who seek bilateral implantation are pursuing “normalcy” for their<br />

child. Their objectives are for their child to go to school with their hearing<br />

peers and develop as a child with normal hearing. There are cases in which<br />

this is possible, but surgery alone cannot assure this outcome. Interdisciplinary<br />

collaboration and communication among the family, pediatric audiologists,<br />

physicians, early interventionists and other therapists are necessary for<br />

parents and professionals to understand the complexities of each case and to<br />

attempt to establish reasonable expectations for all involved. Many factors<br />

affect outcomes for each child, including the age of the child at the first and<br />

second implants and the length of time between the two implants. Resources<br />

for therapy in the family’s community and their commitment to following<br />

through with therapy and reinforcing it at home significantly affect outcomes.<br />

To the degree possible, all of the factors that can influence the child and<br />

family in their daily routines should be identified and discussed in relation to<br />

the cochlear implants.<br />

Timing the Second Implant Surgery<br />

A growing body of research (Govaerts et al., 2002; Manrique, Cervera-Paz,<br />

Huarte, & Molina 2004; Zwolan et al., 2004; Keilmann, Gaida, & Limberger,<br />

2005; Kral, Hartmann, Tillein, Heid, & Klinke, 2002a; Kral, Hartmann, Tillein,<br />

Heid, & Klinke, 2002b; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl, 1998; Yoshinaga-Itano,<br />

2002) has provided evidence that children with profound hearing<br />

loss who receive a cochlear implant by their second birthday achieve higher<br />

scores on speech discrimination testing and do so at a faster rate than children<br />

with similar hearing losses who are implanted later in life. The results of<br />

studies demonstrating improved success of children unilaterally implanted at<br />

younger ages (Sharma, Dorman, Spah, & Todd, 2002), combined with studies<br />

regarding the benefits of binaural hearing in normally hearing children, have<br />

provided support for bilateral cochlear implantation in children at younger<br />

ages.<br />

Challenges in Binaural Fitting of Speech Processors<br />

The initial fitting of the second speech processor should be conducted as an<br />

independent fitting, entirely distinct from the fitting of the first speech processor.<br />

Because each cochlea has a unique anatomy, as well as physiologic<br />

346 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


conditions, it is seldom, if ever, possible for the electrode array to be placed<br />

in exactly the same configuration in both ears. As a result, fitting should be<br />

conducted as an individualized process for each ear, even in the same patient.<br />

For these same reasons, the first processor’s map should never be used as the<br />

basis for the initial fitting of the second processor. Among the children fitted<br />

with binaural cochlear implants in this clinic, none have had identical maps<br />

in both of their speech processors.<br />

Perceiving, processing, storing and retrieving auditory information are<br />

complex tasks. Children who are candidates for cochlear implants have a<br />

wide range of abilities and strategies for performing these tasks. Although the<br />

most important and fundamental principle in cochlear implant fitting is that<br />

every fitting is unique, to achieve a “good cochlear implant fitting” for each<br />

child a few universally valid fitting guidelines are recommended.<br />

Most important is that the child should never be overstimulated during the<br />

fitting procedure. Overstimulation creates an association between “hearing”<br />

and pain, which immediately reduces the likelihood of a successful fitting of<br />

the speech processor. A second principle is that within the first fitting session,<br />

all channels that are available for stimulation should be activated. At our<br />

clinic this occurs over the first week of fitting. Once all channels are activated,<br />

subsequent increases in current levels will produce stimulation patterns without<br />

changing the basic sound characteristics. This applies to cochlear implant<br />

systems running the authentic CIS strategy (i.e., fast, continuous stimulation<br />

on all electrode-channels). During each stimulation cycle, all channels will be<br />

activated and are able to provide information. Stimulation patterns produced<br />

by using feature extraction only contain parts of the original input signal.<br />

Interpolation of channel settings should be avoided. Individual testing and<br />

verification of all settings of each channel are necessary to achieve maximum<br />

benefit, as even small changes in fitting data may cause significant changes in<br />

the child’s hearing sensation (Lippert & Bohnert, 2005; Bohnert, 2005). And<br />

finally, the effect of binaural loudness summation must be considered during<br />

fitting of the second ear. Summation intensity may differ considerably from<br />

child to child; thus, careful attention to this phenomenon is essential in each<br />

fitting. In some cases the summation effect leads to an obvious change in the<br />

child’s behavior. When this occurs, the stimulation level of the first fitted ear<br />

is reduced step-by-step until “normal” is reestablished.<br />

Influence of Age on Second Processor Fitting<br />

Children younger than two years appear to experience the least difficulty<br />

adjusting to a second implant and the resulting bilateral auditory signal.<br />

Among children at this age, hearing with a cochlear implant is still in the<br />

initial stages, and integration of the second signal into the child’s existing<br />

auditory processing strategies is more easily accomplished. This factor, combined<br />

with the shorter period between the first and second implants, makes<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 347


fitting the second processor easier in younger children. Regardless of the<br />

child’s age, fitting the second processor must be done cautiously in a stepby-step<br />

process. At the University Clinic in Mainz, speech processor fittings<br />

are accomplished during two one-week inpatient hospital stays. During the<br />

first fitting session, each electrode in the array is activated. As electrodes are<br />

activated and added to the array, the child’s responses to a wide variety of<br />

sounds, experienced in everyday situations, are observed by the family and<br />

clinic staff. On the basis of observations and reports of behavioral changes,<br />

the electrodes may be adjusted. This collaborative effort by the family and<br />

staff is very beneficial, so that by the end of the week the child has been fitted<br />

to an effective level of performance. During the second stay, the child’s behavior<br />

is observed and fine tuning of the speech processor is completed.<br />

After age 2, the process of fitting the second cochlear implant can become<br />

more complex. As the children become older at the age of the second implant,<br />

or as the length of time between implants increases (the two factors are often<br />

related), a variety of responses to bilateral stimulation have been observed. In<br />

either case, the child has had a longer period of time to become accustomed<br />

to the speech signal provided by a single implant. Some children accept the<br />

second implant without complications or resistance, even when there has<br />

been a considerable delay between the first and second cochlear implant<br />

surgeries. In other cases, introduction of additional sound from a second<br />

implant can confuse or irritate the child. To reduce a negative response to<br />

stimulation provided by the second implant, fitting should proceed in a careful<br />

and systematic manner. In some rare cases, it will be helpful to set processor<br />

levels in a way that stimulation is below the threshold of perception at<br />

the initial fitting. Thus, the auditory nerve and auditory cortex are slightly<br />

stimulated without disturbing the child. Increasing stimulation intensity in a<br />

step-by-step manner while carefully observing the child’s behavior appears to<br />

increase acceptance of the second processor.<br />

When the child is able to tolerate an initial stimulation level of the second<br />

implant beyond the threshold of perception, speech discrimination scores<br />

with bilateral activation often decrease. Because each child has a unique<br />

history of auditory experiences, cochlear implant benefit and neurological<br />

maturation, it is not possible to predict accurately the length of time speech<br />

discrimination might be adversely affected. It has been reported, as also observed<br />

at our clinic that, after a period of acclimatization, the child’s speech<br />

discrimination scores improve and scores with bilateral activation become<br />

superior to previous unilateral speech discrimination scores (Kuehn-Inacker<br />

et al., 2004).<br />

Habilitation<br />

In Germany, there are several possible scenarios for habilitation/rehabilitation<br />

that vary according to location. In most cases the initial fitting of the<br />

device takes place in the clinic where the patient received his or her cochlear<br />

348 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


implant. All of the subsequent adjustments as well as the habilitation/<br />

rehabilitation will occur at that clinic. Some clinics provide the option of an<br />

inpatient stay; others can only provide outpatient appointments. The University<br />

Clinic for ENT and Communication Disorders, Mainz, is able to provide<br />

an inpatient stay in the hospital. Younger children under age 5 or children<br />

with multiple handicaps must be accompanied by parents or caregivers.<br />

Many of the older children attend their inpatient stay without parents. Typically,<br />

the child is initially seen for one week. Then, following a four-week<br />

period of habituation, the child has one more week of inpatient stay, during<br />

which programming is adjusted. During this time, the audiologists, speechlanguage<br />

pathologists and other professionals have the opportunity to see the<br />

child several times per day. This provides the opportunity to pay special<br />

attention to the child’s individual needs.<br />

All specialized therapies can be tailored to each child’s specific needs. Most<br />

of the children are very familiar with the clinic because it is the same place<br />

they go for hearing evaluations and hearing aid fittings and know the physicians,<br />

audiologists and other therapists. In most cases the staff has known<br />

the child and family since the child was first diagnosed. Therefore, a relationship<br />

has been established over a period of months and often years that creates a<br />

sense of confidence on the part of both the family and the professionals.<br />

In our clinic, clinical testing occurs every three months during the first year<br />

after cochlear implantation. Between 12 and 24 months after the first fitting,<br />

appointments are made twice a year or more often if needed. In addition, an<br />

annual speech and language evaluation occurs. The cochlear implant team<br />

also meets with the child’s early interventionist once or twice a year to discuss<br />

the child’s progress.<br />

An important aspect in fitting the second processor is balancing pitch and<br />

loudness in comparison to the contralateral side. This has to be checked both<br />

in therapy sessions as well as at home. Families have the opportunity and<br />

responsibility to observe the children at home where the acoustic conditions<br />

are quite different from those in the fitting and therapy environments.<br />

To reinforce the child’s awareness of sound in the newly implanted ear,<br />

therapy should be conducted with the first implant turned off, and parents<br />

are advised to have their child experience hearing with the new processor<br />

alone for a certain portion of the day.<br />

Several factors should be considered in devising an appropriate therapy<br />

plan. The child’s age at implantation and the time between fittings are very<br />

critical factors. Beginning at age 2 1 ⁄2, children become aware of themselves<br />

and their environment. For example, children at that age who have used one<br />

implant for a while are often reluctant to “give away their good ear.” If<br />

children have adequate language development, they can be told that their<br />

second ear has been “sleeping” for a while and now has to be awakened very<br />

carefully. With a shorter time period between the two implantations, integration<br />

of the new implant and auditory synchronization will develop more<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 349


quickly and naturally. Children who receive their second implant during the<br />

preverbal or early language development usually accept bilateral stimulation<br />

after a very short time.<br />

When there has been a period of more than two years between implants, a<br />

more intense and time-consuming habilitation phase should be anticipated<br />

for the second implant. As a general rule, the longer the time between implants,<br />

the more extended the habilitation process is likely to be. In children<br />

who received their first implant at an older age, the training in sound localization<br />

and discrimination of speech in noise will require more intensity and<br />

a longer time period.<br />

Loudness Scaling<br />

As soon as possible, loudness scaling should be conducted to attain more<br />

detailed information about hearing sensations following electrical stimulation<br />

(Zorowka, Heinemann, & Bohnert, 1995). Up to a developmental age of approximately<br />

four years, balancing binaural cochlear implants has to be based<br />

upon the results of aided audiometry and behavioral observation. This will<br />

also be the case for children with multiple disabilities or those who have not<br />

attained a developmental age of four to five years. For all of these children,<br />

testing each ear separately in free sound field, with different frequencies of<br />

narrow-band noise as the test stimuli, can provide basic information for balancing.<br />

Determining balancing information from behavioral observation is<br />

very difficult because competency at localization of the sound source is required<br />

for this task. Most children require a long period of bilateral cochlear<br />

implant use to develop localization abilities.<br />

An effective tool for balancing bilateral cochlear implant systems in slightly<br />

older children is loudness scaling. The scaling method used during fitting<br />

sessions will be very helpful in achieving reliable results. The better the child<br />

is trained to perform the scaling tasks, the more reliable the information used<br />

for balancing will be. Children who have a developmental age of four to five<br />

years will most like be able to identify reliable loudness level with a scale<br />

illustrated by drawings of children reacting to sounds. The drawings depict<br />

children reacting to sound at different loudness levels ranging from “I can’t<br />

hear it” to “too loud.” A picture of the touchscreen used in the University<br />

Clinic in Mainz for scaling loudness levels is presented in Figure 1. Figure 2<br />

shows an example of a scaling profile conducted by a cochlear implant user<br />

for 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000 Hz. The lines through the black dots indicate the<br />

individual loudness progression at a particular frequency, compared to loudness<br />

as evaluated by normal hearing persons.<br />

Description of the Children<br />

The speech discrimination scores reported in this article are based upon<br />

the performance of 41 children with bilateral cochlear implants. All of the<br />

350 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


Figure 1. Touchscreen for loudness scaling (Wuerzburger Hoerfeld, WESTRA).<br />

children involved were implanted in two-stage procedures. The preimplant<br />

variables within the group, such as age at first implant, age at second implant<br />

and the time between implants, vary widely. The age of the children at first<br />

implantation ranged from 6 months to 9 years, 1 month (see Table 1). The<br />

second implant was received at ages ranging from 1 year, 2 months to 11<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 351


Figure 2. Graphic example of a loudness scaling profile for a cochlear implant user.<br />

years, 6 months. Among this group of children, five were bilaterally implanted<br />

before age 2. Twenty-two received the second implant before age 6,<br />

and 14 were bilaterally implanted after age 6. The amount of time between the<br />

first and second implants also varied widely (see Table 2). The shortest period<br />

of time between implant one and implant two was 3 months, whereas the<br />

longest interval was 7 years, 8 months. Among the group, 18 children received<br />

the second implant within two years of the first, whereas 23 children<br />

experienced a period of more than 2 years between the first and second<br />

implant.<br />

The data reported here were collected as part of routine clinical evaluations<br />

and were analyzed retrospectively. It is important to note that the results<br />

were attained in specific test environments and do not necessarily reflect the<br />

children’s skills in “real life” situations.<br />

Audiometric Information<br />

All audiometric tests were conducted within the routine clinical examination<br />

and evaluation of the implant systems, as is the practice at this clinic.<br />

Monaural aided CI thresholds were measured in all children in free sound<br />

field using narrow-band noise. In most cases, aided audiometry was carried<br />

out at 250, 500, 1000, 2000, 4000 and 8000 Hz. Depending upon the child’s<br />

age, tests were conducted via visual reinforcement or play audiometry.<br />

352 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


Table 1. Patients<br />

Patient ID<br />

Age at<br />

First Implant<br />

[y;m]<br />

Age at<br />

Second Implant<br />

[y;m]<br />

Time Lag<br />

[y;m]<br />

Current Age<br />

[y;m]<br />

TT 0;06 1;02 0;08 3;02<br />

EF 0;06 1;09 1;03 4;08<br />

WP 0;08 1;04 0;08 3;08<br />

HL 0;09 3;06 2;09 4;11<br />

BL 0;10 1;02 0;04 1;05<br />

SJ 0;10 2;01 1;03 2;07<br />

ED 1;01 3;08 2;07 4;04<br />

JP 1;01 4;06 3;05 4;11<br />

WC 1;01 3;04 2;03 7;08<br />

RM 1;02 1;07 0;05 3;04<br />

NKP 1;02 2;06 1;04 7;10<br />

SN 1;03 3;08 2;05 5;01<br />

KL 1;05 4;03 2;10 7;11<br />

MAA 1;06 3;02 1;08 6;07<br />

HJ 1;07 2;02 0;07 4;01<br />

HM 1;07 2;05 0;10 5;05<br />

KuK 1;08 3;02 1;06 3;09<br />

RS 1;09 4;01 2;04 7;02<br />

KeK 1;10 2;10 1;00 4;11<br />

WA 1;10 7;06 5;08 8;01<br />

RP 2;00 2;10 0;10 6;09<br />

ZR 2;01 2;04 0;03 5;00<br />

EM 2;03 8;04 6;01 8;07<br />

LC 2;03 9;06 7;03 11;05<br />

VHP 2;04 5;09 3;05 9;08<br />

LT 2;04 7;02 4;10 11;06<br />

PR 2;06 3;02 0;08 4;00<br />

HP 2;06 4;11 2;05 7;09<br />

MS 2;08 8;04 5;08 11;07<br />

MJN 2;10 10;04 7;06 12;04<br />

EJL 2;11 5;07 2;08 6;09<br />

UF 2;11 6;04 3;05 12;05<br />

JF 3;00 3;08 0;08 6;03<br />

LDM 3;00 9;03 6;03 12;10<br />

WJ 3;05 8;10 5;05 9;07<br />

OM 3;06 7;03 3;09 8;06<br />

GM 3;10 11;06 7;08 13;07<br />

TJM 4;00 6;00 2;00 7;10<br />

SM 4;01 7;04 3;03 9;09<br />

SS 5;10 9;09 3;11 15;04<br />

LL 9;01 9;09 0;08 14;00<br />

Note. Children are listed in ascending order, based on age at first implantation.<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 353


Table 2. Patients<br />

Patient ID<br />

Time Lag<br />

[y;m]<br />

Age at<br />

First Implant<br />

[y;m]<br />

Age at<br />

Second Implant<br />

[y;m]<br />

Current Age<br />

[y;m]<br />

ZR 0;03 2;01 2;04 5;00<br />

BL 0;04 0;10 1;02 1;05<br />

RM 0;05 1;02 1;07 3;04<br />

HJ 0;07 1;07 2;02 4;01<br />

TT 0;08 0;06 1;02 3;02<br />

WP 0;08 0;08 1;04 3;08<br />

PR 0;08 2;06 3;02 4;00<br />

JF 0;08 3;00 3;08 6;03<br />

LL 0;08 9;01 9;09 14;00<br />

HM 0;10 1;07 2;05 5;05<br />

RP 0;10 2;00 2;10 6;09<br />

KeK 1;00 1;10 2;10 4;11<br />

EF 1;03 0;06 1;09 4;08<br />

SJ 1;03 0;10 2;01 2;07<br />

NKP 1;04 1;02 2;06 7;10<br />

KuK 1;06 1;08 3;02 3;09<br />

MAA 1;08 1;06 3;02 6;07<br />

TJM 2;00 4;00 6;00 7;10<br />

WC 2;03 1;01 3;04 7;08<br />

RS 2;04 1;09 4;01 7;02<br />

SN 2;05 1;03 3;08 5;01<br />

HP 2;05 2;06 4;11 7;09<br />

ED 2;07 1;01 3;08 4;04<br />

EJL 2;08 2;11 5;07 6;09<br />

HL 2;09 0;09 3;06 4;11<br />

KL 2;10 1;05 4;03 7;11<br />

SM 3;03 4;01 7;04 9;09<br />

JP 3;05 1;01 4;06 4;11<br />

VHP 3;05 2;04 5;09 9;08<br />

UF 3;05 2;11 6;04 12;05<br />

OM 3;09 3;06 7;03 8;06<br />

SS 3;11 5;10 9;09 15;04<br />

LT 4;10 2;04 7;02 11;06<br />

WJ 5;05 3;05 8;10 9;07<br />

WA 5;08 1;10 7;06 8;01<br />

MS 5;08 2;08 8;04 11;07<br />

EM 6;01 2;03 8;04 8;07<br />

LDM 6;03 3;00 9;03 12;10<br />

LC 7;03 2;03 9;06 11;05<br />

MJN 7;06 2;10 10;04 12;04<br />

GM 7;08 3;10 11;06 13;07<br />

Note. Children sorted in ascending order according to time lag between first and second implant.<br />

354 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


Figure 3. Averaged aided audiogram of children with a cochlear implant.<br />

The averaged aided audiogram for all ears of all bilaterally implanted<br />

children is shown in Figure 3, with standard deviations. The figure indicates<br />

that variability of detection is nearly equal at each of the different test frequencies.<br />

The mean threshold for all children is in the range of 30 dB HL.<br />

Mean values are also shown for the aided audiograms with the first implant,<br />

(Figure 4, left) and with the second implant (Figure 4, right).<br />

Both averaged audiograms show similar results and correspond with the<br />

mean values for all ears in Figure 3. These results indicate that the children<br />

have similar thresholds. This fact alone, however, does not guarantee similar<br />

speech discrimination abilities, which vary considerably among the children.<br />

Other factors, such as the child’s chronological and developmental ages, the<br />

presence of co-occurring health, learning or cognitive challenges and the time<br />

between first and second implant surgery, also influence performance in<br />

general as well as performance on specific measures.<br />

Figure 4. Averaged aided audiograms of children with first (left) and second<br />

implants (right).<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 355


Speech Discrimination Tasks<br />

In addition to aided audiograms, some children were able to participate in<br />

speech discrimination testing with their cochlear implants. Speech discrimination<br />

tests were selected according to the child’s age and speech development.<br />

Listed in Table 3 are the speech discrimination tests, with appropriate<br />

age ranges, used at the University Clinic in Mainz. The age ranges listed are<br />

for normal hearing children who are typically developing. All speech discrimination<br />

tests were conducted in open set both in quiet and in noise<br />

conditions. In the quiet condition, the child was seated in front of the speaker<br />

at a distance of 1 meter. Tests were conducted first with the child wearing a<br />

monaural implant and again with input from both implants. It is important to<br />

obtain information about each implant in a monaural listening situation, as<br />

well as information regarding binaural listening performance. Comparison of<br />

these different sets of information can be useful in determining the contribution<br />

of each implant in binaural performance. This can be especially important<br />

information in fitting the second implant to maintain optimal speech<br />

discrimination and compatibility with the first implant.<br />

Children’s Speech Discrimination Performance in Quiet<br />

Speech discrimination in quiet was conducted with open-set materials for<br />

all children. Of the 41 children implanted, 33 were able to participate in<br />

speech discrimination testing. Due to differences in developmental ages<br />

among the children, the number and complexity of the tests completed by<br />

each child varied. Fifteen of the younger children were able to attend to only<br />

one binaurally presented speech discrimination test. Eighteen of the children<br />

were able to respond to speech discrimination testing in both binaural and<br />

monaural modalities. Discrimination scores of speech presented at 65 dB HL<br />

Table 3. Speech Discrimination Tests Used at the University Clinic in Mainz with<br />

Children Who Have Normal Hearing<br />

Test Material Age Ranges<br />

Mainzer I mono- and bisyllabic words 2 years, 6 months to 3 years,<br />

5 months<br />

Mainzer II mono- and bisyllabic words 3 years, 6 months to 4 years,<br />

5 months<br />

Mainzer III mono- and bisyllabic words 4 years, 6 months and older<br />

Göttinger I monosyllabic words 4 years, 6 months to 5 years,<br />

11 months<br />

Göttinger II monosyllabic words 5 years, 11 months and older<br />

Freiburger Zahlen four-syllabic numerals 8 years and older<br />

Freiburger Wörter monosyllabic words 8 years and older<br />

356 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


with the first implant (monaural), the second implant (monaural) and with<br />

two implants (binaural) are shown in Figure 5.<br />

Three of the 18 children (SM, WJ and OM) were not able to discriminate<br />

speech with their second implant monaurally because it had just been fitted.<br />

Despite this, two had binaural speech discrimination scores that exceeded<br />

monaural scores with the first implant alone. The third child’s binaural scores<br />

were slightly less than those attained with the first implant.<br />

Seven of the children demonstrated better binaural speech discrimination<br />

than either of their two monaural scores, indicating the presence of binaural<br />

advantage. These children ranged in age from 4 years, 11 months to 12 years,<br />

5 months. The length of time between implant one and implant two ranged<br />

between 8 months and 7 years, 6 months. In nine children, binaural speech<br />

discrimination scores and their better monaural scores were the same. Overall,<br />

their performance ranged between 60% and 100%, and in five of the nine,<br />

performance was at the 90 th percentile or higher. Of the five children who<br />

scored at the 90 th percentile, all had received their first implant by age 2 years,<br />

4 months. Among these children the time between first and second implant<br />

ranged from 10 months to 7 years, 3 months. At the time of testing they had<br />

worn both implants between 3 years, 11 months and 4 years, 4 months.<br />

Two other children attained monaural and binaural scores at the 80 th percentile.<br />

They received their first implant later, between age 2 years, 8 months<br />

and 9 years, 1 month. At the time of testing they had worn bilateral implants<br />

for 3 years, 3 months and 4 years, 3 months, respectively.<br />

Figure 5. Monaural and binaural speech discrimination scores with a cochlear<br />

implant at 65 dB.<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 357


The last two children with equal monaural (better ear) and binaural scores<br />

had speech discrimination scores of 60% and 70%. Their ages at the first<br />

implant were 1 year, 2 months and 2 years, 10 months. At the time of testing<br />

they had been binaurally fitted for 2 years, 6 months, and 11 years, 4 months.<br />

In two children, binaural speech discrimination scores were worse than in<br />

the monaural condition. One child was initially implanted at age 2 years, 11<br />

months and the other at age 3 years, 5 months. The second implant occurred<br />

at the age of 6 years, 4 months for the first child and 8 years, 10 months for<br />

the second. The monaural versus binaural scores for both children differedby<br />

10%, which equals a difference of one word.<br />

These results indicate that the time between implants does not necessarily<br />

predict binaural performance. It should also be stressed once again that the<br />

fitting of the second implant can initially have a deleterious effect upon<br />

speech discrimination scores. This is more likely to be the case when the time<br />

period between the first and second implant is substantial. The integration of<br />

a new auditory signal is a process that requires adaptation over a period of<br />

time that varies from one individual to another.<br />

Speech Discrimination Performance in Noise<br />

Speech discrimination tests in noise were also carried out in monaural and<br />

binaural conditions. In the test setting, the child was seated in front of the<br />

speaker at a distance of 1 m. The broad-band noise signal was presented by<br />

a speaker placed directly behind the child at a distance of 1 m. Speech was<br />

presented at a level of 65 dB HL, and the noise signal was presented at 60 dB<br />

HL creating a signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of +5 dB.<br />

Speech discrimination in noise was tested in a total of 24 children. Of this<br />

group, 12 children were tested in both binaural and monaural conditions. Six<br />

other children were tested monaurally in both ears, but not in a bilateral<br />

condition. Six additional children were tested monaurally in one ear only,<br />

because they were unable to concentrate on the test any longer. Test results<br />

for all children are shown in Figure 6.<br />

Nine of the 24 children could discriminate speech better binaurally compared<br />

to the monaural test situation. The age at time of first implant ranged<br />

from 1 year, 1 month to 9 years, 1 month, with a time lag between first and<br />

second implant of between 10 months to 7 years, 6 months. At the time of<br />

testing, they had been implanted binaurally between 2 years and 6 years, 1<br />

month. The improvement between binaural and monaural conditions ranged<br />

from 10% to 40%.<br />

For two children there was no difference between monaural and binaural<br />

test performance. The children were first implanted at ages 2 years, 1 month<br />

and 9 years, 1 month. The first child was bilaterally implanted eight months<br />

later, and the other child received a second implant 2 years, 8 months later.<br />

The binaural performance of one child (WJ) was worse than monaural<br />

358 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


Figure 6. Speech discrimination in noise in monaural and binaural conditions.<br />

performance. This particular child was first implanted at the age of 3 years, 5<br />

months and received the second implant 5 years, 5 months later. At the time<br />

of testing, the child had been bilaterally implanted for a period of only nine<br />

months.<br />

Localization<br />

The ability of the children with bilateral implants to localize sound was<br />

tested in a five-speaker arrangement, usually at 60 dB with 1 kHz narrowband<br />

noise. The speaker arrangement is shown in Figure 7. The child was<br />

instructed to point in the direction of the presented sound signal. When the<br />

child pointed correctly, he was reinforced by a light inside the speaker. Localization<br />

was rated only after a child was able to reliably understand the<br />

directions “left” and “right.” To assist the child in learning to localize sound<br />

sources, early interventionists provided special training emphasizing directional<br />

hearing during the fitting and rehabilitation phase. This training was<br />

even more important when the child had not had sufficient hearing to localize<br />

prior to implantation.<br />

Localization Performance<br />

Ten of the 41 children were able to localize sound in a test situation. They<br />

ranged in age from 3 years, 2 months to 12 years, 5 months. All had been<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 359


Figure 7. Loudspeaker arrangement for testing localization.<br />

bilaterally fitted for a minimum of two years. The children had been bilaterally<br />

implanted for a period of time ranging from 2 years to 6 years, 1 month.<br />

The children received their first implant at ages ranging from 6 months to 4<br />

years, 1 month and the second between 1 year, 2 months and 10 years, 4<br />

months. The length of time between the first and second surgeries varied<br />

from 8 months to 7 years, 6 months. The results indicate that even children<br />

who were implanted at older ages were able to develop sound localization<br />

ability. The children who were able to demonstrate localization in the test<br />

situation had worn two implants between 2 years and 6 years, 1 month. The<br />

parents of those children who had less than one year of unilateral implant<br />

experience reported that they were also able to localize the sources of sounds<br />

in daily life. Among the children who did not demonstrate localization in the<br />

clinical setting, the majority of their parents reported that they were able to<br />

localize the direction of sound sources in daily life.<br />

On the basis of parents’ reports and clinical experiences, localization<br />

abilities typically become more precise over time (Mueller, 2005). Important<br />

factors in localizing environmental sounds and voices, especially in an uncontrolled<br />

acoustic environment are room acoustics and the characteristics of<br />

the sound produced.<br />

Discussion and Summary<br />

It should be reiterated that the results reported here were all obtained as<br />

part of routine clinical evaluations of performance with cochlear implants and<br />

are not research data. The group of children was heterogeneous. Their<br />

chronological and developmental ages varied widely, which affected their<br />

attention spans and motivation to complete a task. Because of the variability<br />

in child characteristics, not all children responded to the same tasks. Thus, the<br />

360 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


findings cannot be statistically analyzed but are presented as descriptive<br />

information.<br />

All of the children reported upon here wore bilateral cochlear implants,<br />

and as is typically the case for children with implants, had average aided<br />

thresholds of 30 dB HL across the speech spectrum. Thus, all the children had<br />

aided thresholds allowing them to detect sound in both binaural and monaural<br />

conditions.<br />

Ten of 41 children were able to reliably localize narrow-band noise at least<br />

in terms of “left” and “right.” All of the children who were able to localize<br />

had been bilaterally implanted for a minimum of two years. In a similar<br />

study, Mueller (2005) showed that children implanted bilaterally for 18<br />

months demonstrated localization to a sound source. Because the information<br />

reported here is drawn from clinical records, it is not possible to report the<br />

results of fixed time interval testing.<br />

As reported by Kuehn-Inacker et al. (2004), several children who were able<br />

to respond to both monaural and binaural speech discrimination testing demonstrated<br />

improvement in speech discrimination with two implants compared<br />

to the monaural condition. Performance could not be attributed to the<br />

age at which the child received the initial implant or the second implant, or<br />

the amount of time that had elapsed between the first and second fittings. A<br />

similar lack of correlation between age of implantations or time between the<br />

fittings was reported by Kuehn-Inacker et al. (2004).<br />

Testing of speech discrimination in noise was successfully conducted in 12<br />

of the children. Most of these children showed better performance in binaural<br />

compared to the monaural condition. As in discrimination of speech in quiet,<br />

there was no correlation between scores and the age of the first or the second<br />

implant, or the amount of time that had elapsed between the two implantations.<br />

All of the children accepted the second implant from the time of initial<br />

fitting and have worn both implants consistently. Generally, bilateral fitting<br />

has brought about improved performance in complex listening situations in<br />

all of the children discussed here. This improved performance was not obvious<br />

in all of the test situation results, but parents and children have reported<br />

the benefits of bilateral implantation. These include increased self-esteem and<br />

self-assurance, faster orientation to acoustical changes in the environment,<br />

decreased listening effort, longer attention span, and better concentration.<br />

Parents reported feeling more confident about their children’s safety in situations<br />

such as roads with busy traffic. They also reported that their children<br />

are able to become comfortable more quickly in unfamiliar situations. Parents<br />

and children reported feeling positive about their decision to have a second<br />

implant.<br />

An advantage of bilateral implantation in general is the fact that, in those<br />

cases of technical or mechanical malfunction or defect of one device, the<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 361


children will retain monaural stimulation. For school-age children, this auditory<br />

input can be critical. In addition, the children have access to auditory<br />

information in their everyday lives (Brill, 2004).<br />

In general, it has been the experience in this clinic that when there is a<br />

shorter time period between the first and second implant fitting, the aural<br />

habilitation therapy can be conducted simultaneously. In this situation there<br />

is less effort for the child to integrate the new sound provided by the second<br />

implant and use both implants comfortably within a shorter period of time.<br />

Not all of the children demonstrated improved bilateral performance in the<br />

test situations. It is important to know, however, that all of the children<br />

accepted the second implant and have worn bilateral fittings constantly. Additionally,<br />

the parents and the children both report decreased listening effort<br />

and improvements in daily life activities with the second implant.<br />

Among all of the children who were bilaterally implanted at an older age,<br />

the timespan between the first and second surgeries was longer than the time<br />

between the first and second implant surgeries of children implanted at a<br />

younger age. They required a longer period of aural habilitation and a longer<br />

time was required to achieve performance comparable to that of the younger<br />

children with bilateral fittings.<br />

The decision to proceed with a second cochlear implant must be made<br />

carefully. It requires close cooperation among the child the family, and professionals<br />

from a variety of disciplines, including pediatric audiologists, early<br />

interventionists, otolaryngologists, speech-language pathologists, teachers<br />

and other specialists. The decision to proceed with bilateral cochlear implantation<br />

must always be made with within the context of each child’s unique<br />

family situation as well as their social, emotional and educational status and<br />

not simply upon improving hearing levels in both ears. Although having two<br />

ears available for listening is beneficial, it will not necessarily solve all of a<br />

child’s problems. In fact, occasionally the expectations placed upon a child<br />

with bilateral implants by parents, teachers and professionals can be overwhelming<br />

to the child. Thus, expectations must be reasonable and realistic.<br />

References<br />

Biesalski, P., Lippert, K., & Bohnert, A. (1985). Erfahrungen in der audiologisch-rehabilitativen<br />

Betreuung von gehoerlosen Schulkindern mit einer<br />

Kochlea-Implant-Therapie. Sprache-Stimme-Gehoer, 9, 29-33.<br />

Blauert, J. (1974). Raeumliches Hoeren. Stuttgart: S. Hirzel Verlag.<br />

Bohnert, A. (2005). Best-practice procedures: The benefit in daily routine—A<br />

case study. In R. Seewald & J. Bamford (Eds.), A Sound Foundation Through<br />

Early Amplification 2004 (pp. 261-269). Staefa, Switzerland: Phonak AG.<br />

Brill, S. (2004). Bilaterale CI-anpassung. Die Schnecke, 5, 13-15.<br />

Govaerts, P., DeBeukelaer, C., Daemers, K., DeCeulaer, G., Yperman, M.,<br />

362 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


Somers, T., et al. (2002). Outcome of cochlear implantation at different ages<br />

from 0-6 years. Otology & Neurotology, 23(6), 885-890.<br />

Hoth, S. (2006). Die bilaterale Versorgung mit Cochlea Implantaten. Von der<br />

ADANO erarbeitetes und vom Praesidium der Deutschen Gesellschaft fuer<br />

HNO-Heilkunde, Kopf- und Halschirurgie e.V. verabschiedetes Positionspapier.<br />

HNO, 54, 77.<br />

Keilmann, A., Gaida, A., & Limberger, A. (2005). Laesst sich der Erfolg einer<br />

CI-Operation bei Kindern voraussagen? Die Schnecke, 17, 18-20.<br />

Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., Heid, S., & Klinke, R. (2002a). Delayed<br />

maturation and sensitive periods in the auditory cortex. Audiology and Neurotology,<br />

6, 346-362.<br />

Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., Heid, S., & Klinke, R. (2002b). Hearing after<br />

congenital deafness: Central auditory plasticity and sensory deprivation.<br />

Cerebral Cortex, 12, 797-807.<br />

Kuehn-Inacker, H., Shehata-Dieler, W., Mueller, J., & Helms, J. (2004). Bilateral<br />

cochlear implants: A way to optimize auditory perception abilities in<br />

deaf children? International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 1257-<br />

1266.<br />

Laszig, R., Aschendorff, A., Stecker, M., Mueller-Deile, J., Maune, S., Dillier,<br />

N., et al. (2004). Benefits of bilateral electrical stimulation with the nucleus<br />

cochlear implant in adults: 6-month postoperative results. Otology & Neurotology,<br />

25, 958-968.<br />

Lippert, K., & Bohnert, A. (2005). Optimierung bei bimodaler Versorgung—<br />

Eine Fallstudie. Zeitschrift für Audiologie/Audiological Acoustics, 44, 95-96.<br />

Manrique, M., Cervera-Paz, F., Huarte, A., & Molina, M. (2004). Advantages<br />

of a cochlear implantation in prelingual deaf children before 2 years of age<br />

when compared with later implantation. Laryngoscope, 114, 1462-1469.<br />

Mueller, J. (1998). Erste Ergebnisse der bilateralen Cochlea Implant Versorgung.<br />

European Archives of Otorhinolaryngology, 255 (38), 743-749.<br />

Mueller, J. (1999). Angemeldete Diskussionsbemerkung. Aachen: Deutscher<br />

HNO-Kongress.<br />

Mueller, J. (2005). Die apparative Versorgung der Schwerhoerigkeit: Cochlea-<br />

Implantate und Hirnstammimplantate—Aktuelle Entwicklungen der letzten<br />

10 Jahre. Laryngo-Rhino-Otologie, 84(Suppl.), 60-73.<br />

Offeciers, E., Morera, C., Mueller, J., Huarte, A., Shallop, J., & Cavallé, L.<br />

(2005). International consensus on bilateral cochlear implants and bimodal<br />

stimulation. Second Meeting Consensus on Auditory Implants, 19-21 February<br />

2004, Valencia, Spain. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 125(9), 918-919.<br />

Schleich, P., Nopp, P., & D’Haese, P. (2004). Head shadow, squelch, and<br />

summation effects in bilateral users of the MED-EL COMBI40/40+ cochlear<br />

implant. Ear and Hearing, 25(3), 197-204.<br />

Sharma, A., Dorman, M., Spah, A., & Todd, N. (2002). Early cochlear implantation<br />

in children allows normal development of central auditory pathways.<br />

Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology 189(Suppl.), 38-41.<br />

Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children 363


van Hoesel, R., Tong, Y., Hallow, R., & Clark, G. (1992). Psychophysical and<br />

speech perception studies: A case report on a binaural cochlear implant<br />

subject. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94, 3178-3189.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2002). The social-emotional ramifications of universal<br />

newborn hearing screening early identification and intervention of children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing. In R. Seewald & J. Gravel, J. (Eds.), A sound<br />

foundation through early amplification 2001. Proceedings of the second international<br />

conference (pp. 221-231). Staefa, Switzerland: Phonak AG.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A., Coulter, D., & Mehl, A. (1998). Language of<br />

early and late identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics, 102, 1161-<br />

1171.<br />

Zorowka, P., Heinemann, M., & Bohnert, A. (1995). Der Einsatz der Lautheitsskalierung<br />

der Hoergeraetefeinanpassung im Kindesalter. In M.<br />

Gross (Ed)., Aktuelle Phoniatrisch-Paedaudiologische Aspekte, Volume 3 (pp.<br />

205-210). Berlin: Renate Gross Verlag.<br />

Zwolan, T., Ashbaugh, C., Alarfaj, A., Kileny, P., Arts, H., El-Kashlan, H., &<br />

Telian, S. (2004). Pediatric cochlear implant patient performance as a function<br />

of age at implantation. Otology & Neurotology, 25, 112-120.<br />

364 Bohnert, Spitzlei, Lippert, & Keilmann


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 365-379<br />

Early Cochlear Implant<br />

Experience and Emotional<br />

Functioning During<br />

Childhood: Loneliness in<br />

Middle and Late Childhood<br />

Efrat A. Schorr, Ph.D.<br />

The importance of early intervention for children with hearing loss has been demonstrated<br />

persuasively in areas including speech perception and production and<br />

spoken language. The present research shows that feelings of loneliness, a significant<br />

emotional outcome, are affected by the age at which children receive intervention with<br />

cochlear implants. Earliest cochlear implantation is associated with lowest levels of<br />

loneliness during middle and late childhood. Compared to children with normal<br />

hearing, children with cochlear implants are not more lonely than their peers with<br />

normal hearing.<br />

Introduction<br />

Awareness of the importance of early intervention for all children with<br />

disabilities has steadily grown in recent decades. Great strides are being made<br />

in the early detection and intervention for infants and children with hearing<br />

loss. In the United States and other countries, the introduction and increased<br />

use of universal newborn hearing screening programs has made it possible to<br />

detect hearing loss in the first days of life. The Joint Committee on Infant<br />

Hearing (2000) has advocated hearing screening for all newborns, with the<br />

goal of confirming hearing loss before 3 months of age and beginning intervention<br />

services before 6 months of age.<br />

The availability of improved technological means to assess hearing in infants<br />

and young children and to habilitate hearing loss with advanced digital<br />

hearing aids and cochlear implants has changed the prospects for many children<br />

with severe and profound hearing loss. Research has shown that infants<br />

Efrat A. Schorr, Ph.D., is a postdoctoral fellow at the Gonda Brain Research Center at Bar Ilan<br />

University, Ramat Gan, Israel.<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 365


with hearing loss who receive early intervention services before 6 months of<br />

age may display age-appropriate language skills at 5 years (Yoshinaga-Itano,<br />

1995; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl, 1998). A growing body of<br />

research literature demonstrates that children with congenital hearing loss<br />

who receive cochlear implants at young ages show improved cognitive<br />

(Sharma, Dorman, & Spahr, 2003), language (Nicholas & Geers, 2006; Tomblin,<br />

Barker, Spencer, Zhang, & Gantz, 2005) and hearing and speech perception<br />

outcomes (Harrison, Gordon, & Mount, 2005; Nikolopoulos, Gibbin, &<br />

Dyar, 2004; Schorr, Fox, vanWassenhove, & Knudsen, 2005; Svirsky, Teoh, &<br />

Neuberger, 2004) compared with children who receive the implants later.<br />

Less is known about the effects of early intervention for children with<br />

hearing loss on social and emotional outcomes. Because more children are<br />

receiving cochlear implants at younger ages, it is imperative that professionals<br />

and researchers from across disciplines work together to learn more about<br />

the effects of early cochlear implantation on the wide range of social and<br />

emotional aspects of child development, such as the development of selfregulation<br />

and self-control, social competence, and peer relationships. Understanding<br />

the social and emotional implications of an early period of<br />

deafness followed by the unique experience of hearing with a cochlear implant<br />

is essential to supporting the development of this growing group of<br />

children. The study presented here is an introductory chapter of the multifaceted<br />

research goals that lie ahead.<br />

Children as young as 5 years demonstrate an understanding of feelings of<br />

loneliness (Cassidy & Asher, 1992). They are able to explain that loneliness<br />

involves both being alone and feeling sad or depressed, a very close definition<br />

to that given by adults (see Peplau & Perlman, 1982, for a general discussion<br />

of loneliness). Research has demonstrated that loneliness in middle childhood<br />

is not a fleeting mood; rather, it is a stable and often long-lasting problem for<br />

children (Hymel & Franke, 1985; Renshaw & Brown, 1993).<br />

Loneliness during childhood develops when children feel that they are not<br />

socially competent and feel rejected by their peers over a relatively long<br />

period of time (Asher & Wheeler, 1985; Renshaw & Brown, 1993). The children<br />

who are most susceptible to feelings of loneliness are those who are<br />

rejected by their peers and are characterized as being socially withdrawn or<br />

inhibited (Cassidy & Asher, 1992; Crick & Ladd, 1993; Parkhurst & Asher,<br />

1992). In addition, children who express feelings of loneliness and social<br />

dissatisfaction in the school setting often have distinct behavioral characteristics.<br />

Young children who reported feeling very lonely at school demonstrated<br />

more aggressive and disruptive behaviors and were viewed as being<br />

less prosocial and shyer than children who did not feel very lonely (Cassidy<br />

& Asher, 1992). Having a close friend, however, has been identified as a<br />

protective factor that buffers children from feeling very lonely even if they are<br />

socially rejected from the peer group (Renshaw & Brown, 1993; Rubin, Coplan,<br />

Nelson, Cheah, & Lagace-Seguin, 1999).<br />

366 Schorr


Hearing loss places children at a specific risk for failure to establish and<br />

maintain positive social relationships with peers (Marschark, 1993). Evidence<br />

suggests that children with hearing loss (without cochlear implants) who are<br />

placed in mainstream schools and classrooms with children with normal<br />

hearing do not form close relationships with their peers (Antia, 1982; Calderon,<br />

2000; Farrigua & Austin, 1980; Greenberg & Kusche, 1989, 1993; Marschark,<br />

1993; Suarez, 2000). For example, Stinson and Lang (1994) described<br />

social outcomes for children with hearing loss (without cochlear implants) in<br />

mainstream school settings, reporting that “the likely consequence of mainstreaming<br />

is social isolation, not integration, and this kind of social experience<br />

is not conducive to the deaf child’s social development” (p. 157). Research on<br />

the social experiences of children with hearing loss (without cochlear implants)<br />

in mainstream educational settings has found that these children feel<br />

isolated and lonely in their school environments (Gaustad & Kluwin, 1992;<br />

Mertens, 1989; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995; Saur, Popp-Stone, & Hurley-<br />

Lawrence, 1987).<br />

It is important to examine the feelings of loneliness in children with cochlear<br />

implants, especially in light of the possibility that they are often the<br />

only child with hearing loss in their mainstream educational setting (Stinson<br />

& Lang, 1994). Furthermore, critics of cochlear implants have warned about<br />

the potential identity problems facing children with prelingual hearing loss<br />

who receive cochlear implants as young children. They argue that these children<br />

are unable to truly take part in the hearing world and will inevitably be<br />

excluded from full social lives with hearing people (Crouch, 1997; Lane, 1997).<br />

In the present investigation, we sought to assess the feelings of loneliness<br />

in children with cochlear implants. We asked several specific questions about<br />

loneliness. (1) Are children with cochlear implants lonelier than children with<br />

normal hearing? (2) Do early experience-related factors, such as the age at<br />

which hearing loss is confirmed, age at which a child first receives hearing<br />

aids or age at which child receives a cochlear implant, influence loneliness<br />

later on in childhood? (3) Are present-day factors such as speech perception<br />

or language proficiency related to loneliness?<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Thirty-seven children (19 girls and 18 boys) with cochlear implants, ages 5<br />

to 14 (M = 9 years), participated in this study. All of the children were born<br />

deaf, had no additional significant disabilities (e.g., blindness, autism), used<br />

spoken English as their primary mode of communication and their native<br />

language, had nonverbal IQ scores in the average range (a minimum score of<br />

85 on the Matrices subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test [K-BIT;<br />

Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990]) and had a general language proficiency level of<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 367


5 years (i.e., met the age cut-off on both the Peabody Picture Vocabulary<br />

Test-III [PPVT-III; Dunn & Dunn, 1997] and the age-appropriate Grammatic<br />

Understanding subtest of the Test of Language Development-Primary<br />

[TOLD-P; Newcomer & Hammill, 1997], Test of Language Development-<br />

Intermediate [TOLD-I; Hammill & Newcomer, 1997] or Test of Adolescent<br />

and Adult Language-Third Edition [TOAL-3; Hammill, Brown, Larsen, &<br />

Wiederholt, 1994]).<br />

The age at which the children received their cochlear implant ranged from<br />

16 months to 8 years, 3 months, with a mean age at implant of 3 years, 2<br />

months. All children had a minimum of one year of cochlear implant experience<br />

at the time of participation in this study. The duration of cochlear<br />

implant use ranged from 19 months to 10 years, 6 months, with a mean of 5<br />

years, 9 months. The children were from the mid-Atlantic United States.<br />

Parent report indicated that 32 children were educated exclusively in mainstream<br />

or general education classrooms, three were educated in partial mainstream<br />

classrooms and two were educated in self-contained special education<br />

classrooms.<br />

The comparison group consisted of 37 children with normal hearing whose<br />

native language was English. They were matched individually to the children<br />

with cochlear implants by gender and age, within three months of the birth<br />

dates of the children with cochlear implants. All the children with normal<br />

hearing had nonverbal IQ scores in the average range (a minimum score of 85<br />

on the K-BIT Matrices subtest), and no additional disabilities.<br />

Loneliness Assessment<br />

Materials<br />

The Loneliness Scale (Asher & Wheeler, 1985, revised from Asher, Hymel,<br />

& Renshaw, 1984) was used to provide an account of feelings of loneliness.<br />

Responses to this 24-item questionnaire are provided on a five-point Likert<br />

scale. Sixteen items focus on feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction in<br />

school, and 8 items are “filler” items. The 16 primary items include four<br />

domains: (1) children’s feelings of loneliness (“I am lonely at school”), (2)<br />

children’s appraisal of their current peer relationships (“I don’t have any<br />

friends in class”), (3) children’s perceptions of the degree to which important<br />

relationship needs are being met (“There are no other kids I can go to when<br />

I need help at school”) and (4) children’s perceptions of their social competence<br />

(“I’m good at working with other children in my class”). Split-half<br />

reliability between forms is 0.83, and Cronbach’s alpha is 0.90 (Asher et al.,<br />

1984; Crick & Ladd, 1993). The participants were asked to respond to each<br />

item on a five-point scale (1 = that’s always true of me, 2 = that’s true about<br />

me most of the time, 3 = that’s sometimes true about me, 4 = that’s hardly ever<br />

true about me and 5 = that’s not true about me at all).<br />

368 Schorr


Nonverbal IQ Assessment<br />

Nonverbal intelligence was assessed with Matrices subtest of the K-<br />

BIT (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990). All test items contain pictures and abstract<br />

designs and require a pointing response so that cognitive ability can be assessed<br />

independently of oral language proficiency. Cognitive functioning information<br />

was used to ensure that each participant’s nonverbal intelligence<br />

was within normal limits.<br />

Speech Perception Assessment<br />

The participants’ ability to understand spoken language accurately was<br />

assessed with the Lexical Neighborhood Test (LNT) and Multisyllabic Lexical<br />

Neighborhood Test (MLNT) (Kirk, Pisoni, & Osberger, 1995). The LNT and<br />

MLNT measure speech perception through assessment of open-set word recognition.<br />

The LNT contains two 50-item lists of monosyllabic words, and the<br />

MLNT consists of two 24-item lists of two- and three-syllable words. In each<br />

test, half of the words are considered lexically “easy,” meaning that they are<br />

common and have few phonologically similar words. Half are considered<br />

lexically “hard,” meaning that they are words that occur infrequently and<br />

have many phonologically similar words. This pair of tests was designed<br />

specifically for use with children with cochlear implants. Good test-retest<br />

reliability (0.83 for lists one and two, both easy and hard components) and<br />

interlist equivalency have been demonstrated (Kirk, Eisenberg, Martinez, &<br />

Hay-McCutcheon, 1999). Percentage of correct words for the easy and hard<br />

word lists of both the LNT and MLNT were scored, and an average of the four<br />

subtests was calculated.<br />

Language Assessment<br />

General language proficiency was assessed with measures of receptive semantics<br />

and syntax. The PPVT-III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) was administered to<br />

all participants, and one of three different subtests of receptive syntax performance<br />

was administered to each. For participants ages 5 years to 7 years,<br />

11 months, the Grammatic Understanding subtest of the TOLD-P (Newcomer<br />

& Hammill, 1997) was used. For children ages 8 years to 11 years, 11 months<br />

the Grammatic Understanding subtest of the TOLD-I (Hammill & Newcomer,<br />

1997) was administered. For children ages 12 years to 14 years, 11 months, the<br />

Listening/Grammar subtest of the TOAL-3 (Hammill et al., 1994) was used.<br />

Language proficiency scores were computed with the sum of standard score<br />

obtained on the PPVT and the age-appropriate subtest of syntax.<br />

Procedure<br />

All of the participants took part in a one-day assessment at the Child<br />

Development Lab at the University of Maryland, College Park, or at another<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 369


location. Informed consent was obtained from the accompanying parent before<br />

beginning testing. The accompanying parent completed a questionnaire,<br />

providing information on the child’s audiological, educational and family<br />

background.<br />

All tests were administered individually to each participant in a quiet<br />

room. The loneliness, IQ, speech perception and language tests were administered<br />

in variable order. The examiner explained to the participant that his or<br />

her answers to all of the questions would remain confidential. All participants<br />

were given a written copy of the Loneliness Scale (Asher & Wheeler, 1985),<br />

and the examiner read each test item aloud, allowing participants time to<br />

respond on paper and ask questions if clarification was necessary. The test<br />

items were read orally to minimize the concern that reading ability might<br />

affect the reliability of the assessments. Scores were summed, with a range of<br />

16 (low loneliness) to 80 (high loneliness).<br />

Results<br />

Description of Feelings of Loneliness of Children with Cochlear Implants<br />

The participants with cochlear implants reported an average score of 32.62<br />

(SD = 10.78) on the Loneliness Scale out of the lowest possible score of 16 to<br />

the highest possible score of 80. This mean loneliness score is comparable to<br />

that of the sample reported in the Asher et al. (1984) original study describing<br />

the Loneliness Scale, which was 32.51 (SD = 11.82). There was considerable<br />

variability in the scores for the participants with cochlear implants, which<br />

ranged from 16 to 60 in the present study.<br />

Loneliness of Children with Cochlear Implants Compared to Children with<br />

Normal Hearing<br />

As a group, the participants with normal hearing reported an average score<br />

of 28.19 on the Loneliness Scale (SD = 8.32). An analysis of variance (ANOVA)<br />

was conducted to determine whether the cochlear implant and normal<br />

hearing groups differed in their reports of loneliness (see Table 1). The<br />

Table 1. Mean Differences in Loneliness by Group for Children with Cochlear<br />

Implants and Children with Normal Hearing<br />

Children With<br />

Cochlear Implants<br />

M (SD)<br />

Children With<br />

Normal Hearing<br />

M (SD) df F<br />

Loneliness average score 32.62 (10.78) 28.19 (8.32) 1,72 3.920<br />

Note. N = 37 for both study groups.<br />

370 Schorr


participants with cochlear implants did not significantly differ from the participants<br />

with normal hearing in their report of feelings of loneliness at school<br />

(p > 0.05).<br />

Effects of Early Experience-Related Variables on Loneliness<br />

Although the participants with cochlear implants did not report feeling<br />

significantly more lonely than the participants with normal hearing, quite a<br />

bit of variability was found in the scores obtained in the study group. Zeroorder<br />

correlations were generated to examine the relationship between loneliness<br />

and early experience-related variables (see Table 2).<br />

A linear regression procedure was used to assess the relationship between<br />

early experience-related variables and mean loneliness scores. Specifically,<br />

we tested whether the age hearing loss was confirmed, age at first amplification<br />

with hearing aids or age at activation of cochlear implant predicted the<br />

loneliness score. We used present chronological age as a covariate in the<br />

regression models to minimize the influence of maturation-related changes<br />

because of the wide age range of participants. The analyses revealed a nonsignificant<br />

relationship between the age hearing loss was confirmed [F(1,31)<br />

= 0.868, p > 0.05, R 2 change = 0.026] and age at first amplification with hearing<br />

aids [F(1,32) = 0.538, p > 0.05, R 2 change = 0.016]. A significant relationship<br />

between age at implant and loneliness score was found [F(1,34) = 4.836, p <<br />

0.05, R 2 change = 0.119), indicating that older age at implant was associated<br />

with higher loneliness scores (ß = 0.450). This model as a whole accounted for<br />

16.6% of the variance in loneliness scores. Figure 1 presents scatter plots of<br />

these relationships.<br />

In addition, we found that the type of amplification used before cochlear<br />

implant surgery was related to loneliness score. Thirty-five of 37 participants<br />

had used hearing aids before their cochlear implant activation (data were not<br />

available for two of the participants). Of those 35, nine also used FM personal<br />

sound systems (see Flexer, 1999, for details on FM technology) with their<br />

hearing aids. An ANOVA indicated that the participants who had used FM<br />

Table 2. Correlations Between Early Experience-Related Variables and Loneliness<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

1. Loneliness score — –0.113 –0.084 –0.121 0.403*<br />

2. Age hearing loss confirmed — 0.984** 0.852** 0.379*<br />

3. Age at first amplification — 0.841** 0.379*<br />

4. Age intervention program began — 0.265<br />

5. Age at implantation —<br />

Note. Significant correlations appear in bold type.<br />

*Significant at the 0.05 level.<br />

**Significant at the 0.01 level.<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 371


Figure 1. Loneliness score and age at implantation, age that hearing loss was<br />

confirmed and age at first use of amplification.<br />

systems were significantly less lonely than participants who had only used<br />

hearing aids [F(1,33) = 6.529, p > 0.05). The participants who had used FM<br />

systems (n = 9) had a mean loneliness score of 26.33 (SD = 5.20), with a<br />

minimum score of 20 and a maximum score of 34. The participants who used<br />

only hearing aids (n = 26) had a mean loneliness score of 35.92 (SD = 10.76),<br />

372 Schorr


with a minimum score of 16 and a maximum score of 60. The hearing aids<br />

group versus hearing aids plus FM group also differed in chronological age<br />

and age at implantation. The participants who had used FM systems were<br />

younger (91 versus 116 months of age at testing) and received their cochlear<br />

implant at a younger age (23 versus 45 months). We interpret these results<br />

with caution because of the unequal size of the groups (n = 26 versus n=9),<br />

but they can provide some guidance for best practices for intervention with<br />

children with cochlear implants.<br />

Relationship Between Present-Day Factors and Loneliness<br />

Regression models showed a relationship between the age at which participants<br />

received their cochlear implant and their loneliness score. We<br />

wanted to examine whether speech perception or spoken language skills<br />

served as the mediator for this association. In other words, we asked whether<br />

early age of implantation leads to better speech perception and spoken language<br />

skills, which, in turn, affect loneliness; i.e., the better one can communicate,<br />

the less lonely one might feel. In this scenario, loneliness is affected<br />

only indirectly by early experience variability in age of implantation because<br />

speech perception or language skills are the factors that truly influence loneliness.<br />

Zero-order correlations were computed to examine the relationships between<br />

loneliness and speech perception and language skills (see Table 3).<br />

Speech perception and language skills were closely related to each other, but<br />

neither of these variables corresponded to the mean loneliness score in a<br />

significant way. Regression models using chronological age and speech perception<br />

and chronological age and language skills as covariates were computed.<br />

In the two models, neither speech perception [F(1,33) = 1.548, p > 0.05,<br />

R 2 change = 0.043] nor language skills [F(1,34) = 0.495, p > 0.05, R 2 change =<br />

0.014] accounted for the variability in the loneliness scores.<br />

Because of the null findings in these regression analyses, a post-hoc power<br />

Table 3. Correlations Between Speech Perception and Language Skills<br />

and Loneliness<br />

1 2 3 4<br />

1. Loneliness score — –0.<strong>106</strong> –0.203 0.403*<br />

2. Speech perception score — 0.703** 0.126<br />

3. Composite language score — –0.056<br />

4. Age at implantation —<br />

Note. Significant correlations appear in bold type.<br />

*Significant at the 0.05 level.<br />

**Significant at the 0.01 level.<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 373


analysis was conducted to determine whether there was sufficient power in<br />

the research design to permit detection of a true effect. For the regression<br />

analysis involving speech perception as the dependent variable, the observed<br />

power value of 0.35 was indicated (confidence intervals of 95% for B = –0.133<br />

to 0.551). For the regression analysis involving language skill as the dependent<br />

variable, the observed power was 0.24 (confidence intervals of 95% for<br />

B=–0.256 to 0.124). Both of these effect sizes are relatively small, indicating<br />

that the failure to find significant effects of the speech and language variables<br />

on the loneliness measure could be the result of insufficient sample size rather<br />

than the weak connection between these variables. Nonetheless, our findings<br />

indicate that in this sample, speech perception and language skills are not the<br />

factors that influence loneliness. Rather, there appears to be a direct link<br />

between age of implantation and feelings of loneliness in middle and late<br />

childhood that is present regardless of speech perception and language skills.<br />

Discussion<br />

The purpose of this study was to assess the feelings of loneliness of children<br />

with cochlear implants and to try to place this investigation within the<br />

broader context of loneliness among a normative sample of children with<br />

normal hearing. As well, we wanted to investigate the influence of factors that<br />

are unique to children with cochlear implants on loneliness. The participants<br />

with cochlear implants reported feelings of loneliness at school that were<br />

relatively low. These children appear to perceive a sense of support and feel<br />

included in their school settings. These findings are in contrast with the<br />

reports of social exclusion in school from children with hearing loss without<br />

cochlear implants (i.e., Stinson & Lang, 1994). Several potentially significant<br />

differences were found between the experiences of the present sample of<br />

children with cochlear implants and those without cochlear implants reported<br />

previously in the literature. First, children with cochlear implants in<br />

the present study all used spoken language fluently, which allowed them to<br />

participate in their classrooms without the need for a sign language interpreter.<br />

This ability also allowed them to spend most, if not all, of their time<br />

in the inclusive classroom as opposed to being a “visitor” who came and went<br />

from the classroom frequently. These factors could permit the child to become<br />

a true “member” of the classroom social group, despite his or her hearing loss<br />

(see Antia, Stinson, & Gaustad, 2002, for a discussion of “membership” and<br />

“visitorship” for students with hearing loss in inclusive education).<br />

The participants with cochlear implants reported feelings of loneliness<br />

that were very similar to the participants with normal hearing. This result<br />

seems to run counter to the arguments of cochlear implant opponents who<br />

felt that children with cochlear implants would inevitably feel excluded from<br />

the hearing social world around them. It may be helpful for parents and<br />

professionals to know that children with cochlear implants are not necessarily<br />

374 Schorr


lonelier than their peers with normal hearing. Indeed, we found significant<br />

variability in the feelings of loneliness of the participants with cochlear implants—some<br />

reported feeling quite lonely at school—but the results overall<br />

show that being a child with hearing loss with a cochlear implant in a mainstream<br />

school does not necessarily mean they will be lonely.<br />

There appears to be a strong connection between the age at which a child<br />

receives a cochlear implant and feelings of loneliness at school years later.<br />

Also, it seems that while the development of speech and language skills is<br />

essential to social functioning, it alone does not determine whether a child<br />

will feel lonely in school. Rather, one reasonable interpretation of the results<br />

is that the opportunity for children with congenital hearing loss to truly<br />

participate in their family and school environments begins when they receive<br />

their cochlear implants. Thus, perhaps the participants who received their<br />

implants at the youngest age developed feelings of belonging and inclusionin<br />

their school settings in a similar manner as the participants with normal<br />

hearing. Although they may catch up impressively in their speech and language<br />

skills, children who received their cochlear implants at older ages may<br />

take longer to attain a feeling of belonging or may have difficulty feeling that<br />

they belong at all.<br />

The curious results of the relationship of FM system use to loneliness,<br />

despite the nonsignificant relationships between other early amplification<br />

factors, may be understood in the following manner. It seems that, auditory<br />

experience before cochlear implantation per se does not have the same effect<br />

on feelings of loneliness as it does after the cochlear implant. Auditory experience<br />

prior to implantation may not play a significant or obvious role in<br />

later emotional outcomes because children with profound to severe hearing<br />

loss may derive very little benefit from the hearing aids used prior to implantation.<br />

However, FM system use may have an important influence on the<br />

attitudes of parents, teachers and other significant adults. Perhaps using an<br />

FM system encourages parents and teachers to talk to the child with hearing<br />

loss, to include the child in the conversation, and to think of him or her as a<br />

part of the goings on in a way that they quite naturally might not if the FM<br />

system were not in use. Thus, the use of an FM system with hearing aids<br />

before early cochlear implantation represents the “best practice” to prepare<br />

children with hearing loss to tune in to the conversations taking place in their<br />

environment and be primed to pay attention. If understood in this light, the<br />

finding that early FM system use is associated with feelings of loneliness in<br />

school later on in childhood is understandable.<br />

The results of the present study imply that the advantages of early cochlear<br />

implantation extend to the domain of emotional functioning. They seem to<br />

indicate that although cochlear implants provide the potential to acquire<br />

good speech and language skills; this is not all they do. Arguably, they provide<br />

the potential for children to develop healthy emotional perspectives of<br />

themselves and their place in the world around them. Once the children<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 375


eceive cochlear implants, significant others may feel that they are real communication<br />

partners, and importantly, the children begin to view themselves<br />

as capable communication partners.<br />

“Best practice” is an important idea to consider for children with hearing<br />

loss. Although early identification of hearing loss and early amplification<br />

with hearing aids did not predict feelings of loneliness outcomes, early cochlear<br />

implantation did. However, they are essentially related because early<br />

cochlear implantation is only a possibility for children if hearing loss is detected<br />

and the intervention process is begun early. In light of the reality of the<br />

cochlear implant candidacy process and the often cumbersome journey leading<br />

up to cochlear implant surgery, early cochlear implantation only is possible<br />

when hearing loss is detected at birth or soon afterward. Based on the<br />

results of the present study, the provision of cochlear implants to appropriate<br />

candidates as early as suitable should be considered the best practice for<br />

children with severe to profound hearing loss. As well, the results of this<br />

study imply that we should not discount the importance of amplification with<br />

appropriate hearing aids and FM systems before the implant surgery because<br />

their use may be important for reasons that extend beyond the raw numbers<br />

of decibel gain with the devices. Rather, hearing aids and FM systems may<br />

influence the important adults (as well as the child him or herself) to consider<br />

the child with hearing loss as a listener.<br />

Finally, some of the participants with cochlear implants reported feeling<br />

quite lonely in their classrooms. It is important to have this information<br />

because strategies can be sought to help these children feel less lonely in<br />

school. Although it would not be a simple proposition to intervene and ameliorate<br />

feelings of loneliness, awareness of the problem is a necessary first step<br />

in that direction (see Margalit, 1996, for a discussion of intervention strategies<br />

for loneliness in children with disabilities).<br />

This study contributes insights into the exploration of the emotional and<br />

social impact of cochlear implant use on the lives of children. Further research<br />

is needed to learn whether loneliness is related to other emotional or social<br />

issues for children with cochlear implants. More information also is needed to<br />

gain a better understanding of many other emotional aspects of development<br />

that may be affected by hearing loss and the use of a cochlear implant.<br />

Longitudinal research would provide a more comprehensive portrait of emotional<br />

development in children with cochlear implants. We hope this study<br />

will raise awareness of the importance of emotional issues in development<br />

and that more research will follow.<br />

In conclusion, the study reported here emphasizes the importance of early<br />

detection and intervention for children with hearing loss as essential to<br />

speech and language development. Early intervention also has important<br />

effects on feelings of loneliness in these children. The trends in the United<br />

States and other countries toward earlier detection and intervention for children<br />

with hearing loss are encouraging, and it is crucial to raise awareness so<br />

376 Schorr


that all children can benefit from the earliest detection and appropriate intervention<br />

for hearing loss.<br />

Acknowledgments<br />

This research was supported by the American Hearing Research Foundation<br />

and a National Research Service Award grant (F31 DC006204-01A1) to<br />

E.A.S. from the National Institutes of Deafness and Other Communication<br />

Disorders, National Institutes of Health. The author would like to thank the<br />

children and parents who participated in the study. Sincere appreciation and<br />

thanks go to Nathan A. Fox and Froma P. Roth at the University of Maryland<br />

for their guidance and support for this project.<br />

References<br />

Antia, S. (1982). Social interaction of partially mainstreamed hearingimpaired<br />

children. American Annals of the Deaf, 127, 18-25.<br />

Antia, S.D., Stinson, M.S., & Gaustad, M.G. (2002). Developing membershipin<br />

the education of deaf and hard-of-hearing students in inclusive settings.<br />

Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7, 214-229.<br />

Asher, S.R., & Wheeler, V.A. (1985). Children’s loneliness: A comparison of<br />

rejected, and neglected peer status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,<br />

53, 500-505.<br />

Asher, S.R., Hymel, S., & Renshaw, P.D. (1984). Loneliness in children. Child<br />

Development, 55, 1456-1464.<br />

Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children’s education programs<br />

as a predictor of child’s language, early reading, and social emotional<br />

development. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 140-155.<br />

Cassidy, J., & Asher, S.R. (1992). Loneliness and peer relations in young<br />

children. Child Development, 63, 350-365.<br />

Crick, N.R., & Ladd, G.W. (1993). Children’s perceptions of their peer experiences:<br />

Attributions, loneliness, social anxiety, and social avoidance. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 29, 244-254.<br />

Crouch, R.A. (1997). Letting the deaf be deaf: Reconsidering the use of cochlear<br />

implants in prelingually deaf children. Hastings Center Report, 27,<br />

14-21.<br />

Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Circle<br />

Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Farrigua, D., & Austin, G. (1980). A study of socio-emotional adjustment<br />

patterns of hearing-impaired students in different educational settings.<br />

American Annals of the Deaf, 125, 535-541.<br />

Flexer, C.A. (1999). Facilitating Hearing and Listening in Young Children (2 nd<br />

ed.). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.<br />

Gaustad, M., & Kluwin, T. (1992). Patterns of communication among deaf and<br />

hearing adolescents. In T. Kluwin, D. Moores, & M. Gaustad (Eds.), Toward<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 377


effective public school programs for deaf students (pp. 107-128). New York:<br />

Teachers College Press.<br />

Greenberg, M.T., & Kusche, C.A. (1989). Cognitive, personal, and social development<br />

of deaf children and adolescents. In M. Wang, M. Reynolds, &<br />

H. Walberg (Eds.), Handbook of special education: Research and practice (Vol. 3;<br />

pp. 95-129). New York: Pergamon Press.<br />

Greenberg, M.T., & Kusche, C.A. (1993). Promoting social and emotional development<br />

in deaf children: The PATHS project. Seattle, WA: University of Washington<br />

Press.<br />

Hammill, D.D., & Newcomer, P.L. (1997). Test of Language Development–<br />

Intermediate (TOLD-I: 3 rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Hammill, D.D., Brown, V.L., Larsen, S. C., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1994). Test of<br />

Adolescent and Adult Language–Third Edition (TOAL-3). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

Harrison, R.V., Gordon, K.A., & Mount, R.J. (2005). Is there a critical period<br />

for cochlear implantation in congenitally deaf children? Analyses of hearing<br />

and speech perception performance after implantation. Developmental<br />

Psychobiology, 46, 252-261.<br />

Hymel, S., & Franke, S. (1985). Children’s peer relations: Assessing selfperceptions.<br />

In B. H. Schnieder, K. H. Rubin, & J. E. Ledingham (Eds.),<br />

Children’s peer relations: Issues in assessment and intervention (pp. 75-91). New<br />

York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. (2000). Year 2000 position statement:<br />

Principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs.<br />

Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, 798-817.<br />

Kaufman, A.S., & Kaufman, N.L. (1990). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT).<br />

Circle Pines: MN: American Guidance Service.<br />

Kirk, K.I., Pisoni, D.B., & Osberger, M.J. (1995). Lexical effects of spoken word<br />

recognition by pediatric cochlear implant users. Ear and Hearing, 16, 470-481.<br />

Kirk, K.I., Eisenberg, L.S., Martinez, A.S., & Hay-McCutcheon, M. (1999).<br />

Lexical Neighborhood Test: Test-retest reliability and interlist equivalency.<br />

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 10, 113-123.<br />

Lane, H. (1997). Ethical issues in cochlear implant surgery: An exploration<br />

into disease, disability, and the best interests of the child. Kennedy Institute<br />

of Ethics Journal, 7, 231-251.<br />

Margalit, M. (1996). Loneliness among children with special needs: Theory, research,<br />

coping, and intervention. New York: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Marschark, M. (1993). Psychological development of deaf children. New York:<br />

Oxford University Press.<br />

Mertens, D. (1989). Social experiences of hearing-impaired high school youth.<br />

American Annals of the Deaf, 134, 15-19.<br />

Newcomb, A.F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship relations: A<br />

meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 306-347.<br />

Newcomer, P.L., & Hammill, D.D. (1997). Test of Language Development–Primary<br />

(TOLD-P: 3 rd ed). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.<br />

378 Schorr


Nicholas, J.G., & Geers, A.E. (2006). Effects of early auditory experienceonthe<br />

spoken language of deaf children at 3 years of age. Ear and Hearing, 27,<br />

286-289.<br />

Nikolopoulos, T.P., Gibbin, K.P., & Dyar, D. (2004). Predicting speech perception<br />

outcomes following cochlear implantation using Nottingham children’s<br />

implant profile (NChIP). International Journal of Pediatric<br />

Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 137-141.<br />

Parkhurst, J.T., & Asher, S.R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle school: Subgroup<br />

differences in behavior, loneliness, and interpersonal concerns. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 28, 231-241.<br />

Peplau, L.A., & Perlman, D. (1982). Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory,<br />

research, and therapy. New York: Wiley.<br />

Renshaw, P.D., & Brown, P.J. (1993). Loneliness in middle childhood: Concurrent<br />

and longitudinal predictors. Child Development, 64, 1271-1284.<br />

Rubin, K.H., Coplan, R.J., Nelson, L.J., Cheah, C.S.L., & Lagace-Seguin, D.G.<br />

(1999). Peer relationships in childhood. In M.H. Bornstein & M.E. Lamb<br />

(Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (4 th ed., pp. 451-501).<br />

Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Saur, R., Popp-Stone, M., & Hurley-Lawrence, E. (1987). The classroom participation<br />

of mainstreamed hearing-impaired college students. Volta Review,<br />

89, 277-287.<br />

Schorr, E.A., Fox, N.A., vanWassenhove, V., & Knudsen, E.I. (2005). Audiovisual<br />

fusion in speech perception in children with cochlear implants. Proceedings<br />

of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 18748-18750.<br />

Sharma, A., Dorman, M.F., & Spahr, A.J. (2003). A sensitive period for the<br />

development of the central auditory system in children with cochlear implants:<br />

Implications for age of implantation. Ear and Hearing 23, 532-539.<br />

Stinson, M.S., & Lang, H.G. (1994). Full inclusion? A path for integration or<br />

isolation? American Annals of the Deaf, 139, 156-159.<br />

Suarez, M. (2000). Promoting social competence in deaf students: The effects<br />

of an intervention program. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5,<br />

323-336.<br />

Svirsky, M.A., Teoh, S., & Neuberger, H. (2004). Development of language<br />

and speech perception in congenitally, profoundly deaf children as a function<br />

of age at cochlear implantation. Audiology & Neuro-Otology, 9, 224-233.<br />

Tomblin, J.B., Barker, B.A., Spencer, L.J., Zhang, X., & Gantz, B.J. (2005). The<br />

effect of age at cochlear implant initial stimulation on expressive language<br />

growth in infants and toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing<br />

Research, 48, 853-867.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1995). Efficacy of early identification and intervention.<br />

Seminars in Hearing, 16, 115-120.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A.L., Coulter, D.K., & Mehl, A.L. (1998). Language<br />

of early and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics,<br />

102, 1161-1171.<br />

Loneliness in Children with Cochlear Implants 379


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 381-418<br />

Early Hearing Detection and<br />

Intervention in Developing<br />

Countries: Current Status<br />

and Prospects<br />

Bolajoko O. Olusanya, M.D., M.Phil.<br />

Infant hearing screening is emerging rapidly as a silent global revolution for the<br />

early detection of children with congenital or early onset hearing loss to ensure timely<br />

enrollment in family-oriented intervention programs for the development of spoken<br />

language. This article examines the overriding and interrelated scientific, ethical and<br />

economic considerations for early hearing detection and intervention (EHDI) in<br />

developing countries against the backdrop of relevant resolutions of the World Health<br />

Assembly (WHA) and current global programs on early childhood developmentofthe<br />

World Bank, United Nations Children’s Funds (UNICEF) and the United Nations<br />

Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). The need to determine<br />

and develop culturally appropriate EHDI programs is indicated by the significant<br />

proportions of births that occur outside regular hospitals in some countries based<br />

on lessons from ongoing projects. The key challenges and possible approaches to<br />

building local service capacities to advance this vital, social, educational and public<br />

health initiative also are highlighted.<br />

Introduction<br />

Hearing screening for babies dates back to 1944 when Sir Alexander and<br />

Lady Ethel Constance Ewing of Great Britain demonstrated the need to develop<br />

methods of testing hearing in young children in the first year of life<br />

(Ewing & Ewing, 1944). The Ewings were among the earliest to investigate<br />

infant behavioral responses to auditory stimuli using sounds from toys,<br />

Bolajoko Olusanya, M.D., M.Phil., is a research fellow at the Institute of Child Health and the<br />

Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Trust, University College London; a fellow<br />

of the Royal College of Pediatrics and Child Health, United Kingdom; and an international<br />

fellow of the American Academy of Pediatrics. She has successfully pioneered hospital and<br />

community-based infant hearing screening programs in Nigeria in collaboration with the<br />

country’s federal health department.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 381


noisemakers, crumpled paper and the human voice. Further interest in neonatal<br />

and infant hearing screening by various researchers subsequently culminated<br />

in the formation of the U.S. Joint Committee on Infant Hearing<br />

(JCIH) in 1969 to improve and coordinate efforts for the early detection of<br />

congenital hearing loss.<br />

As a result of the limitations of available behavioral screening techniques,<br />

the JCIH recommended in 1972 the screening of only high-risk babies based<br />

on a set of five risk factors, which were expanded to 10 in 1994 (American<br />

Academy of Pediatrics, 1995). However, selective screening based on risk<br />

factors was found to miss a significant number of infants with hearing loss<br />

(Mauk, White, Mortensen, & Behrens, 1991; Watkin, Baldwin, & McEnery,<br />

1991). After much debate (Bess & Paradise, 1994), universal newborn hearing<br />

screening (UNHS) emerged as the preferred method of early hearing detection<br />

and subsequently as a standard of care in the United States and many<br />

developed countries (Bamford, Uus, & Davis, 2005; JCIH, 2000; Lutman &<br />

Grandori, 1999; Russ et al., 2002; Weichbold, Nekahm-Heis, & Welzl-Mueller,<br />

2006; White, 2003). This practice has facilitated a growing enrollment of children<br />

detected with significant hearing loss (averaging 30 to 40 dB or more<br />

over frequencies 500 to 4000 Hz) into family-oriented intervention services in<br />

the first year of life (Harrison & Roush, 2003; JCIH, 2000; Uus & Bamford,<br />

2006; Weichbold et al., 2006). Much of the current success is attributable to<br />

significant advancements in screening technologies and hearing devices as<br />

well as to improved research to establish the efficacy of early intervention on<br />

the communication skills and quality of life of infants with hearing loss.<br />

In a recent report, Morton and Nance (2006) rightly acknowledged that<br />

newborn hearing screening is a silent revolution and that “better hearing for<br />

all nations is an achievable, important goal” (p. 2152). However, current<br />

dispositions toward the globalization of EHDI are varied and seem to range<br />

from cautious optimism to enthusiastic support (Mencher & Devoe, 2001;<br />

White, 2006; Grandori & Hayes, 2006). Reticence towards EHDI initiatives is<br />

commonly linked to the prevailing socioeconomic challenges and the burden<br />

of fatal childhood diseases. This tends to distract attention from the vast<br />

majority of survivors in this region who need to thrive and develop optimally<br />

in their early crucial years. Children with disabilities are the most vulnerable<br />

and those with hidden disabilities like permanent hearing loss are easily the<br />

most neglected and the most disadvantaged (Olusanya, Ruben, & Parving,<br />

2006b).<br />

Another major obstacle to supporting EHDI efforts in developing countries<br />

is the lack of representative data reflecting the peculiarities and the great<br />

diversities within and across the more than 140 countries in this region<br />

(United Nations Children’s Funds [UNICEF], 2005; World Bank, 2006). Although<br />

pilot studies are ongoing in a growing number of countries, progress<br />

towards early hearing detection and intervention (EHDI) is still quite limited<br />

and underreported. Against this backdrop, this review presents the rationale<br />

382 Olusanya


for promoting EHDI in developing countries, highlights the current global<br />

impetus for EHDI, examines current options and progress towards early<br />

hearing detection and concludes with a discussion of the scope, status and<br />

prospects of intervention services.<br />

Rationale for EHDI in Developing Countries<br />

The decision to screen for hearing loss in early childhood in any community<br />

must be based on rational and universally accepted criteria for screening<br />

in order to justify its introduction as an essential public health service. The<br />

major scientific, ethical and economic considerations relevant to screening for<br />

infant hearing loss in developing countries are examined in this section.<br />

Scientific Considerations<br />

A population-based screening may be defined as “the systematic application<br />

of a test or inquiry to identify individuals at sufficient risk of a disorder<br />

that would benefit from further investigation or direct preventive action<br />

among people who have not sought medical attention or intervention because<br />

of symptoms of that disorder” (Wald, 2006 [p. 53]; Strong, Wald, Miller, &<br />

Alwan, 2005 [p. 12]). It entails the early detection of a disorder that is known<br />

to cause significant morbidity, suffering, disability or death if detected at a<br />

later stage and for which early intervention is possible and efficacious.<br />

At least five principles should be satisfied before any screening program is<br />

implemented (American Academy of Pediatrics [AAP], 1999; Bess & Paradise,<br />

1994; National Screening Committee (UK), 2003; Olusanya, Luxon, & Wirz,<br />

2005a; Screening Brief, 2001; Strong et al., 2005):<br />

1. The condition or disorder must be a significant health problem in the<br />

target population.<br />

2. Evidence must exist that the condition cannot be prevented effectively<br />

from occurring (i.e. not fully amenable to primary prevention).<br />

3. The screening procedure or test must be simple, safe and valid.<br />

4. The treatment or intervention must be effective and available.<br />

5. The benefits from the screening program must outweigh the associated<br />

risks.<br />

Significance of PECHL in Developing Countries<br />

As noted aptly by Mencher (2000), a major challenge in many developing<br />

countries is the dearth of epidemiological data on permanent congenital and<br />

early onset hearing loss (PCEHL). Paradoxically, such data can only be obtained<br />

reliably when infant hearing screening is implemented. Prevalence<br />

estimates for PCEHL before the widespread implementation of UNHS ranged<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 383


from 1.5 to 6 per 1,000 live births (Bachmann & Arvedson, 1998; Barsky-<br />

Firkser & Sun, 1997; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, & Mehl, 1998). Basedon<br />

the outcomes of several large-scale UNHS programs in developed countries,<br />

the incidence of bilateral PCEHL (>40 dB HL) is estimated as 2 to 4 per 1,000<br />

live births (Smith, Bale, & White, 2005; White, 2004). If the poorer health and<br />

socioeconomic conditions in developing countries and the significant proportion<br />

of births occurring outside hospitals were considered, the incidence of<br />

PCEHL (>40 dB HL) in many developing countries likely would rise to at<br />

least 6 per 1,000 live births (Olusanya et al., 2006b). For instance, Ansari (2004)<br />

reported a survey conducted in India in which 4 of every 1,000 children born<br />

in that country were identified with severe-to-profound hearing loss. When<br />

moderate-to-severe hearing loss was included, the report suggested an even<br />

higher incidence of hearing loss. These data, therefore, would suggest that of<br />

the 119.7 million annual live births in developing countries (UNICEF, 2005),<br />

about 718,000 are likely to have PCEHL (Olusanya & Newton, in press).<br />

In addition, the World Health Organization (WHO) latest estimates show a<br />

more than two-fold increase within a decade in the population of persons<br />

with significant hearing loss (>40 dB HL) worldwide from 120 million in 1995<br />

to at least 278 million in 2005 (WHO, 2006a).<br />

Two-thirds of the world’s population of children and adults with hearing<br />

loss live in developing countries. Hearing loss in 68 million people is estimated<br />

to have existed since childhood, but this estimate may be understated<br />

because several risk factors for PCEHL, including those listed by the JCIH, are<br />

associated with poor maternal and child health care in developing countries.<br />

PCEHL is also the most common communication disorder that prompts consultation<br />

with physicians (Somefun, Lesi, Danfulani, & Olusanya, 2006).<br />

Based on these facts, PCEHL is unlikely to be a less significant psychosocial,<br />

educational and health condition in developing countries than in developed<br />

countries where UNHS currently is mandatory.<br />

Limitations of Primary Prevention of PCEHL<br />

It has been suggested that at least 50% of the burden of hearing loss across<br />

all age groups can be prevented (Alberti, 1996; Smith, 2003; WHO, 2006a).<br />

Prevention in a generic sense consists of three levels of disease control and<br />

management: primary, secondary and tertiary (see Table 1).<br />

Primary prevention for PCEHL is concerned with the prevention of its<br />

occurrence or any condition that may lead to its occurrence and includes such<br />

activities as immunization, avoidance or rational use of ototoxic drugs and<br />

improved obstetric care, personal hygiene and living conditions. For primary<br />

prevention to be effective, there must be accurate knowledge of the causes<br />

and associated risk factors for PCEHL in a given setting (Morzaria, Westerberg,<br />

& Kozak, 2004). Available studies suggest that the possible causes of<br />

PCEHL in a significant proportion (about 20-57%) of children with hearing<br />

384 Olusanya


Table 1. Levels of Prevention for Congenital and Early Onset Hearing Loss<br />

Level of Prevention Description<br />

Primary<br />

Aim: To prevent<br />

occurrence of PCEHL or<br />

any condition that may<br />

lead to its occurrence<br />

Secondary<br />

Aim: To detect and treat<br />

early or modify the effects<br />

of PCEHL where there is<br />

no treatment.<br />

Tertiary<br />

Aim: To provide an entry<br />

point for the care and<br />

management of the child<br />

with PCEHL.<br />

● Vaccination against rubella, mumps and meningitis<br />

(hemophilus influenzae type B and meningococcus)<br />

● Avoidance and cautious use of ototoxic drugs<br />

● Widespread public education to prevent neonatal<br />

jaundice from<br />

• Iso-immunization<br />

• Exposure of infants who may be G6PD deficient to<br />

hemolytic agents, such as camphor and<br />

menthol-containing household substances<br />

• Umbilical sepsis<br />

● Prenatal diagnosis and termination of pregnancy for<br />

significant congenital abnormalities<br />

● Premarital counseling on the dangers of<br />

consanguinity for genetic diseases in general and<br />

specifically for hearing loss<br />

● Improved maternal and child health services<br />

including prompt decision-making process for<br />

cesarean section when indicated<br />

● Management of maternal HIV infection for the<br />

prevention of vertical transmission through<br />

• Use of antiretroviral drugs (HAART) during<br />

pregnancy<br />

• Delivery by cesarean section<br />

• Avoidance of breast feeding<br />

● Malaria control and prophylaxis<br />

● Parental empowerment through education and<br />

gainful employment<br />

● Early detection through newborn hearing screening<br />

and enrollment in early intervention programs<br />

● Widespread awareness of language milestones and<br />

risk factors for hearing loss during ante-natal classes<br />

for pregnant women and postnatal classes and<br />

immunization clinics for new mothers<br />

● Preschool screening programs to identify children<br />

missed by infant hearing screening<br />

● Technology for auditory-based communication for<br />

mainstreaming<br />

• Provision of hearing aids and support services<br />

• Provision of cochlear implants<br />

● Teachers equipped through education and training<br />

to provide effective classroom auditory-based<br />

communication support for children with mild to<br />

moderate hearing loss<br />

● Classroom accommodations (e.g., preferential seating,<br />

acoustic treatment of classrooms, sound field systems)<br />

● Consideration of alternate visual communication<br />

options (e.g., Cued Speech, Sign Language)<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 385


loss in the developing world are unknown, thus forestalling primary prevention<br />

as a comprehensive solution (Derekoy, 2000; Elango, Chand, & Purohit,<br />

1992; Gray, 1989; Holborow, Martinson, & Anger, 1982; Minja, 1998; Sellars &<br />

Brighton, 1983). Moreover, vaccinations against notable causes of PCEHL,<br />

such as meningitis, mumps and rubella, are rare in many developing countries<br />

(WHO, 2002). The standard of maternal and child health care in many of<br />

these countries is poor and unlikely to reach levels comparable to those of<br />

developed countries in the foreseeable future (UNICEF, 2005).<br />

Secondary prevention, therefore, is imperative in view of the possibilities<br />

offered by newborn or infant screening for early detection and intervention.<br />

Secondary prevention for prelingual hearing loss involves taking action to<br />

prevent PCEHL from becoming a disability through early hearing detection<br />

followed up with appropriate and timely intervention. Infant hearing screening,<br />

therefore, is essentially a preventive and vital social, educational and<br />

health care service. Regardless of the limitations with primary prevention,<br />

however, screening is not an alternative “primary prevention” strategy but a<br />

complement to ongoing efforts to prevent and curtail known risk factors for<br />

PCEHL (Alberti, 1996; Kumar, 1997; WHO, 2006).<br />

Tertiary prevention is concerned with the rehabilitation of persons with<br />

disabilities. Tertiary prevention for PCEHL, therefore, seeks to prevent the<br />

disability posed by restricted hearing sensitivity from becoming a “handicap”<br />

or disability that hinders an individual’s daily functioning and participation<br />

in society at large (Stephens, 1996; WHO, 2001). It embraces the provision of<br />

hearing aids or cochlear implants and associated services, family-oriented<br />

support services, special education and equitable social integration.<br />

Efficiency of Screening Methods<br />

Currently, parental suspicion or concern prompted by a child’s inappropriate<br />

or lack of response to sound is the most common mode of detecting<br />

PCEHL in many developing countries. Parental suspicion often provides<br />

valuable impetus for detection and a prescreening method; however, health<br />

professionals do not always act upon parents’ observations (Glascoe, Maclean,<br />

& Stone, 1991; Roberts, 1996). Moreover, this passive form of detection<br />

often occurs well after the child is 24 months old, which is late for optimal<br />

intervention for speech and language development (Gopal, Hugo, & Louw,<br />

2001; Mukari, Vandort, Ahmad, Saim, & Mohamed, 1999; Olusanya, Luxon, &<br />

Wirz, 2005b). Parental suspicion may be earlier for children with severe-toprofound<br />

hearing loss, but confirmation often is not obtained until the child<br />

is 5 years of age. The delay sometimes is attributable to health professionals<br />

not responding to parental concern (Prendergast, Lartz, & Fiedler, 2002). In<br />

China, the use of a five-item questionnaire was explored as a screening technique<br />

among parents of 6- to 8-month-old infants, and it had varying degrees<br />

of success (Newton, Liu, Ke, Xu, & Bamford, 1999). Though simple and inexpensive<br />

to administer, the questionnaire was unreliable in detecting slight<br />

386 Olusanya


or unilateral hearing loss because it relied solely on parental observation. The<br />

effects of these types of hearing losses may be subtle enough that parents are<br />

not alerted easily to their presence. The use of risk factors to identify children<br />

with hearing loss has been attempted in some countries (Ali, Khan, Baloch, &<br />

Aziz, 2004; D’Mello, 1995; Maisoun & Zakzouk, 2003), but up to 50% of<br />

children with hearing loss were undetected by this method, and the presence<br />

of some risk factors was difficult to elicit reliably. Thus, sole reliance on<br />

parental observation or high-risk screening is inefficient and not advisable.<br />

An ideal screening test should be simple to apply, safe and valid. The test<br />

is valid if it is highly sensitive (i.e., it detects the majority of babies with<br />

hearing loss) and highly specific (i.e., it excludes most babies without hearing<br />

loss). Otoacoustic Emissions (OAE) and Automated Auditory Brainstem Response<br />

(AABR) are the only two objective tests currently that satisfy these<br />

criteria with documented sensitivity and specificity in excess of 90%, especially<br />

when combined into a two-stage screening protocol (Davis et al., 1997;<br />

Hall, Smith, & Popelka, 2004; Kennedy, Kimm, Thornton, & Davis, 2000; Mehl<br />

& Thomson, 2002; Vohr et al., 2001a). They are used presently, in various pilot<br />

newborn hearing screening programs in developing countries (Attias et al.,<br />

2006; Bener, ElHakeem, & Abdulhadi, 2005; Chapchap & Segre, 2001; Habib<br />

& Abdelgaffar, 2005; Khandekar, Khabori, Mohammed, & Gupta, 2006; Lin,<br />

Huang, Lin, Lin, & Wu, 2004; Low, Pang, & Ho, 2005; Mathur & Dhawan,<br />

2006; Mukari, Tan, & Abdullah, 2006; Ng, Hui, Lam, Goh, & Yeung, 2004;<br />

Olusanya & Okolo, 2006a; Quintos, Isleta, Chiong, & Abes, 2003; Swanepoel,<br />

Hugo, & Louw, 2006; Yee-Arellano, Leal-Garza, & Pauli-Muller, 2006). There<br />

is no reason to expect that these tests will be unacceptable among mothers in<br />

any infant hearing program because they are noninvasive, painless and quick<br />

to administer. In the absence of objective screening measures, the age of<br />

detection of PCEHL will remain unacceptably high (Canale et al., 2006; Gopal<br />

et al., 2001; Harrison, Roush, & Wallace, 2003; Parving, 1999; Yoshinaga-Itano,<br />

2004).<br />

Treatment for PCEHL<br />

The value of early detection is optimized if effective intervention exists and<br />

is accessible to children identified with PCEHL. The time from birth to about<br />

5 years of age is a critical or sensitive phase for the acquisition of speech and<br />

language skills (Carney & Moeller, 1998), but optimal outcomes are best<br />

achieved in the first year of life (Kennedy et al., 2006; Moeller, 2000; Yoshinaga-Itano<br />

et al., 1998). The provision of amplification also is essential for<br />

children with PCEHL in the developing world; however, amplification devices<br />

are seldom prescribed for children earlier than 2 years of age because of<br />

late detection of PCEHL and because infants often are considered too young<br />

to be fitted with these devices even when parents can afford to purchase<br />

them. For these reasons, enrollment in schools for children with hearing loss<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 387


still constitutes the predominant intervention option for parents. Often, such<br />

enrollment is delayed until age 10 because of late diagnosis and parental<br />

preference for auditory-based intervention, which physicians rarely offer<br />

(Olusanya et al., 2005b). Even when enrollment into schools for children with<br />

hearing loss occurs at much earlier ages, reading and comprehension abilities<br />

are about 50-60% of the children’s chronological age by the time they leave<br />

school, which is typically at age 18. A survey among hearing parents in<br />

Nigeria indicated that they, like parents in other countries, want their children<br />

with hearing loss to be aided promptly so they can acquire spoken<br />

language–feelings similar to parents in other countries (Luterman & Kurtzer-<br />

White, 1999; Olusanya et al., 2005b; Russ et al., 2004; Young & Tattersall,<br />

2007).<br />

Recognizing the constraints of many developing countries to provide relevant<br />

intervention services due to knowledge gap and scarcity, the WHO has<br />

published guidelines for manufacturing affordable hearing aids, delivery of<br />

audiological services and personnel training to encourage auditory-based<br />

intervention services (Kumar, 2001; WHO, 2004). In addition, low-cost and<br />

solar-powered hearing aids currently are being manufactured in developing<br />

countries to encourage wider use (Parving & Christensen, 2004). It is not<br />

unusual for appropriate early intervention services to be in short supply in<br />

developed countries, a situation that relevant professional associations and<br />

stakeholders in those countries seek to address (JCIH, 2000; Uus, Bamford,<br />

Young, & McCracken, 2005). Anecdotal evidence suggests that as infant hearing<br />

screening programs become established and the health care system increasingly<br />

tasked by unmet patients’ demands, further public and/or private<br />

sector initiatives will emerge towards the development of relevant services to<br />

satisfy growing needs.<br />

Benefits Versus Risks of Screening<br />

Apart from the specific benefits already documented for developed countries,<br />

infant hearing screening programs empower parents to make informed<br />

and timely choices that offer their children the best start in life (Glover, 2003;<br />

Russ et al., 2004; Young & Tattersall, 2007). Additional benefits as they relate<br />

to developing countries have been discussed in greater detail elsewhere (Olusanya,<br />

Luxon, & Wirz, 2004). From a societal standpoint, UNHS has been<br />

associated with significant cost savings in health expenditures in developed<br />

countries (Schroeder et al., 2006), and this also is likely to be applicable to<br />

developing countries.<br />

The emotional/psychological effects of false-positives are the principal adverse<br />

consequences thought to be associated with UNHS (Hewlett & Waisbren,<br />

2006; Vohr, Letourneau, & McDermott, 2001; Watkin, Baldwin, Dixon,<br />

& Beckman, 1998). However, many surveys of parents have shown that only<br />

a few have anxiety about infant hearing screening and that anxiety levels<br />

388 Olusanya


were usually higher when babies were scheduled for rescreening (Vohr et al.,<br />

2001b; Weichbold & Welzl-Mueller, 2001). In fact, maternal anxiety following<br />

positive screening outcomes can be moderated markedly by effective communication<br />

to parents that a referral for subsequent screening test(s) is not by<br />

itself a confirmation of hearing loss (Crockett, Wright, Uus, Bamford, & Marteau,<br />

2006; Hewlett & Waisbren, 2006; Stuart, Moretz, & Yang, 2000).<br />

Maternal anxiety and stress are equally associated with true-positives, but<br />

these effects may not necessarily be considered as adverse consequences of<br />

screening because they are inevitable even without screening. False-negative<br />

screening results give false assurance to parents and health professionals and<br />

delay diagnosis. Similarly, infant hearing screening could miss some children<br />

with PCEHL that is progressive or is delayed-onset, giving parents a false<br />

sense of assurance.<br />

The paucity of high-quality, randomized controlled studies on the benefits<br />

and the potential psychological effects of UNHS remains a continuing global<br />

challenge (Thompson et al., 2001), but as demonstrated in the many countries<br />

where UNHS is offered routinely, the majority of parents feel the benefits of<br />

early detection and intervention through infant hearing screening far outweigh<br />

these potential adverse outcomes (Clemens, Davis, & Bailey, 2000;<br />

Davis et al., 1997; Hergils & Hergils, 2000; Luterman & Kurtzer-White, 1999;<br />

Stuart, Moretz & Yang, 2000; Weichbold & Welzl-Mueller, 2001).<br />

Moreover, the value of knowing about a “hidden” disability that, inevitably,<br />

will become apparent at a later stage when the prospects for optimal or<br />

effective intervention have diminished significantly is unquantifiable, especially<br />

for parents in some traditional communities who would otherwise seek<br />

unorthodox and sometimes potentially harmful treatments for the symptoms<br />

of hearing loss (Andrade & Ross, 2005; Odebiyi & Togonu-Bickersteth, 1987).<br />

In fact, parents often are angry and distraught when health care workers<br />

delay providing vital information on their child’s health status (Glover, 2003;<br />

Magnuson & Hergils, 2000; Russ et al., 2004). Thus, available scientific evidence<br />

clearly demonstrates that screening infants for PCEHL is most likely to<br />

be a worthwhile public health care service for developing countries.<br />

Ethical Considerations<br />

Universal screening is usually offered to people who have not sought medical<br />

attention on account of the symptoms of a disorder. When a condition is<br />

“hidden” or “invisible” and has no major adverse and visible consequences,<br />

it may be difficult to justify the need for screening, especially when intervention<br />

services for those with the condition are limited. However, the adverse<br />

effects of PCEHL are inevitable and likely to become apparent at a time when<br />

intervention is, at the very best, suboptimal. Therefore, it can be argued that<br />

it is unethical to withhold screening for a condition that will manifest itself<br />

eventually with adverse consequences, regardless of the availability of services<br />

to deal adequately with the problem. Nonetheless, screening for such a<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 389


condition must still satisfy the basic ethical principles of autonomy (the right<br />

to choose), non-maleficence (obligation to avoid causing harm), beneficence<br />

(obligation to provide benefits and to balance benefits against risks) and<br />

justice (obligation of fairness in the distribution of benefits and risks) (Olusanya,<br />

Luxon, & Wirz, 2006a).<br />

Parental Autonomy<br />

Parents must be educated sufficiently on the benefits and potential adverse<br />

effects of the screening process, and their consent must be obtained before<br />

conducting the screening. Every pregnant mother expects to give birth to a<br />

healthy baby and leave the birthing center with the assurance that her apparently<br />

well child has every chance to thrive and develop optimally. Parental<br />

consent for screening, therefore, can be obtained readily if it is presented<br />

as a routine neonatal examination that is painless, noninvasive and can be<br />

quickly administered prior to hospital discharge.<br />

The health professional must handle the screening process with sensitivity<br />

because the arrival of a newborn is both a joyous and an emotion-laden event<br />

for the parents and the entire family. Parental reaction to the disclosure of<br />

PCEHL is likely to be one of shock, denial, grief, anger and depression (Hedderly,<br />

Baird, & McConachie, 2003; Kurtzer-White & Luterman, 2003) and may<br />

be compounded by unfavorable or superstitious beliefs toward congenital<br />

disabilities in some traditional communities, which could undermine the effectiveness<br />

of the intervention process (Byford & Veenstra, 2004; Kiyanga &<br />

Moores, 2003; Stephens, Stephens, & Eisenhart-Rothe, 2000). The most opportune<br />

time to begin discussing the importance of screening with parents, therefore,<br />

is probably during maternal and child health clinics, particularly before<br />

delivery (Arnold et al., 2006).<br />

Benefits Versus Harms of Screening<br />

Balancing the benefits of screening against its potential adverse effects is<br />

also an ethical issue because of the inherent limitations of a screening test. The<br />

risks of false assurance that a child has no hearing loss or of unnecessary<br />

anxiety for a child falsely detected with hearing loss as previously mentioned<br />

must not be ignored. The latter is of greater concern for service providers<br />

because it places unnecessary burden on the limited resources. Practical ways<br />

of minimizing these risks include delaying screening beyond 48 hours after<br />

birth to eliminate the effect of vernix plugs, conducting a two-stage screening<br />

with OAE and ABR and minimizing the effect of ambient noise during<br />

screening, which could be a daunting challenge for hospitals located in noisy<br />

environments (Hall, Smith, & Popelka, 2004; Headley, Campbell, & Gravel,<br />

2000; Jacobson & Jacobson, 1994; Kennedy et al., 2000; Maxon, White, Culpepper,<br />

& Vohr, 1997). From an ethical standpoint, these potential adverse<br />

390 Olusanya


Table 2. World Bank Classification of Countries by Income Levels*<br />

Income Group (GNI<br />

Per Capita)<br />

effects or challenges are insignificant when compared with the many benefits<br />

of infant hearing screening in developing countries (Olusanya et al., 2006a).<br />

Distributive Justice<br />

Number of<br />

Countries Partial Listing of Countries<br />

Low income (US$10,725)<br />

Ghana<br />

54 Brazil, Cuba, Indonesia,<br />

Philippines, Syria,<br />

Jordan, Thailand, China<br />

33 Argentina, Botswana,<br />

South Africa, Mexico,<br />

Malaysia<br />

15 Brunei, Pueto Rico, Saudi<br />

Arabia, Kuwait, Qatar,<br />

United Kingdom,<br />

United States<br />

Note. GNI, gross national income. *The World Bank (2006).<br />

Degree of Need for<br />

External Support<br />

Substantial<br />

Some<br />

Minimal<br />

None<br />

The requirement for distributive justice in the use of scarce resources is a<br />

significant consideration for many low- and middle-income countries but is<br />

less so for high-income countries in the developing world (see Table 2). A<br />

simple generalization of the individual capacities of developing countries to<br />

afford EHDI services is fundamentally inappropriate. Some developing countries<br />

in the middle- or high-income group are in a position to implement a<br />

publicly funded EHDI comparable to or approximating the standards of programs<br />

in developed countries. Distributive justice requires the identification<br />

and recognition of best practices in public health care to guide health professionals,<br />

regardless of the economic status of each country. Distributive justice<br />

also requires equity of access to essential health care, especially among the<br />

potential beneficiaries (Landman & Henley, 1999). On a global level, distributive<br />

justice would suggest that EHDI be implemented in developing countries,<br />

which account for 90% of children with PCEHL, especially because<br />

EHDI programs have been considered vital in the developed world, which<br />

accounts for only 10% of children with PCEHL.<br />

Against this backdrop, the implementation of EHDI in developing countries<br />

is ethically justified to warrant the systematic and widespread introduction<br />

and development of relevant services.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 391


Figure 1. How health expenditures are financed globally.<br />

Economic Considerations<br />

A major reservation regarding the introduction of EHDI relates to whether<br />

governments in developing countries can afford these services as part of basic<br />

public health care. This concern often emanates from the perspective in developed<br />

countries in which EHDI services are publicly funded and offered at<br />

minimal or no charge to consumers. There are striking disparities in global<br />

spending patterns for health care services between low/middle-income and<br />

high-income countries that should not be overlooked, as shown in Table 2<br />

and Figure 1 (Gottret & Schieber, 2006). For instance, the public share of total<br />

health expenditures ranges from 65% in high-income countries to 29% in<br />

low-income countries, with private expenditure accounting for the balance. A<br />

common and significant feature in developing countries is that user fees are<br />

charged for most services (including medications) at all levels of health care<br />

delivery. Although such fees are associated more with private providers of<br />

health services, they are not uncommon among public health institutions in<br />

many countries. As a result, out-of-pocket spending in low-income countries<br />

accounts for 66% of total health expenditure (or about 93% of private expenditure)<br />

compared with 20% of total health expenditure (or about 56% of<br />

private expenditure) in high-income countries (see Figure 1). Foreign loans<br />

and grants from multilateral institutions like the World Bank and charitable<br />

contributions from various donor agencies often augment government spending,<br />

but altogether these external sources rarely account for more than 10% of<br />

392 Olusanya


total public expenditure (Gottret & Schieber, 2006). The implication of this<br />

spending pattern is that the introduction of any public health intervention<br />

cannot be solely predicated on the capacity of the government to provide<br />

needed services.<br />

In any country, PCEHL exists among children in all socioeconomic classes.<br />

Just as income varies widely among nations so, too, do individual capacities<br />

within and across communities. Even in low-income countries, there will<br />

always be some individuals who can afford the services for themselves and/<br />

or to help others. In many communities, therefore, the disposable income<br />

available to individuals is the sum of their own resources plus those that may<br />

become available to them from other benevolent individuals who are moved<br />

by the special health needs of others. Besides, the manner in which individuals<br />

choose to prioritize their spending often is unpredictable and complex. For<br />

instance, some communities may consider socially stigmatized diseases more<br />

important and well-deserving of communal support than nonstigmatized<br />

diseases, even if they are less prevalent (Kapiriri & Norheim, 2002), and are<br />

likely to place higher premium on the needs of children than those of adults<br />

(Landman & Henley, 1999).<br />

Given the financial constraints faced by most governments, it is, perhaps,<br />

more plausible for governments to facilitate public-private partnerships for<br />

pilot studies, create public awareness, and set standards for best practices.<br />

Public-private partnerships for EHDI are likely to be more successful if they<br />

are preceded by the inclusion of goals for early childhood development in<br />

general, and early hearing detection in particular, in the national health policy<br />

(The Government of Brazil, 2006; Federal Republic of Nigeria, 2005; Nascimento<br />

et al., 2006; Olusanya et al., 2005a). Governments also can play an<br />

essential role in ensuring that training curricula for health professionals provide<br />

updated skills for the broad spectrum of prevailing health care needs<br />

and are not just limited to fatal childhood diseases. As countries contemplate<br />

national or community-based health insurance schemes (Gottret & Schieber,<br />

2006), it should be possible also to explore the inclusion of EHDI services in<br />

a few countries such as Brazil and South Africa (Swanepoel, 2006).<br />

Current Global Impetus for EHDI<br />

It is very common for governments in developing countries to tailor their<br />

national health programs to the policies and priorities of major multilateral<br />

agencies of the United Nations such as WHO, UNICEF, the World Bank and<br />

the United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization<br />

(UNESCO). In addition, many governments depend on expert advice and<br />

technical inputs from these organizations in constructing their national health<br />

and educational policies. This practice often assures the inflow of external<br />

financing for national programs. For these reasons, it is essential to establish<br />

a link for EHDI within the current global agendas of these institutions.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 393


The World Health Assembly (WHA), which is comprised of the health<br />

ministers of United Nations member states, is responsible for setting the<br />

policies of the WHO to reflect the health care preferences of its members. In<br />

1995, the WHA passed a resolution regarding the prevention and control of<br />

major causes of avoidable hearing loss and the “early detection in babies,<br />

toddlers, and children, as well as in the elderly, within the framework of<br />

primary health care” (WHO, 1995 [p. 1]). This resolution was passed within<br />

the context of the WHO’s definition of health, which seeks to promote physical,<br />

mental and social well-being and not merely amelioration of disease. The<br />

resolution addressed the growing problem of disabling (>40 dB HL) hearing<br />

loss and its adverse consequences for language acquisition, optimal childhood<br />

development and educational achievement. Among other issues, the<br />

resolution recognized the need for early hearing detection but did not recommend<br />

how to achieve this goal. This omission was not unexpected, considering<br />

the keen debate in the United States that was occurring around this<br />

period on the most appropriate and effective strategy for EHDI (Bess &<br />

Paradise, 1994). The resolution also outlined specific roles for United Nations<br />

member states and the WHO while encouraging public-private partnerships<br />

to address this problem.<br />

In 2005, a decade after another resolution relevant to the management of<br />

hearing loss was passed by the WHA on disability management and rehabilitation<br />

(WHO, 2005). Similarly, this resolution urged collaborative action<br />

by the WHO and United Nations member states to reduce risk factors contributing<br />

to disabilities during pregnancy and childhood. The resolution also<br />

encouraged the early identification and intervention for disabilities, especially<br />

during pregnancy and childhood, including family counseling, access<br />

to appropriate assistive technology and community-based rehabilitation programs.<br />

UNICEF is the United Nations’ agency responsible for the well-being of the<br />

world’s children. One of its five cardinal priorities is the promotion of optimal<br />

early childhood development because the organization believes that giving a<br />

child the best start in life lays the foundation for learning and school achievement.<br />

The organization has recently expressed interest for the needs of children<br />

who have been “excluded and made invisible” by hearing loss and other<br />

disabilities and should therefore be supportive of national efforts to help<br />

these children in their early crucial years for optimal development such as<br />

early hearing detection and intervention (UNICEF, 2005).<br />

UNESCO provides for an Early Childhood Care and Education (ECCE)<br />

initiative under its Education For All (EFA) program launched and adopted<br />

by 164 countries in 2000 (UNESCO, 2006). The ECCE is aimed at supporting<br />

children’s survival, growth, development and learning from birth to entry<br />

into primary school, especially for the most vulnerable and disadvantaged<br />

children. It recognizes that early childhood is a time of remarkable brain<br />

development that lays the foundation for later learning. It also asserts that it<br />

394 Olusanya


is more cost effective to institute preventive measures and support for children<br />

early on than to compensate for the disadvantage as they grow older<br />

(UNESCO, 2006). EHDI is therefore consistent with the ECCE priority.<br />

In addition, the ongoing United Nations’ millennium development project,<br />

based on a collective declaration by 189 member countries in 2000, constitutes<br />

the global agenda for the mobilization and prioritization of resources to the<br />

developing world till year 2015 (United Nations, 2005). The first two of eight<br />

millennium development goals (MDGs) are focused on the eradication of<br />

extreme poverty and the completion of full course of primary education by<br />

children everywhere. It has been demonstrated that failure to detect and<br />

manage PCEHL through UNHS in developing countries will significantly<br />

undermine the realization of these two goals (Olusanya et al., 2006a).<br />

These major global initiatives provide valuable platforms and impetus for<br />

the promotion of EHDI in the developing world. Failure to extend UNHS to<br />

developing countries in particular may undermine optimal early childhood<br />

development and exacerbate the already vast health inequality between developed<br />

and developing nations. In fact, it is now widely acknowledged<br />

(inclusive of the World Bank) that an investment in early childhood, which<br />

embraces EHDI, offers significant long-term returns for national development<br />

(Young, 2007). These considerations have also prompted recent efforts to<br />

redress the exclusion of childhood hearing loss and adult hearing loss of<br />

childhood onset in the current global disease control priorities of the World<br />

Bank for developing countries (Jamison et al., 2006; Olusanya & Newton, in<br />

press).<br />

Options for Early Hearing Detection in<br />

Developing Countries<br />

Current pilot infant hearing screening programs in developing countries<br />

are either hospital- or community based. The rationale behind each model,<br />

protocols for implementing screening, possible challenges and highlights<br />

from current programs in developing countries are examined in this section.<br />

Rationale<br />

Hospital-Based Screening<br />

Screening babies in hospitals before discharge is desirable for at least two<br />

primary reasons. First, screening eliminates the need to ask mothers to return<br />

specifically to have their babies tested. Parents are likely to be less enthusiastic<br />

to seek detection of an invisible and non-life-threatening handicap in<br />

their apparently normal babies. Taking an apparently well child to the hospital<br />

for any “check-up” is viewed as socially and culturally inappropriate in<br />

many communities because of the notion that hospitals are established only<br />

to serve the sick. Second, screening helps health care professionals to satisfy<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 395


an important ethical obligation of ensuring that babies have been examined<br />

and tested for hidden, detectable abnormalities before discharge.<br />

Screening Protocols<br />

A common protocol is a two-stage screening, first with Transient-Evoked<br />

Otoacoustic Emissions (TEOAE) followed by AABR for children referred<br />

from the first-stage (i.e., TEOAE) screen. This protocol often is associated with<br />

minimal false-positive rates (specificity >90%) without significant reduction<br />

in sensitivity (Hall et al., 2004; Kennedy et al., 2000). Most screening instruments<br />

are simple to use, allowing a person without prior audiological expertise<br />

to conduct the screening after a brief period of training.<br />

Although nurses are skilled in securing informed parental consent before<br />

screening and may be members of the screening team for this purpose, adding<br />

this responsibility to the nurses’ workload could be counterproductive. In<br />

many settings, nurses are likely to view this task as being less important than<br />

other routine clinical duties. Where mothers are commonly to be discharged<br />

within 48 hours, screening often is recommended as close to discharge as<br />

possible to minimize false referral due to vernix plugs in the baby’s ears.<br />

Possible Challenges<br />

Given the low prevalence rates for congenital hearing loss, the majority of<br />

babies are expected to pass the first-stage screen. Finding a suitable section<br />

within the hospital ward is necessary to minimize false referral rates where<br />

the ambient noise levels are excessive. This could a major challenge for hospitals<br />

with severe space constraints. When the number of babies awaiting<br />

screening is large, some mothers may be too impatient to wait and may never<br />

return. In addition, babies who are unsettled because their mothers have not<br />

established lactation would be difficult to test within the short time available<br />

for screening in “baby-friendly hospitals,” where exclusive breast feeding is<br />

promoted routinely.<br />

Moreover, as shown in Figure 2 (UNICEF, 2005; WHO, 2006b) a significant<br />

proportion of births occur outside regular hospitals in many developing<br />

countries. For instance, in South Asia the proportion of births with skilled<br />

attendants (a proxy for hospital deliveries) varies from as low as 13% in<br />

Bangladesh to 43% in India (compared with about 99% in developed countries).<br />

Consequently, many babies in these regions will be missed by hospitalbased<br />

UNHS programs. The obvious challenge then is to locate where the<br />

majority of babies are born within these communities and establish effective<br />

ways of attracting parents for hearing screening services.<br />

Progress Reports from Current Projects<br />

Hospital-based UNHS pilot projects have been implemented in Nigeria,<br />

South Africa, India, Pakistan, Saudi Arabia, Iran, Qatar, Jordan, Oman, China,<br />

Hong Kong, Taiwan, Malaysia, Philippines, Singapore, Brazil and Mexico<br />

396 Olusanya


Figure 2. Proportion of births in hospital facilities (1996-2004). Based on data from<br />

UNICEF (2005) and WHO (2006b).<br />

(see Table 3). One of the oldest projects was started in a private hospital in<br />

Brazil in 1988 and has expanded to more than 230 sites nationwide (Chapchap<br />

& Segre, 2001). It is possibly one of the most organized and successful UNHS<br />

programs at present in the developing world.<br />

Based on available reports, the number of babies screened has varied from<br />

406 in the Philippines to 36,095 in four hospitals in Singapore. The duration<br />

of the reported data ranges from five months in Nigeria, South Africa and<br />

Hong Kong to 60 months in China. It is pertinent to mention that these pilot<br />

programs still are ongoing in most (if not all) of these countries.<br />

Reports from these countries confirm that hospital-based UNHS is feasible.<br />

The attitudes of parents and health care professionals generally were favorable;<br />

however, some challenges remain unresolved. For instance, high default<br />

rates for follow-up services are common and require effective data management<br />

and tracking systems. Most programs were initiated and entirely managed<br />

by health professionals in hospitals with little or no government<br />

funding. Some have been made possible through equipment donations or<br />

loans from manufacturers, supplemented by financial support from local<br />

non-governmental organizations. Most of these countries have no free national<br />

health care service or publicly administered health insurance schemes;<br />

thus, only parents who can afford to pay seek these services. In the Nigerian<br />

project parents currently are not required to pay for services, including hearing<br />

aids. In other countries, parents pay for hearing aids if required, sometimes<br />

at highly subsidized rates. Data on screening costs per baby still are<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 397


Table 3. Pilot Programs on Infant Hearing Screening in Developing Countries<br />

Country (Reference) Year Started (Location) Setting/ Coverage Protocol Total Screened (Duration)<br />

398 Olusanya<br />

Nigeria (Olusanya & Okolo, 2006) 2005 (Lagos) Hospital, MCC: UNHS OAE, AABR 1,132** (5 months)<br />

South Africa (Swanepoel et al., 2006) 2003 (Pretoria) Hospital, MCC: UNHS OAE 510* (5 months)<br />

India (http://healthlibrary.com) 2003 (Kochi) Hospital: TNHS OAE 2,500* (N/A)<br />

Pakistan (Ali et al., 2000) 1999 (Lahore) Hospital: UNHS OAE 756* (N/A)<br />

Saudi Arabia (Habib & Abdelgaffar, 1996 (Jeddah) Hospital: UNHS OAE 11,986*(96 months)<br />

2005)<br />

Iran (Grandori & Hayes, 2006; Masoud 2002 (Tehran, Mashad) Hospital: UNHS OAE 16,000+** (6 months)<br />

et al., 2006)<br />

Qatar (Bener et al., 2005) 2003 (Doha) Hospital: UNHS OAE 2,800* (11 months)<br />

Jordan (Attias et al., 2006) 2001 (multiple cities) Hospital, MCC: UNHS OAE 8,251** (N/A)<br />

Oman (Khandekar et al., 2006) 2003 (multiple cities) Hospital, MCC: UNHS OAE, AABR 21,387** (12 months)<br />

China (Grandori & Hayes, 2006; Xu 2000 (Nanjing) Hospital: UNHS OAE, AABR 8,800* (60 months)<br />

et al., 2006)<br />

Hong Kong (Ng et al., 2004) 1998 (Hong Kong) Hospital: UNHS OAE, AABR 1,064* (5 months)<br />

Taiwan (Lin et al., 2002) 1998 (Taipei) Hospital: UNHS OAE, AABR 6,765* (24 months)<br />

Malaysia (Mukari et al., 2006) 2000 (Kuala Lumpur) Hospital: UNHS OAE 4,437* (11 months)<br />

Philippines (Quintos et al., 2003) 2000 (Manila) Hospital: UNHS OAE 406* (12 months)<br />

Singapore (Low et al., 2005) 2002 (Singapore) Hospital: UNHS OAE, AABR 36,095** (24 months)<br />

Brazil (Chapchap & Segre, 2001) 1996 (Sao Paulo) Hospital: UNHS OAE 4,196* (36 months)<br />

Mexico (Yee-Arellano et al., 2006) 2005 (Mexico City) Hospital: TNHS, UNHS OAE, AABR 3,066* (24 months)<br />

Note. Abbreviations: MCC, maternal and child health clinic; UNHS, universal newborn hearing screening; OAE, Otoacoustic Emissions; AABR,<br />

automated Auditory Brainstem Response; TNHS, targeted newborn hearing screening; N/A, not available.<br />

*Screening results from one site only.<br />

**Screening results from more than one site.


scant, although they are likely to vary from country to country, depending on<br />

the screening protocol adopted. Generally, screening instruments and associated<br />

consumables remain expensive, but the costs could be reduced significantly<br />

through bulk purchasing among many screening sites. Future product<br />

development by manufacturers may eliminate or minimize expensive consumables,<br />

and donors may be able to provide screening instruments at a<br />

reduced cost. Shortages of relevant manpower and facilities for diagnostic<br />

evaluation as well as poor public awareness also have been cited in some<br />

countries. These issues can be addressed through systematic and ongoing<br />

human capacity building and parent and health professional education. Hospital-based<br />

UNHS programs should be effective in detecting the majority of<br />

babies with congenital hearing loss, as in Latin American countries where a<br />

significant proportion of births occur in hospitals (UNICEF, 2005) (see Figure<br />

2). However, a complementary program would be required in communities<br />

with low rates of hospital-based deliveries.<br />

Community-Based Screening<br />

Rationale<br />

In many developing countries, home births and deliveries at private maternity<br />

homes run by birth attendants account for the majority of babies born<br />

outside of hospitals (WHO, 2006b). The rest are delivered in health facilities<br />

within church premises or before arrival at hospitals. Contemplating newborn<br />

hearing screening programs at these various locations is a logistical<br />

challenge; however, the experiences in most of these countries show that<br />

mothers from all birthing locations take their babies to immunization clinics<br />

at designated community health centers. Routine childhood immunization is<br />

perhaps the most well-established public health program globally due to the<br />

substantial technical and financial support received yearly from UNICEF, the<br />

WHO and several donor agencies and partners. Its popularity is derived from<br />

its preventive value for most fatal childhood diseases and because it is offered<br />

free to parents. Consequently, immunization clinics have been used as platforms<br />

for delivering new child health intervention packages, especially in the<br />

developing world (WHO, 2002). They provide a ready framework for introducing<br />

infant hearing screening (Olusanya et al., 2004). Community-based<br />

screening, however, is not limited to immunization clinics and may be implemented<br />

during infant welfare clinics (Bantock & Croxson, 1998; Kapil, 2002;<br />

Lin et al., 2004; McPherson, Kei, Smyth, Latham, & Loscher, 1998). In addition,<br />

community-based screening can be considered as a complement where<br />

there is a high rate of babies missed due to early discharge or the inability of<br />

the screening personnel to cope with the high birth rates in some public<br />

hospitals in heavily populated areas (Mathur & Dhawan, 2006). Conducting<br />

screening in infants’ homes, as has been reported in some developed<br />

countries, is not a practicable option in developing countries because of the<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 399


Table 4. Typical National Immunization Schedule for Infants in Developing<br />

Countries<br />

enormous logistical challenges associated with this strategy (Oudesluys-<br />

Murphy & Harlaar, 1997; Owen, Webb, & Evans, 2001).<br />

Service Delivery<br />

Reports from the developed world suggest that favorable outcomes still<br />

may be achieved if hearing loss is detected within the first year of life<br />

(Kennedy et al., 2006; Moeller, 2000). A typical national immunization schedule<br />

for developing countries offers a number of options within the first year<br />

of life for infant hearing screening (see Table 4). Bacille Calmette-Guérin<br />

(BCG) immunization often records the highest uptake of all the vaccinations<br />

administered in the first year in many developing countries (UNICEF, 2005).<br />

However, the age at which babies are presented for various immunizations<br />

varies widely within and across communities. For instance, the age at presentation<br />

for BCG immunization in Nigeria typically ranges from 1 to 136<br />

days (75 th percentile, 24 days; 95 th percentile, 61 days) (Olusanya & Okolo,<br />

2006a). A similar variation has been reported in South Africa for the first<br />

diphtheria-pertussis-tetanus (DPT) immunization at 6 weeks (Swanepoel et<br />

al., 2006). This variation in age and timing has the advantage of facilitating<br />

early detection for a significant number of infants with progressive or delayed-onset<br />

congenital hearing loss and those from acquired causes that<br />

would have been missed under hospital-based UNHS. These categories of<br />

hearing loss commonly are associated with prevailing adverse perinatal conditions<br />

in many developing countries (Olusanya & Okolo, 2006b). In addition,<br />

this platform provides the chance to screen sick babies (now recovered) who<br />

could not be tested or immunized during admission.<br />

Possible Challenges<br />

Recommended Age<br />

Vaccine Birth 6 Weeks 10 Weeks 14 Weeks 9 Months<br />

Bacille Calmette-Guérin (BCG) x<br />

Oral polio* x x x x<br />

Diphtheria-pertussis-tetanus (DPT) x x x<br />

Hepatitis B* x x x<br />

Haemophilus influenzae type b* x x x<br />

Yellow fever* x<br />

Measles x<br />

Note. From World Health Organization (2002).<br />

*Not provided in or applicable to all countries.<br />

Difficulties may arise when routine immunization programs are interrupted<br />

because of vaccine shortages or other extraneous reasons. For instance,<br />

400 Olusanya


a widely reported controversy concerning polio vaccination in some Nigerian<br />

states would have disrupted any adjunct programs, including infant hearing<br />

screening (Fleck, 2004; Mitka, 2004; Renne, 2006). Despite the possibility of<br />

program disruptions, the experiences in many countries strongly suggest that<br />

routine immunization programs are still the most effective platform for integrated<br />

child health intervention globally. Another major challenge arises<br />

when screening cannot be completed and babies require follow-up visits in<br />

addition to scheduled immunization clinics. It may be useful in this situation<br />

to ascertain likely difficulties the affected parents may encounter in keeping<br />

follow-up appointments and then to offer appropriate support, such as compensation<br />

or reimbursement for transportation fares. Moreover, a registry of<br />

babies with incomplete screening tests may be maintained at subsequent<br />

immunization clinics for tracking purposes.<br />

Progress Reports from Current Projects<br />

Preliminary results from two pilot programs in South Africa and Nigeria<br />

confirm that infant hearing screening at immunization clinics is feasible and<br />

worthwhile in developing countries, although more reports are needed from<br />

other regions because screening protocols may differ from country to country<br />

(see Table 3). For instance, the screening program in South Africa used a<br />

protocol consisting of a first-stage distortion product OAE and a highfrequency<br />

probe tone (1 kHz) tympanometry for infants ages birth to 12<br />

months. A planned second-stage screen with AABR was discontinued because<br />

of practical difficulties with administering the test (Swanepoel et al.,<br />

2006). Rather, the first-stage protocol was repeated for subsequent stages.<br />

Only 40% of those scheduled for the second-stage screen returned, possibly<br />

due to the timing of follow-up visits outside a routine immunization schedule.<br />

In contrast, a two-stage protocol with an initial TEOAE screen at BCG<br />

immunization clinics was used in the program in Nigeria. Those referred<br />

from the first-stage screen were scheduled for a second-stage screen with<br />

AABR within a week. The uptake for the second-stage screen improved from<br />

50% at the initial stages of the program to 75% subsequently. Babies older<br />

than 3 months often were difficult to test with this screening protocol; thus,<br />

it was necessary to exclude them from the two-stage screening, which reduced<br />

coverage to 83% of babies attending the BCG clinics. Although the<br />

default rates from South Africa and some other developing countries are<br />

high, they are comparable to reported rates in the early stages of most UNHS<br />

programs in the developed world (Korres, Balatsouras, Nikolopoulos, Korres,<br />

& Ferekidis, 2006; Mehl & Thomson, 2002). Effective data management and a<br />

comprehensive tracking system are vital to minimizing avoidable loss to<br />

follow-up.<br />

Additional details on the progress towards early hearing detection in developing<br />

countries based on a collaborative, transnational survey have been<br />

described elsewhere (Olusanya et al., 2007).<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 401


Scope and Status of Intervention<br />

Perhaps the foremost benefit of infant hearing screening is the timely<br />

knowledge it provides for parents who otherwise may experience a wide<br />

range of emotions and seek ineffective solutions as they begin to suspect their<br />

children’s atypical response or lack of response to sound. This is quite typical<br />

in many communities in developing countries where superstitious beliefs and<br />

recourse to unorthodox practices are prevalent (Andrade & Ross, 2005; Byford<br />

& Veenstra, 2004; Kiyanga & Moores, 2003; Odebiyi & Togonu-<br />

Bickersteth, 1987; Stephens et al., 2000). Children with PCEHL are capable of<br />

learning and acquiring spoken language like their peers with normal hearing.<br />

Current technological advances not only facilitate prompt and accurate detection,<br />

but also offer children with PCEHL the opportunity to develop spoken<br />

language. Because the majority of parents use spoken language and lack<br />

interest in any other form of communication, there are major incentives for<br />

parents to seek appropriate help for their children as early as possible when<br />

the most benefit can be provided. Evidence on the status of intervention<br />

services following early hearing detection is scanty because such services are<br />

offered predominantly by private providers who often are reluctant to provide<br />

information on their services for research purposes (Madriz, 2000; Russo,<br />

2000). The target of intervention services is not necessarily to provide the<br />

entire range of options currently offered in developed countries (Gabbard &<br />

Schryer, 2003; Gravel & O’Gara, 2003) but to focus on functionality, affordability<br />

and what is culturally appropriate within an ethical framework (Flett<br />

& Stoffell, 2003). For instance, in Brazil, improved interactions between children<br />

with hearing loss and their families were reported following a specialized<br />

speech and hearing intervention program comprising speech therapy,<br />

sign language and oral language workshops for the children; sign language<br />

workshops for hearing parents; and a parent support group (Grandori &<br />

Hayes, 2006). In addition, the use and effectiveness of “experience books” in<br />

monitoring progress of infants enrolled in early intervention programs from<br />

babbling to first words and language comprehension have been reported also<br />

in Brazil (Novaes, Mendes, & Figueiredo, 2006). In general, the communication<br />

options currently available to parents can be considered under two broad<br />

categories: auditory based and nonauditory based.<br />

Auditory-Based Communication<br />

The term “auditory based” includes the development of spoken language<br />

through the use of hearing devices to facilitate full integration of the child into<br />

mainstream society. It does not preclude the use of speechreading, facial<br />

expressions and naturally occurring gestures.<br />

The first hurdle in this approach is parental acceptance of hearing aids for<br />

402 Olusanya


their babies at such an early age because hearing aids are associated more<br />

commonly with elderly people. The issue of cosmetic acceptability is then<br />

likely to follow (Furuta & Yoshino, 1998; Mutton & Peacock, 2005). Behindthe-ear<br />

hearing aids publicly announce an invisible disability in a baby, which<br />

could be culturally embarrassing to parents and cause them great stress in<br />

responding to inquiries from neighbors and friends about these “strange”<br />

devices in an apparently normal child. During the initial stages, some parents<br />

may be vulnerable to the not uncommon misconception that hearing aids are<br />

not necessary because the child will outgrow the disability. Some even may<br />

be offered unorthodox therapies, such as medicinal plants and animal fat to<br />

treat the “hearing ailment” (Andrade & Ross, 2005; Kale, 1995; Odebiyi &<br />

Togonu-Bickersteth, 1987).<br />

The cost of hearing aids, ear molds, batteries, drying capsules and repairs<br />

is another important consideration. In some countries, such as Brazil, Mauritius<br />

and South Africa, hearing aids are provided free or subsidized in public<br />

hospitals. However, the quality of hearing aids provided is limited to the<br />

most basic models and there are always long waiting lists of eligible patients<br />

of all ages, although preferences may be given to infants and young children<br />

(Swanepoel, 2006).<br />

Many of these factors apply to an even greater extent to the use of cochlear<br />

implants and influence the parents’ decision-making process (Lynas, 2005;<br />

Munoz-Baell & Ruiz, 2000). It is not uncommon to find that even after making<br />

an informed and appropriate choice, parents still seek more time to adjust to<br />

the new reality and it is always prudent not to pressure them to immediately<br />

follow up on their choices.<br />

A great motivation for parents often comes from the assurance and evidence<br />

that all the efforts involved in supporting the child are worthwhile.<br />

Testimonials of other parents with successful experiences are valuable and<br />

just as powerful as the displeasure of unsatisfied parents to prospective parents,<br />

which is perhaps the greatest challenge for service providers in developing<br />

countries. Parents would make any necessary financial sacrifice in<br />

anticipation of visible progress in speech and language development; however,<br />

the challenge for the interventionist is securing the ongoing parental<br />

participation crucial to achieving satisfactory language outcome that is commensurate<br />

with the quality of the services provided.<br />

A further challenge is the multilingual settings in many developing countries<br />

where most families have a second national language along with an<br />

ethnic or tribal language. The language used in the home environment may<br />

be different from that used by the interventionist or in the educational setting.<br />

This may present difficulties for the interventionist and ultimately undermine<br />

the efficacy on Auditory-Verbal intervention programs if not well managed<br />

(Gupta, & Chandler, 1993; Penn, 1998).<br />

Support from primary care physicians, nurses and ancillary health workers<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 403


is essential for the prompt enrollment and retention of children in familyoriented<br />

intervention programs because they are usually the first to be contacted<br />

by parents about health-related concerns. However, this group of<br />

workers requires appropriate education on the range of current possibilities<br />

with EHDI in order to provide well-informed counsel to parents. Such training<br />

also should help them recognize and acknowledge the limits of their<br />

competencies and professional experience and refer parents for appropriate<br />

and timely intervention services within the context of the multidisciplinary<br />

participation required for optimum outcomes. The use of nonspecialists during<br />

an acute shortage of qualified professionals is necessary but should be<br />

limited to providing basic support and referral services (Shrestha, Baral, &<br />

Weir, 2001; Wirz & Lichtig, 1998).<br />

In an attempt to address some of these issues, the WHO (2004) developed<br />

guidelines on hearing aids and hearing services in developing countries. This<br />

initiative includes efforts aimed at providing affordable hearing aids and<br />

services and setting up services for delivery, fitting, follow-up, repair and<br />

training. Recognizing that less than 10% of hearing aids manufactured worldwide<br />

and that only 750,000 of the estimated 32 million hearing aids needed<br />

annually reach developing countries, a global partnership called “WWHearing<br />

— Worldwide Hearing Care for Developing Countries” —has been inaugurated<br />

to address the complex task of providing sufficient quantities of<br />

affordable hearing aids and services for developing countries (WHO, 2006).<br />

This group comprises key stakeholders, such as policymakers, service providers,<br />

trainers and users in developing countries; major donors; and expert<br />

advisers across relevant professional disciplines. Already, low-cost, solarpowered<br />

hearing aids increasingly are being made available (Parving &<br />

Christensen, 2004), usually at less than US$100 each but still far from the<br />

WHO’s target price of US$40. Many groups like Godisa (based in Botswana),<br />

Project IMPACT, Comcare, Hearing International and Christoffel-<br />

Blindenmission are engaged actively in the task of achieving this target price<br />

(McPherson & Brouillette, 2004).<br />

The obvious concern is the trade-off between cost and functionality (Olusanya,<br />

2004). Presently, most of the instruments and services being developed<br />

are primarily for adult users, and these may have very limited use for the<br />

pediatric population. Further research is needed on this age group. In contrast<br />

to the progress made toward affordability and availability of hearing aids,<br />

cochlear implantation is still beyond the reach of many communities in the<br />

developing world, although notable progress is evident in a growing number<br />

of countries (Belal, 1986; Berruecos, 2000; Farhadi, Daneshi, Emamjomeh, &<br />

Hasanzadeh, 2000; Grandori & Hayes, 2006; Swanepoel, 2006; Zeng, 1995). As<br />

the benefits of amplification become more established, the additional benefits<br />

available from cochlear implantation also will become apparent.<br />

Effective auditory-based intervention require more than the fitting of hearing<br />

devices (Meadow-Orlans, Spencer, Koester, & Steinberg, 2004). It requires<br />

404 Olusanya


active, ongoing, culturally appropriate collaboration between the intervention<br />

specialists and the child’s family. Parents must be engaged to be active<br />

participants in the intervention program and discouraged from shifting more<br />

attention to siblings without special needs at the expense of the child with<br />

hearing loss. However, the interventionists must be sensitive to parental emotions,<br />

stress and grief as they adjust to the special and often unfamiliar needs<br />

of an apparently healthy child. To be effective, early intervention must facilitate<br />

developmentally appropriate language skills and enhance the family’s<br />

understanding of its infant’s strengths and needs, build family support and<br />

confidence in parenting the child and promote the family’s ability to advocate<br />

for the child especially for suitable educational placement (JCIH, 2000). Unfortunately,<br />

there are many institutional and cultural barriers to mainstreaming<br />

in developing countries, but these are not insurmountable (Eleweke &<br />

Rodda, 2000). Parents collectively can become catalysts for policy changes<br />

through effective advocacy.<br />

Another major challenge is the acute manpower shortages in many countries;<br />

only few tertiary academic institutions offer audiology-related training<br />

except for special education or ear, nose and throat surgery (Alberti, 1999;<br />

Madriz, 2000; Russo, 2000; Swanepoel, 2006). In this vital area, governments<br />

can play an effective role at minimizing cost by equipping educational institutions<br />

to provide relevant and up-to-date training and promoting exchange<br />

programs with overseas institutions in developed countries. The training<br />

curricula for health professionals, interventionists and teachers need to be<br />

adapted and enhanced appropriately. In addition, because primary care physicians<br />

wield considerable influence over parental health-seeking behaviors,<br />

it also is important to identify and address reasons for their ineffectiveness at<br />

facilitating a prompt referral process for timely/appropriate intervention as<br />

well as their poor adherence to EHDI guidelines for best practices (Moeller,<br />

White, Shisler, 2006; Kemper, Uren, Moseley, Clark, 2006; Cabana, Rand,<br />

Powe, Wu, Wilson, Abboud, Rubib, 1999). For example, as part of efforts to<br />

accelerate training to increase the availability of relevant manpower and enhance<br />

professional involvement, various nongovernmental cross-country initiatives<br />

to provide Internet-based training are being developed by groups in<br />

Nigeria and South Africa with support from nonprofit organizations in the<br />

United States, Australia, the United Kingdom and Canada.<br />

Similarly, free Internet-based training support for parents increasingly is<br />

becoming more widely available from various hearing health-related organizations.<br />

However, such Internet-based educational resources may be of limited<br />

use in areas of developing countries where the literacy rates are low,<br />

computer access limited and social infrastructures poorly developed. Nevertheless,<br />

progress toward self-development through further learning and community<br />

support for persons with special needs is evident in a growing<br />

number of countries. Overall, prospects are still good for high-quality service<br />

delivery for persons with hearing loss (Silverman & Moulton, 1997), and<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 405


international agencies and relevant professional associations can play a crucial<br />

role in fostering the attainment of this goal more rapidly.<br />

Nonauditory-Based Communication<br />

The most common and oldest form of intervention for children with<br />

PCEHL in developing countries is enrollment in schools for children with<br />

hearing loss in which sign language is the predominant mode of communication.<br />

Such enrollment often occurs as late as age 10 because parents are<br />

reluctant to accept this mode of communication. The delay also has been<br />

attributed to past failure in accessing auditory-based intervention. Thus, the<br />

nonauditory-based communication option is fast becoming an alternative<br />

choice when all options for auditory-based intervention have been exhausted,<br />

found unsatisfactory due to the lack of requisite expertise or simply do not<br />

exist. Some parents may be compelled to choose this option for economic<br />

reasons or if they themselves already have sign language skills. Parents, of<br />

course, reserve the right to choose nonauditory-based interventions for other<br />

reasons but in many cultures, the social stigma of having to learn sign language<br />

can be a major disincentive for parents of children with PCEHL (Munoz-Baell<br />

& Ruiz, 2000).<br />

Many developing countries have no provision for persons with disabilities<br />

in general. Public schools for children with hearing loss often are overpopulated,<br />

poorly maintained and dependent on charitable donations, thereby<br />

limiting their capacity to adapt to educational and technological advances.<br />

Government mandates, similar to the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act<br />

or the Americans with Disabilities Act, governing employment rights for people<br />

with disabilities also do not exist in many developing countries. The prevailing<br />

culture of noninclusion isolates persons with hearing loss from wider<br />

society for life. Therefore, it is not uncommon to find that children with<br />

PCEHL grow up without any independent means of livelihood and end up<br />

among the “poorest” of the poor (Elwan, 1999; Olusanya et al., 2006b). This<br />

concern is a major one for indigent parents who desire a better life for their<br />

children so that their children may provide them economic support in their<br />

old age. Parents are unlikely to ignore these considerations when choosing a<br />

communication option for their children with hearing loss. Regardless of the<br />

preferred communication mode, early intervention through early hearing<br />

detection is more efficacious and desirable.<br />

Conclusions<br />

EHDI services in most of the developed world have begun to address<br />

effectively the burden of permanent congenital and early onset hearing loss to<br />

children, their families and society. Hospital-based universal hearing screening<br />

of newborns and infants substantially supported by public funding makes<br />

406 Olusanya


this goal achievable. Reservations toward promoting similar programs in<br />

developing countries are no longer warranted as demonstrated by the voluntary<br />

progress that has been and continues to be made by primarily<br />

nongovernmental organizations in providing sustainable and culturally appropriate<br />

EHDI programs. The prospects of government-backed free EHDI<br />

services may be dim; however, the majority of patients in developing countries<br />

who currently bear the cost of other health services would likely be<br />

willing to take advantage of effective, time-bound hearing health interventions<br />

if they were available. Several opportunities exist for international organizations<br />

and relevant professional associations to be engaged actively in<br />

support of EHDI in developing countries. Positive evidence emerging from<br />

the growing number of pilot programs affirms the value of using alternative,<br />

cost-effective, community-based platforms in which conventional hospitalbased<br />

newborn screening programs miss the majority of babies who are born<br />

outside regular hospitals and who are at greater risk of prelingual hearing<br />

loss. The current resolutions of the WHA on the global prevention of hearing<br />

impairment and the management of disability; as well as the early childhood<br />

initiatives by UNICEF, UNESCO and the World Bank provide additional<br />

moral impetus for extending this “silent, achievable and important revolution”<br />

throughout the developing world to forestall the communicative, educational,<br />

social and economic disabilities associated with early childhood<br />

hearing loss.<br />

References<br />

Alberti, P.W. (1996). The prevention of hearing loss worldwide. Scandinavian<br />

Journal of Audiology, 25(Suppl. 42), 13-17.<br />

Alberti, P.W. (1999). Pediatric ear nose throat services’ demands and resources:<br />

A global perspective. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

49(Suppl. 1), 51-59.<br />

Ali, L., Siddiq S, Khan MA, Maqbool S. (2000). A hospital-based universal<br />

newborn hearing screening programme using transient evoked otoacoustic<br />

emission (TEOAE). Pakistan Pediatric Journal, 24, 117-125.<br />

Ali, L., Khan, M.A., Baloch, G.R., Aziz, M.T. (2004). Hearing screening in high<br />

risk neonates. Pakistan Pediatric Journal, 28, 1-6.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (1995). Joint Committee on Infant<br />

Hearing 1994 position statement. Pediatrics, 95, 152-156.<br />

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (1999). Task Force on Newborn and<br />

Infant Hearing: Newborn and Infant Hearing Loss. Detection and Intervention<br />

Pediatrics, 103, 527-530.<br />

Andrade, V., & Ross, E. (2005). Beliefs and practices of black South African<br />

traditional healers regarding hearing impairment. International Journal of<br />

Audiology, 44, 489-499.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 407


Ansari, M.S. (2004). Screening programme for hearing impairment in newborns:<br />

A challenge during rehabilitation for all. Asia Pacific Disability Rehabilitation<br />

Journal, 15(1), 83-89.<br />

Arnold, C.L., Davis, T.C., Humiston, S.G., Bocchini, J.A., Bass, P.F., et al.<br />

(2006). Infant hearing screening: Stakeholder recommendations for parentcentered<br />

communication. Pediatrics, 117, S341-S354.<br />

Attias, J., Al-Masri, M., AbuKader, L., Cohen, G., Merlov, P., Pratt, H., et al.<br />

(2006). The prevalence of congenital and early-onset hearing loss in Jordanian<br />

and Israeli infants. International Journal of Audiology, 45, 528-536.<br />

Bachmann, K.R., & Arvedson, J.C. (1998). Early identification and intervention<br />

for children who are hearing impaired. Pediatrics in Review, 19, 155-165.<br />

Bamford, J., Uus, K., & Davis, A. (2005). Screening for hearing loss in childhood:<br />

Issues, evidence and current approaches in the UK. Journal of Medical<br />

Screening, 12, 119-124.<br />

Bantock, H.M., & Croxson, S. (1998). Universal hearing screening using transient<br />

otoacoustic emissions in a community health clinic. Archives of Disease<br />

in Childhood, 78, 249-252.<br />

Barsky-Firkser, L., & Sun, S. (1997). Universal newborn hearing screenings: A<br />

three year experience. Pediatrics, 99(6), E4.<br />

Belal, A. (1986). Cochlea implantation in developing countries. American Journal<br />

of Otology, 7(4), 244-247.<br />

Bener, A., ElHakeem, A.A.M., & Abdulhadi, K. (2005). Is there any association<br />

between consanguinity and hearing loss? International Journal of Pediatric<br />

Otolaryngology, 69, 327-333.<br />

Berruecos, P. (2000). Cochlear implants: An international perspective—Latin<br />

American countries and Spain. Audiology, 39, 221-225.<br />

Bess, F.H., & Paradise, J.L. (1994). Universal screening for infant hearing<br />

impairment: Not simple, not risk-free, not necessarily beneficial and not<br />

presently justified. Pediatrics, 93, 330-334. Pediatrics, 94, 948-963.)<br />

Byford, J., & Veenstra, N. (2004). The importance of cultural factors in the<br />

planning of rehabilitation services in a remote area of Papua, New Guinea.<br />

Disability and Rehabilitation, 26, 166-175.<br />

Cabana, M.D., Rand, C.S., Powe, N.R., Wu, A.W., Wilson, M.H., Abboud,<br />

P.C., Rubin, H.R. (1999). Why don’t physicians follow clinical practice<br />

guidelines? JAMA, 282, 1458-1465.<br />

Canale, A., Favero, E., Lacilla, M., Recchia, E., Schindler, A., Roggero, N., et<br />

al. (2006). Age of diagnosis of deaf babies: A retrospective analysis highlighting<br />

the advantage of newborn hearing screening. International Journal<br />

of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 1283-1289.<br />

Carney, A.E., & Moeller, M.P. (1998). Treatment efficacy: Hearing loss in<br />

children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41, S61-S84.<br />

Chapchap, M.J., & Segre, C.M. (2001). Universal newborn hearing screening<br />

and transient evoked otoacoustic emissions: New concepts in Brazil. Scandinavian<br />

Journal of Audiology, 30(Suppl. 53), 33-36.<br />

408 Olusanya


Clemens, C.J., Davis, S.A., & Bailey, A.R. (2000). The false-positive in universal<br />

newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, E1.<br />

Crockett, R., Wright, A.J., Uus, K., Bamford, J., & Marteau, T. (2006). Maternal<br />

anxiety following newborn hearing screening: The moderating role of<br />

knowledge. Journal of Medical Screening, 13, 20-25.<br />

Davis, A., Bamford, J., Wilson, I., Ramkalawan, T., Forshaw, M., Wright, S.<br />

(1997). A critical review of the role of neonatal hearing screening in the<br />

detection of congenital hearing impairment. Health Technology Assessment,<br />

1, 1-176.<br />

Derekoy, F.S. (2000). Etiology of deafness in Afyon school for the deaf in<br />

Turkey. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 55, 125-131.<br />

D’Mello, J. (1995). High-risk register: An economical tool for early identification.<br />

Indian Journal of Pediatrics, 62, 731-735.<br />

Elango, S., Chand, R.P., & Purohit, G.N. (1992). Childhood deafness in Malaysia.<br />

International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 24, 11-17.<br />

Eleweke, C.J., & Rodda, M. (2002). The challenge of enhancing inclusive education<br />

in developing countries. International Journal of Inclusive Education, 6,<br />

113-126.<br />

Elwan, A. (1999). Poverty and disability: A survey of the literature (Social Protection<br />

Discussion Paper No. 9932). Washington, DC: World Bank.<br />

Ewing, I.R., & Ewing, A.W.G. (1944). The ascertainment of deafness in infancy<br />

and early childhood. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 59, 309-333.<br />

Farhadi, M., Daneshi, A., Emamjomeh, H., & Hasanzadeh, S. (2000). Cochlear<br />

implantation in Iran: A report of 190 cases. Advances in Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

57, 435-438.<br />

Federal Republic of Nigeria. (2005). Revised national health policy 2004. Abuja,<br />

Nigeria: Federal Ministry of Health.<br />

Fleck, F. (2004). West Africa polio campaign boycotted by Nigerian states.<br />

British Medical Journal, 328, 485.<br />

Flett, P.J., & Stoffell, B.F. (2003). Ethical issues in pediatric rehabilitation.<br />

Journal of Pediatrics and Child Health, 39, 219-223.<br />

Furuta, H., & Yoshino, T. (1998). The present situation of the use of hearing<br />

aids in rural areas of Sri Lanka: Problems and future prospects. International<br />

Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 21, 103-107.<br />

Gabbard, S.A., & Schryer, J. (2003). Early amplification options. Mental Retardation<br />

and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 236-242.<br />

Glascoe, F.P., Maclean, W.E., & Stone, W.L. (1991). The importance of parents’<br />

concerns about their child’s behavior. Clinical Pediatrics (Philadelphia), 30,<br />

8-11.<br />

Glover, D.M. (2003). The deaf child—challenges in management: A parent’s<br />

perspective. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 67(Suppl.<br />

1), S197-S200.<br />

Gopal, R., Hugo, S.R., & Louw, B. (2001). Identification and follow-up of<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 409


children with hearing loss in Mauritius. International Journal of Pediatric<br />

Otorhinolaryngology, 57, 99-113.<br />

Gottret, P., & Schieber, G. (Eds.). (2006). Health financing revisited: A practitioner’s<br />

guide. Washington, DC: World Bank.<br />

Grandori, F., & Hayes, D. (Eds.). (2006). NHS2006: Beyond newborn hearing<br />

screening: Infant and childhood hearing in science and clinical practice [Book of<br />

abstracts]. Como Lake, Italy: NHS2006.<br />

Gravel, J.S., & O’Gara, J. (2003). Communication options for children with<br />

hearing loss. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews,<br />

9, 243-251.<br />

Gray, R.F. (1989). Causes of deafness in schools for the deaf in Madras. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 18, 97-<strong>106</strong>.<br />

Gupta, A.F., & Chandler, H. (1993). Paediatric speech and language therapy<br />

referral in Singapore: implications for multilingual language disability. European<br />

Journal of Disorders of Communication, 28, 311-317.<br />

Habib, H.S., & Abdelgaffar, H. (2005). Neonatal hearing screening with transient<br />

evoked otoacoustic emissions in Western Saudi Arabia. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 69, 839-842.<br />

Hall, J.W., 3 rd , Smith, S.D., & Popelka, G.R. (2004). Newborn hearing screening<br />

with combined otoacoustic emissions and auditory brainstem responses.<br />

Journal of American Academy of Audiology, 15, 414-425.<br />

Harrison, M., Roush, J., & Wallace, J. (2003). Trends in age of identification<br />

and intervention in infants with hearing loss. Ear and Hearing, 24, 89-95.<br />

Headley, G.M., Campbell, D.E., & Gravel, J.S. (2000). Effect of neonatal test<br />

environment on recording Transient-Evoked Otoacoustic Emissions. Pediatrics,<br />

105, 1279-1285.<br />

Hedderly, T., Baird, G., & McConachie, H. (2003). Parental reaction to disability.<br />

Current Pediatrics, 13, 30-35.<br />

Hergils, L., Hergils, A. (2000). Universal neonatal hearing screening- parental<br />

attitudes and concern. British Journal of Audiology, 34, 321-327.<br />

Hewlett, J., & Waisbren, S.E. (2006). A review of psychological effects of<br />

false-positives on parents and current communication practices in newborn<br />

screening. Journal of Inherited Metabolic Diseases, 29, 677-682.<br />

Holborow, C., Martinson, F., & Anger, N. (1982). A study of deafness in West<br />

Africa. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 4, 107-132.<br />

Jacobson, J.T., & Jacobson, C.A. (1994). The effects of noise in transient EOAE<br />

newborn hearing screening. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

29, 235-248.<br />

Jamison, D.T., Breman, J.G., Measham, A.R., Alleyne, G., Claeson, M., Evans,<br />

D.B., et al. (Eds.). (2006). Disease control priorities in developing countries (2 nd<br />

ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.<br />

Joint Committee on Infant Hearing. (2000). Joint Committee on Infant Hearing<br />

Year 2000 position statement: Principles and guidelines for early hearing<br />

detection and intervention programs. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, 798-817.<br />

410 Olusanya


Kale, R. (1995). Traditional healers in South Africa: A parallel healthcare<br />

system. British Medical Journal, 310, 1182-1185.<br />

Kapil, U. (2002). Integrated Child Development Services (ICDS) scheme: A<br />

program for holistic development of children in India. Indian Journal of<br />

Pediatrics, 69, 597-601.<br />

Kapiriri, L., & Norheim, O.F. (2002). Whose priorities count? Comparison of<br />

community-identified health problems and burden-of-disease-assessed<br />

health priorities in a district in Uganda. Health Expectation, 5, 55-62.<br />

Kemper, A.R., Uren, R.L., Moseley, K.L., Clark, S.J. (2006). Primary care physicians’<br />

attitudes regarding follow-up care for children with positive newborn<br />

screening results. Pediatrics, 118, 1836-1841.<br />

Kennedy, C., Kimm, L., Thornton, R., & Davis, A. (2000). False positives in<br />

universal neonatal screening for hearing impairment. Lancet, 356, 1903-<br />

1904.<br />

Kennedy, C.R., McCann, D.C., Campbell, M.J., Law, C.M., Mullee, M., Petrou,<br />

S., et al. (2006). Language ability after early detection of permanent childhood<br />

hearing impairment. New England Journal of Medicine, 354, 2131-2141.<br />

Khandekar, R., Khabori, M., Mohammed, A.J., & Gupta, R. (2006). Neonatal<br />

screening for hearing impairment: The Oman experience. International Journal<br />

of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 663-670.<br />

Kiyanga, N.B., & Moores, D.F. (2003). Deafness in Sub-Saharan Africa. American<br />

Annals of the Deaf, 148, 18-24.<br />

Korres, S.G., Balatsouras, D.G., Nikolopoulos, T., Korres, G.S., & Ferekidis, E.<br />

(2006). Making universal newborn hearing screening a success. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 241-246.<br />

Kumar S. (1997). Deafness and its prevention–Indian scenario. Indian Journal<br />

of Pediatrics, 64, 801-809.<br />

Kumar, S. (2001). WHO tackles hearing disabilities in developing world. Lancet,<br />

358, 219.<br />

Kurtzer-White, E., & Luterman, D. (2003). Families and children with hearing<br />

loss: Grief and coping. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research<br />

Reviews, 9, 232-235.<br />

Landman, W.A., & Henley, L.D. (1999). Equitable rationing of highly specialized<br />

health care services for children: A perspective from South Africa.<br />

Journal of Medical Ethics, 25, 224-229.<br />

Lin, C.Y., Huang, C.Y., Lin, C.Y., Lin, Y.H., & Wu, J.L. (2004). Communitybased<br />

newborn hearing screening program in Taiwan. International Journal<br />

of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 185-189.<br />

Low, W.K., Pang, K.Y., & Ho, L.Y. (2005). Universal newborn hearing screening<br />

in Singapore: The need, implementation, and challenges. Annals of the<br />

Academy of Medicine of Singapore, 34, 301-306.<br />

Luterman, D., & Kurtzer-White, E. (1999). Identifying hearing loss: parents’<br />

needs. American Journal of Audiology, 8, 13-18.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 411


Lutman, M.E., & Grandori, F. (1999). Screening for neonatal defects European<br />

consensus statement. European Journal of Pediatrics, 158, 95-96.<br />

Lynas, W. (2005). Controversies in the education of deaf children. Current<br />

Pediatrics, 15, 200-206.<br />

Madriz, J. (2000). Hearing impairment in Latin America: An inventory of<br />

limited options and resources. Audiology, 39, 212-220.<br />

Magnuson, M., & Hergils, L. (2000). Late diagnosis of congenital hearing<br />

impairment in children: The parents’ experiences and opinions. Patient Education<br />

and Counselling, 41, 285-294.<br />

Maisoun, A.M., & Zakzouk, S.M. (2003). Hearing screening of neonates at<br />

risk. Saudi Medical Journal, 24, 55-57.<br />

Masoud, A.D., Mohsen, F., Behzad, M., Roshnanak, V., Nikta, H., & Shahrzad,<br />

M. (2006). Preliminary report of newborn hearing screening in Iran [abstract].<br />

In F. Grandori & D. Hayes (Eds.). NHS2006 Conference: Beyond newborn<br />

hearing screening: infant and childhood hearing in science and clinical<br />

practice (pp. 43). Como Lake, Italy: NHS2006.<br />

Mathur, N.N., & Dhawan, R. (2006). An alternative strategy for universal<br />

infants hearing screening in tertiary hospitals with a high delivery rate,<br />

within a developing country, using transient evoked oto-acoustic emissions<br />

and brainstem evoked response audiometry. Journal of Laryngology and<br />

Otology, Nov 20, 1-5 [Epub ahead of print].<br />

Mauk, G.W., White, K.R., Mortensen, L.B., & Behrens, T.R. (1991). The effectiveness<br />

of screening programs based on high-risk characteristics in early<br />

identification of hearing impairment. Ear and Hearing, 12, 312-319.<br />

Maxon, A.B., White, K.R., Culpepper, B., & Vohr, B.R. (1997). Maintaining<br />

acceptably low referral rates in TEOAE-based newborn hearing screening<br />

programs. Journal of Communication Disorders, 30, 457-475.<br />

McPherson, B., & Brouillette, R. (2004). A fair hearing for all: Providing appropriate<br />

amplification in developing countries. Communication Disorders<br />

Quarterly, 25, 219-223.<br />

McPherson, B., Kei, J., Smyth, V., Latham, S., & Loscher, J. (1998). Feasibility<br />

of community-based hearing screening using transient evoked otoacoustic<br />

emissions. Public Health, 112, 147-152.<br />

Meadow-Orlans, K.P., Spencer, P.E., Koester, L.S., & Steinberg, A.G. (2004).<br />

Implications for intervention with infants and families. In K.P. Meadow-<br />

Orlans, P.E. Spencer, & L.S. Koester (Eds.), The world of deaf infants: A<br />

longitudinal study. New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 218-228).<br />

Mehl, A.L., & Thomson, V. (2002). The Colorado Newborn Hearing Screening<br />

Project, 1992-1999: On the threshold of effective population-based universal<br />

newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics, 109, E7.<br />

Mencher, G.T. (2000). Challenge of epidemiological research in the developing<br />

world: Overview. Audiology, 39, 178-183.<br />

Mencher, G.T.A., & Devoe, S.J. (2001). Universal newborn hearing screening:<br />

412 Olusanya


A dream realized or nightmare in the making? Scandinavian Audiology,<br />

30(Suppl. 53), 15-21.<br />

Minja, B.M. (1998). Etiology of deafness among children in Dar es Salaam,<br />

Tanzania. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 42, 225-231.<br />

Mitka, M. (2004). Vaccine rumors, funding shortfall threaten to derail global<br />

polio eradication efforts. JAMA, 291, 1947-1948.<br />

Moeller, M.P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children<br />

who are deaf and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, <strong>106</strong>, E43.<br />

Moeller, M.P., White, K.R., Shisler, L. (2006). Primary care physicians’ knowledge,<br />

attitudes, and practices related to newborn hearing screening, Pediatrics,<br />

118, 1357-1370.<br />

Morton, C.C., & Nance, W.E. (2006). Newborn hearing screening: A silent<br />

revolution. New England Journal of Medicine, 354, 2151-2164.<br />

Morzaria, S., Westerberg, B.D., & Kozak, F.K. (2004). Systematic review of the<br />

etiology of bilateral sensorineural hearing loss in children. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 1193-1198.<br />

Mukari, S.Z., Tan, K.Y., & Abdullah, A. (2006). A pilot project on hospitalbased<br />

universal newborn hearing screening: Lessons learned. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 843-851.<br />

Mukari, S.Z., Vandort, S., Ahmad, K., Saim, L., & Mohamed, A.S. (1999).<br />

Parents’ awareness and knowledge of the special needs of their hearingimpaired<br />

child. Medical Journal of Malaysia, 54, 87-95.<br />

Munoz-Baell, I.M., & Ruiz, M.T. (2000). Empowering the deaf: Let the deaf be<br />

deaf. Journal of Epidemiology Community Health, 54, 40-44.<br />

Mutton, P.E., & Peacock, K. (2005). Neonatal hearing screens: Wessex revisited.<br />

Lancet, 366, 612-613.<br />

Nascimento, C.A.L., Daher, C.V., Nogueira, J.L.S., Pisanesch, E., Silveira,<br />

L.M., & Miranda, S. (2006). The Brazilian national policy for audiological<br />

care [Abstract]. In F. Grandori & D. Hayes (Eds.), NHS2006: Beyond newborn<br />

hearing screening: Infant and childhood hearing in science and clinical practice<br />

(pp. 8-9). Como Lake, Italy: NHS2006.<br />

National Screening Committee U.K. (2003). Criteria for appraising the viability,<br />

effectiveness and appropriateness of a screening programme. National Screening<br />

Committee, London Department of Health.<br />

Newton, V.E., Liu, X., Ke, X., Xu, L., & Bamford, J.M. (1999). Evaluation of the<br />

use of a questionnaire to detect hearing loss in babies in China. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 48, 125-129.<br />

Ng, P.K., Hui, Y., Lam, B.C.C., Goh, W.H.S., & Yeung, C.Y. (2004). Feasibility<br />

of implementing a universal neonatal hearing screening programme using<br />

distortion product otoacoustic emission detection at a university hospital in<br />

Hong Kong. Hong Kong Medical Journal, 10, 6-13.<br />

Novaes, B., Mendes, B., & Figueiredo, R. (2006) Aural habilitation: The use of<br />

experience books with infants less than two years old and implications for<br />

speech and language development [Abstract]. In F. Grandori & D. Hayes<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 413


(Eds.), NHS2006: Beyond newborn hearing screening: Infant and childhood hearing<br />

in science and clinical practice (pp.166-167). Como Lake, Italy: NHS2006.<br />

Odebiyi, A.I., & Togonu-Bickersteth, F. (1987). Concepts and management of<br />

deafness in the Yoruba medical system: A case study of traditional healers<br />

in Ile-Ife, Nigeria. Social Science and Medicine, 24, 645-649.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., & Newton, V.E. (in press). Global burden of childhood hearing<br />

impairment and disease control priorities for developing countries.<br />

Lancet.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., & Okolo, A.A. (2006a). Early hearing detection at immunization<br />

clinics in developing countries. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

70, 1495-1498.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., & Okolo, A.A. (2006b). Adverse perinatal conditions in hearing-impaired<br />

children in a developing country. Pediatric Perinatal Epidemiology,<br />

20, 366-371.<br />

Olusanya, B. (2004). Self-reported outcomes of aural rehabilitation in a developing<br />

country. International Journal of Audiology, 43, 563-571.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Luxon, L.M., & Wirz, S.L. (2004). Benefits and challenges of<br />

newborn hearing screening for developing countries. International Journal of<br />

Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 287-305.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Luxon, L.M., & Wirz, S.L. (2005a). Screening for early childhood<br />

hearing loss in Nigeria. Journal of Medical Screening, 12, 115-118.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Luxon, L.M., & Wirz, S.L. (2005b). Childhood deafness poses<br />

problems in developing countries. British Medical Journal, 330, 480-481.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Luxon, L.M., & Wirz, S.L. (2006a). Ethical issues in newborn<br />

hearing screening for developing countries. Journal of Medical Ethics, 32,<br />

587-590.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Ruben, R.J., & Parving, A. (2006b). Reducing the burden of<br />

communication disorders in the developing world: An opportunity for the<br />

millennium development project. JAMA, 296, 441-444.<br />

Olusanya, B.O., Swanepoel, D.W, Chapchap, M.J., Castillo, S., Habib, H.,<br />

Mukari, S.Z., et al. (2007). Progress towards early detection services for<br />

infants with hearing loss in developing countries. BMC Health Services Research,<br />

7, 14.<br />

Oudesluys-Murphy, A.M., & Harlaar, J. (1997). Neonatal hearing screening<br />

with an automated auditory brainstem response screener in the infant’s<br />

home. Acta Pediatrica, 86, 651-655.<br />

Owen, M., Webb, M., & Evans, K. (2001). Community based universal neonatal<br />

hearing screening by health visitors using otoacoustic emissions. Archives<br />

of Disease of Childhood Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 84, F157-F162.<br />

Parving, A. (1999). Hearing screening: Aspects of epidemiology and identification<br />

of hearing impaired children. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

49(Suppl. 1), S287-S292.<br />

Parving, A., & Christensen, B. (2004). Clinical trial of a low-cost, solarpowered<br />

hearing aid. Acta Otolaryngology, 124, 416-420.<br />

414 Olusanya


Penn, C. (1998). The study of child language in South Africa. Folia Phoniatrica<br />

et Logopaedica, 50, 256-270.<br />

Prendergast, S.G., Lartz, M.N., & Fiedler, B.C. (2002). Ages of diagnosis, amplification,<br />

and early intervention of infants and young children with hearing<br />

loss: Findings from parent interviews. American Annals of the Deaf, 147,<br />

24-30.<br />

Quintos, M.R., Isleta, P.F., Chiong, C.C., & Abes, G.T. (2003). Newborn hearing<br />

screening using the evoked otoacoustic emission: The Philippine General<br />

Hospital experience. Southeast Asian Journal of Tropical Medicine and<br />

Public Health, 34(Suppl. 3), 231-233.<br />

Renne, E. (2004). Perspectives on polio and immunization in Northern Nigeria.<br />

Social Science and Medicine, 63, 1857-1869.<br />

Roberts, H. (1996). Listen to parents: They may know best. British Medical<br />

Journal, 313, 954-955.<br />

Russ, S.A., Rickards, F., Poulakis, Z., Barker, M., Saunders, K., & Wake, M.<br />

(2002). Six year effectiveness of a population based two tier infant hearing<br />

screening programme. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 86, 245-250.<br />

Russ, S.A., Kuo, A.A., Poulakis, Z., Barker, M., Rickards, F., Saunders, K., et<br />

al. (2004). Qualitative analysis of parents’ experience with early detection of<br />

hearing loss. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 89, 353-358.<br />

Russo, I.C.P. (2000). Overview of audiology in Brazil: State of the art. Audiology,<br />

39, 202-206.<br />

Schroeder, L., Petrou, S., Kennedy, C., McCann, D., Law, C., Watkin, P.M., et<br />

al. (2006). The economic costs of congenital bilateral permanent childhood<br />

hearing impairment. Pediatrics, 117, 1101-1112.<br />

Screening Brief. (2001). Screening infants for congenital deafness. Journal of<br />

Medical Screening, 8, 165.<br />

Sellars, S., & Brighton, P. (1983). Childhood deafness in southern Africa: An<br />

etiological survey of 3064 deaf children. Journal of Laryngology and Otolology,<br />

97, 885-889.<br />

Shrestha, R., Baral, K., & Weir, N. (2001). Community ear care delivery by<br />

community ear assistants and volunteers: A pilot program. Journal of Laryngology<br />

Otology, 115, 869-873.<br />

Silverman, F.H., & Moulton, R. (1997). First-class clinical services are possible<br />

in a developing country: Speech, language, and hearing in the Gaza Strip.<br />

American Journal of Speech and Language Pathology, 6, 5-7.<br />

Smith, A. (2003). Preventing deafness: an achievable challenge. The WHO<br />

perspective. International Congress Series, 1240, 183-191.<br />

Smith, R.J., Bale, J.F., & White, K.R. (2005). Sensorineural hearing loss in<br />

children. Lancet, 365, 879-890.<br />

Somefun, A.O., Lesi, F.E.A., Danfulani, M.A., & Olusanya, B.O. (2006). Communication<br />

disorders in Nigerian children. International Journal of Pediatric<br />

Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 697-702.<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 415


Stephens, D. (1996). Hearing rehabilitation in a psychosocial framework.<br />

Scandinavian Audiology, 25(Suppl. 43), 57-66.<br />

Stephens, D., Stephens, R., & Eisenhart-Rothe, A. (2000). Attitudes toward<br />

hearing impaired children in less developed countries: A pilot study. Audiology,<br />

39, 184-191.<br />

Strong, K., Wald, N., Miller, A., & Alwan, A. (2005). Current concepts in<br />

screening for non-communicable disease: World Health Organisation Consultation<br />

Group Report on methodology of non-communicable disease<br />

screening. Journal of Medical Screening, 12, 12-19.<br />

Stuart, A., Moretz, M., & Yang, E.Y. (2000). An investigation of maternal stress<br />

after neonatal hearing screening. American Journal of Audiology, 9, 135-141.<br />

Swanepoel, D.W. (2006). Audiology in South Africa. International Journal of<br />

Audiology, 45, 262-266.<br />

Swanepoel, D.W., Hugo, R., & Louw, B. (2006). Infant hearing screening at<br />

immunization clinics in South Africa. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology,<br />

70, 1241-1249.<br />

The Government of Brazil. (2006). Brazilian Policy for Hearing Health Care.<br />

Brazil: Ministry of Health.<br />

The World Bank. (2006). World Bank list of economies (July 2006). Retrieved<br />

February 13, 2007, from http://siteresources.worldbank.org/<br />

DATASTATISTICS/Resources/CLASS.XLS.<br />

Thompson, D.C., McPhilips, H., Davis, R.S., Lieu, T.A., Homer, C.J., Helfand,<br />

M. (2001). Universal Newborn Hearing Screening: Summary of Evidence.<br />

JAMA 286, 2000-2010.<br />

United Nations. (2005). The millennium development goals report 2005. New<br />

York.<br />

United Nations Children’s Funds (UNICEF). (2005). The state of the world’s<br />

children 2006. New York.<br />

United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO).<br />

(2006). Strong foundations: early childhood care and education. Paris.<br />

Uus, K., & Bamford, J. (2006). Effectiveness of population-based newborn<br />

hearing screening in England: ages of interventions and profile of cases.<br />

Pediatrics, 117, e887-e893.<br />

Uus, K., Bamford, J., Young, A., & McCracken, W. (2005). Readiness of pediatric<br />

audiology services for newborn hearing screening: Findings and implications<br />

from the programme in England. International Journal of<br />

Audiology, 44, 712-720.<br />

Vohr, B.R., Oh, W., Stewart, E.J., Bentkover, J.D., Gabbard, S., Lemons, J., et<br />

al., (2001a). Comparison of costs and referral rates of 3 universal newborn<br />

hearing screening protocols. Journal of Pediatrics, 139, 238-244.<br />

Vohr, B.R., Letourneau, K.S., & McDermott, C. (2001b). Maternal worry about<br />

neonatal hearing screening. Journal of Perinatology, 21, 15-20.<br />

Wald, N.J. (2006). Guidance on terminology. Journal of Medical Screening, 13,<br />

53.<br />

416 Olusanya


Watkin, P.M., Baldwin, M., & McEnery, G. (1991). Neonatal at risk screening<br />

and the identification of deafness. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 66, 1130-<br />

1135.<br />

Watkin, P.M., Baldwin, M., Dixon, R., & Beckman, A. (1998). Maternal anxiety<br />

and attitudes to universal neonatal hearing screening. British Journal of<br />

Audiology, 32, 27-37.<br />

Weichbold, V., & Welzl-Mueller, K. (2001). Maternal concern about positive<br />

test results in universal newborn hearing screening. Pediatrics, 108, 1111-<br />

1116.<br />

Weichbold, V., Nekahm-Heis, D., & Welzl-Mueller, K. (2006). Ten-year outcome<br />

of newborn hearing screening in Austria. International Journal of Pediatric<br />

Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 235-240.<br />

White, K.R. (2003). The current status of EHDI programs in the United States.<br />

Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9, 79-88.<br />

White, K.R. (2004). Early hearing detection and intervention programs: Opportunities<br />

for genetic services. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 130A,<br />

29-36.<br />

White, K.R. (2006). Reply to Olusanya et al.: The role of the JCIH in the global<br />

expansion of newborn hearing screening. Journal of American Academy of<br />

Audiology, 17, 295-296.<br />

Wirz, S.L., & Lichtig, I. (1998). The use of non-specialist personnel in providing<br />

a service for children disabled by hearing impairment. Disability and<br />

Rehabilitation, 20(5), 189-194.<br />

World Health Organization. (1995, May 12). Prevention of hearing impairment<br />

(World Health Assembly Resolution 48.9). Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (1999). Ear and Hearing Disorders Survey: Protocol<br />

(WHO/PBD/PDH/99.8). Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2001). International classification of functioning,<br />

disability and health. Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2004). Guidelines for hearing aids and services for<br />

developing countries. Second edition. Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2002). State of the world’s vaccines and immunizations.<br />

Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2005). Disability, including prevention, management<br />

and rehabilitation (World Health Assembly Resolution 58.23). Geneva,<br />

Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2006a). Primary care and training resource: Advanced<br />

level. Geneva, Switzerland.<br />

World Health Organization. (2006b). Factsheet: Skilled attendant at birth 2006<br />

updates. Geneva, Switzerland. Department of Reproductive Health and<br />

Research.<br />

Xu, A., Zhang, C., & Du, Z. (2006). False-positives result in newborn hearing<br />

screening [Abstract]. In F. Grandori & D. Hayes (Eds.). NHS2006 Conference:<br />

EHDI in Developing Countries 417


Beyond newborn hearing screening: infant and childhood hearing in science and<br />

clinical practice (pp 122). Como Lake, Italy: NHS2006.<br />

Yee-Arellano, H.M., Leal-Garza, F., & Pauli-Muller, K. (2006). Universal newborn<br />

hearing screening in Mexico: Results of the first 2 years. International<br />

Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 70, 1863-1870.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2004). Levels of evidence: Universal newborn hearing<br />

screening (UNHS) and early hearing detection and intervention systems<br />

(EHDI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 451-465.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A.L., Coulter, D.K., & Mehl, A.L. (1998). Language<br />

of early and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics,<br />

102, 1161-1171.<br />

Young, A., & Tattersall, H. (2007). Universal newborn hearing screening and<br />

early identification of deafness: parents’ responses to knowing early and<br />

their expectations of child communication development. Journal of Deaf<br />

Studies and Deaf Education, Feb 2. [Epub ahead of print].<br />

Young, M.E. (ed). (2002). From early child development to human development:<br />

investing in our children’s future. The World Bank. Washington, DC.<br />

Zeng, F. (1995). Cochlear implants in China. Audiology, 34, 61-75.<br />

418 Olusanya


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 419-431<br />

Beyond Early Intervention:<br />

Providing Support to Public<br />

School Personnel<br />

Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT ®<br />

At age 3, children with hearing loss transition from Part C early intervention to<br />

Part B public school services. These children represent a heterogeneous population<br />

when considering factors such as communication approaches; speech, language, auditory<br />

and cognitive skills; social-emotional and motor development; parental involvement;<br />

hearing technology; ethnicity and additional challenges such as learning<br />

disabilities. School personnel must have the necessary knowledge, skills and abilities<br />

to serve this diverse population. This article describes the North Carolina Resource<br />

Support Program (RSP), an outreach program designed to address the needs of public<br />

school personnel serving students who are deaf or hard of hearing ages 3-21. Program<br />

components including staff, services and future directions are discussed.<br />

Introduction<br />

Amazing strides have been made to screen newborns for the presence of<br />

hearing loss and to provide early intervention to infants and toddlers who are<br />

deaf or hard of hearing in the 21st century. The benefits of early identification<br />

and early intervention are well documented. For children identified in the<br />

first months of life, fit with appropriate amplification and enrolled in early<br />

intervention in a timely manner, research suggests that speech, language and<br />

vocabulary outcomes as well as social-emotional development are significantly<br />

better than for children who are identified later (Yoshinaga-Itano, 1995,<br />

2004; Yoshinaga-Itano & Sedey, 2000; Yoshinaga-Itano, Sedey, Coulter, &<br />

Mehl, 1998). The 1-3-6 model for screening, diagnosis, amplification and enrollment<br />

in intervention ensures that more children transition from early<br />

intervention to public school services with age-appropriate speech, language,<br />

vocabulary and social-emotional skills. In this new era of early identification<br />

and intervention, children can be fit with hearing aids in the first months of<br />

Kathryn Wilson, M.A., CCC-SLP, Cert. AVT ® , is director of the Resource Support Program<br />

in the Office of Education Services for the North Carolina Department of Health and Human<br />

Services and an adjunct professor at the University of North Carolina—Chapel Hill.<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 419


life. A growing number of children also are receiving cochlear implants during<br />

the birth-to-three years. With increased access to information, parents<br />

may explore a variety of communication approaches and choose from an<br />

array of options.<br />

In the United States, a transition from early intervention services to preschool<br />

services occurs at age 3. School personnel are faced with the challenge<br />

of meeting the needs of a heterogeneous group of children who are deaf or<br />

hard of hearing as well as those who have additional learning difficulties and<br />

are from ethnically diverse backgrounds. What are successful methods for<br />

training and supporting school personnel in their endeavors to implement<br />

effective programming and practices in public schools? This article describes<br />

the North Carolina Resource Support Program (RSP), an outreach program<br />

that addresses the needs of public school professionals who serve children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing at transition time and beyond.<br />

Background<br />

Although it is increasingly possible for children who are identified early to<br />

develop communication and language skills in close parallel to their hearing<br />

peers as a result of improved screening procedures and appropriate amplification<br />

technology and intervention programs, most of these children and<br />

their families require ongoing intervention, support and guidance during the<br />

preschool years. For example, North Carolina has been screening newborns<br />

for hearing loss for almost seven years and has a comprehensive early intervention<br />

program for children ages birth to 3. During fiscal year 2005-2006,<br />

77% of three year olds transitioning from early intervention to public school<br />

preschool services qualified for ongoing special education services and had<br />

an Individualized Education Program (IEP) in place at the time of transition<br />

(J. Dunn, personal communication, August 21, 2006; R.A. Everett & L. Kendall,<br />

personal communication, August 14, 2006).<br />

Given the robust relationship between oral language and success in reading<br />

(Katz, Shankweiler, & Liberman, 1981; Mann, Shankweiler, & Smith, 1984;<br />

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2005; Shankweiler,<br />

Liberman, Mark, Fowler, & Fischer, 1979; Snow, Tabors, Nicholson, &<br />

Kurland, 1994; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002) and the formal reading process<br />

that now typically begins near the end of the kindergarten year rather than in<br />

first grade, it is vital that children acquire the requisite language, phonological<br />

awareness and vocabulary skills before entering kindergarten in order to<br />

prepare adequately for the learning-to-read process. Variables such as the age<br />

of identification, appropriate amplification and early intervention determine<br />

linguistic outcomes and school success for children who are deaf or hard of<br />

hearing. In addition, the skills and training of the child and family’s service<br />

providers are regarded as key factors that affect the child’s progress. Kozleski,<br />

Mainzer and Deschler (as cited in Luckner, 2003) stated, “Whether in special<br />

420 Wilson


education or general education, there is growing evidence that the single<br />

most important influence in a student’s education is a well-prepared, caring<br />

and qualified teacher” (p. 5). A knowledgeable and well-trained teacher is<br />

essential to providing the necessary support and guidance to children and<br />

their families during early intervention and following transition from early<br />

intervention to public preschool services. This is true for children who transition<br />

from early intervention with age-appropriate or near age-appropriate<br />

language and vocabulary skills to ensure that they receive the ongoing support<br />

necessary to maintain language and vocabulary levels similar to those of<br />

their peers who are typically developing and to those children who demonstrate<br />

significant delays in language and vocabulary. Children in this second<br />

category are particularly at risk for problems related to success in reading and<br />

subsequent academic failure (Ling, 1989; Robertson, 2000). Because of the<br />

potentially significant education delays the children in this group demonstrate,<br />

the need for highly qualified professionals is even more substantial.<br />

That all children are deserving of qualified educators and related service<br />

personnel is a widely accepted view. The challenge is the shortage of appropriately<br />

trained personnel to meet the needs of the new generation of children<br />

with hearing loss identified at or near birth who transition from Part C early<br />

intervention services to public school Part B services at age 3. At the preservice<br />

level, only eight out of 70 colleges and universities with deaf education<br />

programs in the United States offer specialized training in auditory/oral<br />

education (Houston & Harrison, 2006). Because most college and university<br />

deaf education programs don’t focus on developing the specialized skills<br />

professionals need to work with children learning spoken language, teachers<br />

of the deaf, speech-language pathologists and audiologists already working<br />

in the field often pursue continuing education opportunities to develop these<br />

skills. Professionals can take advantage of online training programs such as<br />

FIRST YEARS, workshops and seminars provided by hearing aid and cochlear<br />

implant manufacturers, certificate programs, CEU opportunities offered<br />

through professional organizations and mentoring programs. The<br />

remainder of this article discusses detailed information about the North Carolina<br />

RSP, a state-funded and -operated program that provides training and<br />

support to public school personnel who serve students who are deaf or hard<br />

of hearing ages 3 to 21.<br />

History<br />

The Office of Education Services RSP<br />

In October 1999, North Carolina mandated newborn hearing screening in<br />

the state’s birthing hospitals. Historically, most children were not identified<br />

and enrolled in early intervention until age 2. Before 2000, children who were<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 421


identified as deaf or hard of hearing received early intervention and preschool<br />

services until age 5 and, in some cases, until age 6 through the Department<br />

of Health and Human Services (DHHS). The DHHS served children<br />

until kindergarten entry, although federal and state policy stated that public<br />

school programs—Part B—were responsible for children age 3 and older.<br />

Beginning with the 2001-2002 school year, North Carolina public schools<br />

began assuming responsibility for serving children upon their third birthday.<br />

With this transfer, new service delivery challenges for children who are deaf<br />

or hard of hearing were presented to administrators of public school services<br />

at the state and local levels.<br />

Perhaps the greatest challenge for public school systems is the lack of<br />

appropriately trained educators and speech-language pathologists to meet<br />

the needs of the new generation of infants and toddlers who are deaf and<br />

hard of hearing. Beginning in 1999-2000, at the same time that North Carolina<br />

implemented universal newborn hearing screening, the state began to experience<br />

a paradigm shift in the communication options parents were choosing.<br />

Presently, the combination of universal newborn hearing screening and the<br />

provision of information about all communication options through organizations<br />

such as BEGINNINGS for Parents of Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of<br />

Hearing, Inc., has resulted in the majority of parents choosing a spoken language<br />

approach (BEGINNINGS, 2005) (see the article by Alberg, Roush and<br />

Wilson, this issue, page 259). Data gathered by the Office of Education Services<br />

indicate that for fiscal year 1996-1997, 60% of families chose a sign<br />

language option, and the remaining 40% chose either the Cued Speech, Auditory-Verbal<br />

or auditory/oral options. A review of statistics from the last<br />

quarter of fiscal year 2005-2006 showed that 65% of families in the statewide<br />

birth-to-three program chose either the Auditory-Verbal or auditory/oral<br />

approach, 15% chose Total Communication, 6% identified the “other” category<br />

and 14% were “undecided.” Although the majority of the 115 North<br />

Carolina public school systems employed one or more teachers of children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing and all employed speech-language pathologists,<br />

only a limited number of systems had professionals with specialized<br />

training in working with children with cochlear implants and whose primary<br />

mode of communication was spoken language.<br />

An additional challenge to school systems was serving preschool-age children.<br />

Historically, schools did not begin to provide services to children who<br />

are deaf or hard of hearing until entry into kindergarten. Although the RSP<br />

was established to provide support to public schools serving preschool children,<br />

it also began receiving, and has continued to receive, requests to provide<br />

support for older students. Typical cases include students who were<br />

identified late, implanted late or transitioned from one communication approach<br />

to another; students for whom English is not the primary language<br />

spoken in the home; and students for whom there are concerns regarding<br />

reading comprehension and achievement. To provide the essential support<br />

422 Wilson


for both developmental and remedial learners, the need for a well-trained,<br />

experienced and highly qualified team was identified as a fundamental component<br />

of adequate service delivery.<br />

Staff<br />

The RSP began with two full-time positions and has expanded to include<br />

three full-time and two part-time staff. Program staff are licensed as teachers<br />

of the deaf, speech-language pathologists and audiologists and have specific<br />

expertise and experience in a variety of communication approaches. The team<br />

comprises Certified Auditory-Verbal Therapists ® , individuals fluent in the<br />

Cued Speech approach and skilled signers. Members of the team are either<br />

licensed, certified or demonstrate expertise in more than one communication<br />

approach or area of licensure. Two members of the team are National Board<br />

Certified teachers in special education. These same two staff members have<br />

obtained the Highly Qualified status in keeping with requirements of the No<br />

Child Left Behind Act. Additionally, team members have work experience in a<br />

variety of settings, including public schools, residential schools, early intervention<br />

home-based services, private practice and a hospital-based cochlear<br />

implant program. The average number of years of experience for team members<br />

is 27, with a range of 20 to 35 years. In an effort to maximize resources<br />

already in place, the RSP was established and staffed with existing funding<br />

and vacant positions allocated for one of North Carolina’s schools for the<br />

deaf. No new funding or allocations were required to implement this muchneeded<br />

outreach component.<br />

As described, members of the RSP team have many years of experience in<br />

all communication approaches and with a variety of age groups. The depth<br />

and breadth of staff expertise and specific knowledge allow the program<br />

director to triage each request and assign the most appropriate person or<br />

persons to a case. In addition to staff expertise and experience, other factors<br />

that seem to promote success and changes in teaching practices among professionals<br />

served by the RSP include the following.<br />

All team members have worked in public schools, which increases the<br />

RSP staff’s credibility. As one staff member stated, “They know we have<br />

walked in their shoes.”<br />

Team members understand the high cost of educating children with<br />

special needs and the financial constraints most schools face. Staff members<br />

are skilled in helping public school personnel to identify and maximize<br />

existing resources.<br />

Staff members are able to provide ongoing and continuous support.<br />

Responses to referrals and requests for support are made in a timely<br />

manner (i.e., one to two business days).<br />

RSP staff can develop a clear picture of what is needed by helping<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 423


Services<br />

professionals to develop a plan of action and then to “work their plan.”<br />

The results increase staff members’ motivation.<br />

RSP staff members are willing to work side by side with professionals<br />

not just by telling them what to do, but also by showing them how to<br />

do it.<br />

Emphasis is placed on self-evaluation. School professionals are encouraged<br />

to self-evaluate the effectiveness of their practices and programs<br />

before staff make suggestions and recommendations.<br />

A variety of services are offered to professionals in public schools, including<br />

consultation, observations, evaluations, IEP development, staff development<br />

and mentoring. Although the RSP was established primarily to provide<br />

support to professionals, parents also may request support. In either case (i.e.,<br />

parent or professional request for support), services only are provided at the<br />

invitation of the school system’s exceptional children’s director. RSP staff<br />

strive to work in concert with both school system personnel and families<br />

rather than in the role of advocate for either entity.<br />

Consultation. Consultative services are provided in person or through phone,<br />

fax or e-mail. An onsite consultation usually is provided in conjunction with<br />

an observation of a student in the school setting.<br />

Observations. For students in both special and regular education settings, observations<br />

can be requested for several different purposes, including assessment<br />

of the acoustic environment; level of student participation and ability to<br />

process information in a group setting; appraisal of the placement; use of<br />

appropriate modifications, accommodations, strategies and techniques; and<br />

delivery of services in accordance with the IEP. In some situations, checklists,<br />

such as those available from Children’s Hospital Boston, are used to assist the<br />

observer in assessing a student’s potential to transition from sign language to<br />

a spoken language approach or to a mainstream setting. Typically, a meeting<br />

is held immediately after an observation in order to provide an overview of<br />

findings, respond to questions from school personnel and develop an action<br />

plan if needed. The RSP staff member sends a written report detailing the<br />

observation and suggestions to the school system and parents after the onsite<br />

observation and consultation. Table 1 depicts the type and number of consultative<br />

services the RSP team delivered during the period of July 1, 2005, to<br />

June 31, 2006. For the same period, 21 onsite observations were conducted for<br />

students age 3 to 5, and 23 onsite observations were conducted for students<br />

age 5 to 21.<br />

Evaluations. These are conducted in the areas of speech, receptive and expressive<br />

language, vocabulary, listening, speech perception and written language.<br />

424 Wilson


Table 1. Type and Number of RSP Consultative Services Delivered from July 1,<br />

2005, to June 31, 2006<br />

Consultative Service Phone Fax E-Mail In Person<br />

Parents 126 0 69 114<br />

Professionals 306 13 939 439<br />

Although most school systems in North Carolina have the capacity to evaluate<br />

children with both formal and informal measures, the RSP receives requests<br />

for assistance in this area. A request for evaluation services usually<br />

occurs when a school system does not have a professional with the appropriate<br />

skills to assess the student in one or more areas or, in some situations,<br />

a second opinion is desired. In either case, the RSP team involves the professional(s)<br />

working with the student during the entire evaluation process in an<br />

effort to build capacity at the local level through onsite training in the administration,<br />

scoring and interpretation of a variety of assessments. School<br />

professionals are asked to exhaust all of their available resources for evaluations<br />

before seeking support from the outreach program. During fiscal year<br />

2005-2006, 130 evaluations were completed.<br />

IEP development. The Individuals with Disabilities Education Act requires that an<br />

IEP include the student’s present level of performance, proper goals and<br />

benchmarks and criterion for mastery, how and when progress will be reported<br />

to parents, related services, frequency and intensity of services and<br />

accommodations and modifications needed for a student to access the curriculum.<br />

For families going through the IEP process for the first time as the<br />

child transitions from early intervention, the process can be confusing and<br />

overwhelming. Early intervention programs emphasize family-centered practices.<br />

During the birth-to-three period, an Individualized Family Service Plan<br />

(IFSP) provides the framework for the services the child receives. The goals<br />

set forth in the IFSP primarily are family driven and reflect the parents’<br />

priorities. When a three-year-old child with hearing loss transitions, there is<br />

a shift from the family-centered practices endorsed by early intervention<br />

programs to a more formal and rule-driven process. Parents require information<br />

and guidance about the IEP process well in advance of the child’s<br />

third birthday in order to prepare adequately for transition from early intervention<br />

to public preschool services.<br />

North Carolina has developed procedures to enhance the transition process<br />

for children who are deaf or hard of hearing and their families. Once the child<br />

reaches age 2, the regional early intervention program director notifies, in<br />

writing, the appropriate Local Education Agency (LEA) regarding the child’s<br />

birth date, expected date of transition, communication approach and other<br />

relevant information to benefit the LEA in preparing for transition. Giving the<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 425


school system notification a full year in advance of the actual transition is<br />

recommended to grant the LEA time to identify appropriate personnel to<br />

meet the child’s unique communication needs. North Carolina guidelines also<br />

recommend a meeting with service providers, the infant-toddler service coordinator<br />

and the parents six months before transition. At this meeting parents<br />

are informed of all service options for the child, which may include visits<br />

to various sites and settings. A transition planning meeting is convened 90<br />

days before the child’s third birthday. Evaluations are reviewed, and the need<br />

for additional evaluations is determined. IEP development and possible<br />

placement options may be discussed. One month before the child’s third<br />

birthday the preschool IEP team convenes. The IEP document may be completed<br />

and signed at this time to become effective on the child’s third birthday.<br />

What roles, then, do RSP staff play during the development of the initial<br />

IEP? RSP staff attend very few IEP meetings; however, they often play a key<br />

role in assisting parents and professionals with the development of an appropriate<br />

IEP outside of the formal IEP meeting. This service is provided<br />

through the consultation model. Keeping in mind that the overarching goal<br />

for the RSP is to work effectively and collaboratively with parents and professionals,<br />

the team’s role is to provide information and support rather than<br />

to advocate for either the professionals or the parents. Parents who need an<br />

advocate present for IEP meetings are encouraged to access the services of the<br />

parent educators employed by BEGINNINGS.<br />

In addition to requests for support at transition, RSP staff also receive<br />

requests for IEP development for older students who demonstrate educationally<br />

significant language delays. In general, these students are more than two<br />

years delayed in their understanding and use of language. They often<br />

struggle in mainstream environments, and increased special education services,<br />

alternative testing procedures and non-diploma pathways are being<br />

considered. For a variety of reasons, these students have not met their IEP<br />

goals and objectives for one or more years and continue to fall farther behind<br />

academically. Using a model first introduced by Walker (2004) RSP staff assist<br />

school professionals and parents with developing a long-term, multiyear plan<br />

to close the existing language gap. Closing the language gap is one of the<br />

factors regarded as necessary for success in the mainstream. Once a student<br />

has attained language skills that are approximately age appropriate, he or she<br />

has greater potential to learn a variety of academic subjects and ultimately<br />

complete the requirements required for high school graduation. The longterm<br />

plan consists of specific information regarding projected rate of progress<br />

for each year of the plan in language and vocabulary. A long-term plan<br />

describes both the kinds of special education services and the amount of time<br />

needed daily or weekly to achieve the desired outcomes. Like an IEP, a<br />

long-term plan is developed with both parent and professional input. Unlike<br />

the IEP, a long-term plan defines parental roles and responsibilities in the<br />

426 Wilson


process of closing the language gap in terms of participation in weekly parent<br />

sessions and amount of recommended carryover time outside of the school<br />

environment. All components needed to achieve the target of age-appropriate<br />

language skills are identified and discussed. Assisting local school professionals<br />

and parents of remedial students in the development and implementation<br />

of long-term plans is regarded as an extremely valuable component<br />

offered by the RSP. The expectation is that developing and implementing this<br />

concept will increase the number of students who are deaf and hard of hearing<br />

in the state who will be able to read and write on a par with their hearing<br />

peers upon graduation from high school.<br />

Staff development. Regularly providing a variety of workshop training opportunities<br />

to public school personnel is a primary objective of the RSP. To meet<br />

this objective, the RSP contracts with the Auditory Learning Center, a private,<br />

nonprofit agency in Raleigh funded by the General Assembly of North Carolina.<br />

The RSP director and the executive director of the Auditory Learning<br />

Center meet at least two times a year to discuss training needs identified by<br />

public school personnel and to plan the training calendar. Modules from the<br />

Auditory-Verbal International, Inc., Standardized Curriculum (2000-2003) are<br />

presented regularly, with topics such as hearing and audiology, cochlear<br />

implants, spoken language, parent guidance, development of auditory function,<br />

Auditory-Verbal strategies and techniques and mainstreaming. In addition<br />

to the modules, the Auditory Learning Center presents seven days of<br />

training throughout the course of a school year on the Bloom & Lahey (1978)<br />

model of normal language development. Although the modules are presented<br />

“live,” some can be accessed through satellite set-ups at remote locations,<br />

making training more affordable and accessible for professionals who<br />

might otherwise have to travel long distances. In addition to the annual<br />

calendar for training, the RSP team responds to requests from individual<br />

school systems. The team provides workshops at the invitation of the exceptional<br />

children’s director to address professionals’ training needs at the local<br />

level. There is no charge to public schools for workshops presented by the<br />

RSP, and school systems are encouraged to invite and include personnel from<br />

neighboring districts so that as many professionals as possible can benefit<br />

from the workshops. During fiscal year 2005-2006, 141 training sessions or<br />

workshops were conducted for 883 education professionals and 68 parents.<br />

Mentoring. Workshops are one way of meeting staff development needs; however,<br />

this format cannot meet the needs of all professionals. The RSP staff<br />

recognized the need to develop multiple strategies for personnel development<br />

of adult learners who exhibit a variety of learning styles. Some professionals<br />

find it challenging to apply information presented in workshops or<br />

in-service trainings to their everyday “real-world” school setting. According<br />

to the Westchester Institute for Human Services Research (1998), effective<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 427


training programs emphasize feedback and follow-up. Therefore, in addition<br />

to the series of workshops for public school personnel, a mentoring component<br />

has evolved to provide regular, ongoing, onsite support to assist professionals<br />

in applying workshop content to practice.<br />

Gallacher (1997) described the stages of mentoring relationships, characteristics<br />

of effective mentors, the components of a mentoring program and the<br />

challenges associated with the mentoring process. When a request for mentoring<br />

is received, it is essential that RSP staff first have a clear understanding<br />

of the mentee’s reasons for the request. Is the request based on the needs and<br />

interests of the professional, or is the professional being encouraged to have<br />

a mentor by administrators or perhaps a student’s family? Once the purpose<br />

and goals are established, a plan is developed to define how often and how<br />

long mentoring activities will occur and the specific types of mentoring activities.<br />

The mentor and mentee discuss how feedback will be given both<br />

during and following a session. Because the majority of mentoring activities<br />

that RSP staff provide occur on site rather than by video review, travel time<br />

in addition to the time necessary for the actual visit; review of the lesson plan<br />

before the visit; report writing; and communication through phone, fax and<br />

e-mail all must be taken into consideration when developing a plan. Mentoring<br />

is indeed a time-intensive process for both mentors and mentees, and both<br />

parties must take steps to ensure that all necessary resources are available and<br />

in place. The effectiveness of the mentoring relationship also is examined<br />

regularly. Clearly, this type of personnel development requires planning,<br />

strong organizational skills, openness to feedback, positive attitudes, selfevaluation<br />

skills and sustained commitment to achieve long-term goals.<br />

Future Directions<br />

The RSP has experienced an increase in the number of services requested<br />

and delivered each year since its inception. It is expected that this trend will<br />

continue at least in the foreseeable future as more parents and professionals<br />

become aware of the outreach services available to them; as more children<br />

enter public schools with varying needs; and as the current workforce pursues<br />

the knowledge, skills and abilities to address the needs of a new generation<br />

of students who are deaf or hard of hearing. The RSP will continue to<br />

provide observations, consultations, evaluations and support for IEP and<br />

long-term plan development. The team also will support school systems that<br />

decide to establish classroom programs for students who are deaf and hard of<br />

hearing when needs cannot be met in regular or resource settings. In addition<br />

to the workshops currently offered, the team is working in partnership with<br />

the Auditory Learning Center and BEGINNINGS to develop more workshops,<br />

including a newly developed workshop that will address variables<br />

that influence successful outcomes in reading and literacy; developmental<br />

sequences and intervention strategies for reading aloud, oral narration and<br />

428 Wilson


phonologic and phonemic awareness; and a variety of application activities.<br />

Other topics school system personnel have requested include information<br />

and intervention strategies for working with students who have additional<br />

learning difficulties and providing services to children from bilingual and<br />

non-English-speaking homes. It is expected that the mentoring component<br />

will continue to develop given the growing body of evidence to support the<br />

effectiveness of this type of personnel development. Finally, the RSP staff<br />

envision establishing a Web site that will allow North Carolina professionals<br />

to communicate and network with one another regarding ideas, concerns and<br />

information related to improving the education of students who are deaf and<br />

hard of hearing.<br />

Conclusion<br />

Many positive developments have taken place in recent years for children<br />

who are deaf or hard of hearing, their parents and the professionals who<br />

serve them. Unquestionably, the movement toward early identification increases<br />

potential for communicative competence, success in school and independence.<br />

Many variables contribute to successful outcomes. Chief among these are<br />

the skills and expertise of the professionals who guide the child and family<br />

through early intervention, the preschool years and beyond. Evidence suggests<br />

that preservice programs specializing in auditory/oral and Auditory-<br />

Verbal practices are limited. A variety of in-service programs are available,<br />

although many require professionals to travel and training-related expenses<br />

can be prohibitive.<br />

In response to changes in early identification and the role of public schools<br />

in providing services to preschool children who are deaf or hard of hearing,<br />

the Office of Education Services established the RSP to assist in meeting the<br />

needs of public school personnel. A variety of services and activities designed<br />

to build capacity at the local level are provided by a well-trained and highly<br />

qualified staff at no cost to the public schools. Now in its fourth year of<br />

operation, the RSP continues to expand in terms of number of requests and<br />

services provided. Being able to provide onsite support is one of the major<br />

advantages identified by both RSP staff and recipients of the program’s services.<br />

The challenges encountered in North Carolina are likely to be experienced<br />

in other states, and the RSP can be an effective and replicable model for<br />

providing outreach to school personnel.<br />

References<br />

Auditory-Verbal International, Inc. (2000). Standardized curriculum. Alexandria,<br />

VA: Author.<br />

BEGINNINGS for Parents of Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing, Inc.<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 429


(2005).Communication options. Retrieved August 22, 2006, from http://<br />

www.beginnings.org.<br />

Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders.<br />

New York: Wiley.<br />

Children’s Hospital Boston (2003). Children with cochlear implants who sign:<br />

Guidelines for transitioning to oral education or a mainstream setting. Retrieved<br />

August 23, 2006, from http://www.childrenshospital.org<br />

Gallacher, K. (1997). Supervising, mentoring, and coaching: Methods for supporting<br />

personnel development. In P.J. Winton (Ed.), Reforming personnel<br />

preparation in early intervention: Issues, models, and practical strategies (pp.<br />

191-214). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Houston, K.T., and Harrison, J. (2006). Meeting the needs of children with hearing<br />

loss in the 21st century: The quest for spoken language. PowerPoint presentation<br />

at the Association of College Educators of the Deaf and Hard of Hearing<br />

2006 Conference, Denver, CO.<br />

Katz, R.B., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I.Y. (1981). Memory for item order<br />

and phonetic recoding in the beginning reader. Journal of Experimental Child<br />

Psychology, 32, 474-484.<br />

Ling, D. (1989). Foundations of spoken language for hearing-impaired children.<br />

Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf and<br />

Hard of Hearing.<br />

Luckner, J.L. (2003). Job satisfaction: Perceptions of a national sample of<br />

teachers of students who are deaf or hard of hearing. American Annals of the<br />

Deaf, 148(1), 5-17.<br />

Mann, V.A., Shankweiler, D., & Smith, S. (1984). The association between<br />

comprehension of spoken sentences and early reading ability: The role of<br />

phonetic representation. Journal of Child Language, 2, 627-643.<br />

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care<br />

Research Network. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language<br />

in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 428-442.<br />

Robertson, L. (2000). Literacy learning for children who are deaf or hard of hearing.<br />

Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf and<br />

Hard of Hearing.<br />

Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I.Y., Mark, L.S., Fowler, C.A., & Fischer, F.W.<br />

(1979). The speech code and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Psychology:<br />

Human Learning and Memory, 5, 531-545.<br />

Snow, C.E., Tabors, P.O., Nicholson, P.A., & Kurland, B.F. (1994). SHELL:<br />

Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first-grade children.<br />

Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10(1), 37-48.<br />

Storch, S.A., & Whitehurst, G.J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors<br />

to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 38, 934-947.<br />

Walker, B. (2004). Mainstream module. Presented at the University of North<br />

Carolina–Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, N.C.<br />

430 Wilson


Westchester Institute for Human Services Research. (1998). The balanced view:<br />

Professional development. Retrieved August 16, 2006, from http://www<br />

.sharingsuccess.org.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (1995). Efficacy of early identification and intervention.<br />

Seminars in Hearing, 16, 115-120.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2004). Levels of evidence: universal newborn hearing<br />

screening (UNHS) and early hearing detection and intervention systems<br />

(EHDI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 451-465.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., & Sedey, A.L. (2000). Early speech development in children<br />

who are deaf or hard-of-hearing: Interrelationships with language and<br />

hearing. In C. Yoshinaga-Itano & A.L. Sedey (Eds.), Language, speech, and<br />

social-emotional development of children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing: The<br />

early years. The Volta Review, 100, 181-211.<br />

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A.L., Coulter, D.K., & Mehl, A.L. (1998). Language<br />

of early and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics;<br />

102, 1161-1171.<br />

Beyond Early Intervention 431


The Volta Review, Volume <strong>106</strong>(3) (monograph), 433-434<br />

Author Index for Volume <strong>106</strong><br />

<strong>106</strong>(1)<br />

Charlesworth, Ann; Charlesworth, Robert; Raban, Bridie; and Rickards, Field.<br />

(2006). Reading Recovery for Children with Hearing Loss. The Volta Review<br />

<strong>106</strong>(1), pp. 29-51.<br />

Eriks-Brophy, Alice; Durieux-Smith, Andrée; Olds, Janet; Fitzpatrick, Elizabeth;<br />

Duquette, Cheryll; and Whittingham, JoAnne. (2006). Facilitators and<br />

Barriers to the Inclusion of Orally Educated Children and Youth with Hearing<br />

Loss in Schools: Promoting Partnerships to Support Inclusion. The Volta<br />

Review <strong>106</strong>(1), pp. 53-88.<br />

Most, Tova; Aram, Dorit; and Andorn, Tamar. (2006). Early Literacy in Children<br />

with Hearing Loss: A Comparison Between Two Educational Systems.<br />

The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(1), pp. 5-28.<br />

Verté, Sylvie; Hebbrecht, Lies; and Roeyers, Herbert. (2006). Psychological<br />

Adjustment of Siblings of Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. The<br />

Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(1), pp. 89-110.<br />

<strong>106</strong>(2)<br />

Dillon, Caitlin M.; and Pisoni, David B. (2006). Nonword Repetition and<br />

Reading Skills in Children Who Are Deaf and Have Cochlear Implants. The<br />

Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(2), pp. 121-145.<br />

Neuss, Deirdre. (2006). The Ecological Transition to Auditory-Verbal<br />

Therapy: Experiences of Parents Whose Children Use Cochlear Implants.<br />

The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(2), pp. 195-222.<br />

Reed, Charlotte M.; and Delhorne, Lorraine A. (2006). A Study of the Combined<br />

Use of a Hearing Aid and Tactual Aid in an Adult with Profound<br />

Hearing Loss. The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(2), pp. 171-193.<br />

Robertson, Lyn; Dow, Gina Annunziato; and Hainzinger, Sarah Lynn. (2006).<br />

Story Retelling Patterns Among Children with and Without Hearing Loss:<br />

Effects of Repeated Practice and Parent-Child Attunement. The Volta Review<br />

<strong>106</strong>(2), pp. 147-170.<br />

<strong>106</strong>(3)<br />

Alberg, Joni; Wilson, Kathryn; and Roush, Jackson. (2007). Statewide Collaboration<br />

in the Delivery of EHDI Services. The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 259-<br />

274.<br />

Author Index 433


Bohnert, Andrea; Spitzlei, Vera; Lippert, Karl L.; and Keilmann, Annerose.<br />

(2007). Bilateral Cochlear Implantation in Children: Experiences and Considerations.<br />

The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 343-364.<br />

DesJardin, Jean L. (2007). Family Empowerment: Supporting Language Development<br />

in Young Children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. The Volta<br />

Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 275-298.<br />

McKay, Sarah. (2007). Management of Young Children with Unilateral Hearing<br />

Loss. The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 299-319.<br />

Moore, Jan Allison; Prath, Scott; and Arrieta, Adrianne. (2007). Early Spanish<br />

Speech Acquisition Following Cochlear Implantation. The Volta Review<br />

<strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 321-341.<br />

Olusanya, Bolajoko O. (2007). Early Hearing Detection and Intervention in<br />

Developing Countries: Current Status and Prospects. The Volta Review<br />

<strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 381-418.<br />

Schorr, Efrat A. (2007). Early Cochlear Implant Experience and Emotional<br />

Functioning During Childhood: Loneliness in Middle and Late Childhood.<br />

The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 365-379.<br />

White, Karl R. (2007). Early Intervention for Children with Permanent Hearing<br />

Loss: Finishing the EHDI Revolution. The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 237-<br />

258.<br />

Wilson, Kathryn. (2007). Beyond Early Intervention: Providing Support to<br />

Public School Personnel. The Volta Review <strong>106</strong>(3), pp. 419-431.<br />

434 Author Index

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!