21.05.2013 Views

Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What's ... - DPSS

Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What's ... - DPSS

Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What's ... - DPSS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g: What’s wrong?<br />

This is a prepr<strong>in</strong>t of an article accepted for publication <strong>in</strong> Aggressive Behavior<br />

copyright © 2005 Wiley-Liss Inc.<br />

Gianluca G<strong>in</strong>i<br />

Department of Developmental <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong>isation Psychology<br />

University of Padova<br />

via Venezia 8<br />

35131, Padova - Italy<br />

e-mail: gianluca.g<strong>in</strong>i@unipd.it<br />

Acknowledgements: The author is grateful to all schools, teachers <strong>and</strong> pupils who<br />

participated <strong>in</strong> the study. Moreover, I wish to thank Francesca Happè <strong>and</strong> Jon Sutton for<br />

their stories, translated <strong>and</strong> utilized <strong>in</strong> this study, <strong>and</strong> Elena Arimondo, Alessia<br />

Bortoloso, Giuseppe Carli <strong>and</strong> Simona Pischetola for their help <strong>in</strong> data collection.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, I am very grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on<br />

an earlier version of the manuscript.<br />

1


<strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g: What’s wrong?<br />

Abstract<br />

Two different models have been proposed that describe the bully alternatively as a<br />

child lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> social skills (Crick & Dodge, 1994), or as a cold manipulative<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual, who leads gangs to achieve personal goals (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham,<br />

1999a). The present study exam<strong>in</strong>ed the performance of 204 8-11-year-olds <strong>in</strong> a set of<br />

stories that assessed underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of <strong>cognition</strong>s <strong>and</strong> emotions, <strong>in</strong> relation to their<br />

Participant Role <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g. Moreover, children’s underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of <strong>moral</strong> emotions <strong>and</strong><br />

proneness to <strong>moral</strong> disengagement was assessed. Victims showed some difficulties <strong>in</strong><br />

the social <strong>cognition</strong> task, whereas bullies did not. Aggressive children, <strong>in</strong>stead, were<br />

found to be more ready to show <strong>moral</strong> disengagement mechanisms, whereas defenders<br />

showed higher levels of <strong>moral</strong> sensibility. Results are discussed <strong>in</strong> relation to the two<br />

models, <strong>and</strong> the need for further research <strong>in</strong>to empathy <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> of children<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g episodes is highlighted.<br />

2


<strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g: What’s wrong?<br />

Over the last twenty years, the literature about the development of aggressive<br />

behavior showed an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the study of the relations between <strong>cognition</strong>,<br />

social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> social <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents (e.g.,<br />

Crick & Dodge, 1994; Harvey, Fletcher, & French, 2001). In particular, several authors<br />

studied how the social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g strategies used by children <strong>and</strong> the way <strong>in</strong><br />

which they <strong>in</strong>terpret situational cues <strong>and</strong> use their previous experiences can <strong>in</strong>fluence<br />

their aggressive conduct (Dodge & Price, 1994; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997; R<strong>and</strong>all,<br />

1997). One of the most important results of this field of research has been the possibility<br />

to identify the presence of specific deficits <strong>and</strong> systematic biases <strong>in</strong> some components of<br />

the social <strong>cognition</strong> of aggressive children. These results led Dodge <strong>and</strong> colleagues to<br />

formulate the “<strong>Social</strong> Skills Deficit” model (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980).<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the first formulation of this model, <strong>in</strong> which reactive <strong>and</strong> proactive<br />

categories of aggression were not clearly dist<strong>in</strong>guished, aggressive children show some<br />

difficulties <strong>in</strong> one or more phases of their social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g. For example,<br />

these authors found that aggressive children tend to <strong>in</strong>terpret ambiguous situations <strong>in</strong> an<br />

aggressive way more than their non aggressive peers (hostile attributional bias) (Dodge<br />

& Frame, 1982; Dodge & Somberg, 1987). Another difference between aggressive <strong>and</strong><br />

non aggressive children refers to the selection of the behavioral response <strong>in</strong> a social<br />

situation: the former, <strong>in</strong> fact, seem to have a more limited range of non aggressive<br />

answers than the latter <strong>and</strong>, for this reason, they are more <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed to choose <strong>and</strong> perform<br />

aggressive behaviors, especially <strong>in</strong> the case of <strong>in</strong>terpersonal conflicts (Dodge, 1980;<br />

3


Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Dodge & Newman, 1981). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to the same<br />

approach, victims, who are shy <strong>and</strong> withdrawn <strong>and</strong> lack social skills of assertiveness,<br />

show high levels of anxiety <strong>and</strong> lonel<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>and</strong> have less experience <strong>in</strong> social <strong>in</strong>teraction<br />

<strong>and</strong> play fight<strong>in</strong>g (which may enhance social skills), as well as control <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation<br />

of emotional expression (Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997; Smith, Bowers, B<strong>in</strong>ney, &<br />

Cowie, 1993).<br />

Some authors tried to apply this model to a particular k<strong>in</strong>d of aggressive<br />

phenomenon: school bully<strong>in</strong>g (Boulton & Smith, 1994; Slee, 1993; Smith et al., 1993).<br />

However, despite the popular view of the bully as an “oaf” boy, that is an <strong>in</strong>tellectually<br />

simple or backward boy (Besag, 1989), very few studies confirmed that Dodge’s model<br />

can be applied to bullies <strong>in</strong> exactly the same way as to aggressive children (e.g., Hazler,<br />

1996; R<strong>and</strong>all, 1997). On the contrary, some other authors stressed the role of adaptive<br />

motivation to expla<strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior, argu<strong>in</strong>g that bully<strong>in</strong>g may be seen as an<br />

<strong>in</strong>appropriate way to reach a socially effective goal, such as leadership with<strong>in</strong> a group.<br />

As a consequence, these authors stated that the bully should not necessarily to be seen as<br />

an <strong>in</strong>dividual with specific difficulties. On the contrary, he or she may sometimes be a<br />

skilled <strong>in</strong>dividual who takes advantage of his/her high social-cognitive competence to<br />

reach personal benefits, such as <strong>in</strong>terpersonal dom<strong>in</strong>ance, through a “Machiavellian”<br />

conduct (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham, 1999a).<br />

Some data presented by Sutton <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Sutton, Smith, & Swettenham,<br />

1999b) empirically support this view; <strong>in</strong> fact, they found that 7-10-year-old bullies<br />

performed better than other classmates <strong>in</strong> a social <strong>cognition</strong> task that required the<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of mental states <strong>and</strong> emotions of the ma<strong>in</strong> character of a story (theory of<br />

4


m<strong>in</strong>d task). The relevance of these results, above all, lies <strong>in</strong> the fact that, <strong>in</strong> a bully<strong>in</strong>g<br />

situation, «hav<strong>in</strong>g a grasp of the mental states of those <strong>in</strong>volved, along with an ability to<br />

manipulate these thoughts <strong>and</strong> beliefs, may be crucial for the bully <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g such <strong>in</strong>ter-role relations» (Sutton et al., 1999b, p. 437). In previous studies,<br />

therefore, bullies’ social cognitive skills, such as social perspective tak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of emotional <strong>in</strong>formation, may have been underestimated (Sutton et al., 1999a, 1999b,<br />

1999c). However, some bullies may possess a different conception of the costs <strong>and</strong><br />

benefits of aggression, <strong>and</strong> they may lack <strong>in</strong> empathic underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the victim’s<br />

suffer<strong>in</strong>g. Therefore, it appears more adequate to regard social skills as a neutral tool,<br />

which may be used for both prosocial <strong>and</strong> antisocial purposes (Björkqvist, Österman, &<br />

Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, 2000; Hawley, 2003). From this perspective, the crucial po<strong>in</strong>t to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior refers to the k<strong>in</strong>d of values that guide bullies’ conduct, rather than<br />

their higher or lower ability <strong>in</strong> social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g (Arsenio & Lemerise,<br />

2001).<br />

Follow<strong>in</strong>g this l<strong>in</strong>e of research, the present paper focuses on cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

emotional underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of social situations <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g, us<strong>in</strong>g a theory of m<strong>in</strong>d task<br />

with children who assume different participant roles dur<strong>in</strong>g a bully<strong>in</strong>g episode. In the<br />

above mentioned study, however, Sutton <strong>and</strong> colleagues (1999b) did not explicitly<br />

consider a particular k<strong>in</strong>d of emotion found to be important to account for aggressive<br />

<strong>and</strong> antisocial behaviors: <strong>moral</strong> emotions (<strong>in</strong> particular, guilt <strong>and</strong> shame). If bullies are<br />

cold, manipulative experts <strong>in</strong> social contexts, their behavior may be related to emotions<br />

surround<strong>in</strong>g <strong>moral</strong> transgression. These emotions are important because, as Hoffman<br />

(1976, 2000) po<strong>in</strong>ted out, they are closely connected with empathy <strong>and</strong> they can be<br />

5


considered mediators of conscience. Other authors suggested that <strong>moral</strong> development<br />

plays a major role <strong>in</strong> behavioral regulation (Arsenio, 1988; Nucci, 2001; Smetana, 1995;<br />

Turiel, 1998) <strong>and</strong> that guilt is an <strong>in</strong>terpersonal phenomenon aimed at avoid<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

disruption of relationships <strong>and</strong> bonds (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994;<br />

Tagney, 1992). Moreover, <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are more prone to guilt are less aggressive<br />

<strong>and</strong> less likely to act out behaviors (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988; Quiles & Bybee,<br />

1997). In contrast, if children had lower feel<strong>in</strong>gs of guilt, this could “enable” them to act<br />

aggressively without feel<strong>in</strong>g guilty. However, the reverse causal l<strong>in</strong>k could be true, <strong>in</strong><br />

that children who repeatedly commit acts of aggression may become <strong>in</strong>ured to this type<br />

of behavior, exhibit<strong>in</strong>g dulled emotional responses.<br />

Moral disengagement <strong>and</strong> aggressive behavior<br />

A useful contribution to the analysis of some social-cognitive components of<br />

aggressive behavior is made by the <strong>Social</strong> Cognitive Theory of the Moral Self (B<strong>and</strong>ura,<br />

1986, 1990, 1991). Accord<strong>in</strong>g to this theory, <strong>moral</strong> reason<strong>in</strong>g is l<strong>in</strong>ked to <strong>moral</strong> action<br />

through affective self-regulatory mechanisms by which <strong>moral</strong> agency is exercised. In<br />

their development, children need to develop st<strong>and</strong>ards of right <strong>and</strong> wrong <strong>and</strong> to adopt<br />

these st<strong>and</strong>ards as guides <strong>and</strong> deterrents for their conduct. This self-regulatory process<br />

implies that <strong>in</strong>dividuals usually act <strong>in</strong> ways that give them satisfaction <strong>and</strong> a sense of<br />

self-worth, whereas, on the other side, they tend to avoid behaviors that violate their<br />

<strong>moral</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards <strong>in</strong> order to avoid self-condemnation.<br />

Moreover, B<strong>and</strong>ura (2002) claimed that the construct of <strong>moral</strong> disengagement<br />

could be considered as a mediator between the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s <strong>moral</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>and</strong> his/her<br />

6


eal conduct, which may not necessarily be coherent with those pr<strong>in</strong>ciples. Internal<br />

controls, <strong>in</strong> fact, are not fixed, <strong>and</strong> several psychological <strong>and</strong> social processes can<br />

contribute to their selective deactivation. In particular, B<strong>and</strong>ura described eight different<br />

mechanisms through which <strong>moral</strong> control can be disengaged. One set of disengagement<br />

practices operates on the reconstruction of the behavior itself. For example, an im<strong>moral</strong><br />

conduct is made personally <strong>and</strong> socially acceptable by portray<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong> the service of<br />

<strong>moral</strong> purposes (<strong>moral</strong> justification) or self-deplored acts can be made to appear<br />

righteous by contrast<strong>in</strong>g them with worse conducts (advantageous comparison).<br />

Moreover, <strong>in</strong> some circumstances, activities can take on different appearances<br />

depend<strong>in</strong>g on what they are called (euphemistic label<strong>in</strong>g). The second set of<br />

disengagement practices operates by obscur<strong>in</strong>g or m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g the agentive role <strong>in</strong> the<br />

harm one causes (displacement of responsibility <strong>and</strong> diffusion of responsibility). Other<br />

ways of weaken<strong>in</strong>g <strong>moral</strong> control operate by m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, disregard<strong>in</strong>g or distort<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

effects of one’s action; if m<strong>in</strong>imization does not work, the evidence of harm can be<br />

discredited (m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g or misconstru<strong>in</strong>g consequences). F<strong>in</strong>ally, disengagement<br />

practices may operate on the recipients of detrimental acts by stripp<strong>in</strong>g them of human<br />

qualities (dehumanization) or consider<strong>in</strong>g aggression as provoked by the victim<br />

(attribution of blame). These mechanisms can lead to aggressive behaviors through a<br />

process of <strong>moral</strong> disengagement, that is a partial gap between the “abstract” personal<br />

idea of <strong>moral</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavior <strong>in</strong> real life. In this way, the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dividual protects him/herself from negative feel<strong>in</strong>gs, such as guilt or shame, that<br />

usually follow an im<strong>moral</strong> conduct (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1991).<br />

7


Different studies showed a positive relation between aggressive behavior <strong>and</strong> the<br />

activation of one or more of these mechanisms <strong>in</strong> both adults <strong>and</strong> children (B<strong>and</strong>ura,<br />

Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996; Caprara, Barbaranelli, & B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1995;<br />

Caprara, Barbaranelli, Vic<strong>in</strong>o, & B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1996; Yadava, Sharma, & G<strong>and</strong>hi, 2001). In<br />

particular, Caprara <strong>and</strong> colleagues (1995) confirmed the strong l<strong>in</strong>k between <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement <strong>and</strong> physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression, both self- <strong>and</strong> peer-evaluated,<br />

especially <strong>in</strong> male children. More recently, B<strong>and</strong>ura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Regalia (2001) have replicated these results. They found a positive relation between<br />

<strong>moral</strong> disengagement <strong>and</strong> transgressive behavior, <strong>and</strong> a negative relation between <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement <strong>and</strong> self-regulatory efficacy <strong>and</strong> prosocial behavior.<br />

The above mentioned studies actually referred to aggressive <strong>in</strong>dividuals, but a<br />

similar pattern of data has been found <strong>in</strong> school bully<strong>in</strong>g research, as well. Bacch<strong>in</strong>i,<br />

Amodeo, Ciardi, Valerio, <strong>and</strong> Vitelli (1998) <strong>and</strong> Menes<strong>in</strong>i, Fonzi, <strong>and</strong> Vannucci (1997),<br />

for example, found that male bullies utilized the <strong>moral</strong> disengagement mechanisms more<br />

than did other peers <strong>and</strong>, most of all, they seemed to use the mechanisms of<br />

Dehumanization <strong>and</strong> Moral Justification. A confirmation of these results emerged <strong>in</strong> a<br />

cross-national study, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g Italy <strong>and</strong> Spa<strong>in</strong>, conduced by Menes<strong>in</strong>i <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(Menes<strong>in</strong>i, Codecasa, Benelli, & Cowie, 2003), who assessed <strong>moral</strong> reason<strong>in</strong>g of bullies,<br />

victims <strong>and</strong> children who assumed the role of the defender, us<strong>in</strong>g the Scan Bully<strong>in</strong>g test<br />

(Almeida, del Barrio, Marques, Gutierrez, & van der Meulen, 2001). These authors<br />

confirmed the tendency of bullies to show higher levels of <strong>moral</strong> disengagement, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

presence of a profile of egocentric reason<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> these pupils.<br />

8


Quite recently, the “traditional” classification of schoolchildren <strong>in</strong>to bullies,<br />

victims <strong>and</strong> controls has been criticized <strong>and</strong> several authors stressed the importance of<br />

consider<strong>in</strong>g bully<strong>in</strong>g a group process (Sutton & Smith, 1999). The empirical<br />

confirmation of the importance of peer ecology applied to bully<strong>in</strong>g phenomenon derives<br />

from several observational studies carried out dur<strong>in</strong>g periods of free <strong>in</strong>teraction among<br />

children <strong>in</strong> unstructured contexts, such as dur<strong>in</strong>g recess <strong>and</strong> outdoor play. These studies,<br />

<strong>in</strong> fact, consistently found that peers are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> 85% of bully<strong>in</strong>g episodes (Atlas &<br />

Pepler, 1998; Craig & Pepler, 1997; Craig, Pepler, & Atlas, 2000; Pepler & Craig,<br />

1995). Moreover, Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Björkqvist, Österman, <strong>and</strong> Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en (1996)<br />

identified six different participant roles taken by <strong>in</strong>dividual children <strong>in</strong> the bully<strong>in</strong>g<br />

process (i.e. victim, bully, re<strong>in</strong>forcer of the bully, assistant to the bully, defender of the<br />

victim, <strong>and</strong> outsider). Another study by Salmivalli, Huttunen, <strong>and</strong> Lagerspetz (1997) has<br />

also revealed a strong <strong>in</strong>fluence of how the members of a group behave <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g<br />

situations on an <strong>in</strong>dividual child’s behavior <strong>in</strong> such situations.<br />

In l<strong>in</strong>e with this approach that considers bully<strong>in</strong>g as a group phenomenon, all<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ct roles with<strong>in</strong> the class were considered <strong>in</strong> the present study. Not only bullies <strong>and</strong><br />

victims participated, but also those pupils who, <strong>in</strong> different ways, act important roles <strong>in</strong><br />

re<strong>in</strong>forc<strong>in</strong>g bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior (the so-called “Followers”: the Assistants of the bully <strong>and</strong><br />

the Re<strong>in</strong>forcers) or, <strong>in</strong> contrast, <strong>in</strong>tervene to defend the victim (the Defenders). F<strong>in</strong>ally,<br />

Outsiders, that is children not directly <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the phenomenon, were considered.<br />

9


Aims of the study <strong>and</strong> hypotheses<br />

The first aim of the present study was to assess the performance of these pupils <strong>in</strong><br />

a social-<strong>cognition</strong> task similar to the one used by Sutton et al. (1999b). In this task, the<br />

same two typologies of stories used by Sutton <strong>and</strong> colleagues were used: cognitive <strong>and</strong><br />

emotion stories. The cognitive stories, <strong>in</strong> particular, assessed the child’s ability to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> mental states, beliefs <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions of the character of the story. The<br />

emotion stories, <strong>in</strong>stead, assessed the ability to underst<strong>and</strong> emotional states of the<br />

character <strong>and</strong> the possibility that an emotion could be dissimulated to <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>and</strong><br />

modify others’ knowledge. Contrary to the idea of the bully as a social “oaf”, it is<br />

hypothesized that the Bully group will perform better than, or at least at the same level<br />

as, other pupils <strong>in</strong> a theory of m<strong>in</strong>d task that requires underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the thoughts, beliefs,<br />

<strong>in</strong>tentions that guide others’ behavior.<br />

Moreover, <strong>in</strong> addition to these two k<strong>in</strong>ds of stories, a third category (<strong>moral</strong><br />

stories), not used by Sutton et al. (1999b), was designed <strong>in</strong> order to explore the role of<br />

<strong>moral</strong> emotions <strong>in</strong> the context of aggressive conduct. The second aim of the present<br />

study, therefore, was to analyze the ability to underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> emotions (guilt <strong>and</strong><br />

shame) with<strong>in</strong> hypothetical social scenarios by children who assume different roles.<br />

Consider<strong>in</strong>g the literature about <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>in</strong> aggressive behavior (e.g., Hoffman,<br />

2000; Quiles & Bybee, 1997; Tagney, 1992), it was expected that Bullies would perform<br />

worse than non-aggressive mates <strong>in</strong> <strong>moral</strong> stories, that is be less likely to make the<br />

correct attribution of <strong>moral</strong> emotions to the character of the story ow<strong>in</strong>g to his/her<br />

negative behavior. In contrast, Defenders should be particularly sensitive to this k<strong>in</strong>d of<br />

10


emotion as an expression of what is right <strong>and</strong> what is wrong <strong>and</strong>, therefore, we expected<br />

this group of children to perform better than any other <strong>in</strong> this type of stories.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, the third aim of the study was to consider the relation between <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement <strong>and</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior (e.g., Caprara et al., 1995; Menes<strong>in</strong>i et al., 1999).<br />

The comparison among pupils who assume different roles may lead to a deeper<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the importance of these mechanisms <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g episodes. Specifically,<br />

we expected that the tendency to activate <strong>moral</strong> disengagement mechanisms would be<br />

higher <strong>in</strong> aggressive children (the Bullies <strong>and</strong> their Followers). In contrast, we<br />

hypothesized that Victims <strong>and</strong> those children who frequently <strong>in</strong>tervene to defend their<br />

victimized classmates (Defenders) would show a higher <strong>moral</strong> sensibility <strong>and</strong>,<br />

consequently, a lower level of <strong>moral</strong> disengagement.<br />

Participants<br />

Method<br />

The <strong>in</strong>itial sample comprised 581 Italian primary-school children (295 boys <strong>and</strong><br />

286 girls) aged between 8 <strong>and</strong> 11 years (mean age = 9;7 years, s.d. = 11 months).<br />

Although socio-economic status was not directly measured, our sample <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

students from a wide range of social backgrounds (low <strong>and</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g classes through<br />

upper middle class). In terms of racial/ethnic background, the sample was predom<strong>in</strong>antly<br />

Caucasian (96%), with a small proportion of Asian (2%) <strong>and</strong> North African (2%) orig<strong>in</strong>.<br />

All children received school <strong>and</strong> parental permission to participate prior to the collection<br />

of the data.<br />

11


On the basis of peer nom<strong>in</strong>ations, children were first assigned to one of the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g Role groups: Bully, Assistant, Re<strong>in</strong>forcer, Defender, Outsider <strong>and</strong> Victim,<br />

based on the procedure described below. Seventy-three children did not receive a<br />

specific role because they did not satisfy the classification criteria proposed by<br />

Salmivalli et al. (1996) <strong>and</strong> were not <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al sample. The distribution of<br />

Participant Roles for the whole sample is reported <strong>in</strong> Table I.<br />

As can be seen <strong>in</strong> Table I, cell sizes are rather unequal for both Participant Roles<br />

<strong>and</strong> gender. In order to h<strong>and</strong>le problems connected with strongly unbalanced factorial<br />

designs (Cramer, 1998), especially with small cell numbers, we decided to balance our<br />

Role groups for number <strong>and</strong> gender. Therefore, a total of 204 children, divided <strong>in</strong>to 6<br />

Role group (n=34; 17 males <strong>and</strong> 17 females for each one), were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected <strong>and</strong><br />

participated to the second phase of the study. The analyses presented <strong>in</strong> the result section<br />

have been conducted on this sample with equalized groups.<br />

Material<br />

Participant Role Questionnaire (PRQ)<br />

--- Insert Table I about here ---<br />

The Italian version (Menes<strong>in</strong>i & G<strong>in</strong>i, 2000) of the Participant Role Questionnaire<br />

– PRQ (Salmivalli et al., 1996; Sutton & Smith, 1999) was used to collect peer<br />

nom<strong>in</strong>ations. Children were asked to nom<strong>in</strong>ate up to five boys <strong>and</strong>/or girls <strong>in</strong> their class<br />

who fitted each of the 21 behavioral descriptions of bully<strong>in</strong>g situations that compose the<br />

questionnaire. In the present study, self-nom<strong>in</strong>ations were not considered. The items are<br />

12


divided <strong>in</strong>to six scales, which refer to six different Participant Roles: Bully (4 items,<br />

e.g.: “Starts bully<strong>in</strong>g”), Assistant (2 items, e.g.: “Helps the bully, maybe by catch<strong>in</strong>g or<br />

hold<strong>in</strong>g the victim”), Re<strong>in</strong>forcer (5 items, e.g.: “Laughs at people gett<strong>in</strong>g bullied”),<br />

Defender (5 items, e.g.: “Stick up for the victim”), Outsider (4 items, e.g.: “Isn’t usually<br />

there, stays away”) <strong>and</strong> Victim (1 item, “Gets bullied”). Menes<strong>in</strong>i <strong>and</strong> G<strong>in</strong>i (2000)<br />

reported a good <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability of these scales for the Italian population (α = .78 to α<br />

= .94). In Table II, correlations between different role scores are reported.<br />

Roles 1 were assigned accord<strong>in</strong>g to the orig<strong>in</strong>al procedure, proposed by Salmivalli<br />

et al. (1996):<br />

- for each child a Role Score is calculated <strong>in</strong> each of the six scales of the<br />

questionnaire. A nom<strong>in</strong>ation for “sometimes” show<strong>in</strong>g a behavior is scored as<br />

1, <strong>and</strong> “often” is scored as 2. Then, the nom<strong>in</strong>ations were summed, divided<br />

by the number of nom<strong>in</strong>ators <strong>and</strong> multiplied by 100;<br />

- a Participant Role is assigned on the basis of the child’s highest st<strong>and</strong>ardized<br />

role score. No role is assigned if the child’s score is below the mean (Z


<strong>Social</strong> Cognition Task: The Stories<br />

A set of 15 short stories was designed to test children’s underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of mental<br />

states <strong>and</strong> emotions. Some of these stories were translated from Happé (1994) <strong>and</strong><br />

Sutton et al. (1999b), <strong>and</strong> additional stories were created reflect<strong>in</strong>g the same structure of<br />

the pre-existent stories. Even though some stories (e.g., the “army/war” story) are less<br />

relevant for children than others, they all seemed to be underst<strong>and</strong>able <strong>and</strong> valid for<br />

primary school pupils.<br />

The 15 stories were divided <strong>in</strong>to three categories, five stories for each category:<br />

a) Cognitive stories: these stories require the underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of mental states,<br />

thoughts, beliefs, <strong>in</strong>tentions of characters, which determ<strong>in</strong>e their behavior.<br />

Example of cognitive story (Happé, 1994):<br />

«Dur<strong>in</strong>g the war, the Red army captures a member of the Blue army. They<br />

want him to tell them where his army's tanks are; they know they are either by<br />

the sea or <strong>in</strong> the mounta<strong>in</strong>s. They know that the prisoner will not want to tell<br />

them, he will want to save his army, <strong>and</strong> so he will certa<strong>in</strong>ly lie to them. The<br />

prisoner is very brave <strong>and</strong> very clever, he will not let them f<strong>in</strong>d his tanks. The<br />

tanks are really <strong>in</strong> the mounta<strong>in</strong>s. Now when the other side ask him where his<br />

tanks are, he says, "They are <strong>in</strong> the mounta<strong>in</strong>s"»;<br />

b) Emotion stories: <strong>in</strong> these stories the underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of what k<strong>in</strong>d of emotion is<br />

appropriate with<strong>in</strong> a specific situation is crucial. Moreover, the child needs to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> that emotions can be clearly shown or, on the contrary, dissimulated,<br />

to <strong>in</strong>fluence <strong>and</strong> modify others’ knowledge. In order to facilitate participants’<br />

answers, children were presented with draw<strong>in</strong>gs of faces represent<strong>in</strong>g different<br />

14


emotions (happ<strong>in</strong>ess, sadness, anger, guilt, <strong>and</strong> a neutral face). An example of an<br />

emotion story taken from Sutton et al. (1999b) is:<br />

«Mike wants to go out with his friends, but he has a really bad tummy ache. He<br />

knows that if his Mum notices he is ill, she won’t let him go out to play. Mike<br />

goes downstairs <strong>and</strong> asks his Mum, “Can I go out to play please?”»;<br />

c) Moral stories: <strong>in</strong> these stories the appropriate emotion is a <strong>moral</strong> emotion<br />

because some wrongdo<strong>in</strong>g has happened. In these stories children were also<br />

helped with the draw<strong>in</strong>gs. An example of <strong>moral</strong> story (new) is as follows:<br />

«Claudia has just moved to her new school. She has brown hair, green eyes <strong>and</strong><br />

she is a little bit shorter than her classmates. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the maths class, without<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g noticed by her teacher, Susan, who sits beh<strong>in</strong>d Claudia, sends her a<br />

written note like “Pigmy, dwarf”. Claudia reads this note <strong>and</strong> starts cry<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

front of all her mates».<br />

Each story was followed by a control question based on the content of the story to<br />

verify that the child understood it; then, an experimental question assess<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of mental states or emotions was asked. For the cognitive stories, an<br />

answer was considered correct if the child referred to the mental state, thoughts, or<br />

beliefs of the characters (e.g. critical question: “Where will the Red army look for the<br />

Blue army <strong>and</strong> why?”; answer: “They will look by the sea because they th<strong>in</strong>k that the<br />

prisoner is ly<strong>in</strong>g to them”). For the emotion stories, a correct answer <strong>in</strong>cluded both the<br />

identification of the appropriate emotion (positive or negative) <strong>and</strong> the reference to the<br />

<strong>in</strong>tention/wish of the ma<strong>in</strong> character to hide his/her own real emotion to the other<br />

characters, for example when a character is “sad” but he or she appears “happy” (e.g.<br />

15


“Mike has a bad tummy ache, but he appears happy, otherwise his Mum notices that he<br />

is feel<strong>in</strong>g bad <strong>and</strong> won't let him go out”). F<strong>in</strong>ally, for the <strong>moral</strong> stories, an answer was<br />

considered correct if the child recognized the fact that, <strong>in</strong> the situation described, the<br />

character of the story should have felt a <strong>moral</strong> emotion (guilt or shame) for his/her own<br />

behavior (e.g. “She (Susan) feels guilty because she shouldn’t have said that bad th<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

to her mate Claudia”).<br />

Moral Disengagement Scale<br />

This scale measures the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s tendency to use cognitive mechanisms that<br />

can disengage self-sanctions <strong>and</strong> justify the use of violent <strong>and</strong> aggressive behaviors. This<br />

version of the scale (Caprara et al., 1995), designed for primary school children, consists<br />

of 14 items describ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual’s read<strong>in</strong>ess to construe <strong>in</strong>jurious conduct as serv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

righteous purposes, masquerad<strong>in</strong>g censurable activities by palliative language or<br />

render<strong>in</strong>g them benign by advantageous comparison, m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g the harmful effects of<br />

one’s detrimental conduct, <strong>and</strong> devalu<strong>in</strong>g those who are maltreated (example items:<br />

“Some kids deserve to be treated like animals”; “Kids cannot be blamed for misbehav<strong>in</strong>g<br />

if their friends pressured them to do it”). Children were asked to rate the strength of their<br />

endorsement or rejection of <strong>moral</strong> exoneration of detrimental conduct on a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Likert-type scale (from 1= strongly disagree to 5= strongly agree). Therefore, higher<br />

scores <strong>in</strong> this scale <strong>in</strong>dicate a higher tendency to engage <strong>in</strong> one or more of these<br />

mechanisms. Even though the items of the scale describe different mechanisms, previous<br />

studies us<strong>in</strong>g this scale with Italian samples (Caprara et al., 1995; Caprara et al., 1996;<br />

Menes<strong>in</strong>i et al., 1997) have demonstrated a unidimensional structure of the scale,<br />

16


especially for younger children. This was also confirmed <strong>in</strong> our sample. Therefore,<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g the suggestions of the authors of the scale, analyses was conducted on the total<br />

score <strong>and</strong> not on separate subscales <strong>in</strong> the present study. The reliability of the scale was<br />

α = .69.<br />

Procedure<br />

Stage 1. The PRQ <strong>and</strong> the Moral Disengagement Scale were completed dur<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

s<strong>in</strong>gle classroom session <strong>and</strong> the order of presentation of the two measures was<br />

counterbalanced. This session began by <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g the general purpose of the study.<br />

Then, the follow<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>ition of bully<strong>in</strong>g (Whitney & Smith, 1993) was presented <strong>and</strong><br />

discussed with the class:<br />

“We say a child or young person is be<strong>in</strong>g bullied, or picked on when<br />

another child or young person, or a group of children or young people,<br />

say nasty <strong>and</strong> unpleasant th<strong>in</strong>gs to him or her. It is also bully<strong>in</strong>g when a<br />

child or young person is hit, kicked, threatened, locked <strong>in</strong>side a room,<br />

sent nasty notes, when no one ever talks to them <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs like that.<br />

These th<strong>in</strong>gs can happen frequently <strong>and</strong> it is difficult for the child or<br />

young person be<strong>in</strong>g bullied to defend himself or herself. It is also<br />

bully<strong>in</strong>g when a child or young person is teased repeatedly <strong>in</strong> a nasty<br />

way. But it is not bully<strong>in</strong>g when two children or young people of about<br />

the same strength have the odd fight or quarrel.”<br />

17


Stage 2. When the data from Stage 1 had been analyzed, children selected through<br />

the PRQ were tested <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong> an area of the school that afforded privacy. Dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

this session, the stories were presented <strong>in</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>omized order <strong>and</strong> the children’s answers<br />

were audio-recorded to facilitate the successive transcription <strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g procedure. In<br />

order to avoid any possible <strong>in</strong>fluence upon their responses, the experimenter who<br />

<strong>in</strong>terviewed children did not know their Participant role.<br />

Results<br />

Results are presented us<strong>in</strong>g the categorical measure of “Participant Roles” to<br />

compare group means <strong>in</strong> social <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement with <strong>in</strong>dependent 6<br />

(Roles) x 2 (Gender) ANOVAs. The cont<strong>in</strong>uous measure ”Role Scores” is used to test<br />

for correlations between the extent of different types of behavior <strong>in</strong> the whole sample<br />

<strong>and</strong> the social <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement measures<br />

Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary analysis<br />

A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary one-way ANOVA was performed to <strong>in</strong>vestigate the possible effect<br />

of Participant Roles on the answers to the control questions. No statistically significant<br />

differences emerged among the different roles [F(5, 192) = 0.962, p = n.s.], thus<br />

confirm<strong>in</strong>g that the level of comprehension of the stories presented was similar for all<br />

Participant Roles.<br />

Moreover, <strong>in</strong> order to control for <strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability, two <strong>in</strong>dependent judges<br />

coded the 30% of all children’s answers. Their level of agreement reached 96%. Cohen’s<br />

kappa was also calculated, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> K=.91. Disagreements were due to those answers<br />

18


<strong>in</strong> which the reference to mental states was not completely clear. All discrepancies were<br />

resolved by consultation between the coders.<br />

Participant Roles <strong>and</strong> social <strong>cognition</strong><br />

As the three categories <strong>in</strong>to which our stories were divided (cognitive, emotion<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> stories) are different from each other, three separate ANOVAs were<br />

conducted on the mean performance <strong>in</strong> the three sets of stories, with Role <strong>and</strong> Gender as<br />

between factors. From the analysis, a statistically significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect of Role<br />

emerged only for the cognitive stories [F(5, 192) = 2.376, p = .04, ηp 2 = .058]. Post hoc<br />

analyses revealed that Victims’ performance (M=2.88, s.d.=1.61) was significantly<br />

lower than Defenders (M=4.03, s.d.=1.38), whereas other groups did not significantly<br />

differ from each other (Bonferroni comparisons). The ma<strong>in</strong> effect of Gender <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Role x Gender <strong>in</strong>teraction were not statistically significant.<br />

These data do not support the prediction of Hypothesis 1 that bullies should<br />

perform better than other people <strong>in</strong> the theory of m<strong>in</strong>d tasks, but they also fail to show<br />

social deficits among bullies. In contrast to Hypothesis 2, bullies did not do worse than<br />

the other groups <strong>in</strong> the <strong>moral</strong> stories.<br />

Role scores <strong>and</strong> social <strong>cognition</strong><br />

--- Insert Table III about here ---<br />

A partial correlation analysis between social <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> the actual extent of<br />

different types of behavior (the role scores) was performed. Consider<strong>in</strong>g the fact that the<br />

19


scales are correlated with each other, to exam<strong>in</strong>e the relation of each role with the<br />

cognitive, emotion <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g skills, <strong>in</strong> the correlation analysis<br />

the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g PRQ scales <strong>and</strong> age were partialled out. Moreover, consider<strong>in</strong>g the high<br />

number of comparisons, <strong>in</strong> order to avoid the <strong>in</strong>crease of type 1 error, Bonferroni’s<br />

correction was used (adjusted level of α: 0.05/24=.002). Correlation coefficients are<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> Table IV. In l<strong>in</strong>e with Hypothesis 1 Bully role is positively correlated with the<br />

total social <strong>cognition</strong> score <strong>and</strong> with the cognitive score (the ability to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

thoughts, <strong>in</strong>tentions, beliefs, etc.) <strong>and</strong> the emotion score. Moreover, <strong>in</strong> accordance with<br />

Hypothesis 2, the Defender score is positively correlated with all social <strong>cognition</strong> scores.<br />

Participant Roles <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement<br />

--- Insert Table IV about here ---<br />

As concerns the third aim, a two-way ANOVA on the mean score <strong>in</strong> the Moral<br />

Disengagement Scale was performed with Role <strong>and</strong> Gender as between factors. A<br />

statistically significant ma<strong>in</strong> effect of Role emerged [F(5, 192) = 9.269, p


level of <strong>moral</strong> disengagement than both Outsiders (M=30.35, s.d.=7.63) <strong>and</strong> Victims<br />

(M=32.41, s.d.=8.12). These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs support Hypothesis 3.<br />

Role scores <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement<br />

--- Insert Figure 1 about here ---<br />

A partial correlation analysis, controlled for age of participants, between the score<br />

<strong>in</strong> the <strong>moral</strong> disengagement scale <strong>and</strong> the role scores was performed. Results showed a<br />

positive correlation between <strong>moral</strong> disengagement <strong>and</strong> the role scores <strong>in</strong> Bully<br />

(r(204)=.26, p


models have been proposed <strong>in</strong> the literature to expla<strong>in</strong> the Bully’s behavior <strong>in</strong> terms of<br />

social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g. The first model hypothesizes the presence of deficits <strong>in</strong><br />

the social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g of bullies, similar to those found for aggressive<br />

children (Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980). This model, therefore, proposes the idea<br />

of bullies as “oaf” children. They are considered deficient <strong>in</strong> their social <strong>in</strong>telligence,<br />

that is their ability to <strong>in</strong>terpret <strong>and</strong> manage <strong>in</strong>formation deriv<strong>in</strong>g from social <strong>in</strong>teractions<br />

with peers <strong>and</strong> to produce adequate social responses. The second model, <strong>in</strong> contrast,<br />

states that bullies are quite skilful <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g social cues (e.g., others’ mental<br />

states) <strong>and</strong> that they exploit this ability to their own advantage, act<strong>in</strong>g as “skilled social<br />

manipulators” (Sutton et al., 1999a; 1999b). However, accord<strong>in</strong>g to these authors,<br />

bullies may lack the empathic reactivity towards their mates’ emotions <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> particular,<br />

towards victim’s suffer<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

In the first two hypotheses of the present study, it was anticipated that bullies may<br />

have a good theory of m<strong>in</strong>d, but they may be deficient <strong>in</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong>. The analysis<br />

of variance, <strong>in</strong>deed, <strong>in</strong>dicated that the bully group did not show any difficulty, compared<br />

to non-aggressive peers, <strong>in</strong> the underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of cognitive stories, which required them<br />

to assume the cognitive po<strong>in</strong>t of view of another person, that is to underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions,<br />

beliefs <strong>and</strong> goals that guide a determ<strong>in</strong>ate behavior. Moreover, correlations between the<br />

scores <strong>in</strong> each scale of the PRQ <strong>and</strong> the performance <strong>in</strong> the social <strong>cognition</strong> task<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated a positive relation between bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>and</strong> the ability to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

others’ mental states. As regards the two models, therefore, our results are closer to what<br />

would be expected by the Skilled Manipulator model (Sutton et al., 1999a). The fact that<br />

our bullies did not show particular deficits <strong>in</strong> this specific area of social <strong>cognition</strong> does<br />

22


not suggest a direct application of the <strong>Social</strong> Skills Deficit model (Crick & Dodge, 1994)<br />

to bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior. Further analyses are needed <strong>in</strong>to the social cognitive competence<br />

of children <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g episodes. This is particularly important because theory<br />

of m<strong>in</strong>d explanations per se are not likely to be very helpful for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the<br />

unique nature of bully<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> that hav<strong>in</strong>g a sophisticated theory of m<strong>in</strong>d can lead to<br />

highly prosocial behavior or to bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior. In other words, hav<strong>in</strong>g a “superior”<br />

theory of m<strong>in</strong>d says noth<strong>in</strong>g about how that knowledge will be utilized <strong>in</strong> social<br />

<strong>in</strong>teractions (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001).<br />

As regards the two other types of stories used <strong>in</strong> the present study, bullies’<br />

performance <strong>in</strong> emotion stories <strong>and</strong> also <strong>in</strong> <strong>moral</strong> stories, a category not used by Sutton<br />

<strong>and</strong> colleagues (1999b), was found to be slightly, but not significantly lower, than the<br />

performance obta<strong>in</strong>ed by other groups. Contrary to our second hypothesis about the<br />

possible difficulties of bullies <strong>in</strong> <strong>moral</strong> <strong>cognition</strong>, our results do not completely confirm<br />

the literature on <strong>moral</strong> development <strong>in</strong> aggressive children <strong>and</strong> bullies (e.g., B<strong>and</strong>ura,<br />

1991; Hart, Burock, London, & Atk<strong>in</strong>s, 2003). In our sample, <strong>in</strong> fact, the bullies <strong>and</strong><br />

their followers did not show a significant lack of ability <strong>in</strong> the attribution of appropriate<br />

<strong>moral</strong> emotions to the characters of the stories. These emotions, such as guilt <strong>and</strong> shame,<br />

play an important role <strong>in</strong> the re<strong>cognition</strong> of the damage suffered by another person <strong>and</strong><br />

are closely connected to the sense of responsibility of the person who shows the harmful<br />

behavior. Moreover, these emotions imply the need of reparation as a consequence of a<br />

negative outcome produced by one’s behavior (Bybee, 1998; Hoffman, 2000).<br />

However, for both emotion <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> stories, it is important to note that they<br />

actually assess the “cognitive” ability to process emotional <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> social<br />

23


<strong>in</strong>teractions rather than an “emotional” or “empathic” comprehension of others’ feel<strong>in</strong>gs.<br />

Therefore, the positive performance shown by aggressive children <strong>in</strong> these k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

stories may misrepresent or underestimate the real deficit of these children <strong>in</strong> emotional<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of others. In other words, what bullies may lack <strong>and</strong> what may<br />

differentiate them from prosocial children is the ability to appreciate the emotional<br />

consequences of their behaviors on others’ feel<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> to share <strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> empathize with,<br />

the feel<strong>in</strong>gs of others (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998). In l<strong>in</strong>e<br />

with this perspective, for example, G<strong>in</strong>i, Albiero, <strong>and</strong> Benelli (2004) recently found a<br />

negative relation between bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>and</strong> empathy, measured through Davis’<br />

empathic reactivity <strong>in</strong>dex (1983), <strong>in</strong> a sample of Italian adolescents.<br />

Sutton <strong>and</strong> colleagues (1999a) also argued along these l<strong>in</strong>es, referr<strong>in</strong>g to the “cold<br />

<strong>cognition</strong>” hypothesis proposed by Mealey (1995) as regards sociopathy. They also<br />

spoke of a theory of m<strong>in</strong>d formulated purely <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumental terms, without access to the<br />

empathic underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g that most people rely on. As a consequence, the <strong>in</strong>dividual may<br />

use aggressive behavior <strong>in</strong> a Machiavellian way, without consider<strong>in</strong>g its negative<br />

consequences on others. In contrast, high levels of empathic responsiveness act as an<br />

important mediator <strong>in</strong> social conduct, reduc<strong>in</strong>g aggression <strong>and</strong> favor<strong>in</strong>g prosocial<br />

behavior (Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, 2000; Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, Björkqvist,<br />

Österman, Lagerspetz, & Niskanen, 1994; Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, Björkqvist, Österman, &<br />

Lagerspetz, 1996).<br />

With respect to the other groups considered <strong>in</strong> the present study, victimized<br />

children showed some difficulties <strong>in</strong> the social <strong>cognition</strong> task. If compared to the<br />

defender group <strong>in</strong> particular, they seem to lack the ability to underst<strong>and</strong> others’ m<strong>in</strong>d,<br />

24


that is their thoughts, beliefs <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions that guide social behavior. Our data are<br />

consistent with results reported by Sutton et al. (1999b). Moreover, several authors share<br />

the idea that victims show deficits <strong>in</strong> areas like social skills, social problem solv<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

assertiveness, <strong>and</strong> emotional regulation (e.g., Champion, Vernberg, & Shipman, 2003;<br />

Kochenderfer & Ladd, 1997; Mahady-Wilton, Craig, & Pepler, 2000; Olweus, 1993;<br />

Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988). F<strong>in</strong>ally, the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the social <strong>cognition</strong> task by<br />

the defender group should be noted. These children, <strong>in</strong> fact, showed high levels of<br />

performance <strong>in</strong> all the stories, <strong>and</strong> especially <strong>in</strong> the cognitive stories. Moreover, the<br />

score <strong>in</strong> the defender scale of the PRQ correlated positively with all the scores <strong>in</strong> the<br />

social <strong>cognition</strong> task, thus suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the adoption of this k<strong>in</strong>d of prosocial <strong>and</strong><br />

helpful behavior requires a high level of social ability <strong>and</strong> a well-developed<br />

underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of both cognitive <strong>and</strong> emotional states of others. However, the fact that<br />

the bully<strong>in</strong>g score also correlated with the social <strong>cognition</strong> task, aga<strong>in</strong>, confirms that<br />

hav<strong>in</strong>g a good theory of m<strong>in</strong>d, by itself, does not necessarily mean that this ability will<br />

be used to act prosocially (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001).<br />

Bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement<br />

As regards the third aim of the present study, the analysis of variance on <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement showed higher levels of the tendency to disengage self-sanctions <strong>and</strong><br />

justify the use of aggressive behaviors <strong>in</strong> all the aggressive roles <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> particular, <strong>in</strong><br />

bullies. These results confirmed the role of specific cognitive mechanisms <strong>in</strong> the<br />

regulation of social conduct, such as the <strong>moral</strong> disengagement mechanisms that allow<br />

the assumption of violent behaviors (Bacch<strong>in</strong>i et al., 1998; Caprara et al., 1995;<br />

25


Menes<strong>in</strong>i et al., 1999; Menes<strong>in</strong>i et al., 2003). Regard<strong>in</strong>g this aspect, as suggested by a<br />

reviewer, it could be <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g to collect <strong>in</strong>formation about the relations between<br />

bully<strong>in</strong>g, familial socialization <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> climate at home <strong>and</strong> at school.<br />

In contrast, defenders’ high <strong>moral</strong> sensibility <strong>and</strong> empathic reactivity, already<br />

found <strong>in</strong> other studies (Menes<strong>in</strong>i et al., 2003; G<strong>in</strong>i & Carli, 2003), may be one of the<br />

basic motivations for prosocial behavior, frequently shown by these children towards<br />

their victimized mates. These motivations are very important <strong>and</strong> must be taken <strong>in</strong>to<br />

account dur<strong>in</strong>g the implementation of <strong>in</strong>tervention programs <strong>in</strong> schools (Cowie, 2000;<br />

Cowie, Naylor, Talamelli, Chauhan, & Smith, 2002). However, the fact that the ability<br />

to help other peers requires several <strong>in</strong>terpersonal competencies, that young children do<br />

not necessarily possess, needs to be considered.<br />

In conclusion, the results presented <strong>in</strong> this paper have several potential<br />

implications for future research. On the one h<strong>and</strong>, our results draw attention to the<br />

importance of study<strong>in</strong>g the role of social cognitive competencies <strong>in</strong> the complex circle of<br />

behaviors, attitudes <strong>and</strong> expectations upon which school bully<strong>in</strong>g phenomenon is based.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the relevance of adopt<strong>in</strong>g a more f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed classification of roles<br />

<strong>in</strong> the study of bully<strong>in</strong>g behavior was confirmed.<br />

A possible limitation <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of our results was the fact that we did<br />

not consider the bully-victim group, that is those children who frequently assume both<br />

k<strong>in</strong>ds of role, act<strong>in</strong>g as a bully <strong>and</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g victimized as well (e.g., Boulton & Smith,<br />

1994; Olweus, 1993). Several authors stressed the relevance of this group of pupils, also<br />

called aggressive-victims (Perry et al., 1988; Schwartz, 2000), <strong>in</strong> the study of aggressive<br />

problems. Moreover, these children have been consistently described as lack<strong>in</strong>g social-<br />

26


emotional abilities, hav<strong>in</strong>g psychosocial impairments <strong>and</strong> a wide range of other<br />

problems (e.g., Besag, 1989). A methodological difficulty <strong>in</strong> the study of these pupils is<br />

that it is not always simple to clearly identify bully-victims as a dist<strong>in</strong>ct group. Despite<br />

this complication, future research <strong>in</strong> this field will necessarily need to study the social-<br />

cognitive <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> characteristics of this particular group of children, <strong>and</strong> to compare<br />

their level of cognitive <strong>and</strong> emotional underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g with that of “pure” bullies <strong>and</strong><br />

victims.<br />

References<br />

Almeida, A., del Barrio, C., Marques, M., Gutierrez, H., & van der Meulen, K. (2001).<br />

Scan-bully<strong>in</strong>g: A script-cartoon narrative to assess <strong>cognition</strong>s, emotions <strong>and</strong> cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

strategies <strong>in</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g situations. In M Mart<strong>in</strong>ez. (Ed.), Prevention <strong>and</strong> control of<br />

aggression <strong>and</strong> the impact on its victims (pp. 161 – 168). New York: Kluwer<br />

Academic/Plenum Pub.<br />

Arsenio, W. F. (1988). Children's conceptions of the situational affective consequences<br />

of socio<strong>moral</strong> events. Child Development, 58, 1611-1622.<br />

Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2001). Varieties of childhood bully<strong>in</strong>g: Values,<br />

emotion processes <strong>and</strong> social competence. <strong>Social</strong> Development, 10, 59-73.<br />

Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom. The<br />

Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86-97.<br />

Bacch<strong>in</strong>i, D., Amodeo, A. L., Ciardi, A., Valerio, P., & Vitelli, R. (1998). La relazione<br />

vittima-prepotente: stabilità del fenomeno e ricorso a meccanismi di disimpegno<br />

27


<strong>moral</strong>e [Bully-victim relationship: stability of the phenomenon <strong>and</strong> use of <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement]. Scienze dell’Interazione, 5(1), 29-46.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1986). <strong>Social</strong> foundation of thought <strong>and</strong> action. A social cognitive theory.<br />

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1990). Selective activation <strong>and</strong> disengagement of <strong>moral</strong> control. Journal of<br />

<strong>Social</strong> Issues, 46(1), 27-46.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1991). <strong>Social</strong> cognitive theory of <strong>moral</strong> thought <strong>and</strong> action. In W.M.<br />

Kurt<strong>in</strong>es, & G.L. Gewirtz (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>moral</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong> development:<br />

theory, research <strong>and</strong> applications, Vol. 1 (pp. 71-129). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (2002). Selective <strong>moral</strong> disengagement <strong>in</strong> the exercise of <strong>moral</strong> agency.<br />

Journal of Moral Education, 312, 101-119.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of<br />

<strong>moral</strong> disengagement <strong>in</strong> the exercise of <strong>moral</strong> agency. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Social</strong> Psychology, 71, 364-374.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Regalia, C. (2001).<br />

Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms govern<strong>in</strong>g transgressive behavior.<br />

Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology, 801, 125-135.<br />

Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: an <strong>in</strong>terpersonal<br />

approach. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 115, 243-267.<br />

Besag, V. (1989). Bullies <strong>and</strong> victims <strong>in</strong> schools. Milton Keynes: Open University Press<br />

Milton.<br />

Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, A. (2000). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence – empathy =<br />

aggression? Aggression <strong>and</strong> Violent Behavior, 52, 191-200.<br />

28


Boulton, M., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Bully/victim problems <strong>in</strong> middle-school children:<br />

Stability, self-perceived competence, peer perceptions <strong>and</strong> peer acceptance. British<br />

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 315-329.<br />

Bybee, J. (1998). Guilt <strong>and</strong> children. San Diego: Academic Press.<br />

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Vic<strong>in</strong>o, S., & B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1996). La misura del<br />

disimpegno <strong>moral</strong>e [Measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>moral</strong> disengagement]. Rassegna di Psicologia, 113,<br />

93-105.<br />

Caprara, G. V., Pastorelli, C., & B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1995). La misura del disimpegno <strong>moral</strong>e<br />

<strong>in</strong> età evolutiva [Measur<strong>in</strong>g age differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement]. Età Evolutiva,<br />

51, 18-29.<br />

Champion, K., Vernberg, E., & Shipman, K. (2003). Nonbully<strong>in</strong>g victims of bullies:<br />

Aggression, social skills, <strong>and</strong> friendship characteristics. Journal of Applied<br />

Developmental Psychology, 245, 535-551.<br />

Cowie, H. (2000). Byst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g or st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g by: Gender issues <strong>in</strong> cop<strong>in</strong>g with bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

English schools. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 85-97.<br />

Cowie, H., Naylor, P., Talamelli, L., Chauhan, P., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Knowledge,<br />

use <strong>and</strong> attitudes towards peer support. Journal of Adolescence, 255, 453-467.<br />

Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (1997). Observations of bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> victimization <strong>in</strong> the<br />

schoolyard. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 2, 41-60.<br />

Craig, W. M., Pepler, D. J., & Atlas, R. (2000). Observations of bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the<br />

playground <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the classroom. School Psychology International, 21, 22-36.<br />

Cramer, D. (1998). Fundamental statistics for social research. London: Routledge<br />

29


Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review <strong>and</strong> reformulation of social <strong>in</strong>formation-<br />

process<strong>in</strong>g mechanisms <strong>in</strong> children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 115,<br />

74-101.<br />

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual differences <strong>in</strong> empathy: Evidence for a<br />

multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology, 441,<br />

113-126.<br />

Dodge, K. (1980). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> children’s aggressive behavior. Child<br />

Development, 51, 162-170.<br />

Dodge, K., Bates, J., & Pettit, G. (1990). Mechanisms <strong>in</strong> the cycle of violence. Science,<br />

250, 1678-1683.<br />

Dodge, K., & Frame, C. L. (1982). <strong>Social</strong> cognitive bias <strong>and</strong> deficits <strong>in</strong> aggressive boys.<br />

Child Development, 53, 620-635.<br />

Dodge, K., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g processes <strong>in</strong> aggressive<br />

boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 375-379.<br />

Dodge, K., & Price, J. M. (1994). On the relation between social <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>and</strong> socially competent behavior <strong>in</strong> early school-aged children. Child Development,<br />

65, 1385-1397.<br />

Dodge, K. A., & Somberg, D. (1987). Hostile attributional biases are exacerbated under<br />

conditions of threat to the self. Child Development, 58, 213-224.<br />

Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, & N.<br />

Eisenberg (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology. Vol. 3, 5 th ed. (pp. 701-778). New<br />

York: Wiley.<br />

30


G<strong>in</strong>i, G., Albiero, P., & Benelli, B. (2004). Relazioni tra bullismo, empatia, immag<strong>in</strong>e di<br />

sè ed autoefficacia percepita <strong>in</strong> un campione di adolescenti [Relations between<br />

bully<strong>in</strong>g, empathy, self-image <strong>and</strong> self-efficacy <strong>in</strong> adolescents]. Poster presented at<br />

the 18 th National Conference of Developmental Psychology, Sciacca (AG), Italy.<br />

G<strong>in</strong>i, G., & Carli, G. (2003). Il bullismo a scuola: analisi dei meccanismi di disimpegno<br />

<strong>moral</strong>e <strong>in</strong> una prospettiva di gruppo [Bully<strong>in</strong>g at school: analysis of <strong>moral</strong><br />

disengagement mechanisms <strong>in</strong> a group perspective]. Orientamenti Pedagogici.<br />

Rivista Internazionale per l’Educazione, 502, 303-314.<br />

Happè, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of m<strong>in</strong>d: underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of story<br />

characters’ thoughts <strong>and</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs by able autistic, mentally h<strong>and</strong>icapped, <strong>and</strong> normal<br />

children <strong>and</strong> adults. Journal of Autism <strong>and</strong> Developmental Disorders, 242, 129-154.<br />

Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure of the psychopathy<br />

checklist. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 56, 741-747.<br />

Hart, D., Burock, D., London, B., & Atk<strong>in</strong>s, R. (2003). Prosocial tendencies, antisocial<br />

behavior, <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> development. In A. Slater & G. Bremner (Eds.), An<br />

<strong>in</strong>troduction to developmental psychology (pp. 334-356). Malden, MA, US:<br />

Blackwell Publishers.<br />

Harvey, R. J., Fletcher, J., & French, D. J. (2001). <strong>Social</strong> reason<strong>in</strong>g: A source of<br />

<strong>in</strong>fluence on aggression. Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology Review, 213, 447-469.<br />

Hawley, P. H. (2003). Prosocial <strong>and</strong> coercive configurations of resource control <strong>in</strong> early<br />

adolescence: A case for the well-adapted machiavellian. Merrill Palmer Quarterly,<br />

493, 279-309.<br />

31


Hazler, R. J. (1996). Break<strong>in</strong>g the cycle of violence: Interventions for bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><br />

victimization. Wash<strong>in</strong>gton, DC: Accelerated Development.<br />

Hodges, E. V. E., Malone, M. J., & Perry, D. G. (1997). Individual risk <strong>and</strong> social risk as<br />

<strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g determ<strong>in</strong>ants of victimization <strong>in</strong> the peer group. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 33(6), 1032-1039.<br />

Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathy, role-tak<strong>in</strong>g, guilt, <strong>and</strong> development of altruistic<br />

motives. In T. Likona (Ed.), Moral development <strong>and</strong> behavior: Theory, research,<br />

<strong>and</strong> social issues (pp. 124-143). New York: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart W<strong>in</strong>ston.<br />

Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> development. New York, NY, US:<br />

Cambridge University Press.<br />

Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s normative beliefs about<br />

aggression <strong>and</strong> aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology,<br />

72, 408-419.<br />

Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, A., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1996). <strong>Social</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>telligence <strong>and</strong> empathy as antecedents of different types of aggression. In C. F.<br />

Ferris & T. Grisson (Eds.), Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g aggressive behavior <strong>in</strong> children (pp.<br />

364-366). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 794.<br />

32


Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, A., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Niskanen, L.<br />

(1994). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence <strong>and</strong> the use of <strong>in</strong>direct aggression. Presented at the XIII<br />

Biennal Meet<strong>in</strong>gs of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural<br />

Development, June 28-July 2, Amsterdam, The Nederl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. (1997). Victimized children’s responses to peers<br />

aggression: Behaviors associated with reduced versus cont<strong>in</strong>ued victimization.<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 9, 59-73.<br />

Mahady-Wilton, M. M., Craig, W. M., & Pepler, D. J. (2000). Emotional regulation <strong>and</strong><br />

display <strong>in</strong> classroom victims of bully<strong>in</strong>g: Characteristic expression of affect, cop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

styles <strong>and</strong> relevant contextual factors. <strong>Social</strong> Development, 92, 226-245.<br />

Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An <strong>in</strong>tegrated evolutionary model.<br />

Behavioural <strong>and</strong> Bra<strong>in</strong> Sciences, 18, 523-599.<br />

Menes<strong>in</strong>i, E., Codecasa, E., Benelli, B., & Cowie, H. (2003). Enhanc<strong>in</strong>g children's<br />

responsibility to take action aga<strong>in</strong>st bully<strong>in</strong>g: Evaluation of a befriend<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> Italian middle schools. Aggressive Behavior, 291, 10-14.<br />

Menes<strong>in</strong>i, E., Fonzi, V., & Vannucci, M. (1997). Bullies <strong>and</strong> victims <strong>in</strong> Italy: attitudes<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement. Paper presented at the 7 th European Conference for<br />

Research on Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Instruction EARLI – Athens 26-30 August 1997.<br />

Menes<strong>in</strong>i, E., & G<strong>in</strong>i, G. (2000). Il bullismo come processo di gruppo: adattamento e<br />

validazione del questionario “Ruoli dei partecipanti” alla popolazione italiana<br />

[Bully<strong>in</strong>g as a group process: adaptation <strong>and</strong> validation of the Participant Role<br />

Questionnaire to the Italian population]. Età Evolutiva, 66, 18-32.<br />

33


Menes<strong>in</strong>i, E., Sanchez, V., Fonzi, A., Ortega, R., Costabile, A., & Lo Feudo, G. (2003).<br />

Moral emotions <strong>and</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g: A cross-national comparison of differences between<br />

bullies, victims <strong>and</strong> outsiders. Aggressive Behavior, 296, 515-530.<br />

Nucci, L. (2001). Education <strong>in</strong> the <strong>moral</strong> doma<strong>in</strong>. Cambridge: Cambridge University<br />

Press<br />

Olweus, D. (1993). Bully<strong>in</strong>g at school. What we know <strong>and</strong> what we can do. Blackwell,<br />

Oxford <strong>and</strong> Cambridge.<br />

Pepler, D. J., & Craig, W. M. (1995). A peek beh<strong>in</strong>d the fence: naturalistic observations<br />

of aggressive children with remote audiovisual record<strong>in</strong>g. Developmental<br />

Psychology, 314, 548-553.<br />

Perry, D. G., Kusel, S. J., & Perry, L. C. (1988). Victims of peer aggression.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 24, 807-814.<br />

Quiles, Z. N., & Bybee, J. (1997). Chronic <strong>and</strong> predispositional guilt: Relations to<br />

mental health, prosocial behavior <strong>and</strong> religiosity. Journal of Personality<br />

Assessment, 69, 104-126.<br />

R<strong>and</strong>all, P. (1997). Adult bully<strong>in</strong>g: perpetrators <strong>and</strong> victims. London: Routledge.<br />

Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. M. (1997). Peer networks <strong>and</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

schools. Sc<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>avian Journal of Psychology, 38, 305-312.<br />

Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukia<strong>in</strong>en, A. (1996).<br />

Bully<strong>in</strong>g as a group process: Participant Roles <strong>and</strong> their relations to social status<br />

with<strong>in</strong> the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1-15.<br />

Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims <strong>and</strong> aggressors <strong>in</strong> children’s peer groups.<br />

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181 - 192.<br />

34


Sharp, S., & Smith, P. K. (1994). Tackl<strong>in</strong>g bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> your school: A practical<br />

h<strong>and</strong>book for teachers. London: Routledge.<br />

Slee, P. T. (1993). Bully<strong>in</strong>g: A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the nature <strong>and</strong> effects on<br />

social <strong>cognition</strong>. Early Child Development <strong>and</strong> Care, 87, 47-57.<br />

Smetana, J. G. (1995). Morality <strong>in</strong> context: Abstractions, ambiguities <strong>and</strong> applications.<br />

In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development: A research annual vol. 10 (pp. 83-<br />

130). London: Jessica K<strong>in</strong>gsley.<br />

Smith, P. K., Bowers, L., B<strong>in</strong>ney, V., & Cowie, H. (1993). Relationships of children<br />

<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> bully/victim problems at school. In S. Duck (Ed.), Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g<br />

relationship processes. Vol. 2: Learn<strong>in</strong>g about relationships (pp. 184-212). Sage<br />

Newbury Park, Ca.<br />

Sutton, J., & Smith. P. K. (1999). Bully<strong>in</strong>g as a group process: An adaptation of the<br />

Participant Role approach. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 97-111.<br />

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> “theory of m<strong>in</strong>d”: A<br />

critique of the “social skills deficit” view of anti-social behaviour. <strong>Social</strong><br />

Development, 8(1), 117-134.<br />

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). <strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> bully<strong>in</strong>g: social<br />

<strong>in</strong>adequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,<br />

173, 435-450.<br />

Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999c). <strong>Social</strong>ly undesirable needs not be<br />

<strong>in</strong>competent: A response to Crick <strong>and</strong> Dodge. <strong>Social</strong> Development, 8, 132-134.<br />

Tagney, J. P. (1992). Situational determ<strong>in</strong>ants of shame <strong>and</strong> guilt <strong>in</strong> young adulthood.<br />

Personality <strong>and</strong> <strong>Social</strong> Psychology Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 18, 199-206.<br />

35


Turiel, E. (1998). Moral development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book<br />

of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. <strong>Social</strong>, Emotional, <strong>and</strong> Personality Development 5th<br />

ed. (pp. 863-932). New York: Wiley.<br />

Whitney, I., & Smith, P. K. (1993). A survey of the nature <strong>and</strong> extent of bully<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

junior/middle <strong>and</strong> secondary schools. Educational Research, 35, 3-25.<br />

Yadava, A., Sharma, N. R., & G<strong>and</strong>hi, A. (2001). Aggression <strong>and</strong> <strong>moral</strong> disengagement.<br />

Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Studies, 172, 95-99.<br />

36


Table I. Distribution of Participant Roles (%) with<strong>in</strong> the whole sample<br />

Participant Roles<br />

Bullies Assistants Re<strong>in</strong>forcers Defenders Outsiders Victims No role Total<br />

Males 15.9 16.3 11.9 11.5 12.2 17.6 14.6 100<br />

Females 5.9 6.3 7.7 26.2 25.5 17.8 10.6 100<br />

37


Table II. Role score correlations for the whole sample<br />

Assistant .68 *<br />

Bully Assistant Re<strong>in</strong>forcer Defender Outsider<br />

Re<strong>in</strong>forcer .66 * .60 *<br />

Defender - .16 * - .11 .10<br />

Outsider - .22 * - .24 * - .07 .30 *<br />

Victim .07 .01 .12 - .01 .13<br />

Note: N = 578; * p


Table III. Participant Roles <strong>and</strong> mean score [s.d.] <strong>in</strong> social <strong>cognition</strong> task<br />

Participant<br />

Roles a<br />

Cognitive<br />

stories b<br />

Emotion stories<br />

b<br />

39<br />

Moral<br />

stories b<br />

Total social<br />

<strong>cognition</strong> task c<br />

Bullies 3.06 [1.30] 2.79 [2.20] 3.18 [1.94] 9.03 [4.79]<br />

Assistants 3.18 [1.47] 3.20 [2.04] 3.59 [1.86] 9.97 [4.85]<br />

Re<strong>in</strong>forcers 3.15 [1.56] 3.26 [1.76] 3.62 [1.67] 10.03 [4.28]<br />

Defenders 4.03 [1.38] 4.12 [1.45] 4.00 [1.67] 12.15 [4.04]<br />

Outsiders 3.38 [1.76] 3.68 [1.66] 4.06 [1.63] 11.12 [4.58]<br />

Victims 2.88 [1.61] 3.26 [1.81] 3.44 [1.76] 9.59 [4.73]<br />

Note:<br />

a n=34 for each group; b range = 0-5; c range = 0-15


Table IV. Partial correlations of social <strong>cognition</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual role scores<br />

Role scores<br />

<strong>Social</strong> <strong>cognition</strong> scores<br />

Cognitive Emotion Moral Total score<br />

Bully .195 * .148 * .129 .172 *<br />

Assistant - .021 - .026 .014 - .012<br />

Re<strong>in</strong>forcer - .074 - .059 - .098 - .078<br />

Defender .214 * .226 * .197 * .223 *<br />

Outsider .071 .008 .060 .050<br />

Victim .068 .109 .112 .104<br />

Note: N = 204; * p


Figure 1. Participant Roles <strong>and</strong> Moral Disengagement<br />

40<br />

35<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

Notes:<br />

Defenders, a<br />

n=34 for each group<br />

range = 14-70<br />

Outsiders, c<br />

Victims, c<br />

Assistants, b<br />

a,b; c,d Means with different superscript differ at p < .05.<br />

41<br />

Re<strong>in</strong>forcers,b<br />

Bullies, b,d

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!