20.07.2013 Views

Valency mismatches and the coding of reciprocity in ... - Linguistics

Valency mismatches and the coding of reciprocity in ... - Linguistics

Valency mismatches and the coding of reciprocity in ... - Linguistics

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Valency</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>reciprocity</strong> <strong>in</strong> Australian languages 561<br />

b. Kurdu-jarra-rlu ka-pala-nyanu nya-nyi,<br />

child-du-erg impf-3du.sub-rr see-nspt<br />

karri-nja-karra-rlu.<br />

st<strong>and</strong>-<strong>in</strong>f-refl.subj.comp<br />

‘The two children see each o<strong>the</strong>r (as <strong>the</strong>y are) st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.’ 23 (Hale<br />

1982: 295, also cited <strong>in</strong> Simpson 1991: 165)<br />

The Warlpiri <strong>and</strong> Warluwarra examples, <strong>the</strong>n, clearly illustrate <strong>the</strong> possibility<br />

that languages will encode reciprocal constructions with exactly <strong>the</strong> same argument<br />

structure as <strong>the</strong>ir correspond<strong>in</strong>g non-reciprocal clauses. We now turn<br />

to Australian examples represent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> opposite pole – comparable to <strong>the</strong><br />

Mundari example <strong>in</strong> (3) – where languages effect an unambiguous reduction <strong>in</strong><br />

valency <strong>in</strong> reciprocal clauses.<br />

3.2. <strong>Valency</strong>-reduc<strong>in</strong>g strategies<br />

3.2.1. Basic transitive verbs. Many languages respond to <strong>the</strong> “equation” <strong>of</strong><br />

subject <strong>and</strong> object arguments by fus<strong>in</strong>g antecedent <strong>and</strong> reciprocator <strong>in</strong>to a s<strong>in</strong>gle<br />

argument position, reduc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> clause’s valency by one. Applied to a basic<br />

transitive verb, this yields an <strong>in</strong>transitive verb <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> reciprocal. In this section<br />

we will only consider <strong>the</strong> situation with base transitive verbs, hold<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong>f <strong>the</strong><br />

discussion <strong>of</strong> what happens with base ditransitives until Section 5.<br />

Unambiguous reduction <strong>of</strong> valency <strong>in</strong> reciprocals is claimed for <strong>the</strong> majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> Australian languages, typically accompanied by verbal affixation. 24 Two<br />

tests for valency reduction are usually employed:<br />

(i) <strong>the</strong> overt appearance <strong>of</strong> maximally one argument;<br />

(ii) <strong>the</strong> employment <strong>of</strong> a case frame, or a form <strong>of</strong> pronom<strong>in</strong>al affix, associated<br />

with <strong>in</strong>transitive ra<strong>the</strong>r than transitive constructions. That is, <strong>in</strong>stead<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>ative-accusative, ergative-absolutive, or ergative-accusative<br />

case frames found <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> transitive construction, <strong>the</strong>re is a s<strong>in</strong>gle NP<br />

23. We thank Mary Laughren for supply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong>se sentences, which are <strong>the</strong> reciprocal versions <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g reflexive counterpart, published <strong>in</strong> Hale (1982: 295) <strong>and</strong> Simpson (1991: 165):<br />

(i) Kurdu-ngku ka-Ø-nyanu nya-nyi,<br />

child-erg impf-3sg.sub-rr see-npst<br />

‘The child sees himself st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.’<br />

karri-nja-kurra.<br />

st<strong>and</strong>-<strong>in</strong>f-objcomp<br />

24. Dixon (2002: 520) gives a figure <strong>of</strong> about 105 Australian languages, out <strong>of</strong> 140 surveyed,<br />

<strong>in</strong> which reciprocal constructions are encoded by verbal affix (which may or may not be <strong>the</strong><br />

same as <strong>the</strong> reflexive) with accompany<strong>in</strong>g reduction <strong>in</strong> valency, although it is possible that<br />

some <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> languages <strong>in</strong> this figure may <strong>in</strong> fact exhibit mixed transitivity behaviour.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!