24.07.2013 Views

The Effects of Computer-Mediated Communication on Foreign ...

The Effects of Computer-Mediated Communication on Foreign ...

The Effects of Computer-Mediated Communication on Foreign ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Foreign</strong><br />

Language Anxiety in Heritage and N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage Students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Spanish: A Preliminary Investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

MICHAEL TALLON<br />

University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Incarnate Word<br />

F<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this study was to examine if heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish experience<br />

foreign language anxiety in a sec<strong>on</strong>d-semester Spanish class and, if so, compare that with<br />

the levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety found in n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study also<br />

examined whether computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> had an effect <strong>on</strong> anxiety levels,<br />

both for heritage students and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. A questi<strong>on</strong>naire assessing foreign<br />

language anxiety was administered as a pretest and posttest. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were three groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

students: traditi<strong>on</strong>al class/face-to-face c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s (c<strong>on</strong>trol group), small groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> (experimental group A), and <strong>on</strong>e large<br />

group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> (experimental group B. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group had regular face-to-face discussi<strong>on</strong>s in the classroom. Experimental groups<br />

A and B had electr<strong>on</strong>ic, asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous discussi<strong>on</strong>s using BlackBoard. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> findings show<br />

that heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish experienced less foreign language anxiety than n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

students. Furthermore, the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> can reduce<br />

learners‟ anxiety levels. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediated<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> group did not seem to affect the overall anxiety scores, although the large<br />

group did cause heritage students to experience higher levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety than n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students.<br />

oreign language teachers have l<strong>on</strong>g sought new and better ways to help their students<br />

learn the foreign/sec<strong>on</strong>d language. One area that has provided much excitement is the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

computers and, specifically, computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> (CMC). Research has shown<br />

that CMC can provide many benefits to the sec<strong>on</strong>d language learner. One area that has not been<br />

investigated much yet is the ability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC to reduce anxiety, although there are a few studies<br />

available (see Arnold, 2002; Kern, 1995; Warschauer, 1996; Sullivan & Pratt, 1996; Beauvois,<br />

1995, 1999).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this study is tw<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>old: to determine if heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish<br />

experience foreign language anxiety, and to determine the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computermediated<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> (ACMC) <strong>on</strong> the anxiety levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> heritage and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Spanish. This study is significant because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the increasing number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> heritage students<br />

enrolling in first-year Spanish courses. Because they are different from the traditi<strong>on</strong>al foreign<br />

language student – and thus have different needs – it is important to investigate how teachers can<br />

attend to their affective needs as well. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Literature secti<strong>on</strong> will discuss<br />

computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong>, asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong>, foreign<br />

language anxiety and computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong>, and definiti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

TPFLE Vol.13, No.1, Fall/Winter 2009 39


Spanish. This will be followed by a descripti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the research methodology and a presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the results and their implicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> has been heralded as providing several potential<br />

benefits to the foreign language classroom. It has been suggested that CMC encourages learners<br />

to engage in meaningful interacti<strong>on</strong>s and authentic exchanges in the target language (Chun,<br />

1994; Warschauer, 1996; Warschauer & Meskill, 2000; Blake, 2000; van Lier, 2000; Stockwell<br />

& Harringt<strong>on</strong>, 2003; Pica, 1987; Ellis, 1999). Warschauer (1997) c<strong>on</strong>cludes that “the textmediati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

view links the c<strong>on</strong>cepts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressi<strong>on</strong>, interacti<strong>on</strong>, reflecti<strong>on</strong>, problem-solving,<br />

critical thinking, and literacy with the various uses <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> talk, text, inquiry and collaborati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

classroom” (p. 472). Blake (2000) adds: “Providing students with increased opportunities to<br />

engage in negotiati<strong>on</strong>s…could direct language teachers to accord CMC a more expanded role in<br />

the L2 curriculum” (p. 132).<br />

A sec<strong>on</strong>d benefit <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC is more equal participati<strong>on</strong> by students. Warschauer (1996)<br />

compared face-to-face discussi<strong>on</strong>s and electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong>s and found a tendency toward more<br />

equal participati<strong>on</strong> in the computer mode, with “the overall participati<strong>on</strong> rate twice as equal in<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong> as in face-to-face discussi<strong>on</strong>” (p. 20). Bump (1990), Beauvois (1998,<br />

1999), Kelm (1992) and Kim (1998) also report more student participati<strong>on</strong> using CMC. Sullivan<br />

& Pratt (1996) report a 100% participati<strong>on</strong> rate for a CMC class versus 50% for an oral class.<br />

Warschauer (1997), in his review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the CMC literature, states that a greater amount <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> using CMC has been found according to three measures: (1) percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student<br />

talk versus teacher talk, (2) directi<strong>on</strong>al focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student talk (toward other students or toward the<br />

teacher), and (3) equality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> student participati<strong>on</strong>. Warschauer, Turbee, & Roberts (1996)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cluded that computer learning networks have the potential to empower students by promoting<br />

student aut<strong>on</strong>omy, increasing classroom equality, and helping students develop a critical learning<br />

perspective.<br />

Some studies have shown that students who <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten feel marginalized, or students who<br />

might be shier, in traditi<strong>on</strong>al classroom discussi<strong>on</strong>s become more active in a networked<br />

classroom. Sullivan (1998) illustrated how computer exchanges can promote the self-esteem <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

minority students and build a discourse community within their compositi<strong>on</strong> class. Sullivan<br />

found more participati<strong>on</strong> by minorities and c<strong>on</strong>cluded that they were able to define themselves<br />

by their communicative skills. Warschauer (1996) also reported increased participati<strong>on</strong> via<br />

CMC for certain nati<strong>on</strong>ality groups (Chinese, Japanese, and Vietnamese). Markley (1998)<br />

reports that cultural backgrounds have an effect <strong>on</strong> participati<strong>on</strong> in the classroom and that CMC<br />

can help equalize the participati<strong>on</strong>; in his study, Asians and women participated more via CMC<br />

than in the traditi<strong>on</strong>al classroom.<br />

Another benefit is the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more sophisticated language in CMC. Chun (1994) found<br />

that learners who participated in computer-assisted classroom discussi<strong>on</strong>s (CACD) performed a<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different interacti<strong>on</strong>al speech acts (including initiating and resp<strong>on</strong>ding to simple<br />

statements, asking and answering questi<strong>on</strong>s, and using greetings/farewells) and took the initiative<br />

more than they did in the normal classroom. Chun suggests that the written competence gained<br />

from CACD can gradually be transferred to the students‟ spoken discourse competence. Other<br />

2 40<br />

TPFLE


studies have also found a greater level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sophisticati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g students who used networked<br />

computers for class discussi<strong>on</strong>s (Kern 1995, Beauvois 1998). Warschauer (1996) c<strong>on</strong>cludes that<br />

the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> his study “suggest that electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong> can be a good envir<strong>on</strong>ment for<br />

fostering use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> more formal and complex language, both lexically and syntactically” (p.22).<br />

Beauvois (1998) c<strong>on</strong>cludes that electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong>s show favorable results in the areas <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

quantity, quality, and greater student participati<strong>on</strong> and, therefore, that “the slowing down <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

communicative process seems to bridge the gap between oral and written communicati<strong>on</strong> for a<br />

number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students, allowing them to benefit more fully from the language learning process”<br />

(213).<br />

Asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediate communicati<strong>on</strong> (ACMC) refers to communicati<strong>on</strong> that<br />

does not take place in real time (as opposed to synchr<strong>on</strong>ous computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> -<br />

SCMC). Examples <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ACMC include e-mail and posting messages to an electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

board. Stockwell & Harringt<strong>on</strong> (2003) note several advantages <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> e-mail interacti<strong>on</strong>: (1)<br />

learners have more time to focus <strong>on</strong> the linguistic cues; (2) learners have more time to<br />

comprehend and then resp<strong>on</strong>d; and (3) learners may feel less anxiety than is comm<strong>on</strong> in face-t<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ace<br />

settings “where c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> saving face can heighten learner anxiety” (p. 351).<br />

Kro<strong>on</strong>enberg (1994/1995) states that e-mail “encourages students to use computers in realistic<br />

situati<strong>on</strong>s so that they can develop communicative and thinking skills. Even the technologically<br />

phobic language teacher can become adept at engaging students to use e-mail in skill<br />

development” (p. 24).<br />

Some researchers have found that the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the group can have an effect <strong>on</strong> CMC,<br />

possibly leading to anxiety. For example, Beauvois (1995) found that students expressed<br />

frustrati<strong>on</strong> at trying to keep up with discussi<strong>on</strong> with a large number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students and preferred to<br />

form their own small discussi<strong>on</strong> groups <strong>on</strong> the network. Other researchers (e.g., Bump, 1990;<br />

Colomb & Simutis, 1996; Kelm, 1992) noted that <strong>on</strong>e disadvantage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC is that with large<br />

groups the c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> can produce a large body <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> text, thus making it difficult to keep up with<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>. Warschauer (1997) refers to this as “informati<strong>on</strong> overload,” as participants<br />

become overwhelmed with the number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> messages. One student in Bump‟s (1990) study stated<br />

that “it gets frustrating sometimes when a [chat room c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>] gets really busy and you<br />

would have no time to type anything in if you worried about reading absolutely everything” (p.<br />

61). To overcome such problems, Beauvois (1992) recommends having many small<br />

c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s and Bump (1990) recommends limiting the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> each group to no more than<br />

four or five people.<br />

<strong>Foreign</strong> Language Anxiety<br />

Several researchers have suggested that the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC has the potential to reduce<br />

anxiety associated with learning a foreign/sec<strong>on</strong>d language. Anxiety can be defined as “the<br />

subjective feeling <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tensi<strong>on</strong>, apprehensi<strong>on</strong>, nervousness, and worry associated with an arousal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the aut<strong>on</strong>omic nervous system” (Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope, 1986, p. 125; from Spielberger<br />

1983). “<strong>Foreign</strong> Language Anxiety,” first proposed by Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope (1986), can be<br />

defined as “a distinct complex <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> self-percepti<strong>on</strong>s, beliefs, feelings, and behaviors related to<br />

classroom language learning arising from the uniqueness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the language learning process” (p.<br />

128) and is made up <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong> apprehensi<strong>on</strong>, test anxiety, and fear <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> negative<br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong> (Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope, 1986).<br />

Most researchers accept the idea that foreign language anxiety is a situati<strong>on</strong>-specific<br />

anxiety related to the language learning c<strong>on</strong>text and that it can play a significant causal role in<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 41


creating individual differences in language learning. Horwitz (2001) notes that several studies<br />

have found foreign language anxiety to be largely independent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety.<br />

Some researchers believe that CMC can create an envir<strong>on</strong>ment in which foreign language<br />

anxiety is reduced. Kro<strong>on</strong>enberg (1994/1995) states that “the most timid language students can<br />

come alive while creating meaningful communicati<strong>on</strong> via the keyboard and screen” (p. 24).<br />

Kern (1995) reported that most students enjoyed CMC, with some evidence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reduced anxiety.<br />

Warschauer (1996) states that students reported “that they did not feel stress during electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

discussi<strong>on</strong>” (p. 16). Sullivan & Pratt (1996) state: “Hypothetically, the networked classroom<br />

would <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fer the less pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient speaker more time to think about what to „say,‟ thus reducing<br />

anxiety and the probability <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> error” (p. 492).<br />

In a descriptive study <strong>on</strong> students‟ attitudes and motivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> E-talk, Beauvois (1995)<br />

reports that over 70% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the students felt that the computer lab was much less anxiety-provoking<br />

than the regular classroom and 73% indicated that they would like to spend more time working<br />

in the lab. Based <strong>on</strong> 76 student interviews from all levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> classes over a three-year period,<br />

Beauvois (1999) reports that student resp<strong>on</strong>ses to CMC were unanimously positive <strong>on</strong> the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> stress/anxiety; students also commented <strong>on</strong> the social aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC and how it<br />

creates a sense <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> community and a socially supported envir<strong>on</strong>ment. Beauvois c<strong>on</strong>cludes:<br />

“What we do know now is that…we can use networked computers in our classrooms without<br />

activating anxiety in our students and without inhibiting their participati<strong>on</strong>” (p. 162).<br />

G<strong>on</strong>zález-Bueno & Pérez (2001) explored the significance <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the overall effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> using<br />

e-mail in the quantity and accuracy <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish written language generated by the electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

media through dialogue journals compared to the paper-and-pencil versi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the technique.<br />

Qualitative analysis revealed that a majority (86%) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the experimental students believed the<br />

assignment had improved their attitude towards the language, and half <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them felt that the email<br />

journal had some advantages over its paper-and-pencil counterpart (including learning to<br />

communicate, self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring, using technology appropriately, finding the assignment fun); <strong>on</strong><br />

the other hand, <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e third <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the c<strong>on</strong>trol subjects claimed that this practice was<br />

communicative, creative, meaningful and <strong>on</strong>e that produced low anxiety.<br />

Arnold (2002) investigated the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between CMC and foreign language anxiety<br />

in five secti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> third-semester German classes (N=56) with SCMC, ACMC, and a traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

face-to-face class. She found that students‟ self-ratings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their anxiety and self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence<br />

levels during each discussi<strong>on</strong> did not change significantly between the three treatment groups<br />

(although 29% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the students experienced moderate levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety and 19% experienced high<br />

levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety); all groups generally enjoyed the group discussi<strong>on</strong>s, although the e-mail group<br />

(ACMC) provided some negative feedback (due to the lag time); in all three treatment groups,<br />

students displayed significantly lower levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety after the study (although modality did not<br />

have any effect <strong>on</strong> these changes). Arnold suggests using e-mail exchanges for writing practice<br />

instead <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

In a later study, Arnold (2007) investigated the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between CMC and<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> apprehensi<strong>on</strong> (<strong>on</strong>e aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the foreign language anxiety c<strong>on</strong>struct proposed by<br />

Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope, 1986). Data from pretest and posttest questi<strong>on</strong>naires showed no<br />

significant differences in a reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicati<strong>on</strong> apprehensi<strong>on</strong> between the c<strong>on</strong>trol and<br />

experimental groups.<br />

Perez (2003) found that students who used ACMC (e-mail in this study) “had more time<br />

to think and elaborate while writing their weekly email message; c<strong>on</strong>sequently, average students<br />

2 42<br />

TPFLE


felt more relaxed during this activity” (p. 94). She c<strong>on</strong>cluded that both modes <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC (i.e.,<br />

ACMC and SCMC) created a n<strong>on</strong>threatening atmosphere and lowered the affective filter.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish students in Lee‟s (2004) study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>line discussi<strong>on</strong>s experienced anxiety<br />

when communicating with native speakers at the beginning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the semester, as they felt that their<br />

limited Spanish did not allow them to fully express their ideas. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y were also afraid to make<br />

mistakes.<br />

Heritage Students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish<br />

A heritage student refers to “a language student who is raised in a home where a n<strong>on</strong>-<br />

English language is spoken, who speaks or at least understands the language, and who is to some<br />

degree bilingual in that language and in English” (Valdés 2001). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re are several reas<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> heritage students, especially heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish. According to Campbell &<br />

Peyt<strong>on</strong> (1997), “Interest in this student populati<strong>on</strong> has been triggered by major demographic<br />

changes in this country…Most heritage language speakers (43 percent) are Hispanic<br />

Americans…This group is now the fastest growing and most diverse populati<strong>on</strong> group in the<br />

United States” (p. 38). C<strong>on</strong>sequently, their enrollments in universities are picking up all over the<br />

country. Fishman (1966) argues that heritage languages are a nati<strong>on</strong>al resource that should be<br />

preserved and encouraged. Van Deusen-Scholl (2003) states: “<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> events <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> September 11,<br />

2001, underscored the critical need for language competence in strategic languages…Given the<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>‟s expressed need for strategic linguistic competence, rethinking the role that heritage<br />

learners can play is advisable” (p. 215).<br />

Draper & Hicks (2000) note that “heritage language learners are different from the<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al foreign language student” (p. 20), and, therefore, “the instructi<strong>on</strong>al goals <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

heritage learner may clash with those <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the program in which they seek instructi<strong>on</strong>” (Van<br />

Deusen-Scholl, 2003, p. 223). This has led researchers to try to develop a theoretical foundati<strong>on</strong><br />

for heritage language instructi<strong>on</strong> (Valdés, 2001).<br />

Webb and Miller (2000) point out that heritage language students are pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>icient in their<br />

language in ways that foreign language learners, and sometimes even their foreign language<br />

teachers, are not and may never be. At the same time, they may have gaps in their knowledge <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

their heritage languages that foreign language learners do not have, and these gaps prevent them<br />

from performing certain kinds <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> communicative tasks, reading and writing in particular. As a<br />

result, when teachers attempt to apply a standard foreign language curriculum and foreign<br />

language teaching strategies to the teaching <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a heritage language, it can lead to discomfort, if<br />

not frustrati<strong>on</strong> and failure, for both students and teachers alike. <strong>Foreign</strong> language learners and<br />

heritage language learners are two different groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students, each with their own needs.<br />

Campbell and Rosenthal (2000) provide several characteristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a “typical” heritage<br />

language learner: (1) they have acquired nearly 90% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ph<strong>on</strong>ological system <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their<br />

ancestral language; (2) they have acquired 80% to 90% <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the grammatical rules; (3) they have<br />

acquired extensive vocabularies, although the semantic range is limited to a few sociocultural<br />

domains; (4) they have typically acquired appropriate sociolinguistic rules; (5) they have learned<br />

and adopted many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the customs, values, and traditi<strong>on</strong>s (i.e., the culture) <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their community; (6)<br />

they rarely have opportunities to gain literacy skills in their ancestral languages; and (7) they<br />

present a wide range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> reas<strong>on</strong>s for wanting to study their ancestral languages. It should be noted<br />

that their proposed hypotheses need to be validated by empirical evidence collected from both<br />

heritage students and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students in future research.<br />

Peyt<strong>on</strong>, Ranard and McGinnis (2001) make the point that heritage language learners also<br />

differ from traditi<strong>on</strong>al foreign language learners in that many heritage language learners are<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 43


dealing with deeply felt issues <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> identity. Krashen (2000) adds: “Heritage language [HL]<br />

speakers are in a no-win situati<strong>on</strong> in foreign language classes. If they do well, it is expected. If<br />

HL speakers do not do well in foreign language classes, the experience is especially painful” (p.<br />

441). This can result in a lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> motivati<strong>on</strong>, low academic achievement, high absenteeism, and<br />

a high percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dropouts.<br />

Roca (2000) points out that many heritage students use Spanish at home and with peers,<br />

but lack formal training in the language and have a limited bilingual range. As a result, their<br />

literacy skills in Spanish (most obviously, their n<strong>on</strong>-standard spelling), their registers,<br />

vocabulary, and general ease and fluency with the language, need attenti<strong>on</strong> and expansi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Gutiérrez (1997) states: “<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language teaching pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>essi<strong>on</strong> should clearly understand<br />

that these students need an approach that builds <strong>on</strong> what they bring to the classroom rather than<br />

<strong>on</strong>e that begins at ground zero” (p. 34).<br />

Heritage Students and <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Anxiety<br />

Although many studies have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety<br />

<strong>on</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d language learners, most <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this research has been d<strong>on</strong>e with the more “traditi<strong>on</strong>al”<br />

foreign language learners. To date little research has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> its effects <strong>on</strong> heritage<br />

students. However, there is some anecdotal evidence. For example, Levine (2003) looked at<br />

student and instructor beliefs and attitudes about target language use, first language use, and<br />

anxiety. He states that the findings in his study suggest that “students who come from bi- or<br />

multilingual backgrounds may tend to feel less anxious about TL [target language] use than<br />

students from m<strong>on</strong>olingual backgrounds” (p. 354). Mejías, Applbaum, Applbaum and Trotter<br />

(1991) found that some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Hispanic students in their study experienced anxiety when<br />

speaking Spanish, especially in more formal, less pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />

Tall<strong>on</strong> (2003) found that the mean anxiety score <strong>on</strong> the <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Classroom<br />

Anxiety Scale (FLCAS) (Horwitz, Horwitz and Cope, 1986) was significantly higher for n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

students (cf. 104.6) than for heritage students (cf. 87.1) in first-semester Spanish classes.<br />

It was noted in that study that the anxiety score for the heritage students was somewhat high<br />

(c<strong>on</strong>sidering that no anxiety is represented by a score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> thirty three). In a later study, Tall<strong>on</strong><br />

(2004) found that the mean anxiety score <strong>on</strong> the FLCAS for heritage students in a sec<strong>on</strong>dsemester<br />

Spanish class was 97.0, c<strong>on</strong>sidered “mildly anxious.” In additi<strong>on</strong>, this study showed<br />

that heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish can also experience other types <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety, including listening<br />

anxiety, reading anxiety, and writing anxiety. Finally, it was suggested that the anxiety could<br />

lead to less language output, both orally and in writing, and both in terms <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> quantity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> language<br />

and quality in language, for the heritage students.<br />

In a qualitative study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> self-assessed anxious heritage and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage learners, Coryell<br />

& Clark (2009) examined how and why some adult learners experienced anxiety while studying<br />

Spanish <strong>on</strong>line. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ir participants experienced anxiety because their previous language learning<br />

experiences enforced the c<strong>on</strong>cept <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> language as performance, with a focus <strong>on</strong> grammatical<br />

correctness and precisi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y c<strong>on</strong>cluded that “the findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this study suggest that<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong>al practices <strong>on</strong>line and a lack <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> social language interacti<strong>on</strong> will likely<br />

underserve anxious, interculturally minded learners” (p. 499), advocating instead for pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

and sociocultural c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>s and intercultural communicati<strong>on</strong>s as the focus <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong>line language<br />

courses.<br />

2 44<br />

TPFLE


RESEARCH QUESTIONS<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> purpose <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this study is to determine if “foreign language anxiety” (Horwitz, Horwitz<br />

& Cope 1986) exists for heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish, and, if so, compare that with the levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

foreign language anxiety found in n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> study will also examine if CMC –<br />

specifically, asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous CMC – has an effect <strong>on</strong> foreign language anxiety. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> following<br />

research questi<strong>on</strong>s will be addressed:<br />

1. Do heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish experience foreign language anxiety in a<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d-semester language classroom? If so, how does their level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign<br />

language anxiety compare to the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety found in<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students?<br />

2. Do students (both heritage students and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students) experience a<br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> in foreign language anxiety by using asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous CMC (ACMC)?<br />

3. If so, does the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ACMC group (i.e., small or large) have an effect <strong>on</strong><br />

the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety?<br />

METHODOLOGY<br />

Participants<br />

Participants came from two secti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> sec<strong>on</strong>d-semester Spanish (Elementary Spanish II)<br />

at a private American university in the southwest. Both classes were taught by the same teacher,<br />

who had been teaching Spanish for approximately ten years. One class had 22 students and the<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d class had 9 students. Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> absences, the final numbers were 19 students in the first<br />

class and 7 students in the sec<strong>on</strong>d class (N=26). All subjects were 18 years or older (13 males<br />

and 13 females; average age = 22.5 years). Based <strong>on</strong> self-assessments, 15 students identified<br />

themselves as heritage students and 11 identified themselves as n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students.<br />

Procedure<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re were three groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects: traditi<strong>on</strong>al class/face-to-face c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s (c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

group; n=7), small groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ACMC (experimental group A; two groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three students each<br />

and two groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two students each; n=10), and <strong>on</strong>e large group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ACMC (experimental group<br />

B; n=9) (see Table 1 for a breakdown <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects). Students were randomly assigned to each<br />

group. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol group participated in regular face-to-face discussi<strong>on</strong>s in the classroom during<br />

the scheduled class period.<br />

Table 1<br />

Subjects<br />

Total Heritage N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage<br />

Students Students Students<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol Group 7 4 3<br />

Experimental Group A 10 6 4<br />

Experimental Group B 9 5 4<br />

TOTAL 26 15 11<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 45


Experimental groups A and B participated in electr<strong>on</strong>ic, asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous discussi<strong>on</strong>s (i.e.,<br />

an e-mail bulletin board) using Blackboard outside <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> class. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> topic for all groups was<br />

activities d<strong>on</strong>e in childhood, focusing <strong>on</strong> the Imperfect (past tense) in Spanish. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> assignment<br />

for the ACMC groups c<strong>on</strong>sisted <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> three parts: <strong>on</strong> the first day (M<strong>on</strong>day), students entered the<br />

forum in Blackboard for their particular group and posted answers to questi<strong>on</strong>s regarding<br />

activities they used to do in their childhood (the questi<strong>on</strong>s came from an activity in their<br />

textbook); <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d day (Wednesday), students resp<strong>on</strong>ded to answers posted by their group<br />

members (e.g., stating why they liked a particular answer, asking for more details, replying with<br />

a similar story <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their own); and <strong>on</strong> the third day (Friday), students voted for their favorite<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se and stated why. All posts were required to be in the target language (Spanish). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

following M<strong>on</strong>day a discussi<strong>on</strong> was held in class about the resp<strong>on</strong>ses (letting every<strong>on</strong>e know<br />

what the most popular answers were for each group and why). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol group did the same<br />

assignments in the classroom, spending about 10-15 minutes each class period to complete the<br />

assignment.<br />

A questi<strong>on</strong>naire assessing foreign language anxiety was administered to both groups as a<br />

pre-test and post-test (from Arnold 2002; see Appendix A for the pre-test questi<strong>on</strong>naire and<br />

Appendices B and C for additi<strong>on</strong>al items <strong>on</strong> the post-test questi<strong>on</strong>naires, <strong>on</strong>e for the<br />

experimental groups and <strong>on</strong>e for the c<strong>on</strong>trol group) 1 . <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> pre-test questi<strong>on</strong>naire was designed to<br />

establish an individual‟s level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety and is an adapted form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the “<strong>Foreign</strong><br />

Language Classroom Anxiety Scale” (FLCAS) from Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope (1986) (items<br />

relating to test anxiety were deleted from the original scale for the adapted scale). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> post-test<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire was designed to assess anxiety levels after the treatment, as well as collect<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> about students‟ reacti<strong>on</strong>s to the treatments (for the experimental groups) and to the<br />

classroom group discussi<strong>on</strong>s (for the c<strong>on</strong>trol group). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> post-test secti<strong>on</strong> based <strong>on</strong> the FLCAS<br />

was identical with the pre-test questi<strong>on</strong>naire. Additi<strong>on</strong>al items pertaining to the group<br />

discussi<strong>on</strong>s were added to the post-test questi<strong>on</strong>naire to evaluate students‟ resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the<br />

treatments (see questi<strong>on</strong>s 20 to 32 <strong>on</strong> the post-test questi<strong>on</strong>naires). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> theoretical range for the<br />

adapted anxiety scale was 33 to 95; the higher the number, the higher the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign<br />

language anxiety. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong>naires were designed to collect both quantitative data (using<br />

likert-scale questi<strong>on</strong>s) as well as qualitative data (using open-ended questi<strong>on</strong>s). Participants also<br />

provided some background informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong>naires.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Research Questi<strong>on</strong> #1: Do heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish experience foreign language anxiety in<br />

a sec<strong>on</strong>d-semester language classroom? If so, how does their level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety<br />

compare to the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> foreign language anxiety found in n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students?<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the pre-test questi<strong>on</strong>naire, the heritage students experienced little foreign<br />

language anxiety, with a mean anxiety score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 42.988 (out <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a range <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 33 to 95) (see Table 2<br />

for anxiety scores). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students experienced a higher level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety, with a<br />

mean score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> 52.833. Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the small sample size, tests for statistical significance were<br />

not appropriate. However, these results c<strong>on</strong>firm the findings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> another study (Tall<strong>on</strong>, 2003) in<br />

which the mean anxiety score <strong>on</strong> the FLCAS was significantly higher for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students<br />

(cf. 104.6) than for heritage students (cf. 87.1) (see Table 3, which compares the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> that<br />

study with the results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> two other studies <strong>on</strong> foreign language anxiety – Horwitz, Horwitz &<br />

2 46<br />

TPFLE


Cope, 1986 and Aida, 1994). It was suggested in that study that perhaps the n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students are more anxious in the foreign language classroom due to the presence <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students who<br />

already know some Spanish.<br />

Table 2<br />

Pre-Test Anxiety Scores<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

95% C<strong>on</strong>fidence Interval<br />

HERITAGE Mean Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound<br />

1 heritage 42.988 4.192 34.294 51.681<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 52.833 4.724 43.037 62.630<br />

Table 3<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Anxiety Studies<br />

Horwitz, Horwitz<br />

& Cope (1986) Aida (1994) Tall<strong>on</strong> (2003)<br />

Sample Size 108 96 101<br />

Heritage = 44 (43.6%)<br />

N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage = 57 (56.4%)<br />

Language Level First year First year First semester<br />

Language Spanish Japanese Spanish<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>bach‟s Alpha .93 .94 .96<br />

Mean 94.5 96.7 96.3<br />

Heritage = 87.1<br />

N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage = 104.6<br />

Standard Deviati<strong>on</strong> 21.4 22.1 26.8<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se results suggest that heritage students do not seem to suffer much general foreign<br />

language anxiety in the foreign language classroom. This is not surprising, as many <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> them<br />

come from a background in which they hear and/or speak the language in the home. On the<br />

other hand, n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students in this study experienced a higher level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety, which is<br />

also not surprising. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> difference in anxiety scores between the two groups is about 10 points.<br />

Because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the small sample size, tests for statistical significance were not d<strong>on</strong>e. However, these<br />

results c<strong>on</strong>firm the findings in Tall<strong>on</strong> (2003), which – based <strong>on</strong> a much larger group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> subjects –<br />

found a statistically significant difference in anxiety scores for heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish and<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students.<br />

Research Questi<strong>on</strong> #2: Do students (both heritage students and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students)<br />

experience a reducti<strong>on</strong> in foreign language anxiety by using asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous CMC (ACMC)?<br />

As Table 4 and Figure 1 illustrate, there was not much change in the anxiety scores in the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol group from the pre-test to the post-test (cf. 47.833 to 46.125). However, there was a drop<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 47


<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> about 5 points in the anxiety score <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the experimental group from pre-test to post-test (cf.<br />

51.406 to 46.278).<br />

Table 4<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

recoded<br />

95% C<strong>on</strong>fidence Interval<br />

group TIME Mean Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound<br />

1.00 C<strong>on</strong>trol 1 Pre 47.833 5.807 35.790 59.877<br />

2.00<br />

Experimental<br />

2 Post 46.125 5.442 34.840 57.410<br />

1 Pre 51.406 3.494 44.160 58.651<br />

2 Post 46.278 3.274 39.489 53.067<br />

Figure 1<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

Estimated Marginal Means<br />

52<br />

51<br />

50<br />

49<br />

48<br />

47<br />

46<br />

45<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

2 48<br />

TPFLE<br />

2<br />

recoded group<br />

C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

Experimental<br />

Is there a reducti<strong>on</strong> in foreign language anxiety for heritage students vs. n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students? Table 5 and Figure 2 dem<strong>on</strong>strate that neither heritage students nor n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students in the c<strong>on</strong>trol group experienced any change in their anxiety scores from pre-test to<br />

post-test. Scores for heritage students went from 40.00 to 38.25, while scores for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students went from 55.67 to 54.00.<br />

However, as shown in Table 5 and Figure 3, there was a difference between heritage<br />

students and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students in the experimental groups. While heritage students in the<br />

experimental groups did not experience much change in anxiety scores from pre-test to post-test<br />

(cf. 47.70 to 46.00), the n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students did experience a change (cf. 55.11 to 46.56). Once<br />

again, the small sample size did not allow for tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> significant differences.


Table 5<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

GROUP2 recoded<br />

group HERITAGE Mean Std. Deviati<strong>on</strong> N<br />

SPRETOT 1.00 C<strong>on</strong>trol 1 heritage 40.0000 7.39369 4<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 55.6667 8.08290 3<br />

Total 46.7143 10.91962 7<br />

2.00 Experimental 1 heritage 47.7000 16.89214 10<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 55.1111 16.67666 9<br />

Total 51.2105 16.75503 19<br />

Total 1 heritage 45.5000 14.93962 14<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 55.2500 14.63573 12<br />

Total 50.0000 15.32580 26<br />

SPOSTTOT 1.00 C<strong>on</strong>trol 1 heritage 38.2500 9.42956 4<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 54.0000 7.93725 3<br />

Total 45.0000 11.67619 7<br />

Estimated Marginal Means<br />

2.00 Experimental 1 heritage 46.0000 16.25491 10<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 46.5556 14.56117 9<br />

Total 46.2632 15.04749 19<br />

Total 1 heritage 43.7857 14.71879 14<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>-heritage 48.4167 13.30385 12<br />

Total 45.9231 14.00264 26<br />

Figure 2<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

At recoded group = C<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 49<br />

2<br />

HERITAGE<br />

heritage<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage


Estimated Marginal Means<br />

Figure 3<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for Experimental Groups<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

56<br />

54<br />

52<br />

50<br />

48<br />

46<br />

44<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

At recoded group = Experimental<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

2 50<br />

TPFLE<br />

2<br />

HERITAGE<br />

heritage<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se results suggest that the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ACMC did seem to reduce the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety for<br />

both heritage and n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students in the experimental groups. Anxiety scores dropped<br />

overall from 51.4 to 46.3 after the treatment. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the ACMC seemed to have more <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an<br />

effect <strong>on</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students than heritage students. Whereas the anxiety score for heritage<br />

students went from 47.7 to 46.0 after using ACMC, it dropped from 55.1 to 45.7 for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students. This is not surprising, as the traditi<strong>on</strong>al foreign language student who experiences<br />

foreign language anxiety in the classroom might experience less anxiety in an envir<strong>on</strong>ment<br />

where face-to-face c<strong>on</strong>tact is avoided and where they are not “put <strong>on</strong> the spot” in fr<strong>on</strong>t <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the<br />

entire class.<br />

In the open-ended questi<strong>on</strong> at the end <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the questi<strong>on</strong>naires, both heritage and n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

students left comments that noted some beneficial aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC, including enjoying<br />

the <strong>on</strong>line assignment and feeling more comfortable. According to some n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students:<br />

“I enjoyed it. Time lag allowed for better preparati<strong>on</strong> and no nervousness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> face 2 face.<br />

Was interesting. A good thing.”<br />

“It was easier to resp<strong>on</strong>d <strong>on</strong> the Blackboard assignment because I had enough time to<br />

formulate a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. I was able to enjoy the assignment due to the time<br />

allotted.”<br />

“I thought it was good. We got to know each other, had time to resp<strong>on</strong>d, and could do it<br />

<strong>on</strong> our own time.”<br />

“<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Blackboard assignment helped me to visualize what I was trying to say to my<br />

group. I would not want to do it all semester but <strong>on</strong>ce or twice would be okay.”<br />

Some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the heritage students also left positive comments <strong>on</strong> the open-ended questi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

“I liked it b/c I had time to prepare a resp<strong>on</strong>se and did not have to do [it] face to face.”


“Yes I enjoyed the work that I did. I found it a lot more comfortable and easier.”<br />

“I enjoyed the Blackboard assignment because I had time to think about my answers.<br />

It‟s good practice.”<br />

Research Questi<strong>on</strong> #3: Does the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ACMC group (i.e., small or large) have an effect <strong>on</strong><br />

the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety?<br />

Table 6 shows the anxiety scores from pre-test to post-test for the c<strong>on</strong>trol group and both<br />

experimental groups. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> c<strong>on</strong>trol group did not show any changes (cf. 47.83 to 46.13).<br />

However, both experimental groups – the small groups and the large group – experienced about a<br />

five-point reducti<strong>on</strong> in their anxiety scores from pre-test to post-test. This is shown visually in<br />

Figure 4.<br />

Table 6<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

recoded<br />

95% C<strong>on</strong>fidence Interval<br />

group TIME Mean Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound<br />

1.00 c<strong>on</strong>trol 1 Pre 47.833 5.951 35.419 60.248<br />

2.00 small<br />

group<br />

3.00 large<br />

group<br />

2 Post 46.125 5.596 34.452 57.798<br />

1 Pre 51.167 5.030 40.675 61.659<br />

2 Post 46.208 4.729 36.343 56.073<br />

1 Pre 52.775 5.227 41.871 63.679<br />

2 Post 47.325 4.915 37.073 57.577<br />

Figure 4<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

Estimated Marginal Means<br />

54<br />

52<br />

50<br />

48<br />

46<br />

44<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 51<br />

2<br />

recoded group<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

small group<br />

large group


Table 7 and Figures 5, 6, and 7 show this breakdown for heritage students and n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

students. Heritage students in all three groups experienced a minimal reducti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

anxiety scores from pre-test to post-test (cf. 40.00 to38.25 for the c<strong>on</strong>trol group, 44.33 to 43.17<br />

for the ACMC small groups, and 52.75 to 50.25 for the ACMC large group). N<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students in the c<strong>on</strong>trol group also experienced minimal reducti<strong>on</strong> in anxiety scores from pre-test<br />

to post-test (cf. 55.67 to 54.00). However, n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students in both experimental groups<br />

experienced an 8 to 9 point reducti<strong>on</strong> in anxiety (cf. 58.00 to 49.25 for the ACMC small groups<br />

and 52.80 to 44.40 for the ACMC large group).<br />

Table 7<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group and Experimental Groups<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

95% C<strong>on</strong>fidence Interval<br />

HERITAGE recoded group TIME Mean Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound<br />

1 heritage 1.00 c<strong>on</strong>trol 1 Pre 40.000 7.792 23.746 56.254<br />

2 n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

Estimated Marginal Means<br />

2.00 small<br />

group<br />

3.00 large<br />

group<br />

2 Post 38.250 7.327 22.967 53.533<br />

1 Pre 44.333 6.362 31.062 57.605<br />

2 Post 43.167 5.982 30.688 55.645<br />

1 Pre 52.750 7.792 36.496 69.004<br />

2 Post 50.250 7.327 34.967 65.533<br />

1.00 c<strong>on</strong>trol 1 Pre 55.667 8.998 36.898 74.436<br />

2.00 small<br />

group<br />

3.00 large<br />

group<br />

2 Post 54.000 8.460 36.353 71.647<br />

1 Pre 58.000 7.792 41.746 74.254<br />

2 Post 49.250 7.327 33.967 64.533<br />

1 Pre 52.800 6.970 38.262 67.338<br />

2 Post 44.400 6.553 30.731 58.069<br />

Figure 5<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

At recoded group = c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

2 52<br />

TPFLE<br />

2<br />

HERITAGE<br />

heritage<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage


Estimated Marginal Means<br />

Estimated Marginal Means<br />

Figure 6<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for Experimental Small Groups<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

At recoded group = small group<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 53<br />

2<br />

HERITAGE<br />

heritage<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

Figure 7<br />

Pre- and Post-test Anxiety Scores for Experimental Large Group<br />

(Heritage vs. N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage)<br />

54<br />

52<br />

50<br />

48<br />

46<br />

44<br />

42<br />

Estimated Marginal Means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> MEASURE_1<br />

At recoded group = large group<br />

1<br />

TIME<br />

2<br />

HERITAGE<br />

heritage<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

Overall the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ACMC group did not have an effect <strong>on</strong> anxiety. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety<br />

scores for both the small ACMC groups and the large ACMC group dropped by about five points<br />

from pre-test to post-test. However, in both groups the anxiety scores dropped more for n<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

students than it did for heritage students. In the small ACMC groups the anxiety score<br />

dropped by about nine points (whereas it <strong>on</strong>ly dropped <strong>on</strong>e point for heritage students) and in the<br />

large group the anxiety score dropped about eight points (whereas it <strong>on</strong>ly dropped about three<br />

points for heritage students).<br />

One very interesting finding was that the level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety after the ACMC treatment was<br />

higher for heritage students (cf. 50.25) than for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students (cf. 44.40) in the large<br />

experimental group. This was the <strong>on</strong>ly instance in this study when the heritage students<br />

experienced a higher level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety than the n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. One reas<strong>on</strong> may be that<br />

some <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the heritage students in this study lacked appropriate literacy skills in Spanish (which is


very comm<strong>on</strong> for heritage students, as they usually have not received any formal training in the<br />

heritage language; see Fishman, 2001; Valdés, 2001; Campbell & Rosenthal, 2000; Klee, 1998;<br />

Quintanar-Sarellana, Huebner & Jensen, 1993) and this may have caused their anxiety level to be<br />

higher than that found in the n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. That is, the heritage students may have been<br />

anxious because they were worried that their writing skills were not that good and perhaps they<br />

were being evaluated by their peers. 2 This would be the opposite <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> what they experience in the<br />

classroom, where they are using the two skills with which they usually have extensive practice:<br />

listening and speaking.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> present study c<strong>on</strong>firms the noti<strong>on</strong> that heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish experience less<br />

foreign language anxiety than n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students. Furthermore, it suggests that the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

computer-mediated communicati<strong>on</strong> can reduce learners‟ anxiety levels. This reducti<strong>on</strong> in<br />

anxiety seems to occur more for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage students, which is what is expected. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

the ACMC group did not seem to affect the overall anxiety scores, although the large ACMC<br />

group did cause heritage students to experience higher levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety than n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students.<br />

It is important to note the limitati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> this preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong>. First <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all, the<br />

sample size was small and therefore did not allow for tests <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> statistical significance. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results<br />

are suggestive, but a larger group size needs to be examined for statistical significance. Sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>venience sample was used and thus the intent was not generalizati<strong>on</strong>. Third, this study <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

looked at <strong>on</strong>e level <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong> (sec<strong>on</strong>d semester). Finally, the treatment was fairly short (<strong>on</strong>ly<br />

lasting <strong>on</strong>e week).<br />

This study provides several implicati<strong>on</strong>s for teaching. First <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> all, as pointed out in<br />

Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope (1986), teachers need to help students learn to cope with foreign<br />

language anxiety and also need to try to make the learning c<strong>on</strong>text less stressful. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> results <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

this study suggest that the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC can help with this task, especially for n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

students. It is also suggested that teachers work more <strong>on</strong> literacy skills with heritage students.<br />

Arnold (2002) recommends the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> e-mail for writing practice, which would be perfect for<br />

heritage students who need to focus <strong>on</strong> their literacy skills.<br />

This study also provides several areas for future research. First, it is necessary to<br />

replicate this study with a larger sample size and with a l<strong>on</strong>ger treatment period (e.g., using<br />

ACMC throughout an entire semester). Sec<strong>on</strong>d, anxiety should be investigated in these groups<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> students at different levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> instructi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., beginning students vs. intermediate students vs.<br />

advanced learners). It may be that the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC is more appropriate for more advanced<br />

students. Third, other areas <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anxiety should be examined (e.g., reading anxiety, writing<br />

anxiety), as well as how CMC can affect these areas. Finally, including a qualitative approach<br />

(e.g., informal interviews with students) would probably provide rich data input as to what<br />

students actually think about foreign language anxiety and the use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> CMC in the foreign<br />

language classroom.<br />

2 54<br />

TPFLE


ABOUT THE AUTHOR:<br />

Michael Tall<strong>on</strong> (Ph.D., University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Texas at Austin) is an Assistant Pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>essor <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish at the<br />

University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the Incarnate Word in San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, Texas. His research interests include foreign<br />

language anxiety, heritage students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish, Spanish as a heritage language, and the Spanish<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> San Ant<strong>on</strong>io.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Aida, Y. (1994). Examinati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope‟s C<strong>on</strong>struct <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Foreign</strong> Language<br />

Anxiety: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Case <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Japanese. Modern Language Journal, 78, 155-168.<br />

Arnold, M.N. (2002). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>: Writing to Speak without <strong>Foreign</strong><br />

Language Anxiety? Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>; University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Texas, Austin.<br />

Arnold, N. (2007). Reducing <strong>Foreign</strong> Language <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> Apprehensi<strong>on</strong> with <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>: A Preliminary Study. System, 35(4), 469-486.<br />

Beauvois, M.H. (1992). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-assisted Classroom Discussi<strong>on</strong> in the <strong>Foreign</strong> Language<br />

Classroom: C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> in Slow Moti<strong>on</strong>. <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Annals, 25, 455-464.<br />

Beauvois, M.H. (1995). E-Talk: Attitudes and Motivati<strong>on</strong> in <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-Assisted Classroom<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong>. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>s and the Humanities, 28, 177-190.<br />

Beauvois, M.H. (1998). C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s in Slow Moti<strong>on</strong>: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> in<br />

the <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Classroom. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Canadian Modern Language Review, 54(2), 198-<br />

217.<br />

Beauvois, M. (1999). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>: Reducing Anxiety and Building<br />

Community. In D. Young (Ed.), Affect in <strong>Foreign</strong> Language and Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language<br />

Learning: A Practical Guide to Creating a Low-Anxiety Classroom Atmosphere (pp.<br />

144-165). Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA: McGraw-Hill College.<br />

Blake, R. (2000). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>: A Window <strong>on</strong> L2 Spanish<br />

Interlanguage. Language Learning & Technology, 4(1), 120-136.<br />

Bump, J. (1990). Radical Changes in Class Discussi<strong>on</strong> Using Networked <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>s.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>s and the Humanities, 24(1), 49-65.<br />

Campbell, R., & Peyt<strong>on</strong>, J.K. (1997). Heritage Language Students: A Valuable Language<br />

Resource. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> ERIC Review, 5(3), 38.<br />

Campbell, R., & Rosenthal, J. (2000). Heritage Languages. In J.W. Rosenthal (Ed.), Handbook<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Undergraduate Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> (pp. 165-184). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum Associates, Inc.<br />

Chun, D.M. (1994). Using <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g> Networking to Facilitate the Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Interactive<br />

Competence. System, 22(1), 17-31.<br />

Colomb, G., & Simutis, J.A. (1996). Visible C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> and Academic Inquiry: CMC in a<br />

Culturally Diverse Classroom. In S. Herring (Ed.), <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-mediated <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>:<br />

Linguistic, Social and Cross-cultural Perspectives (pp. 203-222). Amsterdam:<br />

Benjamins.<br />

Coryell, J.E., & Clark, M.C. (2009). One Right Way, Intercultural Participati<strong>on</strong>, and Language<br />

Learning Anxiety: A Qualitative Analysis <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Adult Online Heritage and N<strong>on</strong>heritage<br />

Language Learners. <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Annals, 42(3), 483-504.<br />

Draper, J., & Hicks, J. (2000). Where We‟ve Been; What We‟ve Learned. In J. Webb & B.<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 55


Miller (Eds.), Teaching Heritage Language Learners: Voices from the Classroom (pp.<br />

15-35). Y<strong>on</strong>kers, NY: ACTFL.<br />

Ellis, R. (1999). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>oretical Perspectives <strong>on</strong> Interacti<strong>on</strong> and Language Learning. In R. Ellis<br />

(Ed.), Learning a Language Through Interacti<strong>on</strong> (pp. 3-31). Philadelphia, PA: J<br />

Benjamins.<br />

Fishman, J.A. (1966). Language Loyalty in the United States. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Hague, Netherlands:<br />

Mout<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Fishman, J.A. (2001). 300-Plus Years <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Heritage Language Educati<strong>on</strong> in the United States. In<br />

J.K. Peyt<strong>on</strong>, D.A. Ranard & S. McGinnis (Eds.), Heritage Languages in America:<br />

Preserving a Nati<strong>on</strong>al Resource (pp. 81-89). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Center for Applied<br />

Linguistics.<br />

G<strong>on</strong>zález-Bueno, M., & Pérez, L. (2001). Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Mail in <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Learning<br />

Revisited. Paper presented at the 2001 c<strong>on</strong>ference <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Teachers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English to Speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Other Languages. ERIC Document #ED458817.<br />

Gutiérrez, J.R. (1997). Teaching Spanish as a heritage language: A case for language awareness.<br />

ADFL Bulletin, 29(1), 33-36.<br />

Horwitz, E.K. (1986). Preliminary Evidence for the Reliability and Validity <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a <strong>Foreign</strong><br />

Language Anxiety Scale. TESOL Quarterly, 20, 559-562.<br />

Horwitz, E.K. (2001). Language Anxiety and Achievement. Annual Review <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Applied<br />

Linguistics, 21, 112-126.<br />

Horwitz, E.K., Horwitz, M.B., & Cope, J. (1986). <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Classroom Anxiety.<br />

Modern Language Journal, 70, 125-132.<br />

Kelm, O. (1992). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Use <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Synchr<strong>on</strong>ous <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g> Networks in Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Instructi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A Preliminary Report. <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Annals, 25(5), 441-454.<br />

Kern, R.G. (1995). Restructuring Classroom Interacti<strong>on</strong> with Networked <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>s:<br />

Efects <strong>on</strong> Quantity and Characteristics <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Language Producti<strong>on</strong>. Modern Language<br />

Journal, 79(4), 457-476.<br />

Kim, Y.S. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Effect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a Networked <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> Discussi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Subsequent<br />

Oral Discussi<strong>on</strong> in the ESL Classroom. Doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong>; University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Texas,<br />

Austin.<br />

Klee, C. (1998). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> as an Organizing Principle in the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Standards:<br />

Sociolinguistic Aspects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish Language Teaching. Hispania, 81, 339-351.<br />

Krashen, S. (2000). Bilingual educati<strong>on</strong>, the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> English, and the retenti<strong>on</strong> and loss <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Spanish. In A. Roca (Ed.), Research <strong>on</strong> Spanish in the U.S. (pp. 432-444). Somerville,<br />

MA: Cascadilla Press.<br />

Kro<strong>on</strong>enberg, N. (1994/1995). Developing Communicative and Thinking Skills Via Electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

Mail. TESOL Journal, 4, 24-27.<br />

Lee, L. (2004). Learners‟ Perspectives <strong>on</strong> Networked Collaborative Interacti<strong>on</strong> with Native<br />

Speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish in the US. Language Learning & Technology, 8(1), 83-100.<br />

Levine, G. (2003). Student and Instructor Beliefs and Attitudes about Target Language Use,<br />

First Language Use, and Anxiety: Report <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a Questi<strong>on</strong>naire Study. Modern Language<br />

Journal, 87, 343-364.<br />

Markley, P. (1998). Empowering Students: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Diverse Roles <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Asians and Women in the<br />

ESL Classroom. In J. Swaffar, S. Romano, P. Markley, & K. Arens (Eds.), Language<br />

Learning Online (pp. 81-96). Austin, TX: Labyrinth Publicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Mejias, H., Applbaum, R.L., Applbaum, S.J., & Trotter, R.T. (1991). Oral <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

2 56<br />

TPFLE


Apprehensi<strong>on</strong> and Hispanics: An Explorati<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Oral <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> Apprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

am<strong>on</strong>g Mexican American Students in Texas. In E.K. Horwitz & D.J. Young (Eds.),<br />

Language Anxiety: From <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory and Research to Classroom Implicati<strong>on</strong>s (pp. 87-97).<br />

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc.<br />

Pica, T. (1987). Sec<strong>on</strong>d-Language Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, Social Interacti<strong>on</strong>, and the Classroom. Applied<br />

Linguistics, 8(1), 3-21.<br />

Perez, L.C. (2003). <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Productivity in Synchr<strong>on</strong>ous Versus Asynchr<strong>on</strong>ous<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-mediated <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>. CALICO Journal, 21(1), 89-104.<br />

Peyt<strong>on</strong>, J.K., Ranard, D.A., & McGinnis, S. (2001). Charting a new course: Heritage language<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> in the United States. In J.K. Peyt<strong>on</strong>, D.A. Ranard & S. McGinnis (Eds.),<br />

Heritage Languages in America: Preserving a Nati<strong>on</strong>al Resource (pp. 3-26).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Center for Applied Linguistics.<br />

Quintanar-Sarellana, R., Huebner, T., & Jensen, A. (1993). Tapping a Natural Resource:<br />

Language Minority Students as <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Tutors. In B. Merino, H. Trueba, &<br />

F. Samaniego (Eds.), Language and Culture in Learning: Teaching Spanish to Native<br />

Speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish (pp. 208-221). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Falmer Press.<br />

Roca, A. (2000). Heritage learners <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish. In G. Guntermann (Ed.), Teaching Spanish with<br />

the five C’s: A blueprint for success (pp. 91-106). AATSP Pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>essi<strong>on</strong>al Development<br />

Series Handbook for Teachers K-16, Vol. 2. Mas<strong>on</strong>, OH: Thoms<strong>on</strong>-Heinle.<br />

Spielberger, C. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Palo Alto, CA:<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sulting Psychologists Press.<br />

Stockwell, G., & Harringt<strong>on</strong>, M. (2003). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Incidental Development <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> L2 Pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency in<br />

NS=NNS Email Interacti<strong>on</strong>s. CALICO Journal, 20(2), 337-359.<br />

Sullivan, N. (1998). Developing Critical Reading and Writing Skills: Empowering Minority<br />

Students in a Networked <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g> Classroom. In J. Swaffar, S. Romano, P. Markley,<br />

& K. Arens (Eds.), Language Learning Online (pp. 41-56). Austin, TX: Labyrinth<br />

Publicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Sullivan, N., & Pratt, E. (1996). A Comparative Study <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Two ESL Writing Envir<strong>on</strong>ments:<br />

A <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-Assisted Classroom and a Traditi<strong>on</strong>al Oral Classroom. System, 24(4),<br />

491-501.<br />

Tall<strong>on</strong>, M. (2003). Heritage Speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish and <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Anxiety: A Pilot Study.<br />

Unpublished manuscript; University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Texas, Austin.<br />

Tall<strong>on</strong>, M. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Effects</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>Foreign</strong> Language Anxiety <strong>on</strong> Heritage Students <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish:<br />

A Preliminary Investigati<strong>on</strong>. Unpublished manuscript; University <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Texas, Austin.<br />

Valdés, G. (2001). Heritage Language Students: Pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iles and Possibilities. In J.K. Peyt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

D.A. Ranard, & S. McGinnis (Eds.), Heritage Languages in America: Preserving a<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>alResource (pp. 37-77). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C.: Center for Applied Linguistics.<br />

Van Deusen-Scholl, N. (2003). Toward a Definiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Heritage Language: Sociopolitical and<br />

Pedagogical C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Journal <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Language, Identity, and Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2(3), 211-<br />

230.<br />

van Lier, L. (2000). From Input to Affordance: Social-Interactive Learning from an Ecological<br />

Perspective. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory and Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Learning<br />

(pp. 245-259). Oxford: Oxford University Press.<br />

Warschauer, M. (1996). Comparing Face-to-Face and Electr<strong>on</strong>ic <str<strong>on</strong>g>Communicati<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> in the Sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

Language Classroom. CALICO Journal, 13, 17-26.<br />

Warschauer, M. (1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g>-<str<strong>on</strong>g>Mediated</str<strong>on</strong>g> Collaborative Learning: <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>ory and Practice.<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 57


Modern Language Journal, 81(4), 470-481.<br />

Warschauer, M, Turbee, L., & Roberts, B. (1996). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Computer</str<strong>on</strong>g> Learning Networks and Student<br />

Empowerment. System, 24(1), 1-14.<br />

Warschauer, M., & Meskill, C. (2000). Technology and Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Teaching. In J.W.<br />

Rosenthal (Ed.), Handbook <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Undergraduate Sec<strong>on</strong>d Language Educati<strong>on</strong> (pp. 303-<br />

318). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Webb, J.B., & Miller, B.L. (2000). Teaching heritage language learners: Voices from the<br />

classroom. Y<strong>on</strong>kers, NY: ACTFL.<br />

2 58<br />

TPFLE


Appendix A<br />

Pre-Test Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

(Adapted from Arnold, 2002, based <strong>on</strong> Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope, 1986)<br />

Group #: ____________<br />

SECTION I:<br />

Please provide some demographic informati<strong>on</strong> about yourself.<br />

1. Gender: _____ Male _____ Female<br />

2. Age: _____<br />

3. Major: _______________________________<br />

4. What grade did you get in your previous Spanish class?<br />

___ A ___ B ___ C ___ D or F ___ N/A<br />

5. Have you ever learned any other foreign language(s) other than Spanish?<br />

_____ No _____ Yes<br />

If so, please specify which other language(s): _______________________<br />

6. Have you ever spent a l<strong>on</strong>ger period <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> time around native speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish where you<br />

spoke Spanish <strong>on</strong> a regular basis?<br />

_____ No _____ Yes<br />

If yes, please specify the length <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> that period: ______________________<br />

7. Do you c<strong>on</strong>sider yourself a heritage speaker/learner <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish or a n<strong>on</strong>-heritage<br />

speaker/learner?<br />

NOTE: For this study, a heritage speaker/learner is defined as some<strong>on</strong>e who grew up in a home<br />

where the Spanish language was spoken; you may not even speak Spanish, but you might<br />

understand it. A n<strong>on</strong>-heritage speaker/learner is defined as some<strong>on</strong>e who did not grow up in a<br />

home where the Spanish language was spoken; you may have studied Spanish before in school,<br />

but you were not around the language while growing up.<br />

_____ Heritage speaker/learner _____ N<strong>on</strong>-Heritage Speaker/Learner<br />

8. _____ If Spanish is your native/first language please check here.<br />

9. How <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten do you use computers?<br />

_____ Daily _____ Several times a week _____ Several times a m<strong>on</strong>th<br />

_____ Rarely _____ Never<br />

10. For what purposes to you use computers? Check all that apply.<br />

_____ School _____ Work _____ Entertainment/Fun<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 59


11. What do you do with computers <strong>on</strong> a regular basis? Check all that apply.<br />

_____ Word Processing _____ E-mail _____ Surfing the Web _____ Chat Rooms<br />

SECTION II:<br />

Directi<strong>on</strong>s: Below is a list <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> statements. Be sure to read them carefully and check the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

that best describes your attitudes and feelings. For each statement, indicate whether you (1)<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly agree - SA, (2) agree - A, (3) neither agree nor disagree - N, (4) disagree - D, or (5)<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree - SD by circling the appropriate number <strong>on</strong> the line following each statement.<br />

Please give your first reacti<strong>on</strong> to each statement and mark an answer for every statement.<br />

Answer with “Neither” if you have no opini<strong>on</strong> or if a statement does not apply to your situati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

1. I never feel quite sure about myself when I am speaking Spanish in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

2. I d<strong>on</strong>’t worry about making mistakes in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

3. I tremble when I know that I‟m going to be called <strong>on</strong> in Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

4. It frightens me when I d<strong>on</strong>‟t understand what the teacher is saying in Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

5. I start to panic when I have to speak Spanish without preparati<strong>on</strong> in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

6. It does not embarrass me to volunteer answers in Spanish in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

7. Even if I am well prepared for my Spanish class, I feel anxious about it.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

8. I feel c<strong>on</strong>fident when I speak Spanish in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

9. I can feel my heart pounding when I‟m going to be called <strong>on</strong> in my Spanish class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

2 60<br />

TPFLE


SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

____________________________________________________________________________<br />

10. I always feel that the other students speak Spanish better than I do.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

11. I feel very self-c<strong>on</strong>scious about speaking Spanish in fr<strong>on</strong>t <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> other students.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

12. I feel more tense and nervous in my Spanish class than in my other classes.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

13. I get nervous and c<strong>on</strong>fused when I am speaking Spanish in my language class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

14. I get nervous when I d<strong>on</strong>‟t understand every word the language teacher says.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

15. I am afraid that the other students will laugh at me when I speak Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

16. I would probably feel comfortable around native speakers <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

17. I get nervous when the language teacher asks questi<strong>on</strong>s which I haven‟t prepared for in<br />

advance.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

18. I feel that I can express myself easily when speaking Spanish in the classroom.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

19. I feel intimidated when speaking Spanish in the classroom.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

20. I feel comfortable working with computers.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

21. I work with computers <strong>on</strong> a regular basis.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 61


Other questi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

22. How <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>ten do you speak Spanish in class (i.e., volunteer answers, c<strong>on</strong>tribute to group<br />

work)? Include your experience from previous/other classes to answer this questi<strong>on</strong>. Check<br />

the answer which is the closest.<br />

_____ 5+ times per class period _____ 2-4 times per class period<br />

_____ Once per class period _____ Hardly ever<br />

23. Which expressi<strong>on</strong> best describes the way you feel when the teacher calls <strong>on</strong> you in Spanish<br />

class?<br />

_____ I panic (e.g., get sweaty hands) _____ I get pretty nervous<br />

_____ I get just a little bit nervous _____ I d<strong>on</strong>‟t mind it _____ I enjoy it<br />

24. Which expressi<strong>on</strong> best describes the way you feel when you are supposed to speak Spanish<br />

with your classmates (e.g., during group work)?<br />

_____ I panic (e.g., get sweaty hands) _____ I get pretty nervous<br />

_____ I get just a little bit nervous _____ I d<strong>on</strong>‟t mind it _____ I enjoy it<br />

25. Which expressi<strong>on</strong> best describes the way you feel during an oral exam?<br />

_____ I panic (e.g., get sweaty hands) _____ I get pretty nervous<br />

_____ I get just a little bit nervous _____ I d<strong>on</strong>‟t mind it _____ I enjoy it<br />

26. Describe in your own words how you feel about speaking Spanish (e.g., why do you feel<br />

that way? What do you worry about the most when speaking?).<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

2 62<br />

TPFLE


Appendix B<br />

Post-Test Questi<strong>on</strong>naire for Experimental Groups<br />

(Additi<strong>on</strong>al Items)<br />

Please tell us about your group electr<strong>on</strong>ic discussi<strong>on</strong>s. Possible answers range from<br />

“Str<strong>on</strong>gly Agree” to “Str<strong>on</strong>g Disagree.” Answer with “Neither” if you have no opini<strong>on</strong> or<br />

if a statement does not apply to your situati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

____________________________________________________________________________<br />

20. I enjoyed the regular computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s with my group.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

21. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s helped improve my oral pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency in Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

22. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s did not improve my self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence about speaking<br />

Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

23. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s lowered my feelings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> nervousness about speaking<br />

Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

24. I feel that I speak more Spanish in class now because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the regular computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

25. I do not think that computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s give me a similar learning opportunity as our<br />

regular Speaking activities in class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

26. Not seeing my discussi<strong>on</strong> partners face-to-face was a negative aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the computer<br />

discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 63


____________________________________________________________________________<br />

27. I found the time lag <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s beneficial because it allowed me to take<br />

more time to understand somebody‟s message than in face-to-face interacti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

28. I found the time lag <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s beneficial because it allowed me to take<br />

more time to compose a resp<strong>on</strong>se than in face-to-face interacti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

29. I felt that the time lag hindered the c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

30. I was able to express myself easily <strong>on</strong> the computer.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

31. I participated more during computer discussi<strong>on</strong>s than when we discussed in class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

32. I did not learn very much from working <strong>on</strong> the computer.<br />

Other questi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

38. Describe in your own words your reacti<strong>on</strong>s to this Blackboard assignment. Did you enjoy<br />

it? Why or why not? Do you think it helped you improve your literacy skills? Why or why not?<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

_____________________________________________________________________________<br />

2 64<br />

TPFLE


Appendix C<br />

Post-Test Questi<strong>on</strong>naire for C<strong>on</strong>trol Group<br />

(Additi<strong>on</strong>al Items)<br />

Please tell us about your group discussi<strong>on</strong>s in class. Possible answers range from “Str<strong>on</strong>gly<br />

Agree” to “Str<strong>on</strong>g Disagree.” Answer with “Neither” if you have no opini<strong>on</strong> or if a<br />

statement does not apply to your situati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

____________________________________________________________________________<br />

20. I enjoyed the regular discussi<strong>on</strong>s with my group.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

21. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular discussi<strong>on</strong>s helped improve my oral pr<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>iciency in Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

22. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular discussi<strong>on</strong>s did not improve my self-c<strong>on</strong>fidence about speaking Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

23. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> regular discussi<strong>on</strong>s lowered my feelings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> nervousness about speaking Spanish.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

24. I feel that I speak more Spanish in class now because <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the regular discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

25. I do not think that the discussi<strong>on</strong>s give me a similar learning opportunity as our regular<br />

speaking activities in class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

26. Seeing my discussi<strong>on</strong> partners face-to-face was a good aspect <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the group discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

27. At times, I would have liked to have more time to understand the other students‟ utterances.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

28. At times, I would have liked to have more time to compose a message.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

SA A N D SD<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANXIETY AND CMC 65


____________________________________________________________________________<br />

29. I do not think that the pace <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the discussi<strong>on</strong> hindered the c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

30. I was able to express myself easily during the group discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

31. I participated more during group discussi<strong>on</strong>s than when we discussed in class.<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

32. I did not learn very much from the group discussi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

NOTES<br />

1 2 3 4 5<br />

1 <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se anxiety scales were taken from Arnold (2002). Permissi<strong>on</strong> to use them in this study was granted by Dr.<br />

Nike Arnold. Since these scales were based <strong>on</strong> the original “<strong>Foreign</strong> Language Classroom Anxiety Scale” from<br />

Horwitz, Horwitz & Cope (1986), permissi<strong>on</strong> was also requested – and granted – from Dr. Elaine K. Horwitz.<br />

2 I would like to thank Dr. Zsuzsanna I. Abrams for this observati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

2 66<br />

TPFLE

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!