26.01.2014 Views

Introduction to Syntax Session 1

Introduction to Syntax Session 1

Introduction to Syntax Session 1

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Introduction</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Syntax</strong><br />

<strong>Session</strong> 1<br />

<strong>Introduction</strong><br />

Cornelia Endriss<br />

Cognitive Science Program<br />

University of Osnabrück


The Idea<br />

… of this course is <strong>to</strong> learn more about our language faculty,<br />

in particular about syntax, i.e. about how sentences are<br />

structured.<br />

To try and make this a seminar that actually features some<br />

discussions about the <strong>to</strong>pics we want <strong>to</strong> deal with, the<br />

sessions will usually fall in<strong>to</strong> two parts:<br />

1. I will introduce the basic syntactic concepts <strong>to</strong> you – based<br />

on the textbook “<strong>Syntax</strong>. A generative introduction”, 2nd<br />

edition 2007 from Andrew Carnie.<br />

2. One group of students will give us a 30min presentation<br />

about a <strong>to</strong>pic that is only briefly <strong>to</strong>uched or not mentioned in<br />

the textbook, and we will discuss this <strong>to</strong>pic in class.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 2


The Plan<br />

• The <strong>to</strong>pics that are discussed in the lecture should be<br />

studied at home by reading the respective chapters in the<br />

textbook and working through the given presentations.<br />

• There will be homework exercises from one week <strong>to</strong> the<br />

other so that you can check whether the concepts are<br />

actually unders<strong>to</strong>od.<br />

• Homework exercises and presentations are group work<br />

with groups of 2 or 3 people. Please choose a presentation<br />

<strong>to</strong>pic and sign up for the group associated with this <strong>to</strong>pic<br />

via StudIP (first come first serve). If you sign up for<br />

“Group x – Topic x”, you are “Group x” and you have <strong>to</strong><br />

present “Topic x” on the date it is scheduled.<br />

• There will be a final written exam.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 3


Requirements<br />

Hence, in order <strong>to</strong> acquire a certificate for this course (4 ECTS<br />

points) you have <strong>to</strong>…<br />

1. attend the seminar sessions regularly and read the<br />

corresponding chapters of the course book<br />

2. hand in the homework assigned regularly and on time<br />

(group work)<br />

3. hold one seminar presentation<br />

(group work)<br />

4. take a short written exam in the end of the course<br />

(check also requirements file in StudIP)<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 4


Homework<br />

• Homework exercises are group work.<br />

• Solutions must be sent <strong>to</strong> me by email before 24:00h on the<br />

Friday following the seminar session (docs or pdfs).<br />

• Each assignment will give you a number of points,<br />

depending on the difficulty and amount of work involved.<br />

The fewer correct assignment you hand in (and hand in in<br />

time – the deadline is strict) the fewer points you will<br />

collect.<br />

• The points you score for the homework tasks accounts for<br />

1/3 of the final grade.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 5


Presentations<br />

• Presentations are group work.<br />

• Presentations should be 30min long, accompanied by<br />

handouts or slides.<br />

• They tackle a <strong>to</strong>pic that is only briefly <strong>to</strong>uched in the<br />

textbook and deal with further exciting syntactic problems.<br />

• The materials have <strong>to</strong> be discussed with me at least one<br />

week before the presentation. The presentations should be<br />

more or less ready when we discuss them. My office hours<br />

are Wednesdays, 2-3pm. But we can also arrange other<br />

appointments via email. I am happy <strong>to</strong> give you any help<br />

you may need.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 6


Presentations<br />

• The final version of the presentation (including suggestions<br />

that have been made during the discussion in class)<br />

materials must be uploaded in StudIP not later than one<br />

week after the presentation. The mark is based on the<br />

presentation plus the quality of the final presentation<br />

materials.<br />

• The points you score for the presentation accounts for 1/3<br />

of the final grade.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 7


Final Exam<br />

• The final exam will cover all <strong>to</strong>pics of the textbook that<br />

have been discussed during the seminar.<br />

• The points you score for the exam accounts for 1/3 of the<br />

final grade.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 8


What is <strong>Syntax</strong>?<br />

• The scientific study of sentence structure.<br />

• The psychological (or cognitive) organization of sentence<br />

structure in the mind.<br />

• Underlying thesis: sentences are generated by a<br />

subconscious set of rules.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 9


Rules<br />

Scientific Method throughout this class<br />

• The task of syntax is <strong>to</strong> find out what these rules look like.<br />

• A group of rules are called a Grammar.<br />

• A grammar in the linguistic sense is a cognitive structure.<br />

It is the part of the mind that generates and understands<br />

language.<br />

• Goal of syntactic theory: <strong>to</strong> model what we subconsciously<br />

know about the syntax of our language.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 10


How do we acquire languages?<br />

• We are not instructed.<br />

• Languages are not taught.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 11


Language is creative<br />

• Language is creative.<br />

The dancing hippo saw the rabbit in the kitchen.<br />

• Human languages are recursive.<br />

• Dena likes horses<br />

• Clarissa knows that [Dena likes horses]<br />

• Anne thinks that [Clarissa knows that [Dena likes horses]]<br />

• Christian said that [Anne thinks that [Clarissa knows that<br />

[Dena likes horses]]]<br />

• etc.<br />

• Language is infinite.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 12


Subconscious knowledge<br />

• We have subconscious knowledge about our<br />

grammar:<br />

a) Who(m) did you say Paul hit ___?<br />

b) Who(m) did you say that Paul hit ___ ?<br />

c) Who did you say ___ hit Bill?<br />

d) *Who did you say that ___ hit Bill?<br />

Wen hat Maria __ angerufen?<br />

Who(m) has Maria ring-up?<br />

‘Who(m) did Maria call?’<br />

*Wen weißt du, dass Maria __ angerufen hat?<br />

Who know you that Maria called has<br />

Example in the<br />

original language<br />

Aligned Gloss:<br />

A word by word<br />

translation<br />

(most useful line)<br />

Loose English Translation:<br />

For information purposes only<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 13


Poverty of stimulus<br />

a) Who(m) did you say Paul hit ___?<br />

b) Who(m) did you say that Paul hit ___ ?<br />

c) Who did you say ___ hit Bill?<br />

d) *Who did you say that ___ hit Bill?<br />

• How does the child know that d) is ungrammatical<br />

if it does not get negative evidence?<br />

• How can it be sure that it has heard the crucial set<br />

of data <strong>to</strong> build up a rule on grounds of this input?<br />

(poverty of stimulus argument)?<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 14


Language as an instinct<br />

• Noam Chomsky<br />

The ability of humans <strong>to</strong> use language is innate,<br />

an instinct. We are prewired <strong>to</strong> use language!<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 15


But Languages Differ!<br />

• How can language be an instinct if languages differ?<br />

• Proposal: Languages differ primarily in terms of what<br />

words are used, and in a set number of “parameters”.<br />

• These things are learned but the rest (the basic<br />

architecture of the grammar) is innate.<br />

• A particular language is not innate, it is acquired, but<br />

the basic <strong>to</strong>ols (principles) that any given language uses<br />

are built in.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 16


Presentation Topic 1<br />

Nature versus Nurture<br />

Chomsky has argued that the ability of humans <strong>to</strong> use language is<br />

innate (an instinct). Most linguists nowadays subscribe <strong>to</strong> that idea.<br />

On this view it is predicted that human languages exhibit<br />

interesting generalizations. It is these generalizations that linguists<br />

investigate.<br />

One of the core arguments for the innateness of the human<br />

language capacity is the poverty of stimulus argument. There is a<br />

very recent debate whether this argument is actually compelling.<br />

Read the following papers of the proponents and opponents of the<br />

linguistic nativism thesis and give a short and clear representation of<br />

their views. Think about your positioning. Which arguments do<br />

you find more convincing?<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 17


Presentation Topic 1<br />

Target article--<br />

• Pullum, G. and B. Scholz (2002). Empirical assessment of stimulus<br />

poverty arguments. The Linguistic Review 19: 8–50. [StudIP]<br />

Reply--<br />

• Crain, S. & P. Pietroski (2002): Why language acquisition is a snap.<br />

The Linguistic Review 19: 163-183 [StudIP]<br />

Additional/Background reading--<br />

• Chomsky, N. (1959): A Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior.<br />

Language 35, No. 1: 26-58. [StudIP]<br />

• Crain, S. & P. Pietroski (2005): Innate Ideas, The Cambridge<br />

Companion <strong>to</strong> Chomsky. Ed. J. McGilvray. CUP. [StudIP]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 18


Parts of Speech<br />

• We will go through Carnie’s book “<strong>Syntax</strong>” chapter by<br />

chaper.<br />

• We start with analyzing the units that sentences are built<br />

upon: words. (Chapter 2)<br />

• Famous example (Groucho Marx):<br />

Time flies like an arrow.<br />

Fruit flies like a banana.<br />

(first flies is a verb, second flies is a noun. First like is a<br />

comparative conjunction, second like is a verb. )<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 19


Constituency and Ambiguity<br />

• We carry on with Constitency, Trees, and Rules. (Chapter 3)<br />

• Ambiguity plays a major role here.<br />

• Sentences can be ambiguous (= have more than one<br />

meaning) for numerous reasons.<br />

• We are concerned with the syntactic reasons for ambiguity<br />

in this class .<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 20


Lexical Ambiguity<br />

• Words can have different meanings and thus cause<br />

ambiguities.<br />

• These lexical ambiguities won’t concern us much.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 21


Lexical Ambiguity<br />

Bitte den<br />

Polizisten<br />

umfahren<br />

(Please the<br />

policeman<br />

UMFAHREN)<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 22


umfahren has two meanings:<br />

Lexical Ambiguity<br />

Bitte den<br />

Polizisten<br />

umfahren<br />

umfáhren<br />

To drive round<br />

úmfahren<br />

To knock over<br />

In written form, umfahren<br />

is ambigous.<br />

Pronounciation<br />

disambiguates.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 23


Lexical Ambiguity<br />

• Context often disambiguates.<br />

Bitte den<br />

Polizisten<br />

umfahren<br />

Versuchter Mord wegen Umfahren eines Polizeibeamten<br />

tried murder due-<strong>to</strong> UMFAHREN of-a policeman<br />

(From a blog for legal handling of traffic accidents)<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 24


Morpho-syntactic Ambiguity<br />

• An Example:<br />

(Chicken<br />

Kebap<br />

Children<br />

Kebap)<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 25


Morpho-syntactic Ambiguity<br />

• Ambiguities can be caused by differing interpretations of<br />

word clusters.<br />

• (Complex) words are internally structured. Different<br />

structuring of these complex words can give rise <strong>to</strong><br />

ambiguities.<br />

• We will mostly ignore these morpho-syntactic ambiguities<br />

in this class.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 26


Syntactic Structure<br />

• Ambiguities that are caused by the underlying structure of a<br />

sentence are referred <strong>to</strong> as syntactic ambiguities.<br />

Woman <strong>to</strong> shop assistent:<br />

„Dürfte ich das blaue Kleid im Schaufenster probieren?“<br />

May I the blue dress in-the shop window try<br />

‚Can I try the blue dress in the shop window?‘<br />

-- „Ja! Aber wir haben auch Umkleidekabinen!“<br />

Yes but we have also fitting rooms<br />

‚Yes, but we also have fitting rooms.‘<br />

• Such ambiguities tell us a lot about the structure of sentences.<br />

<strong>Syntax</strong> theory deals with these kinds of ambiguities.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 27


Parsing Preferences<br />

• Extralinguistic knowledge does not always help.<br />

Der Professor küsste die Frau mit dem Briefbeschwerer.<br />

The professor kissed the woman with the paperweight<br />

(sentence from Matthias Schlesewsky)<br />

• Although world knowledge tells us that the act of kissing<br />

does not take an instrument, the sentence is funny.<br />

• The sentence is interpreted as:<br />

Der Professor [[küsste] [die Frau] [mit dem Briefbeschwerer]]<br />

rather than:<br />

Der Professor [[küsste] [die Frau mit dem Briefbeschwerer]]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 28


Parsing Preferences<br />

• Unexpectedly, the sentence from the last slide is hard <strong>to</strong><br />

understand.<br />

• It needs <strong>to</strong> be explained why we cannot interpret the<br />

sentence in the intended way right away. (It is the only<br />

reading that makes sense!)<br />

• There must be general human parsing strategies that<br />

initially block this interpretation.<br />

• People have made different suggestions on how humans<br />

parse and why some sentences are hard <strong>to</strong> understand,<br />

although they do have entirely unproblematic meanings.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 29


Garden Path Sentences<br />

• Some sentences are so hard <strong>to</strong> understand that the listener<br />

actually seems <strong>to</strong> be led in<strong>to</strong> a garden path when trying <strong>to</strong><br />

interpret them. She is stuck.<br />

Es ist eine Lüge, dass Fritz zugunsten von Maria nie<br />

It is a lie that Fritz in aid of Maris never<br />

etwas unternommen worden wäre.<br />

something done have was<br />

Columbus` Feinde glaubten, dass der Entdecker von Amerika erst<br />

Columbus enemies believed that the discoverer of America yet<br />

im Jahre 1502 erfahren hat.<br />

in-the year 1502 learnt has<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 30


Garden Path Sentences<br />

Peter sagt, dass er der Maria anvertraute streng geheime<br />

Peter says that he the Maria entrusted strongly secret<br />

Gerüchte erfahren hat.<br />

Rumors learnt has<br />

The horse raced past the barn fell.<br />

When Fred eats food gets thrown.<br />

Fat people eat accumulates.<br />

The cot<strong>to</strong>n clothing is usually made of grows in Mississippi.<br />

• Here, there are local ambiguities involved, and the parser seems <strong>to</strong><br />

take the wrong route.<br />

• The listener has difficulties understanding the sentence, as she<br />

builds up the wrong structure.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 31


Presentation Topic 2<br />

Sentence Processing<br />

Many sentences are ambiguous in meaning. However, often<br />

only one meaning is predominant. Sometimes this is even the<br />

one that should be excluded by world knowledge or other<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Furthermore, some sentences are virtually<br />

uninterpretable, although they actually do have a perfect<br />

meaning. We call this effect the garden path effect.<br />

People have argued for certain human parsing strategies that<br />

can be held responsible for these effects.<br />

Read the following papers about human parsing in general<br />

and garden path effects in particular and report the results of<br />

these papers <strong>to</strong> the class.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 32


Presentation Topic 2<br />

Overview Article--<br />

• Altmann, Gerry (1998): Ambiguity in Sentence Processing, Trends<br />

in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 146-152 [StudIP]<br />

Suggestions for human parsing strategies—<br />

• Frazier, L. (1987) Sentence processing: a tu<strong>to</strong>rial review. In M.<br />

Coltheart (Hg.), Attention & Performance XII. Hilsdale,<br />

Erlbaum, 559-586. [Reader, my office]<br />

• Crain, Stephen and Mark Steedman (1985): On not being led up<br />

the garden-path: the use of context by the psychological parser. In D.<br />

Dowty, L. Kartunnen, and A. Zwicky (eds.) Natural Language<br />

Parsing. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 33


Binding Theory<br />

• After setting some terminology (Chapter 4), we carry on<br />

with a very important <strong>to</strong>pic in syntax:<br />

Binding Theory (BT) (Chapter 4).<br />

(Yesterday, it was Paula’s birthday.)<br />

Heidi said that she played basketball.<br />

Heidi said that Heidi played basketball.<br />

Heidi said that Paula played basketball.<br />

Heidi bopped herself on the head with a zucchini.<br />

Heidi bopped Heidi on the head with a zucchini.<br />

#Heidi bopped Paula on the head with a zucchini.<br />

Heidi bopped her on the head with a zucchini.<br />

#Heidi bopped Heidi on the head with a zucchini.<br />

Heidi bopped Paula on the head with a zucchini.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 34


Picture NPs<br />

• NPs of the “Xself”-type are usually in complementary<br />

distribution with pronouns such as “him”/”his”/”her”.<br />

• BT explains the distribution of these types of NPs.<br />

• picture-NPs constitute a big challenge <strong>to</strong> BT, as “Xself”-NPs<br />

and pronouns behave abnormally here.<br />

(Paula is happy)<br />

Heidi found a picture of herself.<br />

Heidi found a picture of Heidi.<br />

#Heidi found a picture of Paula.<br />

Heidi found a picture of her.<br />

Heidi found a picture of Heidi.<br />

Heidi found a picture of Paula.<br />

(Paula is happy)<br />

#Heidi found Peter’s picture of herself.<br />

#Heidi found Peter’s picture of Heidi.<br />

#Heidi found Peter’s picture of Paula.<br />

Heidi found Peter’s picture of her.<br />

Heidi found Peter’s picture of Heidi.<br />

Heidi found Peter’s picture of Paula.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 35


Presentation Topic 3<br />

Picture-NPs<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 5 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Read the articles on picture-NPs given on the next slide.<br />

1. Explain why picture- NPs constitute a challenge for<br />

Binding Theory as it is presented in Chapter 5 of the<br />

textbook. Give a short and concise demonstration of your<br />

results.<br />

2. Complete challenge problem sets 1 and 2 of the textbook<br />

(pp. ev: 145f/tv: 147f) and present your results in class.<br />

3. Present the solutions that are suggested in the papers<br />

and -- if there is still time -- explain the experimental<br />

methods used and the results.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 36


Presentation Topic 3<br />

Picture-NPs--<br />

• Asudeh, Ash and Frank Keller. Experimental evidence for a<br />

predication-based Binding Theory. In Mary Andronis,<br />

Chris<strong>to</strong>pher Ball, Heidi Els<strong>to</strong>n, and Sylvain Neuvel, eds.,<br />

CLS 37: The main session. Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic<br />

Society. 1-14. [StudIP]<br />

• Runner, J.T., Sussman, R.S. and Tanenhaus, M.K. (2003).<br />

Assignment of reference <strong>to</strong> reflexives and pronouns in picture<br />

noun phrases: Evidence from eye movements. Cognition 89.1,<br />

B1-B13. [StudIP]<br />

Background reading—<br />

• Chapter 4 of Haegemann, L., 1991. <strong>Introduction</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

Government and Binding Theory. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell.<br />

[Library]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 37


X-bar Theory<br />

• We now discuss X-bar Theory (Chapters 6, 7 & 8), which<br />

gives us a general format for the generative rules we work<br />

with.<br />

• It constrains the form that our rules may have.<br />

• E.g. XP d YP X’ ok; XP d AP BP CP excluded.<br />

• It simplifies the system of rules and captures the crosscategorial<br />

generalizations.<br />

• This sets the stage for the discussion of further empiric<br />

phenomena.<br />

• But X-bar theory as such can still produce sentences that<br />

are NOT well formed.<br />

• It over-generates (generates ungrammatical sentences).<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 38


• E.g. X-bar theory says that complements are optional so that<br />

the rules can generate the following sentences:<br />

The boy hates sweets.<br />

The boy laughed.<br />

• BUT:<br />

Unconstrained X-bar Theory<br />

*The boy loves.<br />

*The boy laughed the horse.<br />

• Certain verbs require objects, others require that they don’t have<br />

them.<br />

• It depends on the particular verb.<br />

overgenerates<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 39


The lexicon<br />

• The information whether a verb requires an object or not is<br />

s<strong>to</strong>red in the lexicon.<br />

• This is the mental dictionary, the s<strong>to</strong>re of information about<br />

particular words.<br />

• To love:<br />

<strong>to</strong> sleep:<br />

Lover<br />

DP<br />

Beloved<br />

DP<br />

Sleeper<br />

DP<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 40


Expletives<br />

• There are verbs that take no arguments at all.<br />

• E.g. the verb <strong>to</strong> rain.<br />

• There is no “rainer” that makes it rain.<br />

• To rain:<br />

• In English (and German), sentences cannot appear without a<br />

subject though.<br />

Expletive/<br />

• Hence, we say<br />

Pleonastic<br />

It rains.<br />

Instead of simply:<br />

Rains.<br />

This is something that needs<br />

<strong>to</strong> be explained!<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 41


Expletives<br />

Presentation Topic 4<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 8 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Expletives or pleonastics are usually explained by the need of<br />

sentences <strong>to</strong> feature subjects. It is hence predicted that they only<br />

appear in subject position. However, there are sentences that seem<br />

<strong>to</strong> suggest <strong>to</strong> the contrary.<br />

I regretted it that he was late.<br />

Here, the it seems superflous and is certainly not a necessary<br />

argument <strong>to</strong> regret. It feels like an expletive.<br />

Read Rothstein‘s paper about pleonastics and list the reasons for<br />

and against treating it in object positions as in the example above<br />

as expletives.<br />

Present Rothstein‘s view and think about your own view.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 42


Presentation Topic 4<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

• Rothstein, S. (1995) Pleonastics and the interpretation of<br />

pronouns, Linguistic Inquiry 26:499-529. [Reader, my office]<br />

Background reading—<br />

• Postal. P. and G. Pullum. (1988). Expletive noun phrases in<br />

subcategorized positions, Linguistic Inquiry 19:633-670.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 43


Movement<br />

• Inspection of new data shows us that we need a new rule<br />

type, i.e. Movement Rules (Chapters 9-12).<br />

• These movement rules can account for word order<br />

variations in different languages.<br />

• It is assumed that there is a D-structure that sentences start<br />

out with, from which we can derive the surface structure<br />

that we actually observe.<br />

• When the verb is moved from some place <strong>to</strong> another, we<br />

refer <strong>to</strong> this as head-movement.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 44


Word Order<br />

• Languages exhibit different word orders.<br />

• SVO: e.g. English<br />

Peter loves Mary.<br />

• SOV: e.g. Turkish<br />

Hasan kitab-i oku-du.<br />

Hasan-subj book-object read-past<br />

‘Hasan reads the book.’<br />

• VSO: e.g. Irish<br />

Phóg Máire an lucharachán<br />

Kissed Mary the leprechaun<br />

‘Mary kissed the leprechaun.’<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 45


Presentation Topic 5<br />

VSO Languages<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 9 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Different languages have different prevailing word orders.<br />

Irish is one of the around 9% of the languages in the world<br />

that features a VSO word order.<br />

It is particularly interesting whether there is a VP (verb +<br />

object) constituent in such languages.<br />

Read the articles given on the next slide and present the<br />

arguments for and against the existence of a VP constituent<br />

in such sentences. Furthermore, give an outline of<br />

McCloskey’s view and his arguments.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 46


Presentation Topic 5<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

• McCloskey, James (1983): A VP in a VSO language. In<br />

Gerald Gazdar, Geoff Pullam and Ivan Sag (eds.), Order<br />

Concord and Constituency. Foris, Dordrecht.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

Background reading—<br />

• Carnie, Andrew (2004): Flat Structure, Phrasal Variability<br />

and Non-Verbal Predication in Irish. Journal of Celtic<br />

Linguistics 9. [StudIP]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 47


• With regards <strong>to</strong> word order, German is a particularly<br />

intriguing case.<br />

• At first, it seems that German is an SVO language like<br />

English.<br />

Clarissa mag Pferde.<br />

Clarissa likes horses<br />

Word Order<br />

• However, when looking at embedded structures, English<br />

and German differ.<br />

*Dena bedauert, dass Clarissa mag Pferde.<br />

Dena regrets that Clarissa likes horses<br />

Dena bedauert, dass Clarissa Pferde mag.<br />

Dena regrets that Clarissa horses likes<br />

Clarissa likes horses.<br />

Dena regrets that<br />

Clarissa likes horses.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 48


Presentation Topic 6<br />

Word Order in German<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 9 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

German shows SVO word order in matrix clauses, but<br />

(usually) SOV in embedded clauses.<br />

So what is the underlying word order of German, SVO or<br />

SOV? How can this strange behavior be explained?<br />

Read the articles given on the next slide and present the<br />

results <strong>to</strong> the class.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 49


Presentation Topic 6<br />

Papers for presentation--<br />

• Koster, Jan (1973): Dutch as an SOV Language, Linguistic<br />

Analysis 1, 111-136.<br />

[http://www.dbnl.org/tekst/kost006dutc01/kost006dutc01_001.htm]<br />

• Bach, E., (1962) The order of elements in a transformational<br />

grammar of German. Language 38, 263-269.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 50


Embedded Clauses in German<br />

• As pointed out before, embedded sentences in German<br />

usually exhibit the SOV word order.<br />

• Dena glaubt, dass Clarissa Pferde mag.<br />

Dena believes that Clarissa horses likes<br />

• However, sometimes we can embed SVO sentences.<br />

Dena glaubt, Clarissa mag Pferde.<br />

Dena believes Clarissa likes horses<br />

• Interestingly, embedding SVO sentences is not always possible.<br />

*Dena bedauert, Clarissa mag Pferde.<br />

Dena regrets Clarissa likes horses<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 51


Embedded Clauses in German<br />

• Predicates that allow for SVO embedding:<br />

believe, know, say, announce, hope, …<br />

• Predicates that don’t allow for SVO embedding:<br />

regret, doubt, want, …<br />

• This strange property of German has <strong>to</strong> be explained.<br />

• People have made different suggestions <strong>to</strong> the problem, but<br />

the question is still under debate.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 52


Presentation Topic 7<br />

Embedded Clauses in German<br />

Note that this is a <strong>to</strong>pic concerning the syntax-semantics interface. The<br />

task requires some knowledge in formal semantics (Intro <strong>to</strong> Semantics or<br />

equivalent skills)!<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 9 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Only some German verbs allow for the embedding of SVO<br />

sentences. It has been suggested that there is a semantic<br />

reason for this. Movement from V <strong>to</strong> C, which is held<br />

responsible for SVO word order, can be associated with<br />

certain semantic effects.<br />

Read Hubert Truckenbrodt’s article given on the next slide<br />

and present the results <strong>to</strong> the class. Please do abstract from<br />

the formal details of his approach!<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 53


Presentation Topic 7<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

• Truckenbrodt, Hubert (2006): On the semantic motivation of<br />

syntactic verb movement <strong>to</strong> C in German. Theoretical<br />

Linguistics 32.3, edited by Hans-Martin Gärtner. [StudIP]<br />

Background Reading<br />

(not obliga<strong>to</strong>ry as paper is written in German)--<br />

• Reis, Marga (1997): Zum syntaktischen Status unselbständiger<br />

Verbzweit-Sätze. In C. Dürscheid and K.-J. Ramers (eds.),<br />

Sprache im Fokus. Festschrift für Heinz Vater zum 65.<br />

Geburtstag. Tübingen: Niemeyer. 121–144.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 54


DP- and WH-Movement<br />

• Not only verbs can move, but also phrases such as the man or<br />

who (cf. Chapters 10-12).<br />

Who(m) did you say Paul hit ___?<br />

Who(m) did you say that Paul hit ___ ?<br />

• In English wh-phrases have <strong>to</strong> move <strong>to</strong> the front.<br />

• In case of embedded questions they move <strong>to</strong> the front of the<br />

embedded clause.<br />

I know who(m) Paul hit ___.<br />

• Embedded questions are not introduced by complementizers.<br />

*I know who(m) that Paul hit ___.<br />

I know that Paul hit Bill.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 55


WH-Movement in German<br />

• The same holds for German.<br />

Wen hat Maria __ angerufen?<br />

Who(m) has Maria ring-up?<br />

‘Who(m) did Maria call?’<br />

• Embedded question:<br />

Ich weiß, wen Maria ___ angerufen hat?<br />

*Ich weiß, wen dass die Maria ___ angerufen hat.<br />

Ich weiß, dass die Maria den Peter angerufen hat.<br />

who<br />

• So far it seems that wh-words and complementizers are in<br />

complementary distribution.<br />

• (Very naïve) explana<strong>to</strong>ry guess: They target the same position.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 56<br />

that


WH-Movement in Bavarian<br />

• But there are languages/dialects that actually allow for<br />

complementizers and fronted wh-words at the same time.<br />

• Bavarian German is such a language.<br />

I woaß ned wos dass –ma <strong>to</strong>a soin.<br />

I know not what that -we do should<br />

‘I don’t know what we should do.’<br />

I woaß ned wann dass da Xaver kummt.<br />

I know not when that the Xaver comes<br />

‘I don’t know when Xaver will come.’<br />

• This shows clearly that wh-phrases actually do not move <strong>to</strong><br />

the same position as complementizers.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 57


Presentation Topic 8<br />

COMP in Bavarian <strong>Syntax</strong><br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 11 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Bavarian German allows for both a wh-phrase and an overt<br />

complementizer. This tells us a lot about wh-movement in<br />

general and the structure of German sentences in particular.<br />

Read Joseph Bayer’s article and present the core ideas.<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

•Bayer, Joseph (1984), COMP in Bavarian syntax. The<br />

Linguistic Review 3: 209-274. [Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 58


Empirical Evidence for Traces<br />

• In order <strong>to</strong> give substance <strong>to</strong> the movement assumption,<br />

people have tried <strong>to</strong> give arguments that support the idea<br />

that the surface structure of a sentence is derived via<br />

movement opera<strong>to</strong>ns.<br />

• One very famous (and wildly debated) argument has <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong><br />

with wanna-contractions in English<br />

want + <strong>to</strong> ⇒ wanna<br />

Who do you wanna kiss __?<br />

Who do you want <strong>to</strong> kiss __ ?<br />

*Who do you wanna kiss the horse?<br />

Who do you want __ <strong>to</strong> kiss the horse?<br />

intervenes, so blocks wanna<br />

contraction<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 59


Presentation Topic 9<br />

Wanna-contraction in English<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 11 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Read the grey box on p. 324 (ev: 322) and explain in detail<br />

why wanna-contractions can be taken <strong>to</strong> constitute an<br />

argument for the movement hypothesis and the existence of<br />

traces. Take also in<strong>to</strong> account the lecture notes of William<br />

Snyder and Susi Wurmbrand.<br />

Then read the notes of Grant Goodall and the paper of<br />

Geffrey Pullum and present the counterarguments for taking<br />

wanna-constructions as evidence for movement approaches.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 60


Presentation Topic 9<br />

Pullum’s Paper--<br />

• Pullum, G. (1997): The morpholexical nature of English <strong>to</strong>contraction,<br />

Language 73, 79-102. [Reader, my office]<br />

Lecture Notes--<br />

• Of Grant Goodall on contraction. [StudIP]<br />

• Of William Snyder and Susi Wurmbrand on wannacontraction.<br />

[StudIP]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 61


Little vP<br />

• There is good reason <strong>to</strong> believe that what appear <strong>to</strong> be<br />

morphologically simple verbs in English may in fact be<br />

morphologically complex.<br />

Ryan cleans the window.<br />

vP<br />

VP<br />

V<br />

clean<br />

V’<br />

DP<br />

the window<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 62<br />

v’<br />

v<br />

CAUSE<br />

VP<br />

V<br />

clean<br />

V’<br />

DP<br />

the window


Crosslinguistic Evidence<br />

for Little vP<br />

• In Japanese for example, certain verbs are realized as<br />

morphologically complex, although their English<br />

correspondents are simple verbs.<br />

Keiko-wa pizza- ag- e -ta.<br />

Keiko-<strong>to</strong>p pizza-acc rise-v(CAUSE)-past<br />

‚Keiko raised the pizza.‘ (Lit.: Keiko made the pizza rise.)<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 63


Semantic Evidence for Little vP<br />

• There is also semantic evidence for the complex VP<br />

analysis, brought up by Arnim von Stechow.<br />

1. Repetitive:<br />

John closed the window again.<br />

Hans closed the window AGAIN.<br />

Hans schloss wieder das Fenster.<br />

Hans had closed the window before.<br />

2. Restitutive:<br />

Hans CLOSED the window again.<br />

Hans schloss das Fenster wieder.<br />

The window had been close at some stage before.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 64


Presentation Topic 10<br />

Little vP and the semantics of again<br />

Note that this is a <strong>to</strong>pic concerning the syntax-semantics interface. The<br />

task requires some knowledge in formal semantics (Intro <strong>to</strong> Semantics or<br />

equivalent skills)!<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 13 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

What appears <strong>to</strong> be a simple verb in German is often realized<br />

as morphologically complex in other languages. This has led<br />

<strong>to</strong> an analysis that splits the VP-domain in<strong>to</strong> vP and VP.<br />

There is also semantic evidence for this kind of analysis,<br />

brought up by Arnim von Stechow, concerning the possible<br />

interpretations of sentences containing again.<br />

Read von Stechow’s paper, present the core ideas and explain<br />

in what respects it strengthens the complex VP analysis.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 65


Presentation Topic 10<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

• von Stechow, Arnim (1996): The Different Readings of Wieder<br />

'Again': A Structural Account, Journal of Semantics 13, 87-<br />

138. [StudIP]<br />

Background Reading--<br />

• Kratzer, A. (1996). Severing the external argument from its<br />

verb. In J. Rooryk and L. Zaring, (eds.), Phrase structure<br />

and the lexicon, Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 109–137.<br />

[Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 66


Raising and Control<br />

• Raising and control structures look strikingly alike at first<br />

sight.<br />

Dena is likely <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

Dena is reluctant <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

Dena wants Brian <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

Dena persuaded Brian <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

Raising<br />

Control<br />

Raising<br />

Control<br />

• Yet, they have <strong>to</strong> be kept apart.<br />

• There is good reason <strong>to</strong> ascribe rather different structures <strong>to</strong><br />

these constructions.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 67


Raising and Control<br />

• One can distinguish these constructions via several tests.<br />

• Here is one, the expletive test.<br />

• Raising:<br />

Dena is likely <strong>to</strong> leave. ⇒ It is likely that Dena will leave.<br />

Dena wants Brian <strong>to</strong> leave. ⇒ ? Dena wants it that Brian leaves.<br />

• Control:<br />

Dena is reluctant <strong>to</strong> leave. ⇒ *It is reluctant that Dena will<br />

leave.<br />

Dena persuaded Brian <strong>to</strong> leave. ⇒ *Dena persuaded it that Brian<br />

leaves.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 68


Dena is reluctant <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

⇒ that who leaves? Dena!<br />

⇒ subject control<br />

Control<br />

Dena persuaded Brian <strong>to</strong> leave.<br />

⇒ that who leaves? Brian!<br />

⇒ object control<br />

• The question of what can be controlled by what in which<br />

configurations is an important <strong>to</strong>pic in syntax.<br />

Robert knows that it is essential <strong>to</strong> be well behaved.<br />

(1. that one is well behaved in general, 2. that he is well behaved)<br />

• We speak of Control Theory (cf. Binding Theory).<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 69


Presentation Topic 11<br />

Control<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 14 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Control Theory is an important <strong>to</strong>pic in syntax.<br />

Read Manzini’s paper on control and give an outline of the<br />

author’s theory.<br />

Paper for presentation--<br />

Manzini, M. R. (1983). On control and control theory. Linguistic<br />

Inquiry 14: 421-446. [Reader, my office]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 70


Syntactic Frameworks<br />

• Finally, we will look at two syntactic frameworks that<br />

many people currently work with:<br />

Minimalism and HPSG.<br />

• These frameworks implement the concepts we will<br />

encounter throughout this course in a rather different<br />

manner.<br />

• Minimalism is the latest framework proposal of Chomsky.<br />

• There will (most probably) be a presentation about<br />

Minimalism in January by Mikko Määttä, University of<br />

Helsinki, currently a guest of the University of Osnabrück.<br />

• HPSG has been developed in a different tradition, but<br />

certainly wants <strong>to</strong> cover the same range of <strong>to</strong>pics and<br />

phenomena.<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 71


Presentation Topic 12<br />

HPSG<br />

(You have <strong>to</strong> have read the textbook up until Chapter 14 <strong>to</strong> complete this<br />

task.)<br />

Read Chapter 17 of Carnie’s book “<strong>Syntax</strong>” and present the<br />

core concepts of HPSG. Complete General Problem Set 1 on<br />

p. 470 (ev: p. 468) and present the results <strong>to</strong> the class. You<br />

should consult the textbook of Sag, Wasow & Bender <strong>to</strong> flesh<br />

out your presentation.<br />

Background reading --<br />

Sag, Ivan A., Thomas Wasow, and Emily M. Bender (2003):<br />

Syntactic Theory - A formal introduction. 2nd Edition. Stanford:<br />

CSLI Publications. [Library]<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 72


<strong>Session</strong> Planning<br />

01 Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 16 <strong>Introduction</strong>, Genrative Grammar<br />

Chapter 1 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

02 Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 23 Parts of Speech, Consituency, Trees, Rules<br />

Chapters 2 & 3 of Carnie‘s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

03 Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 30 No class (due <strong>to</strong> conference attendance)<br />

(There will be an additional session in<br />

January instead)<br />

04 November 6 Linguistic Debates in Cognitive Science<br />

Presentation Topic 1: Nature vs. nurture<br />

Presentation Topic 2: Sentence processing<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 73


<strong>Session</strong> Planning<br />

05 November 13 Structural Relations, Binding Theory<br />

Chapters 4 & 5 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

Presentation Topic 3: Picture-NPs<br />

06 November 20 X-bar Theory<br />

Chapter 6 & 7 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

07 November 27 Constraining X-bar Theory: the Lexicon<br />

Chapter 8 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

Presentation Topic 4: Expletives in object positions<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 74


<strong>Session</strong> Planning<br />

08 December 4 Head-<strong>to</strong>-Head Movement<br />

Chapter 9 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

Presentation Topic 5: VSO-languages<br />

09 December 11 Head-<strong>to</strong>-Head Movement Phenomena<br />

Presentation Topic 6: Word order in German<br />

Presentation Topic 7: Embedded V2<br />

10 December 18 DP Movement, Wh-Movement<br />

& A Unified Theory of Movement<br />

Chapters 10, 11 & 12 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 75


<strong>Session</strong> Planning<br />

11 December 25 no class (Christmas Break)<br />

12 January 1 no class (Christmas Break)<br />

13 January 8 Movement Pheomena & Evidence<br />

Presentation Topic 8: COMP in Bavarian <strong>Syntax</strong><br />

Presentation Topic 9: wanna-contraction<br />

13b tba A Syntactic Framework: Minimalism<br />

An introduction <strong>to</strong> minimalism by Mikko Määttä<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 76


<strong>Session</strong> Planning<br />

14 January 15 Expanded VPs<br />

Chapter 13 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

Presentation Topic 10: Little vP and the semantics of again<br />

15 January 22 Raising, Control, and Empty Categories<br />

Chapter 14 of Carnie`s textbook „<strong>Syntax</strong>“<br />

Presentation Topic 11: Control<br />

16 January 29 A Syntactic Framework: HPSG<br />

Presentation Topic 12: HPSG<br />

17 February 5 Final Exam<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 77


Thank you!<br />

27.05.2008 Some slides are adapted from material of A. Carnie 78

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!