30.10.2012 Views

International Balzan Foundation Luigi Luca Cavalli

International Balzan Foundation Luigi Luca Cavalli

International Balzan Foundation Luigi Luca Cavalli

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>International</strong> <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize<br />

for Science of Human Origins<br />

Excerpts from the publications:<br />

<strong>Balzan</strong> Prizes 1999 (enlarged and revised edition, 2009)<br />

Meeting the Challenges of the Future.<br />

A Discussion between “The Two Cultures”<br />

(Leo S. Olschki Editore, 2003)


Revised and enlarged excerpt published on the occasion<br />

of <strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza’s lectio magistralis<br />

held on 7 September 2009,<br />

at the Fondazione Corriere della Sera, Milano.<br />

© 2009, Fondazione Internazionale <strong>Balzan</strong>, Milano [www.balzan.org]


INDEX<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza,<br />

1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Motivation for the Prize and laudatio 5<br />

Acceptance Speech 6<br />

A Comprehensive Outline of My Research 11<br />

The Developments of My Research after the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize (1999-2009) 25<br />

Are there Limits to Knowledge? 29<br />

Biographical and bibliographical data 36<br />

<strong>International</strong> <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> 41


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza,<br />

1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Motivation of the Prize and laudatio<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

For his comprehensive work on human evolution by integrating genetic and<br />

cultural features.<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza is the world’s expert on human genetic diversity and<br />

what it tells us about the phylogenetic tree of human populations.<br />

He realised that an understanding of the evolution of mankind requires the<br />

knowledge of both genetic mechanisms and cultural, especially linguistic, features.<br />

By collecting genes from a great number of different populations and testing<br />

more than a hundred different alleles, as well as by analysing historical, demographic,<br />

linguistic, etc. data, he was able to reconstruct the origin of ancient<br />

migration, creating a model of diffusion of culture in the Neolithic Age.<br />

In his comprehensive works, his genetic investigations of primitive populations,<br />

especially pygmies of Africa (one of the few remaining groups of hunters-gatherers),<br />

played an important role.<br />

Also, the studies of the genetic consequences of technological development,<br />

particularly the spread of agriculture from its area of origin, the Middle-East, to<br />

Europe, were exemplary. All this, in combination with archaeological data, allowed<br />

the reconstruction of a complete tree of human descent, in which genes<br />

and languages go hand in hand, demonstrating that genetic and cultural data<br />

converge in furnishing a convincing explanation of human evolution.<br />

In summing up, <strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza has created a most comprehensive<br />

synthesis on the differentiation of populations by integrating various fields of research<br />

and providing convincing evidence for genetic and cultural co-evolution.<br />

5


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Acceptance Speech<br />

Berne - 16 November, 1999<br />

Mrs. President of the Swiss Confederation,<br />

Members of the <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong>,<br />

Ladies and Gentlemen,<br />

I would like to begin by thanking all my friends, and other people I do not<br />

know, for proposing my name for the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize; the Prize Committee, for<br />

their decision to act on the nomination; and above all, Eugenio <strong>Balzan</strong> and his<br />

daughter Angela Lina <strong>Balzan</strong> for their great generosity to science and culture.<br />

For about the last thirty years I have been living in the United States where I frequently<br />

have cause to admire the large-scale sponsorship that exists there - particularly<br />

in the scientific field - and from which universities and cultural institutions<br />

benefit to a substantial extent. And I often think what a pity it is that there<br />

is so little sponsorship in Italy. One of the reasons, of course, is that Italian legislation<br />

does not particularly encourage generosity of this type, except in special<br />

cases, and then only recently. For this reason, the way of thinking that would encourage<br />

very wealthy people in Italy to donate part of their money to the country’s<br />

scientific, cultural and intellectual progress has never been able to establish<br />

itself there. However, I hope that the generosity of the <strong>Balzan</strong>s will stimulate<br />

more people in Italy to act in a similar manner. Based on what I know of the life<br />

and work of Eugenio <strong>Balzan</strong> and his daughter I feel enormous admiration for<br />

them, and I am sure their memory will be held in great respect by the many others<br />

who have benefited from their generosity in the same way as I have. I would<br />

like, once again, to express my deepest gratitude to those who decided to make<br />

the award to myself and my field of learning.<br />

The <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize has been awarded to me for my research into the origins of<br />

man. The first ten years of my work as a researcher were devoted to the genetics<br />

of bacteria. However, in 1952, in Italy, I remembered the original teachings<br />

of my great mentor in the field of genetics, Professor Adriano Buzzati-Traverso.<br />

I owe to him my interest in the genetics of population and evolution, and I<br />

decided to turn to this interest, and to concentrate it on humans. I feel the<br />

choice of humans was dictated by my university studies, which I completed in<br />

medicine.<br />

6


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

The study of evolution received a solid framework with the introduction of a<br />

mathematical theory which was developed between 1918 and 1950 by three<br />

great scientists, Sir Ronald A. Fisher, J. B. S. Haldane and Sewall Wright. From<br />

1948 to 1950 I had the good fortune to work with Sir Ronald Fisher in Cambridge<br />

and derived great benefit from the relationship. Soon after this, I found<br />

myself asking the question as to whether it would be possible to reconstruct the<br />

evolutionary history of man by analysing the genetic differences between humans<br />

living today. At that time there were not yet sufficient genetic population<br />

data. Knowledge was limited to the frequency distributions of certain blood<br />

groups, including the so-called ABO system used for blood transfusions, and<br />

the famous rhesus system, as well as a few others. So until enough additional scientific<br />

knowledge could be collected to form a critical mass, the time could be<br />

devoted to studying the theory of evolution. At that time, there were two fundamentally<br />

different views as to the significance of the two major evolutionary<br />

factors: natural selection, on the one hand, and on the other, the effects of<br />

chance due to the limited size of populations. This inevitably leads to fluctuations<br />

in the statistical frequency of occurrence of genetic characteristics, and<br />

their effects would accumulate over generations.<br />

There was absolutely no doubt that natural selection played a central role. The<br />

influence of chance had already been especially investigated by one of the fathers<br />

of the mathematical theory of evolution, Sewall Wright, but there were still<br />

no usable empirical data. In passing, I am pleased to be able to remind you that<br />

Sewall Wright was awarded the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize in 1984, having outlived the other<br />

two great scientists by a good many years. The other geneticist who has so far<br />

had the honour of receiving this prize was John Maynard Smith, who was one of<br />

the most brilliant students of J. B. S. Haldane. So we have the interesting coincidence<br />

that of the three geneticists who have been awarded the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize<br />

until now, Sewall Wright was the last of the three great names who was still alive,<br />

and the other two were students of Haldane and Fisher.<br />

I set out to solve the problem of how to measure the effects of chance on genetic<br />

variation by studying a population group in the neighbourhood of the University<br />

of Parma where I was teaching in the nineteen-fifties. The idea was that<br />

it should be easy to predict the effects of chance on the genetic differences between<br />

the various villages, by using demographic data reconstructed on the basis<br />

of church records of births, marriages and deaths going back to the year<br />

7


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

1500. This would permit us to calculate the genetic variation due to chance predicted<br />

theoretically, and compare it with actual observations. This work turned<br />

out successfully thanks to the assistance of two young students: the priest Antonio<br />

Moroni, and Franco Conterio, who are both now professors at Parma, the<br />

former of ecology and the latter of anthropology.<br />

By 1961 enough genetic data and observations from numerous populations in<br />

the world had been collected, and it was possible to make a first analysis of the<br />

evolutionary tree of human populations. Anthony Edwards of Cambridge University<br />

helped me to develop and use methods of reconstructing evolutionary<br />

trees. We performed the necessary heavy calculations with the Olivetti Elea<br />

computer recently purchased by the University of Pavia, to which I had moved<br />

and where I was continuing my research work. We could thus reconstruct the<br />

first evolutionary tree of human populations, whose basic structure is still valid<br />

today. Another working relationship very fruitful for me was with Sir Walter<br />

Bodmer, also a student of R. A. Fisher, with whom we published two books on<br />

human population genetics. One of these, The Genetics of Human Populations,<br />

was re-printed only a few months ago by Dover Publications, almost thirty years<br />

after it was first published.<br />

In the second half of the nineteen-sixties I extended the model of genetic research<br />

I had developed on the population of Parma – as far as this was possible<br />

– to a group of hunters and gatherers in the African bush: the pygmies. This is<br />

one of the few population groups still alive today with a pre-agricultural economy<br />

similar to that of the Palaeolithic period. Professor Marcello Siniscalco, at<br />

that time Professor at Leiden, was very helpful to me in carrying out in his laboratory<br />

the genetic analysis of countless blood samples I had collected in Africa.<br />

The contact with such a totally different culture stimulated me to embark on research<br />

into cultural evolution which I pursued after I moved to Stanford University<br />

in 1971. I began by analysing archaeological data on the spread of agriculture<br />

in Europe in the Neolithic period in collaboration with the archaeologist<br />

Albert Ammerman. Thanks to a mathematical theory developed by R. A. Fisher<br />

in 1937 we were able to measure the rate at which agriculture spread, and show<br />

that it was consistent with the hypothesis that it was the people themselves that<br />

spread, and not the knowledge of agriculture. In other words, it was a diffusion<br />

of farmers, which we called demic, and not of farming. In the second half of the<br />

nineteen-seventies we carried out with Alberto Piazza and Paolo Menozzi, now<br />

8


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

Professors of Human Genetics in Turin, and Ecology in Parma, an analysis of<br />

data on gene frequencies in Europe. This work enabled us to confirm that genetic<br />

and archaeological data are closely correlated and consistent with the idea<br />

that agriculture spread from the Middle East. This was the start of a 14-yearlong<br />

working relationship with Menozzi and Piazza. Thanks to it we extended<br />

these studies of genetic geography and history to the rest of the world, in the<br />

way they are described in the book The History and Geography of Human Genes<br />

published in 1994.<br />

In the nineteen-eighties the first methods of genetic analysis were used which<br />

made it possible to carry out direct investigations on DNA. This revolutionary<br />

development expanded the bandwidth of genetic variation from a few hundred<br />

genes to several tens of thousands, and genetic analysis could be shifted down to<br />

the smallest unit of DNA, the individual nucleotide. It also became possible to<br />

use new methods of collecting genetic data on significant factors in human evolutionary<br />

history, particularly on the spread of modern man beyond the area<br />

from which he originates: Africa. Four years ago, a researcher from my laboratory,<br />

Peter Underhill, together with Peter Oefner, a colleague from Ron Davis’<br />

laboratory, invented a new and particularly efficient method of investigating<br />

DNA differences. This made it possible to find many genetic variations in the Ychromosome.<br />

For a number of reasons this chromosome turned out to be extremely<br />

valuable, even though it had made practically no contribution to evolutionary<br />

research up to that time. Since then, however, we have entered a phase<br />

of great activity and fascinating discoveries.<br />

The explosive growth in knowledge in the last few years has also had an effect<br />

on certain socio-cultural fields of study such as linguistics and onomastics - the<br />

study of names. In connection with my studies of onomastics and blood relationships<br />

I would like to acknowledge the valuable support of Professor Gianna<br />

Zei of Pavia. We have observed a significant similarity between the development<br />

of language and of genes, which is in part due to common evolutionary<br />

mechanisms and historical expansionary processes of populations. With the<br />

studies of the development of language I have had the assistance of Joseph<br />

Greenberg and Merritt Ruhlen of Stanford, and Bill Wang of Berkeley. These<br />

latest studies result in part from thirty years of collaboration with Professor<br />

Marc Feldman of Stanford, with whom we developed a mathematical theory of<br />

cultural heredity. Until now the studies of cultural evolution have not aroused<br />

9


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

much interest among cultural anthropologists, with one important exception:<br />

the joint research on the pygmies with Professor Barry Hewlett of Washington<br />

University. The studies have, however, proved interesting to economists who<br />

find the mathematical approach more appealing.<br />

My research work has frequently led me into areas normally out of bounds to<br />

the natural sciences, and it can therefore promote a greater interchange between<br />

the “Two Cultures”. Multidisciplinarity is central to fields such as mine. On the<br />

other hand, science demands ever-increasing specialisation, because otherwise<br />

there is the danger of superficiality. However, it is always possible to become<br />

multidisciplinary by seeking out good co-workers in other disciplines. I am<br />

pleased I have had the opportunity to mention the most important ones and to<br />

thank them. My research work would not have been possible without them.<br />

10<br />

LUIGI LUCA CAVALLI-SFORZA


A Comprehensive Outline of My Research<br />

(1999 – revised July 2009)<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

by <strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

Professor Emeritus at the School of Medicine, Stanford University<br />

In my thanks I have given a very preliminary history of my research on human<br />

origins, taking the pleasure of naming all the main collaborators who have<br />

helped me. I now wish to give a summary of the main results.<br />

Drift vs. natural selection as factors of evolution and the importance of demography<br />

Jacques Monod published a great book called Chance and Necessity which, given<br />

its contents, could also have been named with the less attractive, but more<br />

descriptive title of “Mutation and Natural Selection”. In the book, chance was<br />

held partly responsible for evolution because of the randomness of mutation,<br />

but in reality it plays an even greater role, because of random genetic drift, that<br />

is the effect of statistical fluctuations of the frequencies of genes, due to population<br />

size, isolation and poverty of migratory exchanges. At the time I started my<br />

research, enthusiasts of selection tended to downplay the importance of drift.<br />

My early research proved that you can predict the extent and importance of<br />

drift by using demographic information of populations, especially their geographic<br />

structure and reciprocal migrations, and that drift is far from negligible.<br />

It is clear that human evolution is extremely favorable for the study of drift, because<br />

demography, which can predict it quantitatively, is especially easy to study<br />

in humans. But demography is also the basis of natural selection, which is the<br />

outcome of differential survival and reproduction. It is not surprising that Darwin<br />

was greatly influenced by Malthus’ insight on the manner of demographic<br />

growth of human populations. It is now clear that the understanding of evolution<br />

is largely based on the study of demography. In his famous book The Genetic<br />

Theory of Natural Selection, R.A. Fisher adopted Lotka’s equation expressing<br />

the rate of population growth to define the method of measurement of<br />

“Darwinian fitness” in natural selection, that is, the hereditary capacity for individual<br />

adaptation in natural selection, and to use its individual variation to predict<br />

the main aspects of evolution by natural selection.<br />

11


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Thanks to the Japanese population geneticist Motoo Kimura, molecular genetics<br />

has played a key role for understanding the major influence of drift in general<br />

evolution. Drift is important also when there is natural selection, but in its absence<br />

– that is for “selectively neutral” traits – it is almost the only factor, with<br />

mutation and migration, of evolutionary change. Many genetic variants discovered<br />

at DNA level are selectively neutral or nearly so. Kimura introduced the<br />

idea of “survival of the luckiest” to emphasize the role of chance as a counterpoint<br />

to natural selection, as defined by “survival of the fittest”.<br />

Population structure and migratory exchanges between populations are extremely<br />

important for understanding human evolution. They are also beginning<br />

to play a major role in medical genetics, for a simple reason: it is becoming clear<br />

that human population “isolates” are very common. These are populations who<br />

have gone through a demographic bottleneck at some stage in their lifetime and<br />

have had limited migratory exchange with their neighbors. They are strongly<br />

subject to drift, and the result is that their genetic epidemiology is quite different<br />

from that of the general human population as a whole. Many hereditary diseases<br />

common elsewhere are rare or absent in some of these isolates, while other<br />

genetic diseases rare or absent in the general population are common.<br />

Text-book examples of such population isolates are French Canadians, Afrikaners,<br />

Ashkenazi Jews, Sardinians and many other smaller populations like that of<br />

the island of Tristan da Cunha, and others. Most hereditary diseases like schizophrenia<br />

or allergies are due to many different genetic causes and are therefore<br />

difficult to study in the general population, but responsible genes are easier to<br />

identify in these isolates. In fact it is likely that a single gene is responsible for all<br />

of a given disease found in an isolated population, or at least it is much easier to<br />

dissect, through the study of an isolated one, a complex causal genetic system of<br />

diseases or other rare traits.<br />

Evolutionary trees<br />

The first approach I tried for reconstructing the history of human evolution was<br />

the use of evolutionary trees. Their application relies on the principle that the<br />

further the separation of two populations, the greater is, in probability, the magnitude<br />

of the genetic difference between them. This requires measurement of a<br />

quantity we have called “genetic distance”. When one examines the genetic distance<br />

between species which have been separated for very long times, tens or<br />

hundreds of millions of years, one usually examines one individual per species,<br />

12


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

and finds that species differ for many single genetic differences. The percentage<br />

of DNA units which happen to be different in two species is related, in a very<br />

simple way, to their evolutionary time separation. But human populations living<br />

today have been separated for a relatively short time. Archaeology shows that<br />

modern humans appeared only a little over one hundred thousand years ago, in<br />

Africa, and spread first to Asia, between 100,000 and 50,000 years ago, and<br />

from South East Asia to Oceania between 60,000 and 40,000 years ago. Europe<br />

was reached about 40,000 years ago from both West Asia and North Africa, and<br />

America from Siberia beginning 15,000 years ago.<br />

Mutation first appears in one individual, and then it spreads to other individuals<br />

only in successive generations, when the individual carrying the mutated type<br />

has several progeny carrying the mutation, either because mutants are favored<br />

by selection and survive easily or have more children, or they are favored by<br />

chance. It usually takes a great number of generations before the mutation is<br />

found in many individuals, and even more for it to replace completely the original<br />

type. Thus between the first appearance of a mutation and the replacement<br />

of the original type there will usually elapse a very long time. During that period<br />

the mutant type is “polymorphic”, i.e. there coexist in the population both the<br />

original (ancestral) type and the mutant one. There are extremely few mutations<br />

that have reached the whole of the human species after its origin in Africa.<br />

In fact, we established that, although the human population has enormous genetic<br />

variation between individuals, 85% of the total human variation is within<br />

single populations, and only 15% between them. We therefore cannot use for<br />

the comparison of different human populations the same measure of genetic<br />

distance useful for comparing different living species, for which an individual<br />

from each species would suffice. For instance, we find that the RH negative type<br />

has a frequency of 50% in some populations, e.g. in the Basque country, a lower<br />

one in England (40%), and 0% in East Asia. Genetic replacement means<br />

transition from 0% to 100%. The difference between Basques and English people<br />

is only 50%-40% = 10% of a complete replacement, and that between<br />

Basques and East Asians 50% . We actually employ slightly more sophisticated<br />

formulas to calculate the genetic distance between two populations, which is, on<br />

average, proportional to the time since the two were separated. It is also essential<br />

to calculate the averages of many genes for these measurements to be valid.<br />

When chance is one of the main factors responsible for change, the only way to<br />

eliminate the uncertainty it causes is to apply the law of large numbers, that is,<br />

use a large number of genetic differences. However, even if we could confine<br />

13


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

ourselves to studying the differences due to natural selection, we must always<br />

take the averages of many characteristics. The populations of which we can most<br />

usefully study separation by means of evolutionary trees are those which are<br />

most widely separated, and genetic separation is strictly proportional to geographical<br />

separation. Geographically close populations are very similar to each<br />

other, generally speaking, because migration is more frequent between neighbors.<br />

Husband and wife tend to be born at short distance from each other. Migration<br />

acts as a genetic homogenizer and tends to suppress genetic differences<br />

caused by natural selection or drift. But when two groups are located at a great<br />

distance, enough to repress genetic exchange between the original population<br />

and the new colony, differentiation will begin and will continue at a more or less<br />

constant rate, largely dictated by drift, and, if there are important environmental<br />

differences, by natural selection. For this reason, evolutionary trees of populations<br />

enable us to reconstruct their history, and are in reasonable agreement<br />

with archaeological observations.<br />

More recently, we reconstructed genetic trees of individuals, especially for genetic<br />

traits which are inherited through one parent only. These are based on mitochondrial<br />

DNA, which is transmitted through mothers only to all children,<br />

or on Y chromosomes DNA, transmitted only from fathers to sons. DNA sequencences<br />

make it possible to reconstruct the genealogies of mutations that<br />

took place over very long periods, and beautifully confirm the evolutionary<br />

trees made on populations, while being much more detailed and significant. In<br />

practice, they reconstruct the individual genealogy starting from a single “Eve”<br />

(through mitochondrial DNA), or from a single “Adam” (through a Y chromosome)<br />

respectively, who lived between one hundred and two hundred thousand<br />

years ago, but not because there was ever a moment in which only one<br />

couple or only one ancestor was alive. At the beginning of our species, there<br />

was probably one tribe of about a thousand people who lived at some place in<br />

Eastern Africa, but for statistical reasons, both for mitochondria and for Y<br />

chromosomes, there is only one ancestor common to all of the mitochondria<br />

and to all of the Y chromosomes present today: it is the only one that has been<br />

reproduced to this day; all the others have not had children, grandchildren or<br />

great-grandchildren who have survived until the present day. For those who<br />

think that there was actually a time when only one pair of individuals lived, as<br />

the Bible says, we must say that our genetic Adam and Eve lived in very separate<br />

times and perhaps places.<br />

14


Genetics and culture<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

I find it useful to call Culture the heritage of knowledge accumulating over generations,<br />

or, we may also say, culture is what we learn from others and affects our<br />

behavior: it is a much more general definition than the one normally used for the<br />

word “culture” by daily newspapers, which seems to concern only films and<br />

novels. Most animals have culture, though it is clear that humans are the most<br />

cultural animals of all. Culture evolves according to rules which are similar to<br />

those of biology, but the substrate is clearly very different, relying on neuronal<br />

states and relationships, rather than on DNA structure. There are cultural<br />

changes which are equivalent to genetic mutations, such as inventions or innovations,<br />

but they are not as random as biological mutations. On the contrary,<br />

they are often directed toward a specific aim. This is a major difference with biological<br />

evolution. Another great difference is that transmission is not confined<br />

to transmission from parents to children, but can take place, and does more often<br />

take place, between unrelated individuals. This makes cultural change much<br />

faster than biological change. However, there are also cultural traits which are,<br />

in evolutionary terms, much more stable. They are often transmitted from parents<br />

to children (what we call “vertical” transmission), and therefore imitate to<br />

some extent biological transmission, that is known to be very stable. Children<br />

are, to a certain extent malleable, but they also go through critical periods in<br />

which they are especially susceptible to learning specific things, e.g. their own<br />

language must be learned within the first three or four years of life. And the majority<br />

of people have difficulty in learning foreign languages after adolescence.<br />

Most cultural transmission is, however, “horizontal”, i.e. not necessarily influenced<br />

by kinship or age difference between transmitter and transmittee. This<br />

makes the acquisition of cultural novelties potentially very fast, and thus fast<br />

cultural evolution favors assimilation.<br />

What is learnt is not always necessarily favorable to survival or reproduction.<br />

Nevertheless, one can consider culture as an adaptive mechanism that developed<br />

out of the combined use of communication, observation of others, and<br />

learning skills. It obviously relies on the presence of neural structures which<br />

make this possible. Cultural change is not necessarily good or bad, though it<br />

contributes in a significant way to determining our behavior, and, therefore, is<br />

subject to natural selection. One can expect that this will continue to keep cultural<br />

change adaptive, on average, even though maladaptive cultural aberrations<br />

like crime, drugs and the like are to some extent inevitable.<br />

15


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

There has been a tendency in some sociobiological circles and their descendants<br />

to consider cultural traits as dominated by biology, but the reverse is becoming<br />

the rule. There is unquestionably a range of new genetic traits which have been<br />

introduced as an answer to cultural changes. Most of them have to do with acquired<br />

cultural traits introduced by new diets or customs, like getting dressed.<br />

The expansion toward northern Asia would have been impossible without<br />

clothing. Whether clothing was determined by the previous loss of hairiness, or<br />

whether it helped loss of hair is difficult to say, but it is likely that hair loss took<br />

place earliert and made clothing necessary. One may also venture that hair was<br />

lost because fire had become a major presence in human life, and hairiness is<br />

dangerous in proximity to fire. Another genetic change having specific racial<br />

consequences is paleness of the skin. That skin became pale was largely a consequence<br />

of a cereal-based diet, which, in dark-skinned people, causes rickets at<br />

northern latitudes. Wheat does not contain vitamin D, but a chemical substance<br />

that is its precursor, which can be converted into vitamin D only by an enzyme<br />

present in the lower layers of the skin. This enzyme, however, must be activated<br />

by ultraviolet rays, which are too scarce in Europe because of its high latitude.<br />

The melanin responsible for the dark color of the skin stops ultraviolet rays,<br />

whereas pale skin allows the formation of vitamin D in a diet based on cereals<br />

and cancels out the highly harmful consequences of rickets.<br />

Another example of the biological consequences of cultural evolution is intolerance<br />

to milk (and tolerance to it). Practically all mammals lose the ability to use<br />

lactose after being weaned, but among many herding peoples, the custom of<br />

drinking milk among adults became widespread, even though this may give rise<br />

to serious cases of lactose intolerance. A mutation that arose among herders in<br />

the Ural Mountains around 6000 years ago abolished the disappearance after<br />

weaning of the enzyme that makes it possible to use lactose, and the carriers of<br />

this mutation are totally lactose tolerant. Today this mutation is found in 90-<br />

95% of Scandinavians, is very frequent in northern Europe, a little less in central<br />

Europe and present in 20-40% of southern Italy.<br />

The heredity of acquired behavioral characteristics through cultural evolution is<br />

a reality that enables characteristics transmitted via cultural routes to overcome<br />

the great slowness with which normally favorable biological characteristics<br />

spread through populations: thanks to biological heredity alone. The spread of<br />

lactose tolerance is an example of one of the most rapid biological evolutions in<br />

humans, though it was undoubtedly helped enormously by the spread of milk<br />

16


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

consumption, especially among adults, which came about through a Lamarckian<br />

type of heredity (of acquired characteristics during lifetime). The genetic study of<br />

human evolution has shown with extreme clarity that the genetic success of a<br />

population, as proved by its expansion in numbers and across vast regions, was<br />

largely the result of major technological innovations in relatively recent times:<br />

food production (agriculture, stockbreeding and their various developments) or<br />

transportation (cattle, horses, camels, lamas, boats, oceanic navigation), or military<br />

power (bronze, iron, cattle and horses, camels) and, in more recent times,<br />

communication (roads, and again the horse, the telephone, radio, television, and<br />

so on), mathematics (agronomy, geography, astronomy, computers), the experimental<br />

method (engineering, chemistry, modern physics).<br />

With the modern triumph of communication cultural evolution is becoming<br />

more and more the directional force of human evolution, and genetic evolution<br />

may well end up be completely under its control. Even the evolution of animals<br />

and plants is undergoing intense acceleration as a result of cultural evolution in<br />

humans. The remote origins of the success of humans as a species lie in two innovations<br />

which are partly biological, but perhaps in part also cultural. The first<br />

is language, which certainly required a strong development of the essential<br />

parts of our brain, and which was hence largely a biological evolution. However,<br />

once it became possible to use it, it developed enormous driving force. The<br />

other was human ingeniousness, which brought about many cultural innovations,<br />

the usefulness – if not necessity – of which has been proved beyond a<br />

doubt. Unlike genetic innovations due to mutation, which are by chance, cultural<br />

innovations are responses to a necessity. However, they are not always<br />

wholly appropriate, and like all innovations, they may also have costs that are<br />

heavier than their benefits.<br />

Racism<br />

I do not like the word “race” because it corresponds to old subdivisions that are<br />

inconsistent with genetic reality and unjustifiable by a rational classification.<br />

Moreover, there is no real use for such classifications and, what is worse, there<br />

is always an associated racist flavor. Darwin had already recognized the difficulty<br />

of a rational classification of races in what is an almost perfect continuum, and<br />

noted the enormous variety of numbers and definitions of races, from two to almost<br />

one hundred. The current trend toward increased admixture can only<br />

make the idea of race even less clear.<br />

17


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

It is important, however, to note that current classifications depend on external<br />

appearance, which is due to very few genes (hereditary factors) affecting skin,<br />

hair and eye color. Body and face size and shape may involve a few more genes<br />

but, like the former, are the result of an adaptation to climate (including diet and<br />

clothing, which obviously also depend to a large extent on climate and also on<br />

culture). Common belief in the “existence” of races must depend on uniformity<br />

of skin color in different environments: definitely dark in tropical climates,<br />

brown at some distance from the equator, light brown in southern Europe,<br />

reaching the highest degree of paleness in the southern Baltic. To a very superficial<br />

examination, races do exist, in the sense that some groups of individuals<br />

are distinguishable and relatively uniform in terms of a few superficial traits.<br />

However, the existence of “pure” races is pure myth, generated by the fact that<br />

most Europeans are pale, sub-Saharan Africans black, many Asians brown, and<br />

a few other traits may help to distinguish more finely the geographic origin of individuals.<br />

There is no equal uniformity beneath the skin: genetic differences<br />

among populations or races, however defined, are small or trivial compared to<br />

those few perspicuous characteristics that inevitably attract our attention. Below<br />

the superficially uniform veneer created mainly by the color of the skin, there<br />

are no “pure races”. Moreover, cultural differences among ethnic groups have<br />

often been held to be of genetic origin, though the reality is that most of them<br />

disappear after two or three generations of assimilation into another culture,<br />

and if some last longer it is because some cultural traits are more highly conserved<br />

than others. Distinguishing nature from culture is extremely difficult for<br />

most behavioral traits, but cultural differences are often strong in appearance<br />

though only skin-deep when put to the test of time. Some who have studied<br />

twins have, I think, been betrayed by their enthusiasm for genetics, beginning<br />

with the classical example of Sir Cyril Burt, whose aberrations have been exposed.<br />

I am not pressing for a blanket condemnation of all students of twins, but<br />

I think the marvel that some of them have expressed at the fact that a few identical<br />

twins reared apart prefer the same brand of cigarettes should not be taken<br />

very seriously – and in any event, even if there is something genetic about it,<br />

there are many cultural differences that we do not see in twins who grew up together<br />

(and there are very few cases of twins separated at birth). Another myth<br />

which dominated nineteenth century Europe and continues in the present century<br />

is that interracial hybrids are inferior, and that race admixture is to be<br />

avoided at all costs. The degree of genetic differentiation in the human species<br />

is so small that it is impossible for racial admixture to be genetically dangerous.<br />

18


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

On the contrary, the exact opposite, the vigor of hybrids may be deemed probable.<br />

There are, in fact, a few recent examples of very successful interracial hybrids.<br />

They cannot be too many, because there still is a real social handicap confronting<br />

interracial hybrids in most societies, which limits their numbers and<br />

success. Revisiting the Italian translation of this text at a distance of ten years, in<br />

2009, I cannot refrain from making an addition: to note the great satisfaction at<br />

the recent election of a North American who is a hybrid of the two most different<br />

races, and how important that can be for the future of mankind.<br />

I consider racism one of the scourges of humanity. There are very few social environments<br />

from which it is absent, and racism is certainly not only a European<br />

or white social disease. My beloved African Pygmies are considered animals by<br />

most of their Bantu neighbors, who think themselves superior because of the superiority<br />

of an agricultural economy, however primitive, compared with that of<br />

hunter-gatherers. The Saami should not be called Lapps, as they usually are, because<br />

the name “Lapps” means “no good” and they are thus called by their<br />

proximate neighbors because they do not practice agriculture. I find that culture,<br />

not genetics, is more useful in distinguishing people. And every culture has<br />

merits, but these give very different chances of economic and educational success.<br />

Is racism innate in humanity ? I do not know, but I believe a strong educational<br />

effort to eradicate racism is one of the most urgent needs. It may be impossible<br />

to totally eliminate it, but it should be possible the reduce the<br />

criminality with which it is constantly associated.<br />

Political difficulties encountered by genetics<br />

There is today great disagreement on the practical applications of molecular genetics<br />

to plants and animals. It is a good thing that these discussions take place.<br />

Every innovation is associated with potential benefits and costs. The latter are<br />

more difficult to evaluate in advance, leaving room for pessimists to overworry,<br />

and for optimists to be careless. It is a good thing that different countries take<br />

different strategies, so that the overcautious can wait as long as they like, and the<br />

overconfident take the loss, if there is one and teach others in the process.<br />

The study of human evolution has had its share of political difficulties. This has<br />

come from the religious attitudes of primitive cultures and from some political<br />

arenas. At the beginning the Human Genome Project in Washington avoided<br />

the study of variation, mostly, I believe, because it was felt that dealing with one<br />

19


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

single genome was already ambitious enough. The international branch of the<br />

Human Genome Project (the Human Genome Organization, HUGO) helped<br />

me to study human genetic variation. The Human Genetic Diversity Project<br />

(HGDP) – unrelated to the Human Genome project – has been the outcome.<br />

Its aim is that of accumulating samples of DNA from the human world population,<br />

make them available to research workers in human population genetics,<br />

and put their results into a data bank available to the research community.<br />

(Writing ten years later, in 2009, I can say that coordinated effort has brought<br />

substantial advantages). It is a fact that new developments in medical genetics<br />

make this type of research very useful. Pharmaceutical and biotechnologies<br />

firms have promptly understood their interest in genetic variation and have<br />

started working on genetic isolates and patenting polymorphisms. Our work has<br />

been made particularly difficult by totally unjustified attacks full of lies by a special-interest<br />

group, a Canadian NGO, which has an established network among<br />

some indigenous populations of America and Oceania. It accused the Human<br />

Genome Diversity Project of being behind totally unrelated patenting efforts of<br />

cell lines by the U.S. National Institutes of Health, of being interested in profit<br />

and consorting with pharmaceutical industries, and various other lies. The truth<br />

is that the HGDP is a non-profit institution, and has always been against patenting<br />

DNA.<br />

Fortunately several countries and regions, including the European Union, China,<br />

some Moslem countries and Israel have started efforts in the systematic<br />

study of human variation. The Human Genome Project has begun to reverse its<br />

policy of avoiding the study of human variation.<br />

One of the unsolved problems is that a few indigenous populations that are politically<br />

organized are asking for a share in the possible profits from research in<br />

ethnic variation. This may be a reasonable request, especially considering the<br />

poverty of indigenous peoples, but these profits are difficult to predict and ascertain,<br />

and can only come on a very long term basis from medical research,<br />

which is very expensive. Rumours have been spread by some journalists that<br />

knowledge of ethnic variation could be used for new forms of biological warfare<br />

directed against special ethnic groups. This is a hypothetical form of warfare<br />

very unlikely to be ever possible or effective, given the extremely mixed and<br />

complex structure of human populations. We witness daily heinous examples of<br />

ethnic warfare very effectively delivered with conventional or even traditional<br />

weapons, between peoples among whom there cannot exist any, or almost any<br />

genetic difference, but only political or religious ones. Unfortunately there ex-<br />

20


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

ists already a very dangerous biological arsenal, but the weapons developed so<br />

far have fortunately no connection whatsoever with ethnic differences.<br />

The future of human genetics<br />

There are a few signs of directions which human evolution may take. There has<br />

been, especially in the last five hundred years, an increasing migration across<br />

countries and continents, which has already had some effects in increasing overall<br />

admixture. Le Comte de Gobineau, who feared that admixture would ruin<br />

the qualities of human races (especially of the white race, of course), could be<br />

reassured today. There is absolutely no danger, and possibly even some advantage,<br />

from increased admixture. Human individual variation is the best insurance<br />

against future challenges from parasites or other environmental dangers,<br />

and the genetic structure of populations guarantees that this will not change.<br />

Global variation will not change, but the distribution of variation between and<br />

within continents or “races” may change, with a decrease in the variation between,<br />

and an increase in that within populations. Today there is, however, a<br />

major difference between the net reproductive rates of different countries and<br />

continents. Europeans grew in numbers in the last millennium but went<br />

through a demographic transition in the last two centuries, which is almost completed,<br />

and are practically not increasing any more in numbers, except through<br />

immigration. The rest of the world, with the exception of northern America and<br />

part of Oceania is increasing at maximum rate. This is the result of the arrival in<br />

the developing countries of Western hygiene and medicine, even if still very limited.<br />

Unaccompanied by birth limitation, it is causing a very worrying jump in<br />

population numbers. With white people of European origin being demographically<br />

stationary and the rest of the world with an average darker skin multiplying<br />

actively, the consequence will be an average darkening of the skin at a world<br />

level. This change may chagrin white racists, but is really not worrying from a<br />

genetic point of view.<br />

What is more worrying is that medicine has a dysgenic effect, because it has become<br />

successful in curing diseases which have a genetic component, and therefore<br />

these diseases will increase in frequency in the future. In truth, there has<br />

been very little progress in curing hardcore hereditary diseases. The main hope<br />

seems to be mostly gene therapy, which is due in a not necessarily close future.<br />

Progress of medicine has been more common in surgical treatment of many diseases,<br />

and in that of infectious ones, where the genetic component is less dra-<br />

21


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

matic, though not absent. As diseases which will increase in frequency are those<br />

which are curable, there will be no worsening of general health, but there will be<br />

an increase in the global cost of medical treatment, which will be compounded<br />

with the increase due to the greater expense of modern medical treatment, independently<br />

of the augmented frequency of disease. This cost increase is already<br />

wrecking current systems of social medicine. A third cause of increase is the extension<br />

of medical treatment to developing countries, which however is very<br />

slow and in any event to be desired.<br />

In the last century and in the first part of the present one there was great confidence<br />

in eugenics, a movement which recommended improvement of the human<br />

species by encouraging the multiplication of people endowed with successful<br />

characters (positive eugenics), and discouraging that of antisocial and<br />

medically unfit individuals (negative eugenics). Basically this was inspired by the<br />

practice of animal and plant breeders, who try to improve breeds by artificial selection,<br />

i.e. the choice of the best reproducers. However, there are serious traps<br />

in eugenic policies: it is very difficult to tell “good genes” from “bad genes”, or<br />

even good people from bad people – even if, of course, some cases are clear. It<br />

has been noted that selecting against serious psychiatric diseases like schizophrenia<br />

and manic depressive psychosis might destroy major sources of entertainment<br />

and pleasure like the theater, literature, and the arts, given that there is<br />

a significant association between these diseases and artistic creativity. A very<br />

good geneticist, H.J.Muller, gave involuntarily another famous example. He was<br />

in favor of conserving sperm from famous people for insemination of female<br />

volunteers, and gave a list of examples of men whose sperm should be conserved<br />

for propagation of their genes. He was a communist and went to the<br />

USSR before World War II in the hope of convincing the Soviet authorities to<br />

try his program. But he was not successful and, on his return to the free world,<br />

he decided to cancel Lenin and Stalin from his list of great men who should donate<br />

their sperm for his eugenic program.<br />

We can now predict the birth of children with major genetic diseases in time for<br />

early and safe pregnancy interruption. This practice, which avoids many pains<br />

and expenses to families and potential patients of very serious diseases, has met<br />

with considerable success even in traditionally Roman Catholic countries, where<br />

religious views are still contrary to it. There is often confusion between it and<br />

eugenics, but this practice is neither eugenic nor dysgenic. It does almost exactly<br />

what natural selection does, by removing from reproduction individuals who<br />

22


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

would not ordinarily reproduce. With very few exceptions, it simply removes<br />

them before they are born, usually avoiding extreme pains to them and to their<br />

relatives. It therefore does not, in practice, change the genetic incidence of the<br />

disease, but kills it before it appears, in the unborn.<br />

Multidisciplinarity and historical studies<br />

The research of human origins has made it useful or necessary to study parallel<br />

events and phenomena in a number of related disciplines, ranging from genetics<br />

to paleoanthropology, archaeology, ecology, history, demography, sociology, cultural<br />

and physical anthropology, linguistics, toponomastics and anthroponymy,<br />

and this list will probably increase in the future. This has been made possible by<br />

many collaborators whom I have tried to thank in my acceptance speech. There<br />

are definite advantages in this multidisciplinarity. A major one is the intellectual<br />

pleasure of finding so many similarities between disparate fields of study, some<br />

of which belong traditionally to the two opposite sides of culture: science and<br />

the humanities. The unity of scientific method comes out very clearly from such<br />

an exercise. It is also clear that many basic paradigms, especially mathematical<br />

ones, are extremely useful in many different disciplines, including both sciences<br />

and humanities, and are sufficiently few and simple that they can be easily exported<br />

from one field to the other. The book The Two Cultures by C.P. Snow already<br />

gave reasons for not maintaining the gap existing between the two cultures,<br />

but little has happened since it was published.<br />

There is also a specific advantage in multidisciplinarity when studying evolution.<br />

Evolution, like all history, suffers from a major handicap: one cannot use<br />

the experimental method, because one cannot hope to replicate history with the<br />

aim of verifying the causes. For this reason, some philosophers of science have<br />

denied that the study of evolution is a science. But historical events have rarely<br />

had restricted meaning or influence. One finds again and again the same consequences<br />

of the same events in profoundly different things. To give an example,<br />

the history of the settlement of geographical areas by populations expanding demographically<br />

has often similar consequences in genetics and in linguistics, so<br />

much so that their evolutionary trees are similar, in spite of the profound difference<br />

of the physical substrate evolving in biology and language. There is much<br />

fear among some scientists of the possible superficiality of conclusions drawn on<br />

the basis of analogies between different sciences. But one of the great scientists,<br />

Darwin, noted, in chapter 14 of the Origin of Species (second edition), that if we<br />

23


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

knew the genealogical tree of humanity we could predict that of languages. We<br />

are now very close to this aim. In reality, we have seen that he was right, even if<br />

he may have had too much faith in the idea. Many researchers have noted independently<br />

the similarities between the evolution of genes, of languages, and,<br />

more generally, of culture. Without the study of demography, the study of genetic<br />

evolution, especially of the human species, would be fruitless. One could<br />

continue with these parallels, reciprocal interactions, and cross-fertilizations of<br />

different disciplines, but naturally, no two cases where the same model can apply<br />

are truly identical. Analogies, metaphors, and models are powerful instruments<br />

for novel ideas, but critical thinking remains our best protection against<br />

possible superficiality of conclusions derived from them.<br />

Increase in knowledge demands today extreme specialization, but in my experience<br />

the cooperation of scientists from different disciplines can give that degree<br />

of generality without which research would be penalized. Multidisciplinarity is<br />

a major enrichment, especially in those sciences in which, as in history, repetition<br />

of the experiment is impossible, and may provide a sort of analogue of it.<br />

24


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

The Developments of My Research after the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize (1999-2009)<br />

by <strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

Professor Emeritus at the School of Medicine, Stanford University<br />

The <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> has taken the kind initiative of republishing in English<br />

and Italian writings I had prepared for the <strong>Foundation</strong> on the occasion of receiving<br />

the <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize (1999). My scientific activity has since continued and I<br />

thought it useful to prepare a short update.<br />

In 1999 I was, (and had been since 1992, according to the rules then in force), a<br />

professor emeritus in the Department of Genetics at Stanford University Medical<br />

School, though I officially remained “active”, meaning that I kept the right<br />

to maintain my laboratory so long as I had the necessary research funds. In the<br />

USA the best known universities expect professors to finance their own research<br />

with funds from external foundations, which are much more numerous<br />

and active than in Italy.<br />

In 1992 I began to spend half of my year in Italy to continue research I had never<br />

fully abandoned, and also started some new projects. In the first of these I<br />

began a collaboration with my son Francesco to write science textbooks for<br />

Italian secondary schools, which continues. Three scientific projects were effectively<br />

started after the year 2000, even though they had been conceived of<br />

and partly begun long before. One of these, called the Human Genome Diversity<br />

Panel (HGDP) is a collection of lymphocyte cell lines from more than a total<br />

of 1000 individuals, belonging to 52 ethnic groups from the five continents,<br />

for obtaining DNA to distribute to non- commercial research laboratories – to<br />

avoid DNA patenting which would have ruined research. These laboratories<br />

made an undertaking to make their results public in detail before publications<br />

using them, in order to favor further research by laboratories of human population<br />

genetics, which are now numerous, from all over the world. This research<br />

needs a large number of data. The cell lines were collected with the help<br />

of several researchers, mostly personal friends, and are housed in the Paris<br />

CEPH (Centre d’Etudes des Polymorphismes Humains) founded by Jean<br />

Dausset, under the direction of Professor Howard Cann. The distribution of<br />

DNAs to laboratories interested in analyzing them was begun in 2002, and to-<br />

25


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

day over 130 laboratories have received the DNA collection. The richest series<br />

of results was generated, on my proposal, by a group of Stanford researchers<br />

mostly from the Department of Genetics and made available on the Internet in<br />

September 2007. The article analyzing them was published in “Science” in<br />

February 2008 (J.Li et al.). The 948 unrelated individuals of the HGDP collection<br />

were analyzed for 650,000 DNA nucleotides known to be most variable in<br />

humans, with Illumina Microarray, and they are at the moment the largest set<br />

of data available on the genetic variation of any species. Its full statistical analysis<br />

of it will probably take several years. Apart from this experimental work,<br />

collaboration with Stanford is maintained especially with Professor Marcus<br />

Feldman from the Department of Biology at Stanford on cultural evolution, my<br />

other major field of interest.<br />

New activity in Italy includes two major projects, one of which is close to giving<br />

its first results: the Italian Genome Project (PGI), which follows closely the<br />

HGDP model. We already have over 1000 samples of lymphocytes coming<br />

from a province for each Italian region, selected as representative of the<br />

province by anthroponymic methods, and some hundreds of cell lines prepared<br />

from them, designed to get a first analysis of the Italian population<br />

genome. The PGI samples come from blood donors of AVIS, the Italian Association<br />

of Voluntary Blood Donors (more than a million members), naturally all<br />

consenting and informed. BGT (Bio-Genomic Technology) is producing cell<br />

lines. DNA analysis with over one million nucleotides (Illumina) is beginning<br />

now. Results will be used for research on the genetics of the Italian human population,<br />

but its major application will be in terms of the collaboration we can<br />

offer to Italian medical geneticists who have started or are about to start investigations<br />

in the genetic determination of diseases they are researching. Once<br />

this was done by comparing patients with their close and not so close healthy<br />

relatives. Today it is believed that a more efficient method is that of comparing<br />

statistically the DNA (today we say the genome) of a group of patients with a<br />

group of “controls”, healthy people of the same ethnic origin. In spite of the<br />

very scarce support offered in Italy by the government or wealthy people and<br />

foundations there are a number of active medical geneticists. Perhaps it is relatively<br />

easier to find financial help for medical research: once this type of humanitarian<br />

investment went exclusively for increasing the chances of a donor to<br />

go to Paradise. Today medicine is clearly more efficient than it used to be and<br />

this investment is split between a desire to go to Paradise and that which allows<br />

to go there as late as possible.<br />

26


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

These projects are all slow: a second project had begun earlier, around 1995,<br />

and has therefore almost reached its conclusion. It is an encyclopedia called<br />

“Italian Culture”. I use the word “Culture” in the widest possible sense: everything<br />

that we learn at any age and from anybody. It is, in substance, the contribution<br />

to our personality that does not come from our genetic constitution<br />

(what we inherit with DNA), but that which comes from the environment in<br />

which we grew and continue to live, our socialization. From the maternal womb<br />

to what we learn from parents and relatives, teachers, schoolfellows, colleagues,<br />

friends and enemies, all those we encountered in our lives and who taught us<br />

something about real life, in writing, through words or examples, and via many<br />

other modes. Today we can study our genetic constitution in depth, even though<br />

things are not as simple as we hoped at first, and genetics can be more than plain<br />

DNA. And perhaps the contribution of genes and culture can be equally important,<br />

or vary a lot in individual cases.<br />

I have tried to learn more about how we Italians are made, and why. I paid a lot<br />

of attention to history, which is always a great teacher (at the beginning, the idea<br />

was to call the opus “History of Italian Culture”). Perhaps my curiosity was<br />

sharpened by having spent much time in other cultures, American, English,<br />

French and a little bit German. One of the reasons that impelled me was the disagreeable<br />

discovery that Italian-Americans are the Americans least interested in<br />

their origin, at least in the US, and have perhaps the least national pride. One<br />

understands the reasons, remembering that they needed much courage to face<br />

enormous difficulties in a new culture, in order to get away from a country that<br />

had condemned them to hopeless poverty, leaving 80% of them illiterate.<br />

But also Italians who remained in Italy know little about their countrypeople.<br />

Superficially at least, they seem to be divided into two groups: a large one believing<br />

they are better than everybody else, and an equally large one, also equally<br />

wrong, believing we are worse than everybody else. This work tries to tell us<br />

objectively who we were and who we became, with highs and lows, and tries to<br />

answer many questions that one may ask oneself. It was possible to do it because<br />

after many unsatisfactory trials I came across Vittorio Bo, who at the time was a<br />

director at Einaudi publishing house and later founded Codice Edizioni, and he<br />

was willing to take on himself a good share of the directing load. He also convinced<br />

UTET of Turin, a major publishing house with a great tradition, to accept<br />

the challenge. When the work is finished it will consist of twelve large size<br />

volumes, the last two of which are a very detailed index. The contents of the ten<br />

volumes of the text are: 1. Land and People, 2. Tongues and Languages, 3. City,<br />

27


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Home, Landscape, 4. Economy and Communications, 5. Structure of society, 6.<br />

Food, Games, Holidays, Fashion, 7. Humanistic Culture, 8. Science and Technology,<br />

9. Music, Shows, Photography, Design, 10. Art and the Visual. Five volumes<br />

are already out and the remaining five should come out next year. The volumes<br />

are in large size, with paper of excellent quality that allows to enjoy<br />

entirely the great colors that only some Italian publishers can produce. As scientific<br />

director I am already fairly familiar with the contents, but I must confess<br />

that this reading, the first in its final form, gives me extraordinary pleasure,<br />

which almost worries me because I am indulging in it so much that I am getting<br />

late with various urgent commitments.<br />

28


Are there Limits to Knowledge?<br />

by <strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

ForMem RS, Geneticist, Stanford University<br />

<strong>Balzan</strong> Symposium 2002<br />

MEETING THE CHALLENGES OF THE FUTURE<br />

A Discussion between “The Two Cultures”<br />

Royal Society, London; 13 May 2002<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

On one hand, I resent even this question being asked. It looks like an attempt at<br />

trying to limit my freedom of inquiry. It reminds me of the Great Inquisition, the<br />

condemnation of Galileo. Is someone trying to decide what I am allowed to<br />

learn and what I should not even try to learn? In a totally different perspective,<br />

it reminds me of the irrepressible impression of ignorance which every honest<br />

scientist feels when comparing the extent of what we know and what we don’t<br />

know in our own field of knowledge. On a less emotional footing, there is, of<br />

course, Heisenberg’s principle of indetermination. But all the innumerable attempts<br />

at applying it out of context are probably unacceptable. Nevertheless,<br />

we might wonder how many other comparable principles exist in other fields,<br />

ones that we have violated out of ignorance. I find it more interesting, however,<br />

to take a wholly pragmatic approach, and ask: what are the practical limits to<br />

knowledge? Here I find many real limitations.<br />

I find at least three important limits to knowledge. The first and most serious<br />

one is the ambiguity of language. Almost every word has, in most languages, an<br />

enormous variety of meanings. English probably has the greatest number of<br />

words of all languages. This would seem to guarantee that it is closer to an ideal<br />

situation, that there is only one meaning for each word, but the opposite is true.<br />

I know of no statistics, but I suspect that English also has the greatest number<br />

of meanings for each word. We think we are clear when we explain things, and<br />

that we understand what other people tell us. The expectation is usually correct<br />

in most real instances, but I suspect that it loses meaning when we approach<br />

philosophy, where the use of words of a high degree of abstraction is common<br />

and probably increasing. Examples are the verbs ’to be’, ’to exist’, ’to cause’,<br />

and substantives derived from them. It may be because of personal limitations<br />

29


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

that I often feel that I cannot comprehend what philosophers say, but it may also<br />

be that they often use words they do not define, which can be understood in<br />

a great number of different ways. The admiring listener or reader is prepared to<br />

understand, or thinks he understands. By contrast, the sceptic may have a mental<br />

block. Probably both are wrong, and we are all victims of ambiguity, which is<br />

very difficult to avoid.<br />

The failure of most automatic translations is notoriously the consequence of ambiguity,<br />

which can be resolved (when that is possible) only by appropriate consideration<br />

of the context. There are many entertaining examples. A famous one<br />

is the sentence ’Out of sight, out of mind’ which when translated from English<br />

into Chinese and back from Chinese into English became ’invisible idiot’. A<br />

probable therapy for the frequent failure of machine translation will be the use<br />

of an intermediate language completely devoid of ambiguity. This may reduce<br />

errors, especially for translation from this artificial language into a natural one,<br />

but translation into it from a natural language may always involve errors. A complete<br />

catalogue of idiomatic expressions in the language of origin could probably<br />

help to avoid most mistakes, but it remains very likely that a translation freed<br />

of ambiguity will always be horrible. It is said that translations are like women:<br />

if they are faithful they cannot be beautiful, and if they are beautiful they cannot<br />

be faithful. I apologize for an inexcusable macho joke.<br />

I would not have brought up the problems of automatic translation if I did not<br />

think that lack of understanding, or the frequency of misunderstanding, are<br />

among the major causes of unhappiness among people and even among nations.<br />

Communication is the greatest asset of humans compared with other species,<br />

but even in our species it is far from complete. Because of the great multiplicity<br />

of languages, the chance that two random individuals can understand each other<br />

is very small. And the chance of real understanding, implying a good knowledge<br />

of a common language by both individuals, is much lower. However, mutual<br />

understanding is increasing on one hand, and on the other, automatic<br />

translation of simple sentences is more and more able to be done. The cessation<br />

of misunderstanding at the higher levels of philosophical discussion is less likely<br />

to occur, but is also less necessary. Now I must ask the philosophers for forgiveness.<br />

One limit to our knowledge which is most distressing, especially for my work, is<br />

the impossibility of repeating history. I began my research as an experimenter,<br />

and I used to be convinced that science can lead to truth, because you can al-<br />

30


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

ways repeat an experiment and convince yourself, and others, that it is correct.<br />

But when I shifted to the study of human evolution, I was clearly trying to reconstruct<br />

past history, and there is no chance of repeating it when we would like<br />

to do so to solve problems that seem insoluble. There are, however, as Vico said,<br />

courses and recourses of history, so that there is a little room for using analogy,<br />

but it requires great caution. Another resource is more useful: interdisciplinary<br />

research. By and large, for major historical events history must be only one approach,<br />

and the same events may be traced by different routes from different<br />

disciplines, though there is always some shared ground, and synthesis is more<br />

persuasive than any single story. Thus it has been very productive for the history<br />

of human expansions of the last 100,000 years to put together information<br />

from many different disciplines: the genetics of living human populations, archaeology,<br />

and a hybrid between them, archaeogenetics – an important part of<br />

which is the study of fossil DNA – and even linguistics. It has been very gratifying<br />

to see how pieces from the various disciplines seem to be interwoven together<br />

in a single mosaic. There is a mutual complementarity that makes interdisciplinary<br />

research particularly useful. The same general picture of evolution<br />

is obtained for the last 100,000 years from different sources of information. In<br />

the last few years, we have seen some aspects of cultural evolution, especially the<br />

most conservative traits like family and social structure, joining the band-wagon.<br />

Even the evolution of some bacteria and viruses common in modern humans<br />

seems to follow the routes of their long expansion from their place of origin in<br />

East Africa some 100,000 years ago, first through all of Africa, and then, between<br />

40,000 and 60,000, to Asia by both a southern and a northern route, and<br />

finally from Asia to the other three continents. Why is there this concordance?<br />

The explanation is simple: many of these traits, be they cultural like language, or<br />

many of those familiar to cultural anthropologists, or parasitic diseases, are most<br />

probably transmitted not only by contact imitation, as in the accredited cultural<br />

route, but also, and sometimes predominantly, from parent to child, or more<br />

generally within the family. This is expected especially for traits acquired in the<br />

earliest years, a pattern well known to be true for mother tongues. As transmission<br />

from parent to child (also called vertical) is the only way by which genes are<br />

transmitted, it is not at all surprising that patterns of evolution determined by<br />

expansions, followed by divisions and periods of independent evolution in different<br />

territories, will be similar for elements that may have no mutual causal relations,<br />

like genes and languages or other cultural traits, but simply correlations<br />

due to a common general history of resettlement.<br />

31


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Whether it is called inter-, multi-, or trans-disciplinary, collaboration, especially<br />

that between the two cultures, is contributing in an important way to our present<br />

understanding of modern human evolution. Each discipline can generate<br />

knowledge of different aspects of this process: archaeology can generate especially<br />

information on dates, but also on demographic processes. Genetics has also<br />

contributed to understanding the routes and mechanisms of expansions, including<br />

dates, even if so far with large intervals of error. It will be possible to<br />

decrease error substantially in the future by a more organized approach to studies<br />

of ethnic groups, especially if the numbers of individuals and of genes studied<br />

are very substantially increased. A more systematic analysis of cultural and<br />

linguistic diversity can help us to understand better the sometimes truly surprising<br />

vagaries of cultural evolution. It is only a matter of adequate investment,<br />

which so far has been very modest. It is clear that this analysis also has the promise<br />

of helping us to fight racist prejudice effectively. The mosaic of information<br />

formed with pieces coming from different sciences, which usually fit together in<br />

remarkable ways, shows a multidisciplinary agreement that is perhaps the most<br />

pleasant aspect of this research, and also generates the strongest support for<br />

conclusions. It supplies an alternative to the desirable, but impossible, experimental<br />

repetition of history.<br />

Nevertheless, there are examples of spontaneous repetitions of history which do<br />

contain some useful common information. In the past I have been especially interested<br />

in the mechanism of the spread of agriculture from its places of origin.<br />

About 10,000 years ago, Palaeolithic life came to a crisis in the areas with temperate<br />

climates where it had been most successful, like the Middle East and<br />

Turkey, China, and Mexico. The crisis may have been stimulated by a change of<br />

climate or by the foraging population (hunting, gathering, fishing) having come<br />

too close to saturation. Successful attempts were made in all these areas, probably<br />

independently, to cultivate local crops, mostly cereals, and to domesticate local<br />

animals where possible. These innovations generated a new, food-producing<br />

economy which allowed new population growth and favoured expansion from<br />

the place of origin to neighbouring areas where this agropastoral economy could<br />

flourish. A causal nexus may not be excluded, either: the idea may have travelled<br />

from one place to another on the back of some courageous traveller. After<br />

all, someone able to face the perils of the journey and of misunderstanding may<br />

make on foot the tour of the world in a few years. Once, after I had lost my way,<br />

I came across a tent in which lived an Iranian family, a husband with his wife and<br />

two small children, and two goats, who had started out from Iran a year earlier<br />

32


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

and wanted to get to see the Atlantic Ocean. I did not ask why, but I can understand<br />

them: I, too, am motivated by very strong curiosity. However, the move<br />

from food gathering to food production, which started in very distant places at<br />

around the same times, about ten thousand years ago, brought about an almost<br />

exponential growth in population to a thousandfold increase which is now nearing<br />

its limits. Are we ready for a new crisis: expansion outside Earth, self-destruction?<br />

The spread of agriculture from its place of origin was very slow. P. Menozzi, A.<br />

Piazza and I have been able to calculate its rate fairly exactly in Europe, and it<br />

was possible to explain it by the combination of two demographic phenomena,<br />

growth and emigration, to generate population expansion from the area of origin.<br />

Population growth was made possible by the increased availability of food,<br />

and as soon as it reached a new level of saturation it caused geographic expansion<br />

by migration, mostly of family groups or small social nuclei, to neighbouring<br />

areas offering potentially fertile soil. In research in collaboration with archaeologist<br />

Albert Ammerman, we suggested the name of demic diffusion for<br />

this process. It took some time to have this hypothesis accepted by archaeologists,<br />

especially in northern Europe and the USA, because it was totally opposite<br />

to the trend of thought that had developed there, in opposition to ideas popular<br />

in the nineteen-twenties. Clarke, who was mostly responsible for these ideas, explained<br />

every archaeological variation by the movement of people. When, after<br />

the war, the pendulum swung to the opposite pole, the hypothesis of migration<br />

disappeared. Only traders moved, carrying with them products that have been<br />

found and used by archaeologists to describe such expansions : a fashion now<br />

called indigenism. But the idea that it was really the growth and migration of<br />

farmers who brought farming around the nuclear area has been tested by showing<br />

that there are gradients of genes of Middle Eastern origin across Europe,<br />

which follow rather precisely the archaeological paths of the penetration of<br />

farming. Dates and rates of expansion are also in agreement with genetic and<br />

demographic calculations. The observed genetic gradient could have formed<br />

only if, in addition to the demic diffusion of farmers, there was adoption of<br />

farming by local hunter-gatherers, either by marriage or by acculturation.<br />

Cosmologists and astronomers are also afflicted by the problem of the history of<br />

the world being unable to be repeated. One advantage they have is that they can<br />

make much more precise measurements of their observations, and theoretical<br />

predictions to test their hypotheses. Biology is beginning to follow this example.<br />

33


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Genetics is a quantitative science, though errors in measurements and predictions<br />

are usually much greater. But in the study of human evolution we are beginning<br />

to have reasonable measurements, for instance, of the rates of expansions.<br />

[Ten years on, reading this after it was published and translated into<br />

Italian, genome analysis has led to the complete sequencing of DNA, resulting<br />

in a level of precision which is one thousand times greater, and precision is increasing.]<br />

In Europe, the average rate of the advance of farming was of the order<br />

of one kilometre a year, somewhat faster in the expansion from its area of<br />

origin in the Middle East westwards, via the Mediterranean, than towards the<br />

north-west, to Turkey, Greece, the Balkans, and Central Europe. We have approximate<br />

measurements of other rates. That of the Bantus , which started in<br />

Cameroon about 3,000 years ago and ended around 300 years ago in South<br />

Africa, where they met Boer farmers, was 50% faster on average (they were able<br />

to make use of iron). The Malayo-Polynesian expansion originated in South-east<br />

Asia – the precise area of origin is debated – and crossed the Pacific Ocean. It<br />

reached Easter Island about 1,500 years ago, in many jumps from island to island,<br />

and was about five to six times faster. It also expanded towards the west,<br />

reaching Madagascar only slightly later. For earlier expansions we have only one<br />

estimate of some precision: during the last glaciation, between 29,000 and<br />

13,000 years ago, the ice covered most of northern Europe and the population<br />

retired into two non-communicating refuges: south-western France, and the<br />

Balkans and the Ukraine. At the end of the glaciation Palaeolithic hunters began<br />

resettling the north, at a rate of about 0.7 kilometres a year. This rate must have<br />

been to some extent controlled by the rate at which ice withdrew. In general,<br />

rates of expansion are determined by an average (a geometric mean) of the rate<br />

of population growth and that of individual migration. The first extension of<br />

Homo Sapiens from East Africa to South America took approximately 50,000<br />

years to cover 25,000 kilometers (0.5 km on average per year), though it is possible<br />

that the settlement of South America by hunter-gatherers from Alaska to<br />

Chile was much faster. The limit to the quality of such estimates is set by the<br />

quantity and quality of archaeological findings.<br />

Thinking of the genetics of the future, a very serious limit is set by ethical and<br />

practical considerations. Today we know about 10,000 genetic diseases. They<br />

are all Mendelian, that is, they follow Mendel’s laws for characters determined<br />

by single genes; fortunately, they are very rare, as almost all are very serious. The<br />

most important and common diseases, like arthritis, diabetes, hypertension,<br />

34


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

many immunological diseases, schizophrenia, manic-depressive syndrome, are<br />

very poorly known. We are sure there is a genetic component, but there is potentially<br />

a multitude of genes involved in each one, as well as external factors<br />

that are difficult to pin down. Knowledge of the human genome will help, but<br />

there is still much work to do. This ’polygenic or multifactorial inheritance’ can<br />

be studied in animals, in which controlled crosses and experimental treatments<br />

unthinkable in humans are possible, so there is hope, and there has already been<br />

some success in getting help from animals for solving human genetic problems.<br />

But there are serious ethical limits to understanding human physiology and<br />

pathology, caused by the unacceptability of almost all experimentation.<br />

Even so, when I try to imagine what science and technology can do in the remote<br />

future, I find it very difficult to foresee serious limits to our knowledge. All<br />

the imaginations of Jules Verne have come true, plus many more. Perhaps whatever<br />

limits we picture today will be food for laughter among our descendants.<br />

The most serious limit to growth is of another kind: will the world survive the<br />

machinations of a technologically very knowledgeable, very depressed Luddite?<br />

There is a finite probability, fortunately very small for the time being, that this<br />

may cause the end of the only part of the universe where, as far as we know, science<br />

is being practised.<br />

35


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Biographical and bibliographical data<br />

LUIGI LUCA CAVALLI-SFORZA, born on 25 January 1922 in Genoa, is a<br />

dual citizen of Italy and the United States. MD in Medicine and Surgery (1944)<br />

from the University of Pavia, and MA (1950) from the University of Cambridge,<br />

UK. Director of Research in Microbiology, Istituto Sieroterapico Milanese,<br />

Milan (1950-1957); Lecturer in Genetics and in Statistics, Faculty of<br />

Sciences, University of Parma and University of Pavia (1957-1960); Professor<br />

of Genetics, University of Parma (1960-1962); Professor of Genetics and Director<br />

of the Institute of Genetics, University of Pavia (1962-1970); at Stanford<br />

University: Professor of Genetics (1970-1992), Chairman of the Department of<br />

Genetics (1986-1990), and, at present, Professor Emeritus at the School of<br />

Medicine (since 1992). He returned to settle permanently in Milan in September<br />

2008.<br />

From among his many honours:<br />

– Cavaliere di Gran Croce della Repubblica italiana (2000)<br />

– Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, Huxley Lecture<br />

in Anthropology, Londra (1972)<br />

– R.A. Fisher Memorial Lecture, Londra (1974)<br />

– Premio Ibico Reggino, Reggio Calabria (1976)<br />

– Weldon Medal in Biometry, University of Oxford (1978)<br />

– Premio Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei, Roma (1982)<br />

– Fifth <strong>International</strong> San Remo Prize for Human Genetics, Berlin (1986)<br />

– Allen Award in Human Genetics (American Society of Human Genetics,<br />

1987)<br />

– Medaglia d’oro del Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche Italiano (1990)<br />

– <strong>International</strong> Catalonia Award (1992)<br />

– Fyssen <strong>International</strong> Award (1994)<br />

– Premio Nonino (1996)<br />

– Premio Chiron, Accademia Italiana di Medicina (1998)<br />

– President of the Biometric Society (1967-1968)<br />

– Vice President of the <strong>International</strong> Congress of Genetics, Tokyo (1968)<br />

– Foreign Honorary Member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences<br />

(1973) and of the Japanese Society of Human Genetics (1977)<br />

36


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

– Foreign Associate of the US National Academy of Sciences (1978)<br />

– Honorary Fellow of Gonville and Caius College, Cambridge UK (1982)<br />

– President of the American Society of Human Genetics (1989)<br />

– Membre associé du Musée d’Histoire Naturelle de Paris (1990)<br />

– Socio Nazionale dell’Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei (1991)<br />

– Foreign Member of the Royal Society, Londra (1992)<br />

– Doctor Honoris Causa of Columbia University, University of Cambridge and<br />

of the Italian universities of Calabria, Bologna, Cagliari, Roma and Sassari.<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza is the author and co-author of many scientific books<br />

and articles, as well as Popular Science publications.<br />

Scientific books:<br />

– La teoria dell’Urto e le Unità Biologiche Elementari (con A. Buzzati-Traverso),<br />

1948, Milano: Longanesi;<br />

– Analisi Statistica per Medici e Biologi, 1961, prima edizione; 1993, terza edizione,<br />

Torino: Boringhieri;<br />

– The Genetics of Human Populations (with W. Bodmer), 1971, San Francisco:<br />

Freeman, 1999, Mineola, N.Y., Dover Publications;<br />

– Genetics, Evolution, and Man (with W. Bodmer), 1976, San Francisco: Freeman;<br />

edizione italiana Mondatori 1977;<br />

– Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach (with M.W.<br />

Feldman), 1981, Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press;<br />

– The Neolithic Transition and the Genetics of Populations in Europe (with A.<br />

Ammerman), 1984, Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press; edizione italiana<br />

Bollati Boringhieri;<br />

– African Pygmies (ed.), 1986, Orlando, Florida: Academic Press;<br />

– History and Geography of Human Genes (with P. Menozzi e A. Piazza), 1994,<br />

Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press; edizione italiana Adelphi 1997,<br />

2000;<br />

– L’evoluzione della cultura, 2004, Milano: Codice edizioni.<br />

– Consanguinity, Inbreeding, and Genetic Drift in Italy (with Antonio Moroni<br />

and Gianna Zei), 2004, Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.<br />

37


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Scientific articles:<br />

Among his over 500 scientific articles, some of the most recent are:<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL et al., 1988, Reconstruction of Human Evolution: Bringing<br />

Together Genetic Archeologic and Linguistic Data. PNAS. 85:6002-6006;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL et al., 1992, Coevolution of Genes and Languages Revisited.<br />

PNAS. 89:5620-4;<br />

Mountain JL et al., 1992, Evolution of Modern Humans: Evidence from Nuclear<br />

DNA Polymorphisms. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Biol Sci. 337<br />

(1280):159-65;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL et al., 1993, Demic Expansions and Human Evolution. Science.<br />

259:639-46;<br />

Zei et al., 1993, Barriers to Gene Flow Estimated by Surname Distribution in<br />

Italy. Ann Hum Genet. 57:123-40;<br />

– Bowcock AM et al., 1994, High Resolution of Human Evolutionary Trees with<br />

Polymorphic Microsatellites. Nature. 368:455-7;<br />

– Mountain JL e <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1994, Inference of Human Evolution<br />

through Cladistic Analysis of Nuclear DNA Restriction Polymorphisms.<br />

PNAS. 91:6515-9;<br />

– Piazza A et al., 1995, Genetics and the Origin of European Languages. PNAS.<br />

92:5836-40;<br />

– Seielstad M et al., 1994, Construction of Human Y-chromosomal Haplotypes<br />

Using a New Polymorphic A to G Transition. Hum Mol Genet. 3:2159-61;<br />

– Mountain JL et al., 1995, Demographic History of India and mtDNA-sequence<br />

Diversity. Am J Hum Genet. 56:979-92;<br />

Goldstein DB et al., 1995, Genetic Absolute Dating Based on Microsatellites<br />

and the Origin of Modern Humans. PNAS. 92:6723-7;<br />

– Underhill PA et al., 1996, A Pre-Columbian Y chromosome-specific Transition<br />

and its Implications for Human Evolutionary History. PNAS. 93:196-200;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1997, Genetic and Cultural Diversity in Europe. J Anthr<br />

Res. 53:383-404;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1997, Genes, Peoples, and Languages. Proc Natl Acad Sci<br />

USA. 94:7719-24;<br />

– Barbujani AE et al., 1997, Apportionment of Human DNA Diversity. Proc<br />

Natl Acad Sci USA. 94:4516-9;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL e Minch E, 1997, Paleolithic and Neolithic Lineages in the<br />

European Mitochondrial Gene Pool [letter]. Am J Hum Genet. 61:247-54;<br />

38


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

– Underhill PA et al., 1997, Detection of Numerous Y chromosome Biallelic<br />

Polymorphisms by Denaturating High-performance Liquid Chromatography.<br />

Genome Res. 7:996-1005;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1998, The Chinese Human Genome Diversity Project.<br />

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 95:111501-11503;<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1998, The DNA Revolution in Population Genetics.<br />

Trends Genet. 14:60-65.<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 1998, Man and the diversity of his genome. An extraordinary<br />

phase in the history of population genetics. Pathol Biol, Paris. 46: 98-102<br />

– Risch N et al., 1999, A genomic screen of autism: evidence for a multilocus etiology.;<br />

Am J Hum Genet. 65:493-507<br />

– Jin L et al., 1999, Distribution of haplotypes from a chromosome 21 region<br />

distinguishes multiple prehistoric human migrations. Proc Natl Acad Sci<br />

USA. 96:3796-800<br />

– Shen P et al., 2000, Population genetic implications from sequence variation<br />

in four Y chromosome genes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 97 :7354-9<br />

– Underhill PA et al., 2000, Y chromosome sequence variation and the history<br />

of human populations. Nat Genet. 26:358-61<br />

– Passarino G et al., 2000, Y chromosome binary markers to study the high<br />

prevalence of males in Sardinian centenarians and the genetic structure of the<br />

Sardinian population. Hum Hered. 52:136-9<br />

– Underhill PA et al., 2001, The phylogeography of Y chromosome binary haplotypes<br />

and the origins of modern human populations. Ann Hum Genet.<br />

65:43-62<br />

– Passarino G et al., 2001, The Werner syndrome gene and global sequence<br />

variation. Genomics. 71:118-22<br />

– Underhill PA et al., 2001, Maori origins, Y-chromosome haplotypes and implications<br />

for human history in the Pacific. Hum Mutat 17:271-80<br />

– Vlad MO et al., 2002, Neutrality condition and response law for nonlinear reaction-diffusion<br />

equations, with application to population genetics. Phys Rev<br />

E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys. 65:061110<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, Feldman MW, 2003, The application of molecular genetic<br />

approaches to the study of human evolution. Nat Genet. 33(Suppl):266-75<br />

– McIntire JJ et al., 2003, Immunology: hepatitis A virus link to atopic disease.<br />

Nature. 6958(425): 576<br />

– Cinniolu C et al., 2004, Excavating Y-chromosome haplotype strata in Anatolia.<br />

Hum Genet 114:127-48<br />

39


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

– Edmonds CA, Lillie AS, <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 2004, Mutations arising in the<br />

wave front of an expanding population. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 101:975-9<br />

– Vlad MO, <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, Ross J, 2004, Enhanced (hydrodynamic) transport<br />

induced by population growth in reaction-diffusion systems with application<br />

to population genetics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 101:10249-53<br />

– Ramachandran S et al., 2005, Support from the relationship of genetic and geographic<br />

distance in human populations for a serial founder effect originating<br />

in Africa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA<br />

– Kivisild T et al., 2005, The role of selection in the evolution of human mitochondrial<br />

genomes. Genetics 2005<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 2005, Studying diversity. EMBO Rep. 6:713<br />

– Vlad MO et al., 2005 Fisher’s theorems for multivariable, time- and space-dependent<br />

systems, with applications in population genetics and chemical kinetics.<br />

Proc Natl Acad Sci USA<br />

– <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza LL, 2005, The Human Genome Diversity Project: past, present<br />

and future. Nat Rev Genet. 6:333-40<br />

– Sengupta S et al., 2006, Polarity and temporality of high-resolution y-chromosome<br />

distributions in India identify both indigenous and exogenous expansions<br />

and reveal minor genetic influence of central asian pastoralists. Am J<br />

Hum Genet. 78:202-21<br />

Popular Science publications:<br />

– Chi Siamo - La Storia Della Diversità Umana (con F. <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza), 1995,<br />

2007, Milano: Mondadori;<br />

– Geni, Popoli e Lingue, 1996, Milano: Adelphi;<br />

– La Scienza della Felicità (con F. <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza); 1997, Milano: Mondadori.<br />

– Il caso e la necessità - Ragioni e limiti della diversità genetica, 2007, Roma: Di<br />

Renzo Editore.<br />

He has published many articles in Italian daily newspapers such as “La Stampa”,<br />

“Corriere della Sera”, “Il Sole 24 Ore” and “La Repubblica”.<br />

40


<strong>International</strong> <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

The <strong>International</strong> <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> was established in Lugano in 1956 thanks to<br />

the generosity of Lina <strong>Balzan</strong>, who had come into a considerable inheritance on the<br />

death of her father, Eugenio. She destined this wealth to honour her father’s memory.<br />

Eugenio Francesco <strong>Balzan</strong> was born in Badia Polesine, near Rovigo (Northern<br />

Italy), on 20 April 1874, to a family of landowners. He spent almost his entire working<br />

life at Milan’s leading daily paper Corriere della Sera. After joining the paper in<br />

1897, he worked his way through, from editorial assistant, to editor-in-chief and then<br />

special correspondent. In 1903 editor <strong>Luigi</strong> Albertini made him managing director of<br />

the paper’s publishing company; he then became a partner and share-holder in the<br />

company. He was not only a clever and skilful manager but also a leading personality<br />

in Milan. In 1933 he left Italy due to opposition from certain milieus hostile to an<br />

independent Corriere. He then moved to Switzerland, living in Zurich or Lugano,<br />

where for years he had invested his fortune with success. He also continued his charitable<br />

activities in favour of institutions and individuals. After returning to Italy in<br />

1950, Eugenio <strong>Balzan</strong> died in Lugano, Switzerland, on 15 July 1953*.<br />

Today, the <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong>, international in character and scope, acts jointly<br />

through two <strong>Foundation</strong>s: one under Italian law and the other under Swiss law.<br />

In Milan, the <strong>International</strong> E. <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize <strong>Foundation</strong> – “Prize” has the aim to promote,<br />

throughout the world, culture, science, and the most meritorious initiatives in<br />

the cause of humanity, peace and brotherhood among peoples, regardless of nationality,<br />

race or creed. This aim is attained through the annual award of four prizes in<br />

two general fields: literature, the moral sciences and the arts; medicine and the physical,<br />

mathematical and natural sciences. Nominations for the prizes in the scientific<br />

and humanistic fields are received at the <strong>Foundation</strong>’s request from the world’s leading<br />

learned societies. Candidates are selected by the General Prize Committee, composed<br />

of eminent European scholars and scientists. Since 2001, each prize is worth<br />

one million Swiss francs (about 620,000 Euros), half of which the prizewinner must<br />

destine for research work, preferably involving young researchers. At intervals of<br />

not less than hree years, the <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong> also awards a Prize for Humanity,<br />

Peace and Brotherhood among Peoples, of varying amounts.<br />

In Zurich, the <strong>International</strong> E. <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize <strong>Foundation</strong> – “Fund” administers Eugenio<br />

<strong>Balzan</strong>’s estate.<br />

* Renata Broggini, Eugenio <strong>Balzan</strong> 1874-1953. Una vita per il “Corriere”, un progetto per l’umanità, Milano, 2001;<br />

Renata Broggini, Eugenio <strong>Balzan</strong> 1874-1953. A Biography, Milano, 2007.<br />

41


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

Members of the <strong>International</strong> <strong>Balzan</strong> <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

(June 2009)<br />

Board of the “Prize” <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

Bruno Bottai (Italy), Chairman<br />

Carlo Fontana (Italy), Vice-Chairman<br />

Marco Cameroni (Switzerland)<br />

Achille Casanova (Switzerland)<br />

Enrico Decleva (Italy)<br />

Paolo Matthiae (Italy)<br />

Alberto Quadrio Curzio (Italy)<br />

General Prize Committee<br />

Salvatore Veca (Italy), Chairman<br />

M.E.H. Nicolette Mout (Netherlands), Vice-Chairperson<br />

Werner Stauffacher (Switzerland), Vice-Chairman<br />

Enric Banda (Spain)<br />

Giovanni Busino (Italy/Switzerland)<br />

Nicola Cabibbo (Italy)<br />

Étienne Ghys (Francia)<br />

Bengt Gustafsson (Sweden)<br />

John Richard Krebs (United Kingdom)<br />

Nicole Le Douarin (France)<br />

Paolo Matthiae (Italy)<br />

Erwin Neher (Germany)<br />

Antonio Padoa Schioppa (Italy)<br />

Dominique Schnapper (France)<br />

Gottfried Scholz (Austria)<br />

Dmitry O. Shvidkovsky (Russia)<br />

Quentin Skinner (United Kingdom)<br />

Karlheinz Stierle (Germany)<br />

Marc Van Montagu (Belgium)<br />

Luzius Wildhaber (Switzerland)<br />

Suzanne Werder (Italy), Secretary General<br />

42


Board of the “Fund” <strong>Foundation</strong><br />

Achille Casanova (Switzerland), Chairman<br />

Bruno Bottai (Italy)<br />

Luisa Bürkler-Giussani (Switzerland)<br />

Maria Casutt Dietrich (Switzerland)<br />

Carlo Fontana (Italy)<br />

Claudio Generali (Switzerland)<br />

Arina Kowner (Switzerland)<br />

43<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza


<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

<strong>Balzan</strong> Prizewinners<br />

for literature, moral sciences, and the arts;<br />

for physical, mathematical and natural sciences, and medicine<br />

2008 WALLACE S. BROECKER (USA) The Science of Climate Change<br />

MAURIZIO CALVESI (Italy) The Visual Arts since 1700<br />

IAN H. FRAZER (Australia/UK) Preventive Medicine,<br />

including Vaccination<br />

THOMAS NAGEL (USA/Serbia) Moral Philosophy<br />

2007 ROSALYN HIGGINS (UK) <strong>International</strong> Law since 1945<br />

SUMIO IIJIMA (Japan) Nanoscience<br />

MICHEL ZINK (France) European Literature (1000-1500)<br />

JULES HOFFMANN (France)<br />

and BRUCE BEUTLER (USA)<br />

Innate Immunity<br />

2006 LUDWIG FINSCHER (Germany) History of Western Music<br />

since 1600<br />

QUENTIN SKINNER (UK) Political Thought; History and Theory<br />

PAOLO DE BERNARDIS (Italy) Observational Astronomy<br />

and ANDREW LANGE (USA) and Astrophysics<br />

ELLIOT MEYEROWITZ (USA)<br />

and CHRISTOPHER SOMERVILLE<br />

(USA/Canada)<br />

Plant Molecular Genetics<br />

2005 PETER HALL (UK) The Social and Cultural History<br />

of Cities since the Beginning<br />

of the 16th Century<br />

LOTHAR LEDDEROSE (Germany) The History of the Art of Asia<br />

45


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

PETER AND ROSEMARY GRANT Population Biology<br />

(USA/UK)<br />

RUSSELL HEMLEY (USA) Mineral Physics<br />

and HO-KWANG MAO (USA/China)<br />

2004 PIERRE DELIGNE (USA/Belgium) Mathematics<br />

NIKKI RAGOZIN KEDDIE (USA) The Islamic world from the end of<br />

the 19th to the end of the 20th century<br />

MICHAEL MARMOT (UK) Epidemiology<br />

COLIN RENFREW (UK) Prehistoric Archaeology<br />

REINHARD GENZEL (Germany) Infrared Astronomy<br />

ERIC HOBSBAWM (UK/Egypt) European History since 1900<br />

WEN-HSIUNG LI (USA/Taiwan) Genetics and Evolution<br />

SERGE MOSCOVICI<br />

(France/Romania)<br />

Social Psychology<br />

2002 WALTER JAKOB GEHRING Developmental Biology<br />

(Switzerland)<br />

ANTHONY THOMAS GRAFTON History of the Humanities<br />

(USA)<br />

XAVIER LE PICHON Geology<br />

(France/Vietnam)<br />

DOMINIQUE SCHNAPPER (France) Sociology<br />

2001 JAMES SLOSS ACKERMAN (USA) History of Architecture (including<br />

town planning and landscape design)<br />

JEAN-PIERRE CHANGEUX (France) Cognitive Neurosciences<br />

MARC FUMAROLI (France) Literary History and Criticism<br />

(post 1500)<br />

CLAUDE LORIUS (France) Climatology<br />

46


2000 ILKKA HANSKI (Finland) Ecological Sciences<br />

MICHEL MAYOR (Switzerland) Instrumentation and Techniques<br />

in Astronomy and Astrophysics<br />

MICHAEL STOLLEIS (Germany) Legal History since 1500<br />

MARTIN LITCHFIELD WEST (UK) Classical Antiquity<br />

1999 LUIGI LUCA CAVALLI-SFORZA Science of Human Origins<br />

(USA/Italy)<br />

JOHN ELLIOTT (UK) History, 1500-1800<br />

MIKHAEL GROMOV (France/Russia) Mathematics<br />

PAUL RICŒUR (France) Philosophy<br />

1998 HARMON CRAIG (USA) Geochemistry<br />

ROBERT MCCREDIE MAY<br />

(UK/Australia)<br />

Biodiversity<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza<br />

ANDRZEJ WALICKI (USA/Poland) History: The cultural and social<br />

history of the Slavonic world<br />

from the reign of Catherine<br />

the Great to the Russian revolutions<br />

of 1917<br />

1997 CHARLES COULSTON GILLISPIE History and Philosophy of Science<br />

(USA)<br />

THOMAS WILSON MEADE (UK) Epidemiology<br />

STANLEY JEYARAJA TAMBIAH Social Sciences: Social Anthropology<br />

(USA/Sri Lanka)<br />

1996 ARNO BORST (Germany) History: Medieval Cultures<br />

ARNT ELIASSEN (Norway) Meteorology<br />

STANLEY HOFFMANN Political Sciences: Contemporary<br />

(France/USA/Austria) <strong>International</strong> Relations<br />

47


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

1995 YVES BONNEFOY (France) Art History and Art Criticism<br />

(as applied to European Art<br />

from the Middle Ages to our times)<br />

CARLO M. CIPOLLA (Italy) Economic History<br />

ALAN J. HEEGER (USA) Science of New Non-Biological<br />

Materials<br />

1994 NORBERTO BOBBIO (Italy) Law and Political Science<br />

(governments and democracy)<br />

RENÉ COUTEAUX (France) Biology (cell-structure with special<br />

reference to the nervous system)<br />

FRED HOYLE (UK) Astrophysics (evolution of stars)<br />

and MARTIN SCHWARZSCHILD<br />

(USA/Germany)<br />

1993 WOLFGANG H. BERGER Paleontology with special reference<br />

(USA/Germany) to Oceanography<br />

LOTHAR GALL (Germany) History: Societies of the 19th and 20th centuries<br />

JEAN LECLANT (France) Art and Archaeology<br />

of the Ancient World<br />

1992 ARMAND BOREL (USA/Switzerland) Mathematics<br />

GIOVANNI MACCHIA (Italy) History and Criticism of Literatures<br />

EBRAHIM M. SAMBA (Gambia) Preventive Medicine<br />

1991 GYÖRGY LIGETI Music<br />

(Austria/Hungary/Romania)<br />

VITORINO MAGALHÃES GODINHO History: The emergence of Europe<br />

(Portugal) in the 15th and 16th centuries<br />

JOHN MAYNARD SMITH (UK) Genetics and Evolution<br />

48


990 WALTER BURKERT (Germany) Study of the Ancient World<br />

(Mediterranean area)<br />

JAMES FREEMAN GILBERT (USA) Geophysics (solid earth)<br />

PIERRE LALIVE D’EPINAY<br />

(Switzerland)<br />

Private <strong>International</strong> Law<br />

1989 EMMANUEL LÉVINAS<br />

(France/Lithuania)<br />

Philosophy<br />

LEO PARDI (Italy) Ethology<br />

MARTIN JOHN REES (UK) High Energy Astrophysics<br />

1988 SHMUEL NOAH EISENSTADT Sociology<br />

(Israel/Poland)<br />

RENÉ ETIEMBLE (France) Comparative Literature<br />

MICHAEL EVENARI (Israel/France) Applied Botany<br />

and OTTO LUDWIG LANGE (including ecological aspects)<br />

(Germany)<br />

1987 JEROME SEYMOUR BRUNER (USA) Human Psychology<br />

RICHARD W. SOUTHERN (UK) Medieval History<br />

PHILLIP V. TOBIAS (South Africa) Physical Anthropology<br />

1986 OTTO NEUGEBAUER (USA/Austria) History of Science<br />

ROGER REVELLE (USA) Oceanography/Climatology<br />

JEAN RIVERO (France) Basic Human Rights<br />

1985 ERNST H.J. GOMBRICH<br />

(UK/Austria)<br />

History of Western Art<br />

JEAN-PIERRE SERRE (France) Mathematics<br />

1984 JAN HENDRIK OORT (Netherlands) Astrophysics<br />

49<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza


1999 <strong>Balzan</strong> Prize for Science of Human Origins<br />

JEAN STAROBINSKI (Switzerland) History and Criticism of Literatures<br />

SEWALL WRIGHT (USA) Genetics<br />

1983 FRANCESCO GABRIELI (Italy) Oriental Studies<br />

ERNST MAYR (USA/Germany) Zoology<br />

EDWARD SHILS (USA) Sociology<br />

1982 JEAN-BAPTISTE DUROSELLE (France) Social Sciences<br />

MASSIMO PALLOTTINO (Italy) Sciences of Antiquity<br />

KENNETH VIVIAN THIMANN Pure and Applied Botany<br />

(USA/UK)<br />

1981 JOSEF PIEPER (Germany) Philosophy<br />

PAUL REUTER (France) <strong>International</strong> Public Law<br />

DAN PETER MCKENZIE, Geology and Geophysics<br />

DRUMMOND HOYLE MATTHEWS<br />

and FREDERICK JOHN VINE (UK)<br />

1980 ENRICO BOMBIERI (USA/Italy) Mathematics<br />

JORGE LUIS BORGES (Argentina) Philology, Linguistics<br />

and Literary Criticism<br />

HASSAN FATHY (Egypt) Architecture and Urban Planning<br />

1979 TORBJÖRN CASPERSSON (Sweden) Biology<br />

JEAN PIAGET (Switzerland) Social and Political Sciences<br />

ERNEST LABROUSSE (France)<br />

and GIUSEPPE TUCCI (Italy)<br />

History<br />

1962 PAUL HINDEMITH (Germany) Music<br />

ANDREJ KOLMOGOROV (Russia) Mathematics<br />

SAMUEL ELIOT MORISON (USA) History<br />

KARL VON FRISCH (Austria) Biology<br />

50


<strong>Balzan</strong> Prizewinners<br />

for Humanity, Peace and Brotherhood among peoples<br />

2007 KARLHEINZ BÖHM (Austria/Germany)<br />

2004 COMMUNITY OF SANT’EGIDIO<br />

2000 ABDUL SATTAR EDHI (Pakistan/India)<br />

1996 INTERNATIONAL COMMITTEE OF THE RED CROSS<br />

1991 ABBÉ PIERRE (France)<br />

1986 UNITED NATIONS REFUGEE AGENCY (UNHCR)<br />

1978 MOTHER TERESA OF CALCUTTA (India/Macedonia)<br />

1962 H.H. JOHN XXIII (Vatican City/Italy)<br />

1961 NOBEL FOUNDATION<br />

51<br />

<strong>Luigi</strong> <strong>Luca</strong> <strong>Cavalli</strong>-Sforza


Finito di stampare<br />

nel mese di settembre 2009<br />

da Àncora Arti Grafiche - Milano<br />

Printed in Italy

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!