10.11.2012 Views

did you know?

did you know?

did you know?

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

chapter 6>><br />

visual perception<br />

POSSIBLY THE BEST ILLUSTRATION OF THE WAY BODY AND MIND<br />

INTERACT TO ENABLE US TO PERCEIVE THE WORLD AROUND US IS IN<br />

THE PROCESS OF VISION. OUR EYES RECEIVE THE STIMULUS ENERGY<br />

OF LIGHT, REFLECTED FROM AN OBJECT IN THE OUTSIDE WORLD,<br />

LIGHT-SENSITIVE CELLS IN THE EYE SEND MESSAGES TO THE BRAIN<br />

AND THEN WE INTERPRET AND UNDERSTAND WHAT WE SEE.


six stages of visual perception<br />

>>key <strong>know</strong>ledge>><br />

Application of psychological perspectives to explain<br />

visual perception:<br />

> characteristics of the visual perceptual system and<br />

the visual processes involved in detecting and<br />

interpreting visual stimuli<br />

> the effect of psychological factors on perceptual set<br />

> distortions of visual perceptions by illusions.<br />

(VCE Study Design 2009)<br />

A study of the way in which we perceive the world around<br />

us through the sense of vision is a wonderful example of the<br />

way the brain and body work together, depend on each other<br />

and help each other as we go about our everyday activities.<br />

One of the key objectives of this chapter is to help<br />

<strong>you</strong> to understand how the physical reality of light from<br />

the object in the environment—referred to as the distal<br />

stimulus—is cast as an image on the retina—called the<br />

proximal stimulus—and nerve impulses travel to the brain<br />

where higher mental processes enable us to organise and<br />

interpret what we see.<br />

fig 6.1>> The eye<br />

>>chapter overview>><br />

Six stages<br />

of visual<br />

perception<br />

The processes<br />

of visual<br />

perception<br />

The principles<br />

of visual<br />

perception<br />

Distortions of<br />

perception<br />

> Reception<br />

> Transduction<br />

> Transmission<br />

> Selection<br />

> Organisation<br />

> Interpretation<br />

Reception and light energy<br />

Reception and absolute threshold<br />

The role of the eye in visual perception<br />

Selection and organisation of visual<br />

stimuli<br />

> Perceptual constancies<br />

> Size constancy<br />

> Shape constancy<br />

Gestalt principles of perceptual<br />

organisation<br />

> The phi phenomenon<br />

> Figure-ground organisation<br />

> Camouflage<br />

> Closure<br />

> Similarity<br />

> Proximity<br />

Depth perception<br />

> Binocular depth cues<br />

> Retinal disparity<br />

> Convergence<br />

> Monocular depth cues<br />

> Accommodation<br />

> Pictorial depth cues<br />

> Linear perspective<br />

> Interposition<br />

> Texture gradient<br />

> Relative size<br />

> Height in the visual field<br />

The effect of psychological factors on<br />

perceptual set<br />

The Ponzo Illusion<br />

The Müller-Lyer Illusion<br />

> The carpentered world hypothesis<br />

> The perceptual compromise<br />

theory<br />

The Ames Room Illusion<br />

111


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

The actual image on our retina is:<br />

> upside-down<br />

> back-to-front<br />

> blurred<br />

> crisscrossed by a network of veins<br />

> patched by holes.<br />

Yet when this image has been sent to the brain, it<br />

is processed so that we see a crystal-clear picture! The<br />

process itself—from receiving an image to ultimately<br />

interpreting what we see—is complex and has been studied<br />

extensively by a number of psychological perspectives as<br />

discussed in Chapter 5.<br />

The processes involved in sensation and perception<br />

are also thought to be adaptive. From an evolutionary<br />

perspective, the ability to see, hear, touch, smell and<br />

taste has developed over thousands of years and through<br />

millions of changes—leaving our senses perfectly suited<br />

to our environment and to help us survive and reproduce<br />

(Tooby & Cosmides 1992, cited in Westen et al. 2009). Just<br />

like frogs, which have an inbuilt ‘bug-detecting’ function<br />

in their visual system designed to activate when a tasty<br />

insect is in view, humans have specialised areas in the brain<br />

that allow the perception of faces and facial expression.<br />

This can be seen in infants, who have an innate or inborn<br />

tendency to show greater interest in objects that look like a<br />

human face (Adophs, Damasio, Tranel & Damasio 1996).<br />

The eye is a fantastic organ—it is very complex in<br />

construction, but we only need to <strong>know</strong> about a few of its<br />

structures.<br />

fig 6.2>> Movement of light entering eye<br />

112<br />

From the time we receive an image to when we can<br />

identify what we see, six stages have been identified, some<br />

of which occur at about the same time. Essentially these<br />

six stages progress, in sequence, from being reflexive<br />

physical functions of the eye and nervous system to being<br />

psychological functions of the brain, involving memory<br />

and thought processes.<br />

sensation and<br />

perception<br />

1 Reception: Light enters the eye through the cornea,<br />

a tough transparent tissue covering the front of the<br />

eye. It then passes through the pupil—the hole in<br />

the middle of the coloured part of the eye (the iris).<br />

The lens then focuses the light on the retina, which<br />

contains the photoreceptors—light-sensitive cells called<br />

rods and cones.<br />

2 Transduction: The electro-magnetic energy that we<br />

<strong>know</strong> as light energy is converted by the rods and cones<br />

into electro-chemical nerve impulses. This allows the<br />

visual information to travel along the fibres of the optic<br />

nerve to the brain.<br />

3 Transmission: The next task for the rods and cones<br />

is to send the nerve impulses along the optic nerve to<br />

the primary visual cortex in the occipital lobes, at the<br />

very back of the brain where specialised receptor cells<br />

respond as the process of visual perception continues.


fig 6.3>> The brain makes sense of what we see.<br />

4 Selection: We can’t possibly pay attention to all the<br />

millions of stimuli that enter the eye at the same time,<br />

so we pick out the ones that are important to us and<br />

pay attention to those. At this stage of the process, the<br />

image is broken up by specialised cells called feature<br />

detectors. Feature detectors are cells that individually<br />

respond to lines of a certain length, lines at a certain<br />

angle or lines moving in a certain direction. Feature<br />

detector cells are found in the optic nerve and in the<br />

primary visual cortex.<br />

5 Organisation: When visual information reaches the<br />

brain (visual cortex), it is reorganised so that we can<br />

make sense of it. We do this by using certain visual<br />

perceptual principles:<br />

> perceptual constancies<br />

> Gestalt principles<br />

> depth and distance cues<br />

These are explained in the next section of this<br />

chapter.<br />

Once the image is reassembled using these principles,<br />

it travels along two pathways simultaneously: to the<br />

temporal lobe, to identify the object, and to the parietal<br />

lobe, to judge where the object is in space (in relation to<br />

our visual field and ourselves).<br />

6 Interpretation: This is the process whereby the visual<br />

stimulus/object is given meaning. The temporal lobes<br />

identify what the object is by comparing incoming<br />

information with information already stored in memory.<br />

Past experience, motives, values and context help us<br />

understand what we are looking at by contributing to<br />

our perceptual set (as discussed later in this chapter).<br />

At the same time as information is sent to the temporal<br />

fig 6.4>> Do <strong>you</strong> <strong>know</strong> why a person cannot see clearly<br />

underwater? The cornea of the eye is designed to refract or bend<br />

light rays travelling through the air—not through water. So when<br />

<strong>you</strong> put on swimming goggles or a diving mask, the layer of air<br />

between the water and cornea allows <strong>you</strong> to see more clearly.<br />

lobes, it also travels to the parietal lobes, which assist in<br />

judging where the object is in space (in relation to our<br />

visual field and us).<br />

Patients who have damage or tumours in parts of<br />

the temporal lobe responsible for identifying a visual<br />

stimulus may be unable to recognise an object or a<br />

familiar face (prosopagnosia). Patients who have damage<br />

to the parietal lobe will be able to recognise an object but<br />

may constantly bump into furniture or misjudge picking<br />

up their knife and fork.<br />

113


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

Wavelength in<br />

nanometres<br />

Wavelength in<br />

metres<br />

review 6.1<br />

Copy this table into <strong>you</strong>r workbook or use a computer. Complete the table using the information<br />

provided for each stage of the visual perception process. (Note that the image prior to entering<br />

the eye during the reception stage is the correct way up but by the time it reaches the retina, it has<br />

been turned upside down and back to front.)<br />

Gamma<br />

rays<br />

Reception Transduction Transmission Selection Organisation Interpretation<br />

the processes of visual<br />

perception<br />

reception and light energy<br />

The process of light entering the eye is an important part of our ability to receive and interpret<br />

a visual stimulus. However, before the eye can receive the visual stimulus, there are a couple of<br />

elements that must be in place.<br />

First, the light energy must be within the visible part of the electromagnetic spectrum.<br />

Wavelengths of between 360 and 760 nanometres form the visible spectrum (1nm = 1 billionth<br />

of a metre). The energy that enables us to see is what we call light energy, the visible part of the<br />

electromagnetic spectrum.<br />

400 500 600 700<br />

The Visible Spectrum<br />

Ultra-<br />

Infrared<br />

X-rays violet<br />

Radar<br />

rays<br />

rays<br />

Wavelength in nanometres<br />

400 500 600 700<br />

Perceived colour<br />

FM Television<br />

Short<br />

wave<br />

Radio waves<br />

Broadcast<br />

bands<br />

10 – 12 10 – 10 10 – 8 10 – 6 10 – 4 10 – 2 102 1 104 106 fig 6.5>> Electromagnetic spectrum, including the visible spectrum<br />

114<br />

Violet<br />

Indigo<br />

Blue<br />

Green<br />

Yellow<br />

Orange<br />

Red<br />

AC circuits


eception and absolute<br />

threshold<br />

The second important element in the eye detecting the light stimulus is that<br />

the light energy that falls within the visible light spectrum must be intense<br />

enough for the human eye to see. In other words, it must reach absolute<br />

threshold. The absolute threshold is the minimum amount of light energy<br />

needed for an observer to perceive a stimulus, in ideal conditions, 50 per cent<br />

of the time.<br />

One method psychologists use to measure absolute threshold is by<br />

presenting light stimuli at different intensities to see what level of intensity<br />

is needed for a person to detect the light. If that person detects it during the<br />

experiment about 50 per cent of the time at a particular intensity (the point<br />

at which they actually perceive it), then absolute threshold has been reached.<br />

Absolute threshold for the senses are:<br />

> Hearing: ticking of a watch six metres away.<br />

> Smell: one drop of perfume in a large house.<br />

> Taste: one teaspoon of sugar dissolved in 10 litres of water.<br />

> Touch: the wing of a fly falling on the cheek from a height<br />

of one centimetre.<br />

> Vision: the flame of a candle 50 kilometres away on a dark, clear night.<br />

These are based on sound scientific research but may not be the same for<br />

everyone and can vary depending on a range of environmental factors (noise,<br />

amount of light) and psychological factors (fatigue, motivation, stress,<br />

expectations). For example, if a person has had someone break into their<br />

house, they will be more highly attuned to sounds at night, and this may<br />

affect their usual absolute threshold for sound.<br />

the role of the eye in visual<br />

perception<br />

As already mentioned, light enters the eye through the cornea, a tough<br />

transparent tissue covering the front of the eye. It then passes through the<br />

pupil, the hole in the middle of the iris. The lens then focuses the light onto<br />

the retina, which contains photoreceptors (light-sensitive cells).<br />

The retina is nerve tissue that covers more than 50 per cent of the<br />

inner surface of the back of the eye. The retina contains two types of<br />

photoreceptors: rods and cones.<br />

Rods: There are 125 000 000 in each eye.<br />

> They are responsible for vision in low light (that is, they are very<br />

sensitive to light).<br />

> They are responsible for peripheral vision (out of the corner of the eye).<br />

They are concentrated at the edges of the retina.<br />

> They have low visual acuity (they can’t register detail).<br />

> They can register only in black and white.<br />

> They are most sensitive to light of approximately 500 nm wavelength.<br />

fig 6.6>> Your eyes can detect a<br />

candle flame up to 50 km away on a<br />

clear dark night.<br />

fig 6.7>> Your tastebuds can detect one<br />

teaspoon of sugar in 10 litres of water.<br />

115


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

fig 6.8>> Structure of the retina<br />

Cones: There are 6 500 000 in each eye.<br />

> They are concentrated in the middle of the retina.<br />

> They are responsible for vision of detail.<br />

> They are responsible for colour vision (and black-andwhite<br />

vision in daylight).<br />

> They require high levels of light to enable them to respond.<br />

<strong>did</strong> <strong>you</strong> <strong>know</strong>? Psychologists have<br />

learnt a great deal about our vision through<br />

experimentation on animals. They discovered how<br />

receptive fields in ganglion cells respond to an image<br />

after it has been transduced (converted to nerve<br />

impulses) by inserting a tiny electrode into the<br />

brain or retina of an animal. By holding the animal’s<br />

head still and flashing light to different sections of<br />

the visual field, they were able to identify and map<br />

the receptive fields of ganglion cells of the retina<br />

(Westen et al. 2009).<br />

116<br />

review 6.2<br />

1 What is the range of the visible light spectrum in<br />

nanometres?<br />

2 Draw a visible light spectrum in colour and show where<br />

ultraviolet and infrared rays are situated.<br />

3 Explain what happens in transduction.<br />

4 In a table, write down the characteristics for rods and<br />

cones (number, whether they are for daytime vision<br />

and so on).<br />

5 You have soccer practice after school during winter<br />

and <strong>you</strong>r coach wants <strong>you</strong> to play till it’s almost dark.<br />

Which photoreceptor is better suited to low light<br />

conditions?<br />

6 Grandma is busy knitting <strong>you</strong> a pair of woollen socks.<br />

She is sitting in her lounge with her spectacles and a<br />

bright light. She needs to focus on what she’s doing.<br />

Which photoreceptors are best suited to activities that<br />

require clear vision in bright light?


the principles of<br />

visual perception<br />

selection and<br />

organisation of visual<br />

stimuli<br />

At any given moment, vast amounts of visual stimuli enter<br />

the eye—far more than we can pay attention to. The brain<br />

selects and organises visual information according to a<br />

number of visual perceptual principles.<br />

Perceptual constancies<br />

Perceptual constancies enable us to maintain a stable<br />

perception of a stimulus although the image on the retina<br />

may change (e.g. in size or shape). The more familiar we<br />

are with the observed object, the more likely it is that<br />

we will maintain perceptual constancy of it. Perceptual<br />

constancies are usually learnt early in childhood.<br />

Size constancy<br />

This term refers to the fact that we maintain a constant<br />

perception of an object’s size even though the size of the<br />

image on the retina alters as the object moves nearer to<br />

or further from us. For example, when we look at a friend<br />

from a distance of five metres, he produces a retinal<br />

image twice the size of the one he produces when he is ten<br />

metres away. Because we are familiar with his height we<br />

don’t change our perception of it, but we use the size of<br />

the image as a cue to provide information about how far<br />

away he is.<br />

Shape constancy<br />

An object is perceived to maintain its <strong>know</strong>n shape despite<br />

the changing perspective from which it is observed. This<br />

is a learnt skill—a toddler may have difficulty perceiving a<br />

familiar toy if it is viewed from an unusual angle. Objects<br />

that are familiar to us can be accurately interpreted when<br />

viewed from any direction. For example, as a door opens,<br />

its retinal image changes shape from a rectangle to a<br />

trapezium, but we easily maintain our perception of a<br />

rectangular door.<br />

gestalt principles<br />

of perceptual<br />

organisation<br />

Gestalt is a German word that means ‘good form’. Gestalt<br />

psychology developed early in the twentieth century and<br />

is based on the principle that ‘the whole is greater than<br />

the sum of its parts’. In terms of sight, it deals with the<br />

tendency for our visual system to perceive what we see as a<br />

meaningful whole.<br />

How can this be? This answer is described in the<br />

Gestalt principles of visual perception.<br />

The phi phenomenon<br />

Although less common than they used to be, <strong>you</strong> can still<br />

find some local stores—often ‘Fish and Chip’ shops—with<br />

a line of light bulbs round the outside of the window. At<br />

night these bulbs light up one at a time, and as <strong>you</strong> watch<br />

it looks as if one light is running around the window. It’s<br />

not doing that, of course, but the property of motion is<br />

present in the whole, rather than in any of the individual<br />

parts (the light globes).<br />

fig 6.9>> Size constancy—the person casts a larger image on<br />

the retina as he moves closer, but we <strong>know</strong> he remains the same<br />

size, so we use the information to establish his distance from us.<br />

117


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

<strong>did</strong> <strong>you</strong> <strong>know</strong>?<br />

Animals such as leopards need to<br />

hide in trees so they can surprise<br />

their prey. Thinking about the<br />

principle of figure-ground,<br />

complete this sentence:<br />

‘The leopard’s black spots on<br />

its yellow fur mean that when<br />

it is in a tree its is<br />

broken up, it no longer stands<br />

out as a against the<br />

fig 6.11>> Can <strong>you</strong> see the soldier in<br />

camouflage? The contour is broken up<br />

to enable the figure to blend with the<br />

background.<br />

of the leaves and<br />

speckles of sunlight, and as long<br />

as it remains still it will be very<br />

hard for other animals to see.’<br />

118<br />

fig 6.10>> Rubin’s vase. Figure-ground—a special case. This figure may be seen as either<br />

a vase against a dark background or two faces in profile, depending on which is chosen as<br />

figure and which is chosen as the ground.<br />

This can also be seen outside Crown Casino in Melbourne, where lights<br />

look like bubbles flowing out of the top of a champagne bottle.<br />

This effect is <strong>know</strong>n as the phi phenomenon. Have <strong>you</strong> seen it<br />

anywhere else? Of course <strong>you</strong> have! It is the principle that makes cartoons,<br />

movies and television work. One still picture after another gives us the<br />

impression of movement on the screen.<br />

Figure-ground organisation<br />

The first Gestalt principle of visual perception that develops in infants is<br />

figure-ground organisation. The part of the visual field being attended<br />

to and focused on is the figure and its surroundings are the ground. Figure<br />

and ground are separated by an imagined contour (line). The contour<br />

is ‘owned’ by the figure. In children’s drawings, we often see a black line<br />

around a figure emphasising the separation of figure and ground.<br />

Camouflage is where the Gestalt principle of figure-ground is used to<br />

‘blend’ the contour of the figure (which usually stands out) against the<br />

ground (background), making it more difficult to see. We not only see<br />

camouflage in nature, with leopards, tigers and giraffes; the army uses<br />

camouflage uniforms to enable soldiers to blend in to their surroundings<br />

and avoid detection.<br />

Closure<br />

Closure is when we perceive an object as being whole despite its actually<br />

being incomplete. Many company logos use such incomplete figures in<br />

their designs.


Similarity<br />

Similarity is when the individual parts of a stimulus<br />

pattern are similar (e.g. in size, shape or colour).<br />

We tend to group them together as a meaningful<br />

‘whole’—a single unit.<br />

A<br />

X X X X X<br />

O O O O O<br />

X X X X X<br />

O O O O O<br />

We tend to perceive rows of ‘X’ and ‘O’ in group A<br />

and columns of ‘X’ and ‘O’ in group B.<br />

Proximity<br />

When the individual parts of a stimulus pattern are<br />

close to each other, we tend to group them together as<br />

a meaningful ‘whole’—a single unit.<br />

A<br />

X X X X X<br />

X X X X X<br />

X X X X X<br />

depth perception<br />

B<br />

X X X X X X X<br />

X X X X X X X<br />

X X X X X X X<br />

The point made above about Gestalt principles also<br />

holds for depth cues: there are many other depth cues<br />

besides those listed below. These examples show us<br />

how depth and distance are perceived.<br />

Depth and distance cues are vital to us. This is<br />

because we exist in a three-dimensional world but<br />

have only two-dimensional images on our two retinas<br />

from which to judge depth and distance.<br />

Depth cues may be binocular (using both eyes) or<br />

monocular (using one eye alone or both<br />

eyes together).<br />

B<br />

X O X O X<br />

X O X O X<br />

X O X O X<br />

X O X O X<br />

You can choose how to perceive a group of shapes<br />

(as rows or columns) but tend to perceive the ones<br />

on the right as rows, not columns.<br />

> Many other Gestalt principles of visual<br />

perception exist but these examples show how<br />

they work.<br />

fig 6.12>> Cartoonists use the principle of closure when they<br />

draw characters with little detail.<br />

6.1 investigate<br />

1 Make <strong>you</strong>r own animation!<br />

> You will need three sheets of A4 paper cut<br />

into 12 equal rectangles. This will give <strong>you</strong> a<br />

total of 36 pages for <strong>you</strong>r animation.<br />

> Place the 36 pieces together to create a<br />

small booklet and secure one end with a clip<br />

or staple.<br />

> Draw a stick figure of <strong>you</strong>r choice on each<br />

sheet, with each figure slightly different.<br />

Think of each piece of paper as the different<br />

frame of a cartoon or movie.<br />

> Once <strong>you</strong> have drawn a progression of<br />

pictures on the 36 pages, hold the secure<br />

end<br />

and flick through to watch <strong>you</strong>r animation<br />

come to life. You have just created <strong>you</strong>r own<br />

phi phenomenon!<br />

2 Use of Gestalt principles<br />

Look through magazines, newspapers and the<br />

Yellow Pages. How many company logos can <strong>you</strong><br />

find where one or more Gestalt principles is used<br />

in a clever way in the design? Make a table of<br />

<strong>you</strong>r findings using these headings: ‘Company’,<br />

‘Logo’, ‘Gestalt principle used’.<br />

119


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

Binocular depth cues<br />

Retinal disparity<br />

Retinal disparity is a cue that comes about because our<br />

eyes are set about 6–7 centimetres apart. When an object<br />

is within about 20 metres of a viewer (with the strongest<br />

depth cue occurring within 7 metres), each eye receives a<br />

slightly different image on the retina, resulting from the<br />

different angles of view from each eye to the object being<br />

observed. The brain fuses these two images together in<br />

stereoscopic vision. The more different the two images,<br />

the closer the object will be to the viewer.<br />

Try this <strong>you</strong>rself:<br />

> Hold a pen at arm’s length and look past it at the other<br />

side of the room.<br />

> Close one eye and then the other and watch how far the<br />

pen ‘jumps’ from side to side.<br />

> Now bring the pen closer—about 40 cm from <strong>you</strong>r<br />

eyes—and repeat the process. What do <strong>you</strong> notice?<br />

The process of retinal disparity is artificially recreated in<br />

‘magic eye’ pictures from two flat, two-dimensional patterns<br />

fig 6.13>> Magic eye picture—what can <strong>you</strong> see?<br />

120<br />

viewed from about 20 centimetres. Each eye observes a<br />

slightly different view of the same scene and the brain fuses<br />

the two images together in the same way it would when<br />

observing a real (three-dimensional) scene.<br />

If <strong>you</strong> can look ‘through’ the picture below, <strong>you</strong> will see<br />

a ‘three-dimensional’ star in the middle of it.<br />

Convergence<br />

Convergence is also a binocular depth cue. As an object<br />

comes closer to us, the eyes turn inwards to keep the<br />

object centred on the retina. This cue, again, operates<br />

for objects within about seven metres. The brain reads<br />

the amount of turning from the tension of the muscles<br />

that move the eyes and uses this to make judgments of<br />

distance. The more the turning, the closer the object is to<br />

the viewer. Try this <strong>you</strong>rself:<br />

> Hold <strong>you</strong>r pen vertically at arm’s length and slowly<br />

bring it closer to <strong>you</strong>r nose, watching it with both eyes<br />

all the time.<br />

> As the pen gets close to <strong>you</strong>r nose, <strong>you</strong> can feel <strong>you</strong>r<br />

eyes turning and soon <strong>you</strong> go ‘cross-eyed’.<br />

© 2009 Magic Eye Inc.


Monocular depth cues<br />

Two types of monocular depth cues enable us to judge<br />

depth and distance using one eye: accommodation<br />

(a monocular cue) and pictorial cues.<br />

Accommodation<br />

Accommodation involves the lens of the eye (located<br />

behind the iris) changing shape so that it can focus light<br />

rays onto the retina. Small muscles called ciliary muscles<br />

control whether the lens bulges (for closer objects) or<br />

flattens (for more distant objects). At the same time, the<br />

tension in the ciliary muscles is received by the brain<br />

to confirm the location of the object being viewed. The<br />

greater the tension, the closer the object.<br />

Try this <strong>you</strong>rself. Take a pen and close one eye. Move<br />

the pen as close to <strong>you</strong> as <strong>you</strong> can whilest maintaining<br />

focus. You should be able to focus on a close object<br />

(between 8 and 10 cm away—depending on <strong>you</strong>r eyesight).<br />

Keep focusing on the pen until <strong>you</strong> feel the tension within<br />

<strong>you</strong>r eye. That’s <strong>you</strong>r ciliary muscles at work keeping <strong>you</strong>r<br />

brain informed.<br />

Pictorial depth cues<br />

Pictorial depth cues are named ‘pictorial’ because they are<br />

used by artists to create a three-dimensional perception of<br />

something that exists on a two-dimensional surface.<br />

> Linear perspective (first described by Leonardo da<br />

Vinci) is one of the most basic skills an artist uses<br />

to create apparent depth. Parallel lines are made<br />

to converge as they extend along the page to an<br />

imaginary point (where in theory they meet) at the<br />

horizon, as shown in Figure 6.14.<br />

> Interposition (overlap) is based on the partial<br />

blocking or obscuring of one object by another.<br />

The obscured object appears to be further away<br />

than the object obscuring (overlapping) it. This<br />

is an effective cue for determining which objects<br />

are closer than others, but it is not as effective for<br />

actually judging distance.<br />

> Texture gradient is used to make surfaces in a<br />

picture appear to recede into the distance. Artists<br />

draw less and less detail as a surface is more and more<br />

distant, the same way we see it in real life. This is<br />

shown in the boardwalk at Rhyll, Phillip Island. In the<br />

foreground we can see every detail of the wood and<br />

the mesh, but as the boardwalk gets further away it<br />

becomes much less detailed (Figure 6.15).<br />

fig 6.14>> Linear perspective shows parallel lines converging in<br />

the distance.<br />

fig 6.15>> Rhyll boardwalk<br />

> Relative size is a pictorial depth cue based on our<br />

tendency to perceive the object producing the largest<br />

retinal image as being the nearest, and the object<br />

producing the smallest retinal image as being the<br />

farthest. For this cue it is necessary to <strong>know</strong> the real<br />

size of the objects so that accurate comparisons can<br />

be made. Think about watching a game of football<br />

from behind <strong>you</strong>r team’s goal—<strong>you</strong> realise that the<br />

players at the opposite goal are far away; <strong>you</strong> don’t<br />

think they are tiny!<br />

> Height in the visual field is a pictorial depth cue<br />

that shows depth by portraying objects further away<br />

as being closer to the horizon. In a picture, objects in<br />

the sky, such as aeroplanes, clouds and birds, will be<br />

perceived as further away as they become lower in the<br />

visual field (closer to the horizon). On the other hand,<br />

trees, houses and people (objects on the ground) will be<br />

perceived as further away as they become higher in the<br />

visual field (closer to the horizon).<br />

121


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

fig 6.16>> Which of these animals is being hunted by the<br />

warrior?<br />

6.2 investigate<br />

122<br />

because of what we have seen in the past.<br />

You be the artist<br />

1 Draw a picture using the following pictorial depth<br />

cues. (Alternatively, find a picture on the Internet<br />

and use that instead.) Label <strong>you</strong>r picture with the<br />

pictorial cues, making sure that <strong>you</strong> explain each<br />

one to illustrate <strong>you</strong>r understanding.<br />

> linear perspective<br />

> overlap/interposition<br />

> texture gradient<br />

> relative size<br />

> height in the visual field.<br />

2 What is the key difference between monocular<br />

and binocular depth cues?<br />

3 Why do we receive a slightly different image on<br />

each retina?<br />

4 Create a poem or a song using monocular and<br />

binocular depth cues. Make sure that each<br />

term is either defined or explained in some way<br />

and that <strong>you</strong>r poem/song makes sense. Tune<br />

suggestions:<br />

> School anthem<br />

> Mary had a little lamb<br />

> Itsy bitsy spider<br />

> Come up with one of <strong>you</strong>r own!<br />

> Present <strong>you</strong>r creation to the class!<br />

A picture similar to Figure 6.16 was used by<br />

researchers to discover that members of some African<br />

tribes <strong>did</strong> not use the last two cues to estimate depth<br />

and distance. The Africans were very surprised at what<br />

they thought was a picture of a hunter attempting to<br />

spear a very small rhinoceros!<br />

> We <strong>know</strong> that a rhino is much bigger than a zebra,<br />

so we apply our <strong>know</strong>ledge to judge that the zebra<br />

is much closer to us—at about the same distance as<br />

the hunter.<br />

> We would apply the depth cue of ‘height in the<br />

visual field’ to judge that the hunter is about to<br />

spear the zebra, not the rhinoceros, which is in the<br />

distance.<br />

Cross-cultural research has allowed psychologists<br />

to understand that even though every human being<br />

with normal vision receives exactly the same visual<br />

information, our individual and cultural experiences<br />

can influence our interpretation of what we see<br />

the effect of<br />

psychological factors<br />

on perceptual set<br />

Expectations that are created by our previous<br />

experiences, the environment in which a stimulus is<br />

seen, our motivation and even our emotional state can<br />

combine to predispose us to perceive the world in a<br />

certain way.<br />

Perceptual set:<br />

Perceptual set is:<br />

1 A predisposition to perceive stimuli in a specific way,<br />

i.e. interpreting what we see according to certain<br />

preconceptions (interpretation).<br />

2 A predisposition to attend to only certain features or<br />

aspects of our field of view (selection).<br />

Several factors can influence perceptual set,<br />

including:<br />

Previous experience: Previous experience can affect<br />

visual perception, especially if the experience holds<br />

significant personal meaning. The same stimulus can<br />

be interpreted differently by different people.<br />

Context: Context sometimes has an immediate effect<br />

on our expectations. Context refers to the environment<br />

in which a perceived stimulus is observed.


PREVIOUS EXPERIENCE<br />

MOTIVATION<br />

EMOTION<br />

CONTEXT<br />

fig 6.17>> Factors affecting our perceptual set<br />

Motivation: Very often we see what we want to see. On a long drive in the<br />

country, running low on petrol, a sign that reads ‘FOOD AHEAD’ may well<br />

be interpreted as ‘FUEL AHEAD’.<br />

Emotion: We could interpret someone’s facial expression as showing<br />

laughter or crying, depending on how we are feeling ourselves.<br />

fig 6.18>> Quickly look at this illustration. Close <strong>you</strong>r eyes and recall what <strong>you</strong> saw.<br />

<strong>did</strong> <strong>you</strong> <strong>know</strong>? Researchers found that when they<br />

presented a picture of a farmyard to a group of subjects and then<br />

asked them to describe it from memory, they were able to do so<br />

quite well. However, when shown the picture above, the subjects<br />

were surprised by the picture of a giant squid outside the barn—it<br />

is out of context, so they concentrated on this and failed to pay<br />

attention to the other items in the picture.<br />

PERCEPTUAL SET<br />

Bugelski & Alampay (1961) performed the very famous rat-man experiment.<br />

One group of participants was shown a series of line-drawings of faces and<br />

then the ambiguous rat-man stimulus—the majority identified it as an old<br />

man. The other group was shown a series of line-drawings of animals and<br />

then the ambiguous stimulus—the majority identified it as a rat or mouse.<br />

This shows how the perceptual set, created by prior experience, influences<br />

perception.<br />

fig 6.19>> Rat-man. Can <strong>you</strong> see the rat<br />

and the man?<br />

SELECTION<br />

INTERPRETATION<br />

123


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

review 6.3<br />

1 Describe how the brain uses certain principles to<br />

organise visual information.<br />

2 What theory underlies all of the Gestalt principles?<br />

3 Explain the phi phenomenon using local examples.<br />

4 What is figure-ground organisation? Why is a<br />

‘contour’ important to this principle?<br />

5 Why do we find it so difficult to see animals<br />

camouflaged in their environment?<br />

6 Explain how ‘closure’ works.<br />

7 Give an example of similarity and explain why the<br />

example is seen as a single unit.<br />

6.3 investigate<br />

124<br />

Try the rat-man experiment<br />

1 Trace the drawings below onto pieces of card or paper.<br />

8 What is retinal disparity and why is it important to<br />

depth perception?<br />

9 Convergence is also a binocular depth cue. Explain<br />

how this works.<br />

10 Why are depth cues so important to us?<br />

11 What techniques have been developed by artists to<br />

show visual representations of three dimensions on a<br />

two-dimensional surface?<br />

12 Do these visual clues apply to all humans? If not why<br />

not?<br />

13 List the groups of psychological factors that influence<br />

our visual perception.<br />

2 Show either the ‘faces’ group of cards or the ‘animals’ group, one at a time, to volunteer participants.<br />

3 Show the ambiguous rat-man stimulus (Figure 6.19, page 123) to each volunteer and record the response.<br />

4 Compare the responses of the two groups. Did the ‘faces’ group identify an old man more than the ‘animals’ group?<br />

5 Repeat the experiment with other volunteers, but this time show them a mixture of three ‘faces’ and three<br />

‘animals’ before showing the rat-man stimulus.<br />

> Step 5 is an important step in the research. Why should this be done?


distortions of perception<br />

Visual illusions are mistaken perceptions of visual stimuli. We may misjudge length, curvature,<br />

position, speed or direction in a visual illusion. It is the brain that is tricked, not the eye!<br />

A visual illusion occurs when perception consistently differs from objective reality.<br />

the ponzo illusion<br />

In the Ponzo Illusion, for example (Figure 6.20), the upper horizontal line is perceived to be longer<br />

than the lower horizontal line. Why? We mistakenly perceive this because of the depth cues of linear<br />

perspective and height in visual field—both of which make the upper line appear to be further from us.<br />

Because it casts the same-size image on the retina, we perceive the ‘more distant’ line to be longer.<br />

fig 6.20>> The Ponzo Illusion<br />

the müller-lyer illusion<br />

Another illusion, the Müller-Lyer Illusion (Figure 6.21), consists of two lines of equal length, each<br />

having opposite-shaped patterns on the ends. One line has regular arrowheads; the other has<br />

inverted arrowheads (feather-tails). The line with feather-tails is perceived as being longer than the<br />

arrow-headed line.<br />

There are two theories to explain the Müller-Lyer Illusion.<br />

The carpentered world hypothesis<br />

The carpentered world hypothesis (Figure 6.22): British psychologist Richard Gregory believes that<br />

the illusion involves the ‘misapplication of size constancy’. In a three-dimensional world, we maintain a<br />

constant perception of the size of a familiar object by considering its distance from us. Gregory argues<br />

that we mentally make a three-dimensional form of each figure, using familiar features of buildings to do<br />

so. This causes the observer to perceive the arrow-headed line as the leading vertical edge of a building’s<br />

outer wall (closest to the observer). The line with the feather-tails is perceived as being further away<br />

(the inside corner of a room). Because it is perceived as being the more distant of the two, and each line<br />

produces an equal length image on the retina, we interpret the line with feather-tails to be longer.<br />

Evidence to support this theory came when the illusion was shown to country-dwelling Zulu<br />

people who live in round huts with rounded doors and windows. They <strong>did</strong> not perceive the lines to<br />

be different lengths as they had never learnt to judge distance from corners and angles. Zulu people<br />

living in cities in South Africa were fooled by the illusion like everyone else.<br />

The perceptual compromise theory<br />

Ross Day, an Australian psychologist, proposes a different theory.<br />

1 Both parallel lines cast identical-sized images on the retina.<br />

2 The arrowhead or feather-tail lines at the ends of the figures create ‘open’ figures that cause us to<br />

apply the Gestalt principle of closure.<br />

fig 6.21>><br />

The Müller-Lyer<br />

Illusion—which<br />

vertical line is<br />

longer?<br />

125


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

fig 6.22>> Which room has the higher walls?<br />

3 This creates a more ‘solid’ figure as shown by the blue<br />

lines in Figure 6.23.<br />

4 Because of the perceptual compromise made, we<br />

perceive each figure to be the length of the average<br />

between the internal (black) line and the external (blue)<br />

lines (see Figure 6.24).<br />

5 As a result of this, figure A is perceived to be much<br />

shorter than figure B, each being perceived to be as<br />

long as the distance between the green lines shown<br />

in Figure 6.24.<br />

Evidence in support of this theory is that the illusion<br />

occurs even when the ends of the lines are ‘U’-shaped or<br />

completed circles.<br />

6.4 investigate<br />

126<br />

Extend <strong>you</strong>r <strong>know</strong>ledge<br />

Isn’t it amazing how we often look at the full moon<br />

on the horizon and it appears to be huge—yet that<br />

same night, if we look at the moon when it’s high<br />

in the sky, it looks normal size! Perform an Internet<br />

search for ‘Moon Illusion’. Explain how this illusion<br />

occurs.<br />

fig 6.25>> If the arrow ends are replaced with ‘U’ shapes<br />

or circles, the illusion still occurs.<br />

A B A<br />

B<br />

fig 6.23>> The blue lines<br />

show how our mind applies the<br />

Gestalt principle of closure so<br />

that Figure B appears larger.<br />

fig 6.24>> Our mind averages<br />

out the black and blue lines<br />

and we perceive the length as<br />

shown by the green lines.


the ames room illusion<br />

The Ames Room shows that we maintain shape constancy (of the<br />

room) at the expense of size constancy.<br />

The room is constructed in a trapezoidal shape, but made to<br />

appear square when observed through a peephole. It is necessary<br />

to prevent the use of both eyes, so that the strongest (binocular)<br />

depth cues do not work. The rear wall actually runs from right to<br />

left, away from the viewer. The far-left corner is twice as far from<br />

the peephole as the far-right corner. To add to the illusion, the<br />

ceiling is not parallel to the floor. It slopes upwards from right<br />

to left, while the floor slopes downwards from right to left, so<br />

that the height of the room at far left is double that at far right.<br />

When a person walks from the left corner to the right corner, the<br />

observer maintains shape constancy, but cannot also maintain size<br />

constancy—so the person appears to be growing rapidly in size.<br />

review 6.4<br />

1 Describe the Ponzo Illusion.<br />

2 Draw some of <strong>you</strong>r own Müller-Lyer illusions and<br />

try them out on <strong>you</strong>r fellow students.<br />

3 Do these visual clues apply to all humans? If not why not?<br />

4 Explain why the Ames Room Illusion occurs.<br />

5 Why does the person who is viewed through the peep-hole<br />

appear to grow and shrink before <strong>you</strong>r eyes?<br />

6 Go to www.illusionworks.com. Create a PowerPoint<br />

presentation of one other illusion and explain why this occurs.<br />

7 M.C. Escher is a famous lithographic artist who created many<br />

impossible figures. Perfom an Internet search for M.C. Escher<br />

and prepare a presentation on his work.<br />

6.5 investigate<br />

Visual illusions<br />

1 How do visual illusions occur?<br />

2 There are two theories to explain the Müller-Lyer Illusion. Choose one<br />

of these theories and briefly outline the key points.<br />

3 How does the Ames Room work? Describe the construction and how<br />

this influences what is seen when looking through the peephole.<br />

4 Using the Internet, search ‘visual illusions’ and create a PowerPoint<br />

presentation of the illusions <strong>you</strong> have found to share with the class.<br />

Make sure that <strong>you</strong> define ‘Visual Illusions’ at the beginning of <strong>you</strong>r<br />

presentation and create at least 10 slides.<br />

fig 6.26>> The Ames Room is not really rectangular,<br />

but because of our lifetime of experience with<br />

rectangular rooms, we perceive it to be bounded by<br />

right-angles. In fact the ‘smaller’ person is simply twice<br />

as far from the observer as the ‘larger’ person. The<br />

shape of the room is shown below.<br />

127


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

chapter summary<br />

> We come to interpret the reality of our visual world<br />

through the body and mind working together to<br />

enable the process of visual perception.<br />

> Perceptual processes are considered to be adaptive—<br />

our sensory systems (smell, taste, touch, vision, hearing)<br />

have evolved to ensure our survival as a species and to<br />

assist us in reproduction.<br />

> The six stages of visual perception are:<br />

1 Reception: when light energy enters the eye and<br />

is focused on the retina<br />

2 Transduction: when the light energy is converted into<br />

nerve impulses by the rods and cones of the retina<br />

3 Transmission: when the nerve impulses travel via<br />

the optic nerve to the occipital lobes at the rear<br />

of the brain<br />

4 Selection: when the image is pulled apart by cells<br />

called feature detectors that respond to lines of a<br />

certain length, orientation or direction of movement<br />

5 Organisation: when visual information reaching<br />

the brain’s visual cortex is reorganised using three<br />

groups of visual perceptual principles<br />

6 Interpretation: when the brain’s temporal lobes<br />

identify the object by comparing incoming<br />

information with information already stored in<br />

memory, and the parietal lobes assist in judging<br />

where the object is in space.<br />

> The energy that enables us to see (light energy) is<br />

the visible part of the electromagnetic spectrum—<br />

wavelengths between approximately 360 and 760<br />

nanometres (1 nm = 1 billionth of a metre).<br />

> For a visual stimulus to be perceived by a person, it<br />

must reach absolute threshold. This is the minimum<br />

128<br />

amount of light energy needed for an observer to<br />

perceive a stimulus in ideal conditions 50 per cent of<br />

the time.<br />

> The retina, which covers over 50 per cent of<br />

the back of the eye, contains two types of<br />

photoreceptors. The 125 000 000 rods in each<br />

eye are responsible for vision in low light and for<br />

peripheral vision; they only register black and white.<br />

The 6 500 000 cones in each eye are responsible for<br />

vision of detail, and colour vision (and black-and-<br />

white vision in daylight).<br />

> The visual perceptual principles include perceptual<br />

constancies, which enable us to maintain a stable<br />

perception of a stimulus in terms of size or shape;<br />

Gestalt principles (the phi phenomenon, figure-<br />

ground, closure, proximity and similarity), which<br />

relate to the tendency for our visual system to<br />

perceive what we see as a meaningful whole; and<br />

depth and distance cues, which govern our ability<br />

to view the world in 3-D.<br />

> Binocular (requiring two eyes) depth cues are retinal<br />

disparity and convergence. Monocular (one eye)<br />

depth cues are accommodation and pictorial depth<br />

cues (linear perspective, interposition/overlap,<br />

texture gradient, relative size, height in the<br />

visual field).<br />

> Previous experience, motivation, emotion and<br />

context can influence the way we perceive a visual<br />

stimulus. This is referred to as perceptual set.<br />

> Visual illusions are mistaken perceptions of visual<br />

stimuli that occur consistently. They may include<br />

misjudgments of length, curvature, position, speed<br />

or direction. The Ponzo, Müller-Lyer, Ames Room<br />

and Moon illusions are well-<strong>know</strong>n examples.


test <strong>you</strong>r understanding<br />

1 Which of the following statements concerning the<br />

stimulus energy for vision is incorrect?<br />

a Electromagnetic energy of about 380<br />

nanometres is perceived as blue or violet.<br />

b Electromagnetic energy of about 760<br />

nanometres is perceived as red.<br />

c Electromagnetic energy of about 380<br />

nanometres is not visible to humans.<br />

d Electromagnetic energy of about 800<br />

nanometres is not visible to humans.<br />

2 Rods are most responsive to electromagnetic energy<br />

of which colour?<br />

a violet/blue<br />

b blue/green<br />

c green/yellow<br />

d orange/red<br />

3 Which of the following lists the groups of visual<br />

perceptual principles that we use to organise and<br />

help interpret stimuli?<br />

a perceptual constituencies; Gestalt principles;<br />

depth and distance cues<br />

b perceptual set; Gestalt principles; expectancies<br />

c depth and distance cues; perceptual<br />

constancies; Gestalt principles<br />

d visual illusions; Gestalt principles; depth and<br />

distance cues.<br />

4 Which of the following correctly pairs the names<br />

of two sets of visual perceptual principles with<br />

examples from those sets?<br />

a Gestalt principles—linear perspective; depth<br />

and distance cues—convergence<br />

b perceptual set—expectancies; Gestalt<br />

principles—closure<br />

c depth and distance cues—retinal disparity;<br />

Gestalt principles—orientation constancy<br />

d Gestalt principles—figure-ground; depth and<br />

distance cues—texture gradient.<br />

5 As I was driving along the freeway a huge truck<br />

came hurtling up behind me. It seemed to take ages<br />

for the long body to get past as it overtook me.<br />

Afterwards it gradually disappeared up the road in<br />

front of me. Which of the following principles would<br />

I have applied to help me realise that the truck<br />

remained the same object although its image on my<br />

retinas changed?<br />

a shape constancy and size constancy<br />

b size constancy and orientation constancy<br />

c brightness constancy and orientation constancy<br />

d brightness constancy and size constancy.<br />

6 In the figure below, we tend to perceive a building,<br />

rather than a series of disjointed lines.<br />

Which two Gestalt principles are mainly responsible<br />

for this perception?<br />

a closure and similarity<br />

b closure and figure-ground<br />

c figure-ground and similarity<br />

d similarity and figure-ground.<br />

7 The binocular depth cues are:<br />

a linear perspective and convergence<br />

b retinal disparity and accommodation<br />

c relative size and linear perspective<br />

d convergence and retinal disparity.<br />

8 The strongest depth cues are:<br />

a convergence and retinal disparity<br />

b linear perspective and convergence<br />

c pictorial cues<br />

d monocular cues.<br />

129


chapter 6>> visual perception<br />

test <strong>you</strong>r understanding<br />

9 One evening I saw a crowd of tiny dots in the sky.<br />

These dots became larger and suddenly a large flock<br />

of mutton-birds began to land. I was not surprised<br />

that the dots I had seen in the distance were actually<br />

big sea-birds because I had applied the principle of:<br />

a shape constancy<br />

b orientation constancy<br />

c brightness constancy<br />

d size constancy.<br />

10 Jack and his team were doing fielding practice,<br />

catching a ball that the coach hit high in the air.<br />

After a few people had dropped catches, the coach<br />

insisted that all players hold their hands in front of<br />

their face and watch the approaching ball through<br />

their fingers. Jack was surprised that his success<br />

rate improved using the new technique. What is the<br />

reason for his improved performance?<br />

a Using the new method, he could use<br />

convergence and retinal disparity cues to help<br />

him judge depth and distance.<br />

b Using the new method, he could use<br />

accommodation and convergence cues to help<br />

him judge depth and distance.<br />

c Using the new method, he could use pictorial<br />

cues to help him judge depth and distance.<br />

d Using the new method, he could use monocular<br />

cues to help him judge depth and distance.<br />

11 In drawing a landscape, John used the cue of height<br />

130<br />

in the visual field to give the impression of depth and<br />

distance. This meant that:<br />

a objects that were further away were drawn<br />

higher up in the plane of the picture<br />

b objects that were further away were drawn lower<br />

down in the plane of the picture<br />

c objects that were further away were drawn<br />

closer to the horizon in the picture<br />

d objects that were nearer the horizon were drawn<br />

smaller than similar objects in the foreground.<br />

12 As her family was driving across to Perth from<br />

Melbourne, seven-year-old Jodi called out, ‘Look<br />

Mum, there’s hundreds of rabbits!’ As they gradually<br />

got closer, Jodi said ‘Oh! They’re actually great big<br />

kangaroos!’ What had caused Jodi’s initial error of<br />

perception?<br />

a previous experience with rabbits, causing a<br />

perceptual set that led to selection of specific<br />

parts of the scene to be attended to<br />

b previous experience with rabbits, causing<br />

a perceptual set that led to a particular<br />

interpretation of the scene<br />

c the context in which the animals were seen,<br />

causing a perceptual set that led to selection of<br />

specific parts of the scene to be attended to<br />

d the context in which the animals were seen,<br />

causing a perceptual set that led to a particular<br />

interpretation of the scene.<br />

13 Psychologist Colin Turnbull took a pygmy from the<br />

rainforests of South America to the plains of Kenya in<br />

Africa. The pygmy looked at the herds of thousands<br />

of wildebeest in the distance and perceived them to<br />

be a large colony of ants a few metres from him. The<br />

reason for this error in perception is:<br />

a previous experience with ants, causing an<br />

expectancy that led to selection of specific<br />

parts of the scene to be attended to through<br />

perceptual set<br />

b previous experience with ants, causing an<br />

expectancy that led to a particular interpretation<br />

of the scene through perceptual set<br />

c the context in which the animals were seen,<br />

causing a perceptual set that led to selection<br />

of specific parts of the scene to be attended to<br />

through perceptual set<br />

d the context in which the animals were seen,<br />

causing a perceptual set that led to a particular<br />

interpretation of the scene through perceptual<br />

set.


14 The Ames Room Illusion is one in which we<br />

mistakenly perceive a person to change in size as<br />

they cross a room. The illusion is caused by the fact<br />

that we:<br />

a maintain size-constancy for the room at the<br />

expense of size-constancy for the person<br />

b maintain shape-constancy for the room at the<br />

expense of shape-constancy for the person<br />

c maintain size-constancy for the room at the<br />

expense of shape-constancy for the person<br />

d maintain shape-constancy for the room at the<br />

expense of size-constancy for the person.<br />

15 The Müller-Lyer Illusion is one in which we mistakenly<br />

think that one parallel line is longer than the other<br />

because of the pattern on the end of each of the<br />

lines. The best description of the illusion is that:<br />

a the line with the arrowheads on the ends is<br />

perceived to be shorter than the line with the<br />

feather-tails on the ends<br />

b the line with the arrowheads on the ends is<br />

perceived to be longer than the line with the<br />

feather-tails on the ends<br />

c the two lines are perceived to be the same<br />

length but at different distances from us<br />

d the two lines are perceived to be the average<br />

length of the outermost and innermost<br />

measurements.<br />

16 When we are in broad daylight looking at an object,<br />

we like to look straight at it and hold it in the centre<br />

of our visual field. When it is almost dark, we cannot<br />

pick out colours and we see objects better when they<br />

are on the edge of our field of view. With reference<br />

to the retina and photoreceptors, explain why this is<br />

the case.<br />

17 The processes that are carried out by the<br />

photoreceptors during visual perception include<br />

reception, transduction and transmission.<br />

Explain what is meant by transduction.<br />

Explain what is meant by transmission.<br />

18 With reference to the picture shown below, identify<br />

four depth cues that are shown and describe how<br />

each one enables us to perceive depth in this<br />

picture.<br />

19 Explain, using an example, the Gestalt principle of<br />

similarity.<br />

20 Explain what is meant by the term ‘perceptual set’.<br />

Give an example that shows <strong>you</strong>r understanding of<br />

this principle.<br />

21 The Ames Room Illusion is a visual illusion.<br />

a Define ‘visual illusion’<br />

b i Describe the Ames Room Illusion.<br />

(Be careful to state what is perceived.)<br />

ii Why does the illusion occur?<br />

131


unit 1 aos 1>> research methods and ethics<br />

research methods and ethics<br />

132<br />

1 case study<br />

Tuskegee Study of Untreated Syphilis in Negro Males<br />

Syphilis is a sexually transmitted infection and can<br />

affect both men and women. It is transmitted through<br />

close skin-to-skin contact and is highly contagious. It<br />

can also be passed on by an infected mother to her<br />

unborn child. If left untreated, it can damage the heart,<br />

brain, eyes and bones with fatal consequences.<br />

The Tuskegee Study of Untreated Syphilis in Negro<br />

Males was a clinical study that began in 1932 and<br />

ended in 1972 and recruited 399 males with syphilis<br />

and 201 males without syphilis (the control group).<br />

The main organisations involved in the study were<br />

the Tuskegee Institute (a historically black college)<br />

and the Public Health Service Venereal Disease Clinic<br />

in Arkansas. Most of the African American males<br />

recruited to be in the study were poor and illiterate<br />

and were told they had ‘bad blood’. As an incentive to<br />

participate, they were offered free medical treatment,<br />

transportation to the clinic, meals and burial insurance<br />

in case of death.<br />

Questions<br />

Complete the following with reference to this research.<br />

1 When <strong>did</strong> the Tuskegee experiment begin?<br />

2 How were participants selected?<br />

3 How many participants were involved in the study?<br />

4 What experimental design was used (e.g. matched participants, independent groups,<br />

repeated measures)?<br />

5 Were participants offered any incentive(s) to participate in the study? If so, what ethical<br />

principle was breached?<br />

6 What year <strong>did</strong> the experiment begin?<br />

7 Explain what ‘syphilis’ is and how it is acquired. Use the Internet to give additional<br />

information to answer this question.<br />

8 What was the aim of the experiment?<br />

9 How <strong>did</strong> the researchers track the effects of syphilis on the participants?<br />

10 Why is this experiment considered unethical by today’s standards? Explain <strong>you</strong>r answer with<br />

reference to the ethical principles outlined in Chapter 1.<br />

11 What were the long-term problems caused by this experiment?<br />

When the study began in 1932, treatment of syphilis<br />

was often unreliable and many of the medications<br />

used were toxic. The researchers wanted to observe<br />

the progression of the disease and subjected the<br />

participants to dangerous and painful procedures<br />

without actually treating their syphilis. The study also<br />

required all participants to undergo an autopsy after<br />

their death in order for their families to receive the<br />

funeral benefits promised. In 1947, penicillin became<br />

the standard treatment for the disease with excellent<br />

results. However, the participants in the study who<br />

were diagnosed with syphilis were not given treatment.<br />

Some were given a placebo (not the real treatment).<br />

At the end of the study, only 74 of the original<br />

test subjects survived. Twenty-eight of the men died<br />

directly of syphilis, 100 died of related complications,<br />

40 of their wives had been infected and 19 of their<br />

children were born with congenital syphilis.<br />

Reference:


2 case study<br />

The Aversion Project<br />

Aversion therapy is a treatment which has sometimes<br />

been used to stop people smoking or engaging in<br />

other unwanted behaviours. The patient is exposed to<br />

the stimulus (the source of unwanted behaviour, e.g.<br />

smoking cigarettes) and at the same time subjected<br />

to something unpleasant (e.g. electric shock, physical<br />

pain or a nausea-inducing drug). This process is<br />

repeated until the patient begins to associate the<br />

stimulus (cigarette) with the unpleasant consequence.<br />

This method was used in the South African armed<br />

forces to ‘cure’ homosexuality.<br />

From 1971 to 1989 South Africa’s apartheid army<br />

wanted to eliminate homosexuality in their army.<br />

White lesbian and gay soldiers were forced to undergo<br />

‘sex-change’ operations and many were subjected to<br />

Questions<br />

Complete the following with reference to this research.<br />

1 What was the aim of the Aversion Project?<br />

2 Write a hypothesis on which such a project is based.<br />

3 Explain what aversion therapy involves.<br />

4 What methods were used on soldiers to change their sexual preference?<br />

5 What ethical principles were breached in this experiment? Identify as many as <strong>you</strong> can and<br />

explain them in terms of the experiment (refer to Chapter 1).<br />

6 Could this experiment be replicated/repeated today? Explain <strong>you</strong>r answer.<br />

7 MKULTRA was the code name for a covert CIA research program run by the Office of<br />

Scientific Intelligence on mind-control and chemical interrogation. Using the Internet<br />

to research, write approximately 250 words outlining the aim of the project and what it<br />

involved. What ethical principles were breached in this experiment? Outline as many as <strong>you</strong><br />

can and explain each one in relation to this program.<br />

chemical castration, electric shock, and other invasive<br />

medical procedures. According to former apartheid<br />

army surgeons, approximately 900 people were forced<br />

to have ‘sexual reassignment’ operations at military<br />

hospitals as part of a top-secret program to get rid of<br />

homosexuality from the armed forces.<br />

Army psychiatrists and chaplains were instructed<br />

to expose suspected homosexuals in the armed<br />

forces and send them to military psychiatric units<br />

for ‘treatment’. Those who could not be ‘cured’ with<br />

drugs, aversion shock therapy, hormone treatment and<br />

other ‘radical’ psychiatric methods, were chemically<br />

castrated or given sex-change operations. Most of the<br />

victims were white males aged 16 to 24 years who were<br />

drafted (forced) into the apartheid army.<br />

133


unit 1 aos 1>> assessment activities<br />

assessment activities<br />

The following assessment tasks cover a range of topics from<br />

chapters 2 to 6 and are designed to assist <strong>you</strong> in gaining a<br />

deeper understanding of the information covered.<br />

research investigation<br />

Investigate people’s perception and understanding of the<br />

roles of psychologists and psychiatrists.<br />

Working in pairs, construct a short survey to find out what<br />

people actually <strong>know</strong> about psychiatry and psychology.<br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

Each pair is to survey at least five people.<br />

Think of at least 7–10 different statements that relate<br />

to either a psychiatrist or a psychologist. (Note: do<br />

not write statements that are relevant to both.)<br />

Note both correct responses and incorrect responses.<br />

Collate the data.<br />

Formally write up this research investigation,<br />

following the criteria outlined on page 36.<br />

annotated folio of practical<br />

activities<br />

Guided by <strong>you</strong>r teacher, select a range of activities from<br />

chapters 2 to 6 to present for assessment, for example:<br />

><br />

><br />

one research investigation per chapter<br />

written responses to the Review items in chapters.<br />

The activities should be presented in the manner as<br />

instructed within the text or by <strong>you</strong>r teacher.<br />

media response<br />

Using a range of sources, such as newspapers, the<br />

Internet or magazines, find an article on psychology<br />

research related to the biological approach.<br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

><br />

134<br />

What is the article about?<br />

Is it about human or animal subjects?<br />

Is the information presented in a scientifically valid<br />

and accurate manner? Explain.<br />

What was the aim of this research?<br />

Suggest a research hypothesis.<br />

What <strong>did</strong> the researchers conclude?<br />

How reliable was the source? Explain.<br />

oral presentation<br />

Select one of the following topics and present <strong>you</strong>r findings<br />

using a PowerPoint presentation or podcast.<br />

><br />

><br />

Career options for psychologists. Include the range<br />

of specialties and statistics on their numbers.<br />

The cognitive approach. Include the focus of this<br />

approach, the main researchers and their contribution<br />

to <strong>know</strong>ledge of mental processes and behaviour.<br />

visual presentation<br />

Create a concept map, graphic organiser or poster on<br />

one of the following:<br />

><br />

><br />

essay<br />

The biological, cognitive, behavioural and<br />

sociocultural approaches in psychology and what<br />

each has contributed through its research.<br />

The differences between psychology as a science<br />

and pseudo-sciences.<br />

Write an 800-word essay on one of the following topics.<br />

Refer to relevant theories to support <strong>you</strong>r statements.<br />

><br />

><br />

Write a persuasive essay on the question ‘Are<br />

pseudo-sciences dangerous or is the scientific<br />

community simply closed-minded?’<br />

Psychology is said to have developed from the<br />

discipline of philosophy. Explain this in terms of the<br />

contributions made from ancient times.<br />

debate<br />

Choose one of the following topics for <strong>you</strong>r debate:<br />

><br />

><br />

Violent video games and movies have little effect on<br />

adolescent behaviour.<br />

Animal research is necessary for furthering our<br />

<strong>know</strong>ledge in psychology.<br />

data analysis<br />

Using the data collected in the research investigation<br />

above, calculate the mean, median and mode. Draw<br />

appropriate table(s) and graph(s) to show <strong>you</strong>r results.


assessment rubric<br />

Possible total = 20 marks<br />

Criterion 4 marks 3 marks 2 marks 1 mark<br />

1 Communication<br />

clear and concisely written<br />

information<br />

persuading effectively<br />

reading independently<br />

listening and understanding<br />

2 Planning and organisation<br />

collecting, analysing and<br />

organising information<br />

planning the use of resources<br />

time management<br />

3 Presentation of task<br />

use of technology to organise<br />

and present data<br />

speaking clearly and directly<br />

visual presentations<br />

written work follows the<br />

conventions of formal writing<br />

4 Understanding<br />

historical beginnings and<br />

different approaches of<br />

psychology<br />

differences between<br />

psychology and pseudosciences<br />

processes visual perception<br />

5 Analysis<br />

analysis of topic<br />

ability to compare and contrast<br />

different information<br />

6 Research<br />

a range of resources have<br />

been identified and used<br />

resources are all cited using<br />

correct format within the text<br />

and in the reference list<br />

All of the relevant<br />

aspects of this<br />

criterion have been<br />

fully and effectively<br />

addressed and<br />

expressed<br />

Effective planning, use<br />

of time, and use of<br />

resources to maximise<br />

completion of the task<br />

Appropriate and<br />

effective use of<br />

relevant format to<br />

present the task<br />

Clear and complex<br />

understanding of the<br />

topic as evidenced<br />

in the standard of<br />

information in the task<br />

Evidence of complex<br />

analysis of the topic<br />

and competent<br />

contrast/comparison<br />

of information or<br />

concepts<br />

Effective use of a<br />

comprehensive<br />

range of relevant<br />

and appropriate<br />

resources to address<br />

the requirements of<br />

the task, and correct<br />

citation of sources<br />

N.B. For all criteria, 0 marks are awarded for ‘not shown’.<br />

Some of the relevant<br />

aspects of this<br />

criterion have been<br />

fully and effectively<br />

addressed and<br />

expressed<br />

Some effective<br />

planning, use of time,<br />

and use of resources<br />

to complete the task<br />

Appropriate and some<br />

effective use of the<br />

relevant format to<br />

present the task<br />

Clear understanding<br />

of the topic shown<br />

in the standard of<br />

information included<br />

in the task<br />

Evidence of analysis of<br />

the topic and sound<br />

contrast/comparison<br />

of information or<br />

concepts<br />

Use of a range<br />

of relevant and<br />

appropriate resources<br />

to address the<br />

requirements of the<br />

task, and correct<br />

citation of sources<br />

Some of the relevant<br />

aspects of this<br />

criterion have been<br />

addressed<br />

Partial evidence<br />

shown of planning,<br />

use of time, and use of<br />

resources<br />

Appropriate use of<br />

the relevant format to<br />

present the task<br />

Some understanding<br />

of the topic and basic<br />

information evident in<br />

the task<br />

Some evidence<br />

of analysis of the<br />

topic and basic<br />

contrast/comparison<br />

of information or<br />

concepts<br />

Use of some relevant<br />

sources to address<br />

the requirements of<br />

the task, with some<br />

attempt to cite<br />

sources<br />

Some of the relevant<br />

aspects of this<br />

criterion have been<br />

vaguely addressed<br />

Insufficient evidence<br />

shown of planning,<br />

use of time, and use of<br />

resources<br />

An attempt has been<br />

made to use the<br />

relevant format to<br />

present the task<br />

Partial understanding<br />

of the topic and<br />

limited accurate<br />

information evident in<br />

the task<br />

Little evidence of any<br />

analysis and basic<br />

contrast/comparison<br />

of information or<br />

concepts<br />

Limited use and<br />

citation of sources<br />

Your teachers will indicate which aspects of each criterion are applicable to the relevant assessment task that has been set for their class.<br />

135

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!