19.01.2015 Views

Chapter 2 Principles of Stereoscopic Depth Perception and ...

Chapter 2 Principles of Stereoscopic Depth Perception and ...

Chapter 2 Principles of Stereoscopic Depth Perception and ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

2. <strong>Principles</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Stereoscopic</strong> <strong>Depth</strong> <strong>Perception</strong> <strong>and</strong> Reproduction<br />

Stelmach 1998). Temporal effects in stereoscopic vision will be discussed<br />

in more detail in <strong>Chapter</strong> 3.<br />

The single most common cause <strong>of</strong> stereoblindness is strabismus, the misalignment<br />

<strong>of</strong> the two eyes. If this condition is surgically corrected at an<br />

early age, stereoscopic vision may develop normally. Stereopsis also fails<br />

to develop when children have good vision in only one eye, due to monocular<br />

nearsightedness or a cataract. Obviously, stereoblind individuals can<br />

still perceive depth, but are restricted to information sources other than<br />

binocular disparity.<br />

It is well-known that visual abilities deteriorate with age. The major age<br />

differences in visual functioning are the result <strong>of</strong> two types <strong>of</strong> changes in<br />

the structure <strong>of</strong> the eye. The first type <strong>of</strong> change is related to the transmissiveness<br />

<strong>and</strong> accommodative power <strong>of</strong> the lens that begins to manifest<br />

itself between the ages <strong>of</strong> 35 <strong>and</strong> 45. This affects binocular depth perception,<br />

sensitivity to glare, <strong>and</strong> colour sensitivity. The second type <strong>of</strong> change<br />

occurs in the retina <strong>and</strong> the nervous system, <strong>and</strong> usually starts to occur<br />

between 55 <strong>and</strong> 65 years <strong>of</strong> age. This affects the size <strong>of</strong> the visual field,<br />

sensitivity to low light levels, <strong>and</strong> sensitivity to flicker (Hayslip Jr. &<br />

Panek 1989). A recent study by Norman, Dawson & Butler (2000) demonstrated<br />

that older adults were less sensitive than younger adults to perceiving<br />

stereoscopic depth, in particular when image disparity was higher.<br />

Overall however, older adults performed to 75% <strong>of</strong> their expected depth<br />

intervals, demonstrating that their stereoscopic abilities have largely been<br />

preserved during the process <strong>of</strong> aging.<br />

2.2.4 Cue combination in depth perception<br />

Traditionally, monocular <strong>and</strong> binocular depth cues that are important<br />

in achieving a representation <strong>of</strong> distance, depth <strong>and</strong> spatial structure<br />

have been studied in isolation. This research was carried out under<br />

the assumption that depth is processed in separate modules that correspond<br />

to different sources <strong>of</strong> three-dimensional information. However,<br />

it is also interesting to find out how these modules, if independent,<br />

might be integrated to provide a coherent 3-D percept (Johnston, Cumming<br />

& Parker 1993, L<strong>and</strong>y, Maloney, Johnston & Young 1995, L<strong>and</strong>y &<br />

Brenner 2001). The relative efficiency <strong>and</strong> robustness <strong>of</strong> our perception<br />

<strong>of</strong> the natural visual world suggests that we integrate multiple sources <strong>of</strong><br />

information into a single percept. Further interest in cue integration has<br />

58

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!