16.11.2012 Views

Antigen Biotinylated Anti-Rabbit lgs Rabbit Primary Antiserum AB ...

Antigen Biotinylated Anti-Rabbit lgs Rabbit Primary Antiserum AB ...

Antigen Biotinylated Anti-Rabbit lgs Rabbit Primary Antiserum AB ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Light Perception - Discussion<br />

tuning of the shortwave sensitive cone type to the UV-light spectrum should be successively<br />

examined, because it has been discussed in Peichl et al. (2004) that the Zambian mole-rat S-<br />

opsin may be UV sensitive. This functional shift might be one explanation of the unusual<br />

presence of short-wave sensitive opsins in mole-rats. In mammals, ultraviolet vision has been<br />

found in e.g. bats (Winter et al. 2003) and in a number of rodents (Jacobs et al. 1991). Though<br />

the UV sensitivity of the S-opsin is undoubtedly an ancestral mammalian condition (Hunt et<br />

al. 2001), the adaptive meaning in those rodent groups that have retained it, e.g. whether its<br />

formation has been driven by the use of territorial urine marks (Chávez et al. 2003), remains<br />

an object of discussion.<br />

The wavelengths propagated best, i.e. attenuated least in a tunnel, are green and red,<br />

with red being detectable even at 70 cm distance from the light source. Though the amount of<br />

detected red light was meagre, it was twice the photon catch in the blue or green range, and<br />

well in the area of scotopic vision (see above). Thermal radiation, i.e. infrared radiation, was<br />

always detected. This finding did also not, as to the low frequency of long wavelengths,<br />

surprise; and it fits also well the findings by Sun et al. (2003), who describe that far-red and<br />

near infra-red light might leak out of plant tissue. Here, a second explanation for the blue-light<br />

sensitive cone opsin, though a highly speculative one, should be introduced. It is principally<br />

imaginable that the blue cones are a kind of infrared-light detector for orientation along<br />

longwave radiation within the burrow system. Physically, it would be possible that IR quanta,<br />

which are, particularly at low flux rates, difficult to detect for a warm-blooded animal due to<br />

thermal contamination, activate photoreceptors in a multi-stage quantal process that would<br />

provide the necessary energy for excitation of the cis/trans transfer in the ‘blue’ visual<br />

pigments (K. Götz, personal communication). Another option is the so-called “blue eyes”<br />

effect, recently firstly presented at a conference (Kaernbach & Scheibelhofer 2006). The name<br />

of the effect is deduced from the blue halos that occur on both sides of a red light spot<br />

focused on in a dark room; the authors suggest this effect as being retinal. If both a transfer of<br />

this human study to mole-rats and wild speculations are allowed, the rodent could see ‘blue’,<br />

and that is even a symmetrical blue form, by viewing only a single red light spot. By moving<br />

the head and perceiving red light from different spatial positions, the shape of the blue halos<br />

could then yield information on intensity, direction, and distance of the light source.<br />

In any case, whether mole-rats ‘see’ the (infra)red light or detect it by another sensory<br />

system, it would be advantageous for an animal to recognize an open or collapsed tunnel as<br />

soon as possible in order to plug or maintain it. As there are no air currents in an open tunnel<br />

(see above), detecting the opening from a distance of already 60 to 70 cm would allow the<br />

animal to quickly react and plug the tunnel system. Regarding the red light present in a tunnel,<br />

a system sensitive enough to integrate even low photon shares of 0.02% of the total light<br />

37

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!