12.07.2015 Views

Special Issue on Procedural Fairness - American Judges Association

Special Issue on Procedural Fairness - American Judges Association

Special Issue on Procedural Fairness - American Judges Association

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

doubt that aspirati<strong>on</strong>s for, and satisfacti<strong>on</strong> with, particular outcomesreign supreme in litigants’ minds. Distributive justicemight not be litigants’ favorite justice flavor, but it is the flavorthey care about the most. It is therefore the flavor that judgesshould care about the most as well.Procedures of the utmost fairness do not necessarily meanthat litigants will readily accept a court’s outcome or decisi<strong>on</strong>.Hence it is important for legal advisors, professi<strong>on</strong>als, andjudges to be willing to explain outcomes and to express a willingnessto answer litigants’ questi<strong>on</strong>s, particularly if an outcomeis undesirable or unexpected. As menti<strong>on</strong>ed in the individualdifferences secti<strong>on</strong>, supra, it is important further forlegal professi<strong>on</strong>als to recognize that litigants are not cookiecutterreplicas. What makes people different will also influencehow they approach, interpret, and understand the law. Thiswill help to ensure that litigants have a better understanding oftheir outcomes and why those particular outcomes werereached, which would potentially lead to greater satisfacti<strong>on</strong>with the justice system and fewer appeals.Burke and Leben also emphasize the importance of socialscience research in helping legal professi<strong>on</strong>als understand howthe general populati<strong>on</strong> interprets fairness in the legal system.We support this propositi<strong>on</strong> with respect to distributive, aswell as procedural fairness. We similarly recommend that legalprofessi<strong>on</strong>als not <strong>on</strong>ly educate themselves by becoming familiarwith the existing literature, but also support <strong>on</strong>goingresearch. There are two ways in which judges can facilitate thisgoal. First, they can allow researchers to survey litigants abouttheir percepti<strong>on</strong> of legal outcomes as well as legal procedures.Sec<strong>on</strong>d, they can serve as research participants themselves.Social scientists who c<strong>on</strong>duct research <strong>on</strong> the legal systemknow much less about how judges make decisi<strong>on</strong>s than theydo about how juries (and especially individual jurors) makedecisi<strong>on</strong>s. This state of affairs exists for a number of reas<strong>on</strong>s,but primarily because compared to the average juror (or mockjuror), judges are fewer, busier, harder to obtain access to, andless swayed by offers of token compensati<strong>on</strong> for participatingin research studies.As fact finders, judges and juries are similar in manyrespects, yet they differ in subtle ways as well. 43 <strong>Judges</strong> differfrom jurors in terms of their training, background, legalknowledge, and experience with similar cases; evidentiaryrules also mean that judges might make decisi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> slightlydifferent c<strong>on</strong>stellati<strong>on</strong>s of facts than juries. Moreover, preciselybecause of their experience and training, judges are muchmore likely than jurors to have reflected <strong>on</strong> the nature of theirtask and to have formulated principles to which they adhere inadjudicating the cases before them. Interviews with judges, aswell as judge-jury comparis<strong>on</strong>s, could shed a great deal of light<strong>on</strong> the justice principles that legal fact finders rely <strong>on</strong> in determiningtrial outcomes. Reflecti<strong>on</strong> by judges <strong>on</strong> the principlesand goals that they use, often unc<strong>on</strong>sciously, in reaching verdictswould produce a more thoughtful and better informedjudiciary.Brian H. Bornstein, Ph.D. (University ofPennsylvania), M.L.S. (University ofNebraska), is Professor of Psychology andCourtesy Professor of Law at the University ofNebraska-Lincoln, where he is AssociateDirector of the Law-Psychology Program. Hismain research interests are jury decisi<strong>on</strong> making,eyewitness testim<strong>on</strong>y, and percepti<strong>on</strong>s ofjustice. He can be reached at bbornstein2@unl.edu.Hannah Dietrich (M.A.) is a doctoral student atthe University of Nebraska—Lincoln in the jointSocial Psychology PhD and Master of LegalStudies program. She can be reached at hl.dietrich@gmail.com.43. See, e.g., Brian H. Bornstein, <strong>Judges</strong> vs. Juries, 43 CT. REV. 56(2006).Court Review - Volume 44 77

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!