12.07.2015 Views

How to Design and Evaluate Research in Education

How to Design and Evaluate Research in Education

How to Design and Evaluate Research in Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Primarily discussedTypes of <strong>Research</strong> Description <strong>in</strong> chapterAction <strong>Research</strong> <strong>Research</strong> that focuses on specific local situations 1<strong>and</strong> that emphasizes the active <strong>in</strong>volvement ofthose participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the study <strong>and</strong> who areaffected by it. It makes use of one or moreof the other research types.Experiments Compar<strong>in</strong>g two or more treatments <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> 13study their effects, while attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> controlother possible variables that might affect theoutcome under <strong>in</strong>vestigation.S<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies The <strong>in</strong>tensive study of a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual or 14group over time.Correlational studies Investigat<strong>in</strong>g the relationships that may exist 15among two or more variables as well as theirimplications for cause <strong>and</strong> effect.Causal-comparative studies Explor<strong>in</strong>g the cause for, or consequences of, 16differences that exist between groups of people.Surveys Collect<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e various <strong>and</strong> 17specific characteristics of a group.Ethnographies Document<strong>in</strong>g or portray<strong>in</strong>g the everyday 21experiences of <strong>in</strong>dividuals by observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>and</strong> relevant others.His<strong>to</strong>rical studies Study<strong>in</strong>g an aspect of the past either by perus<strong>in</strong>g 22documents of the period or by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividuals who lived dur<strong>in</strong>g that time.


<strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong><strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>SEVENTH EDITIONJack R. FraenkelSan Francisco State UniversityNorman E. WallenSan Francisco State University


Published by McGraw-Hill, an impr<strong>in</strong>t of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., 1221 Avenue ofthe Americas, New York, NY 10020. Copyright © 2009, 2006, 2003, 2000, 1996, 1993, 1990. Allrights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or distributed <strong>in</strong> any form or byany means, or s<strong>to</strong>red <strong>in</strong> a database or retrieval system, without the prior written consent of TheMcGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g, but not limited <strong>to</strong>, <strong>in</strong> any network or other electronics<strong>to</strong>rage or transmission, or broadcast for distance learn<strong>in</strong>g.1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 CCW 0 9 8ISBN: 978-0-07-352596-9MHID: 0-07-352596-0Edi<strong>to</strong>r-<strong>in</strong>-chief: Michael RyanPublisher: Beth MejiaExecutive edi<strong>to</strong>r: David S. PattersonDevelopment edi<strong>to</strong>rs: Vicki Mal<strong>in</strong>ee, Van Brien & AssociatesMarket<strong>in</strong>g manager: James HeadleyProduction edi<strong>to</strong>r: Anne FuzellierArt direc<strong>to</strong>r: Jeanne M. SchreiberArt manager: Rob<strong>in</strong> Mouat<strong>Design</strong> manager: Andrei PasternakInterior designer: Carol<strong>in</strong>e McGowanCover designer: Laurie Entr<strong>in</strong>gerMedia project manager: Thomas BrierlyProduction supervisor: T<strong>and</strong>ra JorgensenProduction service: Matrix Productions Inc.Composition: 10/12 Times Roman by ICC Macmillan Inc.Pr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g: 45# New Era Matte Plus by Courier WestfordCover images: Top: Bill Aron/Pho<strong>to</strong>Edit, Inc.; Bot<strong>to</strong>m: The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Keith Eng, pho<strong>to</strong>grapherLibrary of Congress Catalog<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>in</strong>-Publication DataFraenkel, Jack R., 1932–<strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> design <strong>and</strong> evaluate research <strong>in</strong> education / Jack R. Fraenkel,Norman E. Wallen.—7th ed.p. cm.Includes <strong>in</strong>dex.ISBN-13: 978-0-07-352596-9 (alk. paper)ISBN-10: 0-07-352596-0 (alk. paper)1. <strong>Education</strong>—<strong>Research</strong>—Methodology. 2. <strong>Education</strong>—<strong>Research</strong>—Evaluation.3. Proposal writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> educational research. I. Wallen, Norman E. II. Title.LB1028.F665 2008370.7’2—dc22 2007047463The Internet addresses listed <strong>in</strong> the text were accurate at the time of publication. The <strong>in</strong>clusion ofa Web site does not <strong>in</strong>dicate an endorsement by the authors or McGraw-Hill, <strong>and</strong> McGraw-Hilldoes not guarantee the accuracy of the <strong>in</strong>formation presented at these sites.www.mhhe.com


About the AuthorsJack R. Fraenkel is currentlyProfessor of Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>aryStudies <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>,<strong>and</strong> previously Direc<strong>to</strong>r of the<strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> DevelopmentCenter, College of <strong>Education</strong>,San Francisco State University.He received his Ph.D.from Stanford University <strong>and</strong>has taught courses <strong>in</strong> researchmethodology for more than30 years. In 1997, he received the James A. Michenerprize <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g for his writ<strong>in</strong>gs about the social studies<strong>and</strong> the social sciences. His current work centers aroundadvis<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> assist<strong>in</strong>g faculty <strong>and</strong> students <strong>in</strong> generat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g research endeavors.Norman E. Wallen is ProfessorEmeritus of Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>aryStudies <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>at San Francisco State University,where he taught from1966 <strong>to</strong> 1992. An experiencedresearcher, he received hisPh.D. from Syracuse University<strong>and</strong> taught courses <strong>in</strong> statistics<strong>and</strong> research design <strong>to</strong>master’s <strong>and</strong> doc<strong>to</strong>ral studentsfor many years. He is a former member of the City Councilof Flagstaff, Arizona, <strong>and</strong> the Executive Committee,Gr<strong>and</strong> Canyon Chapter of the Sierra Club.iii


To Marge <strong>and</strong> L<strong>in</strong>a for all their support


Contents <strong>in</strong> BriefPreface xxPART 1Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 11 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 2PART 2The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> 252 The <strong>Research</strong> Problem 263 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 374 Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 525 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 666 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 897 Instrumentation 1098 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 1469 Internal Validity 165PART 3Data Analysis 18310 Descriptive Statistics 18411 Inferential Statistics 21512 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 241PART 4Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies 25913 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 26014 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 29815 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 32716 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 36217 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 389PART 5Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 41918 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 42019 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 43920 Content Analysis 471PART 6Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies 49921 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 50022 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 533PART 7Mixed-Methods Studies 55523 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 556PART 8<strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners 58724 Action <strong>Research</strong> 588PART 9Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong>Reports 61525 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong>Reports 616Appendixes A-1APPENDIX A Portion of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>omNumbers A-2APPENDIX B Selected Values from a Normal CurveTable A-3APPENDIX C Chi-Square Distribution A-4APPENDIX D Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS A-5Glossary G-1Index I-1v


ContentsPreface xxPART 1Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 11 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 2Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 3Some Examples of <strong>Education</strong>al Concerns 3Why <strong>Research</strong> Is of Value 4Ways of Know<strong>in</strong>g 4Types of <strong>Research</strong> 7General <strong>Research</strong> Types 14Critical Analysis of <strong>Research</strong> 16A Brief Overview of the <strong>Research</strong> Process 19Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 21Key Terms 22For Discussion 22Notes 22<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 1 23Problem Sheet 1 23PART 2The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> 252 The <strong>Research</strong> Problem 26Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 27What Is a <strong>Research</strong> Problem? 27<strong>Research</strong> Questions 27Characteristics of Good <strong>Research</strong> Questions 29Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 35Key Terms 35For Discussion 35<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 2 36Problem Sheet 2 363 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 37Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 38The Importance of Study<strong>in</strong>g Relationships 38Variables 39Hypotheses 45Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 48Key Terms 49For Discussion 49<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 3 51Problem Sheet 3 514 Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 52Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 53Some Examples of Unethical Practice 53A Statement of Ethical Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples 53Protect<strong>in</strong>g Participants from Harm 55Ensur<strong>in</strong>g Confidentiality of <strong>Research</strong> Data 56Should Subjects Be Deceived? 56Three Examples Involv<strong>in</strong>g Ethical Concerns 57vi


CONTENTSvii<strong>Research</strong> with Children 59Regulation of <strong>Research</strong> 60Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 63For Discussion 64Notes 64<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 4 65Problem Sheet 4 655 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>gthe Literature 66Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 67The Value of a Literature Review 67Types of Sources 67Steps Involved <strong>in</strong> a Literature Search 68Do<strong>in</strong>g a Computer Search 76Writ<strong>in</strong>g the Literature Review Report 84Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 85Key Terms 86For Discussion 86Notes 87<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 5 88Problem Sheet 5 886 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 89Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 90What Is a Sample? 90R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Methods 93Nonr<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Methods 96A Review of Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Methods 99Sample Size 101External Validity: Generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from a Sample 102Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 105Key Terms 106For Discussion 106<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 6 108Problem Sheet 6 1087 Instrumentation 109Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 110What Are Data? 110Means of Classify<strong>in</strong>g Data-CollectionInstruments 112Examples of Data-Collection Instruments 116Types of Scores 134Norm-Referenced Versus Criterion-ReferencedInstruments 136Measurement Scales 137Prepar<strong>in</strong>g Data for Analysis 140Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 141Key Terms 143For Discussion 143Notes 144<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 7 145Problem Sheet 7 1458 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 146Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 147The Importance of Valid Instruments 147Validity 147Reliability 154Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 162Key Terms 162For Discussion 163Notes 163<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 8 164Problem Sheet 8 1649 Internal Validity 165Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 166What Is Internal Validity? 166Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity 167<strong>How</strong> Can a <strong>Research</strong>er M<strong>in</strong>imize These Threats <strong>to</strong>Internal Validity? 179


viii CONTENTS www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMa<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 180Key Terms 181For Discussion 181Note 181<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 9 182Problem Sheet 9 182PART 3Data Analysis 18310 Descriptive Statistics 184Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 185Statistics Versus Parameters 185Two Fundamental Types of Numerical Data 185Techniques for Summariz<strong>in</strong>g Quantitative Data 187Techniques for Summariz<strong>in</strong>g Categorical Data 207Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 211Key Terms 212For Discussion 213<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 10 214Problem Sheet 10 21411 Inferential Statistics 215Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 216What Are Inferential Statistics? 216The Logic of Inferential Statistics 217Hypothesis Test<strong>in</strong>g 223Practical Versus Statistical Significance 226Inference Techniques 228Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 237Key Terms 238For Discussion 239<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 11 240Problem Sheet 11 24012 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 241Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 242Approaches <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 242Compar<strong>in</strong>g Groups: Quantitative Data 243Relat<strong>in</strong>g Variables With<strong>in</strong> a Group: QuantitativeData 247Compar<strong>in</strong>g Groups: Categorical Data 251Relat<strong>in</strong>g Variables With<strong>in</strong> a Group: CategoricalData 253A Recap of Recommendations 255Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 255Key Terms 256For Discussion 256<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 12 257Problem Sheet 12 257PART 4Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies 25913 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 260Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 261The Uniqueness of Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 261Essential Characteristics of Experimental<strong>Research</strong> 262Control of Extraneous Variables 264Group <strong>Design</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 264Control of Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity:A Summary 276Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the Likelihood of a Threat <strong>to</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> ExperimentalStudies 277Control of Experimental Treatments 280An Example of Experimental<strong>Research</strong> 281<strong>Research</strong> Report 282Analysis of the Study 292Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 293


CONTENTSixKey Terms 295For Discussion 295Notes 296<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 13 297Problem Sheet 13 29714 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 298Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 299Essential Characteristics of S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject<strong>Research</strong> 299S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>s 299Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject<strong>Research</strong> 305An Example of S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 311<strong>Research</strong> Report 312Analysis of the Study 323Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 324Key Terms 326For Discussion 32615 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 327Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 328The Nature of Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 328Purposes of Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 329Basic Steps <strong>in</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 335What Do Correlation CoefficientsTell Us? 337Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Correlational<strong>Research</strong> 337Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Correlational Studies 341An Example of Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 343<strong>Research</strong> Report 344Analysis of the Study 357Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 359Key Terms 360For Discussion 361Notes 36116 Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong> 362Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 363What Is Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>? 363Steps Involved <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 366Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong> 367Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative Studies 369Data Analysis 370Associations Between Categorical Variables 372An Example of Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 373<strong>Research</strong> Report 373Analysis of the Study 384Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 386For Discussion 388Note 38817 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 389Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 390What Is a Survey? 390Why Are Surveys Conducted? 390Types of Surveys 391Survey <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 392Steps <strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong> 392Nonresponse 401Problems <strong>in</strong> the Instrumentation Process <strong>in</strong> Survey<strong>Research</strong> 404Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Survey<strong>Research</strong> 404Data Analysis <strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong> 404An Example of Survey <strong>Research</strong> 404<strong>Research</strong> Report 405Analysis of the Study 414Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 416Key Terms 417For Discussion 417Notes 418


x CONTENTS www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePART 5Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 41918 The Nature of Qualitative<strong>Research</strong> 420Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 421What Is Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>? 421General Characteristics of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 422Philosophical Assumptions Underly<strong>in</strong>g Qualitativeas Opposed <strong>to</strong> Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> 423Postmodernism 424Steps <strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 425Approaches <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 427Generalization <strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 432Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 433Ethics <strong>and</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 433Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>Reconsidered 434Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 435Key Terms 436For Discussion 437Notes 43719 Observation <strong>and</strong>Interview<strong>in</strong>g 439Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 440Observation 440Interview<strong>in</strong>g 445PART 6Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability <strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 453An Example of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 454<strong>Research</strong> Report 455Analysis of the Study 466Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 468Key Terms 469For Discussion 469Notes 47020 Content Analysis 471Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 472What Is Content Analysis? 472Some Applications 473Categorization <strong>in</strong> Content Analysis 474Steps Involved <strong>in</strong> Content Analysis 474An Illustration of Content Analysis 480Us<strong>in</strong>g the Computer <strong>in</strong> Content Analysis 482Advantages of Content Analysis 483Disadvantages of Content Analysis 483An Example of a Content Analysis Study 484<strong>Research</strong> Report 484Analysis of the Study 495Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 496Key Terms 498For Discussion 498Notes 498Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies 49921 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 500Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 501What Is Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>? 501Ethnographic Concepts 503Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 505Do Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>ers Use Hypotheses? 505Data Collection <strong>in</strong> Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 506Data Analysis <strong>in</strong> Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 510Roger Harker <strong>and</strong> His Fifth-Grade Classroom 512Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages of Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong> 513An Example of Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 514<strong>Research</strong> Report 514Analysis of the Study 528Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 530Key Terms 531For Discussion 531Notes 532


CONTENTSxi22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 533Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 534What Is His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>? 534Steps Involved <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 535Data Analysis <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 540Generalization <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 540Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages of His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong> 541An Example of His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 542<strong>Research</strong> Report 543Analysis of the Study 550Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 551Key Terms 552For Discussion 553Notes 553PART 7Mixed-Methods Studies 55523 Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong> 556Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 557What Is Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>? 557Why Do Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>? 558Drawbacks of Mixed-Methods Studies 558A (Very) Brief His<strong>to</strong>ry 559Types of Mixed-Methods <strong>Design</strong>s 560Other Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> <strong>Design</strong> Issues 562Steps <strong>in</strong> Conduct<strong>in</strong>g a Mixed-Methods Study 563Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a Mixed-Methods Study 564Ethics <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 565Summary 565An Example of Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 565<strong>Research</strong> Report 566Analysis of the Study 580Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 583Key Terms 584For Discussion 585Notes 585PART 8<strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners 58724 Action <strong>Research</strong> 588Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 589What Is Action <strong>Research</strong>? 589Types of Action <strong>Research</strong> 590Steps <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong> 592Similarities <strong>and</strong> Differences Between Action<strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Formal Quantitative <strong>and</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 594The Advantages of Action <strong>Research</strong> 596Some Hypothetical Examples of Practical Action<strong>Research</strong> 596An Example of Action <strong>Research</strong> 601A Published Example of Action <strong>Research</strong> 602<strong>Research</strong> Report 603Analysis of the Study 610Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 611Key Terms 612For Discussion 612Notes 613


xii CONTENTS www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePART 9Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 61525 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals<strong>and</strong> Reports 616Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g 617The <strong>Research</strong> Proposal 617The Major Sections of a <strong>Research</strong> Proposalor Report 617Sections Unique <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> Reports 624A Sample <strong>Research</strong> Proposal 627Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts 640For Review 640Key Terms 641For Discussion 641Notes 641Appendixes A-1APPENDIX A Portion of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>omNumbers A-2APPENDIX B Selected Values from a Normal CurveTable A-3APPENDIX C Chi-Square Distribution A-4APPENDIX D Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS A-5Glossary G-1Index I-1


List of FeaturesRESEARCH REPORTSThe Effect of a Computer Simulation Activity Versus aH<strong>and</strong>s-on Activity on Product Creativity<strong>in</strong> Technology <strong>Education</strong> 282Progress<strong>in</strong>g from Programmatic <strong>to</strong> Discovery <strong>Research</strong>: ACase Example with the Overjustification Effect 312When Teachers’ <strong>and</strong> Parents’ Values Differ: Teachers’Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of Academic Competence <strong>in</strong> Childrenfrom Low-Income Families 344Personal, Social, <strong>and</strong> Family Characteristics of AngryStudents 373Russian <strong>and</strong> American College Students’ Attitudes,Perceptions, <strong>and</strong> Tendencies Towards Cheat<strong>in</strong>g 405Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk: An Intervention for BehaviorallyChallenged Youths 455The “Nuts <strong>and</strong> Dolts” of Teacher Images <strong>in</strong> Children’sPicture S<strong>to</strong>rybooks: A Content Analysis 484Dump<strong>in</strong>g Ground or Effective Alternative? Dropout-Prevention Programs <strong>in</strong> Urban Schools 514Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w: Self-Actualization <strong>in</strong> a Periodof Transition 543Perceived Family Support, Acculturation, <strong>and</strong> LifeSatisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American Youth: A Mixed-Methods Exploration 566What Do You Mean “Th<strong>in</strong>k Before I Act”?: ConflictResolution with Choices 603RESEARCH TIPSKey Terms <strong>to</strong> Def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> a <strong>Research</strong> Study 31What a Good Summary of a Journal ArticleShould Conta<strong>in</strong> 75Some Tips About Develop<strong>in</strong>g Your OwnInstrument 113Sample Size 230<strong>How</strong> Not <strong>to</strong> Interview 452What <strong>to</strong> Do About Contradic<strong>to</strong>ry F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs 564Th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> Consider When Do<strong>in</strong>g In-School<strong>Research</strong> 600Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask When Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a <strong>Research</strong>Report 624MORE ABOUT RESEARCHChaos Theory 8The Importance of a Rationale 33Some Important Relationships That Have BeenClarified by <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> 41Patients Given Fake Blood Without Their Knowledge 57An Example of Unethical <strong>Research</strong> 59Department of Health <strong>and</strong> Human Services RevisedRegulations for <strong>Research</strong> with Human Subjects 62The Difficulty <strong>in</strong> Generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from a Sample 102Check<strong>in</strong>g Reliability <strong>and</strong> Validity—An Example 157Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Everyday Life 175Some Thoughts About Meta-Analysis 177Correlation <strong>in</strong> Everyday Life 209Interpret<strong>in</strong>g Statistics 254Significant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 280Important F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 300Examples of Studies Conducted Us<strong>in</strong>g S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject<strong>Design</strong>s 310xiii


xiv LIST OF FEATURES www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 330Significant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong> 372Important F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong> 395Important F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Content Analysis<strong>Research</strong> 475Important F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 502Important F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 541An Important Example of Action <strong>Research</strong> 597CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHShould Some <strong>Research</strong> Methods Be PreferredOver Others? 14Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Trials—Desirable or Not? 55Ethical or Not? 61Sample or Census? 101Which Statistical Index Is Valid? 139High-Stakes Test<strong>in</strong>g 150Is Consequential Validity a Useful Concept? 160Can Statistical Power Analysis Be Mislead<strong>in</strong>g? 236Statistical Inference Tests—Good or Bad? 245Do Placebos Work? 277<strong>How</strong> Should <strong>Research</strong> MethodologiesBe Classified? 365Is Low Response Rate Necessarily a Bad Th<strong>in</strong>g? 403Clarity <strong>and</strong> Postmodernism 426Portraiture: Art, Science, or Both? 428Should His<strong>to</strong>rians Influence Policy? 539Are Some Methods Incompatible with Others? 560<strong>How</strong> Much Should Participants Be Involved <strong>in</strong><strong>Research</strong>? 593TABLESTable 4.1 Criteria for IRB Approval 61Table 5.1 Some of the Major Index<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>Abstract<strong>in</strong>g Services Used <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> 71Table 5.2 Pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of Search Results 79Table 5.3 Major Indexes on the World Wide Web 80Table 5.4 Lead<strong>in</strong>g Search Eng<strong>in</strong>es 81Table 6.1 Part of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>omNumbers 94Table 7.1 Hypothetical Examples of Raw Scores<strong>and</strong> Accompany<strong>in</strong>g Percentile Ranks 135Table 7.2 Characteristics of the Four Types ofMeasurement Scales 139Table 7.3 Hypothetical Results of StudyInvolv<strong>in</strong>g a Comparison of Two Counsel<strong>in</strong>gMethods 140Table 8.1 Example of an Expectancy Table 153Table 8.2 Methods of Check<strong>in</strong>g Validity<strong>and</strong> Reliability 158Table 9.1 General Techniques for Controll<strong>in</strong>gThreats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity 179Table 10.1 Example of a FrequencyDistribution 187Table 10.2 Example of a Grouped FrequencyDistribution 187Table 10.3 Example of the Mode, Median, <strong>and</strong>Mean <strong>in</strong> a Distribution 193Table 10.4 Yearly Salaries of Workers <strong>in</strong> a SmallBus<strong>in</strong>ess 194Table 10.5 Calculation of the St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviationof a Distribution 196Table 10.6 Comparisons of Raw Scores <strong>and</strong> z Scoreson Two Tests 199Table 10.7 Data Used <strong>to</strong> Construct Scatterplot<strong>in</strong> Figure 10.17 202Table 10.8 Frequency <strong>and</strong> Percentage of Total ofResponses <strong>to</strong> Questionnaire 207Table 10.9 Grade Level <strong>and</strong> Gender of Teachers(Hypothetical Data) 208Table 10.10 Repeat of Table 10.9 with ExpectedFrequencies (<strong>in</strong> Parentheses) 208Table 10.11 Position, Gender, <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity ofSchool Leaders (Hypothetical Data) 209Table 10.12 Position <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies (Derivedfrom Table 10.11) 209Table 10.13 Position <strong>and</strong> Gender of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies (Derivedfrom Table 10.11) 209Table 10.14 Gender <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies (Derived fromTable 10.11) 209


LIST OF FEATURESxvTable 10.15 Total of Discrepancies BetweenExpected <strong>and</strong> Observed Frequencies <strong>in</strong>Tables 10.12 Through 10.14 210Table 10.16 Crossbreak Table Show<strong>in</strong>g RelationshipBetween Self-Esteem <strong>and</strong> Gender (HypotheticalData) 210Table 11.1 Cont<strong>in</strong>gency Coefficient Valuesfor Different-Sized Crossbreak Tables 234Table 11.2 Commonly Used InferentialTechniques 234Table 12.1 Ga<strong>in</strong> Scores on Test of Ability <strong>to</strong> Expla<strong>in</strong>:Inquiry <strong>and</strong> Lecture Groups 246Table 12.2 Calculations from Table 12.1 248Table 12.3 Interpretation of CorrelationCoefficients when Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>Hypotheses 249Table 12.4 Self-Esteem Scores <strong>and</strong> Ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> MaritalSatisfaction 250Table 12.5 Gender <strong>and</strong> Political Preference(Percentages) 252Table 12.6 Gender <strong>and</strong> Political Preference(Numbers) 252Table 12.7 Teacher Gender <strong>and</strong> Grade LevelTaught: Case 1 252Table 12.8 Teacher Gender <strong>and</strong> Grade LevelTaught: Case 2 252Table 12.9 Crossbreak Table Show<strong>in</strong>g TeacherGender <strong>and</strong> Grade Level with ExpectedFrequencies Added (Data from Table 12.7) 253Table 12.10 Summary of Commonly Used StatisticalTechniques 254Table 13.1 Effectiveness of Experimental<strong>Design</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Controll<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity 276Table 15.1 Three Sets of Data Show<strong>in</strong>g DifferentDirections <strong>and</strong> Degrees of Correlation 329Table 15.2 Teacher Expectation of Failure <strong>and</strong>Amount of Disruptive Behavior for a Sampleof 12 Classes 331Table 15.3 Correlation Matrix for Variables <strong>in</strong>Student Alienation Study 334Table 15.4 Example of Data Obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>a Correlational <strong>Design</strong> 336Table 16.1 Grade Level <strong>and</strong> Gender of Teachers(Hypothetical Data) 372Table 17.1 Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages of SurveyData-Collection Methods 393Table 17.2 Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages ofClosed-Ended Versus Open-Ended Questions 397Table 18.1 Quantitative Versus Qualitative<strong>Research</strong> 422Table 18.2 Major Characteristics of Qualitative<strong>Research</strong> 424Table 18.3 Differ<strong>in</strong>g Philosophical Assumptions ofQuantitative <strong>and</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>ers 425Table 19.1 Interview<strong>in</strong>g Strategies Used <strong>in</strong><strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> 447Table 19.2 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Questions,Strategies, <strong>and</strong> Data Collection Techniques 454Table 20.1 Cod<strong>in</strong>g Categories for Women <strong>in</strong> SocialStudies Textbooks 477Table 20.2 Sample Tally Sheet (NewspaperEdi<strong>to</strong>rials) 479Table 20.3 Clarity of Studies 480Table 20.4 Type of Sample 480Table 20.5 Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity 482Table 24.1 Basic Assumptions Underly<strong>in</strong>g Action<strong>Research</strong> 590Table 24.2 Similarities <strong>and</strong> Differences BetweenAction <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Formal Quantitative <strong>and</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 595Table 25.1 References APA Style 625FIGURESFigure 1.1 Ways of Know<strong>in</strong>g 10Figure 1.2 Example of Results of Experimental<strong>Research</strong>: Effect of Method of Instruction onHis<strong>to</strong>ry Test Scores 11Figure 1.3 Is the Teacher’s Assumption Correct? 17Figure 1.4 The <strong>Research</strong> Process 19Figure 2.1 <strong>Research</strong>able Versus NonresearchableQuestions 28Figure 2.2 Some Times When OperationalDef<strong>in</strong>itions Would Be Helpful 32Figure 2.3 Illustration of Relationship BetweenVoter Gender <strong>and</strong> Party Affiliation 34Figure 3.1 Quantitative Variables Compared withCategorical Variables 40


xvi LIST OF FEATURES www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 3.2 Relationship Between InstructionalApproach (Independent Variable) <strong>and</strong>Achievement (Dependent Variable), as Moderatedby Gender of Students 43Figure 3.3 Examples of Extraneous Variables 44Figure 3.4 A S<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>Research</strong> Question Can SuggestSeveral Hypotheses 45Figure 3.5 Directional Versus NondirectionalHypotheses 48Figure 4.1 Example of a Consent Form 56Figure 4.2 Examples of Unethical <strong>Research</strong>Practices 58Figure 4.3 Example of a Consent Form for a M<strong>in</strong>or<strong>to</strong> Participate <strong>in</strong> a <strong>Research</strong> Study 60Figure 5.1 Excerpt from CIJE 70Figure 5.2 Excerpt from RIE 70Figure 5.3 Excerpt from Digital Dissertations 73Figure 5.4 Sample Bibliographic Card 74Figure 5.5 Sample Note Card 76Figure 5.6a Example of Abstracts Obta<strong>in</strong>ed Us<strong>in</strong>gthe Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs Question<strong>in</strong>g Techniques <strong>and</strong>Science 77Figure 5.6b Example of Abstracts Obta<strong>in</strong>ed Us<strong>in</strong>gthe Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs Question<strong>in</strong>g Techniques <strong>and</strong>Science 77Figure 5.7 Venn Diagrams Show<strong>in</strong>g the BooleanOpera<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> or 79Figure 5.8 The Yahoo! Web Page 81Figure 5.9 Google Web Page 82Figure 5.10 Librarians’ Index <strong>to</strong> the InternetWeb Page 82Figure 6.1 Representative Versus NonrepresentativeSamples 92Figure 6.2 Select<strong>in</strong>g a Stratified Sample 95Figure 6.3 Cluster R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 96Figure 6.4 R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Methods 97Figure 6.5 Convenience Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 98Figure 6.6 Nonr<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Methods 100Figure 6.7 Population as Opposed <strong>to</strong> EcologicalGeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g 104Figure 7.1 The ERIC Search Eng<strong>in</strong>e 114Figure 7.2 Match<strong>in</strong>g Documents for ERIC Search for“Social Studies Instruments” 114Figure 7.3 Abstract from the ERIC Database 115Figure 7.4 Excerpt from a Behavior Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scalefor Teachers 117Figure 7.5 Excerpt from a Graphic Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scale 117Figure 7.6 Example of a Product Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scale 118Figure 7.7 Interview Schedule (for Teachers)<strong>Design</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> Assess the Effects of a Competency-Based Curriculum <strong>in</strong> Inner-City Schools 119Figure 7.8 Sample Observation Form 120Figure 7.9 Discussion-Analysis Tally Sheet 121Figure 7.10 Participation Flowchart 121Figure 7.11 Performance Checklist Not<strong>in</strong>g StudentActions 122Figure 7.12 Time-<strong>and</strong>-Motion Log 124Figure 7.13 Example of a Self-Checklist 125Figure 7.14 Examples of Items from a LikertScale Measur<strong>in</strong>g Attitude Toward TeacherEmpowerment 126Figure 7.15 Example of the SemanticDifferential 126Figure 7.16 Pic<strong>to</strong>rial Attitude Scale for Use withYoung Children 127Figure 7.17 Sample Items from a PersonalityInven<strong>to</strong>ry 127Figure 7.18 Sample Items from an AchievementTest 128Figure 7.19 Sample Item from an Aptitude Test 129Figure 7.20 Sample Items from an IntelligenceTest 129Figure 7.21 Example from the Blum Sew<strong>in</strong>gMach<strong>in</strong>e Test 130Figure 7.22 Sample Items from the Picture SituationInven<strong>to</strong>ry 130Figure 7.23 Example of a Sociogram 131Figure 7.24 Example of a Group Play 132Figure 7.25 Four Types of MeasurementScales 137Figure 7.26 A Nom<strong>in</strong>al Scale of Measurement 138Figure 7.27 An Ord<strong>in</strong>al Scale: The Outcome of aHorse Race 138Figure 8.1 Types of Evidence of Validity 149Figure 8.2 Reliability <strong>and</strong> Validity 155Figure 8.3 Reliability of a Measurement 155


LIST OF FEATURESxviiFigure 8.4 St<strong>and</strong>ard Error of Measurement 158Figure 8.5 The “Quick <strong>and</strong> Easy” IntelligenceTest 159Figure 8.6 Reliability Worksheet 160Figure 9.1 A Mortality Threat <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity 167Figure 9.2 Location Might Make a Difference 169Figure 9.3 An Example of Instrument Decay 170Figure 9.4 A Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r CharacteristicsThreat 170Figure 9.5 A Test<strong>in</strong>g Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity 171Figure 9.6 A His<strong>to</strong>ry Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity 172Figure 9.7 Could Maturation Be at Work Here? 173Figure 9.8 The Attitude of Subjects Can Make aDifference 174Figure 9.9 Regression Rears Its Head 176Figure 9.10 Illustration of Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity 178Figure 10.1 Example of a Frequency Polygon 188Figure 10.2 Example of a Positively SkewedPolygon 190Figure 10.3 Example of a Negatively SkewedPolygon 190Figure 10.4 Two Frequency PolygonsCompared 190Figure 10.5 His<strong>to</strong>gram of Data <strong>in</strong> Table 10.2 191Figure 10.6 The Normal Curve 192Figure 10.7 Averages Can Be Mislead<strong>in</strong>g! 193Figure 10.8 Different Distributions Compared withRespect <strong>to</strong> Averages <strong>and</strong> Spreads 194Figure 10.9 Boxplots 195Figure 10.10 St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations for Boys’ <strong>and</strong>Men’s Basketball Teams 196Figure 10.11 Fifty Percent of All Scores <strong>in</strong> a NormalCurve Fall on Each Side of the Mean 197Figure 10.12 Percentages under the NormalCurve 197Figure 10.13 z Scores Associated with the NormalCurve 199Figure 10.14 Probabilities under the NormalCurve 199Figure 10.15 Table Show<strong>in</strong>g Probability AreasBetween the Mean <strong>and</strong> Different z Scores 200Figure 10.16 Examples of St<strong>and</strong>ard Scores 201Figure 10.17 Scatterplot of Data fromTable 10.7 202Figure 10.18 Relationship Between FamilyCohesiveness <strong>and</strong> School Achievement <strong>in</strong> aHypothetical Group of Students 203Figure 10.19 Further Examples of Scatterplots 204Figure 10.20 A Perfect Negative Correlation! 205Figure 10.21 Positive <strong>and</strong> NegativeCorrelations 205Figure 10.22 Examples of Nonl<strong>in</strong>ear (Curvil<strong>in</strong>ear)Relationships 206Figure 10.23 Example of a Bar Graph 208Figure 10.24 Example of a Pie Chart 208Figure 11.1 Selection of Two Samples from TwoDist<strong>in</strong>ct Populations 217Figure 11.2 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Error 218Figure 11.3 A Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Distribution ofMeans 219Figure 11.4 Distribution of Sample Means 220Figure 11.5 The 95 Percent ConfidenceInterval 221Figure 11.6 The 99 Percent ConfidenceInterval 221Figure 11.7 We Can Be 99 Percent Confident 222Figure 11.8 Does a Sample Difference Reflect aPopulation Difference? 222Figure 11.9 Distribution of the Difference BetweenSample Means 223Figure 11.10 Confidence Intervals 223Figure 11.11 Null <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> Hypotheses 225Figure 11.12 Illustration of When a <strong>Research</strong>erWould Reject the Null Hypothesis 225Figure 11.13 <strong>How</strong> Much Is Enough? 226Figure 11.14 Significance Area for a One-TailedTest 227Figure 11.15 One-Tailed Test Us<strong>in</strong>g a Distributionof Differences Between Sample Means 228Figure 11.16 Two-Tailed Test Us<strong>in</strong>g a Distributionof Differences Between Sample Means 228Figure 11.17 A Hypothetical Example of Type I <strong>and</strong>Type II Errors 229Figure 11.18 Reject<strong>in</strong>g the Null Hypothesis 235


xviii LIST OF FEATURES www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 11.19 An Illustration of Power Under anAssumed Population Value 235Figure 11.20 A Power Curve 236Figure 12.1 Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of Data <strong>and</strong> Approaches<strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 243Figure 12.2 A Difference That Doesn’t Make aDifference! 246Figure 12.3 Frequency Polygons of Ga<strong>in</strong> Scores onTest of Ability <strong>to</strong> Expla<strong>in</strong>: Inquiry <strong>and</strong> LectureGroups 247Figure 12.4 90 Percent Confidence Interval for aDifference of 1.2 Between Sample Means 247Figure 12.5 Scatterplots with a Pearson rof .50 249Figure 12.6 Scatterplot Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g the RelationshipBetween Initial Self-Esteem <strong>and</strong> Ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> MaritalSatisfaction Among Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Clients 251Figure 12.7 95 Percent Confidence Interval forr .42 251Figure 13.1 Example of a One-Shot Case Study<strong>Design</strong> 265Figure 13.2 Example of a One-Group Pretest-Posttest <strong>Design</strong> 265Figure 13.3 Example of a Static-Group Comparison<strong>Design</strong> 266Figure 13.4 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only Control Group <strong>Design</strong> 267Figure 13.5 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized Pretest-Posttest Control Group <strong>Design</strong> 268Figure 13.6 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized SolomonFour-Group <strong>Design</strong> 269Figure 13.7 A R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only ControlGroup <strong>Design</strong>, Us<strong>in</strong>g Matched Subjects 270Figure 13.8 Results (Means) from a Study Us<strong>in</strong>g aCounterbalanced <strong>Design</strong> 272Figure 13.9 Possible Outcome Patterns <strong>in</strong> a Time-Series <strong>Design</strong> 273Figure 13.10 Us<strong>in</strong>g a Fac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong> <strong>to</strong> StudyEffects of Method <strong>and</strong> Class Size onAchievement 274Figure 13.11 Illustration of Interaction <strong>and</strong> NoInteraction <strong>in</strong> a 2 by 2 Fac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong> 274Figure 13.12 An Interaction Effect 275Figure 13.13 Example of a 4 by 2 Fac<strong>to</strong>rial<strong>Design</strong> 275Figure 13.14 Guidel<strong>in</strong>es for H<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g InternalValidity <strong>in</strong> Comparison Group Studies 278Figure 14.1 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject Graph 300Figure 14.2 A-B <strong>Design</strong> 301Figure 14.3 A-B-A <strong>Design</strong> 302Figure 14.4 A-B-A-B <strong>Design</strong> 303Figure 14.5 B-A-B <strong>Design</strong> 304Figure 14.6 A-B-C-B <strong>Design</strong> 304Figure 14.7 Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong> 305Figure 14.8 Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong> 306Figure 14.9 Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong> Applied<strong>to</strong> Different Sett<strong>in</strong>gs 307Figure 14.10 Variations <strong>in</strong> Basel<strong>in</strong>e Stability 308Figure 14.11 Differences <strong>in</strong> Degree <strong>and</strong> Speedof Change 309Figure 14.12 Differences <strong>in</strong> Return <strong>to</strong> Basel<strong>in</strong>eConditions 310Figure 15.1 Scatterplot Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g a Correlation of1.00 329Figure 15.2 Prediction Us<strong>in</strong>g a Scatterplot 330Figure 15.3 Multiple Correlation 332Figure 15.4 Discrim<strong>in</strong>ant Function Analysis 333Figure 15.5 Path Analysis Diagram 335Figure 15.6 Scatterplots for Comb<strong>in</strong>ations ofVariables 339Figure 15.7 Elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g the Effects of Age ThroughPartial Correlation 340Figure 15.8 Scatterplots Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>How</strong> aFac<strong>to</strong>r (C) May Not Be a Threat <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity 343Figure 15.9 Circle Diagrams Illustrat<strong>in</strong>gRelationships Among Variables 343Figure 16.1 Examples of the Basic Causal-Comparative <strong>Design</strong> 367Figure 16.2 A Subject Characteristics Threat 368Figure 16.3 Does a Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityExist? 371Figure 17.1 Example of an Ideal Versus an ActualTelephone Sample for a Specific Question 394Figure 17.2 Example of Several Cont<strong>in</strong>gencyQuestions <strong>in</strong> an Interview Schedule 399Figure 17.3 Sample Cover Letter for a MailSurvey 400


LIST OF FEATURESxixFigure 17.4 Demographic Data <strong>and</strong>Representativeness 402Figure 18.1 <strong>How</strong> Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative<strong>Research</strong>ers See the World 427Figure 19.1 Variations <strong>in</strong> Approaches <strong>to</strong>Observation 442Figure 19.2 The Importance of a Second Observeras a Check on One’s Conclusions 444Figure 19.3 The Amidon/Fl<strong>and</strong>ers Scheme forCod<strong>in</strong>g Categories of Interaction <strong>in</strong> theClassroom 445Figure 19.4 An Interview of DubiousValidity 450Figure 19.5 Don’t Ask More Than One Questionat a Time 451Figure 20.1 TV Violence <strong>and</strong> Public View<strong>in</strong>gPatterns 473Figure 20.2 What Categories Should I Use? 477Figure 20.3 An Example of Cod<strong>in</strong>g an Interview 478Figure 20.4 Categories Used <strong>to</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> SocialStudies <strong>Research</strong> 481Figure 21.1 Triangulation <strong>and</strong> Politics 511Figure 22.1 What Really Happened? 540Figure 22.2 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> Is Not as Easy asYou May Th<strong>in</strong>k! 542Figure 23.1 Explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong> 560Figure 23.2 Explana<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong> 561Figure 23.3 Triangulation <strong>Design</strong> 561Figure 24.1 Stakeholders 591Figure 24.2 The Role of the “Expert” <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong> 592Figure 24.3 Levels of Participation <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong> 592Figure 24.4 Participation <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong> 595Figure 24.5 Experimental <strong>Design</strong> for the DeMariaStudy 602Figure 25.1 Organization of a <strong>Research</strong> Report 628


Preface<strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> isdirected <strong>to</strong> students tak<strong>in</strong>g their first course <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. Because this field cont<strong>in</strong>ues <strong>to</strong> grow sorapidly with regard <strong>to</strong> both the knowledge it conta<strong>in</strong>s<strong>and</strong> the methodologies it employs, the authors of any<strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry text are forced <strong>to</strong> carefully def<strong>in</strong>e theirgoals as a first step <strong>in</strong> decid<strong>in</strong>g what <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> theirbook. In our case, we cont<strong>in</strong>ually kept three ma<strong>in</strong> goals<strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. We wanted <strong>to</strong> produce a text that would:1. Provide students with the basic <strong>in</strong>formation needed<strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the research process, from idea formulationthrough data analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation.2. Enable students <strong>to</strong> use this knowledge <strong>to</strong> designtheir own research <strong>in</strong>vestigation on a <strong>to</strong>pic of personal<strong>in</strong>terest.3. Permit students <strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the literatureof educational research.The first two goals are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> satisfy the needsof those students who must plan <strong>and</strong> carry out a researchproject as part of their course requirements. The thirdgoal is aimed at students whose course requirements <strong>in</strong>cludelearn<strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the researchof others. Many <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs, ourselves <strong>in</strong>cluded, build allthree goals <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their courses, s<strong>in</strong>ce each one seems <strong>to</strong>re<strong>in</strong>force the others. It is hard <strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> fully comprehendthe research of others if you have not yourself gonethrough the process of design<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a researchproject. Similarly, the more you read <strong>and</strong> evaluatethe research of others, the better equipped you willbe <strong>to</strong> design your own mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>and</strong> creative research.In order <strong>to</strong> achieve the above goals, we have developeda book with the follow<strong>in</strong>g characteristics.CONTENT COVERAGEGoal one, <strong>to</strong> provide students with the basic <strong>in</strong>formationneeded <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the research process, has resulted<strong>in</strong> a n<strong>in</strong>e-part book plan. Part 1 (Chapter 1) <strong>in</strong>troducesstudents <strong>to</strong> the nature of educational research,briefly overviews each of the seven methodologiesdiscussed later <strong>in</strong> the text, <strong>and</strong> presents an overview ofthe research process as well as criticisms of it.xxPart 2 (Chapters 2 through 9) discusses the basicconcepts <strong>and</strong> procedures that must be unders<strong>to</strong>od beforeone can engage <strong>in</strong> research <strong>in</strong>telligently or critique itmean<strong>in</strong>gfully. These chapters expla<strong>in</strong> variables, def<strong>in</strong>itions,ethics, sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, validity, reliability,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. These <strong>and</strong> other conceptsare covered thoroughly, clearly, <strong>and</strong> relatively simply.Our emphasis throughout is <strong>to</strong> show students, by meansof clear <strong>and</strong> appropriate examples, how <strong>to</strong> set up aresearch study <strong>in</strong> an educational sett<strong>in</strong>g on a question of<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> importance.Part 3 (Chapters 10 through 12) describes <strong>in</strong> somedetail the processes <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>gdata.Part 4 (Chapters 13 through 17) describes <strong>and</strong> illustratesthe methodologies most commonly used <strong>in</strong> quantitativeeducational research. Many key concepts presented<strong>in</strong> Part 2 are considered aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> these chapters <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> illustrate their application <strong>to</strong> each methodology.F<strong>in</strong>ally, each methodology chapter concludes with acarefully chosen study from the published researchliterature. Each study is analyzed by the authors withregard <strong>to</strong> both its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. Studentsare shown how <strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> critically analyze a studythey might f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> the literature.Parts 5 (Chapters 18 through 20) <strong>and</strong> 6 (Chapters 21through 22) discuss qualitative research. Part 5 beg<strong>in</strong>s thecoverage by describ<strong>in</strong>g qualitative research, its philosophy,<strong>and</strong> essential features. It has been exp<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludevarious types of qualitative research. This is followed byan exp<strong>and</strong>ed treatment of both data collection <strong>and</strong> analysismethods. Part 6 presents the qualitative methodologies ofethnography <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. As with the quantitativemethodology chapters, these are followed by a carefullychosen research report from the published researchliterature, along with our analysis <strong>and</strong> critique.Part 7 (Chapter 23) discusses Mixed-Methods Studies,which comb<strong>in</strong>e quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods.Aga<strong>in</strong>, as <strong>in</strong> other chapters, the discussion is followed byour analysis <strong>and</strong> critique of a research report we havechosen from the published research literature.Part 8 (Chapter 24) describes the assumptions,characteristics, <strong>and</strong> steps of action research. Classroom


PREFACExxiexamples of action research questions br<strong>in</strong>g the subject <strong>to</strong>life, as does the addition of a critique of a published study.Part 9 (Chapter 25) shows how <strong>to</strong> prepare a researchproposal or report (<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a methodology of choice)that builds on the concepts <strong>and</strong> examples developed <strong>and</strong>illustrated <strong>in</strong> previous chapters.RESEARCH EXERCISESIn order <strong>to</strong> achieve our second goal of help<strong>in</strong>g studentslearn <strong>to</strong> apply their knowledge of basic processes <strong>and</strong>methodologies, we organized the first 12 chapters <strong>in</strong>the same order that students normally follow <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>ga research proposal or conduct<strong>in</strong>g a research project.Then we concluded each of these chapters with aresearch exercise that <strong>in</strong>cludes a fill-<strong>in</strong> problem sheet.These exercises allow students <strong>to</strong> apply their underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof the major concepts of each chapter. Whencompleted, these accumulated problem sheets willhave led students through the step-by-step processes<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g their own research projects. Althoughthis step-by-step development requires somerevision of their work as they learn more about theresearch process, the ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g that resultsas they slowly see their proposal develop “before theireyes” justifies the extra time <strong>and</strong> effort <strong>in</strong>volved.Problem Sheet templates are located <strong>in</strong> the StudentMastery Activities book, <strong>and</strong> electronically at the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center Web site, www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.ACTUAL RESEARCH STUDIESOur third goal, <strong>to</strong> enable students <strong>to</strong> read <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>the literature of educational research, has led us <strong>to</strong> concludeeach of the methodology chapters <strong>in</strong> Parts 4, 5, 6, 7,<strong>and</strong> 8, with an annotated study that illustrates a particularresearch method. At the end of each study we analyze itsstrengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>and</strong> offer suggestions as <strong>to</strong> howit might be improved. Similarly, at the end of our chapteron writ<strong>in</strong>g research proposals <strong>and</strong> reports, we <strong>in</strong>clude astudent research proposal that we have critiqued withmarg<strong>in</strong>al comments. This annotated proposal has provedan effective means of help<strong>in</strong>g students underst<strong>and</strong> bothgood <strong>and</strong> questionable research practices.STYLE OF PRESENTATIONBecause students are typically anxious regard<strong>in</strong>g thecontent of research courses, we have taken extraord<strong>in</strong>arycare not <strong>to</strong> overwhelm them with dry, abstract discussions.More than <strong>in</strong> any text <strong>to</strong> date, our presentations arelaced with clarify<strong>in</strong>g examples <strong>and</strong> with summariz<strong>in</strong>gcharts, tables, <strong>and</strong> diagrams. Our experience <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>gresearch courses for more than 30 years has conv<strong>in</strong>cedus that there is no such th<strong>in</strong>g as hav<strong>in</strong>g “<strong>to</strong>o many” examples<strong>in</strong> a basic text.In addition <strong>to</strong> the many examples <strong>and</strong> illustrationsthat are embedded <strong>in</strong> our (we hope) <strong>in</strong>formal writ<strong>in</strong>gstyle, we have built the follow<strong>in</strong>g pedagogical features<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the book: (1) a graphic organizer for each chapter,(2) chapter objectives, (3) chapter-open<strong>in</strong>g examples,(4) end-of-chapter summaries, (5) key terms with pagereferences, (6) discussion questions, <strong>and</strong> (7) an extensiveend-of-book glossary.CHANGES IN THE SEVENTH EDITIONA number of key additions, new illustrations, <strong>and</strong> ref<strong>in</strong>edexamples, term<strong>in</strong>ology, <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itions have been <strong>in</strong>corporated<strong>to</strong> further meet the goals of the text. Some of therevisions are described below.New Chapter on Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>: Contributedby Michael Gardner (University of Utah),Chapter 23: Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> expla<strong>in</strong>s how <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>tegrate both quantitive <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>one study, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the strengths as well as the pitfalls.Similarly <strong>to</strong> our discussion of other types of researchstudies <strong>in</strong> earlier chapters, this chapter concludes withthe presentation <strong>and</strong> analysis of a published example ofmixed-method research. Once aga<strong>in</strong>, concepts <strong>in</strong>troducedthroughout the text are used <strong>in</strong> the analysis.Updated Practical Guidel<strong>in</strong>es for Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>and</strong>Excel for Data Analysis: The <strong>in</strong>formation related <strong>to</strong>statistical analysis programs found <strong>in</strong> Chapters 10–12<strong>and</strong> Appendix D has been updated <strong>to</strong> reflect the mostrecent versions on the market as of press time.Updated Coverage of Literature Search Tools: Chapters5 <strong>and</strong> 7 have been updated <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude the lates<strong>to</strong>nl<strong>in</strong>e search <strong>to</strong>ols.Exp<strong>and</strong>ed Coverage of Material on InstitutionalReview Boards <strong>in</strong> Chapter 4.Basic Versus Applied <strong>Research</strong>: New coverage <strong>in</strong>cludesa brief discussion of the differences betweenbasic <strong>and</strong> applied research, along with examples of each.Updated References <strong>to</strong> Reflect the Most CurrentTh<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g: The correspond<strong>in</strong>g content has been updatedwhere necessary <strong>to</strong> reflect revised th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the field(such as the various approaches <strong>to</strong> qualitative researchdiscussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 18).New Illustrations: Several new draw<strong>in</strong>gs have beenadded <strong>to</strong> illustrate important concepts covered <strong>in</strong> the text.


xxii PREFACE www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eNew Annotated <strong>and</strong> Analyzed <strong>Research</strong> Reports:Three new <strong>Research</strong> Reports have been added <strong>to</strong> thetext, <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g more research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g diverse populationsas well as help<strong>in</strong>g the student apply the text’sconcepts <strong>and</strong> also practice evaluat<strong>in</strong>g published studies.Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk: An Intervention for BehaviorallyChallenged Youths by Patricia A. DoucetteThe “Nuts <strong>and</strong> Dolts” of Teacher Images <strong>in</strong> Children’sPicture S<strong>to</strong>rybooks: A Content Analysis bySarah Jo S<strong>and</strong>efur <strong>and</strong> Leeann MoorePerceived Family Support, Acculturation, <strong>and</strong> LifeSatisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American Youth: A Mixed-Methods Exploration by Lisa Edwards <strong>and</strong> ShaneLopezSPECIAL FEATURESSupport for Student Learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>helps students become critical consumers of research <strong>and</strong>prepares them <strong>to</strong> conduct <strong>and</strong> report their own research.Chapter-open<strong>in</strong>g Outl<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong> Objectives: Each chapterbeg<strong>in</strong>s with an illustration that visually <strong>in</strong>troduces a<strong>to</strong>pic or issues related <strong>to</strong> the chapter. This is followed byan outl<strong>in</strong>e of chapter content, chapter learn<strong>in</strong>g objectives,the Interactive <strong>and</strong> Applied Learn<strong>in</strong>g feature that listsrelated supplementary material, <strong>and</strong> a related vignette.More About <strong>Research</strong>, <strong>Research</strong> Tips, <strong>and</strong> Controversies<strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong>: These <strong>in</strong>formative sections help students<strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k critically about research while illustrat<strong>in</strong>gimportant techniques <strong>in</strong> educational research.Chapter-end<strong>in</strong>g Learn<strong>in</strong>g Supports: The chaptersconclude with a rem<strong>in</strong>der of the supplementary resourcesavailable, a detailed Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>ts section, a list<strong>in</strong>g ofKey Terms, <strong>and</strong> Questions for Discussion.Chapters 1–12 <strong>in</strong>clude a <strong>Research</strong> Exercise <strong>and</strong> aProblem Sheet <strong>to</strong> aid students <strong>in</strong> the construction of aresearch project.Chapters 13–17 <strong>and</strong> 19–24 <strong>in</strong>clude an actual <strong>Research</strong>Report that has been annotated <strong>to</strong> highlightconcepts discussed <strong>in</strong> the chapter.Practical Resources <strong>and</strong> Examplesfor Do<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong><strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>provides a comprehensive <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> research thatis brought <strong>to</strong> life through practical resources <strong>and</strong> examplesfor do<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g research.<strong>Research</strong> Tips boxes provide practical suggestionsfor do<strong>in</strong>g research.The Annotated <strong>Research</strong> Reports at the conclusionof Chapters 13–17 <strong>and</strong> 19–24 present students withresearch reports <strong>and</strong> author commentary on how thestudy authors have approached <strong>and</strong> supported theirresearch.<strong>Research</strong> Exercises <strong>and</strong> Problems Sheets at theconclusion of Chapters 1–13 are <strong>to</strong>ols for students <strong>to</strong>use when creat<strong>in</strong>g their own research projects.Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>and</strong> Us<strong>in</strong>g Excel boxes show how thesesoftware programs can be used <strong>to</strong> calculate variousstatistics.Chapter 24: Action <strong>Research</strong> details how classroomteachers can <strong>and</strong> should do research <strong>to</strong> improve theirteach<strong>in</strong>g.Chapter 25: Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong>Reports walks the reader through proposal <strong>and</strong> reportpreparation.Resources on the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center Website (see list<strong>in</strong>g below) provide students with a place<strong>to</strong> start when gather<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>to</strong>ols.SUPPLEMENTS THAT SUPPORTSTUDENT LEARNINGOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center Web Site atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center Web site offers <strong>to</strong>ols for study,practice, <strong>and</strong> application. A number of premium featuresare password protected <strong>and</strong> available <strong>to</strong> students who havepurchased a new text or the Student Mastery Activitiesbooklet package. Some of these features <strong>in</strong>clude:Study ResourcesQuizzes, flashcards, <strong>and</strong> crossword activities fortest<strong>in</strong>g content knowledgeLearn More About audio clips that provide additionalexplanation or examples of key conceptsPractice ResourcesInteractive Activities (electronic versions of theactivities found <strong>in</strong> the Student Mastery Activitiesbook) that provide students extra practice with specificconceptsData Analysis Examples <strong>and</strong> Exercises<strong>Research</strong> ResourcesStatistics ProgramCorrelation Coefficient AppletChi Square Applet


PREFACExxiii<strong>Research</strong> Wizard, a wizard version of the ProblemSheetsForms, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a <strong>Research</strong> Worksheet, SampleConsent Forms, <strong>Research</strong> Checklists, electronic versionsof the Problem SheetsA List<strong>in</strong>g of Professional JournalsBibliography Builder, an electronic reference builderThe McGraw-Hill Guide <strong>to</strong> Electronic <strong>Research</strong>Student Mastery Activities Book(ISBN 0-07-332655-0)The Student Mastery Activities book conta<strong>in</strong>s 100activities that allow students <strong>to</strong> practice with key conceptsexpla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the text. The book is designed so itcan be used for homework assignments or <strong>in</strong>dependentstudy.SPSS 15A student version of SPSS 15 can be packaged at areduced additional charge with <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong><strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>. Please contact your salesrepresentative for more <strong>in</strong>formation.Supplements thatSupport Instruc<strong>to</strong>rsOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center Web Site atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe Instruc<strong>to</strong>r’s portion of the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Centeroffers a number of useful resources for classroom <strong>in</strong>struction,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g an Instruc<strong>to</strong>r’s Manual, Test Bank,Computerized Test Bank, chapter-by-chapter Power-Po<strong>in</strong>t presentations, <strong>and</strong> additional resources.CPS by eInstructionClassroom Performance System is a wireless responsesystem that gives you immediate feedback from everystudent <strong>in</strong> the class. These CPS questions are specific <strong>to</strong><strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>,7/e. Contact your local sales representative for detailsabout this resource.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSDirectly <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>directly, many people have contributed<strong>to</strong> the preparation of this text. We will beg<strong>in</strong> by acknowledg<strong>in</strong>gthe students <strong>in</strong> our research classes, who,over the years, have taught us much. Also, we wish <strong>to</strong>thank the reviewers of this edition, whose generouscomments have guided the preparation of this edition.They <strong>in</strong>clude:Ronald Beebe, Clevel<strong>and</strong> State UniversityAlfred Bryant, University of North Carol<strong>in</strong>a–PembrokeCarol Carman, University of Hous<strong>to</strong>n–Clear LakeTarek Chebbi, Florida International UniversityKev<strong>in</strong> Crehan, University of Nevada–Las VegasMary Dereshiwsky, Northern Arizona UniversityMark Garrison, D’Youville CollegeNicole Morgan Gibson, Valdosta State UniversityAthanase Guhungu, Chicago State UniversityJohn Huss, Northern Kentucky UniversityHelen Hyun, San Francisco State UniversityStephen Jenk<strong>in</strong>s, Georgia Southern UniversityJoyce Killian, Southern Ill<strong>in</strong>ois University–CarbondaleCheryl Lovett Kime, University of Central OklahomaJerry McGuire, Concordia UniversityKathleen Norris, Plymouth State UniversityRebecca Robles-Piña, Sam Hous<strong>to</strong>n StateUniversityLea Witta, University of Central FloridaNancy Zeller, East Carol<strong>in</strong>a UniversityWe wish <strong>to</strong> extend a word of thanks <strong>to</strong> our colleague,Enoch Saw<strong>in</strong>, San Francisco State University,whose unusually thorough reviews of the text <strong>in</strong> variousforms led <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>numerable improvements.We want <strong>to</strong> give special thanks <strong>to</strong> Mike Gardner,University of Utah, not only for contribut<strong>in</strong>g the newchapter on mixed-methods research, but also for his assistancewith other revisions. It has been a pleasure <strong>to</strong>work with him.We would also like <strong>to</strong> thank the edi<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> staff atMcGraw-Hill for their efforts <strong>in</strong> turn<strong>in</strong>g the manuscript<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>ished book before you.F<strong>in</strong>ally, we would like <strong>to</strong> thank our wives for their unflagg<strong>in</strong>gsupport dur<strong>in</strong>g the highs <strong>and</strong> lows that <strong>in</strong>evitablyaccompany the preparation of a text of this magnitude.Jack R. FraenkelNorman E. Wallen


A Guided Tour of<strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong><strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>Welcome <strong>to</strong> <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Design</strong><strong>and</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong><strong>Education</strong>.This comprehensive <strong>in</strong>troduction<strong>to</strong> research methods was designed<strong>to</strong> present the basics of educationalresearch <strong>in</strong> as <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>able a way aspossible. To accomplish this,we’ve created the follow<strong>in</strong>gfeatures for each chapter.Open<strong>in</strong>g IllustrationEach chapter opens with an illustrative depiction of a keyconcept that will be covered <strong>in</strong> the chapter.Chapter Outl<strong>in</strong>eNext, a chapter outl<strong>in</strong>e lists the <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>to</strong> follow.Interactive <strong>and</strong> AppliedLearn<strong>in</strong>g ToolsThis special feature lists the practice activities <strong>and</strong>resources related <strong>to</strong> the chapter that are available<strong>in</strong> the student supplements.23 Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>What Is Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Why Do Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Drawbacks of Mixed-Methods StudiesA (Very) Brief His<strong>to</strong>ryTypes of Mixed-Methods<strong>Design</strong>sThe Explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>The Explana<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>The Triangulation <strong>Design</strong>Other Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong> <strong>Design</strong> IssuesSteps <strong>in</strong> Conduct<strong>in</strong>g aMixed-Methods StudyEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g a Mixed-Methods StudyEthics <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>SummaryAn Example of Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>Hypotheses <strong>and</strong> <strong>Design</strong>SampleInstrumentationInternal Validity/CredibilityData AnalysisResults/DiscussionDr. Grahamby Michael K. Gardner,Department of <strong>Education</strong>al Psychology,University of Utah“For my doc<strong>to</strong>raldissertation, I thought I might doa mixed-methods study.”“Oh?”“Yes. In the <strong>in</strong>itial phase I was th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gof do<strong>in</strong>g an ethnographic study of a high school gang <strong>to</strong>try <strong>to</strong> learn why students jo<strong>in</strong> it. Then three years later, I will identifygroups of students who as freshmen jo<strong>in</strong>ed for differentreasons <strong>to</strong> see how they are different.”“I see, comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g an ethnography witha casual-comparative study. Not bad, but how manyyears do you have <strong>to</strong> spend do<strong>in</strong>g this?”OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what a mixed-methods study is. List some of the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>Describe how mixed-methods research conduct<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study.differs from other types of research.List at least five questions that can be usedGive at least three reasons why a researcher <strong>to</strong> evaluate a mixed-methods study.might want <strong>to</strong> do a mixed-methods study. Describe briefly how matters of ethicsDescribe some of the drawbacks <strong>to</strong>affect mixed-methods research.conduct<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study.Recognize a mixed-methods study whenName the three major types of mixedmethodsresearch designs <strong>and</strong> describe literature.you come across one <strong>in</strong> the educationalbriefly how they differ.INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Read the Study Skills PrimerLearn More About Why <strong>Research</strong> Is of ValueAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 1.1: Empirical vs. Nonempirical <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.2: Basic vs. Applied <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.3: Types of <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.4: AssumptionsActivity 1.5: General <strong>Research</strong> TypesHunter? I’m Molly Lev<strong>in</strong>e. I called you about gett<strong>in</strong>g some advice about the master’s degree program <strong>in</strong>Dr.your department.”“Hello, Molly. Pleased <strong>to</strong> meet you. Come on <strong>in</strong>. <strong>How</strong> can I be of help?”“Well, I’m th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the master’s degree program <strong>in</strong> marriage <strong>and</strong> family counsel<strong>in</strong>g, but firstI want <strong>to</strong> know what the requirements are.”“I don’t blame you. It’s always wise <strong>to</strong> know what you are gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>. To obta<strong>in</strong> the degree, you’ll need <strong>to</strong>take a number of courses, <strong>and</strong> there is also an oral exam once you have completed them. You also will have <strong>to</strong>complete a small-scale study.”“What do you mean?”“You actually will have <strong>to</strong> do some research.”“Wow! What does that <strong>in</strong>volve? What do you mean by research, anyway? And how does one do it? What k<strong>in</strong>dsof research are there?”To f<strong>in</strong>d out the answers <strong>to</strong> Molly’s questions, as well as a few others, read this chapter.Chapter-Open<strong>in</strong>g ExampleThe chapter text beg<strong>in</strong>s with a practical example—a dialogue between researchers or a peek <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> aclassroom-related <strong>to</strong> the content <strong>to</strong> follow.ObjectivesChapter objectives prepare the student for thechapter ahead.xxiv


*F t i i li ti i th fi ld f h lMORE ABOUT RESEARCHChaos TheoryThe orig<strong>in</strong>s of what is now known as chaos theory are usuallytraced <strong>to</strong> the 1970s. S<strong>in</strong>ce then, it has come <strong>to</strong> occupya prom<strong>in</strong>ent place <strong>in</strong> mathematics <strong>and</strong> the natural sciences<strong>and</strong>, <strong>to</strong> a lesser extent, <strong>in</strong> the social sciences.Although the physical sciences have primarily been knownfor their basic laws, or “first pr<strong>in</strong>ciples,” it has long beenknown by scientists that most of these laws hold precisely onlyunder ideal conditions that are not found <strong>in</strong> the “real” world.Many phenomena, such as cloud formations, waterfall patterns,<strong>and</strong> even the weather, elude precise prediction. Chaostheorists argue that the natural laws that are so useful <strong>in</strong> sciencemay, <strong>in</strong> themselves, be the exception rather than the rule.Although precise prediction of such phenomena as thesw<strong>in</strong>g of a pendulum or what the weather will be at a particulartime is <strong>in</strong> most cases impossible, repeated patterns, accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> a major pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of chaos theory, can be discovered <strong>and</strong>used, even when the content of the phenomena is chaotic. Developments<strong>in</strong> computer technology, for example, have made itpossible <strong>to</strong> translate an extremely long sequence of “datapo<strong>in</strong>ts,” such as the test scores of a large group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals,<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> colored visual pictures of fasc<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g complexity <strong>and</strong>beauty. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, these pictures show dist<strong>in</strong>ct patterns thatare often quite similar across different content areas, such asphysics, biology, economics, astronomy, <strong>and</strong> geography. Evenmore surpris<strong>in</strong>g is the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that certa<strong>in</strong> patterns recur asthese pictures are enlarged. The most famous example is the“M<strong>and</strong>lebrot Bug,” shown <strong>in</strong> Pho<strong>to</strong>graphs 1.1 <strong>and</strong> 1.2. Notethat Pho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.2 is simply a magnification of a portion ofPho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.1. The t<strong>in</strong>y box <strong>in</strong> the lower left corner of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph1.1 is magnified <strong>to</strong> produce the box <strong>in</strong> the upper left-h<strong>and</strong>corner of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.2. The t<strong>in</strong>y box with<strong>in</strong> this box is then,<strong>in</strong> turn, magnified <strong>to</strong> produce the larger portion of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph1.2, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the reappearance of the “bug” <strong>in</strong> the lower rightcorner. The conclusion is that even with highly complex data(th<strong>in</strong>k of try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> predict the changes that might occur <strong>in</strong> al d f ti ) di t bilit i t if tt b f drevolution <strong>in</strong> science dur<strong>in</strong>g the twentieth century (the theoryof relativity <strong>and</strong> the discovery of quantum mechanics be<strong>in</strong>gthe first two), but that it helps <strong>to</strong> make sense out of what weview as some implications for educational research. What arethese implications?*If chaos theory is correct, the difficulty <strong>in</strong> discover<strong>in</strong>gwidely generalizable rules or laws <strong>in</strong> education, let alone thesocial sciences <strong>in</strong> general, may not be due <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>adequate concepts<strong>and</strong> theories or <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>sufficiently precise measurement<strong>and</strong> methodology, but may simply be an unavoidable factabout the world. Another implication is that whatever “laws”we do discover may be seriously limited <strong>in</strong> their applicability—across geography, across <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong>/or group differences,<strong>and</strong> across time. If this is so, chaos theory provides support forresearchers <strong>to</strong> concentrate on study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>pics at the locallevel—classroom, school, agency—<strong>and</strong> for repeated studiesover time <strong>to</strong> see if such laws hold up.Another implication is that educa<strong>to</strong>rs should pay more attention<strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tensive study of the exceptional or the unusual,rather than treat<strong>in</strong>g such <strong>in</strong>stances as trivial, <strong>in</strong>cidental, or “errors.”Yet another implication is that researchers should focuson predictability on a larger scale—that is, look<strong>in</strong>g for patterns<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups over larger units of time. Thiswould suggest a greater emphasis on long-term studies ratherthan the easier-<strong>to</strong>-conduct (<strong>and</strong> cheaper) short-time <strong>in</strong>vestigationsthat are currently the norm.Not surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, chaos theory has its critics. In education,the criticism is not of the theory itself, but more with mis<strong>in</strong>terpretations<strong>and</strong>/or misapplications of it.† Chaos theorists donot say that all is chaos; quite the contrary, they say that wemust pay more attention <strong>to</strong> chaotic phenomena <strong>and</strong> revise ourconceptions of predictability. At the same time, the laws ofgravity still hold, as, with less certa<strong>in</strong>ty, do many generalizations<strong>in</strong> education.More About <strong>Research</strong>These boxes take a closer look at important<strong>to</strong>pics <strong>in</strong> educational research. See a full list<strong>in</strong>gof these boxes, start<strong>in</strong>g on page xiii.<strong>Research</strong> Tipsb***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSKey Terms <strong>to</strong> Def<strong>in</strong>e<strong>in</strong> a <strong>Research</strong> StudyTerms necessary <strong>to</strong> ensure that the research question issharply focusedTerms that <strong>in</strong>dividuals outside the field of study may notunderst<strong>and</strong>Terms that have multiple mean<strong>in</strong>gsTerms that are essential <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g what the study isaboutTerms <strong>to</strong> provide precision <strong>in</strong> specifications for <strong>in</strong>struments<strong>to</strong> be developed or locatedThese boxes provide practical po<strong>in</strong>ters fordo<strong>in</strong>g research. See a full list<strong>in</strong>g of theseboxes on page xiii.clarify by example. <strong>Research</strong>ers might th<strong>in</strong>k of a fewhumanistic classrooms with which they are familiar <strong>and</strong>then try <strong>to</strong> describe as fully as possible what happens <strong>in</strong>these classrooms. Usually we suggest that people observesuch classrooms <strong>to</strong> see for themselves how theydiffer from other classrooms. This approach also has itsproblems, however, s<strong>in</strong>ce our descriptions may still notbe as clear <strong>to</strong> others as they would like.Thus, a third method of clarification is <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e importantterms operationally. Operational def<strong>in</strong>itionsrequire that researchers specify the actions or operationsnecessary <strong>to</strong> measure or identify the term. For example,here are two possible operational def<strong>in</strong>itions of the termhumanistic classroom.1. Any classroom identified by specified experts asclassroom <strong>to</strong> many people (<strong>and</strong> perhaps <strong>to</strong> you). But it isconsiderably more specific (<strong>and</strong> thus clearer) than thedef<strong>in</strong>ition with which we began.* Armed with this def<strong>in</strong>ition(<strong>and</strong> the necessary facilities), researchers coulddecide quickly whether or not a particular classroomqualified as an example of a humanistic classroom.Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g terms operationally is a helpful way <strong>to</strong> clarifytheir mean<strong>in</strong>g. Operational def<strong>in</strong>itions are useful<strong>to</strong>ols <strong>and</strong> should be mastered by all students of research.Remember that the operations or activities necessary <strong>to</strong>measure or identify the term must be specified. Which ofthe follow<strong>in</strong>g possible def<strong>in</strong>itions of the term motivated<strong>to</strong> learn mathematics do you th<strong>in</strong>k are operational?1. As shown by enthusiasm <strong>in</strong> class2. As judged by the student’s math teacher us<strong>in</strong>g aCONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHShould Some <strong>Research</strong> MethodsBe Preferred over Others?Recently, several researchers* have expressed their concernthat the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong> is show<strong>in</strong>gfavoritism <strong>to</strong>ward the narrow view that experimental research*D. C. Berl<strong>in</strong>er (2002). <strong>Education</strong>al research: The hardest scienceof all. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 18–20; F. E. Erickson <strong>and</strong>K. Gutierrez (2002). Culture, rigor, <strong>and</strong> science <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 21–24.is, if not the only, at least the most respectable form of research<strong>and</strong> the only one worthy of be<strong>in</strong>g called scientific. Sucha preference has implications for both the fund<strong>in</strong>g of schoolprograms <strong>and</strong> educational research. As one writer commented,“<strong>How</strong> scared should we be when the federal governmentendorses a particular view of science <strong>and</strong> rejects others?”††E. A. St. Pierre (2002). Science rejects postmodernism. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 25.Controversies <strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong>represents a different <strong>to</strong>ol for try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> whatgoes on, <strong>and</strong> what works, <strong>in</strong> schools. It is <strong>in</strong>appropriate<strong>to</strong> consider any one or two of these approaches as superior<strong>to</strong> any of the others. The effectiveness of a particularmethodology depends <strong>in</strong> large part on the nature ofthe research question one wants <strong>to</strong> ask <strong>and</strong> the specificcontext with<strong>in</strong> which the particular <strong>in</strong>vestigation is <strong>to</strong>take place. We need <strong>to</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> what goes on <strong>in</strong>education from as many perspectives as possible, <strong>and</strong>hence we need <strong>to</strong> construe research <strong>in</strong> broad rather thannarrow terms.As far as we are concerned, research <strong>in</strong> educationshould ask a variety of questions, move <strong>in</strong> a variety ofdirections, encompass a variety of methodologies, <strong>and</strong>use a variety of <strong>to</strong>ols. Different research orientations,perspectives, <strong>and</strong> goals should be not only allowed butencouraged. The <strong>in</strong>tent of this book is <strong>to</strong> help you learnhow <strong>and</strong> when <strong>to</strong> use several of these methodologies.General <strong>Research</strong> Typessummarize the characteristics (abilities, preferences,behaviors, <strong>and</strong> so on) of <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups or (sometimes)physical environments (such as schools). Qualitativeapproaches, such as ethnographic <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalmethodologies are also primarily descriptive <strong>in</strong> nature.Examples of descriptive studies <strong>in</strong> education <strong>in</strong>cludeidentify<strong>in</strong>g the achievements of various groups of students;describ<strong>in</strong>g the behaviors of teachers, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,or counselors; describ<strong>in</strong>g the attitudes of parents;<strong>and</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g the physical capabilities of schools. Thedescription of phenomena is the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for all researchendeavors.Descriptive research <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, however, is notvery satisfy<strong>in</strong>g, s<strong>in</strong>ce most researchers want <strong>to</strong> have amore complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of people <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs. Thisrequires a more detailed analysis of the various aspects ofphenomena <strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>terrelationships. Advances <strong>in</strong> biology,for example, have come about, <strong>in</strong> large part, as a resul<strong>to</strong>f the categorization of descriptions <strong>and</strong> the subsequentdeterm<strong>in</strong>ation of relationships among these categories.Associational <strong>Research</strong>. <strong>Education</strong>al researchersl t t d th i l d ib it tiThese boxes highlight a controversy <strong>in</strong> research<strong>to</strong> provide you with a greater underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g ofthe issue. See a full list<strong>in</strong>g of these boxes onpage xiv.xxv


PopulationsAE SB D TIC JUHF Y X P G QZ LW N RV M K OA B C D E25%F G H I JK L M N O50%P Q R S T25%ABNOQR PHICDLMEFGSTU JKABNOQR PHICDLMEFGSTU JKFigures <strong>and</strong> TablesDLPHYNB D25%F M O J50%P S25%QREFGCDR<strong>and</strong>omsample of<strong>in</strong>dividualsR<strong>and</strong>omsample ofclustersC, L, TCDSTULMNumerous figures <strong>and</strong> tables expla<strong>in</strong> or extendconcepts presented <strong>in</strong> the text.Simpler<strong>and</strong>omStratifiedr<strong>and</strong>omClusterr<strong>and</strong>omTwo-stager<strong>and</strong>omFigure 6.4 R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsSamples<strong>Research</strong> ReportsPublished research reports are <strong>in</strong>cluded atthe conclusion of methodology chapters.The reports have been annotated <strong>to</strong> illustrateimportant po<strong>in</strong>ts.Justification566RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 53(3) (2006): 279–287. Reproduced with permission.Perceived Family Support, Acculturation,<strong>and</strong> Life Satisfaction <strong>in</strong> MexicanAmerican Youth: A Mixed-MethodsExplorationL. M. EdwardsMarquette UniversityS. J. LopezUniversity of KansasIn this article, the authors describe a mixed-methods study designed <strong>to</strong> explore perceivedfamily support, acculturation, <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction among 266 Mexican Americanadolescents. Specifically, the authors conducted a thematic analysis of open-endedresponses <strong>to</strong> a question about life satisfaction <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> participants’ perceptionsof fac<strong>to</strong>rs that contributed <strong>to</strong> their overall satisfaction with life. The authors also conductedhierarchical regression analyses <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractivecontributions of perceived support from family <strong>and</strong> Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo acculturationorientations on life satisfaction. Convergence of mixed-methods f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs demonstratedthat perceived family support <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation were significant predic<strong>to</strong>rs oflife satisfaction <strong>in</strong> these adolescents. Implications, limitations, <strong>and</strong> directions for furtherresearch are discussed.Psychologists have identified <strong>and</strong> studied a number of challenges faced by Lat<strong>in</strong>oyouth (e.g., juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quency, gang activity, school dropout, alcohol <strong>and</strong> drug abuse),yet little scholarly time <strong>and</strong> energy have been spent on explor<strong>in</strong>g how these adolescentssuccessfully navigate their development <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> adulthood or how they experience wellbe<strong>in</strong>g(Rodriguez & Morrobel, 2004). <strong>Research</strong>ers have yet <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the personalcharacteristics that play a role <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents’ satisfaction with life or how certa<strong>in</strong>cultural values <strong>and</strong>/or strengths <strong>and</strong> resources are related <strong>to</strong> their well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Answers <strong>to</strong>these questions can beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists with a deeper underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof how Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g, which can, <strong>in</strong> turn, hopefully allowresearchers <strong>to</strong> work <strong>to</strong> improve well-be<strong>in</strong>g for those who struggle <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d it.Lat<strong>in</strong>o 1 youth are a grow<strong>in</strong>g presence <strong>in</strong> most communities with<strong>in</strong> the UnitedStates. The U.S. Census Bureau projects that by the year 2010, 20% of young people betweenthe ages of 10 <strong>and</strong> 20 years will be of Hispanic orig<strong>in</strong>. Furthermore, it is projectedthat by the year 2020, one <strong>in</strong> five children will be Hispanic, <strong>and</strong> the Hispanic adolescentpopulation will <strong>in</strong>crease by 50% (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000, 2001). Whereas adolescenceis a unique developmental period for all youth, Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents <strong>in</strong> particular may faceadditional challenges as a result of their ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority status (Vazquez Garcia, Garcia1 In this article, the terms Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>and</strong> Hispanic have been used <strong>in</strong>terchangeably. Specifically, <strong>in</strong> cases <strong>in</strong>which research is summarized, the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs used by the authors were reta<strong>in</strong>ed. The participant sample,however, was restricted <strong>to</strong> adolescents who self-identified as “Mexican” or “Mexican American” <strong>and</strong> arethus described as such.CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 567Coll, Erkut, Alarcon, & Tropp, 2000). These youth generally have undergone socializationexperiences of their Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture (known as enculturation) <strong>and</strong> also must learn <strong>to</strong> Def<strong>in</strong>itionsacculturate <strong>to</strong> the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture <strong>to</strong> some degree (Knight, Bernal, Cota, Garza, &Ocampo, 1993). Navigat<strong>in</strong>g the dem<strong>and</strong>s of these cultural contexts can be challeng<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> yet many Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> positive outcomes. The <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gnumbers of Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth, along with the counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology field’s imperative <strong>to</strong> provideculturally competent services, require that professionals cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> thefull range of psychological function<strong>in</strong>g for members of this unique population.Counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists have cont<strong>in</strong>ually emphasized the importance of wellbe<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g client strengths <strong>in</strong> theory, research, <strong>and</strong> practice(Lopez et al., 2006; Walsh, 2003). This commitment <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the whole person,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal <strong>and</strong> contextual assets <strong>and</strong> challenges, has been one hallmark of thefield (Super, 1955; Tyler, 1973) <strong>and</strong> has <strong>in</strong>fluenced a variety of research about optimalhuman function<strong>in</strong>g (see D. W. Sue & Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003). More recent discussions <strong>in</strong> thisarea have underscored the importance of identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> nurtur<strong>in</strong>g cultural values <strong>and</strong>strengths <strong>in</strong> people of color (e.g., family, religious faith, biculturalism), be<strong>in</strong>g cautious <strong>to</strong>acknowledge that strengths are not universal <strong>and</strong> may differ accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> context or culturalbackground (Lopez et al., 2006; Lopez et al., 2002; D. W. Sue & Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003),<strong>and</strong> may be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by certa<strong>in</strong> with<strong>in</strong>-group differences such as acculturation level(Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003; Zane & Mak, 2003).As scholars respond <strong>to</strong> the emerg<strong>in</strong>g need <strong>to</strong> explore strengths among Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth,the importance of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g these resources <strong>and</strong> values with<strong>in</strong> a cultural context isevident. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g how Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g from their ownperspectives <strong>and</strong> vantage po<strong>in</strong>ts is <strong>in</strong>tegral, as theories from other cultural worldviews maynot be applicable <strong>to</strong> their lives (Auerbach & Silverste<strong>in</strong>, 2003; Lopez et al., 2002; D. W. Sue& Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003). Furthermore, it is necessary <strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> test propositions about Justificationthe role of certa<strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultural values, such as the importance of family, <strong>in</strong> overall wellbe<strong>in</strong>g.Given that many Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents <strong>to</strong>day navigate bicultural contexts <strong>and</strong> adhere<strong>to</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o traditions <strong>and</strong> cus<strong>to</strong>ms <strong>to</strong> differ<strong>in</strong>g degrees (Romero & Roberts, 2003), it is likelythat the role family plays <strong>in</strong> adolescent well-be<strong>in</strong>g is complex <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>in</strong>dividualdifferences such as acculturation. In this study, we sought <strong>to</strong> explore the relationships betweenthese variables by focus<strong>in</strong>g specifically on perceived family support, life satisfaction, Purpose<strong>and</strong> acculturation among Mexican American youth.PERCEIVED FAMILY SUPPORT, ACCULTURATION,AND LIFE SATISFACTION AMONG LATINO YOUTHThe importance of family has been noted as a core Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultural value (Castillo, Conoley,& Brossart, 2004; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003; Paniagua, 1998; Sabogal, Mar<strong>in</strong>, Otero-Sabogal,Mar<strong>in</strong>, & Perez-Stable, 1987). Familismo (familism) is the term used <strong>to</strong> describe the importanceof extended family ties <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture as well as the strong identification <strong>and</strong> Def<strong>in</strong>itionattachment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with their families (Tri<strong>and</strong>is, Mar<strong>in</strong>, Betancourt, Lisansky, &Chang, 1982). Familism is not unique <strong>to</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture <strong>and</strong> has been noted as an importantvalue for other ethnic groups such as African Americans, Asian Americans, <strong>and</strong> AmericanIndians (Cooper, 1999; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003). Nevertheless, it is considered a central aspec<strong>to</strong>f Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> some studies, it has been shown <strong>to</strong> be valued by Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>in</strong>dividualsmore than by non-Lat<strong>in</strong>o Whites (Ga<strong>in</strong>es et al., 1997; Mar<strong>in</strong>, 1993; M<strong>in</strong>del, 1980).In a study of familismo among Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents, Vazquez Garcia et al. (2000)found that the length of time youth had been <strong>in</strong> the United States did not affect their adherence<strong>to</strong> the value of familismo. These results demonstrated that the longerPrior researchadolescentsxxvi


414 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is not explicitly stated. It appears <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>“fill <strong>in</strong> the gap <strong>in</strong> our knowledge about cross-nationaldifferences <strong>in</strong> attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wardscheat<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>and</strong>, more specifically, <strong>to</strong> compare collegebus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> Russia <strong>and</strong> the United States onthese characteristics.The study is justified by cit<strong>in</strong>g both evidence <strong>and</strong>op<strong>in</strong>ion that cheat<strong>in</strong>g is widespread <strong>in</strong> the United States<strong>and</strong>, presumably (although with less documentation),worldwide. Additional justification <strong>in</strong>cludes the unfairnessof cheat<strong>in</strong>g, the likelihood of cheat<strong>in</strong>g carry<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>future life, <strong>and</strong> (<strong>in</strong> the discussion) the need for teachers<strong>in</strong> mult<strong>in</strong>ational classes <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the issues <strong>in</strong>volved.The importance of attitudes <strong>and</strong> perceptionsseems <strong>to</strong> be taken for granted; the only justification forstudy<strong>in</strong>g them is implied <strong>in</strong> the results of the three studiesthat found differences between American students<strong>and</strong> those <strong>in</strong> other countries. We th<strong>in</strong>k a stronger justificationcould <strong>and</strong> should have been made. The f<strong>in</strong>al justificationis that there have been few such studies, nonewith bus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> Russia <strong>and</strong> the United States.The authors’ concern about confidentiality is important,both with regard <strong>to</strong> ethics <strong>and</strong> the validity of <strong>in</strong>formation;they appear <strong>to</strong> have addressed it as effectively as possible.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems of risk or deception.DEFINITIONSDef<strong>in</strong>itions are not provided <strong>and</strong> would be very helpful(as discussed below under “Instrumentation”) becausethe terms attitude, values, <strong>and</strong> beliefs, especially, havemany different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The term tendencies appears<strong>to</strong> mean (from the example items) actual cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>various forms. Some clarity is provided by partial operationaldef<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>in</strong> the form of example items. Weth<strong>in</strong>k a def<strong>in</strong>ition of cheat<strong>in</strong>g should have been provided<strong>to</strong> readers <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> respondents. Based on the itemsprovided, it appears <strong>to</strong> be someth<strong>in</strong>g like “receiv<strong>in</strong>gcredit for work that is not one’s own.”PRIOR RESEARCHThe authors provide extensive citation of evidence <strong>and</strong>summaries of studies on the extent of college-levelcheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> on cross-national comparisons. They givegood brief summaries of what they state are the onlythree directly related studies.HYPOTHESESNo hypothese are stated. A nondirectional hypothesis isclearly implied—i.e., there will be differences betweenthe two groups.SAMPLEThe two groups are convenience (<strong>and</strong> possibly volunteer)samples from the two nations. Each is describedwith respect <strong>to</strong> location, gender, age, <strong>and</strong> academicclass. They consist only of bus<strong>in</strong>ess students, who maynot be representative of all college students. Representativenessis further compromised by the unreportednumber of “unusable” surveys. Sample numbers (443<strong>and</strong> 174) are acceptable.INSTRUMENTATIONThe questionnaire consists of yes-no questions (twobased on brief scenarios) <strong>to</strong> measure “tendencies” <strong>and</strong>seven-po<strong>in</strong>t rat<strong>in</strong>g scales <strong>to</strong> assess attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefsabout cheat<strong>in</strong>g, for a <strong>to</strong>tal of 29 items, of which 21 areshown <strong>in</strong> the report. Neither reliability nor validity isdiscussed. Because the <strong>in</strong>tent was <strong>to</strong> compare groupson <strong>in</strong>dividual items, no summary scores were used.Nevertheless, consistency of response <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualitems is essential <strong>to</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gful results. Though admittedlydifficult, the procedure followed <strong>in</strong> the K<strong>in</strong>seystudy (see page 395) of ask<strong>in</strong>g the same question withdifferent word<strong>in</strong>g might have been used with, at least, asubsample of students <strong>and</strong> items. Similarly, a comparisonof the questionnaire with <strong>in</strong>terview responses <strong>to</strong> thesame content would have provided some evidence ofvalidity.The question of validity is confused by the lack ofclear def<strong>in</strong>itions. The items <strong>in</strong> Table 1 suggest that“tendencies <strong>to</strong> cheat” is taken <strong>to</strong> mean “hav<strong>in</strong>g cheatedor known of others cheat<strong>in</strong>g,” although the two scenarioitems seem <strong>to</strong> be ask<strong>in</strong>g what is considered <strong>to</strong>constitute cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Attitudes <strong>and</strong> perceptions are comb<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> Table 2 as “beliefs,” which seem <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludeboth “op<strong>in</strong>ions about the extent of cheat<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>and</strong> “judgmentsas <strong>to</strong> what behaviors are acceptable”—as well aswhat constitutes <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r responsibility. As such, theitems appear <strong>to</strong> have content validity but omit other behaviors,such as destroy<strong>in</strong>g required library read<strong>in</strong>gs.This does not <strong>in</strong>validate the items used unless they areconsidered <strong>to</strong> represent all forms of cheat<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally,the validity of self-report items cannot be assumed,particularly <strong>in</strong> cross-cultural studies, where mean<strong>in</strong>gsmay differ.Each research report is critiqued by the authors,with both its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses discussed.Chapter ReviewThe chapter ends with a list<strong>in</strong>g of the reviewresources available for students on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center Web site <strong>and</strong> Student MasteryActivities book.CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 105Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsBulleted ma<strong>in</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ts highlight the key conceptsof the chapter.SAMPLES AND SAMPLINGThe term sampl<strong>in</strong>g, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the process of select<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>dividuals who will participate (e.g., be observed or questioned) <strong>in</strong> a researchstudy.A sample is any part of a population of <strong>in</strong>dividuals on whom <strong>in</strong>formation isobta<strong>in</strong>ed. It may, for a variety of reasons, be different from the sample orig<strong>in</strong>allyselected.SAMPLES AND POPULATIONSThe term population, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> all the members of a particulargroup. It is the group of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the researcher, the group <strong>to</strong> whom the researcherwould like <strong>to</strong> generalize the results of a study.A target population is the actual population <strong>to</strong> whom the researcher would like <strong>to</strong>generalize; the accessible population is the population <strong>to</strong> whom the researcher isentitled <strong>to</strong> generalize.A representative sample is a sample that is similar <strong>to</strong> the population on all characteristics.RANDOM VERSUS NONRANDOM SAMPLINGSampl<strong>in</strong>g may be either r<strong>and</strong>om or nonr<strong>and</strong>om. R<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>in</strong>cludesimple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>in</strong>clude systematicsampl<strong>in</strong>g, convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g.RANDOM SAMPLING METHODSA simple r<strong>and</strong>om sample is a sample selected from a population <strong>in</strong> such a mannerthat all members of the population have an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected.A stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sample is a sample selected so that certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics arerepresented <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> the same proportion as they occur <strong>in</strong> the population.A cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sample is one obta<strong>in</strong>ed by us<strong>in</strong>g groups as the sampl<strong>in</strong>g unit ratherthan <strong>in</strong>dividuals.A two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sample selects groups r<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>and</strong> then chooses <strong>in</strong>dividualsr<strong>and</strong>omly from these groups.A table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers lists <strong>and</strong> arranges numbers <strong>in</strong> no particular order <strong>and</strong> canbe used <strong>to</strong> select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsxxvii


FACTORIAL DESIGNSFac<strong>to</strong>rial designs extend the number of relationships that may be exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> anexperimental study.comparison group 262control 264control group 262counterbalanceddesign 271criterion variable 261dependent variable 261design 264experiment 262experimental group 262experimentalresearch 261experimentalvariable 261extraneous variables 263fac<strong>to</strong>rial design 273ga<strong>in</strong> score 271<strong>in</strong>dependentvariable 261<strong>in</strong>teraction 273match<strong>in</strong>g design 268mechanicalmatch<strong>in</strong>g 270modera<strong>to</strong>r variables 273nonequivalent controlgroup design 266one-group pretestposttestdesign 265one-shot case studydesign 265outcome variable 261pretest treatment<strong>in</strong>teraction 268quasi-experimentaldesign 271r<strong>and</strong>omassignment 263r<strong>and</strong>om selection 263r<strong>and</strong>omized posttes<strong>to</strong>nlycontrol groupdesign 267r<strong>and</strong>omized pretestposttestcontrol groupdesign 267r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomonfour-group design 268regressed ga<strong>in</strong> score 271static-group comparisondesign 266static-group pretestposttestdesign 266statistical equat<strong>in</strong>g 270statistical match<strong>in</strong>g 270time-series design 272treatment variable 2611. An occasional criticism of experimental research is that it is very difficult <strong>to</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong> schools. Would you agree? Why or why not?2. Are there any cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect statements you can make that you believe would betrue <strong>in</strong> most schools? Would you say, for example, that a sympathetic teacher“causes” elementary school students <strong>to</strong> like school more?3. Are there any advantages <strong>to</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g more than one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> an experimentaldesign? If so, what are they? What about more than one dependent variable?4. What designs could be used <strong>in</strong> each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g studies? (Note: More than onedesign is possible <strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>stance.)a. A comparison of two different ways of teach<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> first-graders.b. An assessment of the effectiveness of weekly tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g sessions on the read<strong>in</strong>gability of third-graders.Key TermsFor DiscussionKey TermsKey terms are listed with page references.For DiscussionEnd-of-chapter questions are designed for<strong>in</strong>-class discussion.CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 23<strong>Research</strong> ExercisesThe research exercise expla<strong>in</strong>s how <strong>to</strong> fill <strong>in</strong> theProblem Sheet that follows.<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 1: What K<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>Research</strong>?Th<strong>in</strong>k of a research idea or problem you would like <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. Us<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 1,briefly describe the problem <strong>in</strong> a sentence or two. Then <strong>in</strong>dicate the type of researchmethodology you would use <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate this problem.Problem Sheet 1<strong>Research</strong> Method1. A possible <strong>to</strong>pic or problem I am th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of research<strong>in</strong>g is:2. The specific method that seems most appropriate for me <strong>to</strong> use at this time is (circleone, or if plann<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study, circle both types of methods you plan <strong>to</strong>use):An electronic version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t,save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.a. An experimentb. A survey us<strong>in</strong>g a written questionnairec. A survey us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews of several peopled. A correlational studye. A causal-comparative studyf. An ethnographic studyA full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.Problem SheetsIndividually, the problem sheets allow students<strong>to</strong> apply their underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the majorconcepts of each chapter. As a whole, theywalk students through each step of theresearch process.g. A case studyh. A content analysisi. A his<strong>to</strong>rical studyj. An action research studyk. A mixed-methods study3. What questions (if any) might a critical researcher raise with regard <strong>to</strong> your study?(See the section on “Critical Analysis of <strong>Research</strong>” on pp. 16–19 <strong>in</strong> the text forexamples)xxviii


Introduction<strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong>P A R T1<strong>Research</strong> takes many forms. In Part 1, we <strong>in</strong>troduce you <strong>to</strong> the subject of educationalresearch <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why knowledge of the various types of research is of value <strong>to</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs.Because research is but one way <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> knowledge, we also describe severalother ways <strong>and</strong> compare the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses of each. We give a briefoverview of educational research methodologies <strong>to</strong> set the stage for a more extensivediscussion <strong>in</strong> later chapters. Lastly, we discuss criticisms of the research process.


1TheNature of <strong>Research</strong>Some Examples of<strong>Education</strong>al ConcernsWhy <strong>Research</strong> Isof ValueWays of Know<strong>in</strong>gSensory ExperienceAgreement with OthersExpert Op<strong>in</strong>ionLogicThe Scientific MethodTypes of <strong>Research</strong>Experimental <strong>Research</strong>Correlational <strong>Research</strong>Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong>Survey <strong>Research</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Action <strong>Research</strong>All Have ValueGeneral <strong>Research</strong> TypesQuantitative <strong>and</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Meta-analysisCritical Analysisof <strong>Research</strong>A Brief Overview of the<strong>Research</strong> Process“It seems prettyobvious <strong>to</strong> me that themore you know about asubject, the better youcan teach it!”OBJECTIVES“Right! Just likewe know that evidenceis overwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly <strong>in</strong> favorof phonics as the best way<strong>to</strong> teach read<strong>in</strong>g!”Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“educational research” <strong>and</strong> give twoexamples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>to</strong>picseducational researchers might <strong>in</strong>vestigate.Expla<strong>in</strong> why a knowledge of scientific researchmethodology can be of value <strong>to</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs.Name <strong>and</strong> give an example of four waysof know<strong>in</strong>g other than the methodused by scientists.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“scientific method.”Give an example of six different typesof research methodologies used byeducational researchers.“Well, youboth may be <strong>in</strong> fora surprise!”Describe briefly what is meant by criticalresearch.Describe the differences amongdescriptive, associational, <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>tervention-type studies.Describe briefly the difference betweenbasic <strong>and</strong> applied research.Describe briefly the difference betweenquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research.Describe briefly what is meant bymixed-methods research.Describe briefly the basic components<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the research process.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Read the Study Skills PrimerLearn More About Why <strong>Research</strong> Is of ValueAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 1.1: Empirical vs. Nonempirical <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.2: Basic vs. Applied <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.3: Types of <strong>Research</strong>Activity 1.4: AssumptionsActivity 1.5: General <strong>Research</strong> TypesDr. Hunter? I’m Molly Lev<strong>in</strong>e. I called you about gett<strong>in</strong>g some advice about the master’s degree program <strong>in</strong>your department.”“Hello, Molly. Pleased <strong>to</strong> meet you. Come on <strong>in</strong>. <strong>How</strong> can I be of help?”“Well, I’m th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the master’s degree program <strong>in</strong> marriage <strong>and</strong> family counsel<strong>in</strong>g, but firstI want <strong>to</strong> know what the requirements are.”“I don’t blame you. It’s always wise <strong>to</strong> know what you are gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>. To obta<strong>in</strong> the degree, you’ll need <strong>to</strong>take a number of courses, <strong>and</strong> there is also an oral exam once you have completed them. You also will have <strong>to</strong>complete a small-scale study.”“What do you mean?”“You actually will have <strong>to</strong> do some research.”“Wow! What does that <strong>in</strong>volve? What do you mean by research, anyway? And how does one do it? What k<strong>in</strong>dsof research are there?”To f<strong>in</strong>d out the answers <strong>to</strong> Molly’s questions, as well as a few others, read this chapter.Some Examplesof <strong>Education</strong>al ConcernsA high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>in</strong> San Francisco wants <strong>to</strong>improve the morale of her faculty.The direc<strong>to</strong>r of the gifted student program <strong>in</strong> Denverwould like <strong>to</strong> know what happens dur<strong>in</strong>g a typical week<strong>in</strong> an English class for advanced placement students.An elementary school counselor <strong>in</strong> Boise wishes hecould get more students <strong>to</strong> open up <strong>to</strong> him about theirworries <strong>and</strong> problems.A tenth-grade biology teacher <strong>in</strong> Atlanta wonders ifdiscussions are more effective than lectures <strong>in</strong> motivat<strong>in</strong>gstudents <strong>to</strong> learn biological concepts.A physical education teacher <strong>in</strong> Tulsa wonders if ability<strong>in</strong> one sport correlates with ability <strong>in</strong> other sports.A seventh-grade student <strong>in</strong> Philadelphia asks her counselorwhat she can do <strong>to</strong> improve her study habits.The president of the local PTA <strong>in</strong> Little Rock, paren<strong>to</strong>f a sixth-grader at Cabrillo School, wonders howhe can get more parents <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> school-relatedactivities.Each of the above examples, although fictional, representsa typical sort of question or concern fac<strong>in</strong>g manyof us <strong>in</strong> education <strong>to</strong>day. Together, these examples suggestthat teachers, counselors, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, parents,<strong>and</strong> students cont<strong>in</strong>ually need <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> do theirjobs. Teachers need <strong>to</strong> know what k<strong>in</strong>ds of materials,strategies, <strong>and</strong> activities best help students learn. Counselorsneed <strong>to</strong> know what problems h<strong>in</strong>der or preventstudents from learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong> help them with theseproblems. Adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs need <strong>to</strong> know how <strong>to</strong> providean environment for happy <strong>and</strong> productive learn<strong>in</strong>g. Parentsneed <strong>to</strong> know how <strong>to</strong> help their children succeed <strong>in</strong>school. Students need <strong>to</strong> know how <strong>to</strong> study <strong>to</strong> learn asmuch as they can.3


4 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWhy <strong>Research</strong> Is of Value<strong>How</strong> can educa<strong>to</strong>rs, parents, <strong>and</strong> students obta<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>formationthey need? Many ways of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation,of course, exist. One can consult experts, reviewbooks <strong>and</strong> articles, question or observe colleagues withrelevant experience, exam<strong>in</strong>e one’s own past experience,or even rely on <strong>in</strong>tuition. All these approaches suggestpossible ways <strong>to</strong> proceed, but the answers they provideare not always reliable. Experts may be mistaken; sourcedocuments may conta<strong>in</strong> no <strong>in</strong>sights of value; colleaguesmay have no experience <strong>in</strong> the matter; <strong>and</strong> one’s own experienceor <strong>in</strong>tuition may be irrelevant or misunders<strong>to</strong>od.This is why a knowledge of scientific research methodologycan be of value. The scientific method provides uswith another way of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation—<strong>in</strong>formationthat is as accurate <strong>and</strong> reliable as we can get. Let us compareit, therefore, with some of the other ways of know<strong>in</strong>g.Ways of Know<strong>in</strong>gSENSORY EXPERIENCEWe see, we hear, we smell, we taste, we <strong>to</strong>uch. Most ofus have seen fireworks on the Fourth of July, heard thewh<strong>in</strong>e of a jet airplane’s eng<strong>in</strong>es overhead, smelleda rose, tasted chocolate ice cream, <strong>and</strong> felt the wetness ofa ra<strong>in</strong>y day. The <strong>in</strong>formation we take <strong>in</strong> from the worldthrough our senses is the most immediate way we haveof know<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g. Us<strong>in</strong>g sensory experience as ameans of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, the direc<strong>to</strong>r of the giftedstudent program mentioned above, for example, mightvisit an advanced placement English class <strong>to</strong> see <strong>and</strong> hearwhat happens dur<strong>in</strong>g a week or two of the semester.Sensory data, <strong>to</strong> be sure, can be ref<strong>in</strong>ed. See<strong>in</strong>g the temperatureon an outdoor thermometer can ref<strong>in</strong>e our knowledgeof how cold it is; a <strong>to</strong>p-quality stereo system can helpus hear Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony with greater clarity;similarly, smell, taste, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch can all be enhanced, <strong>and</strong>usually need <strong>to</strong> be. Many experiments <strong>in</strong> sensory perceptionhave revealed that we are not always wise <strong>to</strong> trust oursenses <strong>to</strong>o completely. Our senses can (<strong>and</strong> often do) deceiveus: The gunshot we hear becomes a car backfir<strong>in</strong>g;the water we see <strong>in</strong> the road ahead is but a mirage; thechicken we thought we tasted turns out <strong>to</strong> be rabbit.Sensory knowledge is undependable; it is also <strong>in</strong>complete.The data we take <strong>in</strong> through our senses do not accountfor all (or even most) of what we seem <strong>to</strong> feel is therange of human know<strong>in</strong>g. To obta<strong>in</strong> reliable knowledge,therefore, we cannot rely on our senses alone but mustcheck what we th<strong>in</strong>k we know with other sources.AGREEMENT WITH OTHERSOne such source is the op<strong>in</strong>ions of others. Not only canwe share our sensations with others, we can also checkon the accuracy <strong>and</strong> authenticity of these sensations:Does this soup taste salty <strong>to</strong> you? Isn’t that John overthere? Did you hear someone cry for help? Smells likemustard, doesn’t it?Obviously, there is a great advantage <strong>to</strong> check<strong>in</strong>gwith others about whether they see or hear what we do.It can help us discard what is untrue <strong>and</strong> manage ourlives more <strong>in</strong>telligently by focus<strong>in</strong>g on what is true. If,while hik<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the country, I do not hear the sound of anapproach<strong>in</strong>g au<strong>to</strong>mobile but several of my companionsdo <strong>and</strong> alert me <strong>to</strong> it, I can proceed with caution. All ofus frequently discount our own sensations when othersreport that we are miss<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g or “see<strong>in</strong>g” th<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong>correctly. Us<strong>in</strong>g agreement with others as a means ofobta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, the tenth-grade biology teacher<strong>in</strong> Atlanta, for example, might check with her colleagues<strong>to</strong> see if they f<strong>in</strong>d discussions more effectivethan lectures <strong>in</strong> motivat<strong>in</strong>g their students <strong>to</strong> learn.The problem with such common knowledge is thatit, <strong>to</strong>o, can be wrong. A majority vote of a committee isno guarantee of the truth. My friends might be wrongabout the presence of an approach<strong>in</strong>g au<strong>to</strong>mobile, or theau<strong>to</strong>mobile they hear may be mov<strong>in</strong>g away from ratherthan <strong>to</strong>ward us. Two groups of eyewitnesses <strong>to</strong> an accidentmay disagree as <strong>to</strong> which driver was at fault.Hence, we need <strong>to</strong> consider some additional ways <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> reliable knowledge.EXPERT OPINIONPerhaps there are particular <strong>in</strong>dividuals we shouldconsult—experts <strong>in</strong> their field, people who know agreat deal about what we are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out.We are likely <strong>to</strong> believe a noted heart specialist, for example,if he says that Uncle Charlie has a bad heart.Surely, a person with a PhD <strong>in</strong> economics knows morethan most of us do about what makes the economytick. And shouldn’t we believe our family dentist if shetells us that back molar has <strong>to</strong> be pulled? To use exper<strong>to</strong>p<strong>in</strong>ion as a means of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, perhapsthe physical education teacher <strong>in</strong> Tulsa should ask anoted authority <strong>in</strong> the physical education field whetherability <strong>in</strong> one sport correlates with ability <strong>in</strong> another.


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 5Well, maybe. It depends on the credentials of the experts<strong>and</strong> the nature of the question about which they are be<strong>in</strong>gconsulted. Experts, like all of us, can be mistaken. For alltheir study <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, what experts know is still basedprimarily on what they have learned from read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, from listen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g others, <strong>and</strong> fromtheir own experience. No expert, however, has studied orexperienced all there is <strong>to</strong> know <strong>in</strong> a given field, <strong>and</strong> thuseven an expert can never be <strong>to</strong>tally sure.All any expert c<strong>and</strong>o is give us an op<strong>in</strong>ion based on what he or she knows, <strong>and</strong>no matter how much this is, it is never all there is <strong>to</strong> know.Let us consider, then, another way of know<strong>in</strong>g: logic.LOGICWe also know th<strong>in</strong>gs logically. Our <strong>in</strong>tellect—our capability<strong>to</strong> reason th<strong>in</strong>gs out—allows us <strong>to</strong> use sensorydata <strong>to</strong> develop a new k<strong>in</strong>d of knowledge. Consider thefamous syllogism:All human be<strong>in</strong>gs are mortal.Sally is a human be<strong>in</strong>g.Therefore, Sally is mortal.To assert the first statement (called the majorpremise), we need only generalize from our experienceabout the mortality of <strong>in</strong>dividuals. We have never experiencedanyone who was not mortal, so we state that allhuman be<strong>in</strong>gs are. The second statement (called them<strong>in</strong>or premise) is based entirely on sensory experience.We come <strong>in</strong> contact with Sally <strong>and</strong> classify her as ahuman be<strong>in</strong>g. We don’t have <strong>to</strong> rely on our senses, then,<strong>to</strong> know that the third statement (called the conclusion)must be true. Logic tells us it is. As long as the first twostatements are true, the third statement must be true.Take the case of the counselor <strong>in</strong> Philadelphia who isasked <strong>to</strong> advise a student on how <strong>to</strong> improve her studyhabits. Us<strong>in</strong>g logic, she might present the follow<strong>in</strong>g argument:Students who take notes on a regular basis <strong>in</strong> classf<strong>in</strong>d that their grades improve. If you will take notes on aregular basis, then your grades should improve as well.This is not all there is <strong>to</strong> logical reason<strong>in</strong>g, of course,but it is enough <strong>to</strong> give you an idea of another way ofknow<strong>in</strong>g. There is a fundamental danger <strong>in</strong> logical reason<strong>in</strong>g,however: It is only when the major <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>orpremises of a syllogism are both true that the conclusionis guaranteed <strong>to</strong> be true. If either of the premises is false,the conclusion may or may not be true.**In the note-tak<strong>in</strong>g example, the major premise (all students whotake notes on a regular basis <strong>in</strong> class improve their grades) is probablynot true.There is still another way of know<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> consider: themethod of science.THE SCIENTIFIC METHODWhen many people hear the word science, they th<strong>in</strong>k ofth<strong>in</strong>gs like white lab coats, labora<strong>to</strong>ries, test tubes, orspace exploration. Scientists are people who know a lot,<strong>and</strong> the term science suggests a tremendous body ofknowledge. What we are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> here, however, isscience as a method of know<strong>in</strong>g. It is the scientificmethod that is important <strong>to</strong> researchers.What is this method? Essentially it <strong>in</strong>volves test<strong>in</strong>gideas <strong>in</strong> the public arena. Almost all of us humans are capableof mak<strong>in</strong>g connections—of see<strong>in</strong>g relationships<strong>and</strong> associations—among the sensory <strong>in</strong>formation weexperience. Most of us then identify these connections as“facts”—items of knowledge about the world <strong>in</strong> whichwe live. We may speculate, for example, that our studentsmay be less attentive <strong>in</strong> class when we lecture than whenwe engage them <strong>in</strong> discussion. A physician may guessthat people who sleep between six <strong>and</strong> eight hours eachnight will be less anxious than those who sleep more orless than that amount. A counselor may feel that studentsread less than they used <strong>to</strong> because they spend most oftheir free time watch<strong>in</strong>g television. But <strong>in</strong> each of thesecases, we do not really know if our belief is true. Whatwe are deal<strong>in</strong>g with are only guesses or hunches, or asscientists would say, hypotheses.What we must do now is put each of these guesses orhunches <strong>to</strong> a rigorous test <strong>to</strong> see if it holds up undermore controlled conditions. To <strong>in</strong>vestigate our speculationon attentiveness scientifically, we can observe carefully<strong>and</strong> systematically how attentive our students arewhen we lecture <strong>and</strong> when we hold a class discussion.The physician can count the number of hours <strong>in</strong>dividualssleep, then measure <strong>and</strong> compare their anxietylevels. The counselor can compare the read<strong>in</strong>g habits ofstudents who watch different amounts of television.Such <strong>in</strong>vestigations, however, do not constitutescience unless they are made public. This means that allaspects of the <strong>in</strong>vestigation are described <strong>in</strong> sufficientdetail so that the study can be repeated by anyone whoquestions the results—provided, of course, that those<strong>in</strong>terested possess the necessary competence <strong>and</strong>resources. Private procedures, speculations, <strong>and</strong> conclusionsare not scientific until they are made public.There is noth<strong>in</strong>g very mysterious, then, about howscientists work <strong>in</strong> their quest for reliable knowledge. Inreality, many of us proceed this way when we try <strong>to</strong>


6 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ereach an <strong>in</strong>telligent decision about a problem that isbother<strong>in</strong>g us. These procedures can be boiled down <strong>to</strong>five dist<strong>in</strong>ct steps.1. First, there is a problem of some sort—some disturbance<strong>in</strong> our lives that disrupts the normal or desirablestate of affairs. Someth<strong>in</strong>g is bother<strong>in</strong>g us. For most ofus who are not scientists, it may be a tension of somesort, a disruption <strong>in</strong> our normal rout<strong>in</strong>e. Exampleswould be if our students are not as attentive as we wishor if we have difficulty mak<strong>in</strong>g friends. To the professionalscientist, it may be an unexpla<strong>in</strong>ed discrepancy<strong>in</strong> one’s field of knowledge, a gap <strong>to</strong> be closed. Or itcould be that we want <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the practice ofhuman sacrifice <strong>in</strong> terms of its his<strong>to</strong>rical significance.2. Second, steps are taken <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e more precisely theproblem or the questions <strong>to</strong> be answered, <strong>to</strong> becomeclearer about exactly what the purpose of the study is.For example, we must th<strong>in</strong>k through what we mean bystudent attentiveness <strong>and</strong> why we consider it <strong>in</strong>sufficient;the scientist must clarify what is meant by humansacrifice (e.g., how does it differ from murder?).3. Third, we attempt <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e what k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formationwould solve the problem. Generally speak<strong>in</strong>g,there are two possibilities: study what is alreadyknown or carry out a piece of research. As you willsee, the first is a prerequisite for the second; the secondis a major focus of this text. In preparation, wemust be familiar with a wide range of possibilitiesfor obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, so as <strong>to</strong> get firsth<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formationon the problem. For example, the teachermight consider giv<strong>in</strong>g a questionnaire <strong>to</strong> students orhav<strong>in</strong>g someone observe dur<strong>in</strong>g class. The scientistmight decide <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e his<strong>to</strong>rical accounts orspend time <strong>in</strong> societies where the practice of humansacrifice exists (or has until recently). Spell<strong>in</strong>g outthe details of <strong>in</strong>formation gather<strong>in</strong>g is a major aspec<strong>to</strong>f plann<strong>in</strong>g a research study.4. Fourth, we must decide, as far as it is possible, howwe will organize the <strong>in</strong>formation that we obta<strong>in</strong>. Itis not uncommon, <strong>in</strong> both daily life <strong>and</strong> research, <strong>to</strong>discover that we cannot make sense of all the <strong>in</strong>formationwe possess (sometimes referred <strong>to</strong> as <strong>in</strong>formationoverload). Anyone attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>another society while liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> it has probably experiencedthis phenomenon. Our scientist will surely encounterthis problem, but so will our teacher unlessshe has figured out how <strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>le the questionnaire<strong>and</strong>/or observational <strong>in</strong>formation that is obta<strong>in</strong>ed.5. Fifth, after the <strong>in</strong>formation has been collected <strong>and</strong>analyzed, it must be <strong>in</strong>terpreted. While this step mayseem straightforward at first, this is seldom the case.As you will see, one of the most important parts ofresearch is <strong>to</strong> avoid kidd<strong>in</strong>g ourselves. The teachermay conclude that her students are <strong>in</strong>attentive becausethey dislike lectures, but she may be mis<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>formation. The scientist may concludethat human sacrifice is or was a means of try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>control nature, but this also may be <strong>in</strong>correct.In many studies, there are several possible explanationsfor a problem or phenomenon. These are called hypotheses<strong>and</strong> may occur at any stage of an <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Some researchersstate a hypothesis (e.g., “Students are less attentivedur<strong>in</strong>g lectures than dur<strong>in</strong>g discussions”) right at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of a study. In other cases, hypotheses emerge asa study progresses, sometimes even when the <strong>in</strong>formationthat has been collected is be<strong>in</strong>g analyzed <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpreted.The scientist might f<strong>in</strong>d that <strong>in</strong>stances of sacrifice seemed<strong>to</strong> be more common after such societies made contact withother cultures, suggest<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis such as: “Sacrificeis more likely when traditional practices are threatened.”We want <strong>to</strong> stress two crucial features of scientificresearch: freedom of thought <strong>and</strong> public procedures. Atevery step, it is crucial that the researcher be as open ashumanly possible <strong>to</strong> alternative ways of focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>clarify<strong>in</strong>g the problem, collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results. Further, the processmust be as public as possible. It is not a private game <strong>to</strong>be played by a group of <strong>in</strong>siders. The value of scientificresearch is that it can be replicated (i.e., repeated) byanyone <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g so.*The general order of the scientific method, then, is asfollows:Identify<strong>in</strong>g a problem or questionClarify<strong>in</strong>g the problemDeterm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formation needed <strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> itOrganiz<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formationInterpret<strong>in</strong>g the resultsIn short, the essence of all research orig<strong>in</strong>ates <strong>in</strong>curiosity—a desire <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how <strong>and</strong> why th<strong>in</strong>gs happen,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g why people do the th<strong>in</strong>gs they do, as wellas whether or not certa<strong>in</strong> ways of do<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs work betterthan others.*This is not <strong>to</strong> imply that replicat<strong>in</strong>g a study is a simple matter. Itmay require resources <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g—<strong>and</strong> it may be impossible <strong>to</strong>repeat any study <strong>in</strong> exactly the same way it was done orig<strong>in</strong>ally. Theimportant pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, however, is that public evidence (as opposed <strong>to</strong>private experience) is the criterion for belief.


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 7A common misperception of science fosters the ideathat there are fixed, once-<strong>and</strong>-for-all answers <strong>to</strong> particularquestions. This contributes <strong>to</strong> a common, but unfortunate,tendency <strong>to</strong> accept, <strong>and</strong> rigidly adhere <strong>to</strong>, oversimplifiedsolutions <strong>to</strong> very complex problems. Whilecerta<strong>in</strong>ty is appeal<strong>in</strong>g, it is contradic<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> a fundamentalpremise of science: All conclusions are <strong>to</strong> be viewedas tentative <strong>and</strong> subject <strong>to</strong> change, should new ideas <strong>and</strong>new evidence warrant such. It is particularly importantfor educational researchers <strong>to</strong> keep this <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, s<strong>in</strong>cethe dem<strong>and</strong> for f<strong>in</strong>al answers from parents, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,teachers, <strong>and</strong> politicians can often be <strong>in</strong>tense. Anexample of how science changes is shown <strong>in</strong> the MoreAbout <strong>Research</strong> box on page 8.For many years, there has been a strong tendency <strong>in</strong>Western culture <strong>to</strong> value scientific <strong>in</strong>formation over allother k<strong>in</strong>ds. In recent years, the limitations of this viewhave become <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly recognized <strong>and</strong> discussed. Ineducation, we would argue that other ways of know<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> the scientific, should at least be considered.As we have seen, there are many ways <strong>to</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>formation about the world around us. Figure 1.1 onpage 10 illustrates some of these ways of know<strong>in</strong>g.Types of <strong>Research</strong>All of us engage <strong>in</strong> actions that have some of the characteristicsof formal research, although perhaps we do notrealize this at the time. We try out new methods of teach<strong>in</strong>g,new materials, new textbooks. We compare what wedid this year with what we did last year. Teachersfrequently ask students <strong>and</strong> colleagues their op<strong>in</strong>ionsabout school <strong>and</strong> classroom activities. Counselors <strong>in</strong>terviewstudents, faculty, <strong>and</strong> parents about school activities.Adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs hold regular meet<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> gauge howfaculty members feel about various issues. Schoolboards query adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs query teachers,teachers query students <strong>and</strong> each other.We observe, we analyze, we question, we hypothesize,we evaluate. But rarely do we do these th<strong>in</strong>gssystematically. Rarely do we observe under controlledconditions. Rarely are our <strong>in</strong>struments as accurate <strong>and</strong> reliableas they might be. Rarely do we use the variety ofresearch techniques <strong>and</strong> methodologies at our disposal.The term research can mean any sort of “careful, systematic,patient study <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong> some field ofknowledge. 1 Basic research is concerned with clarify<strong>in</strong>gunderly<strong>in</strong>g processes, with the hypothesis usuallyexpressed as a theory. <strong>Research</strong>ers engaged <strong>in</strong> basicresearch studies are not particularly <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe effectiveness of specific educational practices.An example of basic research might be an attempt <strong>to</strong> ref<strong>in</strong>eone or more stages of Erickson’s psychological theoryof development. Applied research, on the otherh<strong>and</strong>, is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the effectiveness ofparticular educational practices. <strong>Research</strong>ers engaged<strong>in</strong> applied research studies may or may not want <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe degree <strong>to</strong> which certa<strong>in</strong> theories are useful<strong>in</strong> practical sett<strong>in</strong>gs. An example might be an attempt bya researcher <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out whether a particular theory ofhow children learn <strong>to</strong> read can be applied <strong>to</strong> first graderswho are non-readers. Many studies comb<strong>in</strong>e the twotypes of research. An example would be a study thatexam<strong>in</strong>es the effects of particular teacher behaviors onstudents while also test<strong>in</strong>g a theory of personality.Many methodologies fit with<strong>in</strong> the framework ofresearch. If we learn how <strong>to</strong> use more of these methodologieswhere they are appropriate <strong>and</strong> if we can becomemore knowledgeable <strong>in</strong> our research efforts, we canobta<strong>in</strong> more reliable <strong>in</strong>formation upon which <strong>to</strong> base oureducational decisions. Let us look, therefore, at some ofthe research methodologies we might use. We shallreturn <strong>to</strong> each of them <strong>in</strong> greater detail <strong>in</strong> Parts 4 <strong>and</strong> 5.EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCHExperimental research is the most conclusive of scientificmethods. Because the researcher actually establishesdifferent treatments <strong>and</strong> then studies their effects,results from this type of research are likely <strong>to</strong> lead <strong>to</strong> themost clear-cut <strong>in</strong>terpretations.Suppose a his<strong>to</strong>ry teacher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gquestion: <strong>How</strong> can I most effectively teach importantconcepts (such as democracy or colonialism) <strong>to</strong> mystudents? The teacher might compare the effectivenessof two or more methods of <strong>in</strong>struction (usually calledthe <strong>in</strong>dependent variable) <strong>in</strong> promot<strong>in</strong>g the learn<strong>in</strong>g ofhis<strong>to</strong>rical concepts. After systematically assign<strong>in</strong>g students<strong>to</strong> contrast<strong>in</strong>g forms of his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction (such as<strong>in</strong>quiry versus programmed units), the teacher couldcompare the effects of these contrast<strong>in</strong>g methods bytest<strong>in</strong>g students’ conceptual knowledge. Student learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> each group could be assessed by an objective tes<strong>to</strong>r some other measur<strong>in</strong>g device. If the average scoreson the test (usually called the dependent variable) differed,they would give some idea of the effectiveness ofthe various methods. A simple graph could be plotted <strong>to</strong>show the results, as illustrated <strong>in</strong> Figure 1.2 on page 12.In the simplest sort of experiment, two contrast<strong>in</strong>gmethods are compared <strong>and</strong> an attempt is made <strong>to</strong> control


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHChaos TheoryThe orig<strong>in</strong>s of what is now known as chaos theory are usuallytraced <strong>to</strong> the 1970s. S<strong>in</strong>ce then, it has come <strong>to</strong> occupya prom<strong>in</strong>ent place <strong>in</strong> mathematics <strong>and</strong> the natural sciences<strong>and</strong>, <strong>to</strong> a lesser extent, <strong>in</strong> the social sciences.Although the physical sciences have primarily been knownfor their basic laws, or “first pr<strong>in</strong>ciples,” it has long beenknown by scientists that most of these laws hold precisely onlyunder ideal conditions that are not found <strong>in</strong> the “real” world.Many phenomena, such as cloud formations, waterfall patterns,<strong>and</strong> even the weather, elude precise prediction. Chaostheorists argue that the natural laws that are so useful <strong>in</strong> sciencemay, <strong>in</strong> themselves, be the exception rather than the rule.Although precise prediction of such phenomena as thesw<strong>in</strong>g of a pendulum or what the weather will be at a particulartime is <strong>in</strong> most cases impossible, repeated patterns, accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> a major pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of chaos theory, can be discovered <strong>and</strong>used, even when the content of the phenomena is chaotic. Developments<strong>in</strong> computer technology, for example, have made itpossible <strong>to</strong> translate an extremely long sequence of “datapo<strong>in</strong>ts,” such as the test scores of a large group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals,<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> colored visual pictures of fasc<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g complexity <strong>and</strong>beauty. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, these pictures show dist<strong>in</strong>ct patterns thatare often quite similar across different content areas, such asphysics, biology, economics, astronomy, <strong>and</strong> geography. Evenmore surpris<strong>in</strong>g is the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that certa<strong>in</strong> patterns recur asthese pictures are enlarged. The most famous example is the“M<strong>and</strong>lebrot Bug,” shown <strong>in</strong> Pho<strong>to</strong>graphs 1.1 <strong>and</strong> 1.2. Notethat Pho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.2 is simply a magnification of a portion ofPho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.1. The t<strong>in</strong>y box <strong>in</strong> the lower left corner of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph1.1 is magnified <strong>to</strong> produce the box <strong>in</strong> the upper left-h<strong>and</strong>corner of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph 1.2. The t<strong>in</strong>y box with<strong>in</strong> this box is then,<strong>in</strong> turn, magnified <strong>to</strong> produce the larger portion of Pho<strong>to</strong>graph1.2, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the reappearance of the “bug” <strong>in</strong> the lower rightcorner. The conclusion is that even with highly complex data(th<strong>in</strong>k of try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> predict the changes that might occur <strong>in</strong> acloud formation), predictability exists if patterns can be foundacross time or when the scale of a phenomenon is <strong>in</strong>creased.IMPLICATIONS FOR EDUCATIONAL RESEARCHWe hope that this brief <strong>in</strong>troduction has not only stimulatedyour <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> what has been called, by some, the thirdrevolution <strong>in</strong> science dur<strong>in</strong>g the twentieth century (the theoryof relativity <strong>and</strong> the discovery of quantum mechanics be<strong>in</strong>gthe first two), but that it helps <strong>to</strong> make sense out of what weview as some implications for educational research. What arethese implications?*If chaos theory is correct, the difficulty <strong>in</strong> discover<strong>in</strong>gwidely generalizable rules or laws <strong>in</strong> education, let alone thesocial sciences <strong>in</strong> general, may not be due <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>adequate concepts<strong>and</strong> theories or <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>sufficiently precise measurement<strong>and</strong> methodology, but may simply be an unavoidable factabout the world. Another implication is that whatever “laws”we do discover may be seriously limited <strong>in</strong> their applicability—across geography, across <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong>/or group differences,<strong>and</strong> across time. If this is so, chaos theory provides support forresearchers <strong>to</strong> concentrate on study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>pics at the locallevel—classroom, school, agency—<strong>and</strong> for repeated studiesover time <strong>to</strong> see if such laws hold up.Another implication is that educa<strong>to</strong>rs should pay more attention<strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tensive study of the exceptional or the unusual,rather than treat<strong>in</strong>g such <strong>in</strong>stances as trivial, <strong>in</strong>cidental, or “errors.”Yet another implication is that researchers should focuson predictability on a larger scale—that is, look<strong>in</strong>g for patterns<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups over larger units of time. Thiswould suggest a greater emphasis on long-term studies ratherthan the easier-<strong>to</strong>-conduct (<strong>and</strong> cheaper) short-time <strong>in</strong>vestigationsthat are currently the norm.Not surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, chaos theory has its critics. In education,the criticism is not of the theory itself, but more with mis<strong>in</strong>terpretations<strong>and</strong>/or misapplications of it.† Chaos theorists donot say that all is chaos; quite the contrary, they say that wemust pay more attention <strong>to</strong> chaotic phenomena <strong>and</strong> revise ourconceptions of predictability. At the same time, the laws ofgravity still hold, as, with less certa<strong>in</strong>ty, do many generalizations<strong>in</strong> education.*For more extensive implications <strong>in</strong> the field of psychology, seeM. P. Duke (1994). Chaos theory <strong>and</strong> psychology: Seven propositions.Genetic, Social <strong>and</strong> General Psychology Monographs,120: 267–286.†See W. Hunter, J. Benson, <strong>and</strong> D. Garth (1997). Arrows <strong>in</strong> time:The misapplication of chaos theory <strong>to</strong> education. Journal ofCurriculum Studies, 29: 87–100.8


Pho<strong>to</strong>graphs 1.1 <strong>and</strong> 1.2 The M<strong>and</strong>lebrot BugSource: He<strong>in</strong>z-Ot<strong>to</strong> Peitgen <strong>and</strong> Peter H. Richter (1986). The beauty of fractals. Berl<strong>in</strong>: Spr<strong>in</strong>ger-Verlag.9


10 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSens<strong>in</strong>g“Boy, that’s hot!”Shar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation with othersBe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ld someth<strong>in</strong>g byan expertLogical reason<strong>in</strong>gIf the cat is <strong>in</strong> the basket<strong>and</strong> the basket is <strong>in</strong> thebox, the cat thereforehas <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> the box.BoxScience“I’ve concludedthat Mr. Johnson hasLyman disease.”“On whatbasis?”“Based onwhat his x-rays <strong>and</strong>his blood sample reveal,both of which can becorroborated byanyone.”Figure 1.1 Ways of Know<strong>in</strong>gfor all other (extraneous) variables—such as studentability level, age, grade level, time, materials, <strong>and</strong>teacher characteristics—that might affect the outcomeunder <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Methods of such control could <strong>in</strong>cludehold<strong>in</strong>g the classes dur<strong>in</strong>g the same or closely relatedperiods of time, us<strong>in</strong>g the same materials <strong>in</strong> bothgroups, compar<strong>in</strong>g students of the same age <strong>and</strong> gradelevel, <strong>and</strong> so on.Of course, we want <strong>to</strong> have as much control as possibleover the assignment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> the varioustreatment groups, <strong>to</strong> ensure that the groups are similar.But <strong>in</strong> most schools, systematic assignment of students <strong>to</strong>treatment groups is difficult, if not impossible, <strong>to</strong> achieve.Nevertheless, useful comparisons are still possible. Youmight wish <strong>to</strong> compare the effect of different teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods (lectures versus discussion, for example) on


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 11Test score (dependent variable)y-axisLectureDiscussionMethod of <strong>in</strong>struction (<strong>in</strong>dependent variable)x-axisFigure 1.2 Example of Results of Experimental<strong>Research</strong>: Effect of Method of Instruction onHis<strong>to</strong>ry Test Scores aa Many of the examples of data presented throughout this text, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g thatshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 1.2, are hypothetical. When actual data are shown, the sourceis <strong>in</strong>dicated.student achievement or attitudes <strong>in</strong> two or more <strong>in</strong>tacthis<strong>to</strong>ry classes <strong>in</strong> the same school. If a difference existsbetween the classes <strong>in</strong> terms of what is be<strong>in</strong>g measured,this result can suggest how the two methods compare,even though the exact causes of the difference would besomewhat <strong>in</strong> doubt. We discuss this type of experimentalresearch <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13.Another form of experimental research, s<strong>in</strong>glesubjectresearch, <strong>in</strong>volves the <strong>in</strong>tensive study of a s<strong>in</strong>gle<strong>in</strong>dividual (or sometimes a s<strong>in</strong>gle group) over time.These designs are particularly appropriate when study<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividuals with special characteristics by means ofdirect observation. We discuss this type of research <strong>in</strong>Chapter 14.CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHAnother type of research is done <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e relationshipsamong two or more variables <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> exploretheir implications for cause <strong>and</strong> effect; this is calledcorrelational research. This type of research can helpus make more <strong>in</strong>telligent predictions.For <strong>in</strong>stance, could a math teacher predict whichsorts of <strong>in</strong>dividuals are likely <strong>to</strong> have trouble learn<strong>in</strong>gthe subject matter of algebra? If we could make fairlyaccurate predictions <strong>in</strong> this regard, then perhaps wecould suggest some corrective measures for teachers <strong>to</strong>use <strong>to</strong> help such <strong>in</strong>dividuals so that large numbers of“algebra-haters” are not produced.<strong>How</strong> do we do this? First, we need <strong>to</strong> collect variousk<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formation on students that we th<strong>in</strong>k are related<strong>to</strong> their achievement <strong>in</strong> algebra. Such <strong>in</strong>formationmight <strong>in</strong>clude their performance on a number of taskslogically related <strong>to</strong> the learn<strong>in</strong>g of algebra (such as computationalskills, ability <strong>to</strong> solve word problems, <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of math concepts), their verbal abilities,their study habits, aspects of their backgrounds, theirearly experiences with math courses <strong>and</strong> math teachers,the number <strong>and</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of math courses they’ve taken,<strong>and</strong> anyth<strong>in</strong>g else that might conceivably po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> howthose students who do well <strong>in</strong> math differ from thosewho do poorly.We then exam<strong>in</strong>e the data <strong>to</strong> see if any relationshipsexist between some or all of these characteristics <strong>and</strong>subsequent success <strong>in</strong> algebra. Perhaps those whoperform better <strong>in</strong> algebra have better computationalskills or higher self-esteem or receive more attentionfrom the teacher. Such <strong>in</strong>formation can help us predictmore accurately the likelihood of learn<strong>in</strong>g difficultiesfor certa<strong>in</strong> types of students <strong>in</strong> algebra courses. It mayeven suggest some specific ways <strong>to</strong> help students learnbetter.In short, correlational research seeks <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe extent <strong>to</strong> which one or more relationships of sometype exist. The approach requires no manipulation or<strong>in</strong>tervention on the part of the researcher other thanadm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>strument(s) necessary <strong>to</strong> collectthe data desired. In general, one would undertakethis type of research <strong>to</strong> look for <strong>and</strong> describe relationshipsthat may exist among naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>gphenomena, without try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> any way <strong>to</strong> alter thesephenomena. We talk more about correlational research<strong>in</strong> Chapter 15.CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE RESEARCHAnother type of research is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e thecause for or the consequences of differences betweengroups of people; this is called causal-comparativeresearch. Suppose a teacher wants <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whetherstudents from s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent families do more poorly <strong>in</strong>her course than students from two-parent families. To <strong>in</strong>vestigatethis question experimentally, the teacher wouldsystematically select two groups of students <strong>and</strong> then assigneach <strong>to</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle- or two-parent family—which isclearly impossible (not <strong>to</strong> mention unethical!).


12 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTo test this question us<strong>in</strong>g a causal-comparative design,the teacher might compare two groups of studentswho already belong <strong>to</strong> one or the other type of family <strong>to</strong>see if they differ <strong>in</strong> their achievement. Suppose thegroups do differ. Can the teacher def<strong>in</strong>itely concludethat the difference <strong>in</strong> family situation produced the difference<strong>in</strong> achievement? Alas, no. The teacher can concludethat a difference does exist but cannot say for surewhat caused the difference.Interpretations of causal-comparative research arelimited, therefore, because the researcher cannot say conclusivelywhether a particular fac<strong>to</strong>r is a cause or a resul<strong>to</strong>f the behavior(s) observed. In the example presentedhere, the teacher cannot be certa<strong>in</strong> whether (1) any perceiveddifference <strong>in</strong> achievement between the two groupsis due <strong>to</strong> the difference <strong>in</strong> home situation, (2) the parentstatus is due <strong>to</strong> the difference <strong>in</strong> achievement between thetwo groups (although this seems unlikely), or (3) someunidentified fac<strong>to</strong>r is at work. Nevertheless, despite problemsof <strong>in</strong>terpretation, causal-comparative studies are ofvalue <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g possible causes of observed variations<strong>in</strong> the behavior patterns of students. In this respect,they are very similar <strong>to</strong> correlational studies. We discusscausal-comparative research <strong>in</strong> Chapter 16.SURVEY RESEARCHAnother type of research obta<strong>in</strong>s data <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>especific characteristics of a group. This is called surveyresearch. Take the case of a high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal whowants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how his faculty feels about his adm<strong>in</strong>istrativepolicies. What do they like about his policies?What do they dislike? Why? Which policies do they likethe best or least?These sorts of questions can best be answeredthrough a variety of survey techniques that measure facultyattitudes <strong>to</strong>ward the policies of the adm<strong>in</strong>istration.A descriptive survey <strong>in</strong>volves ask<strong>in</strong>g the same set ofquestions (often prepared <strong>in</strong> the form of a written questionnaireor ability test) of a large number of <strong>in</strong>dividualseither by mail, by telephone, or <strong>in</strong> person. When answers<strong>to</strong> a set of questions are solicited <strong>in</strong> person, the researchis called an <strong>in</strong>terview. Responses are then tabulated<strong>and</strong> reported, usually <strong>in</strong> the form of frequencies orpercentages of those who answer <strong>in</strong> a particular way <strong>to</strong>each of the questions.The difficulties <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> survey research arema<strong>in</strong>ly threefold: (1) ensur<strong>in</strong>g that the questions areclear <strong>and</strong> not mislead<strong>in</strong>g, (2) gett<strong>in</strong>g respondents <strong>to</strong> answerquestions thoughtfully <strong>and</strong> honestly, <strong>and</strong> (3) gett<strong>in</strong>ga sufficient number of the questionnaires completed <strong>and</strong>returned <strong>to</strong> enable mak<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>gful analyses. The bigadvantage of survey research is that it has the potential <strong>to</strong>provide us with a lot of <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from quitea large sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.If more details about particular survey questions aredesired, the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal (or someone else) can conductpersonal <strong>in</strong>terviews with faculty. The advantages of an<strong>in</strong>terview (over a questionnaire) are that open-endedquestions (those requir<strong>in</strong>g a response of some length)can be used with greater confidence, particular questionsof special <strong>in</strong>terest or value can be pursued <strong>in</strong>depth, follow-up questions can be asked, <strong>and</strong> items thatare unclear can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed. We discuss survey research<strong>in</strong> Chapter 17.ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHIn all the examples presented so far, the questions be<strong>in</strong>gasked <strong>in</strong>volve how well, how much, or how efficientlyknowledge, attitudes, or op<strong>in</strong>ions <strong>and</strong> the like exist orare be<strong>in</strong>g developed. Sometimes, however, researchersmay wish <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a more complete picture of the educationalprocess than answers <strong>to</strong> the above questionsprovide. When they do, some form of qualitative researchis called for. Qualitative research differs from theprevious (quantitative) methodologies <strong>in</strong> both its methods<strong>and</strong> its underly<strong>in</strong>g philosophy. In Chapter 18, wediscuss these differences, along with recent efforts <strong>to</strong>reconcile the two approaches.Consider the subject of physical education. Just howdo physical education teachers teach their subject?What k<strong>in</strong>ds of th<strong>in</strong>gs do they do as they go about theirdaily rout<strong>in</strong>e? What sorts of th<strong>in</strong>gs do students do? Inwhat k<strong>in</strong>ds of activities do they engage? What explicit<strong>and</strong> implicit rules of games <strong>in</strong> PE classes seem <strong>to</strong> help orh<strong>in</strong>der the process of learn<strong>in</strong>g?To ga<strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> such concerns, an ethnographicstudy can be conducted. The emphasis <strong>in</strong> thistype of research is on document<strong>in</strong>g or portray<strong>in</strong>g theeveryday experiences of <strong>in</strong>dividuals by observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>and</strong> relevant others. An elementaryclassroom, for example, might be observed on as regulara basis as possible, <strong>and</strong> the students <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>in</strong>volvedmight be <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong> an attempt <strong>to</strong> describe,as fully <strong>and</strong> as richly as possible, what goes on <strong>in</strong> thatclassroom. Descriptions (a better word might be portrayals)might depict the social atmosphere of the classroom;the <strong>in</strong>tellectual <strong>and</strong> emotional experiences of students;the manner <strong>in</strong> which the teacher acts <strong>to</strong>ward <strong>and</strong>


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 13reacts <strong>to</strong> students of different ethnicities, sexes, or abilities;how the “rules” of the class are learned, modified,<strong>and</strong> enforced; the k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions asked by theteacher <strong>and</strong> students; <strong>and</strong> so forth. The data could <strong>in</strong>cludedetailed prose descriptions by students of classroomactivities, audiotapes of teacher-student conferences,videotapes of classroom discussions, examplesof teacher lesson plans <strong>and</strong> student work, sociogramsdepict<strong>in</strong>g “power” relationships <strong>in</strong> the classroom, <strong>and</strong>flowcharts illustrat<strong>in</strong>g the direction <strong>and</strong> frequency ofcerta<strong>in</strong> types of comments (for example, the k<strong>in</strong>ds ofquestions asked by teacher <strong>and</strong> students of one another<strong>and</strong> the responses that different k<strong>in</strong>ds produce).In addition <strong>to</strong> ethnographic research, qualitativeresearch <strong>in</strong>cludes his<strong>to</strong>rical research (see Chapter 22)<strong>and</strong> several other, less commonly used approaches.Casey, 2 for example, has identified 18 types of “narrative”methods. In Chapter 18, we discuss four of themost dist<strong>in</strong>ctive of these. These <strong>in</strong>clude biography,where the researcher focuses on important experiences<strong>in</strong> the life of an <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teracts with the person<strong>to</strong> clarify mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations (e.g., a study ofthe career of a high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal). In phenomenology,the researcher focuses on a particular phenomenon(such as school board conflict), collects data through <strong>in</strong>depth<strong>in</strong>terviews with participants, <strong>and</strong> then identifieswhat is common <strong>to</strong> their perceptions. A third approachis the case study, <strong>in</strong> which a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual, group, orimportant example is studied extensively <strong>and</strong> varieddata are collected <strong>and</strong> used <strong>to</strong> formulate <strong>in</strong>terpretationsapplicable <strong>to</strong> the specific case (e.g., a particular schoolboard) or <strong>to</strong> provide useful generalizations. Lastly,grounded theory emphasizes cont<strong>in</strong>ual <strong>in</strong>terplay betweenraw data <strong>and</strong> the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations thatemerge from the data. Its central purpose is <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ductivelydevelop a theory from data (e.g., a study ofteacher morale <strong>in</strong> a particular school beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with <strong>in</strong>terviews<strong>and</strong> other types of data).HISTORICAL RESEARCHYou are probably already familiar with his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch. In this type of research, some aspect of thepast is studied, either by perus<strong>in</strong>g documents of theperiod or by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals who lived dur<strong>in</strong>gthe time. The researcher then attempts <strong>to</strong> reconstruct asaccurately as possible what happened dur<strong>in</strong>g that time<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why it did.For example, a curriculum coord<strong>in</strong>a<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a largeurban school district might want <strong>to</strong> know what sorts ofarguments have been made <strong>in</strong> the past as <strong>to</strong> what shouldbe <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the social studies curriculum for gradesK–12. She could read what various social studies <strong>and</strong>other curriculum theorists have written on the <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>and</strong>then compare their positions. The major problems <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch are mak<strong>in</strong>g sure that the documents or <strong>in</strong>dividualsreally did come from (or live dur<strong>in</strong>g) the periodunder study <strong>and</strong>, once this is established, ascerta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g thatwhat the documents or <strong>in</strong>dividuals say is true. We discusshis<strong>to</strong>rical research <strong>in</strong> more detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 22.ACTION RESEARCHAction research differs from all the preced<strong>in</strong>g methodologies<strong>in</strong> two fundamental ways. The first is that generalization<strong>to</strong> other persons, sett<strong>in</strong>gs, or situations is ofm<strong>in</strong>imal importance. Instead of search<strong>in</strong>g for powerfulgeneralizations, action researchers (often teachers orother education professionals, rather than professionalresearchers) focus on gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation that will enablethem <strong>to</strong> change conditions <strong>in</strong> a particular situation<strong>in</strong> which they are personally <strong>in</strong>volved. Examples would<strong>in</strong>clude improv<strong>in</strong>g the read<strong>in</strong>g capabilities of students <strong>in</strong>a specific classroom, reduc<strong>in</strong>g tensions between ethnicgroups <strong>in</strong> the lunchroom at a particular middle school, oridentify<strong>in</strong>g better ways <strong>to</strong> serve special education students<strong>in</strong> a specified school district. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, any ofthe methodologies discussed earlier may be appropriate.The second difference <strong>in</strong>volves the attention paid <strong>to</strong>the active <strong>in</strong>volvement of the subjects <strong>in</strong> a study (i.e.,those on whom data is collected), as well as those likely<strong>to</strong> be affected by the study’s outcomes. Commonly usedterms <strong>in</strong> action research, therefore, are participants orstakeholders, reflect<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>tent <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>volve them directly<strong>in</strong> the research process as part of “the researchteam.” The extent of participation varies from just help<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> select <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>and</strong>/or collect data <strong>to</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> formulate the research purpose <strong>and</strong> question <strong>to</strong> actuallyparticipat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> all aspects of the research <strong>in</strong>vestigationfrom start <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ish. We discuss action research <strong>in</strong>some detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 24.ALL HAVE VALUEIt must be stressed that each of the research methodologiesdescribed so briefly above has value for us <strong>in</strong>education. Each constitutes a different way of <strong>in</strong>quir<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the realities that exist with<strong>in</strong> our classrooms <strong>and</strong>schools <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the m<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>and</strong> emotions of teachers,counselors, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, parents, <strong>and</strong> students. Each


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHShould Some <strong>Research</strong> MethodsBe Preferred over Others?Recently, several researchers* have expressed their concernthat the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong> is show<strong>in</strong>gfavoritism <strong>to</strong>ward the narrow view that experimental research*D. C. Berl<strong>in</strong>er (2002). <strong>Education</strong>al research: The hardest scienceof all. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 18–20; F. E. Erickson <strong>and</strong>K. Gutierrez (2002). Culture, rigor, <strong>and</strong> science <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 21–24.is, if not the only, at least the most respectable form of research<strong>and</strong> the only one worthy of be<strong>in</strong>g called scientific. Sucha preference has implications for both the fund<strong>in</strong>g of schoolprograms <strong>and</strong> educational research. As one writer commented,“<strong>How</strong> scared should we be when the federal governmentendorses a particular view of science <strong>and</strong> rejects others?”††E. A. St. Pierre (2002). Science rejects postmodernism. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 31 (8): 25.represents a different <strong>to</strong>ol for try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> whatgoes on, <strong>and</strong> what works, <strong>in</strong> schools. It is <strong>in</strong>appropriate<strong>to</strong> consider any one or two of these approaches as superior<strong>to</strong> any of the others. The effectiveness of a particularmethodology depends <strong>in</strong> large part on the nature ofthe research question one wants <strong>to</strong> ask <strong>and</strong> the specificcontext with<strong>in</strong> which the particular <strong>in</strong>vestigation is <strong>to</strong>take place. We need <strong>to</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> what goes on <strong>in</strong>education from as many perspectives as possible, <strong>and</strong>hence we need <strong>to</strong> construe research <strong>in</strong> broad rather thannarrow terms.As far as we are concerned, research <strong>in</strong> educationshould ask a variety of questions, move <strong>in</strong> a variety ofdirections, encompass a variety of methodologies, <strong>and</strong>use a variety of <strong>to</strong>ols. Different research orientations,perspectives, <strong>and</strong> goals should be not only allowed butencouraged. The <strong>in</strong>tent of this book is <strong>to</strong> help you learnhow <strong>and</strong> when <strong>to</strong> use several of these methodologies.General <strong>Research</strong> TypesIt is useful <strong>to</strong> consider the various research methodologieswe have described as fall<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> one or moregeneral research categories: descriptive, associational,or <strong>in</strong>tervention-type studies.Descriptive Studies. Descriptive studies describea given state of affairs as fully <strong>and</strong> carefully as possible.One of the best examples of descriptive research is found<strong>in</strong> botany <strong>and</strong> zoology, where each variety of plant <strong>and</strong>animal species is meticulously described <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formationis organized <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> useful taxonomic categories.In educational research, the most common descriptivemethodology is the survey, as when researcherssummarize the characteristics (abilities, preferences,behaviors, <strong>and</strong> so on) of <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups or (sometimes)physical environments (such as schools). Qualitativeapproaches, such as ethnographic <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalmethodologies are also primarily descriptive <strong>in</strong> nature.Examples of descriptive studies <strong>in</strong> education <strong>in</strong>cludeidentify<strong>in</strong>g the achievements of various groups of students;describ<strong>in</strong>g the behaviors of teachers, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,or counselors; describ<strong>in</strong>g the attitudes of parents;<strong>and</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g the physical capabilities of schools. Thedescription of phenomena is the start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for all researchendeavors.Descriptive research <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, however, is notvery satisfy<strong>in</strong>g, s<strong>in</strong>ce most researchers want <strong>to</strong> have amore complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of people <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs. Thisrequires a more detailed analysis of the various aspects ofphenomena <strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>terrelationships. Advances <strong>in</strong> biology,for example, have come about, <strong>in</strong> large part, as a resul<strong>to</strong>f the categorization of descriptions <strong>and</strong> the subsequentdeterm<strong>in</strong>ation of relationships among these categories.Associational <strong>Research</strong>. <strong>Education</strong>al researchersalso want <strong>to</strong> do more than simply describe situations orevents. They want <strong>to</strong> know how (or if), for example,differences <strong>in</strong> achievement are related <strong>to</strong> such th<strong>in</strong>gsas teacher behavior, student diet, student <strong>in</strong>terests, orparental attitudes. By <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g such possible relationships,researchers are able <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> phenomenamore completely. Furthermore, the identification of relationshipsenables one <strong>to</strong> make predictions. If researchersknow that student <strong>in</strong>terest is related <strong>to</strong> achievement, forexample, they can predict that students who are more<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> a subject will demonstrate higher achievement<strong>in</strong> that subject than students who are less <strong>in</strong>terested.<strong>Research</strong> that <strong>in</strong>vestigates relationships is often referred14


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 15<strong>to</strong> as associational research. Correlational <strong>and</strong> causalcomparativemethodologies are the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal examplesof associational research. Other examples <strong>in</strong>cludestudy<strong>in</strong>g relationships (1) between achievement <strong>and</strong>attitude, between childhood experiences <strong>and</strong> adultcharacteristics, or between teacher characteristics <strong>and</strong>student achievement—all of which are correlationalstudies—<strong>and</strong> (2) between methods of <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>and</strong>achievement (compar<strong>in</strong>g students who have been taughtby each method) or between gender <strong>and</strong> attitude(compar<strong>in</strong>g attitudes of males <strong>and</strong> females)—both ofwhich are causal-comparative studies.As useful as associational studies are, they <strong>to</strong>o areultimately unsatisfy<strong>in</strong>g because they do not permitresearchers <strong>to</strong> “do someth<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence or changeoutcomes. Simply determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g that student <strong>in</strong>terestis predictive of achievement does not tell us how <strong>to</strong>change or improve either <strong>in</strong>terest or achievement,although it does suggest that <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest would<strong>in</strong>crease achievement. To f<strong>in</strong>d out whether one th<strong>in</strong>gwill have an effect on someth<strong>in</strong>g else, researchers need<strong>to</strong> conduct some form of <strong>in</strong>tervention study.Intervention Studies. In <strong>in</strong>tervention studies, aparticular method or treatment is expected <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluenceone or more outcomes. Such studies enable researchers<strong>to</strong> assess, for example, the effectiveness of various teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods, curriculum models, classroom arrangements,<strong>and</strong> other efforts <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence the characteristicsof <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups. Intervention studies can alsocontribute <strong>to</strong> general knowledge by confirm<strong>in</strong>g (or fail<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> confirm) theoretical predictions (for <strong>in</strong>stance, thatabstract concepts can be taught <strong>to</strong> young children). Theprimary methodology used <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention research isthe experiment.Some types of educational research may comb<strong>in</strong>ethese three general approaches. Although his<strong>to</strong>rical,ethnographic, <strong>and</strong> other qualitative research methodologiesare primarily descriptive <strong>in</strong> nature, at times they maybe associational if the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r exam<strong>in</strong>es relationships.A descriptive his<strong>to</strong>rical study of college entrancerequirements over time that exam<strong>in</strong>es the relationship betweenthose requirements <strong>and</strong> achievement <strong>in</strong> mathematicsis also associational. An ethnographic study that describes<strong>in</strong> detail the daily activities of an <strong>in</strong>ner-city highschool <strong>and</strong> also f<strong>in</strong>ds a relationship between media attention<strong>and</strong> teacher morale <strong>in</strong> the school is both descriptive<strong>and</strong> associational. An <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the effects ofdifferent teach<strong>in</strong>g methods on concept learn<strong>in</strong>g that alsoreports the relationship between concept learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>gender is an example of a study that is both an <strong>in</strong>tervention<strong>and</strong> an associational-type study.QUANTITATIVE ANDQUALITATIVE RESEARCHAnother dist<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>in</strong>volves the difference betweenquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research. Although weshall discuss the basic differences between these twotypes of research more fully <strong>in</strong> Chapter 18, we will providea brief overview here. In the simplest sense, quantitativedata deal primarily with numbers, whereas qualitativedata primarily <strong>in</strong>volve words. But this is <strong>to</strong>osimple <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>o brief. Quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methodsdiffer <strong>in</strong> their assumptions about the purpose ofresearch itself, methods utilized by researchers, k<strong>in</strong>ds ofstudies undertaken, the role of the researcher, <strong>and</strong> thedegree <strong>to</strong> which generalization is possible.Quantitative researchers usually base their work onthe belief that facts <strong>and</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs can be separated, thatthe world is a s<strong>in</strong>gle reality made up of facts that can bediscovered. Qualitative researchers, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,assume that the world is made up of multiple realities,socially constructed by different <strong>in</strong>dividual views of thesame situation.When it comes <strong>to</strong> the purpose of research, quantitativeresearchers seek <strong>to</strong> establish relationships betweenvariables <strong>and</strong> look for <strong>and</strong> sometimes expla<strong>in</strong> the causesof such relationships. Qualitative researchers, on theother h<strong>and</strong>, are more concerned with underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gsituations <strong>and</strong> events from the viewpo<strong>in</strong>t of the participants.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, the participants often tend <strong>to</strong> bedirectly <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the research process itself.Quantitative research has established widely agreedongeneral formulations of steps that guide researchers<strong>in</strong> their work. Quantitative research designs tend <strong>to</strong> bepreestablished. Qualitative researchers have a muchgreater flexibility <strong>in</strong> both the strategies <strong>and</strong> techniquesthey use <strong>and</strong> the overall research process itself. Their designstend <strong>to</strong> emerge dur<strong>in</strong>g the course of the research.The ideal researcher role <strong>in</strong> quantitative researchis that of a detached observer, whereas qualitative researcherstend <strong>to</strong> become immersed <strong>in</strong> the situations<strong>in</strong> which they do their research. The pro<strong>to</strong>typical study<strong>in</strong> the quantitative tradition is the experiment; for qualitativeresearchers, it is an ethnography.Lastly, most quantitative researchers want <strong>to</strong> establishgeneralizations that transcend the immediate situationor particular sett<strong>in</strong>g. Qualitative researchers, on theother h<strong>and</strong>, often do not even try <strong>to</strong> generalize beyond


16 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ethe particular situation, but may leave it <strong>to</strong> the reader <strong>to</strong>assess applicability. When they do generalize, their generalizationsare usually very limited <strong>in</strong> scope.Many of the dist<strong>in</strong>ctions just described, of course,are not absolute. Sometimes researchers will use bothqualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches <strong>in</strong> the samestudy. This k<strong>in</strong>d of research is referred <strong>to</strong> as mixedmethodsresearch. Its advantage is that by us<strong>in</strong>g multiplemethods, researchers are better able <strong>to</strong> gather <strong>and</strong> analyzeconsiderably more <strong>and</strong> different k<strong>in</strong>ds of data thanthey would be able <strong>to</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g just one approach. Mixedmethodsstudies can emphasize one approach over theother or give each approach roughly equal weight.Consider an example. It is often common <strong>in</strong> surveys<strong>to</strong> use closed-ended questions that lend themselves <strong>to</strong>quantitative analysis (such as through the calculation ofpercentages of different types of responses), but alsoopen-ended questions that permit qualitative analysis(such as follow<strong>in</strong>g up a response that <strong>in</strong>terviewees give<strong>to</strong> a particular question with further questions by theresearcher <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> encourage them <strong>to</strong> elaborate <strong>and</strong>expla<strong>in</strong> their th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g).Studies <strong>in</strong> which researchers use both quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative methods are becom<strong>in</strong>g more common,as we will see <strong>in</strong> Chapter 23.META-ANALYSISMeta-analysis is an attempt <strong>to</strong> reduce the limitations of<strong>in</strong>dividual studies by try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> locate all of the studieson a particular <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>and</strong> then us<strong>in</strong>g statistical means <strong>to</strong>synthesize the results of these studies. In Chapter 5, wediscuss meta-analysis <strong>in</strong> more detail. In subsequentchapters, we exam<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> detail the limitations that arelikely <strong>to</strong> be found <strong>in</strong> various types of research. Someapply <strong>to</strong> all types, while others are more likely <strong>to</strong> apply<strong>to</strong> particular types.Critical Analysis of <strong>Research</strong>There are some who feel that researchers who engage <strong>in</strong>the k<strong>in</strong>ds of research we have just described take a bit<strong>to</strong>o much for granted—<strong>in</strong>deed, that they make a numberof unwarranted (<strong>and</strong> usually unstated) assumptionsabout the nature of the world <strong>in</strong> which we live. Thesecritics (usually referred <strong>to</strong> as critical researchers) raisea number of philosophical, l<strong>in</strong>guistic, ethical, <strong>and</strong> politicalquestions not only about educational research as itis usually conducted but also about all fields of <strong>in</strong>quiry,rang<strong>in</strong>g from the physical sciences <strong>to</strong> literature.In an <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry text, we cannot hope <strong>to</strong> do justice<strong>to</strong> the many arguments <strong>and</strong> concerns these critics haveraised over the years. What we can do is provide an<strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> some of the major questions they haverepeatedly asked.The first issue is the question of reality: As any beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gstudent of philosophy is well aware, there is noway <strong>to</strong> demonstrate whether anyth<strong>in</strong>g “really exists.”There is, for example, no way <strong>to</strong> prove conclusively <strong>to</strong>others that I am look<strong>in</strong>g at what I call a pencil (e.g., othersmay not be able <strong>to</strong> see it; they may not be able <strong>to</strong> tellwhere I am look<strong>in</strong>g; I may be dream<strong>in</strong>g). Further, it iseasily demonstrated that different <strong>in</strong>dividuals maydescribe the same <strong>in</strong>dividual, action, or event quitedifferently—lead<strong>in</strong>g some critics <strong>to</strong> the conclusionthat there is no such th<strong>in</strong>g as reality, only <strong>in</strong>dividual(<strong>and</strong> different) perceptions of it. One implication of thisview is that any search for knowledge about the “real”world is doomed <strong>to</strong> failure.We would acknowledge that what the critics say iscorrect: We cannot, once <strong>and</strong> for all, “prove” anyth<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> there is no deny<strong>in</strong>g that perceptions differ. Wewould argue, however, that our commonsense notion ofreality (that what most knowledgeable persons agreeexists is what is real) has enabled humank<strong>in</strong>d <strong>to</strong> solvemany problems—even the question of how <strong>to</strong> put a manon the moon.The second issue is the question of communication.Let us assume that we can agree that some th<strong>in</strong>gs are“real.” Even so, the critics argue that it is virtually impossible<strong>to</strong> show that we use the same terms <strong>to</strong> identifythese th<strong>in</strong>gs. For example, it is well known thatthe Inuit have many different words (<strong>and</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs)for the English word snow. To put it differently, nomatter how carefully we def<strong>in</strong>e even a simple termsuch as shoe, the possibility always rema<strong>in</strong>s that oneperson’s shoe is not another’s. (Is a slipper a shoe? Isa shower clog a shoe?) If so much of language isimprecise, how then can relationships or laws—whichtry <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate how various terms, th<strong>in</strong>gs, or ideas areconnected—be precise?Aga<strong>in</strong>, we would agree. People often do not agree onthe mean<strong>in</strong>g of a word or phrase. We would argue, however(as we th<strong>in</strong>k would most researchers), that we c<strong>and</strong>ef<strong>in</strong>e terms clearly enough <strong>to</strong> enable different people<strong>to</strong> agree sufficiently about what words mean that theycan communicate <strong>and</strong> thus get on with the acquisition ofuseful knowledge.The third issue is the question of values. His<strong>to</strong>rically,scientists have often claimed <strong>to</strong> be value-free, that is


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 17“You all did wellon the test. You musthave studied hard!”?“I copiedfromAngela.”“I’m a goodguesser.”“I read theright pages <strong>in</strong>the book.”“I knew thisstuff fromlast year.”Figure 1.3 Is the Teacher’s Assumption Correct?“objective,” <strong>in</strong> their conduct of research. Critics haveargued, however, that what is studied <strong>in</strong> the socialsciences, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>and</strong> questions with whicheducational researchers are concerned, is never objectivebut rather socially constructed. Such th<strong>in</strong>gs asteacher-student <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> classrooms, the performanceof students on exam<strong>in</strong>ations, the questions teachersask, <strong>and</strong> a host of other issues <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>pics of concern<strong>to</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs do not exist <strong>in</strong> a vacuum. They are <strong>in</strong>fluencedby the society <strong>and</strong> times <strong>in</strong> which people live. Asa result, such <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>and</strong> concerns, as well as how theyare def<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>in</strong>evitably reflect the values of that society.Further, even <strong>in</strong> the physical sciences, the choice ofproblems <strong>to</strong> study <strong>and</strong> the means of do<strong>in</strong>g so reflect thevalues of the researchers <strong>in</strong>volved.Here, <strong>to</strong>o, we would agree. We th<strong>in</strong>k that mostresearchers <strong>in</strong> education would acknowledge the validityof the critics’ position. Many critical researcherscharge, however, that such agreement is not sufficientlyreflected <strong>in</strong> research reports. They say that many researchersfail <strong>to</strong> admit or identify “where they are com<strong>in</strong>gfrom,” especially <strong>in</strong> their discussions of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof their research.The fourth issue is the question of unstated assumptions.An assumption is anyth<strong>in</strong>g that is taken forgranted rather than tested or checked. Although thisissue is similar <strong>to</strong> the previous issue, it is not limited <strong>to</strong>values but applies <strong>to</strong> both general <strong>and</strong> specific assumptionsthat researchers make with regard <strong>to</strong> a particularstudy. Some assumptions are so generally acceptedthat they are taken for granted by practically all socialresearchers (e.g., the sun will come out; the earth willcont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> rotate). Other assumptions are more questionable.An example given by Krathwohl 3 clarifies this.He po<strong>in</strong>ts out that if researchers change the assumptionsunder which they operate, this may lead <strong>to</strong> differentconsequences. If we assume, for example, that mentallylimited students learn <strong>in</strong> the same way as other studentsbut more slowly, then it follows that given sufficienttime <strong>and</strong> motivation, they can achieve as well as otherstudents. The consequences of this view are <strong>to</strong> givethese <strong>in</strong>dividuals more time, <strong>to</strong> place them <strong>in</strong> classeswhere the competition is less <strong>in</strong>tense, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> motivatethem <strong>to</strong> achieve. If, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, we assume thatthey use different conceptual structures <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> which theyfit what they learn, this assumption leads <strong>to</strong> a searchfor simplified conceptual structures they can learn thatwill result <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g that approximates that of otherstudents. Frequently authors do not make such assumptionsclear.In many studies, researchers implicitly assume thatthe terms they use are clear, that their samples areappropriate, <strong>and</strong> that their measurements are accurate.<strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>g a good study can be seen as try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> reducethese k<strong>in</strong>ds of assumptions <strong>to</strong> a m<strong>in</strong>imum. Readersshould always be given enough <strong>in</strong>formation so that theydo not have <strong>to</strong> make such assumptions. Figure 1.3 illustrateshow an assumption can often be <strong>in</strong>correct.The fifth issue is the question of societal consequences.Critical theorists argue that traditional researchefforts (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those <strong>in</strong> education) predom<strong>in</strong>antlyserve political <strong>in</strong>terests that are, at best, conservative or,


18 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eat worst, oppressive. They po<strong>in</strong>t out that such researchis almost always focused on improv<strong>in</strong>g exist<strong>in</strong>g practicesrather than rais<strong>in</strong>g questions about the practicesthemselves. They argue that, <strong>in</strong>tentional or not, theefforts of most educational researchers have servedessentially <strong>to</strong> re<strong>in</strong>force the status quo. A more extremeposition alleges that educational <strong>in</strong>stitutions (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gresearch), rather than enlighten<strong>in</strong>g the citizenry, haveserved <strong>in</strong>stead <strong>to</strong> prepare them <strong>to</strong> be uncritical functionaries<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dustrialized society.We would agree with this general criticism but notethat there have been a number of <strong>in</strong>vestigations ofthe status quo itself, followed by suggestions for improvement,that have been conducted <strong>and</strong> offered byresearchers of a variety of political persuasions.Let us exam<strong>in</strong>e each of these issues <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> ahypothetical example. Suppose a researcher decides <strong>to</strong>study the effectiveness of a course <strong>in</strong> formal logic <strong>in</strong> improv<strong>in</strong>gthe ability of high school students <strong>to</strong> analyze arguments<strong>and</strong> arrive at defensible conclusions from data.The researcher accord<strong>in</strong>gly designs a study that is soundenough <strong>in</strong> terms of design <strong>to</strong> provide at least a partial answeras <strong>to</strong> the effectiveness of the course. Let us addressthe five issues presented above <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> this study.1. The question of reality: The abilities <strong>in</strong> question (analyz<strong>in</strong>garguments <strong>and</strong> reach<strong>in</strong>g accurate conclusions)clearly are abstractions. They have no physical realityper se. But does this mean that such abilities do not“exist” <strong>in</strong> any way whatsoever? Are they noth<strong>in</strong>gmore than artificial by-products of our conceptuallanguage system? Clearly, this is not the case. Suchabilities do <strong>in</strong>deed exist <strong>in</strong> a somewhat limited sense,as when we talk about the “ability” of a person <strong>to</strong> dowell on tests. But is test performance <strong>in</strong>dicative ofhow well a student can perform <strong>in</strong> real life? If it is not,is the performance of students on such tests important?A critic might allege that the ability <strong>to</strong> analyze,for example, is situation-specific: Some people aregood analyzers on tests; others, <strong>in</strong> public forums; others,of written materials; <strong>and</strong> so forth. If this is so, thenthe concept of a general ability <strong>to</strong> “analyze arguments”would be an illusion. We th<strong>in</strong>k a good argumentcan be made that this is not the case—based oncommonsense experience <strong>and</strong> on some research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.We must admit, however, that the critic has apo<strong>in</strong>t (we don’t know for sure how general this abilityis), <strong>and</strong> one that should not be overlooked.2. The question of communication: Assum<strong>in</strong>g thatthese abilities do exist, can we def<strong>in</strong>e them wellenough so that mean<strong>in</strong>gful communication can result?We th<strong>in</strong>k so, but it is true that even the cleares<strong>to</strong>f def<strong>in</strong>itions does not always guarantee mean<strong>in</strong>gfulcommunication. This is often revealed when we discoverthat the way we use a term differs from howsomeone else uses the same term, despite previousagreement on a def<strong>in</strong>ition. We may agree, for example,that a “defensible conclusion” is one that doesnot contradict the data <strong>and</strong> that follows logicallyfrom the data, yet still f<strong>in</strong>d ourselves disagree<strong>in</strong>g as<strong>to</strong> whether or not a particular conclusion is a defensibleone. Debates among scientists often boil down<strong>to</strong> differences as <strong>to</strong> what constitutes a defensibleconclusion from data.3. The question of values: <strong>Research</strong>ers who decide <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>vestigate outcomes such as the ones <strong>in</strong> this studymake the assumption that the outcomes are eitherdesirable (<strong>and</strong> thus <strong>to</strong> be enhanced) or undesirable(<strong>and</strong> thus <strong>to</strong> be dim<strong>in</strong>ished), <strong>and</strong> they usually po<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut why this is so. Seldom, however, are the values(of the researchers) that led <strong>to</strong> the study of a particularoutcome discussed. Are these outcomes studiedbecause they are considered of highest priority?because they are traditional? socially acceptable?easier <strong>to</strong> study? f<strong>in</strong>ancially reward<strong>in</strong>g?The researcher’s decision <strong>to</strong> study whether acourse <strong>in</strong> logic will affect the ability of students <strong>to</strong>analyze arguments reflects his or her values. Both theoutcomes <strong>and</strong> the method studied reflect Eurocentricideas of value; the Aris<strong>to</strong>telian notion of the “rationalman” (or woman) is not dom<strong>in</strong>ant <strong>in</strong> all cultures.Might some not claim, <strong>in</strong> fact, that we need people <strong>in</strong>our society who will question basic assumptionsmore than we need people who can argue well fromthese assumptions? While researchers probably cannotbe expected <strong>to</strong> discuss such complex issues <strong>in</strong>every study, these critics render a service by urg<strong>in</strong>gall of us <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> research <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about how ourvalues may affect our research endeavors.4. The question of unstated assumptions: In carry<strong>in</strong>g outsuch a study, the researcher is assum<strong>in</strong>g not only thatthe outcome is desirable but that the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of thestudy will have some <strong>in</strong>fluence on educationalpractice. Otherwise, the study is noth<strong>in</strong>g more than anacademic exercise. <strong>Education</strong>al methods research hasbeen often criticized for lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> suggested practicesthat, for various reasons, are unlikely <strong>to</strong> be implemented.While we believe that such studies should stillbe done, researchers have an obligation <strong>to</strong> make suchassumptions clear <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> discuss their reasonableness.


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 195. The question of societal consequences: F<strong>in</strong>ally, letus consider the societal implications of a study suchas this. Critics might allege that this study, whileperhaps defensible as a scientific endeavor, willhave a negative overall impact. <strong>How</strong> so? First byfoster<strong>in</strong>g the idea that the outcome be<strong>in</strong>g studied(the ability <strong>to</strong> analyze arguments) is more importantthan other outcomes (e.g., the ability <strong>to</strong> see novel orunusual relationships). This allegation has, <strong>in</strong> fact,been made for many years <strong>in</strong> education—that researchershave overemphasized the study of someoutcomes at the expense of others.A second allegation might be that such researchserves <strong>to</strong> perpetuate discrim<strong>in</strong>ation aga<strong>in</strong>st the lessprivileged segments of society. If it is true, as somecontend, that some cultures are more “l<strong>in</strong>ear” <strong>and</strong> othersmore “global,” then a course <strong>in</strong> formal logic(be<strong>in</strong>g primarily l<strong>in</strong>ear) may <strong>in</strong>crease the advantagealready held by students from the dom<strong>in</strong>ant l<strong>in</strong>ear culture.4 It can be argued that a fairer approach wouldteach a variety of argumentative methods, therebycapitaliz<strong>in</strong>g on the strengths of all cultural groups.To summarize, we have attempted <strong>to</strong> present themajor issues raised by an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly vocal part of theresearch community. These issues <strong>in</strong>volve the nature ofreality, the difficulty of communication, the recognitionthat values always affect research, unstated assumptions,<strong>and</strong> the consequences of research for society as awhole. While we do not agree with some of the specificcriticisms raised by these writers, we believe theresearch enterprise is the better for their efforts.A Brief Overviewof the <strong>Research</strong> ProcessRegardless of methodology, all researchers engage <strong>in</strong> anumber of similar activities. Almost all research plans<strong>in</strong>clude, for example, a problem statement, a hypothesis,def<strong>in</strong>itions, a literature review, a sample of subjects,tests or other measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments, a description ofprocedures <strong>to</strong> be followed, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a time schedule,<strong>and</strong> a description of <strong>in</strong>tended data analyses. We dealwith each of these components <strong>in</strong> some detail throughoutthis book, but we want <strong>to</strong> give you a brief overviewof them before we proceed.Figure 1.4 presents a schematic of the research components.The solid-l<strong>in</strong>e arrows <strong>in</strong>dicate the sequence <strong>in</strong>which the components are usually presented <strong>and</strong> described<strong>in</strong> research proposals <strong>and</strong> reports. They also <strong>in</strong>dicatea useful sequence for plann<strong>in</strong>g a study (that is,th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about the research problem, followed by thehypothesis, followed by the def<strong>in</strong>itions, <strong>and</strong> so forth).The broken-l<strong>in</strong>e arrows <strong>in</strong>dicate the most likely departuresfrom this sequence (for example, consideration of<strong>in</strong>strumentation sometimes results <strong>in</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> thesample; clarify<strong>in</strong>g the question may suggest which typeof design is most appropriate). The nonl<strong>in</strong>ear patternis <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> po<strong>in</strong>t out that, <strong>in</strong> practice, the processdoes not necessarily follow a precise sequence. In fact,experienced researchers often consider many of thesecomponents simultaneously as they develop theirresearch plan.<strong>Research</strong>problemHypothesesorquestionsInstrumentationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsProcedures/designSampleLiteraturereviewDataanalysisFigure 1.4 The <strong>Research</strong> Process


20 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eStatement of the research problem: The problemof a study sets the stage for everyth<strong>in</strong>g else. Theproblem statement should be accompanied by adescription of the background of the problem(what fac<strong>to</strong>rs caused it <strong>to</strong> be a problem <strong>in</strong> thefirst place) <strong>and</strong> a rationale or justification forstudy<strong>in</strong>g it. Any legal or ethical ramificationsrelated <strong>to</strong> the problem should be discussed <strong>and</strong>resolved.Formulation of an explora<strong>to</strong>ry question or ahypothesis: <strong>Research</strong> problems are usually statedas questions, <strong>and</strong> often as hypotheses. A hypothesisis a prediction, a statement of what specificresults or outcomes are expected <strong>to</strong> occur. The hypothesesof a study should clearly <strong>in</strong>dicate any relationshipsexpected between the variables (thefac<strong>to</strong>rs, characteristics, or conditions) be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated<strong>and</strong> be so stated that they can be testedwith<strong>in</strong> a reasonable period of time. Not all studiesare hypothesis-test<strong>in</strong>g studies, but many are.Def<strong>in</strong>itions: All key terms <strong>in</strong> the problem statement<strong>and</strong> hypothesis should be def<strong>in</strong>ed as clearly aspossible.Review of the related literature: Other studies related<strong>to</strong> the research problem should be located<strong>and</strong> their results briefly summarized. The literaturereview (of appropriate journals, reports,monographs, etc.) should shed light on what isalready known about the problem <strong>and</strong> should<strong>in</strong>dicate logically why the proposed study wouldresult <strong>in</strong> an extension of this prior knowledge.Sample: The subjects* (the sample) of the study<strong>and</strong> the larger group, or population (<strong>to</strong> whomresults are <strong>to</strong> be generalized), should be clearlyidentified. The sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan (the procedures bywhich the subjects will be selected) should be described.Instrumentation: Each of the measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strumentsthat will be used <strong>to</strong> collect data from thesubjects should be described <strong>in</strong> detail, <strong>and</strong> a rationaleshould be given for its use.Procedures: The actual procedures of the study—what the researcher will do (what, when, where,how, <strong>and</strong> with whom) from beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> end, <strong>in</strong>the order <strong>in</strong> which they will occur—should bespelled out <strong>in</strong> detail (although this is not written <strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>ne). This, of course, is much less feasible <strong>and</strong>appropriate <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study. A realistic timeschedule outl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g when various tasks are <strong>to</strong> bestarted, along with expected completion dates,should also be provided. All materials (e.g., textbooks)<strong>and</strong>/or equipment (e.g., computers) thatwill be used <strong>in</strong> the study should also be described.The general design or methodology (e.g., an experimen<strong>to</strong>r a survey) <strong>to</strong> be used should be stated.In addition, possible sources of bias should beidentified, <strong>and</strong> how they will be controlled shouldbe expla<strong>in</strong>ed.Data analysis: Any statistical techniques, both descriptive<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential, <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>in</strong> the dataanalysis should be described. The comparisons <strong>to</strong>be made <strong>to</strong> answer the research question shouldbe made clear.*The term subjects is offensive <strong>to</strong> some because it can imply thatthose be<strong>in</strong>g studied are deprived of dignity. We use it because weknow of no other term of comparable clarity <strong>in</strong> this context.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 21WHY RESEARCH IS OF VALUEThe scientific method provides an important way <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> accurate <strong>and</strong> reliable<strong>in</strong>formation.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsWAYS OF KNOWINGThere are many ways <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g sensory experience, agreementwith others, expert op<strong>in</strong>ion, logic, <strong>and</strong> the scientific method.The scientific method is considered by researchers the most likely way <strong>to</strong> producereliable <strong>and</strong> accurate knowledge.The scientific method <strong>in</strong>volves answer<strong>in</strong>g questions through systematic <strong>and</strong> publicdata collection <strong>and</strong> analysis.TYPES OF RESEARCHSome of the most commonly used research methodologies <strong>in</strong> education are experimentalresearch, correlational research, causal-comparative research, survey research,ethnographic research, his<strong>to</strong>rical research, <strong>and</strong> action research.Experimental research <strong>in</strong>volves manipulat<strong>in</strong>g conditions <strong>and</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g effects.Correlational research <strong>in</strong>volves study<strong>in</strong>g relationships among variables with<strong>in</strong> as<strong>in</strong>gle group <strong>and</strong> frequently suggests the possibility of cause <strong>and</strong> effect.Causal-comparative research <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>g known groups who have had differentexperiences <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e possible causes or consequences of group membership.Survey research <strong>in</strong>volves describ<strong>in</strong>g the characteristics of a group by means of such<strong>in</strong>struments as <strong>in</strong>terview questions, questionnaires, <strong>and</strong> tests.Ethnographic research concentrates on document<strong>in</strong>g or portray<strong>in</strong>g the everydayexperiences of people, us<strong>in</strong>g observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews.Ethnographic research is one form of qualitative research. Other common forms of qualitativeresearch <strong>in</strong>clude the case study, biography, phenomenology, <strong>and</strong> grounded theory.A case study is a detailed analysis of one or a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals.His<strong>to</strong>rical research <strong>in</strong>volves study<strong>in</strong>g some aspect of the past.Action research is a type of research by practitioners designed <strong>to</strong> help improve theirpractice.Each of the research methodologies described constitutes a different way of <strong>in</strong>quir<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> reality <strong>and</strong> is thus a different <strong>to</strong>ol for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g what goes on <strong>in</strong> education.GENERAL RESEARCH TYPESIndividual research methodologies can be classified <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> general research types.Descriptive studies describe a given state of affairs. Associational studies <strong>in</strong>vestigaterelationships. Intervention studies assess the effects of a treatment or method onoutcomes.Quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research methodologies are based on different assumptions;they also differ on the purpose of research, the methods used by researchers,the k<strong>in</strong>ds of studies undertaken, the researcher’s role, <strong>and</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> which generalizationis possible.Mixed-method research <strong>in</strong>corporates both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative approaches.Meta-analysis attempts <strong>to</strong> synthesize the results of all the <strong>in</strong>dividual studies on agiven <strong>to</strong>pic by statistical means.


22 PART 1 Introduction <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCRITICAL ANALYSIS OF RESEARCHCritical analysis of research raises basic questions about the assumptions <strong>and</strong> implicationsof educational research.THE RESEARCH PROCESSAlmost all research plans <strong>in</strong>clude a problem statement, an explora<strong>to</strong>ry question orhypothesis, def<strong>in</strong>itions, a literature review, a sample of subjects, <strong>in</strong>strumentation,a description of procedures <strong>to</strong> be followed, a time schedule, <strong>and</strong> a description of<strong>in</strong>tended data analyses.Key Termsaction research 13applied research 7descriptive studies 14ethnographic study 12problem statement 20procedure 20associational research 15experimental research 7qualitative research 15assumption 17basic research 7causal-comparativeresearch 11chaos theory 8correlationalresearch 11critical researcher 16data analysis 20his<strong>to</strong>rical research 13hypothesis 20<strong>in</strong>struments 20<strong>in</strong>tervention studies 15literature review 20meta-analysis 16mixed-methodsresearch 16population 20quantitativeresearch 15research 7sample 20scientific method 5s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research 11subject 20survey research 12variables 20For Discussion1. “Speculation, procedures, <strong>and</strong> conclusions are not scientific unless they are madepublic.” Is this true? Discuss.2. Most quantitative researchers believe that the world is a s<strong>in</strong>gle reality, whereas mostqualitative researchers believe that the world is made up of multiple realities. Whichposition would you support? Why?3. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of some other ways of know<strong>in</strong>g besides those mentioned <strong>in</strong> thischapter? What are they? What, if any, are the limitations of these methods?4. “While certa<strong>in</strong>ty is appeal<strong>in</strong>g, it is contradic<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> a fundamental premise ofscience.” What does this mean? Discuss.5. Is there such a th<strong>in</strong>g as private knowledge? If so, can you give an example?6. Many people seem <strong>to</strong> be uneasy about the idea of research, particularly research <strong>in</strong>schools. <strong>How</strong> would you expla<strong>in</strong> this?Notes1. Webster’s new world dictionary of the American language, 2nd ed. (1984). New York: Simon <strong>and</strong> Schuster,p. 1208.2. K. Casey (1995, 1996). The new narrative research <strong>in</strong> education. Review of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>,21: 211–253.3. D. R. Krathwohl (1998). Methods of educational <strong>and</strong> social science research, 2nd ed. New York: Longman,p. 88.4. M. Ramirez <strong>and</strong> A. Casteneda (1974). Cultural democracy, biocognitive development <strong>and</strong> education. NewYork: Academic Press.


CHAPTER 1 The Nature of <strong>Research</strong> 23<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 1: What K<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>Research</strong>?Th<strong>in</strong>k of a research idea or problem you would like <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. Us<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 1,briefly describe the problem <strong>in</strong> a sentence or two. Then <strong>in</strong>dicate the type of researchmethodology you would use <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate this problem.Problem Sheet 1<strong>Research</strong> Method1. A possible <strong>to</strong>pic or problem I am th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of research<strong>in</strong>g is:2. The specific method that seems most appropriate for me <strong>to</strong> use at this time is (circleone, or if plann<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study, circle both types of methods you plan <strong>to</strong>use):a. An experimentb. A survey us<strong>in</strong>g a written questionnairec. A survey us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews of several peopled. A correlational studye. A causal-comparative studyf. An ethnographic studyAn electronic version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t,save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.g. A case studyh. A content analysisi. A his<strong>to</strong>rical studyj. An action research studyk. A mixed-methods study3. What questions (if any) might a critical researcher raise with regard <strong>to</strong> your study?(See the section on “Critical Analysis of <strong>Research</strong>” on pp. 16–19 <strong>in</strong> the text forexamples)


The BasicsP A R Tof <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>2In Part 2, we <strong>in</strong>troduce or exp<strong>and</strong> on many of the basic ideas <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. These <strong>in</strong>clude concepts such as hypotheses, variables, sampl<strong>in</strong>g, measurement,validity, reliability, <strong>and</strong> many others. We also beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> supply you with certa<strong>in</strong>skills that will enhance your ability <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> master the research process.These <strong>in</strong>clude such th<strong>in</strong>gs as how <strong>to</strong> select a research problem, formulate a hypothesis,conduct a literature search, choose a sample, def<strong>in</strong>e words <strong>and</strong> phrases clearly,develop a valid <strong>in</strong>strument, plus many others. Regardless of the methodology aresearcher uses, all of these skills are important <strong>to</strong> master. We also discuss the ethicalimplications <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the conduct of research itself.


2The<strong>Research</strong> ProblemWhat Is a <strong>Research</strong>Problem?<strong>Research</strong> QuestionsCharacteristics of Good<strong>Research</strong> Questions<strong>Research</strong> Questions ShouldBe Feasible<strong>Research</strong> Questions ShouldBe Clear<strong>Research</strong> Questions ShouldBe Significant<strong>Research</strong> Questions OftenInvestigate Relationships“My research question is:‘What’s the absolute best way<strong>to</strong> teach his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> n<strong>in</strong>thgraders?’”“Sorry, Suzy, butthat question, as stated,isn’t researchable.”OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Give some examples of potential researchproblems <strong>in</strong> education.Formulate a research question.Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between researchable <strong>and</strong>nonresearchable questions.Name five characteristics that goodresearch questions possess.Describe three ways <strong>to</strong> clarify unclearresearch questions.Give an example of an operationaldef<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> how suchdef<strong>in</strong>itions differ from other k<strong>in</strong>dsof def<strong>in</strong>itions.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant, <strong>in</strong> research, bythe term “relationship” <strong>and</strong> give anexample of a research question that<strong>in</strong>volves a relationship.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About What Makes a Question <strong>Research</strong>ableAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 2.1: <strong>Research</strong> Questions <strong>and</strong> Related <strong>Design</strong>sActivity 2.2: Chang<strong>in</strong>g General Topics <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong>QuestionsActivity 2.3: Operational Def<strong>in</strong>itionsActivity 2.4: JustificationActivity 2.5: Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsRobert Adams, a high school teacher <strong>in</strong> Omaha, Nebraska, wants <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether the <strong>in</strong>quiry methodwill <strong>in</strong>crease the <strong>in</strong>terest of his eleventh-grade students <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry. Phyllis Gomez, a physical educationteacher <strong>in</strong> an elementary school <strong>in</strong> Phoenix, Arizona, wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how her sixth-grade students feel about thenew exercise program recently m<strong>and</strong>ated by the school district. Tami Mendoza, a counselor <strong>in</strong> a large <strong>in</strong>ner-cityhigh school <strong>in</strong> San Francisco, wonders whether a client-centered approach might help ease the hostility that manyof her students display dur<strong>in</strong>g counsel<strong>in</strong>g sessions. Each of these examples presents a problem that could serve as abasis for research. <strong>Research</strong> problems—the focus of a research <strong>in</strong>vestigation—are what this chapter is about.What Is a <strong>Research</strong> Problem?A research problem is exactly that—a problem thatsomeone would like <strong>to</strong> research. A problem can be anyth<strong>in</strong>gthat a person f<strong>in</strong>ds unsatisfac<strong>to</strong>ry or unsettl<strong>in</strong>g, adifficulty of some sort, a state of affairs that needs <strong>to</strong> bechanged, anyth<strong>in</strong>g that is not work<strong>in</strong>g as well as itmight. Problems <strong>in</strong>volve areas of concern <strong>to</strong> researchers,conditions they want <strong>to</strong> improve, difficultiesthey want <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate, questions for which they seekanswers.<strong>Research</strong> QuestionsUsually a research problem is <strong>in</strong>itially posed as a question,which serves as the focus of the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>vestigation.The follow<strong>in</strong>g examples of possible researchquestions <strong>in</strong> education are not sufficiently developed foractual use <strong>in</strong> a research project but would be suitabledur<strong>in</strong>g the early stage of formulat<strong>in</strong>g a research question.An appropriate methodology (<strong>in</strong> parentheses) is providedfor each question. Although there are other possiblemethodologies that might be used, we consider thosegiven here as particularly suitable.Does client-centered therapy produce more satisfaction<strong>in</strong> clients than traditional therapy? (traditionalexperimental research)Does behavior modification reduce aggression<strong>in</strong> autistic children? (s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experimentalresearch)Are the descriptions of people <strong>in</strong> social studies discussionsbiased? (grounded theory research)What goes on <strong>in</strong> an elementary school classroomdur<strong>in</strong>g an average week? (ethnographic research)Do teachers behave differently <strong>to</strong>ward students ofdifferent genders? (causal-comparative research)<strong>How</strong> can we predict which students might have troublelearn<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of subject matter? (correlationalresearch)<strong>How</strong> do parents feel about the school counsel<strong>in</strong>gprogram? (survey research)<strong>How</strong> can a pr<strong>in</strong>cipal improve faculty morale? (<strong>in</strong>terviewresearch)What all these questions have <strong>in</strong> common is that wecan collect data of some sort <strong>to</strong> answer them (at least <strong>in</strong>part). That’s what makes them researchable. For example,a researcher can measure the satisfaction levels of clientswho receive different methods of therapy. Or researcherscan observe <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> describe the27


28 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 2.1<strong>Research</strong>able VersusNonresearchableQuestionsNoNot researchableYes<strong>Research</strong>ableShould I put my youngster<strong>in</strong> preschool?Do children enrolled <strong>in</strong>preschool develop bettersocial skills than childrennot enrolled?What is the best way <strong>to</strong>learn <strong>to</strong> read?At which age is it better<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduce phonics <strong>to</strong>children—age 5, age 6,or age 7?Are some people born bad?Who commits morecrimes—poor peopleor rich people?function<strong>in</strong>g of an elementary school classroom. To repeat,then, what makes these questions researchable is thatsome sort of <strong>in</strong>formation can be collected <strong>to</strong> answer them.There are other k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions, however, thatcannot be answered by collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g data.Here are two examples:Should philosophy be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the high schoolcurriculum?What is the mean<strong>in</strong>g of life?Why can’t these questions be researched? What aboutthem prevents us from collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> answerthem? The reason is both simple <strong>and</strong> straightforward:There is no way <strong>to</strong> collect <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> answer eitherquestion. Both questions are, <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al analysis, notresearchable.The first question is a question of value—it impliesnotions of right <strong>and</strong> wrong, proper <strong>and</strong> improper—<strong>and</strong>therefore does not have any empirical (or observable)referents. There is no way <strong>to</strong> deal, empirically, with theverb should. <strong>How</strong> can we empirically determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether or not someth<strong>in</strong>g “should” be done? What datacould we collect? There is no way for us <strong>to</strong> proceed.<strong>How</strong>ever, if the question is changed <strong>to</strong> “Do people th<strong>in</strong>kphilosophy should be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the high school curriculum?”it becomes researchable. Why? Because nowwe can collect data <strong>to</strong> help us answer the question.The second question is metaphysical <strong>in</strong> nature—that is,beyond the physical, transcendental. Answers <strong>to</strong> thissort of question lie beyond the accumulation of <strong>in</strong>formation.Here are more ideas for research questions. Whichones (if any) do you th<strong>in</strong>k are researchable?1. Is God good?2. Are children happier when taught by a teacher of thesame gender?3. Does high school achievement <strong>in</strong>fluence the academicachievement of university students?4. What is the best way <strong>to</strong> teach grammar?5. What would schools be like <strong>to</strong>day if World War IIhad not occurred?We hope you identified questions 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 as the two thatare researchable. Questions 1, 4, <strong>and</strong> 5, as stated, cannot beresearched. Question 1 is another metaphysical question<strong>and</strong>, as such, does not lend itself <strong>to</strong> empirical research (wecould ask people if they believe God is good, but that wouldbe another question). Question 4 asks for the “best” way <strong>to</strong>do someth<strong>in</strong>g. Th<strong>in</strong>k about this one for a moment. Is thereany way we can determ<strong>in</strong>e the best way <strong>to</strong> do anyth<strong>in</strong>g? Tobe able <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e this, we must exam<strong>in</strong>e every possiblealternative, <strong>and</strong> a moment’s reflection br<strong>in</strong>gs us <strong>to</strong> the realizationthat this can never be accomplished. <strong>How</strong> would weever be sure that all possible alternatives have been exam<strong>in</strong>ed?Question 5 requires the creation of impossible conditions.We can, of course, <strong>in</strong>vestigate what people th<strong>in</strong>kschools would be like. Figure 2.1 illustrates the differencebetween researchable <strong>and</strong> nonresearchable questions.


CHAPTER 2 The <strong>Research</strong> Problem 29Characteristics of Good<strong>Research</strong> QuestionsOnce a research question has been formulated, researcherswant <strong>to</strong> turn it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> as good a question as possible.Good research questions possess four essentialcharacteristics.1. The question is feasible (i.e., it can be <strong>in</strong>vestigatedwithout expend<strong>in</strong>g an undue amount of time, energy,or money).2. The question is clear (i.e., most people would agreeas <strong>to</strong> what the key words <strong>in</strong> the question mean).3. The question is significant (i.e., it is worth <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gbecause it will contribute important knowledgeabout the human condition).4. The question is ethical (i.e., it will not <strong>in</strong>volve physicalor psychological harm or damage <strong>to</strong> human be<strong>in</strong>gsor <strong>to</strong> the natural or social environment of whichthey are a part). We will discuss the subject of ethics<strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 4.Let us discuss some of these characteristics <strong>in</strong> moredetail.RESEARCH QUESTIONS SHOULD BE FEASIBLEFeasibility is an important issue <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g researchstudies. A feasible question is one that can be <strong>in</strong>vestigatedwith available resources. Some questions (such asthose <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g space exploration, for example, or thestudy of the long-term effects of special programs, suchas Head Start) require a great deal of time <strong>and</strong> money;others require much less. Unfortunately, the field ofeducation, unlike medic<strong>in</strong>e, bus<strong>in</strong>ess, law, agriculture,pharmacology, or the military, has never established anongo<strong>in</strong>g research effort tied closely <strong>to</strong> practice. Most ofthe research that is done <strong>in</strong> schools or other educational<strong>in</strong>stitutions is likely <strong>to</strong> be done by “outsiders”—oftenuniversity professors <strong>and</strong> their students—<strong>and</strong> usually isfunded by temporary grants. Thus, lack of feasibilityoften seriously limits research efforts. Follow<strong>in</strong>g aretwo examples of research questions, one feasible <strong>and</strong>one not so feasible.Feasible: <strong>How</strong> do the students at Oceana HighSchool feel about the new guidance program recently<strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong> the district?Not so feasible: <strong>How</strong> would achievement be affectedby giv<strong>in</strong>g each student his or her own lap<strong>to</strong>p computer<strong>to</strong> use for a semester?RESEARCH QUESTIONS SHOULD BE CLEARBecause the research question is the focus of a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation, it is particularly important that the questionbe clear. What exactly is be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated? Let usconsider two examples of research questions that arenot clear enough.Example 1. “Is a humanistically oriented classroomeffective?” Although the phrase humanistically orientedclassroom may seem quite clear, many people may notbe sure exactly what it means. If we ask, What is a humanisticallyoriented classroom? we beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> discoverthat it is not as easy as we might have thought <strong>to</strong> describeits essential characteristics. What happens <strong>in</strong> suchclassrooms that is different from what happens <strong>in</strong> otherclassrooms? Do teachers use certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of strategies?Do they lecture? In what sorts of activities do studentsparticipate? What do such classrooms look like––howis the seat<strong>in</strong>g arranged, for example? What k<strong>in</strong>ds ofmaterials are used? Is there much variation <strong>to</strong> be foundfrom classroom <strong>to</strong> classroom <strong>in</strong> the strategies employedby the teacher or <strong>in</strong> the sorts of activities <strong>in</strong> whichstudents engage? Do the k<strong>in</strong>ds of materials available<strong>and</strong>/or used vary?Another term <strong>in</strong> this question is also ambiguous.What does the term effective mean? Does it mean “results<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased academic proficiency,” “results <strong>in</strong> happierchildren,” “makes life easier for teachers,” or “costsless money”? Maybe it means all these th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> more.Example 2. “<strong>How</strong> do teachers feel about specialclasses for the educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped?” The firstterm that needs clarification is teachers. What age groupdoes this <strong>in</strong>volve? What level of experience (i.e., are probationaryteachers, for example, <strong>in</strong>cluded)? Are teachers<strong>in</strong> both public <strong>and</strong> private schools <strong>in</strong>cluded? Are teachersthroughout the nation <strong>in</strong>cluded, or only those <strong>in</strong> aspecific locality? Does the term refer <strong>to</strong> teachers who donot teach special classes as well as those who do?The phrase feel about is also ambiguous. Does itmean op<strong>in</strong>ions? emotional reactions? Does it suggestactions? or what? The terms special classes <strong>and</strong> educationallyh<strong>and</strong>icapped also need <strong>to</strong> be clarified. An exampleof a legal def<strong>in</strong>ition of an educationally h<strong>and</strong>icappedstudent is:A m<strong>in</strong>or who, by reason of marked learn<strong>in</strong>g or behavioraldisorders, is unable <strong>to</strong> adapt <strong>to</strong> a normal classroom situation.The disorder must be associated with a neurologicalh<strong>and</strong>icap or an emotional disturbance <strong>and</strong> must not be


30 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7edue <strong>to</strong> mental retardation, cultural deprivation, or foreignlanguage problems.Note that this def<strong>in</strong>ition itself conta<strong>in</strong>s some ambiguouswords, such as marked learn<strong>in</strong>g disorders, whichlend themselves <strong>to</strong> a wide variety of <strong>in</strong>terpretations. Thisis equally true of the term cultural deprivation, which isnot only ambiguous but also often offensive <strong>to</strong> membersof ethnic groups <strong>to</strong> whom it is frequently applied.As we beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about these (or other) questions,it appears that terms which seemed at first glance <strong>to</strong> bewords or phrases that everyone would easily underst<strong>and</strong>are really quite complex <strong>and</strong> far more difficult <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ethan we might orig<strong>in</strong>ally have thought.This is true of many current educational concepts<strong>and</strong> methodologies. Consider such terms as core curriculum,client-centered counsel<strong>in</strong>g, active learn<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> quality management. What do such terms mean? Ifyou were <strong>to</strong> ask a sample of five or six teachers, counselors,or adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, you probably would get severaldifferent def<strong>in</strong>itions. Although such ambiguity isvaluable <strong>in</strong> some circumstances <strong>and</strong> for certa<strong>in</strong> purposes,it represents a problem <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs of a researchquestion. <strong>Research</strong>ers have no choice but <strong>to</strong> bespecific about the terms used <strong>in</strong> a research question, <strong>to</strong>def<strong>in</strong>e precisely what is <strong>to</strong> be studied. In mak<strong>in</strong>g this effort,researchers ga<strong>in</strong> a clearer picture of how <strong>to</strong> proceedwith an <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> fact, sometimes decide <strong>to</strong>change the very nature of the research. <strong>How</strong>, then, mightthe clarity of a research question be improved?Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Terms. There are essentially three ways<strong>to</strong> clarify important terms <strong>in</strong> a research question. Thefirst is <strong>to</strong> use a constitutive def<strong>in</strong>ition—that is, <strong>to</strong> usewhat is often referred <strong>to</strong> as the dictionary approach. <strong>Research</strong>erssimply use other words <strong>to</strong> say more clearlywhat is meant. Thus, the term humanistic classroommight be def<strong>in</strong>ed asA classroom <strong>in</strong> which: (1) the needs <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terests ofstudents have the highest priority; (2) students work ontheir own for a considerable amount of time <strong>in</strong> each classperiod; <strong>and</strong> (3) the teacher acts as a guide <strong>and</strong> a resourceperson rather than an <strong>in</strong>formant.Notice, however, that this def<strong>in</strong>ition is still somewhatunclear, s<strong>in</strong>ce the words be<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> the termhumanistic are themselves ambiguous. What does itmean <strong>to</strong> say that the “needs <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terests of studentshave the highest priority” or that “students work on theirown”? What is a “considerable amount” of each class© The New Yorker Collection 1998 Tom Cheney from car<strong>to</strong>onbank.com.All Rights Reserved.period? What does a teacher do when act<strong>in</strong>g as a “guide”or a “resource person”? Further clarification is needed.Students of communication have demonstrated justhow difficult it is <strong>to</strong> be sure that the message sent is themessage received. It is probably true that no one evercompletely underst<strong>and</strong>s the mean<strong>in</strong>g of terms that areused <strong>to</strong> communicate. That is, we can never be certa<strong>in</strong>that the message we receive is the one the sender <strong>in</strong>tended.Some years ago, one of the leaders <strong>in</strong> our fieldwas said <strong>to</strong> have become so depressed by this idea thathe quit talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> his colleagues for several weeks. Amore constructive approach is simply <strong>to</strong> do the best wecan. We must try <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> our terms <strong>to</strong> others. Whilemost researchers try <strong>to</strong> be clear, there is no question thatsome do a much better job than others.Another important po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> remember is that often itis a compound term or phrase that needs <strong>to</strong> be def<strong>in</strong>edrather than only a s<strong>in</strong>gle word. For example, the termnondirective therapy will surely not be clarified byprecise def<strong>in</strong>itions of nondirective <strong>and</strong> therapy, s<strong>in</strong>ce ithas a more specific mean<strong>in</strong>g than the two words def<strong>in</strong>edseparately would convey. Similarly, such terms aslearn<strong>in</strong>g disability, bil<strong>in</strong>gual education, <strong>in</strong>teractive video,<strong>and</strong> home-centered health care need <strong>to</strong> be def<strong>in</strong>ed asl<strong>in</strong>guistic wholes.Here are three def<strong>in</strong>itions of the term motivated <strong>to</strong>learn. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the clearest?1. Works hard2. Is eager <strong>and</strong> enthusiastic3. Susta<strong>in</strong>s attention <strong>to</strong> a task*As you have seen, the dictionary approach <strong>to</strong> clarify<strong>in</strong>gterms has its limitations. A second possibility is <strong>to</strong>*We judge 3 <strong>to</strong> be the clearest, followed by 1 <strong>and</strong> then 2.


***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSKey Terms <strong>to</strong> Def<strong>in</strong>e<strong>in</strong> a <strong>Research</strong> StudyTerms necessary <strong>to</strong> ensure that the research question issharply focusedTerms that <strong>in</strong>dividuals outside the field of study may notunderst<strong>and</strong>Terms that have multiple mean<strong>in</strong>gsTerms that are essential <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g what the study isaboutTerms <strong>to</strong> provide precision <strong>in</strong> specifications for <strong>in</strong>struments<strong>to</strong> be developed or locatedclarify by example. <strong>Research</strong>ers might th<strong>in</strong>k of a fewhumanistic classrooms with which they are familiar <strong>and</strong>then try <strong>to</strong> describe as fully as possible what happens <strong>in</strong>these classrooms. Usually we suggest that people observesuch classrooms <strong>to</strong> see for themselves how theydiffer from other classrooms. This approach also has itsproblems, however, s<strong>in</strong>ce our descriptions may still notbe as clear <strong>to</strong> others as they would like.Thus, a third method of clarification is <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e importantterms operationally. Operational def<strong>in</strong>itionsrequire that researchers specify the actions or operationsnecessary <strong>to</strong> measure or identify the term. For example,here are two possible operational def<strong>in</strong>itions of the termhumanistic classroom.1. Any classroom identified by specified experts asconstitut<strong>in</strong>g an example of a humanistic classroom2. Any classroom judged (by an observer spend<strong>in</strong>g atleast one day per week for four <strong>to</strong> five weeks) <strong>to</strong>possess all the follow<strong>in</strong>g characteristics:a. No more than three children work<strong>in</strong>g with thesame materials at the same timeb. The teacher never spend<strong>in</strong>g more than 20 m<strong>in</strong>utesper day address<strong>in</strong>g the class as a groupc. At least half of every class period open for students<strong>to</strong> work on projects of their own choos<strong>in</strong>gat their own paced. Several (more than three) sets of different k<strong>in</strong>dsof educational materials available for every student<strong>in</strong> the class <strong>to</strong> usee. Nontraditional seat<strong>in</strong>g—students sit <strong>in</strong> circles,small group<strong>in</strong>gs of seats, or even on the floor <strong>to</strong>work on their projectsf. Frequent (at least two per week) discussions <strong>in</strong>which students are encouraged <strong>to</strong> give their op<strong>in</strong>ions<strong>and</strong> ideas on <strong>to</strong>pics be<strong>in</strong>g read about <strong>in</strong> theirtextbooksThe above list<strong>in</strong>g of characteristics <strong>and</strong> behaviors maybe a quite unsatisfac<strong>to</strong>ry def<strong>in</strong>ition of a humanisticclassroom <strong>to</strong> many people (<strong>and</strong> perhaps <strong>to</strong> you). But it isconsiderably more specific (<strong>and</strong> thus clearer) than thedef<strong>in</strong>ition with which we began.* Armed with this def<strong>in</strong>ition(<strong>and</strong> the necessary facilities), researchers coulddecide quickly whether or not a particular classroomqualified as an example of a humanistic classroom.Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g terms operationally is a helpful way <strong>to</strong> clarifytheir mean<strong>in</strong>g. Operational def<strong>in</strong>itions are useful<strong>to</strong>ols <strong>and</strong> should be mastered by all students of research.Remember that the operations or activities necessary <strong>to</strong>measure or identify the term must be specified. Which ofthe follow<strong>in</strong>g possible def<strong>in</strong>itions of the term motivated<strong>to</strong> learn mathematics do you th<strong>in</strong>k are operational?1. As shown by enthusiasm <strong>in</strong> class2. As judged by the student’s math teacher us<strong>in</strong>g arat<strong>in</strong>g scale she developed3. As measured by the “Math Interest” questionnaire4. As shown by attention <strong>to</strong> math tasks <strong>in</strong> class5. As reflected by achievement <strong>in</strong> mathematics6. As <strong>in</strong>dicated by records show<strong>in</strong>g enrollment <strong>in</strong>mathematics electives7. As shown by effort expended <strong>in</strong> class8. As demonstrated by number of optional assignmentscompleted9. As demonstrated by read<strong>in</strong>g math books outsideclass10. As observed by teacher aides us<strong>in</strong>g the “MathematicsInterest” observation record†*This is not <strong>to</strong> say that this list would not be improved by mak<strong>in</strong>gthe guidel<strong>in</strong>es even more specific. These characteristics, however,do meet the criterion for an operational def<strong>in</strong>ition—they specify theactions researchers need <strong>to</strong> take <strong>to</strong> measure or identify the variablebe<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>ed.†The operational def<strong>in</strong>itions are 2, 3, 6, 8, <strong>and</strong> 10. The nonoperationaldef<strong>in</strong>itions are 1, 4, 5, 7, <strong>and</strong> 9, because the activities oroperations necessary for identify<strong>in</strong>g the behavior have not beenspecified.31


32 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“I want <strong>to</strong>buy an<strong>in</strong>expensivecar.”“Under$10,000.”“Hi”“Up <strong>to</strong>$25,000!”“Susan’s teachersays her workisn’t artistic.”“I disagree!She’s verycreative!”PotentialBuyerCarSalesmanPa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g“He doesget really goodgrades.”“No, he has nocommon senseat all.”“He knows how <strong>to</strong> pleasepeople, that’s for sure.”“Nah. He’sno good <strong>in</strong>woodshop.”“Margaret really likesMrs. Rob<strong>in</strong>son.”“Jamie isreally smart,isn’t he?”“No way! He was theonly one who got lost onour camp<strong>in</strong>g trip.”Figure 2.2 Some Times When Operational Def<strong>in</strong>itions Would Be HelpfulIn addition <strong>to</strong> their value <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g readers underst<strong>and</strong>how researchers actually obta<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>formation theyneed, operational def<strong>in</strong>itions are often helpful <strong>in</strong> clarify<strong>in</strong>gterms. Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about how <strong>to</strong> measure job satisfaction,for example, is likely <strong>to</strong> force a researcher <strong>to</strong> clarify,<strong>in</strong> his or her own m<strong>in</strong>d, what he or she means by the term.(For everyday examples of times when operational def<strong>in</strong>itionsare needed, see Figure 2.2.)Despite their virtues, however, operational def<strong>in</strong>itions<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of themselves are often not illum<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g.Read<strong>in</strong>g that “language proficiency is (operationally)def<strong>in</strong>ed as the student’s score on the TOLD test” is notvery helpful unless the reader is familiar with thisparticular test. Even when this is the case, it is moresatisfac<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>formed of what the researchermeans by the term. For these reasons we believe that anoperational def<strong>in</strong>ition should always be accompaniedby a constitutive one.The importance of researchers be<strong>in</strong>g clear about theterms <strong>in</strong> their research questions cannot be overstated.<strong>Research</strong>ers will have difficulty proceed<strong>in</strong>g with plansfor the collection <strong>and</strong> analysis of data if they do notknow exactly what k<strong>in</strong>d of data <strong>to</strong> look for. And theywill not know what data <strong>to</strong> look for if they are unclearabout the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the key terms <strong>in</strong> the researchquestion.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHThe Importance of a Rationale<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> education, as <strong>in</strong> all of social science, has sometimesbeen criticized as trivial. Some years ago, Sena<strong>to</strong>rWilliam Proxmire ga<strong>in</strong>ed considerable publicity for his“golden fleece” awards, which he bes<strong>to</strong>wed on governmentfundedstudies that he considered particularly worthless ortrivial. Some recipients compla<strong>in</strong>ed of “cheap shots,” argu<strong>in</strong>gthat their research had not received a complete or fair hear<strong>in</strong>g.While it is doubtless true that research is often specialized <strong>in</strong>nature <strong>and</strong> not easily communicated <strong>to</strong> persons outside thefield, we believe more attention should be paid <strong>to</strong>:Avoid<strong>in</strong>g esoteric term<strong>in</strong>ologyDef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g key terms clearly <strong>and</strong>, when feasible, both constitutively<strong>and</strong> operationallyMak<strong>in</strong>g a clear <strong>and</strong> persuasive case for the importance ofa studyRESEARCH QUESTIONSSHOULD BE SIGNIFICANT<strong>Research</strong> questions also should be worth <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g.In essence, we need <strong>to</strong> consider whether gett<strong>in</strong>g the answer<strong>to</strong> a question is worth the time <strong>and</strong> energy (<strong>and</strong>often money). What, we might ask, is the value of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>ga particular question? In what ways will it contribute<strong>to</strong> our knowledge about education? <strong>to</strong> ourknowledge of human be<strong>in</strong>gs? Is such knowledge important<strong>in</strong> some way? If so, how? These questions askresearchers <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about why a research question isworthwhile—that is, important or significant.It probably goes without say<strong>in</strong>g that a research questionis of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the person who asks it. But is <strong>in</strong>terestalone sufficient justification for an <strong>in</strong>vestigation? Forsome people, the answer is a clear yes. They say that anyquestion that someone s<strong>in</strong>cerely wants an answer <strong>to</strong> isworth <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g. Others, however, say that personal<strong>in</strong>terest, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, is an <strong>in</strong>sufficient reason. Toooften, they po<strong>in</strong>t out, personal <strong>in</strong>terest can result <strong>in</strong> thepursuit of trivial or <strong>in</strong>significant questions. Becausemost research efforts require some (<strong>and</strong> often a considerable)expenditure of time, energy, materials, money,<strong>and</strong>/or other resources, it is easy <strong>to</strong> appreciate the po<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>f view that some useful outcome or payoff should resultfrom the research. The <strong>in</strong>vestment of oneself <strong>and</strong> others<strong>in</strong> a research enterprise should contribute some knowledgeof value <strong>to</strong> the field of education.Generally speak<strong>in</strong>g, most researchers do not believethat research efforts based primarily on personal <strong>in</strong>terestalone warrant <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Furthermore, there is somereason <strong>to</strong> question a “purely curious” motive on psychologicalgrounds. Most questions probably have somedegree of hidden motivation beh<strong>in</strong>d them, <strong>and</strong> for thesake of credibility, these reasons should be made explicit.One of the most important tasks for any researcher,therefore, is <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k through the value of the <strong>in</strong>tendedresearch before <strong>to</strong>o much prelim<strong>in</strong>ary work is done.Three important questions should be asked:1. <strong>How</strong> might answers <strong>to</strong> this research question advanceknowledge <strong>in</strong> my field?2. <strong>How</strong> might answers <strong>to</strong> this research question improveeducational practice?3. <strong>How</strong> might answers <strong>to</strong> this research question improvethe human condition?As you th<strong>in</strong>k about possible research questions, askyourself: Why would it be important <strong>to</strong> answer thisquestion? Does the question have implications for theimprovement of practice? for adm<strong>in</strong>istrative decisionmak<strong>in</strong>g? for program plann<strong>in</strong>g? Is there an importantissue that can be illum<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>to</strong> some degree by a studyof this question? Is it related <strong>to</strong> a current theory thatI have doubts about or would like <strong>to</strong> substantiate?Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g through possible answers <strong>to</strong> these questionscan help you judge the significance of a potential researchquestion.In our experience, student justifications for a proposedstudy are likely <strong>to</strong> have two weaknesses. First,they assume <strong>to</strong>o much—for example, that everyonewould agree with them (i.e., it is self-evident) that it isimportant <strong>to</strong> study someth<strong>in</strong>g like self-esteem or ability<strong>to</strong> read. In po<strong>in</strong>t of fact, not everyone does agree thatthese are important <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>to</strong> study; nonetheless, it isstill the researcher’s job <strong>to</strong> make the case that they areimportant rather than merely assum<strong>in</strong>g that they are.Second, students often overstate the implications of astudy. Evidence of the effectiveness of a particularteach<strong>in</strong>g method does not, for example, imply that themethod will be generally adopted or that improvement33


34 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eGroup A: No relationshipMaleFemaleMaleGroup B: RelationshipFemaleDemocrat Republican16Democrat Republican161616 16 16 16Figure 2.3 Illustration of Relationship Between Voter Gender <strong>and</strong> Party Affiliation16<strong>in</strong> student achievement will au<strong>to</strong>matically result. Itwould imply, for example, that more attention should begiven <strong>to</strong> the method <strong>in</strong> teacher-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programs.RESEARCH QUESTIONS OFTEN INVESTIGATERELATIONSHIPSThere is an additional characteristic that good researchquestions often possess. They frequently (but not always)suggest a relationship of some sort <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated. (Wediscuss the reasons for this <strong>in</strong> Chapter 3.) A suggestedrelationship means that two qualities or characteristics aretied <strong>to</strong>gether or connected <strong>in</strong> some way. Are motivation<strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g related? If so, how? What about age <strong>and</strong>attractiveness? speed <strong>and</strong> weight? height <strong>and</strong> strength? apr<strong>in</strong>cipal’s adm<strong>in</strong>istrative policies <strong>and</strong> faculty morale?It is important <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> how the term relationshipis used <strong>in</strong> research, s<strong>in</strong>ce the term has other mean<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> everyday life. When researchers use the termrelationship, they are not referr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the nature or qualityof an association between people, for example. Whatwe <strong>and</strong> other researchers mean is perhaps best clarifiedvisually. Look, for example, at the data for groups A <strong>and</strong>B <strong>in</strong> Figure 2.3. What do you notice?The hypothetical data for group A show that out of a<strong>to</strong>tal of 32 <strong>in</strong>dividuals, 16 are Republicans <strong>and</strong> 16 areDemocrats. It also shows that half are male <strong>and</strong> half arefemale. Group B shows the same breakdown by partyaffiliation <strong>and</strong> gender. What is different between the twogroups is that there is no association or relationshipbetween gender <strong>and</strong> political party <strong>in</strong> group A, whereasthere is a very strong relationship between these two fac<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>in</strong> group B. We can express the relationship <strong>in</strong> groupB by say<strong>in</strong>g that males tend <strong>to</strong> be Republicans whilefemales tend <strong>to</strong> be Democrats. We can also express this relationship<strong>in</strong> terms of a prediction. Should another femalejo<strong>in</strong> group B, we would predict she would be a Democrats<strong>in</strong>ce 14 of the previous 16 females are Democrats.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes, practice withkey terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


CHAPTER 2 The <strong>Research</strong> Problem 35RESEARCH PROBLEMA research problem is the focus of a research <strong>in</strong>vestigation.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsRESEARCH QUESTIONSMany research problems are stated as questions.The essential characteristic of a researchable question is that there be some sort of<strong>in</strong>formation that can be collected <strong>in</strong> an attempt <strong>to</strong> answer the question.CHARACTERISTICS OF GOOD RESEARCH QUESTIONS<strong>Research</strong> questions should be feasible—that is, capable of be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated withavailable resources.<strong>Research</strong> questions should be clear—that is, unambiguous.<strong>Research</strong> questions should be significant—that is, worthy of <strong>in</strong>vestigation.<strong>Research</strong> questions should be ethical—that is, their <strong>in</strong>vestigation should not <strong>in</strong>volvephysical or psychological harm or damage <strong>to</strong> human be<strong>in</strong>gs or <strong>to</strong> the natural or socialenvironment of which they are a part.<strong>Research</strong> questions often (although not always) suggest a relationship <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated.The term relationship, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> a connection or associationbetween two or more characteristics or qualities.DEFINING TERMS IN RESEARCHThree common ways <strong>to</strong> clarify ambiguous or unclear terms <strong>in</strong> a research question<strong>in</strong>volve the use of constitutive (dictionary-type) def<strong>in</strong>itions, def<strong>in</strong>ition by example,<strong>and</strong> operational def<strong>in</strong>itions.A constitutive def<strong>in</strong>ition uses additional terms <strong>to</strong> clarify mean<strong>in</strong>g.An operational def<strong>in</strong>ition describes how examples of a term are <strong>to</strong> be measuredor identified.constitutivedef<strong>in</strong>ition 30empiricalreferent 28operationaldef<strong>in</strong>ition 311. Here are three examples of research questions. <strong>How</strong> would you rank them on a scaleof 1 <strong>to</strong> 5 (5 highest, 1 lowest) for clarity? for significance? Why?a. <strong>How</strong> many students <strong>in</strong> the sophomore class signed up for a course <strong>in</strong> driver tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthis semester?b. Why do so many students <strong>in</strong> the district say they dislike English?c. Is <strong>in</strong>quiry or lecture more effective <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g social studies?2. <strong>How</strong> would you def<strong>in</strong>e humanistically oriented classroom?3. Some terms used frequently <strong>in</strong> education, such as motivation, achievement, <strong>and</strong>even learn<strong>in</strong>g, are very hard <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e clearly. Why do you suppose this is so?4. <strong>How</strong> might the term excellence be def<strong>in</strong>ed operationally? Give an example.5. “Even the clearest of def<strong>in</strong>itions does not always guarantee mean<strong>in</strong>gful communication.”Is this really true? Why or why not?6. We would argue that operational def<strong>in</strong>itions should always be accompanied by constitutivedef<strong>in</strong>itions. Would you agree? Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of an <strong>in</strong>stance when this mightnot be necessary?7. Most researchers do not believe that research efforts based primarily on personal <strong>in</strong>terestwarrant <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Do you agree <strong>in</strong> all cases? Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of a possible exception?Key TermsFor Discussion


36 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 2: The <strong>Research</strong> QuestionUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 2, restate the research problem you listed <strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong> Exercise 1 <strong>in</strong> a sentenceor two, <strong>and</strong> then formulate a research question related <strong>to</strong> this problem. Now list all thekey terms <strong>in</strong> the question that you th<strong>in</strong>k are not clear <strong>and</strong> need <strong>to</strong> be def<strong>in</strong>ed. Def<strong>in</strong>e each ofthese terms both constitutively <strong>and</strong> operationally, <strong>and</strong> then state why you th<strong>in</strong>k your questionis an important one <strong>to</strong> study.Problem Sheet 2The <strong>Research</strong> Question1. My (restated) research problem is:2. My research question is:3. Follow<strong>in</strong>g are the key terms <strong>in</strong> the problem or question that are not clear <strong>and</strong> thusneed <strong>to</strong> be def<strong>in</strong>ed:a. d.b. e.c. f.4. Here are my constitutive def<strong>in</strong>itions of these terms:An electronic version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t,save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.5. Here are my operational def<strong>in</strong>itions of these terms:6. My justification for <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g this question/problem (why I would argue that it isan important question <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate) is as follows:


Variables3<strong>and</strong> HypothesesThe Importanceof Study<strong>in</strong>gRelationshipsVariablesWhat Is a Variable?Quantitative VersusCategorical VariablesIndependent VersusDependent VariablesModera<strong>to</strong>r VariablesExtraneous VariablesHypothesesWhat Is a Hypothesis?Advantages of Stat<strong>in</strong>gHypotheses <strong>in</strong> Addition<strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsDisadvantages of Stat<strong>in</strong>gHypothesesSignificant HypothesesDirectional VersusNondirectionalHypotheses<strong>How</strong> many variables can you identify?OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termGive an example of a modera<strong>to</strong>r variable.“variable” <strong>and</strong> name five variables that Expla<strong>in</strong> what a hypothesis is <strong>and</strong> formulatemight be <strong>in</strong>vestigated by educational two hypotheses that might be <strong>in</strong>vestigatedresearchers.<strong>in</strong> education.Expla<strong>in</strong> how a variable differs from aName two advantages <strong>and</strong> twoconstant.disadvantages of stat<strong>in</strong>g research questionsDist<strong>in</strong>guish between a quantitative <strong>and</strong> a as hypotheses.categorical variable.Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between directional <strong>and</strong>Expla<strong>in</strong> how <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> dependent nondirectional hypotheses <strong>and</strong> give anvariables are related.example of each.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Hypotheses: To State or Not <strong>to</strong> StateAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 3.1: Directional vs. Nondirectional HypothesesActivity 3.2: Test<strong>in</strong>g HypothesesActivity 3.3: Categorical vs. Quantitative VariablesActivity 3.4: Independent <strong>and</strong> Dependent VariablesActivity 3.5: Formulat<strong>in</strong>g a HypothesisActivity 3.6: Modera<strong>to</strong>r VariablesMarge Jenk<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Jenna Rodriguez are hav<strong>in</strong>g coffee follow<strong>in</strong>g a meet<strong>in</strong>g of their graduate sem<strong>in</strong>ar <strong>in</strong>educational research. Both are puzzled by some of the ideas that came up <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>day’s meet<strong>in</strong>g of the class.“I’m not sure I agree with Ms. Naser” (their <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r), says Jenna. “She said that there are a lot of advantages <strong>to</strong>predict<strong>in</strong>g how you th<strong>in</strong>k a study will come out.”“Yeah, I know,” replies Marge. “But formulat<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis seems like a good idea <strong>to</strong> me.”“Well, perhaps, but there are some disadvantages, <strong>to</strong>o.”“Really? I can’t th<strong>in</strong>k of any.”“Well, what about . . . ?”Actually, both Jenna <strong>and</strong> Marge are correct. There are both advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages <strong>to</strong> stat<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis<strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> one’s research question. We’ll discuss examples of both <strong>in</strong> this chapter.The Importanceof Study<strong>in</strong>g RelationshipsWe mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 2 that an important characteristicof many research questions is that they suggest a relationshipof some sort <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated. Not all researchquestions, however, suggest relationships. Sometimesresearchers are <strong>in</strong>terested only <strong>in</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g descriptive<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how people th<strong>in</strong>k or feel or <strong>to</strong>describe how they behave <strong>in</strong> a particular situation. Othertimes the <strong>in</strong>tent is <strong>to</strong> describe a particular program or activity.Such questions also are worthy of <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Asa result, researchers may ask questions like the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<strong>How</strong> do the parents of the sophomore class feelabout the counsel<strong>in</strong>g program?What changes would the staff like <strong>to</strong> see <strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong>the curriculum?Has the number of students enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> collegeprepara<strong>to</strong>ry as compared <strong>to</strong> noncollege prepara<strong>to</strong>rycourses changed over the last four years?<strong>How</strong> does the new read<strong>in</strong>g program differ from theone used <strong>in</strong> this district <strong>in</strong> the past?What does an <strong>in</strong>quiry-oriented social studies teacherdo?Notice that no relationship is suggested <strong>in</strong> thesequestions. The researcher simply wants <strong>to</strong> identify characteristics,behaviors, feel<strong>in</strong>gs, or thoughts. It is oftennecessary <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> such <strong>in</strong>formation as a first step <strong>in</strong>design<strong>in</strong>g other research or mak<strong>in</strong>g educational decisionsof some sort.The problem with purely descriptive research questionsis that answers <strong>to</strong> them do not help us underst<strong>and</strong>why people feel or th<strong>in</strong>k or behave a certa<strong>in</strong> way, whyprograms possess certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics, why a particularstrategy is <strong>to</strong> be used at a certa<strong>in</strong> time, <strong>and</strong> so forth.We may learn what happened, or where or when (<strong>and</strong>even how) someth<strong>in</strong>g happened, but not why it happened.As a result, our underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of a situation,group, or phenomenon is limited. For this reason, scientistshighly value research questions that suggest relationships<strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated, because the answers <strong>to</strong>38


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 39them help expla<strong>in</strong> the nature of the world <strong>in</strong> which welive. We learn <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the world by learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>expla<strong>in</strong> how parts of it are related. We beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> detectpatterns or connections between the parts.We believe that underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g is generally enhancedby the demonstration of relationships or connections. Itis for this reason that we favor the formation of a hypothesisthat predicts the existence of a relationship.There may be times, however, when a researcher wants<strong>to</strong> hypothesize that a relationship does not exist. Whyso? The only persuasive argument we know of is that ofcontradict<strong>in</strong>g an exist<strong>in</strong>g widespread (but perhaps erroneous)belief. For example, if it can be shown that agreat many people believe, <strong>in</strong> the absence of adequateevidence, that young boys are less sympathetic thanyoung girls, a study <strong>in</strong> which a researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds no differencebetween boys <strong>and</strong> girls (i.e., no relationshipbetween gender <strong>and</strong> sympathy) might be of value (sucha study may have been done, although we are not awareof one). Unfortunately, most (but by no means all) of themethodological mistakes made <strong>in</strong> research (such asus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>adequate <strong>in</strong>struments or <strong>to</strong>o small a sample ofparticipants) <strong>in</strong>crease the chance of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g no relationshipbetween variables. (We shall discuss several suchmistakes <strong>in</strong> later chapters.)VariablesWHAT IS A VARIABLE?At this po<strong>in</strong>t, it is important <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduce the idea ofvariables, s<strong>in</strong>ce a relationship is a statement about variables.What is a variable? A variable is a concept—anoun that st<strong>and</strong>s for variation with<strong>in</strong> a class of objects,such as chair, gender, eye color, achievement, motivation,or runn<strong>in</strong>g speed. Even spunk, style, <strong>and</strong> lust forlife are variables. Notice that the <strong>in</strong>dividual members <strong>in</strong>the class of objects, however, must differ—or vary—<strong>to</strong>qualify the class as a variable. If all members of a classare identical, we do not have a variable. Such characteristicsare called constants, s<strong>in</strong>ce the <strong>in</strong>dividual membersof the class are not allowed <strong>to</strong> vary, but rather areheld constant. In any study, some characteristics will bevariables, while others will be constants.An example may make this dist<strong>in</strong>ction clearer. Supposea researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the effects ofre<strong>in</strong>forcement on student achievement. The researchersystematically divides a large group of students, all ofwhom are n<strong>in</strong>th-graders, <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> three smaller subgroups.She then tra<strong>in</strong>s the teachers of these subgroups <strong>to</strong> re<strong>in</strong>forcetheir students <strong>in</strong> different ways (one gives verbalpraise, the second gives monetary rewards, the third givesextra po<strong>in</strong>ts) for various tasks the students perform.In this study, re<strong>in</strong>forcement would be a variable (it conta<strong>in</strong>sthree variations), while the grade level of thestudents would be a constant.Notice that it is easier <strong>to</strong> see what some of these conceptsst<strong>and</strong> for than others. The concept of chair, for example,st<strong>and</strong>s for the many different objects that we si<strong>to</strong>n that possess legs, a seat, <strong>and</strong> a back. Furthermore,different observers would probably agree as <strong>to</strong> how particularchairs differ. It is not so easy, however, <strong>to</strong> seewhat a concept like motivation st<strong>and</strong>s for, or <strong>to</strong> agree onwhat it means. The researchers must be specific here—they must def<strong>in</strong>e motivation as clearly as possible. Theymust do this so that it can be measured or manipulated.We cannot mean<strong>in</strong>gfully measure or manipulate a variableif we cannot def<strong>in</strong>e it. As we mentioned above,much educational research <strong>in</strong>volves look<strong>in</strong>g for a relationshipamong variables. But what variables?There are many variables “out there” <strong>in</strong> the worldthat can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated. Obviously, we can’t <strong>in</strong>vestigatethem all, so we must choose. <strong>Research</strong>ers choosecerta<strong>in</strong> variables <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate because they suspect thatthese variables are somehow related <strong>and</strong> believe thatdiscover<strong>in</strong>g the nature of this relationship, if possible,can help us make more sense out of the world <strong>in</strong> whichwe live.QUANTITATIVE VERSUSCATEGORICAL VARIABLESVariables can be classified <strong>in</strong> several ways. One way is<strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish between quantitative <strong>and</strong> categoricalvariables. Quantitative variables exist <strong>in</strong> some degree(rather than all or none) along a cont<strong>in</strong>uum from less <strong>to</strong>more, <strong>and</strong> we can assign numbers <strong>to</strong> different <strong>in</strong>dividualsor objects <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate how much of the variable theypossess. Two obvious examples are height (John is6 feet tall <strong>and</strong> Sally is 5 feet 4 <strong>in</strong>ches) <strong>and</strong> weight(Mr. Adams weighs only 150 pounds <strong>and</strong> his wife140 pounds, but their son tips the scales at an even200 pounds). We can also assign numbers <strong>to</strong> various<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate how much “<strong>in</strong>terest” they have<strong>in</strong> a subject, with a 5 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g very much <strong>in</strong>terest, a4 much <strong>in</strong>terest, a 3 some <strong>in</strong>terest, a 2 little <strong>in</strong>terest, a 1very little <strong>in</strong>terest, down <strong>to</strong> a 0 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g no <strong>in</strong>terest.If we can assign numbers <strong>in</strong> this way, we have thevariable <strong>in</strong>terest.


40 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eQUANTITATIVE VARIABLESHeight (<strong>in</strong>ches)LowHighScience AptitudeLowHighPrice of Houses (Thous<strong>and</strong>s of Dollars)50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600CATEGORICAL VARIABLESVariable: categories of differenttypes of treesVariable: categories of differenttypes of aircraftVariable: categories of differenttypes of animalsFigure 3.1 Quantitative Variables Compared with Categorical VariablesQuantitative variables can often (but not always) besubdivided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> smaller <strong>and</strong> smaller units. Length, forexample, can be measured <strong>in</strong> miles, yards, feet, <strong>in</strong>ches,or <strong>in</strong> whatever subdivision of an <strong>in</strong>ch is needed. By wayof contrast, categorical variables do not vary <strong>in</strong> degree,amount, or quantity but are qualitatively different. Examples<strong>in</strong>clude eye color, gender, religious preference,occupation, position on a baseball team, <strong>and</strong> most k<strong>in</strong>dsof research “treatments” or “methods.” For example,suppose a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> compare certa<strong>in</strong> attitudes<strong>in</strong> two different groups of voters, one <strong>in</strong> which each <strong>in</strong>dividualis registered as a member of one political party<strong>and</strong> the other <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals are members ofanother party. The variable <strong>in</strong>volved would be politicalparty. This is a categorical variable—a person is either <strong>in</strong>one or the other category, not somewhere <strong>in</strong> betweenbe<strong>in</strong>g a registered member of one party <strong>and</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g a registeredmember of another party. All members with<strong>in</strong>each category of this variable are considered the same asfar as party membership is concerned (see Figure 3.1).Can teach<strong>in</strong>g method be considered a variable? Yes, itcan. Suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>gteachers who use different methods <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g. The researcherlocates one teacher who lectures exclusively,another who buttresses her lectures with slides, films,<strong>and</strong> computer images, <strong>and</strong> a third who uses the casestudymethod <strong>and</strong> lectures not at all. Does the teach<strong>in</strong>gmethod “vary”? It does. You may need <strong>to</strong> practice th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gof differences <strong>in</strong> methods or <strong>in</strong> groups of people(teachers compared <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, for example) asvariables, but master<strong>in</strong>g this idea is extremely useful <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g about research.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHSome Important RelationshipsThat Have Been Clarifiedby <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>1. “The more time beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g readers spend on phonics, thebetter readers they become.” (Despite a great deal of researchon the <strong>to</strong>pic, this statement can neither be clearlysupported nor refuted. It is clear that phonic <strong>in</strong>struction isan important <strong>in</strong>gredient; what is not clear is how muchtime should be devoted <strong>to</strong> it.)*2. “The use of manipulatives <strong>in</strong> elementary grades results <strong>in</strong>better math performance.” (The evidence is quite supportiveof this method of teach<strong>in</strong>g mathematics.)†*R. Calfee <strong>and</strong> P. Drum (1986). <strong>Research</strong> on teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g. InM. C. Wittrock (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of research on teach<strong>in</strong>g, 3rd ed.New York: Macmillan, pp. 804–849.†M. N. Suydam (1986). <strong>Research</strong> report: Manipulative materials <strong>and</strong>achievement. Arithmetic Teacher, 10 (February): 32.3. “Behavior modification is an effective way <strong>to</strong> teach simpleskills <strong>to</strong> very slow learners.” (There is a great deal of evidence<strong>to</strong> support this statement.)††4. “The more teachers know about specific subject matter, thebetter they teach it.” (The evidence is <strong>in</strong>conclusive despitethe seem<strong>in</strong>gly obvious fact that teachers must know morethan their students.)§5. “Among children who become deaf before language hasdeveloped, those with hear<strong>in</strong>g parents become better readersthan those with deaf parents.” (The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of manystudies refute this statement.) ||††S. L. Deno (1982). Behavioral treatment methods. In H. E. Mitzel(Ed.), Encyclopedia of educational research, 5th ed. New York:Macmillan, pp. 199–202.§L. Shulman (1986). Paradigms <strong>and</strong> research programs <strong>in</strong> the studyof teach<strong>in</strong>g. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of research onteach<strong>in</strong>g, 3rd ed. New York: Macmillan, pp. 3–36.|| C. M. Kampfe <strong>and</strong> A. G. Turecheck (1987). Read<strong>in</strong>g achievement ofprel<strong>in</strong>gually deaf students <strong>and</strong> its relationship <strong>to</strong> parental method ofcommunication: A review of the literature. American Annals of theDeaf, 10 (March): 11–15.Now, here are several variables. Which ones are quantitativevariables <strong>and</strong> which ones are categorical variables?1. Make of au<strong>to</strong>mobile2. Learn<strong>in</strong>g ability3. Ethnicity4. Cohesiveness5. Heartbeat rate6. Gender*<strong>Research</strong>ers <strong>in</strong> education often study the relationshipbetween (or among) either (1) two (or more) quantitativevariables; (2) one categorical <strong>and</strong> one quantitative variable;or (3) two or more categorical variables. Here aresome examples of each:1. Two quantitative variablesAge <strong>and</strong> amount of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> schoolRead<strong>in</strong>g achievement <strong>and</strong> mathematics achievementClassroom humanism <strong>and</strong> student motivationAmount of time watch<strong>in</strong>g television <strong>and</strong> aggressivenessof behavior*1, 3, <strong>and</strong> 6 represent categorical variables; 2, 4, <strong>and</strong> 5 representquantitative variables.2. One categorical <strong>and</strong> one quantitative variableMethod used <strong>to</strong> teach read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>gachievementCounsel<strong>in</strong>g approach <strong>and</strong> level of anxietyNationality <strong>and</strong> lik<strong>in</strong>g for schoolStudent gender <strong>and</strong> amount of praise given byteachers3. Two categorical variablesEthnicity <strong>and</strong> father’s occupationGender of teacher <strong>and</strong> subject taughtAdm<strong>in</strong>istrative style <strong>and</strong> college majorReligious affiliation <strong>and</strong> political party membershipSometimes researchers have a choice of whether <strong>to</strong>treat a variable as quantitative or categorical. It is notuncommon, for example, <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d studies <strong>in</strong> which a variablesuch as anxiety is studied by compar<strong>in</strong>g a group of“high-anxiety” students <strong>to</strong> a group of “low-anxiety” students.This treats anxiety as though it were a categoricalvariable. While there is noth<strong>in</strong>g really wrong with do<strong>in</strong>gthis, there are three reasons why it is preferable <strong>in</strong> suchsituations <strong>to</strong> treat the variable as quantitative.1. Conceptually, we consider variables such as anxiety<strong>in</strong> people <strong>to</strong> be a matter of degree, not a matter ofeither-or.41


42 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e2. Collaps<strong>in</strong>g the variable <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two (or even several) categorieselim<strong>in</strong>ates the possibility of us<strong>in</strong>g more detailed<strong>in</strong>formationaboutthevariable,s<strong>in</strong>cedifferencesamong <strong>in</strong>dividuals with<strong>in</strong> a category are ignored.3. The divid<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>e between groups (for example, between<strong>in</strong>dividuals of high, middle, <strong>and</strong> low anxiety)is almost always arbitrary (that is, lack<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> anydefensible rationale).INDEPENDENT VERSUSDEPENDENT VARIABLESA common <strong>and</strong> useful way <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about variables is <strong>to</strong>classify them as <strong>in</strong>dependent or dependent. Independentvariables are those that the researcher chooses <strong>to</strong> study <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> assess their possible effect(s) on one or moreother variables. An <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is presumed <strong>to</strong>affect (at least partly cause) or somehow <strong>in</strong>fluence at leas<strong>to</strong>ne other variable. The variable that the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable is presumed <strong>to</strong> affect is called a dependentvariable. In commonsense terms, the dependent variable“depends on” what the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable does <strong>to</strong> it,how it affects it. For example, a researcher study<strong>in</strong>g therelationship between childhood success <strong>in</strong> mathematics<strong>and</strong> adult career choice is likely <strong>to</strong> refer <strong>to</strong> the former asthe <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>and</strong> subsequent career choice asthe dependent variable.It is possible <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate more than one <strong>in</strong>dependent(<strong>and</strong> also more than one dependent) variable <strong>in</strong> astudy. For simplicity’s sake, however, we present examples<strong>in</strong> which only one <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> one dependentvariable are <strong>in</strong>volved.The relationship between <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> dependentvariables can be portrayed graphically as follows:Independentvariable(s)(presumed orpossible cause)AffectsDependentvariable(s)(presumedresults)At this po<strong>in</strong>t, let’s check your underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Supposea researcher plans <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the follow<strong>in</strong>gquestion: “Will students who are taught by a team ofthree teachers learn more science than students taughtby one <strong>in</strong>dividual teacher?” What are the <strong>in</strong>dependent<strong>and</strong> dependent variables <strong>in</strong> this question?**The <strong>in</strong>dependent (categorical) variable is the number of teachers, <strong>and</strong>the dependent (quantitative) variable is the amount of science learn<strong>in</strong>g.Notice that there are two conditions (sometimes calledlevels) <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable—“three teachers” <strong>and</strong>“one teacher.” Also notice that the dependent variable isnot “science learn<strong>in</strong>g” but “amount of science learn<strong>in</strong>g.”Can you see why?At this po<strong>in</strong>t, th<strong>in</strong>gs beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> get a bit complicated. Independentvariables may be either manipulated or selected.A manipulated variable is one that the researchercreates. Such variables are typically found <strong>in</strong> experimentalstudies (see Chapter 13). Suppose, for example, that aresearcher decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effect of differentamounts of re<strong>in</strong>forcement on read<strong>in</strong>g achievement <strong>and</strong>systematically assigns students <strong>to</strong> three different groups.One group is praised cont<strong>in</strong>uously every day dur<strong>in</strong>g theirread<strong>in</strong>g session; the second group is <strong>to</strong>ld simply <strong>to</strong> “keepup the good work”; the third group is <strong>to</strong>ld noth<strong>in</strong>g at all.The researcher, <strong>in</strong> effect, manipulates the conditions <strong>in</strong>this experiment, thereby creat<strong>in</strong>g the variable amount ofre<strong>in</strong>forcement. Whenever a researcher sets up experimentalconditions, one or more variables are created. Suchvariables are called manipulated variables, experimentalvariables, or treatment variables.Sometimes researchers select an <strong>in</strong>dependent variablethat already exists. In this case, the researcher mustlocate <strong>and</strong> select examples of it, rather than creat<strong>in</strong>g it. Inour earlier example of read<strong>in</strong>g methods, the researcherwould have <strong>to</strong> locate <strong>and</strong> select exist<strong>in</strong>g examples ofeach read<strong>in</strong>g method, rather than arrang<strong>in</strong>g for them <strong>to</strong>happen. Selected <strong>in</strong>dependent variables are not limited <strong>to</strong>studies that compare different treatments; they are found<strong>in</strong> both causal-comparative <strong>and</strong> correlational studies (seeChapters 15 <strong>and</strong> 16). They can be either categorical orquantitative. The key idea here, however, is that the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable (either created or selected) is thought <strong>to</strong>affect the dependent variable. Here are a few examplesof some possible relationships between a selected <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable <strong>and</strong> a dependent variable:Independent VariableGender (categorical)Mathematical ability(quantitative)Gang membership(categorical)Test anxiety(quantitative)Dependent VariableMusical aptitude(quantitative)Career choice(categorical)Subsequent marital status(categorical)Test performance(quantitative)Notice that none of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variables <strong>in</strong> the abovepairs could be directly manipulated by the researcher.


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 43Notice also that, <strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>stances, the <strong>in</strong>dependent/dependent relationship might be reversed, depend<strong>in</strong>g onwhich one the researcher thought might be the cause of theother. For example, he or she might th<strong>in</strong>k that test performancecauses anxiety, not the reverse.Generally speak<strong>in</strong>g, most studies <strong>in</strong> education thathave one quantitative <strong>and</strong> one categorical variable arestudies compar<strong>in</strong>g different methods or treatments. As we<strong>in</strong>dicated above, the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> such studies(the different methods or treatments) represents a categoricalvariable. Often the other (dependent) variable isquantitative <strong>and</strong> is referred <strong>to</strong> as an outcome variable.*The reason is rather clear-cut. The <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r, after all,is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effect(s) of the differences <strong>in</strong> methodon one or more outcomes (student achievement, their motivation,<strong>in</strong>terest, <strong>and</strong> so on).Aga<strong>in</strong>, let’s check your underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Suppose aresearcher plans <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the follow<strong>in</strong>g question:“Will students like his<strong>to</strong>ry more if taught by the <strong>in</strong>quirymethod than if taught by the case-study method?” Whatis the outcome variable <strong>in</strong> this question?†MODERATOR VARIABLESA modera<strong>to</strong>r variable is a special type of <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable. It is a secondary <strong>in</strong>dependent variable that hasbeen selected for study <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if it affectsor modifies the basic relationship between the primary<strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>and</strong> the dependent variable. Thus,if an experimenter th<strong>in</strong>ks that the relationship betweenvariables X <strong>and</strong> Y might be altered <strong>in</strong> some way by athird variable Z, then Z could be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the study asa modera<strong>to</strong>r variable.Consider an example. Suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g the effectiveness of a discussionorientedapproach <strong>to</strong> a more visually oriented approachfor teach<strong>in</strong>g a unit <strong>in</strong> a U.S. His<strong>to</strong>ry class. Suppose furtherthat the researcher suspects that the discussion approachmay be superior for the girls <strong>in</strong> the class (who appear <strong>to</strong> bemore verbal <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> learn better through convers<strong>in</strong>g withothers) <strong>and</strong> that the visual approach may be more effectivefor boys (who seem <strong>to</strong> perk up every time a video isshown). When the students are tested <strong>to</strong>gether at the endof the unit, the overall results of the two approaches mayshow no difference, but when the results of the girls areseparated from those of the boys, the two approaches may*It is also possible for an outcome variable <strong>to</strong> be categorical. For example,the variable college completion could be divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the categoriesof dropouts <strong>and</strong> college graduates.†Lik<strong>in</strong>g for his<strong>to</strong>ry is the outcome variable.AchievementGirls GirlsBoysBoysDiscussionVisualInstructional ApproachFigure 3.2 Relationship Between Instructional Approach(Independent Variable) <strong>and</strong> Achievement (DependentVariable), as Moderated by Gender of Studentsreveal different results for each subgroup. If so, then thegender variable moderates the relationship between the<strong>in</strong>structional approach (the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable) <strong>and</strong>effectiveness (the dependent variable). The <strong>in</strong>fluence ofthis modera<strong>to</strong>r variable can be seen <strong>in</strong> Figure 3.2.Here are two examples of hypotheses that conta<strong>in</strong>modera<strong>to</strong>r variables.Hypothesis 1: “Anxiety affects test performance,but the correlation is markedly lower for studentswith test-tak<strong>in</strong>g experience.”Independent variable: anxiety levelModera<strong>to</strong>r variable: test-tak<strong>in</strong>g experienceDependent variable: test performanceHypothesis 2: “High school students taught primarilyby the <strong>in</strong>quiry method will perform better on tests ofcritical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g than will high school studentstaught primarily by the demonstration method,although the reverse will be true for elementaryschool students.”Independent variable: <strong>in</strong>structional methodModera<strong>to</strong>r variable: grade levelDependent variable: performance on criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g testsAs you can see, the <strong>in</strong>clusion of a modera<strong>to</strong>r variable (oreven two or three) <strong>in</strong> a study can provide considerably more<strong>in</strong>formation than just study<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dependent variablealone.We recommend their <strong>in</strong>clusion whenever appropriate.EXTRANEOUS VARIABLESA basic problem <strong>in</strong> research is that there are manypossible <strong>in</strong>dependent variables that could have an effec<strong>to</strong>n the dependent variables. Once researchers have decidedwhich variables <strong>to</strong> study, they must be concernedabout the <strong>in</strong>fluence or effect of other variables that exist.Such variables are usually called extraneous variables.


44 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 3.3 Examples ofExtraneous VariablesThe pr<strong>in</strong>cipal of a high school compares the f<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ation scores of two his<strong>to</strong>ry classes taughtby teachers who use different methods, not realiz<strong>in</strong>g that they are also different <strong>in</strong> many otherways because of extraneous variables. The classes differ <strong>in</strong>:Extraneousvariables• Size of class• Gender of students• Gender of teacher• Age of teacher• Time of day class meets• Days of week class meets••Ethnicity of teacherLength of classMs. Brown’s (age 31) his<strong>to</strong>ry class meets from 9:00 <strong>to</strong> 9:50 A.M., Tuesdays<strong>and</strong> Thursdays. The class conta<strong>in</strong>s 9 students, all girls.Mr. Thompson’s (age 54) his<strong>to</strong>ry class meets from 2:00 <strong>to</strong> 3:00 P.M.Mondays <strong>and</strong> Wednesdays. The class conta<strong>in</strong>s 16 students, all boys.The task is <strong>to</strong> control these extraneous variables somehow<strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate or m<strong>in</strong>imize their effect.Extraneous variables are <strong>in</strong>dependent variables thathave not been controlled. Look aga<strong>in</strong> at the researchquestion about team teach<strong>in</strong>g on page 42. What are someother variables that could have an effect on the learn<strong>in</strong>gof students <strong>in</strong> a classroom situation?There are many possible extraneous variables. The personalityof the teachers <strong>in</strong>volved, the experience level ofthe students, the time of day the classes are taught, thenature of the subject taught, the textbooks used, the type oflearn<strong>in</strong>g activities the teachers employ, <strong>and</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods—all are possible extraneous variables that couldaffect learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this study. Figure 3.3 illustrates the importanceof identify<strong>in</strong>g extraneous variables.One way <strong>to</strong> control extraneous variables is <strong>to</strong> holdthem constant. For example, if a researcher <strong>in</strong>cludesonly boys as the subjects of a study, she is controll<strong>in</strong>gthe variable of gender. We would say that the genderof the subjects does not vary; it is a constant <strong>in</strong> thisstudy.<strong>Research</strong>ers must cont<strong>in</strong>ually th<strong>in</strong>k about how theymight control the possible effect(s) of extraneous variables.We will discuss how <strong>to</strong> do this <strong>in</strong> some detail <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9, but for now you need <strong>to</strong> make sure you underst<strong>and</strong>the difference between <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong>dependent variables <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be aware of extraneous variables.Try your h<strong>and</strong> at the follow<strong>in</strong>g question: “Will femalestudents who are taught his<strong>to</strong>ry by a teacher of thesame gender like the subject more than female students


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 45“Why have thetest scores of ourseniors been go<strong>in</strong>gdown?”“They don’t believethat school<strong>in</strong>g willpay off for them.”“They watch <strong>to</strong>omuch television!”“Many of thisgeneration of studentsare apathetic—they justdon’t care about muchof anyth<strong>in</strong>g.”“No, it’sresources. We justdon’t have the books<strong>and</strong> other materials weneed <strong>to</strong> do aneffective job.”“So manyof them have <strong>to</strong>work at night <strong>and</strong>don’t have time<strong>to</strong> study.”“They are sent<strong>to</strong> us poorly preparedfrom the middle schoolsthey have attended.”“I th<strong>in</strong>k it is us—theteachers. We’re not teach<strong>in</strong>gas well as we used <strong>to</strong> becausewe’re burned out.”Pr<strong>in</strong>cipalFigure 3.4 A S<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>Research</strong> Question Can Suggest Several Hypothesestaught by a teacher of a different gender?” What are thevariables?*HypothesesWHAT IS A HYPOTHESIS?A hypothesis is, simply put, a prediction of the possibleoutcomes of a study. For example, here is a researchquestion followed by its restatement <strong>in</strong> the form of apossible hypothesis:Question: Will students who are taught his<strong>to</strong>ry by ateacher of the same gender like the subject more thanstudents taught by a teacher of a different gender?Hypothesis: Students taught his<strong>to</strong>ry by a teacher ofthe same gender will like the subject more than*The dependent variable is lik<strong>in</strong>g for his<strong>to</strong>ry, the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable is the gender of the teacher. Possible extraneous variables<strong>in</strong>clude the personality <strong>and</strong> ability of the teacher(s) <strong>in</strong>volved; thepersonality <strong>and</strong> ability level of the students; the materials used,such as textbooks; the style of teach<strong>in</strong>g; ethnicity <strong>and</strong>/or age of theteacher <strong>and</strong> students; <strong>and</strong> others. The researcher would want <strong>to</strong>control as many of these variables as possible.students taught his<strong>to</strong>ry by a teacher of a differentgender.Here are two more examples of research questions followedby the restatement of each as a possible hypothesis:Question: Is rapport with clients of counselors us<strong>in</strong>gclient-centered therapy different from that ofcounselors us<strong>in</strong>g behavior-modification therapy?Hypothesis: Counselors who use a client-centeredtherapy approach will have a greater rapport withtheir clients than counselors who use a behaviormodificationapproach.Question: <strong>How</strong> do teachers feel about special classesfor the educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped?Hypothesis: Teachers <strong>in</strong> XYZ School District believethat students attend<strong>in</strong>g special classes for theeducationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped will be stigmatized.orTeachers <strong>in</strong> XYZ School District believe that specialclasses for the educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped willhelp such students improve their academic skills.Many different hypotheses can come from a s<strong>in</strong>glequestion, as illustrated <strong>in</strong> Figure 3.4.


46 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eADVANTAGES OF STATING HYPOTHESESIN ADDITION TO RESEARCH QUESTIONSStat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses has both advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages.What are some of the advantages? First, a hypothesisforces us <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k more deeply <strong>and</strong> specifically aboutthe possible outcomes of a study. Elaborat<strong>in</strong>g on a questionby formulat<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis can lead <strong>to</strong> a moresophisticated underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of what the question implies<strong>and</strong> exactly what variables are <strong>in</strong>volved. Often, as <strong>in</strong> thecase of the third example above, when more than onehypothesis seems <strong>to</strong> suggest itself, we are forced <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>kmore carefully about what we really want <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate.A second advantage of restat<strong>in</strong>g questions as hypotheses<strong>in</strong>volves a philosophy of science. The rationale underly<strong>in</strong>gthis philosophy is as follows: If one is attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>build a body of knowledge <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> answer<strong>in</strong>g aspecific question, then stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses is a good strategybecause it enables one <strong>to</strong> make specific predictionsbased on prior evidence or theoretical argument. If thesepredictions are borne out by subsequent research, theentire procedure ga<strong>in</strong>s both <strong>in</strong> persuasiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency.A classic example is Albert E<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>’s theory ofrelativity. Many hypotheses were formulated as a resul<strong>to</strong>f E<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>’s theory, which were later verified throughresearch. As more <strong>and</strong> more of these predictions wereshown <strong>to</strong> be fact, not only did they become useful <strong>in</strong> theirown right, they also provided <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g support for theorig<strong>in</strong>al ideas <strong>in</strong> E<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>’s theory, which generated thehypotheses <strong>in</strong> the first place.Lastly, stat<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis helps us see if we are, orare not, <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g a relationship. If not, we may beprompted <strong>to</strong> formulate one.DISADVANTAGESOF STATING HYPOTHESESEssentially, the disadvantages of stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses arethreefold. First, stat<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis may lead <strong>to</strong> a bias,either conscious or unconscious, on the part of the researcher.Once <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs state a hypothesis, they maybe tempted <strong>to</strong> arrange the procedures or manipulate thedata <strong>in</strong> such a way as <strong>to</strong> br<strong>in</strong>g about a desired outcome.This is probably more the exception than the rule.<strong>Research</strong>ers are assumed <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>tellectually honest—although there are some famous exceptions. All studiesshould be subject <strong>to</strong> peer review; <strong>in</strong> the past, a review ofsuspect research has, on occasion, revealed such <strong>in</strong>adequaciesof method that the reported results were cast <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>doubt. Furthermore, any particular study can be replicated<strong>to</strong> verify the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the study. Unfortunately,few educational research studies are repeated, so this“protection” is somewhat of an illusion. A dishonest <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rst<strong>and</strong>s a fair chance of gett<strong>in</strong>g away with falsify<strong>in</strong>gresults. Why would a person deliberately dis<strong>to</strong>rthis or her f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs? Probably because professionalrecognition <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ancial reward accrue <strong>to</strong> those whopublish important results.Even for the great majority of researchers who arehonest, however, commitment <strong>to</strong> a hypothesis may lead<strong>to</strong> dis<strong>to</strong>rtions that are un<strong>in</strong>tentional <strong>and</strong> unconscious.But it is probably unlikely that any researcher <strong>in</strong> thefield of education is ever <strong>to</strong>tally dis<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> theoutcomes of a study; therefore, his or her attitudes<strong>and</strong>/or knowledge may favor a particular result. For thisreason, we th<strong>in</strong>k it is desirable for researchers <strong>to</strong> makeknown their predilections regard<strong>in</strong>g a hypothesis so thatthey are clear <strong>to</strong> others <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> their research. Thisalso allows <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> take steps <strong>to</strong> guard (as muchas possible) aga<strong>in</strong>st their personal biases.The second disadvantage of stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses at theoutset is that it may sometimes be unnecessary, or even<strong>in</strong>appropriate, <strong>in</strong> research projects of certa<strong>in</strong> types, suchas descriptive surveys <strong>and</strong> ethnographic studies. Inmany such studies, it would be unduly presumptuous, aswell as futile, <strong>to</strong> predict what the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the <strong>in</strong>quirywill be.The third disadvantage of stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses is that focus<strong>in</strong>gattention on a hypothesis may prevent researchersfrom notic<strong>in</strong>g other phenomena that might be important<strong>to</strong> study. For example, decid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> study the effect of a“humanistic” classroom on student motivation mightlead a researcher <strong>to</strong> overlook its effect on such characteristicsas sex-typ<strong>in</strong>g or decision mak<strong>in</strong>g, which would bequite noticeable <strong>to</strong> another researcher who was not focus<strong>in</strong>gsolely on motivation. This seems <strong>to</strong> be a goodargument aga<strong>in</strong>st all research be<strong>in</strong>g directed <strong>to</strong>wardhypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g.Consider the example of a research question presentedearlier <strong>in</strong> this chapter: “<strong>How</strong> do teachers feelabout special classes for the educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped?”We offered two (of many possible) hypothesesthat might arise out of this question: (1) “Teachersbelieve that students attend<strong>in</strong>g special classes for theeducationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped will be stigmatized” <strong>and</strong>(2) “Teachers believe that special classes for the educationallyh<strong>and</strong>icapped will help such students improvetheir academic skills.” Both of these hypotheses implicitlysuggest a comparison between special classes forthe educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> some other k<strong>in</strong>d ofarrangement. Thus, the relationship <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated is


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 47between teacher beliefs <strong>and</strong> type of class. Notice thatit is important <strong>to</strong> compare what teachers th<strong>in</strong>k aboutspecial classes with their beliefs about other k<strong>in</strong>ds ofarrangements. If researchers looked only at teacherop<strong>in</strong>ions about special classes without also identify<strong>in</strong>gtheir views about other k<strong>in</strong>ds of arrangements, theywould not know if their beliefs about special classeswere <strong>in</strong> any way unique or different.SIGNIFICANT HYPOTHESESAs we th<strong>in</strong>k about possible hypotheses suggested by aresearch question, we beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> see that some of them aremore significant than others. What do we mean by significant?Simply that some may lead <strong>to</strong> more usefulknowledge. Compare, for example, the follow<strong>in</strong>g pairsof hypotheses. Which hypothesis <strong>in</strong> each pair wouldyou say is more significant?Pair 1a. Second-graders like school less than they likewatch<strong>in</strong>g television.b. Second-graders like school less than first-gradersbut more than third-graders.Pair 2a. Most students with academic disabilities preferbe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> regular classes rather than <strong>in</strong> specialclasses.b. Students with academic disabilities will havemore negative attitudes about themselves if theyare placed <strong>in</strong> special classes than if they areplaced <strong>in</strong> regular classes.Pair 3a. Counselors who use client-centered therapy proceduresget different reactions from counseleesthan do counselors who use traditional therapyprocedures.b. Counselees who receive client-centered therapyexpress more satisfaction with the counsel<strong>in</strong>gprocess than do counselees who receive traditionaltherapy.In each of the three pairs, we th<strong>in</strong>k that the secondhypothesis is more significant than the first s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> eachcase (<strong>in</strong> our judgment) not only is the relationship <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>vestigated clearer <strong>and</strong> more specific but also <strong>in</strong>vestigationof the hypothesis seems more likely <strong>to</strong> lead <strong>to</strong> agreater amount of knowledge. It also seems <strong>to</strong> us that the<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> be obta<strong>in</strong>ed will be of more use <strong>to</strong> people<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the research question.DIRECTIONAL VERSUSNONDIRECTIONAL HYPOTHESESLet us make a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between directional <strong>and</strong>nondirectional hypotheses. A directional hypothesis<strong>in</strong>dicates the specific direction (such as higher, lower,more, or less) that a researcher expects <strong>to</strong> emerge <strong>in</strong> arelationship. The particular direction expected is basedon what the researcher has found <strong>in</strong> the literature, frompersonal experience, or from the experience of others.The second hypothesis <strong>in</strong> each of the three pairs aboveis a directional hypothesis.Sometimes it is difficult <strong>to</strong> make specific predictions.If a researcher suspects that a relationship existsbut has no basis for predict<strong>in</strong>g the direction of the relationship,she cannot make a directional hypothesis. Anondirectional hypothesis does not make a specificprediction about what direction the outcome of a studywill take. In nondirectional form, the second hypothesesof the three pairs above would be stated as follows:1. First-, second-, <strong>and</strong> third-graders will feel differently<strong>to</strong>ward school.2. There will be a difference between the scores on anattitude measure of students with academic disabilitiesplaced <strong>in</strong> special classes <strong>and</strong> such studentsplaced <strong>in</strong> regular classes.3. There will be a difference <strong>in</strong> expression of satisfactionwith the counsel<strong>in</strong>g process between counseleeswho receive client-centered therapy <strong>and</strong> counseleeswho receive traditional therapy.Figure 3.5 illustrates the difference between a directional<strong>and</strong> a nondirectional hypothesis. If the personpictured is approach<strong>in</strong>g a street corner, three possibilitiesexist when he reaches the corner:He will cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> look straight ahead.He will look <strong>to</strong> his right.He will look <strong>to</strong> his left.A nondirectional hypothesis would predict that hewill look one way or the other. A directional hypothesiswould predict that he will look <strong>in</strong> a particular direction(for example, <strong>to</strong> his right). S<strong>in</strong>ce a directional hypothesisis riskier (because it is less likely <strong>to</strong> occur), it is moreconv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g when confirmed.*Both directional <strong>and</strong> nondirectional hypothesesappear <strong>in</strong> the literature of research, <strong>and</strong> you should learn<strong>to</strong> recognize each.*If he looks straight ahead, neither a directional nor a nondirectionalhypothesis is confirmed.


48 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eNondirectional hypothesis(The man will look eitherleft or right.)Either – OrDirectional hypothesis (1)(The man will lookonly <strong>to</strong> the right.)Will lookonly <strong>to</strong> rightDirectional hypothesis (2)(The man will lookonly <strong>to</strong> the left.)Will lookonly <strong>to</strong> leftFigure 3.5 Directional Versus Nondirectional HypothesesGo back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTHE IMPORTANCE OF STUDYING RELATIONSHIPSIdentify<strong>in</strong>g relationships among variables enhances underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g relationships helps us <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> the nature of our world.VARIABLESA variable is any characteristic or quality that varies among the members of a particulargroup.A constant is any characteristic or quality that is the same for all members of a particulargroup.


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 49A quantitative variable varies <strong>in</strong> amount or degree, but not <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d.A categorical variable varies only <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d, not <strong>in</strong> degree or amount.Several k<strong>in</strong>ds of variables are studied <strong>in</strong> educational research, the most commonbe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> dependent variables.An <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is presumed <strong>to</strong> affect or <strong>in</strong>fluence other variables.Independent variables are sometimes called experimental variables or manipulatedvariables.A dependent (or outcome) variable is presumed <strong>to</strong> be affected by one or more <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables.Independent variables may be either manipulated or selected. A manipulated variableis created by the researcher. A selected variable is one that already exists that the researcherlocates <strong>and</strong> then chooses <strong>to</strong> study.A modera<strong>to</strong>r variable is a secondary <strong>in</strong>dependent variable that the researcher selects<strong>to</strong> study because he or she th<strong>in</strong>ks it may affect the basic relationship between the primary<strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>and</strong> the dependent variable.An extraneous variable is an <strong>in</strong>dependent variable that may have un<strong>in</strong>tended effectson a dependent variable <strong>in</strong> a particular study.HYPOTHESESThe term hypothesis, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> a prediction of results usuallymade before a study commences.Stat<strong>in</strong>g a research question as a hypothesis has both advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages.A significant hypothesis is one that is likely <strong>to</strong> lead, if it is supported, <strong>to</strong> a greateramount of important knowledge than a nonsignificant hypothesis.A directional hypothesis is a prediction about the specific nature of a relationship—for example, method A is more effective than method B.A nondirectional hypothesis is a prediction that a relationship exists without specify<strong>in</strong>gits exact nature—for example, there will be a difference between method A <strong>and</strong>method B (without say<strong>in</strong>g which will be more effective).bias 46categorical variable 40constant 39dependent variable 42directional hypothesis 47experimental variable 42extraneousvariable 44hypothesis 45<strong>in</strong>dependent variable 42manipulated variable 42modera<strong>to</strong>r variable 43nondirectionalhypothesis 47outcome variable 43quantitative variable 39treatment variable 42variable 39Key Terms1. Here are several research questions. Which ones suggest relationships?a. <strong>How</strong> many students are enrolled <strong>in</strong> the sophomore class this year?b. As the read<strong>in</strong>g level of a text passage <strong>in</strong>creases, does the number of errorsstudents make <strong>in</strong> pronounc<strong>in</strong>g words <strong>in</strong> the passage <strong>in</strong>crease?c. Do <strong>in</strong>dividuals who see themselves as socially “attractive” expect their romanticpartners also <strong>to</strong> be (as judged by others) socially attractive?d. What does the faculty dislike about the new English curriculum?e. Who is the brightest student <strong>in</strong> the senior class?For Discussion


CHAPTER 3 Variables <strong>and</strong> Hypotheses 51<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 3: The <strong>Research</strong> HypothesisTry <strong>to</strong> formulate one testable hypothesis <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a relationship. It should be related <strong>to</strong> theresearch question you developed <strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong> Exercise 2. Us<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 3, state thehypothesis <strong>in</strong> a sentence or two. If you are plann<strong>in</strong>g an open-ended qualitative study, give anexample of a hypothesis that may “emerge” (see p. 6). Check <strong>to</strong> see if it suggests a relationshipbetween at least two variables. If it does not, revise it so that it does. Now <strong>in</strong>dicate which is the<strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>and</strong> which is the dependent variable. Last, list as many extraneousvariables as you can th<strong>in</strong>k of that might affect the results of your study.Problem Sheet 3The <strong>Research</strong> Hypothesis1. My research question is:2. My hypothesis is _______________________________________________________3. This hypothesis suggests a relationship between at least two variables.They are ___________________________ <strong>and</strong> ______________________________4. More specifically, the variables <strong>in</strong> my study are:a. Dependentb. Independent5. The dependent variable is (check one) categorical _____ quantitative _____The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is (check one) categorical _____ quantitative _____6. Possible extraneous variables that might affect my results <strong>in</strong>clude:a.An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe .com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.b.c.d.e.


4Ethics<strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong>Some Examplesof Unethical PracticeA Statement of EthicalPr<strong>in</strong>ciplesProtect<strong>in</strong>g Participantsfrom HarmEnsur<strong>in</strong>g Confidentialityof <strong>Research</strong> DataShould SubjectsBe Deceived?Three Examples Involv<strong>in</strong>gEthical Concerns<strong>Research</strong> with ChildrenRegulation of <strong>Research</strong>“Now, I can’t require you <strong>to</strong>participate <strong>in</strong> this study, but ifyou want <strong>to</strong> get a good grade<strong>in</strong> this course . . .”“Hey, wait a m<strong>in</strong>ute.That’s unethical!”OBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly what is meantby “ethical” research.Describe briefly three important ethicalpr<strong>in</strong>ciples recommended for researchers<strong>to</strong> follow.State the basic question with regard <strong>to</strong>ethics that researchers need <strong>to</strong> ask beforebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g a study.State the three questions researchers need<strong>to</strong> address <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> protect researchparticipants from harm.Describe the procedures researchers mustfollow <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> ensure confidentialityof data collected <strong>in</strong> a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation.Describe when it might be appropriate <strong>to</strong>deceive participants <strong>in</strong> a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>and</strong> the researcher’sresponsibilities <strong>in</strong> such a case.Describe the special considerations<strong>in</strong>volved when do<strong>in</strong>g research withchildren.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About What Constitutes Ethical <strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 4.1: Ethical or Not?Activity 4.2: Some Ethical DilemmasActivity 4.3: Violations of Ethical PracticeActivity 4.4: Why Would These <strong>Research</strong> PracticesBe Unethical?Mary Abrams <strong>and</strong> Lamar Harris, both juniors at a large midwestern university, meet weekly for lunch. “I can’tbelieve it,” Mary says.“What’s the matter?” replies Lamar.“Professor Thomas says that we have <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> one of his research projects if we want <strong>to</strong> pass his course.He says it is a course requirement. I don’t th<strong>in</strong>k that’s right, <strong>and</strong> I’m pretty upset about it. Can you believe it?”“Wow. Can he do that? I mean, is that ethical?”No, it’s not! Mary has a legitimate (<strong>and</strong> ethical) compla<strong>in</strong>t here. This issue—whether professors can requirestudents <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> research projects <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> pass a course—is one example of an unethical practice thatsometimes occurs.The whole question of what is—<strong>and</strong> what isn’t—ethical is the focus of this chapter.Some Examplesof Unethical PracticeThe term ethics refers <strong>to</strong> questions of right <strong>and</strong> wrong.When researchers th<strong>in</strong>k about ethics, they must askthemselves if it is “right” <strong>to</strong> conduct a particular studyor carry out certa<strong>in</strong> procedures. Are there some k<strong>in</strong>ds ofstudies that should not be conducted? You bet! Here aresome examples of unethical practice:A researcherrequires a group of high school sophomores <strong>to</strong> sign aform <strong>in</strong> which they agree <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> a researchstudy.asks first-graders sensitive questions without obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe consent of their parents <strong>to</strong> question them.deletes data he collects that do not support hishypothesis.equires university students <strong>to</strong> fill out a questionnaireabout their sexual practices.<strong>in</strong>volves a group of eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> a researchstudy that may harm them psychologically without<strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g them or their parents of this fact.Each of the above examples <strong>in</strong>volves one or moreviolations of ethical practice. When researchers th<strong>in</strong>kabout ethics, the basic question <strong>to</strong> ask <strong>in</strong> this regard is,Will any physical or psychological harm come <strong>to</strong> anyoneas a result of my research? Naturally, no researcherwants this <strong>to</strong> happen <strong>to</strong> any of the subjects <strong>in</strong> a researchstudy. Because this is such an important (<strong>and</strong> often overlooked)issue, we need <strong>to</strong> discuss it <strong>in</strong> some detail.In a somewhat larger sense, ethics also refers <strong>to</strong>questions of right <strong>and</strong> wrong. By behav<strong>in</strong>g ethically, aperson is do<strong>in</strong>g what is right. But what does it mean <strong>to</strong>be “right” as far as research is concerned?A Statement of EthicalPr<strong>in</strong>ciplesWebster’s New World Dictionary def<strong>in</strong>es ethical (behavior)as “conform<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ards of conduct of a givenprofession or group.” What researchers consider <strong>to</strong> beethical, therefore, is largely a matter of agreement amongthem. Some years ago, the Committee on Scientific<strong>and</strong> Professional Ethics of the American Psychological53


54 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAssociation published a list of ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples for theconduct of research with human subjects. We haveadapted many of these pr<strong>in</strong>ciples so they apply <strong>to</strong> educationalresearch. Please read the follow<strong>in</strong>g statement <strong>and</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k carefully about what it means.The decision <strong>to</strong> undertake research rests upon a consideredjudgment by the <strong>in</strong>dividual educa<strong>to</strong>r about how best<strong>to</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> science <strong>and</strong> human welfare. Once one decides<strong>to</strong> conduct research, the educa<strong>to</strong>r considers variousways by which he might <strong>in</strong>vest his talents <strong>and</strong> resources.Keep<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, the educa<strong>to</strong>r carries out the researchwith respect <strong>and</strong> concern for the dignity <strong>and</strong> welfare ofthe people who participate <strong>and</strong> with cognizance of federal<strong>and</strong> state regulations <strong>and</strong> professional st<strong>and</strong>ards govern<strong>in</strong>gthe conduct of research with human participants.a. In plann<strong>in</strong>g a study, researchers have the responsibility<strong>to</strong> evaluate carefully any ethical concerns. Shouldany of the ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples listed below be compromised,the educa<strong>to</strong>r has a correspond<strong>in</strong>gly serious obligation<strong>to</strong> observe str<strong>in</strong>gent safeguards <strong>to</strong> protect the rightsof human participants.b. Consider<strong>in</strong>g whether a participant <strong>in</strong> a plannedstudy will be a “subject at risk” or a “subject at m<strong>in</strong>imalrisk,” accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> recognized st<strong>and</strong>ards, is of primaryethical concern <strong>to</strong> the researcher.c. The researcher always reta<strong>in</strong>s the responsibility forensur<strong>in</strong>g that a study is conducted ethically. The researcheris also responsible for the ethical treatment of researchparticipants by collabora<strong>to</strong>rs, assistants, students,<strong>and</strong> employees, all of whom, however, <strong>in</strong>cur similarobligations.d. Except <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imal-risk research, the researcherestablishes a clear <strong>and</strong> fair agreement with researchparticipants, before they participate, that clarifies theobligations <strong>and</strong> responsibilities of each. The researcherhas the obligation <strong>to</strong> honor all promises <strong>and</strong> commitments<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> that agreement. The researcher<strong>in</strong>forms the participants of all aspects of the researchthat might reasonably be expected <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence theirwill<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study <strong>and</strong> answershonestly any questions they may have about the research.Failure by the researcher <strong>to</strong> make full disclosure prior<strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formed consent requires additional safeguards<strong>to</strong> protect the welfare <strong>and</strong> dignity of the researchparticipants. Furthermore, research with children or withparticipants who have impairments that would limitunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>/or communication requires specialsafeguard<strong>in</strong>g procedures.e. Sometimes the design of a study makes necessarythe use of concealment or deception. When this is thecase, the researcher has a special responsibility <strong>to</strong>:(i) determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the use of such techniques is justifiedby the study’s prospective scientific or educationalvalue; (ii) determ<strong>in</strong>e whether alternative procedures areavailable that do not use concealment or deception; <strong>and</strong>(iii) ensure that the participants are provided with sufficientexplanation as soon as possible.f. The researcher respects the right of any <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>to</strong> refuse <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study or <strong>to</strong> withdrawfrom participat<strong>in</strong>g at any time. The researcher’sobligation <strong>in</strong> this regard is especially important whenhe or she is <strong>in</strong> a position of authority or <strong>in</strong>fluence overthe participants <strong>in</strong> a study. Such positions of authority<strong>in</strong>clude, but are not limited <strong>to</strong>, situations <strong>in</strong> whichresearch participation is required as part of employmen<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> which the participant is a student, client, oremployee of the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r.g. The researcher protects all participants fromphysical <strong>and</strong> mental discomfort, harm, <strong>and</strong> danger thatmay arise from participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a study. If risks of suchconsequences exist, the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong>forms the participan<strong>to</strong>f that fact. <strong>Research</strong> procedures likely <strong>to</strong> causeserious or last<strong>in</strong>g harm <strong>to</strong> a participant are not usedunless the failure <strong>to</strong> use these procedures might exposethe participant <strong>to</strong> risk of greater harm, or unless theresearch has great potential benefit <strong>and</strong> fully <strong>in</strong>formed<strong>and</strong> voluntary consent is obta<strong>in</strong>ed from each participant.All participants must be <strong>in</strong>formed as <strong>to</strong> how they cancontact the researcher with<strong>in</strong> a reasonable time periodfollow<strong>in</strong>g their participation should stress or potentialharm arise.h. After the data are collected, the researcher providesall participants with <strong>in</strong>formation about the nature of thestudy <strong>and</strong> does his or her best <strong>to</strong> clear up any misconceptionsthat may have developed. Where scientific or humanevalues justify delay<strong>in</strong>g or withhold<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>formation,the researcher has a special responsibility <strong>to</strong>carefully supervise the research <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> ensure that thereare no damag<strong>in</strong>g consequences for the participant.i. Where the procedures of a study result <strong>in</strong> undesirableconsequences for any participant, the researcher hasthe responsibility <strong>to</strong> detect <strong>and</strong> remove or correct theseconsequences, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g long-term effects.j. Information obta<strong>in</strong>ed about a research participantdur<strong>in</strong>g the course of an <strong>in</strong>vestigation is confidentialunless otherwise agreed upon <strong>in</strong> advance. When thepossibility exists that others may obta<strong>in</strong> access <strong>to</strong> such


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHCl<strong>in</strong>ical Trials—Desirable or Not?Cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials are the f<strong>in</strong>al test of a new drug. They offer anopportunity for drug companies <strong>to</strong> prove that new <strong>and</strong> previouslyunused medic<strong>in</strong>es are safe <strong>and</strong> effective <strong>to</strong> use by giv<strong>in</strong>gsuch medic<strong>in</strong>es <strong>to</strong> volunteers. Recently, however, there has beenan <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the number of compla<strong>in</strong>ts aga<strong>in</strong>st such trials. Themost flagrant example was recently cited <strong>in</strong> the San FranciscoChronicle.* A scientist gave a volunteer participant <strong>in</strong> one suchtrial what turned out <strong>to</strong> be a lethal dose of an experimental drug.There has been an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the number of cl<strong>in</strong>icaltrials, as well as a correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the number ofvolunteers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> such trials. In 1995 about 500,000 volunteersparticipated; by 1999 the number had jumped <strong>to</strong> 700,000.†Another concern is that some of the physicians who conductsuch trials may have a f<strong>in</strong>ancial stake <strong>in</strong> the outcome. No uniformpolicy currently exists on the disclosure of <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs’f<strong>in</strong>ancial <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>to</strong> patients who participate <strong>in</strong> such trials.Proponents of cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials argue that, when properly conducted,cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials have paved the way for new medic<strong>in</strong>es<strong>and</strong> procedures that have saved many lives. Volunteers canga<strong>in</strong> access <strong>to</strong> promis<strong>in</strong>g drugs long before they are available<strong>to</strong> the general public. And patients usually get excellent carefrom physicians <strong>and</strong> nurses while they are undergo<strong>in</strong>g suchtrials. Last, but not least, such care often is free.What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Are cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials justified?*T. Abate (2001). Maybe conflicts of <strong>in</strong>terest are scar<strong>in</strong>g cl<strong>in</strong>ical trialpatients. San Francisco Chronicle, May 28.†Report issued at the Association of Cl<strong>in</strong>ical <strong>Research</strong> ProfessionalsConvention, San Francisco, California, May 20, 2001.<strong>in</strong>formation, this possibility, <strong>to</strong>gether with the plansfor protect<strong>in</strong>g confidentiality, is expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> theparticipant as part of the procedure for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formed consent. 1The above statement of ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples suggeststhree very important issues that every researcher shouldaddress: protect<strong>in</strong>g participants from harm, ensur<strong>in</strong>gconfidentiality of research data, <strong>and</strong> the question of deceptionof subjects. <strong>How</strong> can these issues be addressed,<strong>and</strong> how can the <strong>in</strong>terests of the subjects <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>research be protected?Protect<strong>in</strong>g Participantsfrom HarmIt is a fundamental responsibility of every researcher <strong>to</strong>do all <strong>in</strong> his or her power <strong>to</strong> ensure that participants <strong>in</strong>a research study are protected from physical or psychologicalharm, discomfort, or danger that may arise due<strong>to</strong> research procedures. This is perhaps the most importantethical decision of all. Any sort of study that islikely <strong>to</strong> cause last<strong>in</strong>g, or even serious, harm or discomfort<strong>to</strong> any participant should not be conducted, unlessthe research has the potential <strong>to</strong> provide <strong>in</strong>formation ofextreme benefit <strong>to</strong> human be<strong>in</strong>gs. Even when this maybe the case, participants should be fully <strong>in</strong>formed of thedangers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> no way required <strong>to</strong> participate.A further responsibility <strong>in</strong> protect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualsfrom harm is obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g their consent if they may beexposed <strong>to</strong> any risk. (Figure 4.1 shows an example ofa consent form.) Fortunately, almost all educational research<strong>in</strong>volves activities that are with<strong>in</strong> the cus<strong>to</strong>mary,usual procedures of schools or other agencies <strong>and</strong>as such <strong>in</strong>volve little or no risk. Legislation recognizesthis by specifically exempt<strong>in</strong>g most categories ofeducational research from formal review processes.Nevertheless, researchers should carefully considerwhether there is any likelihood of risk <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>and</strong>, ifthere is, provide full <strong>in</strong>formation followed by formalconsent by participants (or their guardians). Three importantethical questions <strong>to</strong> ask about harm <strong>in</strong> anystudy are:1. Could people be harmed (physically or psychologically)dur<strong>in</strong>g the study?2. If so, could the study be conducted <strong>in</strong> another way <strong>to</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d out what the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> know?3. Is the <strong>in</strong>formation that may be obta<strong>in</strong>ed from thisstudy so important that it warrants possible harm <strong>to</strong>the participants?These are difficult questions, <strong>and</strong> they deserve discussion<strong>and</strong> consideration by all researchers.55


56 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 4.1 Example ofa Consent FormCONSENT TO SERVE AS A SUBJECT IN RESEARCHI consent <strong>to</strong> serve as a subject <strong>in</strong> the research <strong>in</strong>vestigation entitled: __________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________The nature <strong>and</strong> general purpose of the research procedure <strong>and</strong> the knownrisks <strong>in</strong>volved have been expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> me by ________________________________.The <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r is authorized <strong>to</strong> proceed on the underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g that I mayterm<strong>in</strong>ate my service as a subject at any time I so desire.I underst<strong>and</strong> the known risks are: _______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I underst<strong>and</strong> also that it is not possible <strong>to</strong> identify all potential risks <strong>in</strong> anexperimental procedure, <strong>and</strong> I believe that reasonable safeguards have beentaken <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imize both the known <strong>and</strong> the potentially unknown risks.Witness _______________________________Signed __________________________(subject)To be reta<strong>in</strong>ed by the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r.Date ____________________________Ensur<strong>in</strong>g Confidentialityof <strong>Research</strong> DataOnce the data <strong>in</strong> a study have been collected, researchersshould make sure that no one else (other than perhaps afew key research assistants) has access <strong>to</strong> the data.Whenever possible, the names of the subjects should beremoved from all data collection forms. This can be doneby assign<strong>in</strong>g a number or letter <strong>to</strong> each form, or subjectscan be asked <strong>to</strong> furnish <strong>in</strong>formation anonymously. Whenthis is done, not even the researcher can l<strong>in</strong>k the data <strong>to</strong> aparticular subject. Sometimes, however, it is important<strong>in</strong> a study <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>in</strong>dividual subjects. When this isthe case, the l<strong>in</strong>kage system should be carefully guarded.All subjects should be assured that any data collectedfrom or about them will be held <strong>in</strong> confidence. Thenames of <strong>in</strong>dividual subjects should never be used <strong>in</strong>any publications that describe the research. And all participants<strong>in</strong> a study should always have the right <strong>to</strong> withdrawfrom the study or <strong>to</strong> request that data collectedabout them not be used.Should Subjects Be Deceived?The issue of deception is particularly troublesome.Many studies cannot be carried out unless some deceptionof subjects takes place. It is often difficult <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>dnaturalistic situations <strong>in</strong> which certa<strong>in</strong> behaviors occurfrequently. For example, a researcher may have <strong>to</strong> waita long time for a teacher <strong>to</strong> re<strong>in</strong>force students <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong>way. It may be much easier for the researcher <strong>to</strong> observethe effects of such re<strong>in</strong>forcement by employ<strong>in</strong>gthe teacher as a confederate.Sometimes it is better <strong>to</strong> deceive subjects than <strong>to</strong> causethem pa<strong>in</strong> or trauma, as <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g a particular researchquestion might require. The famous Milgramstudy of obedience is a good example. 2 In this study, subjectswere ordered <strong>to</strong> give <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly severe electricshocks <strong>to</strong> another subject whom they could not see sitt<strong>in</strong>gbeh<strong>in</strong>d a screen. What they did not know was that the <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>to</strong> whom they thought they were adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>gthe shocks was a confederate of the experimenter, <strong>and</strong> noshocks were actually be<strong>in</strong>g adm<strong>in</strong>istered. The dependent


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHPatients Given Fake Blood WithoutTheir Knowledge*Failed Study Used Change <strong>in</strong> FDA RulesASSOCIATED PRESSChicago—A company conducted an ill-fated blood substitutetrial without the <strong>in</strong>formed consent of patients <strong>in</strong> thestudy—some of whom died, federal officials say.Baxter International Inc. was able <strong>to</strong> test the substitute,known as HemAssist, without consent because of a 1996change <strong>in</strong> federal Food <strong>and</strong> Drug Adm<strong>in</strong>istration regulations.The changes, which broke a 50-year st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>to</strong> get consentfor nearly all experiments on humans, were designed <strong>to</strong>help research <strong>in</strong> emergency medic<strong>in</strong>e that could not happen ifdoc<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong>ok the time <strong>to</strong> get consent.But the problems with the HemAssist trial are prompt<strong>in</strong>gsome medical ethicists <strong>to</strong> question the rule change.*San Francisco Chronicle, January 18, 1999.“People get <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> their detriment withoutany knowledge of it,” George Annas, a professor of health law atthe Bos<strong>to</strong>n University School of Public Health, <strong>to</strong>ld the ChicagoTribune. “We use people. What’s the justification for that?”No other company has conducted a no-consent experimentunder the rule, FDA officials said.Baxter officials halted their cl<strong>in</strong>ical trial of HemAssist lastspr<strong>in</strong>g after review<strong>in</strong>g data on the first 100 trauma patientsplaced <strong>in</strong> the nationwide study.Of the 52 critically ill patients given the substitute, 24 died,represent<strong>in</strong>g a 46.2 percent mortality rate. The Deerfield, Ill.-based company had projected 42.6 percent mortality for criticallyill patients seek<strong>in</strong>g emergency treatment.There has been an <strong>in</strong>tense push <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d a blood substitute <strong>to</strong>ease the effects of whole-blood shortages.<strong>Research</strong>ers say artificial blood lasts longer than conventionalblood, elim<strong>in</strong>ates the time-consum<strong>in</strong>g need <strong>to</strong> matchblood types <strong>and</strong> wipes out the risk of contam<strong>in</strong>ation from suchviruses as HIV <strong>and</strong> hepatitis.The 1996 regulations require a level of community notificationthat is not used <strong>in</strong> most scientific studies, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gcommunity meet<strong>in</strong>gs, news releases <strong>and</strong> post-study follow-up.No lawsuits have arisen from the blood substitute trial,Baxter officials said.variable was the level of shock subjects adm<strong>in</strong>istered beforethey refused <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister any more. Out of a <strong>to</strong>tal of40 subjects who participated <strong>in</strong> the study, 26 followed the“orders” of the experimenter <strong>and</strong> (so they thought) adm<strong>in</strong>isteredthe maximum shock possible of 450 volts!Even though no shocks were actually adm<strong>in</strong>istered, publicationof the study results produced widespread controversy.Many people felt the study was unethical. Othersargued that the importance of the study <strong>and</strong> its results justifiedthe deception. Notice that the study raises questionsabout not only deception but also harm, s<strong>in</strong>ce someparticipants could have suffered emotionally from laterconsideration of their actions.Current professional guidel<strong>in</strong>es are as follows:Whenever possible, a researcher should conduct thestudy us<strong>in</strong>g methods that do not require deception.If alternative methods cannot be devised, theresearcher must determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the use of deceptionis justified by the prospective study’s scientific,educational, or applied value.If the participants are deceived, the researcher mustensure that they are provided with sufficient explanationas soon as possible.Perhaps the most serious problem <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g deceptionis what it may ultimately do <strong>to</strong> the reputation of the scientificcommunity. If people <strong>in</strong> general beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of scientists<strong>and</strong> researchers as liars, or as <strong>in</strong>dividuals who misrepresentwhat they are about, the overall image of sciencemay suffer. Fewer <strong>and</strong> fewer people will be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> participate<strong>in</strong> research <strong>in</strong>vestigations. As a result, the searchfor reliable knowledge about our world may be impeded.Three ExamplesInvolv<strong>in</strong>g Ethical ConcernsHere are brief descriptions of three research studies. Let usconsider each <strong>in</strong> terms of (1) present<strong>in</strong>g possible harm <strong>to</strong> theparticipants, (2) ensur<strong>in</strong>g the confidentiality of the researchdata, <strong>and</strong> (3) know<strong>in</strong>gly practic<strong>in</strong>g deception. (Figure 4.2illustrates some examples of unethical research practices.)Study 1. The researcher plans <strong>to</strong> observe (unobtrusively)students <strong>in</strong> each of 40 classrooms—eight visitseach of 40 m<strong>in</strong>utes’ duration. The purpose of these57


58 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“We are required <strong>to</strong> askyou <strong>to</strong> sign this consentform. You needn’t readit; it’s just rout<strong>in</strong>e.”“A few cases seemed quitedifferent from the rest,so we deleted them.”“Yes, as a student at thisuniversity you are required<strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> this study.”“There is no need <strong>to</strong> tellany of the parents that we aremodify<strong>in</strong>g the school lunchdiet for this study.”“Requir<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong>participate <strong>in</strong> class discussionsmight be harmful <strong>to</strong> some,but it is necessary for ourresearch.”Figure 4.2 Examples of Unethical <strong>Research</strong> Practicesobservations is <strong>to</strong> look for relationships between the behaviorof students <strong>and</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> teacher behavior patterns.Possibility of Harm <strong>to</strong> the Participants. This studywould fall with<strong>in</strong> the exempt category regard<strong>in</strong>g thepossibility of harm <strong>to</strong> the participants. Neither teachersnor students are placed under any risk, <strong>and</strong> observationis an accepted part of school practice.Confidentiality of the <strong>Research</strong> Data. The only issuethat is likely <strong>to</strong> arise <strong>in</strong> this regard is the possible butunlikely observation of a teacher behav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an illegalor unethical way (e.g., physically or verbally abus<strong>in</strong>g astudent). In the former case, the researcher is legallyrequired <strong>to</strong> report the <strong>in</strong>cident. In the latter case, the researchermust weigh the ethical dilemma <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>not report<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>cident aga<strong>in</strong>st that of violat<strong>in</strong>g assurancesof confidentiality.Deception. Although no outright deception is <strong>in</strong>volved,the researcher is go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> have <strong>to</strong> give the teachers arationale for observ<strong>in</strong>g them. If the specific teacher characteristicbe<strong>in</strong>g observed (e.g., need <strong>to</strong> control) is given,the behavior <strong>in</strong> question is likely <strong>to</strong> be affected. To avoidthis, the researcher might expla<strong>in</strong> that the purpose of thestudy is <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate different teach<strong>in</strong>g styles—withoutdivulg<strong>in</strong>g the specifics. To us, this does not seem <strong>to</strong> be unethical.An alternative is <strong>to</strong> tell the teachers that specificdetails cannot be divulged until after data have been collectedfor fear of chang<strong>in</strong>g their behavior. If this alternativeis pursued, some teachers might refuse <strong>to</strong> participate.Study 2. The researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> study the value ofa workshop on suicide prevention for high school students.The workshop is <strong>to</strong> consist of three 2-hour meet<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> which danger signals, causes of suicide, <strong>and</strong>community resources that provide counsel<strong>in</strong>g will bediscussed. Students will volunteer, <strong>and</strong> half will be assigned<strong>to</strong> a comparison group that will not participate<strong>in</strong> the workshop. Outcomes will be assessed by compar<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>formation learned <strong>and</strong> attitudes of thoseattend<strong>in</strong>g the meet<strong>in</strong>gs with those who do not attend.Possibility of Harm <strong>to</strong> the Participants. Whether thisstudy fits the exempt category with regard <strong>to</strong> any possibilityof risk for the participants depends on the extent<strong>to</strong> which it is atypical for the school <strong>in</strong> question. Weth<strong>in</strong>k that <strong>in</strong> most schools, this study would probably beconsidered atypical. In addition, it is conceivable thatthe material presented could place a student at risk bystirr<strong>in</strong>g up emotional reactions. In any case, the researchershould <strong>in</strong>form parents as <strong>to</strong> the nature of thestudy <strong>and</strong> the possible risks <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> theirconsent for their children <strong>to</strong> participate.Confidentiality of the <strong>Research</strong> Data. No problemsare foreseen <strong>in</strong> this regard, although confidentiality as <strong>to</strong>what will occur dur<strong>in</strong>g the workshop cannot, of course,be guaranteed.Deception. No problems are foreseen.Study 3. The researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> study the effects of“failure” versus “success” by teach<strong>in</strong>g junior high students


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHAn Exampleof Unethical <strong>Research</strong>Aseries of studies reported <strong>in</strong> the 1950s <strong>and</strong> 1960s receivedwidespread attention <strong>in</strong> psychology <strong>and</strong> education <strong>and</strong>earned their author much fame, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a knighthood. Theyaddressed the question of how much of one’s performance onIQ tests was likely <strong>to</strong> be hereditary <strong>and</strong> how much was due <strong>to</strong>environmental fac<strong>to</strong>rs.Several groups of children were studied over time, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gidentical tw<strong>in</strong>s raised <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> apart, fraternal tw<strong>in</strong>sraised <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> apart, <strong>and</strong> same-family sibl<strong>in</strong>gs. Theresults were widely cited <strong>to</strong> support the conclusion that IQ isabout 80 percent hereditary <strong>and</strong> 20 percent environmental.Some <strong>in</strong>itial questions were raised when another researcherfound a considerably lower hereditary percentage.Subsequent detailed <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the <strong>in</strong>itial studies* revealedhighly suspicious statistical treatment of data, <strong>in</strong>adequatespecification of procedures, <strong>and</strong> questionable adjustmen<strong>to</strong>f scores, all suggest<strong>in</strong>g unethical massag<strong>in</strong>g of data.Such <strong>in</strong>stances, which are reported occasionally, underscorethe importance of repeat<strong>in</strong>g studies, as well as the essentialrequirement that all procedures <strong>and</strong> data be available for publicscrut<strong>in</strong>y.*L. Kam<strong>in</strong> (1974). The science <strong>and</strong> politics of I.Q. New York: JohnWiley.a mo<strong>to</strong>r skill dur<strong>in</strong>g a series of six 10-m<strong>in</strong>ute <strong>in</strong>structionalperiods. After each tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g period, the students will begiven feedback on their performance as compared withthat of other students. In order <strong>to</strong> control extraneousvariables (such as coord<strong>in</strong>ation), the researcher plans <strong>to</strong>r<strong>and</strong>omly divide the students <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two groups—half willbe <strong>to</strong>ld that their performance was “relatively poor” <strong>and</strong>the other half will be <strong>to</strong>ld that they are “do<strong>in</strong>g well.” Theiractual performance will be ignored.Possibility of Harm <strong>to</strong> the Participants. This studypresents several problems. Some students <strong>in</strong> the “failure”group may well suffer emotional distress. Althoughstudents are normally given similar feedback on theirperformance <strong>in</strong> most schools, feedback <strong>in</strong> this study(be<strong>in</strong>g arbitrary) may conflict dramatically with theirprior experience. The researcher cannot properly <strong>in</strong>formstudents, or their parents, about the deceptive nature ofthe study, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>to</strong> do so would <strong>in</strong> effect destroy the study.Confidentiality of the <strong>Research</strong> Data. Confidentialitydoes not appear <strong>to</strong> be an issue <strong>in</strong> this study.Deception. The deception of participants is clearly anissue. One alternative is <strong>to</strong> base feedback on actual performance.The difficulty here is that each student’s extensiveprior his<strong>to</strong>ry will affect both <strong>in</strong>dividual performance<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of feedback, thus confound<strong>in</strong>gthe results. Some, but not all, of these extraneous variablescan be controlled (perhaps by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g schoolrecords for data on past his<strong>to</strong>ry or by pretest<strong>in</strong>gstudents). Another alternative is <strong>to</strong> weaken the experi-mental treatment by try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> lessen the possibility ofemotional distress (e.g., by say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> participants <strong>in</strong> thefailure group, “You did not do quite as well as most”)<strong>and</strong> conf<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> one time period. Both ofthese alternatives, however, would lessen the chances ofany relationship emerg<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Research</strong> with ChildrenStudies us<strong>in</strong>g children as participants present some specialissues for researchers. The young are more vulnerable<strong>in</strong> some respects, have fewer legal rights, <strong>and</strong> maynot underst<strong>and</strong> the language of <strong>in</strong>formed consent.Therefore, the follow<strong>in</strong>g specific guidel<strong>in</strong>es need <strong>to</strong> beconsidered.Informed consent of parents or of those legally designatedas caretakers is required for participantsdef<strong>in</strong>ed as m<strong>in</strong>ors. Signers must be provided all necessary<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> appropriate language <strong>and</strong> musthave the opportunity <strong>to</strong> refuse. (Figure 4.3 shows anexample of a consent form for a m<strong>in</strong>or.)<strong>Research</strong>ers do not present themselves as diagnosticiansor counselors <strong>in</strong> report<strong>in</strong>g results <strong>to</strong> parents,nor do they report <strong>in</strong>formation given by a child <strong>in</strong>confidence.Children may never be coerced <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> participation <strong>in</strong> astudy.Any form of remuneration for the child’s servicesdoes not affect the application of these (<strong>and</strong> other)ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples.59


60 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 4.3 Example of aConsent Form for a M<strong>in</strong>or<strong>to</strong> Participate <strong>in</strong> a <strong>Research</strong>StudyAUTHORIZATION FOR A MINOR TO SERVEAS A SUBJECT IN RESEARCHI authorize the service of _____________________________________ as a subject<strong>in</strong> the research <strong>in</strong>vestigation entitled: __________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________The nature <strong>and</strong> general purpose of the research procedure <strong>and</strong> the knownrisks have been expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> me. I underst<strong>and</strong> that ___________________________(name of m<strong>in</strong>or)will be given a preservice explanation of the research <strong>and</strong> that he/she may decl<strong>in</strong>e<strong>to</strong> serve. Further, I underst<strong>and</strong> that he/she may term<strong>in</strong>ate his/her service <strong>in</strong> thisresearch at any time he/she so desires.I underst<strong>and</strong> the known risks are: ________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I underst<strong>and</strong> also that it is not possible <strong>to</strong> identify all potential risks <strong>in</strong> anexperimental procedure, <strong>and</strong> I believe that reasonable safeguards have beentaken <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imize both the known <strong>and</strong> the potential but unknown risks.I agree further <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>demnify <strong>and</strong> hold harmless S. F. State University <strong>and</strong> itsagents <strong>and</strong> employees from any <strong>and</strong> all liability, actions, or causes of actions thatmay accrue <strong>to</strong> the subject m<strong>in</strong>or as a result of his/her activities for which thisconsent is granted.Witness _______________________________Signed ___________________________(parent or guardian)To be reta<strong>in</strong>ed by researcher.Date ____________________________Regulation of <strong>Research</strong>The regulation most directly affect<strong>in</strong>g researchers is theNational <strong>Research</strong> Act of 1974. It requires that all research<strong>in</strong>stitutions receiv<strong>in</strong>g federal funds establishwhat are known as Institutional Review Boards <strong>to</strong> review<strong>and</strong> approve research projects. Such a review musttake place whether the research is <strong>to</strong> be done by a s<strong>in</strong>gleresearcher or a group of researchers. In the case of federallyfunded <strong>in</strong>vestigations, failure <strong>to</strong> comply can meanthat the entire <strong>in</strong>stitution (e.g., a university) will lose allof its federal support (e.g., veterans’ benefits, scholarshipmoney). Needless <strong>to</strong> say, this is a severe penalty.The federal agency that has the major responsibilityfor establish<strong>in</strong>g the guidel<strong>in</strong>es for research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>ghuman subjects is the Department of Health <strong>and</strong> HumanServices (HHS).The membership of an IRB must have at least fivemembers, consist of both men <strong>and</strong> women, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludeat least one nonscientist. It must <strong>in</strong>clude one person notaffiliated with the <strong>in</strong>stitution. Individuals competent <strong>in</strong> aparticularly relevant area may be <strong>in</strong>vited <strong>to</strong> assist <strong>in</strong> areview but may not vote. Furthermore, <strong>in</strong>dividuals witha conflict of <strong>in</strong>terest must be excluded, although theymay provide <strong>in</strong>formation.If the IRB regularly reviews research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g avulnerable category of subjects (e.g., such as studies <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gthe mentally retarded), the board must <strong>in</strong>cludeone or more <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are primarily concernedwith the welfare of these subjects.


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHEthical or Not?In September 1998, a U.S. District Court judge halted astudy begun <strong>in</strong> 1994 <strong>to</strong> evaluate the effectiveness of theU.S. Job Corps program. For two years, the researchers hadr<strong>and</strong>omly assigned 1 out of every 12 eligible applicants <strong>to</strong> acontrol group that was denied service for three years—a <strong>to</strong>talof 6,000 applicants. If applicants refused <strong>to</strong> sign a waiveragree<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study, they were <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> reapplytwo years later. The class action lawsuit alleged psychological,emotional, <strong>and</strong> economic harm <strong>to</strong> the control subjects.The basis for the judge’s decision was a failure <strong>to</strong> follow thefederal law that required the methodology <strong>to</strong> be subject <strong>to</strong>public review. A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary settlement pledged <strong>to</strong> locate allof the control subjects by the year 2000, <strong>in</strong>vite them <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> theJob Corps (if still eligible), <strong>and</strong> pay each person $1,000.*In a letter <strong>to</strong> the edi<strong>to</strong>r† of Mother Jones <strong>in</strong> April 1999,however, Judith M. Gueron, the President of ManpowerDemonstration <strong>Research</strong> Corporation (not the companyawarded the evaluation grant) defended the study on twogrounds: (1) s<strong>in</strong>ce there were only limited available open<strong>in</strong>gsfor the program, r<strong>and</strong>om selection of qualified applicants “isarguably fairer” than first-come, first-served; <strong>and</strong> (2) the allegedharm <strong>to</strong> those rejected is unknown, s<strong>in</strong>ce they were free<strong>to</strong> seek other employment or tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?*J. Price (1999). Job Corps lottery. Mother Jones, January/February,pp. 21–22.†Backtalk (1999). Mother Jones, April, p. 13.The IRB exam<strong>in</strong>es all proposed research with respect<strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> basic criteria. The board can request that a studybe modified <strong>to</strong> meet the criteria before it will be approved.If a proposed study fails <strong>to</strong> satisfy any one of these criteria,the study will not be approved (see Table 4.1).TABLE 4.1Criteria for IRB ApprovalM<strong>in</strong>imization of risk <strong>to</strong> participants (e.g., by us<strong>in</strong>gprocedures that do not unnecessarily expose subjects<strong>to</strong> risk).Risks that may occur are reasonable <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong>benefits that are anticipated.Equitable selection—i.e., the proposed research doesnot discrim<strong>in</strong>ate among <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the population.Protection of vulnerable <strong>in</strong>dividuals (e.g., children,pregnant women, prisoners, mentally disabled oreconomically disadvantaged persons, etc.).Informed consent—researchers must provide complete<strong>in</strong>formation about all aspects of the proposed studythat might be on <strong>in</strong>terest or concern <strong>to</strong> a potentialparticipant, <strong>and</strong> this must be presented <strong>in</strong> a form thatparticipants can easily underst<strong>and</strong>.Participants have the right <strong>to</strong> withdraw from the studyat any time without penalty.Informed consent will be appropriately documented.Moni<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g of the data be<strong>in</strong>g collected <strong>to</strong> ensure thesafety of the participants.Privacy <strong>and</strong> confidentiality—ensur<strong>in</strong>g that any <strong>and</strong> all<strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed dur<strong>in</strong>g a study is not released <strong>to</strong>outside <strong>in</strong>dividuals where it might have embarrass<strong>in</strong>g ordamag<strong>in</strong>g consequences.IRB Boards classify research proposals <strong>in</strong> three categories:Category I (Exempt Review)—the proposed studypresents no possible risk <strong>to</strong> adult participants (e.g., ananonymous mailed survey on <strong>in</strong>nocuous <strong>to</strong>pics or ananonymous observation of public behavior). This typeof study is exempt from the requirement of <strong>in</strong>formedconsent.Category II (Expedited Review)—the proposedstudy presents no more than m<strong>in</strong>imal risk <strong>to</strong> participants.A typical example would be a study of <strong>in</strong>dividualor group behavior of adults where there is nopsychological <strong>in</strong>tervention or deception <strong>in</strong>volved. Thiscategory of research does not require written documentationof <strong>in</strong>formed consent, although oral consentis required. Most classroom research projects fall <strong>in</strong>this category.Category III (Full Review)—any proposal that <strong>in</strong>cludesquestionable elements, such as research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gspecial populations, unusual equipment or procedures,deception, <strong>in</strong>tervention, or some form of <strong>in</strong>vasivemeasurement. A meet<strong>in</strong>g of all IRB members is required,<strong>and</strong> the researcher must appear <strong>in</strong> person <strong>to</strong> discuss<strong>and</strong> answer questions about the research.The question of risk for participants is of particular<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the IRB. The board may term<strong>in</strong>ate a studyif it appears that serious harm <strong>to</strong> subjects is likely <strong>to</strong>occur. Any <strong>and</strong> all potential risk(s) <strong>to</strong> subjects must be61


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHDepartment of Health <strong>and</strong> HumanServices Revised Regulations for<strong>Research</strong> with Human SubjectsThe revised guidel<strong>in</strong>es exempt many projects from regulationby HHS. Below is a list of projects now free of theguidel<strong>in</strong>es.1. <strong>Research</strong> conducted <strong>in</strong> educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs, such as <strong>in</strong>structionalstrategy research or studies on the effectivenessof educational techniques, curricula, or classroom managementmethods.2. <strong>Research</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g educational tests (cognitive, diagnostic,aptitude, <strong>and</strong> achievement), provided that subjects rema<strong>in</strong>anonymous.3. Survey or <strong>in</strong>terview procedures, except where all of thefollow<strong>in</strong>g conditions prevail:a. Participants could be identified.b. Participants’ responses, if they became public, couldplace the subject at risk on crim<strong>in</strong>al or civil charges orcould affect the subjects’ f<strong>in</strong>ancial or occupationalst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.c. <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong>volves “sensitive aspects” of the participant’sbehavior, such as illegal conduct, drug use, sexualbehavior, or alcohol use.4. Observation of public behavior (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g observation byparticipants), except where all three of the conditions listed<strong>in</strong> item 3 above are applicable.5. The collection or study of documents, records, exist<strong>in</strong>gdata, pathological specimens, or diagnostic specimens ifthese sources are available <strong>to</strong> the public or if the <strong>in</strong>formationobta<strong>in</strong>ed from the sources rema<strong>in</strong>s anonymous.m<strong>in</strong>imized. What this means is that any risk should notbe any greater than those ord<strong>in</strong>arily encountered <strong>in</strong>daily life or dur<strong>in</strong>g the performance of rout<strong>in</strong>e physicalor psychological exam<strong>in</strong>ations or tests.Some researchers were unhappy with the regulationsthat were issued <strong>in</strong> 1974 by HHS because they felt thatthe rules <strong>in</strong>terfered unnecessarily with risk-free projects.Their opposition resulted <strong>in</strong> a 1981 set of revisedguidel<strong>in</strong>es, as shown <strong>in</strong> the More About <strong>Research</strong> boxon this page. These guidel<strong>in</strong>es apply <strong>to</strong> all researchfunded by HHS. As mentioned above, InstitutionalReview Boards determ<strong>in</strong>e which studies qualify <strong>to</strong> beexempt from the guidel<strong>in</strong>es.Another law affect<strong>in</strong>g research is the Family PrivacyAct of 1974, also known as the Buckley Amendment. Itis <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> protect the privacy of students’educationalrecords. One of its provisions is that data that identifiesstudents may not, with some exceptions, be made availablewithout permission from the student or, if underlegal age, parents or legal guardians. Consent formsmust specify what data will be disclosed, for what purposes<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> whom.The relationship between the current guidel<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong>qualitative research is not as clear as it is for quantitativeresearch. In recent years, therefore, there have been anumber of suggestions for a specific code of ethics forqualitative research. 3 In quantitative studies, subjectscan be <strong>to</strong>ld the content <strong>and</strong> the possible dangers <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> a study. In qualitative studies, however, the relationshipbetween research <strong>and</strong> participant evolves over time.As Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen suggest, do<strong>in</strong>g qualitative researchwith <strong>in</strong>formants can be “more like hav<strong>in</strong>g afriendship than a contract. The people who are studiedhave a say <strong>in</strong> regulat<strong>in</strong>g the relationship <strong>and</strong> they cont<strong>in</strong>uouslymake decisions about their participation.” 4 As a result,Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen offer the follow<strong>in</strong>g suggestionsfor qualitative researchers that might be consideredwhen the criteria used by an IRB may not apply: 51. Avoid research sites where <strong>in</strong>formants may feelcoerced <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the research.2. Honor the privacy of <strong>in</strong>formants—f<strong>in</strong>d a way <strong>to</strong>recruit <strong>in</strong>formants so that they may choose <strong>to</strong> participate<strong>in</strong> the study.3. Tell participants who are be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviewed howlong the <strong>in</strong>terview will take.4. Unless otherwise agreed <strong>to</strong>, the identities of <strong>in</strong>formantsshould be protected so that the <strong>in</strong>formation collecteddoes not embarrass or otherwise harm them.Anonymity should extend not only <strong>to</strong> written reportsbut also <strong>to</strong> the verbal report<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>formation.5. Treat <strong>in</strong>formants with respect <strong>and</strong> seek their cooperation<strong>in</strong> the research. Informants should be <strong>to</strong>ld ofthe researcher’s <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> they should give theirpermission for the researcher <strong>to</strong> proceed. Writtenconsent should always be obta<strong>in</strong>ed.62


CHAPTER 4 Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 636. Make it clear <strong>to</strong> all participants <strong>in</strong> a study the termsof any agreement negotiated with them.7. Tell the truth when f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are written up <strong>and</strong> reported.Mail <strong>in</strong> a separate card <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that theycompleted the questionnaire.One further legal matter should be mentioned.At<strong>to</strong>rneys, physicians, <strong>and</strong> members of the clergy areprotected by laws concern<strong>in</strong>g privileged communications(i.e., they are protected by law from hav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>reveal <strong>in</strong>formation given <strong>to</strong> them <strong>in</strong> confidence).<strong>Research</strong>ers do not have this protection. It is possible,therefore, that any subjects who admit, on a questionnaire,<strong>to</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g committed a crime could be arrested<strong>and</strong> prosecuted. As you can see, it would be a risktherefore for the participants <strong>in</strong> a research study <strong>to</strong>admit <strong>to</strong> a researcher that they had participated <strong>in</strong> acrime. If such <strong>in</strong>formation is required <strong>to</strong> atta<strong>in</strong> the goalsof a study, a researcher can avoid the problem byomitt<strong>in</strong>g all forms of identification from the questionnaire.When mailed questionnaires are used, theresearcher can keep track of nonrespondents by hav<strong>in</strong>geach participant mail <strong>in</strong> a separate card <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g thatthey completed the questionnaire.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes, practice withkey terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.BASIC ETHICAL PRINCIPLESEthics refers <strong>to</strong> questions of right <strong>and</strong> wrong.There are a number of ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciples that all researchers should be aware of <strong>and</strong>apply <strong>to</strong> their <strong>in</strong>vestigations.The basic ethical question for all researchers <strong>to</strong> consider is whether any physical orpsychological harm could come <strong>to</strong> anyone as a result of the research.All subjects <strong>in</strong> a research study should be assured that any data collected from orabout them will be held <strong>in</strong> confidence.The term deception, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentionally mis<strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g thesubjects of a study as <strong>to</strong> some or all aspects of the research <strong>to</strong>pic.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsRESEARCH WITH CHILDRENChildren as research subjects present problems for researchers that are different fromthose of adult subjects. Children are more vulnerable, have fewer legal rights, <strong>and</strong>often do not underst<strong>and</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>formed consent.REGULATION OF RESEARCHBefore any research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g human be<strong>in</strong>gs can be conducted at an <strong>in</strong>stitution thatreceives federal funds, it must be reviewed by an <strong>in</strong>stitutional review board (IRB) atthe <strong>in</strong>stitution.The federal agency that has the major responsibility for establish<strong>in</strong>g the guidel<strong>in</strong>esfor research studies that <strong>in</strong>volve human subjects is the Department of Health <strong>and</strong>Human Services.


64 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFor DiscussionNotes1. Here are three descriptions of ideas for research. Which (if any) might have someethical problems? Why?a. A researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the effects of diet on physical development.He designs a study <strong>in</strong> which two groups are <strong>to</strong> be compared. Both groupsare composed of 11-year-olds. One group is <strong>to</strong> be given an enriched diet, high<strong>in</strong> vitam<strong>in</strong>s, that has been shown <strong>to</strong> have a strengthen<strong>in</strong>g effect on labora<strong>to</strong>ryanimals. A second group is not <strong>to</strong> be given this diet. The groups are <strong>to</strong> be selectedfrom all the 11-year-olds <strong>in</strong> an elementary school near the university where theresearcher teaches.b. A researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of music on attention span. She designs anexperimental study <strong>in</strong> which two similar high school government classes are <strong>to</strong> becompared. For a five-week period, one class has classical music played softly <strong>in</strong>the background as the teacher lectures <strong>and</strong> holds class discussions on the currentunit of study. The other class studies the same material <strong>and</strong> participates <strong>in</strong> thesame activities as the first class but does not have any music played dur<strong>in</strong>g thefive weeks.c. A researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of drugs on human be<strong>in</strong>gs. He asks thewarden of the local penitentiary for subjects <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> an experiment. Thewarden assigns several prisoners <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the experiment but does not tellthem what it is about. The prisoners are <strong>in</strong>jected with a number of drugs whoseeffects are unknown. Their reactions <strong>to</strong> the drugs are then described <strong>in</strong> detail bythe researcher.2. Which, if any, of the above studies would be exempt under the revised guidel<strong>in</strong>esshown <strong>in</strong> the More About <strong>Research</strong> box on p. 62?3. Can you suggest a research study that would present ethical problems if done withchildren but not if done with adults?4. Are there any research questions that should not be <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> schools? If so,why not?5. “Sometimes the design of a study makes necessary the use of concealment or deception.”Discuss. Can you describe a study <strong>in</strong> which deception might be justified?6. “Any sort of study that is likely <strong>to</strong> cause last<strong>in</strong>g, or even serious, harm or discomfort<strong>to</strong> any participant should not be conducted, unless the research has the potential <strong>to</strong>provide <strong>in</strong>formation of extreme benefit <strong>to</strong> human be<strong>in</strong>gs.” Would you agree? If so,why? What might be an example of such <strong>in</strong>formation?1. Adapted from the Committee on Scientific <strong>and</strong> Professional Ethics <strong>and</strong> Conduct (1981). Ethical pr<strong>in</strong>ciplesof psychologists. American Psychologist, 36: 633–638. Copyright 1981 by the American Psychological Association.Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission.2. S. Milgram (1967). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology,67: 371–378.3. For example, see J. Cassell <strong>and</strong> M. Wax (Eds.) (1980). Ethical problems <strong>in</strong> fieldwork. Social Problems27 (3); B. K. Curry <strong>and</strong> J. E. Davis (1995). Represent<strong>in</strong>g: The obligations of faculty as researchers. Academe(Sept.–Oct.): 40–43; Y. L<strong>in</strong>coln (1995). Emerg<strong>in</strong>g criteria for quality <strong>in</strong> qualitative <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretive research.Qualitative Inquiry 1 (3): 275–289.4. R. C. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> S. K. Biklen (2007). Qualitative research <strong>in</strong> education: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> theory <strong>and</strong>methods. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Pearson, p. 49.5. Op cit, pp. 49–50.


CHAPTER 4 Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> 65<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 4: Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong>Us<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 4, restate the research question you developed <strong>in</strong> Problem Sheet 3.Identify any possible ethical problems <strong>in</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out such a study. <strong>How</strong> might such problemsbe remedied?Problem Sheet 4Ethics <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong>1. My research question is: ________________________________________________2. The possibilities of harm <strong>to</strong> participants (if any) are as follows: _________________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.I would h<strong>and</strong>le these problems as follows: __________________________________3. The possibilities of problems of confidentiality (if any) are as follows: ___________A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.I would h<strong>and</strong>le these problems as follows: __________________________________4. The possibilities of problems of deception (if any) are as follows: _______________I would h<strong>and</strong>le these problems as follows: __________________________________5. If you th<strong>in</strong>k your proposed study would fit the guidel<strong>in</strong>es for exempt status, state why.


5Locat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>gthe LiteratureThe Value of aLiterature ReviewTypes of SourcesSteps Involved <strong>in</strong> aLiterature SearchDef<strong>in</strong>e the Problem asPrecisely as PossibleLook through One or TwoSecondary SourcesSelect the AppropriateGeneral ReferencesFormulate Search TermsSearch the GeneralReferencesObta<strong>in</strong> Primary SourcesDo<strong>in</strong>g a ComputerSearchAn Example of a ComputerSearch<strong>Research</strong><strong>in</strong>g the World WideWebInterlibrary LoansWrit<strong>in</strong>g the LiteratureReview ReportMeta-analysis“Hey! There’s Joe.Tak<strong>in</strong>g it easy,as usual.”“No, he says he’sreview<strong>in</strong>g the literaturefor his Biology class!”OBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly why a literature review isof value.Name the steps a researcher goes through<strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a review of the literature.Describe briefly the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formationconta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> a general reference <strong>and</strong> givean example of such a source.Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between a primary<strong>and</strong> a secondary source <strong>and</strong> give anexample of each type.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the phrase“search term” <strong>and</strong> how such terms areused <strong>in</strong> literature searches.Conduct both a manual <strong>and</strong> a computersearch of the literature on a <strong>to</strong>pic of<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> you after a small amount of“h<strong>and</strong>s-on” computer time <strong>and</strong> a littlehelp from a librarian.Write a summary of your literature review.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a meta-analysis is.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Read the Guide <strong>to</strong> Electronic <strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 5.1: Library WorksheetActivity 5.2: Where Would You Look?Activity 5.3: Do a Computer Search of the LiteratureAfter a career <strong>in</strong> the military, Phil Gomez is <strong>in</strong> his first year as a teacher at an adult school <strong>in</strong> Logan, Utah. Heteaches United States his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> students who did not graduate from high school but now are try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> a diploma. He has learned the hard way, through trial <strong>and</strong> error, that there are a number of techniques thatsimply put students <strong>to</strong> sleep. He s<strong>in</strong>cerely wants <strong>to</strong> be a good teacher, but he is hav<strong>in</strong>g trouble gett<strong>in</strong>g his students<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the subject. As he is the only his<strong>to</strong>ry teacher <strong>in</strong> the school, the other teachers are not of much help.He wants <strong>to</strong> get some ideas, therefore, about other approaches, strategies, <strong>and</strong> techniques that he might use. Hedecides <strong>to</strong> do a search of the literature on teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> see what he can f<strong>in</strong>d out. But he has no idea where <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>.Should he look at books? journal articles? reports or papers? Are there references particularly geared <strong>to</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>gof American his<strong>to</strong>ry? What about the possibility of an electronic search? Where should he look?In this chapter, you will learn some answers <strong>to</strong> these (<strong>and</strong> related) questions. When you have f<strong>in</strong>ished read<strong>in</strong>g,you should have a number of ideas about how <strong>to</strong> do both a manual <strong>and</strong> an electronic search of the educationalliterature.The Value of a LiteratureReviewA literature review is helpful <strong>in</strong> two ways. It not onlyhelps researchers glean the ideas of others <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> aparticular research question, but it also lets them read aboutthe results of other (similar or related) studies. A detailedliterature review, <strong>in</strong> fact, is usually required of master’s <strong>and</strong>doc<strong>to</strong>ral students when they design a thesis. <strong>Research</strong>ersthen weigh <strong>in</strong>formation from a literature review <strong>in</strong> light oftheir own concerns <strong>and</strong> situation. Thus, there are two importantpo<strong>in</strong>ts here. <strong>Research</strong>ers need <strong>to</strong> be able not only <strong>to</strong>locate other work deal<strong>in</strong>g with their <strong>in</strong>tended area of studybut also <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> evaluate this work <strong>in</strong> terms of itsrelevance <strong>to</strong> the research question of <strong>in</strong>terest.Types of SourcesA researcher needs <strong>to</strong> be familiar with three basic typesof sources as he or she beg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong> search for <strong>in</strong>formationrelated <strong>to</strong> the research question.1. General references are the sources researchersoften refer <strong>to</strong> first. In effect, they tell where <strong>to</strong> look<strong>to</strong> locate other sources—such as articles, monographs,books, <strong>and</strong> other documents—that deal directlywith the research question. Most general referencesare either <strong>in</strong>dexes, which list the author, title,<strong>and</strong> place of publication of articles <strong>and</strong> other materials,or abstracts, which give a brief summary of variouspublications, as well as their author, title, <strong>and</strong>place of publication. An <strong>in</strong>dex frequently used by researchers<strong>in</strong> education is Current Index <strong>to</strong> Journals<strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>. A commonly used abstract is PsychologicalAbstracts.2. Primary sources are publications <strong>in</strong> which researchersreport the results of their studies. Authorscommunicate their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs directly <strong>to</strong> readers. Mostprimary sources <strong>in</strong> education are journals, such as theJournal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> or the Journal of<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Science Teach<strong>in</strong>g. These journals are usuallypublished monthly or quarterly, <strong>and</strong> the articles <strong>in</strong>them typically report on a particular research study.3. Secondary sources refer <strong>to</strong> publications <strong>in</strong> which authorsdescribe the work of others. The most common67


68 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7esecondary sources <strong>in</strong> education are textbooks. A textbook<strong>in</strong> educational psychology, for example, maydescribe several studies as a way <strong>to</strong> illustrate variousideas <strong>and</strong> concepts <strong>in</strong> psychology. Other commonlyused secondary sources <strong>in</strong>clude educational encyclopedias,research reviews, <strong>and</strong> yearbooks.<strong>Research</strong>ers who seek <strong>in</strong>formation on a given <strong>to</strong>picwould refer first <strong>to</strong> one or more general references <strong>to</strong>locate primary <strong>and</strong> secondary sources of value. For aquick overview of the problem at h<strong>and</strong>, secondarysources are probably the best bet. For detailed <strong>in</strong>formationabout the research that others have done, primarysources should be consulted.Today, there are two ma<strong>in</strong> ways <strong>to</strong> do a literaturesearch—manually, us<strong>in</strong>g the traditional paper approach,<strong>and</strong> electronically, by means of a computer. Let’s first exam<strong>in</strong>ethe traditional approach <strong>and</strong> then consider how <strong>to</strong>do a computer search. (These days, researchers usuallydo a computer search first, go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a manual search onlyif they do not come up with very much electronically.)Steps Involved<strong>in</strong> a Literature SearchThe follow<strong>in</strong>g steps are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a literature review:1. Def<strong>in</strong>e the research problem as precisely as possible.2. Look at relevant secondary sources.3. Select <strong>and</strong> peruse one or two appropriate generalreference works.4. Formulate search terms (key words or phrases)pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> the problem or question of <strong>in</strong>terest.5. Search the general references for relevant primarysources.6. Obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> read relevant primary sources, <strong>and</strong> note<strong>and</strong> summarize key po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the sources.Let us consider each of these steps <strong>in</strong> some detail.DEFINE THE PROBLEMAS PRECISELY AS POSSIBLEThe first th<strong>in</strong>g a researcher needs <strong>to</strong> do is <strong>to</strong> state the researchquestion as specifically as possible. General questionssuch as “What sorts of teach<strong>in</strong>g methods work well<strong>in</strong> the classroom?” or “<strong>How</strong> can a pr<strong>in</strong>cipal be a more effectiveleader?” are <strong>to</strong>o fuzzy <strong>to</strong> be of much help whenlook<strong>in</strong>g through a general reference. The question of<strong>in</strong>terest should be narrowed down <strong>to</strong> a specific area ofconcern. More specific questions, therefore, might be,“Is discussion more effective than us<strong>in</strong>g films <strong>to</strong> motivatestudents <strong>to</strong> learn social studies concepts?” or “Whatsorts of strategies do pr<strong>in</strong>cipals judged <strong>to</strong> be effective bytheir staffs use <strong>to</strong> improve faculty <strong>and</strong> staff morale?” Aserious effort should be made <strong>to</strong> state the question so thatit focuses on the specific issue for <strong>in</strong>vestigation.LOOK THROUGH ONE OR TWOSECONDARY SOURCESOnce the research question has been stated <strong>in</strong> specificterms, it is a good idea <strong>to</strong> look through one or two secondarysources <strong>to</strong> get an overview of previous work thathas been done on the problem. This needn’t be a monumentalchore nor take an overly long time. The ma<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentis <strong>to</strong> get some idea of what is already known aboutthe problem <strong>and</strong> of some of the other questions that arebe<strong>in</strong>g asked. <strong>Research</strong>ers may also get an idea or twoabout how <strong>to</strong> revise or improve the research question.Here are some of the most commonly used secondarysources <strong>in</strong> educational research:Encyclopedia of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> (currentedition): Conta<strong>in</strong>s brief summaries of over 300<strong>to</strong>pics <strong>in</strong> education. Excellent source for gett<strong>in</strong>g abrief overview of the problem.H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Teach<strong>in</strong>g: Conta<strong>in</strong>slonger articles on various aspects of teach<strong>in</strong>g.Most are written by educational researchers whospecialize <strong>in</strong> the <strong>to</strong>pic on which they are writ<strong>in</strong>g.Includes extensive bibliographies.National Society for the Study of <strong>Education</strong> (NSSE)Yearbooks: Published every year, these yearbooksdeal with recent research on various <strong>to</strong>pics. Eachbook usually conta<strong>in</strong>s from 10 <strong>to</strong> 12 chapters deal<strong>in</strong>gwith various aspects of the <strong>to</strong>pic. The societyalso publishes a number of volumes on contemporaryeducational issues that deal <strong>in</strong> part with researchon various <strong>to</strong>pics. A list of these volumes canbe found <strong>in</strong> the back of the most recent yearbook.Review of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>: Published fourtimes a year, this journal conta<strong>in</strong>s reviews of researchon various <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>in</strong> education. Includesextensive bibliographies.Review of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>: Publishedyearly, each volume conta<strong>in</strong>s surveys of researchon important <strong>to</strong>pics written by lead<strong>in</strong>g educationalresearchers.


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 69Subject Guide <strong>to</strong> Books <strong>in</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t (current edition):Each of the above sources conta<strong>in</strong>s reviews of researchon various <strong>to</strong>pics of importance <strong>in</strong> education.There are many <strong>to</strong>pics, however, that have notbeen the subjects of a recent review. If a researchquestion deals with such a <strong>to</strong>pic, the best chancefor locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation discuss<strong>in</strong>g research on the<strong>to</strong>pic lies <strong>in</strong> recent books or monographs on thesubject. The best source for identify<strong>in</strong>g books thatmight discuss research on a <strong>to</strong>pic is the current editionof Books <strong>in</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t.In addition, many professional associations <strong>and</strong> organizationshave published h<strong>and</strong>books of research <strong>in</strong>their fields. These <strong>in</strong>clude:H<strong>and</strong>book of Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on CurriculumH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on <strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istrationH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Mathematics Teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on School SupervisionH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Multicultural <strong>Education</strong>H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Music Teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>Learn<strong>in</strong>gH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Social Studies Teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>gH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on Teacher <strong>Education</strong>H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on the Teach<strong>in</strong>g of EnglishH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> on the <strong>Education</strong> of YoungChildrenEach of these h<strong>and</strong>books <strong>in</strong>cludes a current summary ofresearch deal<strong>in</strong>g with important <strong>to</strong>pics related <strong>to</strong> itsparticular field of study. Other places <strong>to</strong> look for bookson a <strong>to</strong>pic of <strong>in</strong>terest are the card catalog <strong>and</strong> the curriculumdepartment (for textbooks) <strong>in</strong> the library.<strong>Education</strong> Index <strong>and</strong> Psychological Abstracts also listnewly published professional books <strong>in</strong> their fields.SELECT THE APPROPRIATEGENERAL REFERENCESAfter review<strong>in</strong>g a secondary source <strong>to</strong> get a more <strong>in</strong>formedoverview of the problem, researchers shouldhave a clearer idea of exactly what <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. At thispo<strong>in</strong>t, it is a good idea <strong>to</strong> look aga<strong>in</strong> at the research question<strong>to</strong> see if it needs <strong>to</strong> be rewritten <strong>in</strong> any way <strong>to</strong> makeit more focused. Once satisfied, researchers can selec<strong>to</strong>ne or two general references <strong>to</strong> help identify particularjournals or other primary sources related <strong>to</strong> the question.There are many general references a researcher canconsult. Here is a list of the ones most commonly used:<strong>Education</strong> Index: Published monthly, this reference<strong>in</strong>dexes articles from over 300 educational publications,but it gives only bibliographical data(author, title, <strong>and</strong> place of publication). For thisreason, Current Index <strong>to</strong> Journals <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>,or CIJE, is preferred by most educational researchersdo<strong>in</strong>g a literature search on a <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>in</strong>education.Current Index <strong>to</strong> Journals <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> (CIJE):Published monthly by the <strong>Education</strong>al ResourcesInformation Center (ERIC), this <strong>in</strong>dex coversjournal articles. Complete citations <strong>and</strong> abstractsof articles from almost 800 publications, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gmany from foreign countries, are provided,<strong>and</strong> a cumulative <strong>in</strong>dex is <strong>in</strong>cluded at the end ofeach year. The abstract tells what the article isabout; the citation gives the exact page number <strong>in</strong>a specific journal where the entire article appears(Figure 5.1).Resources <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> (RIE): Also publishedmonthly by ERIC, these volumes report on allsorts of documents researchers could not f<strong>in</strong>delsewhere. The monthly issues of RIE reviewspeeches given at professional meet<strong>in</strong>gs, documentspublished by state departments of education,f<strong>in</strong>al reports of federally funded researchprojects, reports from school districts, commissionedpapers written for government agencies,<strong>and</strong> other published <strong>and</strong> unpublished documents.Bibliographic data <strong>and</strong> also an abstract (usually)are provided on all documents. Many reports thatwould otherwise never be published are reported<strong>in</strong> RIE, which makes this an especially valuableresource. RIE should always be consulted, regardlessof the nature of a research <strong>to</strong>pic. An excerptfrom RIE (Figure 5.2) looks quite similar <strong>to</strong> theone from CIJE shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 5.1. Its accessionnumber, however, will beg<strong>in</strong> with ED (<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>git is a document) rather than EJ, as do pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>utsfrom CIJE.Psychological Abstracts: Published monthly by theAmerican Psychological Association, this resourcecovers over 1,300 journals, reports, monographs,<strong>and</strong> other documents (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g books <strong>and</strong> othersecondary sources). Abstracts <strong>and</strong> bibliographicaldata are provided. Although there is considerableoverlap with CIJE, Psych Abstracts (as it is often


Figure 5.1 Excerpt from CIJESource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by Computer SciencesCorporation. www.eric.ed.govFigure 5.2 Excerpt from RIESource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by Computer SciencesCorporation. www.eric.ed.gov70


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 71TABLE 5.1Some of the Major Index<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Abstract<strong>in</strong>g Services Used <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>FocusInclude abstracts?Search via computer?Author <strong>in</strong>dexprovided?First year publishedInclude unpublisheddocuments?Chief advantageChief disadvantagePsychological<strong>Education</strong> Index ERIC Abstracts SSCI<strong>Education</strong>NoYesWith ma<strong>in</strong> entries1929NoIncludes somematerial not <strong>in</strong> CIJENo abstracts<strong>Education</strong>YesYesMonthlyCIJE—1969RIE—1966Yes (<strong>in</strong> RIE)Very comprehensiveUneven quality ofentries <strong>in</strong> RIE<strong>Education</strong>YesYesMonthly1927NoVery comprehensiveMuch that is notrelated <strong>to</strong> educationSocial sciencesNoYesThree times per year1973NoForward search <strong>to</strong> seewhere related articlesof <strong>in</strong>terest may belocatedA bit difficult <strong>to</strong> learnhow <strong>to</strong> use at firstcalled) usually gives a more thorough coverage ofpsychological than educational <strong>to</strong>pics. It shoulddef<strong>in</strong>itely be consulted for any <strong>to</strong>pic deal<strong>in</strong>g withsome aspect of psychology.PsycINFO: Correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the pr<strong>in</strong>ted PsychologicalAbstracts is its onl<strong>in</strong>e version, PsycINFO. In amanner similar <strong>to</strong> Psych Abstracts, PsycINFO <strong>in</strong>cludescitations <strong>to</strong> journal articles, books, <strong>and</strong>book chapters <strong>in</strong> psychology <strong>and</strong> the behavioralsciences, such as anthropology, medic<strong>in</strong>e, psychiatry,<strong>and</strong> sociology. There is a separate file forbooks <strong>and</strong> book chapters. Coverage is from January1987 <strong>to</strong> the present. There are three files forjournal articles, cover<strong>in</strong>g the years 1967–1983,1984–1993, <strong>and</strong> 1993 <strong>to</strong> the present.ERIC onl<strong>in</strong>e: ERIC can also be searched via computeronl<strong>in</strong>e. As <strong>in</strong> the pr<strong>in</strong>ted version, it <strong>in</strong>cludescitations <strong>to</strong> the literature of education, counsel<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> related social science discipl<strong>in</strong>es, <strong>and</strong> it <strong>in</strong>cludesboth CIJE <strong>and</strong> RIE. Citations <strong>to</strong> articlesfrom over 750 journals <strong>and</strong> unpublished curriculumguides, conference papers, <strong>and</strong> research reportsare <strong>in</strong>cluded. There are three files, cover<strong>in</strong>gthe years 1966–1981, 1982–1991, <strong>and</strong> 1992 <strong>to</strong> thepresent. Because ERIC’s coverage is so thorough,a search of RIE <strong>and</strong> CIJE should be sufficient <strong>to</strong>locate most of the relevant references for most researchproblems <strong>in</strong> education. This can now bedone quite easily via a computer search onl<strong>in</strong>e.We’ll show you how <strong>to</strong> do an ERIC search of theliterature later <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Two additional general references that sometimesprovide <strong>in</strong>formation about educational research are thefollow<strong>in</strong>g:Exceptional Child <strong>Education</strong> Resources (ECER):Published quarterly by the Council for ExceptionalChildren, ECER provides <strong>in</strong>formation about exceptionalchildren from over 200 journals. Us<strong>in</strong>g aformat similar <strong>to</strong> CIJE, it provides author, subject,<strong>and</strong> title <strong>in</strong>dexes. It is worth consult<strong>in</strong>g if a research<strong>to</strong>pic deals with exceptional children, s<strong>in</strong>ce it coversseveral journals not searched for <strong>in</strong> CIJE.Social Science Citation Index (SSCI): Another typeof citation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dex<strong>in</strong>g service, SSCI offers theforward search, a unique feature that can be helpful<strong>to</strong> researchers. When a researcher has found anarticle that conta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong>formation of <strong>in</strong>terest, he orshe can locate the author’s name <strong>in</strong> the SSCI (orenter it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a computer search if the library hasSSCI onl<strong>in</strong>e) <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out the names of other authorswho have cited this same article <strong>and</strong> the journals <strong>in</strong>which their articles appeared. These additional articlesmay also be of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the researcher. Heor she can determ<strong>in</strong>e what additional books <strong>and</strong> articleswere cited by these other authors <strong>and</strong> thusconceivably obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation that otherwisemight be missed. (A summary of the major <strong>in</strong>dexes<strong>and</strong> abstracts is shown <strong>in</strong> Table 5.1.)Most doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertations <strong>and</strong> many master’s theses<strong>in</strong> education report on orig<strong>in</strong>al research <strong>and</strong> henceare valuable sources for literature reviews.


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 73Figure 5.3 Excerpt fromDigital DissertationsSource: ProQuest Information<strong>and</strong> Learn<strong>in</strong>g. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted withpermission.


74 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 5.4 SampleBibliographic CardClearly, the abstracts provided <strong>in</strong> Psychological Abstracts,RIE, <strong>and</strong> CIJE are more <strong>in</strong>formative than just thebibliographic data provided <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> Index. Thus itis perhaps somewhat easier <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if an article ispert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> a particular <strong>to</strong>pic. If a <strong>to</strong>pic perta<strong>in</strong>s directly<strong>to</strong> education, little is <strong>to</strong> be ga<strong>in</strong>ed by search<strong>in</strong>g throughPsychological Abstracts. If a <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>in</strong>volves some aspec<strong>to</strong>f psychology, however (such as educational psychology),it often is useful <strong>to</strong> check Psych Abstracts as wellas <strong>Education</strong> Index, RIE, <strong>and</strong> CIJE.In sum, the abstracts <strong>in</strong> RIE <strong>and</strong> Psychological Abstractsare presented <strong>in</strong> more detail than those <strong>in</strong> CIJE.<strong>Education</strong> Index is less comprehensive than CIJE <strong>and</strong>gives only bibliographic <strong>in</strong>formation, not abstracts.CIJE also covers more journals.The best strategy for a thorough search is probably asfollows.1. Before 1965: search <strong>Education</strong> Index.2. From 1966 <strong>to</strong> 1968: search RIE <strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong> Index.3. From 1969 <strong>to</strong> the present: search RIE <strong>and</strong> CIJE, usuallyonl<strong>in</strong>e.OBTAIN PRIMARY SOURCESAfter search<strong>in</strong>g the general references, researchers willhave a pile of bibliographic cards. The next step is <strong>to</strong>locate each of the sources listed on the cards <strong>and</strong> thenread <strong>and</strong> take notes on those relevant <strong>to</strong> the researchproblem. There are two major types of primary sources<strong>to</strong> be familiar with <strong>in</strong> this regard—journals <strong>and</strong> reports.Professional Journals. Many journals <strong>in</strong> educationpublish reports of research. Some publish articles on awide range of educational <strong>to</strong>pics, while others limit whatthey pr<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> a particular specialization, such as socialstudies education. Most researchers become familiar withthe journals <strong>in</strong> their field of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> look them overfrom time <strong>to</strong> time. Examples of such journals <strong>in</strong>clude theAmerican <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, Child Development,<strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istration Quarterly, Journal of<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> ScienceTeach<strong>in</strong>g, Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Quarterly, <strong>and</strong> Theory <strong>and</strong><strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social <strong>Education</strong>.Reports. Many important research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are firstpublished as reports. Almost all funded research projectsproduce a f<strong>in</strong>al report of their activities <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gswhen research is completed. In addition, eachyear many reports on research activities are publishedby the United States government, by state departmentsof education, by private organizations <strong>and</strong> agencies, bylocal school districts, <strong>and</strong> by professional associations.Furthermore, many <strong>in</strong>dividual researchers repor<strong>to</strong>n their recent work at professional meet<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong>conferences.Most reports are abstracted <strong>in</strong> the Documents Résumésection of RIE, <strong>and</strong> ERIC distributes microfichecopies of them <strong>to</strong> most college <strong>and</strong> university libraries.Many papers, such as the reports of presidential taskforces, national conferences, or specially called professionalmeet<strong>in</strong>gs, are published only as reports. They areusually far more detailed than journal articles <strong>and</strong> muchmore up <strong>to</strong> date. Also, they are not copyrighted. Reportsare a very valuable source of up-<strong>to</strong>-date <strong>in</strong>formation thatcould not be obta<strong>in</strong>ed anywhere else.Locat<strong>in</strong>g Primary Sources. Most primary sourcematerial is located <strong>in</strong> journal articles <strong>and</strong> reports, s<strong>in</strong>ce


***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSWhat a Good Summary of aJournal Article Should Conta<strong>in</strong>The problem be<strong>in</strong>g addressedThe purpose of the studyThe hypotheses of the study (if there are any)The methodology the researcher usedA description of the subjects <strong>in</strong>volvedThe resultsThe conclusionsThe particular strengths, weaknesses, <strong>and</strong> limitations ofthe studythat is where most of the research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> educationare published. Although the layout of libraries varies,one often is able <strong>to</strong> go right <strong>to</strong> the stacks where journalsare shelved alphabetically. In some libraries, however,only the librarian can retrieve the journals.As is almost always the case, some of the referencesdesired will be miss<strong>in</strong>g, at the b<strong>in</strong>dery, or checked outby someone else. If an article of particular importanceis unavailable, it can often be obta<strong>in</strong>ed directly fromthe author. Addresses of authors are listed <strong>in</strong> PsychologicalAbstracts or RIE, but not <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> Index orCIJE. Sometimes an author’s address can be found <strong>in</strong>the direc<strong>to</strong>ry of a professional association, such as theAmerican <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association BiographicalMembership Direc<strong>to</strong>ry, or <strong>in</strong> Who’s Who <strong>in</strong> American<strong>Education</strong>. If a repr<strong>in</strong>t cannot be obta<strong>in</strong>ed directlyfrom the author, it may be possible <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> it from anotherlibrary <strong>in</strong> the area or through an <strong>in</strong>terlibrary loan,a service that nearly all libraries provide.Read<strong>in</strong>g Primary Sources. When all the desiredjournal articles are gathered <strong>to</strong>gether, the review canbeg<strong>in</strong>. It is a good idea <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with the most recent articles<strong>and</strong> work backward. The reason for this is that mos<strong>to</strong>f the more recent articles will cite the earlier articles <strong>and</strong>thus provide a quick overview of previous work.<strong>How</strong> should an article be read? While there is no oneperfect way <strong>to</strong> do this, here are some ideas:Read the abstract or the summary first. This will tellwhether the article is worth read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> its entirety.Record the bibliographic data at the <strong>to</strong>p of a note card.Take notes on the article, or pho<strong>to</strong>copy the abstrac<strong>to</strong>r summary. Almost all research articles followapproximately the same format. They usually <strong>in</strong>cludean abstract; an <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry section that presentsthe research problem or question <strong>and</strong> reviewsother related studies; the objectives of the study orthe hypotheses <strong>to</strong> be tested; a description of theresearch procedures, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the subjects studied,the research design, <strong>and</strong> the measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strumentsused; the results or f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the study; a summary(if there is no abstract); <strong>and</strong> the researcher’sconclusions.Be as brief as possible <strong>in</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g notes, yet do not excludeanyth<strong>in</strong>g that might be important <strong>to</strong> describelater <strong>in</strong> the full review.Some researchers prelabel note cards with the essentialsteps mentioned above (problem, hypothesis, procedures,f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, conclusions), leav<strong>in</strong>g space <strong>to</strong> takenotes after each step. For each of these steps, the follow<strong>in</strong>gshould be noted.1. Problem: State it clearly.2. Hypotheses or objectives: List them exactly as stated<strong>in</strong> the article.3. Procedures: List the research methodology used(experiment, case study, <strong>and</strong> so on), the number ofsubjects <strong>and</strong> how they were selected, <strong>and</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>dof <strong>in</strong>strument (questionnaire, tally sheet, <strong>and</strong> so on)used. Make note of any unusual techniques employed.4. F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs: List the major f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Indicate whetherthe objectives of the study were atta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong>whether the hypotheses were supported. Often thef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are summarized <strong>in</strong> a table, which might bepho<strong>to</strong>copied <strong>and</strong> attached <strong>to</strong> the back of the notecard.5. Conclusions: Record or summarize the author’s conclusions.Note your disagreements with the author<strong>and</strong> the reasons for such disagreement. Note strengthsor weaknesses of the study that make the results particularlyapplicable or limited with regard <strong>to</strong> your researchquestion.Figure 5.5 gives an example of a completed note cardbased on the bibliographic card shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 5.4.75


76 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 5.5 Sample Note CardDo<strong>in</strong>g a Computer SearchA computer search of the literature can be performed <strong>in</strong>almost all university libraries <strong>and</strong> most public libraries.Many state departments of education also conduct computersearches, as do some county offices of education<strong>and</strong> some large school systems. Onl<strong>in</strong>e computer term<strong>in</strong>alsare l<strong>in</strong>ked <strong>to</strong> one or more <strong>in</strong>formation retrieval systemsthat draw from a number of databases. The databasemost commonly used by educational researchers isERIC, which can be searched by computer back <strong>to</strong>1966. Other databases <strong>in</strong>clude Psychological Abstracts,Exceptional Child <strong>Education</strong> Resources, <strong>and</strong> the ComprehensiveDissertation Index. Over 200 databases existthat can be computer-searched. Information about themcan be obta<strong>in</strong>ed from most librarians.A typical search of the ERIC database should takeless than an hour. A pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of the search, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g abstractsof sources, can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed, but perhaps most important,more than one descrip<strong>to</strong>r can be searched at thesame time.Suppose a researcher was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formationon the use of question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g science. Asearch of the ERIC database us<strong>in</strong>g the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs question<strong>in</strong>gtechniques <strong>and</strong> science would reveal the abstracts<strong>and</strong> citations of several articles, two of which are shown<strong>in</strong> Figures 5.6a <strong>and</strong> 5.6b. Notice that the word source <strong>in</strong>dicateswhere <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d the articles if the researcher wants<strong>to</strong> read all or part of them—one is <strong>in</strong> the journal <strong>Research</strong><strong>in</strong> Science <strong>and</strong> Technological <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> the other <strong>in</strong>International Journal of Science <strong>Education</strong>.Search<strong>in</strong>g PsycINFO. Search<strong>in</strong>g through PsycINFOis similar <strong>to</strong> search<strong>in</strong>g through RIE <strong>and</strong> CIJE. One firstpulls up the PsycInfo database. Similar <strong>to</strong> a search <strong>in</strong>ERIC, descrip<strong>to</strong>rs can be used s<strong>in</strong>gularly or <strong>in</strong> variouscomb<strong>in</strong>ations <strong>to</strong> locate references. All articles of <strong>in</strong>terestcan then be located <strong>in</strong> the identified journals.AN EXAMPLE OF A COMPUTER SEARCHThe steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a computer search are similar <strong>to</strong>those <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a traditional manual search, except that


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 77Figure 5.6a Example of Abstracts Obta<strong>in</strong>ed Us<strong>in</strong>g the Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs Question<strong>in</strong>g Techniques <strong>and</strong> ScienceSource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by ComputerSciences Corporation. www.eric.ed.govFigure 5.6b Example of Abstracts Obta<strong>in</strong>ed Us<strong>in</strong>g the Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs Question<strong>in</strong>g Techniques <strong>and</strong> ScienceSource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by ComputerSciences Corporation. www.eric.ed.gov


78 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7emuch of the work is done by the computer. To illustratethe steps <strong>in</strong>volved, we can describe an actual search conductedus<strong>in</strong>g the ERIC database.Def<strong>in</strong>e the Problem as Precisely as Possible.As for a traditional manual search, the research problemshould be stated as specifically as possible so that relevantdescrip<strong>to</strong>rs can be identified. A broad statement ofa problem such as, “<strong>How</strong> effective are question<strong>in</strong>g techniques?”is much <strong>to</strong>o general. It is liable <strong>to</strong> produce anextremely large number of references, many of whichprobably will be irrelevant <strong>to</strong> the researcher’s questionof <strong>in</strong>terest. For the purposes of our search, therefore, weposed the follow<strong>in</strong>g research question: “What sorts ofquestion<strong>in</strong>g techniques help students underst<strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalconcepts most effectively?”Decide on the Extent of the Search. The researchermust now decide how many references <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>.For a review for a journal article, a researchermight decide <strong>to</strong> review only 20 <strong>to</strong> 25 fairly recent references.For a more detailed review, such as a master’sthesis, perhaps 30 or 40 might be reviewed. For a veryexhaustive review, as for a doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertation, asmany as 100 or more references might be searched.Decide on the Database. As we mentioned earlier,many databases are available, but the one mostcommonly used is ERIC. Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs must fit a particulardatabase; some descrip<strong>to</strong>rs may not be applicable <strong>to</strong>different databases, although many do overlap. We usedthe ERIC database <strong>in</strong> this example, as it is still the bestfor searches <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g educational <strong>to</strong>pics.Select Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs. Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs are the words theresearcher uses <strong>to</strong> tell the computer what <strong>to</strong> search for.The selection of descrip<strong>to</strong>rs is somewhat of an art. Ifthe descrip<strong>to</strong>r is <strong>to</strong>o general, <strong>to</strong>o many references maybe located, many of which are likely <strong>to</strong> be irrelevant. Ifthe descrip<strong>to</strong>r is <strong>to</strong>o narrow, <strong>to</strong>o few references will belocated, <strong>and</strong> many of those applicable <strong>to</strong> the researchquestion may be missed.Because we used the ERIC database, we selected ourdescrip<strong>to</strong>rs from the Thesaurus of ERIC Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs.Descrip<strong>to</strong>rs can be used s<strong>in</strong>gly or <strong>in</strong> various comb<strong>in</strong>ations<strong>to</strong> locate references. Certa<strong>in</strong> key words, calledBoolean opera<strong>to</strong>rs, enable the retrieval of terms <strong>in</strong> variouscomb<strong>in</strong>ations. The most commonly used Booleanopera<strong>to</strong>rs are <strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> or. For example, by ask<strong>in</strong>g a computer<strong>to</strong> search for a s<strong>in</strong>gle descrip<strong>to</strong>r such as <strong>in</strong>quiry, allreferences conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g this term would be selected. Byconnect<strong>in</strong>g two descrip<strong>to</strong>rs with the word <strong>and</strong>, however,researchers can narrow the search <strong>to</strong> locate only the referencesthat conta<strong>in</strong> both of the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs. Ask<strong>in</strong>g thecomputer <strong>to</strong> search for question<strong>in</strong>g techniques <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong>struction would narrow the search because onlyreferences conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g both descrip<strong>to</strong>rs would be located.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, by us<strong>in</strong>g the word or, a searchcan be broadened, s<strong>in</strong>ce any references with either oneof the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs would be located. Thus, ask<strong>in</strong>g thecomputer <strong>to</strong> search for question<strong>in</strong>g techniques or his<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong>struction would broaden the search because referencesconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g either one of these terms would belocated. Figure 5.7 illustrates the results of us<strong>in</strong>g theseBoolean opera<strong>to</strong>rs.All sorts of comb<strong>in</strong>ations are possible. For example,a researcher might ask the computer <strong>to</strong> search for question<strong>in</strong>gtechniques or <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction orcivics <strong>in</strong>struction. For a reference <strong>to</strong> be selected, itwould have <strong>to</strong> conta<strong>in</strong> either the descrip<strong>to</strong>r term question<strong>in</strong>gtechniques or the descrip<strong>to</strong>r term <strong>in</strong>quiry, aswell as either the descrip<strong>to</strong>r term his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction orthe descrip<strong>to</strong>r term civics <strong>in</strong>struction.For our search, we chose the follow<strong>in</strong>g descrip<strong>to</strong>rs:question<strong>in</strong>g techniques, concept teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong>struction.We also felt there were a number of related terms thatshould also be considered. These <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>quiry,teach<strong>in</strong>g methods, <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g processes under question<strong>in</strong>gtechniques; <strong>and</strong> concept formation <strong>and</strong> cognitivedevelopment under concept teach<strong>in</strong>g. Upon reflection,however, we decided not <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods or learn<strong>in</strong>g processes <strong>in</strong> our search, as we feltthese terms were <strong>to</strong>o broad <strong>to</strong> apply specifically <strong>to</strong> ourresearch question. We also decided not <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude cognitivedevelopment <strong>in</strong> our search for the same reason.Conduct the Search. After determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whichdescrip<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> use, the next step is <strong>to</strong> enter them <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> thecomputer <strong>and</strong> let it do its work. Table 5.2 presents asummary of the search results. As you can see, we askedthe computer first <strong>to</strong> search for question<strong>in</strong>g techniques(search #1), followed by his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction (search #2),followed by a comb<strong>in</strong>ation (search #3) of these two descrip<strong>to</strong>rs(note the use of the Boolean opera<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong>).This resulted <strong>in</strong> a <strong>to</strong>tal of 4,031 references for question<strong>in</strong>gtechniques, 10,143 references for his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction,<strong>and</strong> 62 for a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of these two descrip<strong>to</strong>rs.We then asked the computer <strong>to</strong> search just for the descrip<strong>to</strong>rconcept teach<strong>in</strong>g (search #4). This produced a<strong>to</strong>tal of 7,219 references. Because we were particularly<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> concept teach<strong>in</strong>g as applied <strong>to</strong> question<strong>in</strong>g


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 79Narrow searchFigure 5.7 VennDiagrams Show<strong>in</strong>g theBoolean Opera<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong><strong>and</strong> orQuestion<strong>in</strong>gtechniques(Set 1)His<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong>struction(Set 2)Select question<strong>in</strong>g techniques <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction (Comb<strong>in</strong>e 1 <strong>and</strong> 2)Decreases outputBroader searchQuestion<strong>in</strong>gtechniques(Set 1)His<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong>struction(Set 2)Select question<strong>in</strong>g techniques or his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction (Comb<strong>in</strong>e 1 or 2)Increases outputTABLE 5.2 Pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of Search ResultsSearch Data base Results#1 question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> techniques ERIC 4031#2 his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction ERIC 10143#3 (question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> techniques) ERIC 62<strong>and</strong> (his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction)#4 concept <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g ERIC 7219#5 (question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> techniques)<strong>and</strong> (his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction) ERIC 4<strong>and</strong> (concept <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g)#6 <strong>in</strong>quiry ERIC 5542#7 (concept <strong>and</strong> formation) ERIC 9871#8 (concept <strong>and</strong> formation) or(concept <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g) or ERIC 17856<strong>in</strong>quiry#9 (question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> techniques)<strong>and</strong> (his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>struction)<strong>and</strong> (concept <strong>and</strong> formation) or ERIC(concept <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g) or 18<strong>in</strong>quiry


80 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7etechniques <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction, however, we askedthe computer <strong>to</strong> search for a comb<strong>in</strong>ation (search #5) ofthese three descrip<strong>to</strong>rs (aga<strong>in</strong> note the use of the opera<strong>to</strong>r<strong>and</strong>). This produced only 4 references. This was much<strong>to</strong>o limited a harvest, so we decided <strong>to</strong> broaden ourapproach by ask<strong>in</strong>g the computer <strong>to</strong> look for referencesthat <strong>in</strong>cluded the follow<strong>in</strong>g comb<strong>in</strong>ation of descrip<strong>to</strong>rs:(concept formation or concept teach<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>quiry) <strong>and</strong>(question<strong>in</strong>g techniques <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction). Thisproduced a <strong>to</strong>tal of 18 references. At this po<strong>in</strong>t, we calledfor a pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of these 18 references <strong>and</strong> ended oursearch.If the <strong>in</strong>itial effort of a search produces <strong>to</strong>o few references,the search can be broadened by us<strong>in</strong>g more generaldescrip<strong>to</strong>rs. Thus, we might have used the term socialstudies <strong>in</strong>struction rather than his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>structionhad we not obta<strong>in</strong>ed enough references <strong>in</strong> our search.Similarly, a search can be narrowed by us<strong>in</strong>g more specificdescrip<strong>to</strong>rs. For example, we might have used thespecific descrip<strong>to</strong>r North American his<strong>to</strong>ry rather thanthe <strong>in</strong>clusive term his<strong>to</strong>ry.Obta<strong>in</strong> a Pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of Desired References.Several pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut options are available. Just the title ofthe reference <strong>and</strong> its accession number <strong>in</strong> ERIC (orwhatever database is be<strong>in</strong>g searched) is acceptable. InERIC, this is either the EJ or ED number. Here is anexample:EJ556131Children’s Questions <strong>in</strong> the ClassroomAs you can see, however, this option is not very useful,<strong>in</strong> that titles <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of themselves are often mislead<strong>in</strong>gor not very <strong>in</strong>formative. Hence, most researcherschoose <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> more complete <strong>in</strong>formation, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gbibliographic data, the accession number, the documenttype, the publication year, <strong>and</strong> an abstract of the articleif one has been prepared. Figures 5.1 <strong>and</strong> 5.2 are examplesof more complete pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut options.RESEARCHING THE WORLD WIDE WEBThe World Wide Web (WWW) is part of the Internet,a vast reservoir of <strong>in</strong>formation on all sorts of <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>in</strong> awide variety of areas. Prior <strong>to</strong> 1993, the Internet wasbarely mentioned <strong>in</strong> the research literature. Today, itcannot be ignored. Despite the fact that ERIC <strong>and</strong> (onoccasion) PsycINFO rema<strong>in</strong> the databases of choicewhen it comes <strong>to</strong> research <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g most educational<strong>to</strong>pics, research<strong>in</strong>g the Web should also be considered.Space prevents us from describ<strong>in</strong>g the Internet <strong>in</strong> detail,but we do wish <strong>to</strong> po<strong>in</strong>t out some of its importantfeatures.Us<strong>in</strong>g a Web browser (the computer program thatlets you ga<strong>in</strong> access <strong>to</strong> the WWW), a researcher can f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>formation on almost any <strong>to</strong>pic with just a few clicks ofthe mouse but<strong>to</strong>n. Some of the <strong>in</strong>formation on the Webhas been classified <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dexes, which can be easilysearched by go<strong>in</strong>g from one category <strong>to</strong> another. In addition,several search eng<strong>in</strong>es are available that aresimilar <strong>in</strong> many respects <strong>to</strong> those we used <strong>in</strong> our searchof the ERIC database. Let us consider both <strong>in</strong>dexes <strong>and</strong>search eng<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> a bit more detail.Indexes. Indexes group Web sites <strong>to</strong>gether undersimilar categories, such as Australian universities, Londonart galleries, <strong>and</strong> science labora<strong>to</strong>ries. This is similar<strong>to</strong> what libraries do when they group similar k<strong>in</strong>dsof <strong>in</strong>formation resources <strong>to</strong>gether. The results of an<strong>in</strong>dex search will be a list of Web sites related <strong>to</strong> the<strong>to</strong>pic be<strong>in</strong>g searched. Figure 5.8 shows the Yahoo! Webpage, a particularly good example of an <strong>in</strong>dex. If a researcheris <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g the site for a particularuniversity <strong>in</strong> Australia, for example, he or she should tryus<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dex. Table 5.3 lists the most well-known <strong>in</strong>dexeson the World Wide Web.Indexes often provide an excellent start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t fora review of the literature. This is especially true when aresearcher does not have a clear idea for a researchquestion or <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. Brows<strong>in</strong>g through an<strong>in</strong>dex can be a profitable source of ideas. Felden offersan illustration:For a real-world comparison, suppose I need some householdhardware of some sort <strong>to</strong> perform a repair; I may notalways know exactly what is necessary <strong>to</strong> do the job. ITABLE 5.3 Major Indexes on the World Wide WebDirec<strong>to</strong>ryHRLGalaxyhttp://www.galaxy.comGo Networkhttp://www.go.comHotBothttp://www.hotbot.comLookSmarthttp://www.looksmart.comLibrarians’ Index <strong>to</strong> the Internet http://lii.orgOpen Direc<strong>to</strong>ry Project http://dmoz.orgWebCrawlerhttp://webcrawler.comYahoo!http://www.yahoo.com


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 81Figure 5.8 The Yahoo!Web PageSource: Reproduced withpermission of YAHOO! Inc.© 2007 by Yahoo! Inc. YAHOO!<strong>and</strong> the YAHOO! logo aretrademarks of Yahoo! Inc.TABLE 5.4Lead<strong>in</strong>g Search Eng<strong>in</strong>esAltaVistaExciteGoogleHotBotLycosTeomahttp://www.altavista.comhttp://www.excite.comhttp://www.google.comhttp://www.hotbot.comhttp://www.lycos.comhttp://www.teoma.comFast, powerful, <strong>and</strong> comprehensive. It is extremely good <strong>in</strong> locat<strong>in</strong>g obscure facts<strong>and</strong> offers the best field-search capabilities.Particularly strong on locat<strong>in</strong>g current news articles <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation about travel.Increas<strong>in</strong>gly popular. A very detailed direc<strong>to</strong>ry. Good place <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> first whendo<strong>in</strong>g a search on the Internet.Very easy <strong>to</strong> use when search<strong>in</strong>g for multimedia files or <strong>to</strong> locate Web sites bygeography.Offers very good Web site reviews. Has a very good multimedia search feature.Very good reviews. Easy <strong>to</strong> use.may have a broken part, which I can diligently carry <strong>to</strong> ahardware s<strong>to</strong>re <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> match. Luckily, most hardwares<strong>to</strong>res are fairly well organized <strong>and</strong> have an assortment ofaisles, some with plumb<strong>in</strong>g supplies, others with nails <strong>and</strong>other fasteners, <strong>and</strong> others with rope, tw<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> other materialsfor ty<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong>gether. Proceed<strong>in</strong>g by generalcategory (i.e., electrical, plumb<strong>in</strong>g, woodwork<strong>in</strong>g, etc.), Ican go <strong>to</strong> approximately the right place <strong>and</strong> browse theshelves for items that may fit my repair need. I can exam<strong>in</strong>ethe materials, th<strong>in</strong>k over their potential utility, <strong>and</strong>make my choice. 1Search Eng<strong>in</strong>es. If one wants more specific <strong>in</strong>formation,such as biographical <strong>in</strong>formation about GeorgeOrwell, however, one should use a search eng<strong>in</strong>e, becauseit will search all of the contents of a Web site. Table5.4 presents brief descriptions of the lead<strong>in</strong>g Internetsearch eng<strong>in</strong>es. Search eng<strong>in</strong>es such as Google (Figure5.9) or the Librarians’ Index <strong>to</strong> the Internet (Figure 5.10)use software programs (sometimes called spiders or webcrawlers)that search the entire Internet, look<strong>in</strong>g at millionsof Web pages <strong>and</strong> then <strong>in</strong>dex<strong>in</strong>g all of the words onthem. The search results obta<strong>in</strong>ed are usually ranked <strong>in</strong>


82 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 5.9 Google Web PageSource: Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of Google Br<strong>and</strong> Features.Figure 5.10 Librarians’ Index <strong>to</strong> the Internet Web PageSource: Librarians’ Index <strong>to</strong> the Internet lii.org. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission.


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 83order of relevancy (i.e., the number of times the researcher’ssearch terms appear <strong>in</strong> a document or howclosely the document appears <strong>to</strong> match one of the keywords submitted as query terms by the researcher).A search eng<strong>in</strong>e like Google will search for <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>dthe <strong>in</strong>dividual pages of a Web site that match aresearcher’s search, even if the site itself has noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>do with what the researcher is look<strong>in</strong>g for. As a result,one usually has <strong>to</strong> wade through an awful lot of irrelevant<strong>in</strong>formation. Felden gives us an example:Return<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the hardware s<strong>to</strong>re analogy, if I went <strong>to</strong> thes<strong>to</strong>re <strong>in</strong> search of some screws for my household project<strong>and</strong> employed an au<strong>to</strong>matic robot <strong>in</strong>stead of us<strong>in</strong>g mynative cunn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> browse the (well-arranged) aisles, therobot could conceivably return (after perus<strong>in</strong>g the entires<strong>to</strong>re) with everyth<strong>in</strong>g that had a screw <strong>in</strong> it somewhere.The set of th<strong>in</strong>gs would be a wildly disparate collection. Itwould <strong>in</strong>clude all sorts of boxes of screws, some of themmaybe even the k<strong>in</strong>d I was look<strong>in</strong>g for, but also a wide arrayof other material, much of it of no use for my project. Theremight be birdhouses of wood held <strong>to</strong>gether with screws,<strong>to</strong>ols assembled with screws, a rake with a screw fasten<strong>in</strong>gits h<strong>and</strong>le <strong>to</strong> its prongs. The robot would have done its jobproperly. It had been given someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> match, <strong>in</strong> this casea screw, <strong>and</strong> it went out <strong>and</strong> did its work efficiently <strong>and</strong>thoroughly, although without much <strong>in</strong>telligence. 2In order <strong>to</strong> be satisfied with the results of a search,therefore, one needs <strong>to</strong> know what <strong>to</strong> ask for <strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong>phrase the request <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease the chances of gett<strong>in</strong>g whatis desired. If a researcher wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out <strong>in</strong>formationabout universities, but not English universities, for example,he or she should ask specifically <strong>in</strong> that way.Thus, although it would be a mistake <strong>to</strong> search onlythe Web when do<strong>in</strong>g a literature search (thereby ignor<strong>in</strong>ga plethora of other material that often is so muchbetter organized), it has some def<strong>in</strong>ite advantages forsome k<strong>in</strong>ds of research. Unfortunately, it also has somedisadvantages. Here are some of each:Advantages of Search<strong>in</strong>g the World Wide WebCurrency: Many resources on the Internet are updatedvery rapidly; often they represent the very latest<strong>in</strong>formation about a given <strong>to</strong>pic.Access <strong>to</strong> a wide variety of materials: Many resources,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g works of art, manuscripts, evenentire library collections, can be reviewed at leisureus<strong>in</strong>g a personal computer.Varied formats: Material can be sent over the Internet<strong>in</strong> different formats, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g text, video, sound,<strong>and</strong> animation.Immediacy: The Internet is “open” 24 hours a day.Information can be viewed on one’s own computer<strong>and</strong> can be exam<strong>in</strong>ed as desired or saved <strong>to</strong> a harddrive or disk for later exam<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>and</strong> study.Disadvantages of Search<strong>in</strong>g the World Wide WebDisorganization: Unfortunately, much of the <strong>in</strong>formationon the Web is not well organized. It employsfew of the well-developed classification systemsused by libraries <strong>and</strong> archives. This disorganizationmakes it an absolute necessity for researchers <strong>to</strong> havegood onl<strong>in</strong>e search<strong>in</strong>g skills.Time commitment: There is always a need <strong>to</strong> searchcont<strong>in</strong>ually for new <strong>and</strong> more complete <strong>in</strong>formation.Do<strong>in</strong>g a search on the WWW often (if not usually)can be quite time-consum<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> (regretfully) sometimesless productive than do<strong>in</strong>g a search us<strong>in</strong>g moretraditional sources.Lack (sometimes) of credibility: Anyone can publishsometh<strong>in</strong>g on the Internet. As a result, much of the materialone f<strong>in</strong>ds there may have little, if any, credibility.Uncerta<strong>in</strong> reliability: It is so easy <strong>to</strong> publish <strong>in</strong>formationon the Internet that it often is difficult <strong>to</strong>judge its worth. One of the most valuable aspects ofa library collection is that most of its material hasbeen collected carefully. Librarians make it a po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong>identify <strong>and</strong> select important works that will st<strong>and</strong>the test of time. Much of the <strong>in</strong>formation one f<strong>in</strong>dson the WWW is ill-conceived or trivial.Ethical violations: Because material on the Internetis so easy <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>, there is a greater temptation forresearchers <strong>to</strong> use the material without citation orpermission. Copyright violation is much more likelythan with traditional material.Undue reliance: The amount of <strong>in</strong>formation availableon the Internet has grown so rapidly <strong>in</strong> the lastfew years that some researchers may be misled <strong>to</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k they can f<strong>in</strong>d everyth<strong>in</strong>g they need on the Internet,thereby caus<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> ignore other, more traditionalsources of <strong>in</strong>formation.In search<strong>in</strong>g the WWW, then, here are a few tips <strong>to</strong>get the best search results: 3 Many of these would apply<strong>to</strong> search<strong>in</strong>g ERIC or PsycINFO as well.Use the most specific key word you can th<strong>in</strong>k of. Takesome time <strong>to</strong> list several of the words that are likely <strong>to</strong>appear on the k<strong>in</strong>d of Web page you have <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.Then pick the most unusual word from your list. Forexample, if you’re look<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>in</strong>formation about efforts<strong>to</strong> save tiger populations <strong>in</strong> Asia, don’t use tigers


84 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eas your search term. You’ll be swamped with Webpages about the Detroit Tigers, the Pr<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>to</strong>n Tigers,<strong>and</strong> every other sports team that uses the word tigers<strong>in</strong> its name. Instead, try search<strong>in</strong>g for a particular tigerspecies that you know <strong>to</strong> be on the endangered list—Bengal tiger or Sumatran tiger or Siberian tiger. 4Make it a multistep process. Don’t assume that youwill f<strong>in</strong>d what you want on the first try. Review thefirst couple of pages of your results. Look particularlyat the sites that conta<strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>formation youwant. What unique words appear on those pages?Now do another search us<strong>in</strong>g just those words.Narrow the field by us<strong>in</strong>g just your previous results.If the key words you choose return <strong>to</strong>o much <strong>in</strong>formation,try a second search of just the results you obta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> your first search. This is sometimes referred<strong>to</strong> as set search<strong>in</strong>g. Here’s a tip we th<strong>in</strong>k you’ll f<strong>in</strong>dextremely helpful: Simply add another key word <strong>to</strong>your search request <strong>and</strong> submit it aga<strong>in</strong>.Look for your key word <strong>in</strong> the Web page title. Frequently,the best strategy is <strong>to</strong> look for your uniquekey word <strong>in</strong> the title of Web pages (both AltaVista<strong>and</strong> Infoseek offer this type of search). If you arelook<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>in</strong>formation about <strong>in</strong>quiry teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>secondary school his<strong>to</strong>ry classes, for example, beg<strong>in</strong>with a search of Web pages that have <strong>in</strong>quiry teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> the title. Then do a second search of just thoseresults, look<strong>in</strong>g for secondary school his<strong>to</strong>ry classes.F<strong>in</strong>d out if case counts. Check <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out if the searcheng<strong>in</strong>e you are us<strong>in</strong>g pays any attention <strong>to</strong> upper- <strong>and</strong>lowercase letters <strong>in</strong> your key words. “Will a searchfor java, a microsystems program, for example, alsof<strong>in</strong>d sites that refer <strong>to</strong> the program as JAVA?” 5Check your spell<strong>in</strong>g. If you have used the best keywords that you can th<strong>in</strong>k of <strong>and</strong> the search eng<strong>in</strong>e reports“No results found” (or someth<strong>in</strong>g similar),check your spell<strong>in</strong>g before you do anyth<strong>in</strong>g else. Usually,the fact that a search eng<strong>in</strong>e does not come upwith any results is due <strong>to</strong> a spell<strong>in</strong>g or typ<strong>in</strong>g error.INTERLIBRARY LOANSA problem that every researcher faces at one time or anotheris that a needed book or journal is not available <strong>in</strong> thelibrary. With the com<strong>in</strong>g of computers, however, it is nowvery easy <strong>to</strong> borrow books from distant libraries. Many librarianscan enter <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a computer term<strong>in</strong>al<strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out with<strong>in</strong> seconds which libraries with<strong>in</strong> a designatedarea have a particular book or journal. The librariancan then arrange <strong>to</strong> borrow the material by means of<strong>in</strong>terlibrary loan, usually for only a m<strong>in</strong>imal fee.Writ<strong>in</strong>g the LiteratureReview ReportAfter read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g notes on the various sourcescollected, researchers can prepare the f<strong>in</strong>al review.Literature reviews differ <strong>in</strong> format, but typically theyconsist of the follow<strong>in</strong>g five parts.1. The <strong>in</strong>troduction briefly describes the nature of theresearch problem <strong>and</strong> states the research question.The researcher also expla<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> this section what ledhim or her <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the question <strong>and</strong> why it isan important question <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate.2. The body of the review briefly reports what othershave found or thought about the research problem. Relatedstudies are usually discussed <strong>to</strong>gether, groupedunder subheads (<strong>to</strong> make the review easier <strong>to</strong> read).Major studies are described <strong>in</strong> more detail, while lessimportant work can be referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> just a l<strong>in</strong>e or two.Often this is done by referr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> several studies thatreported similar results <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle sentence, somewhatlike this: “Several other small-scale studies reportedsimilar results (Adams, 1976; Brown, 1980; Cartright,1981; Davis, 1985; Frost, 1987).”3. The summary of the review ties <strong>to</strong>gether the ma<strong>in</strong>threads revealed <strong>in</strong> the literature reviewed <strong>and</strong> presentsa composite picture of what is known orthought <strong>to</strong> date. F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs may be tabulated <strong>to</strong> givereaders some idea of how many other researchershave reported identical or similar f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs or havesimilar recommendations.4. Any conclusions the researcher feels are justifiedbased on the state of knowledge revealed <strong>in</strong> the literatureshould be <strong>in</strong>cluded. What does the literaturesuggest are appropriate courses of action <strong>to</strong> take <strong>to</strong>try <strong>to</strong> solve the problem?5. A bibliography with full bibliographic data for allsources mentioned <strong>in</strong> the review is essential. There aremanyways<strong>to</strong>formatreferencelists,buttheoneoutl<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> the Publication Manual of the American PsychologicalAssociation (2001) is particularly easy <strong>to</strong> use.META-ANALYSISLiterature reviews accompany<strong>in</strong>g research reports <strong>in</strong>journals are usually required <strong>to</strong> be brief. Unfortunately,this largely prevents much <strong>in</strong> the way of critical analysisof <strong>in</strong>dividual studies. Furthermore, traditional literaturereviews basically depend on the judgment of the reviewer<strong>and</strong> hence are prone <strong>to</strong> subjectivity.


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 85In an effort <strong>to</strong> augment (or replace) the subjectivity<strong>and</strong> time required <strong>in</strong> review<strong>in</strong>g many studies on the same<strong>to</strong>pic, therefore, the concept of meta-analysis has beendeveloped. 6 In the simplest terms, when a researcherdoes a meta-analysis, he or she averages the results ofthe selected studies <strong>to</strong> get an overall <strong>in</strong>dex of outcome orrelationship. The first requirement is that results be describedstatistically, most commonly through the calculationof effect sizes <strong>and</strong> correlation coefficients (we expla<strong>in</strong>both later <strong>in</strong> the text). In one of the earliest studiesus<strong>in</strong>g meta-analysis, 7 375 studies on the effectiveness ofpsychotherapy were analyzed, lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the conclusionthat the average client was, after therapy, appreciablybetter off than the average person not <strong>in</strong> therapy.As you might expect, this methodology has had widespreadappeal <strong>in</strong> many discipl<strong>in</strong>es—<strong>to</strong> date, hundreds ofmeta-analyses have been done. Critics raise a number ofobjections, some of which have been at least partly remediedby statistical adjustments. We th<strong>in</strong>k the most seriouscriticisms are that a poorly designed study counts asmuch as one that has been carefully designed <strong>and</strong> executed,<strong>and</strong> that the evaluation of the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the f<strong>in</strong>al<strong>in</strong>dex rema<strong>in</strong>s a judgment call, although an <strong>in</strong>formedone. The former objection can be remedied by delet<strong>in</strong>g“poor” studies, but this br<strong>in</strong>gs back the subjectivity metaanalysiswas designed <strong>to</strong> replace. It is clear that metaanalysisis here <strong>to</strong> stay; we agree with those who arguethat it cannot replace an <strong>in</strong>formed, careful review of <strong>in</strong>dividualstudies, however. In any event, the literature reviewshould <strong>in</strong>clude a search for relevant meta-analysisreports, as well as <strong>in</strong>dividual studies.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE VALUE OF A LITERATURE REVIEWA literature review helps researchers learn what others have written about a <strong>to</strong>pic. Italso lets researchers see the results of other, related studies.A detailed literature review is often required of master’s <strong>and</strong> doc<strong>to</strong>ral students whenthey design a thesis.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTYPES OF SOURCES FOR A LITERATURE REVIEW<strong>Research</strong>ers need <strong>to</strong> be familiar with three basic types of sources (general references,primary sources, <strong>and</strong> secondary sources) <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g a literature review.General references are sources a researcher consults <strong>to</strong> locate other sources.Primary sources are publications <strong>in</strong> which researchers report the results of their <strong>in</strong>vestigations.Most primary source material is located <strong>in</strong> journal articles.Secondary sources refer <strong>to</strong> publications <strong>in</strong> which authors describe the work of others.RIE <strong>and</strong> CIJE are two of the most frequently used general references <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch.Search terms, or descrip<strong>to</strong>rs, are key words researchers use <strong>to</strong> help locate relevantprimary sources.STEPS INVOLVED IN A LITERATURE SEARCHThe essential steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a review of the literature <strong>in</strong>clude: (1) def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g theresearch problem as precisely as possible; (2) perus<strong>in</strong>g the secondary sources;


86 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e(3) select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> perus<strong>in</strong>g an appropriate general reference; (4) formulat<strong>in</strong>g searchterms; (5) search<strong>in</strong>g the general references for relevant primary sources; (6) obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g the primary sources, <strong>and</strong> not<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> summariz<strong>in</strong>g key po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the sources.WAYS TO DO A LITERATURE SEARCHToday, there are two ways <strong>to</strong> do a literature search—manually, us<strong>in</strong>g the traditionalpaper approach, <strong>and</strong> electronically, by means of a computer. The most common <strong>and</strong>frequently used method, however, is <strong>to</strong> search onl<strong>in</strong>e, via computer.There are five essential po<strong>in</strong>ts (problem, hypotheses, procedures, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> conclusions)that researchers should record when tak<strong>in</strong>g notes on a study.DOING A COMPUTER SEARCHComputer searches of the literature have a number of advantages—they are fast, arefairly <strong>in</strong>expensive, provide pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>uts, <strong>and</strong> enable researchers <strong>to</strong> search us<strong>in</strong>g morethan one descrip<strong>to</strong>r at a time.The steps <strong>in</strong> a traditional manual search are similar <strong>to</strong> those <strong>in</strong> a computer search,though computer searches are usually the norm.<strong>Research</strong><strong>in</strong>g the World Wide Web (WWW) should be considered, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong>ERIC <strong>and</strong> PsycINFO, <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g a literature search.Some of the <strong>in</strong>formation on the Web is classified <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dexes, which group Web sites<strong>to</strong>gether under similar categories. Yahoo! is an example of a direc<strong>to</strong>ry.To obta<strong>in</strong> more specific <strong>in</strong>formation, search eng<strong>in</strong>es should be used, because theysearch all of the contents of a Web site.THE LITERATURE REVIEW REPORTThe literature review report consists of an <strong>in</strong>troduction, the body of the review, asummary, the researcher’s conclusions, <strong>and</strong> a bibliography.When a researcher does a meta-analysis, he or she averages the results of a group ofselected studies <strong>to</strong> get an overall <strong>in</strong>dex of outcome or relationship.A literature review should <strong>in</strong>clude a search for relevant meta-analysis reports, as wellas <strong>in</strong>dividual studies.Key termsabstract 67Boolean opera<strong>to</strong>rs 78descrip<strong>to</strong>rs 72general reference 67<strong>in</strong>dex 80literature review 67primary source 67search eng<strong>in</strong>e 80search terms 72secondary source 67Web browser 80World Wide Web 80For Discussion1. Why might it be unwise for a researcher not <strong>to</strong> do a review of the literature beforeplann<strong>in</strong>g a study?2. Many published research articles <strong>in</strong>clude only a few references <strong>to</strong> related studies.<strong>How</strong> would you expla<strong>in</strong> this? Is this justified?3. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k are more important <strong>to</strong> emphasize <strong>in</strong> a literature review—theop<strong>in</strong>ions of experts <strong>in</strong> the field or related studies? Why?4. One rarely f<strong>in</strong>ds books referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> literature reviews. Why do you suppose this isso? Is it a good idea <strong>to</strong> refer <strong>to</strong> books?


CHAPTER 5 Locat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Review<strong>in</strong>g the Literature 875. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of any type of <strong>in</strong>formation that should not be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> a literaturereview? If so, give an example.6. Professor Jones states that he does not have his students do a literature review beforeplann<strong>in</strong>g their master’s theses because they “take <strong>to</strong>o much time,” <strong>and</strong> he wantsthem <strong>to</strong> get started collect<strong>in</strong>g their data as quickly as possible. In light of the <strong>in</strong>formationwe have provided <strong>in</strong> this chapter, what would you say <strong>to</strong> him? Why?7. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of any sorts of studies that would not benefit from hav<strong>in</strong>g the researcherconduct a literature review? If so, what might they be?1. N. Felden (2000). Internet research: Theory <strong>and</strong> practice. (2nd ed.). London: McFarl<strong>and</strong> & Co.,pp. 124–125.2. Ibid., p. 127.3. A. Glossbrenner <strong>and</strong> E. Glossbrenner (1998). Search eng<strong>in</strong>es. San Francisco: San Francisco State UniversityPress, pp. 11–13.4. Ibid., p. 12.5. Ibid.6. M. L. Smith, G. V. Glass, <strong>and</strong> T. I. Miller (1980). Primary, secondary, <strong>and</strong> meta-analysis research. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 5 (10): 3–8.7. Ibid.Notes


88 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 5: Review of the LiteratureUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 5, aga<strong>in</strong> state either your research question or the hypothesis of your study.Then consult an appropriate general reference, <strong>and</strong> list at least three search terms relevant <strong>to</strong>your problem. Next do a computer search <strong>to</strong> locate three studies related <strong>to</strong> your question. Readthe studies, tak<strong>in</strong>g notes similar <strong>to</strong> those shown on the sample note card <strong>in</strong> Figure 5.5. Attacheach of your note cards (one per journal article) <strong>to</strong> Problem Sheet 5.Problem Sheet 5Review of the Literature1. The question or hypothesis <strong>in</strong> my study is: __________________________________2. The general reference(s) I consulted was (were): _____________________________3. The database I used <strong>in</strong> my search was: _____________________________________4. The descrip<strong>to</strong>rs (search terms) I used were (list s<strong>in</strong>gle descrip<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ations<strong>in</strong> the order <strong>in</strong> which you did your search):______________________________________________________________5. The results of my search us<strong>in</strong>g these descrip<strong>to</strong>rs were as follows:______________________________________________________________Search # Descrip<strong>to</strong>r(s) ResultsAn electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.6. Attached is a pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of my search. (Attach <strong>to</strong> the back of this sheet.)7. The title of one of the abstracts located us<strong>in</strong>g the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs identified above is: (Attacha copy of the abstract.) _____________________________________________8. The titles of the studies I read were (Note cards are attached):a.b.c.


Sampl<strong>in</strong>g6Cities of 100,000 peopleare identified by researchers.AEOrange Av.Roberts Av.Brown Av.Green Av.Spruce Av.P<strong>in</strong>e Av.Taylor Av.(Example of one listed block.)Every fourth household hasbeen selected, start<strong>in</strong>g withno. 4, which was selectedat r<strong>and</strong>om.BFStage 4: R<strong>and</strong>omsample of blocksCG1st St.2nd St.3rd St.4th St.5th St.DHStage 3: R<strong>and</strong>omsample of prec<strong>in</strong>cts1. 201 Brown Avenue2. 217 Brown Avenue3. 219 Brown Avenue4. 221 Brown Avenue5. 225 Brown Avenue6. 231 Green Avenue7. 239 Green Avenue8. 245 Green AvenueMULTISTAGE SAMPLINGStage 1: R<strong>and</strong>omsample of citiesFHStage 2: R<strong>and</strong>omsample of wardsStage 5: R<strong>and</strong>om sample of households9. 1008 4th Street10. 1014 4th Street11. 1022 4th Street12. 1030 4th Street13. 1034 5th Street14. 1042 5th Street15. 1050 5th Street16. 1056 5th StreetWhat Is a Sample?Samples <strong>and</strong> PopulationsDef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the PopulationTarget Versus AccessiblePopulationsR<strong>and</strong>om Versus Nonr<strong>and</strong>omSampl<strong>in</strong>gR<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gMethodsSimple R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gStratified R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gCluster R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gTwo-Stage R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gNonr<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gMethodsSystematic Sampl<strong>in</strong>gConvenience Sampl<strong>in</strong>gPurposive Sampl<strong>in</strong>gA Review of Sampl<strong>in</strong>gMethodsSample SizeExternal Validity:Generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from aSamplePopulation GeneralizabilityWhen R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g IsNot FeasibleEcological GeneralizabilityOBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between a sample <strong>and</strong> aExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by “systematicpopulation.sampl<strong>in</strong>g,” “convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g,” <strong>and</strong>Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g.”“representative sample.”Expla<strong>in</strong> how the size of a sample can makeExpla<strong>in</strong> how a target population differs a difference <strong>in</strong> terms of representativenessfrom an accessible population.of the sample.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by “r<strong>and</strong>omExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termsampl<strong>in</strong>g,” <strong>and</strong> describe briefly three ways “external validity.”of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between populationUse a table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers <strong>to</strong> select a generalizability <strong>and</strong> ecologicalr<strong>and</strong>om sample from a population.generalizability <strong>and</strong> discuss whenExpla<strong>in</strong> how stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g it is (<strong>and</strong> when it is not) appropriatediffers from cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>to</strong> generalize the results of a study.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> RepresentativenessAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 6.1: Identify<strong>in</strong>g Types of Sampl<strong>in</strong>gActivity 6.2: Draw<strong>in</strong>g a R<strong>and</strong>om SampleActivity 6.3: When Is It Appropriate <strong>to</strong> Generalize?Activity 6.4: True or False?Activity 6.5: Stratified Sampl<strong>in</strong>gActivity 6.6: <strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>g a Sampl<strong>in</strong>g PlanHelen Hyun, a research professor at a large eastern university, wishes <strong>to</strong> study the effect of a new mathematicsprogram on the mathematics achievement of students who are perform<strong>in</strong>g poorly <strong>in</strong> math <strong>in</strong> elementaryschools throughout the United States. Because of a number of fac<strong>to</strong>rs, of which time <strong>and</strong> money are only two, it isimpossible for Helen <strong>and</strong> her colleagues <strong>to</strong> try out the new program with the entire population of such students.They must select a sample. What is a sample anyway? Are there different k<strong>in</strong>ds of samples? Are some k<strong>in</strong>ds betterthan others <strong>to</strong> study? And just how does one go about obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a sample <strong>in</strong> the first place? Answers <strong>to</strong> questionslike these are some of the th<strong>in</strong>gs you will learn about <strong>in</strong> this chapter.When we want <strong>to</strong> know someth<strong>in</strong>g about a certa<strong>in</strong> groupof people, we usually f<strong>in</strong>d a few members of the groupwhom we know—or don’t know—<strong>and</strong> study them. Afterwe have f<strong>in</strong>ished “study<strong>in</strong>g” these <strong>in</strong>dividuals, we usuallycome <strong>to</strong> some conclusions about the larger group ofwhich they are a part. Many “commonsense” observations,<strong>in</strong> fact, are based on observations of relatively fewpeople. It is not uncommon, for example, <strong>to</strong> hear statementssuch as: “Most female students don’t like math”;“You won’t f<strong>in</strong>d very many teachers vot<strong>in</strong>g Republican”;<strong>and</strong> “Most school super<strong>in</strong>tendents are men.”What Is a Sample?Most people, we th<strong>in</strong>k, base their conclusions about agroup of people (students, Republicans, football players,ac<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> so on) on the experiences they have witha fairly small number, or sample, of <strong>in</strong>dividual members.Sometimes such conclusions are an accurate representationof how the larger group of people acts or whatthey believe, but often they are not. It all depends onhow representative (i.e., how similar) the sample is ofthe larger group.One of the most important steps <strong>in</strong> the researchprocess is the selection of the sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals whowill participate (be observed or questioned). Sampl<strong>in</strong>grefers <strong>to</strong> the process of select<strong>in</strong>g these <strong>in</strong>dividuals.SAMPLES AND POPULATIONSA sample <strong>in</strong> a research study is the group on which <strong>in</strong>formationis obta<strong>in</strong>ed. The larger group <strong>to</strong> which onehopes <strong>to</strong> apply the results is called the population.* All700 (or whatever <strong>to</strong>tal number of) students at State Universitywho are major<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mathematics, for example,constitute a population; 50 of those students constitutea sample. Students who own au<strong>to</strong>mobiles make up anotherpopulation, as do students who live <strong>in</strong> the campusdormi<strong>to</strong>ries. Notice that a group may be both a sample<strong>in</strong> one context <strong>and</strong> a population <strong>in</strong> another context. AllState University students who own au<strong>to</strong>mobiles constitutethe population of au<strong>to</strong>mobile owners at State, yetthey also constitute a sample of all au<strong>to</strong>mobile ownersat state universities across the United States.When it is possible, researchers would prefer <strong>to</strong> studythe entire population of <strong>in</strong>terest. Usually, however, this isdifficult <strong>to</strong> do. Most populations of <strong>in</strong>terest are large,diverse, <strong>and</strong> scattered over a large geographic area. F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g,let alone contact<strong>in</strong>g, all the members can be timeconsum<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> expensive. For that reason, of necessity,*In some <strong>in</strong>stances the sample <strong>and</strong> population may be identical.90


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 91researchers often select a sample <strong>to</strong> study. Some examplesof samples selected from populations follow:A researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the effects ofdiet on the attention span of third-grade students <strong>in</strong> alarge city. There are 1,500 third-graders attend<strong>in</strong>g theelementary schools <strong>in</strong> the city. The researcher selects150 of these third-graders, 30 each <strong>in</strong> five differentschools, as a sample for study.An adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a large urban high school is <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> student op<strong>in</strong>ions on a new counsel<strong>in</strong>g program<strong>in</strong> the district. There are six high schools <strong>and</strong> some14,000 students <strong>in</strong> the district. From a master list of allstudents enrolled <strong>in</strong> the district schools, the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rselects a sample of 1,400 students (350 from eachof the four grades, 9–12) <strong>to</strong> whom he plans <strong>to</strong> mail aquestionnaire ask<strong>in</strong>g their op<strong>in</strong>ion of the program.The pr<strong>in</strong>cipal of an elementary school wants <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe effectiveness of a new U.S. his<strong>to</strong>ry textbookused by some of the teachers <strong>in</strong> the district. Out of a<strong>to</strong>tal of 22 teachers who are us<strong>in</strong>g the text, she selectsa sample of 6. She plans <strong>to</strong> compare the achievemen<strong>to</strong>f the students <strong>in</strong> these teachers’ classes with those ofanother 6 teachers who are not us<strong>in</strong>g the text.DEFINING THE POPULATIONThe first task <strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g a sample is <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e the populationof <strong>in</strong>terest. In what group, exactly, is the researcher<strong>in</strong>terested? To whom does he or she want the results ofthe study <strong>to</strong> apply? The population, <strong>in</strong> other words, is thegroup of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the researcher, the group <strong>to</strong> whomthe researcher would like <strong>to</strong> generalize the results of thestudy. Here are some examples of populations:All high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipals <strong>in</strong> the United StatesAll elementary school counselors <strong>in</strong> the state ofCaliforniaAll students attend<strong>in</strong>g Central High School <strong>in</strong> Omaha,Nebraska, dur<strong>in</strong>g the academic year 2005–2006All students <strong>in</strong> Ms. Brown’s third-grade class atWhar<strong>to</strong>n Elementary SchoolThe above examples reveal that a population can beany size <strong>and</strong> that it will have at least one (<strong>and</strong> sometimesseveral) characteristic(s) that sets it off from any otherpopulation. Notice that a population is always all of the<strong>in</strong>dividuals who possess a certa<strong>in</strong> characteristic (or se<strong>to</strong>f characteristics).In educational research, the population of <strong>in</strong>terest isusually a group of persons (students, teachers, or other<strong>in</strong>dividuals) who possess certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics. In somecases, however, the population may be def<strong>in</strong>ed as a groupof classrooms, schools, or even facilities. For example,All fifth-grade classrooms <strong>in</strong> Delaware (the hypothesismight be that classrooms <strong>in</strong> which teachers displaya greater number <strong>and</strong> variety of student products havehigher achievement)All high school gymnasiums <strong>in</strong> Nevada (the hypothesismight be that schools with “better” physicalfacilities produce more w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g teams)TARGET VERSUS ACCESSIBLE POPULATIONSUnfortunately, the actual population (called the targetpopulation) <strong>to</strong> which a researcher would really like<strong>to</strong> generalize is rarely available. The population <strong>to</strong>which a researcher is able <strong>to</strong> generalize, therefore, is theaccessible population. The former is the researcher’sideal choice; the latter, his or her realistic choice. Considerthese examples:<strong>Research</strong> problem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated: The effectsof computer-assisted <strong>in</strong>struction on the read<strong>in</strong>gachievement of first- <strong>and</strong> second-graders <strong>in</strong>California.Target population: All first- <strong>and</strong> second-grade children<strong>in</strong> California.Accessible population: All first- <strong>and</strong> second-gradechildren <strong>in</strong> the Laguna Salada elementary schooldistrict of Pacifica, California.Sample: Ten percent of the first- <strong>and</strong> second-gradechildren <strong>in</strong> the Laguna Salada district <strong>in</strong> Pacifica,California.<strong>Research</strong> problem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated: The attitudesof fifth-year teachers-<strong>in</strong>-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ward their studentteach<strong>in</strong>g experience.Target population: All fifth-year students enrolled<strong>in</strong> teacher-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programs <strong>in</strong> the United States.Accessible population: All fifth-year students enrolled<strong>in</strong> teacher-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programs <strong>in</strong> the StateUniversity of New York.Sample: Two hundred fifth-year students selectedfrom those enrolled <strong>in</strong> the teacher-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programs<strong>in</strong> the State University of New York.The more narrowly researchers def<strong>in</strong>e the population,the more they save on time, effort, <strong>and</strong> (probably)money, but the more they limit generalizability. It is essentialthat researchers describe the population <strong>and</strong> thesample <strong>in</strong> sufficient detail so that <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>dividualscan determ<strong>in</strong>e the applicability of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> theirown situations. Failure <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> detail the population


92 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 6.1Representative VersusNonrepresentativeSamplesA population of 40men <strong>and</strong> women20 men20 womenA nonrepresentative sample (n=12)(2 women <strong>and</strong> 10 men)A representative sample (n=12)(6 women <strong>and</strong> 6 men)of <strong>in</strong>terest, <strong>and</strong> the sample studied, is one of the mostcommon weaknesses of published research reports. It isimportant <strong>to</strong> note that the actual sample may be differentfrom the sample orig<strong>in</strong>ally selected because somesubjects may refuse <strong>to</strong> participate, some subjects maydrop out, data may be lost, <strong>and</strong> the like. We repeat,therefore, that it is very important <strong>to</strong> describe the characteristicsof the actual sample studied <strong>in</strong> some detail.RANDOM VERSUS NONRANDOM SAMPLINGFollow<strong>in</strong>g is an example of each of the two ma<strong>in</strong> typesof sampl<strong>in</strong>g.R<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: The dean of a school of education<strong>in</strong> a large midwestern university wishes <strong>to</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d out how her faculty feel about the currentsabbatical leave requirements at the university.She places all 150 names of the faculty <strong>in</strong> a hat,mixes them thoroughly, <strong>and</strong> then draws out thenames of 25 <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview.**A better way <strong>to</strong> do this will be discussed shortly, but this gives youthe idea.Nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: The president of the sameuniversity wants <strong>to</strong> know how his junior facultyfeel about a promotion policy that he has recently<strong>in</strong>troduced (with the advice of a faculty committee).He selects a sample of 30 from the <strong>to</strong>talfaculty of 1,000 <strong>to</strong> talk with. Five faculty membersfrom each of the six schools that make upthe university are chosen on the basis of the follow<strong>in</strong>gcriteria: They have taught at the universityfor less than five years, they are nontenured, theybelong <strong>to</strong> one of the faculty associations on campus,<strong>and</strong> they have not been a member of thecommittee that helped the president draft the newpolicy.In the first example, 25 names were selected from ahat after all the names had been mixed thoroughly. Thisis called r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g because every member ofthe population (the 150 faculty members <strong>in</strong> the school)presumably had an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected.There are more sophisticated ways of draw<strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>omsample, but they all have the same <strong>in</strong>tent—<strong>to</strong> select arepresentative sample from the population (Figure 6.1).The basic idea is that the group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals selected is


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 93very much like the entire population. One can never besure of this, of course, but if the sample is selected r<strong>and</strong>omly<strong>and</strong> is sufficiently large, a researcher should getan accurate view of the larger group. The best way <strong>to</strong>ensure this is <strong>to</strong> see that no bias enters the selectionprocess—that the researcher (or other fac<strong>to</strong>rs) cannotconsciously or unconsciously <strong>in</strong>fluence who gets chosen<strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> the sample. We expla<strong>in</strong> more about how <strong>to</strong>m<strong>in</strong>imize bias later <strong>in</strong> this chapter.In the second example, the president wants representativeness,but not as much as he wants <strong>to</strong> makesure there are certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of faculty <strong>in</strong> his sample.Thus, he has stipulated that each of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals selectedmust possess all the criteria mentioned. Eachmember of the population (the entire faculty of theuniversity) does not have an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected;some, <strong>in</strong> fact, have no chance. Hence, this is anexample of nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, sometimes calledpurposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g (see p. 99). Here is another exampleof a r<strong>and</strong>om sample contrasted with a nonr<strong>and</strong>omsample.R<strong>and</strong>om: A researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> conduct a survey ofall social studies teachers <strong>in</strong> a midwestern state <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e their attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward the new stateguidel<strong>in</strong>es for teach<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> the secondaryschools. There are a <strong>to</strong>tal of 725 social studiesteachers <strong>in</strong> the state. The names of these teachersare obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> listed alphabetically. The researcherthen numbers the names on the list from001 <strong>to</strong> 725. Us<strong>in</strong>g a table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers,which he f<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>in</strong> a statistics textbook, he selects100 teachers for the sample.Nonr<strong>and</strong>om: The manager of the campus books<strong>to</strong>reat a local university wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out howstudents feel about the services the books<strong>to</strong>reprovides. Every day for two weeks dur<strong>in</strong>g herlunch hour, she asks every person who entersthe books<strong>to</strong>re <strong>to</strong> fill out a short questionnaireshe has prepared <strong>and</strong> drop it <strong>in</strong> a box near theentrance before leav<strong>in</strong>g. At the end of the twoweekperiod, she has a <strong>to</strong>tal of 235 completedquestionnaires.In the second example, notice that all books<strong>to</strong>reusers did not have an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>the sample, which <strong>in</strong>cluded only those who visited dur<strong>in</strong>gthe lunch hour. That is why the sample is not r<strong>and</strong>om.Notice also that some may not have completed thequestionnaire.R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsAfter mak<strong>in</strong>g a decision <strong>to</strong> sample, researchers try hard,<strong>in</strong> most <strong>in</strong>stances, <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a sample that is representativeof the population of <strong>in</strong>terest—that means they preferr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. The three most common ways ofobta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g this type of sample are simple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g,stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> cluster sampl<strong>in</strong>g.A less common method is two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g.SIMPLE RANDOM SAMPLINGA simple r<strong>and</strong>om sample is one <strong>in</strong> which each <strong>and</strong>every member of the population has an equal <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependentchance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected. If the sample is large,this method is the best way yet devised <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a samplerepresentative of the population of <strong>in</strong>terest. Let’s takean example: Def<strong>in</strong>e a population as all eighth-grade students<strong>in</strong> school district Y. Imag<strong>in</strong>e there are 500 students.If you were one of these students, your chance of be<strong>in</strong>gselected would be 1 <strong>in</strong> 500, if the sampl<strong>in</strong>g procedurewere <strong>in</strong>deed r<strong>and</strong>om. Everyone would have the samechance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected.The larger a r<strong>and</strong>om sample is <strong>in</strong> size, the morelikely it is <strong>to</strong> represent the population. Although there isno guarantee of representativeness, of course, the likelihoodof it is greater with large r<strong>and</strong>om samples thanwith any other method. Any differences between thesample <strong>and</strong> the population should be small <strong>and</strong> unsystematic.Any differences that do occur are the result ofchance, rather than bias on the part of the researcher.The key <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>om sample is <strong>to</strong> ensurethat each <strong>and</strong> every member of the population has anequal <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected. Thiscan be done by us<strong>in</strong>g what is known as a table ofr<strong>and</strong>om numbers—an extremely large list of numbersthat has no order or pattern. Such lists can be found <strong>in</strong>the back of most statistics books. Table 6.1 illustratespart of a typical table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers.For example, <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a sample of 200 from a populationof 2,000 <strong>in</strong>dividuals, us<strong>in</strong>g such a table, select acolumn of numbers, start anywhere <strong>in</strong> the column, <strong>and</strong>beg<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g four-digit numbers. (Why four digits?Because the f<strong>in</strong>al number, 2,000, consists of four digits,<strong>and</strong> we must always use the same number of digits foreach person. Person 1 would be identified as 0001;person 2, as 0002; person 635, as 0635; <strong>and</strong> so forth.)Then proceed <strong>to</strong> write down the first 200 numbers <strong>in</strong> thecolumn that have a value of 2,000 or less.


94 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 6.1Part of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>om Numbers011723 223456 222167 032762 062281 565451912334 379156 233989 109238 934128 987678086401 016265 411148 251287 602345 659080059397 022334 080675 454555 011563 237873666278 106590 879809 899030 909876 198905051965 004571 036900 037700 500098 046660063045 786326 098000 510379 024358 145678560132 345678 356789 033460 050521 342021727009 344870 889567 324588 400567 989657000037 121191 258700 088909 015460 223350667899 234345 076567 090076 345121 121348042397 045645 030032 657112 675897 079326987650 568799 070070 143188 198789 097451091126 021557 102322 209312 909036 342045Let us take the first column of four numbers <strong>in</strong>Table 6.1as an example. Read<strong>in</strong>g only the first four digits, look atthe first number <strong>in</strong> the column: It is 0117, so number 117<strong>in</strong> the list of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the population would be selectedfor the sample. Look at the second number: It is9123. There is no 9123 <strong>in</strong> the population (because thereare only 2,000 <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the entire population). So goon <strong>to</strong> the third number: It is 0864, hence number 864 <strong>in</strong> thelist of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the population would be chosen. Thefourth number is 0593, so number 593 gets selected. Thefifth number is 6662. There is no 6662 <strong>in</strong> the population,so go on <strong>to</strong> the next number, <strong>and</strong> so on, until reach<strong>in</strong>g a<strong>to</strong>tal of 200 numbers, each represent<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>the population who will be selected for the sample.The advantage of r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is that, if largeenough, it is very likely <strong>to</strong> produce a representativesample. Its biggest disadvantage is that it is not easy <strong>to</strong>do. Each <strong>and</strong> every member of the population must beidentified. In most cases, we must be able <strong>to</strong> contact the<strong>in</strong>dividuals selected. In all cases, we must know who117 (for example) is.Furthermore, simple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is not used ifresearchers wish <strong>to</strong> ensure that certa<strong>in</strong> subgroups arepresent <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> the same proportion as they are<strong>in</strong> the population. To do this, researchers must engage <strong>in</strong>what is known as stratified sampl<strong>in</strong>g.STRATIFIED RANDOM SAMPLINGStratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is a process <strong>in</strong> which certa<strong>in</strong>subgroups, or strata, are selected for the sample <strong>in</strong>the same proportion as they exist <strong>in</strong> the population.Suppose the direc<strong>to</strong>r of research for a large school districtwants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out student response <strong>to</strong> a new twelfthgradeAmerican government textbook the district isconsider<strong>in</strong>g adopt<strong>in</strong>g. She <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> compare theachievement of students us<strong>in</strong>g the new book with that ofstudents us<strong>in</strong>g the more traditional text the district haspurchased <strong>in</strong> the past. S<strong>in</strong>ce she has reason <strong>to</strong> believethat gender is an important variable that may affectthe outcomes of her study, she decides <strong>to</strong> ensure that theproportion of males <strong>and</strong> females <strong>in</strong> the study is the sameas <strong>in</strong> the population. The steps <strong>in</strong> the sampl<strong>in</strong>g processwould be as follows:1. She identifies the target (<strong>and</strong> accessible) population:all 365 twelfth-grade students enrolled <strong>in</strong> Americangovernment courses <strong>in</strong> the district.2. She f<strong>in</strong>ds that there are 219 females (60 percent) <strong>and</strong>146 males (40 percent) <strong>in</strong> the population. She decides<strong>to</strong> have a sample made up of 30 percent of thetarget population.3. Us<strong>in</strong>g a table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers, she then r<strong>and</strong>omlyselects 30 percent from each stratum of thepopulation, which results <strong>in</strong> 66 female (30 percent of219) <strong>and</strong> 44 male (30 percent of 146) students be<strong>in</strong>gselected from these subgroups. The proportion ofmales <strong>and</strong> females is the same <strong>in</strong> both the population<strong>and</strong> sample—40 <strong>and</strong> 60 percent (Figure 6.2).The advantage of stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is that it<strong>in</strong>creases the likelihood of representativeness, especiallyif one’s sample is not very large. It virtually ensures thatkey characteristics of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the population are<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the same proportions <strong>in</strong> the sample. The disadvantageis that it requires more effort on the part of theresearcher.CLUSTER RANDOM SAMPLINGIn both r<strong>and</strong>om <strong>and</strong> stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, researcherswant <strong>to</strong> make sure that certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>dividualsare <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the sample. But there are timeswhen it is not possible <strong>to</strong> select a sample of <strong>in</strong>dividualsfrom a population. Sometimes, for example, a list of allmembers of the population of <strong>in</strong>terest is not available.Obviously, then, simple r<strong>and</strong>om or stratified r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g cannot be used. Frequently, researchers cannotselect a sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals due <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istrativeor other restrictions. This is especially true <strong>in</strong> schools.For example, if a target population were all eleventhgradestudents with<strong>in</strong> a district enrolled <strong>in</strong> U.S. his<strong>to</strong>rycourses, it would be unlikely that the researcher could


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 95In a population of 365twelfth-grade American government students,The researcher identifiestwo subgroups, or strata:219 femalestudents(60%)146 malestudents(40%)From these, she r<strong>and</strong>omlyselects a stratified sample of:66 femalestudents(60%)<strong>and</strong>44 malestudents(40%)Figure 6.2 Select<strong>in</strong>g a Stratified Samplepull out r<strong>and</strong>omly selected students <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> anexperimental curriculum. Even if it could be done, thetime <strong>and</strong> effort required would make such selection difficult.About the best the researcher could hope forwould be <strong>to</strong> study a number of <strong>in</strong>tact classes—that is,classes already <strong>in</strong> existence. The selection of groups, orclusters, of subjects rather than <strong>in</strong>dividuals is known ascluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Just as simple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>gis more effective with larger numbers of <strong>in</strong>dividuals,cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is more effective withlarger numbers of clusters.Let us consider another example of cluster r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g. The super<strong>in</strong>tendent of a large unified schooldistrict <strong>in</strong> a city on the East Coast wants <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> someidea of how teachers <strong>in</strong> the district feel about merit pay.There are 10,000 teachers <strong>in</strong> all the elementary <strong>and</strong> secondaryschools of the district, <strong>and</strong> there are 50 schoolsdistributed over a large area. The super<strong>in</strong>tendent doesnot have the funds <strong>to</strong> survey all teachers <strong>in</strong> the district,<strong>and</strong> he needs the <strong>in</strong>formation about merit pay quickly.Instead of r<strong>and</strong>omly select<strong>in</strong>g a sample of teachers fromevery school, therefore, he decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview all theteachers <strong>in</strong> selected schools. The teachers <strong>in</strong> each school,then, constitute a cluster. The super<strong>in</strong>tendent assigns anumber <strong>to</strong> each school <strong>and</strong> then uses a table of r<strong>and</strong>omnumbers <strong>to</strong> select 10 schools (20 percent of the population).All the teachers <strong>in</strong> the selected schools thenconstitute the sample. The <strong>in</strong>terviewer questions all theteachers at each of these 10 schools, rather than hav<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> travel <strong>to</strong> all the schools <strong>in</strong> the district. If these teachersdo represent the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g teachers <strong>in</strong> the district, thenthe super<strong>in</strong>tendent is justified <strong>in</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>g conclusionsabout the feel<strong>in</strong>gs of the entire population of teachers <strong>in</strong>his district about merit pay. It is possible that this sampleis not representative, of course. Because the teachers <strong>to</strong>be <strong>in</strong>terviewed all come from a small number of schools<strong>in</strong> the district, it might be the case that these schools differ<strong>in</strong> some ways from the other schools <strong>in</strong> the district,thereby <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the views of the teachers <strong>in</strong> thoseschools with regard <strong>to</strong> merit pay. The more schools selected,the more likely the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs will be applicable <strong>to</strong>the population of teachers (Figure 6.3).Cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is similar <strong>to</strong> simple r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g except that groups rather than <strong>in</strong>dividuals arer<strong>and</strong>omly selected (that is, the sampl<strong>in</strong>g unit is a grouprather than an <strong>in</strong>dividual). The advantages of clusterr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g are that it can be used when it is difficul<strong>to</strong>r impossible <strong>to</strong> select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals,it is often far easier <strong>to</strong> implement <strong>in</strong> schools, <strong>and</strong> itis frequently less time-consum<strong>in</strong>g. Its disadvantage isthat there is a far greater chance of select<strong>in</strong>g a samplethat is not representative of the population.There is a common error with regard <strong>to</strong> clusterr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g that many beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g researchers


96 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e235In a school district (population)composed of 50 schools, 10schools are r<strong>and</strong>omly selected,<strong>and</strong> then all of the teachers <strong>in</strong>each school are selected.14611181281379101620171918211415252629 302223 2431 32282740424134353633374838394344 4546474950All teachers <strong>in</strong> the selected schools are <strong>in</strong>terviewedFigure 6.3 Cluster R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>gmake: r<strong>and</strong>omly select<strong>in</strong>g only one cluster as a sample<strong>and</strong> then observ<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g all <strong>in</strong>dividuals with<strong>in</strong>that cluster. Even if there is a large number of <strong>in</strong>dividualswith<strong>in</strong> the cluster, it is the cluster that has been r<strong>and</strong>omlyselected, rather than <strong>in</strong>dividuals, <strong>and</strong> hence theresearcher is not entitled <strong>to</strong> draw conclusions about a targetpopulation of such <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Yet, some researchersdo draw such conclusions. We repeat, they should not.TWO-STAGE RANDOM SAMPLINGIt is often useful <strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>gwith <strong>in</strong>dividual r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. This is accomplishedby two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Rather than r<strong>and</strong>omlyselect<strong>in</strong>g 100 students from a population of 3,000 n<strong>in</strong>thgraderslocated <strong>in</strong> 100 classes, the researcher might decide<strong>to</strong> select 25 classes r<strong>and</strong>omly from the populationof 100 classes <strong>and</strong> then r<strong>and</strong>omly select 4 students fromeach class. This is much less time-consum<strong>in</strong>g than visit<strong>in</strong>gmost of the 100 classes. Why would this be betterthan us<strong>in</strong>g all the students <strong>in</strong> four r<strong>and</strong>omly selectedclasses? Because four classes would be <strong>to</strong>o few <strong>to</strong>ensure representativeness, even though they were selectedr<strong>and</strong>omly.Figure 6.4 illustrates the different r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>gmethods we have discussed.Nonr<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsSYSTEMATIC SAMPLINGIn systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g, every nth <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> thepopulation list is selected for <strong>in</strong>clusion <strong>in</strong> the sample.For example, <strong>in</strong> a population list of 5,000 names, <strong>to</strong> selecta sample of 500, a researcher would select everytenth name on the list until reach<strong>in</strong>g a <strong>to</strong>tal of 500names. Here is an example of this type of sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Thepr<strong>in</strong>cipal of a large middle school (grades 6–8) with1,000 students wants <strong>to</strong> know how students feel aboutthe new menu <strong>in</strong> the school cafeteria. She obta<strong>in</strong>s an alphabeticallist of all students <strong>in</strong> the school <strong>and</strong> selectsevery tenth student on the list <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> the sample. Toguard aga<strong>in</strong>st bias, she puts the numbers 1 <strong>to</strong> 10 <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 97PopulationsAE SB D TIC JUHF Y X P G QZ LW N RV M K OA B C D E25%F G H I JK L M N O50%P Q R S T25%AB CDQRNOP LMSTUEFGHIJKAB CDQRNOP LMSTUEFGHIJKR<strong>and</strong>omsample ofclustersCDSTULMDLHNPYB D25%F M O J50%P S25%QREFGCDR<strong>and</strong>omsample of<strong>in</strong>dividualsC, L, TSimpler<strong>and</strong>omStratifiedr<strong>and</strong>omClusterr<strong>and</strong>omTwo-stager<strong>and</strong>omFigure 6.4 R<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsSampleshat <strong>and</strong> draws one out. It is a 3. So she selects the studentsnumbered 3, 13, 23, 33, 43, <strong>and</strong> so on until she hasa sample of 100 students <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed.The above method is technically known as systematicsampl<strong>in</strong>g with a r<strong>and</strong>om start. In addition, thereare two terms that are frequently used when referr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g. The sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terval is the distance<strong>in</strong> the list between each of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals selectedfor the sample. In the example given above, it was 10. Asimple formula <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e it is:Population sizeDesired sample sizeThe sampl<strong>in</strong>g ratio is the proportion of <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>in</strong> the population that is selected for the sample. In theexample above, it was .10, or 10 percent. A simple way<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the sampl<strong>in</strong>g ratio is:Sample sizePopulation sizeThere is a danger <strong>in</strong> systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g that is sometimesoverlooked. If the population has been orderedsystematically—that is, if the arrangement of <strong>in</strong>dividualson the list is <strong>in</strong> some sort of pattern that accidentallyco<strong>in</strong>cides with the sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terval—a markedly biasedsample can result. This is sometimes called periodicity.Suppose that the middle school students <strong>in</strong> the preced<strong>in</strong>gexample had not been listed alphabetically but rather byhomeroom <strong>and</strong> that the homeroom teachers had previouslylisted the students <strong>in</strong> their rooms by grade po<strong>in</strong>taverage, high <strong>to</strong> low. That would mean that the betterstudents would be at the <strong>to</strong>p of each homeroom list. Supposealso that each homeroom had 30 students. If thepr<strong>in</strong>cipal began her selection of every tenth student withthe first or second or third student on the list, her samplewould consist of the better students <strong>in</strong> the school ratherthan a representation of the entire student body. (Do yousee why? Because <strong>in</strong> each homeroom, the poorest studentswould be those who were numbered between 24<strong>and</strong> 30, <strong>and</strong> they would never get chosen.)


98 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWhen plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> select a sample from a list of somesort, therefore, researchers should carefully exam<strong>in</strong>e thelist <strong>to</strong> make sure there is no cyclical pattern present. Ifthe list has been arranged <strong>in</strong> a particular order, researchersshould make sure the arrangement will notbias the sample <strong>in</strong> some way that could dis<strong>to</strong>rt the results.If such seems <strong>to</strong> be the case, steps should be taken<strong>to</strong> ensure representativeness—for example, by r<strong>and</strong>omlyselect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals from each of the cyclical portions.In fact, if a population list is r<strong>and</strong>omly ordered, a systematicsample drawn from the list is a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.CONVENIENCE SAMPLINGMany times it is extremely difficult (sometimes evenimpossible) <strong>to</strong> select either a r<strong>and</strong>om or a systematicnonr<strong>and</strong>om sample. At such times, a researcher may useconvenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g. A convenience sample is agroup of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who (conveniently) are availablefor study (Figure 6.5). Thus, a researcher might decide<strong>to</strong> study two third-grade classes at a nearby elementaryschool because the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal asks for help <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>gthe effectiveness of a new spell<strong>in</strong>g textbook. Here aresome examples of convenience samples:To f<strong>in</strong>d out how students feel about food service <strong>in</strong>the student union at an East Coast university, themanager st<strong>and</strong>s outside the ma<strong>in</strong> door of the cafeteriaone Monday morn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews the first50 students who walk out of the cafeteria.A high school counselor <strong>in</strong>terviews all the studentswho come <strong>to</strong> him for counsel<strong>in</strong>g about their careerplans.A news reporter for a local television station askspassersby on a down<strong>to</strong>wn street corner their op<strong>in</strong>ionsabout plans <strong>to</strong> build a new baseball stadium <strong>in</strong> anearby suburb.A university professor compares student reactions <strong>to</strong>two different textbooks <strong>in</strong> her statistics classes.In each of the above examples, a certa<strong>in</strong> group ofpeople was chosen for study because they were available.The obvious advantage of this type of sampl<strong>in</strong>g isProfessorA class of 40 mathstudents. The professorselects a conveniencesample of 10 studentswho are (conveniently)<strong>in</strong> the front two rows<strong>to</strong> ask how they likethe textbook.11121231341451561671781891910202122232425262728293034567313233343536373839401314151617A convenience sampleFigure 6.5 Convenience Sampl<strong>in</strong>g


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 99convenience. But just as obviously, it has a major disadvantage<strong>in</strong> that the sample will quite likely be biased.Take the case of the TV reporter who is <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gpassersby on a down<strong>to</strong>wn street corner. Many possiblesources of bias exist. First of all, of course, anyone who isnot down<strong>to</strong>wn that day has no chance <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed.Second, those <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are unwill<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> give theirviews will not be <strong>in</strong>terviewed. Third, those who agree <strong>to</strong>be <strong>in</strong>terviewed will probably be <strong>in</strong>dividuals who holdstrong op<strong>in</strong>ions one way or the other about the stadium.Fourth, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the time of day, those who are <strong>in</strong>terviewedquite possibly will be unemployed or have jobsthat do not require them <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>doors. And so forth.In general, convenience samples cannot be consideredrepresentative of any population <strong>and</strong> should beavoided if at all possible. Unfortunately, sometimesthey are the only option a researcher has. When suchis the case, the researcher should be especially careful<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong>formation on demographic <strong>and</strong> othercharacteristics of the sample studied. The studyshould also be replicated, that is, repeated, with anumber of similar samples <strong>to</strong> decrease the likelihoodthat the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed were simply a one-time occurrence.We will discuss replication <strong>in</strong> more depthlater <strong>in</strong> the chapter.PURPOSIVE SAMPLINGOn occasion, based on previous knowledge of a population<strong>and</strong> the specific purpose of the research, <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rsuse personal judgment <strong>to</strong> select a sample. <strong>Research</strong>ersassume they can use their knowledge of thepopulation <strong>to</strong> judge whether or not a particular samplewill be representative. Here are some examples:An eighth-grade social studies teacher chooses thetwo students with the highest grade po<strong>in</strong>t averages <strong>in</strong>her class, the two whose grade po<strong>in</strong>t averages fall <strong>in</strong>the middle of the class, <strong>and</strong> the two with the lowestgrade po<strong>in</strong>t averages <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how her class feelsabout <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a discussion of current events as aregular part of classroom activity. Similar samples <strong>in</strong>the past have represented the viewpo<strong>in</strong>ts of the <strong>to</strong>talclass quite accurately.A graduate student wants <strong>to</strong> know how retired peopleage 65 <strong>and</strong> over feel about their “golden years.” Hehas been <strong>to</strong>ld by one of his professors, an expert onag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the aged population, that the localAssociation of Retired Workers is a representativecross section of retired people age 65 <strong>and</strong> over. Hedecides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview a sample of 50 people who aremembers of the association <strong>to</strong> get their views.In both of these examples, previous <strong>in</strong>formation ledthe researcher <strong>to</strong> believe that the sample selected wouldbe representative of the population. There is a secondform of purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which it is not expectedthat the persons chosen are themselves representative ofthe population, but rather that they possess the necessary<strong>in</strong>formation about the population. For example,A researcher is asked <strong>to</strong> identify the unofficial powerhierarchy <strong>in</strong> a particular high school. She decides <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terview the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal, the union representative, thepr<strong>in</strong>cipal’s secretary, <strong>and</strong> the school cus<strong>to</strong>dianbecause she has prior <strong>in</strong>formation that leads her <strong>to</strong> believethey are the people who possess the <strong>in</strong>formationshe needs.For the past five years, the leaders of the teachers’association <strong>in</strong> a midwestern school district haverepresented the views of three-fourths of the teachers<strong>in</strong> the district on most major issues. This year, therefore,the district adm<strong>in</strong>istration decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewjust the leaders of the association rather than select asample from all the district’s teachers.Purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g is different from conveniencesampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> that researchers do not simply study whoeveris available but rather use their judgment <strong>to</strong> selecta sample that they believe, based on prior <strong>in</strong>formation,will provide the data they need. The major disadvantageof purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g is that the researcher’s judgmentmay be <strong>in</strong> error—he or she may not be correct <strong>in</strong> estimat<strong>in</strong>gthe representativeness of a sample or their expertiseregard<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formation needed. In the secondexample above, this year’s leaders of the teachers’ associationmay hold views markedly different from thoseof their members. Figure 6.6 illustrates the methods ofconvenience, purposive, <strong>and</strong> systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g.A Review of Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsLet us illustrate each of the previous sampl<strong>in</strong>g methodsus<strong>in</strong>g the same hypothesis: “Students with low self-esteemdemonstrate lower achievement <strong>in</strong> school subjects.”Target population: All eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> California.Accessible population: All eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> theSan Francisco Bay Area (seven counties).Feasible sample size: n 200 250.


100 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePopulationsMPWANC RB OS T UQ LXI YGJHDZEKFVJKMAB GH FX IC EL S DQ TNUYVO Z WRPA B C D EF G H I JK L M N OP Q R S TU V W X YZEasilyaccessibleEspeciallyqualifiedQXLB FB GLIYLNVQVConvenience Purposive SystematicFigure 6.6 Nonr<strong>and</strong>om Sampl<strong>in</strong>g MethodsSAMPLESSimple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Identify all eighthgraders<strong>in</strong> all public <strong>and</strong> private schools <strong>in</strong> theseven counties (estimated number of eighth-gradestudents 9,000). Assign each student a number,<strong>and</strong> then use a table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers <strong>to</strong> selecta sample of 200. The difficulty here is that it istime-consum<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> identify every eighth-grader <strong>in</strong>the Bay Area <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> contact (probably) about 200different schools <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister <strong>in</strong>struments<strong>to</strong> one or two students <strong>in</strong> those schools.Cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Identify all public <strong>and</strong>private schools hav<strong>in</strong>g an eighth grade <strong>in</strong> the sevencounties. Assign each of the schools a number, <strong>and</strong>then r<strong>and</strong>omly select four schools <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude alleighth-grade classes <strong>in</strong> each school. (We would estimate2 classes per school 30 students per class 4 schools a <strong>to</strong>tal of 240 students.) Clusterr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is much more feasible than simpler<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> implement, but it is limitedbecause of the use of only four schools, eventhough they are <strong>to</strong> be selected r<strong>and</strong>omly. For example,the selection of only four schools mayexclude the selection of private-school students.Stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Obta<strong>in</strong> data on thenumber of eighth-grade students <strong>in</strong> public versusprivate schools <strong>and</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the proportion ofeach type (e.g., 80 percent public, 20 percent private).Determ<strong>in</strong>e the number from each type <strong>to</strong>be sampled: public 80 percent of 200 160;private 20 percent of 200 40. R<strong>and</strong>omly selectsamples of 160 <strong>and</strong> 40 students from respectivesubpopulations of public <strong>and</strong> private students.Stratification may be used <strong>to</strong> ensure that the sampleis representative on other variables as well. The difficultywith this method is that stratification requiresthat the researcher know the proportions <strong>in</strong>each stratum of the population, <strong>and</strong> it also becomes<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly difficult as more variables are added.Imag<strong>in</strong>e try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> stratify not only on the publicprivatevariable but also (for example) on studentethnicity, gender, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic status, <strong>and</strong> onteacher gender <strong>and</strong> experience.Two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g: R<strong>and</strong>omly select 25schools from the accessible population of schools,<strong>and</strong> then r<strong>and</strong>omly select 8 eighth-grade studentsfrom each school (n 8 25 200). This


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHSample or Census?Samples look at only part of the population. A census tries<strong>to</strong> look at the entire population. The U.S. Census Bureau,charged with conduct<strong>in</strong>g the United States Census every tenyears, estimated that the 2000 census missed 1–2 percent ofthe population—3.4 million people, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g 1.2 percen<strong>to</strong>f the African-Americans who lived largely <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ner city.The procedure for tak<strong>in</strong>g a census consists of send<strong>in</strong>g outmail<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g up with door-<strong>to</strong>-door canvass<strong>in</strong>g ofnon-respondents.Some statisticians have proposed augment<strong>in</strong>g the headcountby survey<strong>in</strong>g a separate representative sample <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>gthis data <strong>to</strong> estimate the size <strong>and</strong> demographics of nonrespondents.Supporters of the idea argue that this would providea better picture of the population; opponents say that theassumptions <strong>in</strong>volved, along with process<strong>in</strong>g errors, wouldproduce more error.It can be argued that a sizable r<strong>and</strong>om sample of the entirepopulation accompanied by more extensive follow-up wouldprovide more accurate data than the current procedure atno greater expense, but this is precluded by the Constitution.(For more on this <strong>to</strong>pic, go <strong>to</strong> Google <strong>and</strong> check on nationalcensus sampl<strong>in</strong>g.)method is much more feasible than simple r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> more representative than clustersampl<strong>in</strong>g. It may well be the best choice <strong>in</strong> thisexample, but it still requires permission from25 schools <strong>and</strong> the resources <strong>to</strong> collect data fromeach.Convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Select all eighth-graders <strong>in</strong>four schools <strong>to</strong> which the researcher has access(aga<strong>in</strong>, we estimate two classes of 30 students perschool, so n 30 4 2 240). This methodprecludes generaliz<strong>in</strong>g beyond these four schools,unless a strong argument with support<strong>in</strong>g data canbe made for their similarity <strong>to</strong> the entire group ofaccessible schools.Purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Select eight classes fromthroughout the seven counties on the basis of demographicdata show<strong>in</strong>g that they are representativeof all eighth-graders. Particular attention mustbe paid <strong>to</strong> self-esteem <strong>and</strong> achievement scores.The problem is that such data are unlikely <strong>to</strong> beavailable <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> any case, cannot elim<strong>in</strong>ate possibledifferences between the sample <strong>and</strong> the populationon other variables—such as teacher attitude<strong>and</strong> available resources.Systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g: Select every 45th studentfrom an alphabetical list for each school.200 students <strong>in</strong> sample9,000 students <strong>in</strong> population 145This method is almost as <strong>in</strong>convenient as simple r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> is likely <strong>to</strong> result <strong>in</strong> a biased sample,s<strong>in</strong>ce the 45th name <strong>in</strong> each school is apt <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> the lastthird of the alphabet (remember there are an estimated60 eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> each school), <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g probableethnic or cultural bias.Sample SizeDraw<strong>in</strong>g conclusions about a population after study<strong>in</strong>ga sample is never <strong>to</strong>tally satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry, s<strong>in</strong>ce researcherscan never be sure that their sample is perfectly representativeof the population. Some differences between thesample <strong>and</strong> the population are bound <strong>to</strong> exist, but if thesample is r<strong>and</strong>omly selected <strong>and</strong> of sufficient size, thesedifferences are likely <strong>to</strong> be relatively <strong>in</strong>significant <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>cidental. The question rema<strong>in</strong>s, therefore, as <strong>to</strong> whatconstitutes an adequate, or sufficient, size for a sample.Unfortunately, there is no clear-cut answer <strong>to</strong> thisquestion. Suppose a target population consists of 1,000eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> a given school district. Some samplesizes, of course, are obviously <strong>to</strong>o small. Samples with1 or 2 or 3 <strong>in</strong>dividuals, for example, are so small thatthey cannot possibly be representative. Probably anysample that has less than 20 <strong>to</strong> 30 <strong>in</strong>dividuals is <strong>to</strong>osmall, s<strong>in</strong>ce that would only be 2 or 3 percent of thepopulation. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, a sample can be <strong>to</strong>olarge, given the amount of time <strong>and</strong> effort the researchermust put <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g it. In this example, a sample of250 or more <strong>in</strong>dividuals would probably be needlesslylarge, as that would constitute a quarter of the population.But what about samples of 50 or 100? Would thesebe sufficiently large? Would a sample of 200 be <strong>to</strong>olarge? At what po<strong>in</strong>t, exactly, does a sample s<strong>to</strong>p be<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>o small <strong>and</strong> become sufficiently large? The best answeris that a sample should be as large as the researcher101


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHThe Difficulty <strong>in</strong> Generaliz<strong>in</strong>gfrom a SampleIn 1936 the Literary Digest, a popular magaz<strong>in</strong>e of the time,selected a sample of voters <strong>in</strong> the United States <strong>and</strong> askedthem for whom they would vote <strong>in</strong> the upcom<strong>in</strong>g presidentialelection—Alf L<strong>and</strong>on (Republican) or Frankl<strong>in</strong> Roosevelt(Democrat). The magaz<strong>in</strong>e edi<strong>to</strong>rs obta<strong>in</strong>ed a sample of2,375,000 <strong>in</strong>dividuals from lists of au<strong>to</strong>mobile <strong>and</strong> telephoneowners <strong>in</strong> the United States (about 20 percent returned themailed postcards). On the basis of their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, the edi<strong>to</strong>rspredicted that L<strong>and</strong>on would w<strong>in</strong> by a l<strong>and</strong>slide. In fact, it wasRoosevelt who won the l<strong>and</strong>slide vic<strong>to</strong>ry. What was wrongwith the study?Certa<strong>in</strong>ly not the size of the sample. The most frequentexplanations have been that the data were collected <strong>to</strong>o farahead of the election <strong>and</strong> that a lot of people changed theirm<strong>in</strong>ds, <strong>and</strong>/or that the sample of voters was heavily biased <strong>in</strong>favor of the more affluent, <strong>and</strong>/or that the 20 percent returnrate <strong>in</strong>troduced a major bias. What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?A common misconception among beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g researchers isillustrated by the follow<strong>in</strong>g statement: “Although I obta<strong>in</strong>ed ar<strong>and</strong>om sample only from schools <strong>in</strong> San Francisco, I am entitled<strong>to</strong> generalize my f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> the entire state of Californiabecause the San Francisco schools (<strong>and</strong> hence my sample) reflecta wide variety of socioeconomic levels, ethnic groups,<strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g styles.” The statement is <strong>in</strong>correct because varietyis not the same th<strong>in</strong>g as representativeness. In order for theSan Francisco schools <strong>to</strong> be representative of all the schools <strong>in</strong>California, they must be very similar (ideally, identical) withrespect <strong>to</strong> characteristics such as the ones mentioned. Askyourself: Are the San Francisco schools representative of theentire state with regard <strong>to</strong> ethnic composition of students? Theanswer, of course, is that they are not.can obta<strong>in</strong> with a reasonable expenditure of time <strong>and</strong>energy. This, of course, is not as much help as onewould like, but it suggests that researchers should try <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> as large a sample as they reasonably can.There are a few guidel<strong>in</strong>es that we would suggest withregard <strong>to</strong> the m<strong>in</strong>imum number of subjects needed. Fordescriptive studies, we th<strong>in</strong>k a sample with a m<strong>in</strong>imumnumber of 100 is essential. For correlational studies, asample of at least 50 is deemed necessary <strong>to</strong> establish theexistence of a relationship. For experimental <strong>and</strong> causalcomparativestudies, we recommend a m<strong>in</strong>imum of 30<strong>in</strong>dividuals per group, although sometimes experimentalstudies with only 15 <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> each group can be defendedif they are very tightly controlled; studies us<strong>in</strong>gonly 15 subjects per group should probably be replicated,however, before <strong>to</strong>o much is made of any f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.*External Validity: Generaliz<strong>in</strong>gfrom a SampleAs <strong>in</strong>dicated earlier <strong>in</strong> this chapter, researchers generalizewhen they apply the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of a particular study <strong>to</strong>people or sett<strong>in</strong>gs that go beyond the particular people*More specific guidel<strong>in</strong>es are provided <strong>in</strong> the <strong>Research</strong> Tips box onpage 230 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 11.or sett<strong>in</strong>gs used <strong>in</strong> the study. The whole notion ofscience is built on the idea of generaliz<strong>in</strong>g. Every scienceseeks <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d basic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples or laws that can beapplied <strong>to</strong> a great variety of situations <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> the case ofthe social sciences, <strong>to</strong> a great many people. Mostresearchers wish <strong>to</strong> generalize their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> appropriatepopulations. But when is generaliz<strong>in</strong>g warranted?When can researchers say with confidence thatwhat they have learned about a sample is also true ofthe population? Both the nature of the sample <strong>and</strong> theenvironmental conditions—the sett<strong>in</strong>g—with<strong>in</strong> which astudy takes place must be considered <strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g aboutgeneralizability. The extent <strong>to</strong> which the results of astudy can be generalized determ<strong>in</strong>es the externalvalidity of the study.POPULATION GENERALIZABILITYPopulation generalizability refers <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong>which a sample represents the population of <strong>in</strong>terest. Ifthe results of a study only apply <strong>to</strong> the group be<strong>in</strong>g studied<strong>and</strong> if that group is fairly small or is narrowlydef<strong>in</strong>ed, the usefulness of any f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs is seriously limited.This is why try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a representative sampleis so important. Because conduct<strong>in</strong>g a study takes aconsiderable amount of time, energy, <strong>and</strong> (frequently)money, researchers usually want the results of an <strong>in</strong>vestigation<strong>to</strong> be as widely applicable as possible.102


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 103When we speak of representativeness, however, weare referr<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>to</strong> the essential, or relevant, characteristicsof a population. What do we mean by relevant?Only that the characteristics referred <strong>to</strong> might possiblybe a contribut<strong>in</strong>g fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>to</strong> any results that are obta<strong>in</strong>ed.For example, if a researcher wished <strong>to</strong> select a sample offirst- <strong>and</strong> second-graders <strong>to</strong> study the effect of read<strong>in</strong>gmethod on pupil achievement, such characteristics asheight, eye color, or jump<strong>in</strong>g ability would be judged <strong>to</strong>be irrelevant—that is, we would not expect any variation<strong>in</strong> them <strong>to</strong> have an effect on how easily a childlearns <strong>to</strong> read, <strong>and</strong> hence we would not be overly concernedif those characteristics were not adequately represented<strong>in</strong> the sample. Other characteristics, such asage, gender, or visual acuity, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, might(logically) have an effect <strong>and</strong> hence should be appropriatelyrepresented <strong>in</strong> the sample.Whenever purposive or convenience samples areused, generalization is made more plausible if data arepresented <strong>to</strong> show that the sample is representative ofthe <strong>in</strong>tended population on at least some relevant variables.This procedure, however, can never guaranteerepresentativeness on all relevant variables.One aspect of generalizability that is often overlooked<strong>in</strong> “methods” or “treatment” studies perta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong>the teachers, counselors, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, or others whoadm<strong>in</strong>ister the various treatments. We must rememberthat such studies <strong>in</strong>volve not only a sample of students,clients, or other recipients of the treatments but also asample of those who implement the various treatments.Thus, a study that r<strong>and</strong>omly selects students but notteachers is only entitled <strong>to</strong> generalize the outcomes <strong>to</strong>the population of students—if they are taught by thesame teachers. To generalize the results <strong>to</strong> other teachers,the sample of teachers must also be selected r<strong>and</strong>omly<strong>and</strong> must be sufficiently large.F<strong>in</strong>ally, we must remember that the sample <strong>in</strong> anystudy is the group about whom data are actuallyobta<strong>in</strong>ed. The best sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan is of no value if <strong>in</strong>formationis miss<strong>in</strong>g on a sizable portion of the <strong>in</strong>itialsample. Once the sample has been selected, everyeffort must be made <strong>to</strong> ensure that the necessary dataare obta<strong>in</strong>ed on each person <strong>in</strong> the sample. This isoften difficult <strong>to</strong> do, particularly with questionnairetypesurvey studies, but the results are well worththe time <strong>and</strong> energy expended. Unfortunately, thereare no clear guidel<strong>in</strong>es as <strong>to</strong> how many subjects canbe lost before representativeness is seriously impaired.Any researchers who lose over 10 percent of theorig<strong>in</strong>ally selected sample would be well advised <strong>to</strong>acknowledge this limitation <strong>and</strong> qualify their conclusionsaccord<strong>in</strong>gly.Do researchers always want <strong>to</strong> generalize? The onlytime researchers are not <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g beyondthe conf<strong>in</strong>es of a particular study is when the resultsof an <strong>in</strong>vestigation are of <strong>in</strong>terest only as applied <strong>to</strong>a particular group of people at a particular time, <strong>and</strong>where all of the members of the group are <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>the study. An example might be the op<strong>in</strong>ions of an elementaryschool faculty on a specific issue such aswhether <strong>to</strong> implement a new math program. This mightbe of value <strong>to</strong> that faculty for decision mak<strong>in</strong>g or programplann<strong>in</strong>g, but not <strong>to</strong> anyone else.WHEN RANDOM SAMPLINGIS NOT FEASIBLEAs we have shown, sometimes it is not feasible or evenpossible <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>om sample. When this is thecase, researchers should describe the sample as thoroughlyas possible (detail<strong>in</strong>g, for example, age, gender,ethnicity, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic status) so that <strong>in</strong>terestedothers can judge for themselves the degree <strong>to</strong> which anyf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs apply, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> whom <strong>and</strong> where. This is clearlyan <strong>in</strong>ferior procedure compared <strong>to</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g,but sometimes it is the only alternative one has.There is another possibility when a r<strong>and</strong>om sample isimpossible <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>: It is called replication. The researcher(or other researchers) repeats the study us<strong>in</strong>gdifferent groups of subjects <strong>in</strong> different situations. If astudy is repeated several times, us<strong>in</strong>g different groupsof subjects <strong>and</strong> under different conditions of geography,socioeconomic level, ability, <strong>and</strong> so on, <strong>and</strong> if the resultsobta<strong>in</strong>ed are essentially the same <strong>in</strong> each case, aresearcher may have additional confidence about generaliz<strong>in</strong>gthe f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.In the vast majority of studies that have been done<strong>in</strong> education, r<strong>and</strong>om samples have not been used.There seem <strong>to</strong> be two reasons for this. First, educationalresearchers may be unaware of the hazards <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g when one does not have a r<strong>and</strong>omsample. Second, <strong>in</strong> many studies it is simply notfeasible for a researcher <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vest the time, money, orother resources necessary <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.For the results of a particular study <strong>to</strong> be applicable <strong>to</strong>a larger group, then, the researcher must argue conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>glythat the sample employed, even though notchosen r<strong>and</strong>omly, is <strong>in</strong> fact representative of the targetpopulation. This is difficult, however, <strong>and</strong> always subject<strong>to</strong> contrary arguments.


104 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eECOLOGICAL GENERALIZABILITYEcological generalizability refers <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong>which the results of a study can be extended <strong>to</strong> othersett<strong>in</strong>gs or conditions. <strong>Research</strong>ers must make clear thenature of the environmental conditions—the sett<strong>in</strong>g—under which a study takes place. These conditions mustbe the same <strong>in</strong> all important respects <strong>in</strong> any new situation<strong>in</strong> which researchers wish <strong>to</strong> assert that their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsapply. For example, it is not justifiable <strong>to</strong> generalizefrom studies on the effects of a new read<strong>in</strong>g programon third-graders <strong>in</strong> a large urban school system <strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>gmathematics, even <strong>to</strong> those students <strong>in</strong> that system.<strong>Research</strong> results from urban school environments maynot apply <strong>to</strong> suburban or rural school environments; resultsobta<strong>in</strong>ed with transparencies may not apply <strong>to</strong>those with textbooks. What holds true for one subject,or with certa<strong>in</strong> materials, or under certa<strong>in</strong> conditions, orat certa<strong>in</strong> times may not generalize <strong>to</strong> other subjects,materials, conditions, or times.An example of <strong>in</strong>appropriate ecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>goccurred <strong>in</strong> a study that found that a particular methodof <strong>in</strong>struction applied <strong>to</strong> map read<strong>in</strong>g resulted <strong>in</strong> greatertransfer <strong>to</strong> general map <strong>in</strong>terpretation on the part offifth-graders <strong>in</strong> several schools. The researcher accord<strong>in</strong>glyrecommended that the method of <strong>in</strong>struction beused <strong>in</strong> other content areas, such as mathematics <strong>and</strong>science, overlook<strong>in</strong>g differences <strong>in</strong> content, materials,<strong>and</strong> skills <strong>in</strong>volved, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> probable differences<strong>in</strong> resources, teacher experience, <strong>and</strong> the like. Improperecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>g such as this rema<strong>in</strong>s the bane ofmuch educational research.Unfortunately, application of the powerful techniqueof r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is virtually never possible with respect<strong>to</strong> ecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>g. While it is conceivablethat a researcher could identify “populations” of organizationpatterns, materials, classroom conditions, <strong>and</strong>so on <strong>and</strong> then r<strong>and</strong>omly select a sizable number ofcomb<strong>in</strong>ations from all possible comb<strong>in</strong>ations, the logisticsof do<strong>in</strong>g so boggle the m<strong>in</strong>d. Therefore, researchersmust be cautious about generaliz<strong>in</strong>g the results fromany one study. Only when outcomes have been shown<strong>to</strong> be similar through replication across specific environmentalconditions can we generalize across thoseconditions. Figure 6.7 illustrates the difference betweenpopulation <strong>and</strong> ecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>g.POPULATION GENERALIZINGECOLOGICAL GENERALIZINGFBZEA STD UJ H IG NY QPK R WVO MOthertextbooksOthermethodsOthertechnologyOthercontentarea(s)OtheraspectsPopulationEnvironmental conditionsDH P NL YSampleTextbooksused<strong>in</strong>studyMethodsused<strong>in</strong>studyFigure 6.7 Population as Opposed <strong>to</strong> Ecological Generaliz<strong>in</strong>gAudiovisualmaterialsusedContentarea(s)<strong>in</strong>study?


CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 105Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.SAMPLES AND SAMPLINGThe term sampl<strong>in</strong>g, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the process of select<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>dividuals who will participate (e.g., be observed or questioned) <strong>in</strong> a researchstudy.A sample is any part of a population of <strong>in</strong>dividuals on whom <strong>in</strong>formation isobta<strong>in</strong>ed. It may, for a variety of reasons, be different from the sample orig<strong>in</strong>allyselected.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsSAMPLES AND POPULATIONSThe term population, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> all the members of a particulargroup. It is the group of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the researcher, the group <strong>to</strong> whom the researcherwould like <strong>to</strong> generalize the results of a study.A target population is the actual population <strong>to</strong> whom the researcher would like <strong>to</strong>generalize; the accessible population is the population <strong>to</strong> whom the researcher isentitled <strong>to</strong> generalize.A representative sample is a sample that is similar <strong>to</strong> the population on all characteristics.RANDOM VERSUS NONRANDOM SAMPLINGSampl<strong>in</strong>g may be either r<strong>and</strong>om or nonr<strong>and</strong>om. R<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>in</strong>cludesimple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>in</strong>clude systematicsampl<strong>in</strong>g, convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g.RANDOM SAMPLING METHODSA simple r<strong>and</strong>om sample is a sample selected from a population <strong>in</strong> such a mannerthat all members of the population have an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected.A stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sample is a sample selected so that certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics arerepresented <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> the same proportion as they occur <strong>in</strong> the population.A cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sample is one obta<strong>in</strong>ed by us<strong>in</strong>g groups as the sampl<strong>in</strong>g unit ratherthan <strong>in</strong>dividuals.A two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sample selects groups r<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>and</strong> then chooses <strong>in</strong>dividualsr<strong>and</strong>omly from these groups.A table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers lists <strong>and</strong> arranges numbers <strong>in</strong> no particular order <strong>and</strong> canbe used <strong>to</strong> select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.


106 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eNONRANDOM SAMPLING METHODSA systematic sample is obta<strong>in</strong>ed by select<strong>in</strong>g every nth name <strong>in</strong> a population.A convenience sample is any group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals that is conveniently available <strong>to</strong>be studied.A purposive sample consists of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who have special qualifications of somesort or are deemed representative on the basis of prior evidence.SAMPLE SIZESamples should be as large as a researcher can obta<strong>in</strong> with a reasonable expenditure oftime <strong>and</strong> energy. A recommended m<strong>in</strong>imum number of subjects is 100 for a descriptivestudy, 50 for a correlational study, <strong>and</strong> 30 <strong>in</strong> each group for experimental <strong>and</strong> causalcomparativestudies.EXTERNAL VALIDITY (GENERALIZABILITY)The term external validity, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the extent that the results ofa study can be generalized from a sample <strong>to</strong> a population.The term population generalizability refers <strong>to</strong> the extent <strong>to</strong> which the results of astudy can be generalized <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tended population.The term ecological generalizability refers <strong>to</strong> the extent <strong>to</strong> which the results of astudy can be generalized <strong>to</strong> conditions or sett<strong>in</strong>gs other than those that prevailed <strong>in</strong>a particular study.REPLICATIONWhen a study is replicated, it is repeated with a new sample <strong>and</strong> sometimes undernew conditions.Key Termsaccessiblepopulation 91cluster r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g 95conveniencesampl<strong>in</strong>g 98ecologicalgeneralizability 104external validity 102generaliz<strong>in</strong>g 102nonr<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g 93periodicity 97population 90populationgeneralizability 102purposivesampl<strong>in</strong>g 99r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g 92r<strong>and</strong>om start 97replication 103representativeness 103sample 90sampl<strong>in</strong>g 90sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terval 97sampl<strong>in</strong>g ratio 97simple r<strong>and</strong>omsample 93stratified r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g 94systematicsampl<strong>in</strong>g 96table of r<strong>and</strong>omnumbers 93target population 91two-stage r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g 96For Discussion1. A team of researchers wants <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e student attitudes about the recreationalservices available <strong>in</strong> the student union on campus. The team s<strong>to</strong>ps the first 100 studentsit meets on a street <strong>in</strong> the middle of the campus <strong>and</strong> asks each of them questions aboutthe union. What are some possible ways that this sample might be biased?


2. Suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the effects of music on learn<strong>in</strong>g. Heobta<strong>in</strong>s permission from a nearby elementary school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>to</strong> use the two third-gradeclasses <strong>in</strong> the school. The ability level of the two classes, as shown by st<strong>and</strong>ardizedtests, grade po<strong>in</strong>t averages, <strong>and</strong> faculty op<strong>in</strong>ion, is quite similar. In one class, theresearcher plays classical music softly every day for a semester. In the other class, nomusic is played. At the end of the semester, he f<strong>in</strong>ds that the class <strong>in</strong> which the musicwas played has a markedly higher average <strong>in</strong> arithmetic than the other class, althoughthey do not differ <strong>in</strong> any other respect. To what population (if any) might the resultsof this study be generalized? What, exactly, could the researcher say about the effectsof music on learn<strong>in</strong>g?3. When, if ever, might a researcher not be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g the results of astudy? Expla<strong>in</strong>.4. “The larger a sample, the more justified a researcher is <strong>in</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from it <strong>to</strong> apopulation.” Is this statement true? Why or why not?5. Some people have argued that no population can ever be studied <strong>in</strong> its entirety.Would you agree? Why or why not?6. “The more narrowly researchers def<strong>in</strong>e the population, the more they limit generalizability.”Is this always true? Discuss.7. “The best sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan is of no value if <strong>in</strong>formation is miss<strong>in</strong>g on a sizable proportionof the <strong>in</strong>itial sample.” Why is this so? Discuss.8. “The use of r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g is almost never possible with respect <strong>to</strong> ecologicalgeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g.” Why is this so? Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of a possible study for which ecologicalgeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g would be possible? If so, give an example.CHAPTER 6 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g 107


108 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 6: Sampl<strong>in</strong>g PlanUse Problem Sheet 6 <strong>to</strong> describe, as fully as you can, your sample—that is, the subjects you will<strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> your study. Describe the type of sample you plan <strong>to</strong> use <strong>and</strong> how you will obta<strong>in</strong> thesample. Indicate whether you expect your study <strong>to</strong> have population generalizability. If so, <strong>to</strong>what population? if not, why not? Then <strong>in</strong>dicate whether the study would have ecologicalgeneralizability. If so, <strong>to</strong> what sett<strong>in</strong>gs? if not, why would it not?Problem Sheet 6Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Plan1. My <strong>in</strong>tended sample (subjects who would participate <strong>in</strong> my study) consists of (tellwho <strong>and</strong> how many): ___________________________________________________2. Demographics (characteristics of the sample) are as follows:a. Age range ______________________________________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.b. Sex distribution ______________________________________c. Ethnic breakdown _______________________________________d. Location (where are these subjects?) ____________________________e. Other characteristics not mentioned above that you deem important (use a sheet ofpaper if you need more space) _________________________________________A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.3. Type of sample: simple r<strong>and</strong>om _____ stratified r<strong>and</strong>om _____ systematic _____cluster r<strong>and</strong>om ____ two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om ____ convenience ____ purposive ____4. I will obta<strong>in</strong> my sample by: ______________________________________________5. External validity (I will generalize <strong>to</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g population):a. To what accessible population? ________________________________________b. To what target population? ____________________________________________c. If results are not generalizable, why not? _________________________________6. Ecological validity (I will generalize <strong>to</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g sett<strong>in</strong>gs/conditions):a. Generalizable <strong>to</strong> what sett<strong>in</strong>g(s)? _______________________________________b. Generalizable <strong>to</strong> what condition(s)? _____________________________________c. If results are not generalizable, why not? _________________________________108


Instrumentation7Instructions: Circle the choice after each statement that <strong>in</strong>dicates your op<strong>in</strong>ion.1. Teachers’ unions should be abolished.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(5) (4) (3) (2) (1)2. School adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs should be required <strong>to</strong> teach at least one class <strong>in</strong> a publicschool every year.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(5) (4) (3) (2) (1)3. Classroom teachers should choose who the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs will be <strong>in</strong> theirschool.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(5) (4) (3) (2) (1)OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“data.”“unobtrusive measures” <strong>and</strong> give twoExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termexamples of such measures.“<strong>in</strong>strumentation.”Name four types of measurement scalesName three ways <strong>in</strong> which data can be <strong>and</strong> give an example of each.collected by researchers.Name three different types of scores usedExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term “datacollection<strong>in</strong>strument.”example of each.<strong>in</strong> educational research <strong>and</strong> give anDescribe five types of researchercompleted<strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> educational norm-referenced <strong>and</strong> criterion-referencedDescribe briefly the difference betweenresearch.<strong>in</strong>struments.Describe five types of subject-completed Describe briefly how <strong>to</strong> score, tabulate,<strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> educational research. <strong>and</strong> code data for analysis.What Are Data?Key QuestionsValidity, Reliability, <strong>and</strong>ObjectivityUsabilityMeans of Classify<strong>in</strong>gData-CollectionInstrumentsWho Provides the Information?Where Did the InstrumentCome from?Written Response VersusPerformanceExamples of Data-Collection Instruments<strong>Research</strong>er-CompletedInstrumentsSubject-CompletedInstrumentsUnobtrusive MeasuresTypes of ScoresRaw ScoresDerived ScoresWhich Scores <strong>to</strong> Use?Norm-Referenced VersusCriterion-ReferencedInstrumentsNorm-Referenced InstrumentsCriterion-ReferencedInstrumentsMeasurement ScalesNom<strong>in</strong>al ScalesOrd<strong>in</strong>al ScalesInterval ScalesRatio ScalesMeasurement ScalesReconsideredPrepar<strong>in</strong>g Datafor AnalysisScor<strong>in</strong>g the DataTabulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cod<strong>in</strong>gthe Data


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Develop<strong>in</strong>g InstrumentsAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 7.1: Major Categories of Instruments <strong>and</strong> Their UsesActivity 7.2: Which Type of Instrument Is Most Appropriate?Activity 7.3: Types of ScalesActivity 7.4: Norm-Referenced vs. Criterion-ReferencedInstrumentsActivity 7.5: Develop<strong>in</strong>g a Rat<strong>in</strong>g ScaleActivity 7.6: <strong>Design</strong> an InstrumentMonica Stuart <strong>and</strong> Ben Colen are discuss<strong>in</strong>g last night’s lecture <strong>in</strong> their educational research class.“I must admit that I was pretty impressed,” says Monica.“<strong>How</strong> come?”“That list of measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments Professor Mendoza described last night. Questionnaires, rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, truefalsetests, sociograms, logs, anecdotal records, tally sheets, . . . It just went on <strong>and</strong> on. I never knew there were somany ways <strong>to</strong> measure someth<strong>in</strong>g—so many measur<strong>in</strong>g devices. And what he said about measurement—that reallyimpressed me! Remember? He said that if someth<strong>in</strong>g exists, you can measure it!”“Yeah, it was quite a feat. I have <strong>to</strong> admit that. I don’t know about the idea that everyth<strong>in</strong>g can be measured,though. What about someth<strong>in</strong>g that is very abstract?”“Like what?”“Well, how about, say, alienation? or motivation? <strong>How</strong> would you measure them? I’m not sure they even can bemeasured!”“Well, here’s what I’d do,” says Monica. “I would . . .”<strong>How</strong> would you measure motivation? Do <strong>in</strong>struments exist that could measure someth<strong>in</strong>g this abstract? To getsome ideas, read this chapter.What Are Data?The term data refers <strong>to</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formationresearchers obta<strong>in</strong> on the subjects of their research.Demographic <strong>in</strong>formation, such as age, gender, ethnicity,religion, <strong>and</strong> so on, is one k<strong>in</strong>d of data; scores from acommercially available or researcher-prepared test areanother. Responses <strong>to</strong> the researcher’s questions <strong>in</strong> anoral <strong>in</strong>terview or written replies <strong>to</strong> a survey questionnaireare other k<strong>in</strong>ds. Essays written by students, gradepo<strong>in</strong>t averages obta<strong>in</strong>ed from school records, performancelogs kept by coaches, anecdotal records ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>edby teachers or counselors—all constitute variousk<strong>in</strong>ds of data that researchers might want <strong>to</strong> collect aspart of a research <strong>in</strong>vestigation. An important decisionfor every researcher <strong>to</strong> make dur<strong>in</strong>g the plann<strong>in</strong>g phaseof an <strong>in</strong>vestigation, therefore, is what k<strong>in</strong>d(s) of data heor she <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> collect. The device (such as a pencil<strong>and</strong>-papertest, a questionnaire, or a rat<strong>in</strong>g scale) the researcheruses <strong>to</strong> collect data is called an <strong>in</strong>strument.*KEY QUESTIONSGenerally, the whole process of prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> collectdata is called <strong>in</strong>strumentation. It <strong>in</strong>volves not only the*Most, but not all, research requires the use of an <strong>in</strong>strument. Instudies where data are obta<strong>in</strong>ed exclusively from exist<strong>in</strong>g records(grades, attendance, etc.), no <strong>in</strong>strument is needed.110


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 111selection or design of the <strong>in</strong>struments but also theprocedures <strong>and</strong> the conditions under which the <strong>in</strong>strumentswill be adm<strong>in</strong>istered. Several questions arise:1. Where will the data be collected? This questionrefers <strong>to</strong> the location of the data collection. Wherewill it be? <strong>in</strong> a classroom? a schoolyard? a privatehome? on the street?2. When will the data be collected? This questionrefers <strong>to</strong> the time of collection. When is it <strong>to</strong> takeplace? <strong>in</strong> the morn<strong>in</strong>g? afternoon? even<strong>in</strong>g? over aweekend?3. <strong>How</strong> often are the data <strong>to</strong> be collected? This questionrefers <strong>to</strong> the frequency of collection. <strong>How</strong> manytimes are the data <strong>to</strong> be collected? only once? twice?more than twice?4. Who is <strong>to</strong> collect the data? This question refers <strong>to</strong> theadm<strong>in</strong>istration of the <strong>in</strong>struments. Who is <strong>to</strong> do this?the researcher? someone selected <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong>ed by theresearcher?These questions are important because howresearchers answer them may affect the data obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Itis a mistake <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k that researchers need only locate ordevelop a “good” <strong>in</strong>strument. The data provided by any<strong>in</strong>strument may be affected by any or all of the preced<strong>in</strong>gconsiderations. The most highly regarded of <strong>in</strong>strumentswill provide useless data, for <strong>in</strong>stance, if adm<strong>in</strong>istered<strong>in</strong>correctly; by someone disliked by respondents; undernoisy, <strong>in</strong>hospitable conditions; or when subjects areexhausted.All the above questions are important for researchers<strong>to</strong> answer, therefore, before they beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> collect thedata they need. A researcher’s decisions about location,time, frequency, <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration are always affectedby the k<strong>in</strong>d(s) of <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> be used. And for it <strong>to</strong> beof any value, every <strong>in</strong>strument, no matter what k<strong>in</strong>d,must allow researchers <strong>to</strong> draw accurate conclusionsabout the capabilities or other characteristics of thepeople be<strong>in</strong>g studied.VALIDITY, RELIABILITY, AND OBJECTIVITYA frequently used (but somewhat old-fashioned) def<strong>in</strong>itionof a valid <strong>in</strong>strument is that it measures what it issupposed <strong>to</strong> measure. A more accurate def<strong>in</strong>ition ofvalidity revolves around the defensibility of the <strong>in</strong>ferencesresearchers make from the data collected throughthe use of an <strong>in</strong>strument. An <strong>in</strong>strument, after all, is adevice used <strong>to</strong> gather data. <strong>Research</strong>ers then use thesedata <strong>to</strong> make <strong>in</strong>ferences about the characteristics of certa<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals.* But <strong>to</strong> be of any use, these <strong>in</strong>ferencesmust be correct. All researchers, therefore, want <strong>in</strong>strumentsthat permit them <strong>to</strong> draw warranted, or valid, conclusionsabout the characteristics (ability, achievement,attitudes, <strong>and</strong> so on) of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals they study.To measure math achievement, for example, aresearcher needs <strong>to</strong> have some assurance that the <strong>in</strong>strumentshe <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> use actually does measure suchachievement. Another researcher who wants <strong>to</strong> knowwhat people th<strong>in</strong>k or how they feel about a particular<strong>to</strong>pic needs assurance that the <strong>in</strong>strument used willallow him <strong>to</strong> make accurate <strong>in</strong>ferences. There are variousways <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> such assurance, <strong>and</strong> we discuss them<strong>in</strong> Chapter 8.A second consideration is reliability. A reliable<strong>in</strong>strument is one that gives consistent results. If aresearcher tested the math achievement of a group of <strong>in</strong>dividualsat two or more different times, for example, heor she should expect <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> close <strong>to</strong> the same resultseach time. This consistency would give the researcherconfidence that the results actually represented theachievement of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>volved. As with validity,a number of procedures can be used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e thereliability of an <strong>in</strong>strument. We discuss several of them<strong>in</strong> Chapter 8.A f<strong>in</strong>al consideration is objectivity. Objectivity refers<strong>to</strong> the absence of subjective judgments. Whenever possible,researchers should try <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate subjectivity fromthe judgments they make about the achievement, performance,or characteristics of subjects. Unfortunately,complete objectivity is probably never atta<strong>in</strong>ed.We discuss each of these concepts <strong>in</strong> much moredetail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8. In this chapter, we look at some ofthe various k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>struments that can be (<strong>and</strong> oftenare) used <strong>in</strong> research <strong>and</strong> discuss how <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>and</strong> selectthem.USABILITYA number of practical considerations face every researcher.One of these is how easy it will be <strong>to</strong> use any*Sometimes <strong>in</strong>struments are used <strong>to</strong> collect data on someth<strong>in</strong>g otherthan <strong>in</strong>dividuals (such as groups, programs, <strong>and</strong> environments), buts<strong>in</strong>ce most of the time we are concerned with <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch, we use this term<strong>in</strong>ology throughout our discussion.


112 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>in</strong>strument he or she designs or selects. <strong>How</strong> long will ittake <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister? Are the directions clear? Is it appropriatefor the ethnic or other groups <strong>to</strong> whom it will beadm<strong>in</strong>istered? <strong>How</strong> easy is it <strong>to</strong> score? <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret theresults? <strong>How</strong> much does it cost? Do equivalent formsexist? Have any problems been reported by others whoused it? Does evidence of its reliability <strong>and</strong> validityexist? Gett<strong>in</strong>g satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry answers <strong>to</strong> such questionscan save a researcher a lot of time <strong>and</strong> energy <strong>and</strong> canprevent a lot of headaches.Means of Classify<strong>in</strong>gData-Collection InstrumentsInstruments can be classified <strong>in</strong> a number of ways. Hereare some of the most useful.WHO PROVIDES THE INFORMATION?In educational research, three general methods areavailable for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation. <strong>Research</strong>ers can getthe <strong>in</strong>formation (1) themselves, with little or no <strong>in</strong>volvemen<strong>to</strong>f other people; (2) directly from the subjects ofthe study; or (3) from others, frequently referred <strong>to</strong> as<strong>in</strong>formants, who are knowledgeable about the subjects.Let us follow a specific example. A researcher wishes <strong>to</strong>test the hypothesis that <strong>in</strong>quiry teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ryclasses results <strong>in</strong> higher-level th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g than does the lecturemethod. The researcher may elect option 1, <strong>in</strong>which case she may observe students <strong>in</strong> the classroom,not<strong>in</strong>g the frequency of oral statements <strong>in</strong>dicative ofhigher-level th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Or, she may exam<strong>in</strong>e exist<strong>in</strong>g studentrecords that may <strong>in</strong>clude test results <strong>and</strong>/or anecdotalmaterial she considers <strong>in</strong>dicative of higher-levelth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. If she elects option 2, the researcher is likely<strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister tests or request student products (essays,problem sheets) for evidence. She may also decide <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terview students us<strong>in</strong>g questions designed <strong>to</strong> revealtheir th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about his<strong>to</strong>ry (or other <strong>to</strong>pics). F<strong>in</strong>ally, ifthe researcher chooses option 3, she is likely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewpersons (teachers, other students) or ask them <strong>to</strong>fill out rat<strong>in</strong>g scales <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>terviewees assesseach student’s th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skills based on their prior experiencewith the student. Examples of each type ofmethod are given below.1. <strong>Research</strong>er <strong>in</strong>strumentsA researcher <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> memorydevelopment counts the number of times it takesdifferent nursery school children <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>to</strong> navigatetheir way correctly through a maze located<strong>in</strong> their school playground. He records his f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gson a tally sheet.A researcher <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the concept of mutualattraction describes <strong>in</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g field notes howthe behaviors of people who work <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>in</strong>various sett<strong>in</strong>gs have been observed <strong>to</strong> differ onthis variable.2. Subject <strong>in</strong>strumentsA researcher <strong>in</strong> an elementary school adm<strong>in</strong>istersa weekly spell<strong>in</strong>g test that requires students <strong>to</strong>spell correctly the new words learned <strong>in</strong> classdur<strong>in</strong>g the week.At a researcher’s request, an adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r passesout a questionnaire dur<strong>in</strong>g a faculty meet<strong>in</strong>gthat asks the faculty’s op<strong>in</strong>ions about the newmathematics curriculum recently <strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong> thedistrict.A researcher asks high school English teachers <strong>to</strong>have their students keep a daily log <strong>in</strong> which theyrecord their reactions <strong>to</strong> the plays they read eachweek.3. Informant <strong>in</strong>strumentsA researcher asks teachers <strong>to</strong> use a rat<strong>in</strong>g scale <strong>to</strong>rate each of their students on their phonic read<strong>in</strong>gskills.A researcher asks parents <strong>to</strong> keep anecdotalrecords describ<strong>in</strong>g the TV characters their preschoolersspontaneously role-play.A researcher <strong>in</strong>terviews the president of the studentcouncil about student views on the school’sdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary code. Her responses are recorded onan <strong>in</strong>terview schedule.WHERE DID THE INSTRUMENT COME FROM?There are essentially two basic ways for a researcher <strong>to</strong>acquire an <strong>in</strong>strument: (1) f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister a previouslyexist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument of some sort or (2) adm<strong>in</strong>isteran <strong>in</strong>strument the researcher personally developed orhad developed by someone else.Develop<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strument has its problems. Primarily,it is not easy <strong>to</strong> do. Develop<strong>in</strong>g a “good” <strong>in</strong>strumentusually takes a fair amount of time <strong>and</strong> effort, not <strong>to</strong>mention a considerable amount of skill.Select<strong>in</strong>g an already developed <strong>in</strong>strument whenappropriate, therefore, is preferred. Such <strong>in</strong>strumentsare usually developed by experts who possess the necessaryskills. Choos<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strument that has already


***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSSome Tips About Develop<strong>in</strong>gYour Own Instrument1. Be sure you are clear about what variables are <strong>to</strong> be assessed.Much time <strong>and</strong> effort can be wasted by def<strong>in</strong>itionsthat are <strong>to</strong>o ambiguous. If more than one variable is <strong>in</strong>volved,be sure that both the mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the items foreach variable are kept dist<strong>in</strong>ct. In general, a particular itemor question should be used for only one variable.2. Review exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments that measure similar variables<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> decide upon a format <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> ideason specific items.3. Decide on a format for each variable. Although it is sometimesappropriate <strong>to</strong> mix multiple-choice, true-false,match<strong>in</strong>g, rat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> open-ended items, do<strong>in</strong>g so complicatesscor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> is usually undesirable. Remember:Different variables often require different formats.4. Beg<strong>in</strong> compil<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>/or writ<strong>in</strong>g items. Be sure that, <strong>in</strong> yourjudgment, each is logically valid—that is, that the item isconsistent with the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the variable. Try <strong>to</strong> ensurethat the vocabulary is appropriate for the <strong>in</strong>tended respondents.5. Have colleagues review the items for logical validity.Supply colleagues with a copy of your def<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>and</strong> adescription of the <strong>in</strong>tended respondents. Be sure <strong>to</strong> havethem evaluate format as well as content.6. Revise items based on colleague feedback. At this po<strong>in</strong>t,try <strong>to</strong> have about twice as many items as you <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> use<strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al form (generally at least 20). Remember thatmore items generally provide higher reliability.7. Locate a group of people with experience appropriate <strong>to</strong>your study. Have them review your items for logical validity.Make any revisions needed, <strong>and</strong> complete youritems. You should have half aga<strong>in</strong> as many items as <strong>in</strong>tended<strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al form.8. Try out your <strong>in</strong>strument with a group of respondents whoare as similar as possible <strong>to</strong> your study respondents. Havethem complete the <strong>in</strong>strument, <strong>and</strong> then discuss it withthem, <strong>to</strong> the extent that this is feasible, given their age,sophistication, <strong>and</strong> so forth.9. If feasible, conduct a statistical item analysis with your tryoutdata (at least 20 respondents are necessary). Suchanalyses are not difficult <strong>to</strong> carry out, especially if you havea computer. The <strong>in</strong>formation provided on each item <strong>in</strong>dicateshow effective it is <strong>and</strong> sometimes even suggests how<strong>to</strong> improve it. See, for example, K. R. Murphy <strong>and</strong> C. O.Davidshofer (1991). Psychological test<strong>in</strong>g: Pr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>and</strong>applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.10. Select <strong>and</strong> revise items as necessary until you have thenumber you want.been developed takes far less time than develop<strong>in</strong>g anew <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> measure the same th<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>g one’s own <strong>in</strong>strument is time-consum<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> we do not recommend it for those without a considerableamount of time, energy, <strong>and</strong> money <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vest <strong>in</strong>the endeavor. Fortunately, a number of already developed,useful <strong>in</strong>struments exist, <strong>and</strong> they can be locatedquite easily by means of a computer. The most comprehensivelist<strong>in</strong>g of test<strong>in</strong>g resources currently availablecan be found by access<strong>in</strong>g the ERIC database at thefollow<strong>in</strong>g Web site: http://eric.ed.gov (Figure 7.1).We typed the words social studies <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> thebox labeled “Search Terms.” This produced a list of1080 documents. As this was far <strong>to</strong>o many <strong>to</strong> peruse, wechanged the search terms <strong>to</strong> social studies competencybased <strong>in</strong>struments. This produced a much more manageablelist of just five references as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.2.We clicked on #1 “Social Studies. Competency-Based<strong>Education</strong> Assessment Series” <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> the descriptionof this <strong>in</strong>strument (Figure 7.3).Almost any <strong>to</strong>pic can be searched <strong>in</strong> this way <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>a list of <strong>in</strong>struments that measure some aspect of the <strong>to</strong>pic.Notice that a pr<strong>in</strong>ted copy of the <strong>in</strong>strument can be obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom the ERIC Document Reproduction Service.A few years ago ERIC underwent considerablechanges. The clear<strong>in</strong>ghouses were closed <strong>in</strong> early 2004,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> September 2004 a new Web site was <strong>in</strong>troducedthat provides users with markedly improved searchcapabilities that utilize more efficient retrieval methods<strong>to</strong> access the ERIC database (1966–present). Users cannow ref<strong>in</strong>e their search results through the use of theERIC thesaurus <strong>and</strong> various ERIC identifiers (check i<strong>to</strong>ut!). In Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 2004 free full-text nonjournal ERICresources were <strong>in</strong>troduced, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g over 100,000full-text documents authorized for electronic ERICdistribution from January 1993 through July 2004.Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> December, new bibliographic records <strong>and</strong>full-text journal <strong>and</strong> nonjournal resources after July2004 were added. The basic ERIC Web site rema<strong>in</strong>shttp://www.eric.ed.gov.113


Figure 7.1 The ERIC Search Eng<strong>in</strong>eSource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by ComputerSciences Corporation. www.eric.ed.govFigure 7.2 Match<strong>in</strong>g Documents from ERIC Search for “Social Studies Instruments”Source: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by ComputerSciences Corporation. www.eric.ed.gov


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 115Figure 7.3 Abstract from the ERIC DatabaseSource: From ERIC (Educa<strong>to</strong>r Resources Information Center). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, operated by Computer SciencesCorporation. www.eric.ed.govNote that the search eng<strong>in</strong>es that we described <strong>in</strong> Chapter5 can be used <strong>to</strong> locate ERIC. What you want <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>dis ERIC’s test collection of more than 9,000 <strong>in</strong>strumentsof various types, as well as The Mental MeasurementsYearbooks. Now produced by the Buros Institute at theUniversity of Nebraska,* the yearbooks are publishedabout every other year, with supplements produced betweenissues. Each yearbook provides reviews of thest<strong>and</strong>ardized tests that have been published s<strong>in</strong>ce thelast issue. The Institute’s Tests <strong>in</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t is a comprehensivebibliography of commercial tests. Unfortunately,only the references <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>and</strong> reviews ofthem are available onl<strong>in</strong>e; the actual <strong>in</strong>struments themselvesare available only <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t form.Here are some other references you can consult thatlist various types of <strong>in</strong>struments:T. E. Backer (1977). A direc<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>in</strong>formation ontests. ERIC TM Report 62-1977. Pr<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>to</strong>n, NJ:*So named for Oscar Buros, who started the yearbooks back <strong>in</strong> 1938.ERIC Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse on Assessment <strong>and</strong> Evaluation,<strong>Education</strong>al Test<strong>in</strong>g Service.K. Corcoran <strong>and</strong> J. Fischer (Eds.) (1994). Measuresfor cl<strong>in</strong>ical practice (2 volumes). New York: FreePress.ETS test collection catalog: Volume 1, Achievementtests (1992); Volume 2, Vocational tests (1988); Volume3, Tests for special populations (1989); Volume4, Cognitive, aptitude, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence tests (1990);Volume 5, Attitude measures (1991); Volume 6, Affectivemeasures <strong>and</strong> personality tests (1992). Phoenix,AZ: Oryx Press.E. Fabiano <strong>and</strong> N. O’Brien (1987). Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formationsources for educa<strong>to</strong>rs. TME Report 94.Pr<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>to</strong>n, NJ: ERIC Clear<strong>in</strong>ghouse on Assessment<strong>and</strong> Evaluation, <strong>Education</strong>al Test<strong>in</strong>g Service. Thissource updates Backer <strong>to</strong> 1987, but it is not ascomprehensive.B. A. Goldman <strong>and</strong> D. F. Mitchell (1974–1995).Direc<strong>to</strong>ry of unpublished experimental mental measures(6 volumes). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.


116 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eM. Hersen <strong>and</strong> A. S. Bellack (1988). Dictionaryof behavioral assessment techniques. New York:Pergamon.J. C. Impara <strong>and</strong> B. S. Plake (1999). Mental measurementsyearbook. L<strong>in</strong>coln, NE: Buros Institute,University of Nebraska.S. E. Krug (published biannually). Psychwaresourcebook. Aust<strong>in</strong>, TX: Pro-Ed, Inc. A direc<strong>to</strong>ry ofcomputer-based assessment <strong>to</strong>ols, such as tests, scor<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation systems.H. I. McCubb<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> A. I. Thompson (Eds.) (1987).Family assessment <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries for research <strong>and</strong>practice. Madison, WI: University of Wiscons<strong>in</strong>–Madison.L. L. Murphy et al. (1999). Tests <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t. L<strong>in</strong>coln,NE: Buros Institute, University of Nebraska.R. C. Sweetl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> D. J. Keyser (Eds.) (1991).Tests: A comprehensive reference for assessments <strong>in</strong>psychology, education, <strong>and</strong> bus<strong>in</strong>ess, 3rd ed. KansasCity, MO: Test Corporation of America.With so many <strong>in</strong>struments now available <strong>to</strong> theresearch community, we recommend that, except <strong>in</strong>unusual cases, researchers devote their energies <strong>to</strong> adapt<strong>in</strong>g(<strong>and</strong>/or improv<strong>in</strong>g) those that now exist rather thantry<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> start from scratch <strong>to</strong> develop entirely newmeasures.WRITTEN RESPONSE VERSUS PERFORMANCEAnother way <strong>to</strong> classify <strong>in</strong>struments is <strong>in</strong> terms ofwhether they require a written or marked responsefrom subjects or a more general evaluation of subjects’performance. Written-response <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong>cludeobjective (e.g., multiple-choice, true-false, match<strong>in</strong>g, orshort-answer) tests, short-essay exam<strong>in</strong>ations, questionnaires,<strong>in</strong>terview schedules, rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, <strong>and</strong> checklists.Performance <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong>clude any devicedesigned <strong>to</strong> measure either a procedure or a product.Procedures are ways of do<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs, such as mix<strong>in</strong>g achemical solution, diagnos<strong>in</strong>g a problem <strong>in</strong> an au<strong>to</strong>mobile,writ<strong>in</strong>g a letter, solv<strong>in</strong>g a puzzle, or sett<strong>in</strong>g themarg<strong>in</strong>s on a typewriter. Products are the end results ofprocedures, such as the correct chemical solution, thecorrect diagnosis of au<strong>to</strong> malfunction, or a properlytyped letter. Performance <strong>in</strong>struments are designed <strong>to</strong>see whether <strong>and</strong> how well procedures can be followed<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> assess the quality of products.Written-response <strong>in</strong>struments are generally preferredover performance <strong>in</strong>struments, s<strong>in</strong>ce the use of the latteris frequently quite time-consum<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> often requiresequipment or other resources that are not readily available.A large amount of time would be required <strong>to</strong> haveeven a fairly small sample of students (imag<strong>in</strong>e 35!)complete the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a high school scienceexperiment.Examples of Data-CollectionInstrumentsWhen it comes <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>to</strong> beused <strong>in</strong> a study, either the researchers (or their assistantsor other <strong>in</strong>formants) must do it themselves, or they mustask the subjects of the study <strong>to</strong> provide the <strong>in</strong>formationdesired. Therefore, we group the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gdiscussion accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> whether they are completedby researchers or by subjects. Examples of these<strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g:<strong>Research</strong>er completes:Rat<strong>in</strong>g scalesInterview schedulesObservation formsTally sheetsFlowchartsPerformancechecklistsAnecdotal recordsTime-<strong>and</strong>-motionlogsSubject completes:QuestionnairesSelf-checklistsAttitude ScalesPersonality (or character)<strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>riesAchievement/aptitude testsPerformance testsProjective devicesSociometric devicesThis dist<strong>in</strong>ction, of course, is by no means absolute.Many of the <strong>in</strong>struments we list might, on a givenoccasion, be completed by either the researcher(s) orsubjects <strong>in</strong> a particular study.RESEARCHER-COMPLETED INSTRUMENTSRat<strong>in</strong>g Scales. A rat<strong>in</strong>g is a measured judgment ofsome sort. When we rate people, we make a judgmentabout their behavior or someth<strong>in</strong>g they have produced.Thus, both behaviors (such as how well a person givesan oral report) <strong>and</strong> products (such as a written copy of areport) of <strong>in</strong>dividuals can be rated.Notice that the terms observations <strong>and</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>gs arenot synonymous. A rat<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> convey therater’s judgment about an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavior orproduct. An observation is <strong>in</strong>tended merely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 117whether a particular behavior is present or absent (seethe time-<strong>and</strong>-motion log <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.12 on page 124).Sometimes, of course, researchers may do both. The activitiesof a small group engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a discussion, forexample, can be both observed <strong>and</strong> rated.Behavior Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scales. Behavior rat<strong>in</strong>g scales appear<strong>in</strong> several forms, but those most commonly used ask theobserver <strong>to</strong> circle or mark a po<strong>in</strong>t on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatethe rat<strong>in</strong>g. The simplest of these <strong>to</strong> construct is anumerical rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, which provides a series of numbers,each represent<strong>in</strong>g a particular rat<strong>in</strong>g.Figure 7.4 shows such a scale designed <strong>to</strong> rate teachers.The problem with this rat<strong>in</strong>g scale is that differen<strong>to</strong>bservers are quite likely <strong>to</strong> have different ideas aboutthe mean<strong>in</strong>g of the terms that the numbers represent(excellent, average, etc.). In other words, the differentrat<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>ts on the scale are not described fullyenough. The same <strong>in</strong>dividual, therefore, might be ratedquite differently by two different observers. One way <strong>to</strong>address this problem is <strong>to</strong> give additional mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>each number by describ<strong>in</strong>g it more fully. For example,<strong>in</strong> Figure 7.4, the rat<strong>in</strong>g 5 could be def<strong>in</strong>ed as “amongthe <strong>to</strong>p 5 percent of all teachers you have had.” In theabsence of such def<strong>in</strong>itions, the researcher must eitherrely on the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the respondents or treat the rat<strong>in</strong>gsas subjective op<strong>in</strong>ions.The graphic rat<strong>in</strong>g scale is an attempt <strong>to</strong> improve onthe vagueness of numerical rat<strong>in</strong>g scales. It describeseach of the characteristics <strong>to</strong> be rated <strong>and</strong> places themon a horizontal l<strong>in</strong>e on which the observer is <strong>to</strong> place acheck mark. Figure 7.5 presents an example of a graphicrat<strong>in</strong>g scale. Here aga<strong>in</strong>, this scale would be improvedInstructions: For each of the behaviors listedbelow, circle the appropriate number, us<strong>in</strong>gthe follow<strong>in</strong>g key: 5 = Excellent, 4 = AboveAverage, 3 = Average, 2 = Below Average,1 = Poor.A. Expla<strong>in</strong>s course material clearly1 2 3 4 5B. Establishes rapport with students1 2 3 4 5C. Asks high-level questions1 2 3 4 5D. Varies class activities1 2 3 4 5Figure 7.4 Excerpt from a Behavior Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scale forTeachersby add<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>itions, such as def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g always as “95 <strong>to</strong>100 percent of the time,” <strong>and</strong> frequently as “70 <strong>to</strong>94 percent of the time.”Product Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scales. As we mentioned earlier,researchers may wish <strong>to</strong> rate products. Examples ofproducts that are frequently rated <strong>in</strong> education are bookreports, maps <strong>and</strong> charts, diagrams, draw<strong>in</strong>gs, notebooks,essays, <strong>and</strong> creative endeavors of all sorts.Whereas behavior rat<strong>in</strong>gs must be done at a particulartime (when the researcher can observe the behavior), abig advantage of product rat<strong>in</strong>gs is that they can beInstructions: Indicate the quality of the student’s participation<strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g class activities by plac<strong>in</strong>g an X anywhere alongeach l<strong>in</strong>e.Figure 7.5 Excerpt froma Graphic Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scale1. Listens <strong>to</strong> teacher’s <strong>in</strong>structions| | | | |Always Frequently Occasionally Seldom Never2. Listens <strong>to</strong> the op<strong>in</strong>ions of other students| | | | |Always Frequently Occasionally Seldom Never3. Offers own op<strong>in</strong>ions <strong>in</strong> class discussions| | | | |Always Frequently Occasionally Seldom Never


118 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 7.6 Exampleof a Product Rat<strong>in</strong>gScaleSource: H<strong>and</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g scale used<strong>in</strong> the California AchievementTests, Form W (1957).CTB/McGraw-Hill, Monterey,CA. Copyright © 1957 byMcGraw-Hill.done at any time.* Figure 7.6 presents an example of ascale rat<strong>in</strong>g the product “h<strong>and</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g.” To use this*Some behavior rat<strong>in</strong>g scales are designed <strong>to</strong> assess behavior over aperiod of time—for example, how frequently a teacher asks highlevelthought questions.scale, an actual sample of the student’s h<strong>and</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g isobta<strong>in</strong>ed. It is then moved along the scale until thequality of the h<strong>and</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the sample is most similar<strong>to</strong> the example shown on the scale. Although more than50 years old, it rema<strong>in</strong>s a classic example of this type of<strong>in</strong>strument.


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 119Interview Schedules. Interview schedules <strong>and</strong>questionnaires are basically the same k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>strument—aset of questions <strong>to</strong> be answered by the subjectsof the study. There are some important differences <strong>in</strong>how they are adm<strong>in</strong>istered, however. Interviews areconducted orally, <strong>and</strong> the answers <strong>to</strong> the questions arerecorded by the researcher (or someone he or she hastra<strong>in</strong>ed). The advantages of this <strong>in</strong>strument are that the<strong>in</strong>terviewer can clarify any questions that are obscure<strong>and</strong> also can ask the respondent <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> on answersthat are particularly important or reveal<strong>in</strong>g. A big disadvantage,on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is that it takes much longerthan the questionnaire <strong>to</strong> complete. Furthermore, thepresence of the researcher may <strong>in</strong>hibit respondents fromsay<strong>in</strong>g what they really th<strong>in</strong>k.Figure 7.7 illustrates a structured <strong>in</strong>terview schedule.Notice that this <strong>in</strong>terview schedule requires the <strong>in</strong>terviewers<strong>to</strong> do considerable writ<strong>in</strong>g, unless the <strong>in</strong>terviewis taped. Some <strong>in</strong>terview schedules phrasequestions so that the responses are likely <strong>to</strong> fall <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong>categories. This is sometimes call precod<strong>in</strong>g. Precod<strong>in</strong>genables the <strong>in</strong>terviewer <strong>to</strong> check appropriateitems rather than transcribe responses, thus prevent<strong>in</strong>gthe respondent from hav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> wait while the <strong>in</strong>terviewerrecords a response.Observation Forms. Paper-<strong>and</strong>-pencil observationforms (sometimes called observation schedules) arefairly easy <strong>to</strong> construct. A sample of such a form is1. Would you rate pupil academic learn<strong>in</strong>g as excellent, good,fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compare pupilacademic learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.2. Would you rate pupil attitude <strong>to</strong>ward school generally asexcellent, good, fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compare pupilattitude <strong>to</strong>ward school generally <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.3. Would you rate pupil attitude <strong>to</strong>ward learn<strong>in</strong>g as excellent,good, fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compareattitude <strong>to</strong>ward learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.4. Would you rate pupil attitude <strong>to</strong>ward self as excellent, good,fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compare pupilattitude <strong>to</strong>ward self <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.5. Would you rate pupil attitude <strong>to</strong>ward other students asexcellent, good, fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compareattitude <strong>to</strong>ward other students <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.6. Would you rate pupil attitude <strong>to</strong>ward you as excellent, good,fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compare pupilattitude <strong>to</strong>ward you <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.7. Would you rate pupil creativity–self-expression as excellent,good, fair, or poor?a. If you were here last year, how would you compare pupilcreativity–self-expression <strong>to</strong> previous years?b. Please give specific examples.Figure 7.7 InterviewSchedule (for Teachers)<strong>Design</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> Assess theEffects of a Competency-Based Curriculum <strong>in</strong>Inner-City Schools


120 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 7.8 SampleObservation FormDirections:1. Place a check mark each time the teacher:a. asks <strong>in</strong>dividual students a questionb. asks questions <strong>to</strong> the class as a wholec. discipl<strong>in</strong>es studentsd. asks for quiete. asks students if they have any questionsf. sends students <strong>to</strong> the chalkboard✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓✓Frequency6213122. Place a check mark each time the teacher asksa question that requires:a. memory or recall of <strong>in</strong>formationb. comparisonc. an <strong>in</strong>ferenced. a generalizatione. specific application✓ ✓ ✓✓✓✓✓ ✓✓✓✓Frequency52301shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.8. As you can see, the form requiresthe observer not only <strong>to</strong> record certa<strong>in</strong> behaviors, butalso <strong>to</strong> evaluate some as they occur.Initially, observation forms should always be used ona trial basis <strong>in</strong> situations similar <strong>to</strong> those <strong>to</strong> be observed<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> work out any bugs or ambiguities. A frequentweakness <strong>in</strong> many observation forms is that they ask theobserver <strong>to</strong> record more behaviors than can be doneaccurately (or <strong>to</strong> watch <strong>to</strong>o many <strong>in</strong>dividuals at the sametime). As is frequently the case, the simpler the <strong>in</strong>strument,the better.Tally Sheets. A tally sheet is a device often used byresearchers <strong>to</strong> record the frequency of student behaviors,activities, or remarks. <strong>How</strong> many high school studentsfollow <strong>in</strong>structions dur<strong>in</strong>g fire drills, for example?<strong>How</strong> many <strong>in</strong>stances of aggression or helpfulnessdo elementary students exhibit on the playground?<strong>How</strong> often do students <strong>in</strong> Mr. Jordan’s fifth-period U.S.his<strong>to</strong>ry class ask questions? <strong>How</strong> often do they ask <strong>in</strong>ferentialquestions? Tally sheets can help researchersrecord answers <strong>to</strong> these k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions efficiently.A tally sheet is simply a list<strong>in</strong>g of various categoriesof activities or behaviors on a piece of paper. Everytime a subject is observed engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> one of these activitiesor behaviors, the researcher places a tally <strong>in</strong> theappropriate category. The k<strong>in</strong>ds of statements that studentsmake <strong>in</strong> class, for example, often <strong>in</strong>dicate the degree<strong>to</strong> which they underst<strong>and</strong> various concepts <strong>and</strong>ideas. The possible category systems that might be devisedare probably endless, but Figure 7.9 presents oneexample.Flowcharts. A particular type of tally sheet is theparticipation flowchart. Flowcharts are particularlyhelpful <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g class discussions. Both the number<strong>and</strong> direction of student remarks can be charted <strong>to</strong> ga<strong>in</strong>some idea of the quantity <strong>and</strong> focus of students’ verbalparticipation <strong>in</strong> class.One of the easiest ways <strong>to</strong> do this is <strong>to</strong> prepare a seat<strong>in</strong>gchart on which a box is drawn for each student <strong>in</strong> theclass be<strong>in</strong>g observed.Atally is then placed <strong>in</strong> the box of aparticular student each time he or she makes a verbalcomment. To <strong>in</strong>dicate the direction of <strong>in</strong>dividual studentcomments, arrows can be drawn from the box of a studentmak<strong>in</strong>g a comment <strong>to</strong> the box of the student <strong>to</strong> whom thecomment is directed. Figure 7.10 illustrates what such aflowchart might look like. This chart suggests thatRobert, Felix, <strong>and</strong> Mercedes dom<strong>in</strong>ated the discussion,with contributions from Al, Gail, Jack, <strong>and</strong> Sam. Joe <strong>and</strong>Nancy said noth<strong>in</strong>g. Note that a subsequent discussion, ora different <strong>to</strong>pic, however, might reveal a quite differentpattern.Performance Checklists. One of the mostfrequently used of all measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments is the


Type of Remark1. Asks question call<strong>in</strong>g for factual <strong>in</strong>formation Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson2. Asks question call<strong>in</strong>g for clarification Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson3. Asks question call<strong>in</strong>g for explanation Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson4. Asks question call<strong>in</strong>g for speculation Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson5. Asks question of another student Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson6. Gives own op<strong>in</strong>ion on issue Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson7. Responds <strong>to</strong> another student Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson8. Summarizes remarks of another student Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson9. Does not respond when addressed by teacher Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lesson10. Does not respond when addressed by another student Related <strong>to</strong> lessonNot related <strong>to</strong> lessonFigure 7.9 Discussion-Analysis Tally SheetFigure 7.10ParticipationFlowchartSource: Adapted from Enoch I.Saw<strong>in</strong> (1969). Evaluation<strong>and</strong> the work of the teacher.Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, p. 179.Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of SagePublishers, Inc.Jack Al MercedesGail Joe NancyRobert Felix Sam


122 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e1. Takes slide ______2. Wipes slide with lens paper ______3. Wipes slide with cloth ______4. Wipes slide with f<strong>in</strong>ger ______5. Moves bottle of culture along thetable______6. Places drop or two of culture on slide ______7. Adds more culture ______8. Adds few drops of water ______9. Hunts for cover glasses ______10. Wipes cover glass with lens paper ______11. Wipes cover glass with cloth ______12. Wipes cover with f<strong>in</strong>ger ______13. Adjusts cover with f<strong>in</strong>ger ______14. Wipes off surplus fluid ______15. Places slide on stage ______16. Looks through eyepiece withright eye______17. Looks through eyepiece with left eye ______18. Turns <strong>to</strong> objective of lowest power ______19. Turns <strong>to</strong> low-power objective ______20. Turns <strong>to</strong> high-power objective ______21. Holds one eye closed ______22. Looks for light ______23. Adjusts concave mirror ______24. Adjusts plane mirror ______25. Adjusts diaphragm ______26. Does not <strong>to</strong>uch diaphragm ______27. With eye at eyepiece, turns downcoarse adjustment screw______28. Breaks cover glass ______29. Breaks slide ______30. With eye away from eyepiece, turnsdown coarse adjustment screw______31. Turns up coarse adjustment screwa great distance______32. With eye at eyepiece, turns downf<strong>in</strong>e adjustment screw a greatdistance______33. With eye away from eyepiece, turnsdown f<strong>in</strong>e adjustment screw a greatdistance______34. Turns up f<strong>in</strong>e adjustment screw agreat distance______35. Turns f<strong>in</strong>e adjustment screw a fewturns______36. Removes slide from stage ______37. Wipes objective with lens paper ______38. Wipes objective with cloth ______39. Wipes objective with f<strong>in</strong>ger ______40. Wipes eyepiece with lens paper ______41. Wipes eyepiece with cloth ______42. Wipes eyepiece with f<strong>in</strong>ger ______43. Makes another mount ______44. Takes another microscope ______45. F<strong>in</strong>ds object ______46. Pauses for an <strong>in</strong>terval ______47. Asks, “What do you want me <strong>to</strong> do?” ______48. Asks whether <strong>to</strong> use high power ______49. Says, “I’m satisfied.” ______50. Says that the mount is all right forhis or her eye______51. Says, “I cannot do it.” ______52. Told <strong>to</strong> start a new mount ______53. Directed <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d object under lowpower______54. Directed <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d object under highpower______Figure 7.11 Performance Checklist Not<strong>in</strong>g Student ActionsSource: <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Bullet<strong>in</strong> (1922–61) by R. W. Tyler. Copyright 1930 by Ohio State University, College of <strong>Education</strong>. Reproduced withpermission of Ohio State University, College of <strong>Education</strong> <strong>in</strong> the format Textbook via Copyright Clearance Center.checklist. A performance checklist consists of a list ofbehaviors that make up a certa<strong>in</strong> type of performance(us<strong>in</strong>g a microscope, typ<strong>in</strong>g a letter, solv<strong>in</strong>g a mathematicsproblem, <strong>and</strong> so on). It is used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether an <strong>in</strong>dividual behaves <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> (usually desired)way when asked <strong>to</strong> complete a particular task. Ifa particular behavior is present when an <strong>in</strong>dividual isobserved, the researcher places a check mark opposite i<strong>to</strong>n the list.Figure 7.11 presents part of a performance checklistdeveloped more than 70 years ago <strong>to</strong> assess a student’sskill <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g a microscope. Note that the items on thischecklist (as any well-constructed checklist should) askthe observer <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate only if the desired behaviorstake place. No subjective judgments are called for onthe part of the observer as <strong>to</strong> how well the <strong>in</strong>dividualperforms. Items that call for such judgments are best left<strong>to</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>g scales.Anecdotal Records. Another way of record<strong>in</strong>gthe behavior of <strong>in</strong>dividuals is the anecdotal record. It isjust what its name implies—a record of observedbehaviors written down <strong>in</strong> the form of anecdotes. Thereis no set format; rather, observers are free <strong>to</strong> recordany behavior they th<strong>in</strong>k is important <strong>and</strong> need not focuson the same behavior for all subjects. To produce themost useful records, however, observers should try <strong>to</strong>be as specific <strong>and</strong> as factual as possible <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> avoid


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 123evaluative, <strong>in</strong>terpretive, or overly generalized remarks.The American Council on <strong>Education</strong> describes fourtypes of anecdotes, stat<strong>in</strong>g that the first three are <strong>to</strong> beavoided. Only the fourth type is desired.1. Anecdotes that evaluate or judge the behavior of thechild as good or bad, desirable or undesirable, acceptableor unacceptable . . . evaluative statements(<strong>to</strong> be avoided).2. Anecdotes that account for or expla<strong>in</strong> the child’sbehavior, usually on the basis of a s<strong>in</strong>gle fact orthesis . . . <strong>in</strong>terpretive statements (<strong>to</strong> be avoided).3. Anecdotes that describe certa<strong>in</strong> behavior <strong>in</strong> generalterms, as happen<strong>in</strong>g frequently, or as characteriz<strong>in</strong>gthe child ...generalized statements (<strong>to</strong> be avoided).4. Anecdotes that tell exactly what the child did orsaid, that describe concretely the situation <strong>in</strong> whichthe action or comment occurred, <strong>and</strong> that tell clearlywhat other persons also did or said . . . specific orconcrete descriptive statements (the type desired). 1Here are examples of each of the four types.Evaluative: Julius talked loud <strong>and</strong> much dur<strong>in</strong>g poetry;wanted <strong>to</strong> do <strong>and</strong> say just what he wanted<strong>and</strong> didn’t consider the right work<strong>in</strong>g out ofth<strong>in</strong>gs. Had <strong>to</strong> ask him <strong>to</strong> sit by me. Showed a badattitude about it.Interpretive: For the last week Sammy has been aperfect wiggle-tail. He is grow<strong>in</strong>g so fast he cannotbe settled. . . . Of course the <strong>in</strong>ward changethat is tak<strong>in</strong>g place causes the restlessness.Generalized: Sammy is awfully restless these days.He is whisper<strong>in</strong>g most of the time he is not keptbusy. In the circle, dur<strong>in</strong>g various discussions,even though he is <strong>in</strong>terested, his arms are mov<strong>in</strong>gor he is punch<strong>in</strong>g the one sitt<strong>in</strong>g next <strong>to</strong> him. Hesmiles when I speak <strong>to</strong> him.Specific (the type desired): The weather was so bitterlycold that we did not go on the playground<strong>to</strong>day. The children played games <strong>in</strong> the roomdur<strong>in</strong>g the regular recess period. Andrew <strong>and</strong>Larry chose sides for a game which is known assteal<strong>in</strong>g the bacon. I was talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a group ofchildren <strong>in</strong> the front of the room while the choos<strong>in</strong>gwas <strong>in</strong> process <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> a moment I heard a loudaltercation. Larry said that all the children wanted<strong>to</strong> be on Andrew’s side rather than on his. Andrewremarked, “I can’t help it if they all want <strong>to</strong> be onmy side.” 2Time-<strong>and</strong>-Motion Logs. There are occasionswhen researchers want <strong>to</strong> make a very detailed observationof an <strong>in</strong>dividual or a group. This is often the case, forexample, when try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> identify the reasons underly<strong>in</strong>ga particular problem or difficulty that an <strong>in</strong>dividual orclass is hav<strong>in</strong>g (work<strong>in</strong>g very slowly, fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> completeassigned tasks, <strong>in</strong>attentiveness, <strong>and</strong> so on).A time-<strong>and</strong>-motion study is the observation <strong>and</strong>detailed record<strong>in</strong>g over a given period of time of the activitiesof one or more <strong>in</strong>dividuals (for example, dur<strong>in</strong>g a 15-m<strong>in</strong>ute labora<strong>to</strong>ry demonstration). Observers try <strong>to</strong> recordeveryth<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual does as objectively as possible<strong>and</strong> at brief, regular <strong>in</strong>tervals (such as every 3 m<strong>in</strong>utes,with a 1-m<strong>in</strong>ute break <strong>in</strong>terspersed between <strong>in</strong>tervals).The late Hilda Taba, a pioneer <strong>in</strong> educational evaluation,once cited an example of a fourth-grade teacherwho believed that her class’s considerable slowness wasdue <strong>to</strong> the fact that they were extremely meticulous <strong>in</strong>their work. To check this out, she decided <strong>to</strong> conduct adetailed time-<strong>and</strong>-motion study of one typical student.The results of her study <strong>in</strong>dicated that this student,rather than be<strong>in</strong>g overly meticulous, was actuallyunable <strong>to</strong> focus his attention on a particular task for anyconcerted period of time. Figure 7.12 illustrates whatshe observed.SUBJECT-COMPLETED INSTRUMENTSQuestionnaires. The <strong>in</strong>terview schedule shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 7.7 on page 119 could be used as a questionnaire.In a questionnaire, the subjects respond <strong>to</strong> the questionsby writ<strong>in</strong>g or, more commonly, by mark<strong>in</strong>g an answersheet. Advantages of questionnaires are that they can bemailed or given <strong>to</strong> large numbers of people at the sametime. The disadvantages are that unclear or seem<strong>in</strong>glyambiguous questions cannot be clarified, <strong>and</strong> therespondent has no chance <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> on or react verbally<strong>to</strong> a question of particular <strong>in</strong>terest or importance.Selection items on questionnaires <strong>in</strong>clude multiplechoice,true-false, match<strong>in</strong>g, or <strong>in</strong>terpretive-exercisequestions. Supply items <strong>in</strong>clude short-answer or essayquestions. We’ll give some examples of each of thesetypes of items when we discuss achievement tests later<strong>in</strong> the chapter.Self-Checklists. A self-checklist is a list of severalcharacteristics or activities presented <strong>to</strong> the subjects of astudy. The <strong>in</strong>dividuals are asked <strong>to</strong> study the list <strong>and</strong>then <strong>to</strong> place a mark opposite the characteristics they


124 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTime Activity11:32 Stacked paperPicked up pencilWrote nameMoved paper closerCont<strong>in</strong>ued with read<strong>in</strong>gRubbed noseLooked at Art’s paperStarted <strong>to</strong> work . . .11:45 Worked <strong>and</strong> watchedMade funny facesGiggled. Looked at Lorrie <strong>and</strong> smiledBorrowed Art’s paperErasedStacked paperReadSlid paper aroundWorked brieflyPicked up paper <strong>and</strong> readThumb <strong>in</strong> mouth, watched Miss D11:47 Worked <strong>and</strong> watchedMade funny faceGiggled. Looked <strong>and</strong> smiled at LorriePaper up—readPicked eyeStudied bullet<strong>in</strong> boardPaper down—read aga<strong>in</strong>Fidgeted with paperPlayed with pencil <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>gersWatched meTimeActivityWatched L.Laughed at herErasedH<strong>and</strong> upLaughed. Watched D.Got help11:50 Looked at LorrieTapped f<strong>in</strong>gers on deskWroteSlid down <strong>in</strong> deskH<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> head, listened <strong>to</strong> D. help<strong>in</strong>gLorrieBlew breath out hardFidgeted with paperLooked at other groupHeld ch<strong>in</strong>Watched CharlesRead, h<strong>and</strong>s hold<strong>in</strong>g headErasedWatched other group, ch<strong>in</strong> on h<strong>and</strong>Made faces—yawned—fidgetedHeld headRead, po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> wordsWrotePut head on arm on deskHeld ch<strong>in</strong>ReadRubbed eye11:55 WroteFigure 7.12 Time-<strong>and</strong>-Motion LogSource: Hilda Taba (1957). Problem identification. In ASCD 1957 Yearbook: <strong>Research</strong> for Curriculum Improvement, pp. 60–61. © 1957 ASCD.Used with permission. The Association for Supervision <strong>and</strong> Curriculum Development is a worldwide community of educa<strong>to</strong>rs advocat<strong>in</strong>g soundpolicies <strong>and</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g best practices <strong>to</strong> achieve the success of each learner. To learn more, visit ASCD at www.ascd.org.possess or the activities <strong>in</strong> which they have engaged fora particular length of time. Self-checklists are oftenused when researchers want students <strong>to</strong> diagnose or <strong>to</strong>appraise their own performance. One example of a selfchecklistfor use with elementary school students isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.13.Attitude Scales. The basic assumption that underliesall attitude scales is that it is possible <strong>to</strong> discover attitudesby ask<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> a series ofstatements of preference. Thus, if <strong>in</strong>dividuals agree withthe statement, “A course <strong>in</strong> philosophy should berequired of all c<strong>and</strong>idates for a teach<strong>in</strong>g credential,”researchers <strong>in</strong>fer that these students have a positive attitude<strong>to</strong>ward such a course (assum<strong>in</strong>g students underst<strong>and</strong>the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the statement <strong>and</strong> are s<strong>in</strong>cere <strong>in</strong>their responses). An attitude scale, therefore, consists ofa set of statements <strong>to</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>dividual responds. Thepattern of responses is then viewed as evidence of oneor more underly<strong>in</strong>g attitudes.Attitude scales are often similar <strong>to</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>g scales <strong>in</strong>form, with words <strong>and</strong> numbers placed on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum.Subjects circle the word or number that best representshow they feel about the <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the questionsor statements <strong>in</strong> the scale. A commonly used attitudescale <strong>in</strong> educational research is the Likert scale, namedafter the man who designed it. 3 Figure 7.14 presents afew examples from a Likert scale. On some items, a 5(strongly agree) will <strong>in</strong>dicate a positive attitude, <strong>and</strong> bescored 5. On other items, a 1 (strongly disagree) will


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 125Date _______________________Name _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________Instructions: Place a check (✓) <strong>in</strong> the space provided for those days, dur<strong>in</strong>g the past week, when youhave participated <strong>in</strong> the activity listed. Circle the activity if you feel you need <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> it morefrequently <strong>in</strong> the weeks <strong>to</strong> come.Mon Tues Wed Thurs Fri1. I participated <strong>in</strong> class discussions. ✓ ✓ ✓2. I did not <strong>in</strong>terrupt others while they werespeak<strong>in</strong>g. ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓3. I encouraged others <strong>to</strong> offer their op<strong>in</strong>ions. ✓ ✓4. I listened <strong>to</strong> what others had <strong>to</strong> say. ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓5. I helped others when asked. ✓6. I asked questions when I was unclear about whathad been said. ✓ ✓7. I looked up words <strong>in</strong> the dictionary that I didnot know how <strong>to</strong> spell.✓8. I considered the suggestions of others. ✓ ✓ ✓9. I tried <strong>to</strong> be helpful <strong>in</strong> my remarks. ✓ ✓ ✓10. I praised others when I thought they did a good job. ✓Figure 7.13 Example of a Self-Checklist<strong>in</strong>dicate a positive attitude <strong>and</strong> be scored 5 (thus, theends of the scale are reversed when scor<strong>in</strong>g), as shown<strong>in</strong> item 2 <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.14.A unique sort of attitude scale that is especially usefulfor classroom research is the semantic differential. 4It allows a researcher <strong>to</strong> measure a subject’s attitude <strong>to</strong>warda particular concept. Subjects are presented with acont<strong>in</strong>uum of several pairs of adjectives (good-bad,cold-hot, priceless-worthless, <strong>and</strong> so on) <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong>place a check mark between each pair <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate theirattitudes. Figure 7.15 presents an example.A scale that has particular value for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g theattitudes of young children uses simply drawn faces.When the subjects of an attitude study are primaryschool children or younger, they can be asked <strong>to</strong> place anX under a face, such as the ones shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.16, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dicate how they feel about a <strong>to</strong>pic.The subject of attitude scales is discussed rather extensively<strong>in</strong> the literature on evaluation <strong>and</strong> test development,<strong>and</strong> students <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> a more extendedtreatment should consult a st<strong>and</strong>ard textbook on thesesubjects. 5Personality (or Character) Inven<strong>to</strong>ries.Personality <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries are designed <strong>to</strong> measure certa<strong>in</strong>traits of <strong>in</strong>dividuals or <strong>to</strong> assess their feel<strong>in</strong>gsabout themselves. Examples of such <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries<strong>in</strong>clude the M<strong>in</strong>nesota Multiphasic Personality Inven<strong>to</strong>ry,the IPAT Anxiety Scale, the Piers-HarrisChildren’s Self-Concept Scale (<strong>How</strong> I Feel AboutMyself), <strong>and</strong> the Kuder Preference Record. Figure7.17 lists some typical items from this type of test.The specific items, of course, reflect the variable(s)the <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry addresses.Achievement Tests. Achievement, or ability,tests measure an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s knowledge or skill <strong>in</strong> agiven area or subject. They are mostly used <strong>in</strong> schools <strong>to</strong>


126 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 7.14 Examplesof Items from a LikertScale Measur<strong>in</strong>gAttitude Toward TeacherEmpowermentInstructions: Circle the choice after each statement that <strong>in</strong>dicatesyour op<strong>in</strong>ion.1. All professors of education should be required <strong>to</strong> spend atleast six months teach<strong>in</strong>g at the elementary or secondarylevel every five years.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(5) (4) (3) (2) (1)2. Teachers’ unions should be abolished.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)3. All school adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs should be required by law <strong>to</strong> teachat least one class <strong>in</strong> a public school classroom every year.StronglyStronglyagree Agree Undecided Disagree disagree(5) (4) (3) (2) (1)Figure 7.15 Example ofthe Semantic DifferentialInstructions: Listed below are several pairs of adjectives. Place acheck mark (✓) on the l<strong>in</strong>e between each pair <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate howyou feel. Example: Hockey:excit<strong>in</strong>g :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: dullIf you feel that hockey is very excit<strong>in</strong>g, you would place a check mark<strong>in</strong> the first space next <strong>to</strong> the word excit<strong>in</strong>g. If you feel that hockey isvery dull, you would place a check mark <strong>in</strong> the space nearest the worddull. If you are sort of undecided, you would place a check mark <strong>in</strong> themiddle space between the two words. Now rate each of the activitiesthat follow [only one is listed]:Work<strong>in</strong>g with other students <strong>in</strong> small groupsfriendly :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: unfriendlyhappy :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:sadeasy :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: hardfun :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: workhot :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:coldgood :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: badlaugh :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:crybeautiful :_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: ugly


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 127<strong>How</strong> do you feel about arithmetic?Figure 7.16 Pic<strong>to</strong>rial Attitude Scale for Use with Young ChildrenInstructions: Check the option that most correctly describes you.QuiteAlmostSELF-ESTEEM ________ often Sometimes _________ ________ never1. Do you th<strong>in</strong>k your friends are smarter than you? _____ _____ _____2. Do you feel good about your appearance? _____ _____ _____3. Do you avoid meet<strong>in</strong>g new people? _____ _____ _____QuiteAlmostSTRESS ________ often Sometimes _________ ________ never1. Do you have trouble sleep<strong>in</strong>g? _______ _______ _______2. Do you feel on <strong>to</strong>p of th<strong>in</strong>gs? _______ _______ _______3. Do you feel you have <strong>to</strong>o much <strong>to</strong> do? _______ _______ _______Figure 7.17 Sample Items from a Personality Inven<strong>to</strong>rymeasure learn<strong>in</strong>g or the effectiveness of <strong>in</strong>struction. TheCalifornia Achievement Test, for example, measuresachievement <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, language, <strong>and</strong> arithmetic. TheStanford Achievement Test measures a variety of areas,such as language usage, word mean<strong>in</strong>g, spell<strong>in</strong>g, arithmeticcomputation, social studies, <strong>and</strong> science. Othercommonly used achievement tests <strong>in</strong>clude the ComprehensiveTests of Basic Skills, the Iowa Tests of BasicSkills, the Metropolitan Achievement Test, <strong>and</strong> theSequential Tests of <strong>Education</strong>al Progress (STEP). Inresearch that <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional methods,achievement is frequently the dependent variable.Achievement tests can be classified <strong>in</strong> several ways.General achievement tests are usually batteries of tests(such as the STEP tests) that measure such th<strong>in</strong>gs asvocabulary, read<strong>in</strong>g ability, language usage, math, <strong>and</strong>social studies. One of the most common generalachievement tests is the Graduate Record Exam<strong>in</strong>ation,which students must pass before they can beadmitted <strong>to</strong> most graduate programs. Specific achievementtests, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, are tests that measure an<strong>in</strong>dividual’s ability <strong>in</strong> a specific subject, such as English,world his<strong>to</strong>ry, or biology. Figure 7.18 illustratesthe k<strong>in</strong>ds of items found on an achievement test.Aptitude Tests. Another well-known type of abilitytest is the so-called general aptitude, or <strong>in</strong>telligence,test, which assesses <strong>in</strong>tellectual abilities that are not,<strong>in</strong> most cases, specifically taught <strong>in</strong> school. Some measureof general ability is frequently used as either an<strong>in</strong>dependent or a dependent variable <strong>in</strong> research. Inattempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> assess the effects of different <strong>in</strong>structionalprograms, for example, it is often necessary (<strong>and</strong> veryimportant) <strong>to</strong> control this variable so that groupsexposed <strong>to</strong> the different programs are not markedly different<strong>in</strong> general ability.


128 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 7.18 SampleItems from anAchievement TestInstructions: Use the map <strong>to</strong> answer questions 1 <strong>and</strong> 2. Circle the correct answer.76521431. Which number shows an area decl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> population?A 1B 3C 5D 72. Which number shows an area that once belonged <strong>to</strong> Mexico?A 3B 5C 6D None of the aboveAptitude tests are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> measure an <strong>in</strong>dividual’spotential <strong>to</strong> achieve; <strong>in</strong> actuality, they measure presentskills or abilities. They differ from achievementtests <strong>in</strong> their purpose <strong>and</strong> often <strong>in</strong> content, usually <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>ga wider variety of skills or knowledge. Thesame test may be either an aptitude or an achievementtest, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the purpose for which it is used. Amathematics achievement test, for example, may alsomeasure aptitude for additional mathematics. Althoughsuch tests are used primarily by counselors <strong>to</strong> help <strong>in</strong>dividualsidentify areas <strong>in</strong> which they may have potential,they also can be used <strong>in</strong> research. In this regard, they areparticularly useful for purposes of control. For example,<strong>to</strong> measure the effectiveness of an <strong>in</strong>structional programdesigned <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>in</strong> mathematics,a researcher might decide <strong>to</strong> use an aptitude test<strong>to</strong> adjust for <strong>in</strong>itial differences <strong>in</strong> ability. Figure 7.19presents an example of one k<strong>in</strong>d of item found on an aptitudetest.Aptitude tests may be adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals orgroups. Each method has both advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages.The big advantage of group tests is that theyare more convenient <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister <strong>and</strong> hence saveconsiderable time. One disadvantage is that they requirea great deal of read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> students who are low <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g ability are thus at a disadvantage. Furthermore,it is difficult for those tak<strong>in</strong>g the test <strong>to</strong> have test <strong>in</strong>structionsclarified or <strong>to</strong> have any <strong>in</strong>teraction with the exam<strong>in</strong>er(which sometimes can raise scores). Lastly, therange of possible tasks on which the student can be exam<strong>in</strong>edis much less with a group-adm<strong>in</strong>istered test thanwith an <strong>in</strong>dividually adm<strong>in</strong>istered test.The California Test of Mental Maturity (CTMM) <strong>and</strong>the Otis-Lennon are examples of group tests. The bestknownof the <strong>in</strong>dividual aptitude tests is the Stanford-B<strong>in</strong>et Intelligence Scale, although the Wechsler scales areused more widely. Whereas the Stanford-B<strong>in</strong>et gives onlyone IQ score, the Wechsler scales also yield a number ofsubscores. The two Wechsler scales are the WechslerIntelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III) for ages 5 <strong>to</strong>15 <strong>and</strong> the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III)for older adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults.Many <strong>in</strong>telligence tests provide reliable <strong>and</strong> validevidence when used with certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>and</strong> for certa<strong>in</strong> purposes (for example, predict<strong>in</strong>g thecollege grades of middle-class Caucasians). On theother h<strong>and</strong>, they have <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly come under attackwhen used with other persons or for other purposes(such as identify<strong>in</strong>g members of certa<strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority groups<strong>to</strong> be placed <strong>in</strong> special classes). Furthermore, there is


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 129Look at the foldout on the left. Which object on the right can be made from it?Figure 7.19 Sample Item from an Aptitude TestA B C D E<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g recognition that most <strong>in</strong>telligence tests fail <strong>to</strong>measure many important abilities, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the ability<strong>to</strong> identify or conceptualize unusual sorts of relationships.As a result, the researcher must be especiallycareful <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g any such test before us<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>and</strong>must determ<strong>in</strong>e whether it is appropriate for thepurpose of the study. (We discuss some ways <strong>to</strong> do thiswhen we consider validity <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8.) Figure 7.20presents examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of items on an <strong>in</strong>telligencetest.Performance Tests. As we have mentioned, a performancetest measures an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s performance ona particular task. An example would be a typ<strong>in</strong>g test, <strong>in</strong>which <strong>in</strong>dividual scores are determ<strong>in</strong>ed by how accurately<strong>and</strong> how rapidly people type.As Saw<strong>in</strong> has suggested, it is not always easy <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether a particular <strong>in</strong>strument should be1. <strong>How</strong> are frog <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>y alike <strong>and</strong> how are they different?2. Here is a sequence of pictures.A B C D EWhich of the follow<strong>in</strong>g would come next <strong>in</strong> the sequence?F G HFigure 7.20 Sample Items from an Intelligence Testcalled a performance test, a performance checklist, or aperformance rat<strong>in</strong>g scale. 6 A performance test is themost objective of the three. When a considerableamount of judgment is required <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if the variousaspects of a performance were done correctly, thedevice is likely <strong>to</strong> be classified as either a checklist orrat<strong>in</strong>g scale. Figure 7.21 illustrates a performance testdeveloped more than 60 years ago <strong>to</strong> measure sew<strong>in</strong>gability. In this test, the <strong>in</strong>dividual is requested <strong>to</strong> sew onthe l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> part A of the test, <strong>and</strong> between the l<strong>in</strong>es onpart B of the test. 7Projective Devices. A projective device is anysort of <strong>in</strong>strument with a vague stimulus that allows<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> project their <strong>in</strong>terests, preferences, anxieties,prejudices, needs, <strong>and</strong> so on through theirresponses <strong>to</strong> it. This k<strong>in</strong>d of device has no “right” answers(or any clear-cut answers of any sort), <strong>and</strong> itsformat allows an <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>to</strong> express someth<strong>in</strong>g of hisor her own personality. There is room for a wide varietyof responses.Perhaps the best-known example of a projectivedevice is the Rorschach Ink Blot Test, <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividualsare presented with a series of ambiguously shaped<strong>in</strong>k blots <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong> describe what the blots look like.Another well-known projective test is the ThematicApperception Test (TAT), <strong>in</strong> which pictures of eventsare presented <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals are asked <strong>to</strong> make up as<strong>to</strong>ry about each picture. One application of the projectiveapproach <strong>to</strong> a classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g is the Picture SituationInven<strong>to</strong>ry, one of the few examples especiallyadapted <strong>to</strong> classroom situations. This <strong>in</strong>strument consistsof a series of car<strong>to</strong>onlike draw<strong>in</strong>gs, each portray<strong>in</strong>ga classroom situation <strong>in</strong> which a child is say<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g.Students tak<strong>in</strong>g the test are <strong>to</strong> enter the responseof the teacher, thereby presumably <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g


130 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 7.21 Examplefrom the Blum Sew<strong>in</strong>gMach<strong>in</strong>e TestSource: M. L. Blum. Selection ofsew<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>e opera<strong>to</strong>rs.Journal of Applied Psychology,27 (1): 36. Copyright 1943 bythe American PsychologicalAssociation. Reproduced withpermission.Directions: Sew on the l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> part A <strong>and</strong> between the l<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> part B.NAMEANAMEBPicture 1 Picture 2Do I have <strong>to</strong>do this? Youdidn’t makeTom.My motherwants me<strong>to</strong> read <strong>in</strong>a harderbook.Figure 7.22 Sample Items from the Picture Situation Inven<strong>to</strong>rySource: N. T. Rowan (1967). The relationship of teacher <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> classroom situations <strong>to</strong> teacherpersonality variables. Unpublished doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertation. Salt Lake City: University of Utah, p. 68.of their own tendencies <strong>in</strong> the situation. Two of the pictures<strong>in</strong> this test are reproduced <strong>in</strong> Figure 7.22.Sociometric Devices. Sociometric devices ask <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>to</strong> rate their peers <strong>in</strong> some way. Two examples<strong>in</strong>clude the sociogram <strong>and</strong> the “group play.” A sociogramis a visual representation, usually by means of arrows, ofthe choices people make about other <strong>in</strong>dividuals withwhom they <strong>in</strong>teract. It is frequently used <strong>to</strong> assess the climate<strong>and</strong> structure of <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationships with<strong>in</strong> aclassroom, but it is by no means limited <strong>to</strong> such an environment.Each student is usually represented by a circle(if female) or a triangle (if male), <strong>and</strong> arrows are thendrawn <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate different student choices with regard <strong>to</strong>a particular question. Students may be asked, for example,<strong>to</strong> list three students whom they consider leaders of


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 131KEYBlack l<strong>in</strong>es = Choices= Mutual choice1, 2, 3, outside circle = order of choiceName1-1No. ofchoicesreceivedNo. ofmutualchoicesJan4-33123Gloria2-1223Pat0-0Date givenGrade/classChoice question:SchoolPresent124seat<strong>in</strong>gGirlsBoys3 1Connie 2Mary4-23 4-21 2AbsentTom3-12321Mary Ann6-33Rose3-321312Irene4-331Felix2-21June2-22Eve3-331Al2-11 1Joe3-2223 23 Doris122-23 1Jean2-2Figure 7.23 Example of a Sociogramthe class; admire the most; f<strong>in</strong>d especially helpful; wouldlike <strong>to</strong> have for a friend; would like <strong>to</strong> have as a partner <strong>in</strong>a research project; <strong>and</strong> so forth. The responses studentsgive are then used <strong>to</strong> construct the sociogram. Figure 7.23illustrates a sociogram.Another version of a sociometric device is the groupplay. Students are asked <strong>to</strong> cast different members oftheir group <strong>in</strong> various roles <strong>in</strong> a play <strong>to</strong> illustrate their <strong>in</strong>terpersonalrelationships. The roles are listed on a pieceof paper, <strong>and</strong> then the members of the group are asked <strong>to</strong>write <strong>in</strong> the name of the student they th<strong>in</strong>k each rolebest describes. Almost any type of role can be suggested.The cast<strong>in</strong>g choices that <strong>in</strong>dividuals make oftenshed considerable light on how some <strong>in</strong>dividuals are


132 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eDirections: Imag<strong>in</strong>e you are the cast<strong>in</strong>g direc<strong>to</strong>r for a large play. Your job is <strong>to</strong> choose the <strong>in</strong>dividualswho will take the various parts (listed below) <strong>in</strong> the play. S<strong>in</strong>ce some of the parts are rather small, youmay select the same <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>to</strong> play more than one part. Choose <strong>in</strong>dividuals you th<strong>in</strong>k would be themost natural for the part, that is, those who are most like the role <strong>in</strong> real life.1. The partsPart 1—someone who is well liked by all members of the group ____________________________________Part 2—someone who is disliked by many people __________________________________________________Part 3—someone who always gets angry about th<strong>in</strong>gs of little consequence __________________________Part 4—someone who has wit <strong>and</strong> a good sense of humor ___________________________________________Part 5—someone who is very quiet <strong>and</strong> rarely says anyth<strong>in</strong>g ________________________________________Part 6—someone who does not contribute much <strong>to</strong> the group ______________________________________Part 7—someone who is angry a lot of the time _____________________________________________________2. Your roleWhich part do you th<strong>in</strong>k you could play best? ________________________________________________________Which part would other members of the group ask you <strong>to</strong> play? _____________________________________Figure 7.24 Example of a Group Playviewed by others. Figure 7.24 presents an example ofthis device.Item Formats. Although the types of items or questionsused <strong>in</strong> different <strong>in</strong>struments can take many forms,each item can be classified as either a selection item ora supply item. A selection item presents a set of possibleresponses from which respondents are <strong>to</strong> select the mostappropriate answer. A supply item, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,asks respondents <strong>to</strong> formulate <strong>and</strong> then supply their ownanswers. Here are some examples of each type.Selection Items. True-false items: True-false itemspresent either a true or a false statement, <strong>and</strong> the respondenthas <strong>to</strong> mark either true (T) or false (F). Frequentlyused variations of the words true <strong>and</strong> false are yes-no orright-wrong, which often are more useful when attempt<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> question or <strong>in</strong>terview young children. Here is anexample of a true-false item.T F I get very nervous whenever I have <strong>to</strong> speak <strong>in</strong> public.Multiple-choice items: Multiple-choice items consis<strong>to</strong>f two parts: the stem, which conta<strong>in</strong>s the question, <strong>and</strong>several (usually four) possible choices. Here is an example:Which of the follow<strong>in</strong>g expresses your op<strong>in</strong>ion on abortion?a. It is immoral <strong>and</strong> should be prohibited.b. It should be discouraged but permitted underunusual circumstances.c. It should be available under a wide range ofconditions.d. It is entirely a matter of <strong>in</strong>dividual choice.Match<strong>in</strong>g items: Match<strong>in</strong>g items are variations of themultiple-choice format. They consist of two groupslisted <strong>in</strong> columns—the left-h<strong>and</strong> column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g thequestions or items <strong>to</strong> be thought about <strong>and</strong> the right-h<strong>and</strong>column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the possible responses <strong>to</strong> the questions.The respondent pairs the choice from the righth<strong>and</strong>column with the correspond<strong>in</strong>g question or item <strong>in</strong>the left-h<strong>and</strong> column. Here is an example:Instructions: For each item <strong>in</strong> the left-h<strong>and</strong> column, selectthe item <strong>in</strong> the right-h<strong>and</strong> column that represents your firstreaction. Place the appropriate letter <strong>in</strong> the blank. Eachlettered item may be used more than once or not at all.Column ASpecial classes for the:___ 1. severely retarded___ 2. mildly retarded___ 3. hard of hear<strong>in</strong>g___ 4. visually impaired___ 5. learn<strong>in</strong>gh<strong>and</strong>icapped___ 6. emotionallydisturbedColumn Ba. should be <strong>in</strong>creasedb. should be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>edc. should be decreasedd. should be elim<strong>in</strong>ated


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 133Interpretive exercises: One difficulty with us<strong>in</strong>g truefalse,multiple-choice, <strong>and</strong> match<strong>in</strong>g items <strong>to</strong> measureachievement is that these items often do not measure complexlearn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes. One way <strong>to</strong> get at more complexlearn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes is <strong>to</strong> use what is called an <strong>in</strong>terpretiveexercise. An <strong>in</strong>terpretive exercise consists of a selectionof <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry material (this may be a paragraph, map,diagram, picture, chart) followed by one or more selectionitems that ask a respondent <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret this material.Two examples of <strong>in</strong>terpretive exercises follow.Example 1.Directions: Read the follow<strong>in</strong>g comments a teachermade about test<strong>in</strong>g. Then answer the question thatfollows the comments by circl<strong>in</strong>g the letter of the bestanswer.“Students go <strong>to</strong> school <strong>to</strong> learn, not <strong>to</strong> take tests. In addition,tests cannot be used <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate a student’s absolutelevel of learn<strong>in</strong>g. All tests can do is rank students <strong>in</strong> orderof achievement, <strong>and</strong> this relative rank<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>fluenced byguess<strong>in</strong>g, bluff<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> the subjective op<strong>in</strong>ions of theteacher do<strong>in</strong>g the scor<strong>in</strong>g. The teach<strong>in</strong>g-learn<strong>in</strong>g processwould benefit if we did away with tests <strong>and</strong> depended onstudent self-evaluation.”1. Which one of the follow<strong>in</strong>g unstated assumptions isthis teacher mak<strong>in</strong>g?a. Students go <strong>to</strong> school <strong>to</strong> learn.b. Teachers use essay tests primarily.c. Tests make no contribution <strong>to</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g.d. Tests do not <strong>in</strong>dicate a student’s absolute level oflearn<strong>in</strong>g.Example 2.Directions: Paragraph A conta<strong>in</strong>s a description of the test<strong>in</strong>gpractices of Mr. Smith, a high school teacher. Readthe description <strong>and</strong> each of the statements that follow it.Mark each statement <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the type of <strong>in</strong>ference thatcan be drawn about it from the material <strong>in</strong> the paragraph.Place the appropriate letter <strong>in</strong> front of each statementus<strong>in</strong>g the follow<strong>in</strong>g key:T—if the statement may be <strong>in</strong>ferred as true.F—if the statement may be <strong>in</strong>ferred as untrue.N—if no <strong>in</strong>ference may be drawn about it from theparagraph.Paragraph AApproximately one week before a test is <strong>to</strong> be given,Mr. Smith carefully goes through the textbook <strong>and</strong>constructs multiple-choice items based on the material<strong>in</strong> the book. He always uses the exact word<strong>in</strong>g of the textbookfor the correct answer so that there will beno question concern<strong>in</strong>g its correctness. He is careful<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude some test items from each chapter. After thetest is given, he lists the scores from high <strong>to</strong> low on theblackboard <strong>and</strong> tells each student his or her score. Hedoes not return the test papers <strong>to</strong> the students, but heoffers <strong>to</strong> answer any questions they might have aboutthe test. He puts the items from each test <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a test file,which he is build<strong>in</strong>g for future use.Statements on Paragraph A(T)(F)(N)(N)(T)(F)1. Mr. Smith’s tests measure a limited range oflearn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes.2. Some of Mr. Smith’s test items measure atthe underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g level.3. Mr. Smith’s tests measure a balanced sampleof subject matter.4. Mr. Smith uses the type of test item that isbest for his purpose.5. Students can determ<strong>in</strong>e where they rank <strong>in</strong>the distribution of scores on Mr. Smith’s tests.6. Mr. Smith’s test<strong>in</strong>g practices are likely <strong>to</strong>motivate students <strong>to</strong> overcome their weaknesses.8Supply Items. Short-answer items: A short-answeritem requires the respondent <strong>to</strong> supply a word, phrase,number, or symbol that is necessary <strong>to</strong> complete a statemen<strong>to</strong>r answer a question. Here is an example:Directions: In the space provided, write the word that bestcompletes the sentence.When the number of items <strong>in</strong> a test is <strong>in</strong>creased, theof the scores on the test is likely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease.(Answer: reliability.)Short-answer items have one major disadvantage: It isusually difficult <strong>to</strong> write a short-answer item so onlyone word completes it correctly. In the question above,for example, many students might argue that the wordrange would also be correct.Essay questions: An essay question is one that respondentsare asked <strong>to</strong> write about at length. As withshort-answer questions, subjects must produce theirown answers. Generally, however, they are free <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ehow <strong>to</strong> answer the question, what facts <strong>to</strong> present,which <strong>to</strong> emphasize, what <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>to</strong> make,<strong>and</strong> the like. For these reasons, the essay question is a


134 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eparticularly useful device for assess<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual’sability <strong>to</strong> organize, <strong>in</strong>tegrate, analyze, <strong>and</strong> synthesize<strong>in</strong>formation. It is especially useful <strong>in</strong> measur<strong>in</strong>g the socalledhigher-level learn<strong>in</strong>g outcomes, such as analysis,synthesis, <strong>and</strong> evaluation. Here are two examples ofessay questions:Example 1Mr. Rogers, a n<strong>in</strong>th-grade science teacher, wants <strong>to</strong> measurehis students’ “ability <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret scientific data” witha paper-<strong>and</strong>-pencil test.1. Describe the steps that Mr. Rogers should follow.2. Give reasons <strong>to</strong> justify each step.Example 2For a course that you are teach<strong>in</strong>g or expect <strong>to</strong> teach,prepare a complete plan for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g student achievement.Be sure <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude the procedures you wouldfollow, the <strong>in</strong>struments you would use, <strong>and</strong> the reasonsfor your choices. 9UNOBTRUSIVE MEASURESMany <strong>in</strong>struments require the cooperation of the respondent<strong>in</strong> one way or another <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>volve some k<strong>in</strong>d of<strong>in</strong>trusion <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g activities. On occasion, respondentswill dislike or even resent be<strong>in</strong>g tested, observed, or<strong>in</strong>terviewed. Furthermore, the reaction of respondents <strong>to</strong>the <strong>in</strong>strumentation process—that is, <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g tested,observed, or <strong>in</strong>terviewed—often will, <strong>to</strong> some degree, affectthe nature of the <strong>in</strong>formation researchers obta<strong>in</strong>.To elim<strong>in</strong>ate this reactive effect, researchers at timesattempt <strong>to</strong> use what are called unobtrusive measures, 10which are data-collection procedures that <strong>in</strong>volve no <strong>in</strong>trusion<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g course of events. Inmost <strong>in</strong>stances, no <strong>in</strong>strument is required, only someform of record keep<strong>in</strong>g. Here are some examples ofsuch procedures:The degree of fear <strong>in</strong>duced by a ghost-s<strong>to</strong>ry-tell<strong>in</strong>gsession can be measured by not<strong>in</strong>g the shr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gdiameter of a circle of seated children.Library withdrawals could be used <strong>to</strong> demonstratethe effect of the <strong>in</strong>troduction of a new unit on Ch<strong>in</strong>esehis<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> a social studies curriculum.The <strong>in</strong>terest of children <strong>in</strong> Christmas or other holidaysmight be demonstrated by the amount of dis<strong>to</strong>rtion <strong>in</strong>the size of their draw<strong>in</strong>gs of Santa Claus or otherholiday figures.Racial attitudes <strong>in</strong> two elementary schools might becompared by not<strong>in</strong>g the degree of cluster<strong>in</strong>g of membersof different ethnic groups <strong>in</strong> the lunchroom <strong>and</strong>on the playground.The values held by people of different countriesmight be compared through analyz<strong>in</strong>g different typesof published materials, such as textbooks, plays,h<strong>and</strong>books for youth organizations, magaz<strong>in</strong>e advertisements,<strong>and</strong> newspaper headl<strong>in</strong>es.Some idea of the attention paid <strong>to</strong> patients <strong>in</strong> a hospitalmight be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by observ<strong>in</strong>g the frequencyof notes, both <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>and</strong> required, made by attend<strong>in</strong>gnurses <strong>to</strong> patients’ bedside records.The degree of stress felt by college students might beassessed by not<strong>in</strong>g the nature <strong>and</strong> frequency of sickcallvisits <strong>to</strong> the college health center.Student attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong>, various <strong>to</strong>picscan be noted by observ<strong>in</strong>g the amount of graffitiabout those <strong>to</strong>pics written on school walls.Many variables of <strong>in</strong>terest can be assessed, at least <strong>to</strong>some degree, through the use of unobtrusive measures.The reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of <strong>in</strong>ferences based on suchmeasures will vary depend<strong>in</strong>g on the procedure used.Nevertheless, unobtrusive measures add an important<strong>and</strong> useful dimension <strong>to</strong> the array of possible data sourcesavailable <strong>to</strong> researchers. They are particularly valuable assupplements <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> questionnaires, often provid<strong>in</strong>ga useful way <strong>to</strong> corroborate (or contradict) whatthese more traditional data sources reveal. 11Types of ScoresQuantitative data are usually reported <strong>in</strong> the form ofscores. Scores can be reported <strong>in</strong> many ways, but animportant dist<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> is the differencebetween raw scores <strong>and</strong> derived scores.RAW SCORESAlmost all measurement beg<strong>in</strong>s with what is called araw score, which is the <strong>in</strong>itial score obta<strong>in</strong>ed. It may bethe <strong>to</strong>tal number of items an <strong>in</strong>dividual gets correct oranswers <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> way on a test, the number of timesa certa<strong>in</strong> behavior is tallied, the rat<strong>in</strong>g given by ateacher, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Examples <strong>in</strong>clude the number ofquestions answered correctly on a science test, the numberof questions answered “positively” on an attitudescale, the number of times “aggressive” behavior is


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 135observed, a teacher’s rat<strong>in</strong>g on a “self-esteem” measure,or the number of choices received on a sociogram.Taken by itself, an <strong>in</strong>dividual raw score is difficult <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret, s<strong>in</strong>ce it has little mean<strong>in</strong>g. What, for example,does it mean <strong>to</strong> say a student received a score of 62 on atest if that is all the <strong>in</strong>formation you have? Even if youknow that there were 100 questions on the test, youdon’t know whether 62 is an extremely high (or extremelylow) score, s<strong>in</strong>ce the test may have been easy ordifficult.We often want <strong>to</strong> know how one <strong>in</strong>dividual’s rawscore compares <strong>to</strong> those of other <strong>in</strong>dividuals tak<strong>in</strong>gthe same test, <strong>and</strong> (perhaps) how he or she has scoredon similar tests taken at other times. This is truewhenever we want <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret an <strong>in</strong>dividual score.Because raw scores by themselves are difficult <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret, they often are converted <strong>to</strong> what are calledderived scores.DERIVED SCORESDerived scores are obta<strong>in</strong>ed by tak<strong>in</strong>g raw scores <strong>and</strong>convert<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> more useful scores on some typeof st<strong>and</strong>ardized basis. They <strong>in</strong>dicate where a particular<strong>in</strong>dividual’s raw score falls <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> all other rawscores <strong>in</strong> the same distribution. They enable a researcher<strong>to</strong> say how well the <strong>in</strong>dividual has performedcompared <strong>to</strong> all others tak<strong>in</strong>g the same test. Examples ofderived scores are age- <strong>and</strong> grade-level equivalents, percentileranks, <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard scores.Age <strong>and</strong> Grade-level Equivalents. Ageequivalentscores <strong>and</strong> grade-equivalent scores tell usof what age or grade an <strong>in</strong>dividual score is typical. Suppose,for example, that the average score on a beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof-the-yeararithmetic test for all eighth-graders <strong>in</strong> acerta<strong>in</strong> state is 62 out of a possible 100. Students whoscore 62 will have a grade equivalent of 8.0 on the testregardless of their actual grade placement—whether <strong>in</strong>sixth, seventh, eighth, n<strong>in</strong>th, or tenth grade, the student’sperformance is typical of beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g eighth-graders. Similarly,a student who is 10 years <strong>and</strong> 6 months old mayhave an age-equivalent score of 12-2, mean<strong>in</strong>g that his orher test performance is typical of students who are12 years <strong>and</strong> 2 months old.Percentile Ranks. A percentile rank refers <strong>to</strong> thepercentage of <strong>in</strong>dividuals scor<strong>in</strong>g at or below a givenraw score. Percentile ranks are sometimes referred <strong>to</strong> aspercentiles, although this term is not quite correct as asynonym.*Percentile ranks are easy <strong>to</strong> calculate. A simple formulafor convert<strong>in</strong>g raw scores <strong>to</strong> percentile ranks (Pr)is as follows:number of studentsall studentsbelow score at scorePr * 100<strong>to</strong>tal number <strong>in</strong> groupSuppose a <strong>to</strong>tal of 100 students <strong>to</strong>ok an exam<strong>in</strong>ation,<strong>and</strong> 18 of them received a raw score above 85, whiletwo students received a score of 85. Eighty students,then, scored somewhere below 85. What is the percentilerank of the two students who received the scoreof 85? Us<strong>in</strong>g the formulaPr 80 2100* 100 82the percentile rank of these two students is 82.Often percentile ranks are calculated for each of thescores <strong>in</strong> a group. Table 7.1 presents a group of scoreswith the percentile rank of each score <strong>in</strong>dicated.TABLE 7.1 Hypothetical Examples of Raw Scores<strong>and</strong> Accompany<strong>in</strong>g Percentile RanksRaw Cumulative PercentileScore Frequency Frequency Rank95 1 25 10093 1 24 9688 2 23 9285 3 21 8479 1 18 7275 4 17 6870 6 13 5265 2 7 2862 1 5 2058 1 4 1654 2 3 1250 1 1 4N 25*A percentile is the po<strong>in</strong>t below which a certa<strong>in</strong> percentage of scoresfall. The 70th percentile, for example, is the po<strong>in</strong>t below which 70percent of the scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution fall. The 99th percentile is thepo<strong>in</strong>t below which 99 percent of the scores fall, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Thus, if20 percent of the students <strong>in</strong> a sample score below 40 on a test, thenthe 20th percentile is a score of 40. A person who obta<strong>in</strong>s a score of40 has a percentile rank of 20.


136 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSt<strong>and</strong>ard Scores. St<strong>and</strong>ard scores provide anothermeans of <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g how one <strong>in</strong>dividual compares <strong>to</strong>other <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> a group. St<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>in</strong>dicatehow far a given raw score is from a reference po<strong>in</strong>t.They are particularly helpful <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual’srelative achievement on different types of <strong>in</strong>struments(such as compar<strong>in</strong>g a person’s performance on achemistry achievement test with an <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r’s rat<strong>in</strong>gof his work <strong>in</strong> a labora<strong>to</strong>ry). Many different systems ofst<strong>and</strong>ard scores exist, but the two most commonly used<strong>and</strong> reported <strong>in</strong> educational research are z scores <strong>and</strong> Tscores. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g them requires some knowledgeof descriptive statistics, however, <strong>and</strong> hence we willpostpone a discussion of them until Chapter 10.WHICH SCORES TO USE?Given these various types of scores, how do researchersdecide which <strong>to</strong> use? Recall that the usefulness of derivedscores is primarily <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual raw scoresmean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>to</strong> students, parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> others.Despite their value <strong>in</strong> this respect, some derived scoresshould not be used <strong>in</strong> research. This is the case if the researcheris assum<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terval scale, as often is done.Interval scales are discussed on pages 138–139. Percentileranks, for example, should never be used becausethey, almost certa<strong>in</strong>ly, do not constitute an <strong>in</strong>terval scale.Age- <strong>and</strong> grade-equivalent scores likewise have seriouslimitations because of the way they are obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Usuallythe best scores <strong>to</strong> use are st<strong>and</strong>ard scores, which aresometimes provided <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument manuals <strong>and</strong>, if not,can easily be calculated. (We discuss <strong>and</strong> show how <strong>to</strong>calculate st<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10.) If st<strong>and</strong>ardscores are not used, it is far preferable <strong>to</strong> use rawscores—convert<strong>in</strong>g derived scores, for example, back <strong>to</strong>the orig<strong>in</strong>al raw scores, if necessary—rather than usepercentile ranks or age/grade equivalents.Norm-Referenced VersusCriterion-ReferencedInstrumentsNORM-REFERENCED INSTRUMENTSAll derived scores give mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual scores bycompar<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> the scores of a group. This meansthat the nature of the group is extremely important.Whenever such scores are used, researchers must besure that the reference group makes sense. Compar<strong>in</strong>g aboy’s score on a grammar test <strong>to</strong> a group of girls’ scoreson that test, for example, may be quite mislead<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>cegirls usually score higher <strong>in</strong> grammar. The group used <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e derived scores is called the norm group, <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>struments that provide such scores are referred <strong>to</strong> asnorm-referenced <strong>in</strong>struments.CRITERION-REFERENCED INSTRUMENTSAn alternative <strong>to</strong> the use of cus<strong>to</strong>mary achievement orperformance <strong>in</strong>struments, most of which are normreferenced,is <strong>to</strong> use a criterion-referenced <strong>in</strong>strument—usuallya test.The <strong>in</strong>tent of such tests is somewhat different from tha<strong>to</strong>f norm-referenced tests; criterion-referenced tests focusmore directly on <strong>in</strong>struction. Rather than evaluat<strong>in</strong>glearner progress through ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> scores (for example, from40 <strong>to</strong> 70 on an achievement test), a criterion-referencedtest is based on a specific goal, or target (called a criterion),for each learner <strong>to</strong> achieve. This criterion formastery, or “pass,” is usually stated as a fairly high percentageof correctly answered questions (such as 80 or90 percent). Examples of criterion-referenced <strong>and</strong> normreferencedevaluation statements are as follows:Criterion-referenced: A student . . .spelled every word <strong>in</strong> the weekly spell<strong>in</strong>g list correctly.solved at least 75 percent of the assigned problems.achieved a score of at least 80 out of 100 on the f<strong>in</strong>alexam.did at least 25 push-ups with<strong>in</strong> a five-m<strong>in</strong>ute period.read a m<strong>in</strong>imum of one nonfiction book a week.Norm-referenced: A student . . .scored at the 50th percentile <strong>in</strong> his group.scored above 90 percent of all the students <strong>in</strong> theclass.received a higher grade po<strong>in</strong>t average <strong>in</strong> English literaturethan any other student <strong>in</strong> the school.ran faster than all but one other student on the team.<strong>and</strong> one other <strong>in</strong> the class were the only ones <strong>to</strong> receiveAs on the midterm.The advantage of a criterion-referenced <strong>in</strong>strument isthat it gives both teacher <strong>and</strong> students a clear-cut goal <strong>to</strong>work <strong>to</strong>ward. As a result, it has considerable appeal as ameans of improv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. In practice, however,several problems arise. First, teachers seldom set or reachthe ideal of <strong>in</strong>dividualized student goals. Rather, classgoals are more the rule, the idea be<strong>in</strong>g that all students


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 137SCALEEXAMPLENom<strong>in</strong>alGenderOrd<strong>in</strong>al4th 3rd 2nd 1stPosition <strong>in</strong> a raceInterval20100–10–20-20° -10° 0° 10° 20° 30° 40°Temperature(<strong>in</strong> Fahrenheit)Ratio$$$$$MoneyFigure 7.25 Four Types of Measurement Scales0 $100 $200 $300 $400 $500will reach the criterion—though, of course, some maynot <strong>and</strong> many will exceed it. The second problem is thatit is difficult <strong>to</strong> establish even class criteria that are mean<strong>in</strong>gful.What, precisely, should a class of fifth-graders beable <strong>to</strong> do <strong>in</strong> mathematics? Solve s<strong>to</strong>ry problems, manywould say. We would agree, but of what complexity? <strong>and</strong>requir<strong>in</strong>g which mathematics subskills? In the absence of<strong>in</strong>dependent criteria, we have little choice but <strong>to</strong> fall backon exist<strong>in</strong>g expectations, <strong>and</strong> this is typically (though notnecessarily) done by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g exist<strong>in</strong>g texts <strong>and</strong> tests.As a result, the specific items <strong>in</strong> a criterion-referencedtest often turn out <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guishable from those <strong>in</strong>the usual norm-referenced test, with one important difference:A criterion-referenced test at any grade level will almostcerta<strong>in</strong>ly be easier than a norm-referenced test. Itmust be easier if most students are <strong>to</strong> get 80 or 90 percen<strong>to</strong>f the items correct. In prepar<strong>in</strong>g such tests, researchersmust try <strong>to</strong> write items that will be answered correctly by80 percent of the students—after all, they don’t want50 percent of their students <strong>to</strong> fail. The desired difficultylevel for norm-referenced items, however, is at or about50 percent, <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> provide the maximum opportunityfor the scores <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish the ability of one studentfrom another.While a criterion-referenced test may be more usefulat times <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> circumstances than the morecus<strong>to</strong>mary norm-referenced test (this issue is still be<strong>in</strong>gdebated), it is often <strong>in</strong>ferior for research purposes.Why? Because, <strong>in</strong> general, a criterion-referenced testwill provide much less variability of scores, because itis easier. Whereas the usual norm-referenced test willprovide a range of scores somewhat less than the possiblerange (that is, from zero <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>tal number ofitems <strong>in</strong> the test), a criterion-referenced test, if it istrue <strong>to</strong> its rationale, will have most of the students(surely at least half) gett<strong>in</strong>g a high score. Because, <strong>in</strong>research, we usually want maximum variability <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> have any hope of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g relationships withother variables, the use of criterion-referenced tests isoften self-defeat<strong>in</strong>g.*Measurement ScalesYou will recall from Chapter 3 that there are two basictypes of variables—quantitative <strong>and</strong> categorical. Eachuses a different type of analysis <strong>and</strong> measurement,requir<strong>in</strong>g the use of different measurement scales. Thereare four types of measurement scales: nom<strong>in</strong>al, ord<strong>in</strong>al,<strong>in</strong>terval, <strong>and</strong> ratio (Figure 7.25).*An exception is <strong>in</strong> program evaluation, where some researchersadvocate the use of criterion-referenced tests because they want <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ehow many students reach a predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed st<strong>and</strong>ard (criterion).


138 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Men arenumber one!”“Oh, no!Women arenumber one!”ORDINAL SCALESAn ord<strong>in</strong>al scale is one <strong>in</strong> which data may be ordered <strong>in</strong>some way—high <strong>to</strong> low or least <strong>to</strong> most. For example, aresearcher might rank-order student scores on a biologytest from high <strong>to</strong> low. Notice, however, that the difference<strong>in</strong> scores or <strong>in</strong> actual ability between the first- <strong>and</strong>second-ranked students <strong>and</strong> between the fifth- <strong>and</strong> sixthrankedstudents would not necessarily be the same.Ord<strong>in</strong>al scales <strong>in</strong>dicate relative st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g among <strong>in</strong>dividuals,as Figure 7.27 demonstrates.Figure 7.26 A Nom<strong>in</strong>al Scale of MeasurementNOMINAL SCALESA nom<strong>in</strong>al scale is the simplest form of measurement researcherscan use. When us<strong>in</strong>g a nom<strong>in</strong>al scale, researcherssimply assign numbers <strong>to</strong> different categories <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> show differences (Figure 7.26). For example, aresearcher concerned with the variable of gender mightgroup data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two categories, male <strong>and</strong> female, <strong>and</strong> assignthe number 1 <strong>to</strong> females <strong>and</strong> the number 2 <strong>to</strong> males.Another researcher, <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g methods ofteach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g, might assign the number 1 <strong>to</strong> the wholewordmethod, the number 2 <strong>to</strong> the phonics method, <strong>and</strong>the number 3 <strong>to</strong> the “mixed” method. In most cases, theadvantage <strong>to</strong> assign<strong>in</strong>g numbers <strong>to</strong> the categories is <strong>to</strong> facilitatecomputer analysis. There is no implication that thephonics method (assigned number 2) is “more” of anyth<strong>in</strong>gthan the whole-word method (assigned number 1).INTERVAL SCALESAn <strong>in</strong>terval scale possesses all the characteristics of anord<strong>in</strong>al scale with one additional feature: The distancesbetween the po<strong>in</strong>ts on the scale are equal. For example,the distances between scores on most commerciallyavailable mathematics achievement tests are usuallyconsidered equal. Thus, the distance between scores of70 <strong>and</strong> 80 is considered <strong>to</strong> be the same as the distancebetween scores of 80 <strong>and</strong> 90. Notice, however, that thezero po<strong>in</strong>t on an <strong>in</strong>terval scale does not <strong>in</strong>dicate a <strong>to</strong>talabsence of what is be<strong>in</strong>g measured. Thus, 0° (zero degrees)on the Fahrenheit scale, which measures temperature,does not <strong>in</strong>dicate no temperature.To illustrate further, consider the commonly used IQscore. Is the difference between an IQ of 90 <strong>and</strong> one of 100(10 po<strong>in</strong>ts) the same as the difference between an IQ of 40<strong>and</strong> one of 50 (also 10 po<strong>in</strong>ts)? or between an IQ of 120 <strong>and</strong>one of 130? If we believe that the scores constitute an <strong>in</strong>tervalscale, we must assume that 10 po<strong>in</strong>ts has the samemean<strong>in</strong>g at different po<strong>in</strong>ts on the scale. Do we knowwhetherthisistrue?No,wedonot,asweshallnowexpla<strong>in</strong>.With respect <strong>to</strong> some measurements, we can demonstrateequal <strong>in</strong>tervals. We do so by hav<strong>in</strong>g an agreed-on4321Old Pa<strong>in</strong>tSlick SamStarlightBellybusterFigure 7.27 An Ord<strong>in</strong>al Scale: The Outcome of a Horse Race


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHWhich Statistical Index Is Valid?Measurements, of course, are not limited <strong>to</strong> test scores<strong>and</strong> the like. For example, a widely used measurementis the “<strong>in</strong>dex of unemployment” provided by the Bureau ofLabor Statistics. One of its many uses is <strong>to</strong> study the relationshipbetween unemployment <strong>and</strong> crime. Its validity for thispurpose has been questioned by the author of a recent studywho found that while this <strong>in</strong>dex showed no relationship <strong>to</strong>property crimes, two different <strong>in</strong>dexes that reflected long-term(rather than temporary) unemployment did show substantialrelationships. The author concluded, “Clearly, we need <strong>to</strong>become as <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the selection of appropriate <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rsas we are with the specification of appropriate models <strong>and</strong> theselection of statistical techniques.”*What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Why did the different <strong>in</strong>dexes give differentresults?*M. B. Chaml<strong>in</strong> (2000). Unemployment, economic theory, <strong>and</strong>property crime: A note on measurement. Journal of QuantitativeCrim<strong>in</strong>ology, 16 (4): 454.st<strong>and</strong>ard unit. This is one reason why we have a Bureauof St<strong>and</strong>ards, located <strong>in</strong> Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC. You could, ifyou wished <strong>to</strong> do so, go <strong>to</strong> the bureau <strong>and</strong> actually “see”a st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>in</strong>ch, foot, ounce, <strong>and</strong> so on, that def<strong>in</strong>esthese units. While it might not be easy, you could conceivablycheck your carpenter’s rule us<strong>in</strong>g the “st<strong>and</strong>ard<strong>in</strong>ch” <strong>to</strong> see if an <strong>in</strong>ch is an <strong>in</strong>ch all along your rule. Youliterally could place the “st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>in</strong>ch” at variouspo<strong>in</strong>ts along your rule.There is no such st<strong>and</strong>ard unit for IQ or for virtuallyany variable commonly used <strong>in</strong> educational research.Over the years, sophisticated <strong>and</strong> clever techniqueshave been developed <strong>to</strong> create <strong>in</strong>terval scales for use byresearchers. The details are beyond the scope of thistext, but you should know that they all are based onhighly questionable assumptions.In actual practice, most researchers prefer <strong>to</strong> “act asif” they have an <strong>in</strong>terval scale, because it permits the useof more sensitive data analysis procedures <strong>and</strong> because,over the years, the results of do<strong>in</strong>g so make sense. Nevertheless,act<strong>in</strong>g as if we have <strong>in</strong>terval scales requires anassumption that (at least <strong>to</strong> date) cannot be proved.RATIO SCALESAn <strong>in</strong>terval scale that does possess an actual, or true,zero po<strong>in</strong>t is called a ratio scale. For example, a scaledesigned <strong>to</strong> measure height would be a ratio scale, becausethe zero po<strong>in</strong>t on the scale represents the absenceof height (that is, no height). Similarly, the zero on abathroom weight scale represents zero, or no, weight.Ratio scales are almost never encountered <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch, s<strong>in</strong>ce rarely do researchers engage <strong>in</strong>measurement <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a true zero po<strong>in</strong>t (even on thoserare occasions when a student receives a zero on a tes<strong>to</strong>f some sort, this does not mean that whatever is be<strong>in</strong>gmeasured is <strong>to</strong>tally absent <strong>in</strong> the student). Some othervariables that do have ratio scales are <strong>in</strong>come, time ontask, <strong>and</strong> age.MEASUREMENT SCALES RECONSIDEREDAt this po<strong>in</strong>t, you may be say<strong>in</strong>g, Well, okay, but sowhat? Why are these dist<strong>in</strong>ctions important? There aretwo reasons why you should have at least a rudimentaryunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the differences between these fourtypes of scales. First, they convey different amounts of<strong>in</strong>formation. Ratio scales provide more <strong>in</strong>formation th<strong>and</strong>o <strong>in</strong>terval scales; <strong>in</strong>terval, more than ord<strong>in</strong>al; <strong>and</strong> ord<strong>in</strong>al,more than nom<strong>in</strong>al. Hence, if possible, researchersshould use the type of measurement that will providethem with the maximum amount of <strong>in</strong>formation needed<strong>to</strong> answer their research question. Second, some types ofstatistical procedures are <strong>in</strong>appropriate for the differentscales. The way <strong>in</strong> which the data <strong>in</strong> a research study areorganized dictates the use of certa<strong>in</strong> types of statisticalanalyses, but not others (we shall discuss this po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>more detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 11). Table 7.2 presents a summaryof the four types of measurement scales.TABLE 7.2 Characteristics of the Four Typesof Measurement ScalesMeasurementScaleCharacteristicsNom<strong>in</strong>al Groups <strong>and</strong> labels data only;reports frequencies or percentages.Ord<strong>in</strong>al Ranks data; uses numbers only <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dicate rank<strong>in</strong>g.Interval Assumes that equal differences betweenscores really mean equal differences <strong>in</strong>the variable measured.RatioAll of the above, plus a true zero po<strong>in</strong>t.139


140 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOften researchers have a choice <strong>to</strong> make. They mustdecide whether <strong>to</strong> consider data as ord<strong>in</strong>al or <strong>in</strong>terval.For example, suppose a researcher uses a self-reportquestionnaire <strong>to</strong> measure self-esteem. The questionnaireis scored for the number of items answered (yes orno) <strong>in</strong> the direction <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g high self-esteem. For agiven sample of 60, the researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds that the scoresrange from 30 <strong>to</strong> 75.The researcher may now decide <strong>to</strong> treat scores as <strong>in</strong>tervaldata, <strong>in</strong> which case she assumes that equal distances(e.g., 30–34, 35–39, 40–44) <strong>in</strong> score represent equal differences<strong>in</strong> self-esteem.* If the researcher is uncomfortablewith this assumption, she could use the scores <strong>to</strong> rankthe <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> her sample from highest (rank 1) <strong>to</strong> lowest(rank 60). If she were then <strong>to</strong> use only these rank<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> subsequent analysis, she would now be assum<strong>in</strong>g thather <strong>in</strong>strument provides only ord<strong>in</strong>al data.Fortunately, researchers can avoid this choice. Theyhave another option—<strong>to</strong> treat the data separately accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> both assumptions (that is, <strong>to</strong> treat the scores as ord<strong>in</strong>aldata, <strong>and</strong> then aga<strong>in</strong> as <strong>in</strong>terval data). The importantth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> realize is that a researcher must be prepared <strong>to</strong> defendthe assumptions underly<strong>in</strong>g her choice of a measurementscale used <strong>in</strong> the collection <strong>and</strong> organization of data.Prepar<strong>in</strong>g Data for AnalysisOnce the <strong>in</strong>struments be<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>in</strong> a study have been adm<strong>in</strong>istered,the researcher must score the data that havebeen collected <strong>and</strong> then organize it <strong>to</strong> facilitate analysis.SCORING THE DATACollected data must be scored accurately <strong>and</strong> consistently.If they are not, any conclusions a researcher drawsfrom the data may be erroneous or mislead<strong>in</strong>g. Each <strong>in</strong>dividual’stest (questionnaire, essay, etc.) should be scoredus<strong>in</strong>g exactly the same procedures <strong>and</strong> criteria. When acommercially purchased <strong>in</strong>strument is used, the scor<strong>in</strong>gprocedures are made much easier. Usually the <strong>in</strong>strumentdeveloper will provide a scor<strong>in</strong>g manual that lists thesteps <strong>to</strong> follow <strong>in</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>strument, along with ascor<strong>in</strong>g key. It is a good idea <strong>to</strong> double-check one’s scor<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> ensure that no mistakes have been made.The scor<strong>in</strong>g of a self-developed test can produce difficulties,<strong>and</strong> hence researchers have <strong>to</strong> take special care <strong>to</strong>*Notice that she cannot treat the scores as ratio data, s<strong>in</strong>ce a score ofzero cannot be assumed <strong>to</strong> represent zero (i.e., no) self-esteem.ensure that scor<strong>in</strong>g is accurate <strong>and</strong> consistent. Essay exam<strong>in</strong>ations,<strong>in</strong> particular, are often very difficult <strong>to</strong> score<strong>in</strong> a consistent manner. For this reason, it is usually advisable<strong>to</strong> have a second person also score the results. <strong>Research</strong>ersshould carefully prepare their scor<strong>in</strong>g plans, <strong>in</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g, ahead of time <strong>and</strong> then try out their <strong>in</strong>strumentby adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g it with a group of <strong>in</strong>dividualssimilar <strong>to</strong> the population they <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> sample <strong>in</strong> theirstudy. Problems with adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>and</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g can thusbe identified early <strong>and</strong> corrected before it is <strong>to</strong>o late.TABULATING AND CODING THE DATAWhen the data have been scored, the researcher must tallyor tabulate them <strong>in</strong> some way. Usually this is done bytransferr<strong>in</strong>g the data <strong>to</strong> some sort of summary data shee<strong>to</strong>r card. The important th<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>to</strong> record one’s data accurately<strong>and</strong> systematically. If categorical data are be<strong>in</strong>grecorded, the number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals scor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> each categoryare tallied. If quantitative data are be<strong>in</strong>g recorded,the data are usually listed <strong>in</strong> one or more columns,depend<strong>in</strong>g on the number of groups <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study.For example, if the data analysis is <strong>to</strong> consist simply of acomparison of the scores of two groups on a posttest, thedata would most likely be placed <strong>in</strong> two columns, one foreach group, <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>g order. Table 7.3, for example,presents some hypothetical results of a study <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g aTABLE 7.3 Hypothetical Results of Study Involv<strong>in</strong>ga Comparison of Two Counsel<strong>in</strong>gMethodsScore for“Rapport” Method A Method B96–100 0 091–95 0 286–90 0 381–85 2 376–80 2 471–75 5 366–70 6 461–65 9 456–60 4 551–55 5 346–50 2 241–45 0 136–40 0 1N 35 35


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 141comparison of two counsel<strong>in</strong>g methods with an <strong>in</strong>strumentmeasur<strong>in</strong>g rapport. If pre- <strong>and</strong> posttest scores are <strong>to</strong>be compared, additional columns could be added. Subgroupscores could also be <strong>in</strong>dicated.When different k<strong>in</strong>ds of data are collected (i.e., scoreson several different <strong>in</strong>struments) <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> biographical<strong>in</strong>formation (gender, age, ethnicity, etc.), they areusually recorded <strong>in</strong> a computer or on data cards, one cardfor each <strong>in</strong>dividual from whom data were collected. Thisfacilitates easy comparison <strong>and</strong> group<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>and</strong> regroup<strong>in</strong>g)of data for purposes of analysis. In addition, the dataare coded. In other words, some type of code is used <strong>to</strong>protect the privacy of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the study. Thus,the names of males <strong>and</strong> females might be coded as 1 <strong>and</strong>2. Cod<strong>in</strong>g of data is especially important when data areanalyzed by computer, s<strong>in</strong>ce any data not <strong>in</strong> numericalform must be coded <strong>in</strong> some systematic way before theycan be entered <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the computer. Thus, categorical data,<strong>to</strong> be analyzed on a computer, are often coded numerically(e.g., pretest scores 1, <strong>and</strong> posttest scores 2).The first step <strong>in</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g data is often <strong>to</strong> assign an IDnumber <strong>to</strong> every <strong>in</strong>dividual from whom data has beencollected. If there were 100 <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> a study, forexample, the researcher would number them from 001<strong>to</strong> 100. If the highest value for any variable be<strong>in</strong>g analyzed<strong>in</strong>volves three digits (e.g., 100), then every <strong>in</strong>dividualcode number must have three digits (e.g., the first<strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>to</strong> be numbered must be 001, not 1).The next step would be <strong>to</strong> decide how any categoricaldata be<strong>in</strong>g analyzed are <strong>to</strong> be coded. Suppose aresearcher wished <strong>to</strong> analyze certa<strong>in</strong> demographic <strong>in</strong>formationobta<strong>in</strong>ed from 100 subjects who answered aquestionnaire. If his study <strong>in</strong>cluded juniors <strong>and</strong> seniors<strong>in</strong> a high school, he might code the juniors as 11 <strong>and</strong>the seniors as 12. Or, if respondents were asked <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatewhich of four choices they preferred (as <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong>multiple-choice questions), the researcher might codeeach of the choices [e.g., (a), (b), (c), (d) as 1, 2, 3, or 4,respectively]. The important th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> remember is thatthe cod<strong>in</strong>g must be consistent—that is, once a decisionis made about how <strong>to</strong> code someone, all others must becoded the same way, <strong>and</strong> this (<strong>and</strong> any other) cod<strong>in</strong>grule must be communicated <strong>to</strong> everyone <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>cod<strong>in</strong>g the data.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.WHAT ARE DATA?The term data refers <strong>to</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formation researchers obta<strong>in</strong> on the subjects oftheir research.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsINSTRUMENTATIONThe term <strong>in</strong>strumentation refers <strong>to</strong> the entire process of collect<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation.VALIDITY AND RELIABILITYAn important consideration <strong>in</strong> the choice of a research <strong>in</strong>strument is validity: theextent <strong>to</strong> which results from it permit researchers <strong>to</strong> draw warranted conclusionsabout the characteristics of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals studied.A reliable <strong>in</strong>strument is one that gives consistent results.


142 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOBJECTIVITY AND USABILITYWhenever possible, researchers try <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate subjectivity from the judgments theymake about the achievement, performance, or characteristics of subjects.An important consideration for any researcher <strong>in</strong> choos<strong>in</strong>g or design<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strumentis its ease of use.WAYS TO CLASSIFY INSTRUMENTS<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong>struments can be classified <strong>in</strong> many ways. Some of the more common are<strong>in</strong> terms of who provides the data, the method of data collection, who collects thedata, <strong>and</strong> what k<strong>in</strong>d of response they require from the subjects.<strong>Research</strong> data are obta<strong>in</strong>ed by directly or <strong>in</strong>directly assess<strong>in</strong>g the subjects of a study.Self-report data are provided by the subjects of a study themselves.Informant data are provided by other people about the subjects of a study.TYPES OF INSTRUMENTSThere are many types of researcher-completed <strong>in</strong>struments. Some of the more commonlyused are rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, <strong>in</strong>terview schedules, observation forms, tally sheets,flowcharts, performance checklists, anecdotal records, <strong>and</strong> time-<strong>and</strong>-motion logs.Many types of <strong>in</strong>struments are completed by the subjects of a study rather than theresearcher. Some of the more commonly used of this type are questionnaires; selfchecklists;attitude scales; personality <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries; achievement, aptitude, <strong>and</strong> performancetests; <strong>and</strong> projective <strong>and</strong> sociometric devices.The types of items or questions used <strong>in</strong> subject-completed <strong>in</strong>struments can take manyforms, but they all can be classified as either selection or supply items. Examples ofselection items <strong>in</strong>clude true-false items, multiple-choice items, match<strong>in</strong>g items, <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpretive exercises. Examples of supply items <strong>in</strong>clude short-answer items <strong>and</strong>essay questions.An excellent source for locat<strong>in</strong>g already available tests is the ERIC database.Unobtrusive measures require no <strong>in</strong>trusion <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the normal course of affairs.TYPES OF SCORESA raw score is the <strong>in</strong>itial score obta<strong>in</strong>ed when us<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strument; a derived score isa raw score that has been translated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a more useful score on some type of st<strong>and</strong>ardizedbasis <strong>to</strong> aid <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation.Age/grade equivalents are derived scores that <strong>in</strong>dicate the typical age or grade associatedwith an <strong>in</strong>dividual raw score.A percentile rank is the percentage of a specific group scor<strong>in</strong>g at or below a givenraw score.A st<strong>and</strong>ard score is a mathematically derived score hav<strong>in</strong>g comparable mean<strong>in</strong>g ondifferent <strong>in</strong>struments.NORM-REFERENCED VERSUS CRITERION-REFERENCED INSTRUMENTSInstruments that provide scores that compare <strong>in</strong>dividual scores <strong>to</strong> the scores of anappropriate reference group are called norm-referenced <strong>in</strong>struments.Instruments that are based on a specific target for each learner <strong>to</strong> achieve are calledcriterion-referenced <strong>in</strong>struments.


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 143MEASUREMENT SCALESFour types of measurement scales—nom<strong>in</strong>al, ord<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>in</strong>terval, <strong>and</strong> ratio—are used <strong>in</strong>educational research.A nom<strong>in</strong>al scale uses numbers <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate membership <strong>in</strong> one or more categories.An ord<strong>in</strong>al scale uses numbers <strong>to</strong> rank or order scores from high <strong>to</strong> low.An <strong>in</strong>terval scale uses numbers <strong>to</strong> represent equal <strong>in</strong>tervals <strong>in</strong> different segments ona cont<strong>in</strong>uum.A ratio scale uses numbers <strong>to</strong> represent equal distances from a known zero po<strong>in</strong>t.PREPARING DATA FOR ANALYSISCollected data must be scored accurately <strong>and</strong> consistently.Once scored, data must be tabulated <strong>and</strong> coded.achievement test 125age-equivalent score 135Likert scale 124nom<strong>in</strong>al scale 138ratio scale 139raw score 134Key Termsaptitude test 127norm group 136reliability 111criterion-referenced<strong>in</strong>strument 136data 110derived scores 135grade-equivalentscore 135<strong>in</strong>formants 112<strong>in</strong>strumentation 110<strong>in</strong>terval scale 138norm-referenced<strong>in</strong>strument 136objectivity 111ord<strong>in</strong>al scale 138percentile rank 135performance<strong>in</strong>strument 116projective device 129questionnaire 112semantic differential 125sociometric device 130st<strong>and</strong>ard score 136tally sheet 112unobtrusive measure 134validity 111written-response<strong>in</strong>strument 1161. What type of <strong>in</strong>strument do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be best suited <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> data about eachof the follow<strong>in</strong>g?a. The free-throw shoot<strong>in</strong>g ability of a tenth-grade basketball teamb. <strong>How</strong> nurses feel about a new management policy recently <strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong> theirhospitalc. Parental reactions <strong>to</strong> a proposed campaign <strong>to</strong> raise money for an addition <strong>to</strong> theschool libraryd. The “best-liked” boy <strong>and</strong> girl <strong>in</strong> the senior classe. The “best” adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a particular school districtf. <strong>How</strong> well students <strong>in</strong> a food management class can prepare a balanced mealg. Characteristics of all students who are biology majors at a midwestern universityh. <strong>How</strong> students at one school compare <strong>to</strong> students at another school <strong>in</strong> mathematicsabilityi. The potential of various high school seniors for college workj. What the members of a k<strong>in</strong>dergarten class like <strong>and</strong> dislike about school2. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be the easiest <strong>to</strong> measure—the attention level of a class,student <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> a poem, or participation <strong>in</strong> a class discussion? Why? Which wouldbe the hardest <strong>to</strong> measure?For Discussion


144 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e3. Is it possible <strong>to</strong> measure a person’s self-concept? If so, how? What about his or herbody image?4. Are there any th<strong>in</strong>gs (ideas, objects, etc.) that cannot be measured? If so, give anexample.5. Of all the <strong>in</strong>struments presented <strong>in</strong> this chapter, which one(s) do you th<strong>in</strong>k would bethe hardest <strong>to</strong> use? the easiest? Why? Which one(s) do you th<strong>in</strong>k would provide themost dependable <strong>in</strong>formation? Why?6. “Any <strong>in</strong>dividual raw score, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, is mean<strong>in</strong>gless.” Would you agree withthis statement? Expla<strong>in</strong>.7. “Derived scores give mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual raw scores.” Is this statement true?Expla<strong>in</strong>.8. It sometimes would not be fair <strong>to</strong> compare an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s score on a test <strong>to</strong> thescores of other <strong>in</strong>dividuals tak<strong>in</strong>g the same test. Why?Notes1. American Council on <strong>Education</strong> (1945). Help<strong>in</strong>g teachers underst<strong>and</strong> children. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: AmericanCouncil on <strong>Education</strong>, pp. 32–33.2. Ibid., p. 33.3. A. Likert (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Archives de Psychologie, 6 (140): 173–177.4. C. Osgood, G. Suci, <strong>and</strong> P. Tannenbaum (1962). The measurement of mean<strong>in</strong>g. Urbana, IL: University ofIll<strong>in</strong>ois Press.5. See, for example, W. J. Popham (1992). <strong>Education</strong>al evaluation, 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall, pp. 150–173.6. E. I. Saw<strong>in</strong> (1969). Evaluation <strong>and</strong> the work of the teacher. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, p. 176.7. M. L. Blum (1943). Selection of sew<strong>in</strong>g mach<strong>in</strong>e opera<strong>to</strong>rs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 27 (2): 35–40.8. N. E. Gronlund (1988). <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> construct achievement tests, 4th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall,pp. 66–67. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of Prentice Hall, Inc. Also see Gronlund’s (1997) Assessment of studentachievement, 6th ed. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon.9. Gronlund (1988), pp. 76–77. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of Prentice Hall, Inc.10. E. J. Webb, D. T. Campbell, R. D. Schwartz, <strong>and</strong> L. Sechrest (1966). Unobtrusive measures: Nonreactiveresearch <strong>in</strong> the social sciences. Chicago: R<strong>and</strong> McNally.11. The use of unobtrusive measures is an art <strong>in</strong> itself. We can only scratch the surface of the <strong>to</strong>pic here. Fora more extended discussion, along with many <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g examples, the reader is referred <strong>to</strong> the book by Webbet al., <strong>in</strong> note 10.


CHAPTER 7 Instrumentation 145<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 7: InstrumentationChoose the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>struments you will use <strong>to</strong> measure the dependent variable(s) <strong>in</strong> yourstudy. Us<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 7, name all the <strong>in</strong>struments you plan <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> your study. If you plan<strong>to</strong> use one or more already exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments, describe each. If you will need <strong>to</strong> develop an<strong>in</strong>strument, give two examples of the k<strong>in</strong>d of questions you would ask (or tasks you would havestudents perform) as a part of each <strong>in</strong>strument. Indicate how you would describe <strong>and</strong> organizethe data on each of the variables yield<strong>in</strong>g numerical data.Problem Sheet 7Instrumentation1. The question or hypothesis <strong>in</strong> my study is: __________________________________2. The types of <strong>in</strong>struments I plan <strong>to</strong> use are: __________________________________3. If I need <strong>to</strong> develop an <strong>in</strong>strument, here are two examples of the k<strong>in</strong>d of questions Iwould ask (or tasks I would have students perform) as a part of my <strong>in</strong>strument:a. ___________________________________________________________________b. ___________________________________________________________________4. These are the exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments I plan <strong>to</strong> use: ______________________________5. The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> my study is: ___________________________________I would describe it as follows (circle the term <strong>in</strong> each set that applies):[quantitative or categorical][nom<strong>in</strong>al or ord<strong>in</strong>al or <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio]6. The dependent variable <strong>in</strong> my study is: _____________________________________I would describe it as follows (circle the term <strong>in</strong> each set that applies):[quantitative or categorical][nom<strong>in</strong>al or ord<strong>in</strong>al or <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio]7. My study does not have <strong>in</strong>dependent/dependent variables. The variable(s) <strong>in</strong> mystudy is (are): _________________________________________________________8. For each variable above that yields numerical data, I will treat it as follows (checkone <strong>in</strong> each column): ___________________________________________________Independent Dependent OtherRaw score __________ __________ __________Age/grade equivalents __________ __________ __________Percentile __________ __________ __________St<strong>and</strong>ard score __________ __________ __________9. I do not have any variables that yield numerical data <strong>in</strong> my study ___________ (✓).An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.


8Validity<strong>and</strong> ReliabilityThe Importanceof Valid InstrumentsValidityContent-Related EvidenceCriterion-Related EvidenceConstruct-Related EvidenceReliabilityErrors of MeasurementTest-Retest MethodEquivalent-Forms MethodInternal-Consistency MethodsThe St<strong>and</strong>ard Error ofMeasurement (SEMeas)Scor<strong>in</strong>g AgreementValidity <strong>and</strong> Reliability <strong>in</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>“I’ve failed Mr. Johnson’stest three times. But he keepsgiv<strong>in</strong>g me problems that he hasnever taught or <strong>to</strong>ldus about.”“Sounds <strong>to</strong> me likean <strong>in</strong>valid test.”“But he saysit’s very reliable.”“Can a testbe reliable butnot valid?”OBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“validity” as it applies <strong>to</strong> the use of<strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> educational research.Name three types of evidence of validitythat can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>and</strong> give an exampleof each type.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“correlation coefficient” <strong>and</strong> describebriefly the difference between positive <strong>and</strong>negative correlation coefficients.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the terms “validitycoefficient” <strong>and</strong> “reliability coefficient.”Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“reliability” as it applies <strong>to</strong> the use of<strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> educational research.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term “errorsof measurement.”Expla<strong>in</strong> briefly the mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> use of theterm “st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement.”Describe briefly three ways <strong>to</strong> estimate thereliability of the scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g aparticular <strong>in</strong>strument.Describe how <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> evaluatescor<strong>in</strong>g agreement.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Validity <strong>and</strong> ReliabilityAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 8.1: Instrument ValidityActivity 8.2: Instrument Reliability (1)Activity 8.3: Instrument Reliability (2)Activity 8.4: What K<strong>in</strong>d of Evidence: Content-Related,Criterion-Related, or Construct-Related?Activity 8.5: What Constitutes Construct-RelatedEvidence of Validity?It isn’t fair, Tony!”“What isn’t, Lily?”“Those tests that Mrs. Leonard gives. Grrr!”“What about them?”“Well, take this last test we had on the Civil War. All dur<strong>in</strong>g her lectures <strong>and</strong> the class discussions over the last fewweeks, we’ve been talk<strong>in</strong>g about the causes <strong>and</strong> effects of the War.”“So?”“Well, then on this test, she asked a lot about battles <strong>and</strong> generals <strong>and</strong> other stuff that we didn’t study.”“Did you ask her how come?”“Yeah, I did. She said she wanted <strong>to</strong> test our th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability. But she was ask<strong>in</strong>g us <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about material shehadn’t even gone over or discussed <strong>in</strong> class. That’s why I th<strong>in</strong>k she isn’t fair.”Lily is correct. Her teacher, <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance, isn’t be<strong>in</strong>g fair. Although she isn’t us<strong>in</strong>g the term, what Lily is talk<strong>in</strong>gabout is a matter of validity. It appears Mrs. Leonard is giv<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>valid test. What this means, <strong>and</strong> why it isn’t agood th<strong>in</strong>g for a teacher (or any researcher) <strong>to</strong> do, is largely what this chapter is about.The Importanceof Valid InstrumentsThe quality of the <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> research is veryimportant, for the conclusions researchers draw arebased on the <strong>in</strong>formation they obta<strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g these <strong>in</strong>struments.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, researchers use a number of procedures<strong>to</strong> ensure that the <strong>in</strong>ferences they draw, based onthe data they collect, are valid <strong>and</strong> reliable.Validity refers <strong>to</strong> the appropriateness, mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness,correctness, <strong>and</strong> usefulness of the <strong>in</strong>ferences aresearcher makes. Reliability refers <strong>to</strong> the consistencyof scores or answers from one adm<strong>in</strong>istration of an<strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> another, <strong>and</strong> from one set of items <strong>to</strong>another. Both concepts are important <strong>to</strong> consider whenit comes <strong>to</strong> the selection or design of the <strong>in</strong>struments aresearcher <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> use. In this chapter, therefore, weshall discuss both validity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong> some detail.ValidityValidity is the most important idea <strong>to</strong> consider whenprepar<strong>in</strong>g or select<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strument for use. More thananyth<strong>in</strong>g else, researchers want the <strong>in</strong>formation theyobta<strong>in</strong> through the use of an <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> serve theirpurposes. For example, <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out what teachers <strong>in</strong> a particularschool district th<strong>in</strong>k about a recent policy passedby the school board, researchers need both an <strong>in</strong>strument147


148 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>to</strong> record the data <strong>and</strong> some sort of assurance that the<strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed will enable them <strong>to</strong> draw correctconclusions about teacher op<strong>in</strong>ions. The draw<strong>in</strong>g of correctconclusions based on the data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from anassessment is what validity is all about. While it is notessential, the comprehension <strong>and</strong> use of <strong>in</strong>formation isgreatly simplified if some k<strong>in</strong>d of score that summarizesthe <strong>in</strong>formation for each person is obta<strong>in</strong>ed. While theideas that follow are not limited <strong>to</strong> the use of scores, wediscuss them <strong>in</strong> this context because the ideas are easier<strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> most <strong>in</strong>struments provide such scores.In recent years, validity has been def<strong>in</strong>ed as referr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> the appropriateness, correctness, mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness, <strong>and</strong>usefulness of the specific <strong>in</strong>ferences researchers makebased on the data they collect. Validation is the process ofcollect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g evidence <strong>to</strong> support such <strong>in</strong>ferences.There are many ways <strong>to</strong> collect evidence, <strong>and</strong> wewill discuss some of them shortly. The important po<strong>in</strong>there is <strong>to</strong> realize that validity refers <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> whichevidence supports any <strong>in</strong>ferences a researcher makesbased on the data he or she collects us<strong>in</strong>g a particular <strong>in</strong>strument.It is the <strong>in</strong>ferences about the specific uses of an<strong>in</strong>strument that are validated, not the <strong>in</strong>strument itself.*These <strong>in</strong>ferences should be appropriate, mean<strong>in</strong>gful,correct, <strong>and</strong> useful.One <strong>in</strong>terpretation of this conceptualization of validityhas been that test publishers no longer have a responsibility<strong>to</strong> provide evidence of validity. We do not agree;publishers have an obligation <strong>to</strong> state what an <strong>in</strong>strumentis <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> measure <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide evidence that itdoes. Nonetheless, researchers must still give attention <strong>to</strong>the way <strong>in</strong> which they <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret the <strong>in</strong>formation.An appropriate <strong>in</strong>ference would be one that isrelevant—that is, related—<strong>to</strong> the purposes of the study.If the purpose of a study were <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e what studentsknow about African culture, for example, it wouldmake no sense <strong>to</strong> make <strong>in</strong>ferences about this from theirscores on a test about the physical geography of Africa.A mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>in</strong>ference is one that says someth<strong>in</strong>gabout the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the <strong>in</strong>formation (such as test scores)obta<strong>in</strong>ed through the use of an <strong>in</strong>strument. What exactlydoes a high score on a particular test mean? What doessuch a score allow us <strong>to</strong> say about the <strong>in</strong>dividual who*This is somewhat of a change from past <strong>in</strong>terpretations. It is based onthe set of st<strong>and</strong>ards prepared by a jo<strong>in</strong>t committee consist<strong>in</strong>g of membersof the American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association, the AmericanPsychological Association, <strong>and</strong> the National Council on Measurement<strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>. See American Psychological Association (1985).St<strong>and</strong>ards for educational <strong>and</strong> psychological test<strong>in</strong>g. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 9–18, 19–23.received it? In what way is an <strong>in</strong>dividual who receives ahigh score different from one who receives a low score?And so forth. It is one th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> collect <strong>in</strong>formation frompeople. We do this all the time—names, addresses, birthdates, shoe sizes, car license numbers, <strong>and</strong> so on. Butunless we can make <strong>in</strong>ferences that mean someth<strong>in</strong>g fromthe <strong>in</strong>formation we obta<strong>in</strong>, it is of little use. The purposeof research is not merely <strong>to</strong> collect data, but <strong>to</strong> use suchdata <strong>to</strong> draw warranted conclusions about the people (<strong>and</strong>others like them) on whom the data were collected.A useful <strong>in</strong>ference is one that helps researchers makea decision related <strong>to</strong> what they were try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out.<strong>Research</strong>ers <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of <strong>in</strong>quiry-relatedteach<strong>in</strong>g materials on student achievement, for example,need <strong>in</strong>formation that will enable them <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fer whetherachievement is affected by such materials <strong>and</strong>, if so, how.Validity, therefore, depends on the amount <strong>and</strong> typeof evidence there is <strong>to</strong> support the <strong>in</strong>terpretationsresearchers wish <strong>to</strong> make concern<strong>in</strong>g data they havecollected. The crucial question is: Do the results of theassessment provide useful <strong>in</strong>formation about the <strong>to</strong>picor variable be<strong>in</strong>g measured?What k<strong>in</strong>ds of evidence might a researcher collect?Essentially, there are three ma<strong>in</strong> types.Content-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> thecontent <strong>and</strong> format of the <strong>in</strong>strument. <strong>How</strong> appropriateis the content? how comprehensive? Doesit logically get at the <strong>in</strong>tended variable? <strong>How</strong>adequately does the sample of items or questionsrepresent the content <strong>to</strong> be assessed? Is the formatappropriate? The content <strong>and</strong> format must beconsistent with the def<strong>in</strong>ition of the variable <strong>and</strong>the sample of subjects <strong>to</strong> be measured.Criterion-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> therelationship between scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g one or moreother <strong>in</strong>struments or measures (often called acriterion). <strong>How</strong> strong is this relationship? <strong>How</strong>well do such scores estimate present or predictfuture performance of a certa<strong>in</strong> type?Construct-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> thenature of the psychological construct or characteristicbe<strong>in</strong>g measured by the <strong>in</strong>strument. <strong>How</strong> welldoes a measure of the construct expla<strong>in</strong> differences<strong>in</strong> the behavior of <strong>in</strong>dividuals or their performanceon certa<strong>in</strong> tasks? We provide furtherexplanation of this rather complex concept later <strong>in</strong>the chapter.Figure 8.1 illustrates these three types of evidence.


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 149Content-related evidenceFigure 8.1 Types ofEvidence of ValidityDef<strong>in</strong>ition Sample Content FormatCriterion-related evidenceJohnnyMariaRaulAmyInstrument A(e.g., test)YesNoNoYesInstrument B(e.g., observation scale)YesNoNoYesConstruct-related evidencePrediction 1 — confirmedTheoryPrediction 2 — confirmedPrediction 3 — confirmedPrediction ? — confirmedCONTENT-RELATED EVIDENCESuppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of a newmath program on the mathematics ability of fifthgraders.The researcher expects that students who completethe program will be able <strong>to</strong> solve a number ofdifferent types of word problems correctly. To assesstheir mathematics ability, the researcher plans <strong>to</strong> givethem a math test conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g about 15 such problems.The performance of the students on this test is importan<strong>to</strong>nly <strong>to</strong> the degree that it provides evidence of their ability<strong>to</strong> solve these k<strong>in</strong>ds of problems. Hence, performanceon the <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> this case (the math test) willprovide valid evidence of the mathematics ability ofthese students if the <strong>in</strong>strument provides an adequatesample of the types of word problems covered <strong>in</strong> theprogram. If only easy problems are <strong>in</strong>cluded on the test,


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHHigh-Stakes Test<strong>in</strong>gHigh-stakes test<strong>in</strong>g refers <strong>to</strong> the use of tests (often only as<strong>in</strong>gle achievement test) as the primary, or only basis fordecisions hav<strong>in</strong>g major consequences. For students, such consequences<strong>in</strong>clude retention <strong>in</strong> grade <strong>and</strong>/or the denial ofdiplomas <strong>and</strong> awards. For schools, they <strong>in</strong>clude public praiseor condemnation, sanctions, <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ancial rewards or punishments.“In state after state, legislatures, governors, <strong>and</strong> stateboards, supported by bus<strong>in</strong>ess leaders, have imposed <strong>to</strong>ugherrequirements <strong>in</strong> mathematics, English, science, <strong>and</strong> otherfields, <strong>to</strong>gether with new tests by which the performance ofboth students <strong>and</strong> schools is <strong>to</strong> be judged.”*For years, tests had been used as one <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>r of performance;what was new was exclusive reliance on them. “Thebacklash, <strong>to</strong>uch<strong>in</strong>g virtually every state that has <strong>in</strong>stitutedhigh-stakes test<strong>in</strong>g, arises from a spectrum of compla<strong>in</strong>ts. Amajor compla<strong>in</strong>t is that the focus on test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> obsessive testpreparation, sometimes beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten, is kill<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>novative teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> curricula <strong>and</strong> driv<strong>in</strong>g out good teachers.Other compla<strong>in</strong>ts are that (conversely) the st<strong>and</strong>ards onwhich the tests are based are <strong>to</strong>o vague <strong>and</strong> that students havenot been taught the material on which the tests are based; orthat the tests are unfair <strong>to</strong> poor <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority students or <strong>to</strong>those who lack test-tak<strong>in</strong>g skills; or that the tests put <strong>to</strong>o muchstress on young children. And some argue that they are <strong>to</strong>o*P. Schrag (2000). High stakes are for <strong>to</strong>ma<strong>to</strong>es. Atlantic Monthly,286 (August): 19.long (<strong>in</strong> Massachusetts they can take up <strong>to</strong> 13 hours!) or <strong>to</strong>o<strong>to</strong>ugh or simply not good enough.”†In response, the American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Associationdeveloped a position statement of “conditions essential <strong>to</strong>sound implementation of high-stakes educational test<strong>in</strong>g programs.”‡It conta<strong>in</strong>ed 14 specific po<strong>in</strong>ts, 4 of the most importantbe<strong>in</strong>g that (1) such decisions about students should not bebased on test scores alone; (2) tests should be made fairer <strong>to</strong> allstudents; (3) tests should match the curriculum; <strong>and</strong> (4) the reliability<strong>and</strong> validity of tests should cont<strong>in</strong>ually be evaluated.Two examples of responses <strong>to</strong> the guidel<strong>in</strong>es were thefollow<strong>in</strong>g:“In the face of <strong>to</strong>o much test<strong>in</strong>g with far <strong>to</strong>o severe consequences,the AERA positions, if implemented, would be astep forward relative <strong>to</strong> current practice.”§“The statement reflects what is desired for all state tests<strong>and</strong> assessments. But, just as all students have not yet metthe st<strong>and</strong>ards, not all state tests <strong>and</strong> assessments will immediatelymeet the goals conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> this statement.” ||What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Are the compla<strong>in</strong>ts about high-stakestests warranted?†Ibid.‡American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association (2000). Position statemen<strong>to</strong>f the American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association concern<strong>in</strong>ghigh-stakes test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> pre-12 education. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er,29 (11): 24–35.§M. Neill (2000). Quoted <strong>in</strong> Initial responses <strong>to</strong> AERA’s positionstatement concern<strong>in</strong>g high-stakes test<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er,29 (11): 28.|| W. Mart<strong>in</strong> (2000). Quoted <strong>in</strong> Ibid., p. 27.or only very difficult or lengthy ones, or only problems<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g subtraction, the test will be unrepresentative<strong>and</strong> hence not provide <strong>in</strong>formation from which valid<strong>in</strong>ferences can be made.One key element <strong>in</strong> content-related evidence ofvalidity, then, concerns the adequacy of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g.Most <strong>in</strong>struments (<strong>and</strong> especially achievement tests)provide only a sample of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of problems thatmight be solved or questions that might be asked. Contentvalidation, therefore, is partly a matter of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gif the content that the <strong>in</strong>strument conta<strong>in</strong>s is anadequate sample of the doma<strong>in</strong> of content it is supposed<strong>to</strong> represent.The other aspect of content validation has <strong>to</strong> do withthe format of the <strong>in</strong>strument. This <strong>in</strong>cludes such th<strong>in</strong>gs asthe clarity of pr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g, size of type, adequacy of workspace (if needed), appropriateness of language, clarity ofdirections, <strong>and</strong> so on. Regardless of the adequacy of thequestions <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>strument, if they are presented <strong>in</strong> an<strong>in</strong>appropriate format (such as giv<strong>in</strong>g a test written <strong>in</strong> English<strong>to</strong> children whose English is m<strong>in</strong>imal), valid resultscannot be obta<strong>in</strong>ed. For this reason, it is important that thecharacteristics of the <strong>in</strong>tended sample be kept <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.<strong>How</strong> does one obta<strong>in</strong> content-related evidence of validity?A common way <strong>to</strong> do this is <strong>to</strong> have someonelook at the content <strong>and</strong> format of the <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong>judge whether or not it is appropriate. The “someone,”of course, should not be just anyone, but rather an <strong>in</strong>dividualwho can be expected <strong>to</strong> render an <strong>in</strong>telligentjudgment about the adequacy of the <strong>in</strong>strument—<strong>in</strong>other words, someone who knows enough about what is<strong>to</strong> be measured <strong>to</strong> be a competent judge.150


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 151The usual procedure is somewhat as follows. Theresearcher writes out the def<strong>in</strong>ition of what he or shewants <strong>to</strong> measure <strong>and</strong> then gives this def<strong>in</strong>ition, alongwith the <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> a description of the <strong>in</strong>tendedsample, <strong>to</strong> one or more judges. The judges look at thedef<strong>in</strong>ition, read over the items or questions <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>strument,<strong>and</strong> place a check mark <strong>in</strong> front of eachquestion or item that they feel does not measure oneor more aspects of the def<strong>in</strong>ition (objectives, for example)or other criteria. They also place a check mark<strong>in</strong> front of each aspect not assessed by any of theitems. In addition, the judges evaluate the appropriatenessof the <strong>in</strong>strument format. The researcher thenrewrites any item or question so checked <strong>and</strong> resubmitsit <strong>to</strong> the judges, <strong>and</strong>/or writes new items forcriteria not adequately covered. This cont<strong>in</strong>ues untilthe judges approve all the items or questions <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> also <strong>in</strong>dicate that they feel the <strong>to</strong>talnumber of items is an adequate representation of the<strong>to</strong>tal doma<strong>in</strong> of content covered by the variable be<strong>in</strong>gmeasured.To illustrate how a researcher might go about try<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> establish content-related validity, let us consider twoexamples.Example 1. Suppose a researcher desires <strong>to</strong> measurestudents’ ability <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong>formation that they have previouslyacquired. When asked what she means by thisphrase, she offers the follow<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>ition.As evidence that students can use previously acquired<strong>in</strong>formation, they should be able <strong>to</strong>:1. Draw a correct conclusion (verbally or <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g) thatis based on <strong>in</strong>formation they are given.2. Identify one or more logical implications that followfrom a given po<strong>in</strong>t of view.3. State (orally or <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g) whether two ideas are identical,similar, unrelated, or contradic<strong>to</strong>ry.<strong>How</strong> might the researcher obta<strong>in</strong> such evidence? Shedecides <strong>to</strong> prepare a written test that will conta<strong>in</strong> variousquestions. Students’ answers will constitute the evidenceshe seeks. Here are three examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds ofquestions she has <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, designed <strong>to</strong> produce each ofthe three types of evidence listed above.1. If A is greater than B, <strong>and</strong> B is greater than C, then:a. A must be greater than C.b. C must be smaller than A.c. B must be smaller than A.d. All of the above are true.2. Those who believe that <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g consumer expenditureswould be the best way <strong>to</strong> stimulate the economywould advocatea. an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest rates.b. an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> depletion allowances.c. tax reductions <strong>in</strong> the lower <strong>in</strong>come brackets.d. a reduction <strong>in</strong> government expenditures.3. Compare the dollar amounts spent by the U.S.government dur<strong>in</strong>g the past 10 years for (a) debtpayments, (b) defense, <strong>and</strong> (c) social services.Now, look at each of the questions <strong>and</strong> the correspond<strong>in</strong>gobjective they are supposed <strong>to</strong> measure. Doyou th<strong>in</strong>k each question measures the objective it wasdesigned for? If not, why not?*Example 2. Here is what another researcher designedas an attempt <strong>to</strong> measure (at least <strong>in</strong> part) the ability ofstudents <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why events occur.Read the directions that follow, <strong>and</strong> then answer thequestion.Directions: Here are some facts.Fact W: A camper started a fire <strong>to</strong> cook food on a w<strong>in</strong>dyday <strong>in</strong> a forest.Fact X: A fire started <strong>in</strong> some dry grass near a campfire<strong>in</strong> a forest.Here is another fact that happened later the same day <strong>in</strong>the same forest.Fact Y: A house <strong>in</strong> the forest burned down.You are <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> what might have caused the house <strong>to</strong>burn down (Fact Y). Would Fact W <strong>and</strong> X be useful asparts of your explanation?a. Yes, both W <strong>and</strong> X <strong>and</strong> the possible cause-<strong>and</strong>-effectrelationship between them would be useful.b. Yes, both W <strong>and</strong> X would be useful, even thoughneither was likely a cause of the other.c. No, because only one of Facts W <strong>and</strong> X was likely acause of Y.d. No, because neither W or X was likely a cause of Y. 1*We would rate correct answers <strong>to</strong> questions 1 (choice d) <strong>and</strong> 2(choice c) as valid evidence, although 1 could be considered questionable,s<strong>in</strong>ce students might view it as somewhat tricky. We wouldnot rate the answers <strong>to</strong> 3 as valid, s<strong>in</strong>ce students are not asked <strong>to</strong>contrast ideas, only facts.


152 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOnce aga<strong>in</strong>, look at the question <strong>and</strong> the objective itwas designed <strong>to</strong> measure. Does it measure this objective?If not, why not?*Attempts like these <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> evidence of some sort(<strong>in</strong> the above <strong>in</strong>stances, the support of <strong>in</strong>dependentjudges that the items measure what they are supposed <strong>to</strong>measure) typify the process of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g content-relatedevidence of validity. As we mentioned previously, however,the qualifications of the judges are always animportant consideration, <strong>and</strong> the judges must keep <strong>in</strong>m<strong>in</strong>d the characteristics of the <strong>in</strong>tended sample.CRITERION-RELATED EVIDENCETo obta<strong>in</strong> criterion-related evidence of validity, researchersusually compare performance on one <strong>in</strong>strument(the one be<strong>in</strong>g validated) with performance onsome other, <strong>in</strong>dependent criterion. A criterion is a secondtest or other assessment procedure presumed <strong>to</strong> measurethe same variable. For example, if an <strong>in</strong>strumenthas been designed <strong>to</strong> measure academic ability, studentscores on the <strong>in</strong>strument might be compared with theirgrade-po<strong>in</strong>t averages (the external criterion). If the <strong>in</strong>strumentdoes <strong>in</strong>deed measure academic ability, then studentswho score high on the test would also be expected<strong>to</strong> have high grade-po<strong>in</strong>t averages. Can you see why?There are two forms of criterion-related validity—predictive <strong>and</strong> concurrent. To obta<strong>in</strong> evidence of predictivevalidity, researchers allow a time <strong>in</strong>terval <strong>to</strong> elapsebetween adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe criterion scores. For example, a researcher might adm<strong>in</strong>istera science aptitude test <strong>to</strong> a group of high schoolstudents <strong>and</strong> later compare their scores on the test withtheir end-of-semester grades <strong>in</strong> science courses.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, when <strong>in</strong>strument data <strong>and</strong> criteriondata are gathered at nearly the same time, <strong>and</strong> theresults are compared, this is an attempt by researchers <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> evidence of concurrent validity. An example iswhen a researcher adm<strong>in</strong>isters a self-esteem <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>to</strong> a group of eighth-graders <strong>and</strong> compares their scoreson it with their teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of student self-esteemobta<strong>in</strong>ed at about the same time.A key <strong>in</strong>dex <strong>in</strong> both forms of criterion-related validityis the correlation coefficient.† A correlation coefficient,symbolized by the letter r, <strong>in</strong>dicates the degree ofrelationship that exists between the scores <strong>in</strong>dividuals*We would rate a correct answer <strong>to</strong> this question as valid evidenceof student ability <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why events occur.†The correlation coefficient, expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10, is anextremely useful statistic. This is one of its many applications or uses.© The New Yorker Collection 2000 Sidney Harris from car<strong>to</strong>onbank.com.All Rights Reserved.obta<strong>in</strong> on two <strong>in</strong>struments. A positive relationship is <strong>in</strong>dicatedwhen a high score on one of the <strong>in</strong>struments isaccompanied by a high score on the other or when a lowscore on one is accompanied by a low score on theother. A negative relationship is <strong>in</strong>dicated when a highscore on one <strong>in</strong>strument is accompanied by a low scoreon the other, <strong>and</strong> vice versa. All correlation coefficientsfall somewhere between 1.00 <strong>and</strong> 1.00. An r of .00<strong>in</strong>dicates that no relationship exists.When a correlation coefficient is used <strong>to</strong> describe therelationship between a set of scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by thesame group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals on a particular <strong>in</strong>strument<strong>and</strong> their scores on some criterion measure, it is called avalidity coefficient. For example, a validity coefficien<strong>to</strong>f 1.00 obta<strong>in</strong>ed by correlat<strong>in</strong>g a set of scores on amathematics aptitude test (the predic<strong>to</strong>r) <strong>and</strong> another se<strong>to</strong>f scores, this time on a mathematics achievement test(the criterion), for the same <strong>in</strong>dividuals would <strong>in</strong>dicatethat each <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> the group had exactly the samerelative st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g on both measures. Such a correlation,if obta<strong>in</strong>ed, would allow the researcher <strong>to</strong> predict perfectlymath achievement based on aptitude test scores.Although this correlation coefficient would be very unlikely,it illustrates what such coefficients mean. Thehigher the validity coefficient obta<strong>in</strong>ed, the more accuratea researcher’s predictions are likely <strong>to</strong> be.


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 153Gronlund suggests the use of an expectancy table asanother way <strong>to</strong> depict criterion-related evidence. 2 Anexpectancy table is noth<strong>in</strong>g more than a two-way chart,with the predic<strong>to</strong>r categories listed down the left-h<strong>and</strong>side of the chart <strong>and</strong> the criterion categories listed horizontallyalong the <strong>to</strong>p of the chart. For each category ofscores on the predic<strong>to</strong>r, the researcher then <strong>in</strong>dicates thepercentage of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who fall with<strong>in</strong> each of thecategories on the criterion.Table 8.1 presents an example. As you can see fromthe table, 51 percent of the students who were classifiedoutst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g by these judges received a grade of A <strong>in</strong>orchestra, 35 percent received a B, <strong>and</strong> 14 percent receiveda C. Although this table refers only <strong>to</strong> this particulargroup, it could be used <strong>to</strong> predict the scores of otheraspir<strong>in</strong>g music students who were evaluated by thesesame judges. If a student obta<strong>in</strong>ed an evaluation of “outst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g,”we might predict (approximately) that he orshe would have a 51 percent chance of receiv<strong>in</strong>g an A,a 35 percent chance of receiv<strong>in</strong>g a B, <strong>and</strong> a 14 percentchance of receiv<strong>in</strong>g a C.Expectancy tables are particularly useful devices forresearchers <strong>to</strong> use with data collected <strong>in</strong> schools. Theyare simple <strong>to</strong> construct, easily unders<strong>to</strong>od, <strong>and</strong> clearlyshow the relationship between two measures.It is important <strong>to</strong> realize that the nature of the criterionis the most important fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> gather<strong>in</strong>g criterion-relatedevidence. High positive correlations do not mean muchif the criterion measure does not make logical sense. Forexample, a high correlation between scores on an <strong>in</strong>strumentdesigned <strong>to</strong> measure aptitude for science <strong>and</strong>scores on a physical fitness test would not be relevantcriterion-related evidence for either <strong>in</strong>strument. Th<strong>in</strong>kback <strong>to</strong> the example we presented earlier of the questionsdesigned <strong>to</strong> measure student ability <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> whyevents occur. What sort of criteria could be used <strong>to</strong> establishcriterion-referenced validity for those items?TABLE 8.1Example of an Expectancy TableCourse Grades<strong>in</strong> Orchestra(Percentage Receiv<strong>in</strong>gJudges’ ClassificationEach Grade)of Music Aptitude A B C DOutst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g 51 35 14 0Above average 20 43 37 0Average 0 6 83 11Below average 0 0 13 87CONSTRUCT-RELATED EVIDENCEConstruct-related evidence of validity is the broades<strong>to</strong>f the three categories of evidence for validity that weare consider<strong>in</strong>g. There is no s<strong>in</strong>gle piece of evidencethat satisfies construct-related validity. Rather, researchersattempt <strong>to</strong> collect a variety of different typesof evidence (the more <strong>and</strong> the more varied the better)that will allow them <strong>to</strong> make warranted <strong>in</strong>ferences—<strong>to</strong>assert, for example, that the scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed from adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>ga self-esteem <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry permit accurate <strong>in</strong>ferencesabout the degree of self-esteem that peoplewho receive those scores possess.Usually, there are three steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gconstruct-related evidence of validity: (1) the variablebe<strong>in</strong>g measured is clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed; (2) hypotheses,based on a theory underly<strong>in</strong>g the variable, are formedabout how people who possess a lot versus a little of thevariable will behave <strong>in</strong> a particular situation; <strong>and</strong> (3) thehypotheses are tested both logically <strong>and</strong> empirically.To make the process clearer, let us consider an example.Suppose a researcher <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g apencil-<strong>and</strong>-paper test <strong>to</strong> measure honesty wants <strong>to</strong> use aconstruct-validity approach. First, he def<strong>in</strong>es honesty.Next he formulates a theory about how “honest” peoplebehave as compared <strong>to</strong> “dishonest” people. For example,he might theorize that honest <strong>in</strong>dividuals, if theyf<strong>in</strong>d an object that does not belong <strong>to</strong> them, will make areasonable effort <strong>to</strong> locate the <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>to</strong> whom theobject belongs. Based on this theory, the researchermight hypothesize that <strong>in</strong>dividuals who score high onhis honesty test will be more likely <strong>to</strong> attempt <strong>to</strong> locatethe owner of an object they f<strong>in</strong>d than <strong>in</strong>dividuals whoscore low on the test. The researcher then adm<strong>in</strong>istersthe honesty test, separates the names of those who scorehigh <strong>and</strong> those who score low, <strong>and</strong> gives all of them anopportunity <strong>to</strong> be honest. He might, for example, leavea wallet with $5 <strong>in</strong> it ly<strong>in</strong>g just outside the test-tak<strong>in</strong>groom so that the <strong>in</strong>dividuals tak<strong>in</strong>g the test can easilysee it <strong>and</strong> pick it up. The wallet displays the name <strong>and</strong>phone number of the owner <strong>in</strong> pla<strong>in</strong> view. If the researcher’shypothesis is substantiated, more of the highscorers than the low scorers on the honesty test will attempt<strong>to</strong> call the owner of the wallet. (This could bechecked by hav<strong>in</strong>g the number answered by a record<strong>in</strong>gmach<strong>in</strong>e ask<strong>in</strong>g the caller <strong>to</strong> leave his or her name <strong>and</strong>number.) This is one piece of evidence that could beused <strong>to</strong> support <strong>in</strong>ferences about the honesty of <strong>in</strong>dividuals,based on the scores they receive on this test.We must stress, however, that a researcher must carryout a series of studies <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a variety of evidence


154 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7esuggest<strong>in</strong>g that the scores from a particular <strong>in</strong>strumentcan be used <strong>to</strong> draw correct <strong>in</strong>ferences about the variablethat the <strong>in</strong>strument purports <strong>to</strong> measure. It is abroad array of evidence, rather than any one particulartype of evidence, that is desired.Consider a second example. Some evidence thatmight be considered <strong>to</strong> support a claim for constructvalidity <strong>in</strong> connection with a test designed <strong>to</strong> measuremathematical reason<strong>in</strong>g ability might be as follows:Independent judges all <strong>in</strong>dicate that all items on thetest require mathematical reason<strong>in</strong>g.Independent judges all <strong>in</strong>dicate that the featuresof the test itself (such as test format, directions,scor<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g level) would not <strong>in</strong> any wayprevent students from engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mathematicalreason<strong>in</strong>g.Independent judges all <strong>in</strong>dicate that the sample oftasks <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the test is relevant <strong>and</strong> representativeof mathematical reason<strong>in</strong>g tasks.A high correlation exists between scores on the test<strong>and</strong> grades <strong>in</strong> mathematics.High scores have been made on the test by studentswho have had specific tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mathematicalreason<strong>in</strong>g.Students actually engage <strong>in</strong> mathematical reason<strong>in</strong>gwhen they are asked <strong>to</strong> “th<strong>in</strong>k aloud” as they goabout try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> solve the problems on the test.A high correlation exists between scores on the test<strong>and</strong> teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of competence <strong>in</strong> mathematicalreason<strong>in</strong>g.Higher scores are obta<strong>in</strong>ed on the test by mathematicsmajors than by general science majors.Other types of evidence might be listed for the abovetask (perhaps you can th<strong>in</strong>k of some), but we hope thisis enough <strong>to</strong> make clear that it is not just one type, butmany types, of evidence that a researcher seeks <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>.Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether the scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed throughthe use of a particular <strong>in</strong>strument measure a particularvariable <strong>in</strong>volves a study of how the test was developed,the theory underly<strong>in</strong>g the test, how the test functionswith a variety of people <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> a variety of situations,<strong>and</strong> how scores on the test relate <strong>to</strong> scores on otherappropriate <strong>in</strong>struments. Construct validation <strong>in</strong>volves,then, a wide variety of procedures <strong>and</strong> many differenttypes of evidence, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g both content-related <strong>and</strong>criterion-related evidence. The more evidence researchershave from many different sources, the moreconfident they become about <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the scoresobta<strong>in</strong>ed from a particular <strong>in</strong>strument.ReliabilityReliability refers <strong>to</strong> the consistency of the scoresobta<strong>in</strong>ed—how consistent they are for each <strong>in</strong>dividualfrom one adm<strong>in</strong>istration of an <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> another <strong>and</strong>from one set of items <strong>to</strong> another. Consider, for example,a test designed <strong>to</strong> measure typ<strong>in</strong>g ability. If the test isreliable, we would expect a student who receives a highscore the first time he takes the test <strong>to</strong> receive a highscore the next time he takes the test. The scores wouldprobably not be identical, but they should be close.The scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed from an <strong>in</strong>strument can be quitereliable but not valid. Suppose a researcher gave agroup of eighth-graders two forms of a test designed <strong>to</strong>measure their knowledge of the Constitution of theUnited States <strong>and</strong> found their scores <strong>to</strong> be consistent:those who scored high on form A also scored high onform B; those who scored low on A scored low on B;<strong>and</strong> so on. We would say that the scores were reliable.But if the researcher then used these same test scores <strong>to</strong>predict the success of these students <strong>in</strong> their physical educationclasses, she would probably be looked at <strong>in</strong>amazement. Any <strong>in</strong>ferences about success <strong>in</strong> physicaleducation based on scores on a Constitution test wouldhave no validity. Now, what about the reverse? Can an<strong>in</strong>strument that yields unreliable scores permit valid <strong>in</strong>ferences?No! If scores are completely <strong>in</strong>consistent for aperson, they provide no useful <strong>in</strong>formation. We have noway of know<strong>in</strong>g which score <strong>to</strong> use <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fer an <strong>in</strong>dividual’sability, attitude, or other characteristic.The dist<strong>in</strong>ction between reliability <strong>and</strong> validity isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 8.2. Reliability <strong>and</strong> validity alwaysdepend on the context <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>strument is used.Depend<strong>in</strong>g on the context, an <strong>in</strong>strument may or maynot yield reliable (consistent) scores. If the data areunreliable, they cannot lead <strong>to</strong> valid (legitimate)<strong>in</strong>ferences—as shown <strong>in</strong> target (a). As reliability improves,validity may improve, as shown <strong>in</strong> target (b), orit may not, as shown <strong>in</strong> target (c). An <strong>in</strong>strument mayhave good reliability but low validity, as shown <strong>in</strong> target(d). What is desired, of course, is both high reliability<strong>and</strong> high validity, as target (e) shows.ERRORS OF MEASUREMENTWhenever people take the same test twice, they will seldomperform exactly the same—that is, their scores oranswers will not usually be identical. This may be due<strong>to</strong> a variety of fac<strong>to</strong>rs (differences <strong>in</strong> motivation, energy,


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 155Figure 8.2 Reliability<strong>and</strong> Validity(a)(b)(c)(d)(e)So unreliableas <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>validFair reliability<strong>and</strong>fair validityFairreliabilitybut <strong>in</strong>validGoodreliabilitybut <strong>in</strong>validGood reliability<strong>and</strong>good validityThe bull’s-eye <strong>in</strong> each target represents the <strong>in</strong>formation that is desired. Each dot represents aseparate score obta<strong>in</strong>ed with the <strong>in</strong>strument. A dot <strong>in</strong> the bull’s-eye <strong>in</strong>dicates that the <strong>in</strong>formationobta<strong>in</strong>ed (the score) is the <strong>in</strong>formation the researcher desires.anxiety, a different test<strong>in</strong>g situation, <strong>and</strong> so on), <strong>and</strong> it is<strong>in</strong>evitable. Such fac<strong>to</strong>rs result <strong>in</strong> errors of measurement(Figure 8.3).Because errors of measurement are always present <strong>to</strong>some degree, researchers expect some variation <strong>in</strong> testscores (<strong>in</strong> answers or rat<strong>in</strong>gs, for example) when an <strong>in</strong>strumentis adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> the same group more thanonce, when two different forms of an <strong>in</strong>strument areused, or even from one part of an <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> another.Reliability estimates provide researchers with an idea ofhow much variation <strong>to</strong> expect. Such estimates are usuallyexpressed as another application of the correlationcoefficient known as a reliability coefficient.As we mentioned earlier, a validity coefficient expressesthe relationship between scores of the same“I can’t believe thatmy blood pressureis 170 over 110!”“Maybe it’s due <strong>to</strong>poor reliability of themeasurement. Let’swait a few m<strong>in</strong>utes,<strong>and</strong> check it aga<strong>in</strong>.”Figure 8.3 Reliability of a Measurement<strong>in</strong>dividuals on two different <strong>in</strong>struments. A reliabilitycoefficient also expresses a relationship, but this time itis between scores of the same <strong>in</strong>dividuals on the same<strong>in</strong>strument at two different times, or on two parts of thesame <strong>in</strong>strument. The three best-known ways <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> areliability coefficient are the test-retest method, theequivalent-forms method; <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal-consistencymethods. Unlike other uses of the correlation coefficient,reliability coefficients must range from .00 <strong>to</strong>1.00—that is, have no negative values.TEST-RETEST METHODThe test-retest method <strong>in</strong>volves adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g thesame test twice <strong>to</strong> the same group after a certa<strong>in</strong> time<strong>in</strong>terval has elapsed. A reliability coefficient is thencalculated <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the relationship between the twosets of scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed.Reliability coefficients will be affected by the length oftime that elapses between the two adm<strong>in</strong>istrations of thetest. The longer the time <strong>in</strong>terval, the lower the reliabilitycoefficient is likely <strong>to</strong> be, s<strong>in</strong>ce there is a greater likelihoodof changes <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividuals tak<strong>in</strong>g the test. In check<strong>in</strong>gfor evidence of test-retest reliability, an appropriate time<strong>in</strong>terval should be selected. This <strong>in</strong>terval should be thatdur<strong>in</strong>g which <strong>in</strong>dividuals would be assumed <strong>to</strong> reta<strong>in</strong> theirrelative position <strong>in</strong> a mean<strong>in</strong>gful group.There is no po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g, or even conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g,a variable that fluctuates wildly <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals forwhom it is measured. When researchers assess someoneas academically talented, for example, or skilled <strong>in</strong> typ<strong>in</strong>gor as hav<strong>in</strong>g a poor self-concept, they assume thatthis characteristic will cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> differentiate <strong>in</strong>dividualsfor some period of time. It is impossible <strong>to</strong> study avariable that has no stability <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual.


156 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong>ers do not expect all variables <strong>to</strong> be equallystable. Experience has shown that some abilities (suchas writ<strong>in</strong>g) are more subject <strong>to</strong> change than others (suchas abstract reason<strong>in</strong>g). Some personal characteristics(such as self-esteem) are considered <strong>to</strong> be more stablethan others (such as teenage vocational <strong>in</strong>terests). Moodis a variable that, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, is considered <strong>to</strong> be stablefor short periods of time—a matter of m<strong>in</strong>utes orhours. But even here, unless the <strong>in</strong>strumentation used isreliable, mean<strong>in</strong>gful relationships with other (perhapscausal) variables will not be found. For most educationalresearch, stability of scores over a two- <strong>to</strong> threemonthperiod is usually viewed as sufficient evidence oftest-retest reliability. In report<strong>in</strong>g test-retest reliabilitycoefficients, therefore, the time <strong>in</strong>terval between thetwo test<strong>in</strong>gs should always be reported.EQUIVALENT-FORMS METHODWhen the equivalent-forms method is used, two differentbut equivalent (also called alternate or parallel)forms of an <strong>in</strong>strument are adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> the samegroup of <strong>in</strong>dividuals dur<strong>in</strong>g the same time period. Althoughthe questions are different, they should samplethe same content <strong>and</strong> they should be constructed separatelyfrom each other. A reliability coefficient is thencalculated between the two sets of scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Ahigh coefficient would <strong>in</strong>dicate strong evidence ofreliability—that the two forms are measur<strong>in</strong>g the sameth<strong>in</strong>g.It is possible <strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e the test-retest <strong>and</strong> equivalentformsmethods by giv<strong>in</strong>g two different forms of thesame test with a time <strong>in</strong>terval between the two adm<strong>in</strong>istrations.A high reliability coefficient would <strong>in</strong>dicate no<strong>to</strong>nly that the two forms are measur<strong>in</strong>g the same sort ofperformance but also what we might expect with regard<strong>to</strong> consistency over time.INTERNAL-CONSISTENCY METHODSThe methods mentioned so far all require two adm<strong>in</strong>istrationor test<strong>in</strong>g sessions. There are several <strong>in</strong>ternalconsistencymethods of estimat<strong>in</strong>g reliability, however,that require only a s<strong>in</strong>gle adm<strong>in</strong>istration of an <strong>in</strong>strument.Split-half Procedure. The split-half procedure <strong>in</strong>volvesscor<strong>in</strong>g two halves (usually odd items versuseven items) of a test separately for each person <strong>and</strong> thencalculat<strong>in</strong>g a correlation coefficient for the two sets ofscores. The coefficient <strong>in</strong>dicates the degree <strong>to</strong> which thetwo halves of the test provide the same results <strong>and</strong>hence describes the <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of the test.The reliability coefficient is calculated us<strong>in</strong>g what isknown as the Spearman-Brown prophecy formula. Asimplified version of this formula is as follows:Reliability ofscores on <strong>to</strong>tal test 2 * reliability for 1 2 test1 reliability for 1 2 testThus, if we obta<strong>in</strong>ed a correlation coefficient of .56by compar<strong>in</strong>g one half of the test items <strong>to</strong> the other half,the reliability of scores for the <strong>to</strong>tal test would be:Reliability ofscores on <strong>to</strong>tal test 2 * .561 .56 1.121.56 .72This illustrates an important characteristic of reliability.The reliability of a test (or any <strong>in</strong>strument) cangenerally be <strong>in</strong>creased by the addition of more items,provided they are similar <strong>to</strong> the orig<strong>in</strong>al ones.Kuder-Richardson Approaches. Perhaps themost frequently employed method for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternalconsistency is the Kuder-Richardson approach,particularly formulas KR20 <strong>and</strong> KR21. The latter formularequires only three pieces of <strong>in</strong>formation—thenumber of items on the test, the mean, <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation. Note, however, that formula KR21 can beused only if it can be assumed that the items are of equaldifficulty.* A frequently used version of the KR21 formulais the follow<strong>in</strong>g:KR21 reliabilitycoefficientKK 1M(K Mc1 K(SD 2 d)where K number of items on the test, M mean ofthe set of test scores, <strong>and</strong> SD st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of theset of test scores.†Although this formula may look somewhat <strong>in</strong>timidat<strong>in</strong>g,its use is actually quite simple. For example, if*Formula KR20 does not require the assumption that all items areof equal difficulty, although it is harder <strong>to</strong> calculate. Computerprograms for do<strong>in</strong>g so are commonly available, however, <strong>and</strong> shouldbe used whenever a researcher cannot assume that all items are ofequal difficulty.†See Chapter 10 for an explanation of st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHCheck<strong>in</strong>g Reliability<strong>and</strong> Validity—An ExampleThe projective device (Picture Situation Inven<strong>to</strong>ry) describedon pages 129–130 consists of 20 pictures, eachscored on the variables control need <strong>and</strong> communication accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> a po<strong>in</strong>t system. For example, here are some illustrativeresponses <strong>to</strong> picture 1 of Figure 7.22. The control needvariable, def<strong>in</strong>ed as “motivated <strong>to</strong> control moment-<strong>to</strong>-momentactivities of their students,” is scored as follows:“I thought you would enjoy someth<strong>in</strong>g special.” (1 po<strong>in</strong>t)“I’d like <strong>to</strong> see how well you can do it.” (2 po<strong>in</strong>ts)“You <strong>and</strong> Tom are two different children.” (3 po<strong>in</strong>ts)“Yes, I would appreciate it if you would f<strong>in</strong>ish it.” (4 po<strong>in</strong>ts)“Do it quickly please.” (5 po<strong>in</strong>ts)In addition <strong>to</strong> the appeal <strong>to</strong> content validity, there is someevidence <strong>in</strong> support of these two measures (control <strong>and</strong>communication).Rowan studied relationships between the two scores <strong>and</strong> severalother measures with a group of elementary school teachers.**N. T. Rowan (1967). The relationship of teacher <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong>classroom situations <strong>to</strong> teacher personality variables. Unpublisheddoc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertation. Salt Lake City: University of Utah.She found that teachers scor<strong>in</strong>g high on control need were morelikely <strong>to</strong> (1) be seen by classroom observers as impos<strong>in</strong>g themselveson situations <strong>and</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g a higher content emphasis, (2) bejudged by <strong>in</strong>terviewers as hav<strong>in</strong>g more rigid attitudes of right<strong>and</strong> wrong, <strong>and</strong> (3) score higher on a test of authoritariantendencies.In a study of ability <strong>to</strong> predict success <strong>in</strong> a program prepar<strong>in</strong>gteachers for <strong>in</strong>ner-city classrooms, evidence was foundthat the Picture Situation Inven<strong>to</strong>ry control score had predictivevalue.†Correlations existed between the control score obta<strong>in</strong>ed onentrance <strong>to</strong> the program <strong>and</strong> a variety of measures subsequentlyobta<strong>in</strong>ed through classroom observation <strong>in</strong> student teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> subsequent first-year teach<strong>in</strong>g assignments. The mostclear-cut f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g was that those scor<strong>in</strong>g higher <strong>in</strong> control needhad classrooms observed as less noisy. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g adds somewhat<strong>to</strong> the validity of the measurement, s<strong>in</strong>ce a teacher withhigher control need would be expected <strong>to</strong> have a quieter room.The reliability of both measures was found <strong>to</strong> be adequate(.74 <strong>and</strong> .81) when assessed by the split-half procedure. Whenassessed by follow-up over a period of eight years, the consistencyover time was considerably lower (.61 <strong>and</strong> .53), aswould be expected.†N. E. Wallen (1971). Evaluation report <strong>to</strong> Step-TTT Project. SanFrancisco, CA: San Francisco State University.K 50, M 40, <strong>and</strong> SD 4, the reliability coefficientwould be calculated as shown below:Reliability 50 40(50 40)c1 49 50(4 2 d) 1.02c1 40(10)50(16) d 1.02c1 400800 d (1.02)(1 .50) (1.02)(.50) .51Thus, the reliability estimate for scores on this testis .51.Is a reliability estimate of .51 good or bad? high orlow? As is frequently the case, there are some benchmarkswe can use <strong>to</strong> evaluate reliability coefficients.First, we can compare a given coefficient with the extremesthat are possible. As you will recall, a coefficien<strong>to</strong>f .00 <strong>in</strong>dicates a complete absence of a relationship,hence no reliability at all, whereas 1.00 is the maximumpossible coefficient that can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Second, wecan compare a given reliability coefficient with the sortsof coefficients that are usually obta<strong>in</strong>ed for measuresof the same type. The reported reliability coefficientsfor many commercially available achievement tests, forexample, are typically .90 or higher when Kuder-Richardson formulas are used. Many classroom tests reportreliability coefficients of .70 <strong>and</strong> higher. Compared<strong>to</strong> these figures, our obta<strong>in</strong>ed coefficient must be judgedrather low. For research purposes, a useful rule of thumbis that reliability should be at least .70 <strong>and</strong> preferablyhigher.Alpha Coefficient. Another check on the <strong>in</strong>ternalconsistency of an <strong>in</strong>strument is <strong>to</strong> calculate an alpha157


158 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 8.2Methods of Check<strong>in</strong>g Validity <strong>and</strong> ReliabilityMethodContent-related evidenceCriterion-related evidenceConstruct-related evidenceValidity (“Truthfulness”)ProcedureObta<strong>in</strong> expert judgmentRelate <strong>to</strong> another measure of the same variableAssess evidence on predictions made from theoryReliability (“Consistency”)Method Content Time Interval ProcedureTest-retest Identical Varies Give identical <strong>in</strong>strument twiceEquivalent forms Different None Give two forms of <strong>in</strong>strumentEquivalent forms Different Varies Give two forms of <strong>in</strong>strument, with/retesttime <strong>in</strong>terval betweenInternal consistency Different None Divide <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> halves <strong>and</strong> scoreeach or use Kuder-Richardson approachScor<strong>in</strong>g observer Identical None Compare scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by two oragreementmore observers or scorerscoefficient (frequently called Cronbach alpha after theman who developed it). This coefficient (α) is a generalform of the KR20 formula <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>gthe reliability of items that are not scored right versuswrong, as <strong>in</strong> some essay tests where more than oneanswer is possible. 3Table 8.2 summarizes the methods used <strong>in</strong> check<strong>in</strong>gthe validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of an <strong>in</strong>strument.THE STANDARD ERROROF MEASUREMENT (SEMeas)The st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement (SEMeas) is an<strong>in</strong>dex that shows the extent <strong>to</strong> which a measurementwould vary under changed circumstances (i.e., theamount of measurement error). Because there are manyways <strong>in</strong> which circumstances can vary, there are manypossible st<strong>and</strong>ard errors for a given score. For example,the st<strong>and</strong>ard error will be smaller if it <strong>in</strong>cludes onlyerror due <strong>to</strong> different content (<strong>in</strong>ternal-consistency orequivalent-forms reliability) than if it also <strong>in</strong>cludeserror due <strong>to</strong> the passage of time (test-retest reliability).Under the assumption that errors of measurement arenormally distributed (see pp. 191–192 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10), arange of scores can be determ<strong>in</strong>ed that shows theamount of error <strong>to</strong> be expected.For many IQ tests, the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measuremen<strong>to</strong>ver a one-year period <strong>and</strong> with different specificcontent is about five po<strong>in</strong>ts. Over a 10-year period, it isabout eight po<strong>in</strong>ts. This means that a score fluctuatesconsiderably more the longer the time between measurements.Thus, a person scor<strong>in</strong>g 110 can expect <strong>to</strong>have a score between 100 <strong>and</strong> 120 one year later; fiveyears later, the score can be expected <strong>to</strong> be between 94<strong>and</strong> 126 (see Figure 8.4). Note that we doubled the st<strong>and</strong>arderrors of measurement <strong>in</strong> comput<strong>in</strong>g the rangeswith<strong>in</strong> which the second score is expected <strong>to</strong> fall. Thiswas done so we could be 95 percent sure that our estimateswere correct.10095% Interval11095% IntervalSE = 5 po<strong>in</strong>ts120SE = 8 po<strong>in</strong>ts94110126Figure 8.4 St<strong>and</strong>ard Error of Measurement


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 159The formula for the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement isSD21 r 11 where SD the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation ofscores <strong>and</strong> r 11 the reliability coefficient appropriate <strong>to</strong>the conditions that vary. In the above example, the st<strong>and</strong>arderror (SEMeas) of 5 <strong>in</strong> the first example wasobta<strong>in</strong>ed as follows:SD 16, r 11 .90SEM 1621 .90 162.10 16(.32) 5.1SCORING AGREEMENTMost tests <strong>and</strong> many other <strong>in</strong>struments are adm<strong>in</strong>isteredwith specific directions <strong>and</strong> are scored objectively, thatis, with a key that requires no judgment on the part ofthe scorer. Although differences <strong>in</strong> the result<strong>in</strong>g scoreswith different adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs or scorers are still possible,it is generally considered highly unlikely that theywould occur. This is not the case with <strong>in</strong>struments thatare susceptible <strong>to</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration, scor<strong>in</strong>g,or both, such as essay evaluations. In particular, <strong>in</strong>strumentsthat use direct observation are highly vulnerable<strong>to</strong> observer differences. <strong>Research</strong>ers who use such<strong>in</strong>struments are obliged <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate <strong>and</strong> report thedegree of scor<strong>in</strong>g agreement. Such agreement is enhancedby tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the observers <strong>and</strong> by <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g thenumber of observation periods.Instruments differ <strong>in</strong> the amount of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g requiredfor their use. In general, observation techniquesrequire considerable tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for optimum use. Suchtra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g usually consists of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>gthe procedures <strong>in</strong>volved, followed by tra<strong>in</strong>ees us<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>struments as they observe videotapes or live situations.All tra<strong>in</strong>ees observe the same behaviors <strong>and</strong>then discuss any differences <strong>in</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g. This process,or some variation thereon, is repeated until <strong>in</strong>dependen<strong>to</strong>bservers reach an acceptable level of agreement.What is desired is a correlation of at least .90 amongscorers or agreement of at least 80 percent. Usually,even after such tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 8 <strong>to</strong> 12 observation periodsare required <strong>to</strong> get evidence of adequate reliabilityover time.To further illustrate the concept of reliability, let’s takean actual test <strong>and</strong> calculate the <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of itsitems. Figure 8.5 presents an example of a non-typicalDirections: Read each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g questions <strong>and</strong> write youranswers on a separate sheet of paper. Suggested time <strong>to</strong> take thetest is ten m<strong>in</strong>utes.Figure 8.5 The “Quick<strong>and</strong> Easy” IntelligenceTest1. There are two people <strong>in</strong> a room. The first is the son of thesecond person, but the second person is not the first person’sfather. <strong>How</strong> are the two people related?2. Who is buried <strong>in</strong> Grant’s <strong>to</strong>mb?3. Some months have thirty days, some have thirty-one. <strong>How</strong>many have twenty-eight days?4. If you had only one match <strong>and</strong> entered a dark room <strong>in</strong>which there was an oil lamp, an oil heater, <strong>and</strong> some firewood,which would you light first?5. If a physician gave you three pills <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ld you <strong>to</strong> take oneevery half hour, how long would they last?6. A person builds a house with four sides <strong>to</strong> it, a rectangularstructure, with each side hav<strong>in</strong>g a southern exposure. A bigbear comes w<strong>and</strong>er<strong>in</strong>g by. What color is the bear?7. A farmer has seventeen sheep. All but n<strong>in</strong>e died. <strong>How</strong>many did he have left?8. Divide 30 by 1 2 . Add 10. What is the correct answer?9. Take two apples from three apples. What do you have?10. <strong>How</strong> many animals of each species did Moses take aboardthe Ark?


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHIs Consequential Validitya Useful Concept?In recent years, <strong>in</strong>creased attention has been given <strong>to</strong> a conceptcalled consequential validity, orig<strong>in</strong>ally proposed bySamuel Messick <strong>in</strong> 1989.* He <strong>in</strong>tended not <strong>to</strong> change the coremean<strong>in</strong>g of validity, but <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> it <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude two new ideas:“value implications” <strong>and</strong> “social consequences.”Pay<strong>in</strong>g attention <strong>to</strong> value implications requires the “appraisalof the value implications of the construct label, of thetheory underly<strong>in</strong>g test <strong>in</strong>terpretation, <strong>and</strong> the ideologies <strong>in</strong>which the theory is imbedded.”† This <strong>in</strong>volves exp<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g theidea of construct-related evidence of validity that we discussedon pages 153–154. Social consequences refers <strong>to</strong> “theappraisal of both potential <strong>and</strong> actual social consequences ofapplied test<strong>in</strong>g.”*S. Messick (1989). Consequential validity. In R. L. L<strong>in</strong>n (Ed.).<strong>Education</strong>al measurement, 3rd ed. New York: American Council on<strong>Education</strong>, pp. 13–103.†Ibid., p. 20.Disagreement with Messick has been primarily withregard <strong>to</strong> apply<strong>in</strong>g his proposal. Us<strong>in</strong>g his experience as adeveloper of a widely used college admissions test battery(ACT) as an example, Reckase systematically analyzed thefeasibility of us<strong>in</strong>g this concept. He concluded that, althoughdifficult, the critical analysis of value implications is bothfeasible <strong>and</strong> useful.‡<strong>How</strong>ever, he argued that assess<strong>in</strong>g the cause-<strong>and</strong>-effectrelationships implied <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g potential <strong>and</strong> actual socialconsequences of the use of a test is difficult or impossible, evenwith a clear <strong>in</strong>tended use such as determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g college admissions.Cit<strong>in</strong>g the concern of the National Commission on Test<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> Public Policy that such tests often underm<strong>in</strong>e vitalsocial policies,§ he argues that obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the necessary dataseems unlikely <strong>and</strong> that, by def<strong>in</strong>ition, apprais<strong>in</strong>g un<strong>in</strong>tendedconsequences is not possible ahead of time, because one doesnot know what they are.What do you th<strong>in</strong>k of Messick’s proposal?‡M. D. Reckase (1998). Consequential validity from the testdeveloper’s perspective. <strong>Education</strong>al Measurement Issues <strong>and</strong>Practice, 17 (2): 13–16.§National Commission on Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Public Policy. From gatekeeper<strong>to</strong> gateway: Transform<strong>in</strong>g test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> America (Technical report).Chestnut Hill, MA: Bos<strong>to</strong>n College.<strong>in</strong>telligence test that we have adapted. Follow the directions<strong>and</strong> take the test. Then we will calculate the splithalfreliability.Now look at the answer key <strong>in</strong> the footnote at the bot<strong>to</strong>mof page 161. Give yourself one po<strong>in</strong>t for each correctanswer. Assume, for the moment, that a score on this testprovides an <strong>in</strong>dication of <strong>in</strong>telligence. If so, each item onthe test should be a partial measure of <strong>in</strong>telligence. Wecould, therefore, divide the 10-item test <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two 5-itemtests. One of these five-item tests can consist of all theodd-numbered items, <strong>and</strong> the other five-item test canconsist of all the even-numbered items. Now, record yourscore on the odd-numbered items <strong>and</strong> also on the evennumbereditems.We now want <strong>to</strong> see if the odd-numbered items providea measure of <strong>in</strong>telligence similar <strong>to</strong> that providedby the even-numbered items. If they do, your scores onthe odd-numbered items <strong>and</strong> the even-numbered itemsshould be pretty close. If they are not, then the two fiveitemtests do not give consistent results. If this is thecase, then the <strong>to</strong>tal test (the 10 items) probably does notgive consistent results either, <strong>in</strong> which case the scorecould not be considered a reliable measure.Score on five- Score on fiveitemtest 1 item test 2Person (#1, 3, 5, 7, 9) (#2, 4, 6, 8, 10)You _____________ _____________#1 _____________ _____________#2 _____________ _____________#3 _____________ _____________#4 _____________ _____________#5 _____________ _____________Figure 8.6 Reliability WorksheetAsk five other people <strong>to</strong> take the test. Record theirscores on the odd <strong>and</strong> even sets of items, us<strong>in</strong>g theworksheet shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 8.6.Take a look at the scores on each of the five-item setsfor each of the five <strong>in</strong>dividuals, <strong>and</strong> compare them withyour own. What would you conclude about the reliabilityof the scores? What would you say about any <strong>in</strong>ferences160


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 161*You might want <strong>to</strong> assess the content validity of this test. <strong>How</strong>would you def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>telligence? As you def<strong>in</strong>e the term, how wouldyou evaluate this test as a measure of <strong>in</strong>telligence?Answer Key for Q-E Intelligence Test on page 159. 1. Mother <strong>and</strong>son; 2. Ulysses S. Grant; 3. All of them; 4. The match; 5. One hour;6. White; 7. N<strong>in</strong>e; 8. 70; 9. Two; 10. None. (It wasn’t Moses, butNoah who <strong>to</strong>ok the animals on the Ark.)about <strong>in</strong>telligence a researcher might make based onscores on this test? Could they be valid?*Note that we have exam<strong>in</strong>ed only one aspect of reliability(<strong>in</strong>ternal consistency) for results of this test. We still donot know how much a person’s score might change if wegave the test at two different times (test-retest reliability).We could get a different <strong>in</strong>dication of reliability if we gaveone of the five-item tests at one time <strong>and</strong> the other fiveitemtest at another time <strong>to</strong> the same people (equivalentforms/retestreliability). Try <strong>to</strong> do this with a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals,us<strong>in</strong>g a worksheet like the one shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 8.6.<strong>Research</strong>ers typically use the procedures just described<strong>to</strong> establish reliability. Normally, however, theytest many more people (at least 100). You should alsorealize that most tests would have many more than10 items, s<strong>in</strong>ce longer tests are usually more reliablethan short ones, presumably because they provide alarger sampl<strong>in</strong>g of a person’s behavior.In sum, we hope it is clear that a major aspect of researchdesign is the obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of reliable <strong>and</strong> valid <strong>in</strong>formation.Because both reliability <strong>and</strong> validity depend onthe way <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>struments are used <strong>and</strong> on the <strong>in</strong>ferencesresearchers wish <strong>to</strong> make from them, researcherscan never simply assume that their <strong>in</strong>strumentation willprovide satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>formation. They can have moreconfidence if they use <strong>in</strong>struments on which there isprevious evidence of reliability <strong>and</strong> validity, providedthey use the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> the same way—that is, underthe same conditions as existed previously. Even then,researchers cannot be sure; even when all else rema<strong>in</strong>sthe same, the mere passage of time may have impairedthe <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> some way.What this means is that there is no substitute for check<strong>in</strong>greliability <strong>and</strong> validity as a part of the research procedure.There is seldom any excuse for fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> check<strong>in</strong>ternal consistency, s<strong>in</strong>ce the necessary <strong>in</strong>formation is ath<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> no additional data collection is required. Reliabilityover time does, <strong>in</strong> most cases, require an additionaladm<strong>in</strong>istration of an <strong>in</strong>strument, but this can often bedone. In consider<strong>in</strong>g this option, it should be noted that notall members of the sample need be retested, though this isdesirable. It is better <strong>to</strong> retest a r<strong>and</strong>omly selected subsample,or even a convenience subsample, than <strong>to</strong> have no evidenceof retest reliability at all. Another option is <strong>to</strong> test<strong>and</strong> retest a different, though very similar, sample.Obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g evidence on validity is more difficult butseldom prohibitive. Content-related evidence can usuallybe obta<strong>in</strong>ed s<strong>in</strong>ce it requires only a few knowledgeable<strong>and</strong> available judges. It is unreasonable <strong>to</strong> expect a greatdeal of construct-related evidence <strong>to</strong> be obta<strong>in</strong>ed, but, <strong>in</strong>many studies, criterion-related evidence can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed.At a m<strong>in</strong>imum, a second <strong>in</strong>strument should be adm<strong>in</strong>istered.Locat<strong>in</strong>g or develop<strong>in</strong>g an additional means of <strong>in</strong>strumentationis sometimes difficult <strong>and</strong> occasionally impossible(for example, there is probably no way <strong>to</strong> validatea self-report questionnaire on sexual behavior), but the resultsare well worth the time <strong>and</strong> energy <strong>in</strong>volved. As withretest reliability, a subsample can be used, or both <strong>in</strong>strumentscan be given <strong>to</strong> a different, but similar, sample.VALIDITY AND RELIABILITYIN QUALITATIVE RESEARCHWhile many qualitative researchers use many of theprocedures we have described, some take the positionthat validity <strong>and</strong> reliability, as we have discussed them,are either irrelevant or not suited <strong>to</strong> their research effortsbecause they are attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> describe a specific situationor event as viewed by a particular <strong>in</strong>dividual. Theyemphasize <strong>in</strong>stead the honesty, believability, expertise,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrity of the researcher. We ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> that allresearchers should ensure that any <strong>in</strong>ferences they drawthat are based on data obta<strong>in</strong>ed through the use of an<strong>in</strong>strument are appropriate, credible, <strong>and</strong> backed up byevidence of the sort we have described <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes, practice withkey terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


162 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMa<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsVALIDITYIt is important for researchers <strong>to</strong> use valid <strong>in</strong>struments, for the conclusions they draware based on the <strong>in</strong>formation they obta<strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g these <strong>in</strong>struments.The term validity, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the appropriateness, mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness,correctness, <strong>and</strong> usefulness of any <strong>in</strong>ferences a researcher draws based on data obta<strong>in</strong>edthrough the use of an <strong>in</strong>strument.Content-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> judgments on the content <strong>and</strong> logicalstructure of an <strong>in</strong>strument as it is <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>in</strong> a particular study.Criterion-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> which <strong>in</strong>formationprovided by an <strong>in</strong>strument agrees with <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed on other, <strong>in</strong>dependent<strong>in</strong>struments.A criterion is a st<strong>and</strong>ard for judg<strong>in</strong>g; with reference <strong>to</strong> validity, it is a second <strong>in</strong>strumentaga<strong>in</strong>st which scores on an <strong>in</strong>strument can be checked.Construct-related evidence of validity refers <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> which the <strong>to</strong>tality ofevidence obta<strong>in</strong>ed is consistent with theoretical expectations.A validity coefficient is a numerical <strong>in</strong>dex represent<strong>in</strong>g the degree of correspondencebetween scores on an <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> a criterion measure.An expectancy table is a two-way chart used <strong>to</strong> evaluate criterion-related evidence ofvalidity.RELIABILITYThe term reliability, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the consistency of scores or answersprovided by an <strong>in</strong>strument.Errors of measurement refer <strong>to</strong> variations <strong>in</strong> scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by the same <strong>in</strong>dividualson the same <strong>in</strong>strument.The test-retest method of estimat<strong>in</strong>g reliability <strong>in</strong>volves adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g the same <strong>in</strong>strumenttwice <strong>to</strong> the same group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals after a certa<strong>in</strong> time <strong>in</strong>terval haselapsed.The equivalent-forms method of estimat<strong>in</strong>g reliability <strong>in</strong>volves adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g twodifferent, but equivalent, forms of an <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> the same group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals atthe same time.The <strong>in</strong>ternal-consistency method of estimat<strong>in</strong>g reliability <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>gresponses <strong>to</strong> different sets of items that are part of an <strong>in</strong>strument.Scor<strong>in</strong>g agreement requires a demonstration that <strong>in</strong>dependent scorers can achievesatisfac<strong>to</strong>ry agreement <strong>in</strong> their scor<strong>in</strong>g.The st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement is a numerical <strong>in</strong>dex of measurement error.Key Termsalpha coefficient 157concurrent validity 152construct-related evidenceof validity 153content-related evidenceof validity 150correlationcoefficient 152criterion 152criterion-related evidenceof validity 152Cronbach alpha 158equivalent-formsmethod 156errors ofmeasurement 155expectancy table 153<strong>in</strong>ternal-consistencymethods 156Kuder-Richardsonapproach 156predictive validity 152reliability 154


CHAPTER 8 Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability 163reliability coefficient 155scor<strong>in</strong>g agreement 159split-half procedure 156st<strong>and</strong>ard error ofmeasurement(SEMeas) 158test-retest method 155validity 148validity coefficient 1521. We po<strong>in</strong>t out <strong>in</strong> the chapter that scores from an <strong>in</strong>strument may be reliable but notvalid, yet not the reverse. Why would this be so?2. What type of evidence—content-related, criterion-related, or construct-related—doyou th<strong>in</strong>k is the easiest <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>? the hardest? Why?3. In what way(s) might the format of an <strong>in</strong>strument affect its validity?4. “There is no s<strong>in</strong>gle piece of evidence that satisfies construct-related validity.” Is thisstatement true? If so, expla<strong>in</strong> why.5. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k is harder <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>, validity or reliability? Why?6. Might reliability ever be more important than validity? Expla<strong>in</strong>.7. <strong>How</strong> would you assess the Q-E Intelligence Test <strong>in</strong> Figure 8.4 with respect <strong>to</strong> validity?Expla<strong>in</strong>.8. The importance of us<strong>in</strong>g valid <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> research cannot be overstated. Why?For Discussion1. N. E. Wallen, M. C. Durk<strong>in</strong>, J. R. Fraenkel, A. J. McNaugh<strong>to</strong>n, <strong>and</strong> E. I. Saw<strong>in</strong> (1969). The Taba CurriculumDevelopment Project <strong>in</strong> Social Studies: Development of a comprehensive curriculum model for socialstudies for grades one through eight, <strong>in</strong>clusive of procedures for implementation <strong>and</strong> dissem<strong>in</strong>ation. MenloPark, CA: Addison-Wesley, p. 307.2. N. E. Gronlund (1988). <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> construct achievement tests, 4th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall,p. 140.3. See L. J. Cronbach (1951). Coefficient alpha <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal structure of tests. Psychometrika,16: 297–334.Notes


164 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 8: Validity <strong>and</strong> ReliabilityUse Problem Sheet 8 <strong>to</strong> describe how you plan <strong>to</strong> check on the validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of scoresobta<strong>in</strong>ed with your <strong>in</strong>struments. If you plan <strong>to</strong> use an exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument, summarize what youhave been able <strong>to</strong> learn about the validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of results obta<strong>in</strong>ed with it. If you plan<strong>to</strong> develop an <strong>in</strong>strument, expla<strong>in</strong> how you will attempt <strong>to</strong> ensure validity <strong>and</strong> reliability. Ineither case, expla<strong>in</strong> how you will obta<strong>in</strong> evidence <strong>to</strong> check validity <strong>and</strong> reliability.Problem Sheet 8Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability1. Indicate any exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments you plan <strong>to</strong> use:_____________________________I have learned the follow<strong>in</strong>g about the validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed withthese <strong>in</strong>struments. _____________________________________________________An electronic version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t,save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.2. I plan <strong>to</strong> develop the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments: _________________________________I will try <strong>to</strong> ensure reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of results obta<strong>in</strong>ed with these <strong>in</strong>strumentsby us<strong>in</strong>g one or more of the tips described on page 113:A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.3. For each <strong>in</strong>strument I plan <strong>to</strong> use:a. This is how I will check <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency: _____________________________b. This is how I will check reliability over time (stability): _____________________c. This is how I will check validity: ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________


Internal Validity9“You th<strong>in</strong>k that the<strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> your blood pressureis due <strong>to</strong> the new class you’vebeen assigned. Is anyth<strong>in</strong>gelse different?”M.D.“Well, I have beendr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g more lately, <strong>and</strong> I’m ona new diet. Also my wife <strong>and</strong> Ihaven’t been gett<strong>in</strong>g alongof late.”TeacherWhat Is Internal Validity?Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValiditySubject CharacteristicsLoss of Subjects (Mortality)LocationInstrumentationTest<strong>in</strong>gHis<strong>to</strong>ryMaturationAttitude of SubjectsRegressionImplementationFac<strong>to</strong>rs That Reduce theLikelihood of F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g aRelationship<strong>How</strong> Can a <strong>Research</strong>erM<strong>in</strong>imize These Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity?Two Po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>to</strong> EmphasizeOBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the terma “his<strong>to</strong>ry” threat“<strong>in</strong>ternal validity.”a “maturation” threatExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by each of thea “subject attitude” threatfollow<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitya “regression” threat<strong>and</strong> give an example of each:an “implementation” threata “subject characteristics” threatIdentify various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternala “mortality” threatvalidity <strong>in</strong> published research articles.a “location” threatSuggest possible remedies for specifican “<strong>in</strong>strumentation” threatexamples of the various threats <strong>to</strong>a “test<strong>in</strong>g” threat<strong>in</strong>ternal validity.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Internal ValidityAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 9.1: Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityActivity 9.2: What Type of Threat?Activity 9.3: Controll<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValiditySuppose the results of a study show that high school students taught by the <strong>in</strong>quiry method score higher on a tes<strong>to</strong>f critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, on the average, than do students taught by the lecture method. Is this difference <strong>in</strong> scoresdue <strong>to</strong> the difference <strong>in</strong> methods—<strong>to</strong> the fact that the two groups have been taught differently? Surely, theresearcher who is conduct<strong>in</strong>g the study would like <strong>to</strong> conclude this. Your first <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ation may be <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k the same.This may not be a legitimate <strong>in</strong>terpretation, however.What if the students who were taught us<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>quiry method were better critical th<strong>in</strong>kers <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with? Whatif some of the students <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>quiry group were also tak<strong>in</strong>g a related course dur<strong>in</strong>g this time at a nearby university?What if the teachers of the <strong>in</strong>quiry group were simply better teachers? Any of these (or other) fac<strong>to</strong>rs might expla<strong>in</strong>why the <strong>in</strong>quiry group scored higher on the critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test. Should this be the case, the researcher may bemistaken <strong>in</strong> conclud<strong>in</strong>g that there is a difference <strong>in</strong> effectiveness between the two methods, for the obta<strong>in</strong>eddifference <strong>in</strong> results may be due not <strong>to</strong> the difference <strong>in</strong> methods but <strong>to</strong> someth<strong>in</strong>g else.In any study that either describes or tests relationships, there is always the possibility that the relationship shown<strong>in</strong> the data is, <strong>in</strong> fact, due <strong>to</strong> or expla<strong>in</strong>ed by someth<strong>in</strong>g else. If so, the relationship observed is not at all what itseems <strong>and</strong> it may lose whatever mean<strong>in</strong>g it appears <strong>to</strong> have. Many alternative hypotheses may exist, <strong>in</strong> other words,<strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> the outcomes of a study. These alternative explanations are often referred <strong>to</strong> as threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity, <strong>and</strong> they are what this chapter is about.What Is Internal Validity?Perhaps unfortunately, the term validity is used <strong>in</strong> threedifferent ways by researchers. In addition <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity, which we discuss <strong>in</strong> this chapter, you will seereference <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument (or measurement) validity, asdiscussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8, <strong>and</strong> external (or generalization)validity, as discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 6.When a study has <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, it means that anyrelationship observed between two or more variablesshould be unambiguous as <strong>to</strong> what it means rather thanbe<strong>in</strong>g due <strong>to</strong> “someth<strong>in</strong>g else.” The “someth<strong>in</strong>g else”may, as we suggested above, be any one (or more) of anumber of fac<strong>to</strong>rs, such as the age or ability of the subjects,the conditions under which the study is conducted,or the type of materials used. If these fac<strong>to</strong>rs are not <strong>in</strong>some way or another controlled or accounted for, the researchercan never be sure that they are not the reason forany observed results. Stated differently, <strong>in</strong>ternal validitymeans that observed differences on the dependent variableare directly related <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable, <strong>and</strong>not due <strong>to</strong> some other un<strong>in</strong>tended variable.Consider this example. Suppose a researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds acorrelation of .80 between height <strong>and</strong> mathematics testscores for a group of elementary school students (grades1–5)—that is, the taller students have higher mathscores. Such a result is quite mislead<strong>in</strong>g. Why? Becauseit is clearly a by-product of age. Fifth-graders are taller<strong>and</strong> better <strong>in</strong> math than first-graders simply becausethey are older <strong>and</strong> more developed. To explore this relationshipfurther is po<strong>in</strong>tless; <strong>to</strong> let it affect school practicewould be absurd.Or consider a study <strong>in</strong> which the researcher hypothesizesthat, <strong>in</strong> classes for educationally h<strong>and</strong>icapped students,teacher expectation of student failure is related <strong>to</strong>amount of disruptive behavior. Suppose the researcherf<strong>in</strong>ds a high correlation between these two variables.Should he or she conclude that this is a mean<strong>in</strong>gfulrelationship? Perhaps. But the correlation might also be166


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 167expla<strong>in</strong>ed by another variable, such as the ability levelof the class (classes low <strong>in</strong> ability might be expected<strong>to</strong> have more disruptive behavior <strong>and</strong> higher teacherexpectation of failure).*In our experience, a systematic consideration of possiblethreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity receives the least attentionof all the aspects of plann<strong>in</strong>g a study. Often, thepossibility of such threats is not discussed at all. Probablythis is because their consideration is not seen asan essential step <strong>in</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out a study. <strong>Research</strong>erscannot avoid decid<strong>in</strong>g on what variables <strong>to</strong> study, orhow the sample will be obta<strong>in</strong>ed, or how the data will becollected <strong>and</strong> analyzed. They can, however, ignoreor simply not th<strong>in</strong>k about possible alternative explanationsfor the outcomes of a study until after the study iscompleted—at which po<strong>in</strong>t it is almost always <strong>to</strong>o late<strong>to</strong> do anyth<strong>in</strong>g about them. Identify<strong>in</strong>g possible threatsdur<strong>in</strong>g the plann<strong>in</strong>g stage of a study, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,can often lead researchers <strong>to</strong> design ways of elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>gor at least m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g these threats.In recent years, many useful categories of possiblethreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity have been identified. Althoughmost of these categories were orig<strong>in</strong>ally designed forapplication <strong>to</strong> experimental studies, some apply <strong>to</strong> othertypes of methodologies as well. We discuss the most importan<strong>to</strong>f these possible threats <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Various ways of controll<strong>in</strong>g for these threats have alsobeen identified. We discuss some of these <strong>in</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong>derof this chapter <strong>and</strong> others <strong>in</strong> subsequent chapters.may expla<strong>in</strong> away whatever differences between groupsare found. The list of such subject characteristics is virtuallyunlimited, but some examples that might affectthe results of a study <strong>in</strong>clude:AgeStrengthMaturityGenderEthnicityCoord<strong>in</strong>ationSpeedIntelligenceVocabularyAttitudeRead<strong>in</strong>g abilityFluencyManual dexteritySocioeconomic statusReligious beliefsPolitical beliefsIn a particular study, the researcher must decide,based on previous research or experience, which variablesare most likely <strong>to</strong> create problems, <strong>and</strong> do his orher best <strong>to</strong> prevent or m<strong>in</strong>imize their effects. In studiescompar<strong>in</strong>g groups, there are several methods of equat<strong>in</strong>ggroups, which we discuss <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 <strong>and</strong> 16. In correlationalstudies, there are certa<strong>in</strong> statistical techniquesthat can be used <strong>to</strong> control such variables, provided <strong>in</strong>formationon each variable is obta<strong>in</strong>ed. We discuss thesetechniques <strong>in</strong> Chapter 15.LOSS OF SUBJECTS (MORTALITY)No matter how carefully the subjects of a study are selected,it is common <strong>to</strong> “lose” some as the study progresses(Figure 9.1).This is known as a mortality threat.Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValiditySUBJECT CHARACTERISTICSThe selection of people for a study may result <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividuals(or groups) differ<strong>in</strong>g from one another <strong>in</strong> un<strong>in</strong>tendedways that are related <strong>to</strong> the variables <strong>to</strong> be studied.This is sometimes referred <strong>to</strong> as selection bias, or a subjectcharacteristics threat. In our example of teacherexpectations <strong>and</strong> class disruptive behavior, the abilitylevel of the class fits this category. In studies that comparegroups, subjects <strong>in</strong> the groups may differ on suchvariables as age, gender, ability, socioeconomic background,<strong>and</strong> the like. If not controlled, these variables“I’m afraid my thesisis <strong>in</strong> big trouble! I lost25 percent of myexperimental group.”“Good grief!What did youdo <strong>to</strong> them?”*Can you suggest any other variables that would expla<strong>in</strong> a highcorrelation (should it be found) between a teacher’s expectation offailure <strong>and</strong> the amount of disruptive behavior that occurs <strong>in</strong> class?Figure 9.1 A Mortality Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity


168 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFor one reason or another (for example, illness, familyrelocation, or the requirements of other activities), some<strong>in</strong>dividuals may drop out of the study. This is especiallytrue <strong>in</strong> most <strong>in</strong>tervention studies, s<strong>in</strong>ce they take placeover time.Subjects may be absent dur<strong>in</strong>g the collection of dataor fail <strong>to</strong> complete tests, questionnaires, or other <strong>in</strong>struments.Failure <strong>to</strong> complete <strong>in</strong>struments is especially aproblem <strong>in</strong> questionnaire studies. In such studies, it is notuncommon <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d that 20 percent or more of the subjects<strong>in</strong>volved do not return their forms. Remember, the actualsample <strong>in</strong> a study is not the <strong>to</strong>tal of those selected bu<strong>to</strong>nly those from whom data are obta<strong>in</strong>ed.Loss of subjects, of course, not only limits generalizabilitybut also can <strong>in</strong>troduce bias—if those subjects whoare lost would have responded differently from thosefrom whom data were obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Many times this is quitelikely, s<strong>in</strong>ce those who do not respond or who are absentprobably act this way for a reason. In the example wepresented earlier <strong>in</strong> which the researcher was study<strong>in</strong>gthe possible relationship between amount of disruptivebehavior by students <strong>in</strong> class <strong>and</strong> teacher expectations ofstudent failure, it is likely that those teachers who failed<strong>to</strong> describe their expectations <strong>to</strong> the researcher (<strong>and</strong> whowould therefore be “lost” for the purposes of the study)would differ from those who did provide this <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong> ways affect<strong>in</strong>g disruptive behavior.In studies compar<strong>in</strong>g groups, loss of subjects probablywill not be a problem if the loss is about the same <strong>in</strong>all groups. But if there are sizable differences betweengroups <strong>in</strong> terms of the numbers who drop out, this is certa<strong>in</strong>lya conceivable alternative explanation for whateverf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs appear. In compar<strong>in</strong>g students taught bydifferent methods (lecture versus discussion, for example),one might expect the poorer students <strong>in</strong> each group<strong>to</strong> be more likely <strong>to</strong> drop out. If more of the poorer studentsdrop out of either group, the other method may appearmore effective than it actually is.Ofallthethreats<strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity,mortalityisperhapsthe most difficult <strong>to</strong> control. A common misconception isthat the threat is elim<strong>in</strong>ated simply by replac<strong>in</strong>g the lostsubjects. No matter how this is done—even if they are replacedby new subjects selected r<strong>and</strong>omly—researcherscan never be sure that the replacement subjects will respondas those who dropped out would have. It is morelikely, <strong>in</strong> fact, that they will not. Can you see why?**S<strong>in</strong>ce those who drop out have done so for a reason, their replacementswill be different at least <strong>in</strong> this respect; thus, they may seeth<strong>in</strong>gs differently or feel differently, <strong>and</strong> their responses may accord<strong>in</strong>glybe different.It is sometimes possible for a researcher <strong>to</strong> arguethat the loss of subjects <strong>in</strong> a study is not a problem. Thisis done by explor<strong>in</strong>g the reasons for such loss <strong>and</strong> thenoffer<strong>in</strong>g an argument as <strong>to</strong> why these reasons are notrelevant <strong>to</strong> the particular study at h<strong>and</strong>. Absence fromclass on the day of test<strong>in</strong>g, for example, probablywould not <strong>in</strong> most cases favor a particular group,s<strong>in</strong>ce it would be <strong>in</strong>cidental rather than <strong>in</strong>tentional—unless the day <strong>and</strong> time of the test<strong>in</strong>g was announcedbeforeh<strong>and</strong>.Another attempt <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate the problem of mortalityis <strong>to</strong> provide evidence that the subjects lost weresimilar <strong>to</strong> those rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g on pert<strong>in</strong>ent characteristicssuch as age, gender, ethnicity, pretest scores, or othervariables that presumably might be related <strong>to</strong> thestudy outcomes. While desirable, such evidence cannever demonstrate conclusively that those subjectswho were lost would not have responded differentlyfrom those who rema<strong>in</strong>ed. When all is said <strong>and</strong>done, the best solution <strong>to</strong> the problem of mortality is<strong>to</strong> do one’s best <strong>to</strong> prevent or m<strong>in</strong>imize the loss ofsubjects.Some examples of a mortality threat <strong>in</strong>clude thefollow<strong>in</strong>g:Ahigh school teacher decides <strong>to</strong> teach his two Englishclasses differently. His one o’clock class spends alarge amount of time writ<strong>in</strong>g analyses of plays,whereas his two o’clock class spends much time act<strong>in</strong>gout <strong>and</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>g portions of the same plays.Halfway through the semester, several students <strong>in</strong> thetwo o’clock class are excused <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the annualschool play—thus they are “lost” from the study.If they, as a group, are better students than the res<strong>to</strong>f their class, their loss will lower the performanceof the two o’clock class.A researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> study the effects of a newdiet on build<strong>in</strong>g endurance <strong>in</strong> long-distance runners.She receives a grant <strong>to</strong> study, over a two-year period,a group of such runners who are on the trackteam at several nearby high schools <strong>in</strong> a large urbanschool district. The study is designed <strong>to</strong> comparerunners who are given the new diet with similar runners<strong>in</strong> the district who are not given the diet. About5 percent of the runners who receive the diet <strong>and</strong>about 20 percent of those who do not receive thediet, however, are seniors, <strong>and</strong> they graduate at theend of the first year of the study. Because seniorsare probably better runners, this loss will cause therema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g no-diet group <strong>to</strong> appear weaker than thediet group.


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 169“What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?”“What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?”Figure 9.2 Location Might Make a DifferenceLOCATIONThe particular locations <strong>in</strong> which data are collected, or <strong>in</strong>which an <strong>in</strong>tervention is carried out, may create alternativeexplanations for results. This is called a locationthreat. For example, classrooms <strong>in</strong> which students aretaught by, say, the <strong>in</strong>quiry method may have more resources(texts <strong>and</strong> other supplies, equipment, parent support,<strong>and</strong> so on) available <strong>to</strong> them than classrooms <strong>in</strong>which students are taught by the lecture method. Theclassrooms themselves may be larger, have better light<strong>in</strong>g,or conta<strong>in</strong> better-equipped workstations. Such variablesmay account for higher performance by students.In our disruptive behavior versus teacher expectationsexample, the availability of support (resources, aides,<strong>and</strong> parent assistance) might expla<strong>in</strong> the correlation betweenthe major variables of <strong>in</strong>terest. Classes with fewerresources might be expected <strong>to</strong> have more disruptive behavior<strong>and</strong> higher teacher expectations of failure.The location <strong>in</strong> which tests, <strong>in</strong>terviews, or other <strong>in</strong>strumentsare adm<strong>in</strong>istered may affect responses (Figure9.2). Parent assessments of their children at homemay be different from assessments of their children atschool. Student performance on tests may be lower iftests are given <strong>in</strong> noisy or poorly lighted rooms. Observationsof student <strong>in</strong>teraction may be affected by thephysical arrangement of certa<strong>in</strong> classrooms. Such differencesmight provide defensible alternative explanationsfor the results <strong>in</strong> a particular study.The best method of control for a location threat is <strong>to</strong>hold location constant—that is, keep it the same for allparticipants. When this is not feasible, the researchershould try <strong>to</strong> ensure that different locations do not systematicallyfavor or jeopardize the hypothesis. This mayrequire the collection of additional descriptions of thevarious locations.Here are some examples of a location threat:A researcher designs a study <strong>to</strong> compare the effectsof team versus <strong>in</strong>dividual teach<strong>in</strong>g of U.S. his<strong>to</strong>ry onstudent attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward his<strong>to</strong>ry. The classrooms <strong>in</strong>which students are taught by a s<strong>in</strong>gle teacher havefewer books <strong>and</strong> materials than the ones <strong>in</strong> whichstudents are taught by a team of three teachers.A researcher decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview counsel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>special education majors <strong>to</strong> compare their attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward their respective master’s degree programs.Over a three-week period, he manages <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewall of the students enrolled <strong>in</strong> the two programs.Although he is able <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview most of the students<strong>in</strong> one of the university classrooms, schedul<strong>in</strong>g conflictsprevent this classroom from be<strong>in</strong>g available forhim <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview the rema<strong>in</strong>der. As a result, he <strong>in</strong>terviews20 of the counsel<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong> the coffeeshop of the student union.INSTRUMENTATIONThe way <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>struments are used may also constitutea threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of a study. As discussed<strong>in</strong> Chapter 7, scores from the <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong>a study can lack evidence of validity. Lack of this k<strong>in</strong>dof validity does not necessarily present a threat <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity—but it may.*Instrument Decay. Instrumentation can createproblems if the nature of the <strong>in</strong>strument (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the*In general, we expect lack of validity of scores <strong>to</strong> make it less likelythat any relationships will be found. There are times, however, when“poor” <strong>in</strong>strumentation can <strong>in</strong>crease the chances of “phony” or“spurious” relationships emerg<strong>in</strong>g.


170 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Boy, I’m bushed!Will I ever get f<strong>in</strong>ishedwith these? Six more<strong>to</strong> go!”“He’s tired.”“Yep! Bound <strong>to</strong>affect how hegrades those lastfew papers!”“Hello!I’m <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> yourattitudes <strong>to</strong>ward the police.Please complete thisquestionnaire.”Figure 9.3 An Example of Instrument Decayscor<strong>in</strong>g procedure) is changed <strong>in</strong> some way or another.This is usually referred <strong>to</strong> as <strong>in</strong>strument decay. This isoften the case when the <strong>in</strong>strument permits different <strong>in</strong>terpretationsof results (as <strong>in</strong> essay tests) or is especiallylong or difficult <strong>to</strong> score, thereby result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> fatigue ofthe scorer (Figure 9.3). Fatigue often happens when a researcherscores a number of tests one after the other; heor she becomes tired <strong>and</strong> scores the tests differently (forexample, more rigorously at first, more generously later).The pr<strong>in</strong>cipal way <strong>to</strong> control <strong>in</strong>strument decay is <strong>to</strong>schedule data collection <strong>and</strong>/or scor<strong>in</strong>g so as <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imizechanges <strong>in</strong> any of the <strong>in</strong>struments or scor<strong>in</strong>g procedures.Here are some examples of <strong>in</strong>strument decay:A professor grades 100 essay-type f<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ationsover a five-hour period without tak<strong>in</strong>g a break.Each essay encompasses between 10 <strong>and</strong> 12 pages.He grades the papers of each class <strong>in</strong> turn <strong>and</strong> thencompares the results.The adm<strong>in</strong>istration of a large school district changesits method of report<strong>in</strong>g absences. Only students whoare considered truant (absence is unexcused) are reportedas absent; students who have a written excuse(from parents or school officials) are not reported.The district reports a 55 percent decrease <strong>in</strong> absencess<strong>in</strong>ce the new report<strong>in</strong>g system has been <strong>in</strong>stituted.Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r Characteristics. The characteristicsof the data gatherers—an <strong>in</strong>evitable part of most<strong>in</strong>strumentation—can also affect results. Gender, age,ethnicity, language patterns, or other characteristics ofthe <strong>in</strong>dividuals who collect the data <strong>in</strong> a study may affectthe nature of the data they obta<strong>in</strong> (Figure 9.4). If thesecharacteristics are related <strong>to</strong> the variables be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated,they may offer an alternative explanation forFigure 9.4 A Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r Characteristics Threatwhatever f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs appear. Suppose both male <strong>and</strong>female data gatherers were used <strong>in</strong> the prior example ofa researcher wish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> study the relationship betweendisruptive behavior <strong>and</strong> teacher expectations. It might bethat the female data collec<strong>to</strong>rs would elicit more confessionsof an expectation of student failure on the part ofteachers <strong>and</strong> generate more <strong>in</strong>cidents of disruptivebehavior on the part of students dur<strong>in</strong>g classroom observationsthan would the males. If so, any correlationbetween teacher expectations of failure <strong>and</strong> the amoun<strong>to</strong>f disruptive behavior by students might be expla<strong>in</strong>ed(at least partly) as an artifact of who collected the data.The primary ways <strong>to</strong> control this threat <strong>in</strong>clude us<strong>in</strong>gthe same data collec<strong>to</strong>r(s) throughout, analyz<strong>in</strong>g dataseparately for each collec<strong>to</strong>r, <strong>and</strong> (<strong>in</strong> comparison-groupstudies) ensur<strong>in</strong>g that each collec<strong>to</strong>r is used equallywith all groups.Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r Bias. There is also the possibilitythat the data collec<strong>to</strong>r(s) <strong>and</strong>/or scorer(s) may unconsciouslydis<strong>to</strong>rt the data <strong>in</strong> such a way as <strong>to</strong> make certa<strong>in</strong>outcomes (such as support for the hypothesis) morelikely. Examples <strong>in</strong>clude some classes be<strong>in</strong>g allowedmore time on tests than other classes; <strong>in</strong>terviewers ask<strong>in</strong>g“lead<strong>in</strong>g” questions of some <strong>in</strong>terviewees; observerknowledge of teacher expectations affect<strong>in</strong>g quantity<strong>and</strong> type of observed behaviors of a class; <strong>and</strong> judges ofstudent essays favor<strong>in</strong>g (unconsciously) one <strong>in</strong>structionalmethod over another.The two pr<strong>in</strong>cipal techniques for h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g datacollec<strong>to</strong>r bias are <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardize all procedures, whichusually requires some sort of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the data collec<strong>to</strong>rs,<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> ensure that the data collec<strong>to</strong>rs lack the <strong>in</strong>formationthey would need <strong>to</strong> dis<strong>to</strong>rt results—also known


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 171as planned ignorance. Data collec<strong>to</strong>rs should be eitherunaware of the hypothesis or unable <strong>to</strong> identify the particularcharacteristics of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups fromwhom the data are be<strong>in</strong>g collected. Data collec<strong>to</strong>rs donot need <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>ld which method group they are observ<strong>in</strong>gor test<strong>in</strong>g or how the <strong>in</strong>dividuals they are test<strong>in</strong>gperformed on other tests.Some examples of data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias are as follows:All teachers <strong>in</strong> a large school district are <strong>in</strong>terviewedregard<strong>in</strong>g their future goals <strong>and</strong> their views on facultyorganizations. The hypothesis is that those plann<strong>in</strong>ga career <strong>in</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration will be more negative<strong>in</strong> their views on faculty organizations than thoseplann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ue teach<strong>in</strong>g. Interviews are conductedby the vice pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>in</strong> each school. Teachersare likely <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the fact that the person<strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g them is the vice pr<strong>in</strong>cipal, <strong>and</strong> this mayaccount for the hypothesis be<strong>in</strong>g supported.An <strong>in</strong>terviewer unconsciously smiles at certa<strong>in</strong> answers<strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> questions dur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terview.An observer with a preference for <strong>in</strong>quiry methodsobserves more “attend<strong>in</strong>g behavior” <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>quiryidentifiedthan non<strong>in</strong>quiry-identified classes.A researcher is aware, when scor<strong>in</strong>g the end-of-studyexam<strong>in</strong>ations, which students were exposed <strong>to</strong> whichtreatment <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tervention study.TESTINGIn <strong>in</strong>tervention studies, where data are collected over a periodof time, it is common <strong>to</strong> test subjects at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the <strong>in</strong>tervention(s). By test<strong>in</strong>g, we mean the use of anyform of <strong>in</strong>strumentation, not just “tests.” If substantial improvementis found <strong>in</strong> posttest (compared <strong>to</strong> pretest)scores, the researcher may conclude that this improvementisdue<strong>to</strong>the<strong>in</strong>tervention.Analternativeexplanation,however, may be that the improvement is due <strong>to</strong> the use ofthe pretest. Why is this? Let’s look at the reasons.Suppose the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> a particular study <strong>in</strong>volvesthe use of a new textbook. The researcher wants<strong>to</strong> see if students score higher on an achievement test ifthey are taught the subject us<strong>in</strong>g this new text than didstudents who have used the regular text <strong>in</strong> the past. Theresearcher pretests the students before the new textbookis <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>and</strong> then posttests them at the end of a sixweekperiod. The students may be “alerted” <strong>to</strong> what isbe<strong>in</strong>g studied by the questions <strong>in</strong> the pretest, however,<strong>and</strong> accord<strong>in</strong>gly make a greater effort <strong>to</strong> learn the material.This <strong>in</strong>creased effort on the part of the students(rather than the new textbook) could account for the improvement.It may also be that “practice” on the pretestby itself is responsible for the improvement. This isknown as a test<strong>in</strong>g threat (Figure 9.5).Consider another example. Suppose a counselor <strong>in</strong> alarge high school is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out whetherstudent attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward mental health are affected by aspecial unit on the subject. He decides <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister anattitude questionnaire <strong>to</strong> the students before the unit is<strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>and</strong> then adm<strong>in</strong>ister it aga<strong>in</strong> after the unit iscompleted. Any change <strong>in</strong> attitude scores may be due <strong>to</strong>the students th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about <strong>and</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>g their op<strong>in</strong>ionsas a result of the pretest rather than as a result of the<strong>in</strong>tervention.Notice that it is not always the adm<strong>in</strong>istration of apretest per se that creates a possible test<strong>in</strong>g effect, butrather the “<strong>in</strong>teraction” that occurs between tak<strong>in</strong>g thetest <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention. A pretest sometimes can makestudents more alert <strong>to</strong> or aware of what may be about <strong>to</strong>take place, mak<strong>in</strong>g them more sensitive <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> responsive<strong>to</strong>ward the treatment that subsequently occurs. Insome studies, the possible effects of pretest<strong>in</strong>g are consideredso serious that such test<strong>in</strong>g is elim<strong>in</strong>ated.“<strong>How</strong>’d youdo on thef<strong>in</strong>al?”“I killed it! I remembered thequestions on the ‘diagnostic’test we <strong>to</strong>ok at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the semester.”Figure 9.5 A Test<strong>in</strong>gThreat <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity


172 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eA similar problem is created if the <strong>in</strong>strumentationprocess permits subjects <strong>to</strong> figure out the nature of thestudy. This is most likely <strong>to</strong> happen <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-group(correlational) studies of attitudes, op<strong>in</strong>ions, or similarvariables other than ability. Students might be askedtheir op<strong>in</strong>ions, for example, about teachers <strong>and</strong> alsoabout different subjects <strong>to</strong> test the hypothesis thatstudent attitude <strong>to</strong>ward teachers is related <strong>to</strong> studentattitude <strong>to</strong>ward the subjects taught. They may see a connectionbetween the two sets of questions, especially ifthey are both <strong>in</strong>cluded on the same form, <strong>and</strong> answeraccord<strong>in</strong>gly.Some examples of test<strong>in</strong>g threats are as follows:A researcher uses exactly the same set of problems <strong>to</strong>measure change over time <strong>in</strong> student ability <strong>to</strong> solvemathematics word problems. The first adm<strong>in</strong>istrationof the test is given at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of a unit of<strong>in</strong>struction; the second adm<strong>in</strong>istration is given at theend of the unit of <strong>in</strong>struction, three weeks later. Ifimprovement <strong>in</strong> scores occurs, it may be due <strong>to</strong>sensitization <strong>to</strong> the problems produced by the firsttest <strong>and</strong> the practice effect rather than <strong>to</strong> any <strong>in</strong>crease<strong>in</strong> problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g ability.A researcher <strong>in</strong>corporates items designed <strong>to</strong> measureself-esteem <strong>and</strong> achievement motivation <strong>in</strong> the samequestionnaire. The respondents may figure out whatthe researcher is after <strong>and</strong> react accord<strong>in</strong>gly.A researcher uses pre- <strong>and</strong> posttests of anxiety level<strong>to</strong> compare students given relaxation tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g withstudents <strong>in</strong> a control group. Lower scores for therelaxation group on the posttest may be due <strong>to</strong> thetra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, but they also may be due <strong>to</strong> sensitivity (createdby the pretest) <strong>to</strong> the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.HISTORYOn occasion, one or more unanticipated, <strong>and</strong> unplannedfor, events may occur dur<strong>in</strong>g the course of a study thatcan affect the responses of subjects (Figure 9.6). Such anevent is referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> educational research as a his<strong>to</strong>rythreat. In the study we suggested of students be<strong>in</strong>gtaught by the <strong>in</strong>quiry versus the lecture method, forexample, a bor<strong>in</strong>g visi<strong>to</strong>r who dropped <strong>in</strong> on <strong>and</strong> spoke<strong>to</strong> the lecture class just before an upcom<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ationwould be an example. If the visi<strong>to</strong>r’s remarks <strong>in</strong> someway discouraged or turned off students <strong>in</strong> the lectureclass, they might have done less well on the exam<strong>in</strong>ationthan if the visi<strong>to</strong>r had not appeared. Another example <strong>in</strong>volvesa personal experience of one of the authors of thistext. He remembers clearly the day that President JohnF. Kennedy died, s<strong>in</strong>ce he had scheduled an exam<strong>in</strong>ationfor that very day. The author’s students at that time,stunned <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> shock by the announcement of the president’sdeath, were unable <strong>to</strong> take the exam<strong>in</strong>ation. Anycomparison of exam<strong>in</strong>ation results taken on this daywith the exam<strong>in</strong>ation results of other classes taken onother days would have been mean<strong>in</strong>gless.<strong>Research</strong>ers can never be certa<strong>in</strong> that one group hasnot had experiences that differ from those of othergroups. As a result, they should cont<strong>in</strong>ually be alert <strong>to</strong>any such <strong>in</strong>fluences that may occur (<strong>in</strong> schools, forFigure 9.6 A His<strong>to</strong>ryThreat <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity“I don’t see how we’resupposed <strong>to</strong> study with thatconstruction noise outsideall the time!”


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 173example) dur<strong>in</strong>g the course of a study. As you will see <strong>in</strong>Chapter 13, some research designs h<strong>and</strong>le this threatbetter than do others.Two examples of a his<strong>to</strong>ry threat follow.A researcher designs a study <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effectsof simulation games on ethnocentrism. She plans <strong>to</strong>select two high schools <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> an experiment.Students <strong>in</strong> both schools will be given a pretestdesigned <strong>to</strong> measure their attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward m<strong>in</strong>oritygroups. School A will then be given the simulationgames dur<strong>in</strong>g their social studies classes over a threedayperiod, <strong>and</strong> school B will watch travel films. Bothschools will then be given the same test <strong>to</strong> see if theirattitude <strong>to</strong>ward m<strong>in</strong>ority groups has changed. Theresearcher conducts the study as planned, but a specialdocumentary on racial prejudice is shown <strong>in</strong>school A between the pretest <strong>and</strong> the posttest.The achievement scores of five elementary schoolswhose teachers use a cooperative learn<strong>in</strong>g approachare compared with those of five schools whoseteachers do not use this approach. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the courseof the study, the faculty of one of the schools wherecooperative learn<strong>in</strong>g is not used is engaged <strong>in</strong> a disruptiveconflict with the school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal.MATURATIONOften, change dur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>tervention may be due <strong>to</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rsassociated with the pass<strong>in</strong>g of time rather than <strong>to</strong>the <strong>in</strong>tervention itself (Figure 9.7). This is known as amaturation threat. Over the course of a semester, forexample, very young students, <strong>in</strong> particular, will change<strong>in</strong> many ways simply because of ag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> experience.Suppose, for example, that a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>study<strong>in</strong>g the effect of special grasp<strong>in</strong>g exercises on theability of 2-year-olds <strong>to</strong> manipulate various objects. Shef<strong>in</strong>ds that such exercises are associated with marked <strong>in</strong>creases<strong>in</strong> the manipulative ability of the children over asix-month period. Two-year-olds mature very rapidly,however, <strong>and</strong> the improvement <strong>in</strong> their manipulativeability may be due simply <strong>to</strong> this fact rather than thegrasp<strong>in</strong>g exercises. Maturation is a serious threat only <strong>in</strong>studies us<strong>in</strong>g pre–post data for the <strong>in</strong>tervention group,or <strong>in</strong> studies that span a number of years. The best way<strong>to</strong> control for maturation is <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude a well-selectedcomparison group <strong>in</strong> the study.Examples of a maturation threat are as follows:A researcher reports that students <strong>in</strong> liberal artscolleges become less accept<strong>in</strong>g of authoritybetween their freshman <strong>and</strong> senior years <strong>and</strong> attributesthis <strong>to</strong> the many “liberat<strong>in</strong>g” experiences theyhave undergone <strong>in</strong> college. This may be the reason,but it also may be because they simply have grownolder.A researcher tests a group of students enrolled <strong>in</strong> aspecial class for “students with artistic potential”every year for six years, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g when they are age5. She f<strong>in</strong>ds that their draw<strong>in</strong>g ability improvesmarkedly over the years.“I’m much moreconfident after a year <strong>in</strong>that self-esteem class.”“That’s great!”Figure 9.7 CouldMaturation Be at WorkHere?


174 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eATTITUDE OF SUBJECTS<strong>How</strong> subjects view a study <strong>and</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> it can alsothreaten <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. One example is the wellknownHawthorne effect, first observed <strong>in</strong> the Hawthorneplant of the Western Electric Company someyears ago. 1 It was accidentally discovered that productivity<strong>in</strong>creased not only when improvements were made<strong>in</strong> physical work<strong>in</strong>g conditions (such as an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong>the number of coffee breaks <strong>and</strong> better light<strong>in</strong>g) but alsowhen such conditions were un<strong>in</strong>tentionally made worse(for <strong>in</strong>stance, the number of coffee breaks was reduced<strong>and</strong> the light<strong>in</strong>g was dimmed). The usual explanation forthis is that the special attention <strong>and</strong> recognition receivedby the workers were responsible; they felt someonecared about them <strong>and</strong> was try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> help them. This positiveeffect, result<strong>in</strong>g from <strong>in</strong>creased attention <strong>and</strong> recognitionof subjects, has subsequently been referred <strong>to</strong> asthe Hawthorne effect.It has also been suggested that recipients of an experimentaltreatment may perform better because of thenovelty of the treatment rather than the specific natureof the treatment. It might be expected, then, that subjectswho know they are part of a study may show improvementas a result of a feel<strong>in</strong>g that they are receiv<strong>in</strong>gsome sort of special treatment—no matter what thistreatment may be (Figure 9.8).An opposite effect can occur whenever, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terventionstudies, the members of the control group receiveno treatment at all. As a result, they may become demoralizedor resentful <strong>and</strong> hence perform more poorly thanthe treatment group. It may thus appear that the experimentalgroup is perform<strong>in</strong>g better as a result of the treatment,when this is not the case.One remedy for these threats is <strong>to</strong> provide the controlor comparison group(s) with a special or novel treatmentcomparable <strong>to</strong> that received by the experimentalgroup. While simple <strong>in</strong> theory, this is not easy <strong>to</strong> do <strong>in</strong>most educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs. Another possibility, <strong>in</strong> somecases, is <strong>to</strong> make it easy for students <strong>to</strong> believe that thetreatment is just a regular part of <strong>in</strong>struction—that is,not part of an experiment. For example, it is sometimesunnecessary <strong>to</strong> announce that an experiment is be<strong>in</strong>gconducted.Here are examples of a subject attitude threat:A researcher decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the possible reductionof test anxiety by play<strong>in</strong>g classical musicdur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations. She r<strong>and</strong>omly selects 10 freshmanalgebra classes from the five high schools <strong>in</strong> alarge urban school district. In five of these classes,she plays classical music softly <strong>in</strong> the backgrounddur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations. In the other five (the controlgroup), she plays no music. The students <strong>in</strong> theFigure 9.8 The Attitudeof Subjects Can Make aDifference“OK, Juan <strong>and</strong> Alicia, I knowyou’re go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> enjoy this. You’rego<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be a part of a very importantstudy with a professor from theuniversity. Won’t thatbe excit<strong>in</strong>g?”


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHThreats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> Everyday LifeConsider the follow<strong>in</strong>g commonly held beliefs:Because failure often precedes suicide, it is thereforethe cause of suicide. (probable his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> mortalitythreats)Boys are genetically more talented <strong>in</strong> mathematics than aregirls. (probable subject attitude <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry threats)Girls are genetically more talented <strong>in</strong> language than areboys. (probable his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> subject attitude threats)M<strong>in</strong>ority students are less academically able than studentsfrom the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture. (probable subject characteristics,subject attitude, location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rythreats)People on welfare are lazy. (probable subject characteristics,location, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry threats)School<strong>in</strong>g makes students rebellious. (probable maturation<strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry threats)A policy of expell<strong>in</strong>g students who don’t “behave” improvesa school’s test scores. (probable mortality threat)Indoctr<strong>in</strong>ation changes attitude. (probable test<strong>in</strong>g threat)So-called miracle drugs cure <strong>in</strong>tellectual retardation.(probable regression threat)Smok<strong>in</strong>g marijuana leads eventually <strong>to</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g coca<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong>hero<strong>in</strong>. (probable mortality threat)control group, however, learn that music is be<strong>in</strong>gplayed <strong>in</strong> the other classes <strong>and</strong> express some resentmentwhen their teachers tell them that the musiccannot be played <strong>in</strong> their class. This resentment mayactually cause them <strong>to</strong> be more anxious dur<strong>in</strong>gexams or <strong>in</strong>tentionally <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>flate their anxiety scores.A researcher hypothesizes that critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skillis correlated with attention <strong>to</strong> detail. He adm<strong>in</strong>istersa somewhat novel test that provides a separate scorefor each variable (critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> attention <strong>to</strong>detail) <strong>to</strong> a sample of eighth-graders. The novelty ofthe test may confuse some students, while othersmay th<strong>in</strong>k it is silly. In either case, the scores of thesestudents are likely <strong>to</strong> be lower on both variables becauseof the format of the test, not because of anylack of ability. It may appear, therefore, that the hypothesisis supported. Neither score is a valid <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rof ability for such students, so this particular attitud<strong>in</strong>alreaction creates a threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.REGRESSIONA regression threat may be present whenever change isstudied <strong>in</strong> a group that is extremely low or high <strong>in</strong> itspre<strong>in</strong>tervention performance (Figure 9.9). Studies <strong>in</strong> specialeducation are particularly vulnerable <strong>to</strong> this threat,s<strong>in</strong>ce the students <strong>in</strong> such studies are frequently selectedon the basis of previous low performance. The regressionphenomenon can be expla<strong>in</strong>ed statistically, but for ourpurposes it simply describes the fact that a group selectedbecause of unusually low (or high) performance will, onthe average, score closer <strong>to</strong> the mean on subsequent test<strong>in</strong>g,regardless of what transpires <strong>in</strong> the meantime. Thus,a class of students of markedly low ability may be expected<strong>to</strong> score higher on posttests regardless of the effec<strong>to</strong>f any <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>to</strong> which they are exposed. Like maturation,the use of an equivalent control or comparisongroup h<strong>and</strong>les this threat—<strong>and</strong> this seems <strong>to</strong> be unders<strong>to</strong>odas reflected <strong>in</strong> published research.Some examples of a possible regression threat are asfollows:An Olympic track coach selects the members of herteam from those who have the fastest times dur<strong>in</strong>gthe f<strong>in</strong>al trials for various events. She f<strong>in</strong>ds that theiraverage time <strong>in</strong>creases the next time they run, however,which she perhaps erroneously attributes <strong>to</strong>poorer track conditions.Those students who score <strong>in</strong> the lowest 20 percent ona math test are given special help. Six months latertheir average score on a test <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g similar problemshas improved, but not necessarily because ofthe special help.IMPLEMENTATIONThe treatment or method <strong>in</strong> any experimental studymust be adm<strong>in</strong>istered by someone—the researcher, theteachers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study, a counselor, or someother person. This fact raises the possibility that the experimentalgroup may be treated <strong>in</strong> ways that are un<strong>in</strong>tended<strong>and</strong> not necessarily part of the method, yet which175


176 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 9.9 RegressionRears Its Head“These guys were all selectedbecause they’ve run the mile under3:58. But you know, I bet that theiraverage time will be slower than3:58 when they run <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>morrow’s race!”“Yeah, no doubt.A likely exampleof a regressioneffect, eh?”give them an advantage of one sort or another. This isknown as an implementation threat. It can happen <strong>in</strong>either of two ways.First, an implementation threat can occur when different<strong>in</strong>dividuals are assigned <strong>to</strong> implement differentmethods, <strong>and</strong> these <strong>in</strong>dividuals differ <strong>in</strong> ways related <strong>to</strong>the outcome. Consider our previous example <strong>in</strong> whichtwo groups of students are taught by either an <strong>in</strong>quiry ora lecture method. The <strong>in</strong>quiry teachers may simply bebetter teachers than the lecture teachers.There are a number of ways <strong>to</strong> control for this possibility.The researcher can attempt <strong>to</strong> evaluate the <strong>in</strong>dividualswho implement each method on pert<strong>in</strong>entcharacteristics (such as teach<strong>in</strong>g ability) <strong>and</strong> then try<strong>to</strong> equate the treatment groups on these dimensions(for example, by assign<strong>in</strong>g teachers of equivalent ability<strong>to</strong> each group). Clearly, this is a difficult <strong>and</strong> timeconsum<strong>in</strong>gtask. Another control is <strong>to</strong> require that eachmethod be taught by all teachers <strong>in</strong> the study. Wherefeasible, this is a preferable solution, though it also isvulnerable <strong>to</strong> the possibility that some teachers mayhave different abilities <strong>to</strong> implement the different methods.Still another control is <strong>to</strong> use several different <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>to</strong> implement each method, thereby reduc<strong>in</strong>g thechances of an advantage <strong>to</strong> either method.Second, an implementation threat can occur whensome <strong>in</strong>dividuals have a personal bias <strong>in</strong> favor of onemethod over the other. Their preference for the method,rather than the method itself, may account for the superiorperformance of students taught by that method. Thisis a good reason why a researcher should, if at all possible,not be one of the <strong>in</strong>dividuals who implements amethod <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tervention study. It is sometimes possible<strong>to</strong> keep <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are implementers ignorant ofthe nature of a study, but it is generally very difficult—<strong>in</strong>part because teachers or others <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a study willusually need <strong>to</strong> be given a rationale for their participation.One solution for this is <strong>to</strong> allow <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong>choose the method they wish <strong>to</strong> implement, but this createsthe possibility of differences <strong>in</strong> characteristics discussedabove. An alternative is <strong>to</strong> have all methods usedby all implementers, but with their preferences knownbeforeh<strong>and</strong>. Note that preference for a method as a resul<strong>to</strong>f us<strong>in</strong>g it does not constitute a threat—it is simplyone of the by-products of the method itself. This is alsotrue of other by-products. If teacher skill or parent <strong>in</strong>volvement,for example, improves as a result of themethod, it would not constitute a threat. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the researchercan observe <strong>in</strong> an attempt <strong>to</strong> see that the methodsare adm<strong>in</strong>istered as <strong>in</strong>tended.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHSome ThoughtsAbout Meta-AnalysisAs we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 5, the ma<strong>in</strong> argument <strong>in</strong> favorof do<strong>in</strong>g a meta-analysis is that the weaknesses <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualstudies should balance out or be reduced by comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe results of a series of studies. In short, researchers who do ameta-analysis attempt <strong>to</strong> remedy the shortcom<strong>in</strong>gs of any particularstudy by statistically comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the results of several(hopefully many) studies that were conducted on the same<strong>to</strong>pic. Thus, the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity that we discussed <strong>in</strong>this chapter should be reduced <strong>and</strong> generalizability should beenhanced.<strong>How</strong> is this done? Essentially by calculat<strong>in</strong>g what is calledeffect size (see Chapter 12). <strong>Research</strong>ers conduct<strong>in</strong>g a metaanalysisdo their best <strong>to</strong> locate all of the studies on a particular<strong>to</strong>pic (i.e., all of the studies hav<strong>in</strong>g the same <strong>in</strong>dependent variable).Once located, effect sizes <strong>and</strong> an overall average effectsize for each dependent variable are calculated.* As an example,Vockell <strong>and</strong> Asher report an average delta (Δ) of .80 on theeffectiveness of cooperative learn<strong>in</strong>g.†*This is not always easy <strong>to</strong> do. Frequently, published reports lack thenecessary <strong>in</strong>formation, although it can sometimes be deduced fromwhat is reported.†E. L. Vockell <strong>and</strong> J. W. Asher (1995). <strong>Education</strong>al research, 2nd ed.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, p. 361.As we have mentioned, meta-analysis is a way of quantify<strong>in</strong>greplications of a study. It is important <strong>to</strong> note, however,that the term replication is used rather loosely <strong>in</strong> this context,s<strong>in</strong>ce the studies that the researcher(s) has collectedmay have little <strong>in</strong> common except that they all have the same<strong>in</strong>dependent variable. Our concerns are twofold: Merelyobta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g several studies, even if they all have the same <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable, does not mean that they will necessarilybalance out each other’s weaknesses—they might all havethe same weakness. Secondly, <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g a meta-analysis,equal weight is given <strong>to</strong> both good <strong>and</strong> bad studies—that is,no dist<strong>in</strong>ction is made between studies that have been welldesigned<strong>and</strong> conducted <strong>and</strong> those that have not been sowell-designed <strong>and</strong>/or conducted. Results of a well-designedstudy <strong>in</strong> which the researchers used a large r<strong>and</strong>om sample,for example, would count the same as results from a poorlycontrolled study <strong>in</strong> which researchers used a convenience orpurposive sample.A partial solution <strong>to</strong> these problems that we support is <strong>to</strong>comb<strong>in</strong>e meta-analysis with judgmental review. This has beendone by judg<strong>in</strong>g studies as good or bad <strong>and</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g theresults; sometimes they agree. If, however, there is a sufficientnumber of good studies (we would argue for a m<strong>in</strong>imum ofseven), we see little <strong>to</strong> be ga<strong>in</strong>ed by <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g poor ones.Meta-analyses are here <strong>to</strong> stay, <strong>and</strong> there is little questionthat they can provide the research community with valuable<strong>in</strong>formation. But we do not th<strong>in</strong>k excessive enthusiasm forthe technique is warranted. Like many th<strong>in</strong>gs, it is a <strong>to</strong>ol, nota panacea.Examples of an implementation threat are as follows:A researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the effects of anew diet on the physical agility of young children.After obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the permission of the parents of thechildren <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>volved, all of whom are firstgraders,he r<strong>and</strong>omly assigns the children <strong>to</strong> an experimentalgroup <strong>and</strong> a control group. The experimentalgroup is <strong>to</strong> try the new diet for three months,<strong>and</strong> the control group is <strong>to</strong> stay with its regular diet.The researcher overlooks the fact, however, that theteacher of the experimental group is an accomplished<strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r of some five years’ experience,while the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r of the control group is a firstyearteacher, newly appo<strong>in</strong>ted.A group of clients who stutter is given a relativelynew method of therapy called generalization tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.Both client <strong>and</strong> therapist <strong>in</strong>teract with people <strong>in</strong>the “real world” as part of the therapy. After sixmonths of receiv<strong>in</strong>g therapy, the fluency of theseclients is compared with that of a group receiv<strong>in</strong>gtraditional <strong>in</strong>-the-office therapy. Speech therapistswho use new methods are likely <strong>to</strong> be more generallycompetent than those work<strong>in</strong>g with the comparisongroup. If so, greater improvement for the generalizationgroup may be due not <strong>to</strong> the new method butrather <strong>to</strong> the skill of the therapist.Figure 9.10 illustrates each of the threats we havediscussed.FACTORS THAT REDUCE THE LIKELIHOODOF FINDING A RELATIONSHIPIn many studies, the various fac<strong>to</strong>rs we have discussedcould also serve <strong>to</strong> reduce, or even prevent, the chancesof a relationship be<strong>in</strong>g found. For example, if the methods(the treatment) <strong>in</strong> a study are not adequately177


178 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Maybe thoseattend<strong>in</strong>g private schools havehigher academic aptitude — so it isnot the type of school thatmakes the difference.”“Hold on — perhapsprivate schools are more likely <strong>to</strong> expelthe poorer students. So it’s the policy,not the nature of the school, thatmakes the difference.”“Wait a m<strong>in</strong>ute.Private schools may have moreresources (materials, technologies,parent support); that could accountfor the differences <strong>in</strong>stead ofthe type of schoolorganization.”The teachers <strong>in</strong> this fictitiousexample are discuss<strong>in</strong>g the resultsof a study which shows that studentswho attend private high schools hadhigher achievement (as shown by testscores) than students who attendedpublic high schools.“Private school studentsmay achieve higher scores, not becauseof the type of school, but because they areexposed <strong>to</strong> a broader range of experiences.Their parents are more affluent.”(Subjectcharacteristics)(Loss ofsubjects)(Location)“Maybe private schoolstudents have more opportunities <strong>to</strong>practice tak<strong>in</strong>g their tests. This couldaccount for their higherperformance.”“Is it likely that thetests <strong>to</strong> assess achievement arebiased <strong>in</strong> favor of the curricula found <strong>in</strong>private schools? Could the procedureused <strong>in</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g favor the private schoolstudents (test<strong>in</strong>g conditions,adherence <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>structions)?”(Instrumentation) (Test<strong>in</strong>g) (His<strong>to</strong>ry)“Perhaps it is the status <strong>and</strong>self-esteem associated with attend<strong>in</strong>g aprivate school that motivates these students <strong>to</strong>achieve at a higher level, rather than the typeof school organization.”“Perhaps privateschool students spend moreyears <strong>in</strong> high school than those<strong>in</strong> public schools.”*“Maybe there were a lo<strong>to</strong>f students who scoredreally low on the pretest<strong>in</strong> the private schools.”“Maybe privateschools have more experiencedor dedicated teachers <strong>and</strong> this isthe reason for a difference.”†(Maturation)Figure 9.10 Illustration of Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity(Attitude (Regression)of subjects)(Implementation)Note: We are not imply<strong>in</strong>g that any of these statements are necessarily true; our guess is that some are <strong>and</strong> some are not.*This seems unlikely.†If these teacher characteristics are a result of the type of school, then they do not constitute a threat.implemented—that is, adequately tried—the effect ofactual differences between them on outcomes may beobscured. Similarly, if the members of a control or comparisongroup become “aware” of the experimentaltreatment, they may <strong>in</strong>crease their efforts because theyfeel “left out,” thereby reduc<strong>in</strong>g real differences <strong>in</strong>achievement between treatment groups that otherwisewould be seen. Sometimes, teachers of a control groupmay unwitt<strong>in</strong>gly give some sort of “compensation” <strong>to</strong>motivate the members of their group, thereby lessen<strong>in</strong>gthe impact of the experimental treatment. F<strong>in</strong>ally, theuse of <strong>in</strong>struments that produce unreliable scores <strong>and</strong>/orthe use of small samples may result <strong>in</strong> a reduced likelihoodof a relationship or relationships be<strong>in</strong>g observed.


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 179<strong>How</strong> Can a <strong>Research</strong>erM<strong>in</strong>imize These Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity?Throughout this chapter, we have suggested a number oftechniques or procedures that researchers can employ <strong>to</strong>control or m<strong>in</strong>imize the possible effects of threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity. Essentially, they boil down <strong>to</strong> four alternatives.Aresearchercan try <strong>to</strong> do any or all of the follow<strong>in</strong>g.1. St<strong>and</strong>ardize the conditions under which the studyoccurs—such as the way(s) <strong>in</strong> which the treatment isimplemented (<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studies), the way(s) <strong>in</strong>which the data are collected, <strong>and</strong> so on. This helpscontrol for location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, subject attitude,<strong>and</strong> implementation threats.2. Obta<strong>in</strong> more <strong>in</strong>formation on the subjects of thestudy—that is, on relevant characteristics of thesubjects—<strong>and</strong> use that <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results. This helps control for asubject characteristics threat <strong>and</strong> (possibly) a mortalitythreat, as well as maturation <strong>and</strong> regression threats.3. Obta<strong>in</strong> more <strong>in</strong>formation on the details of thestudy—that is, where <strong>and</strong> when it takes place, extraneousevents that occur, <strong>and</strong> so on. This helps controlfor location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, his<strong>to</strong>ry, subject attitude,<strong>and</strong> implementation threats.4. Choose an appropriate design. The proper designcan do much <strong>to</strong> control these threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity.Because control by design applies primarily <strong>to</strong> experimental<strong>and</strong> causal-comparative studies, we shalldiscuss it <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 <strong>and</strong> 16. The four alternativesare summarized <strong>in</strong> Table 9.1.TWO POINTS TO EMPHASIZEWe want <strong>to</strong> end this chapter by emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g two th<strong>in</strong>gs.First, these various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity can begreatly reduced by plann<strong>in</strong>g. Second, such plann<strong>in</strong>goften requires collect<strong>in</strong>g additional <strong>in</strong>formation before astudy beg<strong>in</strong>s (or while it is tak<strong>in</strong>g place). It is often <strong>to</strong>olate <strong>to</strong> consider how <strong>to</strong> control these threats once thedata have been collected.TABLE 9.1General Techniques for Controll<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityTechniqueObta<strong>in</strong> More Obta<strong>in</strong> More Choose anSt<strong>and</strong>ardize Information Information AppropriateThreat Conditions on Subjects on Details <strong>Design</strong>Subject characteristics X XMortality X XLocation X X XInstrumentation X XTest<strong>in</strong>gXHis<strong>to</strong>ry X XMaturation X XSubject attitude X X XRegression X XImplementation X X XGo back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes, practice withkey terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


180 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMa<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTHE MEANING OF INTERNAL VALIDITYWhen a study lacks <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, one or more alternative hypotheses exist <strong>to</strong>expla<strong>in</strong> the outcomes. These alternative hypotheses are referred <strong>to</strong> by researchers asthreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.When a study has <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, it means that any relationship observed betweentwo or more variables is unambiguous, rather than be<strong>in</strong>g due <strong>to</strong> someth<strong>in</strong>g else.THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITYSome of the more common threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity are differences <strong>in</strong> subject characteristics,mortality, location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, test<strong>in</strong>g, his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, attitudeof subjects, regression, <strong>and</strong> implementation.The selection of people for a study may result <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups differ<strong>in</strong>g(i.e., the characteristics of the subjects may differ) from one another <strong>in</strong> un<strong>in</strong>tendedways that are related <strong>to</strong> the variables <strong>to</strong> be studied.No matter how carefully the subjects of a study (the sample) are selected, it is common<strong>to</strong> lose some of them as the study progresses. This is known as mortality. Sucha loss of subjects may affect the outcomes of a study.The particular locations <strong>in</strong> which data are collected, or <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>tervention iscarried out, may create alternative explanations for any results that are obta<strong>in</strong>ed.The way <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>struments are used may also constitute a threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity of a study. Possible <strong>in</strong>strumentation threats <strong>in</strong>clude changes <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>strument,characteristics of the data collec<strong>to</strong>r(s), <strong>and</strong>/or bias on the part of the data collec<strong>to</strong>rs.The use of a pretest <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studies sometimes may create a “practice effect”that can affect the results of a study. A pretest can also sometimes affect the way subjectsrespond <strong>to</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tervention.On occasion, one or more unanticipated <strong>and</strong> unplanned for events may occur dur<strong>in</strong>gthe course of a study that can affect the responses of subjects. This is known as ahis<strong>to</strong>ry threat.Sometimes change dur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>tervention study may be due more <strong>to</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs associatedwith the pass<strong>in</strong>g of time than <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention itself. This is known as a maturationthreat.The attitude of subjects <strong>to</strong>ward a study (<strong>and</strong> their participation <strong>in</strong> it) can create athreat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.When subjects are given <strong>in</strong>creased attention <strong>and</strong> recognition because they are participat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> a study, their responses may be affected. This is known as the Hawthorne effect.Whenever a group is selected because of unusually high or low performance on apretest, it will, on average, score closer <strong>to</strong> the mean on subsequent test<strong>in</strong>g, regardlessof what transpires <strong>in</strong> the meantime. This is called a regression threat.Whenever an experimental group is treated <strong>in</strong> ways that are un<strong>in</strong>tended <strong>and</strong> not anecessary part of the method be<strong>in</strong>g studied, an implementation threat can occur.CONTROLLING THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITY<strong>Research</strong>ers can use a number of techniques or procedures <strong>to</strong> control or m<strong>in</strong>imizethreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. Essentially they boil down <strong>to</strong> four alternatives: (1) st<strong>and</strong>ardiz<strong>in</strong>gthe conditions under which the study occurs, (2) obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g more<strong>in</strong>formation on the subjects of the study, (3) obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>in</strong>formation onthe details of the study, <strong>and</strong> (4) choos<strong>in</strong>g an appropriate design.


CHAPTER 9 Internal Validity 181data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias 170Hawthorne effect 174his<strong>to</strong>ry threat 172implementationthreat 176<strong>in</strong>strument decay 170<strong>in</strong>ternal validity 166location threat 169maturation threat 173mortality threat 167regression threat 175subject characteristicsthreat 167test<strong>in</strong>g threat 171threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity 167Key Terms1. Can a researcher prove conclusively that a study has <strong>in</strong>ternal validity? Expla<strong>in</strong>.2. In Chapter 6, we discussed the concept of external validity. In what ways, if any, are<strong>in</strong>ternal <strong>and</strong> external validity related? Can a study have <strong>in</strong>ternal validity but notexternal validity? If so, how? What about the reverse?3. Students often confuse the concept of <strong>in</strong>ternal validity with the idea of <strong>in</strong>strumentvalidity. <strong>How</strong> would you expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between the two?4. What threat (or threats) <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity might exist <strong>in</strong> each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g?a. A researcher decides <strong>to</strong> try out a new mathematics curriculum <strong>in</strong> a nearby elementaryschool <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> compare student achievement <strong>in</strong> math with that of students<strong>in</strong> another elementary school us<strong>in</strong>g the regular curriculum. The researcher is notaware, however, that the students <strong>in</strong> the new-curriculum school have computers<strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> their classrooms.b. A researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> compare two different k<strong>in</strong>ds of textbooks <strong>in</strong> two highschool chemistry classes over a semester. She f<strong>in</strong>ds that 20 percent of one group<strong>and</strong> 10 percent of the other group are absent dur<strong>in</strong>g the adm<strong>in</strong>istration of unit tests.c. In a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the possible relationship between marital status <strong>and</strong> perceivedsocial changes dur<strong>in</strong>g the last five years, men <strong>and</strong> women <strong>in</strong>terviewers getdifferent reactions from female respondents <strong>to</strong> the same questions.d. Teachers of an experimental English curriculum as well as teachers of the regularcurriculum adm<strong>in</strong>ister both pre- <strong>and</strong> posttests <strong>to</strong> their own students.e. Eighth-grade students who volunteer <strong>to</strong> tu<strong>to</strong>r third-graders <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g showgreater improvement <strong>in</strong> their own read<strong>in</strong>g scores than a comparison group thatdoes not participate <strong>in</strong> tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g.f. A researcher compares the effects of weekly <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> group counsel<strong>in</strong>g onthe improvement of study habits. Each week the students counseled as a group fillout questionnaires on their progress at the end of their meet<strong>in</strong>gs. The studentscounseled <strong>in</strong>dividually, however, fill out the questionnaires at home.g. Those students who score <strong>in</strong> the bot<strong>to</strong>m 10 percent academically <strong>in</strong> a school <strong>in</strong> aneconomically depressed area are selected for a special program of enrichment. Theprogram <strong>in</strong>cludes special games, extra <strong>and</strong> specially colored materials, specialsnacks, <strong>and</strong> new books. The students score substantially higher on achievementtests six months after the program is <strong>in</strong>stituted.h. A group of elderly people are asked <strong>to</strong> fill out a questionnaire designed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe possible relationship between activity level <strong>and</strong> sense of life satisfaction.5. <strong>How</strong> could you determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the threats you identified <strong>in</strong> each of the situations<strong>in</strong> question 4 actually exist?6. Which threats discussed <strong>in</strong> this chapter do you th<strong>in</strong>k are the most important for aresearcher <strong>to</strong> consider? Why? Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be the most difficult <strong>to</strong>control? Expla<strong>in</strong>.1. F. J. Roethlisberger <strong>and</strong> W. J. Dickson (1939). Management <strong>and</strong> the worker. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.For DiscussionNote


182 PART 2 The Basics of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 9: Internal ValidityState the question or hypothesis of your study at the <strong>to</strong>p of Problem Sheet 9. In the spaces<strong>in</strong>dicated, place an X after each of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity that apply <strong>to</strong> your study,expla<strong>in</strong> why they are threats, <strong>and</strong> describe how you <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> control for those most likely <strong>to</strong>occur (i.e., prevent them from affect<strong>in</strong>g the outcome of your study).Problem Sheet 9Internal Validity1. My question or hypothesis is: ____________________________________________2. I have placed an X <strong>in</strong> the blank <strong>in</strong> front of four of the threats listed below that apply<strong>to</strong> my study. I expla<strong>in</strong> why I th<strong>in</strong>k each one is a problem <strong>and</strong> then expla<strong>in</strong> how Iwould attempt <strong>to</strong> control for the threat.An electronic version ofthis Problem Sheet thatyou can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.Threats: _____ Subject characteristics _____ Mortality_____ Location_____ Instrumentation _____ Test<strong>in</strong>g _____ His<strong>to</strong>ry_____ Maturation_____ Subject attitude _____ Regression_____ Implementation _____ OtherThreat 1: _______________ Why? _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I will control by __________________________________________________________A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book._______________________________________________________________________Threat 2: _______________ Why? _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I will control by _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________Threat 3: _______________ Why? _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I will control by _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________Threat 4: _______________ Why? _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________I will control by _________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________


Data AnalysisP A R T3Part 3 <strong>in</strong>troduces the subject of statistics—important <strong>to</strong>ols that are frequently used byresearchers <strong>in</strong> the analysis of their data. Chapter 10 presents a discussion of descriptivestatistics <strong>and</strong> provides a number of techniques for summariz<strong>in</strong>g both quantitative <strong>and</strong>categorical data. Chapter 11 deals with <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics—how <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether an outcome can be generalized or not—<strong>and</strong> briefly discusses the most commonlyused <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics. Chapter 12 then places what we have presented <strong>in</strong>the previous two chapters <strong>in</strong> perspective. We provide some examples of compar<strong>in</strong>ggroups <strong>and</strong> of relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> a group. We conclude the chapter with asummary of recommendations for us<strong>in</strong>g both descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.


10Descriptive StatisticsStatistics VersusParametersTwo Fundamental Typesof Numerical DataQuantitative DataCategorical DataTechniques forSummariz<strong>in</strong>gQuantitative DataFrequency PolygonsSkewed PolygonsHis<strong>to</strong>grams <strong>and</strong> Stem-LeafPlotsThe Normal CurveAveragesSpreadsSt<strong>and</strong>ard Scores <strong>and</strong> theNormal CurveCorrelationTechniques forSummariz<strong>in</strong>gCategorical DataThe Frequency TableBar Graphs <strong>and</strong> Pie ChartsThe Crossbreak TableA Normal Distribution?OBJECTIVESExpla<strong>in</strong> the difference between astatistic <strong>and</strong> a parameter.Differentiate between categorical <strong>and</strong>quantitative data <strong>and</strong> give an exampleof each.Construct a frequency polygon fromdata.Construct a his<strong>to</strong>gram <strong>and</strong> a stem-leafplot from data.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the terms“normal distribution” <strong>and</strong> “normal curve.”Calculate the mean, median, <strong>and</strong> modefor a frequency distribution of data.Calculate the range <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation for a frequency distributionof data.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a five-number summary is.Read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what a boxplot displays.Expla<strong>in</strong> how any particular score <strong>in</strong> anormal distribution can be <strong>in</strong>terpreted<strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation units.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a “z score” is <strong>and</strong> tell whyit is advantageous <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> expressscores <strong>in</strong> z score terms.Expla<strong>in</strong> how <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret a normaldistribution.Construct <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret a scatterplot.Expla<strong>in</strong> more fully what a correlationcoefficient is.Calculate a Pearson correlationcoefficient.Prepare <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret a frequency table,a bar graph, <strong>and</strong> a pie chart.Prepare <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret a crossbreak table.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Read the Statistics PrimerLearn More About Techniques for Summariz<strong>in</strong>gQuantitative <strong>and</strong> Categorical DataAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 10.1: Construct a Frequency PolygonActivity 10.2: Compare Frequency PolygonsActivity 10.3: Calculate AveragesActivity 10.4: Calculate the St<strong>and</strong>ard DeviationActivity 10.5: Calculate a Correlation CoefficientActivity 10.6: Analyze Crossbreak TablesActivity 10.7: Compare z ScoresActivity 10.8: Prepare a Five-Number SummaryActivity 10.9: Summarize SalariesActivity 10.10: Compare ScoresActivity 10.11: Cus<strong>to</strong>dial TimesActivity 10.12: Collect DataTim Williams <strong>and</strong> Jack Mart<strong>in</strong>ez have just come from their 9:00 A.M. statistics class.“I need some coffee,” says Tim.“Yeah, me <strong>to</strong>o,” says Jack. “That was a pretty heavy session.”“Let’s go over some of the stuff Dr. Fraenkel talked about. He gave some pretty good examples, but it doesn’thurt <strong>to</strong> see if we can come up with some on our own.”“I agree. You know, I th<strong>in</strong>k I f<strong>in</strong>ally underst<strong>and</strong> what st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation is all about.”“You do? Hey, good go<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>How</strong> about expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g it <strong>to</strong> me then? I don’t quite get it.”“Piece of cake. Th<strong>in</strong>k of it this way . . .”Maybe not exactly a piece of cake, but we th<strong>in</strong>k you’ll f<strong>in</strong>d the concept fairly easy <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> once you’vestudied the ideas <strong>in</strong> this chapter. Read on.Statistics Versus ParametersThe major advantage of descriptive statistics is thatthey permit researchers <strong>to</strong> describe the <strong>in</strong>formation conta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> many, many scores with just a few <strong>in</strong>dices,such as the mean <strong>and</strong> median (more about these <strong>in</strong> amoment). When such <strong>in</strong>dices are calculated for a sampledrawn from a population, they are called statistics;when they are calculated from the entire population,they are called parameters. Because most educationalresearch <strong>in</strong>volves data from samples rather than frompopulations, we refer ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>to</strong> statistics <strong>in</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong>derof this chapter. We present the most commonly usedtechniques for summariz<strong>in</strong>g such data. Some form ofsummary is essential <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret data collected on anyvariable—a long list of scores or categorical representationsis simply unmanageable.Two Fundamental Typesof Numerical DataIn Chapter 7, we presented a number of <strong>in</strong>struments used<strong>in</strong> educational research. The researcher’s <strong>in</strong>tention <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>gthese <strong>in</strong>struments is <strong>to</strong> collect <strong>in</strong>formation of some sort—measures of abilities, attitudes, beliefs, reactions, <strong>and</strong> soforth—that will enable him or her <strong>to</strong> draw some conclusionsabout the sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals be<strong>in</strong>g studied.As we have seen, such <strong>in</strong>formation can be collected<strong>in</strong> several ways, but it can be reported <strong>in</strong> only threeways: through words, through numbers, <strong>and</strong> sometimesthrough graphs or charts that show patterns or describerelationships. In certa<strong>in</strong> types of research, such as <strong>in</strong>terviews,ethnographic studies, or case studies, researchersoften try <strong>to</strong> describe their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs through a narrative185


186 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eUs<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Perform Statistical CalculationsThe Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, more commonly known as SPSS, is a computerprogram that can be used <strong>to</strong> calculate many of the descriptive statistics that we describe <strong>in</strong> thistext, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations, z scores, correlations, <strong>and</strong> regression equations.SPSS can also be used <strong>to</strong> conduct many hypothesis tests, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> repeatedmeasurest-tests, analysis of variance (ANOVA), <strong>and</strong> chi-square tests. We do not have the space<strong>to</strong> present a complete explanation of how <strong>to</strong> use SPSS, but <strong>in</strong> this <strong>and</strong> the next chapter, we shallgive a brief step-by-step description of how the program can be used <strong>to</strong> compute some of thestatistics described <strong>in</strong> these chapters.In Appendix D, however, we do present a more detailed explanation of how <strong>to</strong> use SPSS thatillustrates many of the steps we describe.description of some sort. Their <strong>in</strong>tent is not <strong>to</strong> reduce the<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> numerical form but <strong>to</strong> present it <strong>in</strong> adescriptive form, <strong>and</strong> often as richly as possible. Wegive some examples of this method of report<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong> Chapters 19, 20, <strong>and</strong> 21. In this chapter, however,we concentrate on numerical ways of report<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation.Much of the <strong>in</strong>formation reported <strong>in</strong> educational researchconsists of numbers of some sort—test scores,percentages, grade po<strong>in</strong>t averages, rat<strong>in</strong>gs, frequencies,<strong>and</strong> the like. The reason is an obvious one—numbersare a useful way <strong>to</strong> simplify <strong>in</strong>formation. Numerical <strong>in</strong>formation,usually referred <strong>to</strong> as data, can be classified<strong>in</strong> one of two basic ways: as either categorical or quantitativedata.Just as there are categorical <strong>and</strong> quantitative variables(see Chapter 3), there are two types of numerical data.Categorical data differ <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>d, but not <strong>in</strong> degree oramount. Quantitative data, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, differ<strong>in</strong> degree or amount.QUANTITATIVE DATAQuantitative data are obta<strong>in</strong>ed when the variable be<strong>in</strong>gstudied is measured along a scale that <strong>in</strong>dicates howmuch of the variable is present. Quantitative data arereported <strong>in</strong> terms of scores. Higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicate thatmore of the variable (such as weight, academic ability,self-esteem, or <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> mathematics) is present than dolower scores. Some examples of quantitative data follow.The amount of money spent on sports equipment byvarious schools <strong>in</strong> a particular district <strong>in</strong> a semester(the variable is amount of money spent on sportsequipment)SAT scores (the variable is scholastic aptitude)The temperatures recorded each day dur<strong>in</strong>g themonths of September through December <strong>in</strong> Omaha,Nebraska, <strong>in</strong> a given year (the variable is temperature)The anxiety scores of all first-year students enrolledat San Francisco State University <strong>in</strong> 2002 (the variableis anxiety)CATEGORICAL DATACategorical data simply <strong>in</strong>dicate the <strong>to</strong>tal number of objects,<strong>in</strong>dividuals, or events a researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds <strong>in</strong> a particularcategory. Thus, a researcher who reports the number ofpeople for or aga<strong>in</strong>st a particular government policy, or thenumber of students complet<strong>in</strong>g a program <strong>in</strong> successiveyears, is report<strong>in</strong>g categorical data. Notice that what theresearcher is look<strong>in</strong>g for is the frequency of certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics,objects, <strong>in</strong>dividuals, or events. Many times itis useful, however, <strong>to</strong> convert these frequencies <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>percentages. Some examples of categorical data follow.The representation of each ethnic group <strong>in</strong> a school(the variable is ethnicity); for example, Caucasian,1,462 (41 percent); black, 853 (24 percent); Hispanic,760 (21 percent); Asian, 530 (15 percent)The number of male <strong>and</strong> female students <strong>in</strong> a chemistryclass (the variable is gender)The number of teachers <strong>in</strong> a large school district whouse (1) the lecture <strong>and</strong> (2) the discussion method (thevariable is teach<strong>in</strong>g method)The number of each type of <strong>to</strong>ol found <strong>in</strong> a workroom(the variable is type of <strong>to</strong>ol)The number of each k<strong>in</strong>d of merch<strong>and</strong>ise found <strong>in</strong> alarge department s<strong>to</strong>re (the variable is type of merch<strong>and</strong>ise)


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 187You may f<strong>in</strong>d it helpful at this po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> refer back <strong>to</strong>Figure 7.25 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7. The ord<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>in</strong>terval, <strong>and</strong> ratioscales all perta<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> quantitative data; the nom<strong>in</strong>al scaleperta<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong> categorical data.Techniques for Summariz<strong>in</strong>gQuantitative DataNote: None of the techniques <strong>in</strong> this section for summariz<strong>in</strong>gquantitative data are appropriate for categoricaldata; they are for use only with quantitative data.FREQUENCY POLYGONSListed below are the scores of a group of 50 students ona midsemester biology test.64, 27, 61, 56, 52, 51, 3, 15, 6, 34, 6, 17, 27, 17, 24,64, 31, 29, 31, 29, 31, 29, 29, 31, 31, 29, 61, 59, 56,34, 59, 51, 38, 38, 38, 38, 34, 36, 36, 34, 34, 36, 21,21, 24, 25, 27, 27, 27, 63<strong>How</strong> many students received a score of 34? Did mos<strong>to</strong>f the students receive a score above 50? <strong>How</strong> many receiveda score below 30? As you can see, when the dataare simply listed <strong>in</strong> no apparent order, as they are here,it is difficult <strong>to</strong> tell.To make any sense out of these data, we must put the <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> some sort of order. One of the most commonways <strong>to</strong> do this is <strong>to</strong> prepare a frequency distribution.This is done by list<strong>in</strong>g the scores <strong>in</strong> rank order fromhigh <strong>to</strong> low, with tallies <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the number of subjectsreceiv<strong>in</strong>g each score (Table 10.1). Often, the scores <strong>in</strong> adistribution are grouped <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervals. This results <strong>in</strong> agrouped frequency distribution, as shown <strong>in</strong>Table 10.2.Although frequency distributions like the ones <strong>in</strong>Tables 10.1 <strong>and</strong> 10.2 can be quite <strong>in</strong>formative, often the<strong>in</strong>formation they conta<strong>in</strong> is hard <strong>to</strong> visualize. To furtherthe underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of quantitative data,it is helpful <strong>to</strong> present it <strong>in</strong> a graph. One such graphicaldisplay is known as a frequency polygon. Figure 10.1presents a frequency polygon of the data <strong>in</strong> Table 10.2The steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> construct<strong>in</strong>g a frequencypolygon are as follows:1. List all scores <strong>in</strong> order of size, <strong>and</strong> tally how manystudents receive each score. Group scores, if necessary,<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervals.**Group<strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervals such as five or more is oftennecessary when there are many scores <strong>in</strong> the distribution. Generally,12 <strong>to</strong> 15 <strong>in</strong>tervals on the x-axis is recommended.TABLE 10.1Example of a Frequency Distribution aRaw ScoreFrequency64 263 161 259 256 252 151 238 436 334 531 529 527 525 124 221 217 215 16 23 1n 50a Technically, the table should <strong>in</strong>clude all scores, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those for whichthere are zero frequencies. We have elim<strong>in</strong>ated those <strong>to</strong> simplify thepresentation.TABLE 10.2Example of a Grouped FrequencyDistributionRaw Scores(Intervals of Five)Frequency60–64 555–59 450–54 345–49 040–44 035–39 730–34 1025–29 1120–24 415–19 310–14 05–9 20–4 1n 50


188 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eUs<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Construct Frequency Distribution TablesWhen you open the SPSS program, you will see a two-dimensional table that looks essentiallylike a spreadsheet. Each row <strong>in</strong> the table will represent a particular <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> your dataset.Each column will represent a specific variable <strong>in</strong> the dataset.To construct a frequency distribution table us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS, first enter your scores <strong>in</strong> one columnof the data table, <strong>and</strong> then click Analyze on the <strong>to</strong>olbar at the <strong>to</strong>p of the screen. When a menuappears, click on Descriptive Statistics <strong>and</strong> select Frequencies. A box conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g two smallerboxes will appear. Highlight the label <strong>in</strong> the left box for the column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the set of scores<strong>and</strong> then click the right-h<strong>and</strong> arrow <strong>to</strong> move the column label <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the right (“Variables”) box. Besure that the option “Display Frequency Table” is selected. Then click OK. The score values willbe listed <strong>in</strong> a column from smallest <strong>to</strong> largest, along with the percentage <strong>and</strong> cumulative percentagefor each score.Figure 10.1 Example ofa Frequency Polygon15141312111098765432100–45–910–1415–1920–2425–2930–3435–3940–4445–4950–5455–5960–6465–69FrequencyScore2. Label the horizontal axis by plac<strong>in</strong>g all of the possiblescores (or group<strong>in</strong>gs) on that axis, at equal <strong>in</strong>tervals,start<strong>in</strong>g with the lowest score on the left.3. Label the vertical axis by <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g frequencies, atequal <strong>in</strong>tervals, start<strong>in</strong>g with zero.4. For each score (or group<strong>in</strong>g of scores), f<strong>in</strong>d the po<strong>in</strong>twhere it <strong>in</strong>tersects with its frequency of occurrence,<strong>and</strong> place a dot at that po<strong>in</strong>t. Remember that eachscore (or group<strong>in</strong>g of scores) with zero frequencymust still be plotted.5. Connect all the dots with a straight l<strong>in</strong>e.As you can see by look<strong>in</strong>g at Figure 10.1, the factthat a large number of the students scored <strong>in</strong> the middleof this distribution is illustrated quite nicely.**A common mistake of students is <strong>to</strong> treat the vertical axis as ifthe numbers represented specific <strong>in</strong>dividuals. They do not. Theyrepresent frequencies. Each number on the vertical axis is used <strong>to</strong>plot the number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals at each score. In Figure 10.1, the dotabove the <strong>in</strong>terval 25–29 shows that 11 persons scored somewherewith<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terval 25–29.


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 189Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Draw a His<strong>to</strong>gram or Bar GraphTo draw a his<strong>to</strong>gram or bar graph, enter your scores <strong>in</strong> one column of the data table, clickAnalyze on the <strong>to</strong>olbar, then select Descriptive Statistics <strong>and</strong> then Frequencies. Highlightthe label <strong>in</strong> the left box for the column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the set of scores, <strong>and</strong> then move the column label<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Variables” box <strong>and</strong> click Charts. If the scale of measurement for your data is nom<strong>in</strong>al,select Bar Charts; if it is <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio, select His<strong>to</strong>grams. Then click Cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>and</strong> thenOK. A frequency distribution <strong>and</strong> a graph will be displayed. (Note: SPSS will au<strong>to</strong>maticallygroup scores <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> class <strong>in</strong>tervals if the range of scores is <strong>to</strong>o wide.)SKEWED POLYGONSData can be distributed <strong>in</strong> almost any shape. If aresearcher obta<strong>in</strong>s a set of data <strong>in</strong> which many <strong>in</strong>dividualsreceived low scores, for example, the shape ofthe distribution would look someth<strong>in</strong>g like the frequencypolygon shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.2. As you can see, <strong>in</strong> thisparticular distribution, only a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals receivedthe higher scores. The frequency polygon <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.2is said <strong>to</strong> be positively skewed because the tail of the distributiontrails off <strong>to</strong> the right, <strong>in</strong> the direction of thehigher (more positive) score values. Suppose the reversewere true. Imag<strong>in</strong>e that a researcher obta<strong>in</strong>ed a set ofdata <strong>in</strong> which few <strong>in</strong>dividuals received relatively lowscores. Then the shape of the distribution would looklike the frequency polygon <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.3. This polygonis said <strong>to</strong> be negatively skewed, s<strong>in</strong>ce the longer tail ofthe distribution goes off <strong>to</strong> the left.Frequency polygons are particularly useful <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>gtwo (or sometimes more) groups. In Chapter 7,Table 7.3, we presented some hypothetical results of astudy <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a comparison of two counsel<strong>in</strong>g methods.Figure 10.4 on page 190 shows the polygons constructedus<strong>in</strong>g the data from Table 7.3.This figure reveals several important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. First,it is evident that method B resulted <strong>in</strong> higher scores,overall, than did method A. Second, it is clear thatthe scores for method B are more spread out. Third, itis clear that the reason for method B be<strong>in</strong>g higher overallis not that there are fewer scores at the low end ofthe scale (although this might have happened, it didnot). In fact, the groups are almost identical <strong>in</strong> thenumber of scores below 61: A10, B12. The reasonmethod B is higher overall is that there were fewer cases<strong>in</strong> the middle range of the scores (between 60 <strong>and</strong> 75),<strong>and</strong> more cases above 75. If this is not clear <strong>to</strong> you,study the shaded areas <strong>in</strong> the figure. Many times wewant <strong>to</strong> know not only which group is higher overall butalso where the differences are. In this example we seethat method B resulted <strong>in</strong> more variability <strong>and</strong> that itresulted <strong>in</strong> a substantial number of scores higher thanthose <strong>in</strong> method A.HISTOGRAMS AND STEM-LEAF PLOTSA his<strong>to</strong>gram is a bar graph used <strong>to</strong> display quantitativedata at the <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio level of measurement. Thebars are arranged <strong>in</strong> order from left <strong>to</strong> right on the horizontalaxis, <strong>and</strong> the widths of the bars <strong>in</strong>dicate the rangeof values that fall with<strong>in</strong> each bar. Frequencies are shownon the vertical axis, <strong>and</strong> the po<strong>in</strong>t of <strong>in</strong>tersection of thetwo axes is always zero. Furthermore, the bars <strong>in</strong> a his<strong>to</strong>gram(<strong>in</strong> contrast <strong>to</strong> those <strong>in</strong> a bar graph) <strong>to</strong>uch, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gthat they illustrate quantitative rather than categoricaldata. Figure 10.5 is a his<strong>to</strong>gram of the data presented <strong>in</strong>the grouped frequency distribution shown <strong>in</strong> Table 10.2.A stem-leaf plot is a display that organizes a set ofdata <strong>to</strong> show both its shape <strong>and</strong> its distribution. Eachdata value is split <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a “stem” <strong>and</strong> a “leaf.” The leaf isusually the last digit of the number, <strong>and</strong> the other digits<strong>to</strong> the left of the leaf form the stem. For example, thenumber 149 would be splitstem 14leaf 9Let us construct a stem-leaf plot for the follow<strong>in</strong>g se<strong>to</strong>f scores on a math quiz: 29, 37, 32, 46, 45, 45, 54, 51,55, 55, 55, 60. First, separate each number <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a stem<strong>and</strong> a leaf. S<strong>in</strong>ce these are two-digit numbers, the tensdigit is the stem <strong>and</strong> the units digit is the leaf. Next,group the numbers with the same stems as shownbelow, list<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong> numerical order:Math Quiz ScoresStem Leaf2 93 724 6555 415556 0


190 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 10.2 Exampleof a Positively SkewedPolygonFrequency1312111098765432100 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34ScoreFigure 10.3 Exampleof a Negatively SkewedPolygonFrequency1312111098765432100 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34ScoreFigure 10.4 TwoFrequency PolygonsComparedFrequency9876543210036–4041–4546–5051–5556–6061–6566–7071–7576–8081–8586–9091–9596–100Method AMethod BScore


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 19112111098Frequency765432100–45–910–1415–1920–2425–2930–3435–3940–4445–4950–5455–5960–64Figure 10.5 His<strong>to</strong>gram of Data <strong>in</strong> Table 10.2ScoresF<strong>in</strong>ally, reorder the leaf values <strong>in</strong> sequence:Math Quiz ScoresStem Leaf2 93 274 5565 145556 0One advantage that a stem-leaf plot has over a his<strong>to</strong>gramis that it shows not only the frequency of valueswith<strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>terval but also reveals all of the <strong>in</strong>dividualvalues with<strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>terval.Stem-leaf plots are particularly helpful <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> contrast<strong>in</strong>g two distributions. For example, follow<strong>in</strong>gis a back-<strong>to</strong>-back stem-leaf plot compar<strong>in</strong>g thehome runs hit by Babe Ruth dur<strong>in</strong>g his years with theNew York Yankees with those hit by Mark McGuiredur<strong>in</strong>g his years with the St. Louis Card<strong>in</strong>als.Babe Ruth Mark McGuire0 9,915,2 2 25,4 3 2,3,9,99,7,6,6,6,1,1 4 2,99,4,4 5 2,80 6 57 0Who do you th<strong>in</strong>k was the better home run hitter?<strong>How</strong> would Barry Bonds of the San Francisco Giants,who hit 73 home runs <strong>in</strong> 2003, compare overall?THE NORMAL CURVEOften researchers draw a smooth curve <strong>in</strong>stead of theseries of straight l<strong>in</strong>es <strong>in</strong> a frequency polygon. Thesmooth curve suggests that we are not just connect<strong>in</strong>ga series of dots (that is, the actual frequencies of scores<strong>in</strong> a particular distribution), but rather show<strong>in</strong>g a


192 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eus very much about a distribution, however, it is notused very often <strong>in</strong> educational research.Figure 10.6 The Normal Curvegeneralized distribution of scores that is not limited <strong>to</strong>one specific set of data. These smooth curves are knownas distribution curves.Many distributions of data tend <strong>to</strong> follow a certa<strong>in</strong>specific shape of distribution curve called a normaldistribution. When a distribution curve is normal, thelarge majority of the scores are concentrated <strong>in</strong> the middleof the distribution, <strong>and</strong> the scores decrease <strong>in</strong> frequencythe farther away from the middle they are, asshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.6.The normal curve is based on a precise mathematicalequation. As you can see, it is symmetrical <strong>and</strong> bellshaped.The distribution of some human characteristics,such as height <strong>and</strong> weight, approximate such a curve,while many others, such as spatial ability, manual dexterity,<strong>and</strong> creativity, are often assumed <strong>to</strong> do so. Thenormal curve is very useful <strong>to</strong> researchers, <strong>and</strong> we shalldiscuss it <strong>in</strong> more detail later <strong>in</strong> the chapter.AVERAGESAverages, or measures of central tendency, enable aresearcher <strong>to</strong> summarize the data <strong>in</strong> a frequency distributionwith a s<strong>in</strong>gle number. The three most commonlyused averages are the mode, the median, <strong>and</strong> the mean.Each represents a type of average or typical score atta<strong>in</strong>edby a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals on some measure.The Mode. The mode is the most frequent score <strong>in</strong> adistribution—that is, the score atta<strong>in</strong>ed by more studentsthan any other score. In the follow<strong>in</strong>g distribution,what is the mode?25, 20, 19, 17, 16, 16, 16,14, 14, 11, 10, 9, 9The mode is 16. What about this distribution?25, 24, 24, 23, 22, 20, 19, 19, 18, 11, 10This distribution (called a bimodal distribution) has twomodes, 24 <strong>and</strong> 19. Because the mode really doesn’t tellThe Median. The median is the po<strong>in</strong>t below <strong>and</strong>above which 50 percent of the scores <strong>in</strong> a distributionfall—<strong>in</strong> short, the midpo<strong>in</strong>t. In a distribution that conta<strong>in</strong>san uneven number of scores, the median is the middlemostscore (provided that the scores are listed <strong>in</strong>order). Thus, <strong>in</strong> the distribution 5, 4, 3, 2, 1, the medianis 3. In a distribution that conta<strong>in</strong>s an even number ofscores, the median is the po<strong>in</strong>t halfway between the twomiddlemost scores. Thus, <strong>in</strong> the distribution 70, 74, 82,86, 88, 90, the median is 84. Hence, the median is notnecessarily one of the actual scores <strong>in</strong> the distributionbe<strong>in</strong>g summarized.Note that two very different distributions might havethe same median, as shown below:Distribution A: 98, 90, 84, 82, 76Distribution B: 90, 87, 84, 65, 41In both distributions, the median is 84.It may look like the median is fairly easy <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e.This is usually the case with ungrouped data. Forgrouped data, calculat<strong>in</strong>g the median requires somewhatmore work. It can, however, be estimated by locat<strong>in</strong>g thescore that has half of the area under the frequency polygonabove it <strong>and</strong> half below it.The median is the most appropriate average <strong>to</strong> calculatewhen the data result <strong>in</strong> skewed distributions.The Mean. The mean is another average of all thescores <strong>in</strong> a distribution.* It is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by add<strong>in</strong>g upall of the scores <strong>and</strong> then divid<strong>in</strong>g this sum by the <strong>to</strong>talnumber of scores. The mean of a distribution conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gscores of 52, 68, 74, 86, 95, <strong>and</strong> 105, therefore, is 80.<strong>How</strong> did we determ<strong>in</strong>e this? We simply added up all thescores, which came <strong>to</strong> 480, <strong>and</strong> then divided this sumby 6, the <strong>to</strong>tal number of scores. In symbolic form, theformula for comput<strong>in</strong>g the mean looks like this:X ©Xnwhere Σ represents “sum of,” X represents any rawscore value, n represents the <strong>to</strong>tal number of scores, <strong>and</strong>X – represents the mean.*Actually, there are several k<strong>in</strong>ds of means (geometric, harmonic,etc.), but their use is specialized <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>frequent. We refer here <strong>to</strong> thearithmetic mean.


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 193TABLE 10.3Example of the Mode, Median,<strong>and</strong> Mean <strong>in</strong> a DistributionRaw ScoreFrequency98 197 191 285 180 577 772 565 364 762 1058 345 233 111 15 1n 50Mode = 62; median = 64.5; mean = 66.7Table 10.3 presents a frequency distribution of scoreson a test <strong>and</strong> each of the above measures of centraltendency. As you can see, each of these <strong>in</strong>dices tells ussometh<strong>in</strong>g a little different. The most frequent scorewas 62, but would we want <strong>to</strong> say that this was the most“Poor Tony. Hecalculated the averagedepth only!”typical score? Probably not. The median of the scoreswas 64.5. The mean was 66.7. Perhaps the mean is thebest description of the distribution of scores, but it, <strong>to</strong>o,is not <strong>to</strong>tally satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry because the distribution isskewed. Table 10.3 shows that these <strong>in</strong>dices are onlysummaries of all the scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution <strong>and</strong> oftendo not have the same value. They are not <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong>show variation or spread (Figure 10.7).Which of the three averages (measures of central tendency),then, is best? It depends. The mean is the onlyone of the three that uses all the <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> a distribution,s<strong>in</strong>ce every score is used <strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g it, <strong>and</strong> it isgenerally preferred over the other two measures. <strong>How</strong>ever,it tends <strong>to</strong> be unduly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by extreme scores.(Can you see why?) On occasion, therefore, the mediangives a more accurate <strong>in</strong>dication of the typical score <strong>in</strong>a distribution. Suppose, for example, that the yearlysalaries earned by various workers <strong>in</strong> a small bus<strong>in</strong>esswere as shown <strong>in</strong> Table 10.4.The mean of these salaries is $75,000. Would it becorrect <strong>to</strong> say that this is the average yearly salary paid <strong>in</strong>this company? Obviously it would not. The extremelyhigh salary paid <strong>to</strong> the owner of the company “<strong>in</strong>flates”the mean, so <strong>to</strong> speak. Us<strong>in</strong>g it as a summary figure <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatethe average yearly salary would give an erroneousimpression. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, the median would be themore appropriate average <strong>to</strong> calculate, s<strong>in</strong>ce it would notbe as affected by the owner’s salary. The median is$27,000, a far more accurate <strong>in</strong>dication of the typicalsalary for the year.Figure 10.7 AveragesCan Be Mislead<strong>in</strong>g!“Yep.”9 FEET 9 FEET3 FEET 3 FEET


194 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 10.4SPREADSYearly Salaries of Workers<strong>in</strong> a Small Bus<strong>in</strong>essMr. Davis $ 10,500Mr. Thompson 20,000Ms. Angelo 22,500Mr. Schmidt 24,000Ms. Wills 26,000Ms. Brown 28,000Mr. Greene 36,000Mr. Adams 43,000Ms. Frankl<strong>in</strong> 65,000Mr. Payson (owner) 475,000While measures of central tendency are useful statisticsfor summariz<strong>in</strong>g the scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution, they are notsufficient. Two distributions may have identical means<strong>and</strong> medians, for example, yet be quite different <strong>in</strong> otherways. For example, consider these two distributions:Distribution A: 19, 20, 25, 32, 39Distribution B: 2, 3, 25, 30, 75The mean <strong>in</strong> both of these distributions is 27, <strong>and</strong> themedian <strong>in</strong> both is 25. Yet you can see that the distributionsdiffer considerably. In distribution A, the scoresare closer <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>and</strong> tend <strong>to</strong> cluster around the mean.In distribution B, they are much more spread out. Hencethe two distributions differ <strong>in</strong> what statisticians callvariability. Figure 10.8 illustrates further examples.Thus, measures of central tendency, when presentedwithout any accompany<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation of how spreadout or dispersed the data are, can be mislead<strong>in</strong>g. Tosay that the average annual <strong>in</strong>come of all the players<strong>in</strong> the National Basketball Association <strong>in</strong> 1998 was$275,000.00 hides the fact that some players earned farless while someone like Michael Jordan earned morethan 5 million. The distribution of players’ salaries wasskewed <strong>to</strong> the right <strong>and</strong> very spread out. Know<strong>in</strong>g onlythe mean gives us an <strong>in</strong>adequate description of thedistribution of salaries for players <strong>in</strong> the NBA.There is a need, therefore, for measures researchers canuse <strong>to</strong> describe the spread, or variability, that exists with<strong>in</strong>a distribution. Let us consider three—the <strong>in</strong>terquartilerange, the overall range, <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation.Quartiles <strong>and</strong> the Five-Number Summary.When a distribution is skewed, both the variability <strong>and</strong>the general shape of the distribution can be described byreport<strong>in</strong>g several percentiles. We mentioned percentilesearlier <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7. A percentile <strong>in</strong> a set of numbers is avalue below which a certa<strong>in</strong> percentage of the numbersfall <strong>and</strong> above which the rest of the numbers fall.You may have encountered percentiles if you haveever taken a st<strong>and</strong>ardized test such as the SAT <strong>and</strong> receiveda report say<strong>in</strong>g “Raw score 630, percentile 84.”You received a score of 630, but perhaps more useful isthe fact that 84 percent of those who <strong>to</strong>ok the exam<strong>in</strong>ationscored lower than you did.The median is the 50th percentile. Other percentilesthat are important are the 25th percentile, also known asthe first quartile (Q 1 ), <strong>and</strong> the 75th percentile, the thirdquartile (Q 3 ). A useful way <strong>to</strong> describe a skewed distribution,therefore, is <strong>to</strong> give what is known as a fivenumbersummary, consist<strong>in</strong>g of the lowest score, Q 1 ,the median, Q 3 , <strong>and</strong> the highest score. The <strong>in</strong>ter-quartilerange (IQR) is the difference between the third <strong>and</strong> firstquartiles (Q 3 Q 1 IQR).Boxplots. The five-number summary of a distributioncan be graphically portrayed by means of a boxplot.Boxplots are especially useful <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g two or moredistributions. Figure 10.9 gives boxplots for the distributionsof the midterm scores of two classes tak<strong>in</strong>g thesame biology exam. Each central box has its ends at thequartiles, <strong>and</strong> the median is marked by the l<strong>in</strong>e with<strong>in</strong>the box. The “whiskers” at either end extend <strong>to</strong> the lowest<strong>and</strong> highest scores.*Figure 10.9 permits an immediate comparison betweenthe two classes. Overall, class B did better, but*Boxplots are sometimes called box-<strong>and</strong>-whiskers diagrams.Figure 10.8 DifferentDistributions Comparedwith Respect <strong>to</strong> Averages<strong>and</strong> SpreadsMeanSame average,different spreadMeanMeanSame spread,different average


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 195Class AClass B“I don’t th<strong>in</strong>k I can make it<strong>to</strong> class <strong>to</strong>day, Dr. Roberts. I’m feel<strong>in</strong>ga couple of st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationsbelow the mean.”Figure 10.9 Boxplots45 50 55 60 6570 75 80 85the upper whiskers illustrate that class A had thestudent with the highest score. Figure 10.9 is but anotherexample of how effectively graphs can convey<strong>in</strong>formation.Though the five-number summary is an extremelyuseful numerical description of a distribution, it is notthe most common. That accolade belongs <strong>to</strong> a comb<strong>in</strong>ationof the mean (a measure of center) <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation (a measure of spread). The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation<strong>and</strong> its brother, the variance, measure the spread ofscores from the mean. They should only be used <strong>in</strong> conjunctionwith the mean.The Range. The overall range represents the distancebetween the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution.Thus, if the highest score <strong>in</strong> a distribution is 89<strong>and</strong> the lowest is 11, the range would be 89–11, or 78.Because it <strong>in</strong>volves only the two most extreme scores<strong>in</strong> a distribution, the range is but a crude <strong>in</strong>dication ofvariability. Its ma<strong>in</strong> advantage is that it gives a quick(although rough) estimate of variability.The St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviation. The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation(SD) is the most useful <strong>in</strong>dex of variability. It is a s<strong>in</strong>glenumber that represents the spread of a distribution. Aswith the mean, every score <strong>in</strong> the distribution is used <strong>to</strong>calculate it. The steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation are straightforward.1. Calculate the mean of the distribution.X ©Xn2. Subtract the mean from each score. Each result issymbolized X X – .3. Square each of these scores (X X – ) 2 .4. Add up all the squares of these scores:Σ(X X – ) 2 .5. Divide the <strong>to</strong>tal by the number of scores. The resultis called the variance.6. Take the square root of the variance. This is the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation.The above steps can be summarized as follows:©(X X) 2SD C nwhere SD is the symbol for st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, Σ is thesymbol for “sum of,” X is the symbol for a raw score, X –is the symbol for the mean, <strong>and</strong> n represents the numberof scores <strong>in</strong> the distribution.This procedure sounds more complicated than it is. Itreally is not difficult <strong>to</strong> calculate. Table 10.5 illustratesthe calculation of the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a distributionof 10 scores.You will notice that the more spread out scores are,the greater the deviation scores will be <strong>and</strong> hence thelarger the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. The closer the scores are <strong>to</strong>the mean, the less spread out they are <strong>and</strong> hence thesmaller the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Thus, if we were describ<strong>in</strong>gtwo sets of scores on the same test <strong>and</strong> we stated thatthe st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the scores <strong>in</strong> set 1 was 2.7 <strong>and</strong>the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>in</strong> set 2 was 8.3, we would knowthat there was much less variability <strong>in</strong> set 1—that is, thescores were closer <strong>to</strong>gether.


196 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 10.5Calculation of the St<strong>and</strong>ardDeviation of a DistributionRawScore Mean X X – (X X – ) 285 54 31 96180 54 26 67670 54 16 25660 54 6 3655 54 1 150 54 4 1645 54 9 8140 54 14 19630 54 24 57625 54 29 8413640Variance (SD 2 ©(X X)2) n 364010 364aSt<strong>and</strong>ard deviation (SD) A©(X X) 2a The symbol for the variance of a sample sometimes is shown as s 2 ;thesymbol for the variance of a population is 2 .b The symbol for the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a sample sometimes isshown as s; the symbol for the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a population is .Boys’basketball teamSD = 5.25n 2364 19.08 bAn important po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationis that if a distribution is normal, then the mean plus orm<strong>in</strong>us three st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations will encompass about99 percent of all the scores <strong>in</strong> the distribution. For example,if the mean of a distribution is 72 <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation is 3, then just about 99 percent of thescores <strong>in</strong> the distribution are somewhere between scoresof 63 <strong>and</strong> 81. Figure 10.10 provides an illustration ofst<strong>and</strong>ard deviation.The St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviation of a NormalDistribution. The <strong>to</strong>tal area under the normal curverepresents all of the scores <strong>in</strong> a normal distribution. Insuch a curve, the mean, median, <strong>and</strong> mode are identical,so the mean falls at the exact center of the curve. It thusis also the most frequent score <strong>in</strong> the distribution. Becausethe curve is symmetrical, 50 percent of the scoresmust fall on each side of the mean.Here are some important facts about the normaldistribution:• Fifty percent of all the observations (e.g., scores) fallon each side of the mean (Figure 10.11).• In any normal distribution, 68 percent of the scoresfall with<strong>in</strong> one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the mean. Halfof these (34 percent) fall with<strong>in</strong> one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationabove the mean <strong>and</strong> the other half with<strong>in</strong> onest<strong>and</strong>ard deviation below the mean.Men’sbasketball teamSD = 2.4554" 57" 61" 64" 69" 72" 73" 75" 76" 79"Figure 10.10 St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations for Boys’ <strong>and</strong> Men’s Basketball Teams


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 197–3 –2 –150% 50%01 2Figure 10.11 Fifty Percent of All Scores <strong>in</strong> a NormalCurve Fall on Each Side of the Mean2.15%0.13%13.5%34%68%95%99.7%34%13.5%–3 SD –2 SD –1 SD Mean 1 SD 2 SD 3 SD• Another 27 percent of the observations fall betweenone <strong>and</strong> two st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations away from themean. Hence 95 percent (68 percent plus 27 percent)fall with<strong>in</strong> two st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations of the mean.• In all, 99.7 percent of the observations fall with<strong>in</strong>three st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations of the mean. Figure 10.12illustrates all three of these facts, often referred <strong>to</strong> asthe 68-95-99.7 rule.Hence we see that almost all of the scores <strong>in</strong> a normaldistribution lie between the mean <strong>and</strong> plus or m<strong>in</strong>us threest<strong>and</strong>ard deviations. Only .13 percent of all the scoresfall above 3 SD, <strong>and</strong> .13 percent fall below 3 SD.If a set of scores is normally distributed, we can <strong>in</strong>terpretany particular score if we know how far, <strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation units, it is from the mean. Suppose, forexample, the mean of a normal distribution is 100 <strong>and</strong>the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation is 15. A score that lies one st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation above the mean, therefore, would equal115. A score that lies one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation below themean would equal 85. What would a score that lies 1.5st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations above the mean equal?*32.15%0.13%Figure 10.12 Percentages under the Normal Curve*122.5We also can determ<strong>in</strong>e how a particular <strong>in</strong>dividual’sscore compares with all the other scores <strong>in</strong> a normal distribution.For example, if a person’s score lies exactlyone st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation above the mean, then we knowthat slightly more than 84 percent of all the other scores<strong>in</strong> the distribution lie below his or her score.† If a distributionis normal <strong>and</strong> we know the mean <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation of the distribution, we can determ<strong>in</strong>e thepercentage of scores that lie above <strong>and</strong> below any givenscore (see Figure 10.12). This is one of the most usefulcharacteristics of the normal distribution.STANDARD SCORESAND THE NORMAL CURVE<strong>Research</strong>ers often are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> see<strong>in</strong>g how one person’sscore compares with another’s. As we mentioned<strong>in</strong> Chapter 7, <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e this, researchers often convertraw scores <strong>to</strong> derived scores. We described twotypes of derived scores—age/grade equivalents <strong>and</strong>percentile ranks—<strong>in</strong> that chapter, but mentioned anothertype—st<strong>and</strong>ard scores—only briefly. We discussthem now <strong>in</strong> somewhat more detail, s<strong>in</strong>ce they are veryuseful.St<strong>and</strong>ard scores use a common scale <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatehow an <strong>in</strong>dividual compares <strong>to</strong> other <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> agroup. These scores are particularly helpful <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>gan <strong>in</strong>dividual’s relative position on different <strong>in</strong>struments.The two st<strong>and</strong>ard scores that are most frequentlyused <strong>in</strong> educational research are z scores <strong>and</strong> Tscores.z Scores. The simplest form of st<strong>and</strong>ard score is the zscore. It expresses how far a raw score is from the mean<strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation units. A raw score that is exactlyon the mean corresponds <strong>to</strong> a z score of zero; a rawscore that is exactly one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation above themean equals a z score of 1, while a raw score that isexactly one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation below the mean equals az score of 1. Similarly, a raw score that is exactly twost<strong>and</strong>ard deviations above the mean equals a z score of2, <strong>and</strong> so forth. One z, therefore, equals one st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation (1 z 1 SD), 2 z 2 SD, 0.5 z 0.5 SD,†Fifty percent of the scores <strong>in</strong> the distribution must lie below themean; 34 percent must lie between the mean <strong>and</strong> 1 SD. Therefore84 percent (50 percent 34 percent) of the scores <strong>in</strong> the distributionmust be below 1 SD.


198 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eUs<strong>in</strong>g Excel <strong>to</strong>A B C1 452 563 764 875 886 617 348 679 5510 8811 9212 8513 7814 8415 7716 71.5317 7718 17.7Calculate the Mean, Median, <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviationof a DistributionExcel is another computer program that can be used <strong>to</strong> calculate many descriptive statistics. Simplyclick on an empty cell, <strong>and</strong> then click on Function under the Insert menu. This will open adialogue box. You can search for a function, such Average, Median, or St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviation.Enter the function you wish <strong>to</strong> search for <strong>in</strong> the Search for a function box, <strong>and</strong> click Go. Excelwill present you with a number of possible matches from which <strong>to</strong> choose. You may have <strong>to</strong> accessthe help files <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish between similar functions (e.g., STDEV is the sample st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation, while STDEVP is the population st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation). Click on the function you wish<strong>to</strong> use, <strong>and</strong> then click OK. This will br<strong>in</strong>g up the Function Arguments dialogue box. In this boxyou can enter the <strong>in</strong>dividual cell numbers you wish <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> the calculation, or you can highlighta group of adjo<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g cells on the spreadsheet by click<strong>in</strong>g on the first cell <strong>and</strong> dragg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> thelast cell. F<strong>in</strong>ally, click OK on the Function Argument dialogue box. Excel will calculate the result<strong>and</strong> enter it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the (formerly) empty cell on the spreadsheet that you began with.As you learn the names of functions you commonly use, you can enter them directly, without resort<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> the Insert <strong>and</strong> Function procedure described above. We did this <strong>in</strong> the example <strong>to</strong> the left.First we opened an Excel spreadsheet <strong>and</strong> then listed the data <strong>in</strong> cells A1 through A15. We thenclicked on the cells where we wanted the statistics <strong>to</strong> appear <strong>and</strong> typed <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g comm<strong>and</strong>s:• To obta<strong>in</strong> the mean: AVERAGE(A1:A15)*• To obta<strong>in</strong> the median: MEDIAN(A1:A15)• To obta<strong>in</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation: STDEV(A1:A15)We typed <strong>in</strong> these comm<strong>and</strong>s <strong>in</strong> cells A16, A17, <strong>and</strong> A18, <strong>and</strong> hit “Return” each time.The correspond<strong>in</strong>g values† then appeared <strong>in</strong> those cells: 71.53 (the mean), 77 (the median),<strong>and</strong> 17.7 (the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation).*Note: All comm<strong>and</strong>s must always beg<strong>in</strong> with an equal sign () before typ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the name of the formula<strong>and</strong> the list <strong>to</strong> which the formula is <strong>to</strong> be applied. You must then hit “Return,” <strong>and</strong> the correspond<strong>in</strong>g valuewill appear <strong>in</strong> the designated cell.†Shortened <strong>to</strong> two decimal po<strong>in</strong>ts.Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Calculate the Mean <strong>and</strong> St<strong>and</strong>ard DeviationTo use SPSS <strong>to</strong> calculate the mean for a set of scores, first enter the scores <strong>in</strong> one column of the datatable <strong>and</strong> then click Analyze on the <strong>to</strong>olbar. When the menu appears, click on DescriptiveStatistics <strong>and</strong> select Descriptives. Highlight the label <strong>in</strong> the left box for the column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe set of scores <strong>and</strong> move it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Variables” box. Then click OK. SPSS will produce a summarytable list<strong>in</strong>g the maximum score, the m<strong>in</strong>imum score, the mean, <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation.<strong>and</strong> so on (Figure 10.13). Thus, if the mean of a distributionwas 50 <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation was 2, a rawscore of 52 would equal a z score of 1, a raw score of46 would equal a z score of 2, <strong>and</strong> so forth.A big advantage of z scores is that they allow rawscores on different tests <strong>to</strong> be compared. For example,suppose a student received raw scores of 60 on a biologytest <strong>and</strong> 80 on a chemistry test. A naive observer might be<strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fer, at first glance, that the student wasdo<strong>in</strong>g better <strong>in</strong> chemistry than <strong>in</strong> biology. But this mightbe unwise, for how “well” the student is do<strong>in</strong>g comparativelycannot be determ<strong>in</strong>ed until we know the mean <strong>and</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation for each distribution of scores. Let usfurther suppose that the mean on the biology test was 50,


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 199z score–3 z–3 SD–2 z–2 SD–1 z–1 SD0Mean1 z1 SD2 z2 SD3 z3 SDFigure 10.13 z Scores Associated with the Normal Curve.0215 .3413 .3413.0215.0013.1359 .1359.0013–3 SD –2 SD –1 SD 0 1 SD 2 SD 3 SDMeanFigure 10.14 Probabilities under the Normal CurveTABLE 10.6Comparisons of Raw Scores<strong>and</strong> z Scores on Two TestsRawPercentileTest Score Mean SD z Score RankBiology 60 50 5 2 98Chemistry 80 90 10 1 16but on the chemistry test it was 90. Also assume that thest<strong>and</strong>ard deviation on the biology test was 5, but on thechemistry test it was 10. What does this tell us? The student’sraw score <strong>in</strong> biology (60) is actually two st<strong>and</strong>arddeviations above the mean (a z score of 2), whereas hisraw score <strong>in</strong> chemistry (80) is one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationbelow the mean (a z score of 1). Rather than do<strong>in</strong>g better<strong>in</strong> chemistry, as the raw scores by themselves suggest,the student is actually do<strong>in</strong>g better <strong>in</strong> biology. Table 10.6compares both the raw scores, the z scores, <strong>and</strong> the percentilerank of the student on both tests.Of course, z scores are not always exactly one or twost<strong>and</strong>ard deviations away from the mean. Actually, researchersapply the follow<strong>in</strong>g formula <strong>to</strong> convert a rawscore <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a z score.z score Thus, for a raw score of 80, a mean of 65, <strong>and</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation of 12, the z score will be:z araw score meanst<strong>and</strong>ard deviation80 65b 1.2512Probability <strong>and</strong> z Scores. Another importantcharacteristic of the normal distribution is that the percentagesassociated with areas under the curve can bethought of as probabilities. A probability is a percentstated <strong>in</strong> decimal form <strong>and</strong> refers <strong>to</strong> the likelihood of anevent occurr<strong>in</strong>g. For example, if there is a probabilitythat an event will occur 25 percent of the time, this eventcan be said <strong>to</strong> have a probability of .25. Similarly, anevent that will probably occur 90 percent of the time issaid <strong>to</strong> have a probability of .90. All of the percentagesassociated with areas under a normal curve, therefore,can be expressed <strong>in</strong> decimal form <strong>and</strong> viewed as probabilitystatements. Some of these probabilities are shown<strong>in</strong> Figure 10.14.Consider<strong>in</strong>g the area under the normal curve <strong>in</strong> termsof probabilities is very helpful <strong>to</strong> a researcher. Let usconsider an example. We have previously shown thatapproximately 34 percent of the scores <strong>in</strong> a normal distributionlie between the mean <strong>and</strong> 1 SD. Because50 percent of the scores fall above the mean, roughly16 percent of the scores must therefore lie above 1 SD(50 34 16). Now, if we express 16 percent <strong>in</strong> decimalform <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret it as a probability, we can saythat the probability of r<strong>and</strong>omly select<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividualfrom the population who has a score of 1 SD or moreabove the mean is .16. Usually this is written as p 16,with the p mean<strong>in</strong>g probability. Similarly, we can determ<strong>in</strong>ethe probability of r<strong>and</strong>omly select<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividualwho has a score ly<strong>in</strong>g at or below 2 SD or lower,or between 1 SD <strong>and</strong> 1 SD, <strong>and</strong> so on. Figure 10.14shows that the probability of select<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividualwho has a score lower than 2 SD is p .0228, orroughly 2 <strong>in</strong> 100. The probability of r<strong>and</strong>omly select<strong>in</strong>gan <strong>in</strong>dividual who has a score between 1 SD <strong>and</strong> 1SD is p .6826, <strong>and</strong> so forth.Statistical tables exist (see part of one <strong>in</strong> Appendix B)that give the proportion of scores associated with anyparticular z score <strong>in</strong> the normal distribution (e.g., for z 1.10, the proportion [i.e., the probability] for the area betweenthe mean of z 0 <strong>and</strong> z 1.10 is .3643, <strong>and</strong> for thearea beyond z, it is .1357). Hence, a researcher can be veryprecise <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g the position of any particular score


200 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ez scoreAArea betweenmean <strong>and</strong> z.0000.0987.1915.3413.4332.4750.4772.4938.4951.4987.4998.49997BAreabeyond zMeanAzB0.000.250.501.001.501.962.002.502.583.003.504.00.0000.4013.3085.1587.0668.0250.0228.0062.0049.0013.0002.00003Figure 10.15 Table Show<strong>in</strong>g Probability Areas Between the Mean <strong>and</strong> Different z Scoresrelative <strong>to</strong> other scores <strong>in</strong> a normal distribution. Figure10.15 shows a portion of such a table (see Appendix B).T Scores. Raw scores that are below the mean of adistribution convert <strong>to</strong> negative z scores. This is somewhatawkward. One way <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate negative z scoresis <strong>to</strong> convert them <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> T scores. T scores are simply zscores expressed <strong>in</strong> a different form. To change a z score<strong>to</strong> a T score, we simply multiply the z score by 10<strong>and</strong> add 50. Thus, a z score of 1 equals a T score of 60(1 10 10; 10 50 60). A z score of 2 equals aT score of 30 (2 10 20; 20 50 30). A zscore of zero (which is the equivalent of the mean of theraw scores) equals a T score of 50. You should see that adistribution of T scores has a mean of 50 <strong>and</strong> a st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation of 10. If you th<strong>in</strong>k about it, you should alsosee that a T score of 50 equals the 50th percentile.When a researcher knows, or can assume, that a distributionof scores is normal, both T <strong>and</strong> z scores can be<strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> terms of percentile ranks because there isthen a direct relationship between the two. Figure 10.16illustrates this relationship. There are other systemssimilar <strong>to</strong> T scores, which differ only <strong>in</strong> the choice ofvalues for the mean <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. Two of themost common, those used with the Graduate RecordExam<strong>in</strong>ation (X – 500, SD 100) <strong>and</strong> the Wechsler IntelligenceScales (X – 100, SD 15), are also illustrated<strong>in</strong> Figure 10.16.The Importance of the Normal Curve <strong>and</strong>z Scores. You may have noticed that the preced<strong>in</strong>gdiscussion of the use of z scores, percentages, <strong>and</strong> probabilities<strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> the normal curve was always qualifiedby the words “if or when the distribution of scores isnormal.” You should recall that z scores can be calculatedregardless of the shape of the distribution of orig<strong>in</strong>alscores. But it is only when the distribution is normal thatthe conversion <strong>to</strong> percentages or probabilities as describedabove is legitimate. Fortunately, many distributionsdo approximate the normal curve. This is mostlikely when a sample is chosen r<strong>and</strong>omly from a broadlydef<strong>in</strong>ed population. (It would be very unlikely, for example,with achievement scores <strong>in</strong> a sample that consistedonly of gifted students.)When actual data do not approximate the normalcurve, they can be changed <strong>to</strong> do so. In other words, anydistribution of scores can be “normalized.” The procedurefor do<strong>in</strong>g so is not complicated, but it makes theassumption that the characteristic is “really” normallydistributed. Most published tests that permit use of st<strong>and</strong>ardscores have normalized the score distributions <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> permit the translation of z scores <strong>to</strong> percentages.This relationship—between z scores <strong>and</strong> percentagesof area under the normal curve—is also basic <strong>to</strong>many <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.CORRELATIONIn many places throughout this text we have stated thatthe most mean<strong>in</strong>gful research is that which seeks <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d,or verify, relationships among variables. Compar<strong>in</strong>g theperformance of different groups is, as you have seen, one


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 201Figure 10.16 Examplesof St<strong>and</strong>ard ScoresSt<strong>and</strong>arddeviationsPercentileranksz scores–3 –2 –1 Mean 1 2 30.1 2 16 50 84 98 99.9–3 –2 –1 0 +1 +2 +3T scores20 30 40 50 60 70 80GRE scores200 300 400 500 600 700 800Wechslerscores55 70 85 100 115 130 145way <strong>to</strong> study relationships. In such studies one variable iscategorical—the variable that def<strong>in</strong>es the groups (for example,method A versus method B). The other variable ismost often quantitative, <strong>and</strong> groups are typically comparedus<strong>in</strong>g frequency polygons, averages, <strong>and</strong> spreads.In correlational research, researchers seek <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether a relationship exists between two (ormore) quantitative variables, such as age <strong>and</strong> weight orread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g ability. Sometimes, such relationshipsare useful <strong>in</strong> prediction, but most often the eventualgoal is <strong>to</strong> say someth<strong>in</strong>g about causation. Althoughcausal relationships cannot be proved through correlationalstudies, researchers hope eventually <strong>to</strong> makecausal statements as an outgrowth of their work. The<strong>to</strong>tality of studies show<strong>in</strong>g a relationship between<strong>in</strong>cidence of lung cancer <strong>and</strong> cigarette use is a currentexample. We will discuss correlational research <strong>in</strong> furtherdetail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 15.Scatterplots. What is needed is a means <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether relationships exist <strong>in</strong> data. A useful techniquefor represent<strong>in</strong>g relationships with quantitativedata is the scatterplot. A scatterplot is a pic<strong>to</strong>rial representationof the relationship between two quantitativevariables.Scatterplots are easy <strong>to</strong> construct, provided somecommon pitfalls are avoided. First, <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> be plotted,there must be a score on each variable for each <strong>in</strong>dividual;second, the group<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervals (if any) with<strong>in</strong>each variable (axis) must be of equal size; third, each <strong>in</strong>dividualmust be represented by one, <strong>and</strong> only one,po<strong>in</strong>t of <strong>in</strong>tersection. We used the data <strong>in</strong> Table 10.7 <strong>to</strong>construct the scatterplot <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.17. The steps <strong>in</strong>volvedare the follow<strong>in</strong>g:1. Decide which variable will be represented on eachaxis. It makes no difference which variable is placedon which axis. We have used the horizontal (x) axisfor variable 1 <strong>and</strong> the vertical (y) axis for variable 2.2. Divide each axis <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> about 12 <strong>to</strong> 15 sections. Eachpo<strong>in</strong>t on the axis will represent a particular score orgroup of scores. Be sure all scores can be <strong>in</strong>cluded.3. Group scores if desirable. It was not necessary for us<strong>to</strong> group scores for variable 1, s<strong>in</strong>ce all of the scoresfall with<strong>in</strong> a 15-po<strong>in</strong>t range. For variable 2, however,


TABLE 10.7 Data Used <strong>to</strong> Construct Scatterplot <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.17 Figure 10.17 Scatterplot of Data from Table 10.7IndividualScoresVariable 1Pedro 12GroupedScoresVariable 2Frequency ofGrouped ScoresVariable 280–8475–7970–7465–6960–6455–5950–5445–4940–4435–3930–3425–2920–2415–1910–14Variable 2JohnAliciaCharlesBonniePhilSueOscarJeanDave219713117181514ScoresVariable 24176292143371463543975–7970–7465–6960–6455–5950–5445–4940–4435–3930–3425–2920–2415–1910–14100101022011017 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21Variable 1202


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 203represent<strong>in</strong>g each score on the axis would result <strong>in</strong> agreat many po<strong>in</strong>ts on the vertical axis. Therefore, wegrouped them with<strong>in</strong> equal sized <strong>in</strong>tervals of fivepo<strong>in</strong>ts each.4. Plot, for each person, the po<strong>in</strong>t where the vertical <strong>and</strong>horizontal l<strong>in</strong>es for his or her scores on the two variables<strong>in</strong>tersect. For example, Pedro had a score of 12on variable 1, so we locate 12 on the horizontal axis.He had a score of 41 on variable 2, so we locate thatscore (<strong>in</strong> the 40–44 group<strong>in</strong>g) on the vertical axis. Wethen draw imag<strong>in</strong>ary l<strong>in</strong>es from each of these po<strong>in</strong>tsuntil they <strong>in</strong>tersect, <strong>and</strong> mark an X or a dot at that po<strong>in</strong>t.5. In the same way, plot the scores of all 10 students onboth variables. The completed result is a scatterplot.Interpret<strong>in</strong>g a Scatterplot. <strong>How</strong> do researchers <strong>in</strong>terpretscatterplots? What are they <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> reveal?<strong>Research</strong>ers want <strong>to</strong> know not only if a relationship existsbetween variables, but also <strong>to</strong> what degree. The degree ofrelationship, if one exists, is what a scatterplot illustrates.Consider Figure 10.17. What does it tell us about therelationship between variable 1 <strong>and</strong> variable 2? Thisquestion can be answered <strong>in</strong> several ways.1. We might say that high scores on variable 1 go withhigh scores on variable 2 (as <strong>in</strong> John’s case) <strong>and</strong> thatlow scores also tend <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong>gether (as <strong>in</strong> Sue’s case).2. We might say that by know<strong>in</strong>g a student’s score onone variable, we can estimate his or her score on theother variable fairly closely. Suppose, for example, anew student atta<strong>in</strong>s a score of 16 on variable 1. Whatwould you predict his or her score would be on variable2? You probably would not predict a score of 75or one of 25 (we would predict a score somewherefrom 45 <strong>to</strong> 59).3. The cus<strong>to</strong>mary <strong>in</strong>terpretation of a scatterplot thatlooks like this would be that there is a strong or highdegree of relationship between the two variables.Outliers. Outliers are scores or measurements thatdiffer by such large amounts from those of other <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>in</strong> a group that they must be given careful considerationas special cases. They <strong>in</strong>dicate an unusualexception <strong>to</strong> a general pattern. They occur <strong>in</strong> scatterplots,as well as frequency distribution tables, his<strong>to</strong>grams,<strong>and</strong> frequency polygons. Figure 10.18 showsthe relationship between family cohesiveness <strong>and</strong> schoolachievement. Notice the lonely <strong>in</strong>dividual near the lowerright corner with a high score <strong>in</strong> family cohesiveness buta low score <strong>in</strong> achievement. Why? The answer should beof <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the student’s teacher.AchievementHighLowOutlierLowHighFamily cohesivenessFigure 10.18 Relationship Between Family Cohesiveness<strong>and</strong> School Achievement <strong>in</strong> a Hypothetical Group ofStudentsCorrelation Coefficients <strong>and</strong> Scatterplots.Figure 10.19 presents several other examples of scatterplots.Study<strong>in</strong>g them will help you underst<strong>and</strong> the notionof a relationship <strong>and</strong> also further your underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof the correlation coefficient. As we mentioned <strong>in</strong>Chapter 8, a correlation coefficient, designated by thesymbol r, expresses the degree of relationship betweentwo sets of scores.* A positive relationship is <strong>in</strong>dicatedwhen high scores on one variable are accompanied byhigh scores on the other, when low scores on one are accompaniedby low scores on the other, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Anegative relationship is <strong>in</strong>dicated when high scores onone variable are accompanied by low scores on the other,<strong>and</strong> vice versa (Figure 10.20).You should recall that correlation coefficients arenever more than 1.00, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a perfect positive relationship,or 1.00, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a perfect negative relationship.Perfect positive or negative correlations, however,are rarely, if ever, achieved (Figure 10.21). If thetwo variables are highly related, a coefficient somewhatclose <strong>to</strong> 1.00 or 1.00 will be obta<strong>in</strong>ed (such as .85 or.93). The closer the coefficient is <strong>to</strong> either of theseextremes, the greater the degree of the relationship. Ifthere is no or hardly any relationship, a coefficient of.00 or close <strong>to</strong> it will be obta<strong>in</strong>ed. The coefficient is calculateddirectly from the same scores used <strong>to</strong> constructthe scatterplot.The scatterplots <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.19 illustrate differentdegrees of correlation. Both positive <strong>and</strong> negative*In this context, the correlation coefficient <strong>in</strong>dicates the degree ofrelationship between two variables. You will recall from Chapter 8 thatit is also used <strong>to</strong> assess the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of measurements.


204 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 10.19 FurtherExamples of Scatterplotsr = .90r = –.90(a)(e)r = .65r = –.75(b)(f)r = .35r = –.50(c)(g)r = .00r = –.10(d)(h)correlations are shown. Scatterplots (a), (b), <strong>and</strong> (c) illustratedifferent degrees of positive correlation, whilescatterplots (e), ( f ), (g), <strong>and</strong> (h) illustrate different degreesof negative correlation. Scatterplot (d) <strong>in</strong>dicatesno relationship between the two variables <strong>in</strong>volved.In order <strong>to</strong> better underst<strong>and</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g of differentvalues of the correlation coefficient, we suggest thatyou try the follow<strong>in</strong>g two exercises with Figure 10.19.1. Lay a pencil flat on the paper on scatterplot (a) sothat the entire length of the pencil is <strong>to</strong>uch<strong>in</strong>g thepaper. Place it <strong>in</strong> such a way that it <strong>to</strong>uches or coversas many dots as possible. Take note that there isclearly one “best” placement. You would not, forexample, maximize the po<strong>in</strong>ts covered if youplaced the pencil horizontally on the scatterplot.Repeat this procedure for each of the scatterplots,


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 205Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Determ<strong>in</strong>e Correlations <strong>and</strong> Regression EquationsTo use SPSS <strong>to</strong> compute correlations <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d regression equations, you first enter the data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>two columns <strong>in</strong> the data table, with the X values <strong>in</strong> one column <strong>and</strong> the Y values <strong>in</strong> another column.For a correlation, click Analyze on the <strong>to</strong>olbar, select Correlate, <strong>and</strong> click on Bivariate.Next, move the labels for these two columns <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Variables” box, <strong>and</strong> check the Pearsonbox. Then click OK. SPSS will produce a correlation matrix show<strong>in</strong>g all of the possible correlations.The correlation of X <strong>and</strong> Y will be conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the upper-right (or lower-left) corner. Thesignificance level (the p value) for the hypothesis test for a correlation will also be shown.To f<strong>in</strong>d a regression equation, you enter the X <strong>and</strong> Y values <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two columns, click on Analyze, onthe <strong>to</strong>olbar, select Regression, then click on L<strong>in</strong>ear. Enter the column label for Y <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the Dependentbox <strong>and</strong> the column label for X <strong>in</strong> the “Independent” box. Then click OK. Abox will appear show<strong>in</strong>gthe value for the correlation <strong>and</strong> r 2 , as well as the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate for the regression equation.Both the y-<strong>in</strong>tercept <strong>and</strong> the slope of the regression are also calculated. These can be found <strong>in</strong> thetable labeled “Coefficients” under “Unst<strong>and</strong>ardized Coefficients” <strong>in</strong> the subcolumn labeled “B.”“Can I stay overnightat Susie’s house?”“Can I have somemore ice cream?”“Do I have <strong>to</strong>clean my room?”“Yes!”“The more you likesometh<strong>in</strong>g the less youget <strong>to</strong> do it!”“No!”“No!”r = –1.00Figure 10.20 A Perfect Negative Correlation!Figure 10.21 Positive <strong>and</strong> Negative Correlations


206 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eUs<strong>in</strong>g Excel <strong>to</strong>X27243436181613251015191427724Y32273338211517281216181429822r = .97Quiz #240353025201510500Draw a ScatterplotExcel can also be used <strong>to</strong> draw a scatterplot <strong>to</strong> illustrate the relationship between twoquantitative variables. The dataset <strong>to</strong> be illustrated must be entered <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two columns <strong>in</strong>an Excel worksheet, with the X variable <strong>in</strong> one column <strong>and</strong> the Y variable <strong>in</strong> another.Scatterplot Show<strong>in</strong>g RelationshipBetween Two Sets of Quiz Scores1020Quiz #13040Once the data are entered, pull down theInsert menu, then click on Chart. When thechart w<strong>in</strong>dow opens, click on Scatter. Thenclick on Next at the bot<strong>to</strong>m of the dialoguebox. Another dialogue box will open. Clickon the first cell <strong>in</strong> the upper left of the twocolumns of data, <strong>and</strong> drag down <strong>to</strong> the lastcell <strong>in</strong> the lower right. Click on F<strong>in</strong>ish at thebot<strong>to</strong>m of the dialogue box <strong>to</strong> display thef<strong>in</strong>ished chart. Shown here is an exampleof the relationship between two quizzes fora hypothetical class of 15 high schoolchemistry students. Note that the correlationcoefficient is shown <strong>to</strong> the left of the scatterplot.In this <strong>in</strong>stance, r .97, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g avery strong, positive relationship betweenthe two sets of quiz scores for this group ofstudents.not<strong>in</strong>g what occurs as you move from one scatterplot<strong>to</strong> another.2. Draw a horizontal l<strong>in</strong>e on scatterplot (a) so thatabout half of the dots are above the l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> half arebelow it. Next, draw a vertical l<strong>in</strong>e so that about halfof the dots are <strong>to</strong> the left of the l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> half are <strong>to</strong>the right. Repeat the procedure for each scatterplot<strong>and</strong> note what you observe as you move from onescatterplot <strong>to</strong> another.The Pearson Product-Moment Coefficient.There are many different correlation coefficients, eachapply<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a particular circumstance <strong>and</strong> each calculatedby means of a different computational formula. The onewe have been illustrat<strong>in</strong>g is the one most frequently used:the Pearson product-moment coefficient of correlation(also known as the Pearson r). It is symbolized by thelowercase letter r. When the data for both variables areexpressed <strong>in</strong> terms of quantitative scores, the Pearson r isthe appropriate correlation coefficient <strong>to</strong> use. (It is notdifficult <strong>to</strong> calculate, <strong>and</strong> we’ll show you how <strong>in</strong> Chapter12.) It is designed for use with <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio data.Eta. Another <strong>in</strong>dex of correlation that you should becomefamiliar with is called eta (symbolized as η). Weshall not illustrate how <strong>to</strong> calculate eta (s<strong>in</strong>ce it requirescomputational methods beyond the scope of this text),but you should know that it is used when a scatterplotshows that a straight l<strong>in</strong>e is not the best fit for the plottedpo<strong>in</strong>ts. In the examples shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 10.22, forFigure 10.22 Examplesof Nonl<strong>in</strong>ear(Curvil<strong>in</strong>ear)RelationshipsTask performanceDependence on othersAnxietyAge


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 207example, you can see that a curved l<strong>in</strong>e provides a muchbetter fit <strong>to</strong> the data than would a straight l<strong>in</strong>e.Eta is <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong> much the same way as the Pearsonr, except that it ranges from .00 <strong>to</strong> 1.00, ratherthan from 1.00 <strong>to</strong> 1.00. Higher values, as with theother correlation coefficients, <strong>in</strong>dicate higher degreesof relationship.Techniques for Summariz<strong>in</strong>gCategorical DataTHE FREQUENCY TABLESuppose a researcher, us<strong>in</strong>g a questionnaire, has beencollect<strong>in</strong>g data from a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 50 teachers <strong>in</strong>a large urban school district. The questionnaire coversmany variables related <strong>to</strong> their activities <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terests.One of the variables is learn<strong>in</strong>g activity I use most frequently<strong>in</strong> my classroom. The researcher arranges herdata on this variable (<strong>and</strong> others) <strong>in</strong> the form of a frequencytable, which shows the frequency with whicheach type, or category, of learn<strong>in</strong>g activity is mentioned.The researcher simply places a tally mark for each <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>in</strong> the sample alongside the activity mentioned.When she has tallied all 50 <strong>in</strong>dividuals, her results looklike the follow<strong>in</strong>g frequency list<strong>in</strong>g.Response Tally FrequencyLectures |||| |||| |||| 15Class discussions |||| |||| 10Oral reports |||| 4Library research || 2Seatwork |||| 5Demonstrations |||| ||| 8Audiovisualpresentations |||| | 6n 50The tally marks have been added up at the end of eachrow <strong>to</strong> show the <strong>to</strong>tal number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who listedthat activity. Often with categorical data, researchers are<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> proportions, because they wish <strong>to</strong> make anestimate (if their sample is r<strong>and</strong>om) about the proportions<strong>in</strong> the <strong>to</strong>tal population from which the sample wasselected. Thus, the <strong>to</strong>tal numbers <strong>in</strong> each category areoften changed <strong>to</strong> percentages. This has been done <strong>in</strong>Table 10.8, with the categories arranged <strong>in</strong> descend<strong>in</strong>gorder of frequency.TABLE 10.8Frequency <strong>and</strong> Percentage of Totalof Responses <strong>to</strong> QuestionnairePercentageResponse Frequency of Total (%)Lectures 15 30Class discussions 10 20Demonstrations 8 16Audiovisualpresentations 6 12Seatwork 5 10Oral reports 4 8Library research 2 4Total 50 100BAR GRAPHS AND PIE CHARTSThere are two other ways <strong>to</strong> illustrate a difference <strong>in</strong>proportions. One is a bar graph portrayal, as shown<strong>in</strong> Figure 10.23; another is a pie chart, as shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 10.24.THE CROSSBREAK TABLEWhen a relationship between two categorical variables isof <strong>in</strong>terest, it is usually reported <strong>in</strong> the form of acrossbreak table (sometimes called a cont<strong>in</strong>gencytable). The simplest crossbreak is a 2 by 2 table, as shown<strong>in</strong> Table 10.9. Each <strong>in</strong>dividual is tallied <strong>in</strong> one, <strong>and</strong> onlyone, cell that corresponds <strong>to</strong> the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of gender<strong>and</strong> grade level. You will notice that the numbers <strong>in</strong> eachof the cells <strong>in</strong> Table 10.9 represent <strong>to</strong>tals—the <strong>to</strong>tal numberof <strong>in</strong>dividuals who fit the characteristics of the cell(for example, junior high male teachers). Although percentages<strong>and</strong> proportions are sometimes calculated forcells, we do not recommend it, as this is often mislead<strong>in</strong>g.It probably seems obvious that Table 10.9 reveals arelationship between teacher gender <strong>and</strong> grade level. Ajunior high school teacher is more likely <strong>to</strong> be female; ahigh school teacher is more likely <strong>to</strong> be male. Often,however, it is useful <strong>to</strong> calculate “expected” frequencies<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> see results more clearly. What do we mean by“expected”? If there is no relationship between variables,we would “expect” the proportion of cases with<strong>in</strong>each cell of the table correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a category of avariable <strong>to</strong> be identical <strong>to</strong> the proportion with<strong>in</strong> that category<strong>in</strong> the entire group. Look, for example, atTable 10.10. Exactly one-half (50 percent) of the <strong>to</strong>tal


208 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e403030%PERCENTAGE201020%16%12%10%8%4%0n =15Lectures10Classdiscussions8Demonstrations6Audiovisualpresentations5Seatwork4Oralreports2LibraryresearchACTIVITYPercentages of learn<strong>in</strong>g activities used most frequently by 50 teachersFigure 10.23 Example of a Bar GraphTABLE 10.9Grade Level <strong>and</strong> Gender ofTeachers (Hypothetical Data)Male Female TotalJunior high school teachers 40 60 100High school teachers 60 40 100Total 100 100 200Learn<strong>in</strong>g activities used most frequentlyby 50 teachers15 Lectures10 Class discussions8 Demonstrations6 Audiovisual presentations5 Seatwork4 Oral reports2 Library researchFigure 10.24 Example of a Pie Chartgroup of teachers <strong>in</strong> this table are female. If gender isunrelated <strong>to</strong> grade level, we would “expect” that thesame proportion (exactly one-half) of the junior highschool teachers would be female. Similarly, we would“expect” that one-half of the high school teachers wouldbe female. The “expected” frequencies, <strong>in</strong> other words,TABLE 10.10Repeat of Table 10.9 with ExpectedFrequencies (<strong>in</strong> Parentheses)Male Female TotalJunior high school teachers 40 (50) 60 (50) 100High school teachers 60 (50) 40 (50) 100Total 100 100 200would be 50 female junior high school teachers <strong>and</strong> 50female high school teachers, rather than the 60 femalejunior high school <strong>and</strong> 40 female high school teachersthat were actually obta<strong>in</strong>ed. These expected <strong>and</strong> actual,or “observed,” frequencies are shown <strong>in</strong> each box(or “cell”) <strong>in</strong> Table 10.10. The expected frequencies areshown <strong>in</strong> parentheses.**Expected frequencies can also be provided ahead of time, based ontheory or prior experience. In this example, the researcher might havewanted <strong>to</strong> know whether the characteristics of junior high schoolteachers <strong>in</strong> a particular school fit the national pattern. Nationalpercentages would then be used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e expected frequencies.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHCorrelation <strong>in</strong> Everyday LifeMany commonplace relationships (true or not) can be expressedas correlations. For example, Boyle’s law statesthat the volume <strong>and</strong> pressure of a gas vary <strong>in</strong>versely if kept ata constant temperature. Another way <strong>to</strong> express this is that thecorrelation between volume <strong>and</strong> pressure is 1.00. This relationship,however, is only theoretically true—that is, it existsonly for a perfect gas <strong>in</strong> a perfect vacuum. In real life, the correlationis lower.Consider the follow<strong>in</strong>g say<strong>in</strong>gs:1. “Spare the rod <strong>and</strong> spoil the child” implies a negative correlationbetween punishment <strong>and</strong> spoiled behavior.2. “Idle h<strong>and</strong>s are the devil’s workplace” implies a positivecorrelation between idleness <strong>and</strong> mischief.3. “There’s no fool like an old fool” suggests a positive correlationbetween foolishness <strong>and</strong> age.4. “A stitch <strong>in</strong> time saves n<strong>in</strong>e” suggests a positive correlationbetween how long one waits <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> a corrective action <strong>and</strong>the amount of time (<strong>and</strong> effort) required <strong>to</strong> fix the problem.5. “The early bird catches the worm” suggests a positive correlationbetween early ris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> success.6. “You can’t teach an old dog new tricks” implies a negativecorrelation between age of adults <strong>and</strong> ability <strong>to</strong> learn.7. “An apple a day keeps the doc<strong>to</strong>r away” suggests a negativecorrelation between the consumption of apples <strong>and</strong> illness.8. “Fa<strong>in</strong>t heart never won fair maiden” suggests a negativecorrelation between timidity <strong>and</strong> female receptivity.TABLE 10.11Position, Gender, <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity ofSchool Leaders (Hypothetical Data)Adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rsTeachersWhite Nonwhite White Nonwhite TotalMale 50 20 150 80 300Female 20 10 150 120 300Total 70 30 300 200 600TABLE 10.12Position <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies(Derived from Table 10.11)Adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs Teachers TotalWhite 70 (62) 300 (308) 370Nonwhite 30 (38) 200 (192) 230Total 100 500 600Compar<strong>in</strong>g expected <strong>and</strong> actual frequencies makesthe degree <strong>and</strong> direction of the relationship clearer. Thisis particularly helpful with more complex tables. Look,for example, at Table 10.11. This table conta<strong>in</strong>s not two,but three, variables.The researcher who collected <strong>and</strong> summarized thesedata hypothesized that appo<strong>in</strong>tment <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istrative (orother nonteach<strong>in</strong>g) positions rather than teach<strong>in</strong>g positionsis related <strong>to</strong> (1) gender <strong>and</strong> (2) ethnicity. While it ispossible <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e Table 10.11 <strong>in</strong> its entirety <strong>to</strong> evaluatethese hypotheses, it is much easier <strong>to</strong> see the relationshipsby extract<strong>in</strong>g components of the table. Let us lookat the relationship of each variable <strong>in</strong> the table <strong>to</strong> theother two variables. By tak<strong>in</strong>g two variables at a time, wecan compare (1) position <strong>and</strong> ethnicity; (2) position <strong>and</strong>gender; <strong>and</strong> (3) gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity. Table 10.12 presentsthe data for position <strong>and</strong> ethnicity, Table 10.13 presentsthe data for position <strong>and</strong> gender, <strong>and</strong> Table 10.14presents the data for gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity.Let us review the calculation of expected frequenciesby referr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> Table 10.12. This table shows theTABLE 10.13Position <strong>and</strong> Gender of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies(Derived from Table 10.11)Adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs Teachers TotalMale 70 (50) 230 (250) 300Female 30 (50) 270 (250) 300Total 100 500 600TABLE 10.14Gender <strong>and</strong> Ethnicity of SchoolLeaders with Expected Frequencies(Derived from Table 10.11)White Nonwhite TotalMale 200 (185) 100 (115) 300Female 170 (185) 130 (115) 300Total 370 230 600209


210 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 10.15Total of Discrepancies Between Expected <strong>and</strong> ObservedFrequencies <strong>in</strong> Tables 10.12 Through 10.14Table 10.12 Table 10.13 Table 10.14(70 vs. 62) 8 (70 vs. 50) 20 (200 vs. 185) 15(30 vs. 38) 8 (30 vs. 50) 20 (170 vs. 185) 15(300 vs. 308) 8 (230 vs. 250) 20 (100 vs. 115) 15(200 vs. 192) 8 (270 vs. 250) 20 (130 vs. 115) 15Total 32 80 60relationship between ethnicity <strong>and</strong> position. S<strong>in</strong>ce onesixthof the <strong>to</strong>tal group (100/600) are adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, we1would expect 62 whites <strong>to</strong> be adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs ( 6 of 370).Likewise, we would expect 38 of the nonwhites <strong>to</strong> be adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs( 6 of 230). S<strong>in</strong>ce five-sixths of the <strong>to</strong>tal group15are teachers, we would expect 308 of the whites ( 6 of 370)5<strong>and</strong> 192 of the nonwhites ( 6 of 230) <strong>to</strong> be teachers.As youcan see, however, the actual frequencies for adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rswere 70 (rather than 62) whites, <strong>and</strong> 30 (ratherthan 38) nonwhites, <strong>and</strong> the actual frequencies for teacherswere 300 (rather than 308) whites, <strong>and</strong> 200 (rather than192) nonwhites. This tells us that there is a discrepancybetween what we would expect (if there is no relationship)<strong>and</strong> what we actually obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Discrepancies betweenthe frequencies expected <strong>and</strong> those actually obta<strong>in</strong>ed canalso be seen <strong>in</strong> Tables 10.13 <strong>and</strong> 10.14.An <strong>in</strong>dex of the strength of the relationships can beobta<strong>in</strong>ed by summ<strong>in</strong>g the discrepancies <strong>in</strong> each table. InTable 10.12, the sum equals 32, <strong>in</strong> Table 10.13, it equals80, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> Table 10.14 it equals 60. The calculation ofthese sums is shown <strong>in</strong> Table 10.15. The discrepancybetween expected <strong>and</strong> observed frequencies is greatest<strong>in</strong> Table 10.13, position <strong>and</strong> gender; less <strong>in</strong> Table 10.14,gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity; <strong>and</strong> least <strong>in</strong> Table 10.12, position<strong>and</strong> ethnicity. A numerical <strong>in</strong>dex show<strong>in</strong>g degree ofrelationship—the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient—will be discussed<strong>in</strong> Chapter 11.Thus, the data <strong>in</strong> the crossbreak tables reveal that thereis a slight tendency for there <strong>to</strong> be more white adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>and</strong> more nonwhite teachers than would be expected(Table 10.12). There is a stronger tendency <strong>to</strong>ward morewhitemales<strong>and</strong>nonwhitefemalesthanwouldbeexpected(Table 10.14). The strongest relationship <strong>in</strong>dicates moremale adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> more female teachers than wouldbe expected (Table 10.13). In sum, the chances of hav<strong>in</strong>gan adm<strong>in</strong>istrative position appear <strong>to</strong> be considerablygreater if one is male, <strong>and</strong> slightly enhanced if one is white.In contrast <strong>to</strong> the preced<strong>in</strong>g example, where eachvariable (ethnicity, gender, role) is clearly categorical, aTABLE 10.16Crossbreak Table Show<strong>in</strong>gRelationship Between Self-Esteem<strong>and</strong> Gender (Hypothetical Data)Self EsteemGender Low Middle HighMale 10 15 5Female 5 10 15researcher sometimes has a choice whether <strong>to</strong> treat dataas quantitative or as categorical. Take the case of aresearcher who measures self-esteem by a self-reportquestionnaire scored for number of items answered (yesor no) <strong>in</strong> the direction <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g high self-esteem. Theresearcher might decide <strong>to</strong> use these scores <strong>to</strong> divide thesample (n 60) <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> high, middle, <strong>and</strong> low thirds. He orshe might use only this <strong>in</strong>formation for each <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>and</strong> subsequently treat the data as categorical, as isshown, for example, <strong>in</strong> Table 10.16. Most researcherswould advise aga<strong>in</strong>st treat<strong>in</strong>g the data this way, however,s<strong>in</strong>ce it “wastes” so much <strong>in</strong>formation—for example,differences <strong>in</strong> scores with<strong>in</strong> each category areignored. A quantitative analysis, by way of contrast,would compare the mean self-esteem scores of males<strong>and</strong> females.In such situations (i.e., when one variable is quantitative<strong>and</strong> the other is treated as categorical), correlationis another option. When data on a variable assumed <strong>to</strong>be quantitative have been divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two categories, abiserial correlation coefficient can be calculated <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpreted the same as a Pearson r coefficient.* If thecategories are assumed <strong>to</strong> reflect a true division, calculationof a po<strong>in</strong>t biserial coefficient is an option, butmust be <strong>in</strong>terpreted with caution.*A detailed explanation of these statistics is beyond the scope of thistext. For further details, consult any statistics text.


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 211Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.STATISTICS VERSUS PARAMETERS• A parameter is a characteristic of a population. It is a numerical or graphic way <strong>to</strong>summarize data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from the population.• A statistic, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is a characteristic of a sample. It is a numerical orgraphic way <strong>to</strong> summarize data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a sample.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTYPES OF NUMERICAL DATA• There are two fundamental types of numerical data a researcher can collect. Quantitativedata are obta<strong>in</strong>ed by determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g placement on a scale that <strong>in</strong>dicates amount ordegree. Categorical data are obta<strong>in</strong>ed by determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the frequency of occurrences <strong>in</strong>each of several categories.TECHNIQUES FOR SUMMARIZING QUANTITATIVE DATA• A frequency distribution is a two-column list<strong>in</strong>g, from high <strong>to</strong> low, of all the scoresalong with their frequencies. In a grouped frequency distribution, the scores havebeen grouped <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> equal <strong>in</strong>tervals.• A frequency polygon is a graphic display of a frequency distribution. It is a graphicway <strong>to</strong> summarize quantitative data for one variable.• A graphic distribution of scores <strong>in</strong> which only a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals receive high scoresis called a positively skewed polygon; one <strong>in</strong> which only a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals receivelow scores is called a negatively skewed polygon.• A his<strong>to</strong>gram is a bar graph used <strong>to</strong> display quantitative data at the <strong>in</strong>terval or ratiolevel of measurement.• A stem-leaf plot is similar <strong>to</strong> a his<strong>to</strong>gram, except it lists specific values <strong>in</strong>stead of bars.• The normal distribution is a theoretical distribution that is symmetrical <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> whicha large proportion of the scores are concentrated <strong>in</strong> the middle.• A distribution curve is a smoothed-out frequency polygon.• The distribution curve of a normal distribution is called a normal curve. It is bellshaped,<strong>and</strong> its mean, median, <strong>and</strong> mode are identical.• There are several measures of central tendency (averages) that are used <strong>to</strong> summarizequantitative data. The two most common are the mean <strong>and</strong> the median.• The mean of a distribution is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by add<strong>in</strong>g up all of the scores <strong>and</strong> divid<strong>in</strong>gthis sum by the <strong>to</strong>tal number of scores.• The median of a distribution marks the po<strong>in</strong>t above <strong>and</strong> below which half of thescores <strong>in</strong> the distribution lie.• The mode is the most frequent score <strong>in</strong> a distribution.• The term variability, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the extent <strong>to</strong> which the scores on aquantitative variable <strong>in</strong> a distribution are spread out.


212 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e• The most common measure of variability used <strong>in</strong> educational research is the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation.• The range, another measure of variability, represents the difference between thehighest <strong>and</strong> lowest scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution.• A five-number summary of a distribution reports the lowest score, the first quartile,the median, the third quartile, <strong>and</strong> the highest score.• Five-number summaries of distributions are often portrayed graphically by the use ofboxplots.STANDARD SCORES AND THE NORMAL CURVE• St<strong>and</strong>ard scores use a common scale <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate how an <strong>in</strong>dividual compares <strong>to</strong> other<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> a group. The simplest form of st<strong>and</strong>ard score is a z score. A z scoreexpresses how far a raw score is from the mean <strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation units.• The major advantage of st<strong>and</strong>ard scores is that they provide a better basis for compar<strong>in</strong>gperformance on different measures than do raw scores.• The term probability, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> a prediction of how often a particularevent will occur. Probabilities are usually expressed <strong>in</strong> decimal form.CORRELATION• A correlation coefficient is a numerical <strong>in</strong>dex express<strong>in</strong>g the degree of relationshipbetween two quantitative variables. The one most commonly used <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch is the Pearson r.• A scatterplot is a graphic way <strong>to</strong> describe a relationship between two quantitativevariables.TECHNIQUES FOR SUMMARIZING CATEGORICAL DATA• <strong>Research</strong>ers use various graphic techniques <strong>to</strong> summarize categorical data, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gfrequency tables, bar graphs, <strong>and</strong> pie charts.• A crossbreak table is a graphic way <strong>to</strong> report a relationship between two or morecategorical variables.Key Terms68-95-99.7 rule 197averages 192bar graph 207boxplot 194correlationcoefficient 203crossbreak table/cont<strong>in</strong>gency table 207descriptive statistics 185distribution curves 192eta 206five-numbersummary 194frequencydistribution 187frequency polygon 187grouped frequencydistribution 187his<strong>to</strong>gram 190mean 192measures of centraltendency 192median 192mode 192negatively skewed 188normal curve 192normal distribution 192outlier 203parameter 185Pearson productmomentcoefficient/Pearson r 206percentile 194pie chart 207positively skewed 188probability 199quantitative data 186range 195scatterplot 201st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation 195st<strong>and</strong>ard score 197statistic 185stem-leaf plot 190T score 200variability 194variance 195z score 197


CHAPTER 10 Descriptive Statistics 2131. Would you expect the follow<strong>in</strong>g correlations <strong>to</strong> be positive or negative? Why?a. Bowl<strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>and</strong> golf scoresb. Read<strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>and</strong> arithmetic scores for sixth-gradersc. Age <strong>and</strong> weight for a group of 5-year-olds; for a group of people over 70d. Life expectancy at age 40 <strong>and</strong> frequency of smok<strong>in</strong>ge. Size <strong>and</strong> strength for junior high students2. Why do you th<strong>in</strong>k so many people mistrust statistics? <strong>How</strong> might such mistrust bealleviated?3. Would it be possible for two different distributions <strong>to</strong> have the same st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationbut different means? What about the reverse? Expla<strong>in</strong>.4. “The larger the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a distribution, the more heterogeneous thescores <strong>in</strong> that distribution.” Is this statement true? Expla<strong>in</strong>.5. “The most complete <strong>in</strong>formation about a distribution of scores is provided by a frequencypolygon.” Is this statement true? Expla<strong>in</strong>.6. Group<strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>in</strong> a frequency distribution has its advantages but also its disadvantages.What might be some examples of each?7. “Any s<strong>in</strong>gle raw score, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, tells us noth<strong>in</strong>g.” Would you agree? Expla<strong>in</strong>.8. The relationship between age <strong>and</strong> strength is said <strong>to</strong> be curvil<strong>in</strong>ear. What does thismean? Might there be exceptions <strong>to</strong> this relationship? What might cause such anexception?For Discussion


<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 10: Descriptive StatisticsUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 10, state aga<strong>in</strong> the question or hypothesis of your study <strong>and</strong> list yourvariables. Then <strong>in</strong>dicate how you would summarize the results for each variable <strong>in</strong> your study.Lastly, <strong>in</strong>dicate how you would describe the relationship between variables 1 <strong>and</strong> 2. If your studyis qualitative, answer <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> any possible hypothesis that may emerge, <strong>and</strong> any statisticsthat would be useful.Problem Sheet 10Descriptive Statistics1. The question or hypothesis of my study is: __________________________________2. My variables are: (1) ___________________________________________________(2) _________________________ (others) _________________________________3. I consider variable 1 <strong>to</strong> be: quantitative _________ categorical _________4. I consider variable 2 <strong>to</strong> be: quantitative _________ categorical _________5. I would summarize the results for each variable checked below (<strong>in</strong>dicate with a checkmark):Variable 1: Variable 2: Other:a. Frequency polygonb. Five-number summaryc. Box plot____________ ____________ ____________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.d. Meane. Medianf. Percentagesg. St<strong>and</strong>ard deviationh. Frequency tablei. Bar graphj. Pie chart5. I would describe the relationship between variables 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 by (<strong>in</strong>dicate with a checkmark):a. Comparison of frequency polygons ________b. Comparison of averages ________c. Crossbreak table(s) ________d. Correlation coefficient ________e. Scatterplot ________f. Report<strong>in</strong>g of percentages ________214


Inferential Statistics11“Survey resultsshow that 60 percen<strong>to</strong>f those we <strong>in</strong>terviewedsupport ourbond issue.”“Great! Can we be surethen that 60 percent of thevoters will go for it?”“No, not exactly.But it is very likely thatbetween 55 percent <strong>and</strong>65 percent will!”OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termExpla<strong>in</strong> the difference between parametric“<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.”<strong>and</strong> nonparametric tests of significance.Expla<strong>in</strong> the concept of sampl<strong>in</strong>g error. Name three examples of parametric testsDescribe briefly how <strong>to</strong> calculate aused by educational researchers.confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.Name three examples of nonparametricState the difference between a research tests used by educational researchers.hypothesis <strong>and</strong> a null hypothesis.Describe what is meant by the termDescribe briefly the logic underly<strong>in</strong>g“power” with regard <strong>to</strong> statistical tests.hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g.Expla<strong>in</strong> the importance of r<strong>and</strong>omState what is meant by the termssampl<strong>in</strong>g with regard <strong>to</strong> the use of“significance level” <strong>and</strong> “statistically<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.significant.”Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between a one<strong>and</strong>a two-tailed test of significance.What Are InferentialStatistics?The Logic of InferentialStatisticsSampl<strong>in</strong>g ErrorDistribution of SampleMeansSt<strong>and</strong>ard Error of the MeanConfidence IntervalsConfidence Intervals <strong>and</strong>ProbabilityCompar<strong>in</strong>g More Than OneSampleThe St<strong>and</strong>ard Error of theDifference BetweenSample MeansHypothesis Test<strong>in</strong>gThe Null HypothesisHypothesis Test<strong>in</strong>g:A ReviewPractical VersusStatistical SignificanceOne- <strong>and</strong> Two-Tailed TestsUse of the Null Hypothesis:An EvaluationInference TechniquesParametric Techniques forAnalyz<strong>in</strong>g QuantitativeDataNonparametric Techniquesfor Analyz<strong>in</strong>gQuantitative DataParametric Techniques forAnalyz<strong>in</strong>g CategoricalDataNonparametric Techniquesfor Analyz<strong>in</strong>g CategoricalDataSummary of TechniquesPower of a Statistical Test


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Practice with Graphs <strong>and</strong> CurvesLearn More About the Purpose of Inferential StatisticsAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 11.1: ProbabilityActivity 11.2: Learn <strong>to</strong> Read a t-TableActivity 11.3: Calculate a t-TestActivity 11.4: Perform a Chi-Square TestActivity 11.5: Conduct a t-TestActivity 11.6: The Big GamePaulo, I’m worried.”“Why, Julie?”“Well, <strong>in</strong> my job as direc<strong>to</strong>r of the new elementary math program for the district, I have <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out how well thestudents <strong>in</strong> the program are do<strong>in</strong>g. This year, we’re test<strong>in</strong>g the fifth-graders.”“Yeah, so? What’s <strong>to</strong> worry about?”“Well, I gave the end-of-the-semester exam <strong>to</strong> one of the fifth-grade classes at Hoover Elementary last week. Igot the results back just <strong>to</strong>day.”“And?”“Get this. Their average score was only 65 out of a possible 100 po<strong>in</strong>ts! Of course this is just one class, but still . . .I’m afraid that it may be true of all the fifth-grade classes.”“Not necessarily, Julie. That would depend on how similar the Hoover kids are <strong>to</strong> the other fifth-graders <strong>in</strong> thedistrict. What you need is some way <strong>to</strong> estimate the average score of all of the district’s fifth-graders—but you can’tdo it from that class.”“I assume you’re th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about some sort of <strong>in</strong>ferential test, eh?”“Yep, you got it.”<strong>How</strong> might Julie make such an estimate? To f<strong>in</strong>d out, read this chapter.What Are Inferential Statistics?Descriptive statistics are but one type of statistic thatresearchers use <strong>to</strong> analyze their data. Many times theywish <strong>to</strong> make <strong>in</strong>ferences about a population based ondata they have obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a sample. To do this, theyuse <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics. Let us consider how.Suppose a researcher adm<strong>in</strong>isters a commerciallyavailable IQ test <strong>to</strong> a sample of 65 students selectedfrom a particular elementary school district <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>dstheir average score is 85. What does this tell her aboutthe IQ scores of the entire population of students <strong>in</strong> thedistrict? Does the average IQ score of students <strong>in</strong> thedistrict also equal 85? Or is this sample of studentsdifferent, on the average, from other students <strong>in</strong> thedistrict? If these students are different, how are theydifferent? Are their IQ scores higher—or lower?What the researcher needs is some way <strong>to</strong> estimatehow closely statistics, such as the mean of the IQ scores,obta<strong>in</strong>ed on a sample agree with the correspond<strong>in</strong>gparameters of the population without actually obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdata on the entire population. Inferential statisticsprovide such a way.Inferential statistics are certa<strong>in</strong> types of proceduresthat allow researchers <strong>to</strong> make <strong>in</strong>ferences about a populationbased on f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from a sample. In Chapter 6, wediscussed the concept of a r<strong>and</strong>om sample <strong>and</strong> po<strong>in</strong>tedout that obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>om sample is desirable becauseit helps ensure that one’s sample is representative of alarger population. When a sample is representative, allthe characteristics of the population are assumed <strong>to</strong> be216


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 217present <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> the same degree. No sampl<strong>in</strong>gprocedure, not even r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, guarantees a <strong>to</strong>tallyrepresentative sample, but the chance of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gone is greater with r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g than with any othermethod. And the more a sample represents a population,the more researchers are entitled <strong>to</strong> assume that whatthey f<strong>in</strong>d out about the sample will also be true of thatpopulation. Mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferences about populations on thebasis of r<strong>and</strong>om samples is what <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics isall about.As with descriptive statistics, the techniques of<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics differ depend<strong>in</strong>g on which type ofdata—categorical or quantitative—a researcher wishes<strong>to</strong> analyze. This chapter beg<strong>in</strong>s with techniques applicable<strong>to</strong> quantitative data, because they provide the best<strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> the logic beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>ference techniques<strong>and</strong> because most educational research <strong>in</strong>volves suchdata. Some techniques for the analysis of categoricaldata are presented at the end of the chapter.The Logic of InferentialStatisticsSuppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the differencebetween males <strong>and</strong> females with respect <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong>his<strong>to</strong>ry. He hypothesizes that female students f<strong>in</strong>d his<strong>to</strong>rymore <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g than do male students. To test thehypothesis, he decides <strong>to</strong> perform the follow<strong>in</strong>g study.He obta<strong>in</strong>s one r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 30 male his<strong>to</strong>rystudents from the population of 500 male tenth-gradestudents tak<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> a nearby school district <strong>and</strong>another r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 30 female his<strong>to</strong>ry studentsfrom the female population of 550 female tenth-gradehis<strong>to</strong>ry students <strong>in</strong> the district. All students are givenan attitude scale <strong>to</strong> complete. The researcher now hastwo sets of data: the attitude scores for the male group<strong>and</strong> the attitude scores for the female group. Thedesign of the study is shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 11.1 Theresearcher wants <strong>to</strong> know whether the male populationis different from the female population—that is, willthe mean score of the male group on the attitude testdiffer from the mean score of the female group? Butthe researcher does not know the means of the twopopulations. All he has are the means of the two samples.He has <strong>to</strong> rely on the two samples <strong>to</strong> provide <strong>in</strong>formationabout the populations.Population of malehis<strong>to</strong>ry studentsn = 500Sample 1n = 30Population of femalehis<strong>to</strong>ry studentsn = 550Sample 2n = 30Figure 11.1 Selection of Two Samples from Two Dist<strong>in</strong>ctPopulationsIs it reasonable <strong>to</strong> assume that each sample will givea fairly accurate picture of its population? It certa<strong>in</strong>ly ispossible, s<strong>in</strong>ce each sample was r<strong>and</strong>omly selected fromits population. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the students <strong>in</strong> eachsample are only a small portion of their population,<strong>and</strong> only rarely is a sample absolutely identical <strong>to</strong> itsparent population on a given characteristic. The data theresearcher obta<strong>in</strong>s from the two samples will depend onthe <strong>in</strong>dividual students selected <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> each sample. Ifanother two samples were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected, theirmakeup would differ from the orig<strong>in</strong>al two, their meanson the attitude scale would be different, <strong>and</strong> the researcherwould end up with a different set of data. <strong>How</strong>can the researcher be sure that any particular sample hehas selected is, <strong>in</strong>deed, a representative one? He cannot.Maybe another sample would be better.SAMPLING ERRORThis is the basic difficulty that confronts us when wework with samples: Samples are not likely <strong>to</strong> be identical<strong>to</strong> their parent populations. This difference betweena sample <strong>and</strong> its population is referred <strong>to</strong> as sampl<strong>in</strong>gerror (Figure 11.2). Furthermore, no two samples willbe the same <strong>in</strong> all their characteristics. Two differentsamples from the same population will not be identical:they will be composed of different <strong>in</strong>dividuals, they willhave different scores on a test (or other measure), <strong>and</strong>they will probably have different sample means.Consider the population of high school students <strong>in</strong>the United States. It would be possible <strong>to</strong> select literallythous<strong>and</strong>s of different samples from this population.Suppose we <strong>to</strong>ok two samples of 25 students each fromthis population <strong>and</strong> measured their heights. What wouldyou estimate our chances would be of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g exactlythe same mean height <strong>in</strong> both samples? Very, very unlikely.In fact, we could probably take sample after sample<strong>and</strong> very seldom obta<strong>in</strong> two sets of people hav<strong>in</strong>gexactly the same mean height.


218 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePopulation of 100 adultwomen <strong>in</strong> the United StatesSampleSample Sample SampleFigure 11.2 Sampl<strong>in</strong>g ErrorNotice that none of the samplesis exactly like the population.This difference is what is knownas sampl<strong>in</strong>g error.DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE MEANSAll of this might suggest that it is impossible <strong>to</strong> formulateany rules that researchers can use <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>esimilarities between samples <strong>and</strong> populations. Not so.Fortunately, large collections of r<strong>and</strong>om samples dopattern themselves <strong>in</strong> such a way that it is possible forresearchers <strong>to</strong> predict accurately some characteristics ofthe population from which the sample was selected.Were we able <strong>to</strong> select an <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite number of r<strong>and</strong>omsamples (all of the same size) from a population,calculate the mean of each, <strong>and</strong> then arrange thesemeans <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a frequency polygon, we would f<strong>in</strong>d thatthey shaped themselves <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a familiar pattern. Themeans of a large number of r<strong>and</strong>om samples tend<strong>to</strong> be normally distributed, unless the size of each ofthe samples is small (less than 30) <strong>and</strong> the scores<strong>in</strong> the population are not normally distributed. Oncesample size reaches 30, however, the distribution ofsample means is very nearly normal, even if the populationis not normally distributed. (We realize that thisis not immediately obvious; should you wish moreexplanation of why this is true, consult any <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>rystatistics text.)Like all normal distributions, a distribution of samplemeans (called a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution) has its ownmean <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. The mean of a sampl<strong>in</strong>gdistribution (the “mean of the means”) is equal <strong>to</strong> themean of the population. In an <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ite number of samples,some will have means larger than the populationmean <strong>and</strong> some will have means smaller than the populationmean (Figure 11.3). These data tend <strong>to</strong> neutralizeeach other, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an overall average that is equal<strong>to</strong> the mean of the population. Consider an example.Suppose you have a population of only three scores—1,2, 3. The mean of this population is 2. Now, take all ofthe possible types of samples of size two. <strong>How</strong> many


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 219Population(mean = 70)Illustrative samples eachof same size (e.g., 60)drawn from this populationSample ASample BSample CSample DSample EMean ofsample A = 68Mean ofsample B = 73Mean ofsample C = 82Mean ofsample D = 73Mean ofsample E = 77NormalcurveFrequencyofoccurrence55Figure 11.3 A Sampl<strong>in</strong>g Distribution of Means60 65 70 75 80 85would there be? N<strong>in</strong>e—(1, 1); (1, 2); (1, 3); (2, 1); (2, 2);(2, 3); (3, 1); (3, 2); (3, 3). The means of these samplesare 1, 1.5, 2, 1.5, 2, 2.5, 2, 2.5, <strong>and</strong> 3, respectively. Addup all these means <strong>and</strong> divide by n<strong>in</strong>e (that is, 18 ÷ 9),<strong>and</strong> you see that the mean of these means equals 2, thesame as the population mean.STANDARD ERROR OF THE MEANThe st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution ofmeans is called the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean(SEM). As <strong>in</strong> all normal distributions, therefore, the68-95-99.7 rule holds: approximately 68 percent of thesample means fall between 1 SEM; approximately 95percent fall between 2 SEM; <strong>and</strong> 99.7 percent fallbetween 3 SEM (Figure 11.4).Thus, if we know or can accurately estimate themean <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution,we can determ<strong>in</strong>e whether it is likely or unlikelythat a particular sample mean could be obta<strong>in</strong>ed fromthat population. Suppose the mean of a population is100, for example, <strong>and</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean is10. A sample mean of 110 would fall at 1 SEM, a samplemean of 120 would fall at 2 SEM, a sample meanof 130 would fall at 3 SEM; <strong>and</strong> so forth.It would be very unlikely <strong>to</strong> draw a sample from thepopulation whose mean fell above 3 SEM. Why?Because, as <strong>in</strong> all normal distributions (<strong>and</strong> remember,the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution is a normal distribution—ofmeans), only 0.0013 of all values (<strong>in</strong> this case, samplemeans) fall above 3 SEM. It would not be unusual <strong>to</strong>select a sample from this population <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d that itsmean is 105, but select<strong>in</strong>g a sample with a mean of 130would be unlikely—very unlikely!It is possible <strong>to</strong> use z scores <strong>to</strong> describe the positionof any particular sample mean with<strong>in</strong> a distribution of


220 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 11.4 Distributionof Sample Means99+%95%68%Less likelyUnlikely.0013Extremelyunlikely–3 SEM–3 z–2 SEM–2 z–1 SEM–1 zLikelyoccurrence.1359 .3413 .3413 .1359.0215 .0215+1 SEM+1 zPopulation mean+2 SEM+2 z+3 SEM+3 zLess likelyUnlikely.0013Extremelyunlikelysample means. We discussed z scores <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10. Wenow want <strong>to</strong> express means as z scores. Remember thata z score simply states how far a score (or mean) differsfrom the mean of scores (or means) <strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationunits. The z score tells a researcher exactly where aparticular sample mean is located relative <strong>to</strong> all othersample means that could have been obta<strong>in</strong>ed. For example,a z score of 2 would <strong>in</strong>dicate that a particular samplemean is two st<strong>and</strong>ard errors above the populationmean. Only about 2 percent of all sample means fallabove a z score of 2. Hence, a sample with such amean would be unusual.Estimat<strong>in</strong>g the St<strong>and</strong>ard Error of the Mean.<strong>How</strong> do we obta<strong>in</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean?Clearly, we cannot calculate it directly, s<strong>in</strong>ce we wouldneed, literally, <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a huge number of samples <strong>and</strong>their means.* Statisticians have shown, however, thatthe st<strong>and</strong>ard error can be calculated us<strong>in</strong>g a simple formularequir<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the population<strong>and</strong> the size of the sample. Although we seldom knowthe st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the population, fortunately itcan be estimated† us<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of thesample. To calculate the SEM, then, simply divide the*If we did have these means, we would calculate the st<strong>and</strong>arderror just like any other st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, treat<strong>in</strong>g each mean asa score.†The fact that the st<strong>and</strong>ard error is based on an estimated valuerather than a known value does <strong>in</strong>troduce an unknown degree ofimprecision <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> this process.st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the sample by the square root ofthe sample size m<strong>in</strong>us one:SEM SD4n 1Let’s review the basic ideas we have presented so far.1. The sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of the mean (or anydescriptive statistic) is the distribution of the means(or other statistic) obta<strong>in</strong>ed (theoretically) froman <strong>in</strong>f<strong>in</strong>itely large number of samples of the samesize.2. The shape of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution <strong>in</strong> many (butnot all) cases is the shape of the normal distribution.3. The SEM (st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean)—that is, thest<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution ofmeans—can be estimated by divid<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>arddeviation of the sample by the square root of thesample size m<strong>in</strong>us one.4. The frequency with which a particular sample meanwill occur can be estimated by us<strong>in</strong>g z scores basedon sample data <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate its position <strong>in</strong> the sampl<strong>in</strong>gdistribution.CONFIDENCE INTERVALSWe now can use the SEM <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate boundaries, orlimits, with<strong>in</strong> which the population mean lies. Suchboundaries are called confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals. <strong>How</strong> arethey determ<strong>in</strong>ed?Let us return <strong>to</strong> the example of the researcher whoadm<strong>in</strong>istered an IQ test <strong>to</strong> a sample of 65 elementary


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 221school students. You will recall that she obta<strong>in</strong>ed asample mean of 85 <strong>and</strong> wanted <strong>to</strong> know how much thepopulation mean might differ from this value. We arenow <strong>in</strong> a position <strong>to</strong> give her some help <strong>in</strong> this regard.Let us assume that we have calculated the estimatedst<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean for her sample <strong>and</strong> found it<strong>to</strong> equal 2.0. Apply<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>to</strong> a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distributionof means, we can say that 95 percent of the time thepopulation mean will be between 85 1.96 (2) 85 3.92 81.08 <strong>to</strong> 88.92. Why 1.96? Because thearea between 1.96 z equals 95 percent (.95) of the<strong>to</strong>tal area under the normal curve.* This is shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 11.5.†Suppose this researcher then wished <strong>to</strong> establish an<strong>in</strong>terval that would give her more confidence than p .95 <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g a statement about the population mean.This can be done by calculat<strong>in</strong>g the 99 percent confidence<strong>in</strong>terval. The 99 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval isdeterm<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> a manner similar <strong>to</strong> that for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe 95 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval. Given the characteristicsof a normal distribution, we know that0.5 percent of the sample means will lie below–2.58 SEM <strong>and</strong> another 0.5 percent will lie above2.58 SEM (see Figure 10.12 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10). Us<strong>in</strong>gthe previous example, <strong>in</strong> which the mean of the samplewas 85 <strong>and</strong> the SEM was 2, we calculate the <strong>in</strong>tervalas follows: 85 2.58 (SEM) 85 2.58(2.0) 85 5.16 79.84 <strong>to</strong> 90.16. Thus the 99 confidence<strong>in</strong>terval lies between 79.84 <strong>and</strong> 90.16, as shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 11.6.Our researcher can now answer her question about approximatelyhow much the population mean differs fromthe sample mean. While she cannot know exactly whatthe population mean is, she can <strong>in</strong>dicate the “boundaries”or limits with<strong>in</strong> which it is likely <strong>to</strong> fall (Figure 11.7). Torepeat, these limits are called confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals. The95 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval spans a segment on thehorizontal axis that we are 95 percent certa<strong>in</strong> conta<strong>in</strong>sthe population mean. The 99 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>tervalspans a segment on the horizontal axis with<strong>in</strong> whichwe are even more certa<strong>in</strong> (99 percent certa<strong>in</strong>) that the*By look<strong>in</strong>g at the normal curve table <strong>in</strong> Appendix B, we see thatthe area between the mean <strong>and</strong> 1.96 z .4750. Multiplied by 2, thisequals .95, or 95 percent, of the <strong>to</strong>tal area under the curve.†Strictly speak<strong>in</strong>g, it is not proper <strong>to</strong> consider a distribution ofpopulation means around the sample mean. In practice, we <strong>in</strong>terpretconfidence <strong>in</strong>tervals <strong>in</strong> this way. The legitimacy of do<strong>in</strong>g so requiresa demonstration beyond the level of an <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry text.95%81.0888.92–1.96 SEM+1.96 SEM–1.96 z+1.96 z85Sample meanFigure 11.5 The 95 Percent Confidence Interval79.84–2.58 SEM99%85Sample meanFigure 11.6 The 99 Percent Confidence Interval90.16+2.58 SEMpopulation mean falls.‡ We could be mistaken, ofcourse—the population mean could lie outside these<strong>in</strong>tervals—but it is not very likely.§‡Notice that it is not correct <strong>to</strong> say that the population mean fallswith<strong>in</strong> the 95 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval 95 times out of 100. Thepopulation mean is a fixed value, <strong>and</strong> it either does or does not fallwith<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>terval. The correct way <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of a confidence <strong>in</strong>tervalis <strong>to</strong> view it <strong>in</strong> terms of replicat<strong>in</strong>g the study. Suppose we replicatedthe study with another sample <strong>and</strong> calculated the 95 percent confidence<strong>in</strong>terval for that sample. Suppose we then replicated the studyonce aga<strong>in</strong> with a third sample <strong>and</strong> calculated the 95 percent confidence<strong>in</strong>terval for this third sample. We would cont<strong>in</strong>ue until we haddrawn 100 samples <strong>and</strong> calculated the 95 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>tervalfor each of these 100 samples. We would f<strong>in</strong>d that the populationmean lay with<strong>in</strong> 95 percent of these <strong>in</strong>tervals.§The likelihood of the population mean be<strong>in</strong>g outside the 95 percentconfidence <strong>in</strong>terval is only 5 percent, <strong>and</strong> that of be<strong>in</strong>g outside the99 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval only 1 percent. Analogous reason<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> procedures can be used with sample sizes less than 30.


222 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“You say the averageyears of school<strong>in</strong>g for peopleover 30 <strong>in</strong> the U.S. is 13.7 years?But that’s based on a sample. Youdon’t know what it wouldbe if you had dataon everyone!”“That’s true, but because oursample was r<strong>and</strong>omly selected, <strong>and</strong> thest<strong>and</strong>ard error is 0.3, we can be pretty sure—<strong>in</strong> fact, 99 percent confident—that the averageyears of school<strong>in</strong>g for the whole populationof the country is between 12.8<strong>and</strong> 14.6 years!”Figure 11.7 We Can Be 99 Percent ConfidentCONFIDENCE INTERVALSAND PROBABILITYLet us return <strong>to</strong> the concept of probability <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> Chapter 10. As we use the term here, probability isnoth<strong>in</strong>g more than predicted relative occurrence, or relativefrequency. When we say that someth<strong>in</strong>g wouldoccur 5 times <strong>in</strong> 100, we are express<strong>in</strong>g a probability.We could just as well say the probability is 5 <strong>in</strong> 100.In our earlier example, we can say, therefore, that theprobability of the population mean be<strong>in</strong>g outside the81.08–88.92 limits (the 95 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval)is only 5 <strong>in</strong> 100. The probability of it be<strong>in</strong>g outside the79.84–90.16 limits (the 99 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval)is even less—only 1 <strong>in</strong> 100. Remember that it is cus<strong>to</strong>mary<strong>to</strong> express probabilities <strong>in</strong> decimal form, e.g.,p .05 or p .01. What would p .10 signify?**A probability of 10 <strong>in</strong> 100.COMPARING MORE THAN ONE SAMPLEUp <strong>to</strong> this po<strong>in</strong>t, we have been expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> make<strong>in</strong>ferences about the population mean us<strong>in</strong>g data fromjust one sample. More typically, however, researcherswant <strong>to</strong> compare two or more samples. For example, aresearcher might want <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if there is a difference<strong>in</strong> attitude between fourth-grade boys <strong>and</strong> girls <strong>in</strong>mathematics; whether there is a difference <strong>in</strong> achievementbetween students taught by the discussion methodas compared <strong>to</strong> the lecture method; <strong>and</strong> so forth.Our previous logic also applies <strong>to</strong> a difference betweenmeans. For example, if a difference between means isfound between the test scores of two samples <strong>in</strong> a study,a researcher wants <strong>to</strong> know if a difference exists <strong>in</strong> thepopulations from which the two samples were selected(Figure 11.8). In essence, we ask the same question weasked about one mean, only this time we ask it about a differencebetween means. Hence we ask, “Is the differencePopulationMean = ??Sample AMean = 25PopulationMean = ??Sample BMean = 22Figure 11.8 Does a Sample Difference Reflecta Population Difference? aa Question: Does the three-po<strong>in</strong>t difference between the means of sample A<strong>and</strong> sample B reflect a difference between the means of population A <strong>and</strong>population B?


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 223we have found a likely or an unlikely occurrence?” It ispossible that the difference can be attributed simply <strong>to</strong>sampl<strong>in</strong>g error—<strong>to</strong> the fact that certa<strong>in</strong> samples, ratherthan others, were selected (the “luck of the draw,” so <strong>to</strong>speak). Once aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics help us out.99+%95%THE STANDARD ERROR OF THE DIFFERENCEBETWEEN SAMPLE MEANSFortunately, differences between sample means are alsolikely <strong>to</strong> be normally distributed. The distribution of differencesbetween sample means also has its own mean<strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation. The mean of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distributionof differences between sample means is equal<strong>to</strong> the difference between the means of the two populations.The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of this distribution iscalled the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference (SED).The formula for comput<strong>in</strong>g the SED is:SED 21SEM 1 2 2 1SEM 2 2 2where 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 refer <strong>to</strong> the respective samples.Because the distribution is normal, slightly more than68 percent of the differences between sample means willfall between 1 SED (aga<strong>in</strong>, remember that the st<strong>and</strong>arderror of the difference is a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation); about95 percent of the differences between sample means willfall between 2 SED, <strong>and</strong> 99 percent of these differenceswill fall between 3 SED (Figure 11.9).Now we can proceed similarly <strong>to</strong> the way we did with<strong>in</strong>dividual sample means. A researcher estimates thest<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference between means <strong>and</strong> thenuses it, along with the difference between the two samplemeans <strong>and</strong> the normal curve, <strong>to</strong> estimate probable limits(confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals) with<strong>in</strong> which the difference betweenthe means of the two populations is likely <strong>to</strong> fall.Let us consider an example. Imag<strong>in</strong>e that the differencebetween two sample means is 14 raw score po<strong>in</strong>ts<strong>and</strong> the calculated SED is 3. Just as we did with onesample population mean, we can now <strong>in</strong>dicate limitswith<strong>in</strong> which the difference between the means of thetwo populations is likely <strong>to</strong> fall. If we say that the differencebetween the means of the two populations isbetween 11 <strong>and</strong> 17 (1 SED), we have slightly morethan a 68 percent chance of be<strong>in</strong>g right. We have somewhatmore than a 95 percent chance of be<strong>in</strong>g right if wesay the difference between the means of the two populationsis between 8 <strong>and</strong> 20 (2 SED), <strong>and</strong> better than a99 percent chance of be<strong>in</strong>g right if we say the differencebetween the means of the two populations is between5 <strong>and</strong> 23 (3 SED). Figure 11.10 illustrates these confidence<strong>in</strong>tervals.68%–3 SED –2 SED –1 SED +1 SED +2 SED +3 SEDDifferencebetween the meansof the two populationsFigure 11.9 Distribution of the Difference BetweenSample Means5–3 SED8–2 SED11–1 SED99+%95%68%14Suppose the difference between two other samplemeans is 12. If we calculated the SED <strong>to</strong> be 2, would itbe likely or unlikely for the difference between populationmeans <strong>to</strong> fall between 10 <strong>and</strong> 14?*Hypothesis Test<strong>in</strong>g17+1 SEDObta<strong>in</strong>ed differencebetween sample meansFigure 11.10 Confidence Intervals20+2 SED23+3 SED<strong>How</strong> does all this apply <strong>to</strong> research questions <strong>and</strong>research hypotheses? You will recall that many hypothesespredict a relationship. In Chapter 10, we presentedtechniques for exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g data for the existence of relationships.We po<strong>in</strong>ted out <strong>in</strong> previous chapters that virtuallyall relationships <strong>in</strong> data can be exam<strong>in</strong>ed throughone (or more) of three procedures: a comparison of*Likely, s<strong>in</strong>ce 68 percent of the differences between populationmeans fall between these values.


224 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7emeans, a correlation, or a crossbreak table. In each <strong>in</strong>stance,some degree of relationship may be found. If arelationship is found <strong>in</strong> the data, is there likely <strong>to</strong> be asimilar relationship <strong>in</strong> the population, or is it simply due<strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g error—<strong>to</strong> the fact that a particular sample,rather than another, was selected for study? Once aga<strong>in</strong>,<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics can help.The logic discussed earlier applies <strong>to</strong> any particularform of a hypothesis <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> many procedures used <strong>to</strong>exam<strong>in</strong>e data. Thus, correlation coefficients <strong>and</strong> differencesbetween them can be evaluated <strong>in</strong> essentially thesame way as means <strong>and</strong> differences between means; wejust need <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the correlationcoefficient(s). The procedure used with crossbreaktables differs <strong>in</strong> technique, but the logic is the same. Wewill discuss it later <strong>in</strong> the chapter.When test<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses, it is cus<strong>to</strong>mary <strong>to</strong> proceed<strong>in</strong> a slightly different way. Instead of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g theboundaries with<strong>in</strong> which the population mean (or otherparameter) can be said <strong>to</strong> fall, a researcher determ<strong>in</strong>esthe likelihood of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a sample value (for example,a difference between two sample means) if there is norelationship (that is, no difference between the means ofthe two populations) <strong>in</strong> the populations from which thesamples were drawn. The researcher formulates both aresearch hypothesis <strong>and</strong> a null hypothesis. To test the researchhypothesis, the researcher must formulate a nullhypothesis.THE NULL HYPOTHESISAs you will recall, the research hypothesis specifies thepredicted outcome of a study. Many research hypothesespredict the nature of the relationship the researcherth<strong>in</strong>ks exists <strong>in</strong> the population; for example: “The populationmean of students us<strong>in</strong>g method A is greater thanthe population mean of students us<strong>in</strong>g method B.”The null hypothesis most commonly used specifiesthere is no relationship <strong>in</strong> the population; for example:“There is no difference between the population mean ofstudents us<strong>in</strong>g method A <strong>and</strong> the population mean ofstudents us<strong>in</strong>g method B.” (This is the same th<strong>in</strong>g assay<strong>in</strong>g the difference between the means of the twopopulations is zero.) Figure 11.11 offers a comparisonof research <strong>and</strong> null hypotheses.The researcher then proceeds <strong>to</strong> test the null hypothesis.The same <strong>in</strong>formation is needed as before: the knowledgethat the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution is normal, <strong>and</strong> thecalculated st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference (SED). What isdifferent <strong>in</strong> a hypothesis test is that <strong>in</strong>stead of us<strong>in</strong>g theobta<strong>in</strong>ed sample value (e.g., the obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencebetweensamplemeans)asthemeanofthesampl<strong>in</strong>gdistribution(aswedidwithconfidence<strong>in</strong>tervals),weusezero.*We then can determ<strong>in</strong>e the probability of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g aparticular sample value (such as an obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencebetween sample means) by see<strong>in</strong>g where such a valuefalls on the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution. If the probability issmall, the null hypothesis is rejected, thereby provid<strong>in</strong>gsupport for the research hypothesis. The results are said<strong>to</strong> be statistically significant.What counts as “small”? In other words, what constitutesan unlikely outcome? Probably you have guessed.It is cus<strong>to</strong>mary <strong>in</strong> educational research <strong>to</strong> view asunlikely any outcome that has a probability of .05(p .05) or less. This is referred <strong>to</strong> as the .05 level ofsignificance. When we reject a null hypothesis at the.05 level, we are say<strong>in</strong>g that the probability of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gsuch an outcome is only 5 times (or less) <strong>in</strong> 100. Someresearchers prefer <strong>to</strong> be even more str<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>and</strong> choosea .01 level of significance. When a null hypothesis isrejected at the .01 level, it means that the likelihood ofobta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the outcome is only 1 time (or less) <strong>in</strong> 100.HYPOTHESIS TESTING: A REVIEWLet us review what we have said. The logical sequencefor a researcher who wishes <strong>to</strong> engage <strong>in</strong> hypothesistest<strong>in</strong>g is as follows:1. State the research hypothesis (e.g., “There is a differencebetween the population mean of studentsus<strong>in</strong>g method A <strong>and</strong> the population mean of studentsus<strong>in</strong>g method B”).2. State the null hypothesis (e.g., “There is no differencebetween the population mean of students us<strong>in</strong>gmethod A <strong>and</strong> the population mean of students us<strong>in</strong>gmethod B,” or “The difference between the two populationmeans is zero”).3. Determ<strong>in</strong>e the sample statistics pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> the hypothesis(e.g., the mean of sample A <strong>and</strong> the mean ofsample B).4. Determ<strong>in</strong>e the probability of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the sampleresults (i.e., the difference between the mean ofsample A <strong>and</strong> the mean of sample B) if the nullhypothesis is true.5. If the probability is small, reject the null hypothesis,thus affirm<strong>in</strong>g the research hypothesis.6. If the probability is large, do not reject the nullhypothesis, which means you cannot affirm theresearch hypothesis.*Actually, any value could be used, but zero is used <strong>in</strong> virtually alleducational research.


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 225(Null Hypothesis)“You know, I readrecently that most researchersbelieve there really is no difference betweenthe average score of students taught mathematicsby the ’new’ math <strong>and</strong> the average score ofthose students taught by the traditionallecture/discussion approach.”(<strong>Research</strong> Hypothesis)“I don’t believe it.Let’s compare a sample of ourstudents this year <strong>and</strong> see. I bet thatthe students taught by the newmath will do better.”Figure 11.11 Null <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> HypothesesFigure 11.12Illustration of When a<strong>Research</strong>er Would Rejectthe Null Hypothesis–9–3 SED–6–2 SED–3–1 SED0 3 6 9+1 SED +2 SED +3 SED12+4 SEDRawscores14Obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencebetween sample meansLet us use our previous example <strong>in</strong> which the differencebetween sample means was 14 po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>and</strong> the SEDwas 3 (see Figure 11.10). In Figure 11.12 we see that thesample difference of 14 falls far beyond 3 SED; <strong>in</strong>fact, it exceeds 4 SED. Thus, the probability of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gsuch a sample result is considerably less than .01,<strong>and</strong> as a result, the null hypothesis is rejected. If thedifference <strong>in</strong> sample means had been 4 <strong>in</strong>stead of 14,would the null hypothesis be rejected?**No. The probability of a difference of 4 is <strong>to</strong>o high—much largerthan .05.


226 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePractical VersusStatistical SignificanceThe fact that a result is statistically significant (not due<strong>to</strong> chance) does not mean that it has any practical or educationalvalue <strong>in</strong> the real world <strong>in</strong> which we all work<strong>and</strong> live. Statistical significance only means that one’sresults are likely <strong>to</strong> occur by chance less than a certa<strong>in</strong>percentage of the time, say 5 percent. So what? Rememberthat this only means the observed relationship mostlikely would not be zero <strong>in</strong> the population. But it doesnot mean necessarily that it is important! Whenever wehave a large enough r<strong>and</strong>om sample, almost any resultwill turn out <strong>to</strong> be statistically significant. Thus, a verysmall correlation coefficient, for example, may turn out<strong>to</strong> be statistically significant but have little (if any)practical significance. In a similar sense, a very smalldifference <strong>in</strong> means may yield a statistically significantresult but have little educational import.Consider a few examples. Suppose a r<strong>and</strong>om sampleof 1,000 high school baseball pitchers on the East Coastreveals an average fastball speed of 75 mph, while asecond r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 1,000 high school pitchers <strong>in</strong>the Midwest shows an average fastball speed of 71 mph.Now this difference of 4 mph might be statistically significant(due <strong>to</strong> the large sample size), but we doubt thatbaseball fans would say that it is of any practical importance(see Figure 11.13). Or suppose that a researchertries out a new method of teach<strong>in</strong>g mathematics <strong>to</strong> highschool juniors. She f<strong>in</strong>ds that those students exposed <strong>to</strong>method A (the new method) score, on the average, twopo<strong>in</strong>ts higher on the f<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ation than the studentsexposed <strong>to</strong> method B (the older, more traditional method),<strong>and</strong> that this difference is statistically significant. Wedoubt that the mathematics department would immediatelyencourage all of its members <strong>to</strong> adopt method A onthe basis of this two-po<strong>in</strong>t difference. Would you?Ironically, the fact that most educational studies <strong>in</strong>volvesmaller samples may actually be an advantagewhen it comes <strong>to</strong> practical significance. Becausesmaller sample size makes it harder <strong>to</strong> detect a differenceeven when there is one <strong>in</strong> the population, a largerdifference <strong>in</strong> means is therefore required <strong>to</strong> reject thenull hypothesis. This is so because a smaller sample results<strong>in</strong> a larger st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference <strong>in</strong> means(SED). Therefore, a larger difference <strong>in</strong> means is required<strong>to</strong> reach the significance level (see Figure 11.10).“I th<strong>in</strong>k we can say thatmy guy’s fastball is pretty impressive—<strong>in</strong> fact, you might say it issignificantly faster thanyour guy’s.”“Are you kidd<strong>in</strong>g?Your guy has been clocked at79 mph, <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>e at 77 mph. Youcall a difference of 2 mphsignificant?”Figure 11.13 <strong>How</strong> Much Is Enough?


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 227It is also possible that relationships of potential practicalsignificance may be overlooked or dismissed becausethey are not statistically significant (more on this <strong>in</strong> thenext chapter).One should always take care <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results—just because one br<strong>and</strong> of radios is significantly morepowerful than another br<strong>and</strong> statistically does not meanthat those look<strong>in</strong>g for a radio should rush <strong>to</strong> buy the firstbr<strong>and</strong>.–2 z –1 z 0+1 z +2 zShaded arearepresents5% of areaunder the curveONE- AND TWO-TAILED TESTSIn Chapter 3, we made a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between directional<strong>and</strong> nondirectional hypotheses. There is sometimes anadvantage <strong>to</strong> stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses <strong>in</strong> directional form thatis related <strong>to</strong> significance test<strong>in</strong>g. We refer aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> a hypotheticalexample of a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of differencesbetween means <strong>in</strong> which the calculated SEDequals 3. Previously, we <strong>in</strong>terpreted the statistical significanceof an obta<strong>in</strong>ed difference between samplemeans of 14 po<strong>in</strong>ts. The statistical significance of thisdifference was quite clear-cut, s<strong>in</strong>ce it was so large.Suppose, however, that the obta<strong>in</strong>ed difference was not14, but 5.5 po<strong>in</strong>ts. To determ<strong>in</strong>e the probability associatedwith this outcome, we must know whether the researcher’shypothesis was a directional or a nondirectionalone. If the hypothesis was directional, theresearcher specified ahead of time (before collect<strong>in</strong>gany data) which group would have the higher mean (forexample, “The mean score of students us<strong>in</strong>g method Awill be higher than the mean score of students us<strong>in</strong>gmethod B.”).If this had been the case, the researcher’s hypothesiswould be supported only if the mean of sample A werehigher than the mean of sample B. The researcher mustdecide beforeh<strong>and</strong> that he or she will subtract the meanof sample B from the mean of sample A. A large differencebetween sample means <strong>in</strong> the opposite directionwould not support the research hypothesis. A differencebetween sample means of 2 is <strong>in</strong> the hypothesized direction,therefore, but a difference of –2 (should themean of sample B be higher than the mean of sample A)is not. Because the researchers’ hypothesis can be supportedonly if he or she obta<strong>in</strong>s a positive difference betweenthe sample means, the researcher is justified <strong>in</strong>us<strong>in</strong>g only the positive tail of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution<strong>to</strong> locate the obta<strong>in</strong>ed difference. This is referred <strong>to</strong> as aone-tailed test of statistical significance (Figure 11.14).At the 5 percent level of significance (p .05), thenull hypothesis may be rejected only if the obta<strong>in</strong>ed1.65 zFigure 11.14 Significance Area for a One-Tailed Testdifference between sample means reaches or exceeds1.65 SED* <strong>in</strong> the one tail. As shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 11.15,this requires a difference between sample means of 5po<strong>in</strong>ts or more.† Our previously obta<strong>in</strong>ed difference of5.5 would be significant at this level, therefore, s<strong>in</strong>ce itnot only reaches, but exceeds, 1.65 SED.What if the hypothesis were nondirectional? If thiswere the case, the researcher would not have specifiedbeforeh<strong>and</strong> which group would have the higher mean.The hypothesis would then be supported by a suitabledifference <strong>in</strong> either tail. This is called a two-tailed tes<strong>to</strong>f statistical significance. If a researcher uses the .05level of significance, this requires that the 5 percent ofthe <strong>to</strong>tal area must <strong>in</strong>clude both tails—that is, there is2.5 percent <strong>in</strong> each tail. As a result, a difference <strong>in</strong> samplemeans of almost 6 po<strong>in</strong>ts (either 5.88 or –5.88) isrequired <strong>to</strong> reject the null hypothesis (Figure 11.16),s<strong>in</strong>ce 1.96(3) 5.88.USE OF THE NULL HYPOTHESIS:AN EVALUATIONThere appears <strong>to</strong> be much misunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g regard<strong>in</strong>gthe use of the null hypothesis. First, it often is stated <strong>in</strong>place of a research hypothesis. While it is easy <strong>to</strong> replacea research hypothesis (which predicts a relationship)with a null hypothesis (which predicts no relationship),there is no good reason for do<strong>in</strong>g so. As we have*By look<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the normal curve table <strong>in</strong> Appendix B, we see thatthe area beyond 1.65 z equals .05, or 5 percent of the area under thecurve.†S<strong>in</strong>ce an area of .05 <strong>in</strong> one tail equals a z of 1.65, <strong>and</strong> the SEDis 3, we multiply 1.65(3) <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d the score value at this po<strong>in</strong>t:1.65(3) 4.95, or 5 po<strong>in</strong>ts.


228 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 11.15 One-Tailed Test Us<strong>in</strong>g aDistribution ofDifferences BetweenSample Means1 SEDSED = 3All obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencesbetween sample meansthat fall <strong>in</strong> this shadedarea (i.e., reach 1.65 zor beyond) would cause aresearcher <strong>to</strong> reject thenull hypothesis..0503+1 z55.51.65 zFigure 11.16 Two-Tailed Test Us<strong>in</strong>g aDistribution ofDifferences BetweenSample Means.0251 SED1 SEDSED = 3All obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencesbetween sample meansthat fall <strong>in</strong> this shadedarea (i.e., reach either+1.96 z or –1.96 z or beyond)would cause a researcher <strong>to</strong>reject the null hypothesis.–5.88–1.96 z–3–1 z03+1 z5.88+1.96 z.025seen, the null hypothesis is merely a useful methodologicaldevice.Second, there is noth<strong>in</strong>g sacred about the cus<strong>to</strong>mary .05<strong>and</strong> .01 significance levels—they are merely conventions.It is a little ridiculous, for example, <strong>to</strong> fail <strong>to</strong> reject the nullhypothesis with a sample value that has a probability of.06. To do so might very well result <strong>in</strong> what is known as aType II error—this error results when a researcher fails <strong>to</strong>reject a null hypothesis that is false.AType I error, on theother h<strong>and</strong>, results when a researcher rejects a null hypothesisthat is true. In our example <strong>in</strong> which there was a14-po<strong>in</strong>t difference between sample means, for example,we rejected the null hypothesis at the .05 level. In do<strong>in</strong>g so,we realized that a 5 percent chance rema<strong>in</strong>ed of be<strong>in</strong>gwrong—that is, a 5 percent chance that the null hypothesiswas true. Figure 11.17 provides an example of Type I <strong>and</strong>Type II errors.F<strong>in</strong>ally, there is also noth<strong>in</strong>g sacrosanct about test<strong>in</strong>gan obta<strong>in</strong>ed result aga<strong>in</strong>st zero. In our previous example,for <strong>in</strong>stance, why not test the obta<strong>in</strong>ed value of 14(or 5.5, etc.) aga<strong>in</strong>st a hypothetical population differenceof 1 (or 3, etc.)? Test<strong>in</strong>g only aga<strong>in</strong>st zero canmislead one <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> exaggerat<strong>in</strong>g the importance of theobta<strong>in</strong>ed relationship. We believe the report<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>ferentialstatistics should rely more on confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals<strong>and</strong> less on whether a particular level of significance hasbeen atta<strong>in</strong>ed.Inference TechniquesIt is beyond the scope of this text <strong>to</strong> treat <strong>in</strong> detail eachof the many techniques that exist for answer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferencequestions about data. We shall, however, present abrief summary of the more commonly used tests of statisticalsignificance that researchers employ <strong>and</strong> then illustratehow <strong>to</strong> do one such test.In Chapter 10, we made a dist<strong>in</strong>ction between quantitative<strong>and</strong> categorical data. We po<strong>in</strong>ted out that thetype of data a researcher collects often <strong>in</strong>fluences thetype of statistical analysis required. A statistical techniqueappropriate for quantitative data, for example,will generally be <strong>in</strong>appropriate for categorical data.There are two basic types of <strong>in</strong>ference techniquesthat researchers use. Parametric techniques makevarious k<strong>in</strong>ds of assumptions about the nature of the


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 229Susie has pneumonia.Susie does nothave pneumonia.Doc<strong>to</strong>r says that symp<strong>to</strong>mslike Susie’s occur only 5 percen<strong>to</strong>f the time <strong>in</strong> healthy people.To be safe, however, he decides<strong>to</strong> treat Susie for pneumonia.Doc<strong>to</strong>r is correct. Susiedoes have pneumonia <strong>and</strong>the treatment cures her.Doc<strong>to</strong>r is wrong. Susie’streatment was unnecessary<strong>and</strong> possibly unpleasant<strong>and</strong> expensive. Type Ierror ()Doc<strong>to</strong>r says that symp<strong>to</strong>mslike Susie’s occur 95 percent ofthe time <strong>in</strong> healthy people.In his judgment, therefore,her symp<strong>to</strong>ms are a falsealarm <strong>and</strong> do not warranttreatment, <strong>and</strong> he decides not<strong>to</strong> treat Susie for pneumonia.Doc<strong>to</strong>r is wrong. Susie isnot treated <strong>and</strong> may sufferserious consequences. TypeII error ()Doc<strong>to</strong>r is correct.Unnecessary treatmentis avoided.Figure 11.17 A Hypothetical Example of Type I <strong>and</strong> Type II Errorspopulation from which the sample(s) <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the researchstudy are drawn. Nonparametric techniques,onthe other h<strong>and</strong>, make few (if any) assumptions about thenature of the population from which the samples aretaken. An advantage of parametric techniques is thatthey are generally more powerful* than nonparametrictechniques <strong>and</strong> hence much more likely <strong>to</strong> reveal a truedifference or relationship if one really exists. Their disadvantageis that often a researcher cannot satisfy the assumptionsthey require (for example, that the populationis normally distributed on the characteristic of <strong>in</strong>terest).The advantage of nonparametric techniques is that theyare safer <strong>to</strong> use when a researcher cannot satisfy the assumptionsunderly<strong>in</strong>g the use of parametric techniques.PARAMETRIC TECHNIQUESFOR ANALYZING QUANTITATIVE DATA †The t-Test for Means. The t-test is a parametricstatistical test used <strong>to</strong> see whether a difference between*We discuss the power of a statistical test later <strong>in</strong> this chapter.†Many texts dist<strong>in</strong>guish between techniques appropriate for nom<strong>in</strong>al,ord<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terval scales of measurement (see Chapter 7). It turnsout that <strong>in</strong> most cases parametric techniques are most appropriate for<strong>in</strong>terval data, while nonparametric techniques are more appropriatefor ord<strong>in</strong>al <strong>and</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>al data. <strong>Research</strong>ers rarely know for certa<strong>in</strong>whether their data justify the assumption that <strong>in</strong>terval scales haveactually been used.the means of two samples is significant. The test producesa value for t (called an obta<strong>in</strong>ed t), which the researcherthen checks <strong>in</strong> a statistical table (similar <strong>to</strong> theone shown <strong>in</strong> Appendix B) <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the level of significancethat has been reached. As we mentioned earlier,if the .05 level of significance is reached, the researchercus<strong>to</strong>marily rejects the null hypothesis <strong>and</strong>concludes that a real difference does exist.There are two forms of this t-test, a t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependentmeans <strong>and</strong> a t-test for correlated means. Thet-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent means is used <strong>to</strong> compare themean scores of two different, or <strong>in</strong>dependent, groups.For example, if two r<strong>and</strong>omly selected groups of eighthgraders(31 <strong>in</strong> each group) were exposed <strong>to</strong> two differentmethods of teach<strong>in</strong>g for a semester <strong>and</strong> then giventhe same achievement test at the end of the semester,their achievement scores could be compared us<strong>in</strong>g at-test. Let us assume that the researcher suspected thatthose students exposed <strong>to</strong> method A would score significantlyhigher on the end-of-semester achievement test.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, she formulated the follow<strong>in</strong>g null <strong>and</strong> alternativehypotheses:Null hypothesis: population mean of method A population mean of method B<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: population mean of methodA population mean of method BShe f<strong>in</strong>ds that the mean score on the achievement tes<strong>to</strong>f the students who were taught by method A equals 85


RESEARCH TIPSSample SizeStudents frequently ask for more specific rules on samplesize. Unfortunately, there are no simple answers. <strong>How</strong>ever,under certa<strong>in</strong> conditions, some guidel<strong>in</strong>es are available.The most important condition is r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, but thereare other specific requirements that are discussed <strong>in</strong> statisticstexts. Assum<strong>in</strong>g these assumptions are met, the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>the <strong>to</strong>p table apply:Sample size required for conclud<strong>in</strong>g that a sample correlationcoefficient is statistically significant (i.e., different fromzero <strong>in</strong> the population) at the .05 level of confidence.Sample size required for conclud<strong>in</strong>g that a difference <strong>in</strong>sample means is statistically significant (i.e., the differencebetween the means of the two populations is not zero) at the.05 level of confidence. These calculations require that thepopulation st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation be known or estimated fromthe sample st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations. Let us assume, for example,that the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation <strong>in</strong> both populations is 15 <strong>and</strong> eachof the samples is the same size.Value of sample r .05 .10 .15 .20 .25 .30 .40 .50Sample size required 1,539 400 177 100 64 49 25 16Difference between 2 5 10 15the sample means po<strong>in</strong>ts po<strong>in</strong>ts po<strong>in</strong>ts po<strong>in</strong>tsRequired size 434 71 18 8of each sample<strong>and</strong> that the mean score of the students taught bymethod B equals 80. Obviously, these two samplemeans are different. <strong>How</strong>ever, this difference could bedue <strong>to</strong> chance (i.e., sampl<strong>in</strong>g error). The key question iswhether these means are different enough that ourresearcher can conclude that the difference is mostlikely not due <strong>to</strong> chance but actually <strong>to</strong> the differencebetween the two teach<strong>in</strong>g methods.To evaluate her research hypothesis, our researcherconducts a one-tailed t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent samples <strong>and</strong>f<strong>in</strong>ds that the t-value (the test statistic) is 2.18. To be statisticallysignificant at the .05 level, with 60 degrees offreedom (df ), a t-value of at least 1.67 is required, s<strong>in</strong>cethe test is a one-tailed test. Because the obta<strong>in</strong>ed t-valueof 2.18 is beyond 1.67, it is an unlikely value, <strong>and</strong> hencethe null hypothesis is rejected. Our researcher concludesthat the difference between the two means is statisticallysignificant—that is, it was not merely a chance occurrencebut <strong>in</strong>deed represents a real difference betweenthe achievement scores of the two groups.Let us say a word about the concept degrees of freedom(df ), which we referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> the example above.This concept is quite important <strong>in</strong> many <strong>in</strong>ferential statisticstests. In essence, it refers <strong>to</strong> the number of scores <strong>in</strong> afrequency distribution that are “free <strong>to</strong> vary”—that is,that are not fixed. For example, suppose you had a distributionof only three scores, a, b, <strong>and</strong> c, that must add up <strong>to</strong>10. It is apparent that a, b, <strong>and</strong> c can have a number of differentvalues (such as 3, 5, <strong>and</strong> 2; or 1, 6, <strong>and</strong> 3; or 2, 2, <strong>and</strong>6) <strong>and</strong> still add up <strong>to</strong> 10. But once any two of these valuesare fixed, or set, the third value is also set; it cannot vary.Thus, should a 3 <strong>and</strong> b 5, c must equal 2. Hence,we say that there are two degrees of freedom <strong>in</strong> this distribution;any two of the values are “free <strong>to</strong> vary,” so <strong>to</strong>speak, but once they are set, the third is also fixed.Degrees of freedom are calculated <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dependentsamples t-test by subtract<strong>in</strong>g 2 from the <strong>to</strong>tal number ofvalues <strong>in</strong> both groups.* In this example, there are twogroups, with 31 students <strong>in</strong> each group; hence there are30 values <strong>in</strong> each group that are free <strong>to</strong> vary, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>a <strong>to</strong>tal of 60 degrees of freedom.The t-test for correlated means is used <strong>to</strong> comparethe mean scores of the same group before <strong>and</strong> after atreatment of some sort is given, <strong>to</strong> see if any observedga<strong>in</strong> is significant, or when the research design <strong>in</strong>volves*In a study with only one group, you would subtract 1 from the <strong>to</strong>talvalue of the group.230


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 231Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS <strong>to</strong>Conduct an Independent Samples t-TestLet us suppose that a researcher conducts an experiment <strong>in</strong> which she exposes two r<strong>and</strong>omlyselected groups of fourth-graders (10 <strong>in</strong> each group) <strong>to</strong> two different methods of teach<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>gfor a week <strong>and</strong> then gives them a 35-word spell<strong>in</strong>g test at the end of the week. The researcherrecords the number of words correctly spelled by each student. Here are the results:Group A: 19, 20, 24, 30, 31, 32, 30, 27, 25, 22 mean 26Group B: 23, 22, 15, 16, 18, 12, 16, 19, 14, 25 mean 1819 120 124 130 131 132 130 127 125 122 123 222 215 216 218 212 216 219 214 225 2To use SPSS <strong>to</strong> conduct an <strong>in</strong>dependent samples t-test on this data, you first enter the scores <strong>in</strong>what is called a stacked format, mean<strong>in</strong>g that all of the scores from both samples are entered <strong>in</strong>one column of the data table. Values are then entered <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a second column <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the treatmentcondition correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> each of the scores. For example, <strong>in</strong> the second column, the numeral1 could be entered beside each score from sample group A <strong>and</strong> the numeral 2 beside eachscore from sample group B, as shown. Then proceed us<strong>in</strong>g the follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions:Click Analyze on the <strong>to</strong>olbar. When the menu appears, select Compare Means, <strong>and</strong> click onIndependent-Samples T Test. Highlight the label <strong>in</strong> the left box for the column conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g theset of scores <strong>and</strong> move it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Test Variable” box. Then highlight <strong>and</strong> move the label for thecolumn that conta<strong>in</strong>s the group number for each score <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Group<strong>in</strong>g Variable” box. Next,click on Def<strong>in</strong>e Groups. If a 1 is used <strong>to</strong> identify sample group A <strong>and</strong> a 2 <strong>to</strong> identify sample groupB, enter the values 1 <strong>and</strong> 2 <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the appropriate group boxes, then click Cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>and</strong> OK.SPSS will produce a summary table conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g two boxes show<strong>in</strong>g the results of the analysis,the ma<strong>in</strong> parts of which we reproduce here:Group n Mean S.D. SEM1 10 26.0 4.7 1.492 10 18.0 4.2 1.3395%Significance Mean Std. Error Confidencet df (2-tailed) difference Difference Interval4.00 18 .001 8.00 2.00 3.8–12.2Thus, SPSS reveals that the difference <strong>in</strong> means is statistically significant at the .001 level,<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that a difference this large (8 po<strong>in</strong>ts) could occur by chance only 1 out of 1,000 times.two matched groups. It is also used when the same subjectsreceive two different treatments <strong>in</strong> a study.Consider an example. Suppose a sports psychologistbelieves that anxiety often makes basketball players performpoorly when shoot<strong>in</strong>g free throws <strong>in</strong> close games.She decides <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effectiveness of relaxationtra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for reduc<strong>in</strong>g the level of anxiety such athletesexperience <strong>and</strong> thus improv<strong>in</strong>g their performance at thefree throw l<strong>in</strong>e. She formulates these hypotheses:Null hypothesis: There will be no change <strong>in</strong> performanceat the free throw l<strong>in</strong>e.<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: Performance at the free throwl<strong>in</strong>e will improve.She r<strong>and</strong>omly selects a sample of 15 athletes <strong>to</strong> undergothe tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the week before the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gsessions (the treatment), she measures the athletes’ levelof anxiety. She then exposes the athletes <strong>to</strong> the treatment.


232 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFor a week afterwards, she aga<strong>in</strong> measures the athletes’level of anxiety.She f<strong>in</strong>ds that the mean number of free throwscompleted equals five per game before the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> seven after the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g has been completed. Sheconducts a t-test for repeated measures <strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>s at-statistic of 2.43. Statistical significance at the .05 level,for a one-tailed test, with 14 degrees of freedom,requires a t-statistic of at least 1.76. S<strong>in</strong>ce the obta<strong>in</strong>edt-value is more than that, the researcher concludes thatsuch a result is unusual (i.e., unlikely <strong>to</strong> be a chance result);therefore, she rejects the null hypothesis. She concludesthat relaxation therapy can reduce the anxietylevel of these athletes.Analysis of Variance. When researchers desire <strong>to</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d out whether there are significant differences betweenthe means of more than two groups, they commonlyuse a technique called analysis of variance(ANOVA), which is actually a more general form of thet-test that is appropriate <strong>to</strong> use with three or moregroups. (It can also be used with two groups.) In brief,variation both with<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> between each of the groups isanalyzed statistically, yield<strong>in</strong>g what is known as an Fvalue. As <strong>in</strong> a t-test, this F value is then checked <strong>in</strong> a statisticaltable <strong>to</strong> see if it is statistically significant. It is <strong>in</strong>terpretedquite similarly <strong>to</strong> the t-value, <strong>in</strong> that the largerthe obta<strong>in</strong>ed value of F, the greater the likelihood thatstatistical significance exists.For example, imag<strong>in</strong>e that a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe effectiveness of three drugs used <strong>to</strong> relieveheadaches. She r<strong>and</strong>omly selects three groups of <strong>in</strong>dividualswho rout<strong>in</strong>ely suffer from headaches (with 20 <strong>in</strong>each group) <strong>and</strong> formulates the follow<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses:Null hypothesis: There is no difference betweengroups.<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: There is a difference betweengroups.The researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s an F value of 3.95. The criticalregion for the correspond<strong>in</strong>g degrees of freedom at the.05 level, for a two-tailed test, is 3.17. Therefore, shewould reject the null hypothesis <strong>and</strong> conclude that theeffectiveness of the three drugs is not the same.When only two groups are be<strong>in</strong>g compared, the Ftest is sufficient <strong>to</strong> tell the researcher whether significancehas been achieved. When more than two groupsare be<strong>in</strong>g compared, the F test will not, by itself, tellus which of the means are different. A further (butquite simple) procedure, called a post hoc analysis, isrequired <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d this out. ANOVA is also used whenmore than one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is <strong>in</strong>vestigated, as<strong>in</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rial designs, which we discuss <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13.Analysis of Covariance. Analysis of covariance(ANCOVA) is a variation of ANOVA used when, forexample, groups are given a pretest related <strong>in</strong> some way<strong>to</strong> the dependent variable <strong>and</strong> their mean scores on thispretest are found <strong>to</strong> differ. ANCOVA enables the researcher<strong>to</strong> adjust the posttest mean scores on the dependentvariable for each group <strong>to</strong> compensate for the <strong>in</strong>itialdifferences between the groups on the pretest. Thepretest is called the covariate. <strong>How</strong> much the posttestmean scores must be adjusted depends on how large thedifference between the pretest means is <strong>and</strong> the degreeof relationship between the covariate <strong>and</strong> the dependentvariable. Several covariates can be used <strong>in</strong> an ANCOVAtest, so <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> (or <strong>in</strong>stead of) adjust<strong>in</strong>g for apretest, the researcher can adjust for the effect of othervariables. (We discuss this further <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13). LikeANOVA, ANCOVA produces an F value, which is thenlooked up <strong>in</strong> a statistical table <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether it isstatistically significant.Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Multivariateanalysis of variance (MANOVA) differs fromANOVA <strong>in</strong> only one respect: It <strong>in</strong>corporates two ormore dependent variables <strong>in</strong> the same analysis, thus permitt<strong>in</strong>ga more powerful test of differences amongmeans. It is justified only when the researcher has reason<strong>to</strong> believe correlations exist among the dependent variables.Similarly, multivariate analysis of covariance(MANCOVA) extends ANCOVA <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude two ormore dependent variables <strong>in</strong> the same analysis. The specificvalue that is calculated is Wilk’s lambda, a numberanalogous <strong>to</strong> F <strong>in</strong> analysis of variance.The t-Test for r. This t-test is used <strong>to</strong> see whethera correlation coefficient calculated on sample data issignificant—that is, whether it represents a nonzero correlation<strong>in</strong> the population from which the sample wasdrawn. It is similar <strong>to</strong> the t-test for means, except thathere the statistic be<strong>in</strong>g dealt with is a correlation coefficient(r) rather than a difference between means. Thetest produces a value for t (aga<strong>in</strong> called an obta<strong>in</strong>ed t),which the researcher checks <strong>in</strong> a statistical probabilitytable <strong>to</strong> see whether it is statistically significant. As withthe other parametric tests, the larger the obta<strong>in</strong>ed valuefor t, the greater the likelihood that significance hasbeen achieved.


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 233For example, a researcher is us<strong>in</strong>g a regular twotailedtest, with alpha .05, <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if a nonzerocorrelation exists <strong>in</strong> a particular population. He r<strong>and</strong>omlyselects a sample of 30 <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Check<strong>in</strong>g theappropriate statistical table (critical values for the Pearsoncorrelation), he f<strong>in</strong>ds that with α .05 <strong>and</strong> n 30(28 df ), the table lists a critical value of 0.361. The samplecorrelation, therefore (<strong>in</strong>dependent of sign) musthave a value equal <strong>to</strong> or greater than 0.361 for the researcher<strong>to</strong> reject the null hypothesis <strong>and</strong> conclude thatthere is a significant correlation <strong>in</strong> the population. Anysample correlation between 0.361 <strong>and</strong> –0.361 would beconsidered likely (that is, due <strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g error) <strong>and</strong>hence not statistically significant.NONPARAMETRIC TECHNIQUESFOR ANALYZING QUANTITATIVE DATAThe Mann-Whitney U Test. The Mann-WhitneyU test is a nonparametric alternative <strong>to</strong> the t-test usedwhen a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> analyze ranked data. Theresearcher <strong>in</strong>term<strong>in</strong>gles the scores of the two groups <strong>and</strong>then ranks them as if they were all from just one group.The test produces a value (U), whose probability ofoccurrence is then checked by the researcher <strong>in</strong> the appropriatestatistical table. The logic of the test is as follows:If the parent populations are identical, then thesum of the pooled rank<strong>in</strong>gs for each group should beabout the same. If the summed ranks are markedly different,on the other h<strong>and</strong>, then this difference is likely <strong>to</strong>be statistically significant.The Kruskal-Wallis One-Way Analysis ofVariance. The Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis ofvariance is used when researchers have more than two<strong>in</strong>dependent groups <strong>to</strong> compare. The procedure is quitesimilar <strong>to</strong> the Mann-Whitney U test. The scores of the<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the several groups are pooled <strong>and</strong> thenranked as though they all came from one group. Thesums of the ranks added <strong>to</strong>gether for each of the separategroups are then compared. This analysis produces avalue (H), whose probability of occurrence is checkedby the researcher <strong>in</strong> the appropriate statistical table.The Sign Test. The sign test is used when a researcherwants <strong>to</strong> analyze two related (as opposed <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dependent) samples. Related samples are connected<strong>in</strong> some way. For example, often a researcher will try <strong>to</strong>equalize groups on IQ, gender, age, or some othervariable. The groups are matched, so <strong>to</strong> speak, on thesevariables. Another example of a related sample is whenthe same group is both pre- <strong>and</strong> posttested (that is, testedtwice). Each <strong>in</strong>dividual, <strong>in</strong> other words, is tested on twodifferent occasions (as with the t-test for correlatedmeans).This test is very easy <strong>to</strong> use. The researcher simplyl<strong>in</strong>es up the pairs of related subjects <strong>and</strong> then determ<strong>in</strong>eshow many times the paired subjects <strong>in</strong> one group scoredhigher than those <strong>in</strong> the other group. If the groups do notdiffer significantly, the <strong>to</strong>tals for the two groups shouldbe about equal. If there is a marked difference <strong>in</strong> scor<strong>in</strong>g(such as many more <strong>in</strong> one group scor<strong>in</strong>g higher), thedifference may be statistically significant. Aga<strong>in</strong>, theprobability of this occurrence can be determ<strong>in</strong>ed byconsult<strong>in</strong>g the appropriate statistical table.The Friedman Two-Way Analysis of Variance.If more than two related groups are <strong>in</strong>volved,then the Friedman two-way analysis of variance testcan be used. For example, if a researcher employs fourmatched groups, this test would be appropriate.PARAMETRIC TECHNIQUESFOR ANALYZING CATEGORICAL DATAt-Test for Proportions. The most commonly usedparametric tests for analyz<strong>in</strong>g categorical data are the t-tests for a difference <strong>in</strong> proportions—that is, whetherthe proportion <strong>in</strong> one category (e.g., males) is differentfrom the proportion <strong>in</strong> another category (e.g., females).As is the case with the t-tests for means, there are twoforms: one t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent proportions <strong>and</strong> onet-test for correlated proportions. The latter is used primarilywhen the same group is be<strong>in</strong>g compared, as <strong>in</strong> theproportion of <strong>in</strong>dividuals agree<strong>in</strong>g with a statement before<strong>and</strong> after receiv<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>tervention of some sort.NONPARAMETRIC TECHNIQUESFOR ANALYZING CATEGORICAL DATAThe Chi-Square Test. The chi-square test is used<strong>to</strong> analyze data that are reported <strong>in</strong> categories. For example,a researcher might want <strong>to</strong> compare how manymale <strong>and</strong> female teachers favor a new curriculum <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong> a particular school district. He asks asample of 50 teachers if they favor or oppose the newcurriculum. If they do not differ significantly <strong>in</strong> theirresponses, he would expect that about the same proportionof males <strong>and</strong> females would be <strong>in</strong> favor of (oropposed <strong>to</strong>) <strong>in</strong>stitut<strong>in</strong>g the curriculum.


234 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe chi-square test is based on a comparison betweenexpected frequencies <strong>and</strong> actual, obta<strong>in</strong>ed frequencies.If the obta<strong>in</strong>ed frequencies are similar <strong>to</strong> theexpected frequencies, then researchers conclude that thegroups do not differ (<strong>in</strong> our example above, they do notdiffer <strong>in</strong> their attitude <strong>to</strong>ward the new curriculum). Ifthere are considerable differences between the expected<strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>ed frequencies, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, then researchersconclude that there is a significant difference<strong>in</strong> attitude between the two groups.The calculation of chi square is a necessary step <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient—a descriptivestatistic referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10.As with all of these <strong>in</strong>ference techniques, the chisquaretest yields a value (χ 2 ). The chi-square test is notlimited <strong>to</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g expected <strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>ed frequenciesfor only two variables. See Table 10.11 for an example.After the value for χ 2 has been calculated, we want <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e how likely it is that such a result could occur ifthere were no relationship <strong>in</strong> the population—that is,whether the obta<strong>in</strong>ed pattern of results does not exist <strong>in</strong> thepopulation but occurred because of the particular samplethat was selected. As with all <strong>in</strong>ferential tests, we determ<strong>in</strong>ethis by consult<strong>in</strong>g a probability table (Appendix C).You will notice that the chi-square table <strong>in</strong> AppendixC also has a column headed “degrees of freedom.” Degreesof freedom are calculated <strong>in</strong> crossbreak tables asfollows, us<strong>in</strong>g an example of a table with three rows <strong>and</strong>two columns.Step 1: Subtract 1 from the number of rows: 3 – 1 2Step 2: Subtract 1 from the number of columns:2 – 1 1Step 3: Multiply step 1 by step 2: (2)(1) 2Thus, <strong>in</strong> this example, there are two degrees of freedom.Cont<strong>in</strong>gency Coefficient. The f<strong>in</strong>al step <strong>in</strong> the chisquaretest process is <strong>to</strong> calculate the cont<strong>in</strong>gencyTABLE 11.1Size of Table(No. of Cells)Cont<strong>in</strong>gency Coefficient Values forDifferent-Sized Crossbreak TablesUpper limit afor C Calculated2 by 2 .713 by 3 .824 by 4 .875 by 5 .896 by 6 .91a The upper limits for unequal-sized tables (such as 2 by 3 or 3 by 4) areunknown but can be estimated from the values given. Thus, the upperlimit for a 3 by 4 table would approximate .85.coefficient, symbolized by the letter C, <strong>to</strong> which wereferred <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10. It is a measure of the degree ofassociation <strong>in</strong> a cont<strong>in</strong>gency table. We show how <strong>to</strong>calculate both the chi-square test <strong>and</strong> the cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficient <strong>in</strong> Chapter 12.The cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient cannot be <strong>in</strong>terpreted <strong>in</strong>exactly the same way as the correlation coefficient. Itmust be <strong>in</strong>terpreted by us<strong>in</strong>g Table 11.1. This table givesthe upper limit for C, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the number of cells<strong>in</strong> the crossbreak table.SUMMARY OF TECHNIQUESThe names of the most commonly used <strong>in</strong>ferential procedures<strong>and</strong> the data type appropriate <strong>to</strong> their use aresummarized <strong>in</strong> Table 11.2.This summary should be useful <strong>to</strong> you whenever youencounter these terms <strong>in</strong> your read<strong>in</strong>g. While the detailsof both mathematical rationale <strong>and</strong> calculation differgreatly among these techniques, the most importantth<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> remember are as follows.1. The end product of all <strong>in</strong>ference procedures is thesame: a statement of probability relat<strong>in</strong>g the sampledata <strong>to</strong> hypothesized population characteristics.TABLE 11.2Commonly Used Inferential TechniquesParametricNonparametricQuantitative t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent means Mann-Whitney U testt-test for correlated meansKruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of varianceAnalysis of variance (ANOVA)Sign testAnalysis of covariance (ANCOVA)Friedman two-way analysis of varianceMultivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)t-test for rCategorical t-test for difference <strong>in</strong> proportions Chi square


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 2352. All <strong>in</strong>ference techniques assume r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g.Without r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, the result<strong>in</strong>g probabilitiesare <strong>in</strong> error—<strong>to</strong> an unknown degree.3. Inference techniques are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> answer onlyone question: Given the sample data, what are probablepopulation characteristics? These techniques donot help decide whether the data show results thatare mean<strong>in</strong>gful or useful—they <strong>in</strong>dicate only the extent<strong>to</strong> which they may be generalizable.Null hypothesis= 0Criticalregion= 5%Critical value= 5.8Figure 11.18 Reject<strong>in</strong>g the Null Hypothesisβ(won’t detect)Criticalvalue = 5.8Truevalue = 8.01–β(will detect)Ourdifferenceof 7.6Figure 11.19 An Illustration of Power Under an AssumedPopulation ValuePOWER OF A STATISTICAL TESTThe power of a statistical test is similar <strong>to</strong> the power ofa telescope. Astronomers look<strong>in</strong>g at Mars or Venus witha low-power telescope probably can see that these planetslook like spheres, but it is unlikely that they can seemuch by way of differences. With high-power telescopes,however, they can see such differences asmoons <strong>and</strong> mounta<strong>in</strong>s. When the purpose of a statisticaltest is <strong>to</strong> assess differences, power is the probability thatthe test will correctly lead <strong>to</strong> the conclusion that there isa difference when, <strong>in</strong> fact, a difference exists.Suppose a football coach wants <strong>to</strong> study a new techniquefor kick<strong>in</strong>g field goals. He asks a (r<strong>and</strong>om) sampleof 30 high school players <strong>to</strong> each kick 30 goals.They take the same “test” after be<strong>in</strong>g coached <strong>in</strong> thenew technique. The mean number of goals is 11.2 beforecoach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> 18.8 after coach<strong>in</strong>g—a difference of7.6. The null hypothesis is that any positive difference(i.e., the number of goals after coach<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>us thenumber of goals before coach<strong>in</strong>g) is a chance differencefrom the true population difference of zero. A onetailedt-test is the technique used <strong>to</strong> test for statisticalsignificance.In this example, the critical value for reject<strong>in</strong>g thenull hypothesis at the .05 level is calculated. Assume thecritical value turns out <strong>to</strong> be 5.8. Any difference <strong>in</strong>means that is larger than 5.8 would result <strong>in</strong> rejectionof the null hypothesis. S<strong>in</strong>ce our difference is 7.6, wetherefore reject the null hypothesis. Figure 11.18 illustratesthis condition.Now assume that we somehow know that the realdifference <strong>in</strong> the population is actually 8.0. This isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 11.19. The dark-shaded part shows theprobability that the null hypothesis (which we now“know” <strong>to</strong> be wrong) will not be rejected by us<strong>in</strong>g thecritical value of 5.8—that is, it shows how often onewould get a value of 5.8 or less, which is not sufficient<strong>to</strong> cause the null hypothesis <strong>to</strong> be rejected. This area isbeta, <strong>and</strong> is determ<strong>in</strong>ed from a t-table. The power underthis particular “known” value of the population differenceis the lightly shaded area 1 – β. You can see, then,that <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance, if the real value is 8, a t-test us<strong>in</strong>gp .05 is unlikely <strong>to</strong> detect it.The power (1 – β) for a series of assumed “true” valuescan be obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the same way. When power isplotted aga<strong>in</strong>st assumed “real” values, the result is calleda power curve <strong>and</strong> looks someth<strong>in</strong>g like Figure 11.20.Compar<strong>in</strong>g such power curves for different techniques(e.g., a t-test versus the Mann-Whitney U test) <strong>in</strong>dicatestheir relative efficiency for use <strong>in</strong> a particular circumstance.Parametric tests (e.g., ANOVA, t-tests) aregenerally, but not always, more powerful than nonparametrictests (e.g., chi square, Mann-Whitney U test).Clearly, researchers want <strong>to</strong> use a powerful statisticaltest—one that can detect a relationship if one exists <strong>in</strong>the population—if they possibly can. If at all possible,therefore, power should be <strong>in</strong>creased. <strong>How</strong> might thisbe done?


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHCan Statistical Power AnalysisBe Mislead<strong>in</strong>g?Can statistical power analysis mislead researchers? Anexample is provided by a study which found that <strong>in</strong> theyear follow<strong>in</strong>g the 1994 federal ban on assault weapons, gunhomicides decl<strong>in</strong>ed 6.7 percent more than would have beenexpected, based on preexist<strong>in</strong>g trends. This difference was notstatistically significant. The authors carried out a statisticalpower analysis <strong>and</strong> concluded that a larger sample <strong>and</strong> longertime period were needed <strong>to</strong> have a sufficiently powerful statisticaltest that could detect an effect if any truly existed.**C. S. Koper <strong>and</strong> J. A. Roth (2001). The impact of the 1994 assaultweapon ban on gun violence outcomes: An assessment of multipleoutcome measures <strong>and</strong> some lessons for policy evaluation. Journalof Quantitative Crim<strong>in</strong>ology, 17(1): 33–74.Kleck argued that even with a much longer time frame <strong>and</strong>larger sample (<strong>and</strong> therefore greater statistical power), thepossible impact of the ban was so slight (because assaultweapons comprised only two percent of the guns used <strong>in</strong>crime) that “we could not reliably detect so m<strong>in</strong>uscule animpact.”† He agreed with the study’s authors that the observeddecl<strong>in</strong>e may well have been due <strong>to</strong> other fac<strong>to</strong>rs, such asreduced crack use <strong>in</strong> high-crime neighborhoods (uncontrolledvariables). He asked, <strong>in</strong> light of such known limitations,whether such studies are worth do<strong>in</strong>g. Koper <strong>and</strong> Roth arguedthat they are because of the importance of decisions regard<strong>in</strong>gsuch social policy issues.‡What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Are such studies worth do<strong>in</strong>g?†G. Kleck (2001). Impossible policy evaluations <strong>and</strong> impossibleconclusions: A comment on Koper <strong>and</strong> Roth. Journal of QuantitativeCrim<strong>in</strong>ology, 17(1): 80.‡C. S. Koper <strong>and</strong> J. A. Roth (2001). A priori assertions versus empirical<strong>in</strong>quiry: A reply <strong>to</strong> Kleck. Journal of Quantitative Crim<strong>in</strong>ology,17(1): 81–88.Power = 1–β1.0.9.8.7.6.5.4.3.2.10–4Figure 11.20 A Power Curve0 4True value of difference8 12There are at least four ways <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease power <strong>in</strong>addition <strong>to</strong> the use of parametric tests when appropriate:1. Decrease sampl<strong>in</strong>g error by:a. Increas<strong>in</strong>g sample size. An estimate of the necessarysample size can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed by do<strong>in</strong>g a statisticalpower analysis. This requires an estimationof all of the values (except n sample size) used<strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g the statistic you plan <strong>to</strong> use <strong>and</strong> solv<strong>in</strong>gfor n (see, for example, Table 12.2 on page 248for the formulas used <strong>to</strong> calculate the t-test).*b. Us<strong>in</strong>g reliable measures <strong>to</strong> decrease measurementerror.2. Controll<strong>in</strong>g for extraneous variables, as these mayobscure the relationship be<strong>in</strong>g studied.3. Increas<strong>in</strong>g the strength of the treatment (if there isone), perhaps by us<strong>in</strong>g a larger time period.4. Us<strong>in</strong>g a one-tailed test, when such is justifiable.*Tables are available for estimat<strong>in</strong>g sample size based on a comb<strong>in</strong>ationof desired effect size (see page 244) <strong>and</strong> desired level of power.See also M. W. Lipsey (1990). <strong>Design</strong> sensitivity: Statistical powerfor experimental research. Newbury Park, CA, Sage.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.236


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 237WHAT ARE INFERENTIAL STATISTICS?Inferential statistics refer <strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> procedures that allow researchers <strong>to</strong> make<strong>in</strong>ferences about a population based on data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a sample.The term probability, as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> the predicted relative frequencywith which a given event will occur.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsSAMPLING ERRORThe term sampl<strong>in</strong>g error refers <strong>to</strong> the variations <strong>in</strong> sample statistics that occur as aresult of repeated sampl<strong>in</strong>g from the same population.THE DISTRIBUTION OF SAMPLE MEANSA sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of means is a frequency distribution result<strong>in</strong>g from plott<strong>in</strong>gthe means of a very large number of samples from the same population.The st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean is the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution ofmeans. The st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference between means is the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationof a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of differences between sample means.CONFIDENCE INTERVALSA confidence <strong>in</strong>terval is a region extend<strong>in</strong>g both above <strong>and</strong> below a sample statistic(such as a sample mean) with<strong>in</strong> which a population parameter (such as the populationmean) may be said <strong>to</strong> fall with a specified probability of be<strong>in</strong>g wrong.HYPOTHESIS TESTINGStatistical hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g is a way of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the probability that an obta<strong>in</strong>edsample statistic will occur, given a hypothetical population parameter.A research hypothesis specifies the nature of the relationship the researcher th<strong>in</strong>ksexists <strong>in</strong> the population.The null hypothesis typically specifies that there is no relationship <strong>in</strong> the population.SIGNIFICANCE LEVELSThe term significance level (or level of significance), as used <strong>in</strong> research, refers <strong>to</strong> theprobability of a sample statistic occurr<strong>in</strong>g as a result of sampl<strong>in</strong>g error.The significance levels most commonly used <strong>in</strong> educational research are the .05 <strong>and</strong>.01 levels.Statistical significance <strong>and</strong> practical significance are not necessarily the same. Evenif a result is statistically significant, it may not be practically (i.e., educationally)significant.TESTS OF STATISTICAL SIGNIFICANCEA one-tailed test of significance <strong>in</strong>volves the use of probabilities based on one-half ofa sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution because the research hypothesis is a directional hypothesis.A two-tailed test, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong>volves the use of probabilities based on bothsides of a sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution because the research hypothesis is a nondirectionalhypothesis.


238 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePARAMETRIC TESTS FOR QUANTITATIVE DATAA parametric statistical test requires various k<strong>in</strong>ds of assumptions about the nature ofthe population from which the samples <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the research study were taken.Some of the commonly used parametric techniques for analyz<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data<strong>in</strong>clude the t-test for means, ANOVA, ANCOVA, MANOVA, MANCOVA, <strong>and</strong> thet-test for r.PARAMETRIC TESTS FOR CATEGORICAL DATAThe most common parametric technique for analyz<strong>in</strong>g categorical data is the t-testfor differences <strong>in</strong> proportions.NONPARAMETRIC TESTS FOR QUANTITATIVE DATAA nonparametric statistical technique makes few, if any, assumptions about the natureof the population from which the samples <strong>in</strong> the study were taken.Some of the commonly used nonparametric techniques for analyz<strong>in</strong>g quantitativedata are the Mann-Whitney U test, the Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance,the sign test, <strong>and</strong> the Friedman two-way analysis of variance.NONPARAMETRIC TESTS FOR CATEGORICAL DATAThe chi-square test is the nonparametric technique most commonly used <strong>to</strong> analyzecategorical data.The cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient is a descriptive statistic <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the degree of relationshipbetween two categorical variables.POWER OF A STATISTICAL TESTThe power of a statistical test for a particular set of data is the likelihood of identify<strong>in</strong>ga difference, when <strong>in</strong> fact it exists, between population parameters.Parametric tests are generally, but not always, more powerful than nonparametric tests.Key Termsanalysis of covariance(ANCOVA) 232analysis of variance(ANOVA) 232chi-square test 233confidence <strong>in</strong>terval 220cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficient 234degrees of freedom 230Friedman two-wayanalysis ofvariance 233<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics 216Kruskal-Wallisone-way analysisof variance 233level of significance 224Mann-WhitneyU test 233multivariate analysisof covariance(MANCOVA) 232multivariate analysisof variance(MANOVA) 232nonparametrictechnique 229null hypothesis 224one-tailed test 227parametrictechnique 228power of a statisticaltest 235practical significance 226probability 222research hypothesis 224sampl<strong>in</strong>gdistribution 218sampl<strong>in</strong>g error 217sign test 233


CHAPTER 11 Inferential Statistics 239st<strong>and</strong>ard error ofthe difference 223st<strong>and</strong>ard error ofthe mean 219statisticalsignificance 226t-test for a difference<strong>in</strong> proportions 233t-test for correlatedmeans 230t-test for correlatedproportions 233t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependentmeans 229t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependentproportions 233two-tailed test 227Type I error 228Type II error 228Wilk’s lambda 2321. “Hypotheses can never be proven, only supported.” Is this statement true or not?Expla<strong>in</strong>.2. No two samples will be the same <strong>in</strong> all of their characteristics. Why won’t they?3. When might a researcher not need <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics <strong>to</strong> analyze his or herdata?4. “No sampl<strong>in</strong>g procedure, not even r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, guarantees a <strong>to</strong>tally representativesample.” Is this true? Discuss.5. Could a relationship that is practically significant be ignored because it is not statisticallysignificant? What about the reverse? Can you suggest an example of each?For Discussion


240 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 11: Inferential StatisticsUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 11, once aga<strong>in</strong> state the question or hypothesis of your study. Summarizethe descriptive statistics you would use <strong>to</strong> describe the relationship you are hypothesiz<strong>in</strong>g.Indicate which <strong>in</strong>ference technique(s) is appropriate. Tell whether you would or would notdo a significance test <strong>and</strong>/or calculate a confidence <strong>in</strong>terval, <strong>and</strong> if not, expla<strong>in</strong> why. Lastly,describe the type of sample used <strong>in</strong> your study <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> any limitations that are placedon your use of <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics ow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the nature of the sample.Problem Sheet 11Inferential Statistics1. The question or hypothesis of my study is: _________________________________2. The descriptive statistic(s) I would use <strong>to</strong> describe the relationship I am hypothesiz<strong>in</strong>gwould be: _________________________________________________________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.3. The appropriate <strong>in</strong>ference technique for my study would be: ____________________4. Circle the appropriate word <strong>in</strong> the sentences below.a. I would use a parametric or a nonparametric technique because:______________b. I would or would not do a significance test because: ______________________A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.c. I would or would not calculate a confidence <strong>in</strong>terval because: ________________5. The type of sample used <strong>in</strong> my study is: ____________________________________6. The type of sample used <strong>in</strong> my study places the follow<strong>in</strong>g limitation(s) on my use of<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics: ____________________________________________________


Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective12“Our methodshows significantlyhigher test results!”“Hmm. Do you meanstatistical significanceor ‘real-world’significance?”Approaches <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong>Compar<strong>in</strong>g Groups:Quantitative DataTechniquesInterpretationRelat<strong>in</strong>g VariablesWith<strong>in</strong> a Group:Quantitative DataTechniquesInterpretationCompar<strong>in</strong>g Groups:Categorical DataTechniquesInterpretationRelat<strong>in</strong>g VariablesWith<strong>in</strong> a Group:Categorical DataA Recap ofRecommendationsTest consultantPr<strong>in</strong>cipalOBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Apply several recommendations when Describe briefly how <strong>to</strong> use frequencycompar<strong>in</strong>g data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from two or polygons, scatterplots, <strong>and</strong> crossbreakmore groups.tables <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret data.Apply several recommendations when Differentiate between statisticallyrelat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle group. significant <strong>and</strong> practically significantExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termresearch results.“effect size.”


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Centerat www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Statistical Versus Practical SignificanceAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 12.1: Statistical vs. Practical SignificanceActivity 12.2: Appropriate TechniquesActivity 12.3: Interpret the DataActivity 12.4: Collect Some DataWell, the results are <strong>in</strong>,” said Tamara Phillips. “I got the consultant’s report about that study we did lastsemester.”“What study was that?” asked Felicia Lee, as the two carpooled <strong>to</strong> Eisenhower Middle School, where they bothtaught eighth-grade social studies.“Don’t you remember? That guy from the university came down <strong>and</strong> asked some of us who taught social studies<strong>to</strong> try an <strong>in</strong>quiry approach?”“Oh, yeah, I remember. I was <strong>in</strong> the experimental group—we used a series of <strong>in</strong>quiry-oriented lessons that theydesigned. They compared the results of our students with those of students who were similar <strong>in</strong> ability whoseteachers did not use those lessons. What did they f<strong>in</strong>d out?”“Well, the report states that those students whose teachers used the <strong>in</strong>quiry lessons had significantly higher testscores. But I’m not quite sure what that suggests.”“It means that the <strong>in</strong>quiry method is superior <strong>to</strong> whatever method the teachers of the other group used,doesn’t it?”“I’m not sure. It depends on whether the significance they’re talk<strong>in</strong>g about refers <strong>to</strong> practical or only statisticalsignificance.”“What’s the difference?”This difference—between statistical <strong>and</strong> practical significance—is an important one when it comes <strong>to</strong> talk<strong>in</strong>gabout the results of a study. It is one of the th<strong>in</strong>gs you will learn about <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Now that you are somewhat familiar with both descriptive<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics, we want <strong>to</strong> relate themmore specifically <strong>to</strong> practice. What are appropriate usesof these statistics? What are appropriate <strong>in</strong>terpretationsof them? What are the common errors or mistakes youshould watch out for as either a participant <strong>in</strong> or consumerof research?There are appropriate uses for both descriptive <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>ferential statistics. Sometimes, however, either orboth types can be used <strong>in</strong>appropriately. In this chapter,therefore, we want <strong>to</strong> discuss the appropriate use of thedescriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics described <strong>in</strong> theprevious two chapters. We will present a number of recommendationsthat we believe all researchers shouldconsider when they use either type of statistics.Approaches <strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong>Much research <strong>in</strong> education is done <strong>in</strong> one of two ways:either two or more groups are compared or variableswith<strong>in</strong> one group are related. Furthermore, as you haveseen, the data <strong>in</strong> a study may be either quantitative orcategorical. Thus, four different comb<strong>in</strong>ations of researchare possible, as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 12.1.Remember that all groups are made up of <strong>in</strong>dividualunits. In most cases, the unit is one person <strong>and</strong> the groupis a group of people. Sometimes, however, the unit is itselfa group (for example, a class of students). In suchcases, the “group” would be a collection of classes. Thisis illustrated by the follow<strong>in</strong>g hypothesis: “Teacher242


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 243Two or moregroups are comparedVariables with<strong>in</strong> onegroup are relatedQuantitativeDataCategoricalFigure 12.1Comb<strong>in</strong>ations of Data<strong>and</strong> Approaches <strong>to</strong><strong>Research</strong>friendl<strong>in</strong>ess is related <strong>to</strong> student learn<strong>in</strong>g.” This hypothesiscould be studied with a group of classes <strong>and</strong> a measureof both teacher “friendl<strong>in</strong>ess” <strong>and</strong> average studentlearn<strong>in</strong>g for each class.Another complication arises <strong>in</strong> studies <strong>in</strong> which thesame <strong>in</strong>dividuals receive two or more different treatmentsor methods. In compar<strong>in</strong>g treatments, we are notthen compar<strong>in</strong>g different groups of people but differentgroups of scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by the same group at differenttimes. Nevertheless, the statistical analysis fits the comparisongroup model. We discuss this po<strong>in</strong>t further <strong>in</strong>Chapter 13.Compar<strong>in</strong>g Groups:Quantitative DataTECHNIQUESWhenever two or more groups are compared us<strong>in</strong>gquantitative data, the comparisons can be made <strong>in</strong> a varietyof ways: through frequency polygons, calculationof one or more measures of central tendency (averages),<strong>and</strong>/or calculation of one or more measures of variability(spreads). Frequency polygons provide the most <strong>in</strong>formation;averages are useful summaries of eachgroup’s performance; <strong>and</strong> spreads provide <strong>in</strong>formationabout the degree of variability <strong>in</strong> each group.When analyz<strong>in</strong>g data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from two groups,therefore, the first th<strong>in</strong>g researchers should do is constructa frequency polygon of each group’s scores. Thiswill show all the <strong>in</strong>formation available about each group<strong>and</strong> also help researchers decide which of the shorter<strong>and</strong> more convenient <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>to</strong> calculate. For example,exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the frequency polygon of a group’sscores can <strong>in</strong>dicate whether the median or the mean isthe most appropriate measure of central tendency <strong>to</strong> use.When compar<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data from two groups,therefore, we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Recommendation 1: As a first step, prepare a frequencypolygon of each group’s scores.Recommendation 2: Use these polygons <strong>to</strong> decidewhich measure of central tendency is appropriate<strong>to</strong> calculate. If any polygon shows extreme scoresat one end, use medians for all groups rather than,or <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong>, means.INTERPRETATIONOnce the descriptive statistics have been calculated, theymust be <strong>in</strong>terpreted. At this po<strong>in</strong>t, the task is <strong>to</strong> describe,<strong>in</strong> words, what the polygons <strong>and</strong> averages tell researchersabout the question or hypothesis be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated.A key question arises: <strong>How</strong> large does a difference <strong>in</strong>means between two groups have <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> be important?When will this difference make a difference?<strong>How</strong> does one decide? You will recall that this is theissue of practical versus statistical significance that wediscussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 11.Use Information About Known Groups.Unfortunately, <strong>in</strong> most educational research, this <strong>in</strong>formationis very difficult <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>. Sometimes, prior experiencecan be helpful. One of the advantages of IQscores is that, over the years, many educa<strong>to</strong>rs have hadenough experience with them <strong>to</strong> make differences betweenthem mean<strong>in</strong>gful. Most experienced counselors,adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> teachers realize, for example, that adifference <strong>in</strong> means of less than 5 po<strong>in</strong>ts between twogroups has little useful mean<strong>in</strong>g, no matter how statisticallysignificant the difference may be. They also knowthat a difference between means of 10 po<strong>in</strong>ts is enough<strong>to</strong> have important implications. At other times, a researchermay have available a frame of reference, or


244 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7edivid<strong>in</strong>g the difference between the means of the twogroups be<strong>in</strong>g compared by the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of thecomparison group. Thus:ES mean of experimental group mean of comparison groupst<strong>and</strong>ard deviationof comparison groupWhen pre-<strong>to</strong>-post ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> the mean scores oftwo groups are compared, the formula is modified asfollows:ES mean experimental ga<strong>in</strong> mean comparison ga<strong>in</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of ga<strong>in</strong>of comparison group© The New Yorker Collection 1977 Joseph Mirachi fromcar<strong>to</strong>onbank.com. All Rights Reserved.st<strong>and</strong>ard, <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the magnitude of a differencebetween means. One such st<strong>and</strong>ard consists ofthe mean scores of known groups. In a study of criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which one of the present authors participated,for example, the end-of-year mean score for agroup of eleventh-graders who received a special curriculumwas higher than is typical of the mean scores ofeleventh-graders <strong>in</strong> general <strong>and</strong> close <strong>to</strong> the mean scoreof a group of college students, whereas a comparisongroup scored lower than both. Because the specialcurriculumgroup also demonstrated a fall-<strong>to</strong>-spr<strong>in</strong>gmean ga<strong>in</strong> that was twice that of the comparison group,the <strong>to</strong>tal evidence obta<strong>in</strong>ed through compar<strong>in</strong>g theirperformance with other groups <strong>in</strong>dicated that the ga<strong>in</strong>smade by the special-curriculum group were important.Calculate the Effect Size. Another techniquefor assess<strong>in</strong>g the magnitude of a difference between themeans of two groups is <strong>to</strong> calculate what is known aseffect size (ES).*Effect size takes <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account the size of the differencebetween means that is obta<strong>in</strong>ed, regardless ofwhether it is statistically significant. One of the mostcommonly used <strong>in</strong>dexes of effect size is obta<strong>in</strong>ed by*The term effect size is used <strong>to</strong> identify a group of statistical <strong>in</strong>dices,all of which have the common purpose of clarify<strong>in</strong>g the magnitudeof relationship.The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of ga<strong>in</strong> score is obta<strong>in</strong>ed by firstgett<strong>in</strong>g the ga<strong>in</strong> (post – pre) score for each <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>and</strong> then calculat<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation as usual.†While effect size is a useful <strong>to</strong>ol for assess<strong>in</strong>g themagnitude of a difference between the means of twogroups, it does not, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of itself, answer the questionof how large it must be for researchers <strong>to</strong> consider anobta<strong>in</strong>ed difference important. As is the case with significancelevels, this is essentially an arbitrary decision.Most researchers consider that any effect size of .50(that is, half a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of the comparisongroup’s scores) or larger is an important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g. If thescores fit the normal distribution, such a value <strong>in</strong>dicatesthat the difference <strong>in</strong> means between the two groups isabout one-twelfth the distance between the highest <strong>and</strong>lowest scores of the comparison group. When assess<strong>in</strong>gthe magnitude of a difference between the means of twogroups, therefore, we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Recommendation 3: Compare obta<strong>in</strong>ed results withdata on the means of known groups, if possible.Recommendation 4: Calculate an effect size. Interpretan ES of .50 or larger as important.Use Inferential Statistics. A third method forjudg<strong>in</strong>g the importance of a difference between the meansof two groups is by the use of <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics. Itis common <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d, even before exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g polygons ordifferences <strong>in</strong> means, that a researcher has applied an<strong>in</strong>ference technique (a t-test, an analysis of variance,†There are more effective ways <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> scores, but we willdelay a discussion until subsequent chapters.


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHStatistical Inference Tests—Good or Bad?Our recommendations regard<strong>in</strong>g statistical <strong>in</strong>ference arenot free of controversy. At one extreme are the views ofCarver* <strong>and</strong> Schmidt,† who argue that the use of statistical<strong>in</strong>ference tests <strong>in</strong> educational research should be banned. And<strong>in</strong> 2000 a survey of AERA members (American <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> Association) <strong>in</strong>dicated that 19 percent agreed.‡At the other extreme are those who agree with Rob<strong>in</strong>son<strong>and</strong> Lev<strong>in</strong> that “authors should first <strong>in</strong>dicate whether the observedeffect is a statistically improbable one, <strong>and</strong> only if it isshould they <strong>in</strong>dicate how large or important it is (is it a differencethat makes a difference).”§Cahan argued, <strong>to</strong> the contrary, that the way <strong>to</strong> avoid mislead<strong>in</strong>gconclusions regard<strong>in</strong>g effects is not by us<strong>in</strong>g significancetests, but rather us<strong>in</strong>g confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals accompaniedby <strong>in</strong>creased sample size. ||In 1999 the American Psychological Association Task Forceon Statistical Inference recommended that <strong>in</strong>ference tests not bebanned, but that researchers should “always provide some effectsize estimate when report<strong>in</strong>g a p value,” <strong>and</strong> further that“report<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g effect sizes <strong>in</strong> the context of previouslyreported research is essential <strong>to</strong> good research.”#What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Should significance tests be banned <strong>in</strong>educational research?*R. P. Carver (1993). The case aga<strong>in</strong>st statistical significance test<strong>in</strong>grevisited. Journal of Experimental <strong>Education</strong>, 61: 287–292.†F. L. Schmidt (1996). Statistical significance test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> cumulativeknowledge <strong>in</strong> psychology: Implications for tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of researchers.Psychological Methods, 1: 115–129.‡K. C. Mittag <strong>and</strong> B. Thompson (2000). A national survey of AERAmembers’ perceptions of statistical significance tests <strong>and</strong> other statisticalissues. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 29(3): 14–19.§D. H. Rob<strong>in</strong>son <strong>and</strong> J. R. Lev<strong>in</strong> (1997). Reflections on statistical<strong>and</strong> substantive significance, with a slice of replication. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 26 (January/February): 22.|| S. Cahan (2000). Statistical significance is not a “Kosher Certificate”for observed effects: A critical analysis of the two-step approach <strong>to</strong> theevaluation of empirical results. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 29(5): 34.#L. Wilk<strong>in</strong>son <strong>and</strong> the APA Task Force on Statistical Inference(1999). Statistical methods <strong>in</strong> psychology journals: Guidel<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong>explanations. American Psychologist, 54: 599.<strong>and</strong> so on) <strong>and</strong> then used the results as the only criterionfor evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the importance of the results. This practicehas come under <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g attack for the follow<strong>in</strong>greasons:1. Unless the groups compared are r<strong>and</strong>om samplesfrom specified populations (which is unusual), theresults (probabilities, significance levels, <strong>and</strong> confidence<strong>in</strong>tervals) are <strong>to</strong> an unknown degree <strong>in</strong> error<strong>and</strong> hence mislead<strong>in</strong>g.2. The outcome is greatly affected by sample size. With100 cases <strong>in</strong> each of two groups, a mean difference <strong>in</strong>IQ score of 4.2 po<strong>in</strong>ts is statistically significant at the.05 level (assum<strong>in</strong>g the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation is 15, as istypical with most IQ tests). Although statistically significant,this difference is so small as <strong>to</strong> be mean<strong>in</strong>gless<strong>in</strong> any practical sense.3. The actual magnitude of difference is m<strong>in</strong>imized orsometimes overlooked.4. The purpose of <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics is <strong>to</strong> provide <strong>in</strong>formationpert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g sample results <strong>to</strong>populations, not <strong>to</strong> evaluate sample results.With regard <strong>to</strong> the use of <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics, therefore,we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Recommendation 5: Consider us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferential statisticsonly if you can make a conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g argumentthat a difference between means of the magnitudeobta<strong>in</strong>ed is important (Figure 12.2).Recommendation 6: Do not use tests of statisticalsignificance <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitude of a differencebetween sample means. Use them only asthey were <strong>in</strong>tended: <strong>to</strong> judge the generalizabilityof results.Recommendation 7: Unless r<strong>and</strong>om samples wereused, <strong>in</strong>terpret probabilities <strong>and</strong>/or significancelevels as crude <strong>in</strong>dices, not as precise values.Recommendation 8: Report the results of <strong>in</strong>ferencetechniques as confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals rather than (or<strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong>) significance levels.Example. Let us give an example <strong>to</strong> illustrate thistype of analysis. We shall present the appropriate calculations<strong>in</strong> detail <strong>and</strong> then <strong>in</strong>terpret the results. Imag<strong>in</strong>ethat we have two groups of eighth-grade students, 60 <strong>in</strong>each group, who receive different methods of socialstudies <strong>in</strong>struction for one semester. The teacher of onegroup uses an <strong>in</strong>quiry method of <strong>in</strong>struction, while theteacher of the other group uses the lecture method. The245


246 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 12.2 ADifference That Doesn’tMake a Difference!“I’m pretty depressed.My class scored lower thanthe overall school average —statistically significant atthe 1% level, <strong>to</strong>o!”“Come on. Theschool average was only140 out of 200 — <strong>and</strong> yourclass average was 136!Big deal!”researcher’s hypothesis is that the <strong>in</strong>quiry method willresult <strong>in</strong> greater improvement than the lecture method <strong>in</strong>expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g skills as measured by the “test of ability <strong>to</strong>expla<strong>in</strong>” (see page 151) <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8. Each student istested at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> at the end of the semester.The test consists of 40 items; the range of scores on thepretest is from 3 <strong>to</strong> 32, or 29 po<strong>in</strong>ts. A ga<strong>in</strong> score(posttest–pretest) is obta<strong>in</strong>ed. These ga<strong>in</strong> scores areshown <strong>in</strong> the frequency distributions <strong>in</strong> Table 12.1 <strong>and</strong>the frequency polygons <strong>in</strong> Figure 12.3.These polygons <strong>in</strong>dicate that a comparison of meansis appropriate. Why?* The mean of the <strong>in</strong>quiry group is5.6 compared <strong>to</strong> the mean of 4.4 for the lecture group.The difference between means is 1.2. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, acomparison with the means of known groups is not possible,s<strong>in</strong>ce such data are not available. A calculation ofeffect size results <strong>in</strong> an ES of .44, somewhat below the.50 that most researchers recommend for significance.Inspection of Figure 12.3, however, suggests that thedifference between the means of the two groups shouldnot be discounted. Figure 12.4 <strong>and</strong> Table 12.2 show thatthe number of students ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 7 or more po<strong>in</strong>ts is 25 <strong>in</strong>the <strong>in</strong>quiry group <strong>and</strong> 13 (about half as many) <strong>in</strong> the lecturegroup. A ga<strong>in</strong> of 7 po<strong>in</strong>ts on a 40-item test can beconsidered substantial, even more so when it is recalledthat the range was 29 po<strong>in</strong>ts (3–32) on the pretest. Ifa ga<strong>in</strong> of 8 po<strong>in</strong>ts is used, the numbers are 16 <strong>in</strong> the*The polygons are nearly symmetrical without extreme scores ateither end.TABLE 12.1Ga<strong>in</strong> Scores on Test of Ability <strong>to</strong>Expla<strong>in</strong>: Inquiry <strong>and</strong> Lecture GroupsInquiryLectureGa<strong>in</strong> Cumulative CumulativeScores a Frequency Frequency Frequency Frequency11 1 60 0 6010 3 59 2 609 5 56 3 588 7 51 4 557 9 44 4 516 9 35 7 475 6 26 9 404 6 20 8 313 5 14 7 232 4 9 6 161 2 5 4 100 3 3 5 6–1 0 0 1 1a A negative score <strong>in</strong>dicates the pretest was higher than the posttest.<strong>in</strong>quiry group <strong>and</strong> 9 <strong>in</strong> the lecture group. If a ga<strong>in</strong> of 6po<strong>in</strong>ts is used, the numbers become 34 <strong>and</strong> 20. Wewould argue that these discrepancies are large enough,<strong>in</strong> context, <strong>to</strong> recommend the <strong>in</strong>quiry method over thelecture method.The use of an <strong>in</strong>ference technique (a t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependentmeans) <strong>in</strong>dicates that p < .05 <strong>in</strong> one tail


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 247Frequency987654321InquiryLecture–2 –1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12Ga<strong>in</strong> scores on test of ability <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong>*Figure 12.3 Frequency Polygons of Ga<strong>in</strong> Scores on Tes<strong>to</strong>f Ability <strong>to</strong> Expla<strong>in</strong>: Inquiry <strong>and</strong> Lecture Groups*A negative score <strong>in</strong>dicates the pretest was higher than the posttest..05 .050.39 1.2 2.01Obta<strong>in</strong>ed differencebetween sample meansFigure 12.4 90 Percent Confidence Interval fora Difference of 1.2 Between Sample Means(Table 12.2).* This leads the researcher <strong>to</strong> conclude thatthe observed difference between means of 1.2 po<strong>in</strong>tsprobably is not due <strong>to</strong> the particular samples used.Whether this probability can be taken as exact dependsprimarily on whether the samples were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected.The 90 percent confidence <strong>in</strong>terval is shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 12.4.† Notice that a difference of zero between thepopulation means is not with<strong>in</strong> the confidence <strong>in</strong>terval.*A directional hypothesis <strong>in</strong>dicates use of a one-tailed test (see p. 227).†1.65 SED gives .05 <strong>in</strong> one tail of the normal curve. 1.65 (SED) 1.65(.49) .81. 1.2 ± .81 equals .39 <strong>to</strong> 2.01. This is the 90 percentconfidence <strong>in</strong>terval. Use of 1.65 rather than 1.96 is justified becausethe researcher’s hypothesis is concerned only with a positive ga<strong>in</strong>(a one-tailed test). The 95 percent or any other confidence <strong>in</strong>tervalcould, of course, have been used.Relat<strong>in</strong>g Variables With<strong>in</strong> aGroup: Quantitative DataTECHNIQUESWhenever a relationship between quantitative variableswith<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle group is exam<strong>in</strong>ed, the appropriate techniquesare the scatterplot <strong>and</strong> the correlation coefficient.The scatterplot illustrates all the data visually, while thecorrelation coefficient provides a numerical summaryof the data. When analyz<strong>in</strong>g data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a s<strong>in</strong>glegroup, therefore, researchers should beg<strong>in</strong> by construct<strong>in</strong>ga scatterplot. Not only will it provide all the <strong>in</strong>formationavailable, but it will help them judge whichcorrelation coefficient <strong>to</strong> calculate (the choice usuallywill be between the Pearson r, which assumes a l<strong>in</strong>ear,or straight-l<strong>in</strong>e relationship, <strong>and</strong> eta, which describesa curvil<strong>in</strong>ear, or curved, relationship).‡Consider Figure 12.5. All of the five scatterplotsshown represent a Pearson correlation of about .50.Only <strong>in</strong> (a), however, does this coefficient (.50) completelyconvey the nature of the relationship. In (b) therelationship is understated, s<strong>in</strong>ce it is a curvil<strong>in</strong>ear one,<strong>and</strong> eta would give a higher coefficient. In (c) the coefficientdoes not reflect the fan-shaped nature of the relationship.In (d) the coefficient does not reveal that thereare two dist<strong>in</strong>ct subgroups. In (e) the coefficient isgreatly <strong>in</strong>flated by a few unusual cases. While theseillustrations are a bit exaggerated, similar results areoften found <strong>in</strong> real data.When exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g relationships with<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle group,therefore, we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Recommendation 9: Beg<strong>in</strong> by construct<strong>in</strong>g a scatterplot.Recommendation 10: Use the scatterplot <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhich correlation coefficient is appropriate<strong>to</strong> calculate.Recommendation 11: Use both the scatterplot <strong>and</strong>the correlation coefficient <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret results.INTERPRETATIONInterpret<strong>in</strong>g scatterplots <strong>and</strong> correlations presents problemssimilar <strong>to</strong> those we discussed <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong>‡Because both of these correlations describe the magnitude ofrelationship, they are also examples of effect size (see footnote,page 244).


248 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 12.2 Calculations from Table 12.1Inquiry GroupLecture GroupGa<strong>in</strong>Ga<strong>in</strong>Score ƒ a ƒX b X X c (X X) 2d f(X X) 2e Score ƒ ƒX X X (X X) 2 f(X X) 211 1 11 5.4 29.2 29.2 11 0 0 6.6 43.6 0.010 3 30 4.4 19.4 58.2 10 2 20 5.6 31.4 62.89 5 45 3.4 11.6 58.0 9 3 27 4.6 21.2 63.68 7 56 2.4 5.8 40.6 8 4 32 3.6 13.0 52.07 9 63 1.4 2.0 18.0 7 4 28 2.6 6.8 27.26 9 54 0.4 0.2 1.8 6 7 42 1.6 2.6 18.25 6 30 0.6 0.4 2.4 5 9 45 0.6 0.4 3.64 6 24 1.6 2.6 15.6 4 8 32 0.4 0.2 1.63 5 15 2.6 6.8 34.0 3 7 21 1.4 2.0 14.02 4 8 3.6 13.0 52.0 2 6 12 2.4 5.8 34.81 2 2 4.6 21.2 42.4 1 4 4 3.4 11.6 46.40 3 0 5.6 31.4 94.2 0 5 0 4.4 19.4 97.01 0 0 6.6 43.6 0.0 1 1 1 5.4 29.2 29.22 0 0 7.6 57.8 0.0 2 0 0 6.4 41.0 0.0Total © 338 © 446.4 © 262 © 450.4262X 1 = ∑fX 338=X 2= =n 60 = 5.6 ∑fXn 60= 4.4SD 1=f(X – X ) 2 446.4f(X – X) 2 450.4== 7.4 = 2.7SDn602==n60= 7.5 = 2.7SEM 1=SD 2.7 2.7SD 2.7 2.7= = = .35SEMn – 1 59 7.72= = =n – 1 59 7.7= .35SED = (SEM 1 ) 2 + SEM 2 ) 2 = .35 2 + .35 2 = .12 + .12 = .24 = .49t =X 1 – X 2SED1.2= = 2.45.49


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 249(a) (b) (c) (d) (e)Figure 12.5 Scatterplots with a Pearson r of .50significant at the .05 level with a two-tailed test. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly,we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g when <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>gscatterplots <strong>and</strong> correlation coefficients:Recommendation 12: Draw a l<strong>in</strong>e that best fits allpo<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> a scatterplot, <strong>and</strong> note the extent ofdeviations from it. The smaller the deviations allalong the l<strong>in</strong>e, the more useful the relationship.*Recommendation 13: Consider us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferentialstatistics only if you can give a conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g argumentfor the importance of the size of the relationshipfound <strong>in</strong> the sample.Recommendation 14: Do not use tests of statisticalsignificance <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitude of a relationship.Use them, as they were <strong>in</strong>tended, <strong>to</strong>judge generalizability.Recommendation 15: Unless a r<strong>and</strong>om sample wasused, <strong>in</strong>terpret probabilities <strong>and</strong>/or significancelevels as crude <strong>in</strong>dices, not as precise values.Recommendation 16: Report the results of <strong>in</strong>ferencetechniques as confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals ratherthan as significance levels.*Try this with Figure 12.5.TABLE 12.3 Interpretation of CorrelationCoefficients when Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesMagnitude of r Interpretation.00 <strong>to</strong> .40 Of little practical importanceexcept <strong>in</strong> unusual circumstances;perhaps of theoretical value.*.41 <strong>to</strong> .60 Large enough <strong>to</strong> be of practical aswell as theoretical use..61 <strong>to</strong> .80 Very important, but rarely obta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> educational research..81 or above Possibly an error <strong>in</strong> calculation; ifnot, a very sizable relationship.*When select<strong>in</strong>g a very few people from a large group, even correlationsthis small may have predictive value.Example. Let us now consider an example <strong>to</strong> illustratethe analysis of a suspected relationship betweenvariables. Suppose a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> test the hypothesisthat, among counsel<strong>in</strong>g clients, improvement<strong>in</strong> marital satisfaction after six months of counsel<strong>in</strong>gis related <strong>to</strong> self-esteem at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of counsel<strong>in</strong>g.In other words, people with higher self-esteemwould be expected <strong>to</strong> show more improvement <strong>in</strong>marital satisfaction after undergo<strong>in</strong>g therapy for aperiod of six months than people with lower selfesteem.The researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s a group of 30 clients,each of whom takes a self-esteem <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> amarital satisfaction <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry prior <strong>to</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g. Themarital satisfaction <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry is taken aga<strong>in</strong> at the endof six months of counsel<strong>in</strong>g. The data are shown <strong>in</strong>Table 12.4.The calculations shown <strong>in</strong> Table 12.4 are not as hardas they look. Here are the steps that we followed <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> r .42.1. Multiply n by ΣXY: 30(7,023) 210,6902. Multiply ΣX by ΣY: (1,007)(192) 193,3443. Subtract step 2 from step 1: 210,690 – 193,344 17,3464. Multiply n by ΣX 2 : 30(35,507) 1,065,2105. Square ΣX: (1,007) 2 1,014,0496. Subtract step 5 from step 4: 1,065,210 – 1,014,049 51,1617. Multiply n by ΣY 2 : 30(2,354) 70,6208. Square ΣY: (192) 2 36,8649. Subtract step 8 from step 7: 70,620 – 36,864 33,75610. Multiply step 6 by step 9: (51,161)(33,756) 1,726,990,71611. Take the square root of step 10: 21,726,990,716 41,55712. Divide step 3 by step 11: 17,346/41,557 .42Us<strong>in</strong>g the data presented <strong>in</strong> Table 12.4, the researcherplots a scatterplot <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ds that it reveals two th<strong>in</strong>gs.First, there is a tendency for <strong>in</strong>dividuals with higher <strong>in</strong>itialself-esteem scores <strong>to</strong> show greater improvement <strong>in</strong>


250 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 12.4Self-Esteem Scores <strong>and</strong> Ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Marital SatisfactionSelf-EsteemGa<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> MaritalScore beforeSatisfaction afterClient Counsel<strong>in</strong>g (X) X 2 Counsel<strong>in</strong>g (Y) Y 2 XY1 20 400 –4 16 –802 21 441 –2 4 –423 22 484 –7 49 –1544 24 576 1 1 245 24 576 4 16 966 25 625 5 25 1257 26 676 –1 1 –268 27 729 8 64 2169 29 841 2 4 5810 28 784 5 25 14011 30 900 5 25 15012 30 900 14 196 42013 32 1024 7 49 21914 33 1089 15 225 49515 35 1225 6 36 21016 35 1225 16 256 56017 36 1269 11 121 39618 37 1396 14 196 51819 36 1296 18 324 64820 38 1444 9 81 34221 39 1527 14 196 54622 39 1527 15 225 58523 40 1600 4 16 16024 41 1681 8 64 32825 42 1764 0 0 026 43 1849 3 9 12927 43 1849 5 25 21528 43 1849 8 64 34429 44 1936 4 16 17630 45 2025 5 25 225Total () 1,007 35,507 192 2,354 7,023rnΣXY – ΣX ΣY[nΣX 2 – (ΣX ) 2 ][ nΣY 2 – (ΣY 2 )] == 30(7023) – (1007)(192)[30(35507) – (1007) 2 ][30(2354) – (192) 2 ]==210690 – 193344(1065210 – 1014049)(70620 – 36864) = 17346(51161)(33756)173461726990716 = 1734641557= .42marital satisfaction than those with lower <strong>in</strong>itial selfesteemscores. Second, it also shows that the relationshipis more correctly described as curvil<strong>in</strong>ear—that is,clients with low or high self-esteem show less improvementthan those with a moderate level of self-esteem (remember,these data are fictional). Pearson r equals .42.


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 251Ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> marital satisfaction after counsel<strong>in</strong>g25 <strong>to</strong> 2722 <strong>to</strong> 2419 <strong>to</strong> 2116 <strong>to</strong> 1813 <strong>to</strong> 1510 <strong>to</strong> 127 <strong>to</strong> 94 <strong>to</strong> 61 <strong>to</strong> 30 <strong>to</strong> –2–3 <strong>to</strong> –5–6 <strong>to</strong> –8–9 <strong>to</strong> –1020 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42 44Self-esteem scores before counsel<strong>in</strong>gFigure 12.6 Scatterplot Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g the Relationship Between InitialSelf-Esteem <strong>and</strong> Ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> Marital Satisfaction Among Counsel<strong>in</strong>g ClientsThe value of eta obta<strong>in</strong>ed for these same data is .82,<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a substantial degree of relationship betweenthe two variables. We have not shown the calculationsfor eta s<strong>in</strong>ce they are somewhat more complicatedthan those for r. The relationship is illustrated by thesmoothed curve shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 12.6.The researcher calculates the appropriate <strong>in</strong>ferencestatistic (a t-test for r), as shown, <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whetherr .42 is significant.1St<strong>and</strong>ard error of r SE r 2n 1 1229 .185t r r .00SE r 2.3; p


252 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 12.5INTERPRETATIONGender <strong>and</strong> Political Preference(Percentages)Percentageof MalesPercentageof FemalesDemocrat 20 50Republican 70 45Other 10 5Total 100 100TABLE 12.6 Gender <strong>and</strong> Political Preference(Numbers)MalesFemalesDemocrat 2 30Republican 7 27Other 1 3TABLE 12.7Teacher Gender <strong>and</strong> Grade LevelTaught: Case 1Grade Grade Grade Grade4 5 6 7 TotalMale 10 20 20 30 80Female 40 30 30 20 120Total 50 50 50 50 200TABLE 12.8Teacher Gender <strong>and</strong> Grade LevelTaught: Case 2Grade Grade Grade Grade4 5 6 7 TotalMale 22 22 25 28 97Female 28 28 25 22 103Total 50 50 50 50 200Once aga<strong>in</strong>, we must look at summary statistics—evenpercentages—carefully. Percentages can be mislead<strong>in</strong>gunless the number of cases is also given. At first glance,Table 12.5 may look impressive—until one discoversthat the data <strong>in</strong> it represent 60 females <strong>and</strong> only 10males. In crossbreak form, Table 12.6 represents the actualnumbers, as opposed <strong>to</strong> percentages, of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.Table 12.7 illustrates a fictitious relationship betweenteacher gender <strong>and</strong> grade level taught.As you can see, thelargest number of male teachers is <strong>to</strong> be found <strong>in</strong> grade 7,<strong>and</strong> the largest number of female teachers is <strong>to</strong> be found<strong>in</strong> grade 4. Here, <strong>to</strong>o, however, we must ask: <strong>How</strong> muchdifference must there be between these frequencies for us<strong>to</strong> consider them important? One of the limitations of categoricaldata is that such evaluations are even harder thanwith quantitative data. One possible approach is <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>eprior experience or knowledge. Table 12.7 does suggesta trend <strong>to</strong>ward an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly larger proportion ofmale teachers <strong>in</strong> the higher grades—but, aga<strong>in</strong>, is thetrend substantial enough <strong>to</strong> be considered important?The data <strong>in</strong> Table 12.8 show the same trend, but thepattern is much less strik<strong>in</strong>g. Perhaps prior experience orresearch shows (somehow) that gender differences becomeimportant whenever the with<strong>in</strong>-grade difference ismore than 10 percent (or a frequency of 5 <strong>in</strong> these data).Such knowledge is seldom available, however, whichleads us <strong>to</strong> consider the summary statistic (similar <strong>to</strong> thecorrelation coefficient) known as the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient(see Chapter 11). In order <strong>to</strong> use it, however, rememberthat the data must be presented <strong>in</strong> crossbreaktables. Calculat<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient is easilydone by h<strong>and</strong> or by computer. You will recall that thisstatistic is not as straightforward <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation as thecorrelation coefficient, s<strong>in</strong>ce its <strong>in</strong>terpretation dependson the number of cells <strong>in</strong> the crossbreak table. Nevertheless,we recommend its use.Perhaps because of the difficulties mentioned above,most research reports us<strong>in</strong>g percentages or crossbreaksrely on <strong>in</strong>ference techniques <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitudeof relationships. In the absence of r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g,their use suffers from the same liabilities as with quantitativedata. When analyz<strong>in</strong>g categorical data, therefore,we recommend the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Recommendation 17: Whenever possible, place alldata <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> crossbreak tables.Recommendation 18: To clarify the importance ofrelationships, patterns, or trends, calculate a cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficient.Recommendation 19: Do not use tests of statisticalsignificance <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitude of relationships.Use them, as <strong>in</strong>tended, <strong>to</strong> judgegeneralizability.Recommendation 20: Unless a r<strong>and</strong>om sample wasused, <strong>in</strong>terpret probabilities <strong>and</strong>/or significancelevels as crude <strong>in</strong>dices, not as precise values.Example. Once aga<strong>in</strong>, let us consider an example <strong>to</strong>illustrate an analysis, this time <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g categoricaldata when compar<strong>in</strong>g groups. Let us return <strong>to</strong> Tables12.7 <strong>and</strong> 12.8 <strong>to</strong> illustrate the major recommendationsfor analyz<strong>in</strong>g categorical data. We shall consider Table12.7 first. Because there are 50 teachers, or 25 percent,


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 253TABLE 12.9 Crossbreak Table Show<strong>in</strong>g TeacherGender <strong>and</strong> Grade Level withExpected Frequencies Added(Data from Table 12.7)Grade Grade Grade Grade4 5 6 7 TotalMale 10 (20) 20 (20) 20 (20) 30 (20) 80Female 40 (30) 30 (30) 30 (30) 20 (30) 120Total 50 50 50 50 200of the <strong>to</strong>tal of 200 teachers at each grade level (4–7), wewould expect that there would be 25 percent of the <strong>to</strong>talnumber of male teachers <strong>and</strong> 25 percent of the <strong>to</strong>talnumber of female teachers at each grade level as well.Out of the <strong>to</strong>tal of 200 teachers, 80 are male <strong>and</strong> 120 arefemale. Hence, the expected frequency for male teachersat each of the grade levels would be 20 (25 percen<strong>to</strong>f 80), <strong>and</strong> for female teachers 30 (25 percent of 120).These expected frequencies are shown <strong>in</strong> parentheses <strong>in</strong>Table 12.9. We then calculate the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient,which equals .28.By referr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> Table 11.1 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 11, we estimatethat the upper limit for a 2 by 4 table (which we havehere) is approximately .80. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, a cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficient of .28 <strong>in</strong>dicates only a slight degree of relationship.As a result, we would not recommend test<strong>in</strong>gfor significance. Were we <strong>to</strong> do so, however, we wouldf<strong>in</strong>d by look<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a chi-square probability table that threedegrees of freedom requires a chi-square value of 7.81 <strong>to</strong>be considered significant at the .05 level. Our obta<strong>in</strong>edvalue for chi square was 16.66, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that the smallrelationship we have discovered probably does exist <strong>in</strong>the population from which the sample was drawn.* Thisis a good example of the difference between statistical<strong>and</strong> practical significance. Our obta<strong>in</strong>ed correlation of.28 is statistically significant but practically <strong>in</strong>significant.A correlation of .28 would be considered by mostresearchers as hav<strong>in</strong>g little practical importance.If we carry out the same analysis for Table 12.8, theresult<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient is .10. Such a correlationis, for all practical purposes, mean<strong>in</strong>gless, butshould we (for some reason) wish <strong>to</strong> see if it was statisticallysignificant, we would f<strong>in</strong>d that it is not significantat the .05 level (the chi-square value 1.98, farbelow the 7.82 needed for significance).Aga<strong>in</strong>, the calculations from Table 12.9 are not difficult.Here are the steps we followed:*Assum<strong>in</strong>g the sample is r<strong>and</strong>om.1. For the first cell above (Grade 4-male), subtract Efrom O: 10 20 102. Square the result: (O E) 2 (10) 2 1003. Divide the result by E:1O E22© 100E 20 5.00O E O − E (O − E) 2 (O − E) 2E10 20 –10 100 100/20 5.0040 30 10 100 100/30 3.3320 20 0 0 0 030 30 0 0 0 020 20 0 0 0 030 30 0 0 0 030 20 10 100 100/20 5.0020 30 –10 100 100/30 3.334. Repeat this process for each cell. (Be sure <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludeall cells.)5. Add the results of all cells:5.00 3.33 5.00 3.33 16.66 χ 26. To calculate the cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient, we used theformula:x 2C B x 2 n 16.66B 16.66 200 .28Relat<strong>in</strong>g Variables With<strong>in</strong>a Group: Categorical DataAlthough the preced<strong>in</strong>g section <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>ggroups, the reason<strong>in</strong>g also applies <strong>to</strong> hypotheses that exam<strong>in</strong>erelationships among categorical variables with<strong>in</strong>just one group.Amoment’s thought shows why. The proceduresavailable <strong>to</strong> us are the same—percentages orcrossbreak tables. Suppose our hypothesis is that amongcollege students, gender is related <strong>to</strong> political preference.To test this we must divide the data we obta<strong>in</strong> from thisgroup by gender <strong>and</strong> political preference. This gives usthe crossbreak <strong>in</strong> Table 12.6. Because all such hypothesesmust be tested by divid<strong>in</strong>g people <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> groups, thestatistical analysis is the same whether seen as onegroup, subdivided, or as two or more different groups.A summary of the most commonly used statisticaltechniques, both descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential, as used withquantitative <strong>and</strong> categorical data, is shown <strong>in</strong> Table 12.10.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHInterpret<strong>in</strong>g StatisticsSuppose a researcher found a correlation of .08 betweendr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g grapefruit juice <strong>and</strong> subsequent <strong>in</strong>cidence ofarthritis <strong>to</strong> be statistically significant. Is that possible?(Yes, it is quite possible. If the sample had been r<strong>and</strong>omlyselected, <strong>and</strong> the sample size was around 500, a correlationof .08 would be statistically significant at the .05 level. Butbecause of the small relationship—<strong>and</strong> many uncontrolledvariables—we would not s<strong>to</strong>p dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g grapefruit juicebased on an r of only .08!)Suppose an early <strong>in</strong>tervention program was found <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>creaseIQ scores on average by 12 po<strong>in</strong>ts, but that this wasnot statistically significant at the .05 level. <strong>How</strong> much attentionwould you give <strong>to</strong> this report? (We would pay considerableattention; 12 IQ po<strong>in</strong>ts is a lot <strong>and</strong> could be veryimportant if confirmed <strong>in</strong> replications. Evidently the samplesize was rather small.)Suppose the difference <strong>in</strong> poll<strong>in</strong>g preference for a particularc<strong>and</strong>idate was found <strong>to</strong> be 52 percent for the Democrat asopposed <strong>to</strong> 48 percent for the Republican, with a marg<strong>in</strong> oferror of 2 percent at the .05 level. Would you consider thisdifference important? (One way of report<strong>in</strong>g such results isthat the probability of the difference be<strong>in</strong>g due <strong>to</strong> chance isless than .01.* In addition, a difference of only 4 po<strong>in</strong>ts is ofgreat practical importance s<strong>in</strong>ce the w<strong>in</strong>ner <strong>in</strong> a two-personelection needs only 51 percent of the vote <strong>to</strong> w<strong>in</strong>. A verysimilar prediction proved wrong <strong>in</strong> the 1948 presidentialelection, when Truman defeated Dewey. The usual explanationsare that the sample was not r<strong>and</strong>om <strong>and</strong> thus not representative,<strong>and</strong>/or that a lot of people changed their m<strong>in</strong>dsbefore they entered the vot<strong>in</strong>g booth.)*The SE of each percentage must be 2.00 (the marg<strong>in</strong> of error) dividedby 1.96 (the number of st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations required at the 5 percentlevel), or approximately 1.00. The st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference(SED) equals the square root of (1 2 +1 2 ) or 1.4. The difference between48 percent <strong>and</strong> 52 percent—4 percent—divided by 1.4 (theSED) equals 2.86, which yields a probability of less than .01.TABLE 12.10 Summary of Commonly Used Statistical TechniquesDATAQuantitativeCategoricalTwo or more groups are compared:Descriptive Statistics Frequency polygons PercentagesAveragesBar graphsSpreadsPie chartsEffect sizeCrossbreak (cont<strong>in</strong>gency) tablesInferential Statistics t-test for means Chi squareANOVAt-test for proportionsANCOVAMANOVAMANCOVAConfidence <strong>in</strong>tervalsMann-Whitney U testKruskal-Wallis ANOVASign testFriedman two-way ANOVARelationships among variablesare studied with<strong>in</strong> one group:Descriptive Statistics Scatterplot Crossbreak (cont<strong>in</strong>gency)Correlation coefficient (r) tablesetaCont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficientInferential Statistics t-test for r Chi squareConfidence <strong>in</strong>tervalst-test for proportions254


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 255A Recap of RecommendationsYou may have noticed that many of our recommendationsare essentially the same, regardless of the methodof statistical analysis <strong>in</strong>volved. To stress their importance,we want <strong>to</strong> state them aga<strong>in</strong> here, all <strong>to</strong>gether,phrased more generally.We recommend that researchers:Use graphic techniques before calculat<strong>in</strong>g numericalsummary <strong>in</strong>dices. Pay particular attention <strong>to</strong> outliers.Use both graphs <strong>and</strong> summary <strong>in</strong>dices <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretresults of a study.Make use of external criteria (such as prior experienceor scores of known groups) <strong>to</strong> assess the magnitude ofa relationship whenever such criteria are available.Use professional consensus when evaluat<strong>in</strong>g themagnitude of an effect size (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g correlationcoefficients).Consider us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics only if you canmake a conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g case for the importance of thesize of the relationship found <strong>in</strong> the sample.Use tests of statistical significance only <strong>to</strong> evaluategeneralizability, not <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitude ofrelationships.When r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g has not occurred, treat probabilitiesas approximations or crude <strong>in</strong>dices ratherthan as precise values.Report confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals rather than, or <strong>in</strong> addition<strong>to</strong>, significance levels whenever possible.We also want <strong>to</strong> make a f<strong>in</strong>al recommendation <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gthe dist<strong>in</strong>ction between parametric <strong>and</strong> nonparametricstatistics. S<strong>in</strong>ce the calculation of statisticshas now become rather easy <strong>and</strong> quick ow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> theavailability of many computer programs, we concludewith the follow<strong>in</strong>g suggestion <strong>to</strong> researchers:Use both parametric <strong>and</strong> nonparametric techniques<strong>to</strong> analyze data. When the results are consistent,<strong>in</strong>terpretation will thereby be strengthened. Whenthe results are not consistent, discuss possiblereasons.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.APPROACHES TO RESEARCHA good deal of educational research is done <strong>in</strong> one of two ways: either two or moregroups are compared, or variables with<strong>in</strong> one group are related.The data <strong>in</strong> a study may be either quantitative or categorical.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsCOMPARING GROUPS USING QUANTITATIVE DATAWhen compar<strong>in</strong>g two or more groups us<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data, researchers can comparethem through frequency polygons, calculation of averages, <strong>and</strong> calculation of spreads.We recommend, therefore, construct<strong>in</strong>g frequency polygons, us<strong>in</strong>g data on themeans of known groups, calculat<strong>in</strong>g effect sizes, <strong>and</strong> report<strong>in</strong>g confidence <strong>in</strong>tervalswhen compar<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data from two or more groups.


256 PART 3 Data Analysis www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eRELATING VARIABLES WITHIN A GROUP USING QUANTITATIVE DATAWhen researchers exam<strong>in</strong>e a relationship between quantitative variables with<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>glegroup, the appropriate techniques are the scatterplot <strong>and</strong> the correlation coefficient.Because a scatterplot illustrates all the data visually, researchers should beg<strong>in</strong> theiranalysis of data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a s<strong>in</strong>gle group by construct<strong>in</strong>g a scatterplot.Therefore, we recommend construct<strong>in</strong>g scatterplots <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g both scatterplots <strong>and</strong>correlation coefficients when relat<strong>in</strong>g variables <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data with<strong>in</strong> as<strong>in</strong>gle group.COMPARING GROUPS USING CATEGORICAL DATAWhen the data are categorical, groups can be compared by report<strong>in</strong>g either percentagesor frequencies <strong>in</strong> crossbreak tables.It is a good idea <strong>to</strong> report both the percentage <strong>and</strong> the number of cases <strong>in</strong> a crossbreaktable, as percentages alone can be mislead<strong>in</strong>g.Therefore, we recommend construct<strong>in</strong>g crossbreak tables <strong>and</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficients when compar<strong>in</strong>g categorical data <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g two or more groups.RELATING VARIABLES WITHIN A GROUP USING CATEGORICAL DATAWhen you are exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g relationships among categorical data with<strong>in</strong> one group,we aga<strong>in</strong> recommend construct<strong>in</strong>g crossbreak tables <strong>and</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gencycoefficients.TWO FINAL RECOMMENDATIONSWhen tests of statistical significance can be applied, it is recommended that they beused <strong>to</strong> evaluate generalizability only, not <strong>to</strong> evaluate the magnitude of relationships.Confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals should be reported <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> significance levels.Both parametric <strong>and</strong> nonparametric techniques should be used <strong>to</strong> analyze data ratherthan either one alone.Key Termscorrelationcoefficient 247curvil<strong>in</strong>earrelationship 247effect size (ES) 244<strong>in</strong>ferentialstatistics 244l<strong>in</strong>earrelationship 247scatterplot 247For Discussion1. Give some examples of how the results of a study might be significant statisticallyyet unimportant educationally. Could the reverse be true?2. Are there times when a slight difference <strong>in</strong> means (e.g., an effect size of less than.50) might be important? Expla<strong>in</strong> your answer.3. When compar<strong>in</strong>g groups, the use of frequency polygons helps us decide which measureof central tendency is the most appropriate <strong>to</strong> calculate. <strong>How</strong> so?4. Why is it important <strong>to</strong> consider outliers <strong>in</strong> scatterplots?5. “When analyz<strong>in</strong>g data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from two groups, the first th<strong>in</strong>g researchers should do isconstruct a frequency polygon of each group’s scores.” Why is this important—or is it?6. Why is it important <strong>to</strong> use both graphs <strong>and</strong> summary <strong>in</strong>dices (e.g., the means) <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret the results of a study—or is it?7. A picture, supposedly, is worth a thous<strong>and</strong> words. Would this statement also apply <strong>to</strong>analyz<strong>in</strong>g the results of a study? Can numbers alone ever give a complete picture ofa study’s results? Why or why not?


CHAPTER 12 Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective 257<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 12: Statistics <strong>in</strong> PerspectiveUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 12, once aga<strong>in</strong> state the question or hypothesis of your study. Summarizethe descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics you would use <strong>to</strong> describe the relationship you arehypothesiz<strong>in</strong>g. Then tell how you would evaluate the magnitude of any relationship you mightf<strong>in</strong>d. F<strong>in</strong>ally, describe the changes <strong>in</strong> techniques <strong>to</strong> be used from those you described <strong>in</strong> ProblemSheets 10 <strong>and</strong> 11, if any. If your study is qualitative, you will probably omit question 3.Problem Sheet 12Statistics <strong>in</strong> Perspective1. The question or hypothesis of my study is: __________________________________2. My expected relationship(s) would be described us<strong>in</strong>g the follow<strong>in</strong>g descriptivestatistics:_____________________________________________________________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.3. The <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics I would use are:____________________________________A full-sized versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.4. I would evaluate the magnitude of the relationship(s) I f<strong>in</strong>d by: __________________5. The changes (if any) <strong>in</strong> my use of descriptive or <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics from those Idescribed <strong>in</strong> Problem Sheets 10 <strong>and</strong> 11 are as follows: _________________________


Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>MethodologiesP A R T4In Part 4, we beg<strong>in</strong> a more detailed discussion of some of the methodologies thateducational researchers use. We concentrate here on quantitative research, with aseparate chapter devoted <strong>to</strong> group-comparison experimental research, s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectexperimental research, correlational research, causal-comparative research, <strong>and</strong>survey research. In each chapter, we not only discuss the method <strong>in</strong> some detail, butwe also provide examples of published studies <strong>in</strong> which the researchers used one ofthese methods. We conclude each chapter with an analysis of a particular study’sstrengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses.


13Experimental <strong>Research</strong>The Uniqueness ofExperimental <strong>Research</strong>Essential Characteristicsof Experimental<strong>Research</strong>Comparison of GroupsManipulation of theIndependent VariableR<strong>and</strong>omizationControl of ExtraneousVariablesGroup <strong>Design</strong>s <strong>in</strong>Experimental <strong>Research</strong>Weak Experimental <strong>Design</strong>sTrue Experimental <strong>Design</strong>sQuasi-Experimental <strong>Design</strong>sFac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong>sControl of Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity:A SummaryEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g theLikelihood of a Threat<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Experimental StudiesControl of ExperimentalTreatmentsAn Example ofExperimental <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData Analysis/ResultsDiscussion/InterpretationOBJECTIVES“What is adouble-bl<strong>in</strong>dstudy anyway?”Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly the purpose ofexperimental research.Describe the basic steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>conduct<strong>in</strong>g an experiment.Describe two ways <strong>in</strong> which experimentalresearch differs from other forms ofeducational research.Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between r<strong>and</strong>omassignment <strong>and</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om selection <strong>and</strong> theimportance of each.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the phrase“manipulation of variables” <strong>and</strong> describethree ways <strong>in</strong> which such manipulationcan occur.Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between examples of weak <strong>and</strong>strong experimental designs <strong>and</strong> drawdiagrams of such designs.Identify various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validityassociated with different experimentaldesigns.“This seemslike more thanjust a double!”Expla<strong>in</strong> three ways <strong>in</strong> which various threats<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> experimental researchcan be controlled.Expla<strong>in</strong> how match<strong>in</strong>g can be used<strong>to</strong> equate groups <strong>in</strong> experimentalstudies.Describe briefly the purpose of fac<strong>to</strong>rial<strong>and</strong> counterbalanced designs <strong>and</strong> drawdiagrams of such designs.Describe briefly the purpose of a timeseriesdesign <strong>and</strong> draw a diagram of thisdesign.Describe briefly how <strong>to</strong> assess probablethreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> anexperimental study.Recognize an experimental study whenyou see one <strong>in</strong> the literature.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Centerat www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About What Constitutes an ExperimentAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 13.1: Group Experimental <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 13.2: <strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>g an ExperimentActivity 13.3: Characteristics of Experimental <strong>Research</strong>Activity 13.4: R<strong>and</strong>om Selections vs. R<strong>and</strong>om AssignmentDoes team-teach<strong>in</strong>g improve the achievement of students <strong>in</strong> high school social studies classes? AbigailJohnson, the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal of a large high school <strong>in</strong> M<strong>in</strong>neapolis, M<strong>in</strong>nesota, hav<strong>in</strong>g heard encourag<strong>in</strong>g remarksabout the idea at a recent educational conference, wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, she asks some of her eleventhgradeworld his<strong>to</strong>ry teachers <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> an experiment. Three teachers are <strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e their classes <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> onelarge group. These teachers are <strong>to</strong> work as a team, shar<strong>in</strong>g the plann<strong>in</strong>g, teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> evaluation of these students.Three other teachers are assigned <strong>to</strong> teach a class <strong>in</strong> the same subject <strong>in</strong>dividually, with the usual arrangement ofone teacher per class. The students selected <strong>to</strong> participate are similar <strong>in</strong> ability, <strong>and</strong> the teachers will teach at thesame time, us<strong>in</strong>g the same curriculum. All are <strong>to</strong> use the same st<strong>and</strong>ardized tests <strong>and</strong> other assessment <strong>in</strong>struments,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g written tests prepared jo<strong>in</strong>tly by the six teachers. Periodically dur<strong>in</strong>g the semester, Mrs. Johnson willcompare the scores of the two groups of students on these tests.This is an example of an experiment—the comparison of a treatment group with a nontreatment group. In thischapter, you will learn about various procedures that researchers use <strong>to</strong> carry out such experiments, as well as howthey try <strong>to</strong> ensure that it is the experimental treatment rather than some uncontrolled variable that causes thechanges <strong>in</strong> achievement.Experimental research is one of the most powerful researchmethodologies that researchers can use. Of themany types of research that might be used, the experimentis the best way <strong>to</strong> establish cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect relationshipsamong variables. Yet experiments are notalways easy <strong>to</strong> conduct. In this chapter, we will showyou both the power of, <strong>and</strong> the problems <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>,conduct<strong>in</strong>g experiments.The Uniquenessof Experimental <strong>Research</strong>Of all the research methodologies described <strong>in</strong> thisbook, experimental research is unique <strong>in</strong> two very importantrespects: It is the only type of research that directlyattempts <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence a particular variable, <strong>and</strong>when properly applied, it is the best type for test<strong>in</strong>ghypotheses about cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect relationships. In anexperimental study, researchers look at the effect(s) ofat least one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable on one or more dependentvariables. The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> experimentalresearch is also frequently referred <strong>to</strong> as theexperimental, or treatment, variable. The dependentvariable, also known as the criterion, or outcome,variable, refers <strong>to</strong> the results or outcomes of the study.The major characteristic of experimental researchthat dist<strong>in</strong>guishes it from all other types of research isthat researchers manipulate the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable.They decide the nature of the treatment (that is, what isgo<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> happen <strong>to</strong> the subjects of the study), <strong>to</strong> whom itis <strong>to</strong> be applied, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> what extent. Independent variablesfrequently manipulated <strong>in</strong> educational research <strong>in</strong>cludemethods of <strong>in</strong>struction, types of assignment,learn<strong>in</strong>g materials, rewards given <strong>to</strong> students, <strong>and</strong> typesof questions asked by teachers. Dependent variablesthat are frequently studied <strong>in</strong>clude achievement, <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> a subject, attention span, motivation, <strong>and</strong> attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward school.261


262 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAfter the treatment has been adm<strong>in</strong>istered for an appropriatelength of time, researchers observe or measurethe groups receiv<strong>in</strong>g different treatments (by means of aposttest of some sort) <strong>to</strong> see if they differ. Another wayof say<strong>in</strong>g this is that researchers want <strong>to</strong> see whether thetreatment made a difference. If the average scores of thegroups on the posttest do differ <strong>and</strong> researchers cannotf<strong>in</strong>d any sensible alternative explanations for this difference,they can conclude that the treatment did have aneffect <strong>and</strong> is likely the cause of the difference.Experimental research, therefore, enables researchers<strong>to</strong> go beyond description <strong>and</strong> prediction, beyond theidentification of relationships, <strong>to</strong> at least a partial determ<strong>in</strong>ationof what causes them. Correlational studiesmay demonstrate a strong relationship between socioeconomiclevel <strong>and</strong> academic achievement, for <strong>in</strong>stance,but they cannot demonstrate that improv<strong>in</strong>g socioeconomiclevel will necessarily improve achievement.Only experimental research has this capability. Someactual examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of experimental studiesthat have been conducted by educational researchers areas follows:“Quality of learn<strong>in</strong>g with an active versus passivemotivational set” 1“Comparison of computer-assisted cooperative,competitive, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualistic learn<strong>in</strong>g” 2“An <strong>in</strong>tensive group counsel<strong>in</strong>g dropout prevention<strong>in</strong>tervention: . . . isolat<strong>in</strong>g at-risk adolescents with<strong>in</strong>high schools” 3“The effects of student questions <strong>and</strong> teacher questionson concept acquisition” 4“Chang<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g practices <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream classrooms<strong>to</strong> improve bond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> behavior of lowachievers” 5“Mnemonic versus nonmnemonic vocabulary-learn<strong>in</strong>gstrategies for children” 6Essential Characteristicsof Experimental <strong>Research</strong>The word experiment has a long <strong>and</strong> illustrious his<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong> the annals of research. It has often been hailed as themost powerful method that exists for study<strong>in</strong>g cause<strong>and</strong> effect. Its orig<strong>in</strong>s go back <strong>to</strong> the very beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs ofhis<strong>to</strong>ry when, for example, primeval humans first experimentedwith ways <strong>to</strong> produce fire. One can imag<strong>in</strong>ecountless trial-<strong>and</strong>-error attempts on their part beforeachiev<strong>in</strong>g success by spark<strong>in</strong>g rocks or by sp<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gwooden sp<strong>in</strong>dles <strong>in</strong> dry leaves. Much of the success ofmodern science is due <strong>to</strong> carefully designed <strong>and</strong> meticulouslyimplemented experiments.The basic idea underly<strong>in</strong>g all experimental researchis really quite simple: Try someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> systematicallyobserve what happens. Formal experiments consis<strong>to</strong>f two basic conditions. First, at least two (bu<strong>to</strong>ften more) conditions or methods are compared <strong>to</strong>assess the effect(s) of particular conditions or “treatments”(the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable). Second, the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable is directly manipulated by the researcher.Change is planned for <strong>and</strong> deliberately manipulated<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> study its effect(s) on one or more outcomes(the dependent variable). Let us discuss some importantcharacteristics of experimental research <strong>in</strong> a bitmore detail.COMPARISON OF GROUPSAn experiment usually <strong>in</strong>volves two groups of subjects, anexperimental group <strong>and</strong> a control or a comparison group,although it is possible <strong>to</strong> conduct an experiment with onlyone group (by provid<strong>in</strong>g all treatments <strong>to</strong> the same subjects)or with three or more groups. The experimentalgroup receives a treatment of some sort (such as a newtextbook or a different method of teach<strong>in</strong>g), while thecontrol group receives no treatment (or the comparisongroup receives a different treatment). The control or thecomparison group is crucially important <strong>in</strong> all experimentalresearch, for it enables the researcher <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether the treatment has had an effect or whether onetreatment is more effective than another.His<strong>to</strong>rically, a pure control group is one that receivesno treatment at all. While this is often the case <strong>in</strong> medicalor psychological research, it is rarely true <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. The control group almost always receives adifferent treatment of some sort. Some educational researchers,therefore, refer <strong>to</strong> comparison groups ratherthan <strong>to</strong> control groups.Consider an example. Suppose a researcher wished<strong>to</strong> study the effectiveness of a new method of teach<strong>in</strong>gscience. He or she would have the students <strong>in</strong> the experimentalgroup taught by the new method, but the students<strong>in</strong> the comparison group would cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> betaught by their teacher’s usual method. The researcherwould not adm<strong>in</strong>ister the new method <strong>to</strong> the experimentalgroup <strong>and</strong> have a control group do noth<strong>in</strong>g. Anymethod of <strong>in</strong>struction would likely be more effectivethan no method at all!


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 263MANIPULATION OF THE INDEPENDENTVARIABLEThe second essential characteristic of all experiments isthat the researcher actively manipulates the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables. What does this mean? Simply put, it meansthat the researcher deliberately <strong>and</strong> directly determ<strong>in</strong>eswhat forms the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable will take <strong>and</strong> thenwhich group will get which form. For example, if the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable <strong>in</strong> a study is the amount of enthusiasman <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r displays, a researcher might tra<strong>in</strong> twoteachers <strong>to</strong> display different amounts of enthusiasm asthey teach their classes.Although many <strong>in</strong>dependent variables <strong>in</strong> educationcan be manipulated, many others cannot. Examples of<strong>in</strong>dependent variables that can be manipulated <strong>in</strong>cludeteach<strong>in</strong>g method, type of counsel<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g activities,assignments given, <strong>and</strong> materials used; examples of <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables that cannot be manipulated <strong>in</strong>cludegender, ethnicity, age, <strong>and</strong> religious preference. <strong>Research</strong>erscan manipulate the k<strong>in</strong>ds of learn<strong>in</strong>g activities<strong>to</strong> which students are exposed <strong>in</strong> a classroom, but theycannot manipulate, say, religious preference—that is,students cannot be “made <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>” Protestants, Catholics,Jews, or Muslims, for example, <strong>to</strong> serve the purposes ofa study. To manipulate a variable, researchers must decidewho is <strong>to</strong> get someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> when, where, <strong>and</strong> howthey will get it.The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> an experimental studymay be established <strong>in</strong> several ways—either (1) oneform of the variable versus another; (2) presence versusabsence of a particular form; or (3) vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees ofthe same form. An example of (1) would be a studycompar<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>quiry method with the lecture methodof <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g chemistry. An example of(2) would be a study compar<strong>in</strong>g the use of transparenciesversus no transparencies <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g statistics. Anexample of (3) would be a study compar<strong>in</strong>g the effectsof different specified amounts of teacher enthusiasm onstudent attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward mathematics. In both (1) <strong>and</strong>(2), the variable (method) is clearly categorical. In (3), avariable that <strong>in</strong> actuality is quantitative (degree of enthusiasm)is treated as categorical (the effects of onlyspecified amounts of enthusiasm will be studied) <strong>in</strong>order for the researcher <strong>to</strong> manipulate (that is, <strong>to</strong> controlfor) the amount of enthusiasm.RANDOMIZATIONAn important aspect of many experiments is the r<strong>and</strong>omassignment of subjects <strong>to</strong> groups. Although there arecerta<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of experiments <strong>in</strong> which r<strong>and</strong>om assignmentis not possible, researchers try <strong>to</strong> use r<strong>and</strong>omizationwhenever feasible. It is a crucial <strong>in</strong>gredient <strong>in</strong> thebest k<strong>in</strong>ds of experiments. R<strong>and</strong>om assignment is similar,but not identical, <strong>to</strong> the concept of r<strong>and</strong>om selectionwe discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 6. R<strong>and</strong>om assignment meansthat every <strong>in</strong>dividual who is participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an experimenthas an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g assigned <strong>to</strong> any of theexperimental or control conditions be<strong>in</strong>g compared.R<strong>and</strong>om selection, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, means that everymember of a population has an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>gselected <strong>to</strong> be a member of the sample. Under r<strong>and</strong>omassignment, each member of the sample is given a number(arbitrarily), <strong>and</strong> a table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers (seeChapter 6) is then used <strong>to</strong> select the members of theexperimental <strong>and</strong> control groups.Three th<strong>in</strong>gs should be noted about the r<strong>and</strong>om assignmen<strong>to</strong>f subjects <strong>to</strong> groups. First, it takes placebefore the experiment beg<strong>in</strong>s. Second, it is a process ofassign<strong>in</strong>g or distribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> groups, not a resul<strong>to</strong>f such distribution. This means that you cannotlook at two groups that have already been formed <strong>and</strong>be able <strong>to</strong> tell, just by look<strong>in</strong>g, whether or not they wereformed r<strong>and</strong>omly. Third, the use of r<strong>and</strong>om assignmentallows the researcher <strong>to</strong> form groups that, right at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the study, are equivalent—that is, they differonly by chance <strong>in</strong> any variables of <strong>in</strong>terest. In otherwords, r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate thethreat of extraneous, or additional, variables—no<strong>to</strong>nly those of which researchers are aware but alsothose of which they are not aware—that might affectthe outcome of the study. This is the beauty <strong>and</strong> thepower of r<strong>and</strong>om assignment. It is one of the reasonswhy experiments are, <strong>in</strong> general, more effective thanother types of research for assess<strong>in</strong>g cause-<strong>and</strong>-effectrelationships.This last statement is tempered, of course, by the realizationthat groups formed through r<strong>and</strong>om assignmentmay still differ somewhat. R<strong>and</strong>om assignmentensures only that groups are equivalent (or at least asequivalent as human be<strong>in</strong>gs can make them) at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof an experiment.Furthermore, r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is no guarantee ofequivalent groups unless both groups are sufficientlylarge. No one would expect r<strong>and</strong>om assignment <strong>to</strong> result<strong>in</strong> equivalence if only five subjects were assigned <strong>to</strong>each group, for example. There are no rules for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ghow large groups must be, but most researchersare uncomfortable rely<strong>in</strong>g on r<strong>and</strong>om assignment withfewer than 40 subjects <strong>in</strong> each group.


264 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eControl of ExtraneousVariables<strong>Research</strong>ers <strong>in</strong> an experimental study have an opportunity<strong>to</strong> exercise far more control than <strong>in</strong> most otherforms of research. They determ<strong>in</strong>e the treatment (ortreatments), select the sample, assign <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong>groups, decide which group will get the treatment, try<strong>to</strong> control other fac<strong>to</strong>rs besides the treatment that might<strong>in</strong>fluence the outcome of the study, <strong>and</strong> then (f<strong>in</strong>ally)observe or measure the effect of the treatment on thegroups when the treatment is completed.In Chapter 9, we <strong>in</strong>troduced the idea of <strong>in</strong>ternal validity<strong>and</strong> discussed several k<strong>in</strong>ds of threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity. It is very important for researchers conduct<strong>in</strong>gan experimental study <strong>to</strong> do their best <strong>to</strong> control for—that is, <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate or <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imize the possible effec<strong>to</strong>f—these threats. If researchers are unsure whether anothervariable might be the cause of a result observed <strong>in</strong>a study, they cannot be sure what the cause really is. Forexample, if a researcher attempted <strong>to</strong> compare the effectsof two different methods of <strong>in</strong>struction on studentattitudes <strong>to</strong>ward his<strong>to</strong>ry but did not make sure that thegroups <strong>in</strong>volved were equivalent <strong>in</strong> ability, then abilitymight be a possible alternative explanation (rather thanthe difference <strong>in</strong> methods) for any differences <strong>in</strong> attitudesof the groups found on a posttest.In particular, researchers who conduct experimentalstudies try their best <strong>to</strong> control any <strong>and</strong> all subject characteristicsthat might affect the outcome of the study.They do this by ensur<strong>in</strong>g that the two groups are asequivalent as possible on all variables other than the oneor ones be<strong>in</strong>g studied (that is, the <strong>in</strong>dependent variables).<strong>How</strong> do researchers m<strong>in</strong>imize or elim<strong>in</strong>ate threatsdue <strong>to</strong> subject characteristics? Many ways exist. Hereare some of the most common.R<strong>and</strong>omization: As we mentioned before, if subjectscan be r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> the variousgroups <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> an experimental study, researcherscan assume that the groups are equivalent.This is the best way <strong>to</strong> ensure that the effectsof one or more possible extraneous variables havebeen controlled.Hold<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> variables constant: The idea hereis <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate the possible effects of a variable byremov<strong>in</strong>g it from the study. For example, if a researchersuspects that gender might <strong>in</strong>fluence theoutcomes of a study, she could control for it byrestrict<strong>in</strong>g the subjects of the study <strong>to</strong> females <strong>and</strong>by exclud<strong>in</strong>g all males. The variable of gender, <strong>in</strong>other words, is held constant. <strong>How</strong>ever, there is acost <strong>in</strong>volved (as there almost always is) for thiscontrol, as the generalizability of the results of thestudy are correspond<strong>in</strong>gly reduced.Build<strong>in</strong>g the variable <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the design: This solution<strong>in</strong>volves build<strong>in</strong>g the variable(s) <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the study<strong>to</strong> assess their effects. It is the exact opposite ofthe previous idea. Us<strong>in</strong>g the preced<strong>in</strong>g example, theresearcher would <strong>in</strong>clude both females <strong>and</strong> males(as dist<strong>in</strong>ct groups) <strong>in</strong> the design of the study <strong>and</strong>then analyze the effects of both gender <strong>and</strong>method on outcomes.Match<strong>in</strong>g: Often pairs of subjects can be matchedon certa<strong>in</strong> variables of <strong>in</strong>terest. If a researcher feltthat age, for example, might affect the outcome ofa study, he might endeavor <strong>to</strong> match students accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> their ages <strong>and</strong> then assign one memberof each pair (r<strong>and</strong>omly if possible) <strong>to</strong> each of thecomparison groups.Us<strong>in</strong>g subjects as their own controls: When subjectsare used as their own controls, their performanceunder both (or all) treatments is compared.Thus, the same students might be taught algebraunits first by an <strong>in</strong>quiry method <strong>and</strong> later by a lecturemethod. Another example is the assessmen<strong>to</strong>f an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g a period oftime before <strong>and</strong> after a treatment is implemented<strong>to</strong> see whether changes <strong>in</strong> behavior occur.Us<strong>in</strong>g analysis of covariance: As mentioned <strong>in</strong>Chapter 11, analysis of covariance can be used <strong>to</strong>equate groups statistically on the basis of a pretes<strong>to</strong>r other variables. The posttest scores of the subjects<strong>in</strong> each group are then adjusted accord<strong>in</strong>gly.We will shortly show you a number of research designsthat illustrate how several of the above controlscan be implemented <strong>in</strong> an experimental study.Group <strong>Design</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Experimental<strong>Research</strong>The design of an experiment can take a variety offorms. Some of the designs we present <strong>in</strong> this sectionare better than others, however. Why “better”? Becauseof the various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity identified <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9: Good designs control many of these threats,while poor designs control only a few. The quality of anexperiment depends on how well the various threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity are controlled.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 265WEAK EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNS<strong>Design</strong>s that are “weak” do not have built-<strong>in</strong> controls forthreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. In addition <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable, there are a number of other plausible explanationsfor any outcomes that occur. As a result, anyresearcher who uses one of these designs has difficultyassess<strong>in</strong>g the effectiveness of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable.The One-Shot Case Study. In the one-shot casestudy design, a s<strong>in</strong>gle group is exposed <strong>to</strong> a treatmen<strong>to</strong>r event <strong>and</strong> a dependent variable is subsequently observed(measured) <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> assess the effect of thetreatment. A diagram of this design is as follows:The One-Shot Case Study <strong>Design</strong>XOTreatmentObservation(Dependent variable)The symbol X represents exposure of the group <strong>to</strong> thetreatment of <strong>in</strong>terest, while O refers <strong>to</strong> observation (measurement)of the dependent variable. The placement ofthe symbols from left <strong>to</strong> right <strong>in</strong>dicates the order <strong>in</strong> timeof X <strong>and</strong> O. As you can see, the treatment, X, comes beforeobservation of the dependent variable, O.Suppose a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> see if a new textbook<strong>in</strong>creases student <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry. He uses the textbook(X) for a semester <strong>and</strong> then measures student <strong>in</strong>terest(O) with an attitude scale. A diagram of this example isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.1.The most obvious weakness of this design is its absenceof any control. The researcher has no way ofknow<strong>in</strong>g if the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed at O (as measured by theattitude scale) are due <strong>to</strong> treatment X (the textbook).The design does not provide for any comparison, so theresearcher cannot compare the treatment results (asmeasured by the attitude scale) with the same group beforeus<strong>in</strong>g the new textbook, or with those of anothergroup us<strong>in</strong>g a different textbook. Because the group hasnot been pretested <strong>in</strong> any way, the researcher knowsnoth<strong>in</strong>g about what the group was like before us<strong>in</strong>g theXNew textbookOAttitude scale<strong>to</strong> measure <strong>in</strong>terest(Dependent variable)Figure 13.1 Example of a One-Shot Case Study <strong>Design</strong>text. Thus, he does not know whether the treatment hadany effect at all. It is quite possible that the students whouse the new textbook will <strong>in</strong>dicate very favorable attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward his<strong>to</strong>ry. But the question rema<strong>in</strong>s, werethese attitudes produced by the new textbook? Unfortunately,the one-shot case study does not help us answerthis question. To remedy this design, a comparisoncould be made with another group of students who hadthe same course content presented <strong>in</strong> the regular textbook.(We shall show you just such a design shortly.)Fortunately, the flaws <strong>in</strong> the one-shot design are so wellknown that it is seldom used <strong>in</strong> educational research.The One-Group Pretest-Posttest <strong>Design</strong>.In the one-group pretest-posttest design, a s<strong>in</strong>glegroup is measured or observed not only after be<strong>in</strong>gexposed <strong>to</strong> a treatment of some sort, but also before. Adiagram of this design is as follows:The One-Group Pretest-Posttest <strong>Design</strong>O X OPretest Treatment PosttestConsider an example of this design. A pr<strong>in</strong>cipal wants<strong>to</strong> assess the effects of weekly counsel<strong>in</strong>g sessions on theattitudes of certa<strong>in</strong> “hard-<strong>to</strong>-reach” students <strong>in</strong> her school.She asks the counselors <strong>in</strong> the program <strong>to</strong> meet once aweek with these students for a period of 10 weeks, dur<strong>in</strong>gwhich sessions the students are encouraged <strong>to</strong> expresstheir feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> concerns. She uses a 20-item scale <strong>to</strong>measure student attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward school both immediatelybefore <strong>and</strong> after the 10-week period. Figure 13.2presents a diagram of the design of the study.This design is better than the one-shot case study (theresearcher at least knows whether any change occurred),but it is still weak. N<strong>in</strong>e uncontrolled-for threats <strong>to</strong>OPretest:Twenty-itemattitude scalecompleted bystudents(Dependentvariable)XTreatment:Ten weeks ofcounsel<strong>in</strong>gOPosttest:Twenty-itemattitude scalecompleted bystudents(Dependentvariable)Figure 13.2 Example of a One-Group Pretest-Posttest<strong>Design</strong>


266 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>in</strong>ternal validity exist that might also expla<strong>in</strong> the resultson the posttest. They are his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, <strong>in</strong>strumentdecay, data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics, data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias,test<strong>in</strong>g, statistical regression, attitude of subjects, <strong>and</strong>implementation. Any or all of these may <strong>in</strong>fluence theoutcome of the study. The researcher would not know ifany differences between the pretest <strong>and</strong> the posttest aredue <strong>to</strong> the treatment or <strong>to</strong> one or more of these threats. Toremedy this, a comparison group, which does not receivethe treatment, could be added. Then if a change <strong>in</strong> attitudeoccurs between the pretest <strong>and</strong> the posttest, the researcherhas reason <strong>to</strong> believe that it was caused by thetreatment (symbolized by X).The Static-Group Comparison <strong>Design</strong>. Inthe static-group comparison design, two already exist<strong>in</strong>g,or <strong>in</strong>tact, groups are used. These are sometimes referred<strong>to</strong> as static groups, hence the name for the design.This design is sometimes called a nonequivalentcontrol group design. A diagram of this design is asfollows:The Static-Group Comparison <strong>Design</strong>XThe dashed l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicates that the two groups be<strong>in</strong>gcompared are already formed—that is, the subjects arenot r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> the two groups. X symbolizesthe experimental treatment. The blank space <strong>in</strong> the design<strong>in</strong>dicates that the “control” group does not receive the experimentaltreatment; it may receive a different treatmen<strong>to</strong>r no treatment at all. The two Os are placed exactly vertical<strong>to</strong> each other, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that the observation or measuremen<strong>to</strong>f the two groups occurs at the same time.Consider aga<strong>in</strong> the example used <strong>to</strong> illustrate the oneshotcase study design. We could apply the static-groupcomparison design <strong>to</strong> this example. The researcherwould (1) f<strong>in</strong>d two <strong>in</strong>tact groups (two classes), (2) assignthe new textbook (X) <strong>to</strong> one of the classes but have theother class use the regular textbook, <strong>and</strong> then (3) measurethe degree of <strong>in</strong>terest of all students <strong>in</strong> both classesat the same time (for example, at the end of the semester).Figure 13.3 presents a diagram of this example.Although this design provides better control overhis<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, test<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> regression threats,* it is*His<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> maturation rema<strong>in</strong> possible threats because the researchercannot be sure that the two groups have been exposed <strong>to</strong> thesame extraneous events or have the same maturational processes.OOXNew textbookRegular textbookOAttitude scale<strong>to</strong> measure <strong>in</strong>terestOAttitude scale<strong>to</strong> measure <strong>in</strong>terestFigure 13.3 Example of a Static-Group Comparison<strong>Design</strong>more vulnerable not only <strong>to</strong> mortality <strong>and</strong> location,† butalso, more importantly, <strong>to</strong> the possibility of differentialsubject characteristics.The Static-Group Pretest-Posttest <strong>Design</strong>.The static-group pretest-posttest design differs fromthe static-group comparison design only <strong>in</strong> that a pretestis given <strong>to</strong> both groups. A diagram for this design is asfollows:The Static-Group Pretest-Posttest <strong>Design</strong>O X OOOIn analyz<strong>in</strong>g the data, each <strong>in</strong>dividual’s pretest score issubtracted from his or her posttest score, thus permitt<strong>in</strong>ganalysis of “ga<strong>in</strong>” or “change.” While this provides bettercontrol of the subject characteristics threat (s<strong>in</strong>ce it is thechange <strong>in</strong> each student that is analyzed), the amount ofga<strong>in</strong> often depends on <strong>in</strong>itial performance; that is, thegroup scor<strong>in</strong>g higher on the pretest is likely <strong>to</strong> improvemore (or <strong>in</strong> some cases less), <strong>and</strong> thus subject characteristicsstillrema<strong>in</strong>ssomewha<strong>to</strong>fathreat.Further,adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>ga pretest raises the possibility of a test<strong>in</strong>g threat. In theevent that the pretest is used <strong>to</strong> match groups, this designbecomes the match<strong>in</strong>g-only pretest-posttest control groupdesign (see page 269), a much more effective design.TRUE EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNSThe essential <strong>in</strong>gredient of a true experimental design isthat subjects are r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> treatment groups.As discussed earlier, r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is a powerfultechnique for controll<strong>in</strong>g the subject characteristicsthreat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, a major consideration <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch.†This is because the groups may differ <strong>in</strong> the number of subjects lost<strong>and</strong>/or <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of resources provided.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 267The R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only ControlGroup <strong>Design</strong>. The r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only controlgroup design <strong>in</strong>volves two groups, both of which areformed by r<strong>and</strong>om assignment. One group receives theexperimental treatment while the other does not, <strong>and</strong> thenboth groups are posttested on the dependent variable. Adiagram of this design is as follows:The R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-OnlyControl Group <strong>Design</strong>Treatment group R X OControl group R C OAs before, the symbol X represents exposure <strong>to</strong> the treatment<strong>and</strong> O refers <strong>to</strong> the measurement of the dependentvariable. R represents the r<strong>and</strong>om assignment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>to</strong> groups. C now represents the control group.In this design, the control of certa<strong>in</strong> threats is excellent.Through the use of r<strong>and</strong>om assignment, the threatsof subject characteristics, maturation, <strong>and</strong> statisticalregression are well controlled for. Because none of thesubjects <strong>in</strong> the study are measured twice, test<strong>in</strong>g is not apossible threat. This is perhaps the best of all designs <strong>to</strong>use <strong>in</strong> an experimental study, provided there are at least40 subjects <strong>in</strong> each group.There are, unfortunately, some threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitythat are not controlled for by this design. The firstis mortality. Because the two groups are similar, wemight expect an equal dropout rate from each group.<strong>How</strong>ever, exposure <strong>to</strong> the treatment may cause more <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>in</strong> the experimental group <strong>to</strong> drop out (or stay<strong>in</strong>) than <strong>in</strong> the control group. This may result <strong>in</strong> the twogroups becom<strong>in</strong>g dissimilar <strong>in</strong> terms of their characteristics,which <strong>in</strong> turn may affect the results on theposttest. For this reason, researchers should always reporthow many subjects drop out of each group dur<strong>in</strong>gan experiment. An attitud<strong>in</strong>al threat (Hawthorne effect)is possible. In addition, implementation, data collec<strong>to</strong>rbias, location, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry threats may exist. Thesethreats can sometimes be controlled by appropriatemodifications <strong>to</strong> this design.As an example of this design, consider a hypotheticalstudy <strong>in</strong> which a researcher <strong>in</strong>vestigates the effects of aseries of sensitivity tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g workshops on facultymorale <strong>in</strong> a large high school district. The researcherr<strong>and</strong>omly selects a sample of 100 teachers from all theteachers <strong>in</strong> the district. The researcher then (1) r<strong>and</strong>omlyassigns the teachers <strong>in</strong> the district <strong>to</strong> two groups;(2) exposes one group, but not the other, <strong>to</strong> the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g;<strong>and</strong> then (3) measures the morale of each group us<strong>in</strong>ga questionnaire. Figure 13.4 presents a diagram of thishypothetical experiment.Aga<strong>in</strong> we stress that it is important <strong>to</strong> keep clear thedist<strong>in</strong>ction between r<strong>and</strong>om selection <strong>and</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om assignment.Both <strong>in</strong>volve the process of r<strong>and</strong>omization,but for a different purpose. R<strong>and</strong>om selection, you willrecall, is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> provide a representative sample.But it may or may not be accompanied by the r<strong>and</strong>omassignment of subjects <strong>to</strong> groups. R<strong>and</strong>om assignmentis <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> equate groups, <strong>and</strong> often is not accompaniedby r<strong>and</strong>om selection.The R<strong>and</strong>omized Pretest-Posttest ControlGroup <strong>Design</strong>. The r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttestcontrol group design differs from the r<strong>and</strong>omizedposttest-only control group design solely <strong>in</strong> the use of apretest. Two groups of subjects are used, with bothgroups be<strong>in</strong>g measured or observed twice. The first100 teachersr<strong>and</strong>omly selectedRR<strong>and</strong>om assignmen<strong>to</strong>f50 teachers <strong>to</strong>experimental groupRR<strong>and</strong>om assignmen<strong>to</strong>f50 teachers <strong>to</strong>control groupXTreatment:Sensitivity tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gworkshopsCNo treatment:Do not receivesensitivity tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gOPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependent variable)OPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependent variable)Figure 13.4 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only Control Group <strong>Design</strong>


268 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7emeasurement serves as the pretest, the second as theposttest. R<strong>and</strong>om assignment is used <strong>to</strong> form thegroups. The measurements or observations are collectedat the same time for both groups. A diagram of this designfollows.The R<strong>and</strong>omized Pretest-PosttestControl Group <strong>Design</strong>Treatment group R O X OControl group R O C OThe use of the pretest raises the possibility of a pretesttreatment <strong>in</strong>teraction threat, s<strong>in</strong>ce it may “alert” themembers of the experimental group, thereby caus<strong>in</strong>gthem <strong>to</strong> do better (or more poorly) on the posttest than themembers of the control group. A trade-off is that it providesthe researcher with a means of check<strong>in</strong>g whetherthe two groups are really similar—that is, whether r<strong>and</strong>omassignment actually succeeded <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g thegroups equivalent. This is particularly desirable if thenumber <strong>in</strong> each group is small (less than 30). If the pretestshows that the groups are not equivalent, the researchercan seek <strong>to</strong> make them so by us<strong>in</strong>g one of the match<strong>in</strong>gdesigns we will discuss shortly. A pretest is also necessaryif the amount of change over time is <strong>to</strong> be assessed.Let us illustrate this design by us<strong>in</strong>g our previousexample <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the use of sensitivity workshops.Figure 13.5 presents a diagram of how this designwould be used.The R<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon Four-Group<strong>Design</strong>. The r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-groupdesign is an attempt <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate the possible effect of apretest. It <strong>in</strong>volves r<strong>and</strong>om assignment of subjects <strong>to</strong>four groups, with two of the groups be<strong>in</strong>g pretested <strong>and</strong>two not. One of the pretested groups <strong>and</strong> one of theunpretested groups is exposed <strong>to</strong> the experimental treatment.All four groups are then posttested. A diagram ofthis design is as follows:The R<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon Four-Group <strong>Design</strong>Treatment group R O X OControl group R O C OTreatment group R X OControl group R C OThe r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-group design comb<strong>in</strong>esthe pretest-posttest control group <strong>and</strong> posttest-onlycontrol group designs. The first two groups represent thepretest-posttest control group design, while the last twogroups represent the posttest-only control group design.Figure 13.6 presents an example of the r<strong>and</strong>omizedSolomon four-group design.The r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-group design providesthe best control of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitythat we have discussed. A weakness, however, is that itrequires a large sample because subjects must be assigned<strong>to</strong> four groups. Furthermore, conduct<strong>in</strong>g a study<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g four groups at the same time requires a considerableamount of energy <strong>and</strong> effort on the part of theresearcher.R<strong>and</strong>om Assignment with Match<strong>in</strong>g. In anattempt <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood that the groups ofsubjects <strong>in</strong> an experiment will be equivalent, pairs of100 teachersr<strong>and</strong>omlyselectedRR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of50 teachers <strong>to</strong>experimental groupRR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of50 teachers <strong>to</strong>control groupOPretest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)OPretest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)XTreatment:Sensitivitytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gworkshopsCTreatment:Workshops thatdo not <strong>in</strong>cludesensitivity tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gOPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)OPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)Figure 13.5 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized Pretest-Posttest Control Group <strong>Design</strong>


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 269RR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of25 teachers <strong>to</strong>experimentalgroup (Group I)OPretest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)XTreatment:Sensitivitytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gworkshopsOPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)100teachersr<strong>and</strong>omlyselectedRR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of25 teachers <strong>to</strong>comparisongroup (Group II)RR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of25 teachers <strong>to</strong>experimentalgroup (Group III)OPretest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)CWorkshopsthat do not<strong>in</strong>cludesensitivitytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gXTreatment:Sensitivitytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gworkshopsOPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)OPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)RR<strong>and</strong>omassignment of25 teachers <strong>to</strong>comparisongroup (Group IV)CWorkshopsthat do not<strong>in</strong>cludesensitivitytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gOPosttest:Faculty moralequestionnaire(Dependentvariable)Figure 13.6 Example of a R<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon Four-Group <strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals may be matched on certa<strong>in</strong> variables. Thechoice of variables on which <strong>to</strong> match is based on previousresearch, theory, <strong>and</strong>/or the experience of the researcher.The members of each matched pair are then assigned<strong>to</strong> the experimental <strong>and</strong> control groups at r<strong>and</strong>om.This adaptation can be made <strong>to</strong> both the posttest-onlycontrol group design <strong>and</strong> the pretest-posttest controlgroup design, although the latter is more common. Diagramsof these designs are provided below.The R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only ControlGroup <strong>Design</strong>, Us<strong>in</strong>g Matched SubjectsTreatment group M r X OControl group M r C OThe R<strong>and</strong>omized Pretest-Posttest ControlGroup <strong>Design</strong>, Us<strong>in</strong>g Matched SubjectsTreatment group M r O X OControl group M r O C OThe symbol M r refers <strong>to</strong> the fact that the members ofeach matched pair are r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> the experimental<strong>and</strong> control groups.Although a pretest of the dependent variable is commonlyused <strong>to</strong> provide scores on which <strong>to</strong> match, a measuremen<strong>to</strong>f any variable that shows a substantial relationship<strong>to</strong> the dependent variable is appropriate. Match<strong>in</strong>gmay be done <strong>in</strong> either or both of two ways: mechanicallyor statistically. Both require a score for each subject oneach variable on which subjects are <strong>to</strong> be matched.


270 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSample of women r<strong>and</strong>omlyselected <strong>and</strong> then matched withregard <strong>to</strong> GPA.Experimentalgroup receivescoach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>mathematics.Effects ofcoach<strong>in</strong>gon GPAarecompared.One member of each pair isr<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> theexperimental group <strong>and</strong> one<strong>to</strong> the comparison group.Control group receivesno coach<strong>in</strong>g.Figure 13.7 A R<strong>and</strong>omized Posttest-Only Control Group <strong>Design</strong>, Us<strong>in</strong>g Matched SubjectsMechanical match<strong>in</strong>g is a process of pair<strong>in</strong>g twopersons whose scores on a particular variable are similar.Two girls, for example, whose mathematics aptitudescores <strong>and</strong> test anxiety scores are similar might bematched on those variables. After the match<strong>in</strong>g is completedfor the entire sample, a check should be made(through the use of frequency polygons) <strong>to</strong> ensure thatthe two groups are <strong>in</strong>deed equivalent on each match<strong>in</strong>gvariable. Unfortunately, two problems limit the usefulnessof mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g. First, it is very difficult <strong>to</strong>match on more than two or three variables—people justdon’t pair up on more than a few characteristics, mak<strong>in</strong>git necessary <strong>to</strong> have a very large <strong>in</strong>itial sample <strong>to</strong> drawfrom. Second, <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> match, it is almost <strong>in</strong>evitablethat some subjects must be elim<strong>in</strong>ated from the studybecause no “matches” for them can be found. Samplesthen are no longer r<strong>and</strong>om even though they may havebeen before match<strong>in</strong>g occurred.As an example of a mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g design withr<strong>and</strong>om assignment, suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>the effects of academic coach<strong>in</strong>g on the grade po<strong>in</strong>taverages (GPA) of low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong> scienceclasses. The researcher r<strong>and</strong>omly selects a sample of60 students from a population of 125 such students <strong>in</strong> alocal elementary school <strong>and</strong> matches them by pairs onGPA, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that she can match 40 of the 60. She thenr<strong>and</strong>omly assigns each subject <strong>in</strong> the result<strong>in</strong>g 20 pairs <strong>to</strong>either the experimental or the control group. Figure 13.7presents a similar example.Statistical match<strong>in</strong>g,* on the other h<strong>and</strong>, does notnecessitate a loss of subjects, nor does it limit the numberof match<strong>in</strong>g variables. Each subject is given a“predicted” score on the dependent variable, based onthe correlation between the dependent variable <strong>and</strong> thevariable (or variables) on which the subjects are be<strong>in</strong>gmatched. The difference between the predicted <strong>and</strong>actual scores for each <strong>in</strong>dividual is then used <strong>to</strong> compareexperimental <strong>and</strong> control groups.*Statistical equat<strong>in</strong>g is a more common term than its synonym,statistical match<strong>in</strong>g. We believe the mean<strong>in</strong>g for the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gstudent is better conveyed by the term match<strong>in</strong>g.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 271When a pretest is used as the match<strong>in</strong>g variable, thedifference between the predicted <strong>and</strong> actual score iscalled a regressed ga<strong>in</strong> score. This score is preferable<strong>to</strong> the more straightforward ga<strong>in</strong> scores (posttest m<strong>in</strong>uspretest score for each <strong>in</strong>dividual) primarily because it ismore reliable. We discuss a similar procedure under partialcorrelation <strong>in</strong> Chapter 15.If mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g is used, one member of eachmatched pair is r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> the experimentalgroup, the other <strong>to</strong> the control group. If statisticalmatch<strong>in</strong>g is used, the sample is divided r<strong>and</strong>omly at theoutset, <strong>and</strong> the statistical adjustments are made after alldata have been collected. Although some researchersadvocate the use of statistical over mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g,statistical match<strong>in</strong>g is not <strong>in</strong>fallible. Its majorweakness is that it assumes that the relationshipbetween the dependent variable <strong>and</strong> each predic<strong>to</strong>r variablecan be properly described by a straight l<strong>in</strong>e ratherthan a curved l<strong>in</strong>e. Whichever procedure is used, theresearcher must (<strong>in</strong> this design) rely on r<strong>and</strong>om assignment<strong>to</strong> equate groups on all other variables related <strong>to</strong>the dependent variable.QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNSQuasi-experimental designs do not <strong>in</strong>clude the use ofr<strong>and</strong>om assignment. <strong>Research</strong>ers who employ these designsrely <strong>in</strong>stead on other techniques <strong>to</strong> control (or atleast reduce) threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. We shall describesome of these techniques as we discuss severalquasi-experimental designs.The Match<strong>in</strong>g-Only <strong>Design</strong>. This design differsfrom r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with match<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>in</strong> the factthat r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is not used. The researcher stillmatches the subjects <strong>in</strong> the experimental <strong>and</strong> controlgroups on certa<strong>in</strong> variables, but he or she has no assurancethat they are equivalent on others. Why? Becauseeven though matched, subjects already are <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tactgroups. This is a serious limitation but often is unavoidablewhen r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is impossible—that is,when <strong>in</strong>tact groups must be used. When several (say, 10or more) groups are available for a method study <strong>and</strong> thegroups can be r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> different treatments,this design offers an alternative <strong>to</strong> r<strong>and</strong>om assignment ofsubjects. After the groups have been r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned<strong>to</strong> the different treatments, the <strong>in</strong>dividuals receiv<strong>in</strong>g onetreatment are matched with <strong>in</strong>dividuals receiv<strong>in</strong>g theother treatments. The design shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.7 is stillpreferred, however.It should be emphasized that match<strong>in</strong>g (whether mechanicalor statistical) is never a substitute for r<strong>and</strong>omassignment. Furthermore, the correlation between thematch<strong>in</strong>g variable(s) <strong>and</strong> the dependent variable shouldbe fairly substantial. (We suggest at least .40.) Realizealso that unless it is used <strong>in</strong> conjunction with r<strong>and</strong>omassignment, match<strong>in</strong>g controls only for the variable(s)be<strong>in</strong>g matched. Diagrams of each of the match<strong>in</strong>g-onlycontrol group designs follow.The Match<strong>in</strong>g-Only Posttest-OnlyControl Group <strong>Design</strong>Treatment group M X OControl group M C OThe Match<strong>in</strong>g-Only Pretest-PosttestControl Group <strong>Design</strong>Treatment group M O X OControl group M O C OThe M <strong>in</strong> this design means that the subjects <strong>in</strong> eachgroup have been matched (on certa<strong>in</strong> variables) but notr<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> the groups.Counterbalanced <strong>Design</strong>s. Counterbalanceddesigns represent another technique for equat<strong>in</strong>g experimental<strong>and</strong> comparison groups. In this design, each groupis exposed <strong>to</strong> all treatments, however many there are, but<strong>in</strong> a different order. Any number of treatments may be <strong>in</strong>volved.An example of a diagram for a counterbalanceddesign <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g three treatments is as follows:A Three-Treatment Counterbalanced <strong>Design</strong>Group I X 1 O X 2 O X 3 OGroup II X 2 O X 3 O X 1 OGroup III X 3 O X 1 O X 2 OThis arrangement <strong>in</strong>volves three groups. Group I receivestreatment 1 <strong>and</strong> is posttested, then receives treatment2 <strong>and</strong> is posttested, <strong>and</strong> last receives treatment 3 <strong>and</strong>is posttested. Group II receives treatment 2 first, thentreatment 3, <strong>and</strong> then treatment 1, be<strong>in</strong>g posttested aftereach treatment. Group III receives treatment 3 first, thentreatment1,followedbytreatment2,alsobe<strong>in</strong>gposttestedafter each treatment. The order <strong>in</strong> which the groups receivethe treatments should be determ<strong>in</strong>ed r<strong>and</strong>omly.<strong>How</strong> do researchers determ<strong>in</strong>e the effectiveness of thevarious treatments? Simply by compar<strong>in</strong>g the averagescores for all groups on the posttest for each treatment. In


272 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eGroup IGroup IIStudy 1Weeks Weeks1--45--8Method X = 12 Method Y = 8Method Y = 8 Method X = 12Study 2Weeks1--4Method X = 10Method Y = 10Overall Means: Method X = 12; Method Y = 8 Method X = 12; Method Y = 8Figure 13.8 Results (Means) from a Study Us<strong>in</strong>g a Counterbalanced <strong>Design</strong>Weeks5--8Method Y = 6Method X = 14other words, the averaged posttest score for all groups fortreatment 1 can be compared with the averaged posttestscore for all groups for treatment 2, <strong>and</strong> so on, for howevermany treatments there are.This design controls well for the subject characteristicsthreat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity but is particularly vulnerable<strong>to</strong> multiple-treatment <strong>in</strong>terference—that is, performancedur<strong>in</strong>g a particular treatment may be affected byone or more of the previous treatments. Consequently,the results of any study <strong>in</strong> which the researcher has useda counterbalanced design must be exam<strong>in</strong>ed carefully.Consider the two sets of hypothetical data shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 13.8.The <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>in</strong> study 1 is clear: Method X is superiorfor both groups regardless of sequence <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> thesame degree. The <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>in</strong> study 2, however, ismuch more complex. Overall, method X appears superior,<strong>and</strong> by the same amount as <strong>in</strong> study 1. In both studies,the overall mean for X is 12, while for Y it is 8. Instudy 2, however, it appears that the difference betweenX <strong>and</strong> Y depends on previous exposure <strong>to</strong> the othermethod. Group I performed much worse on method Ywhen it was exposed <strong>to</strong> it follow<strong>in</strong>g X, <strong>and</strong> group II performedmuch better on X when it was exposed <strong>to</strong> it aftermethod Y. When either X or Y was given first <strong>in</strong> the sequence,there was no difference <strong>in</strong> performance. It is notclear that method X is superior <strong>in</strong> all conditions <strong>in</strong> study2, whereas this is quite clear <strong>in</strong> study 1.Time-Series <strong>Design</strong>s. The typical pre- <strong>and</strong>posttest designs exam<strong>in</strong>ed up <strong>to</strong> now <strong>in</strong>volve observationsor measurements taken immediately before <strong>and</strong>after treatment. A time-series design, however, <strong>in</strong>volvesrepeated measurements or observations over aperiod of time both before <strong>and</strong> after treatment. It is reallyan elaboration of the one-group pretest-posttest designpresented <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.2. An extensive amount ofdata is collected on a s<strong>in</strong>gle group. If the group scoresessentially the same on the pretests <strong>and</strong> then considerablyimproves on the posttests, the researcher has moreconfidence that the treatment is caus<strong>in</strong>g the improvementthan if just one pretest <strong>and</strong> one posttest weregiven. An example might be a teacher who gives aweekly test <strong>to</strong> her class for several weeks before giv<strong>in</strong>gthem a new textbook <strong>to</strong> use, <strong>and</strong> then moni<strong>to</strong>rs how theyscore on a number of weekly tests after they have usedthe text. A diagram of the basic time-series design is asfollows:A Basic Time-Series <strong>Design</strong>O 1 O 2 O 3 O 4 O 5 X O 6 O 7 O 8 O 9 O 10The threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity that endanger use ofthis design <strong>in</strong>clude his<strong>to</strong>ry (someth<strong>in</strong>g could happen betweenthe last pretest <strong>and</strong> the first posttest), <strong>in</strong>strumentation(if, for some reason, the test be<strong>in</strong>g used ischanged at any time dur<strong>in</strong>g the study), <strong>and</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g(due <strong>to</strong> a practice effect). The possibility of a pretesttreatment<strong>in</strong>teraction is also <strong>in</strong>creased with the use ofseveral pretests.The effectiveness of the treatment <strong>in</strong> a time-series designis basically determ<strong>in</strong>ed by analyz<strong>in</strong>g the pattern oftest scores that results from the several tests. Figure 13.9illustrates several possible outcome patterns that mightresult from the <strong>in</strong>troduction of an experimental variable(X). The vertical l<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>dicates the po<strong>in</strong>t at which theexperimental treatment is <strong>in</strong>troduced. In this figure, thechange between time periods 5 <strong>and</strong> 6 gives the samek<strong>in</strong>d of data that would be obta<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g a one-grouppretest-posttest design. The collection of additional databefore <strong>and</strong> after the <strong>in</strong>troduction of the treatment,however, shows how mislead<strong>in</strong>g a one-group pretestposttestdesign can be. In (A), the improvement isshown <strong>to</strong> be no more than that which occurs fromone data collection period <strong>to</strong> another—regardless ofmethod. You will notice that performance does improve


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 273O 1 O 2 O 3 O 4 O 5 O 6 O 7 O 8 O 9 O 10XStudy(D)Study(C)Study(B)Study(A)Figure 13.9 Possible Outcome Patterns <strong>in</strong> a Time-Series<strong>Design</strong>from time <strong>to</strong> time, but no trend or overall <strong>in</strong>crease isapparent. In (B), the ga<strong>in</strong> from periods 5 <strong>to</strong> 6 appears <strong>to</strong>be part of a trend already apparent before the treatmentwas begun (quite possibly an example of maturation). In(D) the higher score <strong>in</strong> period 6 is only temporary, asperformance soon approaches what it was before thetreatment was <strong>in</strong>troduced (suggest<strong>in</strong>g an extraneousevent of transient impact). Only <strong>in</strong> (C) do we have evidenceof a consistent effect of the treatment.The time-series design is a strong design, althoughit is vulnerable <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry (an extraneous event couldoccur after period 5) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumentation (ow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> theseveral test adm<strong>in</strong>istrations at different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time).The extensive amount of data collection required, <strong>in</strong>fact, is a likely reason why this design is <strong>in</strong>frequentlyused <strong>in</strong> educational research. In many studies, especially<strong>in</strong> schools, it simply is not feasible <strong>to</strong> give the same<strong>in</strong>strument eight <strong>to</strong> ten times. Even when it is possible,serious questions are raised concern<strong>in</strong>g the validity of <strong>in</strong>strument<strong>in</strong>terpretation with so many adm<strong>in</strong>istrations.An exception is the use of unobtrusive devices that canbe applied over many occasions, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>terpretationsbased on them should rema<strong>in</strong> valid.FACTORIAL DESIGNSFac<strong>to</strong>rial designs extend the number of relationshipsthat may be exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> an experimental study. Theyare essentially modifications of either the posttest-onlycontrol group or pretest-posttest control group designs(with or without r<strong>and</strong>om assignment), which permit the<strong>in</strong>vestigation of additional <strong>in</strong>dependent variables. Anothervalue of a fac<strong>to</strong>rial design is that it allows a researcher<strong>to</strong> study the <strong>in</strong>teraction of an <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable with one or more other variables, sometimescalled modera<strong>to</strong>r variables. Modera<strong>to</strong>r variables maybe either treatment variables or subject characteristicvariables. A diagram of a fac<strong>to</strong>rial design is as follows:Fac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong>Treatment R O X Y 1 OControl R O C Y 1 OTreatment R O X Y 2 OControl R O C Y 2 OThis design is a modification of the pretest-posttestcontrol group design. It <strong>in</strong>volves one treatment <strong>and</strong> onecontrol group, <strong>and</strong> a modera<strong>to</strong>r variable hav<strong>in</strong>g two levels(Y 1 <strong>and</strong> Y 2 ). In this example, two groups would receivethe treatment (X) <strong>and</strong> two would not (C). Thegroups receiv<strong>in</strong>g the treatment would differ on Y, however,as would the two groups not receiv<strong>in</strong>g the treatment.Because each variable, or fac<strong>to</strong>r, has two levels,the above design is called a 2 by 2 fac<strong>to</strong>rial design. Thisdesign can also be illustrated as follows:Alternative Illustration of the Above ExampleY 1Y 2XCA variation of this design uses two or more differenttreatment groups <strong>and</strong> no control groups. Consider theexample we have used before of a researcher compar<strong>in</strong>gthe effectiveness of <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> lecture methods of<strong>in</strong>struction on achievement <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry. The <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable <strong>in</strong> this case (method of <strong>in</strong>struction) has twolevels—<strong>in</strong>quiry (X 1 ) <strong>and</strong> lecture (X 2 ). Now imag<strong>in</strong>e theresearcher wants <strong>to</strong> see whether achievement is also<strong>in</strong>fluenced by class size. In that case, Y 1 might representsmall classes <strong>and</strong> Y 2 might represent large classes.As we suggest above, it is possible us<strong>in</strong>g a fac<strong>to</strong>rialdesign <strong>to</strong> assess not only the separate effect of each <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable but also their jo<strong>in</strong>t effect. In otherwords, the researcher is able <strong>to</strong> see how one of the variablesmight moderate the other (hence the reason forcall<strong>in</strong>g these variables modera<strong>to</strong>r variables).


274 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eClass sizeSmall (Y 1 )Large (Y 2 )MethodInquiry (X 1 ) Lecture (X 2 )Figure 13.10 Us<strong>in</strong>g a Fac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong> <strong>to</strong> Study Effectsof Method <strong>and</strong> Class Size on AchievementLet us cont<strong>in</strong>ue with the example of the researcherwho wished <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effects of method of <strong>in</strong>struction<strong>and</strong> class size on achievement <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry. Figure13.10 illustrates how various comb<strong>in</strong>ations of thesevariables could be studied <strong>in</strong> a fac<strong>to</strong>rial design.Fac<strong>to</strong>rial designs, therefore, are an efficient way <strong>to</strong>study several relationships with one set of data. Let usemphasize aga<strong>in</strong>, however, that their greatest virtue lies<strong>in</strong> the fact that they enable a researcher <strong>to</strong> study <strong>in</strong>teractionsbetween variables.Figure 13.11, for example, illustrates two possibleoutcomes for the 2 by 2 fac<strong>to</strong>rial design shown <strong>in</strong> Figure13.10. The scores for each group on the posttest(a 50-item quiz on American his<strong>to</strong>ry) are shown <strong>in</strong>the boxes (usually called cells) correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> eachcomb<strong>in</strong>ation of method <strong>and</strong> class size.In study (a) <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.11, the <strong>in</strong>quiry method wasshown <strong>to</strong> be superior <strong>in</strong> both small <strong>and</strong> large classes,<strong>and</strong> small classes were superior <strong>to</strong> large classes for bothmethods. Hence no <strong>in</strong>teraction effect is present. In study(b), students did better <strong>in</strong> small than <strong>in</strong> large classeswith both methods; however, students <strong>in</strong> small classesdid better when they were taught by the <strong>in</strong>quiry method,but students <strong>in</strong> large classes did better when they weretaught by the lecture method. Thus, even though studentsdid better <strong>in</strong> small than <strong>in</strong> large classes <strong>in</strong> general,how well they did depended on the teach<strong>in</strong>g method. Asa result, the researcher cannot say that either methodwas always better; it depended on the size of the class <strong>in</strong>which students were taught. There was an <strong>in</strong>teraction,50(a) No <strong>in</strong>teraction between class size <strong>and</strong> methodMethodClass size Inquiry (X 1 ) Lecture (X 2 )Small (Y 1 )4638Mean42Scores454035InquiryLectureLarge (Y 2 )Mean =404332353630Small LargeClass size50(b) Interaction between class size <strong>and</strong> methodMethodClass sizeSmall (Y 1 )Large (Y 2 )Inquiry (X 1 )4832Lecture (X 2 )4238Mean4535Mean =4040Figure 13.11 Illustration of Interaction <strong>and</strong> No Interaction <strong>in</strong> a 2 by 2 Fac<strong>to</strong>rial <strong>Design</strong>Scores45Inquiry4035 Lecture30Small LargeClass size


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 275<strong>in</strong> other words, between class size <strong>and</strong> method, <strong>and</strong> this <strong>in</strong>turn affected achievement.Suppose a fac<strong>to</strong>rial design was not used <strong>in</strong> study (b). Ifthe researcher simply compared the effect of the twomethods, without tak<strong>in</strong>g class size <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account, he wouldhave concluded that there was no difference <strong>in</strong> their effec<strong>to</strong>n achievement (notice that the means of both groups 40). The use of a fac<strong>to</strong>rial design enables us <strong>to</strong> see that theeffectiveness of the method, <strong>in</strong> this case, depended on thesize of the class <strong>in</strong> which it was used. It appears that an <strong>in</strong>teractionexisted between method <strong>and</strong> class size. Figure13.12 illustrates an example of an <strong>in</strong>teraction effect.A fac<strong>to</strong>rial design <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g four levels of the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g a modification of theposttest-only control group design was employed byTuckman. 7 In this design, the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable wastype of <strong>in</strong>struction, <strong>and</strong> the modera<strong>to</strong>r was amount ofmotivation. It is a 4 by 2 fac<strong>to</strong>rial design (Figure 13.13).Many additional variations are also possible, such as 3by 3, 4 by 3, <strong>and</strong> 3 by 2 by 3 designs. Fac<strong>to</strong>rial designscan be used <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate more than two variables, althoughrarely are more than three variables studied <strong>in</strong>one design.Figure 13.12 AnInteraction EffectA little dr<strong>in</strong>k can be fun . . . as can a drive around <strong>to</strong>wn . . .but watch outfor the <strong>in</strong>teraction.RRRRRRRRX 1 Y 1X 2 Y 1X 3 Y 1X 4 Y 1X 1 Y 2X 2X 3X 4Y 2Y 2Y 2OOOOOOOOX 1X 2X 3Y 1Treatments (X)X 4YComputer-assisted <strong>in</strong>structionProgrammed textTelevised lectureLecture-discussionTreatmentsModera<strong>to</strong>r (Y)X 1 X 2 X 3 X 4High motivationY 2 Low motivation1Y 2Figure 13.13 Exampleof a 4 by 2 Fac<strong>to</strong>rial<strong>Design</strong>


276 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eControl of Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity: A SummaryTable 13.1 presents our evaluation of the effectiveness ofeach of the preced<strong>in</strong>g designs <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g the threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity that we discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9. Youshould remember that these assessments reflect our judgment;not all researchers would necessarily agree. Wehave assigned two pluses () <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate a strong control(the threat is unlikely <strong>to</strong> occur); one plus () <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatesome control (the threat might occur); a m<strong>in</strong>us () <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicatea weak control (the threat is likely <strong>to</strong> occur); <strong>and</strong> aquestion mark (?) <strong>to</strong> those threats whose likelihood, ow<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> the nature of the study, we cannot determ<strong>in</strong>e.You will notice that these designs are most effective <strong>in</strong>controll<strong>in</strong>g the threats of subject characteristics, mortality,his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, <strong>and</strong> regression. Note that mortality iscontrolled <strong>in</strong> several designs because any subject lost islost <strong>to</strong> both the experimental <strong>and</strong> control methods, thusTABLE 13.1Effectiveness of Experimental <strong>Design</strong>s <strong>in</strong> Controll<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityThreatDataAtti-Collec- DatatudeSubject Instru- <strong>to</strong>r Col- of Imple-Charac- Mor- Loca- ment Charac- lec<strong>to</strong>rMatura- Sub- Regres- men-<strong>Design</strong> teristics tality tion Decay teristics Bias Test<strong>in</strong>g His<strong>to</strong>ry tion jects sion tationOne-shot casestudy (NA) (NA) One grouppretest-posttest ? Static-groupcomparison ? R<strong>and</strong>omizedposttest-onlycontrol group R<strong>and</strong>omizedpretest-posttestcontrol group R<strong>and</strong>omizedSolomon fourgroup R<strong>and</strong>omizedposttest-onlycontrol groupwith matchedsubjects Match<strong>in</strong>g-onlypretest-posttestcontrol group Counterbalanced Time-series Fac<strong>to</strong>rial withr<strong>and</strong>omization Fac<strong>to</strong>rial withoutr<strong>and</strong>omization ? ? ? Key: () strong control, threat unlikely <strong>to</strong> occur; () some control, threat may possibly occur; () weak control, threat likely <strong>to</strong>occur; (?) can’t determ<strong>in</strong>e; (NA) threat does not apply.


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHreport, published <strong>in</strong> the New Engl<strong>and</strong> Journal of Medic<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>May 2001, showed that “placebos offer no significant advantageover ‘no treatment’ for dozens of conditions rang<strong>in</strong>g fromDo Placebos Work?colds <strong>and</strong> seasickness <strong>to</strong> hypertension <strong>and</strong>Alzheimer’s disease.The placebo effect—the expectation that some patients will (The exception was pa<strong>in</strong> relief, which sugar pills seem <strong>to</strong> br<strong>in</strong>gshow improvement if they are given any k<strong>in</strong>d of treatment <strong>to</strong> about 15 percent of patients.)”* The researchers speculatedat all, even a sugar pill—has long been acknowledged by that one explanation of the placebo effect may simply have beenphysicians <strong>and</strong> others <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials. But does this an unconscious desire by patients <strong>to</strong> please their doc<strong>to</strong>rs.effect really exist?What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Do some patients try (unconsciously)Two researchers <strong>in</strong> Denmark recently suggested it often <strong>to</strong> please their doc<strong>to</strong>rs?does not. They reviewed 114 cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials <strong>in</strong> which patientswere given real medic<strong>in</strong>e, a placebo, or no treatment at all.Their *Reported <strong>in</strong> Time, June 4, 2001, p. 65.<strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g no advantage <strong>to</strong> either. A location threat isa m<strong>in</strong>or problem <strong>in</strong> the time-series design because thelocation where the treatment is adm<strong>in</strong>istered is usuallyconstant throughout the study; the same is true for datacollec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics, although such characteristicsmay be a problem <strong>in</strong> other designs if different collec<strong>to</strong>rsare used for different methods. This is usually easy <strong>to</strong>control, however. Unfortunately, time-series designs dosuffer from a strong likelihood of <strong>in</strong>strument decay <strong>and</strong>data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, s<strong>in</strong>ce data (by means of observations)must be collected over many trials, <strong>and</strong> the data collec<strong>to</strong>rcan hardly be kept <strong>in</strong> the dark as <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tent of the study.Unconscious bias on the part of data collec<strong>to</strong>rs is notcontrolled by any of these designs, nor is an implementationeffect. Either implementers or data collec<strong>to</strong>rs can,un<strong>in</strong>tentionally, dis<strong>to</strong>rt the results of a study. The datacollec<strong>to</strong>r should be kept ignorant as <strong>to</strong> who receivedwhich treatment, if this is feasible. It should be verifiedthat the treatment is adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>and</strong> the data collectedas the researcher <strong>in</strong>tended.As you can see <strong>in</strong> Table 13.1, a test<strong>in</strong>g threat may bepresent <strong>in</strong> many of the designs, although its magnitudedepends on the nature <strong>and</strong> frequency of the <strong>in</strong>strumentation<strong>in</strong>volved. It can occur only when subjects respond<strong>to</strong> an <strong>in</strong>strument on more than one occasion.The attitud<strong>in</strong>al (or demoralization) effect is best controlledby the counterbalanced design s<strong>in</strong>ce each subjectreceives both (or all) special treatments. In the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdesigns, it can be controlled by provid<strong>in</strong>g another“special” experience dur<strong>in</strong>g the alternative treatment.Special mention should be made of the double-bl<strong>in</strong>d(see page 260) experiment. Such studies are common <strong>in</strong>medic<strong>in</strong>e but hard <strong>to</strong> arrange <strong>in</strong> education. The key elementis that neither the subjects nor the researcher knowthe identity of those receiv<strong>in</strong>g each treatment. This ismost easily accomplished <strong>in</strong> medical studies by meansof a placebo (sometimes a sugar pill) that is <strong>in</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guishablefrom the actual medic<strong>in</strong>e.Regression is not likely <strong>to</strong> be a problem except <strong>in</strong> theone-group pretest-posttest design, s<strong>in</strong>ce it should occurequally <strong>in</strong> experimental <strong>and</strong> control conditions if it occursat all. It could, however, possibly occur <strong>in</strong> a staticgrouppretest-posttest control group design, if there arelarge <strong>in</strong>itial differences between the two groups.Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the Likelihoodof a Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> Experimental StudiesAn important consideration <strong>in</strong> plann<strong>in</strong>g an experimentalstudy or <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the results of a reported study is thelikelihood of threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. As we haveshown, a number of possible threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitymay exist. The question that a researcher must ask is: <strong>How</strong>likely is it that any particular threat exists <strong>in</strong> this study?To aid <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g this likelihood, we suggest thefollow<strong>in</strong>g procedures.Step 1: Ask: What specific fac<strong>to</strong>rs either are known<strong>to</strong> affect the dependent variable or may logicallybe expected <strong>to</strong> affect this variable? (Note that researchersneed not be concerned with fac<strong>to</strong>rs unrelated<strong>to</strong> what they are study<strong>in</strong>g.)Step 2: Ask: What is the likelihood of the comparisongroups differ<strong>in</strong>g on each of these fac<strong>to</strong>rs? (Adifference between groups cannot be expla<strong>in</strong>edaway by a fac<strong>to</strong>r that is the same for all groups.)Step 3: <strong>Evaluate</strong> the threats on the basis of howlikely they are <strong>to</strong> have an effect, <strong>and</strong> plan <strong>to</strong> controlfor them. If a given threat cannot be controlled,acknowledge this.277


278 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe importance of step 2 is illustrated <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.14.In each diagram, the thermometers depict the performanceof subjects receiv<strong>in</strong>g methodAcompared <strong>to</strong> thosereceiv<strong>in</strong>g method B. In diagram (a), subjects receiv<strong>in</strong>gmethod A performed higher on the posttest but also performedhigher on the pretest; thus, the difference <strong>in</strong>pretest achievement accounts for the difference on theposttest. In diagram (b), subjects receiv<strong>in</strong>g method Aperformed higher on the posttest but did not performhigher on the pretest; thus, the posttest results cannotbe expla<strong>in</strong>ed by, or attributed <strong>to</strong>, different achievementlevels prior <strong>to</strong> receiv<strong>in</strong>g the methods.Let us consider an example <strong>to</strong> illustrate how thesedifferent steps might be employed. Suppose a researcherwishes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effects of two different teach<strong>in</strong>gmethods (for example, lecture versus <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>in</strong>struction)on critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability of students (as measuredby scores on a critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test). The researcherplans <strong>to</strong> compare two groups of eleventh-graders, onegroup be<strong>in</strong>g taught by an <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r who uses the lecturemethod, the other group be<strong>in</strong>g taught by an <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rwho uses the <strong>in</strong>quiry method. Assume that <strong>in</strong>tact classeswill be used rather than r<strong>and</strong>om assignment <strong>to</strong> groups.Several of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity discussed <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9 are considered <strong>and</strong> evaluated us<strong>in</strong>g the stepsjust presented. We would argue that this is the k<strong>in</strong>d ofth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g researchers should engage <strong>in</strong> when plann<strong>in</strong>g aresearch project.Subject Characteristics. Although many possiblesubject characteristics might affect critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gability, we identify only two here—(1) <strong>in</strong>itial criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability <strong>and</strong> (2) gender.1. Critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability. Step 1: Posttreatment criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability of students <strong>in</strong> the two groups isalmost certa<strong>in</strong>ly related <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gability. Step 2: Groups may well differ unless r<strong>and</strong>omlyassigned or matched. Step 3: Likelihood ofhav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.Figure 13.14Guidel<strong>in</strong>es for H<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>gInternal Validity <strong>in</strong>Comparison GroupStudies(a)ThreatpresentMethod A Method B Method A Method BPretestachievementPosttestachievementMethod A Method B Method A Method B(b)ThreatcontrolledPretestachievementPosttestachievement


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 2792. Gender. Step 1: Posttreatment critical ability maybe related <strong>to</strong> gender. Step 2: If groups differ significantly<strong>in</strong> proportions of each gender, threat exists.Although possible, this is unlikely. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.Mortality. Step 1: Mortality is likely <strong>to</strong> affect posttreatmentscores on any measure of critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong>ce those subjects who drop out or are otherwise lostwould likely have lower scores. Step 2: Groups probablywould not differ <strong>in</strong> numbers lost, but this should beverified. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unlesscontrolled: moderate.Location. Step 1: If location of implementation oftreatment <strong>and</strong>/or of data collection differs for the twogroups, this could affect posttreatment scores on the criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test. Posttreatment scores would be expected<strong>to</strong> be affected by such resources as class size,availability of read<strong>in</strong>g materials, films, <strong>and</strong> so forth.Step 2: This threat may differ for groups unless controlledfor by st<strong>and</strong>ardiz<strong>in</strong>g locations for implementation <strong>and</strong>data collection. The classrooms us<strong>in</strong>g each method maydiffer systematically unless steps are taken <strong>to</strong> ensure thatresources are comparable. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: moderate <strong>to</strong> high.Instrumentation.1. Instrument decay. Step 1: Instrument decay may affectany outcome. Step 2: Instrument decay could differfor groups. This should not be a major problem,provided all <strong>in</strong>struments used are carefully exam<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>and</strong> any alterations found are corrected. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.2. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics. Step 1: Data collec<strong>to</strong>rcharacteristics might affect scores on criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test. Step 2: This threat might differ forgroups unless controlled by us<strong>in</strong>g the same data collec<strong>to</strong>r(s)for all groups. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: moderate.3. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias. Step 1: Bias could certa<strong>in</strong>ly affectscores on critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test. Step 2: Thisthreat might differ for groups unless controlled bytra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g implementers <strong>in</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the <strong>in</strong>strument<strong>and</strong>/or keep<strong>in</strong>g them ignorant as <strong>to</strong> whichtreatment group is be<strong>in</strong>g tested. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.Test<strong>in</strong>g. Step 1: Pretest<strong>in</strong>g, if used, might well affectposttest scores on critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g test. Step 2: Presumablythe pretest would affect both groups equally, however,<strong>and</strong> would not be likely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>teract with method,s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs us<strong>in</strong>g each method are teach<strong>in</strong>g criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skills. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effectunless controlled: low.His<strong>to</strong>ry. Step 1: Extraneous events that might affectcritical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skills are difficult <strong>to</strong> conjecture, butthey might <strong>in</strong>clude such th<strong>in</strong>gs as a special TV series onth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, attendance at a district workshop on criticalth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g by some students, or participation <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong>extracurricular activities (e.g., debates) that occur dur<strong>in</strong>gthe course of the study. Step 2: In most cases, theseevents would likely affect both groups equally <strong>and</strong>hence are not likely <strong>to</strong> constitute a threat. Such eventsshould be noted <strong>and</strong> their impact on each group assessed<strong>to</strong> the degree possible. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g aneffect unless controlled: low.Maturation. Step 1: Maturation could affect outcomescores s<strong>in</strong>ce critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g is presumably related<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual growth. Step 2: Presum<strong>in</strong>g that the<strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs teach each method over the same time period,maturation should not be a threat. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.Attitude of Subjects. Step 1: Subjects’ attitudescould affect posttest scores. Step 2: If the members ofeither group perceive that they are receiv<strong>in</strong>g any sort of“special attention,” this could be a threat. The extent <strong>to</strong>which either treatment is “novel” should be evaluated.Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled:low <strong>to</strong> moderate.Regression. Step 1: Regression is unlikely <strong>to</strong> affectposttest scores unless subjects are selected on the basisof extreme scores. Step 2: This threat is unlikely <strong>to</strong>affect groups differently, although it could do so. Step 3:Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.Implementation. Step 1: Instruc<strong>to</strong>r characteristicsare likely <strong>to</strong> affect posttreatment scores. Step 2: Becausedifferent <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs teach the methods, they may welldiffer. This could be controlled by hav<strong>in</strong>g several <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rsfor each method, by hav<strong>in</strong>g each <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r teachboth methods, or by moni<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.The trick, then, <strong>to</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validityis, first, <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of different variables (conditions,


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHSignificant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> Experimental <strong>Research</strong>In our op<strong>in</strong>ion, some of the most important research <strong>in</strong> socialpsychology, with obvious implications for education, hasbeen that on the effects of cooperative social <strong>in</strong>teraction onnegative attitudes, or the tendency of people <strong>to</strong> dislike others.A series of experimental studies begun <strong>in</strong> the 1940s led <strong>to</strong> thegeneralization that lik<strong>in</strong>g for group members, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g thoseof different backgrounds <strong>and</strong> ethnicity, is <strong>in</strong>creased by cooperativeactivities that lead <strong>to</strong> a successful outcome.* A recent applicationof this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is the “jigsaw technique,” which requireseach member of a group <strong>to</strong> teach other members asection of material <strong>to</strong> be learned.† Experimental studies generallysupport the effectiveness of this procedure.*W. G. Stephan (1985). Intergroup relations. In G. L<strong>in</strong>dzey <strong>and</strong> E.Aronson (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of social psychology. New York: R<strong>and</strong>omHouse.†E. Aronson, C. Stephan, J. Sikes, N. Blaney, <strong>and</strong> M. Snapp (1978).The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills: Sage.subject characteristics, <strong>and</strong> so on) that might affect theoutcome variable of the study <strong>and</strong>, second, <strong>to</strong> decide,based on evidence <strong>and</strong>/or experience, whether theseth<strong>in</strong>gs would affect the comparison groups differently.If so, the <strong>in</strong>fluence of these fac<strong>to</strong>rs may provide an alternativeexplanation for the results. If this seems likely,a threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of the study may <strong>in</strong>deedbe present <strong>and</strong> needs <strong>to</strong> be m<strong>in</strong>imized or elim<strong>in</strong>ated.It should then be discussed <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al report on theresearch project.Control of ExperimentalTreatmentsThe designs discussed <strong>in</strong> this chapter are all <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong>improve the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of an experimental study.As you have seen, each has its advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages,<strong>and</strong> each provides a way of h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g somethreats but not others.Another issue, however, cuts across all designs.While it has been <strong>to</strong>uched on <strong>in</strong> earlier sections, particularly<strong>in</strong> connection with location <strong>and</strong> implementationthreats, it deserves more attention than it cus<strong>to</strong>marilyreceives. The issue is that of researcher control over theexperimental treatment(s). Of course, an essentialrequirement of a well-conducted experiment is thatresearchers have control over the treatment—that is,they control the what, who, when, <strong>and</strong> how of it. A clearexample of researcher control is the test<strong>in</strong>g of a newdrug; clearly, the drug is the treatment <strong>and</strong> the researchercan control who adm<strong>in</strong>isters it, under whatconditions, when it is given, <strong>to</strong> whom, <strong>and</strong> how much.Unfortunately, researchers seldom have this degree ofcontrol <strong>in</strong> educational research.In the ideal situation, a researcher can specify preciselythe <strong>in</strong>gredients of the treatment; <strong>in</strong> actual practice,many treatments or methods are <strong>to</strong>o complex <strong>to</strong> describeprecisely. Consider the example we have previouslygiven of a study compar<strong>in</strong>g the effectiveness of <strong>in</strong>quiry<strong>and</strong> lecture methods of <strong>in</strong>struction. What, exactly, is the<strong>in</strong>dividual who implements each method <strong>to</strong> do? <strong>Research</strong>ersmay differ greatly <strong>in</strong> their answers <strong>to</strong> this question.Ambiguity <strong>in</strong> specify<strong>in</strong>g exactly what the implementerof the treatment is <strong>to</strong> do leads <strong>to</strong> major problems<strong>in</strong> implementation. <strong>How</strong> are researchers <strong>to</strong> tra<strong>in</strong> teachers<strong>to</strong> implement the methods <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a study if theycan’t specify the essential characteristics of those methods?Even suppos<strong>in</strong>g that adequate specification can beachieved <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g methods developed, how can researchersbe sure the methods are implemented correctly?These problems must be faced by any researcherus<strong>in</strong>g any of the designs we have discussed.A consideration of this issue frequently leads <strong>to</strong> consideration(<strong>and</strong> assessment) of possible trade-offs. Thegreatest control is likely <strong>to</strong> occur when the researcher isthe one implement<strong>in</strong>g the treatment; this, however, alsoprovides the greatest opportunity for an implementationthreat <strong>to</strong> occur. The more the researcher diffuses implementationby add<strong>in</strong>g other implementers <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>teres<strong>to</strong>f reduc<strong>in</strong>g threats, however, the more he or she risksdis<strong>to</strong>rtion or dilution of the treatment. The extreme caseis the use of exist<strong>in</strong>g treatment groups—that is, groupslocated by the researcher that already are receiv<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong>treatments. Most authors refer <strong>to</strong> these as causalcomparativeor ex post fac<strong>to</strong> studies (see Chapter 16),<strong>and</strong> do not consider them <strong>to</strong> fall under the category of280


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 281experimental research. In such studies, the researchermust locate groups receiv<strong>in</strong>g the specified treatment(s)<strong>and</strong> then use a match<strong>in</strong>g-only design or, if sufficientlead time exists before implementation of the treatment,a time-series design. We are not persuaded that suchstudies, if treatments are carefully identified, are necessarily<strong>in</strong>ferior with respect <strong>to</strong> cause-effect conclusionscompared with studies <strong>in</strong> which treatments are assigned<strong>to</strong> teachers (or others) by the researcher. Both areequally open <strong>to</strong> most of the threats we have discussed.The exist<strong>in</strong>g groups are more susceptible <strong>to</strong> subjectcharacteristics, location, <strong>and</strong> regression threats than trueexperiments, but not necessarily more so than quasiexperiments.One would expect fewer problems with anattitud<strong>in</strong>al effect, s<strong>in</strong>ce exist<strong>in</strong>g practice is not altered.Greater his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> maturation threats exist because theresearcher would have less control. Implementation isdifficult <strong>to</strong> assess. Teachers who are already implement<strong>in</strong>ga new method may be enthusiastic if they <strong>in</strong>itiallychose the method, but they also may be better teachers.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, teachers assigned <strong>to</strong> a method that isnew <strong>to</strong> them may be either enthusiastic or resentful. Weconclude that both types of study are legitimate.An Example of Experimental<strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of experimental research. Along with a repr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>f the actual study itself, we critique the study, identifyits strengths, <strong>and</strong> discuss areas we th<strong>in</strong>k could be improved.We also do this at the end of Chapters 14 <strong>to</strong> 17<strong>and</strong> 19 <strong>to</strong> 24, <strong>in</strong> each case analyz<strong>in</strong>g the type of studydiscussed <strong>in</strong> the chapter. In select<strong>in</strong>g the studies for review,we used the follow<strong>in</strong>g criteria:The study had <strong>to</strong> exemplify typical, but not outst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g,methodology <strong>and</strong> permit constructive criticism.The study had <strong>to</strong> have enough <strong>in</strong>terest value <strong>to</strong> holdthe attention of students, even though specific professional<strong>in</strong>terests may not be directly addressed.The study had <strong>to</strong> be concisely reported.In <strong>to</strong>tal, these studies represent the diversity of special<strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> the field of education.In critiqu<strong>in</strong>g each of these studies, we used a seriesof categories <strong>and</strong> questions that should, by now, befamiliar <strong>to</strong> you. They are:Purpose/justification: Is it logical? Is it conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g?Is it sufficient? Do the authors show how theresults of the study have important implicationsfor theory, practice, or both? Are assumptionsmade explicit?Def<strong>in</strong>itions: Are major terms clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed? If not,are they clear <strong>in</strong> context?Prior research: Has previous work on the <strong>to</strong>pic beencovered adequately? Is it clearly connected <strong>to</strong> thepresent study?Hypotheses: Are they stated? implied? appropriatefor the study?Sample: What type of sample is used? Is it a r<strong>and</strong>omsample? If not, is it adequately described? Do theauthors recommend or imply generaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> apopulation? If so, is the target population clearly<strong>in</strong>dicated? Are possible limits <strong>to</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>gdiscussed?Instrumentation: Is it adequately described? Is evidenceof adequate reliability presented? Is evidenceof validity provided? <strong>How</strong> persuasive is theevidence or the argument for validity of <strong>in</strong>ferencesmade from the <strong>in</strong>struments?Procedures/<strong>in</strong>ternal validity: What threats are evident?Were they controlled? If not, were theydiscussed?Data analysis: Are data summarized <strong>and</strong> reportedappropriately? Are descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics(if any) used appropriately? Are the statistics<strong>in</strong>terpreted correctly? Are limitations discussed?Results: Are they clearly presented? Is the writtensummary consistent with the data reported?Discussion/<strong>in</strong>terpretations: Do the authors placethe study <strong>in</strong> a broader context? Do they recognizelimitations of the study, especially with regard<strong>to</strong> population <strong>and</strong> ecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>g ofresults?


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of Technology <strong>Education</strong>, 13 (2001): 31–43.The Effect of a Computer SimulationActivity Versus a H<strong>and</strong>s-on Activityon Product Creativity<strong>in</strong> Technology <strong>Education</strong>Kurt Y. MichaelCentral Shen<strong>and</strong>oah Valley Regional Governor’s School, Virg<strong>in</strong>iaDef<strong>in</strong>ition?JustificationComputer use <strong>in</strong> the classroom has become a popular method of <strong>in</strong>struction for manytechnology educa<strong>to</strong>rs. This may be due <strong>to</strong> the fact that software programs have advancedbeyond the early days of drill <strong>and</strong> practice <strong>in</strong>struction. With the <strong>in</strong>troduction ofthe graphical user <strong>in</strong>terface, <strong>in</strong>creased process<strong>in</strong>g speed, <strong>and</strong> affordability, computer use<strong>in</strong> education has f<strong>in</strong>ally come of age. Software designers are now able <strong>to</strong> design multidimensionaleducational programs that <strong>in</strong>clude high-quality graphics, stereo sound, <strong>and</strong>real time <strong>in</strong>teraction (Bilan, 1992). One area of noticeable improvement is computersimulations.Computer simulations are software programs that either replicate or mimic realworld phenomena. If implemented correctly, computer simulations can help studentslearn about technological events <strong>and</strong> processes that may otherwise be unatta<strong>in</strong>able due<strong>to</strong> cost, feasibility, or safety. Studies have shown that computer simula<strong>to</strong>rs can:1. Be equally as effective as real life, h<strong>and</strong>s-on labora<strong>to</strong>ry experiences <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>gstudents scientific concepts (Choi & Gennaro, 1987).2. Enhance the learn<strong>in</strong>g achievement levels of students (Betz, 1996).3. Enhance the problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g skills of students (Gokhale, 1996).4. Foster peer <strong>in</strong>teraction (Bilan, 1992).The educational benefits of computer simulations for learn<strong>in</strong>g are promis<strong>in</strong>g.Some researchers even suspect that computer simulations may enhance creativity (e.g.,Betz, 1996; Gokhale, 1996; Harkow, 1996); however, after an extensive review of literature,no empirical research has been found <strong>to</strong> support this claim. For this reason, the follow<strong>in</strong>gstudy was conducted <strong>to</strong> compare the effect of a computer simulation activity versusa traditional h<strong>and</strong>s-on activity on students’ product creativity.BACKGROUNDProduct Creativity <strong>in</strong> Technology <strong>Education</strong>His<strong>to</strong>rically, technology educa<strong>to</strong>rs have chosen the creation of products or projects as ameans <strong>to</strong> teach technological concepts (Knoll, 1997). Olson (1973), <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g the importantrole projects play <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts/technology classroom, remarked, “Theproject represents human creative achievement with materials <strong>and</strong> ideas <strong>and</strong> results <strong>in</strong> anexperience of self-fulfillment” (p. 21). Lewis (1999) reiterated this belief by stat<strong>in</strong>g,“Technology is <strong>in</strong> essence a manifestation of human creativity. Thus, an important way <strong>in</strong>which students can come <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> it would be by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> acts of technologicalcreation” (p. 46). The result of technological creation is the creative product.282


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 283The creative product embodies the very essence of technology. The American Associationfor the Advancement of Science (Johnson, 1989) stated, “Technology is best describedas a process, but is most commonly known by its products <strong>and</strong> their effects on society”(p. 1). A product can be described as a physical object, article, patent, theoreticalsystem, an equation, or new technique (Brogden & Sprecher, 1964). A creative product isone that possesses some degree of unusualness (orig<strong>in</strong>ality) <strong>and</strong> usefulness (Moss, 1966).When given the opportunity for self-expression, a student’s project becomes noth<strong>in</strong>g lessthan a creative product.The creative product can be viewed as a physical representation of a person’s“true” creative ability encapsulat<strong>in</strong>g both the creative person <strong>and</strong> process (Besemer &O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1993). By exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the literature related <strong>to</strong> the creative person <strong>and</strong>process, technology educa<strong>to</strong>rs may ga<strong>in</strong> a deeper underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the creative productitself.Def<strong>in</strong>itionsJustificationThe Creative PersonInven<strong>to</strong>rs such as Edison <strong>and</strong> Ford have been recognized as be<strong>in</strong>g highly creative. Whysome people reach a level of creative genius while others do not is still unknown. <strong>How</strong>ever,Maslow (1962), after study<strong>in</strong>g several of his subjects, determ<strong>in</strong>ed that all people arecreative, not <strong>in</strong> the sense of creat<strong>in</strong>g great works, but rather, creative <strong>in</strong> a universal sensethat attributes a portion of creative talent <strong>to</strong> every person. In try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>predict a person’s creative ability, two fac<strong>to</strong>rs have often been considered: <strong>in</strong>telligence<strong>and</strong> personality.IntelligenceA frequently asked question among educa<strong>to</strong>rs is “What is the relationship between creativity<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence?” <strong>Research</strong> has shown that there is no direct correlation betweencreativity <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence quotient (I.Q.) (Edmunds, 1990; Hayes, 1990; Moss, 1966;Torrance, 1963). Edmunds (1990) conducted a study <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there was arelationship between creativity <strong>and</strong> I.Q. Two hundred <strong>and</strong> eighty-one r<strong>and</strong>omly selectedstudents, grades eight <strong>to</strong> eleven, from three different schools <strong>in</strong> New Brunswick, Canada,participated. The <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>to</strong> collect data were the Torrance Test of CreativeTh<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the Otis-Lennon School Ability Test, used <strong>to</strong> test <strong>in</strong>tellectual ability. Basedon a Pearson product moment analysis, results showed that I.Q. scores did not significantlycorrelate with creativity scores. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were consistent with the literaturedeal<strong>in</strong>g with creativity <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligence.On a practical level, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs similar <strong>to</strong> the one above may expla<strong>in</strong> why I.Q. measureshave proven <strong>to</strong> be unsuccessful <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g creative performance. Hayes (1990)po<strong>in</strong>ted out that creative performance may be better predicted by isolat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gpersonality traits.<strong>How</strong> is thisdef<strong>in</strong>ed?Evidence?Personality Traits<strong>Research</strong>ers have shown that there are certa<strong>in</strong> personality traits associated with creativepeople (e.g., DeVore, Hor<strong>to</strong>n, & Lawson, 1989; Hayes, 1990; Runco, Nemiro, & Walberg,1998; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 1974). Runco, Nemiro, <strong>and</strong> Walberg (1998) identified <strong>and</strong> conducted a survey<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g personality traits associated with the creative person. The survey wasmailed <strong>to</strong> 400 <strong>in</strong>dividuals who had submitted papers <strong>and</strong>/or published articles related <strong>to</strong>creativity. The researchers asked participants <strong>to</strong> rate, <strong>in</strong> order of importance, varioustraits that they believed affected creative achievement. The survey conta<strong>in</strong>ed 16 creative


284 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePoor returnachievement clusters consist<strong>in</strong>g of 141 items. One hundred <strong>and</strong> forty-three surveys werereturned reflect<strong>in</strong>g a response of 35.8%. Results demonstrated that <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation,problem f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> question<strong>in</strong>g skills were considered the most important traits <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g creative achievement. Though personality traits play an importantpart <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g creative ability, an equally important area of creativity theorylies <strong>in</strong> the identification of the creative process itself.The Creative ProcessCreativity is a process (Hayes, 1990; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 1974; Taylor, 1959; Torrance, 1963) that hasbeen represented us<strong>in</strong>g various models. Wallas (1926) offered one of the earliest explanationsof the creative process. His model consisted of four stages that are brieflydescribed below:1. Preparation: This is the first stage <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>dividual identifies then <strong>in</strong>vestigatesa problem from many different angles.2. Incubation: At this stage the <strong>in</strong>dividual s<strong>to</strong>ps all conscious work related <strong>to</strong> theproblem.3. Illum<strong>in</strong>ation: This stage is characterized by a sudden or immediate solution <strong>to</strong>the problem.4. Verification: This is the last stage at which time the solution is tested.Wallas’ model has served as a foundation upon which other models have beenbuilt. Some researchers have added the communication stage <strong>to</strong> the creative process(e.g., Ste<strong>in</strong>, 1974; Taylor, 1959; Torrance, 1966). The communication stage is the f<strong>in</strong>alstage of the creative process. At this stage, the new idea conf<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> one’s m<strong>in</strong>d is transformed<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a verbal or nonverbal product. The product is then shared with<strong>in</strong> a socialcontext <strong>in</strong> order that others may react <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> possibly accept or reject it. A more comprehensivedescription of the creative process is captured with<strong>in</strong> a def<strong>in</strong>ition offered byTorrance (1966):Creativity is a process of becom<strong>in</strong>g sensitive <strong>to</strong> problems, deficiencies, gaps <strong>in</strong>knowledge, miss<strong>in</strong>g elements, disharmonies, <strong>and</strong> so on; identify<strong>in</strong>g the difficult;search<strong>in</strong>g for solutions, mak<strong>in</strong>g guesses or formulat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses about thedeficiencies, test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> re-test<strong>in</strong>g these hypotheses <strong>and</strong> possibly modify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>re-test<strong>in</strong>g them, <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ally communicat<strong>in</strong>g the results (p. 8).Torrance’s def<strong>in</strong>ition resembles what some have referred <strong>to</strong> as problem solv<strong>in</strong>g.For example, technology educa<strong>to</strong>rs Savage <strong>and</strong> Sterry (1990), generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from thework of several scholars, identified six steps <strong>to</strong> the problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g process:• Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the problem: Analyz<strong>in</strong>g, gather<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation, <strong>and</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>g limitationsthat will isolate <strong>and</strong> identify the need or opportunity.• Develop<strong>in</strong>g alternative solutions: Us<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, ideation, <strong>and</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>rm<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> develop alternate ways <strong>to</strong> meet the opportunity or solve the problem.• Select<strong>in</strong>g a solution: Select<strong>in</strong>g the most plausible solution by identify<strong>in</strong>g, modify<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong>/or comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ideas from the group of possible solutions.• Implement<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the solution: Model<strong>in</strong>g, operat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>gthe effectiveness of the selected solution.• Redesign<strong>in</strong>g the solution: Incorporat<strong>in</strong>g improvements <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the design of thesolution that address needs identified dur<strong>in</strong>g the evaluation phase.• Interpret<strong>in</strong>g the solution: Synthesiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> communicat<strong>in</strong>g the characteristics<strong>and</strong> operat<strong>in</strong>g parameters of the solution (p. 15).


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 285By closely compar<strong>in</strong>g Torrance’s (1966) def<strong>in</strong>ition of creativity with that of Savage<strong>and</strong> Sterry’s (1990) problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g process, one can easily see similarities between thedescriptions. Guilford (1976), a lead<strong>in</strong>g expert <strong>in</strong> the study of creativity, made a similarcomparison between steps of the creative process offered by Wallas (1926) with those ofthe problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g process proposed by the noted educational philosopher, JohnDewey. In do<strong>in</strong>g so, Guilford simply concluded that “Problem solv<strong>in</strong>g is creative; there isno other k<strong>in</strong>d” (p. 98).H<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>n (1968) comb<strong>in</strong>ed the creative process <strong>and</strong> problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>what is now known as creative problem solv<strong>in</strong>g. He believed that creativity would bebetter unders<strong>to</strong>od if placed with<strong>in</strong> a problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g structure. Creative problem solv<strong>in</strong>gis a subset of problem solv<strong>in</strong>g based on the assumption that not all problems require acreative solution. He surmised that when a problem is solved with a learned response, nocreativity has been expressed. <strong>How</strong>ever, when a simple problem is solved with an<strong>in</strong>sighful response, a small measure of creativity has been expressed; when a complexproblem is solved with a novel solution, genu<strong>in</strong>e creativity has occurred.Genu<strong>in</strong>e creativity is the result of the creative process that manifests itself <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> acreative product. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the creative process as well as the creative person mayplay an important role <strong>in</strong> realiz<strong>in</strong>g the true nature of the creative product. Though researchershave not reached a consensus as <strong>to</strong> what attributes make up the creative product(Besemer & Treff<strong>in</strong>gger, 1981; Joram, Woodruff, Bryson, & L<strong>in</strong>dsay, 1992; Ste<strong>in</strong>, 1974),identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the creative product has been a concern of some researchers.Notable is the work of Moss (1966) <strong>and</strong> Duenk (1966).<strong>How</strong> is thisdef<strong>in</strong>ed?Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the Creative Product <strong>in</strong> Industrial Arts/Technology <strong>Education</strong>Moss (1966) <strong>and</strong> Duenk (1966) have arguably conducted the most extensive researchestablish<strong>in</strong>g criteria for evaluat<strong>in</strong>g creative products with<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts/technologyeducation. Moss (1966), <strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the criterion problem, concluded that unusualness(orig<strong>in</strong>ality) <strong>and</strong> usefulness were the def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g characteristics of the creativeproduct produced by <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts students. A description of his model is presentedbelow:1. Unusualness: To be creative, a product must possess some degree of unusualness[or orig<strong>in</strong>ality]. The quality of unusualness may, theoretically, be measured<strong>in</strong> terms of probability of occurrence; the less the probability of its occurrence,the more unusual the product (Moss, 1966, p. 7).2. Usefulness: While some degree of unusualness is a necessary requirement forcreative products, it is not a sufficient condition. To be creative, an <strong>in</strong>dustrialarts student’s product must also satisfy the m<strong>in</strong>imal pr<strong>in</strong>ciple requirements ofthe problem situation; <strong>to</strong> some degree it must “work” or be potentially “workable.”Completely <strong>in</strong>effective, irrelevant solutions <strong>to</strong> teacher-imposed orstudent-<strong>in</strong>itiated problems are not creative (Moss, 1966, p. 7).3. Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Unusualness <strong>and</strong> Usefulness: When a product possesses some degreeof both unusualness <strong>and</strong> usefulness, it is creative. But, because these twocriterion qualities are considered variables, the degree of creativity amongproducts will also vary. The extent of each product’s departure from the typical<strong>and</strong> its value as a problem solution will, <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation, determ<strong>in</strong>e the degreeof creativity of each product. Giv<strong>in</strong>g the two qualities equal weight, as the unusualness<strong>and</strong>/or usefulness of a product <strong>in</strong>creases so does its rated creativity;similarly, as the product approaches the conventional <strong>and</strong>/or uselessness itsrated creativity decreases (Moss, 1966, p. 8).In society?


286 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eJustificationUnclearIn establish<strong>in</strong>g the construct validity of his theoretical model, Moss (1966) submittedhis work for review <strong>to</strong> 57 <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts educa<strong>to</strong>rs, two measurement specialists, <strong>and</strong>six educational psychologists. Results of the review found the proposed model was compatiblewith exist<strong>in</strong>g theory <strong>and</strong> practice of both creativity <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts. No onedisagreed with the major premise of us<strong>in</strong>g unusualness <strong>and</strong> usefulness as def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g characteristicsfor evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the creative products of <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts students.To date, little additional research has been conducted <strong>to</strong> establish criteria for evaluat<strong>in</strong>gthe creative products of <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts <strong>and</strong>/or technology education students. Iftechnology is best known by its creative products, then technology educa<strong>to</strong>rs are obligated<strong>to</strong> identify characteristics that make a product more or less creative. Furthermore,educa<strong>to</strong>rs must f<strong>in</strong>d ways <strong>to</strong> objectively measure these attributes <strong>and</strong> then teach students<strong>in</strong> a manner that enhances the creativity of their products. A possible approach <strong>to</strong> enhanc<strong>in</strong>gproduct creativity is by <strong>in</strong>corporat<strong>in</strong>g computer simulation technology <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> theclassroom. <strong>How</strong>ever, no research has been done <strong>in</strong> this area <strong>to</strong> measure the true effect ofcomputer simulation on product creativity. For that reason, other studies address<strong>in</strong>gcomputer use <strong>in</strong> general <strong>and</strong> product creativity will be explored.Studies Related <strong>to</strong> Computers <strong>and</strong> the Creative ProductGood summariesJustificationA study conducted by Joram, Woodruff, Bryson, <strong>and</strong> L<strong>in</strong>dsay (1992) found that averagestudents produced their most creative work us<strong>in</strong>g word processors as compared <strong>to</strong> studentsus<strong>in</strong>g pencil <strong>and</strong> paper. The researchers hypothesized that word process<strong>in</strong>g wouldh<strong>in</strong>der product creativity due <strong>to</strong> constant evaluation <strong>and</strong> edit<strong>in</strong>g of their work. To test thehypotheses, average <strong>and</strong> above-average eighth-grade writers were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong>one of two groups. The first group was asked <strong>to</strong> compose us<strong>in</strong>g word processors while thesecond group was asked <strong>to</strong> compose us<strong>in</strong>g pencil <strong>and</strong> paper. After collect<strong>in</strong>g the compositions,both the word-processed <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>written texts were typed so that they would be<strong>in</strong> the same format for the evalua<strong>to</strong>rs. Based on the results, the researchers concluded thatword process<strong>in</strong>g enhances the creative abilities of average writers. The researchers attributedthis <strong>to</strong> the prospect that word process<strong>in</strong>g may allow the average writer <strong>to</strong> generatea number of ideas, know<strong>in</strong>g that only a few of them will be usable <strong>and</strong> the rest can be easilyerased. <strong>How</strong>ever, the researchers also found that word process<strong>in</strong>g had a negative effec<strong>to</strong>n the creativity of above-average writers. These mixed results suggest that the use ofword process<strong>in</strong>g may not be appropriate for all students relative <strong>to</strong> creativity.Similar <strong>to</strong> word process<strong>in</strong>g, computer graphics programs may also help studentsimprove the creativeness of their products. In a study conducted by <strong>How</strong>e (1992), two advancedundergraduate classes <strong>in</strong> graphics design were assigned <strong>to</strong> one of two treatments.The first treatment group was <strong>in</strong>structed <strong>to</strong> use a computer graphic program <strong>to</strong>complete a design project whereas the other group was asked <strong>to</strong> use conventionalgraphic design equipment <strong>to</strong> design their product. Upon completion of the assignment,both groups’ projects were collected <strong>and</strong> pho<strong>to</strong>copied so that they would be <strong>in</strong> the sameformat before be<strong>in</strong>g evaluated. Based on the results, the researcher concluded that studentsus<strong>in</strong>g computer graphics technology surpassed the conventional method <strong>in</strong> productcreativity. The researchers attributed this <strong>to</strong> the prospect that computer graphics programsmay enable graphic designers <strong>to</strong> generate an abundance of ideas, then capturethe most creative ones <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>corporate them <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their designs. <strong>How</strong>ever, due <strong>to</strong> a lackof r<strong>and</strong>om assignment, results of the study should be generalized with caution.Like word process<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> computer graphics, simulation technology is a type ofcomputer application that allows users <strong>to</strong> freely manipulate <strong>and</strong> edit virtual objects.Thus, it was surmised that computer simulation may enhance creativity. This notion led<strong>to</strong> the development of the study reported here<strong>in</strong>.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 287PURPOSE OF THE STUDYThis study compared the effect of a computer simulation activity versus a traditional h<strong>and</strong>sonactivity on students’ product creativity. A creative product was def<strong>in</strong>ed as one thatpossesses some measure of both unusualness (orig<strong>in</strong>ality) <strong>and</strong> usefulness. The follow<strong>in</strong>ghypothesis <strong>and</strong> sub-hypotheses were exam<strong>in</strong>ed.Description, notpurposeMajor <strong>Research</strong> HypothesisThere is no difference <strong>in</strong> product creativity between the computer simulation <strong>and</strong> traditionalh<strong>and</strong>s-on groups.<strong>Research</strong> Sub-Hypotheses1. There is no difference <strong>in</strong> product orig<strong>in</strong>ality between the computer simulation<strong>and</strong> traditional h<strong>and</strong>s-on groups.2. There is no difference <strong>in</strong> product usefulness between the computer simulation<strong>and</strong> traditional h<strong>and</strong>s-on groups.Null hypothesesNull hypothesesMETHODSubjectsThe subjects selected for this study were seventh-grade technology education studentsfrom three different middle schools located <strong>in</strong> Northern Virg<strong>in</strong>ia, a middle-<strong>to</strong>-upper<strong>in</strong>comesuburb outside of Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, D.C. The school system’s middle school technologyeducation programs provide learn<strong>in</strong>g situations that allow the students <strong>to</strong> exploretechnology through problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g activities. The three participat<strong>in</strong>g schools werechosen because of the teachers’ will<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study.ConveniencesampleMaterialsKits of Classic Lego Bricks TM were used with the h<strong>and</strong>s-on group. The demonstrationversion of Gryphon Bricks TM (Gryphon Software Corporation, 1996) was used with thesimulation group. This software allows students <strong>to</strong> assemble <strong>and</strong> disassemble computergeneratedLego-type bricks <strong>in</strong> a virtual environment on the screen of the computer.Subjects <strong>in</strong> the computer simulation group were each assigned <strong>to</strong> a Mac<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>sh computeron which the Gryphon Bricks software was <strong>in</strong>stalled. Each subject <strong>in</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s-on treatmentgroup was given a conta<strong>in</strong>er of Lego bricks identical <strong>to</strong> those available virtually <strong>in</strong>the Gryphon software.Test InstrumentProducts were evaluated based on a theoretical model proposed by Moss (1966). Mossused the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of unusualness (or orig<strong>in</strong>ality) <strong>and</strong> usefulness as criteria fordeterm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g product creativity. <strong>How</strong>ever, Moss’ actual <strong>in</strong>strument was not used <strong>in</strong> thisstudy due <strong>to</strong> low <strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability. Instead, a portion of the Creative Product SemanticScale or CPSS (Besemer & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1989) was used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e product creativity.Sub-scales “Orig<strong>in</strong>al” <strong>and</strong> “Useful” from the CPSS were chosen <strong>to</strong> be consistent withMoss’ theoretical model.The CPSS has proven <strong>to</strong> be a reliable <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a variety of creativeproducts based on objective, analytical measures of creativity (Besemer & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1986,1987, 1989, 1993). This was accomplished by the use of a bipolar, semantic differentialNot clear <strong>to</strong> usEvidence?


288 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eInsufficientdemonstrationof reliability<strong>and</strong> validityInternal consistencyonlyscale. In general, semantic differential scales are good for measur<strong>in</strong>g mental concepts orimages (Alreck, 1995). Because creativity is a mental concept, the semantic differentialnaturally lends itself <strong>to</strong> measur<strong>in</strong>g the creative product. Furthermore, the CPSS is flexibleenough <strong>to</strong> allow researchers <strong>to</strong> pick various subscales based on the theoretical constructbe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated, like the use of the “Orig<strong>in</strong>al” <strong>and</strong> “Useful” subscales <strong>in</strong> this study. Insupport of this, Besemer <strong>and</strong> O’Qu<strong>in</strong> (1986) stated, “. . . the subscale structure of the<strong>to</strong>tal scale lends itself <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istration of relevant portions of the <strong>in</strong>strument ratherthan the whole” (p. 125).The CPSS was used <strong>in</strong> a study conducted by <strong>How</strong>e (1992). His reliability analysis,based on Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, yielded good <strong>to</strong> high reliability across all subscalesof the CPSS. Important <strong>to</strong> this study were the high reliability results for subscales “Orig<strong>in</strong>al”(.93) <strong>and</strong> “Useful” (.92). These high reliability coefficients are consistent with earlierstudies conducted by Besemer <strong>and</strong> O’Qu<strong>in</strong> (1986, 1987, 1989).The Pilot StudyA pilot study was conducted <strong>in</strong> which a seventh-grade technology education class from aSouthwest Virg<strong>in</strong>ia middle school was selected. The pilot study consisted of 16 subjectswho were r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> either a h<strong>and</strong>s-on treatment group or a simulation treatmentgroup. As a result of the pilot study, the time allocated for the students <strong>to</strong> assembletheir creative products was reduced from 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>to</strong> 25 m<strong>in</strong>utes s<strong>in</strong>ce most of them hadf<strong>in</strong>ished with<strong>in</strong> the shorter time. Precedence for limit<strong>in</strong>g the time needed <strong>to</strong> complete acreative task was found <strong>in</strong> Torrance’s (1966) work <strong>in</strong> which 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes was the time limitfor a variety of approaches <strong>to</strong> measur<strong>in</strong>g creativity.ProcedureTrue experimentBy whom?Internal validityOne class from each of the three participat<strong>in</strong>g schools was selected for the study. Fiftyeightsubjects participated, 21 females <strong>and</strong> 37 males, with an average age of 12.4 years.Subjects were given identification numbers, then r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned <strong>to</strong> either the h<strong>and</strong>sonor the computer simulation treatment group. The r<strong>and</strong>om assignment helped ensurethe equivalence of groups <strong>and</strong> controlled for extraneous variables such as students’ priorexperience with open-ended problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g activities, use of Lego blocks <strong>and</strong>/or computersimulation programs, <strong>and</strong> other extraneous variables that may have confoundedthe results. The <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> this study was the <strong>in</strong>structional activity <strong>and</strong>the dependent variable was the subjects’ creative product scores as determ<strong>in</strong>ed by thecomb<strong>in</strong>ation of the “Orig<strong>in</strong>al” <strong>and</strong> “Useful” subscales from the CPSS (Besemer &O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1989).Subjects <strong>in</strong> both the h<strong>and</strong>s-on <strong>and</strong> the simulation groups were asked <strong>to</strong> constructa “creature” that they believed would be found on a Lego planet. The “creature” scenariowas chosen because it was an open-ended problem <strong>and</strong> possessed the greatest potentialfor imag<strong>in</strong>ative student expression. The only difference <strong>in</strong> treatment between thetwo groups was that the h<strong>and</strong>s-on group used real Lego bricks <strong>in</strong> construct<strong>in</strong>g their productswhereas the simulation treatment group used a computer simula<strong>to</strong>r. Treatmentswere adm<strong>in</strong>istered simultaneously <strong>and</strong> overall treatment time was the same for bothgroups. The h<strong>and</strong>s-on treatment group met <strong>in</strong> its regular classroom whereas the simulationtreatment group met <strong>in</strong> a computer lab. The classroom teacher at each school proc<strong>to</strong>redthe h<strong>and</strong>s-on treatment group <strong>and</strong> the researcher proc<strong>to</strong>red the simulation treatmentgroup.The subjects <strong>in</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s-on treatment group were given five m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>to</strong> sort theirbricks by color while subjects <strong>in</strong> the simulation treatment group watched a five-m<strong>in</strong>ute


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 289<strong>in</strong>structional video expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> use the simulation software. By hav<strong>in</strong>g the studentssort their bricks for five m<strong>in</strong>utes, the overall treatment time was the same for bothgroups, thus elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g a variable that may otherwise <strong>in</strong>fluence the results. Then, thesubjects <strong>in</strong> both groups were given the follow<strong>in</strong>g scenario:Internal validityPretend you are a <strong>to</strong>y designer work<strong>in</strong>g for the Lego Company. Your job is <strong>to</strong>create a “creative” us<strong>in</strong>g Lego bricks that will be used <strong>in</strong> a <strong>to</strong>y set called LegoPlanet. What types of creatures might be found on a Lego planet? Use yourcreativity <strong>and</strong> make a creature that is orig<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong> appearance yet useful <strong>to</strong> the<strong>to</strong>y manufacturer.One more th<strong>in</strong>g: The creature you construct must be able <strong>to</strong> fit with<strong>in</strong> afive-<strong>in</strong>ch cubed box. That means you must stay with<strong>in</strong> the limits of your greenbase plate <strong>and</strong> make your creature no higher than 13 bricks.You will have 25 m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>to</strong> complete this activity. If you f<strong>in</strong>ish early, spendmore time th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about how you can make your creature more creative. Youmust rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> your seat the whole time. If there are no questions, you may beg<strong>in</strong>.When the time was up, the subjects were asked <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p work<strong>in</strong>g. The h<strong>and</strong>s-ontreatment group’s products were labeled, collected, <strong>and</strong> then reproduced <strong>in</strong> the computersimulation software by the researcher. This was done so that the raters could notdist<strong>in</strong>guish from which treatment group the products were created. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the imagesof the products from both groups were pr<strong>in</strong>ted us<strong>in</strong>g a color pr<strong>in</strong>ter.Internal validityProduct EvaluationTo evaluate the students’ solutions, two raters were recruited: a middle school artteacher <strong>and</strong> a middle school science teacher. The teachers were chosen because of theirwill<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study <strong>and</strong> had a comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong>tal of 36 years of teach<strong>in</strong>gexperience. To help establish <strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability, a rater tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g session was conducteddur<strong>in</strong>g the pilot study. The same teacher-raters used <strong>in</strong> the pilot study were used <strong>in</strong> thef<strong>in</strong>al study. The tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g session provided the teacher-raters with <strong>in</strong>structions on how <strong>to</strong>use the rat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> allowed them <strong>to</strong> practice rat<strong>in</strong>g sample products. Dur<strong>in</strong>gthe session, disagreements on product rat<strong>in</strong>gs were discussed <strong>and</strong> rules were developedby the raters <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease consistency. The pilot study confirmed that there was good<strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability across all the scales <strong>and</strong> thus the experimental procedures proceededas designed. No significant difference <strong>in</strong> creativity, orig<strong>in</strong>ality, or usefulness wasfound between the two treatment groups dur<strong>in</strong>g the pilot study.For the actual study, the teacher-raters were each given the pr<strong>in</strong>ted images of theproducts from each of the 58 subjects <strong>and</strong> were <strong>in</strong>structed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependently rate themus<strong>in</strong>g the “Orig<strong>in</strong>al” <strong>and</strong> “Useful” subscales of the CPSS (Besemer & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1989). Threeweeks were allowed for the rat<strong>in</strong>g process.FINDINGSOnce the rat<strong>in</strong>gs from the two raters had been obta<strong>in</strong>ed, an <strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability analysis,based on Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, was conducted. Analysis yielded moderate <strong>to</strong>good <strong>in</strong>ter-rater reliability (.74 <strong>to</strong> .88) across all the scales. The stated hypotheses werethen tested us<strong>in</strong>g one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA).• No difference <strong>in</strong> product Creativity scores was found between the computersimulation group (M = 41.7, SD = 7.67) <strong>and</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s-on group (M 42.0, SD 5.58). Therefore, the null hypothesis was not rejected, F (5,52) 0.54, p 0.75.GoodEvidence shouldbe providedOperationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionDef<strong>in</strong>itionsprovided?Inappropriate?We would expecthigherBasic assumptionviolated (seepages 232,244–245)


290 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCalculation of effectsize is appropriatestatistic (see page244)• No difference <strong>in</strong> product Orig<strong>in</strong>ality scores was found between the computersimulation group (M 20.59, SD 4.44) <strong>and</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s-on group (M 21.10.SD 3.10). Thus, the null hypothesis was not rejected, F (5,52) 1.07, p 0.39.• No difference <strong>in</strong> product Usefulness scores between the computer simulationgroup (M 21.15, SD 4.17) <strong>and</strong> the traditional h<strong>and</strong>s-on group (M 20.90,SD 3.20). Once aga<strong>in</strong>, the researcher failed <strong>to</strong> reject the null hypothesis,F (5,52) 0.49, p 0.78.CONCLUSION<strong>How</strong> is this def<strong>in</strong>ed?Only one productwas studiedWhy?RightThough there are only a few empirical studies <strong>to</strong> support their claims, some researchersbelieve that computers <strong>in</strong> general may improve student product creativity by allow<strong>in</strong>gstudents <strong>to</strong> generate an abundance of ideas, capture the most creative ones, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>corporatethem <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their product (<strong>How</strong>e, 1992; Joram, Woodruff, Bryson, & L<strong>in</strong>dsay, 1992).Similarly, some researchers speculate that the use of computer simulations may enhanceproduct creativity as well (Betz, 1996; Gokhale, 1996; Harkow, 1996). <strong>How</strong>ever, based onthe results of this study, the use of computer simulation <strong>to</strong> enhance product creativitywas not supported. The creativity, usefulness, or orig<strong>in</strong>ality of the result<strong>in</strong>g productsappears <strong>to</strong> be the same whether students use a computer simulation of Lego blocks orwhether they manipulated the actual blocks.Because the simulation activity <strong>in</strong> this study was nearly identical <strong>to</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s-ontask, one might conclude that product creativity may be more reliant upon the <strong>in</strong>dividual’screative cognitive ability rather than the <strong>to</strong>ols or means by which the product wascreated. This would st<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> reason based on Besemer <strong>and</strong> O’Qu<strong>in</strong>’s (1993) belief thatthe creative product is unique <strong>in</strong> that it comb<strong>in</strong>es both the creative person <strong>and</strong> process<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a tangible object represent<strong>in</strong>g the “true” measure of a person’s creative ability.With this <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, when study<strong>in</strong>g a computer simulation’s effect on student product creativity,researchers may want <strong>to</strong> focus more attention on the creative person’s traits <strong>and</strong>the cognitive process used <strong>to</strong> create the product rather than focus<strong>in</strong>g on the <strong>to</strong>ol ormeans by which the product was created. This approach <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g student productcreativity may lend itself more <strong>to</strong> qualitative rather than quantitative research.If quantitative research is <strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>in</strong> this area of study, researchers may wish <strong>to</strong>consider us<strong>in</strong>g a different theoretical model <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument for measur<strong>in</strong>g the creativeproduct. For example, if replicat<strong>in</strong>g this experiment, rather than us<strong>in</strong>g only the two subscalesof the Creative Product Semantic Scale (Bessemer & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, 1989), the complete<strong>in</strong>strument might be used, yield<strong>in</strong>g additional dimensions of creativity. Additional researchregard<strong>in</strong>g the various types of simulation programs is needed, along with the differenteffects they might have on student creativity <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g products. The use ofcomputer simulations <strong>in</strong> technology education programs appears <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g withlittle research <strong>to</strong> support their effectiveness or viable use.ReferencesAlreck, T. L., & Settle, B. R. (1995). The survey researchh<strong>and</strong>book (2nd Ed.). Chicago: Irw<strong>in</strong> Inc.Besemer, S. P., & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, K. (1993). Assess<strong>in</strong>g creativeproducts: Progress <strong>and</strong> potentials. In S. G. Isaksen (Ed.),Nurtur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g creativity: The emergence of adiscipl<strong>in</strong>e (pp. 331–349). Norwood, New Jersey: AblexPublish<strong>in</strong>g Corp.Besemer, S. P., & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, K. (1989). The development,reliability, <strong>and</strong> validity of the revised creative productsemantic scale. Creativity <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 2, 268–279.Besemer, S. P., & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, K. (1987). Creative product analysis:Test<strong>in</strong>g a model by develop<strong>in</strong>g a judg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument. InS. G. Isaksen, Frontiers of creativity research: Beyond thebasics. (pp. 341–357). Buffalo, NY: Bearly Ltd.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 291Besemer, S. P., & O’Qu<strong>in</strong>, K. (1986). Analysis of creativeproducts: Ref<strong>in</strong>ement <strong>and</strong> test of a judg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument.Journal of Creative Behavior, 20(2), 115–126.Besemer, S. P., & Treff<strong>in</strong>gger, D. (1981). Analysis of creativeproducts: Review <strong>and</strong> synthesis. Journal of CreativeBehavior, 15, 158–178.Betz, J. A. (1996). Computer games: Increase learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an<strong>in</strong>teractive multidiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary environment. Journal ofTechnology Systems, 24(2), 195–205.Bilan, B. (1992). Computer simulations: An Integrated <strong>to</strong>ol.Paper presented at the SAGE/6th Canadian Symposium,The University of Calgary.Brogden, H., & Sprecher, T. (1964). Criteria of creativity. InC. W. Taylor, Creativity, progress <strong>and</strong> potential. New York:McGraw Hill.Choi, B., & Gennaro, E. (1987). The effectiveness of us<strong>in</strong>gcomputer simulated experiments on junior high students’underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the volume displacementconcept. Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Science Teach<strong>in</strong>g, 24(6),539–552.DeVore, P., Hor<strong>to</strong>n, A., & Lawson, A. (1989). Creativity,design, <strong>and</strong> technology. Worcester, Massachusetts: DavisPublications, Inc.Duenk, L. G. (1966). A study of the concurrent validity of theM<strong>in</strong>nesota Test of Creative Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, Abbr. Form VII, foreighth-grade <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts students. M<strong>in</strong>neapolis:M<strong>in</strong>nesota University. (Report No. BR-5-0113).Edmunds, A. L. (1990). Relationships among adolescentcreativity, cognitive development, <strong>in</strong>telligence, <strong>and</strong> age.Canadian Journal of Special <strong>Education</strong>, 6(1), 61–71.Gokhale, A. A. (1996). Effectiveness of computer simulationfor enhanc<strong>in</strong>g higher order th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of IndustrialTeacher <strong>Education</strong>, 33(4), 36–46.Gryphon Software Corporation (1996). Gryphon BricksDemo (Version 1.0) [Computer Software]. Glendale, CA:Knowledge Adventure. [On-l<strong>in</strong>e] Available: http://www.kidsdoma<strong>in</strong>.com/down/mac/bricksdemo.htmlGuilford, J. (1976). Intellectual fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong> productive th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.In R. Mooney & T. Rayik (Eds.), Explorations <strong>in</strong> creativity.New York: Harper & Row.Harkow, R. M. (1996). Increas<strong>in</strong>g creative th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong>second <strong>and</strong> third grade gifted students us<strong>in</strong>g imagery,computers, <strong>and</strong> creative problem solv<strong>in</strong>g. Unpublishedmaster’s thesis, NOVA Southeastern University.Hayes, J. R. (1990). Cognitive processes <strong>in</strong> creativity. (PaperNo. 18). University of California, Berkeley.H<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>n, B. L. (1968, Spr<strong>in</strong>g). A model for the study ofcreative problem solv<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of Creative Behavior,2(2), 133–142.<strong>How</strong>e, R. (1992). Uncover<strong>in</strong>g the creative dimensions ofcomputer-graphic design products. Creativity <strong>Research</strong>Journal, 5(3), 233–243.Johnson, J. R. (1989). Project 2061: Technology (Associationfor the Advancement of Science Publication 89-06S).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Association for theAdvancement of Science.Joram, E., Woodruff, E., Bryson, M., & L<strong>in</strong>dsay, P. (1992). Theeffects of revis<strong>in</strong>g with a word processor on writ<strong>in</strong>gcomposition. <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> the Teach<strong>in</strong>g of English, 26(2),167–192.Knoll, M. (1997). The project method: Its vocationaleducation orig<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternational development. Journalof Industrial Teacher <strong>Education</strong>, 34(3), 59–80.Lewis, T. (1999). <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> technology education: Someareas of need. Journal of Technology <strong>Education</strong>, 10(2),41–56.Maslow, A. (1962). Toward a psychology of be<strong>in</strong>g. Pr<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>to</strong>n,NJ: Van Nostr<strong>and</strong>.Moss, J. (1966). Measur<strong>in</strong>g creative abilities <strong>in</strong> junior highschool <strong>in</strong>dustrial arts. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Councilon Industrial Arts Teacher <strong>Education</strong>.Olson, D. W. (1973). Tecnol-o-gee. Raleigh: North Carol<strong>in</strong>aUniversity School of <strong>Education</strong>, Office of Publications.Runco, R. A., Nemiro, J., & Walberg, H. J. (1998). Personalexplicit theories of creativity. Journal of CreativeBehavior, 32(1), 1–17.Savage, E., & Sterry, L. (1990). A conceptual frameworkfor technology education. Res<strong>to</strong>n, VA: InternationalTechnology <strong>Education</strong> Association.Ste<strong>in</strong>, M. (1974). Stimulat<strong>in</strong>g creativity: Vol. 1. Individualprocedures. New York: Academic Press.Taylor, I. A. (1959). The nature of the creative process. InP. Smith (Ed.), Creativity: An exam<strong>in</strong>ation of the creativeprocess (pp. 51–82). New York: Hast<strong>in</strong>gs HousePublishers.Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance test on creative th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g:Norms-technical manual (<strong>Research</strong> Edition). Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,MA: Personal Press.Torrance, E. P. (1963). Creativity. In F. W. Hubbard (Ed.), Whatresearch says <strong>to</strong> the teacher (Number 28). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,DC: Department of Classroom Teachers American<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association of the National<strong>Education</strong> Association.Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt,Brace <strong>and</strong> Company.


292 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is made clear (repeatedly), though it is notfound under the “Purpose of the Study” head<strong>in</strong>g. It is <strong>to</strong>study the effects of computer simulation on the productionof “creative products” by middle school students.Study<strong>in</strong>g computer simulation is justified by cit<strong>in</strong>g bothop<strong>in</strong>ion <strong>and</strong> evidence as <strong>to</strong> its value, particularly <strong>in</strong> technologyeducation. The use of “product creativity” as theoutcome variable is justified at length as <strong>to</strong> its importance<strong>in</strong> technology <strong>and</strong>, by implication, <strong>in</strong> broader human endeavors.A f<strong>in</strong>al justification is that no research had beenreported specifically on simulation effects on creativity(though use of the term true effects causes us <strong>to</strong> wonder).There appear <strong>to</strong> be no ethical problems related <strong>to</strong>confidentiality, risk, or deception.DEFINITIONSThe dependent variable, product creativity, is def<strong>in</strong>ed as aproduct “that possesses some degree of unusualness(orig<strong>in</strong>ality) <strong>and</strong> usefulness.” A product is def<strong>in</strong>ed as “aphysical object, article, patent, theoretical system, anequation, or new technique.” By this def<strong>in</strong>ition, productpresumably does not <strong>in</strong>clude such th<strong>in</strong>gs as an essay ordance. The def<strong>in</strong>ition of product creativity is, we th<strong>in</strong>k,confused by the extended discussion of creativity <strong>and</strong>problem solv<strong>in</strong>g because it gets <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the perennial questionas <strong>to</strong> whether orig<strong>in</strong>ality <strong>and</strong> usefulness perta<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the<strong>in</strong>dividual or <strong>to</strong> society. An <strong>in</strong>dividual’s problem solutioncan be both orig<strong>in</strong>al <strong>and</strong> useful <strong>to</strong> the person but not necessarily<strong>in</strong> any broader sense. Fortunately, the reference<strong>to</strong> Moss (see page 285) seems <strong>to</strong> make it clear that societyis the reference po<strong>in</strong>t. An unidentified operational def<strong>in</strong>itionis provided <strong>in</strong> the last paragraph before “F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.”Computer simulations is def<strong>in</strong>ed as “software programsthat either replicate or mimic real world phenomena.”An operational def<strong>in</strong>ition is found under“Materials,” as “The demonstration version of GryphonBricks,” which is further described, as are the details ofimplementation. The comparison treatment is def<strong>in</strong>edas “h<strong>and</strong>s-on” experience with the same materials <strong>and</strong>under the same procedures.PRIOR RESEARCHWe accept (with the qualification mentioned earlier) theauthors’ statement that there are no prior studies on this<strong>to</strong>pic. The brief summaries of two studies on outcomes ofcomputer graphics <strong>and</strong> word processors are well done,but we must assume they are the only ones. The literaturecited <strong>in</strong> the discussion of creative people <strong>and</strong> of the creativeprocess appears thorough <strong>and</strong> is relevant, althoughmix<strong>in</strong>g differ<strong>in</strong>g def<strong>in</strong>itions of creativity is a problem.HYPOTHESESThe major hypothesis <strong>and</strong> two subhypotheses areclearly stated. They are <strong>in</strong> the common “null” form. Ourpreference is for directional hypotheses when, as seemsthe case <strong>in</strong> this study, they are clearly implied.SAMPLEAs is common, unfortunately, a convenience sample wasused. Description of students is limited <strong>to</strong> geographiclocation, grade level, gender, <strong>and</strong> economic level, whichis of some but limited value is assess<strong>in</strong>g generalizability.The <strong>to</strong>tal sample size is 58, limit<strong>in</strong>g comparisons <strong>to</strong> 29 <strong>in</strong>each group.INSTRUMENTATIONProduct creativity was measured by means of a semanticdifferential rat<strong>in</strong>g scale (see page 126) used by judges <strong>to</strong>evaluate orig<strong>in</strong>ality <strong>and</strong> usefulness. It is implied but notclear that these scales follow the def<strong>in</strong>itions of Moss. Itis also unclear whether raters were provided def<strong>in</strong>itionsor allowed <strong>to</strong> implicitly use their own.Prior results on <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency for these scalesare good but demonstrate only consistency among thefive items <strong>in</strong> each scale. The authors are <strong>to</strong> be commendedfor check<strong>in</strong>g agreement between raters, but it isnot clear <strong>to</strong> us how Cronbach’s alpha coefficient (appropriatefor <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency) could have been used <strong>to</strong>compare raters.Validity is not adequately addressed; the statement(cit<strong>in</strong>g Alreck) that such scales are “good” is <strong>in</strong>sufficient.Validity data should have been reviewed.PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYThe study design is a true experiment <strong>in</strong> that each ofthree classes was r<strong>and</strong>omly divided <strong>and</strong> assigned <strong>to</strong> oneof the two treatment methods. This is the best way <strong>to</strong>control the subject characteristic threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.Familiarity with materials was controlled by giv<strong>in</strong>gequivalent preexposure <strong>to</strong> both groups; <strong>in</strong>structions wereidentical as was treatment time. Because the method


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 293groups worked <strong>in</strong> different rooms, a location threat ispossible but seems unlikely. Raters were ignorant oftreatment group identity, elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>strumentationthreat. An implementation threat is possible becausemethod groups had different proc<strong>to</strong>rs but is unlikely unlessthey were active participants. Mortality evidentlydid not occur. Test<strong>in</strong>g, his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation <strong>and</strong> regressionthreats do not apply or were controlled by the design.DATA ANALYSIS/RESULTSBecause of the lack of r<strong>and</strong>om selection <strong>and</strong> the absenceof a rationale for consider<strong>in</strong>g the sample representativeof a def<strong>in</strong>ed population, use of ANOVA (see page 245)is <strong>in</strong>appropriate except as a rough guide <strong>and</strong> with thenecessary qualifications. Instead effect size (see page244) is the correct technique <strong>and</strong> (us<strong>in</strong>g data provided)gives m<strong>in</strong>imal values of less than .10 for all three comparisons.Exam<strong>in</strong>ation of means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationsalone demonstrates the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g of no differencesworth attend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>.DISCUSSION/INTERPRETATIONWe agree that the results do not support “the use of computersimulation <strong>to</strong> enhance product creativity.” Theconclusion is enhanced by the good control of threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity. <strong>How</strong>ever, this conclusion must be limited<strong>to</strong> this particular product—a severe limitation onecological generalizability (see page 104). The authoracknowledges a similar limitation, that f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are limited<strong>to</strong> this particular form of simulation. We do notagree with the suggestion of us<strong>in</strong>g other theories <strong>and</strong>other scales of the CPSS because this would mean ab<strong>and</strong>on<strong>in</strong>ga well-defended approach based on the results ofone seriously limited study. Instead, we would recommendretra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of raters (provided with clear def<strong>in</strong>itions),because rater agreement was not up <strong>to</strong> cus<strong>to</strong>marylevels, <strong>and</strong> redo<strong>in</strong>g the product rat<strong>in</strong>gs. If replication ofthis study with a different simulation <strong>and</strong> product producedthe same outcomes, we would f<strong>in</strong>d these suggestionsmore persuasive.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE UNIQUENESS OF EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCHExperimental research is unique <strong>in</strong> that it is the only type of research that directlyattempts <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence a particular variable, <strong>and</strong> it is the only type that, when usedproperly, can really test hypotheses about cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect relationships. Experimentaldesigns are some of the strongest available for educational researchers <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g cause <strong>and</strong> effect.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsESSENTIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCHExperiments differ from other types of research <strong>in</strong> two basic ways—comparison oftreatments <strong>and</strong> the direct manipulation of one or more <strong>in</strong>dependent variables by theresearcher.


294 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eRANDOMIZATIONR<strong>and</strong>om assignment is an important <strong>in</strong>gredient <strong>in</strong> the best k<strong>in</strong>ds of experiments. Itmeans that every <strong>in</strong>dividual who is participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the experiment has an equalchance of be<strong>in</strong>g assigned <strong>to</strong> any of the experimental or control conditions that arebe<strong>in</strong>g compared.CONTROL OF EXTRANEOUS VARIABLESThe researcher <strong>in</strong> an experimental study has an opportunity <strong>to</strong> exercise far more controlthan <strong>in</strong> most other forms of research.Some of the most common ways <strong>to</strong> control for the possibility of differential subjectcharacteristics (<strong>in</strong> the various groups be<strong>in</strong>g compared) are r<strong>and</strong>omization, hold<strong>in</strong>gcerta<strong>in</strong> variables constant, build<strong>in</strong>g the variable <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the design, match<strong>in</strong>g, us<strong>in</strong>gsubjects as their own controls, <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g analysis of the covariance.WEAK EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNSThree weak designs that are occasionally used <strong>in</strong> experimental research are the oneshotcase study design, the one-group pretest-posttest design, <strong>and</strong> the static-groupcomparison design. They are considered weak because they do not have built-<strong>in</strong> controlsfor threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.In a one-shot case study, a s<strong>in</strong>gle group is exposed <strong>to</strong> a treatment or event, <strong>and</strong> itseffects are assessed.In the one-group pretest-posttest design, a s<strong>in</strong>gle group is measured or observed bothbefore <strong>and</strong> after exposure <strong>to</strong> a treatment.In the static-group comparison design, two <strong>in</strong>tact groups receive different treatments.TRUE EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNSThe essential <strong>in</strong>gredient of a true experiment is r<strong>and</strong>om assignment of subjects <strong>to</strong>treatment groups.The r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only control group design <strong>in</strong>volves two groups formed byr<strong>and</strong>om assignment.The r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttest control group design differs from the r<strong>and</strong>omizedposttest-only control group only <strong>in</strong> the use of a pretest.The r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-group design <strong>in</strong>volves r<strong>and</strong>om assignment of subjects<strong>to</strong> four groups, with two be<strong>in</strong>g pretested <strong>and</strong> two not.MATCHINGTo <strong>in</strong>crease the likelihood that groups of subjects will be equivalent, pairs of subjectsmay be matched on certa<strong>in</strong> variables. The members of the matched groups are thenassigned <strong>to</strong> the experimental <strong>and</strong> control groups.Match<strong>in</strong>g may be either mechanical or statistical.Mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g is a process of pair<strong>in</strong>g two persons whose scores on a particularvariable are similar.Two difficulties with mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g are that it is very difficult <strong>to</strong> match onmore than two or three variables, <strong>and</strong> that <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> match, some subjects must beelim<strong>in</strong>ated from the study when no matches can be found.Statistical match<strong>in</strong>g does not necessitate a loss of subjects.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 295QUASI-EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNSThe match<strong>in</strong>g-only design differs from r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with match<strong>in</strong>g only <strong>in</strong>that r<strong>and</strong>om assignment is not used.In a counterbalanced design, all groups are exposed <strong>to</strong> all treatments, but <strong>in</strong> a differen<strong>to</strong>rder.A time-series design <strong>in</strong>volves repeated measurements or observations over time,both before <strong>and</strong> after treatment.FACTORIAL DESIGNSFac<strong>to</strong>rial designs extend the number of relationships that may be exam<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> anexperimental study.comparison group 262control 264control group 262counterbalanceddesign 271criterion variable 261dependent variable 261design 264experiment 262experimental group 262experimentalresearch 261experimentalvariable 261extraneous variables 263fac<strong>to</strong>rial design 273ga<strong>in</strong> score 271<strong>in</strong>dependentvariable 261<strong>in</strong>teraction 273match<strong>in</strong>g design 268mechanicalmatch<strong>in</strong>g 270modera<strong>to</strong>r variables 273nonequivalent controlgroup design 266one-group pretestposttestdesign 265one-shot case studydesign 265outcome variable 261pretest treatment<strong>in</strong>teraction 268quasi-experimentaldesign 271r<strong>and</strong>omassignment 263r<strong>and</strong>om selection 263r<strong>and</strong>omized posttes<strong>to</strong>nlycontrol groupdesign 267r<strong>and</strong>omized pretestposttestcontrol groupdesign 267r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomonfour-group design 268regressed ga<strong>in</strong> score 271static-group comparisondesign 266static-group pretestposttestdesign 266statistical equat<strong>in</strong>g 270statistical match<strong>in</strong>g 270time-series design 272treatment variable 261Key Terms1. An occasional criticism of experimental research is that it is very difficult <strong>to</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong> schools. Would you agree? Why or why not?2. Are there any cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect statements you can make that you believe would betrue <strong>in</strong> most schools? Would you say, for example, that a sympathetic teacher“causes” elementary school students <strong>to</strong> like school more?3. Are there any advantages <strong>to</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g more than one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable <strong>in</strong> an experimentaldesign? If so, what are they? What about more than one dependent variable?4. What designs could be used <strong>in</strong> each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g studies? (Note: More than onedesign is possible <strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>stance.)a. A comparison of two different ways of teach<strong>in</strong>g spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> first-graders.b. An assessment of the effectiveness of weekly tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g sessions on the read<strong>in</strong>gability of third-graders.For Discussion


296 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ec. A comparison of a third-period high school English class taught by the discussionmethod with a third-period (same high school) English class taught by the lecturemethod.d. The effectiveness of re<strong>in</strong>forcement <strong>in</strong> decreas<strong>in</strong>g stutter<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> students with thisspeech defect.e. The effects of a year-long weight-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g program on a group of high schoolathletes.f. The possible effects of age, gender, <strong>and</strong> method on student lik<strong>in</strong>g for his<strong>to</strong>ry.5. What flaw can you f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g studies?a. A teacher tries out a new mathematics textbook with her class for a semester. At theend of the semester, she reports that the class’s <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> mathematics is markedlyhigher than she has ever seen it <strong>in</strong> the past with other classes us<strong>in</strong>g another text.b. A teacher divides his class <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two subgroups, with each subgroup be<strong>in</strong>g taughtspell<strong>in</strong>g by a different method. Each group listens <strong>to</strong> the teacher <strong>in</strong>struct the othergroup while they wait their turn.c. A researcher calls for eighth-grade students <strong>to</strong> volunteer <strong>to</strong> tu<strong>to</strong>r third-grade studentswho are hav<strong>in</strong>g difficulty <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. She compares their effectiveness astu<strong>to</strong>rs with a control group of students who are assigned <strong>to</strong> be tu<strong>to</strong>rs (they do notvolunteer). The students of the volunteers have a much greater level of improvement<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g than the students who were assigned <strong>to</strong> tu<strong>to</strong>r.d. A teacher decides <strong>to</strong> try out a new textbook <strong>in</strong> one of her social studies classes.She uses it for four weeks <strong>and</strong> then compares this class’s scores on a unit test withthe scores of her previous classes. All classes are study<strong>in</strong>g the same material.Dur<strong>in</strong>g the unit test, however, a fire drill occurs, <strong>and</strong> the class loses about 10 m<strong>in</strong>utesof the time allotted for the test.e. Two groups of third-graders are compared with regard <strong>to</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g ability, subsequent<strong>to</strong> different tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g schedules. One group is tested dur<strong>in</strong>g physical educationclass <strong>in</strong> the school gymnasium, while the other is tested after school on thefootball field.f. A researcher compares a third-period English class with a fifth-period chemistryclass <strong>in</strong> terms of student <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the subject taught. The English class is taught bythe discussion method, while the chemistry class is taught by the lecture method.Notes1. C. A. Benware <strong>and</strong> E. L. Deci (1984). Quality of learn<strong>in</strong>g with an active versus passive motivational set.American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 21(4): 755–766.2. R. T. Johnson, D. W. Johnson, <strong>and</strong> M. B. Stanne (1986). Comparison of computer-assisted cooperative,competitive, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualistic learn<strong>in</strong>g. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 23(3): 382–392.3. J. S. Catterall (1987). An <strong>in</strong>tensive group counsel<strong>in</strong>g dropout prevention <strong>in</strong>tervention: Some cautions onisolat<strong>in</strong>g at-risk adolescents with<strong>in</strong> high schools. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 24(4): 521–540.4. A. G. Gilmore <strong>and</strong> C. W. McK<strong>in</strong>ney (1986). The effects of student questions <strong>and</strong> teacher questions on conceptacquisition. Theory <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social <strong>Education</strong>, 14(3): 225–244.5. J. D. Hawk<strong>in</strong>s, H. J. Doueck, <strong>and</strong> D. M. Lishner (1988). Chang<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g practices <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream classrooms<strong>to</strong> improve bond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> behavior of low achievers. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 25(1):31–50.6. J. R. Lev<strong>in</strong>, C. B. McCormick, G. E. Miller, J. K. Berry, <strong>and</strong> M. Pressley (1982). Mnemonic versusnonmnemonic vocabulary-learn<strong>in</strong>g strategies for children. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 19(1):121–136.7. B. W. Tuckman (1994). Conduct<strong>in</strong>g educational research, 4th ed. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,p. 152.


CHAPTER 13 Experimental <strong>Research</strong> 297<strong>Research</strong> Exercise 13: <strong>Research</strong> MethodologyUs<strong>in</strong>g Problem Sheet 13, once aga<strong>in</strong> state the question or hypothesis of your study <strong>and</strong> identifywhich methodology you plan <strong>to</strong> use. Then describe, <strong>in</strong> as much detail as you can, theprocedures of your study, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g analysis of results—that is, what you <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> do, when,where, <strong>and</strong> how. Lastly, <strong>in</strong>dicate any unresolved problems you see at this po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> yourplann<strong>in</strong>g.Problem Sheet 13<strong>Research</strong> MethodologyYou should complete Problem Sheet 13 once you have decided which of the methodologiesdescribed <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13–17 <strong>and</strong> 19–24 you plan <strong>to</strong> use. You might wish <strong>to</strong>consider, however, whether your research question could be <strong>in</strong>vestigated by othermethodologies.1. The question or hypothesis of my study is: __________________________________An electronic versionof this Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>pr<strong>in</strong>t, save, or e-mail isavailable on the Onl<strong>in</strong>eLearn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e.2. The methodology I <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> use is: _______________________________________3. A brief summary of what I <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> do, when, where, <strong>and</strong> how is as follows: _______A full-sized version ofthis Problem Sheetthat you can fill <strong>in</strong> orpho<strong>to</strong>copy is <strong>in</strong> yourStudent MasteryActivities book.4. The major problems I foresee at this po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g: ________________


14S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>Essential Characteristicsof S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject<strong>Research</strong>S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>sThe Graph<strong>in</strong>g of S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>sThe A-B <strong>Design</strong>The A-B-A <strong>Design</strong>The A-B-A-B <strong>Design</strong>The B-A-B <strong>Design</strong>The A-B-C-B <strong>Design</strong>Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong>sThreats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject<strong>Research</strong>Control of Threats <strong>to</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>External Validity <strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>:The Importance ofReplicationOther S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>sAn Example ofS<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData Analysis/ResultsDiscussion/IntrepretationNumber of cigarettes smoked by C<strong>in</strong>dy Adams454035302520151050Basel<strong>in</strong>e Nicot<strong>in</strong>e patch Basel<strong>in</strong>e0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16DaysOBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly the purpose of s<strong>in</strong>glesubjectresearch.Describe the essential characteristics ofsuch research.Describe two ways <strong>in</strong> which s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectresearch differs from other forms ofexperimental research.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a basel<strong>in</strong>e is <strong>and</strong> why it isused.Expla<strong>in</strong> what an A-B design is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a reversal (A-B-A) design is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what an A-B-A-B design is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a B-A-B design is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what an A-B-C-B design is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>edesign is.Identify various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validityassociated with s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies.Expla<strong>in</strong> three ways <strong>in</strong> which threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studiescan be controlled.Discuss the external validity of s<strong>in</strong>glesubjectresearch.Critique research articles that <strong>in</strong>volves<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Centerat www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Essential Characteristics of S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 14.1: S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 14.2: Characteristics of S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>Activity 14.3: Analyze Some S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject DataJasm<strong>in</strong> Wong, a third-grade teacher <strong>in</strong> a small elementary school <strong>in</strong> San Diego, California, f<strong>in</strong>ds her teach<strong>in</strong>gcont<strong>in</strong>ually <strong>in</strong>terrupted by Alex, a student who can’t seem <strong>to</strong> keep quiet. Distressed, she asks herself what shemight do <strong>to</strong> control this student <strong>and</strong> wonders whether some k<strong>in</strong>d of “time-out” activity might work. With this <strong>in</strong>m<strong>in</strong>d, she asks some of the other teachers <strong>in</strong> the school whether brief periods of remov<strong>in</strong>g Alex from the class mightdecrease the frequency of his disruptive behavior.This question is exactly the sort that can be answered best by means of a s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject A-B-A-B design. Inthis chapter, you will learn what an A-B-A-B design <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>and</strong> how it works, as well as some other ideas abouts<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research.Essential Characteristicsof S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>All of the designs described <strong>in</strong> the previous chapter onexperimental research <strong>in</strong>volve the study of groups. Attimes, however, group designs are not appropriate for aresearcher <strong>to</strong> use, particularly when the usual <strong>in</strong>strumentsare not pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>and</strong> observation must be themethod of data collection. Sometimes there are just notenough subjects available <strong>to</strong> make the use of a group designpractical. In other cases, <strong>in</strong>tensive data collectionon a very few <strong>in</strong>dividuals makes more sense. <strong>Research</strong>erswho wish <strong>to</strong> study children who suffer frommultiple h<strong>and</strong>icaps (who are both deaf <strong>and</strong> bl<strong>in</strong>d, for example)may have only a small number of children available<strong>to</strong> them, say six or less. It would make little sense<strong>to</strong> form two groups of three each <strong>in</strong> such an <strong>in</strong>stance.Further, each child would probably need <strong>to</strong> be observed<strong>in</strong> great detail.S<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs are adaptations of the basictime-series design shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 13.9 <strong>in</strong> the previouschapter. The difference is that data are collected <strong>and</strong>analyzed for only one subject at a time. They are mostcommonly used <strong>to</strong> study the changes <strong>in</strong> behavior an <strong>in</strong>dividualexhibits after exposure <strong>to</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tervention ortreatment of some sort. Developed primarily <strong>in</strong> specialeducation, where much of the usual <strong>in</strong>strumentation is<strong>in</strong>appropriate, s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs have been used byresearchers <strong>to</strong> demonstrate that children with Downsyndrome, for example, are capable of far more complexlearn<strong>in</strong>g than was previously believed.*S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>sTHE GRAPHINGOF SINGLE-SUBJECT DESIGNSS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject researchers primarily use l<strong>in</strong>e graphs <strong>to</strong>present their data <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> illustrate the effects of a particular<strong>in</strong>tervention or treatment. Figure 14.1 presents anillustration of such a graph. The dependent (outcome)variable is displayed on the vertical axis (the ord<strong>in</strong>ate,or y-axis). For example, if we were teach<strong>in</strong>g a self-helpskill <strong>to</strong> a severely limited child, the number of correctresponses would be shown on the vertical axis.The horizontal axis (the abscissa, or x-axis) is used<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate a sequence of time, such as sessions, days,weeks, trials, or months. As a rough rule of thumb, thehorizontal axis should be anywhere from one <strong>and</strong> onehalf<strong>to</strong> two times as long as the vertical axis.*Increas<strong>in</strong>gly, s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs are be<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dsof studies where the unit of observation is a s<strong>in</strong>gle group rather thana s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual.299


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>For a long time, it was thought that there were many th<strong>in</strong>gs,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependent liv<strong>in</strong>g skills, that severely <strong>in</strong>tellectuallylimited or emotionally disturbed children <strong>and</strong> adultscould not be expected <strong>to</strong> learn. A series of studies <strong>in</strong> the 1960s,however, proved that they could learn a great deal throughprocedures known orig<strong>in</strong>ally as operant condition<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>more recently as behavior management or applied behavioralanalysis.* Recent studies have ref<strong>in</strong>ed these methods.†*G. J. Bensberg, C. N. Colwell, <strong>and</strong> R. H. Cassel (1965). Teach<strong>in</strong>gthe profoundly retarded self-help activities by behavior shap<strong>in</strong>gtechniques. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 69: 674–679;O. I. Lovaas, L. Freitag, K. Nelson, <strong>and</strong> C. Whalen (1967). Theestablishment of imitation <strong>and</strong> its use for the development ofcomplex behavior <strong>in</strong> schizophrenic children. Behavior <strong>Research</strong><strong>and</strong> Therapy, 5: 171–181.†M. Wolery, D. B. Bailey, <strong>and</strong> G. Sugai (1998). Effective teach<strong>in</strong>gpr<strong>in</strong>ciples <strong>and</strong> practices of applied behavior analysis withexceptional students. Needham, MA: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon.Condition identificationsIndependent variable10Basel<strong>in</strong>eOrd<strong>in</strong>ateSelf-help <strong>in</strong>structionCondition change l<strong>in</strong>eDependentmeasureFrequency of correct responses86420Data pathsData po<strong>in</strong>tsAbscissa0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12Unit of timeSessionsMeasure of timePlace the first po<strong>in</strong>t above the abscissa.Figure 14.1 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject GraphPercentage of correct responses across basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> self-help conditionsA description of the conditions <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the studyis listed just above the graph. The first condition is usuallythe basel<strong>in</strong>e, followed by the <strong>in</strong>tervention (the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable). Condition l<strong>in</strong>es, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g whenthe condition has changed, separate the conditions. Thedots are data po<strong>in</strong>ts. They represent the data collected atvarious times dur<strong>in</strong>g the study. They are placed on thegraph by f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tersection of the time when thedata po<strong>in</strong>t was collected (e.g., session 6) <strong>and</strong> the resultsat that time (six correct responses). These data po<strong>in</strong>tsare then connected <strong>to</strong> illustrate trends <strong>in</strong> the data. Lastly,there is a figure caption near the bot<strong>to</strong>m of the graph,which is a summary of the figure, usually list<strong>in</strong>g boththe <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> the dependent variables.THE A-B DESIGNThe basic approach of researchers us<strong>in</strong>g an A-B design is<strong>to</strong> collect data on the same subject, operat<strong>in</strong>g as his or herown control, under two conditions or phases. The first300


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 301condition is the pretreatment condition, typically called(as mentioned before) the basel<strong>in</strong>e period, <strong>and</strong> identifiedas A. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the basel<strong>in</strong>e period, the subject is assessedfor several sessions until it appears that his or her typicalbehavior has been reliably determ<strong>in</strong>ed. The basel<strong>in</strong>e isextremely important <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research s<strong>in</strong>ce it isthe best estimate of what would have occurred if the <strong>in</strong>terventionwere not applied. Enough data po<strong>in</strong>ts must beobta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e a clear picture of the exist<strong>in</strong>g condition;certa<strong>in</strong>ly one should collect a m<strong>in</strong>imum of threedata po<strong>in</strong>ts before implement<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Thebasel<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>in</strong> effect, provides a comparison <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terventioncondition.Once the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition has been established, atreatment or <strong>in</strong>tervention condition, identified as B, is<strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed for a period of time. Typically,though not necessarily, a highly specific behavioris taught dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition, with the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rserv<strong>in</strong>g as the data collec<strong>to</strong>r—usually by record<strong>in</strong>gthe number of correct responses (e.g., answers <strong>to</strong>questions) or behaviors (e.g., look<strong>in</strong>g at the teacher)given by the subject dur<strong>in</strong>g a fixed number of trials.As an example of an A-B design, consider a researcher<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of verbal praise on aparticularly nonresponsive junior high school studentdur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> mathematics. The researcher couldobserve the student’s behavior for, say, five days while<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> math is occurr<strong>in</strong>g, then praise him verballyfor five sessions <strong>and</strong> observe his behavior immediatelyafter the praise. Figure 14.2 illustrates this A-B design.As you can see, five measures were taken before the<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>and</strong> five more dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Look<strong>in</strong>gat the data <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.2, the <strong>in</strong>tervention appears <strong>to</strong>have been effective. The amount of responsiveness afterthe <strong>in</strong>tervention (the praise) <strong>in</strong>creased markedly. <strong>How</strong>ever,there is a major problem with the A-B design.Similar <strong>to</strong> the one-shot case study that it resembles, theresearcher does not know whether any behavior changeoccurred because of the treatment. It is possible that someother variable (other than praise) actually caused thechange, or even that the change would have occurred naturally,without any treatment at all. Thus the A-B designfails <strong>to</strong> control for various threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity; itdoes not determ<strong>in</strong>e the effect of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable(praise) on the dependent variable (responsiveness) whilerul<strong>in</strong>g out the possible effect(s) of extraneous variables.As a result, researchers usually try <strong>to</strong> improve on the A-Bdesign by us<strong>in</strong>g an A-B-A design.**Another option is <strong>to</strong> replicate this design with additional <strong>in</strong>dividualswith treatment beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g at different times, thereby reduc<strong>in</strong>g thelikelihood that the passage of time or other conditions are responsiblefor changes.87ABasel<strong>in</strong>eBPraise6Frequency of response5432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12DaysFrequency of response across basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> praise conditionsFigure 14.2 A-B <strong>Design</strong>


302 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTHE A-B-A DESIGNWhen us<strong>in</strong>g an A-B-A design (sometimes called reversaldesigns), researchers simply add another basel<strong>in</strong>eperiod. This improves the design considerably. If thebehavior dur<strong>in</strong>g the treatment period differs fromthe behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g either basel<strong>in</strong>e period, we havestronger evidence for the effectiveness of the <strong>in</strong>tervention.In our previous example, the researcher, afterprais<strong>in</strong>g the student for say, five days, could elim<strong>in</strong>atethe praise <strong>and</strong> observe the student’s behavior for anotherfive days with no praise. This would reducethreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, because it is unlikely thatsometh<strong>in</strong>g would occur at the precise time the <strong>in</strong>terventionis presented <strong>to</strong> cause an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the behavior<strong>and</strong> at the precise time the <strong>in</strong>tervention is removed <strong>to</strong>cause a decrease <strong>in</strong> the behavior. Figure 14.3 illustratesthe A-B-A design.Although the decrease <strong>in</strong> threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validityis a def<strong>in</strong>ite advantage of the A-B-A design, thereis a significant ethical disadvantage <strong>to</strong> this design: It<strong>in</strong>volves leav<strong>in</strong>g the subjects <strong>in</strong> the A condition.Many researchers would feel uncomfortable aboutend<strong>in</strong>g this type of study without some degree of f<strong>in</strong>alimprovement be<strong>in</strong>g shown. As a result, an extensionof this design—the A-B-A-B design, is frequentlyused.THE A-B-A-B DESIGNIn the A-B-A-B design, two basel<strong>in</strong>e periods are comb<strong>in</strong>edwith two treatment periods. This further strengthensany conclusion about the effectiveness of the treatment,because it permits the effectiveness of the treatment<strong>to</strong> be demonstrated twice. In fact, the second treatmentcan be extended <strong>in</strong>def<strong>in</strong>itely if a researcher so desires. Ifthe behavior of the subject is essentially the same dur<strong>in</strong>gboth treatment phases <strong>and</strong> better (or worse) than bothbasel<strong>in</strong>e periods, the likelihood of another variable be<strong>in</strong>gthe cause of the change is decreased markedly. Anotheradvantage here is evident—the ethical problem of leav<strong>in</strong>gthe subject(s) without an <strong>in</strong>tervention is avoided.To implement an A-B-A-B design <strong>in</strong> the previous example,the researcher would re<strong>in</strong>state the experimenttreatment, B (praise), for five days after the secondbasel<strong>in</strong>e period <strong>and</strong> observe the subject’s behavior. Aswith the A-B-A design, the researcher hopes <strong>to</strong> demonstratethat the dependent variable (responsiveness)changes whenever the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable (praise) isapplied. If the subject’s behavior changes from the firstbasel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong> the first treatment period, from the first treatment<strong>to</strong> the second basel<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> so on, the researcherhas evidence that praise is <strong>in</strong>deed the cause of thechange. Figure 14.4 illustrates the results of a hypotheticalstudy <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g an A-B-A-B design.87ABasel<strong>in</strong>eBPraiseABasel<strong>in</strong>e6Frequency of response5432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15DaysFrequency of response across basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> praise conditionsFigure 14.3 A-B-A <strong>Design</strong>


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 30387ABasel<strong>in</strong>eBPraiseABasel<strong>in</strong>eBPraise6Frequency of response5432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21DaysFrequency of response across basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> praise conditionsFigure 14.4 A-B-A-B <strong>Design</strong>Notice that a clear basel<strong>in</strong>e is established, followed byan <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> response dur<strong>in</strong>g treatment, followed by adecrease <strong>in</strong> response when treatment is s<strong>to</strong>pped, followedby an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> response once the treatment is <strong>in</strong>stitutedaga<strong>in</strong>. This pattern provides fairly strong evidence that itis the treatment, rather than his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, or someth<strong>in</strong>gelse, that is responsible for the improvement.Although evidence such as that shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.4would be considered a strong argument for causation, youshould be aware that the A-B-A <strong>and</strong> A-B-A-B designssuffer from limitations: The possibility of data-collec<strong>to</strong>rbias (the <strong>in</strong>dividual who is giv<strong>in</strong>g the treatment also usuallycollects the data) <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>strumentation effect (theneed for an extensive number of data collection periods)can lead <strong>to</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> the conditions of data collection.THE B-A-B DESIGNOccasionally there are times when an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavioris so severe or disturb<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., excessive fight<strong>in</strong>gboth <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> outside of class) that a researcher cannotwait for a basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong> be established. In such cases, aB-A-B design may be used. This design <strong>in</strong>volves atreatment followed by a basel<strong>in</strong>e followed by a return <strong>to</strong>the treatment. This design is also appropriate whenthere is a lack of behavior—for example, if the subjectshave never exhibited the desired (e.g., pay<strong>in</strong>g attention)behaviors <strong>in</strong> the past—or when an <strong>in</strong>tervention is alreadyongo<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., an after-school detention program)<strong>and</strong> a researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> establish its effect. Figure14.5 illustrates the B-A-B design.THE A-B-C-B DESIGNThe A-B-C-B design is a further modification of theA-B-A design. The C <strong>in</strong> this design refers <strong>to</strong> a variationof the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> the B condition. In the firsttwo conditions, the basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention data arecollected. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the C condition, the <strong>in</strong>tervention ischanged <strong>to</strong> control for any extra attention the subjectmay have received dur<strong>in</strong>g the B phase. For example, <strong>in</strong>our earlier example, one might argue that it was notthe praise that was responsible for any improved responsiveness(should that occur) on the part of thesubject, but rather the extra attention that the subjectreceived.The C condition, therefore, might be praise given nomatter how the subject responds (i.e., whether he offersresponses or not). Thus, as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.6, a conclusioncould be reached that cont<strong>in</strong>gent (or selective)praise is critical for improved responsiveness, as compared<strong>to</strong> the mere <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> overall praise.


304 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e(Unlikely that an extraneous variable causedthe observed changes at these two po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time)Number of fights32302826242220181614121086420BAfter-schooldetentionABasel<strong>in</strong>e0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16WeeksBAfter-schooldetentionNumber of fights across after-school detention conditionsFigure 14.5 B-A-B <strong>Design</strong>(Likely that cont<strong>in</strong>gent praisecaused the observed changes)1009080ABasel<strong>in</strong>eB C BNoncont<strong>in</strong>gentpraiseCont<strong>in</strong>gentpraiseCont<strong>in</strong>gentpraiseFrequency of response7060504030201000 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20WeeksFrequency of response across basel<strong>in</strong>e, cont<strong>in</strong>gent praise, <strong>and</strong> noncont<strong>in</strong>gentpraise conditionsFigure 14.6 A-B-C-B <strong>Design</strong>


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 305MULTIPLE-BASELINE DESIGNSAn alternative <strong>to</strong> the A-B-A-B design is the multiplebasel<strong>in</strong>edesign. Multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designs are typicallyused when it is not possible or ethical <strong>to</strong> withdrawa treatment <strong>and</strong> return <strong>to</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition. Whenus<strong>in</strong>g a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design, researchers do morethan collect data on one behavior for one subject <strong>in</strong> onesett<strong>in</strong>g; they collect on several behaviors for one subject,obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a basel<strong>in</strong>e for each dur<strong>in</strong>g the same periodof time.When us<strong>in</strong>g a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design across behaviors,the researcher systematically applies the treatmentat different times for each behavior until all of them areundergo<strong>in</strong>g the treatment. If behavior changes <strong>in</strong> eachcase only after the treatment has been applied, the treatmentis judged <strong>to</strong> be the cause of the change. It is importantthat the behaviors be<strong>in</strong>g treated, however, rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>dependent of each other. If behavior 2, for example, isaffected by the <strong>in</strong>troduction of the treatment <strong>to</strong> behavior1, then the effectiveness of the treatment cannot be determ<strong>in</strong>ed.A diagram of a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gthree behaviors is shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.7.In this design, treatment is applied first <strong>to</strong> changebehavior 1, then behavior 2, <strong>and</strong> then behavior 3 untilall three behaviors are undergo<strong>in</strong>g the treatment. Forexample, a researcher might <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effects of“time-out” (remov<strong>in</strong>g a student from class activities fora period of time) on decreas<strong>in</strong>g various undesirable behaviorsof a particular student. Suppose the behaviorsare (a) talk<strong>in</strong>g out of turn; (b) tear<strong>in</strong>g up worksheets;<strong>and</strong> (c) mak<strong>in</strong>g deroga<strong>to</strong>ry remarks <strong>to</strong>ward another student.The researcher beg<strong>in</strong>s by apply<strong>in</strong>g the treatment(“time-out”) first <strong>to</strong> behavior 1, then <strong>to</strong> behavior 2, <strong>and</strong>then <strong>to</strong> behavior 3. At that po<strong>in</strong>t, the treatment will havebeen applied <strong>to</strong> all three behaviors. The more behaviorsthat are elim<strong>in</strong>ated or reduced, the more effective thetreatment can be judged <strong>to</strong> be. <strong>How</strong> many times the researchermust apply the treatment is a matter of judgment<strong>and</strong> depends on the subjects <strong>in</strong>volved, the sett<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> the behaviors the researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> decrease orelim<strong>in</strong>ate (or encourage). Multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designs alsoare sometimes used <strong>to</strong> collect data on several subjectswith regard <strong>to</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle behavior, or <strong>to</strong> measure a subject’sbehavior <strong>in</strong> two or more different sett<strong>in</strong>gs.Figure 14.8 illustrates the effects of a treatment <strong>in</strong> ahypothetical study us<strong>in</strong>g a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design.Notice that each of the behaviors changed only whenthe treatment was <strong>in</strong>troduced. Figure 14.9 illustrates thedesign applied <strong>to</strong> different sett<strong>in</strong>gs.In practice, the results of the studies described hererarely fit the ideal model <strong>in</strong> that the data po<strong>in</strong>ts oftenshow more fluctuation, mak<strong>in</strong>g trends less clear-cut.This feature makes data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias even more of aproblem, particularly when the behavior <strong>in</strong> question ismore complex than just a simple response such as pick<strong>in</strong>gup an object. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias <strong>in</strong> multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>estudies rema<strong>in</strong>s a serious concern.Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>As we mentioned earlier, there are unfortunately severalthreats <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies.Some of the most important <strong>in</strong>volve the length of thebasel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention conditions, the number ofvariables changed when mov<strong>in</strong>g from one condition <strong>to</strong>another, the degree <strong>and</strong> speed of any change that occurs,a return—or not—of the behavior <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e levels, the<strong>in</strong>dependence of behaviors, <strong>and</strong> the number of basel<strong>in</strong>es.Let us discuss each of these <strong>in</strong> more detail.Condition Length. Condition length refers <strong>to</strong> howlong the basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention conditions are <strong>in</strong> effect.It is essentially the number of data po<strong>in</strong>ts gathereddur<strong>in</strong>g a condition. A researcher must have enough datapo<strong>in</strong>ts (a m<strong>in</strong>imum of three) <strong>to</strong> establish a clear patternor trend. Take a look at Figure 14.10(a) on page 308. Thedata shown <strong>in</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition appear <strong>to</strong> be stable,<strong>and</strong> hence it would be appropriate for the researcher <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>troduce the <strong>in</strong>tervention. In Figure 14.10(b), the datapo<strong>in</strong>ts appear <strong>to</strong> be mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a direction opposite <strong>to</strong>Behavior 1Behavior 2Behavior 3OOOOOOOOOO X O XO X O X O X O X O X O X O X OO O O O X O X O X O X O X O X O X OO O O O O O O X O X O X O X O X OFigure 14.7Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e<strong>Design</strong>


306 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 14.8 Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong>Behavior 1f9876543210Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14Behavior 2f9876543210Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14Behavior 39Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment876f 5432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14Data Collection Periodsthat which is desired, <strong>and</strong> hence here <strong>to</strong>o it would beappropriate for the researcher <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduce the <strong>in</strong>tervention.In Figure 14.10(c), the data po<strong>in</strong>ts vary greatly; notrend has been established, <strong>and</strong> hence the researchershould stay <strong>in</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition for a longer periodof time. Note that the data po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.10(d) appear<strong>to</strong> be mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the same direction as that which isdesired. If the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition were <strong>to</strong> be ended atthis time <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>troduced, the effects ofthe <strong>in</strong>tervention might be difficult <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e.In the real world, of course, it is often difficult <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>enough data po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>to</strong> see a clear trend. Often thereare practical problems such as a need <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> the studydue <strong>to</strong> a shortage of time or an ethical concern such as asubject display<strong>in</strong>g very dangerous behavior. Nevertheless,the stability of data po<strong>in</strong>ts must always be taken<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account by those who conduct (<strong>and</strong> those who read)s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies.Number of Variables Changed WhenMov<strong>in</strong>g from One Condition <strong>to</strong> Another.This is one of the most important considerations <strong>in</strong>s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research. It is important that only onevariable be changed at a time when mov<strong>in</strong>g from onecondition <strong>to</strong> another. For <strong>in</strong>stance, consider our previousexample <strong>in</strong> which a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the effects of time-out on decreas<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong>undesirable behaviors of a student. The researchershould take care that the only treatment she <strong>in</strong>troducesdur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition is the time-out experience.This is chang<strong>in</strong>g only one variable. If the researcherwere <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduce not only the time-out experience


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 30710090807060504030Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatmentPercentage of time-on-task20100100908070605040302010Condition l<strong>in</strong>e is staggeredLibraryGeneral classroom00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13SessionsPercentage of time-on-task across basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> treatmentconditions across library <strong>and</strong> classroom sett<strong>in</strong>gsFigure 14.9 Multiple-Basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>Design</strong> Applied <strong>to</strong> Different Sett<strong>in</strong>gsbut also another experience (e.g., counsel<strong>in</strong>g the studentdur<strong>in</strong>g the time-out), she would be chang<strong>in</strong>g two variables.In effect, the treatment would be confounded. The<strong>in</strong>tervention would now consist of two variables mixed<strong>to</strong>gether. Unfortunately, the only th<strong>in</strong>g the researchercould now conclude would be whether the comb<strong>in</strong>edtreatment was or was not effective. He or she would notknow if it was the counsel<strong>in</strong>g or the time-out that wasthe cause. Thus, when analyz<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject design,it is always important <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether only


308 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e98Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment98Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment77Frequency of fights6543Frequency of fights65432211(a)00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11Weeks(b)00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11Weeks9879Basel<strong>in</strong>e Treatment Basel<strong>in</strong>e Treatment87Frequency of fights6543Frequency of fights6543221100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11Weeks(c)Figure 14.10 Variations <strong>in</strong> Basel<strong>in</strong>e Stability(d)00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11Weeksone variable at a time has been changed. If this is not thecase, any conclusions that are drawn may be erroneous.Degree <strong>and</strong> Speed of Change. <strong>Research</strong>ersmust also take <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account the magnitude with which thedata change at the time the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition isimplemented (i.e., when the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is<strong>in</strong>troduced or removed). Look, for example, at Figure14.11(a). The basel<strong>in</strong>e condition reveals that the datahave stability. When the <strong>in</strong>tervention is <strong>in</strong>troduced, however,the subject’s behavior does not change for a periodof three sessions. This does not <strong>in</strong>dicate a very strong experimentaleffect. If the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable (whatever itmay be) were effective, one would assume that the subject’sbehavior would have changed more quickly. It ispossible, of course, that the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable was effective,but not of sufficient strength <strong>to</strong> br<strong>in</strong>g about an immediatechange (or the behavior may have been resistant<strong>to</strong> change). Nevertheless, researchers must consider allsuch possibilities if there is a slow or delayed changeonce the <strong>in</strong>tervention is <strong>in</strong>troduced. Figure 14.11(b) <strong>in</strong>dicatesthere was a fairly immediate change but that it wasof small magnitude. Only <strong>in</strong> Figure 14.11(c) do we see adramatic <strong>and</strong> rapid change once the <strong>in</strong>tervention was <strong>in</strong>troduced.A researcher would be more likely <strong>to</strong> concludethat the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable was effective <strong>in</strong> this casethan he or she would <strong>in</strong> either of the other two.Return <strong>to</strong> Basel<strong>in</strong>e Level. Look at Figure 14.12(a).Notice that <strong>in</strong> return<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition, therewas not a rapid change <strong>in</strong> behavior. This suggests thatsometh<strong>in</strong>g else may have occurred when the <strong>in</strong>terventioncondition was <strong>in</strong>troduced. We would expect that thebehavior of the subject would have returned <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>elevels fairly quickly if the <strong>in</strong>tervention had been thecausal fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g the subject’s behavior. Thefact that the subject’s behavior did not return <strong>to</strong> theorig<strong>in</strong>al basel<strong>in</strong>e level suggests that one or more extraneousvariables may have produced the effects observeddur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition. On the other h<strong>and</strong>,


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 309Frequency of fightsFrequency of fightsFrequency of fights9876543210Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9Weeks(a)9Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment8765432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9Weeks(b)9Basel<strong>in</strong>eTreatment8765432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9Weeks(c)Figure 14.11 Differences <strong>in</strong> Degree <strong>and</strong> Speedof Change10 1110 1110 11look at Figure 14.12(b). Here we see that the changefrom <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e levels was abrupt <strong>and</strong>rapid. This suggests that the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable waslikely the cause of the changes <strong>in</strong> the dependent variable.Note, however, that, because the treatment was <strong>in</strong>tended<strong>to</strong> have a last<strong>in</strong>g impact, a slower return <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>emay have been desirable, though it would havecomplicated <strong>in</strong>terpretation.Independence of Behaviors. This concern ismost applicable <strong>to</strong> multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e studies. Imag<strong>in</strong>efor a moment that a researcher is <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g variousmethods of teach<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>ry. The researcher def<strong>in</strong>es twoseparate behaviors that she is go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> measure. These<strong>in</strong>clude (1) ability <strong>to</strong> locate the central idea, <strong>and</strong> (2) ability<strong>to</strong> summarize the important po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> various his<strong>to</strong>ricaldocuments. The researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s basel<strong>in</strong>e data foreach of these skills <strong>and</strong> then implements an <strong>in</strong>tervention(provid<strong>in</strong>g worksheets that give clues about how <strong>to</strong> locateimportant ideas <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical documents). The subject’sability <strong>to</strong> locate the central idea <strong>in</strong> a document improvesquickly <strong>and</strong> considerably. <strong>How</strong>ever, the subject’sability <strong>to</strong> summarize important po<strong>in</strong>ts also improves. Itis quite evident that these two skills are not <strong>in</strong>dependent.They appear <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>in</strong> some way, conceivablydependent on the same underly<strong>in</strong>g cognitive ability,<strong>and</strong> hence they improve <strong>to</strong>gether.Number of Basel<strong>in</strong>es. In order <strong>to</strong> have a multiplebasel<strong>in</strong>edesign, a researcher must have at least twobasel<strong>in</strong>es. Although the basel<strong>in</strong>es beg<strong>in</strong> at the same time,the <strong>in</strong>terventions are <strong>in</strong>troduced at different times. As wementioned earlier, the chances that an extraneous variablecaused the results when us<strong>in</strong>g a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>edesign across two behaviors are lessened, s<strong>in</strong>ce it is lesslikely that the same extraneous event caused the observedchanges for both behaviors at different times. Theprobability that an extraneous event caused the changes<strong>in</strong> a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design across three behaviors,therefore, is even less.Thus, the greater the number of basel<strong>in</strong>es, the greaterthe probability that the <strong>in</strong>tervention is the cause of anychanges <strong>in</strong> behavior, s<strong>in</strong>ce the likelihood that an extraneousvariable caused the changes is correspond<strong>in</strong>glydecreased the more behaviors we have.There is a problem with a large number of basel<strong>in</strong>es,however. The more basel<strong>in</strong>es there are, the longer thelater behaviors must rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e—that is, are keptfrom receiv<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention. For example, if we followthe recommendation mentioned earlier of establish<strong>in</strong>gstable data po<strong>in</strong>ts before we <strong>in</strong>troduce the <strong>in</strong>terventioncondition, this would mean that the first behavior is<strong>in</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e for a m<strong>in</strong>imum of three sessions, the secondfor six sessions, <strong>and</strong> the third for n<strong>in</strong>e. Should we usefour basel<strong>in</strong>es, the fourth behavior would be <strong>in</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>econdition for 12 sessions! This is a very long time for abehavior <strong>to</strong> be kept from receiv<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention. As ageneral rule, however, it is important <strong>to</strong> remember thatthe fewer the number of basel<strong>in</strong>es, the less likely we can


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHExamples of Studies ConductedUs<strong>in</strong>g S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Design</strong>sDeterm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the collateral effects of peer tu<strong>to</strong>r-tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ona student with severe disabilities (an A-B design).*Effects of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> rapid decod<strong>in</strong>g on the read<strong>in</strong>g comprehensionof adult learners (an A-B-A design).†*R. C. Martella, N. E. March<strong>and</strong>-Martella, K. R. Young, <strong>and</strong> C. A.McFarl<strong>and</strong> (1995). Behavior Modification, 19: 170–191.†A. Tan, D. W. Moore, R. S. Dixon, <strong>and</strong> T. Nichelson (1994). Journalof Behavioral <strong>Education</strong>, 4: 177–189.Self-record<strong>in</strong>g: Effects on reduc<strong>in</strong>g off-task behavior witha high school student with an attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder (an A-B-A-B design).‡Assess<strong>in</strong>g the acquisition of first-aid treatments byelementary-aged children (a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e across subjectsdesign).§The effects of a self-management procedure on the classroom<strong>and</strong> academic behavior of students with mild h<strong>and</strong>icaps(a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e across sett<strong>in</strong>gs design). ||‡K. G. Stewart <strong>and</strong> T. F. McLaughl<strong>in</strong>. (1992). Child <strong>and</strong> FamilyBehavior Therapy 14(3): 53–59.§N. E. March<strong>and</strong>-Martella, R. C. Martella, M. Agran, <strong>and</strong> K. R.Young (1991). Child <strong>and</strong> Family Behavior Therapy 13(4): 29–43.|| D. J. Smith, J. R. Nelson, K. R. Young, <strong>and</strong> R. P. West (1992).School Psychology Review, 21: 59–72.987Basel<strong>in</strong>e Intervention Basel<strong>in</strong>e 987Basel<strong>in</strong>e Intervention Basel<strong>in</strong>eFrequency of fights654321Frequency of fights65432100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9Weeks(a)10 11 12 13 14Figure 14.12 Differences <strong>in</strong> Return <strong>to</strong> Basel<strong>in</strong>e Conditions00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9Weeks(b)10 11 12 13 14conclude that it is the <strong>in</strong>tervention rather than some othervariable that causes any changes <strong>in</strong> behavior.CONTROL OF THREATS TO INTERNALVALIDITY IN SINGLE-SUBJECT RESEARCHS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs are most effective <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>gfor subject characteristics, mortality, test<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rythreats; they are less effective with location, data collec<strong>to</strong>rcharacteristics, maturation, <strong>and</strong> regression threats;<strong>and</strong> they are def<strong>in</strong>itely weak when it comes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumentdecay, data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, attitud<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> implementationthreats.A location threat is most often only a m<strong>in</strong>or threat <strong>in</strong>multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e studies, because the location where thetreatment is adm<strong>in</strong>istered is usually constant throughoutthe study. The same is true for data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics,although such characteristics can be a problem if thedata collec<strong>to</strong>r is changed over the course of the study.S<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs unfortunately do suffer from astrong likelihood of <strong>in</strong>strument decay <strong>and</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>rbias, s<strong>in</strong>ce data must be collected (usually by means ofobservations) over many trials, <strong>and</strong> the data collec<strong>to</strong>r canhardly be kept <strong>in</strong> the dark as <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tent of the study.Neither implementation nor attitud<strong>in</strong>al effect threatsare well controlled for <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research. Eitherimplementers or data collec<strong>to</strong>rs can, un<strong>in</strong>tentionally, dis<strong>to</strong>rtthe results of a study. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias is a particularproblem when the same person is both implementer(e.g., act<strong>in</strong>g as the teacher) <strong>and</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>r. A secondobserver, record<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependently, reduces this threatbut <strong>in</strong>creases the amount of staff needed <strong>to</strong> complete the310


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 311study. A test<strong>in</strong>g threat is usually not a threat, s<strong>in</strong>ce presumablythe subject cannot affect observational data.EXTERNAL VALIDITYIN SINGLE-SUBJECT RESEARCH:THE IMPORTANCE OF REPLICATIONS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies are weak when it comes <strong>to</strong> externalvalidity—i.e., generalizability. One would hardly advocateuse of a treatment shown <strong>to</strong> be effective with only onesubject! As a result, studies <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectdesigns that show a particular treatment <strong>to</strong> be effective<strong>in</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g behaviors must rely on replications—across<strong>in</strong>dividuals rather than groups—if such results are <strong>to</strong> befound worthy of generalization.OTHER SINGLE-SUBJECT DESIGNSThere are a variety of other, less used designs that fallwith<strong>in</strong> the s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject category. One is the multitreatmentdesign, which <strong>in</strong>troduces a different treatment<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> an A-B-A-B design (i.e., A-B-A-C-A). Thealternat<strong>in</strong>g-treatments design alternates two or moredifferent treatments after an <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e period (e.g.,A-B-C-B-C). A variation of this is illustrated <strong>in</strong> thestudy analysis <strong>in</strong> this chapter, which elim<strong>in</strong>ates thebasel<strong>in</strong>e, becom<strong>in</strong>g a B-C-B, B-C-B-C, or B-C-B-C-Bdesign. The multiprobe design differs from a multiplebasel<strong>in</strong>edesign only <strong>in</strong> that fewer data po<strong>in</strong>ts are used,<strong>in</strong> an attempt <strong>to</strong> reduce the data collection burden <strong>and</strong>avoid threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. F<strong>in</strong>ally, features of allthese designs can be comb<strong>in</strong>ed.*An Example ofS<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research followed by a critiqueof its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> ourcritique of the group comparison experimental researchstudy <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13, we use the concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong>earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.*For a more detailed discussion of various types of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectdesigns, see D. H. Barlow <strong>and</strong> M. Hersen (1984). S<strong>in</strong>gle-caseexperimental designs: Strategies for study<strong>in</strong>g behavior change, 2nded. New York: Pergamon Press.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(1) (Spr<strong>in</strong>g 2003): 35–46.Progress<strong>in</strong>g from Programmatic<strong>to</strong> Discovery <strong>Research</strong>: A Case Examplewith the Overjustification EffectHenry S. RoaneMarcus <strong>and</strong> Kennedy Krieger Institutes <strong>and</strong> Emory University School of Medic<strong>in</strong>eWayne W. FisherMarcus <strong>and</strong> Kennedy Krieger Institutes <strong>and</strong> Johns Hopk<strong>in</strong>s University School of Medic<strong>in</strong>eEr<strong>in</strong> M. McDonoughMarcus InstitutePurposePurposeDef<strong>in</strong>itionLiterature reviewScientific research progresses along planned (programmatic research) <strong>and</strong> unplanned(discovery research) paths. In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation, we attempted <strong>to</strong> conduct a s<strong>in</strong>glecaseevaluation of the overjustification effect (i.e., programmatic research). Results ofthe <strong>in</strong>itial analysis were contrary <strong>to</strong> the overjustification hypothesis <strong>in</strong> that removal ofthe reward cont<strong>in</strong>gency produced an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g. Based on this unexpectedf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g, we conducted subsequent analyses <strong>to</strong> further evaluate the mechanisms underly<strong>in</strong>gthese results (i.e., discovery research). Results of the additional analyses suggestedthat the reward cont<strong>in</strong>gency functioned as punishment (because the participant preferredthe task <strong>to</strong> the rewards) <strong>and</strong> that withdrawal of the cont<strong>in</strong>gency produced punishmentcontrast.DESCRIPTORS: autism, behavioral contrast, discovery research, overjustification,punishmentProgress <strong>in</strong> scientific research often advances on two different paths. Sometimes a researcherfollows a planned l<strong>in</strong>e of research <strong>in</strong> which specific hypotheses are tested (referred<strong>to</strong> as programmatic research; Mace, 1994). At other times, unplanned events or serendipi<strong>to</strong>usf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs occur that are <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g or noteworthy <strong>and</strong> that lead the researcher <strong>in</strong> a previouslyunforeseen direction (referred <strong>to</strong> as discovery research; Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, 1956). The current<strong>in</strong>vestigation started as a planned with<strong>in</strong>-subject analysis of the phenomenon referred <strong>to</strong>as the overjustification effect (programmatic research), but when the results were <strong>in</strong> direc<strong>to</strong>pposition <strong>to</strong> the overjustification hypothesis, we under<strong>to</strong>ok a different set of analyses <strong>in</strong>an attempt <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> this serendipi<strong>to</strong>us f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g (discovery research). In the rema<strong>in</strong>derof the <strong>in</strong>troduction, we review the relevant literature that led <strong>to</strong> our <strong>in</strong>itial analysis of theoverjustification effect <strong>and</strong> then review studies relevant <strong>to</strong> discovery research.The overjustification hypothesis, which is an often-cited criticism of reward-basedprograms, states that the delivery of extr<strong>in</strong>sic rewards decreases an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the behavior that produced the rewards (Greene & Lepper, 1974). For example,an <strong>in</strong>dividual may play guitar simply because it is a preferred activity. If the <strong>in</strong>dividualis subsequently paid for play<strong>in</strong>g the guitar, the overjustification hypothesis predictsthat guitar play<strong>in</strong>g will decrease when payment is no longer received. From a generalcognitive perspective, the use of the external reward may devalue the <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> the behavior <strong>in</strong> that the <strong>in</strong>dividual changes the concept of why he or she is engag<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> the response <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terprets the behavior as “work” rather than “pleasure” (see Deci,1971, for a more detailed discussion of this <strong>in</strong>terpretation).312


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 313It should be noted that the overjustification hypothesis does not predict whateffect the use of rewards will have on the target response (i.e., whether those rewardswill function as re<strong>in</strong>forcement <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease the future probability of the response). Inaddition, the nontechnical term reward is used <strong>to</strong> describe a preferred stimulus that ispresented cont<strong>in</strong>gent on a response with the goal of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the future occurrence ofthat response. By contrast, the term positive re<strong>in</strong>forcement is reserved for conditions <strong>in</strong>which cont<strong>in</strong>gent presentation of a stimulus actually produces an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the futureprobability of the target response. Unfortunately, most studies on the overjustificationeffect have been conducted us<strong>in</strong>g between-groups designs <strong>and</strong> arbitrarily determ<strong>in</strong>edrewards (Reitman, 1998), which do not allow a proper evaluation of whether the stimulifunctioned as positive re<strong>in</strong>forcers (rather than so-called rewards).Several <strong>in</strong>vestigations have been conducted <strong>to</strong> evaluate the validity of the overjustificationhypothesis <strong>and</strong> have produced mixed results. Deci (1971), for example, showedevidence of overjustification by compar<strong>in</strong>g the puzzle completion of two groups of participants.Follow<strong>in</strong>g basel<strong>in</strong>e observation, one group received a $1 reward for puzzlecompletion <strong>and</strong> the other group did not. For the reward group, puzzle completion decreasedbelow the <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e level follow<strong>in</strong>g cessation of the reward cont<strong>in</strong>gency,whereas stable levels of completion were observed for the control group. Greene <strong>and</strong>Lepper (1974) compared levels of color<strong>in</strong>g across three groups of children <strong>and</strong> found thatchildren who received a reward for color<strong>in</strong>g showed less <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> color<strong>in</strong>g once the rewardcont<strong>in</strong>gency was removed relative <strong>to</strong> children who were never <strong>to</strong>ld that they wouldreceive a reward.By contrast, Vasta <strong>and</strong> Stirpe (1979) showed evidence that did not support theoverjustification hypothesis. FIrst, basel<strong>in</strong>e data were collected on worksheet completionfor two groups of children. Follow<strong>in</strong>g basel<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>to</strong>ken delivery was <strong>in</strong>itiated with onegroup. This resulted <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the target response, however, participants <strong>in</strong> theexperimental group returned <strong>to</strong> their <strong>in</strong>itial response levels dur<strong>in</strong>g the reversal <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e.That is, no evidence of the overjustification effect was obta<strong>in</strong>ed.From a behavior-analytic perspective, the overjustification effect might be conceptualizedas behavioral contrast (Balsam & Bondy, 1983). Behavioral contrast <strong>in</strong>volves an<strong>in</strong>teraction between two schedules <strong>in</strong> which manipulation of one schedule produces an<strong>in</strong>verse (or contrast<strong>in</strong>g) change <strong>in</strong> the response associated with the unchanged schedule(e.g., <strong>in</strong>troduction of ext<strong>in</strong>ction for Response A not only decreases Response A but also<strong>in</strong>creases Response B). Behavioral contrast has been reported most frequently for schedule<strong>in</strong>teractions that occur dur<strong>in</strong>g multiple <strong>and</strong> concurrent schedules (Catania, 1992;Reynolds, 1961), but contrast effects can sometimes occur across successive phases with as<strong>in</strong>gle response (Azr<strong>in</strong> & Holz, 1966).The overjustification effect, when it occurs, is an example of successive behavioralcontrast <strong>in</strong> which a schedule change <strong>in</strong> one phase affects the level of a s<strong>in</strong>gle response <strong>in</strong>a subsequent phase. That is, dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e, the target response is presumablyma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed by au<strong>to</strong>matic re<strong>in</strong>forcement (e.g., play<strong>in</strong>g guitar 1 hr per day). Follow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>troductionof the external reward (e.g., payment for play<strong>in</strong>g guitar), any <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g(e.g., play<strong>in</strong>g guitar 2 hr per day) would be attributable <strong>to</strong> the re<strong>in</strong>forcementeffect of the reward. If withdrawal of the external reward decreases respond<strong>in</strong>g belowthe levels <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e (e.g., play<strong>in</strong>g guitar 1 hr every 2 days), the difference <strong>in</strong>respond<strong>in</strong>g between the two basel<strong>in</strong>e phases (i.e., the one preced<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the one follow<strong>in</strong>gthe re<strong>in</strong>forcement phase) would represent a contrast (or overjustification) effect.Negative behavioral contrast has been def<strong>in</strong>ed as response suppression for one re<strong>in</strong>forcerfollow<strong>in</strong>g prior exposure <strong>to</strong> a more favorable re<strong>in</strong>forcer (Mack<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>sh, 1974). In theabove example, the decrease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g the second basel<strong>in</strong>e phase would beBut it is used thisway.JustificationLiteratureBehaviorLower response?LiteratureBehaviorGood explanation


314 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAfter the factjustificationattributable <strong>to</strong> the prior <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> re<strong>in</strong>forcement (i.e., au<strong>to</strong>matic re<strong>in</strong>forcement pluspayment) <strong>and</strong> would represent negative behavioral contrast. Interpret<strong>in</strong>g overjustificationas negative behavioral contrast may be a more parsimonious <strong>in</strong>terpretation of theeffect, as opposed <strong>to</strong> cognitive perspectives, because of the observability of the responseunder question across successive phases. In addition, <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the overjustificationeffect as behavioral contrast may help <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why prior research on this phenomenonhas produced such mixed results, <strong>in</strong> that contrast effects tend <strong>to</strong> be transient <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>consistentphenomena (Balsam & Bondy, 1983; Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996).Although programmatic l<strong>in</strong>es of research often lead <strong>to</strong> scientific advances, <strong>in</strong> manycases serendipi<strong>to</strong>us f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs may also lead <strong>to</strong> new areas of research. Many of Sk<strong>in</strong>ner’searly discoveries were the result of unplanned f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> his labora<strong>to</strong>ry. For example, theproduction of an ext<strong>in</strong>ction curve was due <strong>to</strong> equipment failure (i.e., a jam <strong>in</strong> the foodmagaz<strong>in</strong>e), <strong>in</strong>termittent re<strong>in</strong>forcement schedules were developed based on the need <strong>to</strong>conserve food pellets, <strong>and</strong> the development of the fixed-ratio schedule occurred with<strong>in</strong>the context of controll<strong>in</strong>g for deprivation under fixed-<strong>in</strong>terval schedules (Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, 1956). Inaddition, many research programs have been developed based on unexpected or accidentalf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> the labora<strong>to</strong>ry (see Brady, 1958). Unplanned results are important <strong>to</strong> researchersbecause such f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs often produce a l<strong>in</strong>e of “curiosity-test<strong>in</strong>g” research <strong>in</strong>which novel scientific f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are obta<strong>in</strong>ed (Sidman, 1960).In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation, we describe a case example <strong>in</strong> which a planned l<strong>in</strong>e ofprogrammatic research (i.e., a s<strong>in</strong>gle-case evaluation of the overjustification hypothesis)produced unexpected results. Based on these results, additional analyses were conducted<strong>to</strong> evaluate the mechanisms underly<strong>in</strong>g these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.GENERAL METHODParticipant <strong>and</strong> Sett<strong>in</strong>gSampleArnold, a 14-year-old boy who had been diagnosed with autism, cerebral palsy, moderatemental retardation, <strong>and</strong> visual impairments, had been admitted <strong>to</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tensive daytreatmentprogram for the assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of self-<strong>in</strong>jurious behavior (headbang<strong>in</strong>g). He had a vocabulary of approximately 1,000 words <strong>and</strong> was able <strong>to</strong> followmultiple-step <strong>in</strong>structions <strong>to</strong> complete complex tasks (e.g., fold<strong>in</strong>g laundry, operat<strong>in</strong>g adishwasher) but required some assistance with self-help skills (e.g., dress<strong>in</strong>g, ambulat<strong>in</strong>glong distances) due primarily <strong>to</strong> his visual impairment. Throughout this <strong>in</strong>vestigation,Arnold received constant dosages of fluvoxam<strong>in</strong>e, divalproex, <strong>and</strong> olanzap<strong>in</strong>e.All sessions were conducted <strong>in</strong> a padded room (approximately 4 m by 3 m) thatconta<strong>in</strong>ed chairs, a table, <strong>and</strong> other stimuli (e.g., <strong>to</strong>ys, work materials) needed for thecondition <strong>in</strong> effect. A therapist was present <strong>in</strong> the room with Arnold across all conditions,<strong>and</strong> one or two observers were seated <strong>in</strong> unobtrusive locations <strong>in</strong> the room.Response Measurement <strong>and</strong> ReliabilityAmbiguousdef<strong>in</strong>ition?Observers collected data on sort<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>in</strong> the reward <strong>and</strong> time-out analyses), <strong>in</strong>-seat behavior(<strong>in</strong> the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment <strong>and</strong> the reward analysis), <strong>and</strong> orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior (<strong>in</strong> thetime-out analysis). Sort<strong>in</strong>g was def<strong>in</strong>ed as plac<strong>in</strong>g a piece of silverware <strong>in</strong> a plastic utensiltray that was divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> different spaces, each shaped like a particular type of silverware(i.e., knife, fork, or spoon). Sort<strong>in</strong>g was scored only when Arnold placed a piece ofsilverware <strong>in</strong> the correct space <strong>in</strong> the tray. Sort<strong>in</strong>g was identified as the target behaviorbased on reports from home <strong>and</strong> school that this was a task that Arnold completed <strong>in</strong>dependently.In-seat behavior was def<strong>in</strong>ed as contact of the but<strong>to</strong>cks <strong>to</strong> the seat of a chair.Orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior consisted of responses that were necessary for an <strong>in</strong>dividual with


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 315visual impairments <strong>to</strong> locate the task materials <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>to</strong>uch<strong>in</strong>g areas of the tableuntil the tray was located or <strong>to</strong>uch<strong>in</strong>g the various utensil spaces on the tray. For the purposeof data analysis, sort<strong>in</strong>g was recorded as a frequency measure <strong>and</strong> was converted <strong>to</strong>responses per m<strong>in</strong>ute. Durations of <strong>in</strong>-seat behavior <strong>and</strong> orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior were converted<strong>to</strong> percentage of session time by divid<strong>in</strong>g the duration of the behavior by the durationof the session (i.e., 600 s of work time) <strong>and</strong> multiply<strong>in</strong>g by 100%.A second observer <strong>in</strong>dependently collected data on 46.3% of all sessions. Exactagreement was calculated by compar<strong>in</strong>g observer agreement on the exact number (or duration)of occurrences or nonoccurrences of a response dur<strong>in</strong>g each 10-s <strong>in</strong>terval. Theagreement coefficient was computed by divid<strong>in</strong>g the number of exact agreements on theoccurrence or nonoccurrence of behavior by the number of agreements plus disagreements<strong>and</strong> multiply<strong>in</strong>g by 100%. Agreement on sort<strong>in</strong>g averaged 86.6% (range, 78.7% <strong>to</strong>98.3%) <strong>in</strong> the reward analysis <strong>and</strong> 88.4% (range, 81.9% <strong>to</strong> 92.6%) <strong>in</strong> the time-out analysis.Agreement on <strong>in</strong>-seat behavior averaged 96.8% (range, 90.3% <strong>to</strong> 100%) <strong>in</strong> rewardanalysis <strong>and</strong> 98.9% (range, 96.8% <strong>to</strong> 100%) <strong>in</strong> the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment. Agreement onorient<strong>in</strong>g behavior averaged 88.1% (range, 85.2% <strong>to</strong> 91.1%) <strong>in</strong> the time-out analysis.Ambiguousdef<strong>in</strong>ition?Good procedureAcceptable <strong>to</strong> goodagreementEXPERIMENT 1: REWARD ANALYSISMethodPreference Assessment. A modified stimulus-choice preference assessment was conducted<strong>to</strong> identify a hierarchy of preferred stimuli (Fisher et al., 1992; Paclawskyj & Vollmer, 1995).Stimuli <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> this assessment were based on <strong>in</strong>formal observations of Arnold’s <strong>in</strong>teractionswith various stimuli <strong>and</strong> on caregiver report of preferred items (Fisher, Piazza,Bowman, & Amari, 1996). Eight stimuli were <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the preference assessment, <strong>and</strong>each stimulus was paired once with every other stimulus <strong>in</strong> a r<strong>and</strong>om order. At the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof each presentation, the therapist (a) held a pair of stimuli <strong>in</strong> front of Arnold, (b) vocally<strong>to</strong>ld Arnold which item was located <strong>to</strong> the left <strong>and</strong> which was <strong>to</strong> the right, (c) guidedArnold <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teract with each item for approximately 5 s, <strong>and</strong> (d) said, “Pick one.”Cont<strong>in</strong>gent on a selection, Arnold received access <strong>to</strong> the item for 20 s. After the 20-s <strong>in</strong>tervalelapsed, the stimulus was withdrawn, <strong>and</strong> two different stimuli were presented <strong>in</strong> thesame manner. Simultaneous approaches <strong>to</strong>ward both stimuli were blocked, <strong>and</strong> the itemswere briefly withdrawn <strong>and</strong> re-presented <strong>in</strong> the manner described above.Reward Analysis. This analysis consisted of two conditions, basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gentreward. Dur<strong>in</strong>g basel<strong>in</strong>e, Arnold was seated at a table with a box of silverware locatedon the floor <strong>to</strong> the left of his chair. A plastic tray was located approximately 25 cm fromthe edge of the table (the location was marked by a piece of tape). Throughout the session,Arnold was prompted <strong>to</strong> engage <strong>in</strong> the target behavior (i.e., the therapist said“Arnold, sort the silverware”) on a fixed-time (FT) 60-s schedule. No differential consequenceswere arranged for the emission of the sort<strong>in</strong>g response, <strong>and</strong> all other behaviorwas ignored. In the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward condition, Arnold received 20-s access <strong>to</strong> the twopreferred stimuli (<strong>to</strong>y telephone <strong>and</strong> radio) for sort<strong>in</strong>g silverware on a fixed-ratio (FR)1 schedule. When Arnold ga<strong>in</strong>ed access <strong>to</strong> the preferred stimuli, the tray <strong>and</strong> the box ofsilverware were removed, <strong>and</strong> the preferred stimuli were placed on the table. After the20-s <strong>in</strong>terval elapsed, the preferred stimuli were removed, the tray <strong>and</strong> the box of silverwarewere returned <strong>to</strong> their <strong>in</strong>itial positions, <strong>and</strong> Arnold could resume sort<strong>in</strong>g. With theexception of the presentation of preferred stimuli, the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward condition wasidentical <strong>to</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition (i.e., silverware <strong>and</strong> tray were present, prompts weredelivered on an FT 60-s schedule, <strong>and</strong> all other behavior was ignored).ImplementerGood description


316 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward conditions were alternated <strong>in</strong> a reversal(ABABA) design. All sessions consisted of 10 m<strong>in</strong> of work time (i.e., the session clocks<strong>to</strong>pped dur<strong>in</strong>g each 20-s <strong>in</strong>terval <strong>in</strong> which preferred stimuli were delivered).Results <strong>and</strong> DiscussionPreference Assessment. Two stimuli were chosen on over 80% of presentations dur<strong>in</strong>g thestimulus-choice preference assessment. A <strong>to</strong>y telephone was chosen on 100% of presentations<strong>and</strong> a radio was chosen on 86% of presentations.DirectionalhypothesisResultsInterpretationReward Analysis. This analysis was conducted <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if cont<strong>in</strong>gent presentationof preferred <strong>to</strong>ys would <strong>in</strong>crease the target response while the cont<strong>in</strong>gency was <strong>in</strong> effect<strong>and</strong> then decrease this response below its <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e levels once the cont<strong>in</strong>gency waswithdrawn (i.e., would produce negative behavioral contrast or an overjustification effect).Results of the reward analysis are shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 1. The <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e resulted <strong>in</strong> moderatelyhigh levels of sort<strong>in</strong>g (M 4.6 responses per m<strong>in</strong>ute). Contrary <strong>to</strong> expectations, cont<strong>in</strong>gentaccess <strong>to</strong> preferred <strong>to</strong>ys actually decreased the rate of sort<strong>in</strong>g (M 3.5). A reversal<strong>to</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition showed that sort<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>to</strong> levels that exceeded the <strong>in</strong>itialbasel<strong>in</strong>e (M 6.1). Subsequent <strong>in</strong>troduction of the <strong>to</strong>ys produced another decrease <strong>in</strong> sort<strong>in</strong>g(M 3.6) that was followed by a recovery of <strong>in</strong>creased sort<strong>in</strong>g rates <strong>in</strong> the secondreversal <strong>to</strong> the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition (M 5.9). In summary, cont<strong>in</strong>gent presentation of thepreferred <strong>to</strong>ys decreased respond<strong>in</strong>g relative <strong>to</strong> its <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e levels, <strong>and</strong> removal ofthe cont<strong>in</strong>gency produced <strong>in</strong>creased response rates that exceeded <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e levels.Because the reward cont<strong>in</strong>gency decreased respond<strong>in</strong>g while it was <strong>in</strong> effect <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creasedrespond<strong>in</strong>g above the <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e levels after it was withdrawn (<strong>in</strong> direct opposition<strong>to</strong> the prediction of the overjustification hypothesis), subsequent analyses wereconducted <strong>to</strong> evaluate several potential explanations of the observed effects of the cont<strong>in</strong>gency.One potential explanation was that cont<strong>in</strong>gent access <strong>to</strong> the preferred stimulifunctioned as punishment (time-out from the au<strong>to</strong>matic re<strong>in</strong>forcement produced bysort<strong>in</strong>g) because the delivery of the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>in</strong>terrupted an even more preferred activity(sort<strong>in</strong>g the silverware). A second potential explanation of the effects of the cont<strong>in</strong>gencywas that presentation of the preferred stimuli <strong>in</strong>creased the complexity of the taskbecause the participant was visually impaired <strong>and</strong> had <strong>to</strong> reorient <strong>to</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g materials8Basel<strong>in</strong>eCont<strong>in</strong>gentRewardBasel<strong>in</strong>eCont<strong>in</strong>gentRewardBasel<strong>in</strong>e7Sort<strong>in</strong>g responses per m<strong>in</strong>ute65432100 5 1015 20SessionsFigure 1 Sort<strong>in</strong>g Responses per M<strong>in</strong>ute Dur<strong>in</strong>g the Reward Analysis25


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 317after each delivery of the preferred stimuli. To evaluate these possibilities, we conductedan additional analysis. The second (time-out) analysis was a direct test of the effects oftime-out from the sort<strong>in</strong>g task, while the duration of orient<strong>in</strong>g behaviors was measured(<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the reductions <strong>in</strong> sort<strong>in</strong>g were attributable <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>creasedcomplexity result<strong>in</strong>g from these prerequisite responses). If time-out produced reductions<strong>in</strong> silverware sort<strong>in</strong>g similar <strong>to</strong> those produced dur<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward condition, itwould strongly suggest that cont<strong>in</strong>gent access <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>ys functioned as punishment for silverwaresort<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the subsequent <strong>in</strong>creases resulted from behavioral contrast. Alternatively,high levels of orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>in</strong> the time-out condition would suggest that theresults obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the reward analysis were due <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased task complexity.Directionalhypothesis?EXPERIMENT 2: TIME-OUT ANALYSISMethodThe basel<strong>in</strong>e condition was identical <strong>to</strong> the one conducted <strong>in</strong> the reward analysis (i.e., silverwarelocated <strong>to</strong> the left of the chair, a tray present on the table, <strong>and</strong> prompts deliveredevery 60 s). The time-out condition was identical <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e except that the tray <strong>and</strong> boxof silverware were removed for 20 s cont<strong>in</strong>gent on the sort<strong>in</strong>g response on an FR 1 schedule.Thus, this condition was similar <strong>to</strong> the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward condition of the rewardanalysis except that the preferred stimuli were not delivered follow<strong>in</strong>g each sort<strong>in</strong>g response.At the end of the 20-s time-out, the therapist returned the tray <strong>and</strong> box of silverware<strong>and</strong> Arnold could resume sort<strong>in</strong>g. All other responses were ignored. The basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong>time-out conditions were compared <strong>in</strong> a multielement design. All sessions consisted of10 m<strong>in</strong> of work time (i.e., the session clock s<strong>to</strong>pped dur<strong>in</strong>g each 20-s time-out <strong>in</strong>terval).Results <strong>and</strong> DiscussionResults of the time-out analysis are presented <strong>in</strong> Figure 2. Rates of sort<strong>in</strong>g (M 6.4 responsesper m<strong>in</strong>ute) dur<strong>in</strong>g basel<strong>in</strong>e were similar <strong>to</strong> the rates observed dur<strong>in</strong>g the lasttwo basel<strong>in</strong>e phases of the reward analysis. Lower rates of sort<strong>in</strong>g were observed <strong>in</strong> thetime-out condition (M 3.4). This rate is similar <strong>to</strong> the rates observed <strong>in</strong> the cont<strong>in</strong>gentreward phases of the reward analysis.Given Arnold’s visual impairments, it was possible that the lower rates observeddur<strong>in</strong>g the time-out condition could be due <strong>to</strong> orient<strong>in</strong>g responses that may have beenInterpretation87Basel<strong>in</strong>eSort<strong>in</strong>g responses per m<strong>in</strong>ute65432Time-out101 2 3 4 56 7SessionsFigure 2 Sort<strong>in</strong>g Responses per M<strong>in</strong>ute Dur<strong>in</strong>g the Time-out Analysis8


318 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWe agree.needed <strong>to</strong> re<strong>in</strong>itiate the sort<strong>in</strong>g response after each time-out <strong>in</strong>terval (i.e., orient<strong>in</strong>g thematerials prior <strong>to</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g). Thus, dur<strong>in</strong>g the time-out condition, observers collected dataon the time Arnold allocated <strong>to</strong> such orient<strong>in</strong>g responses. These data revealed that thedifferences between the amount of time Arnold allocated <strong>to</strong> orient<strong>in</strong>g responses dur<strong>in</strong>gbasel<strong>in</strong>e (M 0.6 s per session) <strong>and</strong> the time-out conditions (M 2.4 s per session) werenegligible <strong>and</strong> could not account for the observed reductions <strong>in</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g response.Results of the time-out analysis suggested that <strong>in</strong>terruption of the ongo<strong>in</strong>g sort<strong>in</strong>gresponse functioned as punishment <strong>and</strong> reduced the occurrence of sort<strong>in</strong>g. Thus, it waslikely that the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the reward analysis were attributable <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terruptionof the sort<strong>in</strong>g response via the cont<strong>in</strong>gent presentation of the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys. Also,results of the reward <strong>and</strong> time-out analyses suggested that sort<strong>in</strong>g was a highly preferredresponse, which was possibly more preferred than play<strong>in</strong>g with the <strong>to</strong>y telephone <strong>and</strong>radio. To exam<strong>in</strong>e this possibility, a third analysis was conducted <strong>to</strong> evaluate the relativere<strong>in</strong>forc<strong>in</strong>g efficacy of the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys when no alternative stimulation was available<strong>and</strong> when Arnold had a choice between the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> sort<strong>in</strong>g silverware.EXPERIMENT 3: REINFORCER ASSESSMENTSMethodGood descriptionInternal validityA re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment (based on Roane, Vollmer, R<strong>in</strong>gdahl, & Marcus, 1998) was conducted<strong>to</strong> evaluate the re<strong>in</strong>forc<strong>in</strong>g effects of the preferred stimuli when no alternativestimulation was available (Phase 1) <strong>and</strong> when Arnold had a choice between the preferredstimuli <strong>and</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g response (Phase 2). Dur<strong>in</strong>g each phase of the assessment, two chairswere concurrently available <strong>in</strong> the room. Dur<strong>in</strong>g Phase 1, sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> one chair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uousaccess <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>y telephone <strong>and</strong> radio (the preferred stimuli identified dur<strong>in</strong>g thepreference assessment), whereas sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the other chair produced no consequence (controlchair). Dur<strong>in</strong>g Phase 2, sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> one chair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uous access <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>y telephone<strong>and</strong> radio, whereas sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the other chair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uous access <strong>to</strong> thesort<strong>in</strong>g task. Prior <strong>to</strong> each session, Arnold was guided <strong>to</strong> sit <strong>in</strong> each chair, <strong>and</strong> he receivedthe consequence associated with that chair. At the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the session, Arnold wasmoved 1.5 m from the chairs, was <strong>to</strong>ld which chair was located <strong>to</strong> his left <strong>and</strong> right, <strong>and</strong> wasprompted <strong>to</strong> select one of the chairs. After 5 m<strong>in</strong> elapsed, the session clock was paused <strong>and</strong>Arnold was guided <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong> up <strong>and</strong> walk <strong>to</strong> the start<strong>in</strong>g area (i.e., 1.5 m from the chairs). Atthis po<strong>in</strong>t the chairs <strong>and</strong> their respective cont<strong>in</strong>gencies were reversed (e.g., the re<strong>in</strong>forcementchair became the control chair <strong>and</strong> vice versa). Arnold was aga<strong>in</strong> prompted <strong>to</strong> choosea chair, the session clock resumed, <strong>and</strong> the session cont<strong>in</strong>ued as described above.ResultsResultsResults of the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment are shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 3. In Phase 1, when sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> onechair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uous access <strong>to</strong> the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the other chair producedno consequence, Arnold allocated all of his respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ward the chair associatedwith the <strong>to</strong>ys (M 94.1% of the session time) <strong>to</strong> the exclusion of the control chair.By contrast, <strong>in</strong> Phase 2, when one chair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uous access <strong>to</strong> these same preferred<strong>to</strong>ys but the other chair produced cont<strong>in</strong>uous access <strong>to</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g task, Arnold allocatedall of his respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the chair associated with the sort<strong>in</strong>g materials (M 92.3% of the session time) <strong>to</strong> the exclusion of the chair associated with preferred stimuli.These results <strong>in</strong>dicate that the preferred <strong>to</strong>ys functioned as re<strong>in</strong>forcement for <strong>in</strong>-seatbehavior when the alternative was no stimulation but not when the alternative wasengagement <strong>in</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g task. Arnold clearly preferred the sort<strong>in</strong>g task <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>ys.


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 319100Phase 1 Phase 29080Toy AccessPercentage of session time <strong>in</strong> seat70605040302010ControlSort<strong>in</strong>g TaskToy Access01 2 3 4 56 7SessionsFigure 3 Percentage of Session Time of In-seat Behavior Dur<strong>in</strong>g Re<strong>in</strong>forcer Assessment8GENERAL DISCUSSIONIn the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation, a young man sorted silverware <strong>in</strong> the absence of external rewarddelivery. This behavior met the def<strong>in</strong>ition of <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sically motivated behavior describedby Deci (1971). The overjustification hypothesis states that levels of an <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sicallymotivated behavior will decrease <strong>to</strong> levels below the prereward basel<strong>in</strong>e follow<strong>in</strong>gcessation of the reward cont<strong>in</strong>gency. Not only was this effect not evident <strong>in</strong> the current<strong>in</strong>vestigation, but the results were directly opposite of the prediction of the overjustificationhypothesis.Results of the <strong>in</strong>itial (reward) analysis revealed what might be termed an antioverjustificationeffect <strong>in</strong> that (a) cont<strong>in</strong>gent presentation of high-preference stimuli resulted<strong>in</strong> a decrease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g relative <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> (b) respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased when thebehavior no longer produced the external reward. The unexpected results of the <strong>in</strong>itialanalysis led <strong>to</strong> the development of additional hypotheses that were evaluated throughsubsequent analyses. These additional analyses suggested that <strong>in</strong>terruption of the sort<strong>in</strong>gtask (via the removal of sort<strong>in</strong>g materials) functioned as punishment <strong>and</strong> that thesort<strong>in</strong>g task was a more preferred response relative <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>y play.Two operant mechanisms appear <strong>to</strong> provide the most parsimonious accounts for theresults observed <strong>in</strong> the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Results of the reward <strong>and</strong> time-out analysessuggest that decreased response levels were attributable <strong>to</strong> the removal of the sort<strong>in</strong>gmaterials, which <strong>in</strong>terrupted the ongo<strong>in</strong>g sort<strong>in</strong>g response. Cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>terruption ofau<strong>to</strong>matically re<strong>in</strong>forced behavior has been used <strong>to</strong> reduce the occurrence of such responses<strong>and</strong> has been reported as a punishment effect (e.g., Barmann, Croyle-Barmann, &McLa<strong>in</strong>, 1980; Lerman & Iwata, 1996). Likewise, <strong>in</strong>terruption of the sort<strong>in</strong>g task appeared<strong>to</strong> function as punishment. The removal of the response manipul<strong>and</strong>a <strong>in</strong> the reward <strong>and</strong>time-out analyses is similar <strong>to</strong> the time-out procedures used <strong>in</strong> labora<strong>to</strong>ry research. Ferster<strong>and</strong> Sk<strong>in</strong>ner (1957) def<strong>in</strong>ed time-out as “any period of time dur<strong>in</strong>g which the organism isprevented from emitt<strong>in</strong>g the behavior under observation” (p. 34). Time-out periods frequentlyresult <strong>in</strong> a decreased rate of respond<strong>in</strong>g (Ferster & Sk<strong>in</strong>ner). In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation,Arnold could not emit the target response (sort<strong>in</strong>g) dur<strong>in</strong>g the reward <strong>in</strong>terval ofthe reward analysis or dur<strong>in</strong>g the time-out <strong>in</strong>terval of the time-out analysis because access<strong>to</strong> the silverware <strong>and</strong> tray was restricted. Thus, it appears that the decrease <strong>in</strong> behaviorDirectionalhypothesisNot stated as suchInterpretationInterpretation


320 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eComplex discussionImplicationdur<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward <strong>and</strong> time-out conditions was due <strong>to</strong> punishment <strong>in</strong> the formof time-out from the more preferred re<strong>in</strong>forcer (the sort<strong>in</strong>g task).The second general effect observed <strong>in</strong> the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation (i.e., <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>grelative <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e) is <strong>in</strong>dicative of behavioral contrast. Specifically, acontrast effect was noted <strong>in</strong> that respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased follow<strong>in</strong>g prior exposure <strong>to</strong> a lesspreferred consequence (i.e., <strong>in</strong>terruption). Recall that the overjustification hypothesis maybe <strong>in</strong>terpreted as negative behavioral contrast (i.e., respond<strong>in</strong>g for one re<strong>in</strong>forcer decreasesfollow<strong>in</strong>g exposure <strong>to</strong> a more preferred re<strong>in</strong>forcer). By contrast, <strong>in</strong> the current <strong>in</strong>vestigationthe target behavior decreased <strong>in</strong>itially <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>in</strong> the subsequent basel<strong>in</strong>e phases.Given that the behavior decreased dur<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward <strong>and</strong> time-outconditions, it is not appropriate <strong>to</strong> conceptualize the current results as re<strong>in</strong>forcementcontrast. The current results appear <strong>to</strong> be more accurately characterized as an exampleof punishment contrast (i.e., <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g for a re<strong>in</strong>forcer follow<strong>in</strong>g exposure<strong>to</strong> punishment). Ferster <strong>and</strong> Sk<strong>in</strong>ner (1957) found higher rates of respond<strong>in</strong>g follow<strong>in</strong>g atime-out period relative <strong>to</strong> the levels of respond<strong>in</strong>g observed prior <strong>to</strong> the time-out. Similarly,Azr<strong>in</strong> (1960) showed that respond<strong>in</strong>g follow<strong>in</strong>g the cessation of a punishment cont<strong>in</strong>gency<strong>in</strong>creased <strong>to</strong> levels that exceeded prepunishment basel<strong>in</strong>e levels.Although the mechanism underly<strong>in</strong>g punishment contrast rema<strong>in</strong>s uncerta<strong>in</strong>, itseems that <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g follow<strong>in</strong>g a punishment cont<strong>in</strong>gency may be related <strong>to</strong>decreased amounts of re<strong>in</strong>forcement dur<strong>in</strong>g the punishment phase. In other words, punishmentmay create a deprivation state that results <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a subsequent(nonpunishment) phase (Azr<strong>in</strong> & Holz, 1966), an <strong>in</strong>terpretation that is also consistentwith the response-deprivation hypothesis (Timberlake & Allison, 1974; for more<strong>in</strong>-depth reviews of this <strong>and</strong> other potential explanations of punishment contrast, seeAzr<strong>in</strong> & Holz or Crosbie, Williams, Lattal, Anderson, & Brown, 1997).An alternative <strong>to</strong> the punishment contrast explanation is that the decrease <strong>in</strong> thetarget response observed dur<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gent reward <strong>and</strong> time-out conditions may havebeen due <strong>to</strong> disrupted response momentum (Nev<strong>in</strong>, 1996). Specifically, presentation of the<strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> removal of the sort<strong>in</strong>g materials may have functioned <strong>to</strong> disrupt the ongo<strong>in</strong>ghigh-probability sort<strong>in</strong>g response, such that response levels dropped relative <strong>to</strong> the nondisruptedbasel<strong>in</strong>e. <strong>How</strong>ever, if the decrease <strong>in</strong> the target response observed dur<strong>in</strong>g the cont<strong>in</strong>gentreward phase were due <strong>to</strong> disrupted response momentum, one would not expectrespond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> the second basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong> levels above those observed dur<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e. To the contrary, if the response’s momentum were disrupted, one wouldexpect lower levels of respond<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g the second basel<strong>in</strong>e relative <strong>to</strong> the first.One potentially important aspect of the current results is that they illustrate the relativenature of re<strong>in</strong>forcement, <strong>and</strong> of punishment for that matter (Herrnste<strong>in</strong> & Lovel<strong>and</strong>,1975; Premack, 1971; Timberlake & Allison, 1974). Typically, stimuli identified as highlypreferred <strong>in</strong> stimulus preference assessments function as effective positive re<strong>in</strong>forcers(e.g., Fisher et al., 1992; Roane et al., 1998). In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation, cont<strong>in</strong>gent access<strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>y telephone <strong>and</strong> radio (the items identified as highly preferred dur<strong>in</strong>g the preferenceassessment) did not function as re<strong>in</strong>forcement for the sort<strong>in</strong>g response dur<strong>in</strong>g thereward analysis. Results of the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment helped <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> this f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g byshow<strong>in</strong>g that these stimuli (the <strong>to</strong>ys) functioned as re<strong>in</strong>forcement (for <strong>in</strong>-seat behavior)when the alternative was sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a chair associated with no alternative re<strong>in</strong>forcementbut not when the choice was between the <strong>to</strong>ys <strong>and</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g task.In light of the results of the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessment, it is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g that a re<strong>in</strong>forcementeffect was not obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the reward analysis. In fact, if the re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessmenthad been conducted first, the results of the reward analysis could have been predictedus<strong>in</strong>g either the probability-differential hypothesis (i.e., the Premack pr<strong>in</strong>ciple;


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 321Premack, 1959) or the response-deprivation hypothesis (Timberlake & Allison, 1974). Theprobability-differential hypothesis states that a higher probability response will <strong>in</strong>creasethe occurrence of a lower probability response, if the cont<strong>in</strong>gency is arranged such that thehigh-probability response is cont<strong>in</strong>gent on the low-probability response. In the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation,the probability-differential hypothesis would predict that cont<strong>in</strong>gent access <strong>to</strong>the <strong>to</strong>ys would function as punishment for the sort<strong>in</strong>g response because a lower probabilityresponse was presented cont<strong>in</strong>gent on a higher probability response (Premack, 1971).The response-deprivation hypothesis states that restrict<strong>in</strong>g a response below its freeoperantbasel<strong>in</strong>e probability will establish its effectiveness as re<strong>in</strong>forcement for anotherresponse. Response deprivation would predict the absence of a re<strong>in</strong>forcement effect (butnot necessarily a punishment effect), because play<strong>in</strong>g with the <strong>to</strong>ys did not occur when thisresponse <strong>and</strong> the sort<strong>in</strong>g response were concurrently available. Under this condition, it wasnot possible <strong>to</strong> produce response deprivation for <strong>to</strong>y play (which would be necessary <strong>to</strong> establishits effectiveness as re<strong>in</strong>forcement accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> response-deprivation theory) becausethe <strong>in</strong>itial probability of <strong>to</strong>y play was zero (see Konarski, Johnson, Crowell, & Whitman, 1980,for a more complete discussion of the convergent <strong>and</strong> divergent predictions of the Premackpr<strong>in</strong>ciple <strong>and</strong> the response-deprivation hypothesis).Future research should consider the relativity of re<strong>in</strong>forcement when design<strong>in</strong>g behavioral<strong>in</strong>terventions. Specifically, researchers should consider conduct<strong>in</strong>g concurrentarrangements of potential <strong>in</strong>strumental (e.g., tasks) <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gent (e.g., preferred stimuli)responses <strong>in</strong> conjunction with either the Premack pr<strong>in</strong>ciple or the response-deprivation hypothesis<strong>to</strong> help <strong>to</strong> ensure that a re<strong>in</strong>forcement cont<strong>in</strong>gency will be arranged appropriately.Additional research should also be directed at extend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>itial unexpected or negativef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the fac<strong>to</strong>rs that contribute <strong>to</strong> such results (e.g., Piazza,Fisher, Hanley, Hilker, & Derby, 1996; R<strong>in</strong>gdahl, Vollmer, Marcus, & Roane, 1997). In thecurrent <strong>in</strong>vestigation, the reward analysis failed <strong>to</strong> yield the anticipated results. That is,the orig<strong>in</strong>al purpose of our analysis was <strong>to</strong> conduct a s<strong>in</strong>gle-case evaluation of the overjustificationeffect us<strong>in</strong>g empirically derived preferred stimuli. From this perspective, the<strong>in</strong>itial results could be <strong>in</strong>terpreted as a failure. <strong>How</strong>ever, the negative results of the rewardanalysis led <strong>to</strong> further experimentation designed <strong>to</strong> address additional hypotheses.These additional analyses allowed us <strong>to</strong> pursue other research questions (i.e., throughdiscovery research; Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, 1956).Future research should also cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> evaluate the overjustification hypothesisus<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle-case designs <strong>and</strong> methods appropriate <strong>to</strong> the evaluation of contrast effects(Crosbie et al., 1997). In addition, <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs should exam<strong>in</strong>e the effects of varioustypes of contrast effects on behavioral <strong>in</strong>terventions. As with other operant pr<strong>in</strong>ciples,contrast mechanisms may vary <strong>in</strong> terms of their effect on subsequent behavior (i.e.,<strong>in</strong>crease or decrease) <strong>and</strong> the conditions under which they occur (i.e., simultaneousor successive schedules; Mack<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>sh, 1974). In addition, contrast effects are generallyconsidered <strong>to</strong> be transient phenomena <strong>in</strong> that response rates generally return <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>elevels over time (Azr<strong>in</strong> & Holz, 1966). F<strong>in</strong>ally, future research could help <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhether the overjustification effect represents an example of a transient negativecontrast, which may add perspective regard<strong>in</strong>g the importance of the phenomenon.ImplicationReferencesAzr<strong>in</strong>, N. H. (1960). Effects of punishment <strong>in</strong>tensity dur<strong>in</strong>gvariable-<strong>in</strong>terval re<strong>in</strong>forcement. Journal of the ExperimentalAnalysis of Behavior, 3, 123–142.Azr<strong>in</strong>, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig(Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research <strong>and</strong> application(pp. 380–447). New York: Apple<strong>to</strong>n-Century-Crofts.


322 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eBalsam, P. D., & Bondy, A. S. (1983). The negative sideeffects of reward. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,16, 283–296.Barmann, B. C., Croyle-Barmann, C., & McLa<strong>in</strong>, B. (1980).The use of cont<strong>in</strong>gent-<strong>in</strong>terrupted music <strong>in</strong> the treatmen<strong>to</strong>f disruptive bus-rid<strong>in</strong>g behavior. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 13, 693–698.Brady, J. V. (1958). Ulcers <strong>in</strong> “executive” monkeys. ScientificAmerican, 199, 95–100.Catania, A. C. (1992). Learn<strong>in</strong>g (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs,NJ: Prentice Hall.Crosbie, J., Williams, A. M., Lattal, K. A., Anderson,M. M.,& Brown, S. M. (1997). Schedule <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>gpunishment with pigeons <strong>and</strong> humans. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 161–175.Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewardson <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> SocialPsychology, 18, 105–115.Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effectsof reward. American Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166.Ferster, C. B., & Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of re<strong>in</strong>forcement.New York: Apple<strong>to</strong>n-Century-Crofts.Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., & Amari, A.(1996). Integrat<strong>in</strong>g caregiver report with a systematicchoice assessment <strong>to</strong> enhance re<strong>in</strong>forcer identification.American Journal on Mental Retardation, 101, 15–25.Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P.,Owens, J. C., & Slev<strong>in</strong>, I. (1992). A comparison of twoapproaches for identify<strong>in</strong>g re<strong>in</strong>forcers for persons withsevere <strong>to</strong> profound disabilities. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 25, 491–498.Greene, D., & Lepper, M. R. (1974). Effects of extr<strong>in</strong>sicrewards on children’s subsequent <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic <strong>in</strong>terest. ChildDevelopment, 45, 1141–1145.Herrnste<strong>in</strong>, R. J., & Lovel<strong>and</strong>, D. H. (1975). Maximiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>match<strong>in</strong>g on concurrent ratio schedules. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 24, 107–116.Konarski, E. A., Jr., Johnson, M. R., Crowell, C. R., &Whitman, T. L. (1980). Response deprivation <strong>and</strong>re<strong>in</strong>forcement <strong>in</strong> applied sett<strong>in</strong>gs: A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary analysis.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 595–609.Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). A methodology fordist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g between ext<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>and</strong> punishment effectsassociated with response block<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 29, 231–233.Mace, F. C. (1994). Basic research needed for stimulat<strong>in</strong>g thedevelopment of behavioral technologies. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 529–550.Mack<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>sh, N. J. (1974). The psychology of animal learn<strong>in</strong>g.London: Academic Press.Nev<strong>in</strong>, J. A. (1996). The momentum of compliance. Journalof Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 535–547.Paclawskyj, T. R., & Vollmer, T. R. (1995). Re<strong>in</strong>forcer assessmentfor children with developmental disabilities <strong>and</strong>visual impairments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,28, 219–224.Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Hilker, K., & Derby,K. M. (1996). A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary procedure for predict<strong>in</strong>g thepositive <strong>and</strong> negative effects of re<strong>in</strong>forcement-basedprocedures. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,137–152.Premack, D. (1959). Toward empirical behavioral laws: I. Positivere<strong>in</strong>forcement. Psychological Review, 66, 219–233.Premack, D. (1971). Catch<strong>in</strong>g up with common sense or twosides of a generalization: Re<strong>in</strong>forcement <strong>and</strong> punishment.In R. Glaser (Ed.), The nature of re<strong>in</strong>forcement(pp. 121–150). New York: Academic Press.Reitman, D. (1998). The real <strong>and</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>ed harmful effects ofrewards: Implications for cl<strong>in</strong>ical practice. Journal of BehaviorTherapy <strong>and</strong> Experimental Psychiatry, 29, 101–113.Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Behavioral contrast. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 57–71.R<strong>in</strong>gdahl, J. E., Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Roane, H. S.(1997). An analogue evaluation of environmentalenrichment: The role of stimulus preference. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 30, 203–216.Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., R<strong>in</strong>gdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A.(1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference assessment.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 605–620.Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research: Evaluat<strong>in</strong>gexperimental data <strong>in</strong> psychology. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: AuthorsCooperative.Sk<strong>in</strong>ner, B. F. (1956). A case his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> scientific method.American Psychologist, 11, 221–233.Timberlake, W., & Allison, J. (1974). Response deprivation:An empirical approach <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumental performance.Psychological Review, 81, 146–164.Vasta, R., & Stirpe, L. A. (1979). Re<strong>in</strong>forcement effects onthree measures of children’s <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> math. BehaviorModification, 3, 223–244.


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 323Analysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is not clearly stated as such but is neverthelessmade clear: “<strong>to</strong> conduct a s<strong>in</strong>gle-case evaluationof the overjustification effect.” A secondary purpose(emerg<strong>in</strong>g later) appears <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong> illustrate how “programmaticresearch” can lead <strong>to</strong> productive “discoveryresearch.” The latter was one reason we selected thisstudy for review.Justification of the study is theoretical <strong>in</strong> nature. Althoughthe focus is on the overjustification effect, othertheoretical concepts are discussed at length <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong>it. The importance of this effect appears <strong>to</strong> be taken forgranted; we th<strong>in</strong>k support<strong>in</strong>g reasons should have beenpresented.Apractical justification might have been given<strong>in</strong> terms of implications for Arnold <strong>and</strong> others like him.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems of risk, confidentiality,or deception.DEFINITIONSThe essential terms are overjustification effect, positivere<strong>in</strong>forcement, cont<strong>in</strong>gency reward, <strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>forcer.These are either explicitly def<strong>in</strong>ed or, we th<strong>in</strong>k, adequatelyexpla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> context. The dependent variables(sort<strong>in</strong>g response <strong>and</strong> response preference) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables (reward, time-out, <strong>and</strong> choice) areoperationally def<strong>in</strong>ed. Terms that are not <strong>in</strong>itially crucial<strong>to</strong> the study but which provide important theoreticalcontext <strong>in</strong>clude behavioral contrast, complexity oftask, punishment, cont<strong>in</strong>gent <strong>in</strong>terruption, disrupted responsemomentum, probability-differential hypothesis,<strong>and</strong> response-deprivation hypothesis. We th<strong>in</strong>k theseterms are made as clear as is feasible <strong>in</strong> a brief treatmentgiven their complexity.PRIOR RESEARCHThe authors provide good brief summaries of three ofthe most pert<strong>in</strong>ent prior studies <strong>and</strong> state that the conclusionsof such studies are mixed or contradic<strong>to</strong>ry. <strong>Research</strong>on related theoretical issues is summarized,sometimes <strong>in</strong> the “General Discussion” section, whichis appropriate because of the nature of the study.HYPOTHESESNo hypotheses are directly stated at the outset, but it isclear that the primary one was the overjustificationhypothesis itself <strong>in</strong> the context of this study—i.e., deliveryof an extr<strong>in</strong>sic reward (access <strong>to</strong> preferred <strong>to</strong>ys) willdecrease the frequency of the behavior (sort<strong>in</strong>g) belowits <strong>in</strong>itial level when the reward is term<strong>in</strong>ated. This hypothesisis directional, though the authors do not statewhether they predicted support for it.The second <strong>and</strong> third experiments tested hypotheseson the effect of time-out as an <strong>in</strong>terruption <strong>and</strong> of re<strong>in</strong>forcerpreference, although hypotheses were not statedas such.SAMPLEThe sample consisted of one 14-year-old boy with multiplespecial needs. He is adequately described, <strong>in</strong> ourop<strong>in</strong>ion. As always <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, generalization<strong>to</strong> any population, <strong>in</strong> the absence of replication,is not defensible.INSTRUMENTATIONInstrumentation consists entirely of the observationalsystem for collect<strong>in</strong>g data. The observers were presumablyprovided the necessary def<strong>in</strong>itions of behaviors <strong>to</strong>be tallied. Sort<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>-seat behavior seem straightforward;orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior, less so. Observer agreementranged from 78.7 percent <strong>to</strong> 100 percent; the former ismarg<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> the others are acceptable <strong>to</strong> good with averagesfrom 86.6 percent <strong>to</strong> 98.6 percent. The <strong>to</strong>tal numberof observation periods was two <strong>in</strong> experiment 1 <strong>and</strong>eight <strong>in</strong> experiments 2 <strong>and</strong> 3.Reliability, other than observer agreement, is not discussed.Inconsistency across time is <strong>to</strong> be expected; thequestion is whether there is sufficient consistencywith<strong>in</strong> treatments <strong>to</strong> allow between-treatment differences<strong>to</strong> emerge. This is clearly the case <strong>in</strong> all three experiments.As is typical <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies, the absence ofdiscussion of validity reflects the reliance on content(logical) validity. The description of behaviors <strong>and</strong> goodobserver agreement are often considered sufficient <strong>to</strong>ensure that the variable be<strong>in</strong>g tallied is <strong>in</strong>deed the<strong>in</strong>tended (conceptualized) variable. This argument ismuch more persuasive <strong>in</strong> studies of this k<strong>in</strong>d than <strong>in</strong>those study<strong>in</strong>g more ambiguous variables such as read<strong>in</strong>gability or assertiveness; nevertheless, one can questionwhether orient<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>in</strong> experiment 2 isunambiguous. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, observer agreement washigher for this than for sort<strong>in</strong>g.


324 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYProcedures are clearly described. Experiment 1 used anABAB reversal design (actually, ABABA), <strong>and</strong> experiment2 used a BABA-type design but modified (with noexplanation) <strong>to</strong> a BAABABBA design. Both designsprovide good control of several threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity—i.e., subject characteristics, mortality, his<strong>to</strong>ry,maturation, location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation (observer fatigue)<strong>and</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics. An implementationthreat is possible if the therapist behaved differentlydur<strong>in</strong>g different treatments, but this seems unlikely.Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias is a threat because observers obviouslyknew which treatment was be<strong>in</strong>g observed; goodobserver agreement is a partial control. Subject attitude isan unlikely threat because lik<strong>in</strong>g for a particular treatmentis part of the rationale for its effects. Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>regression threats don’t apply.Experiment 3 consisted of two phases, <strong>in</strong> each ofwhich Arnold was allowed <strong>to</strong> choose where he sat, an<strong>in</strong>dication of which conditions he preferred. The positionof each choice was alternated <strong>to</strong> prevent respond<strong>in</strong>gby “habit.”Experiments 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 were conducted not <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong>away the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of experiment 1, but rather <strong>to</strong> furtherexpla<strong>in</strong> those results. Experiment 2 attempted <strong>to</strong> clarifythe possibility of <strong>in</strong>terruption of task as punishment byus<strong>in</strong>g time-out as the <strong>in</strong>terruption. The possibility thatreorientation was operat<strong>in</strong>g was checked by observ<strong>in</strong>gthis variable. Experiment 3 evaluated the possibility thatArnold preferred the task itself <strong>to</strong> the reward.DATA ANALYSIS/RESULTSData are presented with the cus<strong>to</strong>mary charts. Resultsare clearly <strong>and</strong> correctly discussed. All three experimentsshow unusually strong treatment effects for cont<strong>in</strong>gentreward (though opposite <strong>to</strong> the overjustificationhypothesis), time-out, <strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>forcer preference.DISCUSSION/INTREPRETATIONThe f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are reviewed succ<strong>in</strong>ctly <strong>and</strong> accurately, followedby a lengthy discussion of possible explanationswith references <strong>to</strong> research not previously mentioned.This is largely the result of the explora<strong>to</strong>ry nature of experiments2 <strong>and</strong> 3. This discussion, though appropriate<strong>and</strong>, we th<strong>in</strong>k, accurate, could have been written moreclearly despite the admittedly complex theoretical issuesit addresses. Implications for future research are presentedat length <strong>and</strong> serve <strong>to</strong> further illustrate the complexityof the matter studied.The serious limitations on both population <strong>and</strong>ecological generaliz<strong>in</strong>g should have been mentioned,although they are arguably less crucial <strong>in</strong> a study <strong>in</strong>tended<strong>to</strong> test theory than one <strong>in</strong>tended for application.The implications for address<strong>in</strong>g Arnold’s head bang<strong>in</strong>gmight, with proper cautions, have been a useful addition.They are (1) that rewards may function as punishment<strong>and</strong> (2) that punishment for head bang<strong>in</strong>g might bepredicted <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease its frequency follow<strong>in</strong>g cessationof the cont<strong>in</strong>gent punishment.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsESSENTIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF SINGLE-SUBJECT RESEARCHS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research <strong>in</strong>volves the extensive collection of data on one subject at atime.An advantage of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs is that they can be applied <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>gs wheregroup designs are difficult <strong>to</strong> put <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> play.


CHAPTER 14 S<strong>in</strong>gle-Subject <strong>Research</strong> 325SINGLE-SUBJECT DESIGNSS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs are most commonly used <strong>to</strong> study the changes <strong>in</strong> behavior an<strong>in</strong>dividual exhibits after exposure <strong>to</strong> a treatment or <strong>in</strong>tervention of some sort.S<strong>in</strong>gle-subject researchers primarily use l<strong>in</strong>e graphs <strong>to</strong> present their data <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> illustratethe effects of a particular <strong>in</strong>tervention or treatment.The basic approach of researchers us<strong>in</strong>g an A-B design is <strong>to</strong> expose the same subject,operat<strong>in</strong>g as his or her own control, <strong>to</strong> two conditions or phases.When us<strong>in</strong>g an A-B-A design (sometimes called a reversal design), researchers simplyadd another basel<strong>in</strong>e period <strong>to</strong> the A-B design.In the A-B-A-B design, two basel<strong>in</strong>e periods are comb<strong>in</strong>ed with two treatment periods.The B-A-B design is used when an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavior is so severe or disturb<strong>in</strong>gthat a researcher cannot wait for a basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong> be established.In the A-B-C-B design, the C condition refers <strong>to</strong> a variation of the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> theB condition. The <strong>in</strong>tervention is changed dur<strong>in</strong>g the C phase typically <strong>to</strong> control forany extra attention the subject may have received dur<strong>in</strong>g the B phase.MULTIPLE-BASELINE DESIGNSMultiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designs are used when it is not possible or ethical <strong>to</strong> withdraw atreatment <strong>and</strong> return <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e.When a multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design is used, researchers do more than collect data onone behavior for one subject <strong>in</strong> one sett<strong>in</strong>g; they collect on several behaviors for onesubject, obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a basel<strong>in</strong>e for each dur<strong>in</strong>g the same period of time.Multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designs also are sometimes used <strong>to</strong> collect data on several subjectswith regard <strong>to</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle behavior, or <strong>to</strong> measure a subject’s behavior <strong>in</strong> two or moredifferent sett<strong>in</strong>gs.THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITY IN SINGLE-SUBJECT RESEARCHSeveral threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity exist with regard <strong>to</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs. These<strong>in</strong>clude the length of the basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention conditions, the number of variableschanged when mov<strong>in</strong>g from one condition <strong>to</strong> another, the degree <strong>and</strong> speed of anychange that occurs, a return—or not—of the behavior <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e levels, the <strong>in</strong>dependenceof behaviors, <strong>and</strong> the number of basel<strong>in</strong>es.CONTROLLING THREATS IN SINGLE-SUBJECT STUDIESS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs are most effective <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g for subject characteristics,mortality, test<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry threats.They are less effective with location, data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics, maturation, <strong>and</strong>regression threats.They are especially weak when it comes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument decay, data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, attitude,<strong>and</strong> implementation threats.EXTERNAL VALIDITY AND SINGLE-SUBJECT RESEARCHS<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies are weak when it comes <strong>to</strong> generalizability.It is particularly important <strong>to</strong> replicate s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studies <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whetherthey are worthy of generalization.


326 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOTHER SINGLE-SUBJECT DESIGNSVariations on the basic designs discussed <strong>in</strong> this chapter <strong>in</strong>clude the A-B-A-C-Adesign; the A-B-C-B-C design; <strong>and</strong> the multiprobe design.Key TermsA-B design 300A-B-A design 302A-B-A-B design 302A-B-C-B design 303B-A-B design 303basel<strong>in</strong>e 300externalvalidity 311multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>edesign 305s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectdesign 299For Discussion1. Could s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs be implemented <strong>in</strong> secondary schools? If so, what difficultiesdo you th<strong>in</strong>k one might encounter?2. Professor Jones has a very difficult student <strong>in</strong> his <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry statistics class whokeeps <strong>in</strong>terrupt<strong>in</strong>g the other students when they attempt <strong>to</strong> answer the professor’squestions. <strong>How</strong> might the professor use one of the designs described <strong>in</strong> this chapter<strong>to</strong> reduce the student’s <strong>in</strong>terruptions?3. Can you suggest any <strong>in</strong>stances where a B-A-B design might be required <strong>in</strong> a typicalelementary school? What might they be?4. Would r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g be possible <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research? Why or why not?5. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k is easier <strong>to</strong> conduct: s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject or group comparison research?Why?6. What sorts of questions lend themselves better <strong>to</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject than <strong>to</strong> other k<strong>in</strong>dsof research?7. What sorts of behaviors might require only a few data po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>to</strong> establish a basel<strong>in</strong>e?Give some examples.8. When might it be unethical <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> an A-B-Adesign? Give an example.9. In terms of difficulty, how would you rate s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research on a scale of 1 <strong>to</strong>10? What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the most difficult aspect of this k<strong>in</strong>d of research? Why?


Correlational <strong>Research</strong>15Parent<strong>in</strong>come(.72)(–.40)(.55)Student loanaccount(.35)Years ofeducation(–.15)OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly what is meant byIdentify <strong>and</strong> describe briefly the stepsassociational research.<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a correlationalState the two major purposes ofstudy.correlational studies.Interpret correlation coefficients ofDist<strong>in</strong>guish between predic<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong>different magnitude.criterion variables.Expla<strong>in</strong> the rationale underly<strong>in</strong>g partialExpla<strong>in</strong> the role of correlational studies correlation.<strong>in</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>g causation.Describe some of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalExpla<strong>in</strong> how a scatterplot can be used <strong>to</strong> validity that exist <strong>in</strong> correlation studies <strong>and</strong>predict an outcome.expla<strong>in</strong> how <strong>to</strong> identify them.Describe what is meant by a prediction Discuss how <strong>to</strong> control for these threats.equation.Recognize a correlation study when youExpla<strong>in</strong> briefly the ma<strong>in</strong> ideas underly<strong>in</strong>g come across one <strong>in</strong> the educationalmultiple correlation, fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis, <strong>and</strong> research literature.path analysis.(.38)Net worthat age 45The Nature ofCorrelational <strong>Research</strong>Purposes of Correlational<strong>Research</strong>Explana<strong>to</strong>ry StudiesPrediction StudiesMore Complex CorrelationalTechniquesBasic Steps <strong>in</strong>Correlational <strong>Research</strong>Problem SelectionSampleInstruments<strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> ProceduresData CollectionData Analysis <strong>and</strong>InterpretationWhat Do CorrelationCoefficients Tell Us?Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity <strong>in</strong> Correlational<strong>Research</strong>Subject CharacteristicsLocationInstrumentationTest<strong>in</strong>gMortalityEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Correlational StudiesAn Example ofCorrelational <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData Analysis/ResultsDiscussion/Interpretation


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Centerat www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About What Correlation Coefficients Tell UsAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 15.1: Correlational <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 15.2: What K<strong>in</strong>d of Correlation?Activity 15.3: Th<strong>in</strong>k Up an ExampleActivity 15.4: Match the Correlation Coefficient <strong>to</strong> ItsScatterplotActivity 15.5: Calculate a Correlation CoefficientActivity 15.6: Construct a ScatterplotActivity 15.7: Correlation <strong>in</strong> Everyday LifeActivity 15.8: RegressionJust<strong>in</strong>e Gibbs, a high school biology teacher, was bothered last year by the fact that many of her 10th-gradestudents had considerable difficulty learn<strong>in</strong>g many of the concepts <strong>in</strong> biology while some learned them rathereasily. Before the semester beg<strong>in</strong>s this year, therefore, she would like <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> predict which sorts of <strong>in</strong>dividualsare likely <strong>to</strong> have trouble learn<strong>in</strong>g these concepts. If she could make some fairly accurate predictions, she might beable <strong>to</strong> suggest some corrective measures (e.g., special tu<strong>to</strong>rial sessions) so that fewer students would have difficulty<strong>in</strong> her biology classes.The appropriate methodology called for here is correlational research. What Gibbs might do is <strong>to</strong> collect differentk<strong>in</strong>ds of data on her students that might be related <strong>to</strong> the difficulties they do—or do not—have with biology. Anyvariables that might be related <strong>to</strong> success—or failure—<strong>in</strong> biology (e.g., their anxiety <strong>to</strong>ward the subject, their previousknowledge, how well they underst<strong>and</strong> abstractions, their performance <strong>in</strong> other science courses, etc.) would beuseful. This might give her some ideas about how those students who learn biological concepts easily differ from thosewho f<strong>in</strong>d them difficult. This, <strong>in</strong> turn, might help her predict who might have trouble learn<strong>in</strong>g biology next semester.This chapter, therefore, describes for Ms. Gibbs (<strong>and</strong> you) what correlational research is all about.The Nature of Correlational<strong>Research</strong>Correlational research, like causal-comparative research(which we discuss <strong>in</strong> Chapter 16), is an exampleof what is sometimes called associational research. Inassociational research, the relationships among two ormore variables are studied without any attempt <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencethem. In their simplest form, correlational studies<strong>in</strong>vestigate the possibility of relationships between onlytwo variables, although <strong>in</strong>vestigations of more than twovariables are common. In contrast <strong>to</strong> experimental research,however, there is no manipulation of variables<strong>in</strong> correlational research.Correlational research is also sometimes referred <strong>to</strong>as a form of descriptive research because it describes anexist<strong>in</strong>g relationship between variables. The way itdescribes this relationship, however, is quite differentfrom the descriptions found <strong>in</strong> other types of studies. Acorrelational study describes the degree <strong>to</strong> which two ormore quantitative variables are related, <strong>and</strong> it does so byus<strong>in</strong>g a correlation coefficient.*When a correlation is found <strong>to</strong> exist between twovariables, it means that scores with<strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> rangeon one variable are associated with scores with<strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong>range on the other variable. You will recall that apositive correlation means high scores on one variabletend <strong>to</strong> be associated with high scores on the othervariable, while low scores on one are associated with*Although associations among two or more categorical variables canalso be studied, such studies are not usually referred <strong>to</strong> as correlational.They are similar with respect <strong>to</strong> overall design <strong>and</strong> threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity, however, <strong>and</strong> we discuss them further <strong>in</strong> Chapter 16.328


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 329TABLE 15.1Three Sets of Data Show<strong>in</strong>g DifferentDirections <strong>and</strong> Degrees of Correlation(A) (B) (C)r 1.00 r 1.00 r 0X Y X Y X Y5 5 5 1 2 14 4 4 2 5 43 3 3 3 3 42 2 2 4 1 51 1 1 5 4 2low scores on the other. A negative correlation, on theother h<strong>and</strong>, means high scores on one variable are associatedwith low scores on the other variable, <strong>and</strong> lowscores on one are associated with high scores on theother (Table 15.1). As we also have <strong>in</strong>dicated before,relationships like those shown <strong>in</strong> Table 15.1 can be illustratedgraphically through the use of scatterplots.Figure 15.1, for example, illustrates the relationshipshown <strong>in</strong> Table 15.1(A).Purposes of Correlational<strong>Research</strong>Correlational research is carried out for one of two basicpurposes—either <strong>to</strong> help expla<strong>in</strong> important human behaviorsor <strong>to</strong> predict likely outcomes.EXPLANATORY STUDIESA major purpose of correlational research is <strong>to</strong> clarifyour underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of important phenomena by identify<strong>in</strong>grelationships among variables. Particularly <strong>in</strong> developmentalpsychology, where experimental studies areespecially difficult <strong>to</strong> design, much has been learned byanalyz<strong>in</strong>g relationships among several variables. Forexample, correlations found between variables such ascomplexity of parent speech <strong>and</strong> rate of language acquisitionhave taught researchers much about how languageis acquired. Similarly, the discovery that—amongvariables related <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g skill—audi<strong>to</strong>ry memoryshows a substantial correlation with read<strong>in</strong>g ability hasexp<strong>and</strong>ed our underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the complex phenomenonof read<strong>in</strong>g. The current belief that smok<strong>in</strong>g causeslung cancer, although based <strong>in</strong> part on experimentalY5432101Figure 15.1 Scatterplot Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g a Correlationof 1.002studies of animals, rests heavily on correlational evidenceof the relationship between frequency of smok<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cidence of lung cancer.<strong>Research</strong>ers who conduct explana<strong>to</strong>ry studies often<strong>in</strong>vestigate a number of variables they believe are related<strong>to</strong> a more complex variable, such as motivation orlearn<strong>in</strong>g. Variables found not <strong>to</strong> be related or onlyslightly related (i.e., when correlations below .20 areobta<strong>in</strong>ed) are then dropped from further consideration,while those found <strong>to</strong> be more highly related (i.e., whencorrelations beyond .40 or .40 are obta<strong>in</strong>ed) oftenserve as the focus of additional research, us<strong>in</strong>g an experimentaldesign, <strong>to</strong> see whether the relationships are<strong>in</strong>deed causal.Let us say a bit more here about causation. Althoughthe discovery of a correlational relationship does notestablish a causal connection, most researchers whoengage <strong>in</strong> correlational research are probably try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ga<strong>in</strong> some idea about cause <strong>and</strong> effect. A researcher whocarried out the fictitious study whose results are illustrated<strong>in</strong> Figure 15.2, for example, would probably be<strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> conclude that a teacher’s expectation of failureis a partial (or at least a contribut<strong>in</strong>g) cause of theamount of disruptive behavior his or her students display<strong>in</strong> class.It must be stressed, however, that correlational studiesdo not, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of themselves, establish cause <strong>and</strong> effect.In the previous example, one could just as wellargue that the amount of disruptive behavior <strong>in</strong> a classcauses a teacher’s expectation of failure, or that bothteacher expectation <strong>and</strong> disruptive behavior are causedby some third fac<strong>to</strong>r—such as the ability level of theclass.X345


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong>One of the most famous, <strong>and</strong> controversial, examples ofcorrelational research is that relat<strong>in</strong>g frequency of<strong>to</strong>bacco smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cidence of lung cancer. When thesestudies began <strong>to</strong> appear, many argued for smok<strong>in</strong>g as a majorcause of lung cancer. Opponents did not argue for thereverse—that is, that cancer causes smok<strong>in</strong>g—for the obviousreason that smok<strong>in</strong>g occurs first. They did, however, arguethat both smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> lung cancer are caused by other fac<strong>to</strong>rssuch as genetic predisposition, lifestyle (sedentary occupationsmight result <strong>in</strong> more smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> less exercise), <strong>and</strong> environment(smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> lung cancer might be more prevalent<strong>in</strong> smoggy cities).Despite a persuasive theory—smok<strong>in</strong>g clearly could irritatelung tissue—the argument for causation was not sufficientlypersuasive for the surgeon general <strong>to</strong> issue warn<strong>in</strong>gsuntil experimental studies showed that exposure <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>baccosmoke did result <strong>in</strong> lung cancer <strong>in</strong> animals.Amount of disruptive behavior1210864200 2 4 6 8 10 12Teacher expectation of failureFigure 15.2 Prediction Us<strong>in</strong>g a ScatterplotThe possibility of causation is strengthened, however,if a time lapse occurs between measurement of thevariables be<strong>in</strong>g studied. If the teacher’s expectations offailure were measured before assign<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong>classes, for example, it would seem unreasonable <strong>to</strong> assumethat class behavior (or, likewise, the ability levelof the class) would cause the teacher’s failure expectations.The reverse, <strong>in</strong> fact, would make more sense. Certa<strong>in</strong>other causal explanations, however, rema<strong>in</strong> persuasive,such as the socioeconomic level of the students<strong>in</strong>volved. Teachers might have higher expectations offailure for economically poor students. Such studentsalso might exhibit a greater amount of disruptive behavior<strong>in</strong> class regardless of their teacher’s expectations.The search for cause <strong>and</strong> effect <strong>in</strong> correlational studies,therefore, is fraught with difficulty. Nonetheless, it canbe a fruitful step <strong>in</strong> the search for causes. We return <strong>to</strong>this matter later <strong>in</strong> the discussion of threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity <strong>in</strong> correlational research.PREDICTION STUDIESA second purpose of correlational research is prediction:If a relationship of sufficient magnitude existsbetween two variables, it becomes possible <strong>to</strong> predict ascore on one variable if a score on the other variable isknown. <strong>Research</strong>ers have found, for example, thathigh school grades are highly related <strong>to</strong> college grades.Hence, high school grades can be used <strong>to</strong> predict collegegrades. We would predict that a person with ahigh GPA <strong>in</strong> high school would be likely <strong>to</strong> have a highGPA <strong>in</strong> college. The variable that is used <strong>to</strong> make theprediction is called the predic<strong>to</strong>r variable; the variableabout which the prediction is made is called thecriterion variable. Hence, <strong>in</strong> the above example, highschool grades would be the predic<strong>to</strong>r variable, <strong>and</strong> collegegrades would be the criterion variable. As wementioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8, prediction studies are alsoused <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the predictive validity of measur<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>struments.Us<strong>in</strong>g Scatterplots <strong>to</strong> Predict a Score. Predictioncan be illustrated through the use of scatterplots.Suppose, for example, that we obta<strong>in</strong> the data shown <strong>in</strong>Table 15.2 from a sample of 12 classes. Us<strong>in</strong>g thesedata, we f<strong>in</strong>d a correlation of .71 between the variablesteacher expectation of failure <strong>and</strong> amount of disruptivebehavior.Plott<strong>in</strong>g the data <strong>in</strong> Table 15.2 produces the scatterplotshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.2. Once a scatterplot such asthis has been constructed, a straight l<strong>in</strong>e, known as aregression l<strong>in</strong>e, can be calculated mathematically. Thecalculation of this l<strong>in</strong>e is beyond the scope of this text,but a general underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of its use can be obta<strong>in</strong>edby look<strong>in</strong>g at Figure 15.2. The regression l<strong>in</strong>e comes theclosest <strong>to</strong> all of the scores depicted on the scatterplot of330


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 331TABLE 15.2 Teacher Expectation of Failure<strong>and</strong> Amount of Disruptive Behaviorfor a Sample of 12 ClassesTeacherAmount ofExpectation Disruptiveof FailureBehaviorClass (Rat<strong>in</strong>gs) (Rat<strong>in</strong>gs)1 10 112 4 33 2 24 4 65 12 106 9 67 8 98 8 69 6 810 5 511 5 912 7 4any straight l<strong>in</strong>e that could be drawn. A researcher canthen use the l<strong>in</strong>e as a basis for prediction. Thus, as youcan see, a teacher with a score of 10 on expectation offailure would be predicted <strong>to</strong> have a class with a score of9 on amount of disruptive behavior, <strong>and</strong> a teacher withan expectation score of 6 would be predicted <strong>to</strong> have aclass with a disruptive behavior score of 6. Similarly, asecond regression l<strong>in</strong>e can be drawn <strong>to</strong> predict a scoreon teacher expectation of failure if we know his or herclass’s score on amount of disruptive behavior.Be<strong>in</strong>g able <strong>to</strong> predict a score for an <strong>in</strong>dividual (orgroup) on one variable based on the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s (orgroup’s) score on another variable is extremely useful.A school adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r, for example, could use Figure15.2 (if it were based on real data) <strong>to</strong> (1) identify <strong>and</strong>select teachers who are likely <strong>to</strong> have less disruptiveclasses; (2) provide tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> those teachers who arepredicted <strong>to</strong> have a large amount of disruptive behavior<strong>in</strong> their classes; or (3) plan for additional assistance forsuch teachers. Both the teachers <strong>and</strong> students <strong>in</strong>volvedwould benefit accord<strong>in</strong>gly.A Simple Prediction Equation. Although scatterplotsare fairly easy devices <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g predictions,they are <strong>in</strong>efficient when pairs of scores from alarge number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals have been collected. Fortunately,the regression l<strong>in</strong>e we just described can beexpressed <strong>in</strong> the form of a prediction equation, whichhas the follow<strong>in</strong>g form:Y 1 œ a bX 1where Y 1 œ the predicted score on Y (the criterion variable)for <strong>in</strong>dividual i, X 1 <strong>in</strong>dividual i’s score on X (thepredic<strong>to</strong>r variable), <strong>and</strong> a <strong>and</strong> b are values calculatedmathematically from the orig<strong>in</strong>al scores. For any givenset of data, a <strong>and</strong> b are constants.We mentioned earlier that high school GPA has beenfound <strong>to</strong> be highly related <strong>to</strong> college GPA. In this example,therefore, the symbol Y œ st<strong>and</strong>s for the predictedfirst-semester college GPA (the criterion variable), <strong>and</strong>X 1 st<strong>and</strong>s for the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s high school GPA (the predic<strong>to</strong>rvariable). Let us assume that a .18 <strong>and</strong> b .73.By substitut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the equation, we can predict a student’sfirst-semester college GPA. Thus, if an <strong>in</strong>dividual’shigh school GPA is 3.5, we would predict that hisor her first-semester college GPA would be 2.735 (thatis, .18 .73 (3.5) 2.735). We later can compare thestudent’s actual first-semester college GPA <strong>to</strong> the predictedGPA. If there is a close similarity between thetwo, we ga<strong>in</strong> confidence <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g the prediction equation<strong>to</strong> make future predictions.This predicted score will not be exact, however, <strong>and</strong>hence researchers also calculate an <strong>in</strong>dex of predictionerror, known as the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate. This<strong>in</strong>dex gives an estimate of the degree <strong>to</strong> which the predictedscore is likely <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>correct. The smaller thest<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate, the more accurate the prediction.This <strong>in</strong>dex of error, as you would expect, is muchlarger for small values of r than for large r’s.*Furthermore, if we have more <strong>in</strong>formation on the <strong>in</strong>dividualsabout whom we wish <strong>to</strong> predict, we should beable <strong>to</strong> decrease our errors of prediction. This is what atechnique known as multiple regression (or multiplecorrelation) is designed <strong>to</strong> do.MORE COMPLEXCORRELATIONAL TECHNIQUESMultiple Regression. Multiple regression is atechnique that enables researchers <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e a correlationbetween a criterion variable <strong>and</strong> the best comb<strong>in</strong>ationof two or more predic<strong>to</strong>r variables. Let us return<strong>to</strong> our previous example <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the high positive correlationbetween high school GPA <strong>and</strong> first-semester*If the reason for this is unclear <strong>to</strong> you, refer aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the scatterplots<strong>in</strong> Figures 15.2 <strong>and</strong> 10.19.


332 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ecollege GPA. Suppose it is also found that a high positivecorrelation (r .68) exists between first-semestercollege GPA <strong>and</strong> the verbal scores on the SAT collegeentrance exam<strong>in</strong>ation, <strong>and</strong> a moderately high positivecorrelation (r .51) exists between the mathematicsscores on the SAT <strong>and</strong> first-semester college GPA. It ispossible, us<strong>in</strong>g a multiple regression prediction formula,<strong>to</strong> use all three of these variables <strong>to</strong> predict whata student’s GPA will be dur<strong>in</strong>g his or her first semester<strong>in</strong> college. The formula is similar <strong>to</strong> the simple predictionequation, except that it now <strong>in</strong>cludes more than onepredic<strong>to</strong>r variable <strong>and</strong> more than two constants. It takesthe follow<strong>in</strong>g form:High schoolGPAFirst-yearcollege GPAY œ a b 1 X 1 b 2 X 2 b 3 X 3where Y œ once aga<strong>in</strong> st<strong>and</strong>s for the predicted firstsemestercollege GPA; a, b 1 , b 2 , <strong>and</strong> b 3 are constants;X 1 the high school GPA; X 2 the verbal SAT score;<strong>and</strong> X 3 the mathematics SAT score. Let us imag<strong>in</strong>e thata .18, b 1 .73, b 2 .0005, <strong>and</strong> b 3 .0002. We knowthat the student’s high school GPA is 3.5. Suppose his orher SAT verbal <strong>and</strong> mathematics scores are 580 <strong>and</strong> 600,respectively. Substitut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the formula, we would predictthat the student’s first-semester GPA would be 3.15.Y œ .18 .73(3.5) .0005(580) .0002(600) .18 2.56 .29 .12 3.15Aga<strong>in</strong>, we could later compare the actual first-semestercollege GPA obta<strong>in</strong>ed by this student with the predictedscore <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e how accurate our prediction was.The Coefficient of Multiple Correlation.The coefficient of multiple correlation, symbolized byR, <strong>in</strong>dicates the strength of the correlation between thecomb<strong>in</strong>ation of the predic<strong>to</strong>r variables <strong>and</strong> the criterionvariable. It can be thought of as a simple Pearson correlationbetween the actual scores on the criterion variable<strong>and</strong> the predicted scores on that variable. In the previousexample, we used a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of high school GPA,SAT verbal score, <strong>and</strong> SAT mathematics score <strong>to</strong> predictthat a particular student’s first-semester college GPAwould be 3.15. We then could obta<strong>in</strong> that same student’sactual first-semester college GPA (it might be 2.95, forexample). If we did this for 100 students, we could thencalculate the correlation (R) between predicted <strong>and</strong>actual GPA. If R turned out <strong>to</strong> be 1.00, for example, itwould mean that the predicted scores correlated perfectlywith the actual scores on the criterion variable.Test scoreFigure 15.3 Multiple CorrelationAn R of 1.00, of course, would be most unusual <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>.In actual practice, Rs of .70 or .80 are consideredquite high. The higher R is, of course, the more reliablea prediction will be. Figure 15.3 illustrates the relationshipsamong a criterion <strong>and</strong> two predic<strong>to</strong>rs. The amoun<strong>to</strong>f college GPA accounted for by high school GPA(about 36 percent) is <strong>in</strong>creased by about 13 percent byadd<strong>in</strong>g test score as a second predic<strong>to</strong>r.The Coefficient of Determ<strong>in</strong>ation. Thesquare of the correlation between a predic<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> a criterionvariable is known as the coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation,symbolized by r 2 . If the correlation between highschool GPA <strong>and</strong> college GPA, for example, equals .60,then the coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation would equal .36.What does this mean? In short, the coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation<strong>in</strong>dicates the percentage of the variabilityamong the criterion scores that can be attributed <strong>to</strong> differences<strong>in</strong> the scores on the predic<strong>to</strong>r variable. Thus, ifthe correlation between high school GPA <strong>and</strong> collegeGPA for a group of students is .60, 36 percent (.60) 2 ofthe differences <strong>in</strong> the college GPAs of those students canbe attributed <strong>to</strong> differences <strong>in</strong> their high school GPAs.The <strong>in</strong>terpretation of R 2 (for multiple regression) issimilar <strong>to</strong> that of r 2 (for simple regression). Suppose <strong>in</strong>our example that used three predic<strong>to</strong>r variables, themultiple correlation coefficient is equal <strong>to</strong> .70. The


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 333coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation, then, is equal <strong>to</strong> (.70) 2 , or.49. Thus, it would be appropriate <strong>to</strong> say that 49 percen<strong>to</strong>f the variability <strong>in</strong> the criterion variable is predictableon the basis of the three predic<strong>to</strong>r variables. Anotherway of say<strong>in</strong>g this is that high school GPA, verbal SATscores, <strong>and</strong> mathematics SAT scores (the three predic<strong>to</strong>rvariables), taken <strong>to</strong>gether, account for about 49 percen<strong>to</strong>f the variability <strong>in</strong> college GPA (the criterion variable).The value of a prediction equation depends onwhether it can be used with a new group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.<strong>Research</strong>ers can never be sure the prediction equationthey develop will work successfully when it is used <strong>to</strong>predict criterion scores for a new group of persons. Infact, it is quite likely that it will be less accurate when soused, s<strong>in</strong>ce the new group will not be identical <strong>to</strong> theone used <strong>to</strong> develop the prediction equation. The successof a particular prediction equation with a newgroup, therefore, usually depends on the group’s similarity<strong>to</strong> the group used <strong>to</strong> develop the prediction equationorig<strong>in</strong>ally.Discrim<strong>in</strong>ant Function Analysis. In most predictionstudies, the criterion variable is quantitative—that is, it <strong>in</strong>volves scores that can fall anywhere along acont<strong>in</strong>uum from low <strong>to</strong> high. Our previous example ofcollege GPA is a quantitative variable, for scores on thevariable can fall anywhere at or between 0.00 <strong>and</strong> 4.00.Sometimes, however, the criterion variable may be acategorical variable—that is, it <strong>in</strong>volves membership <strong>in</strong>a group (or category) rather than scores along a cont<strong>in</strong>uum.For example, a researcher might be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>predict<strong>in</strong>g whether an <strong>in</strong>dividual is more like eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>gmajors or bus<strong>in</strong>ess majors. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, the criterionvariable is dicho<strong>to</strong>mous—an <strong>in</strong>dividual is either<strong>in</strong> one group or the other. Of course, a categorical variablecan have more than just two categories (for example,eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g majors, bus<strong>in</strong>ess majors, education majors,science majors, <strong>and</strong> so on). The technique ofmultiple regression cannot be used when the criterionvariable is categorical; <strong>in</strong>stead, a technique known asdiscrim<strong>in</strong>ant function analysis is used. The purpose ofthe analysis <strong>and</strong> the form of the prediction equation,however, are similar <strong>to</strong> those for multiple regression.Figure 15.4 illustrates the logic; note that the scores ofthe <strong>in</strong>dividual represented by the six faces rema<strong>in</strong> thesame for both categories! The person’s score is comparedfirst <strong>to</strong> the scores of research chemists, <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong>the scores of chemistry teachers.Where do I belong?<strong>Research</strong>chemistsMeasure A BC Measure AHighChemistryteachersBCMeMeScoresMeMeMeMeLowFigure 15.4 Discrim<strong>in</strong>ant Function Analysis


334 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFac<strong>to</strong>r Analysis. When a number of variables are<strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle study, analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretationof data can become rather cumbersome. It is oftendesirable, therefore, <strong>to</strong> reduce the number of variablesby group<strong>in</strong>g those that are moderately or highly correlatedwith one another <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs.Fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis is a technique that allows a researcher<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if many variables can be describedby a few fac<strong>to</strong>rs. The mathematical calculations<strong>in</strong>volved are beyond the scope of this book, but the techniqueessentially <strong>in</strong>volves a search for “clusters” ofvariables, all of which are correlated with each other.Each cluster represents a fac<strong>to</strong>r. Studies of group IQtests, for example, have suggested that the many specificscores used could be expla<strong>in</strong>ed as a result of a relativelysmall number of fac<strong>to</strong>rs. While controversial,these results did provide one means of comprehend<strong>in</strong>gthe mental abilities required <strong>to</strong> perform well on suchtests. They also led <strong>to</strong> new tests designed <strong>to</strong> test theseidentified abilities more effectively.Path Analysis. Path analysis is used <strong>to</strong> test the likelihoodof a causal connection among three or morevariables. Some of the other techniques we havedescribed can be used <strong>to</strong> explore theories about causality,but path analysis is far more powerful than the rest.Although a detailed explanation of this technique is <strong>to</strong>otechnical for <strong>in</strong>clusion here, the essential idea beh<strong>in</strong>dpath analysis is <strong>to</strong> formulate a theory about the possiblecauses of a particular phenomenon (such as studentalienation)—that is, <strong>to</strong> identify causal variables thatcould expla<strong>in</strong> why the phenomenon occurs—<strong>and</strong> then<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether correlations among all the variablesare consistent with the theory.Suppose a researcher theorizes as follows: (1) Certa<strong>in</strong>students are more alienated <strong>in</strong> school than others becausethey do not f<strong>in</strong>d school enjoyable <strong>and</strong> because they havefew friends; (2) they do not f<strong>in</strong>d school enjoyable partlybecause they have few friends <strong>and</strong> partly because they donot perceive their courses as be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> any way related <strong>to</strong>their needs; <strong>and</strong> (3) perceived relevance of courses is relatedslightly <strong>to</strong> number of friends. The researcher wouldthen measure each of these variables (degree of alienation,personal relevance of courses, enjoyment <strong>in</strong> school, <strong>and</strong>number of friends) for a number of students. Correlationsbetween pairs of each of the variables would then be calculated.Let us imag<strong>in</strong>e that the researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s the correlationsshown <strong>in</strong> the correlation matrix <strong>in</strong> Table 15.3.What does this table reveal about possible causes ofstudent alienation? Two of the variables (relevance ofTABLE 15.3 Correlation Matrix for Variables<strong>in</strong> Student Alienation StudySchool NumberEnjoyment of Friends AlienationRelevance of courses .65 .24 .48School enjoyment .58 .53Number of friends .27courses at .48 <strong>and</strong> school enjoyment at .53) shown<strong>in</strong> the table are sizable predic<strong>to</strong>rs of such alienation.Nevertheless, <strong>to</strong> rem<strong>in</strong>d you aga<strong>in</strong>, just because thesevariables predict student alienation, you should not assumethat they cause it. Furthermore, someth<strong>in</strong>g of aproblem exists <strong>in</strong> the fact that the two predic<strong>to</strong>r variablescorrelate with each other. As you can see, schoolenjoyment <strong>and</strong> perceived relevance of courses not onlypredict student alienation, but they also correlate highlywith each other (r .65). Now, does perceived relevanceof courses affect student alienation <strong>in</strong>dependentlyof school enjoyment? Does school enjoyment affect studentalienation <strong>in</strong>dependently of perception of courserelevance? Path analysis can help the researcher determ<strong>in</strong>ethe answers <strong>to</strong> these questions.Path analysis, then, <strong>in</strong>volves four basic steps. First,a theory that l<strong>in</strong>ks several variables is formulated <strong>to</strong>expla<strong>in</strong> a particular phenomenon of <strong>in</strong>terest. In our example,the researcher theorized the follow<strong>in</strong>g causalconnections: (1) When students perceive their coursesas be<strong>in</strong>g unrelated <strong>to</strong> their needs, they will not enjoyschool; (2) if they have few friends <strong>in</strong> school, this willcontribute <strong>to</strong> their lack of enjoyment, <strong>and</strong> (3) the more astudent dislikes school <strong>and</strong> the fewer friends he or shehas, the more alienated he or she will be. Second, thevariables specified by the theory are then measured <strong>in</strong>some way.* Third, correlation coefficients are computed<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the strength of the relationship between eachof the pairs of variables postulated <strong>in</strong> the theory. And,fourth, relationships among the correlation coefficientsare analyzed <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> the theory.Path analysis variables are typically shown <strong>in</strong> thetype of diagram illustrated <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.5.† Each variable<strong>in</strong> the theory is shown <strong>in</strong> the figure. Each arrow*Note that this step is very important. The measures must be validrepresentations of the variables. The results of the path analysis willbe <strong>in</strong>valid if this is not the case.†The process of path analysis <strong>and</strong> the diagrams drawn are, <strong>in</strong> practice,often more complex than the one shown here.


(.10)CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 335Relevance ofcourses(–.30)Figure 15.5 PathAnalysis Diagram(.40)Enjoyment of school(.30)Number of friends(–.55)(–.60)Alienation<strong>in</strong>dicates a hypothesized causal relationship <strong>in</strong> the directionof the arrow. Thus, lik<strong>in</strong>g for school is hypothesized<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence alienation; number of friends <strong>in</strong>fluencesschool enjoyment, <strong>and</strong> so on. Notice that <strong>in</strong> this exampleall of the arrows po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> one direction only. Thismeans that the first variable is hypothesized <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencethe second variable, but not vice versa. Numbers similar(but not identical) <strong>to</strong> correlation coefficients are calculatedfor each pair of variables. If the results were asshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.5, the causal theory of the researcherwould be supported. Do you see why?*Structural Model<strong>in</strong>g. Structural model<strong>in</strong>g is a sophisticatedmethod for explor<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> possibly confirm<strong>in</strong>gcausation among several variables. Its complexity isbeyond the scope of this text. Suffice it <strong>to</strong> say that itcomb<strong>in</strong>es multiple regression, path analysis, <strong>and</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>ranalysis. The computations are greatly simplified by useof computer programs; the computer program mostwidely used is probably LISREL. 1Basic Steps<strong>in</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong>PROBLEM SELECTIONThe variables <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> a correlational studyshould be based on a sound rationale grow<strong>in</strong>g out ofexperience or theory. The researcher should have somereason for th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> variables may be related. Asalways, clarity <strong>in</strong> def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g variables will avoid many*Because alienation is “caused” primarily by lack of enjoyment(–.55) <strong>and</strong> number of friends (–.60). The perceived lack of relevanceof courses does contribute <strong>to</strong> degree of alienation, but primarilybecause relevance “causes” enjoyment. Enjoyment is partly causedby number of friends. Perceived relevance of courses is only slightlycaused by number of friends.problems later on. In general, three major types of problemsare the focus of correlational studies:1. Is variable X related <strong>to</strong> variable Y?2. <strong>How</strong> well does variable P predict variable C?3. What are the relationships among a large number ofvariables, <strong>and</strong> what predictions can be made that arebased on them?Almost all correlational studies will revolve aroundone of these types of questions. Some examples of publishedcorrelational studies are as follows.“The accuracy of pr<strong>in</strong>cipals’ judgments of teacherperformance.” 2“Teacher clarity <strong>and</strong> its relationship <strong>to</strong> studentachievement <strong>and</strong> satisfaction.” 3“Fac<strong>to</strong>rs associated with the drug use of fifththrougheighth-grade students.” 4“Moral development <strong>and</strong> empathy <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g.” 5“The relationship among health beliefs, health values,<strong>and</strong> health promotion activity.” 6“The relationship of student ability <strong>and</strong> small-group<strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>to</strong> student achievement.” 7“Predict<strong>in</strong>g students’ outcomes from their perceptionsof classroom psychosocial environment.” 8SAMPLEThe sample for a correlational study, as <strong>in</strong> any type ofstudy, should be selected carefully <strong>and</strong>, if possible, r<strong>and</strong>omly.The first step <strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g a sample, of course, is<strong>to</strong> identify an appropriate population, one that is mean<strong>in</strong>gful<strong>and</strong> from which data on each of the variables of<strong>in</strong>terest can be collected. The m<strong>in</strong>imum acceptable samplesize for a correlational study is considered by mostresearchers <strong>to</strong> be no less than 30. Data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from asample smaller than 30 may give an <strong>in</strong>accurate estimateof the degree of relationship. Samples larger than 30 aremuch more likely <strong>to</strong> provide mean<strong>in</strong>gful results.


336 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eINSTRUMENTSThe <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>to</strong> measure the two (or more) variables<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a correlational study may take any oneof a number of forms (see Chapter 7), but they mustyield quantitative data. Although data sometimes can becollected from records of one sort or another (gradetranscripts, for example), most correlational studies <strong>in</strong>volvethe adm<strong>in</strong>istration of some type of <strong>in</strong>strument(tests, questionnaires, <strong>and</strong> so on) <strong>and</strong> sometimes observation.As with any study, whatever <strong>in</strong>struments areused must yield reliable scores. In an explana<strong>to</strong>ry study,the <strong>in</strong>struments must also show evidence of validity. Ifthey do not truly measure the <strong>in</strong>tended variables, thenany correlation that is obta<strong>in</strong>ed will not be an <strong>in</strong>dicationof the <strong>in</strong>tended relationship. In a prediction study, it isnot essential that we know what variable is actuallybe<strong>in</strong>g measured—if it works as a predic<strong>to</strong>r, it is useful.<strong>How</strong>ever, prediction studies are most likely <strong>to</strong> be successful,<strong>and</strong> certa<strong>in</strong>ly more satisfy<strong>in</strong>g, when we knowwhat we are measur<strong>in</strong>g!DESIGN AND PROCEDURESThe basic design used <strong>in</strong> a correlational study is quitestraightforward. Us<strong>in</strong>g the symbols <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> ourdiscussion of experimental designs <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13, thisdesign can be diagrammed as shown below:<strong>Design</strong> for a Correlational StudyObservationsSubjects O 1 O 2A — —B — —C — —D — —E — —F — —G — —etc.As you can see, two (or more) scores are obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom each <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong> the sample, one score for eachvariable of <strong>in</strong>terest. The pairs of scores are then correlated,<strong>and</strong> the result<strong>in</strong>g correlation coefficient <strong>in</strong>dicatesthe degree of relationship between the variables.Notice, aga<strong>in</strong>, that we cannot say that the variablebe<strong>in</strong>g measured by the first <strong>in</strong>strument (O 1 ) is thecause of any differences <strong>in</strong> scores we may f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> thevariable be<strong>in</strong>g measured by the second <strong>in</strong>strument (O 2 ).TABLE 15.4Example of Data Obta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> a Correlational <strong>Design</strong>(O 2 )(O 1 ) MathematicsStudent Self-Esteem AchievementJosé 25 95Felix 23 88Rosita 25 96Phil 18 81Jenny 12 65Natty 23 73L<strong>in</strong>a 22 92Jill 15 71Jack 24 93James 17 78As we have mentioned before, three possibilities exist:1. The variable be<strong>in</strong>g measured by O 1 may cause thevariable be<strong>in</strong>g measured by O 2 .2. The variable be<strong>in</strong>g measured by O 2 may cause thevariable be<strong>in</strong>g measured by O 1 .3. Some third, perhaps unidentified <strong>and</strong> unmeasured,variable may cause both of the other variables.Different numbers of variables can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong>correlational studies, <strong>and</strong> sometimes quite complex statisticalprocedures are used. The basic research designfor all correlational studies, however, is similar <strong>to</strong> theone just shown. An example of data obta<strong>in</strong>ed with a correlationaldesign is shown <strong>in</strong> Table 15.4.DATA COLLECTIONIn an explana<strong>to</strong>ry study, all the data on both variableswill usually be collected with<strong>in</strong> a fairly short time.Often, the <strong>in</strong>struments used are adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>glesession, or <strong>in</strong> two sessions one immediately after theother. Thus, if a researcher were <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> measur<strong>in</strong>gthe relationship between verbal aptitude <strong>and</strong> memory, atest of verbal aptitude <strong>and</strong> another of memory would beadm<strong>in</strong>istered closely <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>to</strong> the same group of subjects.In a prediction study, the measurement of thecriterion variables often takes place sometime after themeasurement of the predic<strong>to</strong>r variables. If a researcherwere <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the predictive value of amathematics aptitude test, the aptitude test might beadm<strong>in</strong>istered just prior <strong>to</strong> the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of a course <strong>in</strong>mathematics. Success <strong>in</strong> the course (the criterion variable,as <strong>in</strong>dicated by course grades) would then bemeasured at the end of the course.


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 337DATA ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATIONAs we have mentioned previously, when variables arecorrelated, a correlation coefficient is produced. Thiscoefficient will be a decimal, somewhere between 0.00<strong>and</strong> 1.00 or 1.00. The closer the coefficient is <strong>to</strong>1.00 or 1.00, the stronger the relationship. If thesign is positive, the relationship is positive, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gthat high scores on one variable tend <strong>to</strong> go with highscores on the other variable. If the sign is negative, therelationship is negative, <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that high scores onone variable tend <strong>to</strong> go with low scores on the othervariable. Coefficients that are at or near .00 <strong>in</strong>dicate thatno relationship exists between the variables <strong>in</strong>volved.What Do CorrelationCoefficients Tell Us?It is important <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret correlation coefficientssensibly s<strong>in</strong>ce they appear so frequently <strong>in</strong> articlesabout education <strong>and</strong> educational research. Unfortunately,they are seldom accompanied by scatterplots,which usually help <strong>in</strong>terpretation <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.The mean<strong>in</strong>g of a given correlation coefficientdepends on how it is applied. Correlation coefficientsbelow .35 show only a slight relationship between variables.Such relationships have almost no value <strong>in</strong> anypredictive sense. (It may, of course, be important <strong>to</strong>know that certa<strong>in</strong> variables are not related. Thus wewould expect <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d a very low correlation, for<strong>in</strong>stance, between years of teach<strong>in</strong>g experience <strong>and</strong>number of students enrolled.) Correlations between .40<strong>and</strong> .60 are often found <strong>in</strong> educational research <strong>and</strong> mayhave theoretical or practical value, depend<strong>in</strong>g on thecontext. A correlation of at least .50 must be obta<strong>in</strong>edbefore any crude predictions can be made about <strong>in</strong>dividuals.Even then, such predictions will be subject <strong>to</strong>sizable errors. Only a correlation of .65 or higher willallow <strong>in</strong>dividual predictions that are reasonably accuratefor most purposes. Correlations over .85 <strong>in</strong>dicate aclose relationship between the variables correlated <strong>and</strong>are useful <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual performance, butcorrelations this high are rarely obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch, except when check<strong>in</strong>g on reliability.As we illustrated <strong>in</strong> Chapter 8, correlation coefficientsare also used <strong>to</strong> check the reliability <strong>and</strong> validityof scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed from tests <strong>and</strong> other <strong>in</strong>struments used<strong>in</strong> research; when so used, they are called reliability <strong>and</strong>validity coefficients. When used <strong>to</strong> check reliability ofscores, the coefficient should be at least .70, preferablyhigher; many tests achieve reliability coefficients of .90.The correlation between two different scorers, work<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dependently, should be at least .90. When used <strong>to</strong>check validity of scores, the coefficient should be atleast .50, <strong>and</strong> preferably higher.Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong>Recall from Chapter 9 that a major concern <strong>to</strong> researchersis that extraneous variables may expla<strong>in</strong> awayany results that are obta<strong>in</strong>ed.* A similar concern applies<strong>to</strong> correlational studies. A researcher who conducts acorrelational study should always be alert <strong>to</strong> alternativeexplanations for relationships found <strong>in</strong> the data. Whatmight account for any correlations that are reported asexist<strong>in</strong>g between two or more variables?Consider aga<strong>in</strong> the hypothesis that teacher expectationof failure is positively correlated with student disruptivebehavior. A researcher conduct<strong>in</strong>g this studywould almost certa<strong>in</strong>ly have a cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect sequence<strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, most likely that teacher expectation is apartial cause of disruptive behavior. Why? Because disruptivebehavior is undesirable (because it clearly <strong>in</strong>terfereswith both academic learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> a desirable classroomclimate). Thus, it would be helpful <strong>to</strong> know whatmight be done <strong>to</strong> reduce it. While teacher expectation offailure might be considered the dependent variable, itseems less likely s<strong>in</strong>ce such expectations would be oflittle <strong>in</strong>terest if they have no effect on students.If, <strong>in</strong>deed, the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>tentions are as we havedescribed, he might have carried out an experiment.<strong>How</strong>ever, it is difficult <strong>to</strong> see how teacher expectationcould be experimentally manipulated. It might, however,be possible <strong>to</strong> study whether attempts <strong>to</strong> change teacherexpectations result <strong>in</strong> subsequent changes <strong>in</strong> amount ofdisruptive behavior, but such a study requires develop<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> implement<strong>in</strong>g tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g methods. Beforeembark<strong>in</strong>g on such development <strong>and</strong> implementation,therefore, one might well ask whether there is any*It can be argued that such threats are irrelevant <strong>to</strong> the predictive useof correlational research. The argument is that one can predict even ifthe relationship is an artifact of other variables. Thus, predictions ofcollege achievement can be made from high school grades even ifboth are highly related <strong>to</strong> socioeconomic status. While we agree withthe practical utility of such predictions, we believe that researchshould seek <strong>to</strong> illum<strong>in</strong>ate relationships that have at least the potentialfor explanation.


338 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7erelationship between the primary variables. This is whya correlational study is an appropriate first step.A positive correlation result<strong>in</strong>g from such a studywould most likely be viewed as at least some evidence<strong>to</strong> suggest that modify<strong>in</strong>g teacher expectationswould result <strong>in</strong> less disruptive behavior, thereby justify<strong>in</strong>gfurther experimental efforts. (It may also be thatsome pr<strong>in</strong>cipals or teacher-tra<strong>in</strong>ers would wish <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>stitutemechanisms for chang<strong>in</strong>g teacher expectationsbefore wait<strong>in</strong>g for experimental confirmation, just asthe medical profession began warn<strong>in</strong>g about the effectsof smok<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the absence of conclusive experimentalevidence.) Before <strong>in</strong>vest<strong>in</strong>g time <strong>and</strong> resources<strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g methods <strong>and</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out anexperiment, the researcher needs <strong>to</strong> be as confident aspossible that he is not mis<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g his correlation.If the relationship he has found really reflects theopposite cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect sequence (student behaviorcaus<strong>in</strong>g teacher expectations), or if both are a result ofother causes, such as student ability or socioeconomicstatus, changes <strong>in</strong> teacher expectation are not likely <strong>to</strong>be accompanied by a reduction <strong>in</strong> disruptive behavior.The former problem (direction of cause <strong>and</strong> effect) canbe largely elim<strong>in</strong>ated by assess<strong>in</strong>g teacher expectationsprior <strong>to</strong> direct <strong>in</strong>volvement with the studentgroup. The latter problem—of other causes—is theone we turn <strong>to</strong> now.Some of the threats we discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9 do notapply <strong>to</strong> correlational studies. Implementation, his<strong>to</strong>ry,maturation, attitude of subjects, <strong>and</strong> regression threatsare not applicable s<strong>in</strong>ce no <strong>in</strong>tervention occurs. Thereare some threats, however, that do apply.SUBJECT CHARACTERISTICSWhenever two or more characteristics of <strong>in</strong>dividuals (orgroups) are correlated, there exists the possibility that ye<strong>to</strong>ther characteristics can expla<strong>in</strong> any relationships thatare found. In such cases, the other characteristics canbe controlled through a statistical technique known aspartial correlation. Let us illustrate the logic <strong>in</strong>volvedby us<strong>in</strong>g the example of the relationship between teachers’expectations of failure <strong>and</strong> the amount of disruptivebehavior by students <strong>in</strong> their classes. This relationship isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.6(a).The researcher desires <strong>to</strong> control, or “get rid of,” thevariable of “ability level” for the classes <strong>in</strong>volved, s<strong>in</strong>ceit is logical <strong>to</strong> assume that it might be a cause of variation<strong>in</strong> the other two variables. In order <strong>to</strong> control forthis variable, the researcher needs <strong>to</strong> measure the abilitylevel of each class. She can then construct scatterplotsas shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.6(b) <strong>and</strong> (c). Scatterplot (b)shows the correlation between amount of disruptivebehavior <strong>and</strong> class ability level; scatterplot (c) showsthe correlation between teacher expectation of failure<strong>and</strong> class ability level.The researcher can now use scatterplot (b) <strong>to</strong> predictthe disruptive behavior score for class 1, based on theability score for class 1. In do<strong>in</strong>g so, the researcherwould be assum<strong>in</strong>g that the regression l<strong>in</strong>e shown <strong>in</strong>scatterplot (b) correctly represents the relationship betweenthese variables (class ability level <strong>and</strong> amount ofdisruptive behavior) <strong>in</strong> the data. Next, the researchersubtracts the predicted disruptive behavior score fromthe actual disruptive behavior score. The result is calledthe adjusted disruptive behavior score—that is, the scorehas been “adjusted” by tak<strong>in</strong>g out the <strong>in</strong>fluence of abilitylevel. For class 1, the predicted disruptive behavior scoreis 7 (based on a class ability score of 5). In actuality thisclass scored 11 (higher than expected), so the adjustedscore for amount of disruptive behavior is (11 7), or 4.The same procedure is then followed <strong>to</strong> adjust teacherexpectation scores for class ability level, as shown <strong>in</strong>scatterplot (c) (10 7 3). After repeat<strong>in</strong>g this processfor the entire sample of classes, the researcher is now <strong>in</strong>a position <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the correlation between the adjusteddisruptive behavior scores <strong>and</strong> the adjustedteacher expectation scores. The result is the correlationbetween the two major variables with the effect of classability hav<strong>in</strong>g been elim<strong>in</strong>ated, <strong>and</strong> thus controlled.Methods of calculation, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the use of relativelysimple formulas are available <strong>to</strong> greatly simplify thisprocedure. 9 Figure 15.7 shows another way <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>kabout partial correlation. The <strong>to</strong>p circles illustrate (byamount of overlap) the correlation between A <strong>and</strong> B. Thebot<strong>to</strong>m circles show the same overlap but reduced by“tak<strong>in</strong>g out” the overlap of C with A <strong>and</strong> B. What rema<strong>in</strong>s(the diagonally l<strong>in</strong>ed section) illustrates the partialcorrelation of A <strong>and</strong> B with the effects of C removed.LOCATIONA location threat is possible whenever all <strong>in</strong>strumentsare adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> each subject at a specified location,but the location is different for different subjects. It isnot uncommon for researchers <strong>to</strong> encounter differences<strong>in</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g conditions, particularly when <strong>in</strong>dividual testsare required. In one school, a comfortable, well-lit, <strong>and</strong>ventilated room may be available. In another, a cus<strong>to</strong>dian’scloset may have <strong>to</strong> do. Such conditions can


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 339Amount of disruptive behaviorClass 11210864200 2 4 6 8 10 12Teacher expectation of failure(a)Amount of disruptive behavior <strong>in</strong> class as related <strong>to</strong> teacher expectation of failureFigure 15.6Scatterplots forComb<strong>in</strong>ationsof VariablesAmount of disruptive behavior12108642Class 100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Class ability level(b)Amount of disruptive behavior as related <strong>to</strong> ability level of classActual score = 11Predicted scoreAdjusted score==74Teacher expectation of failure12108642Class 100 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Class ability level(c)Teacher expectation of failure as related <strong>to</strong> ability level of classActual score =10Predicted scoreAdjusted score==73


340 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e(A)Height(B)Weightsolutions <strong>to</strong> location problems such as these are either<strong>to</strong> measure the extraneous variables (such as resourcelevel) <strong>and</strong> use partial correlation or <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e correlationsseparately for each location, provided the numberof students at each location is sufficiently large (am<strong>in</strong>imum n of 30).(A)HeightOrig<strong>in</strong>al correlation(B)Weight(C)AgePartial correlationshow<strong>in</strong>g reduced overlapFigure 15.7 Elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g the Effects of Age ThroughPartial Correlation<strong>in</strong>crease (or decrease) subject scores. If both measuresare not adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> all subjects under the same conditions,the conditions rather than the variables be<strong>in</strong>gstudied may account for the relationship. If only part ofa group, for example, responds <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> an uncomfortable,poorly lit room, they might score lower onan achievement test <strong>and</strong> respond more negatively <strong>to</strong> arat<strong>in</strong>g scale measur<strong>in</strong>g student lik<strong>in</strong>g for school, thusproduc<strong>in</strong>g a mislead<strong>in</strong>g correlation coefficient.Similarly, conditions <strong>in</strong> different schools may accountfor observed relationships. A high negative correlationbetween amount of disruptive behavior <strong>in</strong> class <strong>and</strong>achievement may be simply a reflection of differ<strong>in</strong>g resources.Students <strong>in</strong> schools with few science materialscan be expected <strong>to</strong> do poorly <strong>in</strong> science <strong>and</strong> also <strong>to</strong> bedisruptive because of boredom or hostility. The onlyINSTRUMENTATIONInstrument Decay. In any study us<strong>in</strong>g a particular<strong>in</strong>strument many times, thought must be given <strong>to</strong> thepossibility of <strong>in</strong>strument decay. This is most likely <strong>in</strong>observational studies s<strong>in</strong>ce most other correlationalstudies do not use <strong>in</strong>struments many times (with thesame subjects at least). When both variables are measuredby an observational device at the same time, caremust be taken <strong>to</strong> ensure that observers don’t becometired, bored, or <strong>in</strong>attentive (this may require us<strong>in</strong>g additionalobservers). In a study <strong>in</strong> which observers areasked <strong>to</strong> record (dur<strong>in</strong>g the same time period) both thenumber of “thought questions” asked by the teacher <strong>and</strong>the attentiveness of students, for example, a tired (orbored) observer might miss <strong>in</strong>stances of each, result<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> low scores for the class on both variables, <strong>and</strong> thusdis<strong>to</strong>rtion <strong>in</strong> the correlation.Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r Characteristics. Characteristicsof data collec<strong>to</strong>rs can create a threat if differentpersons adm<strong>in</strong>ister both <strong>in</strong>struments. Gender, age, or ethnicity,for example, may affect specific responses, particularlywith op<strong>in</strong>ion or attitud<strong>in</strong>al <strong>in</strong>struments, as well asthe seriousness with which respondents answer certa<strong>in</strong>questions. One might expect an Air Force colonel <strong>in</strong> uniform,for example, <strong>to</strong> engender different scores on <strong>in</strong>strumentsmeasur<strong>in</strong>g attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward the military <strong>and</strong> (separately)<strong>to</strong>ward the aerospace <strong>in</strong>dustry than a civilian datacollec<strong>to</strong>r. If each data collec<strong>to</strong>r gives both <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>to</strong>several groups, the correlation between these scores willbe higher as a result of the impact of the data collec<strong>to</strong>r.Fortunately, this threat is easily avoided by hav<strong>in</strong>g each<strong>in</strong>strument adm<strong>in</strong>istered by a different <strong>in</strong>dividual.Data Collec<strong>to</strong>r Bias. Another <strong>in</strong>strumentationthreat can result from unconscious bias on the part of thedata gatherers whenever both <strong>in</strong>struments are given orscored by the same person. It is not uncommon, particularlywith <strong>in</strong>dividually adm<strong>in</strong>istered performance tests,for the same person <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister both tests <strong>to</strong> the samestudent, <strong>and</strong> even dur<strong>in</strong>g the same time period. It islikely that the observed or scored performance on the


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 341first test will affect the way <strong>in</strong> which the second test isadm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>and</strong>/or scored. It is almost impossible <strong>to</strong>avoid expectations based on the first test, <strong>and</strong> these maywell affect the exam<strong>in</strong>er’s behavior on the second test<strong>in</strong>g.A high score on the first test, for example, may lead<strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>er expectation of a high score on the second,result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> students be<strong>in</strong>g given additional time orencouragement on the second test. While precise <strong>in</strong>structionsfor adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments are helpful, a bettersolution is <strong>to</strong> have different adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs for each test.TESTINGThe experience of respond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the first <strong>in</strong>strument that isadm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> a correlational study may <strong>in</strong>fluence subjectresponses <strong>to</strong> the second <strong>in</strong>strument. Students asked <strong>to</strong>respond first <strong>to</strong> a “lik<strong>in</strong>g for teacher” scale, <strong>and</strong> thenshortly thereafter <strong>to</strong> a “lik<strong>in</strong>g for social studies” scale arelikely <strong>to</strong> see a connection. You can imag<strong>in</strong>e them say<strong>in</strong>g,perhaps, someth<strong>in</strong>g like, “Oh, I see, if I don’t like theteacher, I’m not supposed <strong>to</strong> like the subject.” To theextent that this happens, the results obta<strong>in</strong>ed can be mislead<strong>in</strong>g.The solution is <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister <strong>in</strong>struments, ifpossible, at different times <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> different contexts.MORTALITYMortality, strictly speak<strong>in</strong>g, is not a problem of <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity <strong>in</strong> correlational studies s<strong>in</strong>ce anyone “lost”must be excluded from the study—correlations cannotbe obta<strong>in</strong>ed unless a researcher has a score for each personon both of the variables be<strong>in</strong>g measured.There are times, however, when loss of subjects maymake a relationship more (or less) likely <strong>in</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdata, thus creat<strong>in</strong>g a threat <strong>to</strong> external validity. Why externalvalidity? Because the sample actually studied isoften not the sample <strong>in</strong>itially selected, because of mortality.Let us refer aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the study hypothesiz<strong>in</strong>g thatteacher expectation of failure would be positively correlatedwith amount of disruptive student behavior. It mightbe that those teachers who refused <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> thestudy were those who had a very low expectation offailure—who, <strong>in</strong> fact, expected their students <strong>to</strong> achieveat unrealistically high levels. It also seems likely that theclasses of those same teachers would exhibit a lot ofdisruptive behavior as a result of such unrealistic pressurefrom these teachers. Their loss would serve <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>creasethe correlation obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Because there is no way <strong>to</strong> knowwhether this possibility is correct, the only th<strong>in</strong>g theresearcher can do is <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> avoid los<strong>in</strong>g subjects.Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity <strong>in</strong> Correlational StudiesThe evaluation of specific threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>correlational studies follows a procedure similar <strong>to</strong> thatfor experimental studies.Step 1: Ask: What are the specific fac<strong>to</strong>rs that areknown <strong>to</strong> affect or could logically affect one ofthe variables be<strong>in</strong>g correlated? It does not matterwhich variable is selected.Step 2: Ask: What is the likelihood of each of thesefac<strong>to</strong>rs also affect<strong>in</strong>g the other variable be<strong>in</strong>g correlatedwith the first? We need not be concernedwith any fac<strong>to</strong>r unrelated <strong>to</strong> either variable. A fac<strong>to</strong>rmust be related <strong>to</strong> both variables <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> bea threat.*Step 3: <strong>Evaluate</strong> the various threats <strong>in</strong> terms of theirlikelihood, <strong>and</strong> plan <strong>to</strong> control them. If a giventhreat cannot be controlled, this should be acknowledged<strong>and</strong> discussed.As we did <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13, let us consider an example<strong>to</strong> show how these steps might be applied. Suppose aresearcher wishes <strong>to</strong> study the relationship betweensocial skills (as observed) <strong>and</strong> job success (as rated bysupervisors) of a group of severely disabled youngadults <strong>in</strong> a career education program. Listed belowaga<strong>in</strong> are several threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity discussed <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9 <strong>and</strong> our evaluation of each.Subject Characteristics. We consider here onlyfour of many possible characteristics.1. Severity of h<strong>and</strong>icap. Step 1: Rated job success canbe expected <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> severity of h<strong>and</strong>icap.Step 2: Severity of h<strong>and</strong>icap can also be expected <strong>to</strong>be related <strong>to</strong> social skills. Therefore, severity shouldbe assessed <strong>and</strong> controlled (us<strong>in</strong>g partial correlation).Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unlesscontrolled: high.2. Socioeconomic level of parents. Step 1: Parents’socioeconomic level is likely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> socialskills. Step 2: Parental socioeconomic status is notlikely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> job success for this group.While it is desirable <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> socioeconomic data*This rule must be modified with respect <strong>to</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> test<strong>in</strong>gthreats, where knowledge about the first <strong>in</strong>strument (or scores on it)may <strong>in</strong>fluence performance or assessment on the second <strong>in</strong>strument.


342 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e(<strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out more about the sample), it is not of highpriority. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unlesscontrolled: low.3. Physical strength <strong>and</strong> coord<strong>in</strong>ation. Step 1: Thesecharacteristics may be related <strong>to</strong> job success. Step 2:Strength <strong>and</strong> coord<strong>in</strong>ation are not likely <strong>to</strong> be related<strong>to</strong> social skills. While it is desirable <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> such<strong>in</strong>formation, it is not of high priority. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.4. Physical appearance. Step 1: Physical appearanceis likely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> social skills. Step 2: It is alsolikely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> rated job success. Therefore,this variable should be assessed <strong>and</strong> controlled(aga<strong>in</strong> by us<strong>in</strong>g partial correlation). Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.Mortality. Step 1: Subjects “lost” are likely <strong>to</strong> havepoorer job performance. Step 2: Lost subjects are alsomore likely <strong>to</strong> have poorer social skills. Thus, loss ofsubjects can be expected <strong>to</strong> reduce magnitude of correlation.Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unlesscontrolled: moderate <strong>to</strong> high.Location. Step 1: Because the subjects of the studywould (<strong>in</strong>evitably) be work<strong>in</strong>g at different job sites <strong>and</strong>under different conditions, location may well be related<strong>to</strong> rated job success. Step 2: If social skill is observedon-site, it may be related <strong>to</strong> the specific site conditions.While it is possible that this threat could be controlledby <strong>in</strong>dependently assess<strong>in</strong>g the job-site environments, abetter solution would be <strong>to</strong> assess social skills at a commonsite such as that used for group tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Step 3:Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.Instrumentation1. Instrument decay. Step 1: Instrument decay, if ithas occurred, is likely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> how accuratelysocial skills are measured. Observationsshould be scheduled, therefore, <strong>to</strong> preclude this possibility.Step 2: Instrument decay would be unlikely<strong>to</strong> affect job rat<strong>in</strong>gs. Therefore, its occurrence wouldnot be expected <strong>to</strong> account for any relationshipfound between the major variables. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: low.2. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics. Step 1: Data collec<strong>to</strong>rcharacteristics might well be related <strong>to</strong> job rat<strong>in</strong>gss<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>teraction of data collec<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> supervisorsis a necessary part of this study. Step 2:Characteristics of data collec<strong>to</strong>rs presumably wouldnot be related <strong>to</strong> their observation of social skills;nevertheless, <strong>to</strong> be on the safe side, this possibilityshould be controlled by hav<strong>in</strong>g the same data collec<strong>to</strong>rsobserve all subjects. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: moderate.3. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias. Step 1: Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of job successshould not be subject <strong>to</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, s<strong>in</strong>cedifferent supervisors will rate each subject. Step 2:Observations of social skills may be related <strong>to</strong>preconceptions of observers about the subjects,especially if they have prior knowledge of job successrat<strong>in</strong>gs. Therefore, observers should have noknowledge of job rat<strong>in</strong>gs. Step 3: Likelihood ofhav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.Test<strong>in</strong>g. Step 1: In this example, performance on thefirst <strong>in</strong>strument adm<strong>in</strong>istered cannot, of course, be affectedby performance on the second. Step 2: In thisstudy, scores on the second <strong>in</strong>strument cannot be affectedby performance on the first, s<strong>in</strong>ce the subjects are unawareof their performance on the first <strong>in</strong>strument. Step 3:Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: zero.Rationale for the Process of Evaluat<strong>in</strong>gThreats <strong>in</strong> Correlational Studies. We will try<strong>to</strong> demonstrate the logic beh<strong>in</strong>d the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple that a fac<strong>to</strong>rmust be related <strong>to</strong> both correlated variables <strong>in</strong> order<strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> a correlation between them. Consider thethree scatterplots shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.8, which representthe scores of a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals on three variables:A, B, <strong>and</strong> C. Scatterplot 1 shows a substantial correlationbetween A <strong>and</strong> B; scatterplot 2 shows a substantialcorrelation between A <strong>and</strong> C; scatterplot 3 shows a zerocorrelation between B <strong>and</strong> C.Suppose the researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gwhether the correlation between variables A <strong>and</strong> B canbe “expla<strong>in</strong>ed” by variable C. A <strong>and</strong> B, <strong>in</strong> other words,represent the variables be<strong>in</strong>g studied, while C representsa third variable be<strong>in</strong>g evaluated as a potential threat <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity. If the researcher tries <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> thecorrelation between A <strong>and</strong> B as due <strong>to</strong> C, he or shecannot. Here’s why.Suppose we say that person 1, shown <strong>in</strong> scatterplot 1,is high on A <strong>and</strong> B because he or she is high on C. Sureenough, be<strong>in</strong>g high on C would predict be<strong>in</strong>g high on A.You can see this <strong>in</strong> scatterplot 2. <strong>How</strong>ever, be<strong>in</strong>g highon C does not predict be<strong>in</strong>g high on B, because althoughsome <strong>in</strong>dividuals who scored high on C did score highon B, others who scored high on C scored <strong>in</strong> the middleor low on B. You can see this <strong>in</strong> scatterplot 3.


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 343Person1Person1Person1?Person1?BABPerson1?ACCScatterplot 1Scatterplot 2Scatterplot 3Figure 15.8 Scatterplots Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>How</strong> a Fac<strong>to</strong>r (C) May Not Be a Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityABABABCDiagram 1Diagram 2Figure 15.9 Circle Diagrams Illustrat<strong>in</strong>g Relationships Among VariablesCDiagram 3Another way of portray<strong>in</strong>g this logic is with circle diagrams,as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.9.Diagram 1 <strong>in</strong> Figure 15.9 illustrates a correlationbetween A <strong>and</strong> B. This is shown by the overlap <strong>in</strong> circles;the greater the overlap, the greater the correlation.Diagram 2 shows a third circle, C, which representsthe additional variable that is be<strong>in</strong>g considered asa possible threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. Because it is correlatedwith both A <strong>and</strong> B, it may be considered a possibleexplanation for at least part of the correlation betweenthem. This is shown by the fact that circle Coverlaps both A <strong>and</strong> B. By way of contrast, diagram 3shows that whereas C is correlated with A, it is not correlatedwith B (there is no overlap). Because C overlapsonly with A (i.e., it does not overlap with bothvariables), it cannot be considered a possible alternativeexplanation for the correlation between A <strong>and</strong> B.Diagram 3, <strong>in</strong> other words, shows what the three scatterplots<strong>in</strong> Figure 15.8 do, namely, that A is correlatedwith B, <strong>and</strong> that A is correlated with C, but that B is notcorrelated with C.An Exampleof Correlational <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of correlational research, followed by a critiqueof its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> ourcritique of the experimental <strong>and</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject studiesanalyzed <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 <strong>and</strong> 14, we use several of theconcepts <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> ouranalysis.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al Psychology, 95, no. 4 (2003): 813–820. Copyright © 2003 by the American PsychologicalAssociation. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission.When Teachers’ <strong>and</strong> Parents’Values Differ: Teachers’ Rat<strong>in</strong>gsof Academic Competence <strong>in</strong> Childrenfrom Low-Income FamiliesPenny Hauser-Cram <strong>and</strong> Selcuk R. Sir<strong>in</strong>Bos<strong>to</strong>n CollegeDeborah StipekStanford UniversityThe authors exam<strong>in</strong>ed predic<strong>to</strong>rs of teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of academic competence of 105k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children from low-<strong>in</strong>come families. Teachers rated target children’s expectedcompetence <strong>in</strong> literacy <strong>and</strong> math <strong>and</strong> completed questions about their perceptions of congruence–dissonancebetween themselves <strong>and</strong> the child’s parents regard<strong>in</strong>g education-relatedvalues. Independent exam<strong>in</strong>ers assessed children’s literacy <strong>and</strong> math skills. Teachers’<strong>in</strong>structional styles were observed <strong>and</strong> rated along dimensions of curriculum-centered <strong>and</strong>student-centered practices. Controll<strong>in</strong>g for children’s skills <strong>and</strong> socioeconomic status,teachers rated children as less competent when they perceived value differences with parents.These patterns were stronger for teachers who exhibited curriculum-centered, ratherthan student-centered, practices. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest a mechanism by which some childrenfrom low-<strong>in</strong>come families enter a path of dim<strong>in</strong>ished expectations.Prior researchChildren from low-<strong>in</strong>come families typically beg<strong>in</strong> their school experience with feweracademic skills than their middle-<strong>in</strong>come peers (Lee & Burkam, 2002), <strong>and</strong> they rema<strong>in</strong>on a path of relatively low performance (Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Entwisle, & Horsey, 1997; Den<strong>to</strong>n &West, 2002; Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997). A range of explanations are offered for theperformance discrepancies associated with family socioeconomic status (SES). Family<strong>and</strong> community <strong>in</strong>fluences are implicated <strong>in</strong> some research; other studies suggest thatsystematic differences <strong>in</strong> school resources, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the quality of teachers, furtherdisadvantage low-<strong>in</strong>come children (Augenblick, Myers, & Anderson, 1997; Betts, Rueben,& Danenberg, 2000; Parrish & Fowler, 1995; Unnever, Kerckhoff, & Rob<strong>in</strong>son, 2000).Many researchers <strong>and</strong> policymakers contend also that teachers expect less of childrenfrom low-<strong>in</strong>come <strong>and</strong> other stigmatized groups <strong>and</strong> therefore provide less rigorousacademic <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>and</strong> lower st<strong>and</strong>ards for achievement. Consistent with this view, relativelylow expectations exist <strong>in</strong> many schools serv<strong>in</strong>g low-<strong>in</strong>come students (Hall<strong>in</strong>ger,Bickman, & Davis, 1996; Hall<strong>in</strong>ger & Murphy, 1986; Kennedy, 1995; Leithwood, Begley, &Cous<strong>in</strong>s, 1990; McLoyd, 1998). Kennedy (1995), for example, analyzed data on the academicclimate of 250 third-grade classrooms <strong>in</strong> a stratified sample of 76 schools <strong>in</strong> Louisiana.The proportion of low-<strong>in</strong>come students was strongly negatively correlated with teachers’perceptions of students’ ability. Although the SES of the student body was also a strongpredic<strong>to</strong>r of academic norms (i.e., peer support for academic performance), the peernorm differences disappeared when teacher expectations entered <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the regressionanalysis. In addition <strong>to</strong> lower expectations for academic performance, teachers perceivechildren from low-SES families as be<strong>in</strong>g less mature <strong>and</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g poorer self-regula<strong>to</strong>ry344


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 345skills than their peers (McLoyd, 1998). In a study of first graders from low-SES families, forexample, Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Entwisle, <strong>and</strong> Thompson (1987) found that teachers from higherstatus backgrounds gave more adverse evaluations of the maturity of m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>and</strong> low-SES-status children as well as held lower expectations for their academic performance.Although the methods typically used <strong>to</strong> study teacher-expectation effects have beencriticized (e.g., Babad, 1993; Brophy, 1983), teacher expectations for student performancedo <strong>in</strong>fluence teachers’ behavior <strong>to</strong>ward students <strong>and</strong> students’ learn<strong>in</strong>g (Jussim & Eccles,1992; Jussim, Eccles, & Madon, 1996; Rosenthal & Jacobson, 1968; see Stipek, 2002; Wigfield& Harold, 1992, for reviews). Children who typically receive relatively low expectations maybe the most affected by teacher expectations. Jussim et al. (1996) provided evidence thatteacher-expectancy effects are stronger among stigmatized groups, such as African Americans,children from families with low SES, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> a lesser extent, girls. In a study of low-<strong>in</strong>comeAfrican American students, Gill <strong>and</strong> Reynolds (2000) found that teacher expectationshad a powerful direct <strong>in</strong>fluence on academic achievement. Thus, children <strong>in</strong> stigmatizedgroups are both prone <strong>to</strong> more adverse expectations by teachers <strong>and</strong> also are more likely <strong>to</strong>have such expectations lead <strong>to</strong> self-fulfill<strong>in</strong>g prophecies of poor academic performance. Lowexpectations <strong>in</strong> particular are likely <strong>to</strong> have susta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g effects on children’s performance.Teacher expectations appear <strong>to</strong> be particularly important <strong>in</strong> the early elementarygrades. In their classic study, Rosenthal <strong>and</strong> Jacobson (1968) found that the first <strong>and</strong> secondgraders, but not the older children <strong>in</strong> the study, evidenced teachers’ self-fulfill<strong>in</strong>gprophecies. Kukl<strong>in</strong>ski <strong>and</strong> We<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong> (2001), likewise, reported that teacher expectanciesaccentuated achievement differences <strong>to</strong> a greater extent <strong>in</strong> the early elementary gradesthan <strong>in</strong> the later elementary grades. And <strong>in</strong> a meta-analytic review, Raudenbush (1984)found teacher expectancies <strong>to</strong> produce their greatest effects on children <strong>in</strong> the earlygrades, but also noted an effect <strong>in</strong> seventh grade. Jussim et al. (1996) suggested that childrenmay be most vulnerable <strong>to</strong> teacher-expectation effects at key transition po<strong>in</strong>ts, suchas school entry or change of school (as often occurs <strong>in</strong> seventh grade), rather than at aparticular developmental age per se.Given the effects of teacher expectations on student learn<strong>in</strong>g, it is important <strong>to</strong>underst<strong>and</strong> what fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>fluence teacher judgments about students’ academic competence.One robust f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is that teacher expectations are strongly associated with children’sactual skills (Brophy, 1983; Jussim & Eccles, 1992; Jussim et al., 1996; Wigfield,Galper, Den<strong>to</strong>n, & Seefeldt, 1999). Jussim et al. (1996) ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed that children’s skill levels<strong>in</strong>fluence teachers’ expectations, which <strong>in</strong> turn affect children’s future performance.Thus, children’s school performance becomes part of a cycle of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g or decreas<strong>in</strong>gexpectations, which, <strong>in</strong> turn, leads <strong>to</strong> future performance. Consistent with this view,when children’s skills are considered, the statistical effects of teacher expectations onstudent learn<strong>in</strong>g are dim<strong>in</strong>ished. Teacher expectations, nevertheless, predict studentachievement, even with students’ previous achievement held constant (Jussim et al.,1996; Kukl<strong>in</strong>ski & We<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>, 2001), suggest<strong>in</strong>g that other fac<strong>to</strong>rs enter <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> teacherjudgments <strong>and</strong> that teacher judgments affect students’ learn<strong>in</strong>g regardless of whetherthey are based on students’ academic skills.In brief, young, low-<strong>in</strong>come children <strong>and</strong> young children of color may be particularlyvulnerable <strong>to</strong> negative effects of teacher expectations. These effects may be especiallypowerful as children make the transition <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> school. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, this study focusedon k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children from various ethnic groups, liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come families.Not all young children from low-<strong>in</strong>come families perform poorly, however, <strong>and</strong> notall teachers expect poor performance from such children. Less is known about thesources of bias <strong>in</strong> teachers’ judgments. For example, researchers have not tried <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong>why teachers perceive children from low-<strong>in</strong>come families <strong>to</strong> be less academically competentGood summaryInternal validityJustification


346 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePurpose<strong>and</strong> what fac<strong>to</strong>rs contribute <strong>to</strong> variation <strong>in</strong> teachers’ perceptions of children from low-<strong>in</strong>comefamilies. The purpose of this <strong>in</strong>vestigation was <strong>to</strong> assess possible predic<strong>to</strong>rs ofteacher expectations of students from low-<strong>in</strong>come families. Specifically, we assessed theextent <strong>to</strong> which family SES <strong>and</strong> teachers’ perceptions of value differences between themselves<strong>and</strong> students’ parents expla<strong>in</strong> variation <strong>in</strong> teacher perceptions <strong>and</strong> expectations ofstudents’ academic competence. Further, we <strong>in</strong>vestigated whether such variation exists<strong>in</strong> classrooms with dist<strong>in</strong>ctly different styles of <strong>in</strong>structional practice.TEACHER–PARENT VALUE DIFFERENCESJustificationRationaleImplied hypothesisMost teachers <strong>in</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come communities differ from the families <strong>in</strong> those communities<strong>in</strong> terms of educational background <strong>and</strong> ethnicity (Alex<strong>and</strong>er et al., 1987). Much hasbeen written about the potential negative consequences for children of a mismatchbetween the culture of the school <strong>and</strong> the culture of their families (e.g., Delpit, 1995;Ogbu, 1993). But this literature focuses on children’s experience of cultural differences.In contrast, teachers’ perceptions of the values <strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> cultural <strong>and</strong> socioeconomicdifferences <strong>and</strong> the effects of these perceptions on their judgments of children have notbeen studied. We focus here on values that are directly related <strong>to</strong> education—effectiveteach<strong>in</strong>g practices, classroom discipl<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> parent <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g(Okagaki & French, 1998; Okagaki & Sternberg, 1993).Parents hold particular ethnotheories about rais<strong>in</strong>g their children (Super & Harkness,1997), <strong>and</strong> their perspectives may differ substantially from those of teachers. For example,beliefs about appropriate parent<strong>in</strong>g practices <strong>and</strong> ways <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>teract with schools vary accord<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> ethnic identity (Ogbu, 1993) <strong>and</strong> social class (Lareau, 1987). Accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> Weisner,Gallimore, <strong>and</strong> Jordan (1988), the scripts used by participants <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g contextsreflect belief systems, which differ by ethnocultural group. Because teachers oftenhave children from diverse cultural groups with<strong>in</strong> one classroom, they need <strong>to</strong> become familiarwith a range of cultural scripts <strong>and</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>g belief systems. This may pose a difficultchallenge for some teachers. For example, Lasky (2000) found that teachers were morecomfortable with parents who shared a similar value system <strong>to</strong> their own <strong>and</strong> often becamedemoralized, angry, <strong>and</strong> discouraged with parents who did not share the same values.Children are presumably disadvantaged when their parents <strong>and</strong> teachers hold differentvalues with respect <strong>to</strong> desired classroom practices <strong>and</strong> behavior. One negativeconsequence of such a mismatch may be lowered teacher expectations. Teachers mayreason, for example, that parents who do not share the teachers’ views of appropriatechild rear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g will fail <strong>to</strong> provide the support that children need <strong>to</strong> learn effectively.As a result, teachers may (even unknow<strong>in</strong>gly) lower their expectations of theschool achievement of such children. Therefore, <strong>in</strong> this study, we assessed associationsbetween teachers’ perceptions of education-related value differences between themselves<strong>and</strong> parents <strong>and</strong> their perception of the children’s current <strong>and</strong> future academiccompetencies. Values related <strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g academic subjects (math, read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g)<strong>and</strong> discipl<strong>in</strong>e were selected because teachers’ attention is largely focused on these doma<strong>in</strong>s,<strong>and</strong> they are frequently discussed <strong>in</strong> parent–teacher conferences. The issue of parents’role <strong>in</strong> assist<strong>in</strong>g their child <strong>in</strong> schoolwork was also <strong>in</strong>cluded because it is a commonsource of conflict or confusion (Baker, 1997; L<strong>in</strong>ek, Ras<strong>in</strong>ski, & Hark<strong>in</strong>s, 1997).Even with<strong>in</strong> a sample of low-<strong>in</strong>come families, there may be considerable variation<strong>in</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> which parents’ values differ from teachers. We suspected, however, thatperceptions of value differences might be confounded with parents’ SES. Perceived valuedifferences are not the only reason why teachers may have relatively low expectationsfor the academic success of children from low-SES families. For example, they may


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 347assume that the lower the children’s SES is, the more stress there is on the family, or theless stable <strong>and</strong> more crowded home conditions are. To be able <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e the <strong>in</strong>dependentpredictive value of perceived value differences, we also <strong>in</strong>cluded a measure of SES.CLASSROOM PRACTICESTeachers may vary <strong>in</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong> which their expectations for students are affected bytheir perception of discrepant values. Teachers who are sensitive <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences<strong>and</strong> adjust <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>and</strong> discipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual children’s skills, learn<strong>in</strong>g styles,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terests may not view differences between themselves <strong>and</strong> parents as an impediment<strong>to</strong> children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g. They may assume that they can adjust <strong>and</strong> effectively teachchildren regardless of whether their values differ from the children’s parents. Teacherswho have a rigid whole-class curriculum <strong>and</strong> do not adjust <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>and</strong> discipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dividual children may, <strong>in</strong> contrast, assume that children who do not experience similardiscipl<strong>in</strong>e approaches <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g at home will have difficulty adjust<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> their curriculum<strong>and</strong> management strategies <strong>and</strong> thus perform less well. To test this hypothesis,we observed each participat<strong>in</strong>g child’s classroom <strong>and</strong> rated teachers on the degree <strong>to</strong>which they had a flexible teach<strong>in</strong>g style that adjusted <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual children’s needs(referred <strong>to</strong> as a student-centered approach) versus a uniform approach dictated by acurriculum (referred <strong>to</strong> as curriculum centered). We predicted that curriculum-centeredteachers’ perceptions of value differences with parents would be more strongly l<strong>in</strong>ked<strong>to</strong> their perceptions of students’ academic skills than would be true for studentcenteredteachers.The notions of student-centered <strong>and</strong> curriculum-centered <strong>in</strong>struction are rooted<strong>in</strong> a debate about effective educational practices. The National Association for the <strong>Education</strong>of Young Children <strong>and</strong> many subject-matter experts embrace an educational approachthat <strong>in</strong>dividualizes <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>to</strong> address differences <strong>in</strong> children’s skill levels <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> which children work <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>and</strong> collaboratively <strong>to</strong> constructtheir own underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Student-centered lessons <strong>in</strong>volveconversations with students as well as some direct teach<strong>in</strong>g (Berk & W<strong>in</strong>sler, 1995;Committee on the Prevention of Read<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties <strong>in</strong> Young Children, 1998; NationalAcademy of <strong>Education</strong>, Commission on Read<strong>in</strong>g, 1985; National Council of Teachers ofMathematics, 1991; National <strong>Research</strong> Council, Committee on the Prevention of Read<strong>in</strong>gDifficulties <strong>in</strong> Young Children, 1998). In contrast, there are also proponents of highlyteacher-directed <strong>in</strong>struction (e.g., Becker & Gersten, 1982; Carn<strong>in</strong>e, Carn<strong>in</strong>e, Karp, &Weisberg, 1988; Meyer, Gersten, & Gutk<strong>in</strong>, 1983). Some researchers claim that explora<strong>to</strong>rylearn<strong>in</strong>g emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g au<strong>to</strong>nomy <strong>and</strong> creativity is a luxury that poor childrencannot afford <strong>and</strong> is <strong>in</strong>congruous with the teach<strong>in</strong>g styles <strong>and</strong> goals of low-<strong>in</strong>come families(Delpit, 1995). These more directive methods, which we refer <strong>to</strong> as curriculum centered,typically <strong>in</strong>volve structured lessons—sometimes even scripted lessons—which arefully teacher led. Student work is usually <strong>in</strong> the form of workbooks that all students areasked <strong>to</strong> complete.In summary, we <strong>in</strong>vestigated the extent <strong>to</strong> which a demographic marker (i.e.,SES) <strong>and</strong> a measure of value discrepancies (i.e., teachers’ perceived differences withparents regard<strong>in</strong>g education-related values) related <strong>to</strong> teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’sacademic competence. Further, we considered the potential moderat<strong>in</strong>g effects of theclassroom teach<strong>in</strong>g style on these relations. We have purposefully selected <strong>to</strong> studystudents from low-<strong>in</strong>come families, who are already at a disadvantage when theybeg<strong>in</strong> school, at a vulnerable transition po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> terms of their school experience, thek<strong>in</strong>dergarten year.Secondaryhypothesis


348 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMETHODConveniencesamplePossible replicationGood descriptionParticipantsParticipants <strong>in</strong>cluded 105 k<strong>in</strong>dergarten students (53% girls) who were orig<strong>in</strong>ally enrolledas <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>in</strong> a longitud<strong>in</strong>al study of very low-<strong>in</strong>come families. Data for the present <strong>in</strong>vestigationwere collected <strong>in</strong> the spr<strong>in</strong>g of children’s k<strong>in</strong>dergarten year, when most childrenwere 5 or 6 years old. All participat<strong>in</strong>g children were from low-<strong>in</strong>come families <strong>in</strong> threedifferent localities, an urban area <strong>in</strong> the northeast, a rural area <strong>in</strong> the northeast, <strong>and</strong> anurban area on the west coast. The average reported annual family <strong>in</strong>come was between$9,000 <strong>and</strong> $12,000; 57.2% of mothers reported receiv<strong>in</strong>g food stamps. About half of themothers were employed, 27.9% full time <strong>and</strong> 23.1% part time. Mothers varied <strong>in</strong> educationlevel: 31.4% had less than a high school degree, 27.3% completed high school or itsequivalent, <strong>and</strong> 41.3% had some tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g beyond high school (e.g., community collegecourses or specialized vocational courses). About one third of children (36.2%) lived withmarried parents, <strong>and</strong> most had at least one sibl<strong>in</strong>g (86.4%). Children were from a rangeof ethnic groups: African American (30%), Euro-American (33%), Lat<strong>in</strong>o (27%), <strong>and</strong> multiracial(10%).The 105 children were distributed among 56 classrooms. All teachers were female,<strong>and</strong> most had a master’s degree or some graduate school tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (78%). Theyranged <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g experience from 1 <strong>to</strong> 38 years (M 22.8 years). Teachers also varied<strong>in</strong> their ethnicity, although most were Euro-American (76% Euro-American, 9% AfricanAmerican, 7% Lat<strong>in</strong>o, <strong>and</strong> 8% Asian American). Most children were enrolled <strong>in</strong> schoolsthat serve primarily a low-<strong>in</strong>come population, as 80% of schools had more than half ofenrolled students eligible for free or reduced lunch. Schools ranged <strong>in</strong> size from a lowof 73 students <strong>to</strong> a high of 1,077 students, <strong>and</strong> most schools (66%) served gradesk<strong>in</strong>dergarten through sixth grade.Procedure <strong>and</strong> MeasuresFour sources of data were used for this <strong>in</strong>vestigation, all collected dur<strong>in</strong>g a 3-month period<strong>in</strong> the spr<strong>in</strong>g of children’s k<strong>in</strong>dergarten year: (a) questionnaires were completed byteachers, (b) children’s academic skills were assessed by an <strong>in</strong>dependent exam<strong>in</strong>er, (c) demographicdata were gathered from parents dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews, <strong>and</strong> (d) observationswere made of the k<strong>in</strong>dergarten classrooms by tra<strong>in</strong>ed field staff.<strong>How</strong> were theyselected?Good descriptionTeacher Questionnaires. Teachers were either given or mailed questionnaires onparticipat<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong> return them by mail. Most teachers (89%) had only1 or 2 participat<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>in</strong> their classrooms. In addition <strong>to</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g demographic <strong>in</strong>formationabout themselves, teachers were asked <strong>to</strong> rate children’s academic competencies<strong>in</strong> math <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g separately (“Please rate the child’s read<strong>in</strong>g–math-related skills”).They were asked <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate their expectations of the child’s future performance one yearfrom that time (“<strong>How</strong> well do you expect the child <strong>to</strong> do next year <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g–math?”). A5-po<strong>in</strong>t response scale was used for both questions (1 well below children this age, 2 below children this age, 3 about average, 4 above children this age, 5 well abovechildren this age). Teachers were also asked <strong>to</strong> predict children’s performance <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> math (separately) by the end of third grade (“Do you expect the child <strong>to</strong> be on gradelevel or above <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g–math by the end of third grade?”). A 4-po<strong>in</strong>t response scale wasused (1 def<strong>in</strong>itely no, 2 probably no, 3 probably yes, 4 def<strong>in</strong>itely yes). Teachers’rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s current competence <strong>and</strong> their expectations for children’s first- <strong>and</strong>


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 349third-grade performance were so highly correlated (r .75 <strong>and</strong> r .88 for read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> r .78<strong>and</strong> r .93 for math) that it did not seem reasonable <strong>to</strong> treat judgments of currentcompetencies <strong>and</strong> expectations for future performance as separate constructs. Thus, allthree items were comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> create two composite measures of teacher perceptions ofchildren’s competency, one for literacy (.93) <strong>and</strong> one for math (.94).On the basis of a review of the literature on teach<strong>in</strong>g practices, teachers were alsogiven a list of goals identified as potentially important for young children <strong>to</strong> develop <strong>in</strong>school. They were asked <strong>to</strong> rate the importance of each goal relative <strong>to</strong> the other goalson a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t scale, rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (not at all important) <strong>to</strong> 5 (very important). Afac<strong>to</strong>r analysis revealed three scales reflect<strong>in</strong>g: (a) traditional basic skills goals (e.g., workhabits, factual knowledge, basic math <strong>and</strong> literacy skills; M 3.73, SD 0.66, .59),(b) higher order th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g goals (e.g., critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong>dependence <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>itiative, creativity;M 3.97, SD 0.53, .63), <strong>and</strong> (c) social development goals (e.g., social skills,cooperation; M 4.31, SD 0.63, .51).In a separate section of the questionnaire, teachers were asked whether they consideredtheir education-related values <strong>to</strong> be similar or different from those of the participat<strong>in</strong>gchild’s parent(s). A set of five questions asked teachers <strong>to</strong> rate congruence with achild’s parent(s) with regard <strong>to</strong> discipl<strong>in</strong>e, parents’ role <strong>in</strong> a child’s education, <strong>and</strong> theteach<strong>in</strong>g of math, literacy, <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g (“Are there differences between the parents’ valuesor preferences <strong>and</strong> your values with respect <strong>to</strong> the educational program <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gareas: discipl<strong>in</strong>e, read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, math, parents’ role <strong>in</strong> assist<strong>in</strong>g their child?”). Aresponse scale of 3 po<strong>in</strong>ts was used (1 no difference, 2 some difference, 3 greatdifference). The Cronbach’s alpha for this sample on this set of items was .92.See our “Analysisof the study,”p. 358. We don’tagree.Good reliabilitySee p. 334Assessment of Children’s Skills. Children’s skills were assessed <strong>in</strong>dependently bytra<strong>in</strong>ed exam<strong>in</strong>ers. The exam<strong>in</strong>ers presented the material <strong>in</strong> English or Spanish, depend<strong>in</strong>gon the child’s language preference. The math assessment measured children’s count<strong>in</strong>gabilities <strong>and</strong> familiarity with numbers (items from the Peabody Individual AchievementTest—Revised; Dunn & Dunn, 1981), their strategies for solv<strong>in</strong>g word problems(Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, & Fennema, 1993; Carpenter, Fennema, & Franke, 1996), <strong>and</strong>their skills <strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>g (us<strong>in</strong>g a calculation subscale of the Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-<strong>Education</strong>al Battery—Revised [WJ-R]; Woodcock & Johnson, 1990). Four composite variableswere created from the items <strong>in</strong> the math assessment: count<strong>in</strong>g—early numbertasks; problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g, pencil–paper calculations; <strong>and</strong> geometric items. The compositevariables were st<strong>and</strong>ardized <strong>and</strong> averaged <strong>to</strong> create a <strong>to</strong>tal math skills score.The literacy assessment measured children’s abilities <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>and</strong> preread<strong>in</strong>g),writ<strong>in</strong>g, comprehension, <strong>and</strong> verbal fluency (Saunders, 1999; letter-word identification<strong>and</strong> passage comprehension subscales of the WJ–R; Woodcock & Johnson, 1990). Six compositevariables were created: letter–sound identification, word read<strong>in</strong>g, overall read<strong>in</strong>g,writ<strong>in</strong>g, oral comprehension, <strong>and</strong> verbal fluency. The composite variables were st<strong>and</strong>ardized<strong>and</strong> averaged <strong>to</strong> create a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>to</strong>tal literacy skill score.Classroom Observation Measure. Tra<strong>in</strong>ed observers used the Early Childhood ClassroomObservation Measure (ECCOM) developed by Stipek <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Byler &Stipek, 2003; Stipek et al., 1998). Observations were conducted dur<strong>in</strong>g the spr<strong>in</strong>g of theparticipat<strong>in</strong>g child’s k<strong>in</strong>dergarten year <strong>to</strong> document the teach<strong>in</strong>g approach used <strong>in</strong> theclassroom. Observers began their observations at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the school day <strong>and</strong>rema<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the classroom for at least 3 hr, return<strong>in</strong>g the follow<strong>in</strong>g day if they had no<strong>to</strong>bserved a math <strong>and</strong> a literacy activity.


350 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Judged <strong>to</strong> be”AdequateGoodGoodTwo sets of 17 items <strong>in</strong> the ECCOM were used for this <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ethe classroom <strong>in</strong>structional environment. Observers gave a score of 1 (low) <strong>to</strong> 5 (high)<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the extent <strong>to</strong> which the classroom looked like each descrip<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> then wrotea justification for each score. One set of observation items was used <strong>to</strong> assess the degree<strong>to</strong> which teachers were student centered, <strong>and</strong> another set of items was used <strong>to</strong> assesshow curriculum centered the teacher was. Teachers provided self-reports of their <strong>in</strong>structionalgoals regard<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g of basic skills <strong>and</strong> higher order th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g processes.The set of student-centered descrip<strong>to</strong>rs is aligned with the developmentally appropriatepractice guidel<strong>in</strong>es issued by the National Association for the <strong>Education</strong> of YoungChildren (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Teachers receiv<strong>in</strong>g a high score on these itemswere respectful <strong>and</strong> responsive <strong>to</strong> children, encouraged children <strong>to</strong> communicate <strong>and</strong>elaborate on their thoughts, <strong>and</strong> celebrated each other’s achievements, at whateverlevel they occurred. They applied rules consistently but not rigidly, <strong>and</strong> children had responsibility<strong>and</strong> opportunities for leadership roles <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> solve problems on their own.The teacher <strong>in</strong>dividually moni<strong>to</strong>red, assisted, <strong>and</strong> challenged children. They also solicitedchildren’s questions, ideas, solutions, or <strong>in</strong>terpretations. Mathematics <strong>and</strong> literacy <strong>in</strong>structionbalanced an emphasis on underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> opportunities <strong>to</strong> practice, <strong>and</strong>children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g was assessed regularly. Interrater reliability for the summary score onthese items based on the 17 rat<strong>in</strong>gs, with two raters rat<strong>in</strong>g 18 classrooms, was .79.The parallel set of 17 curriculum-centered items rated classrooms on how directive<strong>and</strong> rigid teachers were. The items described practices <strong>in</strong> which teachers enforced strictrules <strong>and</strong> gave children few opportunities <strong>to</strong> take responsibility or <strong>to</strong> choose activities;children were held accountable <strong>to</strong> rigid st<strong>and</strong>ards that were not adjusted <strong>to</strong> children’s <strong>in</strong>dividualskill levels. Tasks were fully def<strong>in</strong>ed by the teacher or a published curriculum, <strong>and</strong>the teacher dom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>and</strong> controlled discussion <strong>and</strong> conversation. Math <strong>and</strong> literacy <strong>in</strong>structionfocused on discrete skills <strong>and</strong> heavy reliance on workbooks, with correctness emphasized.Additionally, there was relatively little attention given <strong>to</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g social <strong>and</strong>communication skills, children did not have much time <strong>to</strong> work collaboratively, <strong>and</strong> activitieswere not adjusted <strong>to</strong> children’s <strong>in</strong>dividual skills <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terests. Interrater reliability onthe summary score of a subset of 25 classrooms for this set of descrip<strong>to</strong>rs was .95.Teachers who were high on one set of descrip<strong>to</strong>rs tended <strong>to</strong> be low on the other(r .90, p .001). Therefore, we created a composite measure of classroom practicesby st<strong>and</strong>ardiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> reverse scor<strong>in</strong>g the items high on the curriculum-centered scale <strong>and</strong>add<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> those on the student-centered scale (st<strong>and</strong>ardized). The f<strong>in</strong>al scale had apotential range of 5.0 (<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g highly curriculum-centered practices) <strong>to</strong> 5.0 (<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>ghighly student-centered practices; the actual scores ranged from 2.78 <strong>to</strong> 3.21).Cronbach’s alpha for the composite score was .94.RESULTSNeed descriptivedata hereInappropriate <strong>and</strong>mislead<strong>in</strong>g (see ouranalysis (p. 358)Teacher competency rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> math <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g did not differ by children’s gender,race–ethnicity, or geographical location. Further, teachers’ perceptions of value differenceswith parents did not differ by teachers’ race–ethnicity, school geographic location,or children’s race–ethnicity, although there was a trend <strong>to</strong>ward greater value discrepancybetween teachers <strong>and</strong> African American parents than between teachers <strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o orEuro-American parents, F(2, 84) 2.95, p .06. There were <strong>to</strong>o few teachers of color <strong>to</strong>assess whether shar<strong>in</strong>g or not shar<strong>in</strong>g ethnicity with parents predicted teachers’ value discrepancyjudgments. Euro-American parents were more likely <strong>to</strong> have the same ethnicityas their child’s teacher than were African American <strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o parents (because most ofthe teachers were Euro-American), but the proportion of African American parents


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 351whose ethnicity differed from their child’s teacher’s ethnicity (86%) was not greater thanthat of Lat<strong>in</strong>o parents (85%). Although ethnicity differences may have contributed <strong>to</strong>teachers’ greater value discrepancy rat<strong>in</strong>gs for African American parents, if it were simplya matter of hav<strong>in</strong>g different ethnic backgrounds, discrepancy scores should havebeen higher for Lat<strong>in</strong>o parents (who also often spoke a different language from teachers)than Euro-American parents.Teachers rated 48% of children <strong>to</strong> be currently at grade level, 18% above gradelevel, <strong>and</strong> 34% below grade level <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g; for math they rated 51% of their studentsat grade level, 21% above grade level, <strong>and</strong> 28% below grade level. They expected 74%of children <strong>to</strong> be at grade level or above grade level by third grade <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> 78%of children <strong>to</strong> be at or above grade level by third grade <strong>in</strong> math skills. The higher proportionof children be<strong>in</strong>g rated below grade level than above grade level would be expected<strong>in</strong> a sample of very low-<strong>in</strong>come children who entered school with below-average cognitiveskills (Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [Dunn & Dunn, 1981] score average of 88.63,SD 16.30, at 60 months).As a check on the validity of the observation measure, we computed correlations betweenobservers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of teachers’ practices <strong>and</strong> teachers’ self-reported <strong>in</strong>structionalgoals. Teachers with observed student-centered practices reported plac<strong>in</strong>g relatively moreemphasis on the development of children’s higher order th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g strategies (r .30, p .001) <strong>and</strong> less emphasis on develop<strong>in</strong>g basic skills (r .22, p .01). In comparison, teachersobserved <strong>to</strong> use curriculum-centered practices reported less emphasis on higher orderth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g strategies (r .35, p .001) <strong>and</strong> more emphasis on teach<strong>in</strong>g basic skills (r .38,p .001). Therefore, teachers’ reported goals were consistent with their observed practices.To test the ma<strong>in</strong> questions posed here, we used hierarchical regression analyses.Given the scatter of students across classrooms, we could not apply methods such ashierarchical l<strong>in</strong>ear model<strong>in</strong>g that take advantage of students nested <strong>in</strong> classrooms. On thebasis of prior research, we expected children’s actual skills <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gsof their competencies. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, the variable represent<strong>in</strong>g children’s performanceon the academic skills assessment was entered first. Our questions of <strong>in</strong>terest related<strong>to</strong> the variables added after the academic skills’ variable. We constructed acomposite measure of maternal education <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>come (based on maternal report) as aproxy variable for SES. SES was entered next <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether teachers rated children’scompetencies differently on the basis of SES, controll<strong>in</strong>g for children’s actual levelof skills. Third, the value-difference variable was entered <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether teachers’rat<strong>in</strong>gs varied by their perception of value differences with parents, after children’s academicskills <strong>and</strong> SES were accounted for. F<strong>in</strong>ally, we tested whether the type of classroom<strong>in</strong>struction practices predicted teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s academic competence <strong>and</strong>whether such practices moderated associations found between perceived value differences<strong>and</strong> child competency rat<strong>in</strong>gs. Consistent with Baron <strong>and</strong> Kenny (1986), an <strong>in</strong>teractionterm was created as the product of the cont<strong>in</strong>uous variables, <strong>and</strong> a hierarchical,<strong>in</strong>cremental F test was used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the <strong>in</strong>teraction added significantlyover <strong>and</strong> above the account predicted by the additive model, which <strong>in</strong>cluded the otherpredic<strong>to</strong>rs. Bivariate correlations among study variables can be found <strong>in</strong> Table 1, <strong>and</strong>results of regression analyses are shown <strong>in</strong> Tables 2 <strong>and</strong> 3.For teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of read<strong>in</strong>g competence (presented <strong>in</strong> Table 2), children’s <strong>in</strong>dependentliteracy skill assessment added 13% of the variance <strong>and</strong> was significant. The secondvariable, SES, did not add significant variance. The value difference (VD) variable, added<strong>in</strong> Step 3, contributed a significant additional 17% of the variance. The negative directionon the coefficient (.57) <strong>in</strong>dicates that greater discrepancy <strong>in</strong> value differencespredicted lower teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s academic competency. In Step 4, the classroomInternal validityGood checkLow r’sMultipleregression: seep. 331SocioeconomicmeasureChangedhypothesisInternal validity


352 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCircled values areof primaryimportanceTABLE 1 Bivariate Correlations among Study VariablesVariable 1 2 3 4 5 6 71. SES —2. Value differences –.13 —3. Classroom practices –.03 –.05 —4. Literacy skills .22* –.23* .11 —5. Math skills .21* –.38*** .02 .55*** —6. Teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs (literacy) .17 –.48*** .01 .36*** .63*** —7. Teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs (math) .16 –.47*** .05 .27** .59*** .93*** —Note: N 105. SES socioeconomic status.*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.See our commentunder “Analysis ofthe Study.”R 2.34 .58TABLE 2Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Teacher Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of Children’sCompetence <strong>in</strong> Literacy SkillsStep–predic<strong>to</strong>r at f<strong>in</strong>al step a R 2 R 21. Child skills .21** .13 .13***2. SES .04 .14 .013. Value differences (VD) .57*** .31 .17***4. Classroom practices (CP) .22* .31 .005. VD CP .14* .34 .04*Note. SES socioeconomic status.a Unst<strong>and</strong>ardized regression coefficients are reported because st<strong>and</strong>ardized coefficients are <strong>in</strong>appropriate with<strong>in</strong>teraction terms (see Aiken & West, 1991, pp. 40–47).*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.<strong>in</strong>structional practices (CP) variable was entered <strong>and</strong> did not add significantly <strong>to</strong> theequation. When <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>al step, the <strong>in</strong>teraction term (VD CP) was entered, the R 2was 4% <strong>and</strong> significant. That is, perceived value differences had dist<strong>in</strong>ct effects <strong>in</strong> different<strong>in</strong>structional sett<strong>in</strong>gs.Us<strong>in</strong>g the regression analysis f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs reported <strong>in</strong> Table 2, we calculated the predictedvalues of teacher expectancy rat<strong>in</strong>gs for different classroom practices <strong>and</strong> foundthat perceived value differences had greater effects on teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s competencies<strong>in</strong> literacy <strong>in</strong> more curriculum-centered classrooms. Teachers with curriculumorientedpractices rated children of parents whom they perceived <strong>to</strong> have discrepant values<strong>to</strong> be more than one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation (1.09 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) lower on literacyskills than children whose parents were perceived <strong>to</strong> have educational values congruentwith the teacher’s. Teachers with student-centered practices also rated children of parentswith discrepant values <strong>to</strong> be less competent than other children <strong>in</strong> literacy skills but<strong>to</strong> a lesser extent, about two fifths of a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation (0.39 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation).A similar pattern of results occurred <strong>in</strong> analyses of teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’smath competencies (Table 3). Children’s <strong>in</strong>dependently assessed math skills expla<strong>in</strong>ed asignificant 34% of the variance <strong>in</strong> teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s math competencies. SESdid not add significant variance. Perceived value differences added a unique 7% <strong>and</strong>were negatively related <strong>to</strong> teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s math competencies. The style ofclassroom <strong>in</strong>structional practices did not contribute additional variance. An <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween value differences <strong>and</strong> classroom practices, however, added 6% <strong>and</strong> was significant,<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that value differences were a better predic<strong>to</strong>r of teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> one


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 353TABLE 3 Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Teacher Rat<strong>in</strong>gs of Children’sCompetence <strong>in</strong> Math SkillsStep–predic<strong>to</strong>r at f<strong>in</strong>al step a R 2 R 21. Child skills .39*** .34 .34***2. SES .02 .35 .013. Value differences (VD) .34** .42 .07**4. Classroom practices (CP) .24** .42 .015. VD CP .17*** .48 .06**R 2.48 .69Note. SES socioeconomic status.a Unst<strong>and</strong>ardized regression coefficients are reported because st<strong>and</strong>ardized coefficients are <strong>in</strong>appropriate with<strong>in</strong>teraction terms (see Aiken & West, 1991, pp. 40–47).**p .01. ***p .001.type of classroom. When the <strong>in</strong>teraction effects were calculated, they <strong>in</strong>dicated thatperceived teacher–parent value differences had greater effects on teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>more curriculum-centered classrooms. When value differences were high, teachers withcurriculum-centered practices rated children as one st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation lower (0.97st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation) <strong>in</strong> math skills than children whose parents held values similar <strong>to</strong> theteachers. Teachers with student-centered practices, however, rated both groups ofchildren <strong>to</strong> be almost identical (0.04 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation difference).DISCUSSIONThis study produced several important f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. First, as predicted, teachers’ rat<strong>in</strong>gs ofchildren’s academic competence <strong>and</strong> their expectations for children’s future performancerelated highly <strong>to</strong> children’s actual skills, assessed <strong>in</strong>dependently for this study. Therelatively low level of children’s actual skills found <strong>in</strong> the study is similar <strong>to</strong> that reported<strong>in</strong> other studies, which document that on average low-<strong>in</strong>come children’s academic skillslag beh<strong>in</strong>d their middle-class peers (Lee & Burkam, 2002). Even dur<strong>in</strong>g the spr<strong>in</strong>g of thek<strong>in</strong>dergarten year, only about one third of children (36.0%) knew the names of all letters<strong>in</strong> the alphabet <strong>and</strong> about one quarter (25.8%) did not know sound–symbol associations.In terms of math, only one half (50.0%) could count 30 objects correctly; one quarter ofthe sample (24.6%) could not count 20 objects correctly. Despite the relatively modestlevel of children’s skills, teachers held generally positive beliefs about their academiccompetence; this positive evaluation by teachers has been noted <strong>in</strong> other studies ofchildren liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come families (Wigfield et al., 1999).Teachers varied <strong>in</strong> their judgments of children’s competence, however. Althoughchildren’s academic skills on our <strong>in</strong>dependent assessment predicted teachers’ perceptionsof children’s academic competence, other fac<strong>to</strong>rs also expla<strong>in</strong>ed variance <strong>in</strong> teachers’judgments. When teachers believed the education-related values of parents differedfrom their own, they rated children as less competent academically <strong>and</strong> had lower expectationsfor their future academic success. The dim<strong>in</strong>ished rat<strong>in</strong>gs were evident evenwhen children’s actual academic skills <strong>and</strong> SES were controlled. Thus, value differencesappeared <strong>to</strong> be a central feature <strong>in</strong> teacher judgments of these children’s competencies.Alex<strong>and</strong>er et al. (1987) suggested that social status differences between students <strong>and</strong>teachers produce teachers’ negative perceptions of students. In fact, <strong>in</strong> this study, wherestudents came from low-<strong>in</strong>come families, perceptions of value differences with parentsseemed <strong>to</strong> be even more important <strong>in</strong>dexes of social distance between students <strong>and</strong>teachers than demographic markers, such as SES.


354 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eRightAlthough teachers’ perceptions of value differences predicted their perceptions ofchildren’s academic competence <strong>in</strong> both math <strong>and</strong> literacy, the prediction was strongerfor literacy. In the United States, teachers <strong>and</strong> parents place more emphasis on earlyread<strong>in</strong>g skills than on math skills (Stevenson et al., 1990). We speculate that teachersview early literacy as an area of academic performance that is affected by the home environment(e.g., whether parents read <strong>to</strong> children), whereas they may know less aboutthe relation between the home environment <strong>and</strong> children’s emerg<strong>in</strong>g math skills. Therefore,literacy is an academic doma<strong>in</strong> where teachers’ perspectives of fac<strong>to</strong>rs other thanchildren’s actual skills have greater <strong>in</strong>fluence on their rat<strong>in</strong>gs of children’s competence.The relation between perceived value differences <strong>and</strong> teacher judgments of children’sread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> math competencies was greater <strong>in</strong> classrooms with certa<strong>in</strong> styles of<strong>in</strong>struction. Teachers <strong>in</strong> classrooms that were teacher dom<strong>in</strong>ated <strong>and</strong> driven by curriculumwere more likely <strong>to</strong> expect less of students from families with discrepant values thanwere teachers <strong>in</strong> classrooms <strong>in</strong> which the teacher was more responsive <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual differences<strong>in</strong> students. The children <strong>in</strong> student-centered classrooms were less likely <strong>to</strong>be disadvantaged by low expectations based on teachers’ perceptions of parents’ valuedifferences—perceptions that may not be valid <strong>and</strong> may not be relevant <strong>to</strong> children’sability <strong>to</strong> succeed <strong>in</strong> school. We demonstrated the importance of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g bothteachers’ beliefs <strong>and</strong> values <strong>and</strong> the educational contexts <strong>in</strong> which they are enacted.Delpit (1995) has argued for the benefits of value matches between teachers <strong>and</strong>parents, especially for children of color. Given the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g diversity of the U.S. population<strong>and</strong> the demographics of the population of teachers, value matches are <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>glyless likely <strong>to</strong> occur, however. Many classrooms <strong>in</strong>clude children from a range of diversecultural backgrounds, mak<strong>in</strong>g it difficult for teachers <strong>to</strong> “match” their approach <strong>to</strong> thecultural backgrounds of all students. Children of color <strong>and</strong> students for whom English isnot their first language comprise the majority <strong>in</strong> many schools, especially <strong>in</strong> urban communities.Thirty-eight percent of public school students were considered <strong>to</strong> be membersof m<strong>in</strong>ority groups <strong>in</strong> 1999 (U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, National Center for <strong>Education</strong>Statistics, 2001). In contrast, 90% of teachers who work with these children are Euro-American (National <strong>Education</strong> Association, 1997). These statistics underscore the needfor teachers <strong>to</strong> adapt their teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> meet diverse children’s needs rather than lowertheir expectations for students whose parents have different values or practices fromtheir own. To this end, teacher preparation <strong>and</strong> professional development programs canplay an essential role <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g teachers learn <strong>to</strong> bridge cultural differences betweenthemselves <strong>and</strong> their students’ families.This study has several limitations. We did not assess parents’ views of their valuedifferences with teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents may have dist<strong>in</strong>ct views on value differences. Also,the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are, by design, limited <strong>to</strong> children <strong>in</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come families, <strong>and</strong> given thetruncated range of SES <strong>in</strong> this study, the lack of differences by SES should be consideredwith caution. Further, we do not know the extent <strong>to</strong> which teacher–parent value differencesexist <strong>and</strong> are important <strong>in</strong> a wider range of families.Despite these limitations, this <strong>in</strong>vestigation adds an important dimension <strong>to</strong> theliterature on teachers’ judgments of the competence <strong>and</strong> future academic success of low<strong>in</strong>comechildren dur<strong>in</strong>g the k<strong>in</strong>dergarten year. In previous studies of teacher expectations,researchers focused on the effect of student characteristics <strong>and</strong> behavior. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof this study are particularly remarkable <strong>in</strong> that they demonstrate that fac<strong>to</strong>rs thatare not directly observed <strong>in</strong> children themselves may affect teachers’ judgments <strong>and</strong>potentially their behavior <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> turn children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs thus add a newdimension <strong>to</strong> the literature on teacher expectancy <strong>and</strong> suggest one mechanism by whichsome children from low-<strong>in</strong>come families enter a path of dim<strong>in</strong>ished expectations.


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 355ReferencesAiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression:Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Alex<strong>and</strong>er, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Horsey, C. S. (1997). Fromfirst grade forward: Early foundations of high schooldropout. Sociology of <strong>Education</strong>, 70, 87–107.Alex<strong>and</strong>er, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Thompson, M. (1987).School performance, status relations, <strong>and</strong> the structureof sentiment: Br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g the teacher back <strong>in</strong>. AmericanSociological Review, 52, 665–682.Augenblick, J., Myers, J., & Anderson, A. (1997). Equity <strong>and</strong>adequacy <strong>in</strong> school fund<strong>in</strong>g. The Future of Children, 7(3),63–78.Babad, E. (1993). Pygmalion—25 years after <strong>in</strong>terpersonalexpectations <strong>in</strong> the classroom. In P. D. Blanck (Ed.),Interpersonal expectations: Theory, research, <strong>and</strong>applications (pp. 125–153). Cambridge, Engl<strong>and</strong>:Cambridge University Press.Baker, A. (1997). Improv<strong>in</strong>g parent <strong>in</strong>volvement programs<strong>and</strong> practices: A qualitative study of teacher perceptions.The School Community Journal, 7, 27–55.Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The modera<strong>to</strong>r–media<strong>to</strong>r variable dist<strong>in</strong>ction <strong>in</strong> social psychologicalresearch: Conceptual, strategic, <strong>and</strong> statisticalconsiderations. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> SocialPsychology, 51, 1173–1182.Becker, W., & Gersten, R. (1982). A follow-up of FollowThrough: The later effects of the Direct Instruction Modelon children <strong>in</strong> fifth <strong>and</strong> sixth grades. American<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 19, 75–92.Berk, L., & W<strong>in</strong>sler, A. (1995). Scaffold<strong>in</strong>g children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g:Vygotsky <strong>and</strong> early childhood education. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC:National Association for the <strong>Education</strong> of Young Children.Betts, J., Rueben, K., & Danenberg, A. (2000). Equal resources,equal outcomes? The distribution of schoolresources <strong>and</strong> student achievement <strong>in</strong> California. SanFrancisco: Public Policy Institute of California.Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriatepractice <strong>in</strong> early childhood programs. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC:National Association for the <strong>Education</strong> of Young Children.Brophy, J. (1983). <strong>Research</strong> on the self-fulfill<strong>in</strong>g prophecy <strong>and</strong>teacher expectations. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al Psychology,75, 631–661.Byler, P., & Stipek, D. (2003). The Early Childhood Classroom ObservationMeasure. Manuscript submitted for publication.Carn<strong>in</strong>e, D., Carn<strong>in</strong>e, L., Karp, J., & Weisberg, P. (1988).K<strong>in</strong>dergarten for economically disadvantaged children:The direct <strong>in</strong>struction component. In C. Warger (Ed.),A resource guide <strong>to</strong> public school early childhoodprograms (pp. 73–98). Alex<strong>and</strong>ria, VA: Association forSupervision <strong>and</strong> Curriculum Development.Carpenter, T., Ansell, E., Franke, M., & Fennema, E. (1993).Models of problem solv<strong>in</strong>g: A study of k<strong>in</strong>dergartenchildren’s problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g processes. Journal for <strong>Research</strong><strong>in</strong> Mathematics <strong>Education</strong>, 24, 428–441.Carpenter, T., Fennema, E., & Franke, M. (1996). Cognitivelyguided <strong>in</strong>struction: A knowledge base for reform <strong>in</strong> primarymathematics <strong>in</strong>struction. Elementary School Journal, 97,3–20.Committee on the Prevention of Read<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties <strong>in</strong>Young Children. (1998). Prevent<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong>young children. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: National Academy ofScience.Delpit, L. (1995). Other people’s children: Cultural conflict <strong>in</strong>the classroom. New York: New Press.Den<strong>to</strong>n, K., & West, J. (2002). Children’s read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>mathematics achievement <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten <strong>and</strong> first grade.Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: National Center for <strong>Education</strong> Statistics.Duncan, G. J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Income effectsacross the life span: Integration <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation. InG. J. Duncan & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Consequences ofgrow<strong>in</strong>g up poor (pp. 596–610). New York: Sage.Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody IndividualAchievement Test—Revised. Circle P<strong>in</strong>es, MN: AmericanGuidance Service.Gill, S., & Reynolds, A. J. (2000). <strong>Education</strong>al expectations<strong>and</strong> school achievement of urban African Americanchildren. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 403–424.Hall<strong>in</strong>ger, P., Bickman, L., & Davis, K. (1996). School context,pr<strong>in</strong>cipal leadership, <strong>and</strong> student read<strong>in</strong>g achievement.The Elementary School Journal, 96, 527–549.Hall<strong>in</strong>ger, P., & Murphy, J. (1986). The social context of effectiveschools. American Journal of <strong>Education</strong>, 94, 328–355.Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1992). Teacher expectations: II.Construction <strong>and</strong> reflection of student achievement.Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 63,947–961.Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perceptions,social stereotypes, <strong>and</strong> teacher expectations. In M. P.Zanna (Ed.), Advances <strong>in</strong> experimental social psychology(Vol. 28, pp. 281–388). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Kennedy, E. (1995). Contextual effects on academic normsamong elementary school students. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Quarterly, 18, 5–13.Kukl<strong>in</strong>ski, M. R., & We<strong>in</strong>ste<strong>in</strong>, R. S. (2001). Classroom <strong>and</strong>developmental differences <strong>in</strong> a path model of teacherexpectancy effects. Child Development, 72, 1554–1578.


356 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eLareau, A. (1987). Social class differences <strong>in</strong> family–schoolrelationships: The importance of cultural capital.Sociology of <strong>Education</strong>, 60, 73–85.Lasky, S. (2000). The cultural <strong>and</strong> emotional politics ofteacher–parent <strong>in</strong>teractions. Teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Teacher<strong>Education</strong>, 16, 843–860.Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2002). Inequality at the start<strong>in</strong>ggate: Social background differences <strong>in</strong> achievement aschildren beg<strong>in</strong> school. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: Economic PolicyInstitute.Leithwood, K., Begley, P., & Cous<strong>in</strong>s, B. (1990). The nature,causes <strong>and</strong> consequences of pr<strong>in</strong>cipals’ practices: Anagenda for future research. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>alAdm<strong>in</strong>istration, 28, 5–31.L<strong>in</strong>ek, W., Ras<strong>in</strong>ski, T., & Hark<strong>in</strong>s, D. (1997). Teacherperceptions of parent <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> literacy education.Read<strong>in</strong>g Horizons, 38, 90–107.McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage <strong>and</strong> childdevelopment. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204.Meyer, L., Gersten, R., & Gutk<strong>in</strong>, J. (1983). Direct <strong>in</strong>struction:A Project Follow Through success s<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-cityschool. Elementary School Journal, 84, 241–252.National Academy of <strong>Education</strong>, Commission on Read<strong>in</strong>g.(1985). Becom<strong>in</strong>g a nation of readers. Pittsburgh, PA:Author.National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1991).Professional st<strong>and</strong>ards for teach<strong>in</strong>g mathematics. Res<strong>to</strong>n,VA: Author.National <strong>Education</strong> Association. (1997). Status of theAmerican public school teacher. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: Author.National <strong>Research</strong> Council, Committee on the Preventionof Read<strong>in</strong>g Difficulties <strong>in</strong> Young Children. (1998).Read<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> young children. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,DC: Author.Ogbu, J. (1993). Variability <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority school performance: Aproblem <strong>in</strong> search of an explanation. In E. Jacob & C.Jordon (Eds.), M<strong>in</strong>ority education: Anthropologicalperspectives (pp. 83–111), New Jersey: Ablex.Okagaki, L., & French, P. A. (1998). Parent<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> children’sachievement: A multiethnic perspective. American<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 35, 123–144.Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental beliefs <strong>and</strong>children’s school performance. Child Development, 64,36–56.Parrish, T. B., & Fowler, W. J., Jr. (1995). Disparities <strong>in</strong>public school spend<strong>in</strong>g 1989–1990 (NCES PublicationNo. 95-300). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: U.S. GovernmentPr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g Office.Raudenbush, S. W. (1984). Magnitude of teacher expectancyeffects on pupil IQ as a function of credibility <strong>in</strong>duction:A synthesis of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from 18 experiments. Journal of<strong>Education</strong>al Psychology, 76, 85–97.Rosenthal, R. J., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion <strong>in</strong> theclassroom: Teacher expectations <strong>and</strong> pupils’ <strong>in</strong>tellectualdevelopment. New York: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart & W<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>n.Saunders, W. (1999). Improv<strong>in</strong>g literacy achievement forEnglish learners <strong>in</strong> transitional bil<strong>in</strong>gual programs.<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Evaluation, 5, 345–381.Stevenson, H., Lee, S., Chen, C., Stigler, J. W., Hsu, C., &Kitamura, S. (1990). Contexts of achievement: A study ofAmerican, Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, <strong>and</strong> Japanese children. Monographsof the Society for <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Child Development,55(1–2, Serial No. 221).Stipek, D. (2002). Motivation <strong>to</strong> learn: Theory <strong>and</strong> practice(4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Stipek, D., Feiler, R., Byler, P., Ryan, R., Milburn, S., & Salmon,J. (1998). Good beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs: What difference does theprogram make <strong>in</strong> prepar<strong>in</strong>g young children for school?Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19,41–66.Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1997). The cultural structur<strong>in</strong>gof child development. In J. W. Berry, P. R. Dassen, & T. S.Saraswathi (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of cross-cultural psychology:Vol. 2. Basic processes <strong>and</strong> human development (pp.3–39). Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon.Unnever, J. D., Kerckhoff, A. C., & Rob<strong>in</strong>son, T. J. (2000).District variations <strong>in</strong> educational resources <strong>and</strong> studen<strong>to</strong>utcomes. Economics of <strong>Education</strong> Review, 19,245–259.U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, National Center for <strong>Education</strong>Statistics. (2001). The condition of education 2001 (NCESPublication No. 2001-0172). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: U.S.Government Pr<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g Office.Weisner, T. S., Gallimore, R., & Jordan, C. (1988). Unpack<strong>in</strong>gcultural effects on classroom learn<strong>in</strong>g: Native Hawaiianpeer assistance <strong>and</strong> child-generated activity. Anthropology<strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong> Quarterly, 19, 327–351.Wigfield, A., Galper, A., Den<strong>to</strong>n, K., & Seefeldt, C. (1999).Teachers’ beliefs about former Head Start <strong>and</strong> non-HeadStart first-grade children’s motivation, performance, <strong>and</strong>future educational prospects. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>alPsychology, 91, 98–104.Wigfield, A., & Harold, R. (1992). Teacher beliefs <strong>and</strong>children’s achievement self-perceptions: A developmentalperspective. In D. Schunk & J. Meece (Eds.), Studentperceptions <strong>in</strong> the classroom (pp. 95–121). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1990). Woodcock–-Johnson Psycho<strong>Education</strong>al Battery—Revised. Allen, TX:DLM Teach<strong>in</strong>g Resources.


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 357Analysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose was “<strong>to</strong> assess possible predic<strong>to</strong>rs ofteacher expectations of students from low-<strong>in</strong>come families.”More specifically, the purpose was <strong>to</strong> study the<strong>in</strong>fluence of “socioeconomic status <strong>and</strong> teacher perceptionsof value differences between themselves <strong>and</strong> students’parents.”The primary justification is based on (1) evidence ofpoorer academic performance on the part of low-<strong>in</strong>comestudents, (2) evidence <strong>and</strong> op<strong>in</strong>ion as <strong>to</strong> the importanceof teacher expectations, <strong>and</strong> (3) evidence that teachershave lower expectations for low-<strong>in</strong>come students. Furtherjustification is based on evidence that teacher expectationsare more important <strong>in</strong> the early grades <strong>and</strong> thatexpectations are based on more than observed skill levels.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the emphasis on value differences betweenteachers <strong>and</strong> parents is justified by research show<strong>in</strong>g thatsuch differences exist <strong>and</strong> by a rationale support<strong>in</strong>g theirprobable impact on teacher expectations.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems of risk, confidentiality,or deception.DEFINITIONSDef<strong>in</strong>itions are not provided <strong>and</strong> would be helpful. Theprimary terms are teacher expectations <strong>and</strong> perceivedvalue differences. The former is operationally def<strong>in</strong>edby questions ask<strong>in</strong>g teachers <strong>to</strong> rate, on a five-po<strong>in</strong>tscale, future performance (<strong>in</strong> first <strong>and</strong> third grades) <strong>in</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> math. Perceived value differences is alsooperationally def<strong>in</strong>ed as teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs on a three-po<strong>in</strong>tscale <strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> the question, “Are there differencesbetween the parents’ values or preferences <strong>and</strong> your valueswith respect <strong>to</strong> the educational program <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gareas: discipl<strong>in</strong>e, read<strong>in</strong>g, writ<strong>in</strong>g, math, parents’role <strong>in</strong> assist<strong>in</strong>g their child.”As is common with operational def<strong>in</strong>itions, the natureof these perceived differences is not clear. For example,do these perceived differences regard<strong>in</strong>g discipl<strong>in</strong>eperta<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the nature or extent of discipl<strong>in</strong>e? If thelatter, are parents perceived as want<strong>in</strong>g more or less discipl<strong>in</strong>e?It can be argued that the nature of these differencesis not crucial <strong>to</strong> the study, but clarification wouldhelp <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results.Other terms <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> math skills, teacherperception of read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> math skills, student centered,curriculum centered, <strong>and</strong> the list of student centereddescrip<strong>to</strong>rs are all operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong>, we th<strong>in</strong>k,clear enough <strong>in</strong> context for the purposes of this study.PRIOR RESEARCHExtensive documentation <strong>and</strong> good summaries of bothresearch <strong>and</strong> op<strong>in</strong>ion are provided for most of the backgroundargument. The exception is material specificallyon the effects of “value discrepancies,” which the authorsattribute <strong>to</strong> the lack of prior studies.HYPOTHESESThe primary hypothesis, though not explicitly stated,is clearly implied. It is that the larger the teacherperceivedvalue differences between themselves <strong>and</strong> astudent’s parents, the lower their expectations for the student.A secondary hypothesis is stated; it is that the primaryhypothesis would be more clearly supported among“curriculum-centered” teachers as compared <strong>to</strong> “studentcentered”teachers. Both are directional hypotheses (seepage 47). As noted under “Results,” the authors actuallymodified these hypotheses for purposes of data analysis.SAMPLEThe sample consisted of 105 k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children previouslyenrolled as <strong>in</strong>fants <strong>in</strong> a study of low-<strong>in</strong>come families.They were located <strong>in</strong> three different areas of theUnited States, which differed <strong>in</strong> urbanization as well asgeography. The teacher sample was 56. It is not clearhow the orig<strong>in</strong>al sample of <strong>in</strong>fants was selected, nor howthose <strong>in</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>dergarten sample were obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Theyclearly do not constitute a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of low-<strong>in</strong>comek<strong>in</strong>dergartners nationwide, <strong>and</strong> it seems unlikely that thegroups from the three locations were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected.Students, their families, <strong>and</strong> the teachers are all very welldescribed, which makes some generalization potentiallyfeasible. The use of different geographic regions madereplication (analysis of data separately by region), feasiblebut this was apparently not done.INSTRUMENTATIONAll <strong>in</strong>struments are well described or identified. The rat<strong>in</strong>gscales used a well-known format; the tests <strong>and</strong> observationsystem are known <strong>in</strong> the field.The reliability of the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal measures (predictions ofskill <strong>and</strong> perception of value differences) was not discussed.The correlations between the three similar, but not identical,skill perception scales suggest moderate <strong>to</strong> high <strong>in</strong>ternal


358 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7econsistency (.75 <strong>to</strong> .93). The reliability of the comb<strong>in</strong>edscale is very good, but we do not agree that the correlationsbetween “current competence” <strong>and</strong> future “expectations”are high enough <strong>to</strong> justify the comb<strong>in</strong>ed score. For read<strong>in</strong>g,they were .75 <strong>and</strong> .88, which <strong>in</strong>dicate at most 61 percentcommon variance. Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g scores <strong>in</strong>creased reliabilitybut may have sacrificed validity. The <strong>in</strong>ternal consistencyof the “value differences” scale is excellent at .92.The validity of these measures is not discussed, <strong>and</strong> itshould be. It may appear self-evident that the rat<strong>in</strong>g scalesmust reflect teacher perceptions/expectations, <strong>and</strong> weagree that the appeal <strong>to</strong> content validity is much more satisfy<strong>in</strong>gthan with measures of more ambiguous variables.But such assumptions are always dangerous. Such scalesare easy <strong>to</strong> deliberately dis<strong>to</strong>rt <strong>and</strong> subject <strong>to</strong> un<strong>in</strong>tendedbias, that is, they do not necessarily <strong>in</strong>dicate the teachers’true perceptions. This is especially a problem becauseboth scales were apparently expected <strong>to</strong> be filled out witha short <strong>in</strong>terven<strong>in</strong>g time, rais<strong>in</strong>g the possibility of oneaffect<strong>in</strong>g the other. It is, we admit, difficult <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of alternativemeasures that could be used <strong>to</strong> check validity.Of the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g measures, the reliability of the testsshould have been discussed, but it is probably adequate.The check on observer agreement showed it <strong>to</strong> be fairfor the “student-centered” score at .79 <strong>and</strong> very good forthe “curriculum-centered” score at .95. In this case, weth<strong>in</strong>k comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g scores is justified because the twoscales form a logical cont<strong>in</strong>uum also found <strong>in</strong> otherstudies—as opposed <strong>to</strong> the “current” <strong>and</strong> “expected”skill scales discussed earlier.The use of teacher’s self-reported goals as a check onthe validity of the observation measures is surely commendable;unfortunately, the results (correlations of–.22 <strong>to</strong> .38) do not provide strong evidence of validity.We are unclear as <strong>to</strong> why the goal questions were notmore directly focused on the observation variables; thismay account for the low correlations. The SES measure,not described until the “Results” section, appears reasonablebut is of unknown reliability <strong>and</strong> validity.PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYThe procedures, which consist of adm<strong>in</strong>istration of thevarious <strong>in</strong>struments, are well described. Although notdiscussed as such, many of the measures can be seen ascontroll<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. The authorsfrequently describe their study as one of prediction,<strong>in</strong> which case <strong>in</strong>ternal validity is, strictly speak<strong>in</strong>g, irrelevant.<strong>How</strong>ever it is clear that the authors’ <strong>in</strong>tention isreally <strong>to</strong> explore the causes of teacher expectations, <strong>in</strong>particular, perceived value differences with parents.In the event that the hypotheses were supported,other variables that might expla<strong>in</strong> the correlationsare student ability <strong>and</strong> family socioeconomic status,because each is known <strong>to</strong> be correlated with both primaryvariables. Subsequent data analysis allows thesepossibilities <strong>to</strong> be assessed. The observation measuresallowed the effect of <strong>in</strong>structional style <strong>to</strong> be assessed.We discussed a possible <strong>in</strong>strumentation threat aboveunder “Instrumentation.”DATA ANALYSIS/RESULTSAs discussed previously, we do not agree with the comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gof “perceived present skills” <strong>and</strong> “predicted futureperformance” scales. A teacher might well predictsubstantial future improvement or decl<strong>in</strong>e over a threeyearperiod for many reasons, <strong>and</strong> the correlations, especiallyfor read<strong>in</strong>g, are, <strong>in</strong> our op<strong>in</strong>ion, <strong>to</strong>o low <strong>to</strong> justifycomb<strong>in</strong>ation.The overall data analysis is appropriate, thoughperhaps confus<strong>in</strong>g. It addresses a somewhat differenthypothesis <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> answer the same study question.The revised hypothesis is: Teacher perception of differenceswith parent values will importantly affectteacher expectations after present skill level <strong>and</strong> SESare accounted for. The results show, as the authorsstate, that the “perceived value difference” variabledid add <strong>to</strong> the predictability of teacher expectationsbeyond the <strong>in</strong>fluence of child skills, whereas SES <strong>and</strong>classroom practices did not. The secondary hypothesisof difference between curriculum-oriented <strong>and</strong> student-orientedteachers was appropriately tested <strong>and</strong>supported.The use of statistical significance is not justified asother than a rough guide due <strong>to</strong> the lack of r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g.It is the magnitude of the correlations that ismean<strong>in</strong>gful, particularly <strong>in</strong> light of a fairly large samplesize of 105. The difference between African American<strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o or Euro-American parents should not havebeen dismissed on the basis of a technically <strong>in</strong>appropriatep value of .06. Descriptive statistics such as the calculationof effect size (see page 244) should have beenprovided. (Note: Do not be confused by the ß column <strong>in</strong>Table 2. These numbers are part of the regression analysisbut can be ignored for our purposes.)We th<strong>in</strong>k the study would have been markedly improvedby analyz<strong>in</strong>g data separately for each of thethree geographic subgroups. If such replication showedconsistent results, generalization would be greatly enhanced.It appears that approximately 22 teachers <strong>and</strong>36 students would have been <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> each group.


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 359DISCUSSION/INTERPRETATIONWe agree that the support for both hypotheses has importantimplications <strong>and</strong> that the authors’ suggestionsare consistent with the results. We th<strong>in</strong>k, however, thatclearer details of the perceived teacher–parent differenceswould have helped with implications. If such differencesare undesirable, as this study <strong>in</strong>dicates, whatmight be done <strong>to</strong> reduce them? For example, could theybe due <strong>to</strong> failure <strong>to</strong> communicate educational practicesadequately <strong>to</strong> parents, or are they due <strong>to</strong> more basic culturaldifferences that are harder <strong>to</strong> resolve? The resultsof the secondary hypotheses imply that student-centeredteach<strong>in</strong>g is more likely <strong>to</strong> reduce these perceived differences<strong>and</strong> should, therefore, be encouraged.We th<strong>in</strong>k the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that teacher prediction of futureliteracy was related much more highly <strong>to</strong> current mathskill (.63) than <strong>to</strong> current literacy skill (.36) deserveddiscussion. Perhaps k<strong>in</strong>dergarten teachers are betterable <strong>to</strong> assess math skills <strong>and</strong> this, <strong>in</strong> turn, colors theirprediction of future literacy. This could be checked bycorrelat<strong>in</strong>g teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of current skill with testedskill <strong>in</strong> both read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> math.For the general reader, the authors should havepo<strong>in</strong>ted out that the results of a multiple regressionanalysis depend on the variables selected as predic<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>and</strong> on the sequence <strong>in</strong> which they are entered. Chang<strong>in</strong>gthe variables, or the sequence, would change theamount of additional variance contributed by each,though, <strong>in</strong> this case, we th<strong>in</strong>k this would not change theconclusions regard<strong>in</strong>g the hypotheses.The authors recognize that they studied teacher perceptionsof value differences, not necessarily a valid<strong>in</strong>dex of actual differences. They also acknowledgethat the study is limited <strong>to</strong> low-<strong>in</strong>come families, butthis was <strong>in</strong>tended. They fail <strong>to</strong> acknowledge the seriouslimitation on generaliz<strong>in</strong>g because of the lack of a r<strong>and</strong>omsample, as well as the accompany<strong>in</strong>g fallacy ofus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics as more than a rough guide.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE NATURE OF CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHThe major characteristic of correlational research is seek<strong>in</strong>g out associations amongvariables.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsPURPOSES OF CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHCorrelational studies are carried out either <strong>to</strong> help expla<strong>in</strong> important human behaviorsor <strong>to</strong> predict likely outcomes.If a relationship of sufficient magnitude exists between two variables, it becomespossible <strong>to</strong> predict a score on one variable if a score on the other variable is known.The variable that is used <strong>to</strong> make the prediction is called the predic<strong>to</strong>r variable.The variable about which the prediction is made is called the criterion variable.Both scatterplots <strong>and</strong> regression l<strong>in</strong>es are used <strong>in</strong> correlational studies <strong>to</strong> predicta score on a criterion variable.A predicted score is never exact. As a result, researchers calculate an <strong>in</strong>dex of predictionerror, which is known as the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate.359


360 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCOMPLEX CORRELATIONAL TECHNIQUESMultiple regression is a technique that enables a researcher <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e a correlationbetween a criterion variable <strong>and</strong> the best comb<strong>in</strong>ation of two or more predic<strong>to</strong>rvariables.The coefficient of multiple correlation (R) <strong>in</strong>dicates the strength of the correlationbetween the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of the predic<strong>to</strong>r variables <strong>and</strong> the criterion variable.The value of a prediction equation depends on whether it predicts successfully witha new group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.When the criterion variable is categorical rather than quantitative, discrim<strong>in</strong>ant functionanalysis (rather than multiple regression) must be used.Fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis is a technique that allows a researcher <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether manyvariables can be described by a few fac<strong>to</strong>rs.Path analysis is a technique used <strong>to</strong> test the likelihood of causal connections amongthree or more variables.BASIC STEPS IN CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHThese <strong>in</strong>clude, as <strong>in</strong> most research, select<strong>in</strong>g a problem, choos<strong>in</strong>g a sample, select<strong>in</strong>gor develop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments, determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g procedures, collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g data,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results.CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS AND THEIR MEANINGThe mean<strong>in</strong>g of a given correlation coefficient depends on how it is applied.Correlation coefficients below .35 show only a slight relationship between variables.Correlations between .40 <strong>and</strong> .60 may have theoretical <strong>and</strong>/or practical valuedepend<strong>in</strong>g on the context.Only when a correlation of .65 or higher is obta<strong>in</strong>ed can reasonably accurate predictionsbe made.Correlations over .85 <strong>in</strong>dicate a very strong relationship between the variablescorrelated.EVALUATING THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITY IN CORRELATIONALRESEARCHThreats <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of correlational studies <strong>in</strong>clude subject characteristics,location, <strong>in</strong>strument decay, data collection, test<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> mortality.Results of correlational studies must always be <strong>in</strong>terpreted with caution, becausethey may suggest, but they cannot establish, causation.Key Termscoefficient ofdeterm<strong>in</strong>ation 332coefficient of multiplecorrelation 332correlationcoefficient 337criterionvariable 330discrim<strong>in</strong>ant functionanalysis 333fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis 334multiple regression 331partial correlation 338path analysis 334prediction 330prediction equation 331prediction studies 330predic<strong>to</strong>r variable 330regression l<strong>in</strong>e 330st<strong>and</strong>ard errorof estimate 331


CHAPTER 15 Correlational <strong>Research</strong> 3611. Which type of relationship would a researcher be more pleased <strong>to</strong> have the results ofa study reveal—positive or negative—or would it matter? Expla<strong>in</strong>.2. What is the difference between an effect <strong>and</strong> a relationship? Which is more important,or can this be determ<strong>in</strong>ed?3. Are there any types of <strong>in</strong>struments that could not be used <strong>in</strong> a correlational study? Ifso, why?4. Would it be possible for a correlation <strong>to</strong> be statistically significant yet educationally<strong>in</strong>significant? If so, give an example.5. Why do you suppose people often <strong>in</strong>terpret correlational results as prov<strong>in</strong>g causation?6. What is the difference, if any, between the sign of a correlation <strong>and</strong> the strength of acorrelation?7. “Correlational studies, <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> of themselves, do not establish cause <strong>and</strong> effect.” Isthis true? Why or why not?8. “The possibility of causation (<strong>in</strong> a correlational study) is strengthened if a time lapseoccurs between measurement of the variables be<strong>in</strong>g studied.” Why?9. To <strong>in</strong>terpret correlation coefficients sensibly, it is a good idea <strong>to</strong> show the scatterplotson which they are based. Why is this true? Expla<strong>in</strong>.For Discussion1. K. G. Joreskog <strong>and</strong> D. Sorbom (1988). LISREL VII. Analysis of l<strong>in</strong>ear structural relationships by maximumlikelihood <strong>and</strong> least squares methods: Statistical package for the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.2. D. M. Medley <strong>and</strong> H. Coker (1987). The accuracy of pr<strong>in</strong>cipals’ judgments of teacher performance.Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 80(4): 242–247.3. C. V. H<strong>in</strong>es, D. R. Cruickshank, <strong>and</strong> J. J. Kennedy (1985). Teacher clarity <strong>and</strong> its relationship <strong>to</strong> studentachievement <strong>and</strong> satisfaction. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 22(1): 87–100.4. L. D. Ried, O. B. Mart<strong>in</strong>son, <strong>and</strong> L. C. Weaver (1987). Fac<strong>to</strong>rs associated with the drug use of fifththrougheighth-grade students. Journal of Drug <strong>Education</strong>, 17(2): 149–161.5. J. T. Bowman <strong>and</strong> T. G. Reeves (1987). Moral development <strong>and</strong> empathy <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g. Counselor <strong>Education</strong><strong>and</strong> Supervision, 6: 293–297.6. N. Brown, A. Muhlenkamp, L. Fox, <strong>and</strong> M. Osborn (1983). The relationship among health beliefs, healthvalues, <strong>and</strong> health promotion activity. Western Journal of Nurs<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>, 5(2): 155–163.7. S. R. Sw<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> P. L. Peterson (1982). The relationship of student ability <strong>and</strong> small-group <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>to</strong>student achievement. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 19(2): 259–274.8. B. J. Fraser <strong>and</strong> D. L. Fisher (1982). Predict<strong>in</strong>g students’ outcomes from their perceptions of classroompsychosocial environment. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 19(4): 498–518.9. D. E. H<strong>in</strong>kle, W. Wiersma, <strong>and</strong> S. G. Jurs (1981). Applied statistics for the behavioral sciences. Chicago:R<strong>and</strong> McNally.Notes


16 Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong>What Is Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>?Similarities <strong>and</strong> DifferencesBetween Causal-Comparative <strong>and</strong>Correlational <strong>Research</strong>Similarities <strong>and</strong> DifferencesBetween Causal-Comparative <strong>and</strong>Experimental <strong>Research</strong>Steps Involved <strong>in</strong>Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong>Problem FormulationSampleInstrumentation<strong>Design</strong>Threats <strong>to</strong> InternalValidity <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>Subject CharacteristicsOther ThreatsEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats <strong>to</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Causal-ComparativeStudiesData AnalysisAssociations BetweenCategorical VariablesAn Example of Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisResultsDiscussion/InterpretationOBJECTIVESIs there a difference between natural grass <strong>and</strong> Astro-turf?Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“causal-comparative research.”Describe briefly how causal-comparativeresearch is both similar <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> different fromboth correlational <strong>and</strong> experimentalresearch.Identify <strong>and</strong> describe briefly the steps<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a causalcomparativestudy.Draw a diagram of a design for acausal-comparative study.Describe how data are collected <strong>in</strong>causal-comparative research.Describe some of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity that exist <strong>in</strong> causal-comparativestudies <strong>and</strong> discuss how <strong>to</strong> control forthese threats.Recognize a causal-comparative studywhen you come across one <strong>in</strong> theeducational research literature.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Important Causal-Comparative<strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 16.1: Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 16.2: Experiment or Causal-Comparative StudyActivity 16.3: Causal-Comparative vs. ExperimentalHypothesesActivity 16.4: Analyze Some Causal-Comparative DataJoseph Perea has just completed his first year of teach<strong>in</strong>g chemistry <strong>in</strong> a small high school <strong>in</strong> rural Idaho. Hismethod of teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volved primarily lectur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> his students. At the end-of-the-year faculty party, he isdiscuss<strong>in</strong>g the year with Mary Roberts, one of the other teachers <strong>in</strong> the school.“Ah, Mary, I’m a bit discouraged. Th<strong>in</strong>gs did not turn out the way I had hoped.”“<strong>How</strong> come?”“A lot of my students didn’t seem <strong>to</strong> like my teach<strong>in</strong>g very much. And they didn’t do well on the f<strong>in</strong>al exam that I gave.”“You know, Joe, Bruce (Bruce Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, who taught chemistry last year) used some <strong>in</strong>quiry science materials.I remember him say<strong>in</strong>g that his students really liked them. Maybe you should try them next term.”“I wonder whether his approach would work for me?”An appropriate method for this k<strong>in</strong>d of question is a causal-comparative study. What that <strong>in</strong>volves is the focusof this chapter.What Is Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>?In causal-comparative research, <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs attempt <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e the cause or consequences of differences thatalready exist between or among groups of <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Asa result, it is sometimes viewed, along with correlationalresearch, as a form of associational research, s<strong>in</strong>ce bothdescribe conditions that already exist. A researcher migh<strong>to</strong>bserve, for example, that two groups of <strong>in</strong>dividuals differon some variable (such as teach<strong>in</strong>g style) <strong>and</strong> then attempt<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the reason for, or the results of, thisdifference. The difference between the groups, however,has already occurred. Because both the effect(s) <strong>and</strong> thealleged cause(s) have already occurred, <strong>and</strong> hence arestudied <strong>in</strong> retrospect, causal-comparative research is alsoreferred <strong>to</strong> sometimes as ex post fac<strong>to</strong> (from the Lat<strong>in</strong> for“after the fact”) research. This is <strong>in</strong> contrast <strong>to</strong> an experimentalstudy, <strong>in</strong> which a researcher creates a differencebetween or among groups <strong>and</strong> then compares their performance(on one or more dependent variables) <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ethe effects of the created difference.The group difference variable <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparativestudy is either a variable that cannot be manipulated(such as ethnicity) or one that might have been manipulatedbut for one reason or another has not been (such asteach<strong>in</strong>g style). Sometimes ethical constra<strong>in</strong>ts prevent avariable from be<strong>in</strong>g manipulated, thus prevent<strong>in</strong>g the effectsof variations <strong>in</strong> the variable from be<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ed bymeans of an experimental study. A researcher might be<strong>in</strong>terested, for example, <strong>in</strong> the effects of a new diet onvery young children. Ethical considerations, however,might prevent the researcher from deliberately vary<strong>in</strong>gthe diet <strong>to</strong> which the children are exposed. Causalcomparativeresearch, however, would allow the researcher<strong>to</strong> study the effects of the diet if he or she couldf<strong>in</strong>d a group of children who have already been exposed<strong>to</strong> the diet. The researcher could then compare them witha similar group of children who had not been exposed <strong>to</strong>the diet. Much of the research <strong>in</strong> medic<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> sociologyis causal-comparative <strong>in</strong> nature.Another example is the comparison of scientists <strong>and</strong>eng<strong>in</strong>eers <strong>in</strong> terms of their orig<strong>in</strong>ality. As <strong>in</strong> correlationalresearch, explanations or predictions can be madefrom either variable <strong>to</strong> the other: orig<strong>in</strong>ality could be363


364 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7epredicted from group membership, or group membershipcould be predicted from orig<strong>in</strong>ality. <strong>How</strong>ever, mostsuch studies attempt <strong>to</strong> explore causation rather than <strong>to</strong>foster prediction. Are “orig<strong>in</strong>al” <strong>in</strong>dividuals more likely<strong>to</strong> become scientists? Do scientists become more orig<strong>in</strong>alas they become immersed <strong>in</strong> their work? And soforth. Notice that if it were possible, a correlationalstudy might be preferable, but that it is not appropriatewhen one of the variables (<strong>in</strong> this case, the nature of thegroups) is a categorical variable.Follow<strong>in</strong>g are some examples of different types ofcausal-comparative research.Type 1: Exploration of effects (dependent variable)caused by membership <strong>in</strong> a given groupQuestion: What differences <strong>in</strong> abilities are causedby gender?<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: Females have a greateramount of l<strong>in</strong>guistic ability than males.Type 2: Exploration of causes (<strong>in</strong>dependent variable)of group membershipQuestion: What causes <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> jo<strong>in</strong> a gang?<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: Individuals who are membersof gangs have more aggressive personalities than<strong>in</strong>dividuals who are not members of gangs.Type 3: Exploration of the consequences (dependentvariable) of an <strong>in</strong>terventionQuestion: <strong>How</strong> do students taught by the <strong>in</strong>quirymethod react <strong>to</strong> propag<strong>and</strong>a?<strong>Research</strong> hypothesis: Students who were taught bythe <strong>in</strong>quiry method are more critical of propag<strong>and</strong>athan are those who were taught by the lecturemethod.Causal-comparative studies have been used frequently<strong>to</strong> study the differences between males <strong>and</strong> females. Theyhave demonstrated the superiority of girls <strong>in</strong> language<strong>and</strong> of boys <strong>in</strong> math at certa<strong>in</strong> age levels. The attribut<strong>in</strong>gof these differences <strong>to</strong> gender—as cause—must be tentative.One could hardly view gender as be<strong>in</strong>g caused byability, but there are many other probable l<strong>in</strong>ks <strong>in</strong> thecausal cha<strong>in</strong>, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g societal expectations of males <strong>and</strong>females.The basic causal-comparative approach, therefore, is<strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with a noted difference between two groups<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> look for possible causes for, or consequences of,this difference. A researcher might be <strong>in</strong>terested, for example,<strong>in</strong> the reason(s) why some <strong>in</strong>dividuals becomeaddicted <strong>to</strong> alcohol while others develop a dependenceon pills. <strong>How</strong> can this be expla<strong>in</strong>ed? Descriptions of thetwo groups (alcoholics <strong>and</strong> pill poppers) might be compared<strong>to</strong> see if their characteristics differ <strong>in</strong> ways thatmight account for the difference <strong>in</strong> choice of drug.Sometimes causal-comparative studies are conductedsolely as an alternative <strong>to</strong> experiments. Suppose, for example,that the curriculum direc<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a large, urbanhigh school district is consider<strong>in</strong>g implement<strong>in</strong>g a newEnglish curriculum. The direc<strong>to</strong>r might try out the curriculumexperimentally, select<strong>in</strong>g a few classes at r<strong>and</strong>omthroughout the district, <strong>and</strong> compare studentperformance <strong>in</strong> these classes with comparison groupswho cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> experience the regular curriculum. Thismight take a considerable amount of time, however, <strong>and</strong>be quite costly <strong>in</strong> terms of materials, teacher preparationworkshops, <strong>and</strong> so on. As an alternative, the direc<strong>to</strong>rmight consider a causal-comparative study <strong>and</strong> comparethe achievement of students <strong>in</strong> school districts that arecurrently us<strong>in</strong>g this curriculum with the achievement ofstudents <strong>in</strong> similar districts that do not use the new curriculum.If the results show that students <strong>in</strong> districts (similar<strong>to</strong> his) with the new curriculum are achiev<strong>in</strong>g higherscores <strong>in</strong> English, the direc<strong>to</strong>r would have a basis forgo<strong>in</strong>g ahead <strong>and</strong> implement<strong>in</strong>g the new curriculum <strong>in</strong> hisdistrict. Like correlational studies, causal-comparative<strong>in</strong>vestigations often identify relationships that later arestudied experimentally.Despite their advantages, however, causal-comparativestudies do have serious limitations. The most serious lie<strong>in</strong> the lack of control over threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.Because the manipulation of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variablehas already occurred, many of the controls we discussed<strong>in</strong> Chapter 13 cannot be applied. Thus, considerablecaution must be expressed <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the outcomesof a causal-comparative study. As with correlationalstudies, relationships can be identified, but causationcannot be fully established. As we have po<strong>in</strong>ted out before,the alleged cause may really be an effect, the effectmay be a cause, or there may be a third variable thatproduced both the alleged cause <strong>and</strong> effect.SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCESBETWEEN CAUSAL-COMPARATIVEAND CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHCausal-comparative research is sometimes confusedwith correlational research. Although similarities doexist, there are notable differences.


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCH<strong>How</strong> Should <strong>Research</strong>Methodologies Be Classified?Op<strong>in</strong>ions differ as <strong>to</strong> how the different types of researchmethodology should be classified. No s<strong>in</strong>gle system forclassify<strong>in</strong>g research methods has been widely accepted. To besure, clear dist<strong>in</strong>ctions have been drawn between experimental<strong>and</strong> nonexperimental methods <strong>and</strong> between group-comparison<strong>and</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject forms of experimental research. <strong>How</strong>ever,different authors use different categories <strong>to</strong> describe nonexperimentalresearch, with the most common be<strong>in</strong>g the ones weuse <strong>in</strong> this text (correlational, causal-comparative, <strong>and</strong> survey).These categories, however, are mostly a matter of convenience<strong>and</strong> cus<strong>to</strong>m rather than reflect<strong>in</strong>g essential differences. Correlational<strong>and</strong> causal-comparative methods differ largely <strong>in</strong> thenature of the variables <strong>in</strong>vestigated (quantitative versus categorical)<strong>and</strong> the methods of data analysis. Survey researchdiffers from the other two primarily <strong>in</strong> its purpose. We mustadmit that such a system is not very satisfy<strong>in</strong>g.Recently, Johnson has proposed a new means of classification.*He suggests us<strong>in</strong>g a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of purpose (descriptive,predictive, or explana<strong>to</strong>ry) <strong>and</strong> time frame (retrospective,cross-sectional or longitud<strong>in</strong>al) <strong>to</strong> identify different methods.Such comb<strong>in</strong>ations produce a <strong>to</strong>tal of n<strong>in</strong>e different types.While we would agree that his typology is logically more consistent,we do not f<strong>in</strong>d it useful nor appropriate for an <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>rytext. Why? Because the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> correlational,causal-comparative, <strong>and</strong> survey research are quite different,<strong>and</strong> we believe strongly that students need <strong>to</strong> learn these steps.We see no reason <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease the complexity <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>gso. We also note that a fairly recent survey of teachers of educationalresearch showed that 80 percent favored reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g thecorrelational versus causal-comparative dist<strong>in</strong>ction, apparentlyf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g it useful despite its deficiencies.†*B. Johnson (2001). Towards a new classification of nonexperimentalquantitative research. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 30(2): 3–13.†Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bacon (1996). <strong>Research</strong> methods survey. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn& Bacon.Similarities. Both causal-comparative <strong>and</strong> correlationalstudies are examples of associational research—that is, researchers who conduct them seek <strong>to</strong> explorerelationships among variables. Both attempt <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong>phenomena of <strong>in</strong>terest. Both seek <strong>to</strong> identify variablesthat are worthy of later exploration through experimentalresearch, <strong>and</strong> both often provide guidance forsubsequent experimental studies. Neither permits themanipulation of variables by the researcher, however.Both attempt <strong>to</strong> explore causation, but, <strong>in</strong> both cases,causation must be argued; the methodology alone doesnot permit causal statements.Differences. Causal-comparative studies typicallycompare two or more groups of subjects, while correlationalstudies require a score on each variable for eachsubject. Correlational studies <strong>in</strong>vestigate two (or more)quantitative variables, whereas causal-comparativestudies typically <strong>in</strong>volve at least one categorical variable(group membership). Correlational studies oftenanalyze data us<strong>in</strong>g scatterplots <strong>and</strong>/or correlation coefficients,while causal-comparative studies often compareaverages or use crossbreak tables.SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCESBETWEEN CAUSAL-COMPARATIVEAND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCHSimilarities. Both causal-comparative <strong>and</strong> experimentalstudies typically require at least one categoricalvariable (group membership). Both compare group performances(average scores) <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e relationships.Both typically compare separate groups of subjects.*Differences. In experimental research, the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable is manipulated; <strong>in</strong> causal-comparativeresearch, no manipulation takes place. causal-comparativestudies are likely <strong>to</strong> provide much weaker evidencefor causation than do experimental studies. In experimentalresearch, the researcher can sometimes assignsubjects <strong>to</strong> treatment groups; <strong>in</strong> causal-comparativeresearch, the groups are already formed—the researchermust locate them. In experimental studies, the researcherhas much greater flexibility <strong>in</strong> formulat<strong>in</strong>g thestructure of the design.*Except <strong>in</strong> counterbalanced, time-series, or s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experimentaldesigns (see Chapters 13 <strong>and</strong> 14).365


366 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSteps Involved <strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>PROBLEM FORMULATIONThe first step <strong>in</strong> formulat<strong>in</strong>g a problem <strong>in</strong> causalcomparativeresearch is usually <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ethe particular phenomena of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> considerpossible causes for, or consequences of, these phenomena.Suppose, for example, that a researcher is<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> student creativity. What causes creativity?Why are a few students highly creative while most arenot? Why do some students who <strong>in</strong>itially appear <strong>to</strong> becreative seem <strong>to</strong> lose this characteristic? Why do otherswho at one time are not creative later become so? Andso forth.The researcher speculates, for example, that highlevelcreativity might be caused by a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of socialfailure, on the one h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> personal recognitionfor artistic or scientific achievement, on the other. Theresearcher also identifies a number of alternative hypothesesthat might account for a difference betweenhighly creative <strong>and</strong> noncreative students. Both the quantity<strong>and</strong> quality of a student’s <strong>in</strong>terests, for example,might account for differences <strong>in</strong> creativity. Highly creativestudents might tend <strong>to</strong> have many diverse <strong>in</strong>terests.Parental encouragement <strong>to</strong> explore ideas mightalso account partly for creativity, as might some typesof <strong>in</strong>tellectual skills.Once possible causes of the phenomena have beenidentified, they are (usually) <strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a moreprecise statement of the research problem the researcherwishes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, the researchermight state that the objective of his research is “<strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>epossible differences between students of high<strong>and</strong> low creativity.” Note that differences <strong>in</strong> a number ofvariables can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparativestudy <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e which variable (or comb<strong>in</strong>ationof variables) seems most likely <strong>to</strong> cause the phenomenon(creativity, <strong>in</strong> this case) be<strong>in</strong>g studied. Thistest<strong>in</strong>g of several alternative hypotheses is a basic characteristicof good causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,whenever possible, should be the basis for identify<strong>in</strong>gthe variables on which the comparison groups are <strong>to</strong> becontrasted. This provides a rational basis for selectionof the variables <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated, rather than rely<strong>in</strong>g onwhat is often called the shotgun approach, <strong>in</strong> whicha large number of measures are adm<strong>in</strong>istered simplybecause they seem <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g or are available. Theyalso serve <strong>to</strong> rem<strong>in</strong>d the researcher that the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of acausal-comparative study are open <strong>to</strong> a variety of causalexplanations.SAMPLEOnce the researcher has formulated the problem statement(<strong>and</strong> hypotheses, if any) the next step is <strong>to</strong> selectthe sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> be studied. The most importanttask here is <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e carefully the characteristic <strong>to</strong>be studied <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> select groups that differ <strong>in</strong> thischaracteristic. In the above example, this means def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gas clearly as possible the term creativity. If possible,operational def<strong>in</strong>itions should be employed. A highlycreative student might be def<strong>in</strong>ed, for example, as onewho “has developed an award-w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g scientific orartistic product.”The researcher also needs <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about whether thegroup obta<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g the operational def<strong>in</strong>ition is likely<strong>to</strong> be reasonably homogeneous <strong>in</strong> terms of fac<strong>to</strong>rs caus<strong>in</strong>gcreativity. For example, are students who are creative<strong>in</strong> science similar <strong>to</strong> students who are creative <strong>in</strong>art with respect <strong>to</strong> causation? This is a very importantquestion <strong>to</strong> ask. If creativity has different “causes” <strong>in</strong>different fields, the search for causation is only confusedby comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g students from such fields. Do ethnic,age, or gender differences produce differences <strong>in</strong>creativity? The success of a causal-comparative studydepends <strong>in</strong> large degree on how carefully the comparisongroups are def<strong>in</strong>ed.It is very important <strong>to</strong> select groups that are homogeneouswith regard <strong>to</strong> at least some important variables.For example, if the researcher assumes that the samecauses are operat<strong>in</strong>g for all creative students, regardlessof gender, ethnicity, or age, he or she may f<strong>in</strong>d no differencesbetween comparison groups simply because <strong>to</strong>omany other variables are <strong>in</strong>volved. If all creative studentsare treated as a homogeneous group, no differencesmay be found between highly creative <strong>and</strong> noncreativestudents, whereas if only creative <strong>and</strong>noncreative female art students are compared, differencesmay be found.Once the def<strong>in</strong>ed groups have been selected, they canbe matched on one or more variables. This process ofmatch<strong>in</strong>g controls certa<strong>in</strong> variables, thereby elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>gany group differences on these variables. This is desirable<strong>in</strong> type 1 <strong>and</strong> type 3 studies (see page 364), s<strong>in</strong>ce theresearcher wants the groups as similar as possible <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> differences on the dependent variable(s)as be<strong>in</strong>g due <strong>to</strong> group membership. Match<strong>in</strong>g is not as


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 367appropriate <strong>in</strong> type 2 studies, because the researcher presumablyknows little about the extraneous variables thatmight be related <strong>to</strong> group differences <strong>and</strong> as a result cannotmatch on them.INSTRUMENTATIONThere are no limits on the types of <strong>in</strong>struments that maybe used <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies. Achievementtests, questionnaires, <strong>in</strong>terview schedules, attitud<strong>in</strong>almeasures, observational devices—any of the devicesdiscussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7 can be used.DESIGNThe basic causal-comparative design <strong>in</strong>volves select<strong>in</strong>gtwo or more groups that differ on a particular variableof <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g them on another variableor variables. No manipulation is <strong>in</strong>volved. Thegroups differ <strong>in</strong> one of two ways: One group eitherpossesses a characteristic (often called a criterion) thatthe other does not, or the groups differ on known characteristics.These two variations of the same basicdesign (sometimes called a criterion-group design) areas follows:The Basic Causal-Comparative <strong>Design</strong>sIndependent DependentGroup variable variable(a) I C O(Group possesses (Measurement)characteristic)II –C O(Group does (Measurement)not possesscharacteristic)(b) I C 1 O(Group possesses (Measurement)characteristic 1)II C 2 O(Group possesses (Measurement)characteristic 2)The letter C is used <strong>in</strong> this design <strong>to</strong> represent the presenceof the characteristic. The dashed l<strong>in</strong>e is used <strong>to</strong>show that <strong>in</strong>tact groups are be<strong>in</strong>g compared. Examplesof these causal-comparative designs are presented <strong>in</strong>Figure 16.1.(a) Group IndependentvariableDependentvariableI CODropouts Level ofself-esteemII(–C)NondropoutsOLevel ofself-esteem(b) Group IndependentvariableDependentvariableI C 1OCounselors Amount ofjob satisfactionIIC 2TeachersFigure 16.1 Examples of the Basic Causal-Comparative <strong>Design</strong>OAmount ofjob satisfactionThreats <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>Two weaknesses <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research arelack of r<strong>and</strong>omization <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ability <strong>to</strong> manipulate an <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable. As we have mentioned, r<strong>and</strong>om assignmen<strong>to</strong>f subjects <strong>to</strong> groups is not possible <strong>in</strong> causalcomparativeresearch s<strong>in</strong>ce the groups are alreadyformed. Manipulation of the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable is notpossible because the groups have already been exposed<strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable.SUBJECT CHARACTERISTICSThe major threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of a causalcomparativestudy is the possibility of a subject characteristicsthreat. Because the researcher has had no say <strong>in</strong>either the selection or formation of the comparisongroups, there is always the likelihood that the groupsare not equivalent on one or more important variablesother than the identified group membership variable(Figure 16.2). A group of girls, for example, might beolder than a comparison group of boys.There are a number of procedures that a researchercan use <strong>to</strong> reduce the chance of a subject characteristicsthreat <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparative study. Many of these arealso used <strong>in</strong> experimental research (see Chapter 13).


368 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 16.2 A SubjectCharacteristics Threat“The results ofthe study show thatvegetarians are healthierthan meat eaters.”“Whoa!Those vegetarianswere <strong>in</strong> better shape<strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with!”Match<strong>in</strong>g of Subjects. One way <strong>to</strong> control for anextraneous variable is <strong>to</strong> match subjects from the comparisongroups on that variable. In other words, pairs ofsubjects, one from each group, are found that are similaron that variable. Students might be matched on GPA, forexample, <strong>in</strong> a study of attitudes. Individuals with similarGPAs would be matched. If a match cannot be found fora particular subject, he or she is then elim<strong>in</strong>ated from thestudy. As you have probably realized, the problem withmatch<strong>in</strong>g is that often matches cannot be found for manysubjects, <strong>and</strong> hence the size of the sample is accord<strong>in</strong>glyreduced. Match<strong>in</strong>g becomes even more difficult whenthe researcher tries <strong>to</strong> match on two or more variables.F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g or Creat<strong>in</strong>g Homogeneous Subgroups.Another way <strong>to</strong> control for an extraneousvariable is either <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d, or restrict one’s comparison <strong>to</strong>,groups that are relatively homogeneous on that variable.In the attitude study, the researcher could either seek <strong>to</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d two groups that have similar GPAs (say, all 3.5GPA or above) or form subgroups that represent variouslevels of the extraneous variable (divide the groups <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>high, middle, <strong>and</strong> low GPA subgroups, for example),<strong>and</strong> then compare the comparable subgroups (low GPAsubgroup with the other low GPA subgroup, <strong>and</strong> so on).Statistical Match<strong>in</strong>g. The third way <strong>to</strong> controlfor an important extraneous variable is <strong>to</strong> match thegroups on that variable, us<strong>in</strong>g the technique of statisticalmatch<strong>in</strong>g. As described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13, statistical match<strong>in</strong>gadjusts scores on a posttest for <strong>in</strong>itial differences onsome other variable that is assumed <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong>performance on the dependent variable.OTHER THREATSThe likelihood of the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitydepends on the type of study be<strong>in</strong>g considered. Innon<strong>in</strong>tervention studies, the ma<strong>in</strong> additional concernsare loss of subjects, location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, <strong>and</strong> sometimeshis<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> maturation. If the persons who are lost<strong>to</strong> data collection are different from those who rema<strong>in</strong>(as is often probable) <strong>and</strong> if more are lost from one groupthan the other(s), <strong>in</strong>ternal validity is threatened. If unequalnumbers are lost, an effort should be made <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ethe probable reasons.A location threat is possible if the data are collectedunder different conditions for different groups. Similarly,if different data collec<strong>to</strong>rs are used with differentgroups, an <strong>in</strong>strumentation threat is <strong>in</strong>troduced. Fortunately,it is usually relatively easy <strong>to</strong> ensure that variations<strong>in</strong> location <strong>and</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>rs do not exist.The possibility of data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias can usually becontrolled, as <strong>in</strong> experimental studies, by ensur<strong>in</strong>g thatwhoever collects the data lacks any <strong>in</strong>formation thatmight bias results. Instrument decay may occur <strong>in</strong> observationalstudies <strong>and</strong> with repeated adm<strong>in</strong>istration ofthe same test <strong>to</strong> the same group(s). It can be controlledas <strong>in</strong> experimental studies.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 369In <strong>in</strong>tervention-type studies, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> thethreats just discussed, all of the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g threats thatwe discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13 may be present. Unfortunately,most are harder <strong>to</strong> control <strong>in</strong> causal-comparativestudies than <strong>in</strong> experimental research. The factthat the researcher does not directly manipulate thetreatment variable makes it more likely that a his<strong>to</strong>rythreat may exist. It may also mean that the length ofthe treatment time may have varied, thus creat<strong>in</strong>g apossible maturation threat. An attitude threat is lesslikely because noth<strong>in</strong>g “special” is <strong>in</strong>troduced. Regressionmay be a threat if one of the groups was <strong>in</strong>itiallyselected on the basis of extreme scores. F<strong>in</strong>ally,a pretest/treatment <strong>in</strong>teraction effect, as <strong>in</strong> experimentalstudies, may exist if a pretest was used <strong>in</strong> the study.As we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13 (see page 280), weth<strong>in</strong>k both experimental <strong>and</strong> causal-comparative <strong>in</strong>terventionstudies are useful.Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> Causal-ComparativeStudiesThe evaluation of specific threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>causal-comparative studies <strong>in</strong>volves a set of steps similar<strong>to</strong> those presented <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13 for experimentalstudies.Step 1: Ask: What specific fac<strong>to</strong>rs either are known<strong>to</strong> affect or may logically be expected <strong>to</strong> affect thevariable on which groups are be<strong>in</strong>g compared?Note that this is the dependent variable for type 1<strong>and</strong> type 3 studies (see page 364), but the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable for type 2 studies. As we mentionedwith regard <strong>to</strong> experimental studies, the researcherneed not be concerned with fac<strong>to</strong>rsunrelated <strong>to</strong> what is be<strong>in</strong>g studied.Step 2: Ask: What is the likelihood of the comparisongroups differ<strong>in</strong>g on each of these fac<strong>to</strong>rs?(Remember that a difference between groups cannotbe expla<strong>in</strong>ed away by a fac<strong>to</strong>r that is the samefor all groups.)Step 3: <strong>Evaluate</strong> the threats on the basis of howlikely they are <strong>to</strong> have an effect, <strong>and</strong> plan <strong>to</strong> controlfor them. If a given threat cannot be controlled,this should be acknowledged.Aga<strong>in</strong>, let us consider an example <strong>to</strong> illustrate howthese steps might be employed. Suppose a researcherwishes <strong>to</strong> explore possible causes of students dropp<strong>in</strong>gout <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ner-city high schools. He or she hypothesizesthree possible causes: (1) family <strong>in</strong>stability, (2) low studentself-esteem, <strong>and</strong> (3) lack of a support system related<strong>to</strong> school <strong>and</strong> its requirements. The researchercompiles a list of recent dropouts <strong>and</strong> r<strong>and</strong>omly selectsa comparison group of students still <strong>in</strong> school. He then<strong>in</strong>terviews students <strong>in</strong> both groups <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> data oneach of the three possible causal variables.As we did <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 <strong>and</strong> 15, we list below anumber of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity discussed <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9, followed by our evaluation of each as theymight apply <strong>to</strong> this study.Subject Characteristics. Although many possiblesubject characteristics might be considered, we dealwith only four here—socioeconomic level of the family,gender, ethnicity, <strong>and</strong> marketable job skills.1. Socioeconomic level of the family. Step 1: Socioeconomiclevel may be related <strong>to</strong> all three of the hypothesizedcausal variables. Step 2: Socioeconomiclevel can be expected <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> dropp<strong>in</strong>g outversus stay<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> school. It should therefore be controlledby some form of match<strong>in</strong>g. Step 3: Likelihoodof hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unless controlled: high.2. Gender. Step 1: Gender may also be related <strong>to</strong> eachof the three hypothesized causal variables. Step 2: Itmay well be related <strong>to</strong> dropp<strong>in</strong>g out. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly,the researcher should either restrict this study only<strong>to</strong> males or females or ensure that the comparisongroup has the same gender proportions as thedropout group.* Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effectunless controlled: high.3. Ethnicity. Step 1: Ethnicity may also be related <strong>to</strong>all three of the hypothesized causal variables.Step 2: It may be related <strong>to</strong> dropp<strong>in</strong>g out. Therefore,the two groups should be matched with respect <strong>to</strong>ethnicity. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effectunless controlled: moderate <strong>to</strong> high.4. Marketable job skills. Step 1: Job skills may berelated <strong>to</strong> each of the three hypothesized causal variables.Step 2: They are likely <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> dropp<strong>in</strong>gout, s<strong>in</strong>ce students often drop out if they areable <strong>to</strong> make money work<strong>in</strong>g. It would be desirable,*This is an example of stratify<strong>in</strong>g a sample—<strong>in</strong> this case, thecomparison group.


370 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7etherefore, <strong>to</strong> assess job skills <strong>and</strong> then control bysome form of match<strong>in</strong>g. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: moderate <strong>to</strong> high.Mortality. Step 1: It is probable that refus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>terviewed is related <strong>to</strong> each of the three hypothesizedcausal variables. Step 2: It is also probable that morestudents <strong>in</strong> the dropout group will refuse <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed(s<strong>in</strong>ce they may be work<strong>in</strong>g, it may be harder <strong>to</strong>arrange time for an <strong>in</strong>terview) than will students <strong>in</strong> thecomparison group. The only solution would be <strong>to</strong> makeevery effort <strong>to</strong> get cooperation for the <strong>in</strong>terviews fromall subjects <strong>in</strong> both groups. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: high.Location. Step 1: While it seems unlikely that thecausal variables would differ for different schools, thismight be the case. Step 2: It is quite likely that location(that is, the specific high schools <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study)is related <strong>to</strong> dropp<strong>in</strong>g out. (Dropout rates typically differacross schools.) The best solution is <strong>to</strong> analyze the dataseparately for each school. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: moderate.Instrumentation1. Instrument decay. Step 1: Instrument decay <strong>in</strong> thisstudy means <strong>in</strong>terviewer fatigue. This certa<strong>in</strong>lycould affect the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from students<strong>in</strong> both groups. Step 2: The fatigue fac<strong>to</strong>r could bedifferent for the two groups, depend<strong>in</strong>g on how <strong>in</strong>terviewsare scheduled; the solution is <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong>schedule <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>to</strong> prevent fatigue from occurr<strong>in</strong>g.Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effect unlesscontrolled: moderate.2. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics. Step 1: Data collec<strong>to</strong>rcharacteristics can be expected <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencethe <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed on the three hypothesizedcausal variables; for this reason, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>terviewers<strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardize the <strong>in</strong>terview process is veryimportant. Step 2: Despite such tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, different<strong>in</strong>terviewers might elicit different <strong>in</strong>formation.Therefore, <strong>in</strong>terviewers should be balanced acrossthe two groups; that is, each <strong>in</strong>terviewer should bescheduled <strong>to</strong> do the same number of <strong>in</strong>terviews witheach group. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>g an effectunless controlled: moderate.3. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias. Step 1: Bias might well be related<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed on the three hypothesizedcausal variables. Step 2: Bias might differ forthe two groups; for example, an <strong>in</strong>terviewer mightbehave differently when <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g dropouts. Thesolution is <strong>to</strong> keep <strong>in</strong>terviewers ignorant as <strong>to</strong> whichgroup subjects belong <strong>to</strong>. To do this, care has <strong>to</strong> betaken both with the questions <strong>to</strong> be asked <strong>and</strong> thetra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>terviewers. Step 3: Likelihood of hav<strong>in</strong>gan effect unless controlled: high.Other Threats. Implementation, his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation,attitud<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> regression threats do not affect thisk<strong>in</strong>d (type 2) of causal-comparative study.The trick <strong>to</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>causal-comparative studies (as <strong>in</strong> experimental studies)is, first, <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of various th<strong>in</strong>gs (conditions, othervariables, <strong>and</strong> so on) that might affect the outcome variableof the study. Then, second, <strong>to</strong> decide, based on evidenceor experience, whether these th<strong>in</strong>gs would belikely <strong>to</strong> affect the comparison groups differently. If so,this may provide an alternative explanation for the results.If this seems likely, a threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validityof the study may <strong>in</strong>deed be present <strong>and</strong> needs <strong>to</strong> be controlled.Many of these threats can be greatly reduced ifcausal-comparative studies are replicated. Figure 16.3summarizes the process of evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the presence ofthreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.Data AnalysisThe first step <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparativestudy is <strong>to</strong> construct frequency polygons <strong>and</strong> then calculatethe mean <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of each group if thevariable is quantitative. These descriptive statistics arethen assessed for magnitude (see Chapter 12). A statistical<strong>in</strong>ference test may or may not be appropriate, depend<strong>in</strong>gon whether r<strong>and</strong>om samples were used fromidentified populations (such as creative versus noncreativehigh school seniors). The most commonly used test<strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies is a t-test for differencesbetween means. When more than two groups are used,then either an analysis of variance or an analysis ofcovariance is the appropriate test. Analysis of covarianceis particularly helpful <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research becausea researcher cannot always match the comparisongroups on all relevant variables other than the ones ofprimary <strong>in</strong>terest. As mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 11, analysis ofcovariance provides a way <strong>to</strong> match groups “after thefact” on such variables as age, socioeconomic status,


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 371Results forgroup AResults forgroup BDoes a threat<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity exist?OutcomevariableExtraneous variableDifferenceexistsDifferenceexistsYesOutcome variableExtraneous variableDifferenceexistsDifferenceexistsNoOutcomevariableExtraneous variableDifferenceexistsNo differenceexistsNo(Shaded areas <strong>in</strong>dicate tha<strong>to</strong>utcome <strong>and</strong> extraneousvariables are related.)Figure 16.3 Does a Threat <strong>to</strong> Internal Validity Exist?aptitude, <strong>and</strong> so on. Before analysis of covariance can beused, however, the data <strong>in</strong>volved need <strong>to</strong> satisfy certa<strong>in</strong>assumptions. 1The results of a causal-comparative study must be <strong>in</strong>terpretedwith caution. As with correlational studies,causal-comparative studies are good at identify<strong>in</strong>g relationshipsbetween variables, but they do not prove cause<strong>and</strong> effect.There are two ways <strong>to</strong> strengthen the <strong>in</strong>terpretabilityof causal-comparative studies. First, as we mentionedearlier, alternative hypotheses should be formulated <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>vestigated whenever possible. Second, if the dependentvariables <strong>in</strong>volved are categorical, the relationshipsamong all of the variables <strong>in</strong> the study should beexam<strong>in</strong>ed us<strong>in</strong>g the technique of discrim<strong>in</strong>ant functionanalysis, which we briefly described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 15.The most powerful way <strong>to</strong> check on the possiblecauses identified <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparative study, ofcourse, is <strong>to</strong> perform an experiment. The presumed cause(or causes) identified can sometimes be manipulated.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHSignificant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>Awidely cited causal-comparative study was conducted bytwo researchers <strong>in</strong> the 1940s.* They compared twogroups of 500 boys, one group identified as juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quentsbased on their hav<strong>in</strong>g been <strong>in</strong>stitutionalized (7 monthson average), <strong>and</strong> a second group not so identified. Both groupswere from the same “high-risk” area of Bos<strong>to</strong>n. Pairs of boys,one from each group, were matched on ethnicity, IQ, <strong>and</strong> age.The major differences they found between the groups were*S. Gluek <strong>and</strong> E. Gluek (1950). Unravel<strong>in</strong>g juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quency.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.that the boys <strong>in</strong> the del<strong>in</strong>quent group had more solid muscularbodies, were more energetic <strong>and</strong> extroverted, were more unconventional<strong>and</strong> defiant, were less methodical <strong>and</strong> abstract,<strong>and</strong> came from less cohesive, less affectionate families. Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthese characteristics resulted <strong>in</strong> a table for predict<strong>in</strong>gprobable del<strong>in</strong>quency that has received considerable validation<strong>in</strong> other sett<strong>in</strong>gs over the years.† Nonetheless, argumentcont<strong>in</strong>ues as <strong>to</strong> the nature of cause <strong>and</strong> effect <strong>and</strong> as <strong>to</strong> the desirabilityof us<strong>in</strong>g such predictive <strong>in</strong>formation. It could beused either for benevolent <strong>in</strong>tervention (as envisioned by theresearchers who did the orig<strong>in</strong>al study) or <strong>to</strong> stigmatize <strong>and</strong>coerce.†S. Gluek <strong>and</strong> E. Gluek (1974). Of del<strong>in</strong>quency <strong>and</strong> crime. Spr<strong>in</strong>gfield,IL: C. Thomas.Should differences between experimental <strong>and</strong> controlgroups now be found, the researcher then has a muchbetter reason for <strong>in</strong>ferr<strong>in</strong>g causation.Associations BetweenCategorical VariablesUp <strong>to</strong> this po<strong>in</strong>t, our discussion of associational methodshas considered only the situations <strong>in</strong> which (1) onevariable is categorical <strong>and</strong> the other(s) is(are) quantitative(causal-comparative), <strong>and</strong> (2) both variables arequantitative (correlational). It is also possible <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigateassociations between categorical variables. Bothcrossbreak tables (see Chapter 10) <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficientsare used. An example of a relationship betweencategorical variables is shown <strong>in</strong> Table 16.1.TABLE 16.1 Grade Level <strong>and</strong> Gender of Teachers(Hypothetical Data)Grade Level Males Females TotalElementary 40 70 110Junior High 50 40 90Senior High 80 60 140Total 170 170 340As was true with correlation, such data can be usedfor purposes of prediction <strong>and</strong>, with caution, <strong>in</strong> thesearch for cause <strong>and</strong> effect. Know<strong>in</strong>g that a person is ateacher <strong>and</strong> male, for example, we can predict, withsome degree of confidence (on the basis of the data <strong>in</strong>Table 16.1), that he teaches either junior or senior highschool, s<strong>in</strong>ce 76 percent of males who are teachers doso. We can also estimate how much <strong>in</strong> error our predictionis likely <strong>to</strong> be. Based on the data <strong>in</strong> Table 16.1, theprobability of our prediction be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> error is 40/170, or.24. In this example, the possibility that gender is amajor cause of teach<strong>in</strong>g level seems quite remote—there are other variables, such as his<strong>to</strong>rical patterns ofteacher preparation <strong>and</strong> hir<strong>in</strong>g, that make more sensewhen one tries <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> the relationship.There are no techniques analogous <strong>to</strong> partial correlation(see Chapter 15) or the other techniques that haveevolved from correlational research that can be usedwith categorical variables. Further, prediction fromcrossbreak tables is much less precise than from scatterplots.Fortunately, there are relatively few questions of<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> education that <strong>in</strong>volve two categorical variables.It is common, however, <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d a researcher treat<strong>in</strong>gvariables that are conceptually quantitative (<strong>and</strong>measured accord<strong>in</strong>gly) as if they were categorical. Forexample, a researcher arbitrarily may divide a set ofquantitative scores <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> high, middle, <strong>and</strong> low groups.Noth<strong>in</strong>g is ga<strong>in</strong>ed by this procedure, <strong>and</strong> it suffers fromtwo serious defects: the loss of the precision that is acquiredthrough the use of correlational techniques <strong>and</strong>372


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 373the essential arbitrar<strong>in</strong>ess of the division <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> groups.<strong>How</strong> does one decide which score separates “high”scores from “middle” scores, for example? In general,therefore, such arbitrary division should be avoided.**There are times when a quantitative variable is justifiably treated asa categorical variable. For example, creativity is generally considered<strong>to</strong> be a quantitative variable. One might, however, establish criteriafor divid<strong>in</strong>g this cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> only two categories—“highlycreative” <strong>and</strong> “typically creative”—as a way of study<strong>in</strong>g relationshipswith other variables more efficiently.An Example of Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of causal-comparative research, followed by acritique of its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong>our critiques of the different types of research studieswe analyzed <strong>in</strong> other chapters, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis. Thisstudy is an example of mixed-methods research.RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Professional School Counsel<strong>in</strong>g, 4, no. 2 (December 2000): 86–94. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission, AmericanSchool Counselor Association.Personal, Social, <strong>and</strong> FamilyCharacteristics of Angry StudentsDale Fryxell, Ph.D.Cham<strong>in</strong>ade University, Honolulu, HI.Douglas C. Smith, Ph.D.University of Hawaii, Honolulu.Amid grow<strong>in</strong>g public concern about youth violence <strong>in</strong> school sett<strong>in</strong>gs, school <strong>and</strong> mentalhealth counselors are <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly be<strong>in</strong>g asked <strong>to</strong> play a leadership role <strong>in</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>gpolicies with regard <strong>to</strong> violence prevention <strong>and</strong> school safety. Although school violenceis a complex issue with multiple causal pathways, one potentially important variable isthe high degree of anger <strong>and</strong> hostility exist<strong>in</strong>g among some students <strong>to</strong>day. Angerrelatedproblems are among the most common reasons for referral <strong>to</strong> school counselors(Smith & Furlong, 1994). Interest <strong>in</strong> anger <strong>and</strong> hostility as precursors of aggressive behaviorcan be seen <strong>in</strong> the grow<strong>in</strong>g number of anger management programs currently operat<strong>in</strong>gwith<strong>in</strong> school sett<strong>in</strong>gs (Larson, 1994; Smith, Larson, DeBaryshe, & Salzman, 2000).Children identified as manifest<strong>in</strong>g high levels of anger <strong>and</strong> hostility at school appear<strong>to</strong> be at risk for a number of behavioral, social, academic, <strong>and</strong> physical concerns(Smith & Furlong, 1994). Longitud<strong>in</strong>al research suggests that angry, aggressive childrenare likely <strong>to</strong> grow up <strong>to</strong> be angry, aggressive adults (Elliot, Huiz<strong>in</strong>ga, & Age<strong>to</strong>n, 1985;Kazd<strong>in</strong>, 1987; Olweus, 1979). Thus, it is important <strong>to</strong> identify children manifest<strong>in</strong>g angerrelatedproblems early <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide appropriate <strong>in</strong>terventions. With regard <strong>to</strong> specificoutcomes for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents, the few studies available suggest that high levelsof anger <strong>and</strong> hostility are associated with poorer academic performance (Smith, Adelman,Nelson, Taylor, & Phares, 1987; Smith, Furlong, Bates, & Laughl<strong>in</strong>, 1998) <strong>and</strong> with a widerange of social <strong>and</strong> behavioral difficulties both <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> out of school (Lochman, White, &Wayl<strong>and</strong>, 1991; Smith et al., 1998). Deffenbacher <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Deffenbacher, Lynch,Justification <strong>and</strong>prior research


374 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePrior researchToo strongType 2 studyType 1 study(see page 364)PurposeOett<strong>in</strong>g, & Kemper, 1996; Liebsohn, Oett<strong>in</strong>g, & Deffenbacher, 1994) have provided somepromis<strong>in</strong>g directions for future research with <strong>in</strong>itial f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g high levels of anger<strong>in</strong> youth <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased risk for substance abuse, del<strong>in</strong>quency, <strong>in</strong>terpersonal difficulties, vocational<strong>and</strong> school-related problems, <strong>and</strong> other maladaptive behaviors.In exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g causal <strong>in</strong>fluences on anger, hostility, <strong>and</strong> aggression <strong>in</strong> children,Greenberg, Speltz, <strong>and</strong> DeKlyen (1993) proposed a four-fac<strong>to</strong>r model that <strong>in</strong>cludes familystressors, parent discipl<strong>in</strong>e styles, attachment relationships, <strong>and</strong> with<strong>in</strong>-child characteristicssuch as <strong>in</strong>dividual temperaments. Tolan, Guerra, <strong>and</strong> Kendall (1995) suggestedthat researchers consider multiple developmental-ecological pathways <strong>to</strong> children’s antisocialbehavior. These might <strong>in</strong>clude both <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal characteristics aswell as contextual variables, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g specific cultural <strong>and</strong> community contexts. In mostcases, it appears that such fac<strong>to</strong>rs mutually <strong>in</strong>fluence children’s behavior. For example,Patterson <strong>and</strong> colleagues (Patterson, 1982; Patterson, DeBaryshe, & Ramsey, 1989; Reid &Patterson, 1991) suggested that <strong>in</strong>dividual temperament <strong>and</strong> dispositions with regard <strong>to</strong>anger <strong>and</strong> aggression are shaped <strong>and</strong> modified through <strong>in</strong>teractions both with<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>outside the family. Specifically, early coercive family <strong>in</strong>teractions serve as models of aggressivebehaviors, <strong>and</strong> children are themselves re<strong>in</strong>forced for such behaviors by avoidanceof negative stimuli. Degrees of nurturance <strong>and</strong> family support are also critical determ<strong>in</strong>antsof children’s social <strong>and</strong> emotional development (Reid & Patterson, 1991).Personal fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>clude personality traits, disposition, reactivity, <strong>and</strong> temperament.In terms of anger <strong>and</strong> hostility, these fac<strong>to</strong>rs might <strong>in</strong>clude a propensity <strong>to</strong> react <strong>in</strong>tensely<strong>to</strong> perceived transgressions. It might also <strong>in</strong>clude such social-cognitive processesas causal attributions, which assess <strong>in</strong>tentionality on the part of others. Previous research<strong>in</strong>dicates that aggressive children frequently attribute <strong>in</strong>tentionality <strong>to</strong> negative actionsby peers, thereby justify<strong>in</strong>g aggressive retaliation (Dodge, 1991).School fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>clude not only the immediate physical environment, size of school,<strong>and</strong> general school climate, but also such variables as attitudes of teachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,discipl<strong>in</strong>e policies, <strong>and</strong> student academic performance. Smith et al. (1987) concludedthat at least some of the anger <strong>and</strong> hostility expressed by students <strong>in</strong> schoolsett<strong>in</strong>gs may be the result of academic failures <strong>and</strong>/or lack of academic competence.Peer relationships are also important determ<strong>in</strong>ants of anger <strong>and</strong> hostility at school.It is becom<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly clear that students who lack social competence <strong>and</strong> have fewfriends at school are more prone <strong>to</strong> aggressive behavior. In a longitud<strong>in</strong>al study of emotionalcompetence <strong>and</strong> peer relations (Eisenberg et al., 1995), it was determ<strong>in</strong>ed that thefrequency of children’s negative emotions such as anger <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ability <strong>to</strong> effectivelyregulate negative feel<strong>in</strong>gs were <strong>in</strong>versely related <strong>to</strong> peer acceptance.Most of the above-cited work has focused on underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the complex <strong>in</strong>terplaybetween <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> contextual fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> children’s displays of aggressivebehavior. Clearly more work is needed <strong>to</strong> provide a more detailed picture both of thefac<strong>to</strong>rs promot<strong>in</strong>g high levels of anger <strong>and</strong> hostility <strong>in</strong> youth <strong>and</strong> the impact of anger<strong>and</strong> hostility upon social, behavioral, <strong>and</strong> academic function<strong>in</strong>g.The purpose of the present research was <strong>to</strong> address this need by collect<strong>in</strong>g descriptivedata on personal, family, peer, <strong>and</strong> school fac<strong>to</strong>rs associated with students manifest<strong>in</strong>ganger-related problems at school. In conceptualiz<strong>in</strong>g this study, we decided <strong>to</strong> use amodified case-study approach focus<strong>in</strong>g on detailed analyses of a relatively small numberof angry, hostile, <strong>and</strong> aggressive preadolescents. Given the perceived importance of <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gmultiple sources of <strong>in</strong>formation, we <strong>in</strong>terviewed not only students but also theirparents <strong>and</strong> teachers. We supplemented semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviews with school recordreviews <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized measures of behavior, attitudes at school, self-esteem, <strong>and</strong>academic performance.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 375METHODParticipantsParticipants for this study were 24 high-anger, elementary-age students identified on thebasis of teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized assessment <strong>and</strong> an equal number of low-angerstudents who served as a comparison sample. Both groups were drawn from 12 elementaryschools <strong>in</strong> the Honolulu, Hawaii, area. In general, all schools serve an economically<strong>and</strong> ethnically diverse population <strong>in</strong> a high-density area of the city.The 24 students compris<strong>in</strong>g the high-anger group <strong>in</strong>cluded 20 males <strong>and</strong> 4 femalesequally distributed between fifth <strong>and</strong> sixth grades. The mean age for this group was 10.5years. The ethnic distribution of this group mirrored the composition of the communityfrom which it was drawn <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cluded six Caucasians, four Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong>ers, one NativeAmerican, one Hispanic, <strong>and</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong>der Asian-American children of Japanese,Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Korean, <strong>and</strong>/or Filip<strong>in</strong>o descent.The 24 students <strong>in</strong> the low-anger sample <strong>in</strong>cluded 8 males <strong>and</strong> 16 females equallydistributed between fifth <strong>and</strong> sixth grades. The mean age for this group was 10.6 years.Ethnic distribution of this group <strong>in</strong>cluded three Caucasians, five Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong>ers, oneAfrican-American, two Hispanics, <strong>and</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong>der Asian-American <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Japanese,Ch<strong>in</strong>ese, Korean, <strong>and</strong> Filip<strong>in</strong>o youth.ConveniencesampleUnclearGender imbalanceInstrumentsThe Behavioral Assessment System for Children (BASC)—Aggression subscale (Reynolds &Kamphaus, 1992) was used <strong>to</strong> measure teachers’ perceptions of students’ physical <strong>and</strong> verbalaggression at school. Items are scored accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a 4-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-like format with 1be<strong>in</strong>g “not at all” <strong>and</strong> 4 be<strong>in</strong>g “all the time.” The 14-item aggression subscale has an <strong>in</strong>ternalconsistency of .95 <strong>and</strong> test-retest reliability of .91 (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992).The Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPP; Harter, 1985) is a 36-item measure ofperceived self-worth, which consists of six subscales <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g scholastic competence, socialacceptance, athletic competence, physical appearance, behavioral conduct, <strong>and</strong>global self-worth. Items are scored accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a 4-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert-like scale with 1 be<strong>in</strong>g“not at all like me” <strong>and</strong> 4 be<strong>in</strong>g “very much like me.” Internal consistency for the SPP hasbeen established as rang<strong>in</strong>g from .71 <strong>to</strong> .82 (Harter, 1985).Group used?Too strongProcedureTeachers <strong>in</strong> each of the 12 participat<strong>in</strong>g fifth- <strong>and</strong> sixth-grade classrooms were asked <strong>to</strong>nom<strong>in</strong>ate as many students as they wished from their classes who fit the criteria of“high” <strong>and</strong> “low” anger at school. High-anger students were def<strong>in</strong>ed as those studentswho displayed frequent emotional outbursts related <strong>to</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs of anger; demonstrateda consistent cynical, hostile, or angry attitude; or were <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> frequent <strong>in</strong>cidents offight<strong>in</strong>g or act<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>in</strong> an angry, aggressive manner. Low-anger students were def<strong>in</strong>ed asthose students who rarely or never displayed these characteristics.As an additional screen<strong>in</strong>g measure, all students <strong>in</strong> the 12 classrooms were adm<strong>in</strong>isteredthe Multidimensional School Anger Inven<strong>to</strong>ry (MSAI; Smith et al., 1998), whichyields subscale scores <strong>in</strong> the areas of anger arousal, cynical hostility, <strong>and</strong> negative angerexpression. Both <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of subscales <strong>and</strong> construct validity of the scale havebeen well-established (Smith et al., 1998). Internal consistency of the MSAI subscalesranged from .58 <strong>to</strong> .88. Criterion-related validity has been established as rang<strong>in</strong>g from.39 <strong>to</strong> .54 between MSAI subscales <strong>and</strong> other measures of anger <strong>and</strong> aggression.Def<strong>in</strong>itionsToo strongWhat are they?


376 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOperationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionHelpful detailThe 24 students designated as high anger <strong>in</strong> this study were those students nom<strong>in</strong>atedby their teachers as manifest<strong>in</strong>g anger-related problems at school who also scored<strong>in</strong> the <strong>to</strong>p one third of their class on one or more subscales of the MSAI. The 24 studentsdesignated as low anger were those students nom<strong>in</strong>ated by their teachers as manifest<strong>in</strong>grelatively few anger-related problems at school <strong>and</strong> who scored <strong>in</strong> the bot<strong>to</strong>m onethird of their class on one or more subscales of the MSAI.The <strong>to</strong>tal sample of 48 students compris<strong>in</strong>g both high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups was<strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong>terviewed by one of six tra<strong>in</strong>ed graduate students. All <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>to</strong>okplace at each child’s school <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cluded semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terview questions designed <strong>to</strong>elicit <strong>in</strong>formation with regard <strong>to</strong> school experiences, friendships, perceptions of self,home environment, <strong>and</strong> specific anger experiences. In addition, the students completedthe Behavioral Assessment System for Children <strong>and</strong> the Self-Perception Profile forChildren. Teachers for each student were also <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong> completeboth semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> a portion of the BASC. Teacher <strong>in</strong>terviewquestions elicited <strong>in</strong>formation about teachers’ backgrounds, perceptions of students’function<strong>in</strong>g at school, <strong>and</strong> specific impressions with regard <strong>to</strong> students’ anger. F<strong>in</strong>ally,parents of the high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger students were <strong>in</strong>dividually <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong>provide <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> semi-structured questions about family demographics,students’ developmental his<strong>to</strong>ries, social <strong>and</strong> leisure activities, anger experiences,<strong>and</strong> future expectations.In order <strong>to</strong> facilitate analysis of qualitative data, we coded student, teacher, <strong>and</strong>parent responses <strong>to</strong> open-ended questions on a 3-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scale where higher scoresrepresented more problematic, dysfunctional environments, feel<strong>in</strong>gs, behaviors, <strong>and</strong> attitudes,<strong>and</strong> lower scores <strong>in</strong>dicated more positive <strong>and</strong> prosocial function<strong>in</strong>g.ResultsGood detailData from parent, teacher, <strong>and</strong> student <strong>in</strong>terviews were organized accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> fourmajor doma<strong>in</strong>s—personal, family, peer, <strong>and</strong> school fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Personal variables <strong>in</strong>cludedparticipants’ gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity, perceived self-worth as measured by the SPP, <strong>and</strong>both negative self-descriptions <strong>and</strong> anger-related self-descriptions <strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> semistructured<strong>in</strong>terview questions. Family variables <strong>in</strong>cluded parents’ marital status <strong>and</strong>teachers’ perceptions of parent support. Peer variables <strong>in</strong>cluded number of friends reportedby participants, extent of teas<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> general peer relations as reported byteachers <strong>and</strong> students. School variables <strong>in</strong>cluded students’ grade po<strong>in</strong>t average (GPA),most recent Stanford Achievement Test (SAT) scores, attitudes <strong>and</strong> motivation <strong>to</strong>wardschool, <strong>and</strong> academic performance as reported by teachers.With<strong>in</strong> each doma<strong>in</strong>, results are reported first accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the relationship betweenglobal anger scores on the MSAI <strong>and</strong> other relevant outcome variables. This is followedby descriptions of contrasts between high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups on selected variablesof <strong>in</strong>terest. These descriptions were compiled from student, teacher, <strong>and</strong> parentresponses <strong>to</strong> semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> are reported as qualitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.Personal Doma<strong>in</strong>These probabilitiesare <strong>in</strong>appropriate,not a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.For the entire sample of students compris<strong>in</strong>g both high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups, moderatebut statistically significant negative correlations were found between MSAI global scores<strong>and</strong> the physical appearance, behavioral conduct, <strong>and</strong> global self-worth scales of the SPP(r –.35, –.45, <strong>and</strong> –.45, respectively; p < .05). Global anger was essentially unrelated <strong>to</strong>perceived athletic competence on the SPP (r .07). A moderate positive correlation wasfound between anger-related self-descriptions <strong>and</strong> MSAI global scores (r .45, p < .05),


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 377<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that students report<strong>in</strong>g high levels of anger at school also tended <strong>to</strong> characterizethemselves as angry <strong>in</strong> various other aspects of their lives.With regard <strong>to</strong> specific differences between the high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups <strong>in</strong> thisstudy, a number of personal doma<strong>in</strong> variables dist<strong>in</strong>guished the two. Most dramatic werethe disproportions among genders <strong>in</strong> the two groups, with males compris<strong>in</strong>g roughly83% of students <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group but only 33% of students <strong>in</strong> the low-angergroup. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, there were no differences <strong>in</strong> the ethnic composition of each group.Roughly equal numbers of Caucasians, Pacific Isl<strong>and</strong>ers, <strong>and</strong> Asian-Americans were identifiedas manifest<strong>in</strong>g either high levels or low levels of anger at school.Several themes emerged from qualitative analysis of student, teacher, <strong>and</strong> parent<strong>in</strong>terviews with regard <strong>to</strong> personal characteristics of students. One perta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> the natureof self-descriptions reported by high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger students. Negative self-descriptionsor self-deroga<strong>to</strong>ry comments were much more common among students <strong>in</strong> the highangergroup. One student, for example, stated that “sometimes I don’t want <strong>to</strong> be myself.”Another described himself as “not very popular because I don’t have many friends.”Yet another stated that “I don’t like anyth<strong>in</strong>g about myself.” Other negative terms used <strong>in</strong>the self-descriptions of high-anger students <strong>in</strong>cluded weird, bad, <strong>and</strong> lazy. In contrast, selfdescriptionsof low-anger students were overwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly positive with only occasional references<strong>to</strong> undesirable personal qualities.Also <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> some of the participants’ self-descriptions was <strong>in</strong>formation relat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> their experience of anger. When asked what th<strong>in</strong>gs they would like <strong>to</strong> changeabout themselves, all of the high-anger students acknowledged problems <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>gor regulat<strong>in</strong>g anger. One participant described himself as a person who “gets angry easily.”Another stated that “I lose my temper fast.” A third participant described himself as“mad when I don’t get my own way.” Another participant stated that he would like <strong>to</strong>“change my temper” but <strong>in</strong>dicated “you can’t change that.” In contrast, only one of thelow-anger participants mentioned anger control as someth<strong>in</strong>g she would like <strong>to</strong> changeabout herself. In this case, she stated that she would like <strong>to</strong> change her frequency of“gett<strong>in</strong>g really angry at my sisters.”Right!UnclearFrequencies neededhereGood specificityFamily Doma<strong>in</strong>A significant <strong>in</strong>verse relationship was found between teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs of parental support<strong>and</strong> students’ level of anger (r .53, p < .05). Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, there was no relationship betweenparents’ marital status <strong>and</strong> the degree of school anger reported by their children.Students from divorced, separated, s<strong>in</strong>gle parent, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tact families were equally representedbetween high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups.Teachers’ responses <strong>to</strong> semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviews provided some of the more<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g qualitative <strong>in</strong>formation regard<strong>in</strong>g the home life of their students. In manycases, families of high-anger students were described by teachers as fac<strong>in</strong>g significantchallenges, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g economic, occupational, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal woes. One teacher describedher student’s home life as an environment where “mom <strong>and</strong> dad often quarrel”<strong>and</strong> where “the student hides from the father <strong>to</strong> escape his anger.” Another teacher describedher student’s home life as “up <strong>and</strong> down” because the father is often away due<strong>to</strong> military duty, <strong>and</strong> the mother faces long work<strong>in</strong>g hours. Two of the participants <strong>in</strong> thehigh-anger group had particularly distress<strong>in</strong>g family backgrounds. One had never knownher real mother <strong>and</strong> spent several years liv<strong>in</strong>g with gr<strong>and</strong>parents before be<strong>in</strong>g takenback by her remarried father. Another was described by his teacher as concerned that hisreal father could show up at any time <strong>to</strong> kidnap both him <strong>and</strong> his younger sister <strong>and</strong> takethem away from their mother. Indeed, only a small m<strong>in</strong>ority of the high-anger groupwere described by teachers as hav<strong>in</strong>g even moderately positive, supportive family lives.r ?<strong>How</strong> many?<strong>How</strong> many?


378 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>How</strong> many?Def<strong>in</strong>ition?On the other h<strong>and</strong>, teachers described the majority of the families of low-angerparticipants as extremely supportive <strong>and</strong> positive. Typical descrip<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>cluded“extremely supportive, a lot of parental <strong>in</strong>volvement” <strong>and</strong> “excellent family, parents arevery <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> school, very giv<strong>in</strong>g people, encourag<strong>in</strong>g.” Even the families of low-angerstudents from divorced or separated parents were described <strong>in</strong> mostly positive terms. Forexample, one teacher mentioned that her student “lives with a mom who is very supportive<strong>and</strong> on <strong>to</strong>p of th<strong>in</strong>gs; is very <strong>in</strong>volved with her daughter.”Peer Doma<strong>in</strong>Low r’sr ?n ?Significant <strong>in</strong>verse correlations were found between MSAI global scores <strong>and</strong> the numberof friends students reported hav<strong>in</strong>g at school (r –.32, p .05) <strong>and</strong> teacher rat<strong>in</strong>gs ofsocial relationships (r –.64, p .01). Significant positive relationships were found betweenthe extent of teas<strong>in</strong>g experienced by students <strong>and</strong> their level of anger (r .43,p < .05 <strong>and</strong> between self-reported anger <strong>and</strong> the perceived anger level of friends (r .34,p < .05). This latter f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> that it implies that angry students at schooltend <strong>to</strong> associate with like-m<strong>in</strong>ded peers.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, global anger scores were essentially unrelated <strong>to</strong> students’ perceptionsof their own social competence (as evidenced by SPP scores), <strong>to</strong> number offriends outside of school, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> overall satisfaction with friends. Thus, it appears thatalthough teachers report social difficulties experienced by high-anger students, thestudents themselves may not acknowledge such problems other than the fact of simplyhav<strong>in</strong>g fewer friends at school.Qualitative analysis of student <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>in</strong>terviews suggested several themesrelated <strong>to</strong> high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger students’ <strong>in</strong>terpersonal function<strong>in</strong>g. First, teachersdescribed all of the low-anger students as hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractionswith their classmates. One low-anger student was described, for example, as “well likedby her peers; looked up <strong>to</strong> as a role model; has many friends; is really accept<strong>in</strong>g of others.”Another was described as “well liked; very friendly; has a high <strong>to</strong>lerance for otherpeople; has lots of friends.” Teacher descriptions of high-anger students, on the otherh<strong>and</strong>, were laced with comments about difficulties build<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g friendships.One teacher described her student as “not gett<strong>in</strong>g along very well; very poor relationships;the low man on the <strong>to</strong>tem pole; less power; very defensive <strong>and</strong> can’t let th<strong>in</strong>gs go.”Another participant was described as “an outcast; other students don’t accept her;she says th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a sly, devious manner.” Yet another participant was described as “aloner . . . no close friends, no attachments.”The social portrait pa<strong>in</strong>ted by teachers of high-anger students was not always sobleak, however. A number of students <strong>in</strong> this group were described by their teachersas hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships with their classmates. A few were described as “gett<strong>in</strong>galong well with others” <strong>and</strong> one was described as “the cool guy <strong>in</strong> class; bad butpopular.”Some <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g qualitative data regard<strong>in</strong>g students’ <strong>in</strong>terpersonal function<strong>in</strong>gwere also obta<strong>in</strong>ed from <strong>in</strong>terviews with parents. Once aga<strong>in</strong>, all the parents of lowangerstudents described their children’s relationships with others <strong>in</strong> positive terms. Typicalresponses <strong>in</strong>cluded “gets along with others, has a lot of friends,” or “she is friendly<strong>and</strong> outgo<strong>in</strong>g, so she gets along well with other children.” The majority of parents of thehigh-anger group also saw their children as hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships with others.Only a few concerns were noted. One parent said that his daughter “did not get alongwith other children very good <strong>and</strong> only had one or two close friends.” Another parentdescribed her son as “hav<strong>in</strong>g okay relationships” but stated that “he still isn’t close <strong>to</strong>any of his friends.”


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 379Another important theme perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> peer relationships related <strong>to</strong> teas<strong>in</strong>g atschool. Participants <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group were more likely <strong>to</strong> admit <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g teasedby others both with<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> outside the classroom. The reasons for be<strong>in</strong>g teased could beclassified <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> four general themes <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g: a) appearance, e.g., “fatty,” “scareface,”“small;” b) ability, e.g., “stupid,” “junk art;” c) names, e.g., “tiffy-taffy;” d) behavior, e.g.,“because I like a boy,” “because of the way I act.” Appearance was the most often citedreason for be<strong>in</strong>g teased by both high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger students, although this appearedmore frequently <strong>in</strong> the responses of high-anger students. Teas<strong>in</strong>g with regard <strong>to</strong> abilityoccurred almost exclusively among the experiences of high-anger students.Frequencies <strong>in</strong>each?School Doma<strong>in</strong>Moderate, significant <strong>in</strong>verse relationships were found between MSAI global scores <strong>and</strong>school success as measured by GPA (r –.57, p < .05), school work habits (r –.56, p < .05),<strong>and</strong> school-related personal <strong>and</strong> social attitudes (r –.71, p < .05). In addition, participantswho scored higher on their SAT were less likely <strong>to</strong> experience high levels of anger<strong>and</strong> hostility at school (r –.47, p < .05). A significant <strong>in</strong>verse relationship was also foundbetween teacher <strong>and</strong> student rat<strong>in</strong>gs of academic ability <strong>and</strong> anger (r –.33 <strong>and</strong> –.36, respectively;p < .05). In addition, students <strong>in</strong> the low-anger group were much more likely<strong>to</strong> be categorized as <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sically motivated than were students <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group(42% vs. 0%, respectively).Most of the qualitative <strong>in</strong>formation perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> students’ school performanceswas obta<strong>in</strong>ed from semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviews with teachers. Only one high-anger studentwas described as an above average student academically, <strong>and</strong> this <strong>in</strong>cluded a qualifier.She stated that the student “is very smart, but is sometimes lazy.” Laz<strong>in</strong>ess or lack ofmotivation was a common theme among teacher descriptions of the high-anger group.One teacher said of her student, “If he didn’t have <strong>to</strong>, he wouldn’t do anyth<strong>in</strong>g except sit<strong>and</strong> draw . . . he tries <strong>to</strong> just get by.” Another teacher described her student as “a girlthat if I let her, will just do enough <strong>to</strong> get by.”In addition <strong>to</strong> motivational problems, some high-anger students were described ashav<strong>in</strong>g social or emotional problems that <strong>in</strong>terfered with their academics. One teacherdescribed her student <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g manner: “He has made good progress (academically),but social <strong>and</strong> emotional problems pull him down.” Another student wasdescribed as “an average student that lacks confidence.” A third was described as “onewho gives up easily <strong>and</strong> is easily frustrated.”Teachers identified several participants from the high-anger group as below averageacademically. These students were described as “very low <strong>in</strong> most areas,” “nonexistentacademics,” or “has a difficult time <strong>in</strong> all subject areas.”A sharp contrast was found <strong>in</strong> teacher descriptions of the academic performanceof low-anger students. The vast majority of these students were classified as averageor above average academically. Of these, most were described as be<strong>in</strong>g highly motivatedat school. Only one of the low-anger students was described <strong>in</strong> terms <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g thatmotivation was a concern.Teachers also provided <strong>in</strong>formation on the k<strong>in</strong>ds of strategies used <strong>to</strong> motivate students<strong>in</strong> the two groups. As a group, high-anger students were more likely <strong>to</strong> be motivatedby the prospect of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g material rewards (i.e., extr<strong>in</strong>sic fac<strong>to</strong>rs). Low-angerstudents, although still responsive <strong>to</strong> external rewards, were considerably more likely <strong>to</strong>be motivated by fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic <strong>to</strong> the task (i.e., sense of accomplishment, for the sakeof learn<strong>in</strong>g) than were students from the high-anger group. One high-anger student wasdescribed by his teacher as “unmotivated; noth<strong>in</strong>g seems <strong>to</strong> work at school.”These correlationsare worth not<strong>in</strong>g.These percentagesare helpful.


380 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eDISCUSSIONChange of purposeUnclearThe data don’tshow that theseare the samestudents acrossdoma<strong>in</strong>s.CausationCausationClarifiesFrequency should begiven.The <strong>in</strong>tent of this study was <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e possible risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs related <strong>to</strong> the development<strong>and</strong> expression of anger <strong>in</strong> preadolescents. The results support a conceptualization ofanger development <strong>in</strong> children based on multiple pathways across four doma<strong>in</strong>s <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dividual, school, family, <strong>and</strong> peer fac<strong>to</strong>rs. In general, it appears that cumulativepositive <strong>and</strong> negative experiences at home, <strong>in</strong> school, <strong>and</strong> with peers have a major impac<strong>to</strong>n the frequency <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tensity of anger experienced at school. This conclusion wasevidenced throughout the data where preadolescents who had negative experiences<strong>and</strong> relationships across doma<strong>in</strong>s were also more likely <strong>to</strong> experience high levels ofanger. There also appears <strong>to</strong> be an additive effect across doma<strong>in</strong>s, <strong>in</strong> which negativeexperiences with<strong>in</strong> one doma<strong>in</strong> have an impact on the relationships <strong>and</strong> experienceswith<strong>in</strong> other doma<strong>in</strong>s.One possible <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the data is that <strong>in</strong>dividual traits <strong>and</strong> dispositions arefirst shaped with<strong>in</strong> the family <strong>and</strong> home environment <strong>and</strong> later modified through <strong>in</strong>teractions<strong>and</strong> experiences <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> with peers. Through these <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>and</strong> experiences,the child develops a sense of self-worth, abilities <strong>to</strong> regulate emotions, perceptionsabout themselves as social be<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> feedback on appropriate behavioralconduct. The first environment normally experienced by a child is the home, where <strong>in</strong>itialsocialization <strong>and</strong> development takes place. In this doma<strong>in</strong>, parental support as wellas family stability was found <strong>to</strong> be related <strong>to</strong> level of anger at school. Preadolescents whohad little parental support were at risk for experienc<strong>in</strong>g chronic anger. Further f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong>dicate that hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions at home is likely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencesubsequent relations outside the home both <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> with peers.In addition <strong>to</strong> family fac<strong>to</strong>rs, school was also an important contribu<strong>to</strong>r <strong>to</strong> students’overall levels of anger. Participants <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group had lower academic GPAs,nonacademic GPAs, <strong>and</strong> achievement scores as measured by the SAT than did participants<strong>in</strong> the low-anger group. Zagar, Arbit, Hughes, Busell, <strong>and</strong> Busch (1989) discoveredsimilar patterns <strong>in</strong> achievement test scores among a sample of juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quents. Thehigh-anger participants were also more likely <strong>to</strong> have low levels of motivation, poorwork habits, <strong>and</strong> negative attitudes <strong>to</strong>wards school. Overall, the results from this studysupport the view that low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g preadolescents are more at risk for anger-relatedproblems than their higher achiev<strong>in</strong>g peers.Social skills also appear <strong>to</strong> play an important role <strong>in</strong> chronic anger at school. Preadolescentswho have poor social skills <strong>and</strong> who have difficulty mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> keep<strong>in</strong>g friendsmanifest more anger problems. Further, it was <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> note that angry students tend<strong>to</strong> associate themselves more often with angry peers. In this study, conflict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formationwas obta<strong>in</strong>ed from the participant <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>in</strong>terviews regard<strong>in</strong>g friendship. The majorityof students <strong>in</strong> both the high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups felt that they had friends <strong>and</strong>about equal numbers from both groups thought that they had either enough friends orwished they had more friends. The teachers, however, had a somewhat different view oftheir students’ social relationships. The teachers described all of the low-anger students ashav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships with peers while only half of the high-anger participantswere described as hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships with peers. The parents of both groupsgenerally saw their children as hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships with other children. Therelated <strong>to</strong>pic of teas<strong>in</strong>g was identified as a common problem for preadolescents withchronic anger as they saw themselves as more frequent victims of teas<strong>in</strong>g than did the lowangerchildren. Further, high-anger children were more likely <strong>to</strong> be teased based on somelack of ability than were low-anger children.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 381IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL COUNSELORSResults of this study suggest there are strong patterns with<strong>in</strong> each of the four lifedoma<strong>in</strong>s associated with chronically angry preadolescent youth. This pattern of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rsmay help <strong>in</strong> the assessment <strong>and</strong> identification of students experienc<strong>in</strong>g chronicanger <strong>and</strong> its associated impact on future health <strong>and</strong> development (Williams & Williams,1995). This <strong>in</strong>formation is especially important given the body of research l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g anger<strong>to</strong> such diverse negative consequences as coronary heart disease (Seigel, 1992), cancer,depression, family violence (Smith & Furlong, 1994; Smith & Christensen, 1992), <strong>and</strong> theuse of drugs <strong>and</strong> alcohol (Lochman, 1992; Scherwitz & Rugilies, 1992). Efforts, therefore,<strong>to</strong> assess, identify, treat, <strong>and</strong> prevent chronic anger <strong>and</strong> the negative consequencesassociated with it must beg<strong>in</strong> as early as possible.The rationale for conduct<strong>in</strong>g this study with fifth <strong>and</strong> sixth grade students was thatthey are about <strong>to</strong> enter a turbulent stage <strong>in</strong> their lives. Adolescence is a time when emotionssuch as anger can become heightened. Thus, it is important <strong>to</strong> identify studentsmost at risk for chronic anger <strong>and</strong> teach them the skills they need <strong>to</strong> deal with theiranger before its consequences become more severe.Several important risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs associated with chronically angry students are discussed<strong>to</strong> help <strong>in</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g students need<strong>in</strong>g anger management skill tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>tervention.These student groups <strong>in</strong>clude:Prior researchJustification• Preadolescent youth who are not gett<strong>in</strong>g sufficient emotional or material supportat home. This group may <strong>in</strong>clude children from divorced or separated parentsor those who live <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-parent households.• Preadolescent youth who for various reasons (e.g., ability, motivation) dopoorly <strong>in</strong> school.• Students who are identified by their teachers as hav<strong>in</strong>g difficulty mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>keep<strong>in</strong>g friends, who are constant victims of teas<strong>in</strong>g, or who have poor socialskills. Teachers should play an active role <strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g those studentswho are commonly left out or who are subjected <strong>to</strong> ridicule from theirpeers.• Preadolescent youth with low self-worth or low self-esteem. Both teachers <strong>and</strong>parents may play a crucial role <strong>in</strong> assist<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong> acquire new skills for boost<strong>in</strong>gself-esteem.• Students who self-select themselves as hav<strong>in</strong>g a problem with anger. Preadolescentyouth <strong>in</strong> this study appeared capable of underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g their difficultieswith anger. This may be an especially important strategy, because many preadolescents,especially females, may express anger <strong>in</strong> more <strong>in</strong>direct ways not readilyobservable by others.School counselors <strong>and</strong> other mental health professionals function<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> schoolsett<strong>in</strong>gs may want <strong>to</strong> consider some of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from this study <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g angermanagement programs for youth. These <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g:1. Include family members <strong>in</strong> anger management programs. It appears that thehome life <strong>and</strong> family structure of students are strongly associated with anger.Thus, programs should be designed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude parents <strong>and</strong> other family membersas <strong>in</strong>tegral parts of the <strong>in</strong>tervention.2. Provide anger management or emotional control programs as part of prenatalor early childhood parent<strong>in</strong>g programs so that parents will have the skills <strong>and</strong>strategies necessary <strong>to</strong> act as emotional coaches for their children.


382 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e3. Include anger prevention <strong>and</strong>/or emotional regulation components as part ofcounsel<strong>in</strong>g for <strong>in</strong>dividuals or groups who are at risk for chronic anger. For example,a student tak<strong>in</strong>g part <strong>in</strong> a school counsel<strong>in</strong>g group for students who arego<strong>in</strong>g through divorce will most likely benefit from a comprehensive angermanagement program. In addition, a student identified as at risk for dropp<strong>in</strong>gout of school <strong>in</strong> the future because of academic failure may benefit from a programwhich teaches anger management skills.4. All children could potentially benefit from anger management or emotionalregulation programs that teach positive social skills <strong>and</strong> methods for build<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g friendships with peers. With these skills, children could helpeach other deal with anger problems <strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> turn, become part of an angrychild’s social support system. Schools could also benefit from school-wide angermanagement programs aimed at develop<strong>in</strong>g the skills of teachers for identify<strong>in</strong>g,underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g with students who may have trouble controll<strong>in</strong>gtheir anger. By learn<strong>in</strong>g proactive strategies, they could possibly preventmany angry <strong>and</strong> aggressive outbreaks <strong>and</strong> also learn <strong>to</strong> help students control<strong>and</strong> deal with their anger.LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY AND FUTURE DIRECTIONSLittle evidenceprovidedWe agreeThreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidityThis study was not able <strong>to</strong> capture possible genetic, biological, neurological, <strong>and</strong> manyearly developmental characteristics, which may play an important role <strong>in</strong> the developmen<strong>to</strong>f chronic anger. Future studies may want <strong>to</strong> consider <strong>in</strong>tergenerational family patternsrelated <strong>to</strong> chronic levels of anger <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the contributions of both genetic<strong>and</strong> environmental fac<strong>to</strong>rs. There was some support for the notion that chronic anger<strong>and</strong> hostility tend <strong>to</strong> be present <strong>in</strong> both children <strong>and</strong> adults with<strong>in</strong> the same family. Forexample, the <strong>in</strong>terviewer comments about a father of one of the high-anger students<strong>in</strong>dicated that the father was “pretty hostile” dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terview. He refused <strong>to</strong> answerseveral of the questions <strong>and</strong> made comments dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terview such as “this is tak<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>o much time,” or “I’ll give you a few more m<strong>in</strong>utes.”Further, it was found that preadolescent males were more likely <strong>to</strong> be identified aschronically angry than were females. Additional research is needed <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whythis appears <strong>to</strong> be true. The question of whether males are more genetically predisposed<strong>to</strong> anger or whether they are just socialized <strong>to</strong> be more angry is an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g one.Achenbach (1982) found some developmental stability <strong>in</strong> the aggressive behavior displayedby males, but did not f<strong>in</strong>d this same stability <strong>in</strong> females. Achenbach attributedsome of the aggressive behavior displayed by males <strong>to</strong> their more muscular body type.This muscular structure makes males more effective users of aggression <strong>and</strong> thus, theyf<strong>in</strong>d it more reward<strong>in</strong>g. Future studies may want <strong>to</strong> control for gender by select<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>compar<strong>in</strong>g high-anger males <strong>and</strong> females with low-anger students of the same sex.An additional limitation of this study was that students, teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents mayhave been reluctant <strong>to</strong> disclose <strong>in</strong>formation that was embarrass<strong>in</strong>g or negative <strong>in</strong> nature.In two of the <strong>in</strong>terviews with high-anger students, <strong>in</strong>formation was given which had <strong>to</strong> bereported <strong>to</strong> school personnel as possible <strong>in</strong>stances of child abuse. The parents of one ofthese students asked that her child discont<strong>in</strong>ue participation <strong>in</strong> the study. Abuse, whichwas identified by Lewis (Lewis, Lovely, Yeager, & Fem<strong>in</strong>a, 1989) as one of the most importantcontribu<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> the development of violent behavior, was not likely <strong>to</strong> be admitted <strong>to</strong>dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terviews with either parents or children. In fact, dur<strong>in</strong>g the sample selectionprocess, some of the parents may have refused <strong>to</strong> give consent for their child <strong>to</strong> participatedue <strong>to</strong> a fear that their child may expose negative <strong>in</strong>formation about the family.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 383A conclud<strong>in</strong>g thought: Consistent with the theme of early <strong>in</strong>tervention, a logicalnext step <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs associated with chronic anger <strong>and</strong> howit may develop <strong>in</strong> children would be <strong>to</strong> conduct similar studies with younger cohorts ofchildren. Such studies could use a similar research design <strong>and</strong> could be conducted no<strong>to</strong>nly <strong>in</strong> the earlier grades of elementary school but also <strong>in</strong> preschool <strong>and</strong> Head Startprograms. A further <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g next step would be <strong>to</strong> follow some of the students whoparticipated <strong>in</strong> this study <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the future <strong>to</strong> track the course <strong>and</strong> consequences of anger<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> adolescence <strong>and</strong> adulthood.ReferencesAchenbach, T. (1982). Developmental psychopathology.New York: John Wiley.Deffenbacher, J. L., Lynch, R. S., Oett<strong>in</strong>g, E. R., & Kemper,C. C. (1996). Anger reduction <strong>in</strong> early adolescents.Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 43: 149–157.Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure <strong>and</strong> function of reactive<strong>and</strong> proactive aggression. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rub<strong>in</strong>s(Eds.), Development <strong>and</strong> treatment of childhoodaggression (pp. 201–218). Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum.Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M.,& Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality <strong>and</strong>regulation <strong>in</strong> children’s social function<strong>in</strong>g: A longitud<strong>in</strong>alstudy. Child Development, 66: 1360–1384.Elliot, D. S., Huiz<strong>in</strong>ga, D., & Age<strong>to</strong>n, S. S. (1985). Expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdel<strong>in</strong>quency <strong>and</strong> drug use. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Greenberg, M. T., Speltz, M. L., & DeKlyen, M. (1993).The role of attachment <strong>in</strong> the early developmen<strong>to</strong>f disruptive behavior problems. Development <strong>and</strong>Psychopathology, 3: 413–430.Harter, S. (1985). Self-perception profile for children. Denver,CO: University of Denver.Kazd<strong>in</strong>, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior <strong>in</strong>children: Current status <strong>and</strong> future directions.Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 102: 187–203.Larson, J. D. (1994). Violence prevention <strong>in</strong> the schools: Areview of selected programs <strong>and</strong> procedures. SchoolPsychology Review, 23: 151–164.Lewis, D. D., Lovely, R., Yeager, C., & Fem<strong>in</strong>a, D. D. (1989).Toward a theory of the genesis of violence: A follow-upstudy of del<strong>in</strong>quents. Journal of the American Academyof Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry, 28: 431–436.Liebsohn, M. T., Oett<strong>in</strong>g, E. R., & Deffenbacher, J. L. (1994).Effects of trait anger on alcohol consumption <strong>and</strong>consequences. Journal of Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent SubstanceAbuse, 3: 17–32.Lochman, J. E. (1992). Cognitive-behavioral <strong>in</strong>tervention withaggressive boys: Three-year follow-up <strong>and</strong> preventativeeffects. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology,60: 426–432.Lochman, J. E., White, K. J., & Wayl<strong>and</strong>, K. K. (1991).Cognitive-behavioral assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment withaggressive children. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Child <strong>and</strong>adolescent therapy: Cognitive-behavioral procedures(pp. 25–65). New York: Guilford.Olweus, D. (1979). Stability of aggressive reaction patterns<strong>in</strong> males: A review. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 86: 852–875.Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene,OR: Castalia.Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989).A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior.American Psychologist, 2: 329–335.Reid, J. B., & Patterson, G. R. (1991). Early prevention <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>tervention with conduct problems: A social <strong>in</strong>teractionalmodel for the <strong>in</strong>tegration of research <strong>and</strong> practice. InG. S<strong>to</strong>ner, M. R. Sch<strong>in</strong>n, & H. M. Walker (Eds.),Interventions for achievement <strong>and</strong> behavior problems(pp. 725–739). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: National Associationof School Psychologists.Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). BehaviorAssessment System for Children. Circle P<strong>in</strong>es, MN:American Guidance Service.Scherwitz, L., & Rugilies, R. (1992). Life-style <strong>and</strong> hostility.In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Hostility, cop<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> health(pp. 77–98). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.Siegel, J. M. (1992). Anger <strong>and</strong> cardiovascular health. InH. S. Friedman (Ed.), Hostility, cop<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> health(pp. 49–64). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.Smith, D.C., Adelman, H. S., Nelson, P., Taylor, L., &Phares, V. (1987). Perceived control at school <strong>and</strong>problem behavior <strong>and</strong> attitudes. Journal of SchoolPsychology, 25: 167–176.Smith, D. C. & Furlong, M. J. (1994). Correlates of anger,hostility, <strong>and</strong> aggression <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. In


384 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eM. J. Furlong & D. C. Smith (Eds.), Anger, hostility, <strong>and</strong>aggression: Assessment, prevention, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terventionstrategies for youth (pp. 15–38). New York: John Wiley.Smith, D. C., Furlong, M. E., Bates, M., & Laughl<strong>in</strong>, J. (1998).Development of the Multidimensional School AngerInven<strong>to</strong>ry for Males. Psychology <strong>in</strong> the Schools, 35: 1–15.Smith, D. C., Larson, J. D., DeBaryshe, B., & Salzman, M.(2000). Anger management for youth: What works<strong>and</strong> for whom? In D. S. S<strong>and</strong>hu & C. B. Aspy (Eds.),Violence <strong>in</strong> American schools: A practical guide forcounselors (pp. 217–230). Alex<strong>and</strong>ria, VA: AmericanCounsel<strong>in</strong>g Association.Smith, T. W., & Christensen, A. J. (1992). Hostility, health,<strong>and</strong> social contexts. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Hostility,cop<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> health (pp. 33–48). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC:American Psychological Association.Tolan, P. H., Guerra, N. G., & Kendall, P. C. (1995). Adevelopmental-ecological perspective on antisocialbehavior <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents: Toward a unifiedrisk <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention framework. Journal of Consult<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 63: 579–584.Williams, R. B., & Williams, V. (1995). Anger kills. New York:Times Books.Zagar, R., Arbit, J., Hughes, J. R., Busell, R. E., & Busch, K.(1989). Developmental <strong>and</strong> disruptive behavior disordersamong del<strong>in</strong>quents. Journal of the American Academy ofChild <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry, 28: 437–440.Analysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is stated near the end of the <strong>in</strong>troduction asaddress<strong>in</strong>g the need “<strong>to</strong> provide a more detailed pictureboth of the fac<strong>to</strong>rs promot<strong>in</strong>g high levels of anger <strong>and</strong>hostility <strong>in</strong> youth <strong>and</strong> the impact of anger <strong>and</strong> hostilityupon social, behavioral, <strong>and</strong> academic function<strong>in</strong>g.”Extensive justification is provided on grounds of practicalnecessity <strong>and</strong> prior research. What is not clear <strong>to</strong> usis how the present study was <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> add <strong>to</strong> the bodyof prior knowledge, particularly <strong>in</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>gcauses of anger from consequences.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems of confidentiality,risk, or deception.DEFINITIONSAlthough def<strong>in</strong>ition of all the variables <strong>in</strong> this studymight seem excessive, we th<strong>in</strong>k some should have beenprovided, especially for those emphasized <strong>in</strong> outcomes,for example, “family support,” “social skills,” “positiverelationships with peers,” <strong>and</strong> “low level of motivation.”Such terms may seem clear, but their ambiguity isillustrated by the “typical descriptions” for “supportivefamilies” that are given as “extremely supportive, a lo<strong>to</strong>f parental <strong>in</strong>volvement” (<strong>in</strong> what?) <strong>and</strong> “excellentfamily, parents are very <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> school, very giv<strong>in</strong>gpeople, encourag<strong>in</strong>g.” These suggest that the term <strong>in</strong>cludesmore than encouragement <strong>and</strong> availability <strong>to</strong>their children. The primary term, anger, is operationallydef<strong>in</strong>ed as “teacher nom<strong>in</strong>ation of students who did ordid not display frequent emotional outbursts related <strong>to</strong>feel<strong>in</strong>gs of anger; demonstrated a consistent cynical,hostile, or angry attitude; or were <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> frequent<strong>in</strong>cidents of fight<strong>in</strong>g or act<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>in</strong> an angry, aggressivemanner.” This def<strong>in</strong>ition suggests that angry is implicitlydef<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms of behavior, not feel<strong>in</strong>gs.PRIOR RESEARCHThe authors cite numerous pert<strong>in</strong>ent references <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>troduction (<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the implications) perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> bothcauses <strong>and</strong> effects of anger <strong>in</strong> students. These are appropriatelyorganized <strong>and</strong> summarized.HYPOTHESESNone are stated but many are implied (i.e., that studentanger is related <strong>to</strong> lack of parental support, parent maritalstatus, number of friends, extent of teas<strong>in</strong>g, generalpeer relations, GPA, SAT scores, attitude <strong>and</strong> motivation<strong>to</strong>ward school, <strong>and</strong> teacher perception of academicperformance). Many of these appear <strong>to</strong> be directional.Other themes emerged from the qualitative analysis of<strong>in</strong>terview data.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 385SAMPLEThe sample is a convenience sample of fifth- <strong>and</strong> sixthgradersdrawn from 12 elementary schools <strong>in</strong> Honolulu.Consequently, generalization beyond these schools is notjustified methodologically, nor is argumentation providedfor such generalization. Some description is provided ofthe schools as serv<strong>in</strong>g an economically <strong>and</strong> ethnically diversepopulation.Theethnicdistributionofstudentsispredom<strong>in</strong>antlyAsian-American(over 50 percent).The implicationsof this are not discussed. The identification ofstudents for high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups is well described.INSTRUMENTATIONFormal <strong>in</strong>struments given <strong>to</strong> students <strong>and</strong> teachers arewell described. We th<strong>in</strong>k, however, that use of the term“established” <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> the validity of the SPP<strong>and</strong> MSAI <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency of the MSAI isunfortunate <strong>in</strong> light of coefficients as low as .71 <strong>and</strong> .39<strong>and</strong> without description of the groups <strong>in</strong>volved. Moredescription of the semistructured <strong>in</strong>terview questions isneeded, partly <strong>to</strong> assess how much <strong>in</strong>terviewer biasmight have affected responses. Scor<strong>in</strong>g agreement onthe Likert scales should be reported s<strong>in</strong>ce they are,apparently, those used <strong>in</strong> quantitative analyses.PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYProcedures are adequately described. Internal validityis always a problem <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies.This study is a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of types 1 <strong>and</strong> 2(pages 363–364). The high- <strong>and</strong> low-anger groups maydiffer <strong>in</strong> many ways other than level of anger. Two suchvariables are gender <strong>and</strong> ethnicity. The authors, <strong>in</strong> theirlimitations section, suggest controll<strong>in</strong>g gender <strong>in</strong> futurestudies; it seems <strong>to</strong> us there was good reason <strong>to</strong> havecontrolled it (by match<strong>in</strong>g or same-sex selection) <strong>in</strong> thisstudy. Ethnicity was not controlled, but the groups aresimilar, although not identical. Many of the variables <strong>in</strong>this study are subject characteristics, but none were controlled.A mortality threat is likely, because someparents, presumably <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group, refusedpermission. A data collec<strong>to</strong>r threat is possible because itis unclear whether <strong>in</strong>terviewers were balanced acrossgroups. Data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias is possible, especially withthe open-ended questions; presumably this was addressed<strong>in</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. An <strong>in</strong>strumentation threat seemslikely, as is discussed <strong>in</strong> the limitations section. Thoughless likely, <strong>in</strong>strument decay <strong>and</strong> location threats arepossible. Implementation, attitude, <strong>and</strong> regressionthreats do not apply.DATA ANALYSISAlthough this is a causal-comparative study, the authorschose <strong>to</strong> give much attention <strong>to</strong> the relationships betweenthe anger scores on the self-report <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry(MSAI) <strong>and</strong> other student characteristics <strong>and</strong> used correlationcoefficients as the primary means of data analysis.This is legitimate, but we th<strong>in</strong>k comparisons of thetwo anger groups (identified by a comb<strong>in</strong>ation ofteacher nom<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>and</strong> MSAI scores) us<strong>in</strong>g means,st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations, <strong>and</strong> frequency polygons wouldhave been more mean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>and</strong> consistent with the effortexpended <strong>to</strong> identity two dist<strong>in</strong>ct groups.RESULTSResults are appropriately presented. Too much is madeof low values of r, even though they are said <strong>to</strong> be “statisticallysignificant.” Because the study clearly focuseson practical, as dist<strong>in</strong>ct from theoretical, uses, we wouldpay little attention <strong>to</strong> correlations below .50. We wouldhave emphasized the follow<strong>in</strong>g correlations with degreeof anger: .53 with parental support; .64 with (positive)social relationships; 57 with GPA; .56 with (positive)work habits; .71 with (positive) attitude; <strong>and</strong>,because it is consistent with GPA, .47 with SATscores. Qualitative differences between anger groupsthat seem important are that the high-anger group <strong>in</strong>dicatedmuch greater desire <strong>to</strong> control anger (all versusone), more negative self-descriptions (frequencies wouldbe helpful here <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> subsequent comparisons), morefamily problems, more teas<strong>in</strong>g at school, <strong>and</strong> less motivation<strong>in</strong> terms of fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic <strong>to</strong> school tasks.DISCUSSION/INTERPRETATIONThis section is generally consistent with the results <strong>and</strong>attempts <strong>to</strong> provide an <strong>in</strong>tegration. It highlights somesuggestive f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, for example, that parents’ <strong>and</strong>teachers’ perceptions of student social relations differedmarkedly <strong>in</strong> the high-anger group <strong>and</strong> that for them,teas<strong>in</strong>g may be more related <strong>to</strong> lack of academic ability.The statement that effects are cumulative across doma<strong>in</strong>s,while it may well be true, is not justified from thedata provided <strong>in</strong> the report. The reason is that data arepresented separately for each doma<strong>in</strong> with no data on <strong>in</strong>dividualsacross doma<strong>in</strong>s. The authors commendablyaddress several limitations but omit others. We are concernedwith the implications regard<strong>in</strong>g causality. Inoffer<strong>in</strong>g one possible <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the data, <strong>in</strong>appropriatestatements are made (i.e., the data do not “<strong>in</strong>dicatethat hav<strong>in</strong>g positive relationships <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractions at


386 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ehome is likely <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence subsequent relations outsidethe home both <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> with peers.” Nor do resultsshow that “school was also an important contribu<strong>to</strong>r <strong>to</strong>students’ overall levels of anger.” While these are plausible<strong>in</strong>terpretations, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs can just as legitimatelybe used <strong>to</strong> support the reverse (e.g., that student anger isan important contribu<strong>to</strong>r <strong>to</strong> poor school achievement<strong>and</strong> attitude <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> poor parental support or that bothvariables are caused by other variables.Causal-comparative study f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs cannot determ<strong>in</strong>ethe nature of causality <strong>and</strong> are therefore poorly suited <strong>to</strong>dist<strong>in</strong>guish<strong>in</strong>g causes of anger from effects. In addition<strong>to</strong> the problem of causality, the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity<strong>in</strong> this study are severe. As one example, because theanger groups differ dramatically <strong>in</strong> gender composition,it is important <strong>to</strong> assess whether gender is related <strong>to</strong>other outcome variables <strong>and</strong> could, therefore, accountfor the relationships (see pages 367–368). Recogniz<strong>in</strong>gthat the sample is atypical of U.S. schools <strong>in</strong> general,<strong>and</strong> of our experience, we nevertheless th<strong>in</strong>k it likelythat gender differences could account for a large portionof the relationships found.The problems with causality are avoided by the redef<strong>in</strong>itionof purpose (<strong>in</strong> the discussion section) as identificationof risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs related <strong>to</strong> the development <strong>and</strong>expression of anger <strong>in</strong> preadolescents. As such, risk fac<strong>to</strong>rswould <strong>in</strong>clude low family support, poor academicperformance, teacher assessment as hav<strong>in</strong>g poor socialrelationships, low <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation for school, <strong>and</strong>so forth. The danger here is one of overgeneralization.In a society prone <strong>to</strong> oversimplification, it is easy <strong>to</strong>forget that, with correlations even as high as .71, many(perhaps most) students identified by one or a comb<strong>in</strong>ationof these fac<strong>to</strong>rs will be misidentified as angry oranger-prone. Students so identified should get help <strong>in</strong>cop<strong>in</strong>g with such problems if their existence is verified.We agree that students who identify themselves as need<strong>in</strong>ghelp with anger should get it.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTHE NATURE OF CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE RESEARCHCausal-comparative research, like correlational research, seeks <strong>to</strong> identify associationsamong variables.Causal-comparative research attempts <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the cause or consequences ofdifferences that already exist between or among groups of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.The basic causal-comparative approach is <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with a noted difference betweentwo groups <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> look for possible causes for, or consequences of, thisdifference.There are three types of causal-comparative research (exploration of effects, explorationof causes, <strong>and</strong> exploration of consequences), which differ <strong>in</strong> their purposes <strong>and</strong>structure.When an experiment would take a considerable length of time <strong>and</strong> be quite costly <strong>to</strong>conduct, a causal-comparative study is sometimes used as an alternative.As <strong>in</strong> correlational studies, relationships can be identified <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparativestudy, but causation cannot be fully established.


CHAPTER 16 Causal-Comparative <strong>Research</strong> 387CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE VERSUS CORRELATIONAL RESEARCHThe basic similarity between causal-comparative <strong>and</strong> correlational studies is thatboth seek <strong>to</strong> explore relationships among variables. When relationships are identifiedthrough causal-comparative research (or <strong>in</strong> correlational research), they often arestudied at a later time by means of experimental research.CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE VERSUS EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCHIn experimental research, the group membership variable is manipulated; <strong>in</strong> causalcomparativeresearch, the group differences already exist.STEPS IN CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE RESEARCHThe first step <strong>in</strong> formulat<strong>in</strong>g a problem <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research is usually <strong>to</strong>identify <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e the particular phenomena of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> consider possiblecauses for, or consequences of, these phenomena.The most important task <strong>in</strong> select<strong>in</strong>g a sample for a causal-comparative study is <strong>to</strong>def<strong>in</strong>e carefully the characteristic <strong>to</strong> be studied <strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> select groups that differ <strong>in</strong>this characteristic.There are no limits <strong>to</strong> the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>struments that can be used <strong>in</strong> a causalcomparativestudy.The basic causal-comparative design <strong>in</strong>volves select<strong>in</strong>g two groups that differ on a particularvariable of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> then compar<strong>in</strong>g them on another variable or variables.THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITYIN CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE RESEARCHTwo weaknesses <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research are lack of r<strong>and</strong>omization <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>ability <strong>to</strong> manipulate an <strong>in</strong>dependent variable.A major threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of a causal-comparative study is the possibilityof a subject selection bias. The chief procedures that a researcher can use <strong>to</strong> reducethis threat <strong>in</strong>clude match<strong>in</strong>g subjects on a related variable, creat<strong>in</strong>g homogeneoussubgroups, <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g the technique of statistical match<strong>in</strong>g.Other threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies <strong>in</strong>clude location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation,<strong>and</strong> loss of subjects. In addition, type 3 studies are subject <strong>to</strong> implementation,his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, attitude of subjects, regression, <strong>and</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g threats.DATA ANALYSIS IN CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE STUDIESThe first step <strong>in</strong> a data analysis of a causal-comparative study is <strong>to</strong> constructfrequency polygons.Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations are usually calculated if the variables <strong>in</strong>volved arequantitative.The most commonly used test <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies is a t-test for differencesbetween means.Analysis of covariance is particularly useful <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative studies.The results of causal-comparative studies should always be <strong>in</strong>terpreted with caution,because they do not prove cause <strong>and</strong> effect.


388 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN CATEGORICAL VARIABLESBoth crossbreak tables <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficients can be used <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate possibleassociations between categorical variables, although predictions from crossbreaktables are not precise. Fortunately, there are relatively few questions of <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> education that <strong>in</strong>volve two categorical variables.For Discussion1. Suppose a researcher was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out what fac<strong>to</strong>rs cause del<strong>in</strong>quentbehavior <strong>in</strong> teenagers. What might be a suitable comparison group for the researcher<strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g this question?2. Could observation be used <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparative study? If so, how?3. When, if ever, might a researcher prefer <strong>to</strong> conduct a causal-comparative studyrather than an experimental study? Suggest an example.4. What sorts of questions might lend themselves better <strong>to</strong> causal-comparative researchthan <strong>to</strong> experimental research? Why?5. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be easier <strong>to</strong> conduct, causal-comparative or experimentalresearch? Why?6. Is r<strong>and</strong>om assignment possible <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research? What about r<strong>and</strong>omselection? Expla<strong>in</strong>.7. Suppose a researcher was <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of team teach<strong>in</strong>g on student attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward his<strong>to</strong>ry. Could such a <strong>to</strong>pic be studied by means of causal-comparativeresearch? If so, how?8. What sorts of variables might it be wise for a researcher <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about controll<strong>in</strong>g for<strong>in</strong> a causal-comparative study? What sorts of variables, if any, might be irrelevant?9. Might a researcher ever study the same variables <strong>in</strong> an experimental study that he orshe studied <strong>in</strong> a causal-comparative study? If so, why?10. We state <strong>in</strong> the text that, <strong>in</strong> general, quantitative variables should not be collapsed<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categorical variables because (a) the decision <strong>to</strong> do so is almost always an arbitraryone <strong>and</strong> (b) <strong>to</strong>o much <strong>in</strong>formation is lost by do<strong>in</strong>g so. Can you suggest anyquantitative variables that, for these reasons, should not be collapsed <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categoricalvariables? Can you suggest some quantitative variables that could justifiably betreated as categorical variables?11. Suppose a researcher reports a higher <strong>in</strong>cidence of childhood sexual abuse <strong>in</strong> adultwomen who have eat<strong>in</strong>g disorders than <strong>in</strong> a comparison group of women withouteat<strong>in</strong>g disorders. Which variable is more likely <strong>to</strong> be the cause of the other? Wha<strong>to</strong>ther variables could be alternative or contribut<strong>in</strong>g causes?12. Are there any research questions that cannot be studied by the causal-comparativemethod?13. A professor at a private women’s college wishes <strong>to</strong> assess the degree of alienationpresent <strong>in</strong> undergraduates as compared <strong>to</strong> graduate students at her <strong>in</strong>stitution. Shewill use an <strong>in</strong>strument that she has developed.a. Which method, causal-comparative or experimental, would you recommend sheuse <strong>in</strong> her <strong>in</strong>quiry? Why?b. Would the fact that the researcher plans <strong>to</strong> use an <strong>in</strong>strument that she herselfdeveloped make any difference <strong>in</strong> your recommendation?Note1. The <strong>in</strong>terested reader is referred <strong>to</strong> J. D. Elashoff (1969). Analysis of covariance: A delicate <strong>in</strong>strument.American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 6: 383–399.


Survey <strong>Research</strong>17"30 percent of me says 'yes,'30 percent says 'no,' <strong>and</strong>40 percent of me is justpla<strong>in</strong> confused."OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what a survey is.Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between a closedended<strong>and</strong> an open-ended question.Name three types of surveys conducted<strong>in</strong> educational research.Expla<strong>in</strong> why nonresponse is a problem <strong>in</strong>Expla<strong>in</strong> the purpose of surveys.survey research <strong>and</strong> name two ways <strong>to</strong>Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between a crosssectional<strong>and</strong> a longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey.Name two threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument validityimprove the rate of response <strong>in</strong> surveys.Describe how survey research differs from that can affect survey results. Expla<strong>in</strong>other types of research.how such threats can be controlled.Describe briefly how mail surveys,Describe possible threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternaltelephone surveys, <strong>and</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-facevalidity <strong>in</strong> survey research.<strong>in</strong>terviews differ <strong>and</strong> state two advantages Recognize an example of survey research<strong>and</strong> disadvantages of each type.when you come across it <strong>in</strong> theDescribe the most common pitfalls <strong>in</strong> educational literature.develop<strong>in</strong>g survey questions.What Is a Survey?Why Are SurveysConducted?Types of SurveysCross-sectional SurveysLongitud<strong>in</strong>al SurveysSurvey <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong>Correlational <strong>Research</strong>Steps <strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the ProblemIdentify<strong>in</strong>g the TargetPopulationChoos<strong>in</strong>g the Modeof Data CollectionSelect<strong>in</strong>g the SamplePrepar<strong>in</strong>g the InstrumentPrepar<strong>in</strong>g the Cover LetterTra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g InterviewersUs<strong>in</strong>g an Interview<strong>to</strong> Measure AbilityNonresponseTotal NonresponseItem NonresponseProblems <strong>in</strong> theInstrumentation Process<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>Data Analysis<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>An Example of Survey<strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisDiscussion/Interpretation


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Tak<strong>in</strong>g a CensusAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 17.1: Survey <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 17.2: Types of SurveysActivity 17.3: Open- vs. Closed-Ended QuestionsActivity 17.4: Conduct a SurveyTom Mart<strong>in</strong>ez, the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal of Grover Creek High School, is meet<strong>in</strong>g with his vice pr<strong>in</strong>cipal, Jesse Sullivan.“I wish I knew how more of the faculty felt about this after-school detention program we’ve implementedthis year,” says Tom. “Jose Alcazar s<strong>to</strong>pped me <strong>in</strong> the hall yesterday <strong>to</strong> say he th<strong>in</strong>ks it’s not work<strong>in</strong>g.”“Why?”“He says many of the faculty th<strong>in</strong>k it doesn’t do any good, so they don’t even bother <strong>to</strong> send any students there.”“Really?” answers Jesse. “I’ve heard just the opposite. Just <strong>to</strong>day, at lunch, Becky <strong>and</strong> Felicia were say<strong>in</strong>g theyth<strong>in</strong>k it’s great!”“Hmm, that’s <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g. It seems we need more data.”A survey is an appropriate way for Tom <strong>and</strong> Jesse <strong>to</strong> get such data. <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> conduct a survey is what this chapteris about.What Is a Survey?<strong>Research</strong>ers are often <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the op<strong>in</strong>ions of a largegroup of people about a particular <strong>to</strong>pic or issue. They aska number of questions, all related <strong>to</strong> the issue, <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d answers.For example, imag<strong>in</strong>e that the chairperson of thecounsel<strong>in</strong>g department at a large university is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how students who are seek<strong>in</strong>g a master’s degreefeel about the program. She decides <strong>to</strong> conduct a survey<strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out. She selects a sample of 50 students fromamong those currently enrolled <strong>in</strong> the master’s degree program<strong>and</strong> constructs questions designed <strong>to</strong> elicit their attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward the program. She adm<strong>in</strong>isters the questions<strong>to</strong> each of the 50 students <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewsover a two-week period. The responses given byeach student <strong>in</strong> the sample are coded <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized categoriesfor purposes of analysis, <strong>and</strong> these st<strong>and</strong>ardizedrecords are then analyzed <strong>to</strong> provide descriptions of thestudents <strong>in</strong> the sample. The chairperson draws some conclusionsabout the op<strong>in</strong>ions of the sample, which she thengeneralizes <strong>to</strong> the population from which the sample wasselected, <strong>in</strong> this case, all of the graduate students seek<strong>in</strong>g amaster’s degree <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g from this university.The previous example illustrates the three majorcharacteristics that most surveys possess.1. Information is collected from a group of people <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> describe some aspects or characteristics(such as abilities, op<strong>in</strong>ions, attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong>/orknowledge) of the population of which that group isa part.2. The ma<strong>in</strong> way <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>formation is collectedis through ask<strong>in</strong>g questions; the answers <strong>to</strong> thesequestions by the members of the group constitute thedata of the study.3. Information is collected from a sample rather thanfrom every member of the population.Why Are Surveys Conducted?The major purpose of surveys is <strong>to</strong> describe the characteristicsof a population. In essence, what researcherswant <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out is how the members of a population distributethemselves on one or more variables (for example,age, ethnicity, religious preference, attitudes <strong>to</strong>wardschool). As <strong>in</strong> other types of research, of course, thepopulation as a whole is rarely studied. Instead, a carefullyselected sample of respondents is surveyed <strong>and</strong> adescription of the population is <strong>in</strong>ferred from what isfound out about the sample.390


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 391For example, a researcher might be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>ghow certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics (age, gender, ethnicity,political <strong>in</strong>volvement, <strong>and</strong> so on) of teachers <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ner-city high schools are distributed with<strong>in</strong> the group.The researcher would select a sample of teachers from<strong>in</strong>ner-city high schools <strong>to</strong> survey. Generally, <strong>in</strong> a descriptivesurvey such as this, researchers are not somuch concerned with why the observed distribution existsas with what the distribution is.Types of SurveysThere are two major types of surveys—a cross-sectionalsurvey <strong>and</strong> a longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey.CROSS-SECTIONAL SURVEYSA cross-sectional survey collects <strong>in</strong>formation from asample that has been drawn from a predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed population.Furthermore, the <strong>in</strong>formation is collected atjust one po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time, although the time it takes <strong>to</strong> collectall of the data may take anywhere from a day <strong>to</strong> afew weeks or more. Thus, a professor of mathematicsmight collect data from a sample of all the high schoolmathematics teachers <strong>in</strong> a particular state about their<strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> earn<strong>in</strong>g a master’s degree <strong>in</strong> mathematicsfrom his university, or another researcher might take asurvey of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of personal problems experiencedby students at 10, 13, <strong>and</strong> 16 years of age. All thesegroups could be surveyed at approximately the samepo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time.When an entire population is surveyed, it is called acensus. The prime example is the census conductedby the U.S. Bureau of the Census every 10 years,which attempts <strong>to</strong> collect data about everyone <strong>in</strong> theUnited States.LONGITUDINAL SURVEYSIn a longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong>formationis collected at different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong>study changes over time. Three longitud<strong>in</strong>al designs arecommonly employed <strong>in</strong> survey research: trend studies,cohort studies, <strong>and</strong> panel studies.In a trend study, different samples from a populationwhose members may change are surveyed at differentpo<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time. For example, a researcher mightbe <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the attitudes of high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipals<strong>to</strong>ward the use of flexible schedul<strong>in</strong>g. He would selecta sample each year from a current list<strong>in</strong>g of highschool pr<strong>in</strong>cipals throughout the state. Although thepopulation would change somewhat <strong>and</strong> the same <strong>in</strong>dividualswould not be sampled each year, if r<strong>and</strong>om selectionwere used <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> the samples, the responsesobta<strong>in</strong>ed each year could be considered representativeof the population of high school pr<strong>in</strong>cipals. The researcherwould then exam<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> compare responsesfrom year <strong>to</strong> year <strong>to</strong> see whether any trends wereapparent.Whereas a trend study samples a population whosemembers may change over time, a cohort study samplesa particular population whose members do not changeover the course of the survey. Thus, a researcher mightwant <strong>to</strong> study growth <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g effectiveness of all thefirst-year teachers who had graduated <strong>in</strong> the past yearfrom San Francisco State University. The names of all ofthese teachers would be listed, <strong>and</strong> then a different samplewould be selected from this list<strong>in</strong>g at different times.In a panel study, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, the researchersurveys the same sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals at differenttimes dur<strong>in</strong>g the course of the survey. Because the researcheris study<strong>in</strong>g the same <strong>in</strong>dividuals, she can notechanges <strong>in</strong> their characteristics or behavior <strong>and</strong> explorethe reasons for these changes. Thus, the researcher <strong>in</strong>our previous example might select a sample of lastyear’s graduates from San Francisco State Universitywho are first-year teachers <strong>and</strong> survey the same <strong>in</strong>dividualsseveral times dur<strong>in</strong>g the teach<strong>in</strong>g year. Loss of<strong>in</strong>dividuals is a frequent problem <strong>in</strong> panel studies, however,particularly if the study extends over a fairly longperiod of time.Follow<strong>in</strong>g are the titles of some published reportsof surveys that have been conducted by educationalresearchers.“The status of state his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction.” 1“Dimensions of effective school leadership: Theteacher’s perspective.” 2“Teacher perceptions of discipl<strong>in</strong>e problems <strong>in</strong> a centralVirg<strong>in</strong>ia middle school.” 3“Two thous<strong>and</strong> teachers view their profession.” 4“Grad<strong>in</strong>g problems: A matter of communication.” 5“Peers or parents: Who has the most <strong>in</strong>fluence oncannabis use?” 6“A career ladder’s effect on teacher career <strong>and</strong> workattitudes.” 7“Ethical practices of licensed professional counselors:A survey of state licens<strong>in</strong>g boards.” 8


392 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSurvey <strong>Research</strong><strong>and</strong> Correlational <strong>Research</strong>It is not uncommon for researchers <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e the relationshipof responses <strong>to</strong> one question <strong>in</strong> a survey <strong>to</strong> another,or of a score based on one set of survey questions<strong>to</strong> a score based on another set. In such <strong>in</strong>stances, thetechniques of correlational research described <strong>in</strong> Chapter15 are appropriate.Suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g the relationshipbetween attitude <strong>to</strong>ward school of highschool students <strong>and</strong> their outside-of-school <strong>in</strong>terests. Aquestionnaire conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g items deal<strong>in</strong>g with these twovariables could be prepared <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> a sampleof high school students, <strong>and</strong> then relationships couldbe determ<strong>in</strong>ed by calculat<strong>in</strong>g correlation coefficients orby prepar<strong>in</strong>g cont<strong>in</strong>gency tables. The researcher mayf<strong>in</strong>d that students who have a positive attitude <strong>to</strong>wardschool also have a lot of outside <strong>in</strong>terests, while thosewho have a negative attitude <strong>to</strong>ward school have fewoutside <strong>in</strong>terests.Steps <strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>DEFINING THE PROBLEMThe problem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated by means of a surveyshould be sufficiently <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> important <strong>to</strong> motivate<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> respond. Trivial questions usuallyget what they deserve—they’re <strong>to</strong>ssed <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the nearestwastebasket. You have probably done this yourself <strong>to</strong> asurvey questionnaire you considered unimportant orfound bor<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Research</strong>ers need <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e clearly their objectives <strong>in</strong>conduct<strong>in</strong>g a survey. Each question should relate <strong>to</strong> oneor more of the survey’s objectives. One strategy fordef<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g survey questions is <strong>to</strong> use a hierarchical approach,beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g with the broadest, most general questions<strong>and</strong> end<strong>in</strong>g with the most specific. Jaeger gives adetailed example of such a survey on the question ofwhy many public school teachers “burn out” <strong>and</strong> leavethe profession with<strong>in</strong> a few years. He suggests threegeneral fac<strong>to</strong>rs—economics, work<strong>in</strong>g conditions, <strong>and</strong>perceived social status—around which <strong>to</strong> structure possiblequestions for the survey. Here are the questions hedeveloped with regard <strong>to</strong> economic fac<strong>to</strong>rs.I. Do economic fac<strong>to</strong>rs cause teachers <strong>to</strong> leave the professionearly?A. Do teachers leave the profession early because of<strong>in</strong>adequate yearly <strong>in</strong>come?1. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir monthly <strong>in</strong>come dur<strong>in</strong>g the school year is<strong>to</strong>o small?2. Do teachers leave the profession early becausethey are not paid dur<strong>in</strong>g the summer months?3. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir salary forces them <strong>to</strong> hold a second jobdur<strong>in</strong>g the school year?4. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir lack of <strong>in</strong>come forces them <strong>to</strong> hold a differentjob dur<strong>in</strong>g the summer months?B. Do teachers leave the profession early because ofthe structure of their pay scale?1. Do teachers leave the profession early becausethe upper limit on their pay scale is <strong>to</strong>o low?2. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir rate of progress on the pay scale is <strong>to</strong>oslow?C. Do teachers leave the profession early because of<strong>in</strong>adequate fr<strong>in</strong>ge benefits?1. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir health <strong>in</strong>surance benefits are <strong>in</strong>adequate?2. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir life <strong>in</strong>surance benefits are <strong>in</strong>adequate?3. Do teachers leave the profession early becausetheir retirement benefits are <strong>in</strong>adequate? 9A hierarchical set of research questions like this canhelp researchers identify large categories of issues, suggestmore specific issues with<strong>in</strong> each category, <strong>and</strong> conceiveof possible questions. By determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether aproposed question fits the purposes of the <strong>in</strong>tended survey,researchers can elim<strong>in</strong>ate those that do not. This isimportant, s<strong>in</strong>ce the length of a survey’s questionnaireor <strong>in</strong>terview schedule is a crucial fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe survey’s success.IDENTIFYING THE TARGET POPULATIONAlmost anyth<strong>in</strong>g can be described by means of a survey.That which is studied <strong>in</strong> a survey is called the unit ofanalysis. Although typically people, units of analysiscan also be objects, clubs, companies, classrooms,schools, government agencies, <strong>and</strong> others. For example,<strong>in</strong> a survey of faculty op<strong>in</strong>ion about a new discipl<strong>in</strong>e


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 393policy recently <strong>in</strong>stituted <strong>in</strong> a particular school district,each faculty member sampled <strong>and</strong> surveyed would bethe unit of analysis. In a survey of urban school districts,the school district would be the unit of analysis.Survey data are collected from a number of <strong>in</strong>dividualunits of analysis <strong>to</strong> describe those units; these descriptionsare then summarized <strong>to</strong> describe the populationthat the units of analysis represent. In the examplegiven above, data collected from a sample of facultymembers (the unit of analysis) would be summarized <strong>to</strong>describe the population that this sample represents (allof the faculty members <strong>in</strong> that particular school district).As <strong>in</strong> other types of research, the group of persons(objects, <strong>in</strong>stitutions, <strong>and</strong> so on) that is the focus of thestudy is called the target population. To make trustworthystatements about the target population, it must bevery well def<strong>in</strong>ed. In fact, it must be so well def<strong>in</strong>ed thatit is possible <strong>to</strong> state with certa<strong>in</strong>ty whether or not aparticular unit of analysis is a member of this population.Suppose, for example, that the target population isdef<strong>in</strong>ed as “all of the faculty members <strong>in</strong> a particularschool district.” Is this def<strong>in</strong>ition sufficiently clear sothat one can state with certa<strong>in</strong>ty who is or is not a memberof this population? At first glance, you may betempted <strong>to</strong> say yes. But what about adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs whoalso teach? What about substitute teachers, or those whoteach only part-time? What about student teachers?What about counselors? Unless the target population isdef<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> sufficient detail so that it is unequivocallyclear as <strong>to</strong> who is, or is not, a member of it, any statementsmade about this population, based on a survey ofa sample of it, may be mislead<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>correct.CHOOSING THE MODEOF DATA COLLECTIONThere are four basic ways <strong>to</strong> collect data <strong>in</strong> a survey: byadm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g the survey <strong>in</strong>strument “live” <strong>to</strong> a group;by mail; by telephone; or through face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviews.Table 17.1 presents a summary of the advantages<strong>and</strong> the disadvantages of each of the four survey methods,which are discussed below.Direct Adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>to</strong> a Group. Thismethodis used whenever a researcher has access <strong>to</strong> all (or most)of the members of a particular group <strong>in</strong> one place. The <strong>in</strong>strumentis adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> all members of the group atthe same time <strong>and</strong> usually <strong>in</strong> the same place. Exampleswould <strong>in</strong>clude giv<strong>in</strong>g questionnaires <strong>to</strong> students <strong>to</strong> complete<strong>in</strong> their classrooms or workers <strong>to</strong> complete at theirjob sett<strong>in</strong>gs. The chief advantage of this approach is thehigh rate of response—often close <strong>to</strong> 100 percent (usually<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle sett<strong>in</strong>g). Other advantages <strong>in</strong>clude a generallylow cost fac<strong>to</strong>r, plus the fact that the researcher hasan opportunity <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> the study <strong>and</strong> answer any questionsthat the respondents may have before they completethe questionnaire. The chief disadvantage is that there arenot many types of surveys that can use samples of <strong>in</strong>dividualsthat are collected <strong>to</strong>gether as a group.Mail Surveys. When the data <strong>in</strong> a survey are collectedby mail, the questionnaire is sent <strong>to</strong> each <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>in</strong> the sample, with a request that it be completed<strong>and</strong> then returned by a given date. The advantages ofthis approach are that it is relatively <strong>in</strong>expensive <strong>and</strong> itcan be accomplished by the researcher alone (or withTABLE 17.1Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages of Survey Data Collection MethodsDirectAdm<strong>in</strong>istration Telephone Mail InterviewComparative cost Lowest Intermediate Intermediate HighFacilities needed? Yes No No YesRequire tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of questioner? Yes Yes No YesData-collection time Shortest Short Longer LongestResponse rate Very high Good Poorest Very highGroup adm<strong>in</strong>istration possible? Yes No No YesAllow for r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g? Possibly Yes Yes YesRequire literate sample? Yes No Yes NoPermit follow-up questions? No Yes No YesEncourage response <strong>to</strong> sensitive <strong>to</strong>pics? Somewhat Somewhat Best WeakSt<strong>and</strong>ardization of responses Easy Somewhat Easy Hardest


394 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePopulationIdealActual1,000 800 700 1,000 900R<strong>and</strong>omlyselectedsampleLessthoseresearcherunable <strong>to</strong>contactLess thoserefus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>participatePlusthosereplacedr<strong>and</strong>omlyLess thosenot respond<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> item #10Figure 17.1 Example of an Ideal Versus an Actual Telephone Sample for a Specific Questiononly a few assistants). It also allows the researcher <strong>to</strong>have access <strong>to</strong> samples that might be hard <strong>to</strong> reach <strong>in</strong>person or by telephone (such as the elderly), <strong>and</strong> it permitsthe respondents <strong>to</strong> take sufficient time <strong>to</strong> givethoughtful answers <strong>to</strong> the questions asked.The disadvantages of mail surveys are that there isless opportunity <strong>to</strong> encourage the cooperation of the respondents(through build<strong>in</strong>g rapport, for example) or <strong>to</strong>provide assistance (through answer<strong>in</strong>g their questions,clarify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structions, <strong>and</strong> so on). As a result, mail surveyshave a tendency <strong>to</strong> produce low response rates.Mail surveys also do not lend themselves well <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation from certa<strong>in</strong> types of samples (such as<strong>in</strong>dividuals who are illiterate).Telephone Surveys. In a telephone survey the researcher(or his or her assistants) asks questions of therespondents over the telephone. The advantages of telephonesurveys are they are cheaper than personal <strong>in</strong>terviews,can be conducted fairly quickly, <strong>and</strong> lend themselveseasily <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ardized question<strong>in</strong>g procedures.They also allow the researcher <strong>to</strong> assist the respondent(by clarify<strong>in</strong>g questions, ask<strong>in</strong>g follow-up questions,encourag<strong>in</strong>g hesitant respondents, <strong>and</strong> so on), permit agreater amount of follow-up (through several callbacks),<strong>and</strong> provide better coverage <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> areaswhere personal <strong>in</strong>terviewers often are reluctant <strong>to</strong> go.**Computers are be<strong>in</strong>g used more <strong>in</strong> telephone surveys. Typically, an<strong>in</strong>terviewer sits <strong>in</strong> front of a computer screen. A central computerr<strong>and</strong>omly selects a telephone number <strong>and</strong> dials it. The <strong>in</strong>terviewer,wear<strong>in</strong>g a headset, hears the respondent answer the phone. On thecomputer screen appears a typed <strong>in</strong>troduction, such as “Hello, myname is ______,” for the <strong>in</strong>terviewer <strong>to</strong> read, followed by the firstquestion. The <strong>in</strong>terviewer then types the respondent’s answer <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>the computer. The answer is immediately s<strong>to</strong>red <strong>in</strong>side the centralcomputer. The next question <strong>to</strong> be asked then appears on the screen,<strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terviewer cont<strong>in</strong>ues the question<strong>in</strong>g.The disadvantages of telephone surveys are that access<strong>to</strong> some samples (obviously, those without telephones<strong>and</strong> those whose phone numbers are unlisted) isnot possible. Telephone <strong>in</strong>terviews also prevent visualobservation of respondents <strong>and</strong> are somewhat less effective<strong>in</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation about sensitive issuesor personal questions. Generally, telephone surveys arereported <strong>to</strong> result <strong>in</strong> a 5 percent lower response rate thanthat obta<strong>in</strong>ed by personal <strong>in</strong>terviews. 10 Figure 17.1 illustratesthe difficulty sometimes encountered whenobta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a research sample by telephone.Personal Interviews. In a personal <strong>in</strong>terview, theresearcher (or tra<strong>in</strong>ed assistant) conducts a face-<strong>to</strong>-face<strong>in</strong>terview with the respondent. As a result, this methodhas many advantages. It is probably the most effectivesurvey method for enlist<strong>in</strong>g the cooperation of the respondents.Rapport can be established, questions can beclarified, unclear or <strong>in</strong>complete answers can be followedup, <strong>and</strong> so on. Face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g alsoplaces less of a burden on the read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g skillsof the respondents <strong>and</strong>, when necessary, permits spend<strong>in</strong>gmore time with respondents.The biggest disadvantage of face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewsis that they are more costly than direct, mail, or telephonesurveys. They also require a tra<strong>in</strong>ed staff of <strong>in</strong>terviewers,with all that implies <strong>in</strong> terms of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g costs<strong>and</strong> time. The <strong>to</strong>tal data collection time required is alsolikely <strong>to</strong> be quite a bit longer than <strong>in</strong> any of the otherthree methods. It is possible, <strong>to</strong>o, that the lack ofanonymity (the respondent is obviously known <strong>to</strong> the<strong>in</strong>terviewer, at least temporarily) may result <strong>in</strong> less validresponses <strong>to</strong> personally sensitive questions. Last, sometypes of samples (<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> high-crime areas, workers<strong>in</strong> large corporations, students, <strong>and</strong> so on) are oftendifficult <strong>to</strong> contact <strong>in</strong> sufficient numbers.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>Probably the most famous example of survey research wasthat done by the sociologist Alfred K<strong>in</strong>sey <strong>and</strong> his associateson the sexual behavior of American men (1948)* <strong>and</strong>women (1953).† While these studies are best known for theirshock<strong>in</strong>g (at the time) f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs concern<strong>in</strong>g the frequency ofvarious sexual behaviors, they are equally noteworthy fortheir methodological competence. Us<strong>in</strong>g very large (althoughnot r<strong>and</strong>om) samples <strong>to</strong>tal<strong>in</strong>g some 12,000 men <strong>and</strong> 8,000women, K<strong>in</strong>sey <strong>and</strong> his associates were meticulous <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>gresults from different samples (replication) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g reliability through retest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> validity through*A. C. K<strong>in</strong>sey, W. B. Pomeroy, <strong>and</strong> C. E. Mart<strong>in</strong> (1948). Sexualbehavior <strong>in</strong> the human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.†A. C. K<strong>in</strong>sey, W. B. Pomeroy, C. E. Mart<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> P. H. Gebhard(1953). Sexual behavior <strong>in</strong> the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.<strong>in</strong>ternal cross-check<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> comparison with spouses or otherpartners. One of the more unusual aspects of the basic datagather<strong>in</strong>gprocess—<strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>terviews—was the <strong>in</strong>terviewschedule that conta<strong>in</strong>ed 521 items (although the m<strong>in</strong>imum perrespondent was 300). The same <strong>in</strong>formation was elicited <strong>in</strong>several different questions, all asked <strong>in</strong> rapid-fire successionso as <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>imize conscious dis<strong>to</strong>rtion.A more recent study came <strong>to</strong> somewhat different conclusionsregard<strong>in</strong>g sexual behavior. The researchers used an <strong>in</strong>terviewprocedure very similar <strong>to</strong> that used <strong>in</strong> the K<strong>in</strong>sey studies,but claimed a superior sampl<strong>in</strong>g procedure. They selecteda r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 4,369 adults from a list of nationwidehome addresses, with the household respondent also chosenat r<strong>and</strong>om. While the f<strong>in</strong>al participation rate of 79 percent(sample 3,500) is high, 79 percent of a r<strong>and</strong>om sample is nolonger a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.‡‡E. Laumann, R. Michael, S. Michaels, <strong>and</strong> J. Gagnon (1994). Thesocial organization of sexuality. Chicago: University of ChicagoPress.SELECTING THE SAMPLEThe subjects <strong>to</strong> be surveyed should be selected (r<strong>and</strong>omly,if possible) from the population of <strong>in</strong>terest. <strong>Research</strong>ersmust ensure, however, that the subjects they<strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> question possess the desired <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong>that they will be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> answer these questions. Individualswho possess the necessary <strong>in</strong>formation but whoare un<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the <strong>to</strong>pic of the survey (or who do notsee it as important) are unlikely <strong>to</strong> respond. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly,it is often a good idea for researchers <strong>to</strong> conduct aprelim<strong>in</strong>ary <strong>in</strong>quiry among potential respondents <strong>to</strong> assesstheir receptivity. Frequently, <strong>in</strong> school-based surveys,a higher response rate can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed if a questionnaireis sent <strong>to</strong> persons <strong>in</strong> authority <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister <strong>to</strong>the potential respondents rather than send<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>to</strong> the respondentsthemselves. For example, a researcher mightask classroom teachers <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister a questionnaire <strong>to</strong>their students rather than ask<strong>in</strong>g the students directly.Some examples of samples that have been surveyedby educational researchers are as follows:A sample of all students attend<strong>in</strong>g an urban universityconcern<strong>in</strong>g their views on the adequacy of thegeneral education program at the university.A sample of all faculty members <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city highschool district as <strong>to</strong> the changes needed <strong>to</strong> help “atrisk”students learn more effectively.A sample of all such students <strong>in</strong> the same districtconcern<strong>in</strong>g their views on the same <strong>to</strong>pic.A sample of all women school super<strong>in</strong>tendents <strong>in</strong> aparticular state concern<strong>in</strong>g their views as <strong>to</strong> the problemsthey encounter <strong>in</strong> their adm<strong>in</strong>istrations.A sample of all the counselors <strong>in</strong> a particular highschool district concern<strong>in</strong>g their perceptions as <strong>to</strong> theadequacy of the school counsel<strong>in</strong>g program.PREPARING THE INSTRUMENTThe most common types of <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> survey researchare the questionnaire <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview schedule(see Chapter 7).* They are virtually identical, except thatthe questionnaire is usually self-adm<strong>in</strong>istered by the respondent,while the <strong>in</strong>terview schedule is adm<strong>in</strong>isteredverbally by the researcher (or tra<strong>in</strong>ed assistant). In the caseof a mailed or self-adm<strong>in</strong>istered questionnaire, the appearanceof the <strong>in</strong>strument is very important <strong>to</strong> the overallsuccess of the study. It should be attractive <strong>and</strong> not <strong>to</strong>olong,† <strong>and</strong> the questions should be as easy <strong>to</strong> answer as*Tests of various types can also be used <strong>in</strong> survey research, as whena researcher uses them <strong>to</strong> describe the read<strong>in</strong>g proficiency of students<strong>in</strong> a school district. We restrict our discussion here, however, <strong>to</strong> thedescription of preferences, op<strong>in</strong>ions, <strong>and</strong> beliefs.†This is very important. Long questionnaires discourage people fromcomplet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> return<strong>in</strong>g them.395


396 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7epossible. The questions <strong>in</strong> a survey, <strong>and</strong> the way they areasked, are of crucial importance. Fowler po<strong>in</strong>ts out thatthere are four practical st<strong>and</strong>ards that all survey questionsshould meet:1. Is this a question that can be asked exactly the way itis written?2. Is this a question that will mean the same th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>everyone?3. Is this a question that people can answer?4. Is this a question that people will be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> answer,given the data collection procedures? 11The answers <strong>to</strong> each of the previous questions forevery question <strong>in</strong> a survey should be yes. Any surveyquestion that violates one or more of these st<strong>and</strong>ardsshould be rewritten.In the case of a personal <strong>in</strong>terview or a telephone survey,the manner of the questioner is of paramount importance.He or she must ask the questions <strong>in</strong> such away that the subjects of the study want <strong>to</strong> respond.In either case, the audience <strong>to</strong> whom the questionsare <strong>to</strong> be directed should be clearly identified. Specializedor unusual words should be avoided if possible or,if they must be used, def<strong>in</strong>ed clearly <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>structionswritten on the <strong>in</strong>strument. The most important th<strong>in</strong>gfor researchers <strong>to</strong> keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, however, is that whatevertype of <strong>in</strong>strument is used, the same questionsmust be asked of all respondents <strong>in</strong> the sample. Furthermore,the conditions under which the questionnaireis adm<strong>in</strong>istered or the <strong>in</strong>terview is conducted should beas similar as possible for all respondents.Types of Questions. The nature of the questions<strong>and</strong> the way they are asked are extremely important<strong>in</strong> survey research. Poorly worded questions can dooma survey <strong>to</strong> failure. Hence, they must be clearlywritten <strong>in</strong> a manner that is easily underst<strong>and</strong>able by therespondents. 12Most surveys rely on multiple-choice or otherforms of what are called closed-ended questions.Multiple-choice questions allow a respondent <strong>to</strong> selecthis or her answer from a number of options. Theymay be used <strong>to</strong> measure op<strong>in</strong>ions, attitudes, orknowledge.Closed-ended questions are easy <strong>to</strong> use, score, <strong>and</strong>code for analysis on a computer. Because all subjectsrespond <strong>to</strong> the same options, st<strong>and</strong>ardized data areprovided. They are somewhat more difficult <strong>to</strong> writethan open-ended questions, however. They also posethe possibility that an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s true response isnot present among the options given. For this reason,the researcher usually should provide an “other” choicefor each item, where the subject can write <strong>in</strong> a responsethat the researcher may not have anticipated. Some examplesof closed-ended questions are the follow<strong>in</strong>g:1. Which subject do you like least?a. Social studiesb. Englishc. Scienced. Mathematicse. Other (specify)2. Rate each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g parts of your master’sdegree program by circl<strong>in</strong>g the number under thephrase that describes how you feel.VerydissatisfiedDissatisfiedSatisfieda. Coursework 1 2 3 4b. Professors 1 2 3 4c. Advis<strong>in</strong>g 1 2 3 4d. Requirements 1 2 3 4e. Cost 1 2 3 4f. Other (specify) 1 2 3 4Open-ended questions allow for more <strong>in</strong>dividualizedresponses, but they are sometimes difficult <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret. They are also often hard <strong>to</strong> score, s<strong>in</strong>ce somany different k<strong>in</strong>ds of responses are received. Furthermore,respondents sometimes do not like them.Some examples of open-ended questions are asfollows:1. What characteristics of a person would lead you <strong>to</strong>rate him or her as a good adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r?2. What do you consider <strong>to</strong> be the most important problemfac<strong>in</strong>g classroom teachers <strong>in</strong> high schools <strong>to</strong>day?3. What were the three th<strong>in</strong>gs about this class youfound most useful dur<strong>in</strong>g the past semester?Generally, therefore, closed-ended or short-answerquestions are preferable, although sometimes researchersf<strong>in</strong>d it useful <strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e both formats <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle question,as shown <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g example of a question us<strong>in</strong>gboth open- <strong>and</strong> closed-ended formats.Verysatisfied


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 3971. Please rate <strong>and</strong> comment on each of the follow<strong>in</strong>gaspects of this course:VerydissatisfiedDissatisfiedSatisfieda. Coursework 1 2 3 4Comment ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________b. Professor 1 2 3 4Comment ___________________________________________________________________________________________________________Table 17.2 presents a brief comparison of the advantages<strong>and</strong> disadvantages of closed-ended <strong>and</strong> openendedquestions.TABLE 17.2 Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantages ofClosed-Ended Versus Open-EndedQuestionsClosed-EndedOpen-EndedAdvantagesEnhance consistency ofresponse across respondentsEasier <strong>and</strong> faster <strong>to</strong> tabulateMore popular withrespondentsMay limit breadth ofresponsesTake more time <strong>to</strong>constructRequire more questions <strong>to</strong>cover the research <strong>to</strong>picDisadvantagesVerysatisfiedAllow more freedom ofresponseEasier <strong>to</strong> constructPermit follow-up by<strong>in</strong>terviewerTend <strong>to</strong> produce responsesthat are <strong>in</strong>consistent <strong>in</strong>length <strong>and</strong> content acrossrespondentsBoth questions <strong>and</strong>responses subject <strong>to</strong>mis<strong>in</strong>terpretationHarder <strong>to</strong> tabulate <strong>and</strong>synthesizeSome Suggestions for Improv<strong>in</strong>g Closed-Ended Questions. There are a number of relativelysimple tips that researchers have found <strong>to</strong> be ofvalue <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g good survey questions. A few of themost frequently mentioned ones follow. 131. Be sure the question is unambiguous.Poor: Do you spend a lot of time study<strong>in</strong>g?Better: <strong>How</strong> much time do you spend each daystudy<strong>in</strong>g?a. More than 2 hours.b. One <strong>to</strong> 2 hours.c. Thirty m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>to</strong> 1 hour.d. Less than 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes.e. Other (specify). _______________2. Keep the focus as simple as possible.Poor: Who do you th<strong>in</strong>k are more satisfied withteach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> elementary <strong>and</strong> secondaryschools, men or women?a. Men are more satisfied.b. Women are more satisfied.c. Men <strong>and</strong> women are about equallysatisfied.d. Don’t know.Better: Who do you th<strong>in</strong>k are more satisfied withteach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> elementary schools, men orwomen?a. Men are more satisfied.b. Women are more satisfied.c. Men <strong>and</strong> women are about equallysatisfied.d. Don’t know.3. Keep the questions short.Poor:What part of the district’s English curriculum,<strong>in</strong> your op<strong>in</strong>ion, is of the most importance<strong>in</strong> terms of the overall development ofthe students <strong>in</strong> the program?Better: What part of the district’s English curriculumis the most important?4. Use common language.Poor:What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal reasonschools are experienc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased studentabsenteeism <strong>to</strong>day?a. Problems at home.b. Lack of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> school.c. Illness.d. Don’t know.Better: What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the ma<strong>in</strong> reasonstudents are absent more this year thanpreviously?a. Problems at home.b. Lack of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> school.c. Illness.d. Don’t know.


398 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e5. Avoid the use of terms that might bias responses.Poor: Do you support the super<strong>in</strong>tendent’s “nosmok<strong>in</strong>g”policy on campus grounds whileschool is <strong>in</strong> session?a. I support the policy.b. I am opposed <strong>to</strong> the policy.c. I don’t care one way or the other aboutthe policy.d. I am undecided about the policy.Better: Do you support a “no-smok<strong>in</strong>g” policy oncampus grounds while school is <strong>in</strong> session?a. I support the policy.b. I am opposed <strong>to</strong> the policy.c. I don’t care one way or the other aboutthe policy.d. I am undecided about the policy.6. Avoid lead<strong>in</strong>g questions.Poor:What rules do you consider necessary <strong>in</strong>your classes?Better: Circle each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g that describes arule you set <strong>in</strong> your classes.a. All homework must be turned <strong>in</strong> on thedate due.b. Students are not <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terrupt other studentsdur<strong>in</strong>g class discussions.c. Late homework is not accepted.d. Students are counted tardy if they aremore than 5 m<strong>in</strong>utes late <strong>to</strong> class.e. Other (specify) ________________7. Avoid double negatives.Poor:Would you not be opposed <strong>to</strong> supervis<strong>in</strong>gstudents outside of your classroom?a. Yes.b. No.c. Undecided.Better: Would you be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> supervise studentsoutside of your classroom?a. Yes.b. No.c. Undecided.Pretest<strong>in</strong>g the Questionnaire. Once the questions<strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the questionnaire or the <strong>in</strong>terviewschedule have been written, the researcher is well advised<strong>to</strong> try them out with a small sample similar <strong>to</strong> thepotential respondents. A “pretest” of the questionnaireor <strong>in</strong>terview schedule can reveal ambiguities, poorlyworded questions, questions that are not unders<strong>to</strong>od,<strong>and</strong> unclear choices; it can also <strong>in</strong>dicate whether the<strong>in</strong>structions <strong>to</strong> the respondents are clear.© The New Yorker Collection 1989 George Price fromcar<strong>to</strong>onbank.com. All Rights Reserved.Overall Format. The format of a questionnaire—how the questions look <strong>to</strong> the respondents—is very important<strong>in</strong> encourag<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> respond. Perhaps themost important rule <strong>to</strong> follow is <strong>to</strong> ensure that the questionsare spread out—that is, uncluttered. No more thanone question should be presented on a s<strong>in</strong>gle l<strong>in</strong>e. Whenrespondents have <strong>to</strong> spend a lot of time read<strong>in</strong>g a question,they quickly become discouraged from cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g.There are a variety of ways <strong>to</strong> present the responsecategories from which respondents are asked <strong>to</strong> choose.Babbie suggests that boxes, as shown <strong>in</strong> the questionbelow, are the best. 14Have you ever taught an advanced placement class?[ ] Yes[ ] NoSometimes, certa<strong>in</strong> questions will apply <strong>to</strong> only aportion of the subjects <strong>in</strong> the sample. When this is thecase, follow-up questions can be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the questionnaire.For example, a researcher might ask respondentsif they are familiar with a particular activity, <strong>and</strong>then ask those who say yes <strong>to</strong> give their op<strong>in</strong>ion of theactivity. The follow-up question is called a cont<strong>in</strong>gencyquestion—it is cont<strong>in</strong>gent upon how a respondent answersthe first question. If properly used, cont<strong>in</strong>gencyquestions are a valuable survey <strong>to</strong>ol, <strong>in</strong> that they canmake it easier for a respondent <strong>to</strong> answer a given question<strong>and</strong> also improve the quality of the data a researcherreceives. Although a variety of cont<strong>in</strong>gency formats


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 399Did you substitute at any time dur<strong>in</strong>g the past year?(Include part-time substitut<strong>in</strong>g.)1. Yes 2. Noa. <strong>How</strong> many days did you substitute lastweek, count<strong>in</strong>g all jobs, if more than one?1. Less than one day. 5. Four days.2. One day. 6. Five days.3. Two days. 7. Other4. Three days.b. Would you like <strong>to</strong> substitute more hours,or is that about as much as you want <strong>to</strong>work?1. Want more.2. Don’t want more.3. Don’t know.c. <strong>How</strong> long have you been substituteteach<strong>in</strong>g?1. Less than one year.2. One year.3. 2–3 years.4. 4–5 years.5. 6–10 years.6. More than 10 years.d. In the past year, have there been any weekswhen you were not offered a chance <strong>to</strong>substitute?1. Yes.2. No.3. Don’t know.e. Did you want <strong>to</strong> substitute last week?1. Yes.2. No.f. Did you want <strong>to</strong> substitute at any time dur<strong>in</strong>gthe past 60 days?1. Yes.2. No.g. What were you do<strong>in</strong>g most of last week?1. Keep<strong>in</strong>g house.2. Go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> school.3. On vacation.4. Retired.5. Disabled.6. Other.h. When did you last substitute?1. This month.2. Over a month ago.3. Over six months ago.4. Over a year ago.5. Disabled.6. Never substituted.Figure 17.2 Example of Several Cont<strong>in</strong>gency Questions <strong>in</strong> an Interview ScheduleAdapted from E. S. Babbie (1973). Survey research methods. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, p. 149.may be used, the easiest <strong>to</strong> prepare is simply <strong>to</strong> set offthe cont<strong>in</strong>gency question by <strong>in</strong>dent<strong>in</strong>g it, enclos<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong>a box, <strong>and</strong> connect<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>to</strong> the base question by means ofan arrow <strong>to</strong> the appropriate response, as shown on thefollow<strong>in</strong>g page.Have you ever taught an advanced placement class?[ ] Yes[ ] NoA clear <strong>and</strong> well-organized presentation of cont<strong>in</strong>gencyquestions is particularly important <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewschedules. An <strong>in</strong>dividual who receives a questionnaire<strong>in</strong> the mail can reread a question if it is unclear the firsttime through. If an <strong>in</strong>terviewer becomes confused, however,or reads a question poorly or <strong>in</strong> an unclear manner,the whole <strong>in</strong>terview may become jeopardized. Figure17.2 illustrates a portion of an <strong>in</strong>terview schedulethat <strong>in</strong>cludes several cont<strong>in</strong>gency questions.If yes: Have you ever attended a workshop<strong>in</strong> which you receivedspecial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> teach suchclasses?[ ] Yes[ ] NoPREPARING THE COVER LETTERMailed surveys require someth<strong>in</strong>g that telephone surveys<strong>and</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face personal <strong>in</strong>terviews do not—acover letter expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the purpose of the questionnaire.Ideally, the cover letter also motivates the members ofthe sample <strong>to</strong> respond.


400 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCOLLEGE OF EDUCATIONSan Francisco State UniversityOc<strong>to</strong>ber 1, 200–Mr. Robert R. JohnsonSocial Studies DepartmentOceana High SchoolPacifica, California 96321Dear Mr. Johnson,The Department of Secondary <strong>Education</strong> of San Francisco State University prepares over 100 studentteachers every year <strong>to</strong> teach <strong>in</strong> the public <strong>and</strong> private schools of California. It is our goal <strong>to</strong> help ourgraduates become as well prepared as possible <strong>to</strong> teach <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>day’s schools. The enclosed questionnaire isdesigned <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> your views on how <strong>to</strong> improve the quality of our tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g program. Your suggestionswill be considered <strong>in</strong> plann<strong>in</strong>g for revisions <strong>in</strong> the program <strong>in</strong> the com<strong>in</strong>g academic year. We will alsoprovide you with a copy of the results of our study.We will greatly appreciate it if you will complete the questionnaire <strong>and</strong> return it <strong>in</strong> the enclosed stamped,self-addressed envelope by Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 18th. We realize your schedule is a busy one <strong>and</strong> that your time isvaluable, but we are sure that you want <strong>to</strong> improve the quality of teacher tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g as much as we do. Yourresponses will be kept completely confidential; we ask for no identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation on the questionnaireform. The study has been approved by the University’s <strong>Research</strong> with Human Subjects review committee.We want <strong>to</strong> thank you <strong>in</strong> advance for your cooperation.William P. JonesChair of the DepartmentFigure 17.3 Sample Cover Letter for a Mail SurveyThe cover letter should be brief <strong>and</strong> addressed specifically<strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual be<strong>in</strong>g asked <strong>to</strong> respond. It shouldexpla<strong>in</strong> the purpose of the survey, emphasize the importanceof the <strong>to</strong>pic of the research, <strong>and</strong> (it is hoped) engagethe respondent’s cooperation. If possible, it should<strong>in</strong>dicate the researcher’s will<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> share the resultsof the study once it is completed. Confidentiality <strong>and</strong>anonymity of the respondents should be assured.* It alsohelps if the researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s the sponsorship of an <strong>in</strong>stitutionof some importance that is known <strong>to</strong> the respondent.The letter should specify the date by which thecompleted questionnaire is <strong>to</strong> be returned, <strong>and</strong> it shouldbe <strong>in</strong>dividually signed by the researcher. Every effortshould be made <strong>to</strong> avoid the appearance of a form letter.*If done under a university (or other agency) sponsorship, the lettershould <strong>in</strong>dicate that the study has been approved by the “<strong>Research</strong>with Human Subjects” review committee.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the return should be made as easy as possible;hence, enclos<strong>in</strong>g a stamped, self-addressed envelope isalways a good idea. Figure 17.3 presents an example ofa cover letter.TRAINING INTERVIEWERSBoth telephone <strong>and</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewers need <strong>to</strong> betra<strong>in</strong>ed beforeh<strong>and</strong>. Many suggestions have been made <strong>in</strong>this regard, <strong>and</strong> we have space <strong>to</strong> mention only a few ofthem here. 15 Telephone <strong>in</strong>terviewers need <strong>to</strong> be shownhow <strong>to</strong> engage their <strong>in</strong>terviewees so that they do not hangup on them before the <strong>in</strong>terview has even begun. Theyneed <strong>to</strong> know how <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> quickly the purpose of theircall <strong>and</strong> why it is important <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation from therespondent. They need <strong>to</strong> learn how <strong>to</strong> ask questions <strong>in</strong> away that encourages <strong>in</strong>terviewees <strong>to</strong> respond honestly.


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 401Face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewers need all of the above <strong>and</strong>more. They need <strong>to</strong> learn how <strong>to</strong> establish rapport withtheir <strong>in</strong>terviewees <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> put them at ease. If a respondentseems <strong>to</strong> be resistant <strong>to</strong> a particular l<strong>in</strong>e of question<strong>in</strong>g,the <strong>in</strong>terviewer needs <strong>to</strong> know how <strong>to</strong> move on<strong>to</strong> a new set of questions <strong>and</strong> return <strong>to</strong> the previousquestions later. The <strong>in</strong>terviewer needs <strong>to</strong> know when<strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong> “follow up” on an unusual answer or onethat is ambiguous or unclear. Interviewers also needtra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> gestures, manner, facial expression, <strong>and</strong>dress. A frown at the wrong time can discourage a respondentfrom even attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> answer a question! Insum, the general <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>to</strong> be covered <strong>in</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviewersshould always <strong>in</strong>clude at least the follow<strong>in</strong>g:1. Procedures for contact<strong>in</strong>g respondents <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>gthe study. All <strong>in</strong>terviewers should have a commonunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the purposes of the study.2. The conventions that are used <strong>in</strong> the design of thequestionnaire with respect <strong>to</strong> word<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>structionsfor skipp<strong>in</strong>g questions (if necessary) so that <strong>in</strong>terviewerscan ask the questions <strong>in</strong> a consistent <strong>and</strong>st<strong>and</strong>ardized way.3. Procedures for prob<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>adequate answers <strong>in</strong> anondirective way. Prob<strong>in</strong>g refers <strong>to</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g up <strong>in</strong>completeanswers <strong>in</strong> ways that do not favor one particularanswer over another. Certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of st<strong>and</strong>ardprobes, such as ask<strong>in</strong>g “Anyth<strong>in</strong>g else?” “Tellme more,” or “<strong>How</strong> do you mean that?” usually willh<strong>and</strong>le most situations.4. Procedures for record<strong>in</strong>g answers <strong>to</strong> open-ended <strong>and</strong>closed-ended questions. This is especially importantwith regard <strong>to</strong> answers <strong>to</strong> open-ended questions,which <strong>in</strong>terviewers are expected <strong>to</strong> record verbatim.5. Rules <strong>and</strong> guidel<strong>in</strong>es for h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terpersonalaspects of the <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> a nonbias<strong>in</strong>g way. Of particularimportance here is for <strong>in</strong>terviewers <strong>to</strong> focuson the task at h<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> avoid express<strong>in</strong>g theirviews or op<strong>in</strong>ions (verbally or with body language)on any of the questions be<strong>in</strong>g asked. 16USING AN INTERVIEW TO MEASURE ABILITYAlthough the <strong>in</strong>terview has been used primarily <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>formation on variables other than cognitive ability, animportant exception can be found <strong>in</strong> the field of developmental<strong>and</strong> cognitive psychology. Interviews have beenused extensively <strong>in</strong> this field <strong>to</strong> study both the content <strong>and</strong>processes of cognition. The best-known example of suchuse is <strong>to</strong> be found <strong>in</strong> the work of Jean Piaget <strong>and</strong> hiscolleagues. They used a semistructured sequence ofcont<strong>in</strong>gency questions <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e a child’s cognitivelevel of development.Other psychologists have used <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g procedures<strong>to</strong> study thought processes <strong>and</strong> sequences employed<strong>in</strong> problem solv<strong>in</strong>g. While not used extensively<strong>to</strong> date <strong>in</strong> educational research, an illustrative study isthat of Freyberg <strong>and</strong> Osborne, who studied student underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof basic science concepts. They found frequent<strong>and</strong> important misconceptions of which teacherswere often unaware. Teachers often assumed that studentsused such terms as gravity, condensation, conservationof energy, <strong>and</strong> wastel<strong>and</strong> community <strong>in</strong> the sameway as they did themselves. Many 10-year-olds <strong>and</strong> evensome older children, for example, believed that condensationon the outside of a water glass was caused bywater gett<strong>in</strong>g through the glass. One 15-year-old displayed<strong>in</strong>genious (although <strong>in</strong>correct) th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g as shown<strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g excerpt:(Jenny, aged 15): Through the glass—the particles ofwater have gone through the glass, like diffusion throughair—well, it hasn’t got there any other way. (<strong>Research</strong>er):A lot of younger people I have talked <strong>to</strong> have been worriedabout this water . . . it troubles them. (Jenny): Yes,because they haven’t studied th<strong>in</strong>gs like we have studied.(<strong>Research</strong>er): What have you studied which helps?(Jenny): Th<strong>in</strong>gs that pass through air, <strong>and</strong> concentrations<strong>and</strong> how th<strong>in</strong>gs diffuse. 17Freyberg <strong>and</strong> Osborne make the argument that teachers<strong>and</strong> curriculum developers must have such <strong>in</strong>formationon student conceptions if they are <strong>to</strong> teach effectively.They have also shown how such research canimprove the content of achievement tests by <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gitems specifically directed at common misconceptions.NonresponseIn almost all surveys, some members of the sample willnot respond. This is referred <strong>to</strong> as nonresponse. It maybe due <strong>to</strong> a number of reasons (lack of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the<strong>to</strong>pic be<strong>in</strong>g surveyed, forgetfulness, unwill<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> besurveyed, <strong>and</strong> so on), but it is a major problem that hasbeen <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> recent years as more <strong>and</strong> more peopleseem (for whatever reason) <strong>to</strong> be unwill<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> participate<strong>in</strong> surveys.Why is nonresponse a problem? The chief reason isthat those who do not respond will very likely differfrom the respondents on answers <strong>to</strong> the survey questions.Should this be the case, any conclusions drawn on


402 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ethe basis of the respondents’ replies will be mislead<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> not a true <strong>in</strong>dication of the views of the populationfrom which the sample was drawn.TOTAL NONRESPONSEKal<strong>to</strong>n po<strong>in</strong>ts out that <strong>to</strong>tal nonresponse can occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewsurveys for any of the follow<strong>in</strong>g reasons: Intendedrespondents can refuse <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed, not beat home when the <strong>in</strong>terviewer calls, be unable <strong>to</strong> takepart <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview for various reasons (such as illness,deafness, <strong>in</strong>ability <strong>to</strong> speak the language), or sometimescannot even be located. 18 Of these, refusals <strong>and</strong> not-athomesare the most common.In mail surveys, a few questionnaires may not be deliverable,<strong>and</strong> occasionally a few respondents will returntheir questionnaires unanswered as an <strong>in</strong>dication oftheir refusal <strong>to</strong> participate. Generally, however, all thatis known about most mail survey nonresponse is that thequestionnaire has not been returned. The reason for thelack of return may be any of the ones we have alreadymentioned.A variety of techniques are employed by survey researchers<strong>to</strong> reduce nonresponse. In <strong>in</strong>terview surveys,the <strong>in</strong>terviewers are carefully tra<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> be courteous, <strong>to</strong>ask questions pleasantly <strong>and</strong> sensitively, <strong>to</strong> dress conservatively,or <strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> conduct an <strong>in</strong>terview at amore appropriate time if the situation warrants. Assurancesof anonymity <strong>and</strong> confidentiality are made (this isdone <strong>in</strong> mailed surveys as well). Questions are usuallyorganized <strong>to</strong> start with fairly simple <strong>and</strong> nonthreaten<strong>in</strong>gquestions. Not-at-homes are treated by callbacks (a second,third, or even a fourth visit) on different days <strong>and</strong>at different times dur<strong>in</strong>g the day. Sometimes appo<strong>in</strong>tmentsare set up at a convenient time for the respondent.Mailed questionnaires can be followed up with a rem<strong>in</strong>derletter <strong>and</strong> often a second or sometimes even athird mail<strong>in</strong>g. A frequently overlooked technique is theoffer<strong>in</strong>g of a tangible reward as an <strong>in</strong>ducement <strong>to</strong> respond.There is noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>appropriate about pay<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>in</strong>some manner) respondents for provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation.Nonresponse is a serious problem <strong>in</strong> many surveys.Some observers have stated that response rates for uncomplicatedface-<strong>to</strong>-face surveys by nongovernmentsurvey organizations are about 70 <strong>to</strong> 75 percent. Refusalsmake up the majority of nonrespondents <strong>in</strong> face<strong>to</strong>-face<strong>in</strong>terviews, with not-at-homes constitut<strong>in</strong>g mos<strong>to</strong>f the rema<strong>in</strong>der. Telephone surveys generally havesomewhat lower response rates than face-<strong>to</strong>-face surveys(respondents simply hang up). Response rates <strong>in</strong>mail surveys are quite varied, rang<strong>in</strong>g from as low as10 percent <strong>to</strong> as high as 90 percent. 19 Furthermore, nonresponseis not evenly spread out among various subgroupswith<strong>in</strong> the United States. Nonresponse rates <strong>in</strong>face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terview surveys, for example, are muchhigher <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ner cities than <strong>in</strong> other locations.A procedure commonly used <strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>le nonresponse,especially <strong>in</strong> telephone surveys, is r<strong>and</strong>om replacement,which is cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> add r<strong>and</strong>omly selected cases untilthe desired sample size is reached. This method does notwork for the same reason mentioned earlier: Those whoare not contacted or who refuse <strong>to</strong> respond probably wouldhave answered differently than those who do respond. Remember:A r<strong>and</strong>om sample requires that the sample actuallycomprises those who are orig<strong>in</strong>ally selected.In addition <strong>to</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g as much as possible <strong>to</strong> reducenonresponse, researchers should obta<strong>in</strong>, dur<strong>in</strong>g the surveyor <strong>in</strong> other ways, as much demographic <strong>in</strong>formationas they can on respondents. This not only permits a morecomplete description of the sample, but also may supportan argument for representativeness—if it turns out thatthe sample is very similar <strong>to</strong> the population with regard<strong>to</strong> those demographics that are pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> the study(Figure 17.4). These may <strong>in</strong>clude gender, age, ethnicity,"I'm very pleased.My sample is very similar<strong>to</strong> the population <strong>in</strong> age <strong>and</strong>gender. That makes itrepresentative!""Wait a m<strong>in</strong>ute!What about occupation,<strong>in</strong>come, or othercharacteristics?"Figure 17.4 Demographic Data <strong>and</strong> Representativeness


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHIs Low Response Rate Necessarilya Bad Th<strong>in</strong>g?As po<strong>in</strong>ted out by some researchers, “A basic tenet of surveyresearch is that high response rates are better thanlow response rates. Indeed, a low rate is one of the few outcomesor features that—taken by itself—is considered <strong>to</strong> be amajor threat <strong>to</strong> the usefulness of a survey.”* Two recent studiesof telephone response rates, however, suggest that this isnot necessarily true. In one <strong>in</strong>stance, the authors used an omnibusquestionnaire that <strong>in</strong>cluded demographic, behavioral,attitud<strong>in</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> knowledge items. In the other, the researcherused data from the Index of Consumer Sentiment (a measure of*R. Curt<strong>in</strong>, S. Presser, <strong>and</strong> E. S<strong>in</strong>ger (2000). The effects of responserate changes on the Index of Consumer Sentiment. Public Op<strong>in</strong>ionQuarterly, 64: 413.consumer op<strong>in</strong>ions about the economy). In both studies, acomparison of response rates of 60 <strong>to</strong> 70 percent <strong>to</strong> ratessubstantially lower (i.e., 20 <strong>to</strong> 40 percent) showed m<strong>in</strong>imaldifferences <strong>in</strong> substantive answers.The implication is that the substantial expense of atta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ghigher rates may not be worth it. It is po<strong>in</strong>ted out that“observ<strong>in</strong>g (the) little effect of nonresponse when compar<strong>in</strong>gresponse rates of 60 <strong>to</strong> 70 percent with rates much lower doesnot mean that the surveys with 60 <strong>to</strong> 70 percent response ratesdo not themselves suffer from significant nonresponsebias,”† that is, a 90 percent rate may have given differentresults from the 60 percent rate. Further, these results shouldnot be generalized <strong>to</strong> other types of questions or <strong>to</strong> respondentsother than those <strong>in</strong> these particular surveys.†S. Keeter, C. Miller, A. Kohut, R. Groves, <strong>and</strong> S. Prosser (2000).Consequences of reduc<strong>in</strong>g nonresponse <strong>in</strong> a large national telephonesurvey. Public Op<strong>in</strong>ion Quarterly, 64: 125–148.family size, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Needless <strong>to</strong> say, all such datamust be reported, not just those that support the claim ofrepresentativeness. Such an argument is always <strong>in</strong>conclusives<strong>in</strong>ce it is impossible <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> data on all pert<strong>in</strong>entvariables (or even <strong>to</strong> be sure as <strong>to</strong> what they allare), but it is an important feature of any survey that hasa substantial nonresponse (we would say over 10 percent).A major difficulty with this suggestion is that theneeded demographics may not be available for the population.In any case, the nonresponse rate should alwaysbe reported.ITEM NONRESPONSEPartial gaps <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>formation provided by respondentscan also occur for a variety of reasons: The respondentmay not know the answer <strong>to</strong> a particular question; he orshe may f<strong>in</strong>d certa<strong>in</strong> questions embarrass<strong>in</strong>g or perhapsirrelevant; the respondent may be pressed for time, <strong>and</strong>the <strong>in</strong>terviewer may decide <strong>to</strong> skip over part of the questions;the <strong>in</strong>terviewer may fail <strong>to</strong> record an answer.Sometimes dur<strong>in</strong>g the data analysis phase of a survey,the answers <strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> questions are thrown out becausethey are <strong>in</strong>consistent with other answers. Some answersmay be unclear or illegible.Item nonresponse is rarely as high as <strong>to</strong>tal nonresponse.Generally it varies accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the nature of thequestion asked <strong>and</strong> the mode of data collection. Verysimple demographic questions usually have almost nononresponse. Kal<strong>to</strong>n estimates that items deal<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong>come <strong>and</strong> expenditures may experience item nonresponserates of 10 percent or more, while extremelysensitive or difficult questions may produce nonresponserates that are much higher. 20Listed below is a summary of some of the morecommon suggestions for <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the response rate <strong>in</strong>surveys.1. Adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the questionnaire or <strong>in</strong>terviewschedule:Make conditions under which the <strong>in</strong>terview isconducted, or the questionnaire adm<strong>in</strong>istered, assimple <strong>and</strong> convenient as possible for each <strong>in</strong>dividual<strong>in</strong> the sample.Be sure that the group <strong>to</strong> be surveyed knows someth<strong>in</strong>gabout the <strong>in</strong>formation you want <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>.Tra<strong>in</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face or telephone <strong>in</strong>terviewers <strong>in</strong>how <strong>to</strong> ask questions.Tra<strong>in</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewers <strong>in</strong> how <strong>to</strong> dress.2. Format of the questionnaire or <strong>in</strong>terview schedule:Be sure that sufficient space is provided for respondents(or the <strong>in</strong>terviewer) <strong>to</strong> fill <strong>in</strong> the necessarybiographical data that is needed (age, gender,grade level, <strong>and</strong> so on).Specify <strong>in</strong> precise terms the objectives the questionnaireor <strong>in</strong>terview schedule is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong>achieve—exactly what k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>formation iswanted from the respondents?403


404 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eBe sure each item <strong>in</strong> the questionnaire or <strong>in</strong>terviewschedule is related <strong>to</strong> one of the objectivesof the study—that is, it will help obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formationabout the objective.Use closed-ended (e.g., multiple-choice) ratherthan or <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> open-ended (e.g., free response)questions.Ensure that no psychologically threaten<strong>in</strong>g questionsare <strong>in</strong>cluded.Elim<strong>in</strong>ate any lead<strong>in</strong>g questions.Check for ambiguity of items with a panel ofjudges. Revise as needed.Pretest the questionnaire or <strong>in</strong>terview schedulewith a small group similar <strong>to</strong> the sample <strong>to</strong> besurveyed.Problems <strong>in</strong> theInstrumentation Process<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>Several threats <strong>to</strong> the validity of the <strong>in</strong>strumentationprocess <strong>in</strong> surveys can cause <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> responddifferently from how they might otherwise respond.Suppose, for example, that a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals isbrought <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed all <strong>in</strong> one place<strong>and</strong> an extraneous event (say, a fire drill) occurs dur<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>terview process. The event might upset orotherwise affect various <strong>in</strong>dividuals, caus<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong>respond <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview questions <strong>in</strong> a different wayfrom how they would have responded if the event hadnot occurred.Whenever researchers do not take care <strong>in</strong> prepar<strong>in</strong>gtheir questionnaires—if questions are lead<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>sensitive,for example—it may cause <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> responddifferently. If the conditions under which <strong>in</strong>dividuals arequestioned <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview studies are somewhat unusual(dur<strong>in</strong>g the d<strong>in</strong>ner hour; <strong>in</strong> poorly lit rooms; <strong>and</strong> so on),they may react <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> ways unrelated <strong>to</strong> the nature ofthe questions themselves.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the characteristics of a data collec<strong>to</strong>r (such asgarish dress, <strong>in</strong>sensitivity, rudeness, <strong>and</strong> use of offensivelanguage) can affect how <strong>in</strong>dividuals respond,caus<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> react <strong>in</strong> part <strong>to</strong> the data collec<strong>to</strong>r ratherthan <strong>to</strong> the questions. There is also the possibility of anunconscious bias on the part of the data collec<strong>to</strong>r, aswhen he or she asks lead<strong>in</strong>g questions of some <strong>in</strong>dividualsbut not others.Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g Threats<strong>to</strong> Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Survey <strong>Research</strong>There are four ma<strong>in</strong> threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> surveyresearch: mortality, location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumentdecay.Amortality threat arises <strong>in</strong> longitud<strong>in</strong>al studiesunless all of the data on “lost” subjects are deleted, <strong>in</strong>which case the problem becomes one of appropriate generalization.Alocationthreat can occur if the collection ofdata is carried out <strong>in</strong> places that may affect responses(e.g., a survey of attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward the police conducted <strong>in</strong>a police station). Instrument decay can occur <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewsurveys if the <strong>in</strong>terviewers get tired or are rushed. This, aswell as defects <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>struments themselves, not onlymay reduce the validity of the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed butalso may <strong>in</strong>troduce a systematic bias.Data Analysis<strong>in</strong> Survey <strong>Research</strong>After the answers <strong>to</strong> the survey questions have beenrecorded, there rema<strong>in</strong>s the f<strong>in</strong>al task of summariz<strong>in</strong>g theresponses <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> draw some conclusions from the results.The <strong>to</strong>tal size of the sample should be reported,along with the overall percentage of returns. The percentageof the <strong>to</strong>tal sample respond<strong>in</strong>g for each item shouldthen be reported. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the percentage of respondentswho chose each alternative for each question should begiven. For example, a reported result might be as follows:“For item 26, regard<strong>in</strong>g the approval of a no-smok<strong>in</strong>gpolicy while school is <strong>in</strong> session, 80 percent <strong>in</strong>dicatedthey were <strong>in</strong> favor of such a policy, 15 percent <strong>in</strong>dicatedthey were not <strong>in</strong> favor, <strong>and</strong> 5 percent said they wereneutral.”An Example of Survey <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of survey research, followed by acritique of its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong>our critiques of the different types of research studieswe analyzed <strong>in</strong> other chapters, we use several of theconcepts <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> ouranalysis.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 44 (1) (March 2002): 17–27. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the publisher (Taylor &Francis Ltd., http://www.t<strong>and</strong>f.co.uk/journals).Russian <strong>and</strong> American College Students’Attitudes, Perceptions, <strong>and</strong> TendenciesTowards Cheat<strong>in</strong>gRobert A. Lup<strong>to</strong>nCentral Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n UniversityKenneth J. ChapmanCalifornia State University, ChicoSUMMARY The literature reports that cheat<strong>in</strong>g is endemic throughout the USA.<strong>How</strong>ever, lack<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>ternational comparative studies that have researched cheat<strong>in</strong>gdifferences at the post-secondary bus<strong>in</strong>ess education level. This study <strong>in</strong>vestigates thedifferences between Russian <strong>and</strong> American bus<strong>in</strong>ess college students concern<strong>in</strong>g their attitudes,perceptions <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards academic dishonesty. The study found significantdifferences between Russian <strong>and</strong> American college students’ behaviours <strong>and</strong> beliefsabout cheat<strong>in</strong>g. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are important for bus<strong>in</strong>ess educa<strong>to</strong>rs called <strong>to</strong> teachabroad or <strong>in</strong> classes that are <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly mult<strong>in</strong>ational <strong>in</strong> composition.JustificationINTRODUCTIONThe Ch<strong>in</strong>ese have been concerned about cheat<strong>in</strong>g for longer than most civilizations havebeen <strong>in</strong> existence. Over 2,000 years ago, prospective Ch<strong>in</strong>ese civil servants were given entranceexams <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual cubicles <strong>to</strong> prevent cheat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> searched for crib notes asthey entered the cubicles. The penalty for be<strong>in</strong>g caught at cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> ancient Ch<strong>in</strong>a wasnot a fail<strong>in</strong>g grade or expulsion, but death, which was applicable <strong>to</strong> both the exam<strong>in</strong>ees<strong>and</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ers (Brickman, 1961). Today, while we do not execute students <strong>and</strong> their professorswhen cheat<strong>in</strong>g is discovered, it appears we may not be do<strong>in</strong>g enough <strong>to</strong> detercheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> our classes (e.g., Collison, 1990; McCabe & Trev<strong>in</strong>o, 1996; Paldy, 1996).Cheat<strong>in</strong>g among U.S. college students is well documented <strong>in</strong> a plethora of publishedreports, with a preponderance of U.S. studies report<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cidences <strong>in</strong>excess of 70% (e.g., Baird, 1980; Collison, 1990; Davis et al., 1992; Gail & Bor<strong>in</strong>, 1988;Jendrek, 1989; Lord <strong>and</strong> Chiodo, 1995; McCabe & Trev<strong>in</strong>o, 1996; Oaks, 1975; Stern &Havlicek, 1986; Stevens & Stevens, 1987). Indeed, U.S. academicians have addressed the issuesof cheat<strong>in</strong>g for the past century, publish<strong>in</strong>g over 200 journal articles <strong>and</strong> reports(Payne & Nantz, 1994). 1 The U.S. literature can be divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> five primary areas: (a) report<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>cidences <strong>and</strong> types of cheat<strong>in</strong>g (Baird, 1980; McCabe & Bowers, 1994, 1996),(b) report<strong>in</strong>g the behavioural <strong>and</strong> situational causes of cheat<strong>in</strong>g (Bunn, Caudill, & Gropper,1992; LaBeff et al., 1990), (c) report<strong>in</strong>g the reactions of academicians <strong>to</strong>wards cheat<strong>in</strong>g(Jendrek, 1989; Roberts, 1986), (d) discuss<strong>in</strong>g the prevention <strong>and</strong> control of cheat<strong>in</strong>g(Ackerman, 1971; Hardy, 1981–1982), <strong>and</strong> (e) present<strong>in</strong>g statistical research methodologiesused <strong>to</strong> measure academic misconduct (Frary, Tideman, & Nicholaus, 1997; Frary,Tideman, & Watts, 1977).Literature Review1 For a comprehensive review of the cheat<strong>in</strong>g literature, see Lup<strong>to</strong>n’s (1999) published dissertation.405


406 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eJustificationJustificationGood SummariesJustificationThe U.S. studies on cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours are disturb<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>ce they <strong>in</strong>dicate a widespread,<strong>in</strong>sidious problem. Cheat<strong>in</strong>g devalues the educational experience <strong>in</strong> a number ofways. First, cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours may lead <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>equitable grades <strong>and</strong> a misrepresentationof what a student may actually have learned <strong>and</strong> can use after graduation. Additionally,successful cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours <strong>in</strong> college may carry over as a way of life after college.That is, students may believe that if they can get away with cheat<strong>in</strong>g now, they can getaway with cheat<strong>in</strong>g later. Obviously, academic dishonesty is not <strong>to</strong> be taken lightly, yetcheat<strong>in</strong>g seems <strong>to</strong> be prevalent, at least <strong>in</strong> the USA. This study <strong>in</strong>vestigated if the academicdishonesty problem crosses national boundaries. The researchers <strong>in</strong>vestigated if students’attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g tendencies vary by country—specifically, as part ofan ongo<strong>in</strong>g research agenda (Lup<strong>to</strong>n, Chapman, & Weiss, 2000); the researchers reportdifferences between Russian <strong>and</strong> American students.The <strong>in</strong>ternational literature provides mostly anecdotal evidence of academic dishonesty<strong>and</strong> has few comparative research efforts. International studies <strong>and</strong> reports havelooked at college students <strong>in</strong> Australia (Maslen, 1996; Waugh & Godfrey, 1994), Canada(Black, 1962; Chidley, 1997; Genereux & McLeod, 1995; Harpp & Hogan, 1993, 1998; Jenk<strong>in</strong>son,1996), the UK (Baty, 1997; Bushby, 1997; Franklyn-S<strong>to</strong>kes & Newstead, 1995;Mackenzie & Smith, 1995; Newstead, Franklyn-S<strong>to</strong>kes, & Armstead, 1996), Palest<strong>in</strong>e(Surkes, 1994), Pol<strong>and</strong> (Curry, 1997) <strong>and</strong> Russia (Pol<strong>to</strong>rak, 1995), <strong>and</strong> high school students<strong>in</strong> Austria (Hanisch, 1990), Germany (Rost & Wild, 1990) <strong>and</strong> Italy (TES, 1996).Pol<strong>to</strong>rak (1995), the only major Russian study, measured attitudes about <strong>and</strong> tendencies<strong>to</strong>wards cheat<strong>in</strong>g at four Russian post-secondary technical universities. The researchfound cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be widespread, with over 80% of the students cheat<strong>in</strong>g at leas<strong>to</strong>nce dur<strong>in</strong>g college <strong>and</strong> with many of those <strong>in</strong>cidences occurr<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations.The most common types of cheat<strong>in</strong>g were: us<strong>in</strong>g crib sheets dur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations, look<strong>in</strong>gat someone’s exam<strong>in</strong>ation, us<strong>in</strong>g unauthorized lecture notes dur<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations,us<strong>in</strong>g someone’s f<strong>in</strong>ished homework <strong>to</strong> copy from, <strong>and</strong> purchas<strong>in</strong>g term papers <strong>and</strong> plagiariz<strong>in</strong>g.Moreover, male college students were reported <strong>to</strong> have higher <strong>in</strong>cidences ofcheat<strong>in</strong>g than female students.Only a h<strong>and</strong>ful of studies have <strong>in</strong>vestigated cross-national differences related <strong>to</strong> academicdishonesty (Curtis, 1996; Davis et al., 1994; Diekhoff et al., 1999; Evans, Craig, &Mietzel, 1993; Lup<strong>to</strong>n et al., 2000; Waugh et al., 1995). Davis et al. (1994) reported that amajority of Australian <strong>and</strong> U.S. college students cheated more <strong>in</strong> high school than they did<strong>in</strong> college. The study is unique <strong>in</strong> that cheat<strong>in</strong>g is l<strong>in</strong>ked <strong>to</strong> grade-oriented <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>gorientedattitudes. It appears that Australian college students are more likely <strong>to</strong> attendschool for the sake of learn<strong>in</strong>g, whereas U.S. students tend <strong>to</strong> be much more focused ongrades. Thus, what motivates Australian college students <strong>to</strong> cheat is different from that ofU.S. college students. Diekhoff et al. (1999) found that Japanese college students, as compared<strong>to</strong> U.S. students, report higher levels of cheat<strong>in</strong>g tendencies, have a greater propensity<strong>to</strong> neutralize the severity of cheat<strong>in</strong>g through rationale justification, <strong>and</strong> are not asdisturbed when observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>-class cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, U.S. <strong>and</strong> Japanese studentsagreed guilt is the most effective deterrent <strong>to</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally, Lup<strong>to</strong>n et al. (2000) foundsignificantly different levels of cheat<strong>in</strong>g between Polish <strong>and</strong> U.S. bus<strong>in</strong>ess students. ThePolish students reported much higher frequencies of cheat<strong>in</strong>g than their American counterparts<strong>and</strong> were more likely <strong>to</strong> feel it was not so bad <strong>to</strong> cheat on one exam or tell someone<strong>in</strong> a later section about an exam. The Polish students were also more <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed than theAmerican students <strong>to</strong> feel it was the responsibility of the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r <strong>to</strong> create an environmentthat reduces the likelihood that cheat<strong>in</strong>g could occur.Although cross-national comparative studies are appear<strong>in</strong>g more often <strong>in</strong> academicliterature, it is quite apparent that a major chasm <strong>in</strong> our knowledge still exists


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 407regard<strong>in</strong>g cross-national attitudes, perceptions <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards cheat<strong>in</strong>g at thepost-secondary education level. Moreover, <strong>to</strong> date, no cross-national study has been conductedcompar<strong>in</strong>g Russian <strong>and</strong> U.S. bus<strong>in</strong>ess college students. Russian universities havebeen known <strong>to</strong> produce <strong>to</strong>p students, particularly <strong>in</strong> computer programm<strong>in</strong>g (Chronicleof Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 2000). <strong>How</strong>ever, like many <strong>in</strong>stitutions <strong>in</strong> Russia, education has beenthe recipient of severe sw<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> its support <strong>and</strong> fund<strong>in</strong>g over the years. Some reports<strong>in</strong>dicate the post-secondary educational system is <strong>in</strong> serious disrepair, where bribes forentrance <strong>and</strong> grades are commonplace <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g is m<strong>in</strong>imal (Dolshenko, 1999). Additionally,the value of an education seems <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> question, with only 53% of Russia’scitizens believ<strong>in</strong>g that higher education is important (ibid). It seemed likely that givensome of the problems be<strong>in</strong>g experienced <strong>in</strong> the Russian higher education system, wherethe value of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> education may be <strong>in</strong> a weakened state, cheat<strong>in</strong>g could be commonplace.Substantial differences <strong>in</strong> academic honesty may also be found due <strong>to</strong> Russiabe<strong>in</strong>g a more collective society compared <strong>to</strong> the USA, which is more <strong>in</strong>dividualistic <strong>in</strong>culture (Ryan et al., 1991).Build<strong>in</strong>g on the research conducted <strong>in</strong> the USA, the researchers present a crossnationalstudy that compares attitudes, perceptions, <strong>and</strong> tendencies of college bus<strong>in</strong>essstudents <strong>in</strong> Russia <strong>and</strong> the USA. The research beg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong> fill <strong>in</strong> the gap <strong>in</strong> our knowledgeabout cross-national differences <strong>in</strong> attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards cheat<strong>in</strong>g.JustificationPurpose?METHODOLOGYMethod <strong>and</strong> SampleUndergraduate bus<strong>in</strong>ess students from the USA <strong>and</strong> Russia were asked <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> thestudy. Questionnaires were adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> the classes. Given the sensitive nature of thequestions, respondents were repeatedly <strong>to</strong>ld, orally <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, that their responseswould be anonymous <strong>and</strong> confidential. The respondents were asked <strong>to</strong> answer as manyquestions as possible, as long as they felt comfortable with the particular question.The American student sample was collected from Colorado State University, a midsizeduniversity located <strong>in</strong> the western USA, <strong>and</strong> the Russian sample was collected fromNovgorod State University <strong>and</strong> the Norman School College. Colorado State University islocated <strong>in</strong> Fort Coll<strong>in</strong>s, Colorado, a city of about 120,000 residents. Both Novgorod StateUniversity <strong>and</strong> the Norman School College are located <strong>in</strong> Novgorod, Russia, which has approximately200,000 <strong>in</strong>habitants. A <strong>to</strong>tal of 443 usable surveys were collected <strong>in</strong> the USA<strong>and</strong> 174 <strong>in</strong> Russia. Nearly 50% of the American students <strong>and</strong> 64% of the Russian studentswere male. In both regions, 90% of the sample was between the ages of 17 <strong>and</strong> 25, withan average age of 21 years. The average American grade-po<strong>in</strong>t average (GPA) was 3.02<strong>and</strong> 4.27 for the Russian students (U.S. GPA, A 4.0; Russian GPA, A 5.0). Fifty-two percen<strong>to</strong>f the American sample was juniors <strong>and</strong> 45.8% seniors. In contrast, 56.1% of theRussian survey respondents was freshmen, while sophomores <strong>and</strong> graduate students accountedfor 20.5% <strong>and</strong> 17.5% respectively.Volunteers?Nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampleLimitationSee InternalvalidityThe Survey InstrumentIdentical self-report questionnaires were used <strong>to</strong> collect the data <strong>in</strong> both countries. Thesurvey was translated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> Russian <strong>and</strong> translated back <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> English. To evaluate theattitudes, perceptions, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards academic cheat<strong>in</strong>g, a 29-question survey<strong>in</strong>strument was developed consist<strong>in</strong>g of a series of dicho<strong>to</strong>mous (yes/no) <strong>and</strong> scalar questions,as well as a question that asked students <strong>to</strong> assess what proportion of their peersthey believe cheat. Most of the yes/no questions specifically asked the students about<strong>How</strong> are thesedef<strong>in</strong>ed?


408 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7echeat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours (e.g., “Have you cheated dur<strong>in</strong>g college?” “Have you received <strong>in</strong>formationabout an exam from students <strong>in</strong> earlier sections of the class?”). In addition, studentswere asked <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> a series of statements us<strong>in</strong>g a seven-po<strong>in</strong>t scale anchoredwith Strongly disagree <strong>to</strong> Strongly agree. These scalar questions asked students abouttheir attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefs about cheat<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., “Cheat<strong>in</strong>g on one exam is really not thatbad. I believe tell<strong>in</strong>g someone <strong>in</strong> a later section about an exam you just <strong>to</strong>ok is OK”).Students were also given two scenarios <strong>and</strong> asked <strong>to</strong> decide whether cheat<strong>in</strong>g hadoccurred. Each scenario was <strong>in</strong>tentionally left rather vague. Hav<strong>in</strong>g the scenarios berather ambiguous meant that the student could not easily conclude that cheat<strong>in</strong>g hador had not occurred. In this fashion, students were left more <strong>to</strong> their own personal <strong>in</strong>terpretationsof try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> decide if cheat<strong>in</strong>g had or had not occurred. The first scenario(scenario A) was:John Doe <strong>to</strong>ok Market<strong>in</strong>g 400 <strong>in</strong> the fall semester. His friend, Jane, <strong>to</strong>ok Market<strong>in</strong>g400 <strong>in</strong> the spr<strong>in</strong>g semester. John gave Jane all his prior work from the course.Jane found John’s answers <strong>to</strong> prior exams <strong>and</strong> uses these <strong>to</strong> prepare for tests <strong>in</strong>the course.Students were then asked <strong>to</strong> decide if John <strong>and</strong> Jane had cheated. The next scenario(scenario B) was:Reliability <strong>and</strong>validity should bediscussedJane also discovered that John had received good grades on some written assignmentsfor the class. Many of these assignments required John <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> thelibrary <strong>to</strong> look up articles about various <strong>to</strong>pics. Jane decides <strong>to</strong> forgo the librarywork <strong>and</strong> uses John’s articles for her papers <strong>in</strong> the class.After read<strong>in</strong>g scenario B, students were asked <strong>to</strong> decide if Jane had cheated. F<strong>in</strong>ally,<strong>to</strong> account for possible confounds <strong>and</strong> explore <strong>in</strong>dividual level differences, the surveyalso <strong>in</strong>cluded some basic demographic questions.RESULTSAmerican <strong>and</strong> Russian Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Students’ Positions on Cheat<strong>in</strong>g BehavioursInappropriatestatisticSmall differences(see Table 1)American <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess students had significantly different positions on their selfreportedcheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours, on the degree <strong>to</strong> which they knew or saw otherscheat, <strong>and</strong> on their perception of whether or not cheat<strong>in</strong>g had occurred <strong>in</strong> the two casescenarios.Table 1 highlights the significant differences <strong>in</strong> self-reported cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviourbetween the American <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess students. A larger share of the Russian studentsreported cheat<strong>in</strong>g at some po<strong>in</strong>t. While about 55% of the American students reportedthey had cheated at some po<strong>in</strong>t dur<strong>in</strong>g college, nearly 64% of the Russian studentsreported hav<strong>in</strong>g cheated. Russian students also were much more likely <strong>to</strong> reportcheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the class <strong>in</strong> which the data were collected. In fact, only 2.9% of the Amercianstudents acknowledged cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the class where the data were collected, whereas38.1% of the Russian students admitted <strong>to</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the class. Additionally, Russian studentswere more likely <strong>to</strong> have reported that they knew or had seen a student who hadcheated. The percentage of students who had given or received <strong>in</strong>formation about anexam that had been adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> an earlier section was higher with Russian students.Nearly 92% of the Russian students admitted <strong>to</strong> convey<strong>in</strong>g exam <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> theirpeers <strong>in</strong> a later section, while 68.5% of the American students admitted do<strong>in</strong>g so. Americanstudents, however, reported a greater <strong>in</strong>cidence of us<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ations from a priorterm <strong>to</strong> study for current exams.


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 409TABLE 1Percentage of American or Russian Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Students Respond<strong>in</strong>g“Yes” <strong>to</strong> Questions about Cheat<strong>in</strong>gPercentage respond<strong>in</strong>g “yes”American students Russian studentsn 443 n 174Cheated at some po<strong>in</strong>t dur<strong>in</strong>g college 55.4 64.2***Cheated <strong>in</strong> current class 2.9 38.1*Know student who has cheated on an exam atthe university 77.3 80.9**Know student who has cheated on an exam <strong>in</strong>current class 6.3 66.9*Seen a student cheat on an exam at the university 61.3 72.4**Seen a student cheat on an exam <strong>in</strong> current class 5.6 63.2*Used exam answers from a prior term <strong>to</strong> study fora current exam 88.7 48.6*Given student <strong>in</strong> a later section <strong>in</strong>formation aboutan exam 68.5 91.9*Received exam <strong>in</strong>formation from a student <strong>in</strong> anearlier section 73.9 84.3**Scenario A: John cheated by giv<strong>in</strong>g Jane his past exams 5.2 49.1*Scenario A: Jane cheated by us<strong>in</strong>g John’s past exams 9.7 63.9*Scenario B: Jane cheated by us<strong>in</strong>g John’s articles 77.5 66.9***χ 2 test of differences between nationalities significant at p < 0.000.**c 2 test of differences between nationalities significant at p < 0.01.***c 2 test of differences between nationalities significant at p < 0.05.Appear <strong>to</strong> havecontent validityInappropriatestatisticAmerican <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess students also had very different impressions ofwhether or not cheat<strong>in</strong>g had occurred <strong>in</strong> the scenarios. In scenario A, the Russian studentswere much more likely <strong>to</strong> believe that John <strong>and</strong> Jane had cheated. For example,only 5.2% of the American students felt John had cheated by giv<strong>in</strong>g Jane his past exams,while 49.1% of the Russian students felt the same. Additionally, 9.7% of the Americanstudents compared <strong>to</strong> 63.9% of the Russian students felt Jane had cheated by us<strong>in</strong>gJohn’s past exams. <strong>How</strong>ever, <strong>in</strong> scenario B, a larger share of the American students feltJane had cheated by us<strong>in</strong>g John’s articles. These statistically significant <strong>and</strong> quite largedifferences <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations of the scenarios suggest that American <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>essstudents have extremely different perspectives of what is or is not cheat<strong>in</strong>g.American <strong>and</strong> Russian Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Students’ Differences <strong>in</strong> Beliefs About Cheat<strong>in</strong>gTable 2 reveals that American <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess students have significantly differentbeliefs about cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Students were asked <strong>to</strong> assess what proportion of their peersthey believed <strong>to</strong> cheat. Russian students felt that about 69% of their colleagues cheat onexams, while American students stated that they felt only about 24% of their fellow studentscheat. In a series of Strongly disagree/Strongly agree belief statements, the Russianstudents were more likely than the American students <strong>to</strong> believe that most studentscheat on exams <strong>and</strong> out-of-class assignments, that cheat<strong>in</strong>g on one exam is not so bad,<strong>and</strong> that it is OK <strong>to</strong> tell someone <strong>in</strong> a later section about an exam just completed. <strong>How</strong>ever,as revealed earlier, the Russian students seem <strong>to</strong> have a different position on whatis or is not cheat<strong>in</strong>g. The American students did not believe that giv<strong>in</strong>g someone past


410 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 2American <strong>and</strong> Russian Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Students’ Beliefs about Cheat<strong>in</strong>gAll appear <strong>to</strong> havecontent validityInappropriatestatisticAmerican RussianOverall students studentsmean n 443 n 174Percentage of students believed <strong>to</strong> cheat on exams 36.53 24.18 69.59*Most students cheat on exams 3.45 2.80 5.12*Most students cheat on out-of-class assignments 4.09 3.88 4.64*Cheat<strong>in</strong>g on one exam is not so bad 2.90 2.34 4.36*OK <strong>to</strong> tell someone <strong>in</strong> later section about an exam 4.71 4.07 6.36*Giv<strong>in</strong>g someone your past exams is cheat<strong>in</strong>g 2.26 2.02 2.87*Us<strong>in</strong>g an exam from a prior semester is cheat<strong>in</strong>g 2.65 2.23 3.02*Instruc<strong>to</strong>r must make sure students do not cheat 3.68 3.88 3.18*Instruc<strong>to</strong>r discuss<strong>in</strong>g issues tied <strong>to</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g reducesamount of cheat<strong>in</strong>g 3.92 4.27 3.01*Note: The first item <strong>in</strong> the table is a percentage (e.g., 36.53%). All other items are mean rat<strong>in</strong>gs us<strong>in</strong>g a sevenpo<strong>in</strong>tscale, where 1 Strongly disagree <strong>and</strong> 7 Strongly agree.*t test of mean differences between nationalities significant at p < 0.000.exams or us<strong>in</strong>g exams from a prior semester was cheat<strong>in</strong>g, while the Russian studentswere more neutral on the matter.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the students <strong>in</strong> each country were asked if they believed the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r is responsiblefor ensur<strong>in</strong>g that cheat<strong>in</strong>g does not occur, <strong>and</strong> if by discuss<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g-relatedissues (e.g., ethics, penalties, responsibilities), the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r can reduce cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cidents.The Russian students were less likely than the American students <strong>to</strong> feel that it is the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r’sresponsibility <strong>to</strong> prevent cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>and</strong> were less likely <strong>to</strong> believethat the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r merely discuss<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g-related issues would reduce cheat<strong>in</strong>g.Analysis of Possible ConfoundsInternal validityInappropriatestatistic?Although a number of differences were found based on nationality, it is possible thatthese differences may be due <strong>to</strong> some other issue. Past literature has suggested that anumber of idiosyncratic variables could <strong>in</strong>fluence the likelihood of someone cheat<strong>in</strong>g(e.g., Alschuler & Bliml<strong>in</strong>g, 1995; Bunn et al., 1992; Johnson & Gormly, 1971; Kelly &Worrell, 1978; McCabe & Trev<strong>in</strong>o, 1996; Stern & Havlicek, 1986; Stevens & Stevens, 1987).Therefore, analyses were conducted <strong>to</strong> check if expected grade <strong>in</strong> the course, overallgrade-po<strong>in</strong>t average, college class, gender, or age were hav<strong>in</strong>g any effects on the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> particular, if these fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>teracted with nationality. Of focal concern wasthe extent <strong>to</strong> which these fac<strong>to</strong>rs were <strong>in</strong>fluenc<strong>in</strong>g the number of students that hadreported cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Neither expected grade <strong>in</strong> the course, overall grade-po<strong>in</strong>t average,college class <strong>and</strong> gender, nor age <strong>in</strong>teracted with country. This effectively elim<strong>in</strong>ates thepossibility that they are confounds for the differences found due <strong>to</strong> nationality.CONCLUSIONNot sufficientThis is the first study <strong>to</strong> compare the attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards academicdishonesty of American <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess college students. The study reveals thatAmerican <strong>and</strong> Russian bus<strong>in</strong>ess students hold vastly different attitudes, perceptions, <strong>and</strong>tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards cheat<strong>in</strong>g. It was surpris<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d that Russian students reported muchhigher frequencies of cheat<strong>in</strong>g than their American counterparts. This raises the question:Do Russian students cheat more often than American students? In fact, we believe these


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 411higher self-reported cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviours likely reflect that the Russian students havevery different attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itions regard<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g when compared <strong>to</strong>the American students. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, a few of the questions <strong>and</strong> the answers givenwere unequivocal. The Russian students were much more likely <strong>to</strong> feel it was not so bad<strong>to</strong> cheat on one exam or tell someone <strong>in</strong> a later section about an exam. This may <strong>in</strong>dicatethat the Russians do not take academic dishonesty as seriously as the Americans <strong>and</strong>/orare more motivated <strong>to</strong> cheat. Of course, the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of why the differences existbetween the Russian <strong>and</strong> American students is multidimensional, <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g cultural nuances,societal values, teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> educational philosophies, just <strong>to</strong> name a few. A trueunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of why these differences exist, however, is beyond the scope of this paper,but certa<strong>in</strong>ly worthy of future research endeavours.Yet, educa<strong>to</strong>rs host<strong>in</strong>g foreign students locally <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g abroad need <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>the nuances <strong>and</strong> attitudes of different student populations <strong>and</strong> the associationwith classroom management. The better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g we have of if <strong>and</strong> how<strong>in</strong>ternational students’ attitudes, perceptions, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wards academic dishonestydiffer among countries, the greater the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs’ ability <strong>to</strong> communicatewith expatriate students <strong>and</strong> take actions <strong>to</strong> prevent cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Students from all countriescont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> enroll <strong>in</strong> colleges <strong>and</strong> universities around the world. Of the 1.5 millionstudents who study abroad, nearly one-third of these (481,280) studied <strong>in</strong> the USA(Chronicle of Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 1998). Universities also cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> send faculty abroad<strong>to</strong> teach around the world. Organizations such as the International Institute of <strong>Education</strong>(IIE), the Council for International <strong>Education</strong>al Exchange (CIEE), <strong>and</strong> the Agency forInternational Development (AID) encourage global education <strong>and</strong> resource exchangesabroad (Barron, 1993; Garavalia, 1997). Post-secondary bus<strong>in</strong>ess education has been<strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>to</strong> the former Soviet Union republics <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> East Asia, br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g Americanfaculty <strong>and</strong> resources <strong>to</strong> these regions (Fogel, 1994; Kerr, 1996; Kyj, Kyj, & Marshall,1995; Petkus, 1995). As the student body becomes more <strong>in</strong>ternational <strong>and</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly teach abroad, research of this nature becomes vital for effective classroommanagement.Effective classroom management <strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the predom<strong>in</strong>antnorms with<strong>in</strong> a country or region. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly part of the challenge that emerges forfaculty members is <strong>to</strong> assist students <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g what is or is not academic misconduct.Especially when teach<strong>in</strong>g abroad or <strong>in</strong> courses with a large mult<strong>in</strong>ational composition,the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r needs <strong>to</strong> clearly articulate <strong>to</strong> the students, orally <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g,what behaviours are or are not considered academic misconduct. Instruc<strong>to</strong>rs shouldeducate students on the virtues of not engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the penalties for cheat<strong>in</strong>g,with the hope that this will reduce <strong>in</strong>cidents of academic dishonesty. It should benoted, however, that while the American students felt neutral about the likelihood thatdiscuss<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g-related issues might reduce the degree of cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the course,the Russian students slightly disagreed. Additionally, the Russian students were more<strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed than the American students <strong>to</strong> feel it was not the responsibility of the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r<strong>to</strong> create an environment that reduces the likelihood that cheat<strong>in</strong>g could occur (e.g.,develop<strong>in</strong>g multiple versions of the same exam<strong>in</strong>ation, clean<strong>in</strong>g off desk<strong>to</strong>ps beforeexam<strong>in</strong>ations, arrang<strong>in</strong>g multiple proc<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> oversee the test period, not allow<strong>in</strong>gbathroom breaks).To this end, more research needs <strong>to</strong> be undertaken <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> fully underst<strong>and</strong> howstudents view cheat<strong>in</strong>g. In particular, a cross-national study that compares data from a varietyof diverse countries would greatly illum<strong>in</strong>ate the magnitude of differences that mayexist between countries. This research is the first step <strong>in</strong> highlight<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gthese differences.RightGoodrecommendationWe agree.Op<strong>in</strong>ion


412 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eReferencesAckerman, P. D. (1971). The efforts of honor grad<strong>in</strong>g onstudents’ test scores. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Journal, 8, 321–33.Alschuler, A. S., & Bliml<strong>in</strong>g, G. S. (1995). Curb<strong>in</strong>g epidemiccheat<strong>in</strong>g through systematic change. College Teach<strong>in</strong>g,43, 4, 23–125.Baird, J. S., Jr. (1980). Current trends <strong>in</strong> college cheat<strong>in</strong>g.Psychology <strong>in</strong> the Schools, 17, 4, 515–22.Barron, C. (1993). An Eastern education. Europe, 11, 331, 1–2.Baty, P. (1997). Prosper<strong>in</strong>g cheats on the up. Times Higher<strong>Education</strong> Supplement, 50, 3.Black, D. B. (1962). The falsification of reported exam<strong>in</strong>ationmarks <strong>in</strong> a senior university education course. Journal of<strong>Education</strong> Sociology, 35, 346–54.Brickman, W. W. (1961). Ethics, exam<strong>in</strong>ations, <strong>and</strong> education.School <strong>and</strong> Society, 89, 412–15.Bunn, D. N., Caudill, S. B., & Gropper, D. M. (1992). Crime <strong>in</strong>the classroom: An economic analysis of undergraduatestudent cheat<strong>in</strong>g behavior. Journal of Economic<strong>Education</strong>, 23, 197–207.Bushby, R. (1997). Internet essays cause degrees of concern.Times <strong>Education</strong>al Supplement, 42, 42, 3.Chidley, J. (1997). Tales out of school. Maclean’s, 76–9.Chronicle of Higher <strong>Education</strong>. (1998). Almanac issue, 45,1, 24.Chronicle of Higher <strong>Education</strong>. (2000). Russian universitieseducate world’s <strong>to</strong>p student programmers. Chronicleof Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 47, 8, A43–4.Collison, M. (1990). Apparent rise <strong>in</strong> students’ cheat<strong>in</strong>g hascollege officials worried. Chronicle of Higher <strong>Education</strong>,36, 34–5.Curry, A. (1997). Psst, got the answer? Many say yes.Christian Science Moni<strong>to</strong>r, 89, 157, 7.Curtis, J. (1996). Cheat<strong>in</strong>g—let’s face it. International SchoolsJournal, 15, 2, 37–44.Davis, S. F., Grover, C. A., Becker, A. H., & McGregor, L. N.(1992). Academic dishonesty: Prevalence determ<strong>in</strong>ants,techniques, <strong>and</strong> punishments. Teach<strong>in</strong>g of Psychology,19, 1, 16–20.Davis, S. F., Noble, L. M., Zak, E. N., & Dreyer, K. K. (1994).A comparison of cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g: Grade orientation<strong>in</strong> American <strong>and</strong> Australian college students. CollegeStudent Journal, 28, 353–6.Diekhoff, G. M., Labeff, E. E., Sh<strong>in</strong>ohara, K., & Yasukawa, H.(1999). College cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Japan <strong>and</strong> the United States.<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 40, 3, 343–53.Dolshenko, L. (1999). The college student <strong>to</strong>day: A socialportrait <strong>and</strong> attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward school<strong>in</strong>g. Russian SocialScience Review, 40, 5, 73–83.Evans, E. D., Craig, D., & Mietzel, G. (1993). Adolescents’cognitions <strong>and</strong> attributions for academic cheat<strong>in</strong>g: Across-national study. Journal of Psychology, 127, 6,585–602.Fogel, D. S. (1994). Manag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Emerg<strong>in</strong>g Market Economies.Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Franklyn-S<strong>to</strong>kes, A., & Newstead, S. E. (1995). Undergraduatecheat<strong>in</strong>g: Who does what <strong>and</strong> why? Studies <strong>in</strong> Higher<strong>Education</strong>, 20, 2, 159–72.Frary, R. B., Tideman, T. N., & Nicholaus, T. (1997).Comparison of two <strong>in</strong>dices of answer copy<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>development of a spliced <strong>in</strong>dex. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>and</strong>Psychological Measurement, 57, 1, 20–32.Frary, R. B., Tideman, T. N., & Watts, T. M. (1977). Indices ofcheat<strong>in</strong>g on multiple-choice tests. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>alStatistics, 2, 4, 235–56.Gail, T., & Bor<strong>in</strong>, N. (1988). Cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> academe. Journal of<strong>Education</strong> for Bus<strong>in</strong>ess, 63, 4, 153–7.Garavalia, B. J. (1997). International education: <strong>How</strong> it isdef<strong>in</strong>ed by US students <strong>and</strong> foreign students. Clear<strong>in</strong>gHouse, 70, 4, 215–23.Genereux, R. L., & Mcleod, B. A. (1995). Circumstancessurround<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g: A questionnaire study for collegestudents. <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 36, 6, 687–704.Hanisch, G. (1990). Cheat<strong>in</strong>g: Results of question<strong>in</strong>gViennese pupils. Vienna: Ludwig Boltzmann Institutefur Schulentwicklung und International VergleichendeSchulforschung.Hardy, R. J. (1981–1982). Prevent<strong>in</strong>g academic dishonesty:Some important tips for political science professors.Teach<strong>in</strong>g Political Science, 9, 68–77.Harpp, D. N., & Hogan, S. J. (1993). Detection <strong>and</strong>prevention of cheat<strong>in</strong>g on multiple-choice exams.Journal of Chemical <strong>Education</strong>, 70, 4, 306–10.Harpp, D. N., & Hogan, S. J. (1998). The case of the ultimateidentical tw<strong>in</strong>. Journal of Chemical <strong>Education</strong>, 75, 4, 482–5.Jendrek, M. P. (1989). Faculty reaction <strong>to</strong> academic dishonesty.Journal of College Student Development, 30, 3, 401–6.Jenk<strong>in</strong>son, M. (1996). If you can’t beat ’em, cheat. AlbertaReport, 23, 42, 36–7.Johnson, C. D., & Gormly, J. (1971). Achievement, sociability<strong>and</strong> task importance <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> academic cheat<strong>in</strong>g.Psychological Reports, 28, 302.


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 413Kelly, J. A., & Worrell, L. (1978). Personality characteristics,parent behaviors, <strong>and</strong> sex of the subject <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong>cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Personality, 12, 179–88.Kerr, W. A. (1996). Market<strong>in</strong>g education for Russianmarket<strong>in</strong>g educa<strong>to</strong>rs. Journal of Market<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Education</strong>,19, 3, 39–49.Kyj, L. S., Kyj, M. J., & Marshall, P. S. (1995). Internationalizationof American bus<strong>in</strong>ess programs: Case study Ukra<strong>in</strong>e.Bus<strong>in</strong>ess Horizon, 38, 55–9.Labeff, E. E., Clark, R. E., Ha<strong>in</strong>es, V. J., & Dickhoff, G. M.(1990). Situational ethics <strong>and</strong> college student cheat<strong>in</strong>g.Sociological Inquiry, 60, 2, 190–8.Lord, T., & Chiodo, D. (1995). A look at student cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>college science classes. Journal of Science <strong>and</strong>Technology, 4, 4, 317–24.Lup<strong>to</strong>n, R. A. (1999). Measur<strong>in</strong>g bus<strong>in</strong>ess students’ attitudes,perceptions, <strong>and</strong> tendencies about cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> CentralEurope <strong>and</strong> the USA. ProQuest (dissertation).Lup<strong>to</strong>n, R. A., Chapman, K., & Weiss, J. (2000). American<strong>and</strong> Slovakian university bus<strong>in</strong>ess students’ attitudes,perceptions, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>ward academic cheat<strong>in</strong>g.Journal of <strong>Education</strong> for Bus<strong>in</strong>ess, 75, 4, 231–41.McCabe, D. L., & Bowers, W. J. (1994). Academic dishonestyamong males <strong>in</strong> college: A thirty-year perspective. Journalof College Student Development, 35, 1, 5–10.McCabe, D. L., & Bowers, W. J. (1996). The relationshipbetween student cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> college fraternity orsorority membership. NASPA Journal, 33, 4, 280–91.McCabe, D. L., & Trev<strong>in</strong>o, L. K. (1996). What we know aboutcheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> college. Change, 28, 1, 29–33.Mackenzie, R., & Smith, A. (1995). Do medical studentscheat? Student BMJ, 3, 212.Maslen, G. (1996). Cheats with pagers <strong>and</strong> cordless radiocribs. Times <strong>Education</strong>al Supplement, 4186, 16.Newstead, S. E., Franklyn-S<strong>to</strong>kes, A., & Armstead, P. (1996).Individual differences <strong>in</strong> student cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of<strong>Education</strong>al Psychology, 88, 2, 229–41.Oaks, H. (1975). Cheat<strong>in</strong>g attitudes <strong>and</strong> practices at twostate colleges. Improv<strong>in</strong>g College <strong>and</strong> University Teach<strong>in</strong>g,23, 4, 232–5.Paldy, L. G. (1996). The problems that won’t go away:Address<strong>in</strong>g the causes of cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Journal of CollegeScience Teach<strong>in</strong>g, 26, 1, 4–7.Payne, S. L., & Nantz, K. S. (1994). Social accounts <strong>and</strong>metaphors about cheat<strong>in</strong>g. College Teach<strong>in</strong>g, 42, 3, 90–6.Petkus, E., Jr. (1995). Open for remodel<strong>in</strong>g: Boise State helpsprepare Vietnam’s MBA faculty of the future. Change, 27,64–7.Pol<strong>to</strong>rak, Y. (1995). Cheat<strong>in</strong>g behavior among students offour Moscow <strong>in</strong>stitutes. Higher <strong>Education</strong>, 30, 2, 225–46.Roberts, R. N. (1986). Public university response <strong>to</strong> academicdishonesty: Discipl<strong>in</strong>ary or academic? Journal of Law <strong>and</strong><strong>Education</strong>, 15, 4, 371–84.Rost, D. H., & Wild, K. P. (1990). Academic cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>avoidance of achievement: Components <strong>and</strong> conceptions.Zeitschrift fur Pedagogische Psychologie, 4, 13–27.Ryan, R. M., Chirkov, V. I., Little, T. D., Sheldon, K. M.,Timosh<strong>in</strong>a, E., & Deci, E. L. (1991). The American dream<strong>in</strong> Russia: Extr<strong>in</strong>sic aspirations <strong>and</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> twocultures. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 25,12, 1509–24.Stern, E. B., & Havlicek, L. (1986). Academic misconduct:Results of faculty <strong>and</strong> undergraduate student surveys.Journal of Allied Health, 5, 129–42.Stevens, G. E., & Stevens, F. W. (1987). Ethical <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ations of<strong>to</strong>morrow’s managers revisited: <strong>How</strong> <strong>and</strong> why studentscheat. Journal of <strong>Education</strong> for Bus<strong>in</strong>ess, 63, 24–9.Surkes, S. (1994). Cheat at exams <strong>and</strong> risk go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> prison.Times <strong>Education</strong>al Supplement, 4068, 18.Times <strong>Education</strong>al Supplement. (1996). In brief: Italy. TimesEduc. Suppl., 4187, 16, 27 September.Waugh, R. F., & Godfrey, J. R. (1994). Measur<strong>in</strong>g students’perceptions about cheat<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong>Perspectives, 21, 2, 28–37.Waugh, R. F., Godfrey, J. R., Evans, E. D., & Craig, D. (1995).Measur<strong>in</strong>g students’ perceptions about cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> sixcountries. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 2, 73–82.


414 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is not explicitly stated. It appears <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>“fill <strong>in</strong> the gap <strong>in</strong> our knowledge about cross-nationaldifferences <strong>in</strong> attitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> tendencies <strong>to</strong>wardscheat<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>and</strong>, more specifically, <strong>to</strong> compare collegebus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> Russia <strong>and</strong> the United States onthese characteristics.The study is justified by cit<strong>in</strong>g both evidence <strong>and</strong>op<strong>in</strong>ion that cheat<strong>in</strong>g is widespread <strong>in</strong> the United States<strong>and</strong>, presumably (although with less documentation),worldwide. Additional justification <strong>in</strong>cludes the unfairnessof cheat<strong>in</strong>g, the likelihood of cheat<strong>in</strong>g carry<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>future life, <strong>and</strong> (<strong>in</strong> the discussion) the need for teachers<strong>in</strong> mult<strong>in</strong>ational classes <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the issues <strong>in</strong>volved.The importance of attitudes <strong>and</strong> perceptionsseems <strong>to</strong> be taken for granted; the only justification forstudy<strong>in</strong>g them is implied <strong>in</strong> the results of the three studiesthat found differences between American students<strong>and</strong> those <strong>in</strong> other countries. We th<strong>in</strong>k a stronger justificationcould <strong>and</strong> should have been made. The f<strong>in</strong>al justificationis that there have been few such studies, nonewith bus<strong>in</strong>ess students <strong>in</strong> Russia <strong>and</strong> the United States.The authors’ concern about confidentiality is important,both with regard <strong>to</strong> ethics <strong>and</strong> the validity of <strong>in</strong>formation;they appear <strong>to</strong> have addressed it as effectively as possible.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems of risk or deception.DEFINITIONSDef<strong>in</strong>itions are not provided <strong>and</strong> would be very helpful(as discussed below under “Instrumentation”) becausethe terms attitude, values, <strong>and</strong> beliefs, especially, havemany different mean<strong>in</strong>gs. The term tendencies appears<strong>to</strong> mean (from the example items) actual cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>various forms. Some clarity is provided by partial operationaldef<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>in</strong> the form of example items. Weth<strong>in</strong>k a def<strong>in</strong>ition of cheat<strong>in</strong>g should have been provided<strong>to</strong> readers <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> respondents. Based on the itemsprovided, it appears <strong>to</strong> be someth<strong>in</strong>g like “receiv<strong>in</strong>gcredit for work that is not one’s own.”PRIOR RESEARCHThe authors provide extensive citation of evidence <strong>and</strong>summaries of studies on the extent of college-levelcheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> on cross-national comparisons. They givegood brief summaries of what they state are the onlythree directly related studies.HYPOTHESESNo hypothese are stated. A nondirectional hypothesis isclearly implied—i.e., there will be differences betweenthe two groups.SAMPLEThe two groups are convenience (<strong>and</strong> possibly volunteer)samples from the two nations. Each is describedwith respect <strong>to</strong> location, gender, age, <strong>and</strong> academicclass. They consist only of bus<strong>in</strong>ess students, who maynot be representative of all college students. Representativenessis further compromised by the unreportednumber of “unusable” surveys. Sample numbers (443<strong>and</strong> 174) are acceptable.INSTRUMENTATIONThe questionnaire consists of yes-no questions (twobased on brief scenarios) <strong>to</strong> measure “tendencies” <strong>and</strong>seven-po<strong>in</strong>t rat<strong>in</strong>g scales <strong>to</strong> assess attitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefsabout cheat<strong>in</strong>g, for a <strong>to</strong>tal of 29 items, of which 21 areshown <strong>in</strong> the report. Neither reliability nor validity isdiscussed. Because the <strong>in</strong>tent was <strong>to</strong> compare groupson <strong>in</strong>dividual items, no summary scores were used.Nevertheless, consistency of response <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualitems is essential <strong>to</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gful results. Though admittedlydifficult, the procedure followed <strong>in</strong> the K<strong>in</strong>seystudy (see page 395) of ask<strong>in</strong>g the same question withdifferent word<strong>in</strong>g might have been used with, at least, asubsample of students <strong>and</strong> items. Similarly, a comparisonof the questionnaire with <strong>in</strong>terview responses <strong>to</strong> thesame content would have provided some evidence ofvalidity.The question of validity is confused by the lack ofclear def<strong>in</strong>itions. The items <strong>in</strong> Table 1 suggest that“tendencies <strong>to</strong> cheat” is taken <strong>to</strong> mean “hav<strong>in</strong>g cheatedor known of others cheat<strong>in</strong>g,” although the two scenarioitems seem <strong>to</strong> be ask<strong>in</strong>g what is considered <strong>to</strong>constitute cheat<strong>in</strong>g. Attitudes <strong>and</strong> perceptions are comb<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> Table 2 as “beliefs,” which seem <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludeboth “op<strong>in</strong>ions about the extent of cheat<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>and</strong> “judgmentsas <strong>to</strong> what behaviors are acceptable”—as well aswhat constitutes <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r responsibility. As such, theitems appear <strong>to</strong> have content validity but omit other behaviors,such as destroy<strong>in</strong>g required library read<strong>in</strong>gs.This does not <strong>in</strong>validate the items used unless they areconsidered <strong>to</strong> represent all forms of cheat<strong>in</strong>g. F<strong>in</strong>ally,the validity of self-report items cannot be assumed,particularly <strong>in</strong> cross-cultural studies, where mean<strong>in</strong>gsmay differ.


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 415PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYIf the study is <strong>in</strong>tended simply <strong>to</strong> describe differences,<strong>in</strong>ternal validity is not an issue. If, however, results areused <strong>to</strong> imply causation, alternative explanations fornationality-caus<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>g must be considered. Theauthors are <strong>to</strong> be commended for address<strong>in</strong>g this problem.They report that “neither expected grade <strong>in</strong> thecourse, overall grade-po<strong>in</strong>t-average, college class <strong>and</strong>gender, nor age <strong>in</strong>teracted with country,” thus elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>gthese alternative explanations. It appears, however,that this conclusion may be based on a f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g of no significantdifferences us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>appropriate statistics as discussedunder “Data Analysis” below. The demographicdata on gender <strong>and</strong> academic class <strong>in</strong>dicate substantialdifferences between groups.The authors po<strong>in</strong>t out that other variables such asteach<strong>in</strong>g philosophy <strong>and</strong> societal values may provide abetter underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, but these do not weaken the nationalityexplanation—they clarify it. A variable thatmight well weaken the nationality explanation is “f<strong>in</strong>ancialstatus.” If it is related <strong>to</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> if the Russian<strong>and</strong> U.S. students differed on this variable, the nationality<strong>in</strong>terpretation may be seriously mislead<strong>in</strong>g. Perhapscheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviors <strong>and</strong> beliefs are both highly <strong>in</strong>fluencedby how much money one has.DATA ANALYSISThe descriptive statistics are appropriate, but the <strong>in</strong>ferentialstatistics (t-test <strong>and</strong> Chi square) are not. The samplesare not r<strong>and</strong>om nor arguably representative of anydef<strong>in</strong>ed populations. The appropriate basis for assess<strong>in</strong>gdifferences is direct comparison of percentages <strong>and</strong>means, perhaps augmented with a calculation of effectsize for means (see page 244).Exam<strong>in</strong>ation of Table 1 shows that it does not requirethe <strong>in</strong>correct significance tests <strong>to</strong> show important differencesbetween groups on some items—on the order of2.9 versus 38.1 percent <strong>and</strong> 6.3 versus 66.9 percent.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, the difference between 77.3 <strong>and</strong>80.9 percent is trivial, despite the significance level of.01. While the level of difference that is important is arguable,we would attach importance only <strong>to</strong> differencesof at least 15 percent. This is the case with seven of thetwelve comparisons.With respect <strong>to</strong> Table 2, we can, <strong>in</strong> the absence ofdata, obta<strong>in</strong> a rough estimate of the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationof each distribution of rat<strong>in</strong>gs as 1.5 (estimated range =7 1 6; 4 st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations 95 percent of cases[see page 196]; therefore the estimated st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationis 6 4 1.5). Therefore, an effect size of .75would meet the cus<strong>to</strong>mary .50 requirement. All but oneof the n<strong>in</strong>e comparisons reach this value; three greatlyexceed it—they should receive the most attention.The written results are consistent with Tables 1 <strong>and</strong> 2<strong>and</strong> generally emphasize the larger differences; we disagreeonly with the attention given <strong>to</strong> small differences.DISCUSSION/INTERPRETATIONWe agree that the study suggests large <strong>and</strong> importantdifferences between the Russian <strong>and</strong> U.S. students regard<strong>in</strong>gcheat<strong>in</strong>g. Our only quibble with the discussionof results is with the statement that Russian studentswere more <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> feel it was not the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r’s responsibility<strong>to</strong> create an environment <strong>to</strong> reduce cheat<strong>in</strong>g—true,but the difference is small.The authors’ discussion places the study <strong>in</strong> a broadercontext <strong>and</strong> makes sensible recommendations, some ofwhich follow directly from the results <strong>and</strong> some ofwhich do not—i.e., “<strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs should educate studentson the virtues of not engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> cheat<strong>in</strong>g.”The authors should have discussed the serious limitationson generaliz<strong>in</strong>g their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. These <strong>in</strong>clude aseriously limited sample <strong>and</strong> the lack of evidence ofquestionnaire validity. Their statement that “In fact, webelieve these higher self-reported cheat<strong>in</strong>g behaviourslikely reflect that the Russian students have very differentattitudes, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itions regard<strong>in</strong>g cheat<strong>in</strong>gwhen compared <strong>to</strong> the American students”—a statemen<strong>to</strong>f belief—is not sufficient.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED Learn<strong>in</strong>g feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


416 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMa<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsMAJOR CHARACTERISTICS OF SURVEY RESEARCHMost surveys possess three basic characteristics: (1) the collection of <strong>in</strong>formation(2) from a sample (3) by ask<strong>in</strong>g questions, <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> describe some aspects of thepopulation of which the sample is a part.THE PURPOSE OF SURVEY RESEARCHThe major purpose of all surveys is <strong>to</strong> describe the characteristics of a population.Rarely is the population as a whole studied, however. Instead, a sample is surveyed<strong>and</strong> a description of the population is <strong>in</strong>ferred from what the sample reveals.TYPES OF SURVEYSThere are two major types of surveys: cross-sectional surveys <strong>and</strong> longitud<strong>in</strong>al surveys.Three longitud<strong>in</strong>al designs commonly employed <strong>in</strong> survey research are trend studies,cohort studies, <strong>and</strong> panel studies.In a trend study, different samples from a population whose members change are surveyedat different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time.In a cohort study, different samples from a population whose members do not changeare surveyed at different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time.In a panel study, the same sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals is surveyed at different times overthe course of the survey.Surveys are not suitable for all research <strong>to</strong>pics, especially those that require observationof subjects or the manipulation of variables.STEPS IN SURVEY RESEARCHThe focus of study <strong>in</strong> a survey is called the unit of analysis.As <strong>in</strong> other types of research, the group of persons that is the focus of the study iscalled the target population.There are four basic ways <strong>to</strong> collect data <strong>in</strong> a survey: by direct adm<strong>in</strong>istration of thesurvey <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> a group, by mail, by telephone, or by personal <strong>in</strong>terview. Eachmethod has advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages.The sample <strong>to</strong> be surveyed should be selected r<strong>and</strong>omly if possible.The most common types of <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> survey research are the questionnaire<strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview schedule.QUESTIONS ASKED IN SURVEY RESEARCHThe nature of the questions, <strong>and</strong> the way they are asked, are extremely important <strong>in</strong>survey research.Most surveys use some form of closed-ended question.The survey <strong>in</strong>strument should be pretested with a small sample similar <strong>to</strong> the potentialrespondents.Acont<strong>in</strong>gency question is a question whose answer is cont<strong>in</strong>gent upon how a respondentanswers a prior question <strong>to</strong> which the cont<strong>in</strong>gency question is related. Well-organized<strong>and</strong> sequenced cont<strong>in</strong>gency questions are particularly important <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview schedules.THE COVER LETTERA cover letter is sent <strong>to</strong> potential respondents <strong>in</strong> a mail survey expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the purposeof the survey questionnaire.


CHAPTER 17 Survey <strong>Research</strong> 417INTERVIEWINGBoth telephone <strong>and</strong> face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviewers need <strong>to</strong> be tra<strong>in</strong>ed before they adm<strong>in</strong>isterthe survey <strong>in</strong>strument.Both <strong>to</strong>tal nonresponse <strong>and</strong> item nonresponse are major problems <strong>in</strong> survey researchthat seem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> recent years. This is a problem because those who donot respond are very likely <strong>to</strong> differ from respondents <strong>in</strong> terms of how they would answerthe survey questions.THREATS TO INTERNAL VALIDITY IN SURVEY RESEARCHThreats <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of survey research <strong>in</strong>clude location, <strong>in</strong>strumentation,<strong>in</strong>strument decay, <strong>and</strong> mortality.DATA ANALYSIS IN SURVEY RESEARCHThe percentage of the <strong>to</strong>tal sample respond<strong>in</strong>g for each item on a survey questionnaireshould be reported, as well as the percentage of the <strong>to</strong>tal sample who choseeach alternative for each question.census 391closed-endedquestion 396cohort study 391cont<strong>in</strong>gency question 398cross-sectionalsurvey 391<strong>in</strong>terview schedule 395longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey 391nonresponse 401open-endedquestion 396panel study 391trend study 391unit of analysis 392Key Terms1. For what k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>to</strong>pics might a personal <strong>in</strong>terview be superior <strong>to</strong> a mail or telephonesurvey? Give an example.2. When might a telephone survey be preferable <strong>to</strong> a mail survey? <strong>to</strong> a personal <strong>in</strong>terview?3. Give an example of a question a researcher might use <strong>to</strong> assess each of the follow<strong>in</strong>gcharacteristics of the members of a teacher group:a. Their <strong>in</strong>comeb. Their teach<strong>in</strong>g stylec. Their biggest worryd. Their knowledge of teach<strong>in</strong>g methodse. Their op<strong>in</strong>ions about homogeneous group<strong>in</strong>g of students4. Which mode of data collection—mail, telephone, or personal <strong>in</strong>terview—would bebest for each of the follow<strong>in</strong>g surveys?a. The reasons why some students drop out of college before they graduateb. The feel<strong>in</strong>gs of high school teachers about special classes for the giftedc. Theattitudesofpeopleaboutrais<strong>in</strong>gtaxes<strong>to</strong>payfortheconstructionofnewschoolsd. The duties of secondary school super<strong>in</strong>tendents <strong>in</strong> a midwestern statee. The reasons why <strong>in</strong>dividuals of differ<strong>in</strong>g ethnicity did or did not decide <strong>to</strong> enterthe teach<strong>in</strong>g professionf. The op<strong>in</strong>ions of teachers about the idea of m<strong>in</strong>imum competency test<strong>in</strong>g beforegrant<strong>in</strong>g permanent tenureg. The op<strong>in</strong>ions of parents of students <strong>in</strong> a private school about the elim<strong>in</strong>ation ofcerta<strong>in</strong> subjects from the curriculum5. Some researchers argue that conduct<strong>in</strong>g a careful cross-sectional survey of the populationof the United States would actually be preferable <strong>to</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g a census of theFor Discussion


418 PART 4 Quantitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7epopulation every ten years. What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? What might be some arguments for<strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong>st this idea?6. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be the hardest type of longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey <strong>to</strong> conduct—trend, cohort, or panel? the easiest? Expla<strong>in</strong> your reason<strong>in</strong>g.7. Why do you th<strong>in</strong>k many people do not respond <strong>to</strong> survey questionnaires that theyreceive <strong>in</strong> the mail?8. Are there any questions that researchers could not survey people about through themail? by telephone? personal <strong>in</strong>terview? Expla<strong>in</strong>.9. When conduct<strong>in</strong>g a personal <strong>in</strong>terview, when might it be better <strong>to</strong> ask a closedendedrather than an open-ended question? What about the reverse? Suggest someexamples.10. See if you can suggest a question that you believe almost anyone would be sure <strong>to</strong>answer if asked. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of any they would be sure not <strong>to</strong> answer? Why?11. What suggestions can you offer, beyond those given <strong>in</strong> this chapter, for improv<strong>in</strong>gthe rate of response <strong>in</strong> surveys?Notes1. J. L. Blaga <strong>and</strong> L. E. Nielsen (1983). The status of state his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong>struction. Journal of Social Studies<strong>Research</strong>, 7(1): 45–57.2. J. J. Blase (1987). Dimensions of effective school leadership: The teacher’s perspective. American <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> Journal, 24(4): 589–610.3. J. Tlou <strong>and</strong> C. Bennett (1983). Teacher perceptions of discipl<strong>in</strong>e problems <strong>in</strong> a central Virg<strong>in</strong>ia middleschool. Journal of Social Studies <strong>Research</strong>, 7(2): 37–59.4. C. I. Chase (1985). Two thous<strong>and</strong> teachers view their profession. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 79(1):12–18.5. J. Raths, M. Wojtaszek-Healy, <strong>and</strong> C. Kubo Della-Plana (1987). Grad<strong>in</strong>g problems: A matter of communication.Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 80(3): 133–137.6. M. A. Sheppard, M. S. Goodstadt, <strong>and</strong> M. M. Willett (1987). Peers or parents: Who has the most <strong>in</strong>fluenceon cannabis use? Journal of Drug <strong>Education</strong>, 17(2): 123–128.7. A. Weaver Hart (1987). A career ladder’s effect on teacher career <strong>and</strong> work attitudes. American <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> Journal, 24(4): 479–504.8. B. Herlihy, M. Healy, E. Piel Cook, <strong>and</strong> P. Hudson (1987). Ethical practices of licensed professionalcounselors: A survey of state licens<strong>in</strong>g boards. Counselor <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> Supervision, September: 69–76.9. R. M. Jaeger (1988). Survey research methods <strong>in</strong> education. In Richard M. Jaeger (ed.), Complementarymethods for research <strong>in</strong> education. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC:American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Association, pp. 308–310.10. R. M. Grovers <strong>and</strong> R. L. Kahn (1979). Surveys by telephone: A national comparison with personal <strong>in</strong>terviews.New York: Academic Press.11. F. J. Fowler, Jr. (1984). Survey research methods. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, p. 101.12. The development of survey questions is an art <strong>in</strong> itself. We can only beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> deal with the <strong>to</strong>pic here.For a more detailed discussion, see J. M. Converse <strong>and</strong> S. Presser (1986). Survey questions: H<strong>and</strong>craft<strong>in</strong>gthe st<strong>and</strong>ardized questionnaire. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.13. For further suggestions, see N. E. Gronlund (1988). <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> construct achievement tests. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.14. E. S. Babbie (1973). Survey research methods. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, p. 145.15. For a more detailed discussion, see Fowler, op. cit., Chapter 7.16. Ibid., pp. 109–110.17. P. Freyberg <strong>and</strong> R. Osborne (1981). Who structures the curriculum: Teacher or learner? <strong>Research</strong>Information for Teachers, Number Two. SET, Hamil<strong>to</strong>n, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>.18. G. Kal<strong>to</strong>n (1983). Introduction <strong>to</strong> survey sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, p. 64.19. Ibid., p. 66.20. Ibid., p. 67.


Introduction <strong>to</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>P A R T5Part 5 beg<strong>in</strong>s our discussion of qualitative research. We devote a separate chapter <strong>to</strong>the nature of qualitative research <strong>and</strong> follow it with two chapters on the ma<strong>in</strong>techniques that qualitative researchers use <strong>to</strong> collect <strong>and</strong> analyze their data. These<strong>in</strong>clude observation, <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> content analysis. We provide some examplesof published studies <strong>in</strong> which researchers use these techniques, along with our analysisof the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses of their <strong>in</strong>vestigations.


18The Natureof Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>What Is Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>?General Characteristicsof Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>PhilosophicalAssumptions Underly<strong>in</strong>gQualitative as Opposed<strong>to</strong> Quantitative<strong>Research</strong>PostmodernismSteps <strong>in</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>Approaches <strong>to</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Narrative <strong>Research</strong>PhenomenologyGrounded TheoryCase StudiesEthnographic <strong>and</strong>His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>Generalization <strong>in</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Ethics <strong>and</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>Qualitative <strong>and</strong>Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>Reconsidered"You quantitative"You qualitativeresearchers are soresearchers are sopreoccupied with controlsubjective, you never can bethat you never see thesure what you've foundbig picture!"or where it applies!""Why don't youtwo try <strong>to</strong> work<strong>to</strong>gether? You wouldthen have the best ofboth worlds!"OBJECTIVESExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“qualitative research.”Describe five general characteristics thatmost qualitative studies have <strong>in</strong> common.Describe briefly the philosophicassumptions underly<strong>in</strong>g qualitative<strong>and</strong> quantitative research.Describe briefly some of the steps<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> qualitative research.Describe at least three ways thatqualitative research differs fromquantitative research.Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly at least four differentapproaches <strong>to</strong> qualitative research.Describe the type of samples that areused <strong>in</strong> qualitative research, <strong>and</strong> givesome examples of these types.Expla<strong>in</strong> how generaliz<strong>in</strong>g differs <strong>in</strong>qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative research.Describe briefly how matters of ethicsaffect qualitative research.Suggest some ways that qualitative <strong>and</strong>quantitative approaches <strong>to</strong> researchmight be used <strong>to</strong>gether.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 18.1: Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 18.2: Qualitative vs. Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>Activity 18.3: Approaches <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Hey, Brendan.”“Oh, hi, Melissa. Where’ve you been?”“I just came from my research class. We’re just start<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> learn about qualitative research.”“What’s that?”“Well, sometimes a researcher wants <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>-depth look at a particular <strong>in</strong>dividual, say, or a specificsituation. Maybe even a particular set of <strong>in</strong>structional materials.”“Yeah?”“When they do, they ask some <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g questions. Instead of ask<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g like ‘What do people th<strong>in</strong>kabout this?’ or ‘What would happen if I do this?’ qualitative researchers ask, ‘<strong>How</strong> do these people act?’ or ‘<strong>How</strong>are th<strong>in</strong>gs done?’ or ‘<strong>How</strong> do people give mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> their lives?’”“<strong>How</strong> come?”“Because what they want <strong>to</strong> get at is some idea of the quality of the experiences that people have.”“Sounds different. Tell me more.”We shall <strong>in</strong>deed “tell Brendan (<strong>and</strong> you) more” about qualitative research. The nature of qualitative research, <strong>and</strong>how it differs from quantitative research, is what this chapter is all about.What Is Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>?Most of the questions be<strong>in</strong>g asked by researchers whouse the methodologies discussed <strong>in</strong> previous chapters <strong>in</strong>volvethe extent <strong>to</strong> which various learn<strong>in</strong>gs, attitudes, orideas exist, or how well or how accurately they are be<strong>in</strong>gdeveloped. Thus, possible avenues of research <strong>in</strong>cludecomparisons between alternative methods of teach<strong>in</strong>g(as <strong>in</strong> experimental research); exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g researchamong variables (as <strong>in</strong> correlational relationships); compar<strong>in</strong>ggroups of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> terms of exist<strong>in</strong>g differenceson certa<strong>in</strong> variables (as <strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research);or <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g different groups of educationalprofessionals, such as teachers, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong>counselors (as <strong>in</strong> survey research). These methods arefrequently referred <strong>to</strong> as quantitative research.As we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 1, however, researchersmight wish <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a more holistic impression ofteach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g than answers <strong>to</strong> the above questionscan provide. A researcher might wish <strong>to</strong> knowmore than just “<strong>to</strong> what extent” or “how well” someth<strong>in</strong>gis done. He or she might wish <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a morecomplete picture, for example, of what goes on <strong>in</strong> a particularclassroom or school.Consider the teach<strong>in</strong>g of his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> secondaryschools. Just how do his<strong>to</strong>ry teachers teach their subject?What k<strong>in</strong>ds of th<strong>in</strong>gs do they do as they go about theirdaily rout<strong>in</strong>e? What sorts of th<strong>in</strong>gs do students do? Inwhat k<strong>in</strong>ds of activities do they engage? What are the explicit<strong>and</strong> implicit “rules of the game” <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry classesthat seem <strong>to</strong> help or h<strong>in</strong>der the process of learn<strong>in</strong>g?To ga<strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> these concerns, a researchermight try <strong>to</strong> document or portray the everyday421


422 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 18.1 Quantitative Versus Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Quantitative MethodologiesQualitative MethodologiesPreference for precise hypotheses stated at the outset.Preference for precise def<strong>in</strong>itions stated at the outset.Data reduced <strong>to</strong> numerical scores.Much attention <strong>to</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> improv<strong>in</strong>g reliabilityof scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed from <strong>in</strong>struments.Assessment of validity through a variety of procedureswith reliance on statistical <strong>in</strong>dices.Preference for r<strong>and</strong>om techniques for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gmean<strong>in</strong>gful samples.Preference for precisely describ<strong>in</strong>g procedures.Preference for design or statistical control of extraneousvariables.Preference for specific design control for procedural bias.Preference for statistical summary of results.Preference for break<strong>in</strong>g down complex phenomena <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>specific parts for analysis.Will<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> manipulate aspects, situations, or conditions<strong>in</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g complex phenomena.Preference for hypotheses that emerge as study develops.Preference for def<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>in</strong> context or as study progresses.Preference for narrative description.Preference for assum<strong>in</strong>g that reliability of <strong>in</strong>ferences isadequate.Assessment of validity through cross-check<strong>in</strong>g sources of<strong>in</strong>formation (triangulation).Preference for expert <strong>in</strong>formant (purposive) samples.Preference for narrative/literary descriptions of procedures.Preference for logical analysis <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g or account<strong>in</strong>gfor extraneous variables.Primary reliance on researcher <strong>to</strong> deal with procedural bias.Preference for narrative summary of results.Preference for holistic description of complex phenomena.Unwill<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> tamper with naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>gphenomena.experiences of students (<strong>and</strong> teachers) <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry classrooms.The focus would be on only one classroom (or asmall number of them at most). The researcher wouldobserve the classroom on as regular a basis as possible<strong>and</strong> attempt <strong>to</strong> describe, as fully <strong>and</strong> as richly as possible,what he or she sees.The above example po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>to</strong> the fact that many researchersare more <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the quality of a particularactivity than <strong>in</strong> how often it occurs or how it wouldotherwise be evaluated. <strong>Research</strong> studies that <strong>in</strong>vestigatethe quality of relationships, activities, situations, ormaterials are frequently referred <strong>to</strong> as qualitativeresearch. This type of research differs from themethodologies discussed <strong>in</strong> earlier chapters <strong>in</strong> that thereis a greater emphasis on holistic description—that is, ondescrib<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> detail all of what goes on <strong>in</strong> a particularactivity or situation rather than on compar<strong>in</strong>g the effectsof a particular treatment (as <strong>in</strong> experimental research),say, or on describ<strong>in</strong>g the attitudes or behaviors of people(as <strong>in</strong> survey research).We believe that educational research <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly is,<strong>and</strong> should be, a mixture of quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativeapproaches, (We’ll discuss this <strong>in</strong> more detail later <strong>in</strong>the chapter.) <strong>How</strong>ever, <strong>to</strong> assist you <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe many types of research that exist, we list the essentialdifferences between quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research<strong>in</strong> Table 18.1.General Characteristicsof Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Many different types of qualitative methodologies exist,but there are certa<strong>in</strong> general features that characterizemost qualitative research studies. Not all qualitativestudies will necessarily display all of these characteristicswith equal strength. Nevertheless, taken <strong>to</strong>gether,they give a good overall picture of what is <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>this type of research. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen describe fivesuch features. 11. The natural sett<strong>in</strong>g is the direct source of data, <strong>and</strong>the researcher is the key <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> qualitativeresearch. Qualitative researchers go directly <strong>to</strong> theparticular sett<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> observe <strong>and</strong> collect theirdata. They spend a considerable amount of time actuallybe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a school, sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> on faculty meet<strong>in</strong>gs, attend<strong>in</strong>gparent-teacher association meet<strong>in</strong>gs, observ<strong>in</strong>gteachers <strong>in</strong> their classrooms <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> other locales,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> general directly observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualsas they go about their daily rout<strong>in</strong>es.Sometimes they come equipped only with a pad<strong>and</strong> a pencil <strong>to</strong> take notes, but often they use sophisticatedaudio- <strong>and</strong> videotap<strong>in</strong>g equipment. Evenwhen such equipment is used, however, the data are


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 423collected right at the scene <strong>and</strong> supplemented by theresearcher’s observations <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>sights about wha<strong>to</strong>ccurred. As Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen po<strong>in</strong>t out, qualitativeresearchers go <strong>to</strong> the particular sett<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>terestbecause they are concerned with context—theyfeel that activities can best be unders<strong>to</strong>od <strong>in</strong> the actualsett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> which they occur. They also feel thathuman behavior is vastly <strong>in</strong>fluenced by particularsett<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong>, hence, whenever possible they visitsuch sett<strong>in</strong>gs.2. Qualitative data are collected <strong>in</strong> the form of words orpictures rather than numbers. The k<strong>in</strong>ds of data collected<strong>in</strong> qualitative research <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong>terview transcripts,field notes, pho<strong>to</strong>graphs, audio record<strong>in</strong>gs,videotapes, diaries, personal comments, memos, officialrecords, textbook passages, <strong>and</strong> anyth<strong>in</strong>g elsethat can convey the actual words or actions of people.In their search for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, qualitative researchersdo not usually attempt <strong>to</strong> reduce their data<strong>to</strong> numerical symbols, 2 but rather seek <strong>to</strong> portraywhat they have observed <strong>and</strong> recorded <strong>in</strong> all of itsrichness. Hence, they do their best not <strong>to</strong> ignore anyth<strong>in</strong>gthat might lend <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>to</strong> a situation. Gestures,jokes, conversational gambits, artwork or other decorations<strong>in</strong> a room—all are noted by qualitative researchers.To a qualitative researcher, no data aretrivial or unworthy of notice.3. Qualitative researchers are concerned with process aswell as product. Qualitative researchers are especially<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> how th<strong>in</strong>gs occur. Hence, they are likely<strong>to</strong> observe how people <strong>in</strong>teract with each other; howcerta<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions are answered; the mean<strong>in</strong>gsthat people give <strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> words <strong>and</strong> actions; howpeople’s attitudes are translated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> actions; how studentsseem <strong>to</strong> be affected by a teacher’s manner, gestures,or comments; <strong>and</strong> the like.4. Qualitative researchers tend <strong>to</strong> analyze their data<strong>in</strong>ductively. Qualitative researchers do not, usually,formulate a hypothesis beforeh<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> then seek <strong>to</strong>test it out. Rather, they tend <strong>to</strong> “play it as it goes.”They spend a considerable amount of time collect<strong>in</strong>gtheir data (aga<strong>in</strong>, primarily through observ<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g) before they decide what are theimportant questions <strong>to</strong> consider. As Bogdan <strong>and</strong>Biklen suggest, qualitative researchers are notputt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>gether a puzzle whose picture they alreadyknow. They are construct<strong>in</strong>g a picture that takesshape as they collect <strong>and</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e the parts. 35. <strong>How</strong> people make sense out of their lives is a majorconcern <strong>to</strong> qualitative researchers. A special <strong>in</strong>teres<strong>to</strong>f qualitative researchers lies <strong>in</strong> the perspectives ofthe subjects of a study. Qualitative researchers want<strong>to</strong> know what the participants <strong>in</strong> a study are th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> why they th<strong>in</strong>k what they do. Assumptions, motives,reasons, goals, <strong>and</strong> values—all are of <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>and</strong> likely <strong>to</strong> be the focus of the researcher’s questions.It also is common for a researcher <strong>to</strong> show acompleted videotape or the contents of his or hernotes <strong>to</strong> a participant <strong>to</strong> check on the accuracy of theresearcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations. In other words, the researcherdoes his or her best <strong>to</strong> capture the th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gof the participants from the participants’ perspective(as opposed <strong>to</strong> the researcher merely report<strong>in</strong>g whathe or she th<strong>in</strong>ks) as accurately as possible.Table 18.2 presents a summary of the ma<strong>in</strong> characteristicsof qualitative research.Philosophical AssumptionsUnderly<strong>in</strong>g Qualitativeas Opposed <strong>to</strong> Quantitative<strong>Research</strong>Differences between quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativeresearchers are often discussed <strong>in</strong> terms of differ<strong>in</strong>gparadigms, or worldviews—that is, differences <strong>in</strong> thebasic set of beliefs or assumptions that guide the waythey approach their <strong>in</strong>vestigations. These assumptionsare related <strong>to</strong> the views they hold concern<strong>in</strong>g the natureof reality, the relationship of the researcher <strong>to</strong> that whichhe or she is study<strong>in</strong>g, the role of values <strong>in</strong> a study, <strong>and</strong>the process of research itself.The quantitative approach is associated with the philosophyof positivism, which emerged <strong>in</strong> the n<strong>in</strong>eteenthcentury. Perhaps the person most responsible for thedevelopment <strong>and</strong> spread of this philosophy was AugusteComte (1798–1857). In 1824 he wrote, “I believe that Ishall succeed <strong>in</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g it recognized . . . that there arelaws as well-def<strong>in</strong>ed for the development of the humanspecies as for the fall of a s<strong>to</strong>ne.” 4 Comte argued that the“positive” stage of human knowledge is reached whenpeople beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> rely on empirical data, reason, <strong>and</strong> thedevelopment of scientific laws <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> phenomena.The scientific method, positivists believe, is the surestway <strong>to</strong> produce effective knowledge.Although positivism has changed somewhat over theyears, a basic premise is that there exists a reality “out


424 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 18.2Major Characteristics of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>1. Naturalistic <strong>in</strong>quiry2. Inductive analysis3. Holistic perspective4. Qualitative data5. Personal contact <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>sight6. Dynamic systems7. Unique caseorientation8. Context sensitivity9. Empathic neutrality10. <strong>Design</strong> flexibilityStudy<strong>in</strong>g real-world situations as they unfold naturally; nonmanipulative, unobtrusive,<strong>and</strong> noncontroll<strong>in</strong>g; openness <strong>to</strong> whatever emerges—lack of predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed constra<strong>in</strong>ts onoutcomes.Immersion <strong>in</strong> the details <strong>and</strong> specifics of the data <strong>to</strong> discover important categories,dimensions, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terrelationships; beg<strong>in</strong> by explor<strong>in</strong>g genu<strong>in</strong>ely open questions ratherthan test<strong>in</strong>g theoretically derived (deductive) hypotheses.The whole phenomenon under study is unders<strong>to</strong>od as a complex system that is more thanthe sum of its parts; focus is on complex <strong>in</strong>terdependencies not mean<strong>in</strong>gfully reduced <strong>to</strong>a few discrete variables <strong>and</strong> l<strong>in</strong>ear, cause-effect relationships.Detailed, thick description; <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>in</strong> depth; direct quotations captur<strong>in</strong>g people’spersonal perspectives <strong>and</strong> experiences.The researcher has direct contact with <strong>and</strong> gets close <strong>to</strong> the people, situation, <strong>and</strong>phenomenon under study; researcher’s personal experiences <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>sights are animportant part of the <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> critical <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the phenomenon.Attention <strong>to</strong> process; assumes change is constant <strong>and</strong> ongo<strong>in</strong>g whether the focus is on an<strong>in</strong>dividual or an entire culture.Assumes each case is special <strong>and</strong> unique; the first level of <strong>in</strong>quiry is be<strong>in</strong>g true <strong>to</strong>,respect<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> captur<strong>in</strong>g the details of the <strong>in</strong>dividual cases be<strong>in</strong>g studied; cross-caseanalysis follows from <strong>and</strong> depends on the quality of <strong>in</strong>dividual case studies.Places f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a social, his<strong>to</strong>rical, <strong>and</strong> temporal context; dubious of the possibility ormean<strong>in</strong>gfulness of generalizations across time <strong>and</strong> space.Complete objectivity is impossible; pure subjectivity underm<strong>in</strong>es credibility; theresearcher‘s passion is underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the world <strong>in</strong> all its complexity—not prov<strong>in</strong>gsometh<strong>in</strong>g, not advocat<strong>in</strong>g, not advanc<strong>in</strong>g personal agendas, but underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g; theresearcher <strong>in</strong>cludes personal experience <strong>and</strong> empathic <strong>in</strong>sight as part of the relevant data,while tak<strong>in</strong>g a neutral nonjudgmental stance <strong>to</strong>ward whatever content may emerge.Open <strong>to</strong> adapt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>quiry as underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g deepens <strong>and</strong>/or situations change; avoidsgett<strong>in</strong>g locked <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> rigid designs that elim<strong>in</strong>ate responsiveness; pursues new paths ofdiscovery as they emerge.Source: Qualitative research <strong>and</strong> evaluation methods, by Michael Qu<strong>in</strong>n Pat<strong>to</strong>n. Copyright © 2008 by Sage Publications Inc. Books. Reproducedwith permission of Sage Publications Inc. Books <strong>in</strong> the textbook format via Copyright Clearance Center.there,” <strong>in</strong>dependent of us, wait<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be discovered, thatis driven by stable natural laws. The task of science is <strong>to</strong>discover the nature of this reality <strong>and</strong> how it works. Arelated emphasis is on break<strong>in</strong>g complex phenomenadown <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> manageable pieces for study <strong>and</strong> eventualreassembly <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the whole. The researcher’s role is tha<strong>to</strong>f a “dis<strong>in</strong>terested scientist,” st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g apart from thatwhich is be<strong>in</strong>g studied, with his or her biases <strong>and</strong> valuesexcluded through experimental design <strong>and</strong> control.Challenges <strong>to</strong> the philosophy of positivism havecome from many directions <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> be debated.In general, qualitative researchers are sympathetic <strong>to</strong> theissues raised by critical researchers that we described <strong>in</strong>Chapter 1, <strong>and</strong> they present their methods as an alternative<strong>to</strong> the quantitative approach. Many of them advocatea more “artistic,” as opposed <strong>to</strong> a “scientific,” approach<strong>to</strong> research. Further, their goals are oftendifferent; this is illustrated by the preference of some forfoster<strong>in</strong>g multiple <strong>in</strong>terpretations of events, depend<strong>in</strong>gon how they are perceived by the <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>volved.This is the opposite of what almost all physical scientists(<strong>and</strong> most social scientists) advocate.Table 18.3 reveals the basic differences betweenthe two approaches with regard <strong>to</strong> these philosophicassumptions.PostmodernismRecently, a number of scholars have begun <strong>to</strong> questionwhether research (<strong>and</strong> educational research <strong>in</strong> particular)can really contribute <strong>to</strong> an underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of humanbehavior. These scholars, usually referred <strong>to</strong> aspostmodernists, criticize the relevance of ma<strong>in</strong>stream


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 425TABLE 18.3 Differ<strong>in</strong>g Philosophical Assumptions of Quantitative <strong>and</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>ersAssumptions of Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>ersAssumptions of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>ersThere exists a reality “out there,” <strong>in</strong>dependent of us, wait<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> be known. The task of science is <strong>to</strong> discover the natureof reality <strong>and</strong> how it works.<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigations can potentially result <strong>in</strong> accuratestatements about the way the world really is.It is possible for the researcher <strong>to</strong> remove himself orherself—<strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong> apart—from that which is be<strong>in</strong>g researched.Facts st<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependent of the knower <strong>and</strong> can be known<strong>in</strong> an undis<strong>to</strong>rted way.Facts <strong>and</strong> values are dist<strong>in</strong>ct from one another.The proper design of research <strong>in</strong>vestigations will lead <strong>to</strong>accurate conclusions about the nature of the world.The purpose of educational research is <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> beable <strong>to</strong> predict relationships. The ultimate goal is thedevelopment of laws that make prediction possible.The <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the research situationconstruct reality; thus, realities exist <strong>in</strong> the form ofmultiple mental constructions.<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigations produce alternative visions of whatthe world is like.It is impossible for the researcher <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong> apart from the<strong>in</strong>dividuals he or she is study<strong>in</strong>g.Values are an <strong>in</strong>tegral part of the research process.Facts <strong>and</strong> values are <strong>in</strong>extricably <strong>in</strong>tertw<strong>in</strong>ed.The <strong>in</strong>itial ambiguity that occurs <strong>in</strong> a study is desirable.The purpose of educational research is an underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g ofwhat th<strong>in</strong>gs mean <strong>to</strong> others. Highly generalizable “laws,” assuch, can never be found.research as we have described it <strong>in</strong> many of the chapters<strong>in</strong> this text. They present an even more <strong>in</strong>tensive critiqueof such research, <strong>in</strong> fact, than do the critical researcherswe described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 1.Postmodernists offer a number of criticisms of traditionalresearch, but perhaps the most common are these:First, they deny the existence of underly<strong>in</strong>g structures(e.g., mean<strong>in</strong>g, laws) <strong>in</strong> the doma<strong>in</strong> of social behavior.Foucault, <strong>in</strong> fact, argues that all knowledge <strong>and</strong> truth areproducts of his<strong>to</strong>ry, power, <strong>and</strong> social <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>and</strong>,hence, cannot be “discovered,” as positivists, for example,believe. 5 Second, they argue that all naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g(i.e., nonmathematical) languages are <strong>in</strong>evitablymade up of ambiguous terms that change over time <strong>and</strong>that, therefore, all statements that use these languagescannot be verified. 6 Postmodernism has had an impac<strong>to</strong>n all <strong>in</strong>tellectual discipl<strong>in</strong>es, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gdiscussion of its implications for educational research.What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Can “truth” be verified? Or is it“a product of his<strong>to</strong>ry, power, <strong>and</strong> social <strong>in</strong>terests” aspostmodernists claim?Steps <strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>The steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a qualitative researchstudy are not as dist<strong>in</strong>ct as they are <strong>in</strong> quantitative research;they often overlap <strong>and</strong> are sometimes even conductedconcurrently. Every qualitative study has a dist<strong>in</strong>ctstart<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> end<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t, however. It beg<strong>in</strong>s whenthe researcher identifies the phenomenon he or shewishes <strong>to</strong> study, <strong>and</strong> it ends when the researcher drawshis or her f<strong>in</strong>al conclusions.Although the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> qualitative researchare not as dist<strong>in</strong>ct as they are <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies(they aren’t even necessarily sequential), several stepscan be identified. Let us describe them briefly.1. Identification of the phenomenon <strong>to</strong> be studied. Beforeany study can beg<strong>in</strong>, the researcher must identifythe particular phenomenon he or she is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g. Suppose, for example, a researcherwishes <strong>to</strong> conduct a study <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the <strong>in</strong>teractionbetween m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>and</strong> nonm<strong>in</strong>ority students <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school. The phenomenon of <strong>in</strong>teresthere is student <strong>in</strong>teraction, specifically <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>nercityschool. Admittedly, this is a rather general <strong>to</strong>pic,but it does provide a start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t from which the researchercan proceed. Stated as a research question,the researcher might ask: “To what extent <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>what ways do m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>and</strong> nonm<strong>in</strong>ority students <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school <strong>in</strong>teract?”Such a question suggests what are known asforeshadowed problems. All qualitative studiesbeg<strong>in</strong> with such problems—they are ak<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> theoverall statement of the problem that we discussed<strong>in</strong> Chapter 2. They give the researcher someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>look for. They should not be considered restrictiveor limit<strong>in</strong>g, however, s<strong>in</strong>ce their purpose is <strong>to</strong> providedirection, <strong>to</strong> serve as a guide. For example, asthe <strong>in</strong>vestigation of the question mentioned aboveproceeds, it may become evident that extracurricularas well as <strong>in</strong>-school activities need <strong>to</strong> be looked at,


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHClarity <strong>and</strong> PostmodernismAre the concepts <strong>and</strong> language that postmodernists useunnecessarily difficult <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>? Such criticisms arefrequently made not only by education students, but by othersas well. Jones, for example, argues that this difficulty resultsfrom the fact that most students lack his<strong>to</strong>rical exposure <strong>to</strong> thecontext of the issues.* Constas believes that proponents ofpostmodernism <strong>in</strong> educational research need <strong>to</strong> provide betterclarification† <strong>and</strong> (as paraphrased by Pillow) that sometheorists “exhibit symp<strong>to</strong>ms such as speak<strong>in</strong>g gibberish, whileothers w<strong>and</strong>er aimlessly (<strong>and</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>glessly), <strong>and</strong> still others*A. Jones (1997). Teach<strong>in</strong>g post-structuralist fem<strong>in</strong>ist theory <strong>in</strong>education: Student resistances. Gender <strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong>, 9(3): 266–269.†M. A. Constas (1998). Decipher<strong>in</strong>g postmodern educationalresearch. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 27(9): 36–42.are <strong>in</strong> a state of paralysis.”‡ Lather counters that the search forclarity is part of the “humanist romance of knowledge as cure”<strong>and</strong> that the role of postmodernists is <strong>to</strong> question “taken-forgrantedstructures of <strong>in</strong>telligibility.”§ These would <strong>in</strong>cludesuch concepts as “truth,” “progress,” “rationality,” “gender,”<strong>and</strong> “race.”Pillow argues that Constas misses the po<strong>in</strong>t. “Why, aroundquestions of postmodernism’s <strong>in</strong>fluence on educationalresearch, are we still pursu<strong>in</strong>g questions of truth <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>telligibility?Perhaps what we need more of are examples of what postmodernresearch looks like, does, <strong>and</strong> is committed <strong>to</strong>. Thatis, less about attempts <strong>to</strong> conta<strong>in</strong> what it is, <strong>and</strong> more work<strong>in</strong>gexamples of what it may be.” ||‡W. S. Pillow (2000). Decipher<strong>in</strong>g attempts <strong>to</strong> decipher postmoderneducational research. <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>er, 29 (June–July): 21–24.§P. Lather (1996). Troubl<strong>in</strong>g clarity: The politics of accessiblelanguage. Harvard <strong>Education</strong>al Review 66(3): 525–554.|| Pillow (2000), p. 23.so the k<strong>in</strong>ds of participation by students <strong>in</strong> such activitieswould be observed <strong>and</strong> analyzed. Foreshadowedproblems are often reformulated several timesdur<strong>in</strong>g the course of a qualitative study.2. Identification of the participants <strong>in</strong> the study. Theparticipants <strong>in</strong> the study constitute the sample of <strong>in</strong>dividualswho will be observed (<strong>in</strong>terviewed, etc.)—<strong>in</strong> other words, the subjects of the study. In almostall qualitative research, the sample is a purposivesample (see Chapter 6). R<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g ord<strong>in</strong>arilyis not feasible, s<strong>in</strong>ce the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> ensurethat he or she obta<strong>in</strong>s a sample that is uniquelysuited <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tent of the study. In the current example,<strong>in</strong>ner-city high school students are the subjectsof <strong>in</strong>terest, but not just any group of such studentswill do. They must be found <strong>in</strong> a particular <strong>in</strong>ner-cityhigh school or schools.3. Generation of hypotheses. Unlike <strong>in</strong> most quantitativestudies, hypotheses are not posed at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gof the study by the researcher. Instead, theyemerge from the data as the study progresses. Someare almost immediately discarded; others are modifiedor replaced. New ones are formulated. A typicalqualitative study may beg<strong>in</strong> with few, if any, hypothesesbe<strong>in</strong>g posed by the researcher at the start, but withseveral be<strong>in</strong>g formulated, reconsidered, dropped, <strong>and</strong>modified as the study proceeds. In the current example,a researcher might hypothesize orig<strong>in</strong>ally that<strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school betweenm<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>and</strong> nonm<strong>in</strong>ority students, outside of dailyclass sessions, will be m<strong>in</strong>imal. As he or she observesthe daily go<strong>in</strong>gs-on <strong>in</strong> the school, the hypothesis maybe modified a number of times as the researcherbecomes more aware of times <strong>and</strong> places where thestudents actually do <strong>in</strong>teract fairly regularly.4. Data collection. There is no “treatment” <strong>in</strong> a qualitativestudy, nor is there any “manipulation” of subjects.The participants <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study are notdivided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> groups, with one group be<strong>in</strong>g exposed <strong>to</strong>a treatment of some sort <strong>and</strong> the effects of this treatmentthen measured <strong>in</strong> some way. Data are not collectedat the “end” of the study. Rather, the collectionof data <strong>in</strong> a qualitative research study is ongo<strong>in</strong>g. Theresearcher is cont<strong>in</strong>ually observ<strong>in</strong>g people, events,<strong>and</strong> occurrences, often supplement<strong>in</strong>g his or her observationswith <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews of selected participants<strong>and</strong> the exam<strong>in</strong>ation of various documents<strong>and</strong> records relevant <strong>to</strong> the phenomenon of <strong>in</strong>terest.5. Data analysis. Analyz<strong>in</strong>g the data <strong>in</strong> a qualitativestudy essentially <strong>in</strong>volves analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> synthesiz<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>formation the researcher obta<strong>in</strong>s fromvarious sources (e.g., observations, <strong>in</strong>terviews, documents)<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a coherent description of what he or shehas observed or otherwise discovered. Hypothesesare not usually tested by means of <strong>in</strong>ferential statisticalprocedures, as is the case with experimental orassociational research, although some statistics,such as percentages, may be calculated if it appears426


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 427(A quantitativeperspective)“Wow! I’llbet that’s over420 feet.”(A qualitativeperspective)“Yeah! Poetry <strong>in</strong>motion. That guy’s <strong>in</strong>a class by himself.”400'Figure 18.1 <strong>How</strong> Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative <strong>Research</strong>ers See the Worldthey can illum<strong>in</strong>ate specific details about thephenomenon under <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Data analysis <strong>in</strong>qualitative research, however, relies heavily ondescription; even when certa<strong>in</strong> statistics are calculated,they tend <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>in</strong> a descriptive ratherthan an <strong>in</strong>ferential sense (Figure 18.1). We shall discussthe collection <strong>and</strong> analysis of data <strong>in</strong> qualitativeresearch <strong>in</strong> some detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 19.6. Interpretations <strong>and</strong> conclusions. In qualitative research,<strong>in</strong>terpretations are made cont<strong>in</strong>uously throughoutthe course of a study. Whereas quantitativeresearchers usually leave the draw<strong>in</strong>g of conclusions <strong>to</strong>the very end of their research, qualitative researcherstend <strong>to</strong> formulate their <strong>in</strong>terpretations as they go along.As a result, one f<strong>in</strong>ds the researcher’s conclusions <strong>in</strong> aqualitative study more or less <strong>in</strong>tegrated with othersteps <strong>in</strong> the research process. A qualitative researcherwho is observ<strong>in</strong>g the ongo<strong>in</strong>g activities of an <strong>in</strong>ner-cityclassroom, for example, is likely <strong>to</strong> write up not onlywhat he or she sees each day but also his or her <strong>in</strong>terpretationsof those observations.Approaches <strong>to</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>One f<strong>in</strong>ds a number of approaches <strong>to</strong> qualitative research.Creswell, for example, has identified five, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gnarrative research, phenomenology, grounded theory,case studies, <strong>and</strong> ethnography. 7 Although these five by nomeans exhaust the variety of approaches that exist, we<strong>in</strong>clude them here because: (1) they are frequently seen“<strong>in</strong> the social, behavioral, <strong>and</strong> health sciences literature”8 ; <strong>and</strong> (2) they have “systematic procedures for<strong>in</strong>quiry.” 9 To this list of approaches, we would addhis<strong>to</strong>rical research. Although it is possible <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d twoor more variations or comb<strong>in</strong>ations of these approacheswith<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle study, we separate <strong>and</strong> describe themhere as “pure” approaches <strong>to</strong> research design <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong>simplify underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Let us present a brief descriptionof each.NARRATIVE RESEARCHNarrative research is the study of the life experiences ofan <strong>in</strong>dividual as <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> the researcher or found <strong>in</strong> documents<strong>and</strong> archival material. An important aspect of somenarrative research is that the participant recalls one ormore special events (an “epiphany”) <strong>in</strong> his or her life. Theresearcher <strong>in</strong> narrative research describes, <strong>in</strong> some detail,the sett<strong>in</strong>g or context with<strong>in</strong> which the epiphany occurred.Lastly, the researcher is actively present dur<strong>in</strong>gthe study <strong>and</strong> openly acknowledges that his or her reportis an <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the participant’s experiences.Different forms of narrative research exist. “Abiographical study is a form of narrative study <strong>in</strong> whichthe researcher writes <strong>and</strong> records the experiences ofanother person’s life. Au<strong>to</strong>biography is written <strong>and</strong>recorded by the <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are the subject of


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHPortraiture: Art, Science, or Both?Portraiture is a recent variation on biography. It first appeared<strong>in</strong> 1983 <strong>in</strong> a book by Lawrence-Lightfoot entitledThe Good High School: Portraits of Character <strong>and</strong> Culture.*It won the Outst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g Book Award from the American<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Association <strong>in</strong> 1984. Its dist<strong>in</strong>ctive featureis that the researcher plays an avowedly <strong>in</strong>teractive rolewith the person be<strong>in</strong>g portrayed. In a subsequent book,Lawrence-Lightfoot <strong>and</strong> her coauthor, Hoffman-Davis, arguedthat portraiture meets the criteria <strong>to</strong> be considered ascience.† They described the process as follows: “They (theportraitist <strong>and</strong> the person be<strong>in</strong>g described) both express theirviews <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>gether def<strong>in</strong>e mean<strong>in</strong>g-mak<strong>in</strong>g,” that is, gett<strong>in</strong>gthe essence of the subject. Although the portraitist’s “soulechoes through the piece,” she or he works very hard not <strong>to</strong>simply produce a self-portrait.”‡The controversy <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance is not about the value ofthe method <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g powerful <strong>and</strong> useful results; that wasdemonstrated <strong>in</strong> The Good High School, but rather, whetherthe method can be considered “scientific,” as its proponentsargue. Portraitists cannot, of course, claim generalization beyondthe subject(s) portrayed; they can adopt the position onlythat generalization is left <strong>to</strong> the reader.Like all biographers, portraitists can claim only that otherresearchers would arrive at essentially the same descriptions<strong>and</strong> conclusions as they have. The nature of the <strong>in</strong>teraction betweenthe portraitist <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual be<strong>in</strong>g portrayed makestriangulation virtually impossible—there is no other sourcefor the portraitist <strong>to</strong> check his or her descriptions with. Englishhas argued that the “objective of portraiture <strong>to</strong> capture the‘essence’ of the subject is implicitly a quest for a stable truthwhich, <strong>in</strong> turn, requires the portraitist <strong>to</strong> become omniscient.”He argues, further, that “the claim that the reader of portraiturecan construct his or her own <strong>in</strong>terpretations from ‘thickdescription’ ignores the complete dependence of the reader ona f<strong>in</strong>ished product from which there can be no <strong>in</strong>dependentaccess <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong> alternative explanations” of thatwhich has been described by the portraitist.§What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Is portraiture science?*Sara Lawrence-Lightfoot (1983). The good high school: Portraitsof character <strong>and</strong> culture. New York: Basic Books.†Sara Lawrence-Lightfoot <strong>and</strong> J. Hoffman-Davis (1997). The art<strong>and</strong> science of portraiture. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.‡Ibid., pp. 103, 105.§Fenwick W. English (2000). A critical appraisal of Sara Lawrence-Lightfoot’s portraiture as a method of educational research. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 29: 21.the study (Ellis, 2004). A life his<strong>to</strong>ry portrays an<strong>in</strong>dividual’s entire life, while a personal experience s<strong>to</strong>ryis a narrative study of an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s personal experiencesfound <strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle or multiple episodes, private situations,or communal folklore (Denz<strong>in</strong>, 1989a). An oralhis<strong>to</strong>ry consists of gather<strong>in</strong>g personal reflections ofevents <strong>and</strong> their causes <strong>and</strong> effects from one <strong>in</strong>dividual orseveral <strong>in</strong>dividuals (Plummer, 1983).” 10Narrative research is not easy <strong>to</strong> do, for a number ofreasons:1. The researcher must collect an extensive amount of<strong>in</strong>formation about his or her participant.2. The researcher must have a clear underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g ofthe his<strong>to</strong>rical period with<strong>in</strong> which the participantlived <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> position the participant accuratelywith<strong>in</strong> that period.3. The researcher needs a “sharp eye” <strong>to</strong> uncover thevarious aspects of the participant’s life.4. The researcher needs <strong>to</strong> be reflective about his orher own personal <strong>and</strong> political background, whichmay shape how the participant’s s<strong>to</strong>ry is <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>and</strong>unders<strong>to</strong>od. 11In sum, then, the authors of narrative research focuson a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual, often describe special or importantevents <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual’s life, place the <strong>in</strong>dividualwith<strong>in</strong> a his<strong>to</strong>rical context, <strong>and</strong> try <strong>to</strong> place themselves<strong>in</strong> the research by acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g that the research istheir <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the participant’s life.PHENOMENOLOGYA researcher undertak<strong>in</strong>g a phenomenological study<strong>in</strong>vestigates various reactions <strong>to</strong>, or perceptions of, aparticular phenomenon (e.g., the experience of teachers<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school). The researcher hopes <strong>to</strong>ga<strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the world of his or her participants<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> describe their perceptions <strong>and</strong> reactions (e.g.,what it is like <strong>to</strong> teach <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school). Dataare usually collected through <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g. Theresearcher then attempts <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>and</strong> describe aspects428


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 429of each <strong>in</strong>dividual’s perceptions <strong>and</strong> reactions <strong>to</strong> his orher experience <strong>in</strong> some detail.Phenomenologists generally assume that there issome commonality <strong>to</strong> how human be<strong>in</strong>gs perceive <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret similar experiences; they seek <strong>to</strong> identify, underst<strong>and</strong>,<strong>and</strong> describe these commonalities. This commonalityof perception is referred <strong>to</strong> as the essence—theessential characteristic(s)—of the experience. It is theessential structure of a phenomenon that researcherswant <strong>to</strong> identity <strong>and</strong> describe. They do so by study<strong>in</strong>gmultiple perceptions of the phenomenon as experiencedby different people, <strong>and</strong> by then try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>ewhat is common <strong>to</strong> these perceptions <strong>and</strong> reactions.This search<strong>in</strong>g for the essence of an experience is thecorners<strong>to</strong>ne—the def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g characteristic—of phenomenologicalresearch.Here are some examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>to</strong>pics thatmight serve as the focus for a phenomenological study.<strong>Research</strong>ers might explore the experiences of:African American students <strong>in</strong> a predom<strong>in</strong>antly whitehigh schoolTeachers who have used the <strong>in</strong>quiry approach <strong>in</strong>teach<strong>in</strong>g n<strong>in</strong>th-grade social studiesCivil rights workers <strong>in</strong> the South dur<strong>in</strong>g the 1960sNurses who work <strong>in</strong> the operat<strong>in</strong>g room of a largemedical centerLike narrative research, phenomenological studiesare not easy <strong>to</strong> do. The researcher must get the participants<strong>in</strong> a phenomenological study <strong>to</strong> relive <strong>in</strong> theirm<strong>in</strong>ds the experiences they have had. Often, a numberof tape-recorded <strong>in</strong>terview sessions are necessary. Oncethe <strong>in</strong>terview process is completed, the researcher mustsearch through each participant’s statements for thosethat are especially relevant—those that appear <strong>to</strong> be particularlymean<strong>in</strong>gful <strong>to</strong> the participant <strong>in</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g hisor her experience <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> the phenomenon of<strong>in</strong>terest. The researcher then clusters these statements<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> themes, those aspects of the participants’ experiencesthat they had <strong>in</strong> common. The researcher then attempts<strong>to</strong> describe the fundamental features of the experiencethat have been described by most (ideally, all) ofthe participants <strong>in</strong> the study.In sum, then, researchers who conduct phenomenologicalstudies search for the “essential structure” of as<strong>in</strong>gle phenomenon by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> depth, a numberof <strong>in</strong>dividuals who have experienced the phenomenon.The researcher extracts what he or she considers <strong>to</strong> berelevant statements from each participant’s descriptionof the phenomenon <strong>and</strong> then clusters these statements<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> themes. He or she then <strong>in</strong>tegrates these themes <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>a narrative description of the phenomenon.GROUNDED THEORYIn a grounded theory study, the researchers <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong>generate a theory that is “‘grounded’ <strong>in</strong> data from participantswho have experienced the process (Strauss &Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1998).” 12 Grounded theories are not generatedbefore a study beg<strong>in</strong>s, but are formed <strong>in</strong>ductively fromthe data that are collected dur<strong>in</strong>g the study itself. Inother words, researchers start with the data they havecollected <strong>and</strong> then develop generalizations after theylook at the data. Strauss <strong>and</strong> Corb<strong>in</strong> put it this way:“One does not beg<strong>in</strong> with a theory, then prove it. Ratherone beg<strong>in</strong>s with an area of study <strong>and</strong> what is relevant <strong>to</strong>that area is allowed <strong>to</strong> emerge.” 13<strong>Research</strong>ers do<strong>in</strong>g a grounded theory study use whatis called the constant comparative method. There is acont<strong>in</strong>ual <strong>in</strong>terplay between the researcher, his or herdata, <strong>and</strong> the theory that is be<strong>in</strong>g developed. Potentialcategories for group<strong>in</strong>g items of data are created, triedout, <strong>and</strong> discarded until a “fit” between theory <strong>and</strong> datais achieved. Lancy describes the process as follows:In a study of parental <strong>in</strong>fluence on children’s read<strong>in</strong>g ofs<strong>to</strong>rybooks, Kelly Draper <strong>and</strong> I videotaped 32 parent-childpairs as they read <strong>to</strong> each other. We had few if any preconceptionsabout what we would f<strong>in</strong>d, only that we hopedthat dist<strong>in</strong>ct patterns would emerge <strong>and</strong> that these wouldbe associated with the children’s evident ease/difficulty <strong>in</strong>learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> read. I spent literally dozens of hours view<strong>in</strong>gthese videotapes, develop<strong>in</strong>g, us<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> cast<strong>in</strong>g aside variouscategories until I found two clusters of characteristicswhich I called “reductionist” <strong>and</strong> “expansionist” that accountedfor a large portion of the variation among parents’read<strong>in</strong>g/listen<strong>in</strong>g styles. I was, of course, guided <strong>in</strong> mysearch for appropriate categories by my [experience] withthe sett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> by the transcripts of our <strong>in</strong>terview witheach parent. 14The data <strong>in</strong> a grounded theory study are collectedprimarily through one-on-one <strong>in</strong>terviews, focus group<strong>in</strong>terviews, <strong>and</strong> participant observation by the researcher(s).But it is an ongo<strong>in</strong>g process. Data are collected<strong>and</strong> analyzed; a theory is suggested; more dataare collected; the theory is revised; then more data arecollected; the theory is further developed, clarified, revised;<strong>and</strong> the process cont<strong>in</strong>ues.Let us consider a hypothetical example of agrounded research study. Suppose that a researcher is


430 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> how pr<strong>in</strong>cipals try <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> enhancemorale among the teachers <strong>in</strong> their schools. He or shemight conduct a series of <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews with anumber of pr<strong>in</strong>cipals <strong>in</strong> a few large urban high schools.Suppose the researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds that these pr<strong>in</strong>cipals utilizea variety of strategies <strong>to</strong> keep morale high, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>ghav<strong>in</strong>g frequent one-on-one “praise sessions” <strong>to</strong> rewardgood teach<strong>in</strong>g, acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g the efforts of teachersthrough written <strong>and</strong> oral commendations at facultymeet<strong>in</strong>gs, writ<strong>in</strong>g supportive letters <strong>and</strong> plac<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong>the teachers’ personnel files, provid<strong>in</strong>g extra resources,replac<strong>in</strong>g unnecessary meet<strong>in</strong>gs with rout<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, advis<strong>in</strong>g faculty of policy changes <strong>in</strong> advance<strong>and</strong> ask<strong>in</strong>g for their <strong>in</strong>put <strong>and</strong> approval beforeh<strong>and</strong>,<strong>and</strong> so forth.In addition, the researcher not only observes howthe pr<strong>in</strong>cipals <strong>in</strong>teract with their faculties <strong>and</strong> listen <strong>to</strong>what they have <strong>to</strong> say, but also <strong>in</strong>terviews some of theirteachers <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ually exam<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>ks about thedata he or she has collected through the <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong>observations. Gradually, the researcher develops a theoryabout what effective pr<strong>in</strong>cipals do <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>enhance morale among their teachers. The theory isthen modified over time as the researcher observes <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terviews even more pr<strong>in</strong>cipals <strong>and</strong> teachers. Thepo<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> stress here, however, is that the researcher doesnot go <strong>in</strong> with a theory ahead of time; rather he or shedevelops a theory out of the data that are collected—that is, one that is grounded <strong>in</strong> the data. This approachis obviously highly dependent on the <strong>in</strong>sight of the <strong>in</strong>dividualresearcher.CASE STUDIESThe study of “cases” has been around for some time.Students <strong>in</strong> medic<strong>in</strong>e, law, bus<strong>in</strong>ess, <strong>and</strong> the social sciencesoften study cases as part of their tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Whatcase study researchers have <strong>in</strong> common is that they callthe objects of their research cases, <strong>and</strong> they focus theirresearch on the study of such cases. The case studies ofPiaget <strong>and</strong> Vigotsky, for example, have contributedmuch <strong>to</strong> our underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of cognitive <strong>and</strong> moraldevelopment. 15What is a case? A case comprises just one <strong>in</strong>dividual,classroom, school, or program. Typical cases are a studentwho has trouble learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> read, a social studiesclassroom, a private school, or a national curriculumproject. For some researchers, a case is not just an <strong>in</strong>dividualor situation that can easily be identified (e.g., aparticular <strong>in</strong>dividual, classroom, organization, or project);it may be an event (e.g., a campus celebration), an activity(e.g., learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> use a computer), or an ongo<strong>in</strong>gprocess (e.g., student teach<strong>in</strong>g).Sometimes much can be learned from study<strong>in</strong>g jus<strong>to</strong>ne <strong>in</strong>dividual, one classroom, one school, or oneschool district. For example, there are some studentswho learn a second language rather easily. In hopes ofga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> why this is the case, one such studentcould be observed on a regular basis <strong>to</strong> see if thereare any noticeable patterns or regularities <strong>in</strong> the student’sbehavior. The student, as well as his or her teachers,counselors, parents, <strong>and</strong> friends, might also be <strong>in</strong>terviewed<strong>in</strong> depth. A similar series of observations (<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terviews) might be conducted with a student whof<strong>in</strong>ds learn<strong>in</strong>g another language very difficult. As much<strong>in</strong>formation as possible (study style, attitudes <strong>to</strong>wardthe language, approach <strong>to</strong> the subject, behavior <strong>in</strong> class,<strong>and</strong> so on) would be collected. The hope here is thatthrough the study of a somewhat unique <strong>in</strong>dividual, <strong>in</strong>sightscan be ga<strong>in</strong>ed that will suggest ways <strong>to</strong> help otherlanguage students <strong>in</strong> the future.Similarly, a detailed study might be made of a s<strong>in</strong>gleschool. There might be a particular elementaryschool <strong>in</strong> a given school district, for example, that isnoteworthy for its success with at-risk students. Theresearcher might visit the school on a regular basis,observ<strong>in</strong>g what goes on <strong>in</strong> classrooms, dur<strong>in</strong>g recessperiods, <strong>in</strong> the hallways <strong>and</strong> lunchroom, dur<strong>in</strong>g facultymeet<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> so on. Faculty members, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,support staff, <strong>and</strong> counselors could be <strong>in</strong>terviewed.Aga<strong>in</strong>, as much <strong>in</strong>formation as possible (such as teach<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, adm<strong>in</strong>istrative style, school activities,parental <strong>in</strong>volvement, attitudes of faculty <strong>and</strong> staff <strong>to</strong>wardstudents, classroom <strong>and</strong> other activities) wouldbe collected. Here <strong>to</strong>o, the hope would be that throughthe study of a s<strong>in</strong>gle, rather unique case (<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stancenot an <strong>in</strong>dividual but a school), valuable <strong>in</strong>sightswould be ga<strong>in</strong>ed.Stake has identified three types of case studies. 16 Inan <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic case study, the researcher is primarily <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g a specific <strong>in</strong>dividual or situation.He or she describes, <strong>in</strong> detail, the particulars of thecase <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> shed some light on what is go<strong>in</strong>g on.Thus, a researcher might study a particular student <strong>in</strong>order <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out why that student is hav<strong>in</strong>g troublelearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> read. Another researcher might want <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>how a school’s student council operates. Athird might wish <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e how effectively (orwhether) an after-school detention program is work<strong>in</strong>g.All three of these examples <strong>in</strong>volve the study of a s<strong>in</strong>gle


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 431case. The researcher’s goal <strong>in</strong> each <strong>in</strong>stance is <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>the case <strong>in</strong> all its parts, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g its <strong>in</strong>ner work<strong>in</strong>gs.Intr<strong>in</strong>sic case studies are often used <strong>in</strong> explora<strong>to</strong>ryresearch when researchers seek <strong>to</strong> learn about somelittle-known phenomenon by study<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>in</strong> depth.In an <strong>in</strong>strumental case study, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, aresearcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g morethan just a particular case; the researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>study<strong>in</strong>g the particular case only as a means <strong>to</strong> somelarger goal. A researcher might study how Mrs. Brownteaches phonics, for example, <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> learn someth<strong>in</strong>gabout phonics as a method or about the teach<strong>in</strong>g of read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> general. The researcher’s goal <strong>in</strong> such studies ismore global <strong>and</strong> less focused on the particular <strong>in</strong>dividual,event, program, or school be<strong>in</strong>g studied. <strong>Research</strong>erswho conduct such studies are more <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>gconclusions that apply beyond a particular case than theyare <strong>in</strong> conclusions that apply <strong>to</strong> just one specific case.Third, there is the multiple- (or collective) casestudy <strong>in</strong> which a researcher studies multiple cases at thesame time as part of one overall study. For example, aresearcher might choose several cases <strong>to</strong> study becausehe or she is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the effects of ma<strong>in</strong>stream<strong>in</strong>gchildren with disabilities <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> regular classrooms. Insteadof study<strong>in</strong>g the results of such ma<strong>in</strong>stream<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>just a s<strong>in</strong>gle classroom, the researcher studies its impact<strong>in</strong> a number of different classrooms.Which is <strong>to</strong> be preferred, multiple- or s<strong>in</strong>gle-case designs?Multiple-case designs have both advantages <strong>and</strong>disadvantages when compared <strong>to</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-case designs. Theresults of multiple-case studies are often considered morecompell<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> they are more likely <strong>to</strong> lend themselves <strong>to</strong>valid generalization. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, certa<strong>in</strong> types ofcases (the rare case, the critical case for test<strong>in</strong>g a theory, orthe case that permits a researcher <strong>to</strong> observe a phenomenonpreviously <strong>in</strong>accessible <strong>to</strong> scientific study) requires<strong>in</strong>gle-case research. Furthermore, multiple-case studiesoften require extensive resources <strong>and</strong> time. Any decision<strong>to</strong> undertake multiple-case studies, therefore, cannot betaken lightly. Y<strong>in</strong> argues that researchers who do undertakemultiple-case studies, therefore, should employ whathe calls “replication logic.” Here is his rationale:Thus, if one has access <strong>to</strong> only three cases of a rare, cl<strong>in</strong>icalsyndrome <strong>in</strong>...medical science, the appropriate researchdesign is one <strong>in</strong> which the same results are predictedfor each of the three cases, thereby produc<strong>in</strong>gevidence that the three cases did <strong>in</strong>deed <strong>in</strong>volve the samesyndrome. If similar results are obta<strong>in</strong>ed from all threecases, replication (of results) is said <strong>to</strong> have taken place. 17ETHNOGRAPHIC AND HISTORICALRESEARCHWith regard <strong>to</strong> the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g two approaches <strong>to</strong> qualitativeresearch, we shall not describe them here becauseeach is discussed <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> later chapters. We selectedthese two <strong>to</strong> discuss <strong>in</strong> greater depth because theyrepresent dist<strong>in</strong>ctly different approaches. Ethnographic researchfocuses on the study of culture. His<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch concentrates exclusively on the past. We willdiscuss them <strong>in</strong> Chapters 21 <strong>and</strong> 22.SAMPLING IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCH<strong>Research</strong>ers who engage <strong>in</strong> some form of qualitative researchare likely <strong>to</strong> select a purposive sample (see Chapter6)—that is, they select a sample they feel will yieldthe best underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of what they are study<strong>in</strong>g. Atleast n<strong>in</strong>e types of purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g have been identified.18 These <strong>in</strong>clude:a typical sample, one that is considered or judged <strong>to</strong> betypical or representative of that which is be<strong>in</strong>g studied(e.g., a class of elementary school pupils selected becausethey are judged <strong>to</strong> be typical third-graders).a critical sample, one that is considered <strong>to</strong> be particularlyenlighten<strong>in</strong>g because it is so unusual or exceptional(e.g., <strong>in</strong>dividuals who have atta<strong>in</strong>ed highachievement despite some serious physical limitations).a homogeneous sample, one <strong>in</strong> which all of themembers possess a certa<strong>in</strong> trait or characteristic(e.g., a group of high school students all judged <strong>to</strong>possess exceptional artistic talent).an extreme case sample, one <strong>in</strong> which all of themembers are outliers who do not fit the general patternor who otherwise display extreme characteristics(e.g., students achieve high grades despite lowscores on ability tests <strong>and</strong> poor home environments).a theoretical sample, one that helps the researcher <strong>to</strong>underst<strong>and</strong> a concept or theory (e.g., select<strong>in</strong>g a groupof tribal elders <strong>to</strong> assess the relevance of Piagetiantheory <strong>to</strong> the education of Native Americans).an opportunistic sample, one chosen dur<strong>in</strong>g a study<strong>to</strong> take advantage of new conditions or circumstancesthat have arisen (e.g., eyewitnesses <strong>to</strong> a fracasat a high school football game).a confirm<strong>in</strong>g sample, one that is obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> validateor disconfirm prelim<strong>in</strong>ary f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g., followup<strong>in</strong>terviews with students <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> verify reasonssome students drop out).


432 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ea maximal variation sample, one selected <strong>to</strong> representa diversity of perspectives or characteristics(e.g., a group of students who possess a wide varietyof attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward recent school policies).a snowball sample, one selected as need arisesdur<strong>in</strong>g the conduct of a study (e.g., dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gof a group of pr<strong>in</strong>cipals, they recommendothers who also should be <strong>in</strong>terviewed because theyare particularly knowledgeable about the subject ofthe research).Generalization <strong>in</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>A generalization is usually thought of as a statement orclaim of some sort that applies <strong>to</strong> more than one <strong>in</strong>dividual,group, object, or situation. Thus, when a researchermakes a statement, based on a review of the literature,that there is a negative correlation between age <strong>and</strong>amount of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> school (older children are less <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> school than younger children), he or she is mak<strong>in</strong>ga generalization.The value of a generalization is that it allows us <strong>to</strong>have expectations (<strong>and</strong> sometimes <strong>to</strong> make predictions)about the future. Although a generalization might not betrue <strong>in</strong> every case (e.g., some older children may bemore <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> school than some younger children),it describes, more often than not, what we would expect<strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d. Almost all researchers hope that useful generalizationscan be derived from their research. A limitationof qualitative research is that there is seldom methodologicaljustification for generaliz<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of aparticular study. While this limitation also applies <strong>to</strong>many quantitative studies, it is almost <strong>in</strong>evitable giventhe nature of qualitative research. Because of this,replication of qualitative studies is even more importantthan it is <strong>in</strong> quantitative research.Eisner po<strong>in</strong>ts out that not only ideas but also skills <strong>and</strong>images can be generalized. 19 We generalize a skill whenwe apply it <strong>in</strong> a situation different from the one <strong>in</strong> whichwe learned the skill. Images also generalize. As Eisnerpo<strong>in</strong>ts out, it is this fact—that images generalize—thatleads a qualitative researcher <strong>to</strong> look for certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics<strong>in</strong> a classroom, certa<strong>in</strong> ways of teach<strong>in</strong>g, that he orshe can apply elsewhere. Once a researcher has an imageof “excellence” <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g, for example, he or she canapply this image <strong>to</strong> a variety of situations. “For qualitativeresearch, this means that the creation of an image—a vividportrait of excellent teach<strong>in</strong>g, for example—can become apro<strong>to</strong>type that can be used <strong>in</strong> the education of teachers orfor the appraisal of teach<strong>in</strong>g.” 20 In Eisner’s words:Direct contact with the qualitative world is one of ourmost important sources of generalization. But . . . we donot need <strong>to</strong> learn everyth<strong>in</strong>g first-h<strong>and</strong>. We listen <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>rytellers<strong>and</strong> learn about how th<strong>in</strong>gs were, <strong>and</strong> we use whatwe have been <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> make decisions about what will be.We see pho<strong>to</strong>s <strong>and</strong> learn what <strong>to</strong> expect on our forthcom<strong>in</strong>gtrip <strong>to</strong> Spa<strong>in</strong>. We see the play On the Waterfront <strong>and</strong>learn someth<strong>in</strong>g about corruption <strong>in</strong> the shipp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dustry<strong>and</strong>, more important, about the conflicts <strong>and</strong> tensionsbetween two brothers. We see the film One Flew over theCuckoo’s Nest <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> a bit more about howpeople survive <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>stitution that is hell-bent on theirdomestication. . . .Attention <strong>to</strong> the particular, <strong>to</strong> the case, is descriptivenot only of the case, but of other cases like it. When SaraLawrence-Lightfoot writes about the Brookl<strong>in</strong>e HighSchool or the George Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n Carver High School orthe John F. Kennedy High School, she tells us more thanjust what those particular schools are like; we learn someth<strong>in</strong>gabout what makes a good high school. 21 Do all highschools have <strong>to</strong> be good <strong>in</strong> the same way? No. Can somehigh schools share some of their characteristics? Yes. Canwe learn from Lawrence-Lightfoot what <strong>to</strong> look for?Certa<strong>in</strong>ly. 22There is little question, we th<strong>in</strong>k, that generalizationis possible <strong>in</strong> qualitative research. But it is a type of generalizationthat differs from what is found <strong>in</strong> much quantitativeresearch. In many experimental <strong>and</strong> quasi-experimentalstudies, the researcher generalizes from thesample under <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>to</strong> the population of <strong>in</strong>terest(see Chapter 6). Note that it is the researcher who doesthe generaliz<strong>in</strong>g. 23 He or she is likely <strong>to</strong> suggest <strong>to</strong> practitionersthat the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are of value <strong>and</strong> can (sometimesthey say should) be applied <strong>in</strong> their situations.In qualitative studies, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, the researchermay also generalize, but it is much more likely that anygeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be done will be carried out by <strong>in</strong>terestedpractitioners—by <strong>in</strong>dividuals who are <strong>in</strong> situations similar<strong>to</strong> the one(s) <strong>in</strong>vestigated by the researcher. It is thepractitioner, rather than the researcher, who judges theapplicability of the researcher’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> conclusions,who determ<strong>in</strong>es whether the researcher’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsfit his or her situation. Eisner makes this clear:The researcher might say someth<strong>in</strong>g like this: “This iswhat I did <strong>and</strong> this is what I th<strong>in</strong>k it means. Does it have


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 433any bear<strong>in</strong>g on your situation? If it does <strong>and</strong> if yoursituation is troublesome or problematic, how did it getthat way <strong>and</strong> what can be done <strong>to</strong> improve it?” 24It is worth not<strong>in</strong>g that not all qualitative researcherslook at generaliz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the same way. Some are concernedless “with the question of whether their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsare generalizable, but rather with the question of <strong>to</strong> whichother sett<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> subjects they are generalizable. 25Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen give an example:In the study of an <strong>in</strong>tensive care unit at a teach<strong>in</strong>ghospital, we studied the ways professional staff <strong>and</strong>parents communicate about the condition of the children.As we concentrated on the <strong>in</strong>terchanges, we noticedthat the professional staff not only diagnosed the<strong>in</strong>fants but sized up the parents as well. These parentalevaluations formed the basis for judgments the professionalsmade about what <strong>to</strong> say <strong>to</strong> parents <strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong>say it. Reflect<strong>in</strong>g about parent-teacher conferences <strong>in</strong>public schools <strong>and</strong> other situations where professionalshave <strong>in</strong>formation about children <strong>to</strong> which parents mightwant access, we began <strong>to</strong> see parallels. . . . One tack weare presently explor<strong>in</strong>g is the extent <strong>to</strong> which the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof the <strong>in</strong>tensive care unit are generalizable not <strong>to</strong>other sett<strong>in</strong>gs of the same substantive type, but <strong>to</strong> othersett<strong>in</strong>gs, such as schools, <strong>in</strong> which professionals talk <strong>to</strong>parents. 26Qualitative <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs, then, are less def<strong>in</strong>itive,less certa<strong>in</strong> about the conclusions they draw from theirresearch. They tend <strong>to</strong> view them as ideas <strong>to</strong> be shared,discussed, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigated further. Modification <strong>in</strong> differentcircumstances <strong>and</strong> under different conditions willalmost always be necessary. These issues are oftenreferred <strong>to</strong> as transferability, def<strong>in</strong>ed by Morrow asachieved when “the researcher provides sufficient <strong>in</strong>formationabout self (the researcher as <strong>in</strong>strument) <strong>and</strong>the research context, participants, <strong>and</strong> the researcherparticipantrelationship <strong>to</strong> enable the reader <strong>to</strong> decidehow the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs may transfer.” 27Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>To the extent that a qualitative study does not attempt <strong>to</strong>explore relationships, <strong>in</strong>ternal validity is, strictly speak<strong>in</strong>g,not as important as it is <strong>in</strong> quantitative research.<strong>How</strong>ever, because qualitative research is so dependen<strong>to</strong>n the researcher <strong>in</strong> both collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation, an important consideration, even <strong>in</strong> purelydescriptive studies, is researcher bias. Further, qualitativestudies frequently do conta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>grelationships. Examples of this occur <strong>in</strong> the studiesevaluated <strong>in</strong> Chapters 19 <strong>to</strong> 24. When this is the case, attentionshould be given <strong>to</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>, where possible,controll<strong>in</strong>g each of the threats discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter9. Though more difficult <strong>in</strong> qualitative research, it issometimes possible, as discussed <strong>in</strong> the Chapter 20study critique, <strong>to</strong> control particular threats. The exceptionis his<strong>to</strong>rical research, where<strong>in</strong> control is, we th<strong>in</strong>k,virtually impossible.Ethics <strong>and</strong> Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>Ethical concerns affect qualitative research just as muchas they do any of the other k<strong>in</strong>ds of research that wehave considered <strong>in</strong> this book. Nevertheless, a few po<strong>in</strong>tsbear repeat<strong>in</strong>g because of their importance.First, unless otherwise agreed <strong>to</strong>, the identities of allwho participate <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study should always beprotected; care should be taken <strong>to</strong> ensure that none of the<strong>in</strong>formation collected would embarrass or harm them. Ifconfidentiality cannot be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed, participants mustbe so <strong>in</strong>formed <strong>and</strong> given the opportunity <strong>to</strong> withdrawfrom the study.Second, participants should always be treated withrespect. It is especially important <strong>in</strong> qualitative studies<strong>to</strong> seek the cooperation of all subjects <strong>in</strong> the researchendeavor. Usually, subjects should be <strong>to</strong>ld of the researcher’s<strong>in</strong>terests <strong>and</strong> should give their permission <strong>to</strong>proceed. <strong>Research</strong>ers should never lie <strong>to</strong> subjects norrecord any conversations us<strong>in</strong>g a hidden tape recorderor other mechanical apparatus.Third, researchers should do their best <strong>to</strong> ensure thatno physical or psychological harm will come <strong>to</strong> anyonewho participates <strong>in</strong> the study. This seems rather obvious,perhaps, but researchers sometimes are placed <strong>in</strong> adifficult position because they f<strong>in</strong>d, <strong>in</strong>advertently, thatsubjects are be<strong>in</strong>g harmed. Consider the follow<strong>in</strong>gexample. In certa<strong>in</strong> studies <strong>in</strong> state <strong>in</strong>stitutions for thementally h<strong>and</strong>icapped, researchers have witnessed thephysical abuse of residents. What is their ethical responsibility<strong>in</strong> such cases? Here is what two researchers whoobserved such abuse firsth<strong>and</strong> had <strong>to</strong> say:In the case of physical abuse, the solution may seemobvious at first: <strong>Research</strong>er or not, you should <strong>in</strong>tervene


434 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p the beat<strong>in</strong>gs. In some states, it is illegal not <strong>to</strong>report abuse. That was our immediate disposition. But,through our research, we came <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> that abusewas a pervasive activity <strong>in</strong> most such <strong>in</strong>stitutionsnationally, not only part of this particular sett<strong>in</strong>g. Wasblow<strong>in</strong>g the whistle on one act a responsible way <strong>to</strong>address this problem or was it a way of gett<strong>in</strong>g thematter off our chests? Intervention may get you kickedout. Might not cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g the research, publish<strong>in</strong>g theresults, writ<strong>in</strong>g reports expos<strong>in</strong>g national abuse, <strong>and</strong>provid<strong>in</strong>g research for witnesses <strong>in</strong> court (or be<strong>in</strong>g anexpert witness) do more <strong>to</strong> change the conditions thanthe s<strong>in</strong>gle act of <strong>in</strong>tervention? Was such th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g acopout, an excuse not <strong>to</strong> get <strong>in</strong>volved? 28What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?As the above excerpt reveals, ethical concerns aredifficult ones <strong>in</strong>deed. Two other po<strong>in</strong>ts deserve mention.Many researchers are concerned that subjects do not getvery much <strong>in</strong> return from participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation. After all, the studies that researchers dooften lead <strong>to</strong> the advancement of their careers. They helpprofessors get promoted. Study results are frequentlyreported <strong>in</strong> books that br<strong>in</strong>g their authors royalty checks.<strong>Research</strong>ers get <strong>to</strong> talk about what they have learned;their work, when well done, helps them ga<strong>in</strong> the respec<strong>to</strong>f their colleagues. But what do subjects get? Participantsoften (perhaps usually) do not have a chance <strong>to</strong>reciprocate <strong>and</strong>/or tell what their lives are like. As aresult, subjects sometimes get misrepresented or evendemeaned. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, some researchers have tried <strong>to</strong>design studies <strong>in</strong> which researcher <strong>and</strong> participants aremore like partners <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>vestigation where the subjectsdef<strong>in</strong>itely have a say.Furthermore, there is another ethical concern, somewhatrelated <strong>to</strong> the above, that must be addressed. Thisoccurs when there is the possibility that certa<strong>in</strong> researchf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>in</strong> the h<strong>and</strong>s of the powerful, may lead <strong>to</strong> actionsthat could actually hurt subjects (or people <strong>in</strong> similarcircumstances) <strong>and</strong>/or lead <strong>to</strong> public policies or publicattitudes that are actually harmful <strong>to</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> groups. Whata researcher might see as “a sympathetic portrayal ofpeople liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a hous<strong>in</strong>g project might be read byothers as prov<strong>in</strong>g prejudices about poor people be<strong>in</strong>girresponsible <strong>and</strong> prone <strong>to</strong> violence.” 29 The ethical po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>to</strong> stress here, then, is this: While researchers can neverbe sure how their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs will be received, they mustalways be sure <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k carefully about the implicationsof their work, who the results of this work may affect,<strong>and</strong> how.We offer, then, a number of specific questions that weth<strong>in</strong>k all researchers, no matter what k<strong>in</strong>d of researchthey prefer, should th<strong>in</strong>k about before, dur<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> afterthe completion of any study they undertake:Is the study be<strong>in</strong>g contemplated worth do<strong>in</strong>g?Do the researchers have the necessary expertise <strong>to</strong>carry out a study of good quality?Have the participants <strong>in</strong> the study been given full<strong>in</strong>formation about what the study will <strong>in</strong>volve?Have the participants will<strong>in</strong>gly given their consent <strong>to</strong>participate?Who will ga<strong>in</strong> from this research?Is there a balance between ga<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> costs for bothresearchers <strong>and</strong> participants?Who, if anyone, might be harmed (physically orpsychologically) <strong>in</strong> this study, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> what degree?What is <strong>to</strong> be done should harmful, illegal, or wrongfulbehavior be witnessed?Will the participants <strong>in</strong> the study be deceived <strong>in</strong> anyway?Will confidentiality be assured?Who owns the data that will be collected <strong>and</strong>analyzed <strong>in</strong> this study?<strong>How</strong> will the results of the study be used? Is thereany possibility for misuse? If so, how?Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative<strong>Research</strong> ReconsideredCan qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches be used<strong>to</strong>gether? Of course. And often they should be. In surveyresearch, for example, it is common not only <strong>to</strong>prepare a closed-ended (e.g., multiple-choice) questionnairefor people <strong>to</strong> answer <strong>in</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g, but also <strong>to</strong> conduc<strong>to</strong>pen-ended personal <strong>in</strong>terviews with a r<strong>and</strong>om sampleof the respondents. Descriptive statistics are sometimesused <strong>to</strong> provide quantitative details <strong>in</strong> an otherwisequalitative study. Many his<strong>to</strong>rical studies <strong>in</strong>clude acomb<strong>in</strong>ation of qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative methodologies,<strong>and</strong> their f<strong>in</strong>al reports present both k<strong>in</strong>ds of data.Nevertheless, it must be admitted that carry<strong>in</strong>g out asophisticated quantitative study <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>-depth qualitative<strong>in</strong>vestigation at the same time is difficult <strong>to</strong> pull offsuccessfully. Indeed, it is very difficult. Oftentimeswhat is produced is a study that is neither a good qualitativenor a good quantitative piece of work.Which is the better approach—qualitative or quantitative?Although we hear this question a lot, we th<strong>in</strong>k


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 435it’s pretty much a waste of energy. Oftentimes you willhear overly zealous advocates of one or the otherapproach disparag<strong>in</strong>g the other. They say that theirs isthe best (<strong>in</strong>deed, sometimes the only) method <strong>to</strong> use ifone wants <strong>to</strong> do really useful research on importantquestions <strong>and</strong> that the other is badly flawed <strong>and</strong> canonly lead <strong>to</strong> spurious or trivial results. But here is whattwo em<strong>in</strong>ent qualitative researchers have <strong>to</strong> say:By far, the most widely held (view) is that there is noone best method. It all depends on what you are study<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> what you want <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out. If you want <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat the majority of the American people th<strong>in</strong>k abouta particular issue, survey research which relies heavily onquantitative design <strong>in</strong> pick<strong>in</strong>g your sample, design<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> pretest<strong>in</strong>g your <strong>in</strong>strument, <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g the data isbest. If you want <strong>to</strong> know about the process of change <strong>in</strong>a school <strong>and</strong> how the various school members experiencechange, qualitative methods will do a better job. Withouta doubt there are certa<strong>in</strong> questions <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>pics that thequalitative approach will not help you with, <strong>and</strong> the sameis true of quantitative research. 30We agree. The important th<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>to</strong> know what questionscan best be answered by which method or comb<strong>in</strong>ationof methods.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE NATURE OF QUALITATIVE RESEARCHThe term qualitative research refers <strong>to</strong> studies that <strong>in</strong>vestigate the quality ofrelationships, activities, situations, or materials.The natural sett<strong>in</strong>g is a direct source of data, <strong>and</strong> the researcher is a key part of the<strong>in</strong>strumentation process <strong>in</strong> qualitative research.Qualitative data are collected ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>in</strong> the form of words or pictures <strong>and</strong> seldom<strong>in</strong>volve numbers. Content analysis is a primary method of data analysis.Qualitative researchers are especially <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> how th<strong>in</strong>gs occur <strong>and</strong> particularly<strong>in</strong> the perspectives of the subjects of a study.Qualitative researchers do not, usually, formulate a hypothesis beforeh<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> thenseek <strong>to</strong> test it. Rather, they allow hypotheses <strong>to</strong> emerge as a study develops.Qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative research differ <strong>in</strong> the philosophic assumptions thatunderlie the two approaches.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsSTEPS INVOLVED IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCHThe steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a qualitative study are not as dist<strong>in</strong>ct as theyare <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies. They often overlap <strong>and</strong> sometimes are even conductedconcurrently.All qualitative studies beg<strong>in</strong> with a foreshadowed problem, the particular phenomenonthe researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g.


436 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong>ers who engage <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study of some type usually select a purposivesample. Several types of purposive samples exist.There is no treatment <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study, nor is there any manipulation of variables.The collection of data <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study is ongo<strong>in</strong>g.Conclusions are drawn cont<strong>in</strong>uously throughout the course of a qualitative study.APPROACHES TO QUALITATIVE RESEARCHAbiographical study tells the s<strong>to</strong>ry of the special events <strong>in</strong> the life of a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual.A researcher studies an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s reactions <strong>to</strong> a particular phenomenon <strong>in</strong> aphenomenological study. He or she attempts <strong>to</strong> identify the commonalities amongdifferent <strong>in</strong>dividual perceptions.In a grounded theory study, a researcher forms a theory <strong>in</strong>ductively from the datacollected as a part of the study.A case study is a detailed study of one or (at most) a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals or other socialunits, such as a classroom, a school, or a neighborhood. It can also be a study of anevent, an activity, or an ongo<strong>in</strong>g process.GENERALIZATION IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCHGeneraliz<strong>in</strong>g is possible <strong>in</strong> qualitative research, but it is of a type different from thatfound <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies. Most likely it will be done by <strong>in</strong>terested practitioners.ETHICS AND QUALITATIVE RESEARCHThe identities of all participants <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study should be protected, <strong>and</strong> theyshould be treated with respect.RECONSIDERING QUALITATIVE AND QUANTITATIVE RESEARCHAspects of both qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative research often are used <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>in</strong> astudy. Increased attention is be<strong>in</strong>g given <strong>to</strong> such mixed-methods studies.Whether qualitative or quantitative research is the most appropriate boils down <strong>to</strong>what the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out.Key Termsau<strong>to</strong>biography 427biographical study 427case study 430confirm<strong>in</strong>g sample 431critical sample 431extreme case sample 431foreshadowedproblem 425generalization <strong>in</strong>qualitativeresearch 432grounded theorystudy 429homogeneoussample 431<strong>in</strong>strumental casestudy 431<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic case study 430life his<strong>to</strong>ry 428maximal variationsample 432multiple- (collective)case study 431narrative research 427opportunisticsample 431oral his<strong>to</strong>ry 428phenomenologicalstudy 428portraiture 428positivism 423postmodernism 424purposive sample 426qualitative research 422replication 432snowball sample 432theoretical sample 431typical sample 431436


CHAPTER 18 The Nature of Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> 4371. What do you see as the greatest strength of qualitative research? the biggestweakness?2. Are there any <strong>to</strong>pics or questions that could not be studied us<strong>in</strong>g a qualitative approach?If so, give an example. Is there any type of <strong>in</strong>formation that qualitativeresearch cannot provide? If so, what might it be?3. Qualitative researchers are sometimes accused of be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>o subjective. What do youth<strong>in</strong>k a qualitative researcher might say <strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> such an accusation?4. Qualitative researchers say that “complete” objectivity is impossible. Would youagree? Expla<strong>in</strong> your reason<strong>in</strong>g.5. “The essence of all good research is underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, rather than an attempt <strong>to</strong> provesometh<strong>in</strong>g.” What does this statement mean?6. “All researchers are biased <strong>to</strong> at least some degree. The important th<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>to</strong> beaware of one’s biases!” Is just be<strong>in</strong>g “aware” enough? What else might one do?7. Qualitative researchers often say that “the whole is greater than the sum of its parts.”What does this statement mean? What implications does it have for educationalresearch?8. Would it be possible <strong>to</strong> use r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> qualitative research? Would it bedesirable? Expla<strong>in</strong>.9. In what way is generalization <strong>in</strong> qualitative research different from generalization <strong>in</strong>quantitative research—or is it?10. What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the ethical responsibility of researchers if they witness an <strong>in</strong>stanceof physical abuse <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study they are conduct<strong>in</strong>g?For Discussion1. R. C. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> S. K. Biklen (2007). Qualitative research for education: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> theory <strong>and</strong>methods, 5th ed. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon.2. Some qualitative researchers, however, do use statistical procedures <strong>to</strong> clarify their data. See, for example,M. B. Miles <strong>and</strong> A. M. Huberman (1994). Qualitative data analysis, 2nd ed. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.3. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen, op. cit., p. 6.4. H. R. Bernard (2000). Social research methods: Qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks,CA: Sage.5. M. Foucault (1972). The archaeology of knowledge. New York: Harper <strong>and</strong> Row.6. J. Derrida (1972). Discussion: Structure, sign, <strong>and</strong> plot <strong>in</strong> the discourse of the human sciences. In R.Macksey <strong>and</strong> E. Dona<strong>to</strong> (eds.), The structuralist controversy. Baltimore: Johns Hopk<strong>in</strong>s University Press,pp. 242–272.7. John W. Creswell (2007). Qualitative <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> research design: Choos<strong>in</strong>g among five approaches. Thous<strong>and</strong>Oaks, CA: Sage.8. Ibid., p. 9.9. Ibid., p. 9.10. Ibid, p. 55. Citations with<strong>in</strong> the quotation <strong>in</strong>clude: C. Ellis (2004). The ethnographic it: A methodologicalnovel about au<strong>to</strong>ethnography. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira; N. K. Denz<strong>in</strong> (1989a). Interpretive biography.Newbury Park, CA: Sage; <strong>and</strong> K. Plummer (1983), Documents of life: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> the problems <strong>and</strong> literatureof a humanistic method. London: George Allen & Unw<strong>in</strong>.11. Ibid., p. 57.12. Ibid. p. 63. Citation with<strong>in</strong> the quotation is: A. Strauss <strong>and</strong> J. Corb<strong>in</strong> (1998). Basics of qualitativeresearch: Grounded theory procedures <strong>and</strong> techniques (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.13. A. Strauss <strong>and</strong> J. Corb<strong>in</strong> (1994). Grounded theory methodology: An overview. In A. Denz<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>Y. L<strong>in</strong>coln (eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of qualitative research. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Notes437


438 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e14. David F. Lancy (2001). Study<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>in</strong> schools: Qualitative research traditions. Prospect Heights:Wavel<strong>and</strong> Press, p. 9.15. J. Piaget (1936/1963). The orig<strong>in</strong>s of <strong>in</strong>telligence <strong>in</strong> the child. New York: Nor<strong>to</strong>n; J. Piaget (1932/1965).The moral judgments of the child. New York: Free Press; L. S. Vigotsky (1914/1962). Thought <strong>and</strong> language.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.16. Robert Stake (1997). The art of case study research. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.17. Robert K. Y<strong>in</strong> (1994). Case study research: <strong>Design</strong> <strong>and</strong> methods. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.18. Adapted from John W. Creswell (2005). <strong>Education</strong>al research: Plann<strong>in</strong>g, conduct<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>gquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall, pp. 204–207.19. E. W. Eisner (1991). The enlightened eye: Qualitative <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> the enhancement of educational practice.New York: Macmillan.20. Ibid., p. 199.21. S. L. Lightfoot (1983). The good high school. New York: Basic Books.22. Eisner, op. cit., pp. 202–203.23. Remember that a researcher is entitled <strong>to</strong> generalize only if his or her sample has been r<strong>and</strong>omly selectedfrom the population. Many times, such is not the case.24. Eisner, op. cit., p. 204.25. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen, op. cit., p. 36.26. Ibid.27. S. Morrow (2005). Quality <strong>and</strong> trustworth<strong>in</strong>ess <strong>in</strong> qualitative research <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology. Journalof Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 52: 52.28. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen, op. cit., pp. 51–52.29. Ibid., p. 53.30. Ibid., p. 43.


Observation19<strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>gOBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termDescribe the type of sampl<strong>in</strong>g that occurs“observational research.”<strong>in</strong> observational studies.Describe at least four different roles an Describe briefly four types of <strong>in</strong>terviewsobserver can take <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study. qualitative researchers use.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termExpla<strong>in</strong> what a “key ac<strong>to</strong>r” is.“participant observation.”List at least three expectations that existExpla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termfor all <strong>in</strong>terviews.“nonparticipant observation.”Expla<strong>in</strong> what a focus group <strong>in</strong>terview is.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the termDescribe briefly why an <strong>in</strong>formed consent“naturalistic observation.”form is needed <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview research.Describe what a simulation is <strong>and</strong> how it Give at least four procedures qualitativemight be used by a researcher.researchers use <strong>to</strong> check on or enhanceDescribe what is meant by the termvalidity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong> qualitative“observer effect.”studies.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“observer bias.”ObservationParticipant ObservationNonparticipant ObservationNaturalistic ObservationSimulationsObserver EffectObserver BiasCod<strong>in</strong>g Observational DataThe Use of TechnologyInterview<strong>in</strong>gTypes of InterviewsKey-Ac<strong>to</strong>r InterviewsTypes of Interview QuestionsInterview<strong>in</strong>g BehaviorFocus Group InterviewsRecord<strong>in</strong>g Interview DataEthics <strong>in</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g: TheNecessity for InformedConsentData Collection <strong>and</strong> Analysis<strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability <strong>in</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>An Example ofQualitative StudyAnalysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisResult/InterpretationConclusions


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Interviews <strong>and</strong> ObservationsAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 19.1: Observer RolesActivity 19.2: Types of InterviewsActivity 19.3: Types of Interview QuestionsActivity 19.4: Do Some Observational <strong>Research</strong>What was it like <strong>to</strong> be a student teacher?”“Well, uh, (laughs), it’s sort of, uh, hard <strong>to</strong> describe. I guess I liked it, now that it’s all over (laughs). Butthere were times, uh, . . . I had a lot of trouble at first with discipl<strong>in</strong>e. You know controll<strong>in</strong>g the kids. Couldn’t seem<strong>to</strong> manage them. Especially when they wouldn’t sit down <strong>and</strong> started w<strong>and</strong>er<strong>in</strong>g around the room. Teach<strong>in</strong>g isn’teasy, you know, even for the old pros. And there I was, just a beg<strong>in</strong>ner. Not even sure I wanted <strong>to</strong> be a teacher. Iwas older, <strong>to</strong>o, than most of the other student teachers. Didn’t have a lot <strong>in</strong> common with them, me hav<strong>in</strong>g been<strong>in</strong> the military <strong>and</strong> all. But then, th<strong>in</strong>gs changed.”“What happened?”“Well, I sort of got the hang of it. I learned some th<strong>in</strong>gs. Began <strong>to</strong> learn my craft, you might say (smiles). Ilearned <strong>to</strong> control them better. I, uh, didn’t take any guff, you know (laughs). Oh, I wasn’t mean or anyth<strong>in</strong>g likethat, just firm. Yeah! You know, uh, uh, they respect it if you’re firm. You got <strong>to</strong> be. They don’t like wishy-washyteachers. Took me a while <strong>to</strong> learn that. But then I got better at expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong>o, <strong>and</strong> that made it easier <strong>to</strong>control the kids. And I set up some rules. They had <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> their seats when the bell rang, <strong>and</strong> they got po<strong>in</strong>ts ifthey were. I had an election for a class president who I had sit at the front of the room <strong>and</strong> whose job was <strong>to</strong> keeporder. That worked great. And then I had a weekly class meet<strong>in</strong>g where we talked about th<strong>in</strong>gs they liked <strong>and</strong>th<strong>in</strong>gs they thought could be improved. And I also . . .”The above conversation is part of an <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terview between a qualitative researcher <strong>and</strong> a 55-year-oldretired Air Force Major who has returned <strong>to</strong> school <strong>to</strong> get a middle school teach<strong>in</strong>g credential. In-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gis one of the staples of qualitative research. It is one of the th<strong>in</strong>gs we shall discuss <strong>in</strong> some detail <strong>in</strong> this chapter.Qualitative researchers use three ma<strong>in</strong> techniques <strong>to</strong>collect <strong>and</strong> analyze their data: observ<strong>in</strong>g people as theygo about their daily activities <strong>and</strong> record<strong>in</strong>g what theydo; conduct<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews with people abouttheir ideas, their op<strong>in</strong>ions, <strong>and</strong> their experiences; <strong>and</strong>analyz<strong>in</strong>g documents or other forms of communication(content analysis). Interviews can provide us with <strong>in</strong>formationabout people’s attitudes, their values, <strong>and</strong> whatthey th<strong>in</strong>k they do. If you want <strong>to</strong> know what they actuallydo, however, there is no substitute for watch<strong>in</strong>gthem or exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g documents <strong>and</strong> other forms of communicationthat they create. In this chapter, we discussobservation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> some detail. We willdiscuss the analysis of documents <strong>in</strong> Chapter 20.ObservationCerta<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of research questions can best be answeredby observ<strong>in</strong>g how people act or how th<strong>in</strong>gs look. For example,researchers could <strong>in</strong>terview teachers about howtheir students behave dur<strong>in</strong>g class discussions of sensitiveissues, but a more accurate <strong>in</strong>dication of their activitieswould probably be obta<strong>in</strong>ed by actually observ<strong>in</strong>gsuch discussions while they take place.The degree of observer participation can vary considerably.There are four different roles that a researchercan take, rang<strong>in</strong>g on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum from complete participant<strong>to</strong> complete observer.440


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 441PARTICIPANT OBSERVATIONIn participant observation studies, researchers actuallyparticipate <strong>in</strong> the situation or sett<strong>in</strong>g they are observ<strong>in</strong>g.When a researcher takes on the role of a completeparticipant <strong>in</strong> a group, his identity is not known <strong>to</strong> any ofthe <strong>in</strong>dividuals be<strong>in</strong>g observed. The researcher <strong>in</strong>teractswith members of the group as naturally as possible <strong>and</strong>,for all <strong>in</strong>tents <strong>and</strong> purposes (so far as they are concerned),is one of them. Thus, a researcher might arrange<strong>to</strong> serve for a year as an actual teacher <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-cityclassroom <strong>and</strong> carry out all of the duties <strong>and</strong> responsibilitiesthat are a part of that role, but not reveal that he isalso a researcher. Such covert observation is suspect onethical grounds.When a researcher chooses the role of participant-asobserver,he participates fully <strong>in</strong> the activities of the groupbe<strong>in</strong>g studied, but also makes it clear that he is do<strong>in</strong>g research.As an example, the researcher described abovemight tell the faculty that he is a researcher <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong>describe as thoroughly <strong>and</strong> accurately as he can what goeson <strong>in</strong> the school over the course of a year’s time.Participant observation can be overt, <strong>in</strong> that the researcheris easily identified <strong>and</strong> the subjects know thatthey are be<strong>in</strong>g observed; or it can be covert, <strong>in</strong> whichcase the researcher disguises his or her identity <strong>and</strong> actsjust like any of the other participants. For example, a researchermight ask a n<strong>in</strong>th-grade geography teacher <strong>to</strong>allow him <strong>to</strong> observe one of that teacher’s classes overthe course of a semester. Both teacher <strong>and</strong> studentswould know the researcher’s identity. This would be anexample of overt observation. Overt participant observationis a key <strong>in</strong>gredient <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research,which we will discuss <strong>in</strong> more detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 21.On the other h<strong>and</strong>, another researcher might take thetrouble <strong>to</strong> become certified as an elementary schoolteacher <strong>and</strong> then spend a period of time actually teach<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> an elementary school while observ<strong>in</strong>g what isgo<strong>in</strong>g on. No one would know the researcher’s identity(with the possible exception of the district adm<strong>in</strong>istrationfrom whom permission would have been obta<strong>in</strong>edbeforeh<strong>and</strong>). This would be an example of covert observation.Covert participant observation, although likely <strong>to</strong>produce more valid observations of what really happens,is often criticized on ethical grounds. Observ<strong>in</strong>g peoplewithout their knowledge (<strong>and</strong>/or record<strong>in</strong>g their commentswithout their permission) seems <strong>to</strong> some a highlyquestionable practice.Is it ethical <strong>to</strong> observe people without their knowledge?What about so-called passive deception, such as that <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g people as they go about their bus<strong>in</strong>ess<strong>in</strong> public places, like restaurants <strong>and</strong> airports? Or whatabout observ<strong>in</strong>g children’s schoolyard activities from adistance us<strong>in</strong>g a telepho<strong>to</strong> lens? What do you th<strong>in</strong>k?NONPARTICIPANT OBSERVATIONIn a nonparticipant observation study, researchers donot participate <strong>in</strong> the activity be<strong>in</strong>g observed but rather“sit on the sidel<strong>in</strong>es” <strong>and</strong> watch; they are not directly <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> the situation they are observ<strong>in</strong>g.When a researcher chooses the role of observer-asparticipant,she identifies herself as a researcher butmakes no pretense of actually be<strong>in</strong>g a member of thegroup she is observ<strong>in</strong>g. An example might be a universityprofessor who is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> what goes on <strong>in</strong> an<strong>in</strong>ner-city school. The researcher might conduct a seriesof <strong>in</strong>terviews with teachers <strong>in</strong> the school, visit classes,attend faculty meet<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> collective barga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g negotiations,talk with pr<strong>in</strong>cipals <strong>and</strong> the super<strong>in</strong>tendent, <strong>and</strong>talk with students, but she would not attempt <strong>to</strong> participate<strong>in</strong> the activities of the group other than superficially.She rema<strong>in</strong>s essentially (<strong>and</strong> does not hide the fact thatshe is) an <strong>in</strong>terested observer who is do<strong>in</strong>g research.F<strong>in</strong>ally, the role of complete observer is just that—arole at the opposite extreme from the role of completeparticipant. The researcher observes the activities of agroup without <strong>in</strong> any way participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> those activities.The subjects of the researcher’s observations may,or may not, realize they are be<strong>in</strong>g observed. An examplewould be a researcher who observes the daily activities<strong>in</strong> a school lunchroom.*Each of the observer roles we have described hasboth advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages. The complete participantis probably most likely <strong>to</strong> get the truest pictureof a group’s activities, <strong>and</strong> the others less so, but the ethicalquestion <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g covert observation rema<strong>in</strong>s. Thecomplete observer is probably least likely <strong>to</strong> affect the actionsof the group be<strong>in</strong>g studied, the others more so. Theparticipant-as-observer, s<strong>in</strong>ce he or she is an actualmember of the group be<strong>in</strong>g studied, will have some (<strong>and</strong>often an important) effect on what the group does. Theparticipant-as-observer <strong>and</strong> the observer-as-participantare both likely, <strong>in</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees, <strong>to</strong> focus the attentionof the group on the activities of the researcher <strong>and</strong> awayfrom their normal rout<strong>in</strong>e, thereby mak<strong>in</strong>g their activitiesno longer typical. Figure 19.1 <strong>in</strong>dicates how approaches<strong>to</strong> observation can vary.*Note that many of the techniques described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7 are alsoexamples of nonparticipant observation frequently used <strong>in</strong> bothqualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative studies.


442 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 19.1 Variations<strong>in</strong> Approaches <strong>to</strong>ObservationFull-participan<strong>to</strong>bservationParticipants knowthat observations are be<strong>in</strong>gmade <strong>and</strong> they know who ismak<strong>in</strong>g them.Role of the ObserverPartialparticipation<strong>How</strong> the Observer Is Portrayed <strong>to</strong> OthersSome but notall of theparticipantsknow the observer.<strong>How</strong> the Purpose of the Observation Is Portrayed <strong>to</strong> OthersOnlooker;observer is an outsiderParticipants do not knowthat observations are be<strong>in</strong>gmade or that there issomeone observ<strong>in</strong>g them.The purpose of theobservation is fully expla<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>to</strong> all <strong>in</strong>volved.A s<strong>in</strong>gle observation of limitedduration (e.g., 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes).The purpose of theobservation isexpla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> some ofthe participants.Narrow focus: Only a s<strong>in</strong>gleelement or characteristic is observed.Duration of the ObservationsFocus of the ObservationsNo explanation isgiven <strong>to</strong> any of theparticipants.False explanations aregiven; participants aredeceived about thepurpose of theobservation.Multiple observations; long-termduration (e.g., months, even years).Broad focus: Holistic view of the activity orcharacteristic be<strong>in</strong>g observed <strong>and</strong> all ofits elements is sought.NATURALISTIC OBSERVATIONNaturalistic observation <strong>in</strong>volves observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>in</strong> their natural sett<strong>in</strong>gs. The researcher makes no effortwhatsoever <strong>to</strong> manipulate variables or <strong>to</strong> control theactivities of <strong>in</strong>dividuals, but simply observes <strong>and</strong>records what happens as th<strong>in</strong>gs naturally occur. The activitiesof students at an athletic event, the <strong>in</strong>teractionsbetween students <strong>and</strong> teachers on the playground, or theactivities of very young children <strong>in</strong> a nursery, for example,are probably best unders<strong>to</strong>od through naturalisticobservation.Much of the work of the famous child psychologistJean Piaget <strong>in</strong>volved naturalistic observation. Many ofhis conclusions on cognitive development, which grewout of watch<strong>in</strong>g his own children as they developed,have stimulated further research <strong>in</strong> this area. Insightsobta<strong>in</strong>ed as a result of naturalistic observation, <strong>in</strong> fact,often serve as the basis for more formal experiments.SIMULATIONSTo <strong>in</strong>vestigate certa<strong>in</strong> variables, researchers sometimeswill create a situation <strong>and</strong> ask subjects <strong>to</strong> act out, or simulate,certa<strong>in</strong> roles. In simulations, the researcher, <strong>in</strong>effect, actually tells the subjects what <strong>to</strong> do (but not how<strong>to</strong> do it). This permits a researcher <strong>to</strong> observe what happens<strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of situations, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those tha<strong>to</strong>ccur fairly <strong>in</strong>frequently <strong>in</strong> schools or other educationalsett<strong>in</strong>gs. For example, <strong>in</strong>dividuals might be asked <strong>to</strong> portraya counselor <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with a distraught parent, ateacher discipl<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a student, or two adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs discuss<strong>in</strong>gtheir views on enhanc<strong>in</strong>g teacher morale.Two ma<strong>in</strong> types of role-play<strong>in</strong>g simulations are usedby researchers <strong>in</strong> education: <strong>in</strong>dividual role play<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>team role play<strong>in</strong>g. In <strong>in</strong>dividual role play<strong>in</strong>g, a person isasked <strong>to</strong> role-play how he or she th<strong>in</strong>ks a particular <strong>in</strong>dividualmight act <strong>in</strong> a given situation. The researcherthen observes <strong>and</strong> records what happens. Here is anexample:You are an elementary school counselor. You have anappo<strong>in</strong>tment with a student who is frequently abusive<strong>to</strong>ward his teachers. The student has just arrived for his9:00 A.M. appo<strong>in</strong>tment with you <strong>and</strong> is sitt<strong>in</strong>g beforeyou <strong>in</strong> your office. What do you say <strong>to</strong> this student?In team role play<strong>in</strong>g, a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals is asked<strong>to</strong> act out a particular situation, with the researcheraga<strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> record<strong>in</strong>g what goes on. Particular


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 443attention is paid <strong>to</strong> how the members of the group <strong>in</strong>teract.Here is an example:You <strong>and</strong> five of your faculty colleagues have been appo<strong>in</strong>tedas a temporary special committee <strong>to</strong> discuss <strong>and</strong>come up with solutions <strong>to</strong> the problem of students cutt<strong>in</strong>gclasses, which has been <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g this semester. Many ofthe faculty support a “get <strong>to</strong>ugh” policy <strong>and</strong> have openlyadvocated suspend<strong>in</strong>g students who are frequent cutters.The group’s assignment is <strong>to</strong> come up with other alternativesthat the faculty will accept. What do you propose?The ma<strong>in</strong> disadvantage <strong>to</strong> simulations, as you mighthave recognized, is their artificiality. Situations arebe<strong>in</strong>g acted out, <strong>and</strong> there is no guarantee that what theresearcher sees is what would normally occur <strong>in</strong> a reallifesituation. The results of a simulation often serve ashypotheses <strong>in</strong> other k<strong>in</strong>ds of research <strong>in</strong>vestigations.OBSERVER EFFECTThe presence of an observer can have a considerableimpact on the behavior of those be<strong>in</strong>g observed <strong>and</strong>,hence, on the outcomes of a study; this is known as anobserver effect. Also the observational data (thatwhich the observer records) <strong>in</strong>evitably <strong>to</strong> some extentreflect the biases <strong>and</strong> viewpo<strong>in</strong>ts of the observer. Let usconsider each of these facts a bit further.There is always the problem of reactivity <strong>in</strong> observationalresearch. Gett<strong>in</strong>g around the reactivity problem<strong>in</strong>volves stay<strong>in</strong>g around long enough <strong>to</strong> get people used<strong>to</strong> the observer’s presence. As Bernard suggests, eventually“people just get pla<strong>in</strong> tired of try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> manage yourimpression <strong>and</strong> they act naturally. In [spot sampl<strong>in</strong>g] research,the trick is <strong>to</strong> catch a glimpse of people <strong>in</strong> their naturalactivities before they see you com<strong>in</strong>g on the scene—before they have a chance <strong>to</strong> modify their behavior.” 1Unless a researcher is concealed, it is quite likely thathe or she will have some effect on the behavior of those<strong>in</strong>dividuals who are be<strong>in</strong>g observed. Two th<strong>in</strong>gs canhappen, particularly if an observer is unexpected. First,he or she is likely <strong>to</strong> arouse curiosity <strong>and</strong> result <strong>in</strong> a lackof attention <strong>to</strong> the task at h<strong>and</strong>, thus produc<strong>in</strong>g otherthan-normalbehavior. An <strong>in</strong>experienced researcher whorecords such behavior might easily be misled. It is forthis reason that researchers who observe <strong>in</strong> classrooms,for example, usually alert the teacher beforeh<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>ask <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>troduced. They then may spend four <strong>to</strong> fivedays <strong>in</strong> the classroom before start<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> record observations(<strong>to</strong> enable the students <strong>to</strong> become accus<strong>to</strong>med <strong>to</strong>their presence <strong>and</strong> go about their usual activities).The second th<strong>in</strong>g that can happen is that the behaviorof those who are be<strong>in</strong>g observed might be <strong>in</strong>fluenced bythe researcher’s purpose. For example, suppose a researcheris <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g whether social studiesteachers ask “high-level questions” dur<strong>in</strong>g class discussionsof controversial issues. If the teachers are aware ofwhat the researcher is look<strong>in</strong>g for, they may tend <strong>to</strong> askmore questions than normal, thus giv<strong>in</strong>g a dis<strong>to</strong>rted impressionof what really goes on dur<strong>in</strong>g a typical classdiscussion. The data obta<strong>in</strong>ed by the researcher’s observationwould not be representative of how the teachersnormally behave. It is for this reason that many researchersargue that the participants <strong>in</strong> a study should notbe <strong>in</strong>formed of the study’s purposes until after the datahave been collected. Instead, the researchers should meetwith the participants before the study beg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>and</strong> tell themthat they cannot be <strong>in</strong>formed of the purpose of the studys<strong>in</strong>ce it might affect the study’s outcomes. As soon as thedata have been collected, however, the researcher shouldreveal the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> those who are <strong>in</strong>terested.OBSERVER BIASObserver bias refers <strong>to</strong> the possibility that certa<strong>in</strong> characteristicsor ideas of observers may bias what they“see.” Over the years, qualitative researchers have cont<strong>in</strong>uallyhad <strong>to</strong> deal with the charge that it is very easyfor their prejudices <strong>to</strong> bias their data. But this is someth<strong>in</strong>gwith which all researchers must deal. It is probablytrue that no matter how hard observers try <strong>to</strong> beimpartial, their observations will possess some degreeof bias. No one can be <strong>to</strong>tally objective, as we all are <strong>in</strong>fluenced<strong>to</strong> some degree by our past experiences, which<strong>in</strong> turn affect how we see the world <strong>and</strong> the peoplewith<strong>in</strong> it. Nevertheless, all researchers should do theirbest <strong>to</strong> become aware of, <strong>and</strong> try <strong>to</strong> control, their biases.What qualitative researchers try <strong>to</strong> do is <strong>to</strong> study thesubjective fac<strong>to</strong>rs objectively. They do this <strong>in</strong> a numberof ways. They spend a considerable amount of time at thesite, gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> know their subjects <strong>and</strong> the environment(both physical <strong>and</strong> cultural) <strong>in</strong> which they live. They collectcopious amounts of data <strong>and</strong> check their perceptionsaga<strong>in</strong>st what the data reveal. Realiz<strong>in</strong>g that most situations<strong>and</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>gs are very complex, they do their best <strong>to</strong>collect data from a variety of perspectives, us<strong>in</strong>g a varietyof formats. Not only do they prepare extremely detailedfield notes, but they attempt <strong>to</strong> reflect on their ownsubjectivity as a part of these field notes. Often theywork <strong>in</strong> teams so that they can check their observationsaga<strong>in</strong>st another’s (Figure 19.2). Although they realize


444 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e"These conclusionsare pretty obvious.Surely you agreewith them?""Wait a m<strong>in</strong>ute!Have you anysupport for them by asecond observer?"Figure 19.2 The Importance of a Second Observer as a Check on One’s Conclusions(as should all researchers) that one’s biases can never becompletely elim<strong>in</strong>ated from one’s observations, the importantth<strong>in</strong>g is <strong>to</strong> reflect on how one’s own attitudes may<strong>in</strong>fluence what one perceives.A related concern here is observer expectations.If researchers know they are <strong>to</strong> observe subjects whohave certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics (such as a certa<strong>in</strong> IQ range,ethnicity, or religion), they may “expect” a certa<strong>in</strong> type ofbehavior, which may not be how the subjects normallybehave. It is <strong>in</strong> this regard that audiotap<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> videotap<strong>in</strong>gsare so valuable, as they allow researchers <strong>to</strong> checktheir observations aga<strong>in</strong>st the impressions of others.CODING OBSERVATIONAL DATAOver the years, quantitative researchers have developed anumber of cod<strong>in</strong>g schemes <strong>to</strong> use when they observe. Acod<strong>in</strong>g scheme is a set of categories (e.g., “gives directions”;“asks questions”; “praises”) that an observer uses<strong>to</strong> record the frequency of a person’s or group’s behavior.Cod<strong>in</strong>g schemes have been used <strong>to</strong> measure <strong>in</strong>teractionsbetween parents <strong>and</strong> adolescent children <strong>in</strong> a labora<strong>to</strong>rysett<strong>in</strong>g; 2 <strong>in</strong>teractions of college students dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g alcohol<strong>in</strong> a group sett<strong>in</strong>g; 3 doc<strong>to</strong>r-patient <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong> theoffice of family physicians; 4 <strong>and</strong> student-teacher <strong>in</strong>teractions<strong>in</strong> a classroom. 5 One such cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme, primarilyused <strong>in</strong> quantitative research, was developed by Amidon<strong>and</strong> Fl<strong>and</strong>ers more than 30 years ago but is still <strong>in</strong> use. 6 Itis shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 19.3.These schemes require the observer <strong>to</strong> judge <strong>and</strong> categorizebehavior as it occurs. This is <strong>in</strong> contrast <strong>to</strong> morequalitative approaches that attempt <strong>to</strong> describe all ormost of what occurs <strong>in</strong> a given situation. At a later time,these data are coded <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categories that emerge as theanalysis proceeds. This is particularly true <strong>in</strong> ethnographicresearch. We shall give an example of this typeof cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Chapter 20.THE USE OF TECHNOLOGYEven with a fixed cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme like the one shown <strong>in</strong>Figure 19.3, however, the observer must still choose fromamong alternatives when cod<strong>in</strong>g the behavior of people.When is someone be<strong>in</strong>g “critical,” for example, or “encourag<strong>in</strong>g”?Record<strong>in</strong>g the behavior of people on film orvideo permits the researcher <strong>to</strong> repeatedly view the behaviorof an <strong>in</strong>dividual or a group <strong>and</strong> then decide how <strong>to</strong> codeit at a later, usually more relaxed <strong>and</strong> convenient time.Furthermore, a major difficulty <strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g people isthe fact that much that goes on may be missed by the observer.This is especially true when several behaviors of<strong>in</strong>terest are occurr<strong>in</strong>g rapidly <strong>in</strong> an educational sett<strong>in</strong>g. Inaddition, sometimes a researcher wants <strong>to</strong> have someoneelse (such as an expert on the <strong>to</strong>pic of <strong>in</strong>terest) offer his or


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 445TeacherTalkIndirectInfluenceDirectInfluence1. Accepts feel<strong>in</strong>g: accepts <strong>and</strong> clarifies the feel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ne of the students <strong>in</strong> anonthreaten<strong>in</strong>g manner. Feel<strong>in</strong>gs may be positive or negative. Predict<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>recall<strong>in</strong>g feel<strong>in</strong>gs are <strong>in</strong>cluded.2. Praises or encourages: praises or encourages student action or behavior. Jokesthat release tension, not at the expense of another <strong>in</strong>dividual, nodd<strong>in</strong>g head orsay<strong>in</strong>g “uh huh?” or “go on” are <strong>in</strong>cluded.3. Accepts or uses ideas of student: clarify<strong>in</strong>g, build<strong>in</strong>g, or develop<strong>in</strong>g ideas orsuggestions by a student. As teacher br<strong>in</strong>gs more of his or her own ideas <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> play,shift <strong>to</strong> category five.4. Asks questions: ask<strong>in</strong>g a question about content or procedure with the <strong>in</strong>tent thata student answer.5. Lectures: giv<strong>in</strong>g facts or op<strong>in</strong>ions about content or procedure; express<strong>in</strong>g his or herown ideas; ask<strong>in</strong>g rhe<strong>to</strong>rical questions.6. Gives directions: directions, comm<strong>and</strong>s, or orders with which a student is expected<strong>to</strong> comply.7. Criticizes or justifies authority: statements, <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> change student behaviorfrom nonacceptable <strong>to</strong> acceptable pattern; bawl<strong>in</strong>g someone out; stat<strong>in</strong>g why theteacher is do<strong>in</strong>g what he or she is do<strong>in</strong>g, extreme self-reference.StudentTalk8. Student talk-response: talk by students <strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> teacher. Teacher <strong>in</strong>itiates thecontact or solicits student statement.9. Student talk-<strong>in</strong>itiation: talk by students, which they <strong>in</strong>itiate. If “call<strong>in</strong>g on” studentis only <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate who may talk next, observer must decide whether studentwanted <strong>to</strong> talk. If he or she did, use this category.10. Silence or confusion: pauses, short periods of silence, <strong>and</strong> periods of confusion <strong>in</strong>which communication cannot be unders<strong>to</strong>od by the observer.Figure 19.3 The Amidon/Fl<strong>and</strong>ers Scheme for Cod<strong>in</strong>g Categories of Interaction <strong>in</strong> theClassroomSource: E. J. Amidon <strong>and</strong> J. B. Hough (1967). Interaction analysis: Theory, research, <strong>and</strong> application. Read<strong>in</strong>g,MA: Addison-Wesley.her <strong>in</strong>sights about what is happen<strong>in</strong>g. A researcher whoobserves a number of children’s play sessions <strong>in</strong> a nurseryschool sett<strong>in</strong>g, for example, might want <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> theideas of a qualified child psychologist or an experiencedteacher of preschool children about what is happen<strong>in</strong>g.To overcome these obstacles, researchers may useaudiotapes or videotapes <strong>to</strong> record their observations.These have several advantages. The tapes may be replayedseveral times for cont<strong>in</strong>ued study <strong>and</strong> analysis.Experts or <strong>in</strong>terested others can also hear <strong>and</strong>/or seewhat the researcher observed <strong>and</strong> offer their <strong>in</strong>sights accord<strong>in</strong>gly.And a permanent record of certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds ofbehaviors is obta<strong>in</strong>ed for comparison with later or differentsamples.There are a few disadvantages <strong>to</strong> such tap<strong>in</strong>gs, however,which also should be noted. Videotap<strong>in</strong>gs are notalways the easiest <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> usually require tra<strong>in</strong>edtechnicians <strong>to</strong> do the tap<strong>in</strong>g, unless the researcher hashad some tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> experience <strong>in</strong> this area. Oftenseveral microphones must be set up, which can dis<strong>to</strong>rtthe behavior of those be<strong>in</strong>g observed. Prolonged tap<strong>in</strong>gcan be expensive. Audiotap<strong>in</strong>gs are somewhat easier <strong>to</strong>do, but they of course record only verbal behavior. Furthermore,sometimes it is difficult <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish specificspeakers <strong>in</strong> a record<strong>in</strong>g of many voices. Noise isdifficult <strong>to</strong> control <strong>and</strong> often seriously <strong>in</strong>terferes withthe underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of content. Nevertheless, if these difficultiescan be overcome, the use of audio- <strong>and</strong> videotap<strong>in</strong>goffers considerable promise <strong>to</strong> researchers as away <strong>to</strong> collect, s<strong>to</strong>re, <strong>and</strong> analyze data.Interview<strong>in</strong>gA second method used by qualitative researchers <strong>to</strong> collectdata is <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview selected <strong>in</strong>dividuals. Interview<strong>in</strong>g(i.e., the careful ask<strong>in</strong>g of relevant questions) is animportant way for a researcher <strong>to</strong> check the accuracyof—<strong>to</strong> verify or refute—the impressions he or she hasga<strong>in</strong>ed through observation. Fetterman, <strong>in</strong> fact, describes<strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g as the most important data collection techniquea qualitative researcher possesses. 7


446 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe purpose of <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g people is <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat is on their m<strong>in</strong>ds—what they th<strong>in</strong>k or how theyfeel about someth<strong>in</strong>g. As Pat<strong>to</strong>n has remarked:We <strong>in</strong>terview people <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out from them those th<strong>in</strong>gs wecannot directly observe. The issue is not whetherobservational data is more desirable, valid, or mean<strong>in</strong>gfulthan self-report data. The fact of the matter is that wecannot observe everyth<strong>in</strong>g. We cannot observe feel<strong>in</strong>gs,thoughts, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tentions. We cannot observe behaviors that<strong>to</strong>ok place at some previous po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time. We cannot observesituations that preclude the presence of an observer.We cannot observe how people have organized the world<strong>and</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>gs they attach <strong>to</strong> what goes on <strong>in</strong> the world.We have <strong>to</strong> ask people questions about those th<strong>in</strong>gs. 8TYPES OF INTERVIEWSThere are four types of <strong>in</strong>terviews: structured, semistructured,<strong>in</strong>formal, <strong>and</strong> retrospective. Although thesedifferent types often blend <strong>and</strong> merge <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> one another,we shall describe them separately <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> clarifyhow they differ.Structured <strong>and</strong> semistructured <strong>in</strong>terviews are verbalquestionnaires. Rather formal, they consist of a seriesof questions designed <strong>to</strong> elicit specific answersfrom respondents. Often they are used <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formationthat can later be compared <strong>and</strong> contrasted. Forexample, a researcher <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> how the characteristicsof teachers <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ner-city <strong>and</strong> suburban schools differmight conduct a structured <strong>in</strong>terview (i.e., ask<strong>in</strong>g a se<strong>to</strong>f structured questions) with a group of <strong>in</strong>ner-city highschool teachers <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> background <strong>in</strong>formation aboutthem—their education, their qualifications, their previousexperience, their out-of-school activities, <strong>and</strong> soon—<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> compare these data with the same data(i.e., answers <strong>to</strong> the same questions) obta<strong>in</strong>ed from agroup of teachers who teach <strong>in</strong> the suburbs. In qualitativeresearch, structured <strong>and</strong> semistructured <strong>in</strong>terviewsare often best conducted <strong>to</strong>ward the end of a study, asthey tend <strong>to</strong> shape responses <strong>to</strong> the researcher’s perceptionsof how th<strong>in</strong>gs are. They are most useful for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> test a specific hypothesis that the researcherhas <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d.Informal <strong>in</strong>terviews are much less formal than structuredor semistructured <strong>in</strong>terviews. They tend <strong>to</strong> resemblecasual conversations, pursu<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terests of boththe researcher <strong>and</strong> the respondent <strong>in</strong> turn. They are themost common type of <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> qualitative research.They do not <strong>in</strong>volve any specific type or sequence ofquestions or any particular form of question<strong>in</strong>g. The primary<strong>in</strong>tent of an <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terview is <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out what© The New Yorker Collection 2000 Edward Koren from car<strong>to</strong>onbank.com.All Rights Reserved.people th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>and</strong> how the views of one <strong>in</strong>dividual comparewith those of another.Although at first glance they seem like they would beeasy <strong>to</strong> conduct, <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews are probably themost difficult of all <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>to</strong> do well. Issues ofethics appear almost immediately. <strong>Research</strong>ers oftenneed <strong>to</strong> make some sensitive decisions as an <strong>in</strong>formal<strong>in</strong>terview progresses. When, for example, is a question<strong>to</strong>o personal <strong>to</strong> pursue? To what extent should the researcher“dig deeper” <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> how an <strong>in</strong>dividual feels aboutsometh<strong>in</strong>g? When is it more appropriate <strong>to</strong> refra<strong>in</strong> fromprob<strong>in</strong>g further about an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s response? <strong>How</strong>, <strong>in</strong>fact, does a researcher establish a climate of ease <strong>and</strong> familiaritywhile at the same time try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>in</strong> somedetail about a respondent’s life?Although <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews offer the most naturaltype of situation for the collection of data, there is alwayssome degree of artificiality present <strong>in</strong> any type of<strong>in</strong>terview. A skillful <strong>in</strong>terviewer, however, soon learns<strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> with nonthreaten<strong>in</strong>g questions <strong>to</strong> put a respondentat ease before he or she poses more personal <strong>and</strong>(potentially) threaten<strong>in</strong>g questions. Always, the researchermust establish an atmosphere of trust, cooperation,<strong>and</strong> mutual respect if he or she is <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> accurate<strong>in</strong>formation. Plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> ask<strong>in</strong>g good questions, whiledevelop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g an atmosphere of mutualtrust <strong>and</strong> respect, is an art that anyone who wishes <strong>to</strong> docompetent qualitative research must master.Retrospective <strong>in</strong>terviews can be structured, semistructured,or <strong>in</strong>formal. A researcher who conducts a


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 447retrospective <strong>in</strong>terview tries <strong>to</strong> get a respondent <strong>to</strong> recall<strong>and</strong> then reconstruct from memory someth<strong>in</strong>g that hashappened <strong>in</strong> the past. A retrospective <strong>in</strong>terview is theleast likely of the four <strong>in</strong>terview types <strong>to</strong> provide accurate,reliable data for the researcher.Table 19.1 summarizes some of the major <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gstrategies used <strong>in</strong> educational research. The first threestrategies are more likely (although not exclusively) <strong>to</strong>be utilized <strong>in</strong> qualitative studies, the fourth more likely(but aga<strong>in</strong>, not exclusively) <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies. Thereader is rem<strong>in</strong>ded, however, that it is not uncommon<strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d several of these strategies employed <strong>in</strong> thesame study.KEY-ACTOR INTERVIEWSSome people <strong>in</strong> any group are more <strong>in</strong>formed aboutthe culture <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry of their group, as well as morearticulate, than others. Such <strong>in</strong>dividuals, traditionallycalled key <strong>in</strong>formants, are especially useful sources ofTABLE 19.1 Interview<strong>in</strong>g Strategies Used <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Type of Interview Characteristics Strengths WeaknessesInformalconversational<strong>in</strong>terviewInterview guideapproachSt<strong>and</strong>ardizedopen-ended<strong>in</strong>terviewClosed, fixedresponse<strong>in</strong>terviewQuestions emerge from theimmediate context <strong>and</strong> areasked <strong>in</strong> the natural courseof th<strong>in</strong>gs; there is nopredeterm<strong>in</strong>ation ofquestion <strong>to</strong>pics or word<strong>in</strong>g.Topics <strong>and</strong> issues <strong>to</strong> becovered are specified <strong>in</strong>advance, <strong>in</strong> outl<strong>in</strong>e form;<strong>in</strong>terviewer decidessequence <strong>and</strong> word<strong>in</strong>g ofquestions <strong>in</strong> the course ofthe <strong>in</strong>terview.The exact word<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>sequence of questions aredeterm<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> advance. All<strong>in</strong>terviewees are asked thesame basic questions <strong>in</strong> thesame order. Questions areworded <strong>in</strong> a completelyopen-ended format.Questions <strong>and</strong> responsecategories are determ<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> advance. Responses arefixed; respondent choosesfrom among these fixedresponses.Increases the salience <strong>and</strong>relevance of questions;<strong>in</strong>terviews are built on <strong>and</strong>emerge from observations; the<strong>in</strong>terview can be matched <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong> circumstances.The outl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>creases thecomprehensiveness of the data<strong>and</strong> makes data collectionsomewhat systematic for eachrespondent. Logical gaps <strong>in</strong> datacan be anticipated <strong>and</strong> closed.Interviews rema<strong>in</strong> fairlyconversational <strong>and</strong> situational.Respondents answer the samequestions, thus <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gcomparability of responses; dataare complete for each personon the <strong>to</strong>pics addressed <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>terview. Reduces <strong>in</strong>terviewereffects <strong>and</strong> bias when several<strong>in</strong>terviewers are used. Permitsevaluation users <strong>to</strong> see <strong>and</strong>review the <strong>in</strong>strumentationused <strong>in</strong> the evaluation.Facilitates organization <strong>and</strong>analysis of the data.Data analysis is simple;responses can be directlycompared <strong>and</strong> easilyaggregated; many questions canbe asked<strong>in</strong> a short time.Different <strong>in</strong>formation collectedfrom different people withdifferent questions. Lesssystematic <strong>and</strong> comprehensive ifcerta<strong>in</strong> questions do not arise“naturally.” Data organization <strong>and</strong>analysis can be quite difficult.Important <strong>and</strong> salient <strong>to</strong>pics maybe <strong>in</strong>advertently omitted. Interviewerflexibility <strong>in</strong> sequenc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>word<strong>in</strong>g questions can result <strong>in</strong>substantially different responsesfrom different perspectives, thusreduc<strong>in</strong>g the comparability ofresponses.Little flexibility <strong>in</strong> relat<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>terview <strong>to</strong> particular <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>and</strong> circumstances; st<strong>and</strong>ardizedword<strong>in</strong>g of questions mayconstra<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> limit naturalness<strong>and</strong> relevance of questions <strong>and</strong>answers.Respondents must fit theirexperiences <strong>and</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> theresearcher’s categories; may beperceived as impersonal, irrelevant,<strong>and</strong> mechanistic. Can dis<strong>to</strong>rt whatrespondents really mean or haveexperienced by so completelylimit<strong>in</strong>g their response choices.Source: Qualitative research <strong>and</strong> evaluation methods, by Michael Qu<strong>in</strong>n Pat<strong>to</strong>n, Copyright © 2008 by Sage Publications Inc. Books.Reproduced with permission of Sage Publications Inc. Books <strong>in</strong> the textbook format via Copyright Clearance Center.


448 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>in</strong>formation. Fetterman prefers the term key ac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong>avoid the stigma attached <strong>to</strong> the term <strong>in</strong>formant, as wellas the his<strong>to</strong>rical roots that underlie the term. 9 Key ac<strong>to</strong>rsare especially knowledgeable <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong> thus oftenexcellent sources of <strong>in</strong>formation. They can often providedetailed <strong>in</strong>formation about a group’s past <strong>and</strong> about contemporaryhappen<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> relationships, as well as theeveryday nuances—the ord<strong>in</strong>ary details—that othersmight miss. They offer <strong>in</strong>sights that are often <strong>in</strong>valuable<strong>to</strong> a researcher. Fetterman gives an example of a keyac<strong>to</strong>r who proved helpful <strong>to</strong> him <strong>in</strong> a study of schooldropouts.James was a long-term jani<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> the Detroit dropout program[a program that Fetterman was study<strong>in</strong>g]. He grewup <strong>in</strong> the local community with many of the students<strong>and</strong> was extraord<strong>in</strong>arily perceptive about the differencesbetween the serious <strong>and</strong> less serious students <strong>in</strong> theprogram, as well as between the serious <strong>and</strong> less seriousteachers. I asked him whether he thought the studentswere obey<strong>in</strong>g the new restrictions aga<strong>in</strong>st smok<strong>in</strong>g, wear<strong>in</strong>ghats <strong>in</strong> the build<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> wear<strong>in</strong>g sneakers. He said,“You can tell from the butts on the floor that they is stillsmok<strong>in</strong>’, no matter what dey tell yah. I know, cause Igotta sweep ’em up. ... It’s mostly the new ones, don’tyah know, like Kirk, <strong>and</strong> Dyan, T<strong>in</strong>a. You can catch ’emalmost any ol’ time. I seen ’em dur<strong>in</strong>g class <strong>in</strong> the hallways,here (<strong>in</strong> the cafeteria), <strong>and</strong> afta hours.” He providedempirical evidence <strong>to</strong> support his observations—a pile ofcigarette butts he had swept up while we were talk<strong>in</strong>g. 10Here is another example from Fetterman’s research.In a study of a gifted <strong>and</strong> talented education program, mymost <strong>in</strong>sightful <strong>and</strong> helpful key ac<strong>to</strong>r was a school districtsupervisor. He <strong>to</strong>ld about the politics of the school district<strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong> avoid the turf disputes dur<strong>in</strong>g my study. Hedrove me around the community <strong>to</strong> teach me how <strong>to</strong> identifyeach of the major neighborhoods <strong>and</strong> po<strong>in</strong>ted out correspond<strong>in</strong>gsocioeconomic differences that proved <strong>to</strong> havean important impact on the study. He also described thecyclical nature of the charges of elitism raised aga<strong>in</strong>st theprogram by certa<strong>in</strong> community members <strong>and</strong> a formerschool board member. He confided that his son (who waseligible <strong>to</strong> enter the program) had decided not <strong>to</strong> enter.This <strong>in</strong>formation opened new doors <strong>to</strong> my perception ofpeer pressure <strong>in</strong> that community. 11As you can see, a key ac<strong>to</strong>r can be an extremely valuablesource of <strong>in</strong>formation. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, researchersneed <strong>to</strong> take the time <strong>to</strong> seek out <strong>and</strong> establish a bondof trust with these <strong>in</strong>dividuals. The <strong>in</strong>formation theyprovide can serve as a cross-check on data the researcherobta<strong>in</strong>s from other <strong>in</strong>terviews, from observations, <strong>and</strong>from content analysis. But the mus<strong>in</strong>gs of a key ac<strong>to</strong>rmust also be viewed with some caution. Care must betaken <strong>to</strong> ensure that a key ac<strong>to</strong>r is not merely provid<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation he or she th<strong>in</strong>ks the researcher wants <strong>to</strong>hear. This is why a researcher needs <strong>to</strong> seek out multiplesources of <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> any study.TYPES OF INTERVIEW QUESTIONSPat<strong>to</strong>n has identified six basic types of questions thatcan be asked of people. Any or all of these questionsmight be asked dur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terview. The six types arebackground (or demographic) questions, knowledgequestions, experience (or behavior) questions, op<strong>in</strong>ion(or values) questions, feel<strong>in</strong>gs questions, <strong>and</strong> sensoryquestions. 12Background (or demographic) questions are rout<strong>in</strong>esorts of questions about the background characteristicsof the respondents. They <strong>in</strong>clude questions about education,previous occupations, age, <strong>in</strong>come, <strong>and</strong> the like.Knowledge questions perta<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the factual <strong>in</strong>formation(as contrasted with op<strong>in</strong>ions, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> attitudes)respondents possess. Knowledge questions about aschool, for example, might concern the k<strong>in</strong>ds of coursesavailable <strong>to</strong> students, graduation requirements, the sortsof extracurricular activities provided, school rules, enrollmentpolicies, <strong>and</strong> the like. From a qualitative perspective,what the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out is whatthe respondents consider <strong>to</strong> be factual <strong>in</strong>formation (asopposed <strong>to</strong> beliefs or attitudes).Experience (or behavior) questions focus on whata respondent is currently do<strong>in</strong>g or has done <strong>in</strong> the past.Their <strong>in</strong>tent is <strong>to</strong> elicit descriptions of experience, behaviors,or activities that could have been observed but(for reasons such as the researcher not be<strong>in</strong>g present)were not. Examples might <strong>in</strong>clude, “If I had been <strong>in</strong>your class dur<strong>in</strong>g the past semester, what k<strong>in</strong>ds ofth<strong>in</strong>gs would I have been do<strong>in</strong>g?” or, “If I were <strong>to</strong> followyou through a typical day here at your school, whatexperiences would I be likely <strong>to</strong> see you hav<strong>in</strong>g?”Op<strong>in</strong>ion (or values) questions are aimed at f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gout what people th<strong>in</strong>k about some <strong>to</strong>pic or issue. Answers<strong>to</strong> such questions call attention <strong>to</strong> the respondent’sgoals, beliefs, attitudes, or values. Examplesmight <strong>in</strong>clude such questions as, “What do you th<strong>in</strong>kabout the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal’s new policy concern<strong>in</strong>g absenteeism?”or, “What would you like <strong>to</strong> see changed <strong>in</strong> theway th<strong>in</strong>gs are done <strong>in</strong> your U.S. his<strong>to</strong>ry class?”


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 449Feel<strong>in</strong>gs questions concern how respondents feelabout th<strong>in</strong>gs. They are directed <strong>to</strong>ward people’s emotionalresponses <strong>to</strong> their experiences. Examples might<strong>in</strong>clude such questions as, “<strong>How</strong> do you feel about theway students behave <strong>in</strong> this school?” or, “To what extentare you anxious about go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> gym class?”Feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> op<strong>in</strong>ion questions are often confused. Itis very important for anyone who wishes <strong>to</strong> be a skillful<strong>in</strong>terviewer <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish between the twotypes of questions <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> know when <strong>to</strong> ask each. Tof<strong>in</strong>d out how someone feels about an issue is not thesame th<strong>in</strong>g as f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g out their op<strong>in</strong>ion about the issue.Thus, the question, “What do you th<strong>in</strong>k (what is yourop<strong>in</strong>ion) about your teacher’s homework policy?” asksfor the respondent’s op<strong>in</strong>ion—what he or she th<strong>in</strong>ks—about the policy. The question, “<strong>How</strong> do you feel (whatdo you like or dislike) about your teacher’s homeworkpolicy?” asks how the respondent feels about (his or herattitude <strong>to</strong>ward) the policy. The two, although they appearsomewhat similar, ask for decidedly different k<strong>in</strong>dsof <strong>in</strong>formation.Sensory questions focus on what a respondent hasseen, heard, tasted, smelled, or <strong>to</strong>uched. Examples might<strong>in</strong>clude questions such as, “When you enter your classroom,what do you see?” or, “<strong>How</strong> would you describewhat your class sounds like?” Although this type ofquestion could be considered as a form of experience orbehavior question, it is often overlooked by researchersdur<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terview. Further, such questions are sufficientlydist<strong>in</strong>ct <strong>to</strong> warrant a category of their own.INTERVIEWING BEHAVIORA set of expectations exists for all <strong>in</strong>terviews. Here aresome of the most important.Respect the culture of the group be<strong>in</strong>g studied. Itwould be <strong>in</strong>sensitive, for example, for a researcher <strong>to</strong>wear expensive cloth<strong>in</strong>g while conduct<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terviewwith an impoverished, <strong>in</strong>ner-city high schoolyouth. Of course, a researcher may commit an occasionalfaux pas <strong>in</strong>advertently, which most <strong>in</strong>tervieweeswill forgive. A constant disregard for agroup’s traditions <strong>and</strong> values, however, is bound <strong>to</strong>impede the researcher’s efforts <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> reliable <strong>and</strong>valid <strong>in</strong>formation.Respect the <strong>in</strong>dividual be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviewed. Those whoagree <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed give up time they mightspend elsewhere <strong>to</strong> answer the researcher’s questions.An <strong>in</strong>terview, therefore, should not be viewed as anopportunity <strong>to</strong> criticize or evaluate the <strong>in</strong>terviewee’sactions or ideas; rather, it is an opportunity <strong>to</strong> learnfrom the <strong>in</strong>terviewee. A classroom teacher, a student,a counselor, a school cus<strong>to</strong>dian—all have work <strong>to</strong> do,<strong>and</strong> hence every researcher is well rem<strong>in</strong>ded not <strong>to</strong>waste their time. Interviews should start <strong>and</strong> end atthe scheduled times <strong>and</strong> be conducted courteously.Further, the researcher should pick up on cues givenby the <strong>in</strong>terviewee. As Fetterman po<strong>in</strong>ts out, “repeatedglances at a watch are usually a clear signalthat the time is up. Glazed eyes, a puzzled look, or animpatient scowl is an <strong>in</strong>terviewee’s way of lett<strong>in</strong>g thequestioner know that someth<strong>in</strong>g is wrong. The <strong>in</strong>dividualis lost, bored, or <strong>in</strong>sulted. Common errors <strong>in</strong>volvespend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>o much time talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> not enoughtime listen<strong>in</strong>g, fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> make questions clear, <strong>and</strong>mak<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>advertently <strong>in</strong>sensitive comment.” 13(Figure 19.4 illustrates an example of an <strong>in</strong>tervieweewho is not be<strong>in</strong>g respected.)Be natural. “Act<strong>in</strong>g like an adolescent does not w<strong>in</strong>the confidence of adolescents, it only makes themsuspicious.” 14 Deception <strong>in</strong> any form has no place <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>terview.Develop an appropriate rapport with the participant.Here you have <strong>to</strong> be careful, for dangers lurk. Seidmanpo<strong>in</strong>ts out the problem: “Rapport implies gett<strong>in</strong>galong with each other, a harmony with, a conformity<strong>to</strong>, an aff<strong>in</strong>ity for one another. The problem is that, carried<strong>to</strong> an extreme, the desire <strong>to</strong> build rapport with theparticipant can transform the <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g relationship<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a full ‘We’relationship <strong>in</strong> which the questionof whose experience is be<strong>in</strong>g related <strong>and</strong> whose mean<strong>in</strong>gis be<strong>in</strong>g made is critically confounded. 15 He goeson <strong>to</strong> describe an <strong>in</strong>cident that occurred <strong>in</strong> a study heconducted <strong>in</strong> a community college:In our community college study, one participant <strong>in</strong>vitedmy wife <strong>and</strong> me <strong>to</strong> his house for d<strong>in</strong>ner after (an)<strong>in</strong>terview . . . I had never had such an <strong>in</strong>vitation from aparticipant . . . <strong>and</strong> I did not quite know what <strong>to</strong> do. I didnot want <strong>to</strong> appear ungracious, so we accepted. My wife<strong>and</strong> I went <strong>to</strong> d<strong>in</strong>ner at his home. We had a wonderfulCalifornia backyard cookout <strong>and</strong> it was a pleasure <strong>to</strong>spend time with the participant <strong>and</strong> his family. But a fewdays later, when I met him at his faculty office for thethird <strong>in</strong>terview, he was so warm <strong>and</strong> familiar <strong>to</strong>ward me,that I could not reta<strong>in</strong> the distance that I needed <strong>to</strong> explorehis responses. I felt tentative as an <strong>in</strong>terviewer becauseI did not want <strong>to</strong> risk violat<strong>in</strong>g the spirit of hospitalitythat he had created by <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g us <strong>to</strong> his home. 16


450 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 19.4 AnInterview of DubiousValidity“O.K. Now areyou ready <strong>to</strong> answermy questions?”“When Sallysays she means <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terview somebody,she's not kidd<strong>in</strong>g!”Ask the same question <strong>in</strong> different ways dur<strong>in</strong>g the<strong>in</strong>terview. This enables the researcher <strong>to</strong> check his orher underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of what the <strong>in</strong>terviewee has beensay<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> may even shed new light on the <strong>to</strong>picbe<strong>in</strong>g discussed.Ask the <strong>in</strong>terviewee <strong>to</strong> repeat an answer or statementwhen there is some doubt about the completeness ofa remark. This can stimulate discussion when an <strong>in</strong>tervieweetends <strong>to</strong> respond with terse, short answers<strong>to</strong> the researcher’s questions.Vary who controls the flow of communication. In aformal, structured <strong>in</strong>terview, it is often necessary forthe researcher <strong>to</strong> control the ask<strong>in</strong>g of questions <strong>and</strong>the pace of the discussion. In <strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews,particularly dur<strong>in</strong>g the explora<strong>to</strong>ry or <strong>in</strong>itial phase ofan <strong>in</strong>terview, it is often wise <strong>to</strong> let the <strong>in</strong>tervieweeramble a bit <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> establish a sense of trust <strong>and</strong>cooperation.Avoid lead<strong>in</strong>g questions. Lead<strong>in</strong>g questions presumean answer, as <strong>in</strong> questions like “You wanted <strong>to</strong> dothat, of course?” or “Your friends talked you <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>that, didn’t they?” or “<strong>How</strong> much did that upsetyou?” Each of these questions leads the participant <strong>to</strong>respond <strong>in</strong> a certa<strong>in</strong> way. More appropriate versionsof these questions would be “What did you want <strong>to</strong>do?” <strong>and</strong> “Why did you do that?” <strong>and</strong> “<strong>How</strong> did youfeel about that?”Instead of lead<strong>in</strong>g questions, <strong>in</strong>terviewers oftenask open-ended questions. Open-ended questions<strong>in</strong>dicate an area <strong>to</strong> be explored without suggest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>the participant how it should be explored. They donot presume an answer. Here are some examples:“What was the meet<strong>in</strong>g like for you?” or “Tell mewhat your student teach<strong>in</strong>g experience was like?”There are many possibilities for open-ended questions<strong>and</strong> many ways of ask<strong>in</strong>g them. Perhaps none isbetter than simply ask<strong>in</strong>g “What was that like foryou?” when an <strong>in</strong>terviewer wants <strong>to</strong> get at a participant’ssubjective experience.Do not ask dicho<strong>to</strong>mous questions, that is, questionsthat permit a yes-no answer, when you are try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> geta complete picture. Here are some examples: “Wereyou satisfied with your assignment?” “Have youchanged as a result of teach<strong>in</strong>g at Adams School?”“Was that a good experience for you?” “Did you knowwhat <strong>to</strong> do when you were asked <strong>to</strong> do that?” Andso forth.The problem with dicho<strong>to</strong>mous questions is thatthey do not encourage the respondent <strong>to</strong> talk. Oftentimes,when an <strong>in</strong>terviewer is hav<strong>in</strong>g trouble gett<strong>in</strong>ga participant <strong>to</strong> talk, it is because he or she is ask<strong>in</strong>ga str<strong>in</strong>g of dicho<strong>to</strong>mous questions.Pat<strong>to</strong>n presents what is perhaps the classic exampleof a series of dicho<strong>to</strong>mous questions <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 451conversation between a teenager <strong>and</strong> his parent. Theteen has just returned home from a date:Do you know that you’re late?Yeah.Did you have a good time?Yeah.Did you go <strong>to</strong> a movie?Yeah.Was it a good movie?Yeah, it was okay.So, it was worth see<strong>in</strong>g?Yeah, it was worth see<strong>in</strong>g.I’ve heard a lot about it. Do you th<strong>in</strong>k I would like it?I don’t know. Maybe.Anyth<strong>in</strong>g else you’d like <strong>to</strong> tell me about your even<strong>in</strong>g?No, I guess that’s it.(Teenager goes upstairs <strong>to</strong> bed. One parent turns <strong>to</strong> theother <strong>and</strong> says: “It sure is hard <strong>to</strong> get him <strong>to</strong> talk <strong>to</strong> us.”) 17As you can see, the problem with ask<strong>in</strong>g dicho<strong>to</strong>mousquestions is that they can easily turn an <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> someth<strong>in</strong>g more like a test or <strong>in</strong>terrogation.Ask only one question at a time. Ask<strong>in</strong>g more thanone question is a common error made by novice <strong>in</strong>terviewers,<strong>and</strong> you sometimes see this on poorlydesigned questionnaires as well. Rather than ask<strong>in</strong>gonly a s<strong>in</strong>gle question <strong>and</strong> allow<strong>in</strong>g the participant <strong>to</strong>respond, the <strong>in</strong>terviewer asks several questions oneafter the other without allow<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terviewee <strong>to</strong>answer (Figure 19.5). Here is an example:What was that like for you? Did you participate? You saidyou found it difficult. Was it difficult for you or for theother people who were participat<strong>in</strong>g as well? And how doyou th<strong>in</strong>k they felt about it?• Don’t <strong>in</strong>terrupt. This is perhaps the most importantfeature of good <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g. Don’t <strong>in</strong>terrupt participantswhen they are talk<strong>in</strong>g. And this is especially truewhen a participant says someth<strong>in</strong>g that the <strong>in</strong>terviewerf<strong>in</strong>ds particularly <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g. Often it is tempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terrupt the speaker <strong>to</strong> pursue this <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g item,but <strong>to</strong> do so may <strong>in</strong>terrupt the participant’s tra<strong>in</strong> ofthought. It is better <strong>to</strong> simply jot down a brief note<strong>and</strong> then follow up on it later, when there is a pause<strong>in</strong> the conversation.FOCUS GROUP INTERVIEWSIn a focus group <strong>in</strong>terview, the <strong>in</strong>terviewer asks a smallgroup of people (usually four <strong>to</strong> eight) <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about aseries of questions. The participants are seated <strong>to</strong>gether<strong>in</strong> a group <strong>and</strong> get <strong>to</strong> hear one another’s responses <strong>to</strong> the"The first th<strong>in</strong>g I'd like<strong>to</strong> ask you is how did you f<strong>in</strong>dout about our project — I mean, whodid you talk <strong>to</strong> about it, or did you, orwhy not — <strong>and</strong> what was said?What did you do then?""Huh?"Figure 19.5 Don’tAsk More Than OneQuestion at a Time"PoorMr. Adams.He just can't seem<strong>to</strong> ask a simplequestion."


RESEARCH TIPS<strong>How</strong> Not <strong>to</strong> InterviewFollow<strong>in</strong>g is a hypothetical situation <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a researcher<strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g a teacher who has just f<strong>in</strong>ished us<strong>in</strong>g her district’snew mathematics curriculum.RESEARCHER: This is a very important <strong>to</strong>pic, but don’t benervous. (Fails <strong>to</strong> establish rapport)TEACHER: Okay.RESEARCHER: I assume you had prior experience work<strong>in</strong>gwith this type of mathematics materials?TEACHER: Well, yes, a little.RESEARCHER: That’s <strong>to</strong>o bad. I was hop<strong>in</strong>g you would bemore experienced. (Indicates desired response)TEACHER: Well, actually, now that I th<strong>in</strong>k about it, I diduse similar materials a year or so ago. (Gives desiredresponse)RESEARCHER: Oh, where was that? (Irrelevant comment)TEACHER: In Utah.RESEARCHER: Really? I’m from Utah—how did you like itthere? (Loses focus)TEACHER: I loved it. Ski<strong>in</strong>g was great!RESEARCHER: I’m a tennis player myself.TEACHER: What’s this got <strong>to</strong> do with math?questions. Often they offer additional comments beyondwhat they orig<strong>in</strong>ally had <strong>to</strong> say once they hear theother responses. They may agree or disagree; consensusis neither necessary nor desired. The object is <strong>to</strong> get atwhat people really th<strong>in</strong>k about an issue or issues <strong>in</strong> a socialcontext where the participants can hear the views ofothers <strong>and</strong> consider their own views accord<strong>in</strong>gly.We should stress, however, that a focus group <strong>in</strong>terviewis not a discussion. Neither is it a problem-solv<strong>in</strong>gsession, nor is it a decision-mak<strong>in</strong>g group. It is an<strong>in</strong>terview. 18RECORDING INTERVIEW DATANo matter what k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>terview one conducts, <strong>and</strong> nomatter how carefully one prepares the <strong>in</strong>terview questions,all will be <strong>to</strong> no avail if the <strong>in</strong>terviewer does notcapture what the <strong>in</strong>terviewee actually says. While the<strong>in</strong>terview is go<strong>in</strong>g on, therefore, it is essential <strong>to</strong> recordas faithfully as possible what the participant has <strong>to</strong> say.Some method for record<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>terviewee’s words exactlyis required.A tape recorder, therefore, is often considered an <strong>in</strong>dispensablepart of any qualitative researcher’s equipment.“Tape recorders do not ‘tune out’ conversations,change what has been said because of <strong>in</strong>terpretation(either conscious or unconscious), or record words moreslowly than they are spoken.” 19Us<strong>in</strong>g a tape recorder, however, does not elim<strong>in</strong>atethe need for tak<strong>in</strong>g notes. As Pat<strong>to</strong>n po<strong>in</strong>ts out:Notes can serve at least two purposes: (1) Notes takendur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terview can help the <strong>in</strong>terviewer formulatenew questions as the <strong>in</strong>terview moves along, particularlywhere it may be appropriate <strong>to</strong> check out someth<strong>in</strong>g thatwas said earlier; <strong>and</strong> (2) tak<strong>in</strong>g notes about what is saidwill facilitate later analysis, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g locat<strong>in</strong>g importantquotations from the tape itself . . . the failure <strong>to</strong> takenotes will often <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>to</strong> the respondent that noth<strong>in</strong>gof importance is be<strong>in</strong>g said. 20ETHICS IN INTERVIEWING:THE NECESSITY FOR INFORMED CONSENTIn-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews ask participants <strong>to</strong> reveal much abouttheir lives. Dur<strong>in</strong>g such <strong>in</strong>terviews, a measure of <strong>in</strong>timacycan develop between <strong>in</strong>terviewers <strong>and</strong> participants thatcan lead participants <strong>to</strong> share <strong>in</strong>formation about events <strong>in</strong>their lives that, if misused, could leave them very vulnerable.Participants deserve <strong>to</strong> be protected from suchvulnerability. Furthermore, <strong>in</strong>terviewers also need <strong>to</strong>be protected aga<strong>in</strong>st any misunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g on the par<strong>to</strong>f participants as <strong>to</strong> the nature <strong>and</strong> purpose of the <strong>in</strong>terviewitself.Thus, we believe that it is ethically desirable <strong>in</strong> this<strong>in</strong>stance for <strong>in</strong>terviewers <strong>to</strong> require participants <strong>to</strong> signan <strong>in</strong>formed consent form. We suggest that any suchform <strong>in</strong>clude po<strong>in</strong>ts similar <strong>to</strong> those shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 4.1.DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSISIN QUALITATIVE RESEARCHAs po<strong>in</strong>ted out <strong>in</strong> Chapter 18 <strong>and</strong> described previously,there are important differences between quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative approaches <strong>to</strong> data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis.Although qualitative research can, <strong>and</strong> sometimes452


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 453does, make use of structured <strong>in</strong>struments such as thosedescribed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7, the preference is for less structured,open-ended data collection with structur<strong>in</strong>g tak<strong>in</strong>gplace later through content analysis or emergentthemes (Chapter 20) as the means of data analysis.While other descriptive statistics are often relevant, themost commonly used is report<strong>in</strong>g of frequencies. As theuse of mixed-methods designs cont<strong>in</strong>ues <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease,we expect <strong>to</strong> see more use of quantitative analysis <strong>in</strong>conjunction with more cus<strong>to</strong>mary qualitative analyses.Validity <strong>and</strong> Reliability<strong>in</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>In Chapter 8, we <strong>in</strong>troduced the concepts of validity <strong>and</strong>reliability as they apply <strong>to</strong> the use of <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> educationalresearch. These two concepts are also very important<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, only here they apply <strong>to</strong>the observations researchers make <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> the responsesthey receive <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terview questions. A fundamentalconcern <strong>in</strong> qualitative research, <strong>in</strong> fact, revolves aroundthe degree of confidence researchers can place <strong>in</strong> whatthey have seen or heard. In other words, how can researchersbe sure that they are not be<strong>in</strong>g misled?You will recall that validity refers <strong>to</strong> the appropriateness,mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness, <strong>and</strong> usefulness of the <strong>in</strong>ferencesresearchers make based specifically on the data theycollect, while reliability refers <strong>to</strong> the consistency ofthese <strong>in</strong>ferences over time, location, <strong>and</strong> circumstances.In a qualitative study, much depends on the perspectiveof the researcher. All researchers have certa<strong>in</strong> biases.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, different researchers see someth<strong>in</strong>gs more clearly than others. Qualitative researchersuse a number of techniques, therefore, <strong>to</strong> check theirperceptions <strong>to</strong> ensure that they are not be<strong>in</strong>g mis<strong>in</strong>formed—thatthey are, <strong>in</strong> effect, see<strong>in</strong>g (<strong>and</strong> hear<strong>in</strong>g)what they th<strong>in</strong>k they are. These procedures for check<strong>in</strong>gon or enhanc<strong>in</strong>g validity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong>clude thefollow<strong>in</strong>g:Us<strong>in</strong>g a variety of <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>to</strong> collect data. Whena conclusion is supported by data collected from anumber of different <strong>in</strong>struments, its validity isthereby enhanced. This k<strong>in</strong>d of check<strong>in</strong>g is oftenreferred <strong>to</strong> as triangulation. (See Figure 21.1 <strong>in</strong>Chapter 21.)Check<strong>in</strong>g one <strong>in</strong>formant’s descriptions of someth<strong>in</strong>g(a way of do<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs or a reason for do<strong>in</strong>g someth<strong>in</strong>g)aga<strong>in</strong>st another <strong>in</strong>formant’s descriptions ofthe same th<strong>in</strong>g. Discrepancies <strong>in</strong> descriptions maymean the data are <strong>in</strong>valid.*Learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong>, where appropriate,speak the vocabulary of the group be<strong>in</strong>g studied. Ifresearchers do not underst<strong>and</strong> what <strong>in</strong>formantsmean when they use certa<strong>in</strong> terms (especially slang)or if they take such terms <strong>to</strong> mean someth<strong>in</strong>g thatthey do not, the record<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>valid data will surelyresult.Writ<strong>in</strong>g down the questions asked (<strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> theanswers received). This helps researchers makesense at a later date out of answers recorded earlier,<strong>and</strong> helps them reduce dis<strong>to</strong>rtions ow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> selectiveforgett<strong>in</strong>g.Record<strong>in</strong>g personal thoughts while conduct<strong>in</strong>gobservations <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews. (Also referred <strong>to</strong> asresearcher reflexivity.) Responses that seem unusualor <strong>in</strong>correct can be noted <strong>and</strong> checked later aga<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>ther remarks or observations.Ask<strong>in</strong>g one or more participants <strong>in</strong> the study <strong>to</strong> reviewthe accuracy of the research report. This is frequentlyreferred <strong>to</strong> as member check<strong>in</strong>g.Obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividual outside of the study <strong>to</strong> review<strong>and</strong> evaluate the report. This is called an externalaudit, or peer debrief<strong>in</strong>g.Document<strong>in</strong>g the sources of remarks whenever possible<strong>and</strong> appropriate. This helps researchers makesense out of comments that otherwise might seemmisplaced.Document<strong>in</strong>g the basis for <strong>in</strong>ferences.Describ<strong>in</strong>g the context <strong>in</strong> which questions are asked<strong>and</strong> situations are observed. Also referred <strong>to</strong> as thickdescription.Us<strong>in</strong>g audiotapes <strong>and</strong> videotapes when possible <strong>and</strong>appropriate.Draw<strong>in</strong>g conclusions based on one’s underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof the situation be<strong>in</strong>g observed <strong>and</strong> then act<strong>in</strong>gon these conclusions. If these conclusions are <strong>in</strong>valid,the researcher will soon f<strong>in</strong>d out after act<strong>in</strong>g onthem.Interview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals more than once. Inconsistenciesover time <strong>in</strong> what the same <strong>in</strong>dividual reportsmay suggest that he or she is an unreliable <strong>in</strong>formant.Observ<strong>in</strong>g the sett<strong>in</strong>g or situation of <strong>in</strong>terest over aperiod of time. The length of an observation is*Not necessarily, of course. It may simply mean a difference <strong>in</strong>viewpo<strong>in</strong>t or perception.


454 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eextremely important <strong>in</strong> qualitative research. Consistencyover time with regard <strong>to</strong> what researchers aresee<strong>in</strong>g or hear<strong>in</strong>g is a strong <strong>in</strong>dication of reliability.Furthermore, there is much about a group that doesnot even beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> emerge until some time has passed<strong>and</strong> the members of the group become familiar with,<strong>and</strong> will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> trust, the researcher.Analyz<strong>in</strong>g negative cases. Attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate<strong>in</strong>stances that do not fit the pattern by revis<strong>in</strong>g thatpattern until the <strong>in</strong>stance fits.*Table 19.2 summarizes a number of purposes, researchquestions, strategies, <strong>and</strong> data collection techniquesused <strong>in</strong> qualitative research.An Example of Qualitative<strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of an observational qualitative study, followedby a critique of its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did<strong>in</strong> our critiques of the different types of research studieswe analyzed <strong>in</strong> other chapters, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.*Qualitative researchers often use the term credibility <strong>to</strong> encompassnot only <strong>in</strong>strument validity <strong>and</strong> reliability, but <strong>in</strong>ternal validity aswell.TABLE 19.2Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Questions, Strategies, <strong>and</strong> Data Collection TechniquesPossible <strong>Research</strong> <strong>Research</strong> Examples of DataPurpose of the Study Questions Strategies Collection TechniquesExplora<strong>to</strong>ry:• To <strong>in</strong>vestigate a little-unders<strong>to</strong>odevent, situation, or circumstance• To identify or discover importantvariables• To generate hypotheses forfurther research• What is happen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this school?• What are the important themesor patterns <strong>in</strong> the ways teachersbehave <strong>in</strong> this school?• <strong>How</strong> are these themes orpatterns l<strong>in</strong>ked <strong>to</strong>gether?• Case study• Observation• Field study• Participant observation• Nonparticipan<strong>to</strong>bservation• In-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g• Selected <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gDescriptive:• To document an event, situation,or circumstance of <strong>in</strong>terest• What are the importantbehaviors, events, attitudes,processes, <strong>and</strong>/or structuresoccurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this school?• Case study• Field study• Ethnography• Observation• Participant observation• Nonparticipan<strong>to</strong>bservation• In-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g• Written questionnaire• Content analysisExplana<strong>to</strong>ry:• To expla<strong>in</strong> the forces caus<strong>in</strong>g anevent, situation, or circumstance• To identify plausible causalnetworks shap<strong>in</strong>g an event,situation, or circumstance• What events, beliefs, attitudes,<strong>and</strong>/or policies are shap<strong>in</strong>g thenature of this school?• <strong>How</strong> do these forces <strong>in</strong>teract <strong>to</strong>shape this school?• Case study• Field study• Ethnography• Participant observation• Nonparticipan<strong>to</strong>bservation• In-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g• Written questionnaire• Content analysisPredictive:• To predict the outcomes of anevent, situation, or circumstance• To forecast behaviors or actionsthat might result from an event,situation, or circumstance• What is likely <strong>to</strong> occur <strong>in</strong> thefuture as a result of the policiesnow <strong>in</strong> place at this school?• Who will be affected, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>what ways?• Observation• Interview• In-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g• Written questionnaire• Content analysis


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Adolescence, 39(154) (Summer 2004): 373–388. Libra Publishers, Inc. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission.Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk: An Intervention forBehaviorally Challenged YouthsPatricia A. DoucetteAbstractThis qualitative research explored the question: Do preadolescent <strong>and</strong> adolescent youthswith behavioral challenges benefit from a multimodal <strong>in</strong>tervention of walk<strong>in</strong>g outdoorswhile engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g? The objective of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention is <strong>to</strong> helpthe youth feel better, explore alternative behavioral choices, <strong>and</strong> learn new cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies<strong>and</strong> life skills by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a counsel<strong>in</strong>g process that <strong>in</strong>cludes the benefits of mildaerobic exercise, <strong>and</strong> that nurtures a connection <strong>to</strong> the outdoors, The <strong>in</strong>tervention utilizesa strong therapeutic alliance based on the Rogerian technique of unconditional positiveregard, which is grounded <strong>and</strong> guided by the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of attachment theory. For eightweeks, eight students (aged 9 <strong>to</strong> 13 years) from a middle school <strong>in</strong> Alberta, Canada, participatedweekly <strong>in</strong> the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention. Students’ self-reports <strong>in</strong>dicated thatthey benefited from the <strong>in</strong>tervention. <strong>Research</strong> triangulation with <strong>in</strong>volved adults supportedf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that <strong>in</strong>dicated the students were mak<strong>in</strong>g prosocial choices <strong>in</strong> behavior,<strong>and</strong> were experienc<strong>in</strong>g more feel<strong>in</strong>gs of self-efficacy <strong>and</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g, Limitations, newresearch directions, <strong>and</strong> subsequent longitud<strong>in</strong>al research possibilities are discussed.Western societies have seen an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> violence <strong>and</strong> antisocial behavior <strong>in</strong>schools <strong>and</strong> communities (Pollack, 1998). Juvenile crime rates have <strong>in</strong>creased four timess<strong>in</strong>ce the early 1970s (Cook & Laub, 1997). After the shock of the Columb<strong>in</strong>e schoolmassacre <strong>in</strong> the United States <strong>and</strong> other violent <strong>in</strong>cidents, communities are dem<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>terventions <strong>to</strong> help prevent similar occurrences.Traditional approaches for various youth behavior challenges have assumed thebehavior needs <strong>to</strong> be controlled <strong>and</strong> conta<strong>in</strong>ed by us<strong>in</strong>g behavioral <strong>and</strong> social learn<strong>in</strong>gapproaches (Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998). Many current <strong>in</strong>terventions rely on adaptationsof behavior modification strategies <strong>to</strong> provide structure <strong>and</strong> control. The tenetsof some programs for troubled youth are based on a hierarchy of control, authority, <strong>and</strong>power. The framework of behavior <strong>and</strong> behavioral boundaries is directed by coercivecontrol with <strong>to</strong>ken economies <strong>and</strong> earned privileges that are enforced by systems <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>grevok<strong>in</strong>g social <strong>and</strong> recreational activities (Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998). I question<strong>and</strong> challenge this type of philosophy. Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation for mak<strong>in</strong>g positivebehavioral choices <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g responsibility <strong>and</strong> ownership for behavior is unlikely <strong>to</strong> becomethe behavioral response when behavior is controlled by others. <strong>Research</strong> (Deci &Ryan, 1985) suggests <strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation <strong>in</strong>volves self determ<strong>in</strong>ation, self awareness ofone’s needs <strong>and</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g goals <strong>to</strong> meet those needs. I believe that many behaviorally challengedyouths have experienced <strong>in</strong>teractions with key adults that have been punitive,reject<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> untrustworthy (Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998; Staub, 1996). Therefore,many current <strong>in</strong>terventions based on behavioral strategies <strong>and</strong> coercice control havelimited effectiveness (Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998; Staub, 1996).New treatment methods that adopt a therapeutic approach that is grounded <strong>and</strong>guided by the pr<strong>in</strong>ciples of attachment theory may engage a therapeutic process with theresults of youths’ prosocial behavioral choices (Centers for Disease Control, 1991;Implied directionalhypothesisJustification455


456 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAmbiguousFerguson, 1999; Holl<strong>and</strong>, Moretti, Verlaan, & Peterson, 1993; Keat, 1990; Moffitt, 1993;Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998). By participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a casual walk outdoors, there can bethe physiological advantage of mild aerobic exercise (Franken, 1994; Hays, 1999; Fox,1997; Baum & Posluszny, 1999; Kolb & Whishaw, 1996, 1998). I believe, as do others(Anderson, 2000; Glaser, 2000; Tkachuk & Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1999; Real Age Newsletter, 2001a), thathuman be<strong>in</strong>gs have a natural bond with the outdoors <strong>and</strong> other liv<strong>in</strong>g organisms. Bynurtur<strong>in</strong>g this bond with a walk outdoors, positive well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> health can result(Tkachuk & Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1999; Hays, 1999; Orlick, 1993; Real Age Newsletter, 2001b).WALK AND TALK INTERVENTIONThe Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention has its fundamental philosophy <strong>in</strong> Bronfenbrenner’s(1979) social ecological theory of behavior which views the child, family, school, work,peers, neighborhood, <strong>and</strong> community as <strong>in</strong>terconnected systems. Youths’ problembehavior can be attributed <strong>to</strong> dysfunction between any one or more comb<strong>in</strong>ations ofthese systems (Bordu<strong>in</strong>, 1999). By underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g these dynamics, the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk<strong>in</strong>tervention attempts <strong>to</strong> provide a support network that encourages youths <strong>to</strong> reconnectwith self <strong>and</strong> the environment through an attachment process, a counsel<strong>in</strong>gprocess, <strong>and</strong> a physiological response result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs of self-efficacy.The Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention utilizes three components <strong>to</strong> engage youths. Thecounsel<strong>in</strong>g component of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention borrows seven pr<strong>in</strong>ciples fromthe Or<strong>in</strong>oco program used at the Maples Adolescent Centre near Vancouver, BritishColumbia (Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998, pp. 10–18). These pr<strong>in</strong>ciples are driven by anunderly<strong>in</strong>g underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of attachment theory. These pr<strong>in</strong>ciples are as follows:1. All behavior has mean<strong>in</strong>g. The mean<strong>in</strong>g of the behavior is revealed by underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>ternal work<strong>in</strong>g model of the person generat<strong>in</strong>g the behavior.2. Early <strong>and</strong> repeated experiences with people who care for us set a foundationfor our <strong>in</strong>ternal work<strong>in</strong>g models of relationship with self <strong>and</strong> others. Our earliestexperiences have a profound effect on how we approach relationships,school, work, <strong>and</strong> play.3. Biological legacies such as cognitive, emotional, <strong>and</strong> physical capabilities arean <strong>in</strong>teractive part of our experience <strong>and</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> our work<strong>in</strong>g model ofrelationships with self <strong>and</strong> others.4. Internal work<strong>in</strong>g models are constantly chang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the context of relationships<strong>and</strong> expertise. These models are constantly revised based on experience. Experiencecan be added <strong>to</strong> but not subtracted.5. Interpersonal relationships are a process of cont<strong>in</strong>uous reciprocal <strong>in</strong>terplay ofeach person’s <strong>in</strong>ternal work<strong>in</strong>g model with others. It is not possible <strong>to</strong> hold oneselfapart from this <strong>in</strong>terplay.6. We underst<strong>and</strong> ourselves <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> others. A sense of self <strong>in</strong>cludes our senseof how others view <strong>and</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> us.7. Endur<strong>in</strong>g change <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s behavior occurs only when there is change<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal work<strong>in</strong>g model supported by change <strong>in</strong> the system one lives<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> if there is sufficient time, opportunity, <strong>and</strong> support <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrate the newexperience.The counsel<strong>in</strong>g component of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention is <strong>in</strong>terlaced withnew strategies for positive life skills <strong>and</strong> attempts <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>corporate solution-focused brieftherapy (Riley, 1999). Through counsel<strong>in</strong>g, youths discover solutions by way of simple <strong>in</strong>terventionswhile experienc<strong>in</strong>g positive regard <strong>in</strong> Rogerian fashion (Rogers, 1980). Focus


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 457is kept on the youths’ strengths while collaborat<strong>in</strong>g for change (Riley, 1999; Orlick, 1993).Identify<strong>in</strong>g highlights is an important element of each walk. Highlights are used <strong>to</strong> teachyouths <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k positively so they can reframe their experiences <strong>in</strong> a way that enhanceswell-be<strong>in</strong>g (Orlick, 1993). By be<strong>in</strong>g able <strong>to</strong> illum<strong>in</strong>ate the good <strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs that happen <strong>in</strong>daily life, youths can f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>ner strength <strong>and</strong> resilience when experienc<strong>in</strong>g negativeevents or reactions from others (Orlick, 1993). Youths who have an <strong>in</strong>ner source of rework<strong>in</strong>gsetbacks <strong>in</strong> daily life will be more likely <strong>to</strong> cope with stress effectively.The ecopsychology component of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention is tied <strong>to</strong> the psychologicalprocesses that br<strong>in</strong>g people closer <strong>to</strong> the natural world. Some research suggeststhat humans have a natural bond with other liv<strong>in</strong>g organisms, <strong>and</strong> nurtur<strong>in</strong>g thatconnection may provide a health benefit (Roszak, Gomes, & Kanner, 1995; Real AgeNewsletter, 2001a). By walk<strong>in</strong>g outdoors, the outdoor connection is nurtured, facilitat<strong>in</strong>gyouths’ awareness of their environment.The physiological component engages the youths <strong>in</strong> aerobic exercise. Considerableresearch supports the use of exercise <strong>to</strong> alleviate many types of mental illness <strong>and</strong> enhancefeel<strong>in</strong>gs of well-be<strong>in</strong>g (Tkachuk & Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1999). Some research suggests that aslittle as ten m<strong>in</strong>utes of daily exercise is enough <strong>to</strong> generate mood-elevat<strong>in</strong>g neurochemicals(Real Age Newsletter, 2001b). Recogniz<strong>in</strong>g the importance of exercise <strong>to</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>gis a critical aspect of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention.The <strong>in</strong>tervention for behaviorally challenged youths comb<strong>in</strong>es the benefits of astrong therapeutic alliance based on the Rogerian technique of unconditional positiveregard (Rogers, 1980), <strong>in</strong>tegrated with mild aerobic exercise that occurs outdoors <strong>in</strong> aplace of natural beauty. The research goal is <strong>to</strong> discover if this comb<strong>in</strong>ation has a beneficialeffect on selected youths <strong>and</strong> their problem behaviors.The impetus for this research is <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the epidemiology <strong>and</strong> etiology ofthe problem behaviors while attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> implement an effective preventative <strong>in</strong>tervention.One objective is <strong>to</strong> provide fertile ground for the youths <strong>to</strong> explore <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>alternative behavioral choices. This phenomenological qualitative researchapproach assumes that the participants are existential <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong> as such, actions,verbalizations, everyday patterns, <strong>and</strong> ways of <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g can reveal an underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof human behavior (Addison, 1992). A basic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple of existentialism suggests that each<strong>and</strong> every expression, even the most <strong>in</strong>significant <strong>and</strong> superficial behavior, reveals <strong>and</strong>communicates who that <strong>in</strong>dividual is (Sartre, 1957). It is hoped that the participants willacquire a stronger self underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g via a therapeutic alliance, aerobic exercise, experienc<strong>in</strong>ga connection <strong>to</strong> the outdoors, <strong>and</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> choose <strong>to</strong> make a behavior change.By underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> utiliz<strong>in</strong>g attachment theory (A<strong>in</strong>sworth & Bowlby, 1991;Bowlby, 1969; Centers for Disease Control, 1991; Ferguson, 1999; Holl<strong>and</strong>, Moretti,Verlaan, & Peterson, 1993; Keat, 1990; Moffitt, 1993; Moore, Moretti, & Holl<strong>and</strong>, 1998)<strong>and</strong> Rogerian (1980) methods <strong>to</strong> guide the counsel<strong>in</strong>g with a walk outdoors, it is hopedthat youths’ self-esteem will <strong>in</strong>crease as they become connected <strong>to</strong> another person—myself—<strong>and</strong> the outdoors.Why do some young people sabotage themselves with nonproductive behaviors?I believe if an <strong>in</strong>tervention can be <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>and</strong> then utilized by youths who have a his<strong>to</strong>ryof these behaviors, they can be redirected <strong>to</strong> satisfy<strong>in</strong>g, productive lives regardlessof their prior personal his<strong>to</strong>ry. The <strong>in</strong>tervention will help behaviorally troubled youths <strong>to</strong>feel better <strong>and</strong> do better by be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternally motivated <strong>to</strong> choose prosocial behavior.The plasticity, resilience, <strong>and</strong> remarkable adaptability of youths <strong>to</strong> their uniqueselves <strong>and</strong> situations has been a catalyst for my research. The importance of attachment(as def<strong>in</strong>ed by A<strong>in</strong>sworth, 2000) <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g attachment theory (A<strong>in</strong>sworth, 2000;Bowlby, 1969) cannot be understated. The Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention provides a safePrior researchPurposePossible researcherbias


458 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eplace for youths <strong>to</strong> discover new positive cop<strong>in</strong>g strategies that can benefit themthroughout life.ConveniencesampleAges were 9–13Good clarificationUnclear <strong>to</strong> usParticipan<strong>to</strong>bserverInstrumentationGood cautionMETHODThe middle school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal assigned the student outreach support worker <strong>to</strong> selectappropriate <strong>in</strong>dividuals for the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention. The assistant super<strong>in</strong>tendent,a licensed psychologist, was selected as a resource <strong>and</strong> liaison <strong>in</strong> case crises should arise.A consent form was signed by a school district representative. Further, consent formswere sent <strong>to</strong> the parents of participants.The eight <strong>in</strong>tervention respondents chosen were coded by school assessors as behaviorallychallenged <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> need of special education. I first met with each of the eightyouths for a pre<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>terview that allows us <strong>to</strong> become acqua<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>and</strong> for me<strong>to</strong> familiarize myself with their underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of their behavioral challenges. Specifically,the youths’ problem behaviors as <strong>in</strong>dicated by school representatives, parents<strong>and</strong>/or guardians were identified as conduct disorder as described <strong>in</strong> the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong>Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Conductdisorders <strong>in</strong>clude violat<strong>in</strong>g rules, aggressiveness that threatens or causes physical harm <strong>to</strong>others, bully<strong>in</strong>g, ex<strong>to</strong>rtion, lack of respect for self <strong>and</strong> others, suicide attempts, truancy,<strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g frequent fights, <strong>and</strong> various charges by the police such as break<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> enter<strong>in</strong>g(DSM-IV, 1994). The problem behaviors were repetitive, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> unsuccessfulfunction<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> the school, community, <strong>and</strong> often family sett<strong>in</strong>g.By utiliz<strong>in</strong>g a collaborative, qualitative approach. I disclosed the <strong>in</strong>tentions of theWalk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention. I believe this approach facilitated development of alliance,empowerment of the participant, <strong>and</strong> engagement as the expert (Creswell, 1998; Flick,1998). My role as researcher was that of an active, <strong>in</strong>terested learner (Creswell, 1998;Flick, 1998). This collaborative, qualitative approach bridges the gap between participant<strong>and</strong> researcher. A collaborative approach has been preferred for youths s<strong>in</strong>ce it engages<strong>and</strong> honors them as their own expert (Axl<strong>in</strong>e, 1947/1969; Oakl<strong>and</strong>er, 1978); youthsare usually not <strong>in</strong> control of many decisions that affect them.Interviews were conducted before <strong>and</strong> after the six-week walk <strong>and</strong> talk <strong>in</strong>tervention.The first <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong>cluded an <strong>in</strong>troduction by myself <strong>and</strong> by the youths. They wereasked <strong>to</strong> draw a picture of themselves perform<strong>in</strong>g any activity of their choice. Sheets of 8”by 11” white paper <strong>and</strong> ten assorted gel pens were provided. These pens were chosen becauseof their popularity with children of all ages. Upon completion of the draw<strong>in</strong>gs, theyouths were asked <strong>to</strong> make a list of five of their strengths. Next they were asked <strong>to</strong> list atleast five weaknesses. The f<strong>in</strong>al activity was <strong>to</strong> write a short au<strong>to</strong>biographical <strong>in</strong>cident—about someth<strong>in</strong>g that had made an impression whether positive or negative. After eachactivity, discussion was encouraged. A goal of the <strong>in</strong>terview was <strong>to</strong> start the youths th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gabout self, <strong>and</strong> for me, <strong>to</strong> learn what they th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>and</strong> feel. At the close of the <strong>in</strong>terview,I prepared them for the week of walk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> talk<strong>in</strong>g, emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g that it would be theiropportunity <strong>to</strong> talk about whatever came <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>and</strong> the talks would be confidential—except <strong>in</strong> extreme situations, for <strong>in</strong>stance, statements about harm<strong>in</strong>g themself or others.By conduct<strong>in</strong>g the first <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> this manner, it was hoped the youths wouldstart <strong>to</strong> self disclose <strong>in</strong> some or all of the modalities. Also, it provides basel<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>sight as<strong>to</strong> how the youths feel at that time. The self portraits of each youth were exam<strong>in</strong>ed by alicensed art therapist, Max<strong>in</strong>e Junge, <strong>and</strong> myself. Max<strong>in</strong>e Junge (personal communication,February 18, 2002) provides the caution that what she offered were guesses,hypotheses, <strong>and</strong> impressions. The au<strong>to</strong>biographical pieces gave <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> issues consideredimportant by these youths.


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 459The <strong>in</strong>terview was fairly ambitious, but the researcher did not press the youthswith the agenda. It was hoped that an alliance would be established where<strong>in</strong> trust <strong>and</strong>respect would be shared. This started the counsel<strong>in</strong>g process. It is important <strong>to</strong> discoverwhat this process is for the youths <strong>and</strong> report it. It is important <strong>to</strong> discover the mean<strong>in</strong>gthe youths give <strong>to</strong> events, <strong>and</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g actions (Maxwell, 1996). It was the youths’ realitythat this qualitative approach attempts <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> (Maxwell, 1996). The youthswere the focus <strong>and</strong> their phenomenological experience was explored while psychoeducational<strong>in</strong>terventions were suggested <strong>and</strong> discussed when appropriate.It was the counselor’s role <strong>to</strong> help the youths clarify <strong>and</strong> reframe belief constructswhile help<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>and</strong> translate the subconscious <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the conscious (Hays, 1999).<strong>How</strong> youths behave <strong>and</strong> speak, reflects subconscious thoughts <strong>and</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs (Hunter,1987), It was the counselor’s role <strong>to</strong> help the youths identify the connectedness <strong>to</strong> place<strong>and</strong> others, identify <strong>and</strong> verbalize one or more successful survival skills while <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>gnew conscious approaches that encourage the cognitive strategy of s<strong>to</strong>p, th<strong>in</strong>k, do.Introduc<strong>in</strong>g young people <strong>to</strong> the hope of a future that is reward<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> positive <strong>and</strong> onethey can manage <strong>and</strong> control is a paramount goal. When appropriate, they will be <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>to</strong> various life skills that can improve the quality of their life (Orlick, 1993). Bylearn<strong>in</strong>g about positive th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, positive self talk, stress management, relaxation skills,imagery, anger physiology, anger management, communication with “I statements,”focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>g, new behavioral choices can be made (Orlick, 1993). Learn<strong>in</strong>gone, two, or more key life skills can enhance the youths’ lives.I met with each respondent for six consecutive weeks, once a week, for approximately30–45 m<strong>in</strong>utes per session. Each session entailed a walk on the school grounds.This did not <strong>in</strong>clude the pre <strong>and</strong> post <strong>in</strong>terviews. The eight participants began their firstwalk <strong>and</strong> talk between December 12, 2001 <strong>and</strong> January 28, 2002. This wide range of starttimes was due <strong>to</strong> the wait<strong>in</strong>g period for parental consents <strong>and</strong> then arrang<strong>in</strong>g appropriatetimes with the teachers. Also, at the end of December <strong>and</strong> early January there was atwo-week school break which caused a delay <strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g some first sessions. The <strong>to</strong>talwalk <strong>and</strong> talk time allotted was 45 m<strong>in</strong>utes but because of time needed <strong>to</strong> dress appropriately,actual walk <strong>and</strong> talk time was about 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes. At the start of each walk Iasked the youths what they wanted <strong>to</strong> discuss. If there wasn’t anyth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> particular theywanted <strong>to</strong> say, I asked them for highlights <strong>in</strong> their lives s<strong>in</strong>ce I last saw them a week ago.Highlights are positive events, positive experiences, comments, personal accomplishmentsor anyth<strong>in</strong>g that has lifted the quality of the moment for that child (Orlick, 1993,1998). Next, I asked them about their lowlights. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> verbaliz<strong>in</strong>g that lifeis filled with highs <strong>and</strong> lows beg<strong>in</strong>s the journey of self discovery <strong>and</strong> also allows theyouth <strong>to</strong> discuss alternative strategies for deal<strong>in</strong>g with problems.Throughout the six-week Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention, I <strong>in</strong>troduced strategies fordeal<strong>in</strong>g with stress, identify<strong>in</strong>g what was stressful for the youth, discuss<strong>in</strong>g the importanceof positive self talk, mental imagery, visualization techniques, <strong>and</strong> focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>refocus<strong>in</strong>g techniques (Orlick, 1993, 1998). Most of the youths chosen for this <strong>in</strong>terventionhad anger-management challenges. When appropriate, anger managementtechniques, comb<strong>in</strong>ed with the cognitive strategy of s<strong>to</strong>p, th<strong>in</strong>k, do was <strong>in</strong>troduced.Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g anger cycles <strong>and</strong> the physiology of anger was discussed. One of the lifeskills <strong>in</strong>troduced was learn<strong>in</strong>g the rules of us<strong>in</strong>g assertiveness rather than aggressiveness<strong>and</strong> utiliz<strong>in</strong>g I-statements <strong>to</strong> convey feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> others. When appropriate these types oflife skills were <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>and</strong> practiced <strong>in</strong> mock situations. Positive life skill techniqueswere woven <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the counsel<strong>in</strong>g session dur<strong>in</strong>g most sessions.The <strong>in</strong>tervention was completed with a post <strong>in</strong>terview. When gather<strong>in</strong>g data fromthe youths, respondents were <strong>in</strong>formed that the research was <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> help them <strong>in</strong>ProceduresGood detailFor more detailNeed more detail


460 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eGood detailthe future; therefore, answer<strong>in</strong>g honestly is important. Respondents were <strong>to</strong>ld therewere no right or wrong responses. They were <strong>to</strong> feel free <strong>to</strong> talk openly. Similar <strong>to</strong> thepre <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>terview, youths were asked <strong>to</strong> draw a picture of themselves <strong>in</strong> an activity.Next they were asked <strong>to</strong> write their strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. At that time, Ishowed each youth the draw<strong>in</strong>g from their pre <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>terview, <strong>and</strong> we comparedthe strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses from before <strong>and</strong> after the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Together wenoted the differences. I asked each youth: What has changed s<strong>in</strong>ce we started? What didyou like about walk <strong>and</strong> talk? What didn’t you like about it? What was helpful? Whatwasn’t helpful? What are your conclud<strong>in</strong>g comments <strong>and</strong> remarks? Do you th<strong>in</strong>k it wouldbe good for other youths <strong>to</strong> participate? I asked them what they thought about the artthey produced <strong>and</strong> about the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses they identified. I assessed selfesteemvia the self portrait they had drawn, compar<strong>in</strong>g pre <strong>and</strong> post <strong>in</strong>tervention responses.Several methods of communicat<strong>in</strong>g with the youths, i.e. art, structured exercise,open-ended questions, <strong>and</strong> discussion of their experience, made my report of their phenomenologicalexperience more complete.RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<strong>How</strong> reported?Good cautionSeven of eightI chose a phenomenological approach because I wanted <strong>to</strong> capture the essence of theyouths’ experience as <strong>to</strong>ld by them. Did they feel better <strong>and</strong> do better? The youths’ experiencewas reported as I observed it. I assessed their experience of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk<strong>in</strong>tervention as <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> me by them along with collateral observation <strong>and</strong>/or <strong>in</strong>formationgiven <strong>to</strong> me by parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> other <strong>in</strong>volved school personnel. The ecopsychologyaspect of this <strong>in</strong>tervention can be replicated <strong>in</strong> any safe outdoor environment.The only given variables <strong>in</strong> this research are the common denom<strong>in</strong>a<strong>to</strong>rs of age,youths from 9 <strong>to</strong> 13 years old, <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual, problematic behaviors, although variations<strong>in</strong> etiology <strong>and</strong> epidemiology exist. The fac<strong>to</strong>rs relat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the causes of the behaviorsare <strong>in</strong>dividual. The systemic distribution of impact<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>cidents <strong>and</strong> contribut<strong>in</strong>gcomponents <strong>to</strong> each youth’s behavior vary. By offer<strong>in</strong>g a multimodal approach it washoped that the youths’ experience would be positive <strong>and</strong> result <strong>in</strong> prosocial behavior.As the qualitative researcher it was my m<strong>and</strong>ate <strong>to</strong> utilize rigorous data collectionprocedures (Creswell, 1998). As a researcher it was also my <strong>in</strong>tent <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> my distance<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> promote objectivity but still engage them as a counselor. To achieve this resultrequires walk<strong>in</strong>g a f<strong>in</strong>e l<strong>in</strong>e. To preserve scientific clarity, conscious effort was required.<strong>How</strong>ever, a positive <strong>in</strong>terpersonal relationship was necessary for the success of the research<strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>and</strong> of the qualitative approach. The characteristics <strong>and</strong> assumptions of thephenomenological qualitative approach <strong>to</strong> research necessitates that the participant’s viewbe the entire reality of the study (Creswell, 1998). As such, the reality was purely <strong>and</strong> subjectivelyportrayed as an experiential component of the study. To analyze the data, multipleapproaches <strong>and</strong> multiple traditions were <strong>in</strong>cluded. This was done <strong>to</strong> provide a fuller, holisticview <strong>and</strong> richer underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the process which occurred dur<strong>in</strong>g time <strong>in</strong> the field.Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the three components of counsel<strong>in</strong>g, ecopsychology, <strong>and</strong> physiologicalenhancement creates a new <strong>in</strong>tervention for behaviorally challenged youths. The youthswho completed the <strong>in</strong>tervention stated that it helped them clarify feel<strong>in</strong>gs. Overall, Ibelieve the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention benefited each youth who completed the <strong>in</strong>tervention.The follow<strong>in</strong>g discussion provides specifics about the <strong>in</strong>dividual participants.Youth AWhat evidence?Youth A’s participation helped him <strong>to</strong> become more self aware of his struggles withsister <strong>and</strong> father. Although strategies were discussed, I do not believe that Youth A


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 461assimilated many new life skills. He needed much more <strong>in</strong>dividual time <strong>and</strong> attention <strong>to</strong>help him cope with the number of problems he faces outside of school. <strong>How</strong>ever, his arttherapy work showed a def<strong>in</strong>ite improvement. The first draw<strong>in</strong>g was very small, notgrounded, <strong>and</strong> “float<strong>in</strong>g” which the art therapist suggested <strong>in</strong>dicated a feel<strong>in</strong>g of smallness,powerlessness, <strong>and</strong> lack of self-esteem. The f<strong>in</strong>al draw<strong>in</strong>g depicted a well-def<strong>in</strong>ed boy<strong>and</strong> girl—Youth A <strong>and</strong> little sister—<strong>in</strong> his bedroom with all his prized possessions. Both childrenwere smil<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> he looked like a protective big brother. His teacher’s commentsabout Youth A <strong>in</strong>dicated that the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention had benefited Youth A atleast for the days of each Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk. The teacher believed Youth A needed more cont<strong>in</strong>uous<strong>in</strong>tensive help. Youth A made positive comments about his experience <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention:He liked talk<strong>in</strong>g about his feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>g skills.His before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 12/15 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that he believed he had morestrengths on the completion day of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk than on the start<strong>in</strong>g day. His weaknessesratio was 9/3 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that at the start of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk he believed he had manymore weaknesses than when he f<strong>in</strong>ished.Provid<strong>in</strong>g sourcesof <strong>in</strong>formationYouth BI believe there was a significant improvement with Youth B. Each week he self-disclosedmore <strong>and</strong> more. He was eager <strong>to</strong> talk about his problems <strong>and</strong> challenges as time wen<strong>to</strong>n. Toward the end of the <strong>in</strong>tervention he was walk<strong>in</strong>g with his head held high ratherthan downcast. He was very pleased <strong>to</strong> report his new fun relationship with his bigbrother. His teacher <strong>to</strong>ld me throughout the <strong>in</strong>tervention of his improved cop<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> socialskills <strong>in</strong> the classroom. She gave me detailed accounts of how Youth B avoided confrontationsby us<strong>in</strong>g newly acquired social skill strategies. In the last discussion with theteacher, on the last day of the <strong>in</strong>tervention, she revealed a violent outburst <strong>in</strong> his classroom.It was on that day physical abuse charges were reported <strong>to</strong> social services regard<strong>in</strong>ghis mother. Although the teacher could not underst<strong>and</strong> Youth B’s <strong>in</strong>congruentbehavior, I knew it all fit.His before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 5/8 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that he believed he hadmore strengths on the completion day of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk than on the start<strong>in</strong>g day. In addition,three of the strengths mentioned were social skills. His weaknesses ratio was 4/0<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that at the start of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk he believed he had four weaknesses, <strong>and</strong>when he f<strong>in</strong>ished he had none. Youth B <strong>in</strong>dicated Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk was a helpful <strong>in</strong>terventionfor him.The art therapist’s comments regard<strong>in</strong>g his draw<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>dicate that he was a boypossibly filled with fear <strong>and</strong> anger. The draw<strong>in</strong>gs denoted a developmental problem, <strong>in</strong>that they depicted a small <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>significant figure.Good detailYouth CI th<strong>in</strong>k there was a huge improvement with Youth C. He seemed <strong>to</strong> self-disclose more <strong>and</strong>more each week. He utilized the life skill techniques we discussed, practiced themthroughout the week, <strong>and</strong> eagerly reported back <strong>to</strong> me. His self-esteem soared witheach new success he experienced. He would retell with enthusiasm his weekly attemptsat new life skills, his successes along with some failures. His teacher echoed my sentimentsnotic<strong>in</strong>g a remarkable change of attitude <strong>in</strong> the classroom, his cooperation withpeers, <strong>and</strong> positive choices <strong>in</strong> behavior. His brother commented on their newly improvedrelationship.His before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 5/5. On completion day of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk,three of his five strengths were social skills whereas on start<strong>in</strong>g day none were social


462 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g away”?skills. His weaknesses ratio was 5/2 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that at the start of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk, he hadmany more weaknesses than when he f<strong>in</strong>ished. At the start he <strong>in</strong>dicated that two of hisfive weaknesses were social skills <strong>and</strong> at completion, one of his two weaknesses was histemper. I viewed these changes as exemplify<strong>in</strong>g a raised level of self-awareness. Youth Cvery enthusiastically claimed Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk was a positive event for him.The art therapist noted that his first draw<strong>in</strong>g depicted a small, facetless, <strong>in</strong>significantboy, <strong>and</strong> his f<strong>in</strong>al draw<strong>in</strong>g was very similar. Sadly, after completion of the <strong>in</strong>tervention,charges of parental child abuse were reported <strong>to</strong> social services.Youth DInternal validityYouth D was re<strong>in</strong>tegrated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the regular classroom <strong>to</strong>ward the end of the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk<strong>in</strong>tervention. I th<strong>in</strong>k his participation <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention was one of many support effortsthat helped him improve his overall success <strong>and</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Dur<strong>in</strong>g Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk hetalked about his daily challenges. He seemed <strong>to</strong> develop a self awareness over time. Histeacher reported positive changes: he had started <strong>to</strong> react appropriately <strong>to</strong> accept “no”without burst<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> tears. He utilized self-chosen time outs <strong>and</strong> self talk <strong>to</strong> help himcontrol his emotions. His teacher <strong>in</strong>dicated that he was more polite <strong>and</strong> considerate withothers. Youth D reported that Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk had been a great experience for him.His before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 7/8. On completion day of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk,one of his eight strengths was a social skill. His weaknesses ratio was 5/5. The art therapyassessment for his first draw<strong>in</strong>g suggested an <strong>in</strong>effectual, fearful, <strong>and</strong> avoidant child. Hisf<strong>in</strong>al draw<strong>in</strong>g was grounded, but still revealed a faceless self. Youth D’s before-<strong>and</strong>-afterdraw<strong>in</strong>gs lack depth <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>volvement.Youth EQuestionsvalidityI believe Youth E benefited from his participation <strong>in</strong> the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention, butneeded <strong>in</strong>tensive ongo<strong>in</strong>g help. He seemed <strong>to</strong> have a very low self image that was controlledby external events. His troubled home life, parents’ divorce, <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g a dailydrug cocktail for various problems contributed <strong>to</strong> his need for external support. Histeacher agreed. The teacher also said that Youth E had benefited greatly from participat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk. In the classroom he was much calmer <strong>and</strong> cooperative, therebyexperienc<strong>in</strong>g more personal success, someth<strong>in</strong>g he clearly needed. Youth E said Walk <strong>and</strong>Talk was good for him because he could get his feel<strong>in</strong>gs out.The art therapist’s assessment of his artwork was of a boy with high <strong>in</strong>telligence,with a good self image. This was contradic<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> the boy I knew. Both of his pictureswere grounded but showed an avoidant boy who did not know how <strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>le hisimpulses.His before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 5/8. On completion day of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk,seven of his eight strengths were social skills. This was impressive. His weaknesses ratiowas 5/1. In his first meet<strong>in</strong>g he identified two social skills weaknesses as be<strong>in</strong>g related <strong>to</strong>be<strong>in</strong>g bullied. In our f<strong>in</strong>al meet<strong>in</strong>g he admitted that argu<strong>in</strong>g was his weakness. I believehe had acquired more self awareness over the <strong>in</strong>tervention time <strong>and</strong> learned new cop<strong>in</strong>gstrategies.Youth FIt was difficult for me <strong>to</strong> assess whether Youth F, the only female participant, benefitedfrom the <strong>in</strong>tervention. I often wondered what she was learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> what bothered her.<strong>How</strong>ever, I found her participation <strong>in</strong> the ecopsychology aspect remarkable. She became


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 463transformed from a girl who threw rocks at birds <strong>to</strong> one who tried <strong>to</strong> gently approachthem <strong>and</strong> stroke them. She became <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly aware of the surround<strong>in</strong>g trees, an occasionalw<strong>and</strong>er<strong>in</strong>g dog, <strong>and</strong> the variety of birds. She seemed <strong>to</strong> enjoy the physical aspectsof the <strong>in</strong>tervention. I believe she was extremely athletic <strong>and</strong> often mentioned this<strong>to</strong> her. Her teacher queried me after the second Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>to</strong> learn what life skills wewere concentrat<strong>in</strong>g on. The teacher collaborated with me <strong>to</strong> help the girl control her impulsivityby rem<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g her when it was appropriate <strong>to</strong> focus, refocus, s<strong>to</strong>p, th<strong>in</strong>k, do, rubher lucky penny, <strong>and</strong> apply any other life skill strategies I had mentioned. Also, Youth F’smother phoned me <strong>to</strong> offer collaboration <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g her daughter use life skills at home.Youth F experienced behavioral improvement dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention time as reportedby all triangulation sources. Youth F <strong>to</strong>ld me that Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk was great.The art therapist’s assessment of her artwork suggested possible organic problems.I agreed. Her before-<strong>and</strong>-after strengths ratio was 15/7.Her weaknesses ratio was 5/0. Ibelieve Youth F could use ongo<strong>in</strong>g outside support.Good detailHelps <strong>to</strong> clarifytermSeems <strong>to</strong>contradict firstsentence <strong>in</strong>paragraphYouth GYouth G was a <strong>to</strong>tal pleasure <strong>to</strong> have as a participant of Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk. Although he wasmildly developmentally delayed, he was eager <strong>to</strong> learn new positive life skills. He readilybecame attached <strong>to</strong> the outdoor environment, becom<strong>in</strong>g keenly aware of the birds,trees, <strong>and</strong> sounds. He often made observations that I found remarkable although hisk<strong>in</strong>d, gentle spirit was often squelched <strong>in</strong> his daily struggles with academics <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonalrelationships, but because of his resilience <strong>and</strong> will<strong>in</strong>gness <strong>to</strong> discuss his problemshe could f<strong>in</strong>d solutions readily. His teachers believed Youth G’s success was ongo<strong>in</strong>g afterhe participated <strong>in</strong> behavioral program. Youth G’s teachers concurred that the Walk <strong>and</strong>Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention had probably helped <strong>to</strong> illum<strong>in</strong>ate his positive choices.Youth G’s art assessment denoted his developmental lag. The draw<strong>in</strong>gs before<strong>and</strong>-aftershowed him wear<strong>in</strong>g a sport shirt with the number twelve (his lucky number)<strong>and</strong> play<strong>in</strong>g volleyball. Neither draw<strong>in</strong>g reflected a grounded <strong>in</strong>dividual. His before-<strong>and</strong>afterstrengths ratio was 5/5. In his first meet<strong>in</strong>g he identified two social skills as be<strong>in</strong>gstrengths. In the last meet<strong>in</strong>g he identified three social skills as such. His weaknesses ratiowas 1/3. I believe this <strong>in</strong>dicated a keener self awareness. I believe Youth G benefitedenormously from his participation <strong>in</strong> the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention.Unclear <strong>to</strong> us“Expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g away”?Youth HYouth H identified seven strengths <strong>and</strong> two weaknesses. He liked <strong>to</strong> talk about play<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> watch<strong>in</strong>g hockey. His art was not grounded <strong>and</strong> very simple. The art therapistnoted that his draw<strong>in</strong>g was very protected <strong>and</strong> defensive <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g possible anger <strong>and</strong>aggression.Youth H was removed from the <strong>in</strong>tervention after one meet<strong>in</strong>g. At the time of ourfirst meet<strong>in</strong>g the teacher’s aide strongly argued aga<strong>in</strong>st his be<strong>in</strong>g a participant <strong>in</strong> theWalk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention. Youth H had been selected by the student outreach worker<strong>and</strong> his parents had consented <strong>to</strong> his participation. The new school guidance counselorcontacted me with concerns <strong>and</strong> recommended that he be pulled from the <strong>in</strong>tervention.Due <strong>to</strong> these objections, Youth H was withdrawn. My advice <strong>to</strong> future Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk<strong>in</strong>terventionists is <strong>to</strong> enlist the support of all people who are <strong>in</strong> favor of a youth’sparticipation <strong>in</strong> the program. Otherwise what happened <strong>to</strong> Youth H could happen <strong>to</strong>others.Overall, the research results were positive. From the teachers’ perspective, myperspective, <strong>and</strong> the youths’ comments, the <strong>in</strong>tervention seemed <strong>to</strong> benefit them on


464 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWhat evidence?Redundantmany fronts. Introduc<strong>in</strong>g alternative life skill strategies was a key counsel<strong>in</strong>g componen<strong>to</strong>f the <strong>in</strong>tervention. All youths found the focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>g exercise beneficial <strong>and</strong>many adopted the technique <strong>to</strong> everyday life. Focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>g can facilitatelearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> experience life fully. By practic<strong>in</strong>g focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>g exercises youthscan learn <strong>to</strong> closely observe what is seen, listen <strong>in</strong>tently <strong>to</strong> what is heard, feel fully <strong>and</strong>connect completely when <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g with others (Orlick, 1993). The focus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> refocus<strong>in</strong>gtechnique utilized aspects of the <strong>in</strong>tervention’s ecopsychological component byweav<strong>in</strong>g a life skill technique <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a closer awareness of self <strong>and</strong> facets of the outdoorsthat otherwise would go unnoticed. After apply<strong>in</strong>g the technique outdoors it was readilytransferable <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>door situations.It is my belief that <strong>to</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees, the youths benefited from the experience ofcounsel<strong>in</strong>g outdoors enhanced by the physiological “boost” provided by aerobic exercise.Walk<strong>in</strong>g allowed for physical release, someth<strong>in</strong>g very important for these active youths.Feel<strong>in</strong>gs, problems, <strong>and</strong> sometimes solutions <strong>to</strong> problems materialized. All respondentsfound talk<strong>in</strong>g about such problems <strong>to</strong> be beneficial. These respondents were chosenbecause of their difficulty <strong>in</strong> manag<strong>in</strong>g social situations.Assum<strong>in</strong>g my f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are correct <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention can be deemed successful,will the <strong>in</strong>tervention have long-term effects? I can only speculate. Follow-up longitud<strong>in</strong>alstudies are recommended. Suggestions for future research <strong>in</strong>clude us<strong>in</strong>g control groupswith various problem behaviors as well as groups with no problem behaviors, groupswith <strong>and</strong> without the ecopsychological component, groups with <strong>and</strong> without the walk<strong>in</strong>gcomponent. I also advise utiliz<strong>in</strong>g quantitative methods <strong>to</strong> measure success. Possiblymy strongest recommendation is <strong>to</strong> do the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> warm weather.CONCLUSIONSGood cautionA possible limitation of this research could be its subjective nature. Further, my subjectivitypresupposes that most people with attachment difficulties respond favorably <strong>to</strong> CarlRogers’ (1980) therapeutic approach of positive personal regard.Inclement weather could deter respondents from wholehearted participation.Unfortunately, the session times, once established, were not flexible s<strong>in</strong>ce they were<strong>in</strong>corporated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the school day.This research approached behavioral challenges from an <strong>in</strong>dividual vantage po<strong>in</strong>trather than a systemic or societal perspective. Some researchers (e.g., Grossman, 1999)view youths’ turmoil <strong>and</strong> violence as result<strong>in</strong>g from the ills of society (i.e., television,movies, <strong>and</strong> video game violence). The present research does not address these types ofcultural concerns of society on a macro level.In sum, I would like <strong>to</strong> see the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention used <strong>in</strong> middle schools<strong>and</strong> high schools, <strong>and</strong> utilized by mental health practitioners. Once youths have completedthe <strong>in</strong>tervention, I recommend periodical refreshers on a monthly basis. Walk <strong>and</strong>Talk refreshers will give the youths a time <strong>to</strong> reconnect with the outdoors, self, <strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>statepositive behaviors <strong>and</strong> life skills.ReferencesAddison, R. (1992). Grounded hermeneutic research. In B. F.Crabtree & W. L. Miller (Eds.), Do<strong>in</strong>g qualitative research(pp. 110–124). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.A<strong>in</strong>sworth, M. (2000). Maternal sensitivity scales(Orig<strong>in</strong>al work published <strong>in</strong> 1969). Available athttp://www.psy.sunysb.edu/ewaters/senscoop.htmA<strong>in</strong>sworth, M., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach<strong>to</strong> personality development. American Psychologist, 46,333–341.American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic <strong>and</strong>statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: Author.


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 465Anderson, N. (2000). Testimony of the Office of the Direc<strong>to</strong>rof National Institutes of Health, Department of Health <strong>and</strong>Human Services regard<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>d / body <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>and</strong>health before the United States Senate, September 22,1998. Available at http://www.apa.org/ppo/scitest923.htmlAxl<strong>in</strong>e, V. (1947/1969). Play therapy. New York: Ballant<strong>in</strong>e Books.Baum, A. & Posluszny, D. (1999). Health psychology: mapp<strong>in</strong>gbiobehavioral contributions <strong>to</strong> health <strong>and</strong> illness.Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 137–163.Bordu<strong>in</strong>, C. (1999). Multisystemic treatment of crim<strong>in</strong>ality <strong>and</strong>violence <strong>in</strong> adolescents. Journal of the American Academyof Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 242–249.Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment: Attachment <strong>and</strong> loss (Vol. 1).New York: Basic Books.Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development:Experiments by nature <strong>and</strong> design. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.Centers for Disease Control. (1991). Forum on youth violence<strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority communities: Sett<strong>in</strong>g the agenda forprevention. Public Health Reports, 106, 225–253.Cook, P. J., & Laub, J. H. (1997). The unprecedented epidemic<strong>in</strong> youth violence. In M. Tonry & M. H. Molore (Eds.), Crime<strong>and</strong> justice (pp. 101–138). Chicago, IL: University ofChicago Press.Creswell, J. W. (1998). Qualitative <strong>in</strong>quiry <strong>and</strong> researchdesign: Choos<strong>in</strong>g among five traditions. Thous<strong>and</strong>Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intr<strong>in</strong>sic motivation <strong>and</strong> self determ<strong>in</strong>ation<strong>in</strong> human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.Ferguson, G. (1999). Shout<strong>in</strong>g at the sky: Troubled teens <strong>and</strong>the promise of the wild. New York: Thomas Dunne Books.Flick, U. (1998). An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> qualitative research.Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Fox, K. (1997). Let’s get physical. In K. R. Fox (Ed.), The physicalself: From motivation <strong>to</strong> well be<strong>in</strong>g (pp. vii–xiii).Champaign, IL; Human K<strong>in</strong>etics.Franken, R. (1994). Human motivation. Pacific Grove, CA.:Brooks-Cole Publishers.Glaser, R. (2000). M<strong>in</strong>d-body <strong>in</strong>teractions, immunity, <strong>and</strong>health. Available at http://www.apa.org/ppo/m<strong>in</strong>d.htmlGrossman, D. (1999). S<strong>to</strong>p teach<strong>in</strong>g our kids <strong>to</strong> kill: A call <strong>to</strong>action aga<strong>in</strong>st TV, movie, <strong>and</strong> video game violence. NewYork: Crown Books.Hays, K. (1999). Work<strong>in</strong>g it out. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: AmericanPsychological Association.Holl<strong>and</strong>, R., Moretti, M., Verlaan, V., & Peterson, S. (1993).Attachment <strong>and</strong> conduct disorder: The responseprogram. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 420–431.Hunter, M. (1996). Psych yourself <strong>in</strong>! Vancouver, BC:SeaWalk Press.Keat, D. (1990). Child multimodal therapy. Norwood, NJ:Ablex Publish<strong>in</strong>g.Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. (1996). Human neuropsychology.New York: W. H. Freeman.Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. (1998). Bra<strong>in</strong> plasticity <strong>and</strong> behavior.Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 43–64.Maxwell, J. (1996). Qualitative research design: An<strong>in</strong>teractive approach. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: SagePublications.Moffitt, T. (1993). Adolescence—limited <strong>and</strong> life-coursepersistent antisocial behavior: A developmentaltaxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.Moore, K., Moretti, M., & Holl<strong>and</strong>, R. (1998). A new perspectiveon youth care programs: Us<strong>in</strong>g attachmenttheory <strong>to</strong> guide <strong>in</strong>terventions for troubled youth.Residential Treatment for Children <strong>and</strong> Youth, 15, 1–24.Oakl<strong>and</strong>er, V. (1978). W<strong>in</strong>dows <strong>to</strong> our children: A gestalttherapy approach <strong>to</strong> children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. Moab, UT:Real People Press.Orlick, T. (1993). Free <strong>to</strong> feel great: Teach<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>to</strong> excelat liv<strong>in</strong>g. Carp, Ontario, Canada: Creative Bound.Orlick, T. (1998). Feel<strong>in</strong>g great. Carp, Ontario, Canada:Creative Bound.Pollack, W. (1998). Real boys. New York: Henry Holt & Co.Real Age Newsletter. (2001a, May 18). The call of the wild,tip of the day. Available at http://www.realage.com/Real Age Newsletter (2001b, Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 9). Quick mood fix, tipof the day. Available at http://www.realage.com/Riley, S. (1999). Brief therapy. An adolescent <strong>in</strong>tervention. ArtTherapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association,16, 83–86.Rogers, C. (1980). A way of be<strong>in</strong>g. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Hough<strong>to</strong>n Miffl<strong>in</strong>.Roszak, T., Gomes, M., & Kanner, A. (1995). Ecopsychology:Res<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g the earth, heal<strong>in</strong>g the m<strong>in</strong>d. San Francisco, CA:Sierra Club Books.Sartre, J. P. (1957). Existentialism <strong>and</strong> human emotions. NewYork: The Wisdom Library.Staub, E. (1996). Altruism <strong>and</strong> aggression <strong>in</strong> children <strong>and</strong>youth. In R. Feldman (Ed.), The psychology of adversity(pp. 115–144). Amherst, MA: University of MassachusettsPress.Tkachuk, G. A., & Mart<strong>in</strong>, G. L. (1999). Exercise therapy forpatients with psychiatric disorders: <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>icalimplications. Professional Psychology: <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong>Practice, 30, 275–282.Copyright of Adolescence is the property of Libra Publishers Inc. <strong>and</strong> its content may not be copied or emailed <strong>to</strong> multiple sites or posted <strong>to</strong>a listserv without the copyright holder’s express written permission. <strong>How</strong>ever, users may pr<strong>in</strong>t, download, or email articles for <strong>in</strong>dividual use.


466 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is found on page 457. It is “The research goalis <strong>to</strong> discover if this comb<strong>in</strong>ation has a beneficial effect onselected youths <strong>and</strong> their problem behaviors.” We wouldsubstitute “the Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk <strong>in</strong>tervention” for “this comb<strong>in</strong>ation”but the mean<strong>in</strong>g is, we th<strong>in</strong>k, nonetheless clear.The justification is extensive <strong>and</strong> clear (though somewhatredundant) with respect <strong>to</strong> both the societal/personal needs the study addresses <strong>and</strong> the rationale forthe <strong>in</strong>tervention. It <strong>in</strong>cludes limitations of other <strong>in</strong>terventions<strong>and</strong> the philosophical <strong>and</strong> scientific bases ofthe method.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no ethical issues regard<strong>in</strong>g confidentiallyor deception. Risk <strong>to</strong> students appears m<strong>in</strong>imalbut parental consent forms were obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> a psychologistwas available if needed.DEFINITIONSDef<strong>in</strong>itions are not explicit but are made reasonably clearthrough (sometimes extensive) description of majorterms: “Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk”; beneficial effect; <strong>and</strong> problembehaviors. The mean<strong>in</strong>g of both these <strong>and</strong> other termssuch as: counsel<strong>in</strong>g component; ecopsychology component;physiological component; <strong>and</strong> collaborative qualitativeapproach would be clearer if references <strong>and</strong>justifications were not mixed <strong>in</strong> with descriptions.PRIOR RESEARCHThe author provides extensive references <strong>in</strong> support ofboth rationale for the study <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention procedures.<strong>How</strong>ever, it is often unclear whether the referenceis research, theory, or op<strong>in</strong>ion, <strong>and</strong> whether thereference does, <strong>in</strong> fact, support the method. For example:“there can be the physiological advantage of mildexercise,” page 456.HYPOTHESESNo hypotheses are explicitly stated. The research questionstated <strong>in</strong> the Abstract—“Do preadolescent <strong>and</strong> adolescentyouths with behavioral challenges benefit froma multimodal <strong>in</strong>tervention of walk<strong>in</strong>g outdoors whileengag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g?”—<strong>in</strong> conjunction with subsequentmaterial clearly implies the directional hypothesisthat the students do improve.SAMPLEThe sample is clearly described as eight students (actuallyseven because one was withdrawn for reasons notentirely clear), aged 9 <strong>to</strong> 13 chosen from one school districtas hav<strong>in</strong>g problem behaviors. The method of selectionis clear. Each of the students is further described <strong>in</strong>the section on <strong>in</strong>dividual outcomes. Replication of thestudy would be facilitated by more detail. For example,how many were primarily aggressive, suicidal, lawbreakers,etc. This convenience sample does not permitgeneralization, but that is presumably not the <strong>in</strong>tent ofthe study.INSTRUMENTATIONInstrumentation <strong>in</strong>cluded list<strong>in</strong>g of strengths <strong>and</strong>weaknesses as well as self-draw<strong>in</strong>gs by students <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terviews by the researcher, all done pre- <strong>and</strong> post<strong>in</strong>tervention.It also <strong>in</strong>cluded researcher observations<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations made dur<strong>in</strong>g each of six <strong>in</strong>terventionperiods with each student. Whether a daily log or otherrecord<strong>in</strong>g mechanism was used is not reported; we mustassume these are based on researcher recollection. Also<strong>in</strong>cluded, as we discover <strong>in</strong> the results section, werecomments from teachers <strong>and</strong> family members.No discussion of reliability or validity is provided,which is not unusual <strong>in</strong> qualitative studies. The researcheracknowledges the subjective nature of the studyas well as presents the justification for the methodology.Although the report states that “triangulation with <strong>in</strong>volvedadults supported f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs that <strong>in</strong>dicated the studentswere mak<strong>in</strong>g prosocial choices <strong>in</strong> behavior, <strong>and</strong>were experienc<strong>in</strong>g more feel<strong>in</strong>gs of self-efficacy <strong>and</strong>well-be<strong>in</strong>g,” this is not clear <strong>to</strong> us. As we evaluate the reportson <strong>in</strong>dividual students, it appears that the researcher<strong>and</strong> teacher were <strong>in</strong> clear agreement on three, perhapsfour of the seven students. Comments from family memberswere rare. There also seems <strong>to</strong> be a contradiction <strong>in</strong>one case with the researcher stat<strong>in</strong>g, “it was difficult forme <strong>to</strong> asess whether Youth F . . . benefited from the <strong>in</strong>tervention...”butlaterstat<strong>in</strong>g that “Youth F . . . experiencedbehavioral improvement dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>terventiontime as reported by all triangulation sources.”PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYThe <strong>in</strong>tervention is, <strong>in</strong> general, well described althoughmore detail would be helpful, especially <strong>in</strong> replication.Presumably a reader can turn <strong>to</strong> the Orlick reference on


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 467ways of reduc<strong>in</strong>g stress, but the anger management,cognitive strategies, <strong>and</strong> assertiveness strategies needfurther clarification, as is provided for “life-skills strategies”<strong>in</strong> the report on Youth F.The author recognizes the problem of <strong>in</strong>ternal validity<strong>in</strong> discuss<strong>in</strong>g Youth D with the statement: “I th<strong>in</strong>k hisparticipation <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention was one of many supporteffects that helped him improve. . . .” The effect ofother variables on outcomes exists for all seven students.Although this type of study cannot effectivelycontrol extraneous variables, more discussion is appropriate.It seems <strong>to</strong> us that it is unlikely that many otherthreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity would exist dur<strong>in</strong>g this particularsix-week period, but assessment of such possibilitiesshould be feasible for a researcher who is <strong>in</strong>volvedthis closely with the schools. One <strong>in</strong>stance of a significantevent (physical abuse) <strong>and</strong> its probable impact isdiscussed.DATA ANALYSISStatistical analysis is not appropriate for this study. As isusual <strong>in</strong> studies of this type, the results from various<strong>in</strong>struments are described, <strong>in</strong> this case for <strong>in</strong>dividualstudents.RESULT/INTERPRETATIONThe author recognizes the possibilities for bias <strong>and</strong> subjectivityimpact<strong>in</strong>g her report<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g results.In numerous <strong>in</strong>stances, she gives appropriate cautions.Given this limitation, we f<strong>in</strong>d the results impressive, particularlybecause she is often clear <strong>in</strong> stat<strong>in</strong>g “I believe” sothat the reader should realize that this applies <strong>to</strong> manyother statements as well. She also frequently cites sources:e.g., “Youth B made positive comments about . . .”; “Histeacher <strong>to</strong>ld me . . .” <strong>and</strong> gives behavioral examples suchas “She became transformed from a girl who threw rocksat birds <strong>to</strong> one who tried <strong>to</strong> gently approach them.”Although we th<strong>in</strong>k the <strong>to</strong>tality of evidence <strong>and</strong>impressions justifies the conclusion that students benefited,we th<strong>in</strong>k the amount of benefit is overstated. Itappears that the most common positive outcomes were<strong>in</strong>creased self-awareness as perceived by the researcher<strong>and</strong> the more observable self-disclosure. These are considereddesirable <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g but may have <strong>in</strong>fluencedperception of other outcomes.A problem exists <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the pre-postself-list<strong>in</strong>g of strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. Whenstrengths <strong>in</strong>creased <strong>and</strong> weaknesses decreased, this isusually <strong>in</strong>terpreted as positive. <strong>How</strong>ever, with two studentswhere this is not the case, the result is “expla<strong>in</strong>ed”as due <strong>to</strong> greater self-awareness, hence also positive.While this may be true, researchers cannot change their<strong>in</strong>terpretation of data after the fact, at least not withoutmore justification.CONCLUSIONSWe agree, with the reservation mentioned above,that “. . . <strong>to</strong> vary<strong>in</strong>g degrees the youths <strong>in</strong> this studybenefited from the experience. . . .” We th<strong>in</strong>k the resultsjustify further research, as suggested by the author, <strong>and</strong>that this research is needed before the <strong>in</strong>tervention isrecommended on other than a trial basis.This study illustrates both the richness of such research<strong>and</strong> the difficulty of mak<strong>in</strong>g firm conclusions.It also illustrates a contrast <strong>in</strong> report<strong>in</strong>g styles. More“traditional” researchers are likely <strong>to</strong> prefer, as we do,clearer dist<strong>in</strong>ctions among purpose, justification, def<strong>in</strong>ition,procedures, results, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations than arefound <strong>in</strong> this report. Others argue that <strong>to</strong>o much attention<strong>to</strong> such clarity can severely impair the narrative. Weagree but believe a middle ground is atta<strong>in</strong>able.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.


468 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMa<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsOBSERVER ROLESThere are four roles that an observer can play <strong>in</strong> a qualitative research study, rang<strong>in</strong>gfrom complete participant, <strong>to</strong> participant-as-observer, <strong>to</strong> observer-as-participant, <strong>to</strong>complete observer. The degree of <strong>in</strong>volvement of the observer <strong>in</strong> the observed situationdim<strong>in</strong>ishes accord<strong>in</strong>gly for each of these roles.PARTICIPANT VERSUS NONPARTICIPANT OBSERVATIONIn participant observation studies, the researcher actually participates as an activemember of the group <strong>in</strong> the situation or sett<strong>in</strong>g he or she is observ<strong>in</strong>g.In nonparticipant observation studies, the researcher does not participate <strong>in</strong> an activityor situation but observes “from the sidel<strong>in</strong>es.”The most common forms of nonparticipant observation studies <strong>in</strong>clude naturalisticobservation <strong>and</strong> simulations.A simulation is an artificially created situation <strong>in</strong> which subjects are asked <strong>to</strong> act outcerta<strong>in</strong> roles.OBSERVATION TECHNIQUESA cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme is a set of categories an observer uses <strong>to</strong> record a person’s orgroup’s behaviors.Even with a fixed cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, an observer must still choose what <strong>to</strong>observe.A major problem <strong>in</strong> all observational research is that much that goes on may be missed.OBSERVER EFFECTThe term observer effect refers <strong>to</strong> either the effect the presence of an observer canhave on the behavior of the subjects or observer bias <strong>in</strong> the data reported. The use ofaudio- <strong>and</strong> videotap<strong>in</strong>gs is especially helpful <strong>in</strong> guard<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong>st this effect.For this reason, many researchers argue that the participants <strong>in</strong> a study should not be<strong>in</strong>formed of the study’s purpose until after the data have been collected.OBSERVER BIASObserver bias refers <strong>to</strong> the possibility that certa<strong>in</strong> characteristics or ideas of observersmay affect what they observe.SAMPLING IN OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES<strong>Research</strong>ers who engage <strong>in</strong> observation usually must choose a purposive sample.INTERVIEWINGA major technique commonly used by qualitative researchers is <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g.One purpose of <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g the participants <strong>in</strong> a qualitative study is <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out howthey th<strong>in</strong>k or feel about someth<strong>in</strong>g. Another purpose is <strong>to</strong> provide a check on theresearcher’s observations.Interviews may be structured, semistructured, <strong>in</strong>formal, or retrospective.


CHAPTER 19 Observation <strong>and</strong> Interview<strong>in</strong>g 469The six types of questions asked by <strong>in</strong>terviewers are background (or demographic)questions, knowledge questions, experience (or behavior) questions, op<strong>in</strong>ion (orvalues) questions, feel<strong>in</strong>gs questions, <strong>and</strong> sensory questions.Respect for the <strong>in</strong>dividual be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviewed is a paramount expectation <strong>in</strong> anyproper <strong>in</strong>terview.Key ac<strong>to</strong>rs are people <strong>in</strong> any group who are more <strong>in</strong>formed about the culture <strong>and</strong>his<strong>to</strong>ry of the group <strong>and</strong> who also are more articulate than others.A focus group <strong>in</strong>terview is an <strong>in</strong>terview with a small, fairly homogeneous group ofpeople who respond <strong>to</strong> a series of questions asked by the <strong>in</strong>terviewer.The most effective characteristic of a good <strong>in</strong>terviewer is a strong <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> people<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> listen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> what they have <strong>to</strong> say.RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY IN QUALITATIVE RESEARCHAn important check on the validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations<strong>in</strong> qualitative research is <strong>to</strong> compare one <strong>in</strong>formant’s description of someth<strong>in</strong>g withanother <strong>in</strong>formant’s description of the same th<strong>in</strong>g.Another, although more difficult, check on reliability/validity is <strong>to</strong> compare <strong>in</strong>formationon the same <strong>to</strong>pic with different <strong>in</strong>formation—triangulation.Efforts <strong>to</strong> ensure reliability <strong>and</strong> validity <strong>in</strong>clude use of proper vocabulary, record<strong>in</strong>gquestions used as well as personal reactions, describ<strong>in</strong>g content, <strong>and</strong> document<strong>in</strong>gsources.background(demographic)question 448cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme 444credibility 454dicho<strong>to</strong>mousquestion 450experience (behavior)question 448external audit 453feel<strong>in</strong>gs question 449focus group<strong>in</strong>terview 451<strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terview 446<strong>in</strong>terview 445key ac<strong>to</strong>r(<strong>in</strong>formant) 447knowledge question 448member check<strong>in</strong>g 453naturalisticobservation 442nonparticipan<strong>to</strong>bservation 441observational data 443observer bias 443observer effect 443observerexpectations 444open-endedquestion 450op<strong>in</strong>ion (values)question 448participan<strong>to</strong>bservation 441reliability <strong>in</strong> qualitativeresearch 453retrospective<strong>in</strong>terview 447semistructured<strong>in</strong>terview 446sensory question 449simulation 442structured <strong>in</strong>terview 446triangulation 453validity <strong>in</strong> qualitativeresearch 4531. “Observ<strong>in</strong>g people without their knowledge <strong>and</strong>/or record<strong>in</strong>g their comments withouttheir permission is unethical.” Would you agree with this statement? Expla<strong>in</strong>your reason<strong>in</strong>g.2. Which method do you th<strong>in</strong>k is more likely <strong>to</strong> produce valid <strong>in</strong>formation—participant or nonparticipant observation? Why?3. Are there any k<strong>in</strong>ds of behaviors that should not be observed? Expla<strong>in</strong> your th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.If so, give an example.Key TermsFor Discussion


470 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e4. What would you say is the biggest advantage of participant observation? the biggestdisadvantage?5. “A major difficulty <strong>in</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g people is that much that goes on may be missed bythe observer.” Is this always true? Are there any ways <strong>to</strong> decrease what is misseddur<strong>in</strong>g observational research? If so, give an example of what might be done.6. Is observer effect <strong>in</strong>evitable? Why or why not?7. “What qualitative researchers try <strong>to</strong> do is <strong>to</strong> study the subjective objectively.” Whatdoes this mean?8. Is there any k<strong>in</strong>d of data that cannot be obta<strong>in</strong>ed through observation? through <strong>in</strong>terviews?If so, expla<strong>in</strong>.9. Of the six types of questions we described on pages 448–449, which do you th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>terviewees would f<strong>in</strong>d the hardest <strong>to</strong> answer? the easiest? Why?10. What would you say is the most important quality or characteristic an <strong>in</strong>terviewershould possess? Why?11. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be hardest <strong>to</strong> master <strong>and</strong> do well, observ<strong>in</strong>g or <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g?Why?12. Interviewers are frequently advised <strong>to</strong> “be natural.” What do you th<strong>in</strong>k that means?Is it possible? desirable? always a good idea or not? Expla<strong>in</strong> your th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.Notes1. H. R. Bernard (2000). Social research methods. Qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks,CA: Sage, p. 388.2. D. R. Pap<strong>in</strong>i, N. Datan, <strong>and</strong> K. A. McCluskey-Fawcett (1988). An observational study of affective <strong>and</strong>assertive family <strong>in</strong>teractions dur<strong>in</strong>g adolescence. Journal of Youth <strong>and</strong> Adolescence, 17: 477–492.3. R. L<strong>in</strong>dman, P. Jarv<strong>in</strong>en, <strong>and</strong> J. Vidjeskog (1987). Verbal <strong>in</strong>teractions of aggressively <strong>and</strong> nonaggressivelypredisposed males <strong>in</strong> a dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g situation. Aggressive Behavior, 13: 187–196.4. M. A. Stewart (1984). What is a successful doc<strong>to</strong>r-patient <strong>in</strong>terview? A study of <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>and</strong> outcomes.Social Science <strong>and</strong> Medic<strong>in</strong>e, 19: 167–175.5. B. Devet (1990). A method for observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g writ<strong>in</strong>g lab tu<strong>to</strong>rials. Writ<strong>in</strong>g Center Journal,10: 75–83.6. E. J. Amidon <strong>and</strong> J. B. Hough (1967). Interaction analysis: Theory, research, <strong>and</strong> application. Read<strong>in</strong>g,MA: Addison-Wesley.7. M. Fetterman (1989). Ethnography: Step by Step. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.8. M. Q. Pat<strong>to</strong>n (1990). Qualitative evaluation <strong>and</strong> research methods, 2nd ed. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.9. Fetterman, op. cit., p. 58. Fetterman po<strong>in</strong>ts out that the term <strong>in</strong>formant has its roots <strong>in</strong> anthropological workconducted <strong>in</strong> colonial sett<strong>in</strong>gs, specifically <strong>in</strong> African nations formerly with<strong>in</strong> the British Empire. He refersthe reader <strong>to</strong> E. E. Pritchard (1940). The Nuer: A description of the modes of livelihood <strong>and</strong> political<strong>in</strong>stitutions of a nilotic people. New York: Oxford University Press. The term also conjures up an image ofcl<strong>and</strong>est<strong>in</strong>e activities that he f<strong>in</strong>ds objectionable (as do we) <strong>and</strong> not compatible with ethical research.10. Ibid., pp. 59–60.11. Ibid.12. Pat<strong>to</strong>n, op. cit., pp. 290–293.13. Fetterman, op. cit., p. 55.14. Ibid., p. 56.15. I. E. Seidman (1991). Interview<strong>in</strong>g as qualitative research: A guide for researchers <strong>in</strong> education <strong>and</strong> thesocial sciences. New York: Teacher’s College Press, p. 73.16. Ibid., pp. 73–74.17. Pat<strong>to</strong>n, op. cit., pp. 297–298.18. Pat<strong>to</strong>n, op. cit., p. 335.19. Ibid., p. 348.20. Ibid., pp. 348–349.


Content Analysis20“I’ve sortedall my <strong>in</strong>terview data<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> 38 differentcategories.Now what?”“Whew!That’s a lot!Perhaps you should try<strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>e someof them.”OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what a content analysis is.Describe the k<strong>in</strong>ds of sampl<strong>in</strong>g that canExpla<strong>in</strong> the purpose of content analysis. be done <strong>in</strong> content analysis.Name three or four ways content analysis Describe the two ways <strong>to</strong> codecan be used <strong>in</strong> educational research.descriptive <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categories.Expla<strong>in</strong> why a researcher might wantDescribe two advantages <strong>and</strong> two<strong>to</strong> do a content analysis.disadvantages of content analysis research.Summarize an example of content analysis. Recognize an example of contentDescribe the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>ganalysis research when you come acrossa content analysis.it <strong>in</strong> the educational literature.What Is ContentAnalysis?Some ApplicationsCategorization <strong>in</strong>Content AnalysisSteps Involved <strong>in</strong>Content AnalysisDeterm<strong>in</strong>e ObjectivesDef<strong>in</strong>e TermsSpecify the Unit of AnalysisLocate Relevant DataDevelop a RationaleDevelop a Sampl<strong>in</strong>g PlanFormulate Cod<strong>in</strong>gCategoriesCheck Reliability<strong>and</strong> ValidityAnalyze DataAn Illustrationof Content AnalysisUs<strong>in</strong>g the Computer<strong>in</strong> Content AnalysisAdvantages of ContentAnalysisDisadvantagesof Content AnalysisAn Exampleof a ContentAnalysis StudyAnalysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationInternal ValidityResults/Interpretation


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Content AnalysisAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 20.1: Content Analysis <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 20.2: Content Analysis CategoriesActivity 20.3: Advantages vs. Disadvantages of ContentAnalysisActivity 20.4: Do a Content AnalysisDarrah Hallowitz, a middle school English teacher, is becom<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>and</strong> more concerned about the waysthat women are presented <strong>in</strong> the literature anthologies she has been assigned <strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> her courses. Sheworries that her students are gett<strong>in</strong>g a limited view of the roles that women can play <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>day’s world. After schoolone day, she asks Roberta, another English teacher, what she th<strong>in</strong>ks. “Well,” says Roberta, “Funny you should ask methat. Because I have been k<strong>in</strong>d of worried about the same th<strong>in</strong>g. Why don’t we check this out?”<strong>How</strong> could they “check this out”? What is called for here is content analysis. Darrah <strong>and</strong> Roberta need <strong>to</strong> take acareful look at the ways women are portrayed <strong>in</strong> the various anthologies they are us<strong>in</strong>g. They might f<strong>in</strong>d that suchstudies have been done, or they might do one themselves. That is what this chapter is about.As we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 19, the third method thatqualitative researchers use <strong>to</strong> collect <strong>and</strong> analyze data iswhat is cus<strong>to</strong>marily referred <strong>to</strong> as content analysis, ofwhich the analysis of documents is a major part.What Is Content Analysis?Much of human activity is not directly observable ormeasurable, nor is it always possible <strong>to</strong> get <strong>in</strong>formationfrom people who might know of such activity fromfirsth<strong>and</strong> experience. Content analysis is a techniquethat enables researchers <strong>to</strong> study human behavior <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>direct way, through an analysis of their communications.*It is just what its name implies: the analysisof the usually, but not necessarily, written contentsof a communication. Textbooks, essays, newspapers,novels, magaz<strong>in</strong>e articles, cookbooks, songs, politicalspeeches, advertisements, pictures—<strong>in</strong> fact, the contentsof virtually any type of communication—can be*Many th<strong>in</strong>gs produced by human be<strong>in</strong>gs (e.g., pottery, weapons,songs) were not orig<strong>in</strong>ally <strong>in</strong>tended as communications but subsequentlyhave been viewed as such. For example, the pottery of theMayans tells us much about their culture.analyzed. A person’s or group’s conscious <strong>and</strong> unconsciousbeliefs, attitudes, values, <strong>and</strong> ideas often are revealed<strong>in</strong> their communications.In <strong>to</strong>day’s world, there is a tremendously large numberof communications of one sort or another (newspaperedi<strong>to</strong>rials, graffiti, musical compositions, magaz<strong>in</strong>earticles, advertisements, films, etc.). Analysis of suchcommunications can tell us a great deal about how humanbe<strong>in</strong>gs live. To analyze these messages, a researcherneeds <strong>to</strong> organize a large amount of material. <strong>How</strong> canthis be done? By develop<strong>in</strong>g appropriate categories, rat<strong>in</strong>gs,or scores that the researcher can use for subsequentcomparison <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> illum<strong>in</strong>ate what he or she is <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g.This is what content analysis is all about.By us<strong>in</strong>g this technique, a researcher can study (<strong>in</strong>directly)anyth<strong>in</strong>g from trends <strong>in</strong> child-rear<strong>in</strong>g practices(by compar<strong>in</strong>g them over time or by compar<strong>in</strong>g differences<strong>in</strong> such practices among various groups of people),<strong>to</strong> types of heroes people prefer, <strong>to</strong> the extent of violenceon television. Through an analysis of literature, popularmagaz<strong>in</strong>es, songs, comic strips, car<strong>to</strong>ons, <strong>and</strong> movies,the different ways <strong>in</strong> which sex, crime, religion, education,ethnicity, affection <strong>and</strong> love, or violence <strong>and</strong> hatredhave been presented at different times can be revealed.He or she can also note the rise <strong>and</strong> fall of fads. From472


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 473such data, researchers can make comparisons about theattitudes <strong>and</strong> beliefs of various groups of people separatedby time, geographic locale, culture, or country.Content analysis as a methodology is often used <strong>in</strong>conjunction with other methods, <strong>in</strong> particular his<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>and</strong> ethnographic research. It can be used <strong>in</strong> any context<strong>in</strong> which the researcher desires a means of systematiz<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> (often) quantify<strong>in</strong>g data. It is extremely valuable<strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview data.Let us consider an example. In a series of studies dur<strong>in</strong>gthe 1960s <strong>and</strong> 1970s, Gerbner <strong>and</strong> his colleagues did acontent analysis of the amount of violence on television. 1They selected for their study all of the dramatic televisionprograms that were broadcast dur<strong>in</strong>g a s<strong>in</strong>gle week <strong>in</strong> thefall of each year (<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> make comparisons from year<strong>to</strong> year) <strong>and</strong> looked for <strong>in</strong>cidents that <strong>in</strong>volved violence.They videotaped each program <strong>and</strong> then developed anumber of measures used by tra<strong>in</strong>ed coders <strong>to</strong> analyzeeach of the programs. Prevalence, for example, referred<strong>to</strong> the percentage of programs that <strong>in</strong>cluded one or more<strong>in</strong>cidents of violence; rate referred <strong>to</strong> the number of violent<strong>in</strong>cidents occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> each program; <strong>and</strong> role referred<strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividuals who were <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the violent<strong>in</strong>cidents. (The <strong>in</strong>dividuals who committed the violentact or acts were categorized as “violents,” while the <strong>in</strong>dividualsaga<strong>in</strong>st whom the violence was committed werecategorized as “victims.”) 2Gerbner <strong>and</strong> his associates used these data <strong>to</strong> reporttwo scores: a program score, based on prevalence <strong>and</strong>rate; <strong>and</strong> a character score, based on role. They thencalculated a violence <strong>in</strong>dex for each program, whichwas determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the sum of these two scores. Figure20.1 shows one of the graphs they presented <strong>to</strong>describe the violence <strong>in</strong>dex for different types ofprograms between 1967 <strong>and</strong> 1977. It suggests thatviolence was higher <strong>in</strong> children’s programs than <strong>in</strong>other types of programs <strong>and</strong> that there was littlechange dur<strong>in</strong>g the 10-year period.Some ApplicationsContent analysis is a method that has wide applicability<strong>in</strong> educational research. For example, it can be used <strong>to</strong>:Describe trends <strong>in</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g over time (e.g., theback-<strong>to</strong>-basics movement) by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g professional<strong>and</strong>/or general publications.Underst<strong>and</strong> organizational patterns (e.g., by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gcharts, outl<strong>in</strong>es, etc., prepared by school adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs).Violence <strong>in</strong>dex300250200150100500Weekend morn<strong>in</strong>g(children's programs)All programsPrime-time programs1967–19681969–19701971197219731974–19751975–197619761977Figure 20.1 TV Violence <strong>and</strong> Public View<strong>in</strong>g PatternsShow how different schools h<strong>and</strong>le the same phenomenadifferently (e.g., curricular patterns, schoolgovernance).Inferattitudes,values,<strong>and</strong>culturalpatterns<strong>in</strong>differentcountries (e.g., through an exam<strong>in</strong>ation of what sortsof courses <strong>and</strong> activities are—or are not—sponsored<strong>and</strong> endorsed).Compare the myths that people hold about schoolswith what actually occurs with<strong>in</strong> them (e.g., bycompar<strong>in</strong>g the results of polls taken of the generalpublic with literature written by teachers <strong>and</strong> otherswork<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the schools).Ga<strong>in</strong> a sense of how teachers feel about their work(e.g., by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g what they have written abouttheir jobs).Ga<strong>in</strong> some idea of how schools are perceived (e.g.,by view<strong>in</strong>g films <strong>and</strong> television programs depict<strong>in</strong>gsame).Content analysis can also be used <strong>to</strong> supplemen<strong>to</strong>ther, more direct methods of research. Attitudes <strong>to</strong>wardwomen who are work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> so-called men’s occupations,for example, can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> a variety ofways: questionnaires; <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews; participan<strong>to</strong>bservations; <strong>and</strong>/or content analysis of magaz<strong>in</strong>e articles,television programs, newspapers, films, <strong>and</strong> au<strong>to</strong>biographiesthat <strong>to</strong>uch on the subject.


474 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eLastly, content analysis can be used <strong>to</strong> giveresearchers <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> problems or hypotheses thatthey can then test by more direct methods. A researchermight analyze the content of a student newspaper, forexample, <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation for devis<strong>in</strong>g questionnairesor formulat<strong>in</strong>g questions for subsequent <strong>in</strong>-depth<strong>in</strong>terviews with members of the student body at a particularhigh school.Categorization<strong>in</strong> Content AnalysisAll procedures that are called content analysis havecerta<strong>in</strong> characteristics <strong>in</strong> common. These procedures alsovary <strong>in</strong> some respects, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the purpose of theanalysis <strong>and</strong> the type of communication be<strong>in</strong>g analyzed.All must at some po<strong>in</strong>t convert (i.e., code) descriptive<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categories. There are two waysthat this might be done:1. The researcher determ<strong>in</strong>es the categories before anyanalysis beg<strong>in</strong>s. These categories are based on previousknowledge, theory, <strong>and</strong>/or experience. For example,later <strong>in</strong> this chapter, we use predeterm<strong>in</strong>edcategories <strong>to</strong> describe <strong>and</strong> evaluate a series of journalarticles perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> social studies education(see page 481).2. The researcher becomes very familiar with the descriptive<strong>in</strong>formation collected <strong>and</strong> allows the categories<strong>to</strong> emerge as the analysis cont<strong>in</strong>ues (seeFigure 20.3 on page 478).Steps Involved<strong>in</strong> Content AnalysisDETERMINE OBJECTIVESDecide on the specific objectives you want <strong>to</strong> achieve.There are several reasons why a researcher might want<strong>to</strong> do a content analysis.To obta<strong>in</strong> descriptive <strong>in</strong>formation about a <strong>to</strong>pic.Content analysis is a very useful way <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formationthat describes an issue or <strong>to</strong>pic. For example,a content analysis of child-rear<strong>in</strong>g practices <strong>in</strong> differentcountries could provide descriptive <strong>in</strong>formationthat might lead <strong>to</strong> a consideration of differentapproaches with<strong>in</strong> a particular society. Similarly, acontent analysis of the ways various his<strong>to</strong>rical eventsare described <strong>in</strong> the his<strong>to</strong>ry textbooks of differentcountries might shed some light on why people havedifferent views of his<strong>to</strong>ry (e.g., Adolf Hitler’s role <strong>in</strong>World War II).To formulate themes (i.e., major ideas) that help <strong>to</strong>organize <strong>and</strong> make sense out of large amounts of descriptive<strong>in</strong>formation. Themes are typically group<strong>in</strong>gsof codes that emerge either dur<strong>in</strong>g or after theprocess of develop<strong>in</strong>g codes. An example is shownon page 478.To check other research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Content analysisis helpful <strong>in</strong> validat<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of a study orstudies us<strong>in</strong>g other research methodologies. Statementsof textbook publishers concern<strong>in</strong>g what theybelieve is <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> their company’s high schoolbiology textbooks (obta<strong>in</strong>ed through <strong>in</strong>terviews),for example, could be checked by do<strong>in</strong>g a contentanalysis of such textbooks. Interviews with collegeprofessors as <strong>to</strong> what they say they teach could beverified by do<strong>in</strong>g a content analysis of their lessonplans.To obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation useful <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>g with educationalproblems. Content analysis can helpteachers plan activities <strong>to</strong> help students learn. Acontent analysis of student compositions, for example,might help teachers p<strong>in</strong>po<strong>in</strong>t grammaticalor stylistic errors. A content analysis of math assignmentsmight reveal deficiencies <strong>in</strong> the waysstudents attempt <strong>to</strong> solve word problems. Whilesuch analyses are similar <strong>to</strong> grad<strong>in</strong>g practices, theydiffer <strong>in</strong> that they provide more specific <strong>in</strong>formation,such as the relative frequency of differentk<strong>in</strong>ds of mistakes.To test hypotheses. Content analysis can also be used<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate possible relationships or <strong>to</strong> test ideas.For example, a researcher might hypothesize that socialstudies textbooks have changed <strong>in</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong>which they emphasize the role of m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>in</strong>dividuals<strong>in</strong> the his<strong>to</strong>ry of our country. A content analysis ofa sample of texts published over the last 20 yearswould reveal if this is the case.DEFINE TERMSAs <strong>in</strong> all research, <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong>/or readers are sure<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cur considerable frustration unless important terms,such as violence, m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>in</strong>dividuals, <strong>and</strong> back-<strong>to</strong>basics,are clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed, either beforeh<strong>and</strong> or as thestudy progresses.


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>Content Analysis <strong>Research</strong>One of the classic examples of content analysis was donemore than 50 years ago by Whit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Child.* Theirmethod was <strong>to</strong> have at least two judges assign rat<strong>in</strong>gs on 17characteristics of child rear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> on the presence or absenceof 20 different explanations of illness for 75 “primitive societies”<strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> the United States. Examples of characteristicsare: dependence socialization anxiety, age at wean<strong>in</strong>g,*M. W. Whit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> I. L. Child (1953). Child tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> personality.New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.<strong>and</strong> age at <strong>to</strong>ilet tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Rat<strong>in</strong>gs were based on ethnographicmaterial on each society (see Chapter 21), available at the YaleInstitute of Human Relations, which varied from one pr<strong>in</strong>tedpage <strong>to</strong> several hundred pages.Psychoanalytical theory provided the basis for a series ofcorrelational hypotheses. Among the researchers’ conclusionswas that explanations of illness are related <strong>to</strong> both early deprivation<strong>and</strong> severity of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g (e.g., societies that weanedearliest were more likely <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> illness as due <strong>to</strong> eat<strong>in</strong>g,dr<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, or verbally <strong>in</strong>stigated spells). Another f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g wasthat the U.S. (middle-class) sample was, by comparison, quitesevere <strong>in</strong> its child-rear<strong>in</strong>g practices, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g both wean<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ilet tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g earlier than other societies <strong>and</strong> accompany<strong>in</strong>gboth with exceptionally harsh penalties.SPECIFY THE UNIT OF ANALYSISWhat, exactly, is <strong>to</strong> be analyzed? words? sentences?phrases? pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>gs? The units <strong>to</strong> be used for conduct<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> report<strong>in</strong>g the analysis should be specified before theresearcher beg<strong>in</strong>s the analysis.LOCATE RELEVANT DATAOnce the researcher is clear about the objectives <strong>and</strong>units of analysis, he or she must locate the data (e.g.,textbooks, magaz<strong>in</strong>es, songs, course outl<strong>in</strong>es, lessonplans) that will be analyzed <strong>and</strong> that are relevant <strong>to</strong> theobjectives. The relationship between the content <strong>to</strong> beanalyzed <strong>and</strong> the objectives of the study should be clear.One way <strong>to</strong> help ensure clarity is <strong>to</strong> have a specific researchquestion (<strong>and</strong> possibly a hypothesis) <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d beforeh<strong>and</strong><strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> select a body of material <strong>in</strong> whichthe question or hypothesis can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated.DEVELOP A RATIONALEThe researcher needs a conceptual l<strong>in</strong>k <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> howthe data are related <strong>to</strong> the objectives. The choice of contentshould be clear, even <strong>to</strong> a dis<strong>in</strong>terested observer.Often, the l<strong>in</strong>k between question <strong>and</strong> content is quiteobvious. A logical way <strong>to</strong> study bias <strong>in</strong> advertisements,for example, is <strong>to</strong> study the contents of newspaper <strong>and</strong>magaz<strong>in</strong>e advertisements. At other times, the l<strong>in</strong>k is notso obvious, however, <strong>and</strong> needs <strong>to</strong> be expla<strong>in</strong>ed. Thus, aresearcher who is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> changes <strong>in</strong> attitudes <strong>to</strong>warda particular group (e.g., police officers) over timemight decide <strong>to</strong> look at how they were portrayed <strong>in</strong>short s<strong>to</strong>ries appear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> magaz<strong>in</strong>es published at differenttimes. The researcher must assume that changes <strong>in</strong>how police officers were portrayed <strong>in</strong> these s<strong>to</strong>ries <strong>in</strong>dicatea change <strong>in</strong> attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward them.Many content analyses use available material. But it isalso common for a researcher <strong>to</strong> generate his or her owndata. Thus, open-ended questionnaires might be adm<strong>in</strong>istered<strong>to</strong> a group of students <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e howthey feel about a newly <strong>in</strong>troduced curriculum, <strong>and</strong> thenthe researcher would analyze their responses. Or a seriesof open-ended <strong>in</strong>terviews might be held with a group ofstudents <strong>to</strong> assess their perceptions of the strengths <strong>and</strong>weaknesses of the school’s counsel<strong>in</strong>g program, <strong>and</strong>these <strong>in</strong>terviews would be coded <strong>and</strong> analyzed.DEVELOP A SAMPLING PLANOnce these steps have been accomplished, the researcherdevelops a sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan. Novels, for example,may be sampled at one or any number of levels,such as words, phrases, sentences, paragraphs, chapters,books, or authors. Television programs can be sampledby type, channel, sponsor, producer, or time of dayshown. Any form of communication may be sampled atany conceptual level that is appropriate.One of the purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g designs described <strong>in</strong>Chapter 18 is most commonly used. For example, a researchermight decide <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> transcribed <strong>in</strong>terviewsfrom several students because all of them are exceptionallytalented musicians. Or a researcher might select475


476 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7efrom among the m<strong>in</strong>utes of school board meet<strong>in</strong>gs onlythose <strong>in</strong> which specific curriculum changes were recommended.The sampl<strong>in</strong>g techniques discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 6can also be used <strong>in</strong> content analysis. For example, aresearcher might decide <strong>to</strong> select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample ofchemistry textbooks, curriculum guides, laws perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> education that were passed <strong>in</strong> the state ofCalifornia, lesson plans prepared by his<strong>to</strong>ry teachers<strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school, or an elementary pr<strong>in</strong>cipal’sdaily bullet<strong>in</strong>s. Another possibility would be <strong>to</strong>number all the songs recorded by the Benny Goodmanbig b<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> then select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 50 <strong>to</strong>analyze.Stratified sampl<strong>in</strong>g also can be used <strong>in</strong> contentanalysis. A researcher <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> school board policies<strong>in</strong> a particular state, for example, might beg<strong>in</strong> bygroup<strong>in</strong>g school districts by geographic area <strong>and</strong> size<strong>and</strong> then use r<strong>and</strong>om or systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> selectparticular districts. Stratification ensures that the sampleis representative of the state <strong>in</strong> terms of district size<strong>and</strong> location. A statement of policies would then be obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom each district <strong>in</strong> the sample for analysis.Cluster sampl<strong>in</strong>g can also be used. In the examplejust described, if the unit of analysis were the m<strong>in</strong>utes ofboard meet<strong>in</strong>gs rather than formal policy statements, them<strong>in</strong>utes of all meet<strong>in</strong>gs dur<strong>in</strong>g an academic year couldbe analyzed. Each r<strong>and</strong>omly selected district would thusprovide a cluster of meet<strong>in</strong>g m<strong>in</strong>utes. If m<strong>in</strong>utes of onlyone or two meet<strong>in</strong>gs were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected from eachdistrict, however, this would be an example of twostager<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g (see page 96).There are, of course, less desirable ways <strong>to</strong> select asample of content <strong>to</strong> be analyzed. One could easily selecta convenience sample of content that would makethe analysis virtually mean<strong>in</strong>gless. An example wouldbe assess<strong>in</strong>g the attitudes of American citizens <strong>to</strong>wardfree trade by study<strong>in</strong>g articles published only <strong>in</strong> theNational Review or The Progressive. An improvemen<strong>to</strong>ver convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g would be, as mentionedearlier, purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Rather than rely<strong>in</strong>g onsimply their own or their colleagues’ judgments as <strong>to</strong>what might be appropriate material for analysis, researchersshould, when possible, rely on evidence thatthe materials they select are, <strong>in</strong> fact, representative.Thus, decid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> analyze letters <strong>to</strong> the edi<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> Timemagaz<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> study public attitudes regard<strong>in</strong>gpolitical issues might be justified by previous researchshow<strong>in</strong>g that the letters <strong>in</strong> Time agreed with poll<strong>in</strong>gdata, election results, <strong>and</strong> so on.FORMULATE CODING CATEGORIES*After the researcher has def<strong>in</strong>ed as precisely as possiblewhat aspects of the content are <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated, he orshe needs <strong>to</strong> formulate categories that are relevant <strong>to</strong> the<strong>in</strong>vestigation (Figure 20.2). The categories should be soexplicit that another researcher could use them <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ethe same material <strong>and</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> substantially the sameresults—that is, f<strong>in</strong>d the same frequencies <strong>in</strong> each category.Suppose a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> the accuracy ofthe images or concepts presented <strong>in</strong> high school Englishtexts. She wonders whether the written or visual content<strong>in</strong> these books is biased <strong>in</strong> any way, <strong>and</strong> if it is, how. Shedecides <strong>to</strong> do a content analysis <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> some answers<strong>to</strong> these questions.She must first plan how <strong>to</strong> select <strong>and</strong> order the contentthat is available for analysis—<strong>in</strong> this case, the textbooks.She must develop pert<strong>in</strong>ent categories that willallow her <strong>to</strong> identify that which she th<strong>in</strong>ks is important.Let us imag<strong>in</strong>e that the researcher decides <strong>to</strong> look, <strong>in</strong>particular, at how women are presented <strong>in</strong> these texts.She would first select the sample of textbooks <strong>to</strong> beanalyzed—that is, which texts she will read (<strong>in</strong> thiscase, perhaps, all of the textbooks used at a certa<strong>in</strong>grade level <strong>in</strong> a particular school district). She couldthen formulate categories. <strong>How</strong> are women described?What traits do they possess? What are their physical,emotional, <strong>and</strong> social characteristics? These questionssuggest categories for analysis that can, <strong>in</strong> turn, be brokendown <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> even smaller cod<strong>in</strong>g units such as thoseshown <strong>in</strong> Table 20.1.Another researcher might be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gwhether different attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward <strong>in</strong>timate humanrelationships are implied <strong>in</strong> the mass media of the UnitedStates, Engl<strong>and</strong>, France, <strong>and</strong> Sweden. Films would bean excellent <strong>and</strong> accessible source for this analysis,although the categories <strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g units with<strong>in</strong> eachcategory would be much more difficult <strong>to</strong> formulate. For<strong>in</strong>stance, three general categories could be formed us<strong>in</strong>gHorney’s typology of relationships: “go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ward,”“go<strong>in</strong>g away from,” <strong>and</strong> “go<strong>in</strong>g aga<strong>in</strong>st.” 3 This would bean example of categories formulated ahead of time.The researcher would then look for <strong>in</strong>stances of theseconcepts expressed <strong>in</strong> the films. Other units of behavior,such as hitt<strong>in</strong>g someone, express<strong>in</strong>g a sarcasticremark, kiss<strong>in</strong>g or hugg<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> refus<strong>in</strong>g a request, are*An exception <strong>to</strong> this step occurs when the researcher counts<strong>in</strong>stances of a particular characteristic (e.g., of violence, as <strong>in</strong> theGerbner study) or uses a rat<strong>in</strong>g system (as was done <strong>in</strong> the Whit<strong>in</strong>g& Child study).


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 477“It seemsobvious that youshould base your categorieson Piaget’s theory ofcognitive development!”“But thosecategories don’t makeany sense <strong>in</strong> this k<strong>in</strong>dof study!”“Hey! Thisis my study! I’lldecide what categories<strong>to</strong> use.”The choice ofwhat categories <strong>to</strong> useis crucial!Figure 20.2 What Categories Should I Use?TABLE 20.1Cod<strong>in</strong>g Categories for Women<strong>in</strong> Social Studies TextbooksPhysical Emotional SocialCharacteristics Characteristics CharacteristicsColor of hair Warm RaceColor of eyes Aloof ReligionHeight Stable, secure OccupationWeight Anxious, <strong>in</strong>secure IncomeAge Hostile Hous<strong>in</strong>gHairstyle Enthusiastic Ageetc. etc. etc.illustrations of other categories that might emerge fromfamiliarity with the data.Another way <strong>to</strong> analyze the content of mass media is<strong>to</strong> use “space” or “time” categories. For example, <strong>in</strong> thepast few years, how many <strong>in</strong>ches of newspr<strong>in</strong>t havebeen devoted <strong>to</strong> student demonstrations on campuses?<strong>How</strong> many m<strong>in</strong>utes have television news programs devoted<strong>to</strong> urban riots? <strong>How</strong> much time has been used forprograms that deal with violent <strong>to</strong>pics compared <strong>to</strong> nonviolent<strong>to</strong>pics?The process of develop<strong>in</strong>g categories that emergefrom the data is often complex. An example of cod<strong>in</strong>g an<strong>in</strong>terview is shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 20.3. It is a transcript of an<strong>in</strong>terview with a teacher regard<strong>in</strong>g curriculum change. Inthis example, both the category codes <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itialthemes are identified <strong>in</strong> the text <strong>and</strong> annotated <strong>in</strong> themarg<strong>in</strong>s, along with rem<strong>in</strong>ders <strong>to</strong> the researcher.Manifest Versus Latent Content. In do<strong>in</strong>g acontent analysis, a researcher can code either or both themanifest <strong>and</strong> the latent content of a communication. <strong>How</strong>do they differ? The manifest content of a communicationrefers <strong>to</strong> the obvious, surface content—the words,pictures, images, <strong>and</strong> so on that are directly accessible <strong>to</strong>the naked eye or ear. No <strong>in</strong>ferences as <strong>to</strong> underly<strong>in</strong>gmean<strong>in</strong>g are necessary. To determ<strong>in</strong>e, for example,whether a course of study encourages the development ofcritical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g skills, a researcher might simply countthe number of times the word th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g appears <strong>in</strong> thecourse objectives listed <strong>in</strong> the course outl<strong>in</strong>e.The latent content of a document, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,refers <strong>to</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g underly<strong>in</strong>g what is said or shown.To get at the underly<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g of a course outl<strong>in</strong>e, for


478 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCodesClose-knit communityHealth of community,or community valuesChange is threaten<strong>in</strong>gVisionary skills oftalented peopleFigure 20.3 An Example of Cod<strong>in</strong>g an InterviewTranscriptINTERVIEWER: Lucy, what do you perceive as strengths ofGreenfield as a community <strong>and</strong> how that relates <strong>to</strong> schools?LUCY: Well, I th<strong>in</strong>k Greenfield is a fairly close-knitcommunity. I th<strong>in</strong>k people are <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> what goes on. . . .We like <strong>to</strong> keep track of what our kids are do<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> feel aconnection <strong>to</strong> them because of that. The downside of thatperhaps is that kids can feel that we are look<strong>in</strong>g TOOclose. . . . you said the health of the community itself isreflected <strong>in</strong> schools. . . . I th<strong>in</strong>k . . . this is a pretty conservativecommunity overall, <strong>and</strong> look <strong>to</strong> make sure that what is be<strong>in</strong>gtalked about <strong>in</strong> the schools really carries out the community’svalues. . . . (And I th<strong>in</strong>k there might be a tendency <strong>to</strong> hold backa little bit <strong>to</strong>o much because of that idealization of “you know,we learned the basics, the read<strong>in</strong>g, the writ<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> the arithmetic”).So you know, any change is threaten<strong>in</strong>g . . . Sometimesthat can get <strong>in</strong> the way of try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> do different th<strong>in</strong>gs.INTERVIEWER: In terms of look<strong>in</strong>g at leadership strengths <strong>in</strong>the community, where does Greenfield set <strong>in</strong> a cont<strong>in</strong>uum withplann<strong>in</strong>g process, . . . forward th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, visionary people. . . .LUCY: I th<strong>in</strong>k there are people that have wonderful visionaryskills. I would say that the community as a whole . . . wouldnot reflect that . . . I th<strong>in</strong>k we have some <strong>in</strong>credibly talentedpeople who become frustrated when they try <strong>to</strong> implementwhat they see as their . . . 4ThemesSense of communityPotential theme:Leadersexample, a researcher might read through the entire outl<strong>in</strong>eor a sample of pages, particularly those describ<strong>in</strong>gthe classroom activities <strong>and</strong> homework assignments <strong>to</strong>which students will be exposed. The researcher wouldthen make an overall assessment as <strong>to</strong> the degree <strong>to</strong>which the course is likely <strong>to</strong> develop critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.Although the researcher’s assessment would surely be<strong>in</strong>fluenced by the appearance of the word th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> thedocument, it would not depend <strong>to</strong>tally on the frequencywith which the word (or its synonyms) appeared.There seems little question that both methods havetheir advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages. Cod<strong>in</strong>g the manifestcontent of a document has the advantage of ease ofcod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> reliability—another researcher is likely <strong>to</strong>arrive at the same number of words or phrases counted.It also lets the reader of the report know exactly how theterm th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g was measured. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, itwould be somewhat suspect <strong>in</strong> terms of validity. Justcount<strong>in</strong>g the number of times the word th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g appears<strong>in</strong> the outl<strong>in</strong>e for a course would not <strong>in</strong>dicate all theways <strong>in</strong> which this skill is <strong>to</strong> be developed, nor would itnecessarily <strong>in</strong>dicate “critical” th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.Cod<strong>in</strong>g the latent content of a document has theadvantage of gett<strong>in</strong>g at the underly<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g of whatis written or shown, but it comes at some cost <strong>in</strong> reliability.It is likely that two researchers would assess differentlythe degree <strong>to</strong> which a particular course outl<strong>in</strong>eis likely <strong>to</strong> develop critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. An activity orassignment judged by one researcher as especiallylikely <strong>to</strong> encourage critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g might be seen by asecond researcher as <strong>in</strong>effective. A commonly used criterionis 80 percent agreement. But even if a s<strong>in</strong>gle researcherdoes all the cod<strong>in</strong>g, there is no guarantee thathe or she will rema<strong>in</strong> constant <strong>in</strong> the judgments madeor st<strong>and</strong>ards used. Furthermore, the reader would likelybe uncerta<strong>in</strong> as <strong>to</strong> exactly how the overall judgmentwas made.The best solution, therefore, is <strong>to</strong> use both methodswhenever possible. A given passage or excerpt shouldreceive close <strong>to</strong> the same description if a researcher’s


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 479cod<strong>in</strong>g of the manifest <strong>and</strong> latent contents is reasonablyreliable <strong>and</strong> valid. <strong>How</strong>ever, if a researcher’s (or two ormore researchers’) assessments, us<strong>in</strong>g the two methods,are not fairly close (it is unlikely that there wouldever be perfect agreement), the results should probablybe discarded <strong>and</strong> perhaps the overall <strong>in</strong>tent of theanalysis reconsidered.CHECK RELIABILITY AND VALIDITYAlthough it is seldom done, we believe that some of theprocedures for check<strong>in</strong>g reliability <strong>and</strong> validity (seeChapter 8) could at least <strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>stances be applied <strong>to</strong>content analysis. In addition <strong>to</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g the agreementbetween two or more categorizers, it would be useful <strong>to</strong>know how the categorizations by the same researcheragree over a mean<strong>in</strong>gful time period (test-retestmethod). Furthermore, a k<strong>in</strong>d of equivalent-forms reliabilitycould be done by select<strong>in</strong>g a second sample ofmaterials or divid<strong>in</strong>g the orig<strong>in</strong>al sample <strong>in</strong> half. Onewould expect, for example, that the data obta<strong>in</strong>ed fromone sample of edi<strong>to</strong>rials would agree with those obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom a second sample. Another possibility wouldbe <strong>to</strong> divide each unit of analysis <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> halffor comparison. Thus, if the unit of analysis is a novel,the number of deroga<strong>to</strong>ry statements about foreigners <strong>in</strong>odd-numbered chapters should agree fairly well withthe number <strong>in</strong> even-numbered chapters.With respect <strong>to</strong> validity, we th<strong>in</strong>k it should oftenbe possible not only <strong>to</strong> check manifest aga<strong>in</strong>st latentcontent but also <strong>to</strong> compare either or both with resultsfrom different <strong>in</strong>struments. For example, the relativefrequency of deroga<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> positive statements aboutforeigners found <strong>in</strong> edi<strong>to</strong>rials would be expected <strong>to</strong>correspond with that found <strong>in</strong> letters <strong>to</strong> the edi<strong>to</strong>r, ifboth reflected popular op<strong>in</strong>ion.ANALYZE DATACount<strong>in</strong>g is an important characteristic of some contentanalysis. Each time a unit <strong>in</strong> a pert<strong>in</strong>ent category isfound, it is “counted.” Thus, the end product of the cod<strong>in</strong>gprocess must be numbers. It is obvious that count<strong>in</strong>gthe frequency of certa<strong>in</strong> words, phrases, symbols, pictures,or other manifest content requires the use of numbers.But even cod<strong>in</strong>g the latent content of a documentrequires the researcher <strong>to</strong> represent those cod<strong>in</strong>g decisionswith numbers <strong>in</strong> each category.It is also important <strong>to</strong> record the base, or referencepo<strong>in</strong>t, for the count<strong>in</strong>g. It would not be very <strong>in</strong>formative,for example, merely <strong>to</strong> state that a newspaper edi<strong>to</strong>rialconta<strong>in</strong>ed 15 anti-Semitic statements withoutknow<strong>in</strong>g the overall length of the edi<strong>to</strong>rial. Know<strong>in</strong>g thenumber of speeches a sena<strong>to</strong>r makes <strong>in</strong> which she arguesfor balanc<strong>in</strong>g the budget doesn’t tell us very muchabout how fiscally conservative she is if we don’t knowhow many speeches she has made on economic <strong>to</strong>picss<strong>in</strong>ce the count<strong>in</strong>g began.Let us suppose that we want <strong>to</strong> do a content analysisof the edi<strong>to</strong>rial policies of newspapers <strong>in</strong> various partsof the United States. Table 20.2 illustrates a portion of atally sheet that might be used <strong>to</strong> code such edi<strong>to</strong>rials.The first column lists the newspapers by number (eachnewspaper could be assigned a number <strong>to</strong> facilitateanalysis). The second <strong>and</strong> third columns list location<strong>and</strong> circulation, respectively. The fourth column lists thenumber of edi<strong>to</strong>rials coded for each paper. The fifth columnshows the subjective assessment by the researcherof each newspaper’s edi<strong>to</strong>rial policy (these might laterbe compared with the objective measures obta<strong>in</strong>ed).The sixth <strong>and</strong> seventh columns record the number ofcerta<strong>in</strong> types of edi<strong>to</strong>rials.The last step, then, is <strong>to</strong> analyze the data that havebeen tabulated. As <strong>in</strong> other methods of research, theTABLE 20.2 Sample Tally Sheet (Newspaper Edi<strong>to</strong>rials)Number Number NumberNewspaper of Edi<strong>to</strong>rials Subjective of Pro-Abortion of Anti-AbortionID Number Location Circulation Coded Evaluation a Edi<strong>to</strong>rials Edi<strong>to</strong>rials101 A 3,000,000 29 3 0 1102 B 675,000 21 3 1 1103 C 425,000 33 4 2 0104 D 1,000,000 40 1 0 8105 E 550,000 34 5 7 0a Categories with<strong>in</strong> the subjective evaluation: 1 very conservative; 2 somewhat conservative; 3 middle-of-the-road; 4 moderatelyliberal; 5 very liberal.


480 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7edescriptive statistical procedures discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 10are useful <strong>to</strong> summarize the data <strong>and</strong> assist theresearcher <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g what they reveal.A common way <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret content analysis data isthrough the use of frequencies (i.e., the number ofspecific <strong>in</strong>cidents found <strong>in</strong> the data) <strong>and</strong> the percentage<strong>and</strong>/or proportion of particular occurrences <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>taloccurrences. You will note that we use these statistics<strong>in</strong> the analysis of social studies research articles thatfollows (see Tables 20.3, 20.4, <strong>and</strong> 20.5). In contentanalysis studies designed <strong>to</strong> explore relationships, acrossbreak table (see Chapter 10) or chi-square analysis(see Chapter 11) is often used because both are appropriate<strong>to</strong> the analysis of categorical data.*Other researchers prefer <strong>to</strong> use codes <strong>and</strong> themes asaids <strong>in</strong> organiz<strong>in</strong>g content <strong>and</strong> arriv<strong>in</strong>g at a narrative descriptionof f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.An Illustrationof Content AnalysisIn 1988, we did a content analysis of all the researchstudies published <strong>in</strong> Theory <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social<strong>Education</strong> (TRSE) between the years 1979 <strong>and</strong> 1986. 5TRSE is a journal devoted <strong>to</strong> the publication of socialstudies research. We read 46 studies conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> thoseissues. The follow<strong>in</strong>g presents a breakdown by type ofstudy reviewed.Type of Studies ReviewedTrue experiments 7 (15%)Quasi-experiments 7 (15%)Correlational studies 9 (19%)Questionnaire-type surveys 9 (19%)Interview-type surveys 6 (13%)Ethnographies 9 (19%)n = 47 a (100%)a This <strong>to</strong>tals 47 rather than 46 because the researchers <strong>in</strong> one study usedtwo methodologies.Both of us read every study that was publisheddur<strong>in</strong>g this period that fell <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> one of these categories.We analyzed the studies us<strong>in</strong>g a cod<strong>in</strong>g sheet that wejo<strong>in</strong>tly prepared. To test our agreement concern<strong>in</strong>g themean<strong>in</strong>g of the various categories, we each <strong>in</strong>itially read*In studies <strong>in</strong> which rat<strong>in</strong>gs or scores are used, averages, correlationcoefficients, <strong>and</strong> frequency polygons are appropriate.a sample of (the same) six studies, <strong>and</strong> then met<strong>to</strong> compare our analyses. We found that we were <strong>in</strong>substantial agreement concern<strong>in</strong>g what the categoriesmeant, although it soon became apparent that weneeded some additional subcategories as well as some<strong>to</strong>tally new categories. Figure 20.4 presents the f<strong>in</strong>al se<strong>to</strong>f categories.We then reread the <strong>in</strong>itial six studies us<strong>in</strong>g the revisedset of categories, as well as the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g 40studies. We aga<strong>in</strong> met <strong>to</strong> compare our assessments.Although we had a number of disagreements, the greatmajority were simple oversights by one or the other ofus <strong>and</strong> were easily resolved.* Tables 20.3 through 20.5present some of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of our research.TABLE 20.3TABLE 20.4CategoryClarity of StudiesCategoryNumberA. Focus clear? 46 (100%)B. Variables clear?(1) Initially 31 (67%)(2) Eventually 7 (15%)(3) Never 8 (17%)C. Is treatment <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studiesmade explicit?(1) Yes 12 (26%)(2) No 2 (4%)(3) NA (no treatment) 32 (70%)D. Is there a hypothesis?(1) No 18 (39%)(2) Explicitly stated 13 (28%)(3) Clearly implied 15 (33%)Type of SampleNumberR<strong>and</strong>om selection 2 (4%)Representation based on argument 6 (13%)Convenience 29 (62%)Volunteer 4 (9%)Can’t tell 6 (13%)Note: One study used more than one type of sample. Percent is basedon n 46.*It would have been desirable <strong>to</strong> compare our analysis with the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof a second team as a further check on reliability, but this wasnot feasible.


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 4811. Type of <strong>Research</strong>A. Experimental(1) Pre(2) True(3) QuasiB. CorrelationalC. SurveyD. InterviewE. Causal-comparativeF. Ethnographic2. JustificationA. No mention of justificationB. Explicit argument made with regard <strong>to</strong>worth of studyC. Worth of study is impliedD. Any ethical considerations overlooked?3. ClarityA. Focus clear? (yes or no)B. Variables clear?(1) Initially(2) Eventually(3) NeverC. Is treatment <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studies madeexplicit? (yes, no, or n.a.)D. Is there a hypothesis?(1) No(2) Yes: explicitly stated(3) Yes: clearly implied4. Are Key Terms Def<strong>in</strong>ed?A. NoB. OperationallyC. ConstitutivelyD. Clear <strong>in</strong> context of study5. SampleA. Type(1) R<strong>and</strong>om selection(2) Representation based on argument(3) Convenience(4) Volunteer(5) Can’t tellB. Was sample adequately described?(1 = high; 5 = low)C. Size of sample (n)6. Internal ValidityA. Possible alternative explanations foroutcomes obta<strong>in</strong>ed(1) His<strong>to</strong>ry(2) Maturation(3) Mortality(4) Selection bias/subject characteristics(5) Pretest effect(6) Regression effectFigure 20.4 Categories Used <strong>to</strong> <strong>Evaluate</strong> Social Studies <strong>Research</strong>(7) Instrumentation(8) Attitude of subjectsB. Threats discussed <strong>and</strong> clarified? (yes or no)C. Was it clear that the treatment received anadequate trial (<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studies)? (yes or no)D. Was length of time of treatment sufficient?(yes or no)7. InstrumentationA. Reliability(1) Empirical check made? (yes or no)(2) If yes, was reliability adequate forstudy?B. Validity(1) Empirical check made? (yes or no)(2) If yes, type:(a) Content(b) Concurrent(c) Construct8. External ValidityA. Discussion of population generalizability(1) Appropriate(a) Explicit reference <strong>to</strong> defensibletarget population(b) Appropriate caution expressed(2) Inappropriate(a) No mention of generalizability(b) Explicit reference <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>defensibletarget populationB. Discussion of ecological generalizability(1) Appropriate(a) Explicit reference <strong>to</strong> defensiblesett<strong>in</strong>gs (subject matter, materials,physical conditions, personnel,etc.)(b) Appropriate caution expressed(2) Inappropriate(a) No mention of generalizability(b) Explicit reference <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>defensiblesett<strong>in</strong>gs9. Were Results <strong>and</strong> Interpretations KeptDist<strong>in</strong>ct? (yes or no)10. Data AnalysisA. Descriptive statistics? (yes or no)(1) Correct technique? (yes or no)(2) Correct <strong>in</strong>terpretation? (yes or no)B. Inferential statistics? (yes or no)(1) Correct technique? (yes or no)(2) Correct <strong>in</strong>terpretation? (yes or no)11. Do Data Justify Conclusions? (yes or no)12. Were Outcomes of Study <strong>Education</strong>allySignificant? (yes or no)13. Relevance of Citations


482 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 20.5Threats <strong>to</strong> Internal ValidityPossible Alternative Explanationsfor Outcomes Obta<strong>in</strong>edNumber1. His<strong>to</strong>ry 4 (9%)2. Maturation 0 (0%)3. Mortality 10 (22%)4. Selection bias/subject characteristics 15 (33%)5. Pretest effect 2 (4%)6. Regression effect 0 (0%)7. Instrumentation 21 (46%)8. Attitude of subjects 7 (15%)Threats Discussed<strong>and</strong> Clarified?Number Identified DiscussedType of Articles by Reviewers by AuthorsTrue experiments 7 3 (43%) 2 (29%)Quasi-experiments 7 7 (100%) 4 (57%)Correlational studies 9 5 (56%) 3 (33%)Questionnaire surveys 9 3 (33%) 0 (0%)Interview-type surveys 6 9 (67%) 1 (17%)Causal-comparative 0 — —Ethnographies 9 9 (100%) 0 (0%)These tables <strong>in</strong>dicate that the <strong>in</strong>tent of the studieswas clear; that the variables were generally clear (82percent); that the treatment <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention studies wasclear <strong>in</strong> almost all cases; <strong>and</strong> that most studies were hypothesistest<strong>in</strong>g, although the latter was not alwaysmade clear. Only 17 percent of the studies could claimrepresentative samples, <strong>and</strong> most of these requiredargumentation. Mortality, subject characteristics, <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>strumentation threats existed <strong>in</strong> a substantial proportionof the studies. These were acknowledged <strong>and</strong> discussedby the authors <strong>in</strong> 9 of the 15 experimental orcorrelational studies, but rarely by the authors of any ofthe other types.Us<strong>in</strong>g the Computer<strong>in</strong> Content AnalysisIn recent years, computers have been used <strong>to</strong> offsetmuch of the labor <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g documents.Computer programs have for some time been a boon <strong>to</strong>quantitative research, allow<strong>in</strong>g researchers <strong>to</strong> calculatequite rapidly very complex statistics. Programs <strong>to</strong> assistqualitative researchers <strong>in</strong> their analysis, however, nowalso exist. Many simple word-process<strong>in</strong>g programs canbe used for some k<strong>in</strong>ds of data analysis. The “f<strong>in</strong>d”comm<strong>and</strong>, for example, can locate various passages <strong>in</strong> adocument that conta<strong>in</strong> key words or phrases. Thus, a researchermight ask the computer <strong>to</strong> search for all passagesthat conta<strong>in</strong> the words creative, nonconformist, orpunishment, or phrases such as corporal punishment orartistic creativity.Notable examples of qualitative computer programsthat are currently available <strong>in</strong>clude ATLAS.ti, QSRNUD*IST, Nvivo, <strong>and</strong> Hyper<strong>Research</strong>. These programswill identify words, phrases, or sentences, tabulate theiroccurrence, pr<strong>in</strong>t <strong>and</strong> graph the tabulations, <strong>and</strong> sort <strong>and</strong>regroup words, phrases, or sentences accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> howthey fit a particular set of categories. Computers, ofcourse, presume that the <strong>in</strong>formation of <strong>in</strong>terest is <strong>in</strong>written form. Optical scanners are available that make itpossible for computers <strong>to</strong> “read” documents <strong>and</strong> s<strong>to</strong>rethe contents digitally, thus elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g the need for data


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 483entry by h<strong>and</strong>. Should you have <strong>to</strong> do some qualitativedata analysis, a few of these programs are worth tak<strong>in</strong>gsome time <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e.Advantages of ContentAnalysisAs we mentioned earlier, much of what we know is obta<strong>in</strong>ed,not through direct <strong>in</strong>teraction with others, butthrough books, newspapers, <strong>and</strong> other products ofhuman be<strong>in</strong>gs. A major advantage of content analysis isthat it is unobtrusive. A researcher can “observe” withoutbe<strong>in</strong>g observed, s<strong>in</strong>ce the contents be<strong>in</strong>g analyzedare not <strong>in</strong>fluenced by the researcher’s presence. Informationthat might be difficult, or even impossible, <strong>to</strong>obta<strong>in</strong> through direct observation or other means can bega<strong>in</strong>ed unobtrusively through analysis of textbooks <strong>and</strong>other communications, without the author or publisherbe<strong>in</strong>g aware that it is be<strong>in</strong>g exam<strong>in</strong>ed. Another advantageof content analysis is that, as we have illustrated, itis extremely useful as a means of analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>and</strong> observational data.A third advantage of content analysis is that the researchercan delve <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> records <strong>and</strong> documents <strong>to</strong> getsome feel for the social life of an earlier time. He or sheis not limited by time <strong>and</strong> space <strong>to</strong> the study of presentevents.A fourth advantage accrues from the fact that the logisticsof content analysis are often relatively simple<strong>and</strong> economical—with regard <strong>to</strong> both time <strong>and</strong> resources—ascompared <strong>to</strong> other research methods. Thisis particularly true if the <strong>in</strong>formation is readily accessible,as <strong>in</strong> newspapers, reports, books, periodicals, <strong>and</strong>the like.Lastly, because the data are readily available <strong>and</strong> almostalways can be returned <strong>to</strong> if necessary or desired,content analysis permits replication of a study by otherresearchers. Even live television programs can bevideotaped for repeated analysis at later times.Disadvantagesof Content AnalysisA major disadvantage of content analysis is that it isusually limited <strong>to</strong> recorded <strong>in</strong>formation, although theresearcher may, of course, arrange the record<strong>in</strong>gs, as <strong>in</strong>the use of open-ended questionnaires or projectivetechniques (see page 129). One would not be likely<strong>to</strong> use such record<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> study such variables asproficiency <strong>in</strong> calculus, Spanish vocabulary, or the frequencyof hostile acts because they require demonstratedbehaviors or skills.The other ma<strong>in</strong> disadvantage is <strong>in</strong> establish<strong>in</strong>gvalidity. Assum<strong>in</strong>g that different analysts can achieveacceptable agreement <strong>in</strong> categoriz<strong>in</strong>g, the question rema<strong>in</strong>sas <strong>to</strong> the true mean<strong>in</strong>g of the categories themselves.Recall the earlier discussion of this problemunder the head<strong>in</strong>g “Manifest Versus Latent Content.” Acomparison of the results of these two methodsprovides some evidence of criterion-related validity,although the two measurements obviously are not completely<strong>in</strong>dependent. As with any measurement, additionalevidence of a criterion or construct nature isimportant. In the absence of such evidence, the argumentfor content validity rests on the persuasivenessof the logic connect<strong>in</strong>g each category <strong>to</strong> its <strong>in</strong>tendedmean<strong>in</strong>g. For example, our <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the dataon social studies research assumes that what was clearor unclear <strong>to</strong> us would also be clear or unclear <strong>to</strong>other researchers or readers. Similarly, it assumes thatmost, if not all, researchers would agree as <strong>to</strong> whetherdef<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>and</strong> particular threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitywere present <strong>in</strong> a given article. While we th<strong>in</strong>k theseare reasonable assumptions, that does not makethem so.With respect <strong>to</strong> the use of content analysis <strong>in</strong>his<strong>to</strong>rical research, the researcher normally has recordsonly of what has survived or what someonethought was of sufficient importance <strong>to</strong> write down.Because each generation has a somewhat differentperspective on its life <strong>and</strong> times, what was consideredimportant at a particular time <strong>in</strong> the past may beviewed as trivial <strong>to</strong>day. Conversely, what is consideredimportant <strong>to</strong>day might not even be available fromthe past.F<strong>in</strong>ally, sometimes there is a temptation amongresearchers <strong>to</strong> consider that the <strong>in</strong>terpretations gleanedfrom a particular content analysis <strong>in</strong>dicate the causesof a phenomenon rather than be<strong>in</strong>g a reflection of it.For example, portrayal of violence <strong>in</strong> the media maybe considered a cause of <strong>to</strong>day’s violence <strong>in</strong> thestreets, but a more reasonable conclusion may be thatviolence <strong>in</strong> both the media <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the streets reflectthe attitudes of people. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly much work has <strong>to</strong> be


484 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7edone <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the relationship between the media<strong>and</strong> human behavior. Aga<strong>in</strong>, some people th<strong>in</strong>k thatread<strong>in</strong>g pornographic books <strong>and</strong> magaz<strong>in</strong>es causesmoral decay among those who read such materials.Pornography probably does affect some <strong>in</strong>dividuals,<strong>and</strong> it is likely that it affects different people <strong>in</strong> differentways. It is also quite likely that it does not affec<strong>to</strong>ther <strong>in</strong>dividuals at all, but exactly how people areaffected, <strong>and</strong> why or why not, is unclear.An Exampleof a Content Analysis StudyIn the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of content analysis, followed by a critique ofits strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> our critiquesof other types of research studies, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.RESEARCH REPORTFrom: <strong>Education</strong>, vol. 125, no. 1, Fall 2004. Reproduced by permission of Project Innovation, Inc.The “Nuts <strong>and</strong> Dolts” of Teacher Images<strong>in</strong> Children’s Picture S<strong>to</strong>rybooks:A Content AnalysisSarah Jo S<strong>and</strong>efurUC Foundation Assistant Professor of Literacy <strong>Education</strong>, University of Tennessee–ChattanoogaLeeann MooreAssistant Dean, College of <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> Human Services, Texas A & M University–CommerceRationale orConclusion?Evidence orop<strong>in</strong>ion?Purpose?Statement notconsistent withresultsChildren’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks are rife with contradic<strong>to</strong>ry representations of teachers <strong>and</strong>school. Some of those images are fairly accurate. Some of those images are quite disparatefrom reality. These representations become subsumed <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the collective consciousness of asociety <strong>and</strong> shape expectations <strong>and</strong> behaviors of both students <strong>and</strong> teachers. Teacherscannot effectuate positive change <strong>in</strong> their profession unless <strong>and</strong> until they are aware of the<strong>in</strong>ternal <strong>and</strong> external <strong>in</strong>fluences that def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> shape the educational <strong>in</strong>stitution. Thisethnographic content analysis exam<strong>in</strong>es 62 titles <strong>and</strong> 96 images of teachers <strong>to</strong> probe thepower of stereotypes/clichés. The authors found the follow<strong>in</strong>g: The teacher <strong>in</strong> children’spicture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks is overwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly portrayed as a white, non-Hispanic, woman. Theteacher <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks who is sensitive, competent, <strong>and</strong> able <strong>to</strong> manage a classroomeffectively is a m<strong>in</strong>ority. The negative images outnumbered the positive images. Theteacher <strong>in</strong> children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks is static, unchang<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> flat. The teacher is polarized<strong>and</strong> does not <strong>in</strong>spire <strong>in</strong> his or her students the pursuit of critical <strong>in</strong>quiry.A recent children’s book shares the s<strong>to</strong>ry of a teacher. Miss Malarkey, home with theflu, narrates her concern about how her elementary students will behave with <strong>and</strong>be treated by the potential substitutes available <strong>to</strong> the school. Among the substitutes


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 485represented are Mrs. Boba, a 20-someth<strong>in</strong>g woman who is <strong>to</strong>o busy pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g her <strong>to</strong>enails<strong>to</strong> attend <strong>to</strong> Miss Malarkey’s students. Mr. Doberman is a drill sergeant of a manwho snarls at the children: “So ya th<strong>in</strong>k it’s time for recess, HUH?” Mr. Lemonjello,drawn as a small, bald, nervous man, is taunted by the students with the class iguana<strong>and</strong> is subsequently covered <strong>in</strong> pa<strong>in</strong>t at art time (Miss Malarkey Won’t Be <strong>in</strong> Today,F<strong>in</strong>chler, 1998).In this text, which is representative of many that have been published with teachersas central characters, teachers are portrayed as <strong>in</strong>sensitive; misguided, victimiz<strong>in</strong>g, or<strong>in</strong>competent. We perceive these <strong>in</strong>validat<strong>in</strong>g images as worthy of detailed analysis,based on a hypothesis that a propensity of images pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g teachers <strong>in</strong> an unflatter<strong>in</strong>glight may have broader consequences on cultural perceptions of teachers <strong>and</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g.Our ethnographic content analysis here<strong>in</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>es 96 images of teachers as they arefound <strong>in</strong> 62 picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks from 1965 <strong>to</strong> present. It is our perspective that theseimages <strong>in</strong> part shape <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e the idea of “Teacher” <strong>in</strong> the collective consciousness ofa society.Those of us <strong>in</strong> teacher education realize our students come <strong>to</strong> us with previouslyconstructed images of the profession. What is the orig<strong>in</strong> of those images? When <strong>and</strong> howare these images formed <strong>and</strong> elaborated upon? It appears that the popular culture hasdone much <strong>to</strong> form or modify those images. Weber <strong>and</strong> Mitchell (1995) suggest that thesemultiple, often ambiguous, images are “. . . <strong>in</strong>tegral <strong>to</strong> the form <strong>and</strong> substance of our selfidentitiesas teachers” (p. 32). They suggest that “. . . by study<strong>in</strong>g images <strong>and</strong> prob<strong>in</strong>gtheir <strong>in</strong>fluence, teachers could play a more conscious <strong>and</strong> effective role <strong>in</strong> shap<strong>in</strong>g theirown <strong>and</strong> society’s perceptions of teachers <strong>and</strong> their work” (p. 32). We have supported this“prob<strong>in</strong>g of images” by analyz<strong>in</strong>g children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks, exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g their mean<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>and</strong> metaphors where they <strong>in</strong>tersect with teachers <strong>and</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g. It is our <strong>in</strong>tentionthat by shar<strong>in</strong>g what we have learned about the medium’s responses <strong>to</strong> the profession, wewill better serve teachers <strong>in</strong> play<strong>in</strong>g that “conscious role” <strong>in</strong> def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g their work.We submit that children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks are not benign. Although the illustrationsof teachers are often car<strong>to</strong>on-like <strong>and</strong> at first glance fairly <strong>in</strong>nocent, when taken as awhole they have power not just <strong>in</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>and</strong> their parents about the culture ofschool<strong>in</strong>g, but <strong>in</strong> shap<strong>in</strong>g it, as well. This is of concern particularly when the majority of theimages of teachers are negative, mixed, or neutral as we have found <strong>in</strong> our research <strong>and</strong> willreport here<strong>in</strong>. Gavriel Salomon, well known for his research <strong>in</strong> symbolic representations <strong>and</strong>their impact on children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, has this <strong>to</strong> say about the power of media:Media’s symbolic forms of representation are clearly not neutral or <strong>in</strong>differentpackages that have no effect on the represented <strong>in</strong>formation. Be<strong>in</strong>g part <strong>and</strong>parcel of the <strong>in</strong>formation itself, they <strong>in</strong>fluence the mean<strong>in</strong>gs one arrives at, themental capacities that are called for, <strong>and</strong> the ways one comes <strong>to</strong> view the world.Perhaps more important, the culture that creates the media <strong>and</strong> develops theirsymbolic forms of representation also opens the door for those forms <strong>to</strong> act onthe m<strong>in</strong>ds of the young <strong>in</strong> both more <strong>and</strong> less desirable ways. [italics added](1997, p. 13)We see Salomon’s work here as foundational <strong>to</strong> our own <strong>in</strong> this way: if those imageschildren <strong>and</strong> parents see of “teacher” are generally negative, then they will createa “world view” of “teacher” based upon stereotype. The many negative images ofteachers <strong>in</strong> children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks may be the message <strong>to</strong> readers that teachersare, at best, k<strong>in</strong>d but un<strong>in</strong>spir<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> at worst, roadblocks <strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>rn down <strong>in</strong> order thatchildren may move forward successfully.Evidence orop<strong>in</strong>ionNot researchhypothesis <strong>in</strong>this studySampleRationaleRationale/theoryRationaleNeed def<strong>in</strong>itionof “images”


486 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWHY STUDY IMAGES OF TEACHERS FROM POPULAR CULTURE?Background<strong>Research</strong> orop<strong>in</strong>ion?RationalePurpose?As we were prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> teach a graduate class entitled “Portrayal of Teachers <strong>in</strong> Children’sLiterature <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> Film,” we began gather<strong>in</strong>g a text set of picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks thatfocused on teachers, teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> the school environment. We quickly became aware ofthe propensity of negative images of teachers, from witch <strong>to</strong> dragon, drill sergeant <strong>to</strong>milque<strong>to</strong>ast, <strong>in</strong>competent fool <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>sensitive clod. We realized early <strong>in</strong> the graduatecourse that many teachers had not had the opportunity <strong>to</strong> critically exam<strong>in</strong>e images oftheir own profession <strong>in</strong> the popular media. They were unaware of the negative portrayals<strong>in</strong> exist<strong>in</strong>g texts, particularly <strong>in</strong> children’s literature. Teachers may not have consideredthat the negative images of the teacher “may give the public further justification for alack of support of education” (Crume, 1989, p. 36).Children’s literature is rife with contradic<strong>to</strong>ry representations of teachers <strong>and</strong>school. Some of those images are fairly accurate <strong>and</strong> some of those images are quite disparatefrom reality (Farber, Provenso, & Holm, 1994; Joseph & Burnaford, 1994; Knowles,Cole, & Presswood, 1994; Weber & Mitchell, 1995). These representations become subsumed<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the collective consciousness of a society <strong>and</strong> shape expectations <strong>and</strong> behaviorsof both students <strong>and</strong> teachers. They become a part of the images that children constructwhen they are <strong>in</strong>vited <strong>to</strong> “draw a teacher” or “play school,” <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>deed theimages that teachers draw of themselves. Consider, for example, the three-year old boywith no prior school<strong>in</strong>g experience, who, <strong>in</strong> play<strong>in</strong>g school, puts the dolls <strong>in</strong> straightrows, selects a dom<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g personality for a female teacher, <strong>and</strong> assigns homework(Weber & Mitchell, 1995).This exploration <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> teacher images is a critical one at multiple levels of teachereducation. Pre-service teachers need <strong>to</strong> analyze via media images their personal motivations<strong>and</strong> expectations of the teach<strong>in</strong>g profession <strong>and</strong> enter <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g with clearunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gs of how the broad culture perceives their work. In-service teachers need<strong>to</strong> heighten their awareness of how children, parents, <strong>and</strong> community members perceivethem. These perceptions may be <strong>in</strong> part media-<strong>in</strong>duced <strong>and</strong> not based on the complex realityof a particular teacher. If <strong>in</strong>formation is <strong>in</strong>deed power, then perhaps those of us <strong>in</strong>the profession can better underst<strong>and</strong> that popular images contribute <strong>to</strong> the public’s frequentsuspicion of our efficacy, <strong>and</strong> this heightened awareness can support us <strong>in</strong> address<strong>in</strong>gthe negative images head on.RESEARCH PERSPECTIVESPrior research<strong>How</strong> do we as teachers, prospective teachers, <strong>and</strong> teacher educa<strong>to</strong>rs come <strong>to</strong> so fully subscribe<strong>to</strong> the images we have both experienced <strong>and</strong> imag<strong>in</strong>ed? Have those imagesformed long before adulthood, perhaps even before the child enters school? Weber <strong>and</strong>Mitchell (1994) contend, “Even before children beg<strong>in</strong> school, they have already been exposed<strong>to</strong> a myriad of images of teachers, classrooms <strong>and</strong> schools which have made strong<strong>and</strong> last<strong>in</strong>g impressions on them” (p. 2). Some of those images <strong>and</strong> attitudes form fromdirect experience with teachers. Barone, Meyerson, <strong>and</strong> Mallette (1995) expla<strong>in</strong>, “Whenadults respond <strong>to</strong> the question of which person had the greatest impact on their lives,other than their immediate family, teachers are frequently mentioned” (p. 257). Thoseearly images are not necessarily positive, often convey traditional teach<strong>in</strong>g styles, <strong>and</strong>are marked with commonalities across the United States (Joseph & Burnaford, 1994;Weber & Mitchell, 1995).In addition <strong>to</strong> the years of “on-the-job” experience with teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> teachersthat one acquires as a student sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g “on the other side of the desk,’ aperson has also acquired images <strong>and</strong> stereotypes of teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> teachers from the


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 487person’s experiences with literature <strong>and</strong> media. Lortie calls this “the apprenticeship-ofobservation”(1975, p. 67). These forms of pr<strong>in</strong>t media (literature) <strong>and</strong> visual media arepart of “popular culture,” which is <strong>in</strong>clusive of film, television, magaz<strong>in</strong>es, newspapers,music, video, books, car<strong>to</strong>ons, etc. In the past decade the literature on popular culturehas grown dramatically as an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g number of educa<strong>to</strong>rs, social scientists, <strong>and</strong> othercritical th<strong>in</strong>kers have begun <strong>to</strong> study the field (Daspit & Weaver, 1999; Giroux, 1994;Giroux, 1988; Giroux & Simon, 1989; McLaren, 1994; Trifonas, 2000; Weber & Mitchell,1995). Weber <strong>and</strong> Mitchell (1994) expla<strong>in</strong>, “So pervasive are teachers <strong>in</strong> popular culturethat if you simply ask, as we have, schoolchildren <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>to</strong> name teachers they remember,not from school but from popular culture, a cast of fictionalized charactersemerges that takes on larger than life proportions” (p. 14). These authors challenge us<strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e how it is that children—even young children—would hold such strong images<strong>and</strong> that there be such similarity among the images they hold.Studies of children’s literature have previously exam<strong>in</strong>ed issues of stereotyp<strong>in</strong>g(race, gender, ethnicity, age) as well as moral <strong>and</strong> ethical issues with<strong>in</strong> s<strong>to</strong>ries (Dougherty &Engel, 1987; Hurley & Chadwick, 1998; Lamme, 1996). Recently Barone, Meyerson, <strong>and</strong>Mallette (1995) exam<strong>in</strong>ed the images of teachers <strong>in</strong> children’s literature. They found astartl<strong>in</strong>g paradox: “On one h<strong>and</strong>, teachers are valued as contribut<strong>in</strong>g members of society;on the other h<strong>and</strong>, teachers are frequently portrayed <strong>in</strong> the media <strong>and</strong> literature as<strong>in</strong>ept <strong>and</strong> not very bright” (p. 257).Barone, et al. (1995) found two types of teachers portrayed: traditional, non-childcentered, <strong>and</strong> non-traditional, more child-centered. The more prevalent type, the traditionalteacher, was not usually liked nor respected by the students <strong>in</strong> the s<strong>to</strong>ries. The nontraditionalteacher was seldom portrayed, but when the portrayal was presented, theteacher was shown <strong>to</strong> be valued <strong>and</strong> well liked. They contend that the reality of teach<strong>in</strong>gis far <strong>to</strong>o complex <strong>to</strong> fall <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two such simple categories; that the act of teach<strong>in</strong>g is complex.They po<strong>in</strong>t out that” . . . the authors of children’s books often negate this complexityof teach<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> classify teachers as those who care about students <strong>and</strong>those who are rigid or less sensitive <strong>to</strong> students’ needs” (p. 260). Their study led <strong>to</strong> severaldisturb<strong>in</strong>g conclusions: (a) The ubiqui<strong>to</strong>us portrayal of traditional teachers as mean <strong>and</strong>strict make schools <strong>and</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g appear <strong>to</strong> be a dreadful experience. (b) The portrayal ofteachers is frequently one <strong>in</strong> which the teacher is shown as hav<strong>in</strong>g less <strong>in</strong>telligence thanthe students have. (c) Teachers are portrayed as hav<strong>in</strong>g little or no confidence <strong>in</strong> their students<strong>and</strong> their abilities. Weber <strong>and</strong> Mitchell (1995) assert that “the stereotypes that areprevalent <strong>in</strong> the popular culture <strong>and</strong> experience of childhood play a formative role <strong>in</strong> theevolution of a teacher’s identity <strong>and</strong> are part of the enculturation of teachers <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> theirprofession” (p. 27). Joseph <strong>and</strong> Burnaford (1994) address the numerous examples ofcaricatures or stereotypes as be<strong>in</strong>g somewhat different, but “. . . all are negative <strong>and</strong> allreduce the teacher <strong>to</strong> an object of scorn, disrespect, <strong>and</strong> sometimes fear” (p. 15).What are theresults?Prior researchPrior research<strong>Research</strong>conclusion?<strong>Research</strong>resultsBased on research?<strong>Research</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs?WHAT RESEARCH FRAMEWORK GUIDED OUR STUDY?To answer our questions concern<strong>in</strong>g the elements of the children’s texts, we required amethodological framework from which we could exam<strong>in</strong>e the “character” of the texts.We found that framework <strong>in</strong> access<strong>in</strong>g research theories from anthropology <strong>and</strong> literarycriticism which suggested an appropriate approach <strong>to</strong> content analysis.Submitt<strong>in</strong>g that all research directly or <strong>in</strong>directly <strong>in</strong>volves participant observation,David Altheide (1987) f<strong>in</strong>ds an ethnographic approach applicable <strong>to</strong> content analyses, <strong>in</strong>that the writ<strong>in</strong>gs or electronic texts are ultimately products of social <strong>in</strong>teraction. Ethnographiccontent analysis (ECA) requires a reflexive <strong>and</strong> highly <strong>in</strong>teractive relationshipJustification ofmethod


488 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSee text, p. 485between researcher <strong>and</strong> data with the objective of <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> verify<strong>in</strong>g the communicationof mean<strong>in</strong>g. The mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the text message is assumed <strong>to</strong> be reflected <strong>in</strong>the multiple elements of form, content, context, <strong>and</strong> other nuances. The movement betweenresearcher <strong>and</strong> data throughout the process of concept development, sampl<strong>in</strong>g,data collection, data analysis, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation is systematic but not rigid, <strong>in</strong>itiallystructured but receptive <strong>to</strong> emerg<strong>in</strong>g categories <strong>and</strong> concepts.As we proceeded through the multiple read<strong>in</strong>gs of the picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks, we attempted<strong>to</strong> foreground three ma<strong>in</strong> concepts: (a) To attempt <strong>to</strong> discover “mean<strong>in</strong>g” is anattempt <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude the multiple elements which make up the whole: appearance, language,subject taught, gender issues, racial/ethnic diversity, <strong>and</strong> other nuances as theybecame apparent; (b) The multiple read<strong>in</strong>gs of the selected sample of children’s literature<strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret the structures of the texts are not <strong>to</strong> conform thetexts <strong>to</strong> our analytic notions but <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>form them; <strong>and</strong> (c) In the <strong>in</strong>timacy of our relationshipwith the data we are act<strong>in</strong>g on them <strong>and</strong> chang<strong>in</strong>g them, just as the data are chang<strong>in</strong>gus <strong>and</strong> the way we perceive past <strong>and</strong> present texts. As we encountered new texts, weattempted <strong>to</strong> consistently return <strong>to</strong> previous texts <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be receptive <strong>to</strong> new or revised<strong>in</strong>terpretations that were revealed.WHAT WAS OUR RESEARCH METHODOLOGY?Sample<strong>How</strong> is this known?Same as Image?Good def<strong>in</strong>itionsWe used Follett Library Resources’ database <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d titles address<strong>in</strong>g “teachers” <strong>and</strong>“schools.” This resulted <strong>in</strong> a list of 62 titles <strong>and</strong> 96 teacher images published from 1965<strong>to</strong> present (Appendix A). No chapter books or Magic Schoolbus series books were reviewed,as they did not qualify under the def<strong>in</strong>ition of “picture s<strong>to</strong>rybook” (Huck, 1997,p. 198). We specifically did not attend <strong>to</strong> publication dates or “<strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>t/out of pr<strong>in</strong>t” status,as many of these texts appear on school <strong>and</strong> public library shelves decades afterthey have gone out of pr<strong>in</strong>t. Our approach provided us with the majority of children’spicture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks available for purchase <strong>in</strong> the United States or available through publiclibraries.To better guide our exam<strong>in</strong>ations about the images of teachers, ensure that we reviewedthe titles consistently, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> record the details of the texts we reviewed,we noted details of each teacher representation <strong>in</strong> aspects of Appearance, Language,Subject, Approach, <strong>and</strong> Effectiveness. The specific details we were seek<strong>in</strong>g under eachcategory for each teacher represented <strong>in</strong> the sample literature are further describedbelow:Appearance: observable race, gender, approximate age, name, cloth<strong>in</strong>g, hairstyle,weight (th<strong>in</strong>, average, plump)Language: representative utterances by the teacher represented <strong>in</strong> the book or asreported by the narra<strong>to</strong>r of the bookSubject: the school subject(s) that the teacher was represented as teach<strong>in</strong>g: read<strong>in</strong>g/language arts, math, geography, his<strong>to</strong>ry, etc.Approach: any <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rs of a teach<strong>in</strong>g philosophy, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g whether childrenwere seated <strong>in</strong> rows, were work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g centers, were recit<strong>in</strong>gmemorized material, whether the teacher was shown lectur<strong>in</strong>g, etc.Effectiveness: <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong>cluded narra<strong>to</strong>r’s po<strong>in</strong>t of view, images or Ianguageabout children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g from that teacher; images or language about children’semotional response <strong>to</strong> the teacher etc.We also attempted <strong>to</strong> note the absence of data as well as the presence of data. Forexample, we noted the occurrences of a teacher rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g nameless through the book,


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 489of a teacher not be<strong>in</strong>g represented as teach<strong>in</strong>g any curriculum, or of a teacher fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>spire any critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> her students.We entered data <strong>in</strong> the forego<strong>in</strong>g categories about each teacher representationon<strong>to</strong> forms, which we then reviewed <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> group the <strong>in</strong>dividually representedteachers <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> four more specific categories: positive representations, negative representations,mixed review, <strong>and</strong> neutral. A teacher fitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the category of “positiveteacher” was represented as be<strong>in</strong>g sensitive <strong>to</strong> children’s emotional needs, supportive ofmean<strong>in</strong>gful learn<strong>in</strong>g, compassionate, warm, approachable, able <strong>to</strong> exercise classroommanagement skills without resort<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> punitive measures or yell<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> was respectful<strong>and</strong> protective of children. A teacher would be classified as a “negative teacher” if he orshe were represented as dicta<strong>to</strong>rial, us<strong>in</strong>g harsh language, unable <strong>to</strong> manage classroombehavior, distant or removed, <strong>in</strong>attentive, unable <strong>to</strong> create a learn<strong>in</strong>g environment, allow<strong>in</strong>gteas<strong>in</strong>g or taunt<strong>in</strong>g among students, or unempathetic <strong>to</strong> students’ diverse backgrounds.A teacher was categorized as “mixed review” if they possessed characteristicsthat were both positive <strong>and</strong> negative: for example, if a teacher were otherwise representedas car<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> effective <strong>in</strong> the classroom, but did noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> halt the teas<strong>in</strong>g of achild. The fourth category for consideration was that of “neutral,” <strong>in</strong> which a teacherwas represented <strong>in</strong> the illustration of a text, but had neither a positive nor a negativeeffect on the children.A doc<strong>to</strong>ral student focus<strong>in</strong>g on read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the elementary school <strong>and</strong> who is wellversed<strong>in</strong> children’s literature served as an <strong>in</strong>ter-rater for this part of the analysis. Afterhav<strong>in</strong>g conferred on the characteristics of each category, she read each text <strong>in</strong>dependentlyof the researchers <strong>and</strong> categorized each teacher as “positive,” negative,” “mixedreview,” <strong>and</strong> “neutral.” We achieved 100% agreement <strong>in</strong> the category of “positive representationsof teachers” <strong>and</strong> 93% agreement regard<strong>in</strong>g the “negative” images. We had75% agreement on the “neutral” images <strong>and</strong> 100% agreement on the category of“mixed” images (two images). Upon further discussion of our qualifications for “neutral,”we were able <strong>to</strong> agree on all 14 images as hav<strong>in</strong>g neither a positive nor negativeimpact on the children as represented <strong>in</strong> the text.Good def<strong>in</strong>itionsGood reliabilitycheckWho is “we”?WHAT WERE THE FINDINGS?Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs regard<strong>in</strong>g the preponderance of the images are detailed <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>gparagraphs.The teacher <strong>in</strong> children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks is overwhelm<strong>in</strong>gly portrayed as awhite, non-Hispanic woman. There were only eight representations of African- Americanteachers, <strong>and</strong> only three of them were the protagonists of the books: The Best Teacher <strong>in</strong>the World (Chardiet & Maccarone, 1990); Show <strong>and</strong> Tell (Munsch,1991); <strong>and</strong> Will I Have aFriend? (Cohen, 1967). Two Asians, no Native Americans, <strong>and</strong> no other persons of colorare shown <strong>in</strong> the 96 teacher images, mak<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>to</strong>tal number of culturally diverse imagesrepresented at only 11% of the <strong>to</strong>tal.The teacher <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks who is sensitive, competent, <strong>and</strong> able <strong>to</strong> managea classroom effectively is a m<strong>in</strong>ority. The teacher who met the st<strong>and</strong>ards we described fora “positive teacher,” which <strong>in</strong>clude an ability <strong>to</strong> construct mean<strong>in</strong>gful learn<strong>in</strong>g environments,compassion, respect, <strong>and</strong> management skills for a group of children, exists <strong>in</strong> only42% of the teacher images <strong>in</strong> our sample. This means only 40 images out of a <strong>to</strong>tal 96 imageswere demonstrative of teacher efficacy. Some examples of the “positive teacher”are found <strong>in</strong> Mr. Sl<strong>in</strong>gerl<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> Lilly’s Purple Plastic Purse (Henkes, 1996), Mr. Falker <strong>in</strong>Thank You, Mr. Falker (Polacco, 1998), <strong>and</strong> Arizona Hughes <strong>in</strong> My Great-aunt Arizona(Hous<strong>to</strong>n, 1992).Good detail


490 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eBut 42% <strong>in</strong> eachDisagrees withdescriptions thatfollowSupported <strong>in</strong> thisstudy?But only two ou<strong>to</strong>f 96Good examplesThe negative images outnumbered the positive images. Teachers who were dicta<strong>to</strong>rial,used harsh language with children, were distant or removed, or allowed teas<strong>in</strong>gamong students comprised 42% of the <strong>to</strong>tal number of 96 teacher representations.Examples of the “negative teacher” are found <strong>in</strong> the nameless teacher <strong>in</strong> John PatrickNorman McHennessy—The Boy Who Was Always Late (Burn<strong>in</strong>gham, 1987), Miss Tyler <strong>in</strong>Today Was a Terrible Day (Giff, 1980), <strong>and</strong> Miss L<strong>and</strong>ers <strong>in</strong> The Art Lesson (dePaola, 1989).There were only two teachers <strong>in</strong> the sample who received a “mixed review,” which wasby def<strong>in</strong>ition a generally positive teacher with some negative strategies, approaches, orstatements (Mrs. Chud <strong>in</strong> Chrysanthemum [Henkes, 1991] <strong>and</strong> Mrs. Page <strong>in</strong> Miss Ala<strong>in</strong>eus:A Vocabulary Disaster [Frasier, 2000]). Fourteen teacher images, or 15% of the <strong>to</strong>tal number,were represented as “neutral,” mean<strong>in</strong>g that the teacher <strong>in</strong> the text had neither apositive nor a negative impact on the students. The nameless teachers <strong>in</strong> Oliver But<strong>to</strong>n Isa Sissy (de Paola, 1979) <strong>and</strong> Amaz<strong>in</strong>g Grace (Hoffman, 1991) are representative of “neutral”teacher images.The teacher <strong>in</strong> children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks is static, unchang<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> flat. An unexpectedf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this content analysis was that teachers <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks arenever shown as learners themselves, never portrayed as mov<strong>in</strong>g from less effective <strong>to</strong>more effective. Like the nameless teacher <strong>in</strong> Miriam Cohen’s “Welcome <strong>to</strong> First Grade!”series, if she is a paragon of k<strong>in</strong>dness <strong>and</strong> patience, she will rema<strong>in</strong> so unfail<strong>in</strong>gly fromthe beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the text <strong>to</strong> its conclusion. If he is an <strong>in</strong>competent novice, likeMr. Lemonjello <strong>in</strong> Miss Malarkey Won’t Be <strong>in</strong> Today (F<strong>in</strong>chler, 1998), he will not be shownreflect<strong>in</strong>g, learn<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>vent<strong>in</strong>g himself <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> an <strong>in</strong>formed <strong>and</strong> effective educa<strong>to</strong>r bybook’s end. Perhaps the evolution from mediocrity <strong>to</strong> effectiveness holds little <strong>in</strong> the wayof enterta<strong>in</strong>ment value, but it could hold great value <strong>in</strong> the demonstration that teachersare complex human be<strong>in</strong>gs with a significant capacity for growth. The potential <strong>to</strong> pa<strong>in</strong>trealistic portraits of teachers is present, but we see little evidence of the medium’s desire<strong>to</strong> construct such an image.The teacher <strong>in</strong> children’s picture books is polarized. Other researchers have alsonoted our concerns that we as teachers represented <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks are “healers orwounders . . . sensitive or callous, imag<strong>in</strong>ative or repressive” (Joseph & Burnaford, 1994,p. 12). Only 15% of the teachers presented <strong>in</strong> our sample are neutral images, neither positivelynor negatively impact<strong>in</strong>g the children <strong>in</strong> the fictional classroom, <strong>and</strong> only two imagesout of the 96 exam<strong>in</strong>ed qualified as a “mixed review” of mostly positive characteristicswith some negative aspects of educational practice. Therefore, approximately 84%of the teachers represented <strong>in</strong> our sample are either very good or horrid. The teacherparagon <strong>in</strong> picture books “generally is a woman who never demonstrates the features ofcommonplace motherhood—impatience, frustration, or possibly <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> the worldother than children themselves—demonstrates <strong>to</strong> children that the teacher is a wonderfullybenign creature” (Joseph & Burnaford, 1994, p. 11). Ms. Darcy <strong>in</strong> The Best Teacher<strong>in</strong> the Whole World (Chardiet & Maccarone, 1990), <strong>and</strong> Mrs. Beejorgenhoosen <strong>in</strong> RachelParker, K<strong>in</strong>dergarten Show-off (Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1992) fit neatly <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the mold of “paragon.”They are not represented exhibit<strong>in</strong>g any less-than-perfect, but realistic, characteristics ofexhaustion, short-temperedness, or lapses <strong>in</strong> good judgment.Several texts offer “over the <strong>to</strong>p” representations of bad teachers. The oftenreviewedBlack Lagoon series depicts the teachers <strong>in</strong> children’s imag<strong>in</strong>ations as firebreath<strong>in</strong>gdragons or huge, green gorillas. The well-known Miss Nelson series (Allard) hascreated substitute teacher Viola Swamp <strong>in</strong> the likeness of a witch, complete with<strong>in</strong>credible bulk, large features, warts, <strong>and</strong> a perpetual bad hair day. The teachers <strong>in</strong> TheBig Box (Morrison, 1999) put a child who “just can’t h<strong>and</strong>le her freedom” <strong>in</strong> a big, brownbox. Other books offer slightly more subtle; but still alarm<strong>in</strong>g, representations of negative


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 491teach<strong>in</strong>g practice. Consider Miss Tyler, the heavy-lidded, unsmil<strong>in</strong>g teacher <strong>in</strong> TodayWas a Terrible Day (Giff, 1980), who humiliates Ronald five times <strong>in</strong> the course of thes<strong>to</strong>ry; or Mrs. Bell, who <strong>in</strong> Double Trouble <strong>in</strong> Walla Walla (Clements, 1997), takes a child<strong>to</strong> the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal for her unique language style. Even worse is the nameless teacher whorepeatedly (<strong>and</strong> falsely) accuses a student of ly<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> threatens <strong>to</strong> strike him with a stick(John Patrick Norman McHennessey—The Boy Who Was Always Late, Burn<strong>in</strong>gham,1987). In less drastic representations but, still of concern <strong>to</strong> those of us who believe thatliterature <strong>in</strong>forms expectations about reality, teachers are represented as fail<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> protectchildren from their peers’ taunts. Teachers are shown do<strong>in</strong>g noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p the teas<strong>in</strong>gof children <strong>in</strong> Chrysanthemum (Henkes, 1991), The Br<strong>and</strong> New Kid (Couric, 2000),Today Was a Terrible Day (Giff, 1980), <strong>and</strong> Miss Ala<strong>in</strong>eus: A Vocabulary Disaster (Frasier,2000). If children are learn<strong>in</strong>g about teachers <strong>and</strong> school from the children’s books read<strong>to</strong> them, we propose that there is cause for concern about the unrealistic expectationschildren could develop from such polarized <strong>and</strong> unrealistic images.The teacher <strong>in</strong> children’s picture books does not <strong>in</strong>spire <strong>in</strong> his or her students thepursuit of critical <strong>in</strong>quiry. The overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g majority of texts which represent teachers<strong>in</strong> a positive light—<strong>and</strong> these number <strong>in</strong> our sample only 42% of the <strong>to</strong>tal number ofschool-related children’s literature—show them as k<strong>in</strong>d caregivers who dry tears(Miss Hart <strong>in</strong> Ruby the Copycat, Rathmann, 1991), resolve jealousy between children(Mrs. Beejorgenhoosen <strong>in</strong> Rachel Parker, K<strong>in</strong>dergarten Show-off, Mart<strong>in</strong>, 1992), res<strong>to</strong>reself-esteem (Mrs. Tw<strong>in</strong>kle <strong>in</strong> Chrysanthemum, Henkes, 1991), teach right from wrong(Ms. Darcy <strong>in</strong> The Best Teacher <strong>in</strong> the Whole World, Chardiet & Maccarone, 1990). <strong>How</strong>ever,few teachers are represented as hav<strong>in</strong>g a substantial impact on a child’s learn<strong>in</strong>g.Joseph <strong>and</strong> Burnaford (1994) found that teachers are not seen “lead<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong>ward<strong>in</strong>tellectual pursuits—<strong>to</strong>ward analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> challeng<strong>in</strong>g exist<strong>in</strong>g conditions of community<strong>and</strong> society....The ‘successful’ teacher [<strong>in</strong> children’s literature] . . . does not awakenstudents’ <strong>in</strong>telligence. Such teachers value order; order is what they strive for, what theyare paid for” (p. 16).Our analysis confirms their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Examples are common <strong>in</strong> which teachers actuallyprovide roadblocks <strong>to</strong> children’s success. Tommy <strong>in</strong> The Art Lesson (dePaola, 1989) mustwage battle <strong>to</strong> use his own crayons, use more than just one sheet of paper, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> create artbased on his own vision <strong>and</strong> not the tired model of the art teacher. Miss K<strong>in</strong>caid <strong>in</strong> The Br<strong>and</strong>New Kid (Couric, 2000) actually establishes the opportunity for children <strong>to</strong> tease the newboy who is an immigrant: “We have a new student . . . His name is a different one, Lazlo S.Gasky.” Young Lazlo’s mother must help him f<strong>in</strong>d his way <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the culture of the school <strong>and</strong>community. In David Goes <strong>to</strong> School (Shannon, 1999), young David is met with negativelyframed dem<strong>and</strong>s from his nameless <strong>and</strong> faceless teacher: “No, David!”, “You’re tardy!”,“Keep your h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>to</strong> yourself!”, “Shhhhh!”, <strong>and</strong> “You’re stay<strong>in</strong>g after school!”Only six books <strong>in</strong> our sample represent teachers as <strong>in</strong>tellectually <strong>in</strong>spir<strong>in</strong>g.Mr. Isobe <strong>in</strong> Crow Boy (Yashima, 1967) is represented as child-centered <strong>and</strong> appreciativeof Chibi’s knowledge of agriculture <strong>and</strong> botany, who values his draw<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> stays afterschool <strong>to</strong> talk with young Chibi. He is represented as the catalyst for the crow imitationsat the school talent show which ga<strong>in</strong> Chibi recognition <strong>and</strong> a newfound respect amonghis peers. In Lilly’s Purple Plastic Purse (Henkes, 1996) Mr. Sl<strong>in</strong>gerl<strong>and</strong> is such an effectiveteacher that he <strong>in</strong>spires Lilly <strong>to</strong> want <strong>to</strong> be a teacher (when she isn’t want<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be “adancer or a surgeon or an ambulance driver or a diva . . .”). Mr. Cohen <strong>in</strong> Creativity (Step<strong>to</strong>e,1997) uses the arrival of a new immigrant <strong>in</strong> his class <strong>to</strong> teach about the his<strong>to</strong>ry ofimmigration <strong>in</strong> this country <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> deliver a message about <strong>to</strong>lerance <strong>and</strong> shared his<strong>to</strong>ries.Mrs. Hughes <strong>in</strong> My Great-aunt Arizona (Hous<strong>to</strong>n, 1992) teaches generations of childrenabout “words <strong>and</strong> numbers <strong>and</strong> the faraway places they would visit someday.” TheBut horrid? Seeprior comment.Good examplesSix out of 62


492 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAre theredifferences acrosstime (1965–2005)?nameless teacher <strong>in</strong> When Will I Read? (Cohen, 1977) helps young Jim come <strong>to</strong> the realizationthat he is a reader, <strong>and</strong> Mr. Falker <strong>in</strong> Thank You, Mr. Falker (Polacco, 1998), helpsfifth-grader Trisha learn <strong>to</strong> read <strong>in</strong> three months <strong>and</strong> cries over her achievement whenshe reads her first book <strong>in</strong>dependently. Although these are excellent examples of howteachers can be represented as dedicated supporters of learn<strong>in</strong>g, only six texts out of the62 <strong>in</strong> our sample construct images of teacher as an educated professional.DISCUSSIONEvidence wouldhelp hereOther researchers have found bias, prejudice, <strong>and</strong> stereotypical presentations of characters<strong>in</strong> children’s books, <strong>and</strong> our study specifically about images of teachers does not disputethose f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (Barone, Meyerson, & Mallette, 1995; Hurley & Chadwick, 1998;Hurst, 1981). From our extensive 62-book sample of picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks widely available<strong>to</strong> children, parents, <strong>and</strong> teachers, we have found a parade of teachers who discouragecreativity, ignore teas<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> even threaten <strong>to</strong> hit children with sticks. We have alsofound teachers <strong>in</strong> children’s literature who, <strong>in</strong> great devotion <strong>to</strong> the human good <strong>and</strong>the educative process, save children: from boredom, from illiteracy, <strong>and</strong> from the devastat<strong>in</strong>geffects of social isolation. Our deep concern is that the books <strong>in</strong> which the teacheris demonstrated as <strong>in</strong>telligent <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>spir<strong>in</strong>g (six <strong>in</strong> our 62 book sample) are dwarfed bythe number of books <strong>in</strong> which the image of Teacher is one of daft <strong>in</strong>competence, unreasonableanger, or rigid conformity.We do not f<strong>in</strong>d images of teachers as transformative <strong>in</strong>tellectuals, as educa<strong>to</strong>rswho “go beyond concern with forms of empowerment that promote <strong>in</strong>dividual achievement<strong>and</strong> traditional forms of academic success” (Giroux, 1989, p. 138). Instead, we f<strong>in</strong>drepresentations of teachers whose negatively metaphoric/deroga<strong>to</strong>ry surnames <strong>in</strong>dicatethe level of respect for the profession: Mr. Quackerbot<strong>to</strong>m, Mrs. Nutty, Ima Berpur, MissBonkers, <strong>and</strong> Miss Malarkey.Referr<strong>in</strong>g back <strong>to</strong> the graduate class we taught on representations of teachers <strong>in</strong>popular culture, we perceived a naiveté <strong>in</strong> these teachers as <strong>to</strong> the power of the media,<strong>to</strong> the power of stereotypes <strong>to</strong> shape the teach<strong>in</strong>g profession, <strong>and</strong> the power that teachershave <strong>to</strong> combat the negative images. An overwhelm<strong>in</strong>g majority of our graduate studentsvalued the traditional teacher who ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed order, was nurtur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> car<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> whose focus was on the emotional well-be<strong>in</strong>g of the child. They failed <strong>to</strong> notice thatit was an extremely rare image <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks that showed a teacher as an <strong>in</strong>tellectually<strong>in</strong>spir<strong>in</strong>g force.Teachers cannot effectuate positive change <strong>in</strong> their profession unless <strong>and</strong> untilthey are aware of the <strong>in</strong>ternal <strong>and</strong> external <strong>in</strong>fluences that def<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> shape the educational<strong>in</strong>stitution. We want <strong>to</strong> encourage reflection <strong>and</strong> conversation about school<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g, careful evaluation of extant images <strong>in</strong> popular culture <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> developmean<strong>in</strong>gful dialogue about the accuracy of those images, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> encourage teachers <strong>to</strong>exam<strong>in</strong>e their own memories of teachers <strong>and</strong> how they form current perceptions.IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCHOur explorations <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the representations of teachers <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks have led <strong>to</strong>other <strong>and</strong> further questions regard<strong>in</strong>g images that cultures create of their educationprofessionals.There is much <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> be gleaned from a careful study of the portrayals ofschool adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong> picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks. <strong>How</strong> are teachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs represented<strong>in</strong> basal literature? <strong>How</strong> often do basal publishers select literature or write their


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 493own literature that has school as a sett<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> what is the ratio of positive representations<strong>to</strong> negative ones? Do children’s authors <strong>in</strong> other cultures <strong>and</strong> countries createsimilar negative images of educa<strong>to</strong>rs with the same frequency <strong>and</strong> ire as they do <strong>in</strong> theU.S.? <strong>How</strong> are teachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs portrayed <strong>in</strong> literature for older children, as<strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>termediate chapter books, or young adult novels? <strong>How</strong> have the imagesof teachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs evolved over time <strong>in</strong> our culture? Was there a time<strong>in</strong> our his<strong>to</strong>ry that teachers were consistently portrayed <strong>in</strong> a positive light, <strong>and</strong> wasthere perhaps a national event or series of events which caused the images <strong>to</strong> take onmore negative characteristics?CONCLUSIONBefore we began this study we came across a book entitled Through the Cracks(Sollman, Emmons, & Paol<strong>in</strong>i, 1994), which we decided not <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> our literaturesample as we perceive this text <strong>to</strong> be more for teachers <strong>and</strong> teacher educa<strong>to</strong>rs than children.The text now takes on new importance <strong>in</strong> light of our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. It chronicleschange on one school campus through the eyes of an elementary-age student, Stella.Early <strong>in</strong> the s<strong>to</strong>ry Stella <strong>and</strong> some of her peers beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> physically shr<strong>in</strong>k <strong>and</strong> literally fallthrough the cracks of the classroom floor because of boredom—boredom with both thecontent <strong>and</strong> delivery of the school curriculum. The teachers <strong>in</strong>itially are illustrated aslectur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> daydream<strong>in</strong>g children, runn<strong>in</strong>g off dit<strong>to</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> grad<strong>in</strong>g papers dur<strong>in</strong>g classtime; one image even shows a teacher sharply reprim<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g a child for pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g her pigblue <strong>in</strong>stead of the p<strong>in</strong>k anticipated <strong>in</strong> the teacher’s lesson plan. The children havebecome lost <strong>in</strong> a k<strong>in</strong>d of academic purga<strong>to</strong>ry under the floorboards. Here they rema<strong>in</strong>until substantial changes are made on their campus. The children at first watch, thencome up through the floor <strong>to</strong> become <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>, a curriculum that has become relevant,child-centered, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>tegrative of the arts. Teachers are then represented assupport<strong>in</strong>g children’s learn<strong>in</strong>g through highly <strong>in</strong>tegrated explorations of Egypt, theAmerican Revolution, geometry, life <strong>in</strong> a pond. Their images are shown guid<strong>in</strong>g thechildren <strong>in</strong> recreat<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>and</strong> social events; support<strong>in</strong>g student <strong>in</strong>quiry; explor<strong>in</strong>gpa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g, build<strong>in</strong>g, draw<strong>in</strong>g, danc<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> play<strong>in</strong>g music as a way of know<strong>in</strong>g; cook<strong>in</strong>g;becom<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> community clean-up projects; <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g experts; conduct<strong>in</strong>gscience experiments; <strong>and</strong> more.L<strong>in</strong>da Lamme (1996) concludes that “. . . children’s literature is a resource withample moral <strong>and</strong> ethical activity, that, when shared sensitively with children, can enhancetheir moral development <strong>and</strong> accomplish the lofty goals <strong>to</strong> which educa<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong> ademocracy aspire” (p. 412). Our po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g the contents of Through the Cracks isthis: the picture s<strong>to</strong>rybook format has the potential <strong>to</strong> share with readers the reality ofan effective <strong>and</strong> creative teacher. As opposed <strong>to</strong> an object of ridicule or scath<strong>in</strong>g humor,a teacher can be represented as an <strong>in</strong>tellectual who <strong>in</strong>spires children <strong>to</strong> stretch, grow,<strong>and</strong> explore previously unknown worlds <strong>and</strong> communicate that new knowledge throughmultiple communicative systems. The picture s<strong>to</strong>rybook has the potential <strong>to</strong> encourage achild <strong>to</strong> anticipate the valuable discoveries that are possible <strong>in</strong> the school sett<strong>in</strong>g; it canalso demonstrate <strong>to</strong> parents how school ought <strong>to</strong> be <strong>and</strong> how teachers support children<strong>in</strong> cognitive <strong>and</strong> psychosocial ways. Children’s literature can also provide positive enculturationfor pre-service teachers <strong>and</strong> validation for <strong>in</strong>-service teachers of the possibilities<strong>in</strong>herent <strong>in</strong> their social contributions. Positive representations of teachers have thepotential <strong>to</strong> empower all the partners <strong>in</strong> the academic community: the children, theirparents, teachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> the community at large.This is not aconclusion fromthis study


494 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eReferencesAltheide, D. (1987). Ethnographic content analysis. QualitativeSociology, 10(1), 65–76.Barone, D., Meyerson, M., & Mallette, M. (1995). Images ofteachers <strong>in</strong> children’s literature. The New Advocate, 8(4),257–270.Crume, M. (1989). Images of teachers <strong>in</strong> films <strong>and</strong> literature.<strong>Education</strong> Week, Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 4, 3.Daspit, T. & Weaver, J. (1999). Popular culture <strong>and</strong> criticalpedagogy: Read<strong>in</strong>g, construct<strong>in</strong>g, connect<strong>in</strong>g. New York:Garl<strong>and</strong>.Dougherty, W. & Engle, R. (1987). An 80s look for sexequality <strong>in</strong> Caldecott w<strong>in</strong>ners <strong>and</strong> honor books. TheRead<strong>in</strong>g Teacher, 40, 394–398.Farber, P., Provenzo, E. & Holm, G. (1994). School<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> thelight of popular culture. Albany, NY: State University ofNew York Press.Giroux, H. (1988). Teachers as <strong>in</strong>tellectuals: Toward acritical pedagogy of learn<strong>in</strong>g. Granby, MA: Berg<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>Garvey.Giroux, H. (1989). School<strong>in</strong>g as a form of cultural politics:Toward a pedagogy of <strong>and</strong> for difference. In Henry A.Giroux & Peter L. McLaren (Eds.), Critical pedagogy, thestate <strong>and</strong> cultural struggle (pp. 125–151). Albany, NY:SUNY Press.Giroux, H. (1994). Disturb<strong>in</strong>g pleasures. New York: Routledge.Giroux, H., & Simon, R. (1989). Popular culture, school<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> everyday life. New York: Berg<strong>in</strong> & Garvey.Huck, C., Hepler, S., Hickman, J., & Kiefer, B. (1997). Children’sliterature <strong>in</strong> the elementary school (6th ed.). Bos<strong>to</strong>n:McGraw.Hurley, S., & Chadwick, C. (1998). The images of females,m<strong>in</strong>orities, <strong>and</strong> the aged <strong>in</strong> Caldecott Award-w<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gpicture books, 1958–1997. Journal of Children’sLiterature, 24(1), 58–66.Hurst, J. B. (1981). Images <strong>in</strong> children’s picture books. Social<strong>Education</strong>, 45, 138–143.Joseph, P. B., & Burnaford, G. E. (1994). Images of schoolteachers <strong>in</strong> Twentieth-Century America. New York:St. Mart<strong>in</strong>’s Press.Knowles, J. G., & Cole, A. L. (with Presswood, C. S.). (1994).Through preservice teachers’ eyes: Explor<strong>in</strong>g fieldexperiences through narrative <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>quiry. New York:Merrill.Lamme, L. L. (1996). Digg<strong>in</strong>g deeply: Morals <strong>and</strong> ethics <strong>in</strong>children’s literature. Journal for a Just <strong>and</strong> Car<strong>in</strong>g<strong>Education</strong>, 2(4), 411–419.Leitch, V. B. (1988). American literary criticism from the 30s<strong>to</strong> the 80s. New York: Columbia UP.Lepman, J. (Ed). (1971). <strong>How</strong> children see our world. NewYork: Avon.Lortie, D. C. (1975). Schoolteacher: A sociological study.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.McLaren, P. (1994). Life <strong>in</strong> schools: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> criticalpedagogy <strong>in</strong> the foundations of education. White Pla<strong>in</strong>s,NY: Longman.Salomon, G.(1997). Of m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>and</strong> media: <strong>How</strong> culture’ssymbolic forms affect learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g. Phi DeltaKappan, 78(5), 375–380.Sollman, C., Emmons, B., & Paol<strong>in</strong>i, J. (1994). Through thecracks. Worcester, MA: Davis Publications.Trifonas, P. (2000). Revolutionary pedagogies: Culturalpolitics, <strong>in</strong>stitut<strong>in</strong>g education, <strong>and</strong> the discourse of theory.New York: Routledge.Weber, S. & Mitchell, C. (1995). ‘That’s funny, you don’t looklike a teacher’: Interrogat<strong>in</strong>g images <strong>and</strong> identity <strong>in</strong>popular culture. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: The Falmer Press.Appendix A: Children’s Book ReferencesAllard, H. (1977). Miss Nelson is miss<strong>in</strong>g. Illustrated by JamesMarshall. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Hough<strong>to</strong>n Miffl<strong>in</strong>.Allard, H. (1982). Miss Nelson is back. Illustrated by JamesMarshall. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Hough<strong>to</strong>n Miffl<strong>in</strong>.Allard, H. (1985). Miss Nelson has a field day. Illustrated byJames Marshall. New York: Scholastic.Burn<strong>in</strong>gham, J. (1987). John Patrick Norman McHennessy—The boy who was always late. New York: Crown.Chardiet, B., & Maccarone, G. (1990). The best teacher <strong>in</strong>the world. Illustrated by G. Brian Karas. New York:Scholastic.Clements, A. (1997). Double-trouble <strong>in</strong> Walla-Walla.Illustrated by Sal Murdocca. Brookfield, CT: Millbrook.Cohen, M. (1967). Will I have a friend? Illustrated by LillianHoban. New York: Aladd<strong>in</strong>.Cohen, M. (1977). When will I read? Illustrated by LillianHoban. New York: Bantam.Couric, K. (2000). The br<strong>and</strong> new kid. Illustrated by MarjoriePriceman. New York: Doubleday.de Paola, T. (1979). Oliver But<strong>to</strong>n is a sissy. San Diego, CA: HBJ.de Paola, T. (1989). The art lesson. New York: Putnam.F<strong>in</strong>chler, J. (1995). Miss Malarkey doesn’t live <strong>in</strong> Room 10.Illustrated by Kev<strong>in</strong> O’Malley. New York: Scholastic.F<strong>in</strong>chler, J. (1998). Miss Malarkey won’t be <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>day.Illustrated by Kev<strong>in</strong> O’Malley. New York: Walker.Frasier, D. (2000). Miss Ala<strong>in</strong>eus: A vocabulary disaster. SanDiego, CA: Harcourt.


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 495Giff, P. R. (1980). Today was a terrible day. New York: Puff<strong>in</strong>.Hall<strong>in</strong>an, P. K. (1989). My teacher’s my friend. Nashville,TN: Ideals.Henkes, K. (1991). Chrysanthemum. New York: Greenwillow.Henkes, K. (1996). Lilly’s purple plastic purse. New York:Greenwillow.Hoffman, M. (1991). Amaz<strong>in</strong>g Grace. Illustrated by Carol<strong>in</strong>eB<strong>in</strong>ch. New York: Scholastic.Hous<strong>to</strong>n, G. (1992). My great-aunt Arizona. Illustrated bySusan Condie Lamb. New York: HarperColl<strong>in</strong>s.Mart<strong>in</strong>, A. M. (1992). Rachel Parker, k<strong>in</strong>dergarten show-off.Illustrated by Nancy Poydar. New York: Holiday House.McGovern, A.(1993). Drop everyth<strong>in</strong>g, it’s D.E.A.R. time!Illustrated by Anna DiVi<strong>to</strong>. New York: Scholastic.Morrison, T., & Morrison, S. (1999). The big box. Illustratedby Giselle Potter. New York: Hyperion.Munsch, R. (1985). Thomas’ snowsuit. Illustrated by MichaelMartchenko. Toron<strong>to</strong>, Canada: Annick.Munsch, R. (1991). Show <strong>and</strong> tell. Illustrated by MichaelMartchenko. Toron<strong>to</strong>, Canada: Annick.Polacco, P. (1998). Thank you, Mr. Falker. New York:Philomel.Rathmann, P. (1991). Ruby the copycat. New York: Scholastic.Schwartz, A. (1988). Annabelle Swift, k<strong>in</strong>dergartner. NewYork: Orchard.Seuss, Dr. (1978). Gerald McBo<strong>in</strong>g Bo<strong>in</strong>g. New York: R<strong>and</strong>omHouse.Shannon, D. (1999). David goes <strong>to</strong> school. New York:Scholastic.Yashima, T. (1965). Crow Boy. New York: Scholastic.Copyright of <strong>Education</strong> is the property of Project Innovation, Inc.<strong>and</strong> its content may not be copied or emailed <strong>to</strong> multiple sites orposted <strong>to</strong> a listserv without the copyright holder’s express writtenpermission. <strong>How</strong>ever, users may pr<strong>in</strong>t, download, or email articlesfor <strong>in</strong>dividual use.Analysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONWe do not f<strong>in</strong>d a clear statement of purpose. The abstractsuggests that it is “<strong>to</strong> probe the power of stereotypes/cliches,” but we do not see that the study does this. Itappears <strong>to</strong> us that the purpose is “<strong>to</strong> provide further evidenceon the way <strong>in</strong> which teachers are portrayed <strong>in</strong>children’s picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooks.” An extensive justificationfor the study is given, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g personal experience, theoreticalideas of education writers, <strong>and</strong> previous studiesof children’s literature. Although we would prefer clearerdist<strong>in</strong>ctions among these, we th<strong>in</strong>k the study is adequatelyjustified <strong>in</strong> terms of importance <strong>to</strong> children’seducation <strong>and</strong> public perception of teachers. We wouldlike <strong>to</strong> see more on the contribution of this particularstudy. A justification for the methodology is given.DEFINITIONSClear def<strong>in</strong>itions are provided for the major categories ofthe content analysis <strong>and</strong> for the details of teacher representationthat were focused on by the reviewers. The term“image” should have been def<strong>in</strong>ed because it is prom<strong>in</strong>entthroughout <strong>and</strong> has several possible mean<strong>in</strong>gs. Apparently,it refers not <strong>to</strong> visual images but rather <strong>to</strong> “portrayals”or “representations” <strong>in</strong> both pictures <strong>and</strong> words.PRIOR RESEARCHNumerous references are given, often with the implicationthat they are research studies but sometimes<strong>in</strong>sufficient detail is provided <strong>to</strong> enable the reader <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e whether the “conclusions” cited are based ona study or on op<strong>in</strong>ion (examples <strong>in</strong>clude the referenceson children’s literature <strong>and</strong> on “popular culture”). Onestudy (Bonnie et al.) is discussed <strong>in</strong> some detail, butmethodology <strong>and</strong> grade level are unclear.HYPOTHESESNo hypotheses are stated. The implied hypothesis appears<strong>to</strong> be that “teacher images <strong>in</strong> s<strong>to</strong>rybooks are generallyunrealistic <strong>and</strong> negative.”SAMPLEThe sample was obta<strong>in</strong>ed by locat<strong>in</strong>g all picture s<strong>to</strong>rybooksaddress<strong>in</strong>g “teachers <strong>and</strong> schools” between 1965


496 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>and</strong> (presumably) 2005 as identified from a database. Thesample consisted of 96 teacher images from 62 books.The authors state that this provided the majority of children’ss<strong>to</strong>rybooks available <strong>in</strong> the United States for purchaseor available <strong>in</strong> libraries—presumably the targetpopulation. We are unclear as <strong>to</strong> the basis for this statement.The <strong>in</strong>tended age/grade range for these books is notgiven, but examples suggest it is “primary” grades.INSTRUMENTATIONThe method of deriv<strong>in</strong>g categories is well described.Reliability was asessed through <strong>in</strong>ter-rater agreement;although it is unclear exactly who the “we” refers <strong>to</strong>(there were presumably three categorizers). The level ofagreement is generally good—100% <strong>and</strong> 93% for themajor categories. As is typical of such studies, validityis not discussed. The def<strong>in</strong>itions of major categoriesseem straightforward, <strong>and</strong> this is supported by rateragreement. Good examples are given that also supportvalidity. The very small number (two) of “mixed” imagesis not consistent with our experience with realteachers but supports the author’s “hypothesis.”INTERNAL VALIDITYBecause this study does not explicitly focus on relationships,<strong>in</strong>ternal validity is not a major issue. <strong>How</strong>ever,the def<strong>in</strong>itions of major categories (positive, negative,mixed, <strong>and</strong> neutral) imply high correlations among thevariables (as portrayed) <strong>in</strong> each category. The smallnumber of “mixed” images provides evidence that thisis the case. More serious is the authors’ failure <strong>to</strong> addressthe effect of possible changes over time—from1965 <strong>to</strong> 2005. The question of whether their results areaccurate for recent s<strong>to</strong>rybooks could have been studied,for example, by divid<strong>in</strong>g images <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> three time periods.RESULTS/INTERPRETATIONResults are presented as percentages <strong>in</strong> each of the fourcategories. Extensive examples are given that greatlyhelp clarify the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. In general, we f<strong>in</strong>d the <strong>in</strong>terpretation<strong>to</strong> be consistent with the results. There are, however,important exceptions. Most serious is the statementthat there were more negative than positive images. Thisis not consistent with the data on pages 489–490; bothcategories conta<strong>in</strong>ed 42%—unless there is a typographicalerror. We also question the assertion that 84% of theteachers represented were either very good or horrid.Only two are cited as “paragons,” <strong>and</strong> among the negativeteachers, a number are described as “less drastic”but “still of concern.” We also th<strong>in</strong>k the authors havesometimes overstated their case. For example, the statementthat “we do not f<strong>in</strong>d images of teachers as transformative<strong>in</strong>tellectuals ....” seems <strong>in</strong>consistent with thef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g that six books did conta<strong>in</strong> such images. We alsonote that tha author’s “conclusion” is not the cus<strong>to</strong>maryconclusion based on the study but rather an extension<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> implications from a much broader context.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsWHAT IS CONTENT ANALYSIS?Content analysis is an analysis of the contents of a communication.Content analysis is a technique that enables researchers <strong>to</strong> study human behavior <strong>in</strong>an <strong>in</strong>direct way by analyz<strong>in</strong>g communications.


CHAPTER 20 Content Analysis 497APPLICATIONS OF CONTENT ANALYSISContent analysis has wide applicability <strong>in</strong> educational research.Content analysis can give researchers <strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> problems that they can test bymore direct methods.There are several reasons <strong>to</strong> do a content analysis: <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> descriptive <strong>in</strong>formationof one k<strong>in</strong>d or another; <strong>to</strong> analyze observational <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview data; <strong>to</strong> test hypotheses;<strong>to</strong> check other research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs; <strong>and</strong>/or <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation useful <strong>in</strong> deal<strong>in</strong>gwith educational problems.CATEGORIZATION IN CONTENT ANALYSISPredeterm<strong>in</strong>ed categories are sometimes used <strong>to</strong> code data.Sometimes cod<strong>in</strong>g is done by us<strong>in</strong>g categories that emerge as data is reviewed.STEPS INVOLVED IN CONTENT ANALYSISIn do<strong>in</strong>g a content analysis, researchers should always develop a rationale (a conceptuall<strong>in</strong>k) <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> how the data <strong>to</strong> be collected are related <strong>to</strong> their objectives.Important terms should at some po<strong>in</strong>t be def<strong>in</strong>ed.All of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods used <strong>in</strong> other k<strong>in</strong>ds of educational research can beapplied <strong>to</strong> content analysis. Purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g, however, is the most commonly used.The unit of analysis—what specifically is <strong>to</strong> be analyzed—should be specifiedbefore the researcher beg<strong>in</strong>s an analysis.After precisely def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g what aspects of the content are <strong>to</strong> be analyzed, the researcherneeds <strong>to</strong> formulate cod<strong>in</strong>g categories.CODING CATEGORIESDevelop<strong>in</strong>g emergent cod<strong>in</strong>g categories requires a high level of familiarity with thecontent of a communication.In do<strong>in</strong>g a content analysis, a researcher can code either the manifest or the latentcontent of a communication, <strong>and</strong> sometimes both.The manifest content of a communication refers <strong>to</strong> the specific, clear, surface contents:the words, pictures, images, <strong>and</strong> such that are easily categorized.The latent content of a document refers <strong>to</strong> the mean<strong>in</strong>g underly<strong>in</strong>g what is conta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> a communication.RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY AS APPLIED TO CONTENT ANALYSISReliability <strong>in</strong> content analysis is commonly checked by compar<strong>in</strong>g the results of two<strong>in</strong>dependent scorers (categorizers).Validity can be checked by compar<strong>in</strong>g data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from manifest content <strong>to</strong> tha<strong>to</strong>bta<strong>in</strong>ed from latent content.DATA ANALYSISA common way <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret content analysis data is by us<strong>in</strong>g frequencies (i.e., thenumber of specific <strong>in</strong>cidents found <strong>in</strong> the data) <strong>and</strong> proportion of particular occurrences<strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>tal occurrences.Another method is <strong>to</strong> use cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> develop themes <strong>to</strong> facilitate synthesis.Computer analysis is extremely useful <strong>in</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g data once categories have beendeterm<strong>in</strong>ed. It can also be useful at times <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g such categories.


498 PART 5 Introduction <strong>to</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eADVANTAGES AND DISADVANTAGES OF CONTENT ANALYSISTwo major advantages of content analysis are that it is unobtrusive <strong>and</strong> it is comparativelyeasy <strong>to</strong> do.The major disadvantages of content analysis are that it is limited <strong>to</strong> the analysis ofcommunications <strong>and</strong> it is difficult <strong>to</strong> establish validity.Key Termscluster sampl<strong>in</strong>g 476cod<strong>in</strong>g 476content analysis 472latent content 477manifest content 477r<strong>and</strong>om sample 476reliability 479stratified sampl<strong>in</strong>g 476theme 474validity 479For DiscussionNotes1. When, if ever, might it be more appropriate <strong>to</strong> do a content analysis than <strong>to</strong> use someother k<strong>in</strong>d of methodology?2. When would it be <strong>in</strong>appropriate <strong>to</strong> use content analysis?3. Give an example of some categories a researcher might use <strong>to</strong> analyze data <strong>in</strong> eachof the follow<strong>in</strong>g content analyses:a. To <strong>in</strong>vestigate the amount <strong>and</strong> types of humor on televisionb. To <strong>in</strong>vestigate the k<strong>in</strong>ds of “romantic love” represented <strong>in</strong> popular songsc. To <strong>in</strong>vestigate the social implications of impressionistic pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>gsd. To <strong>in</strong>vestigate whether civil or crim<strong>in</strong>al law makes the most dist<strong>in</strong>ctions betweenmen <strong>and</strong> womene. To describe the assumptions made <strong>in</strong> elementary school science programs4. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k would be more difficult <strong>to</strong> code, the manifest or the latent conten<strong>to</strong>f a movie? Why?5. “Never code only the latent content of a document without also cod<strong>in</strong>g at least someof the manifest content.” Would you agree with this statement? Why or why not?6. In terms of difficulty, how would you compare a content analysis approach <strong>to</strong>the study of social bias on television with a survey approach? <strong>in</strong> terms of useful<strong>in</strong>formation?7. Would it be possible <strong>to</strong> do a content analysis of Hollywood movies? If so, whatmight be some categories you would use?8. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of some th<strong>in</strong>gs produced by humans that were not orig<strong>in</strong>ally <strong>in</strong>tendedas communications but now are considered <strong>to</strong> be? Suggest some examples.9. Content analysis is sometimes said <strong>to</strong> be extremely valuable <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>g observational<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview data. If true, how so?10. The choice of categories <strong>in</strong> a content analysis study is crucial. Would you agree? Ifso, expla<strong>in</strong> why.1. G. Gerbner et al. (1978). Cultural <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rs: Violence profile no. 9. Journal of Communication, 28: 177–207.2. Ibid., p. 181.3. K. Horney (1945). Our <strong>in</strong>ner conflicts. New York: Nor<strong>to</strong>n.4. John W. Creswell (2002). <strong>Education</strong>al research: Plann<strong>in</strong>g, conduct<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g qualitative research.Columbus, OH: Merrill Prentice-Hall, p. 268.5. J. R. Fraenkel <strong>and</strong> N. E. Wallen (1988). Toward improv<strong>in</strong>g research <strong>in</strong> social studies education. Boulder,CO: Social Science Consortium.


Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>MethodologiesP A R T6Part 6 cont<strong>in</strong>ues the discussion of qualitative research we began <strong>in</strong> Part 5. We concentratehere on ethnography <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. As we did <strong>in</strong> Parts 4 <strong>and</strong> 5, we no<strong>to</strong>nly discuss each of these methodologies <strong>in</strong> some detail, but we also provide someexamples of published studies <strong>in</strong> which the researchers used these methods. We thenprovide our analysis of the strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses of these studies.


21Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>What Is Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong>?The Unique Value ofEthnographic <strong>Research</strong>Ethnographic ConceptsTopics that Lend ThemselvesWell <strong>to</strong> Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong>Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Do Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong>ers UseHypotheses?Data Collection <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Field NotesData Analysis <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Advantages <strong>and</strong>Disadvantages ofEthnographic <strong>Research</strong>An Example ofEthnographic <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurposeJustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisResults/DiscussionOBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term“ethnographic research,” <strong>and</strong> give anexample of a research question that mightbe <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> an ethnographic study.Describe briefly what each of the follow<strong>in</strong>gconcepts mean <strong>to</strong> ethnographers:“culture,” “holistic outlook,”“contextualization,” <strong>and</strong> “multiplerealities.”Expla<strong>in</strong> the difference between an “emic”<strong>and</strong> an “etic” perspective.Name at least three <strong>to</strong>pics that would lendthemselves well <strong>to</strong> ethnographic research.Describe the characteristics of the k<strong>in</strong>ds ofsamples used <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research.“Nowcomes thehard part.”Expla<strong>in</strong> how ethnographers employhypotheses <strong>in</strong> their research.Describe the two major data collectiontechniques used <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the term “fieldnotes” <strong>and</strong> how they differ from fieldjott<strong>in</strong>gs, a field diary, <strong>and</strong> a field log.Expla<strong>in</strong> what is meant by the terms“triangulation” <strong>and</strong> “contextualization.”Expla<strong>in</strong> what a “key event” is <strong>in</strong>ethnographic research.Describe briefly how statistics are used <strong>in</strong>ethnographic research.Name at least one advantage <strong>and</strong> onedisadvantage of ethnographic research.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 21.1: Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 21.2: True or False?Activity 21.3: Do Some Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>What do you <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> do for your doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertation, Sam?”“I’m go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> do an ethnography of a school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal.”“No kidd<strong>in</strong>g! Impressive!”“I just talked <strong>to</strong> Elizabeth Rodriguez <strong>to</strong>day—you know, the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal at Roosevelt Elementary. She’s agreed <strong>to</strong> letme follow her around for the next four weeks so that I can see what she does dur<strong>in</strong>g the day—you know, whom shemeets with, where, when, what they talk about, etc.”“You’re go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> keep a record of this, I assume?”“You bet! I’ll carry a notebook <strong>and</strong> make notes about what I see <strong>and</strong> hear. I also have Elizabeth’s permission <strong>to</strong> carrya tape recorder, <strong>and</strong> I <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> tape her conversations. And I have another idea. Dur<strong>in</strong>g two of these days, I want <strong>to</strong>videotape her as she goes about her daily rout<strong>in</strong>e, both <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> as she ventures out <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the community.”“That’s it?”“Nope. I also <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> take a look at the school’s records <strong>and</strong> the daily log she keeps. I’m plann<strong>in</strong>g on do<strong>in</strong>g alot of <strong>in</strong>terviews, <strong>to</strong>o—talk with her secretary, some of the faculty, the cus<strong>to</strong>dial staff, even some students. And, ofcourse, Elizabeth herself. And if she will permit it, the members of her family. I don’t plan <strong>to</strong> do the <strong>in</strong>terviews untilafter I’ve f<strong>in</strong>ished with my observations, however.”“Boy, that sounds like a lot of work.”“No question, it is. Not <strong>to</strong> mention writ<strong>in</strong>g all this up. But it will be worth it. When I’m f<strong>in</strong>ished, I th<strong>in</strong>k I will beable <strong>to</strong> pa<strong>in</strong>t a pretty accurate picture of what the life of an elementary school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal is like.”Sam’s description of what he <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> do is an example of ethnographic research, the subject of this chapter.What Is Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong>?Ethnographic research is, <strong>in</strong> many respects, the mostcomplex of all research methods. A variety of approachesare used <strong>in</strong> an attempt <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> as holistic a picture aspossible of a particular society, group, <strong>in</strong>stitution, sett<strong>in</strong>g,or situation. The emphasis <strong>in</strong> ethnographic researchis on document<strong>in</strong>g or portray<strong>in</strong>g the everyday experiencesof <strong>in</strong>dividuals by observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g them<strong>and</strong> relevant others. The key <strong>to</strong>ols, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong> all ethnographicstudies are <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ual,ongo<strong>in</strong>g participant observation of a situation. <strong>Research</strong>erstry <strong>to</strong> capture as much of what is go<strong>in</strong>g on asthey can—the “whole picture,” so <strong>to</strong> speak. Bernarddescribed the process briefly, but well:It <strong>in</strong>volves establish<strong>in</strong>g rapport <strong>in</strong> a new community;learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> act so that people go about their bus<strong>in</strong>ess asusual when you show up; <strong>and</strong> remov<strong>in</strong>g yourself everyday from cultural immersion so you can <strong>in</strong>tellectualizewhat you’ve learned, put it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> perspective, <strong>and</strong> writeabout it conv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>gly. If you are a successful participan<strong>to</strong>bserver you will know when <strong>to</strong> laugh at what your<strong>in</strong>formants th<strong>in</strong>k is funny; <strong>and</strong> when <strong>in</strong>formants laugh atwhat you say, it will be because you meant it <strong>to</strong> be a joke. 1Wolcott has po<strong>in</strong>ted out that ethnographic proceduresrequire three th<strong>in</strong>gs: a detailed description of theculture-shar<strong>in</strong>g group be<strong>in</strong>g studied, an analysis of this501


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Anthropologist Margaret Mead’s ethnography of life <strong>in</strong>Samoa—<strong>in</strong> particular her study of the adolescence ofgirls—is a social science classic. In the 1920s, she spent n<strong>in</strong>emonths <strong>in</strong> Samoa as a participant observer, rely<strong>in</strong>g mostly onobservation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews with selected <strong>in</strong>formants. Hermajor conclusions were that adolescence <strong>in</strong> Samoa was notthe stressful period it is for adolescents <strong>in</strong> the United States.She believed that this was largely because Samoans were notfaced with the dilemmas that young people <strong>in</strong> the UnitedStates face <strong>and</strong> because the Samoan culture <strong>to</strong>ok a relaxedview <strong>to</strong>ward all forms of behavior. She also concluded that the<strong>in</strong>cidence of emotional disturbance was much lower <strong>in</strong>Samoa, ow<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the diffusion of emotional attachments <strong>and</strong>the clear-cut rules regard<strong>in</strong>g the form<strong>in</strong>g of relationships.In the preface <strong>to</strong> the sixth edition of this report (1973),*Mead po<strong>in</strong>ted out that while neither U.S. nor Samoan culturehas rema<strong>in</strong>ed constant, recent visits had impressed her withthe extraord<strong>in</strong>ary persistence of the Samoan culture.A subsequent ethnography done 20 years later resulted <strong>in</strong>very different conclusions that anthropologists do not attribute<strong>to</strong> the passage of time.† This discrepancy illustrates the rich<strong>and</strong> provocative nature of ethnographic research, as well asthe difficulty <strong>in</strong> arriv<strong>in</strong>g at firm conclusions.*M. Mead (1973). Com<strong>in</strong>g of age <strong>in</strong> Samoa, 6th ed. New York:Morrow Hill.†D. Freeman (1983). Margaret Mead <strong>and</strong> Samoa—The mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>unmak<strong>in</strong>g of an anthropological myth. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.group <strong>in</strong> terms of perceived themes or perspectives, <strong>and</strong>then some <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the group by the researcheras <strong>to</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> generalizations about the social lifeof human be<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> general. 2 The f<strong>in</strong>al product is aholistic cultural portrait of the group—a pull<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>getherby the researcher of everyth<strong>in</strong>g he or she haslearned about the group <strong>in</strong> all its complexity.Here are some titles of studies that ethnographershave conducted <strong>in</strong> education:“Small-Town Teacher” 3“Cultural Organization of Participation Structures <strong>in</strong>Two Classrooms of Indian Students” 4“The Ethnography of Children’s Play” 5“Hempies <strong>and</strong> Squeaks, Truckers <strong>and</strong> Cruisers: AParticipant Observer Study <strong>in</strong> a City High School” 6“The Other Side of the Fence: See<strong>in</strong>g Black <strong>and</strong>White <strong>in</strong> a Small Southern Town” 7“Inside High School: The Student’s Perspective” 8“Boys <strong>in</strong> White: Student Culture <strong>in</strong> Medical School” 9THE UNIQUE VALUEOF ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHEthnographic research has a particular strength thatmakes it especially appeal<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> many researchers. Itcan reveal nuances <strong>and</strong> subtleties that other methodologiesmiss. An excellent example is offered by Babbie.If you were walk<strong>in</strong>g through a public park <strong>and</strong> you threwdown a bunch of trash, you’d discover that your actionwas unacceptable <strong>to</strong> those around you. People wouldglare at you, grumble <strong>to</strong> each other, <strong>and</strong> perhaps someonewould say someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> you about it. Whatever theform, you’d be subjected <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ite, negative sanctionsfor litter<strong>in</strong>g. Now here’s the irony. If you were walk<strong>in</strong>gthrough that same park, came across a bunch of trashthat someone else had dropped, <strong>and</strong> cleaned it up, it’slikely that your action would also be unacceptable <strong>to</strong>those around you. You’d probably be subject <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>ite,negative sanctions for clean<strong>in</strong>g it up.Most [of my students] felt (that this notion) wasabsurd . . . Although we would be negatively sanctionedfor litter<strong>in</strong>g, . . . people would be pleased with us for[clean<strong>in</strong>g up a public place]. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly, all my studentssaid they would be pleased if someone cleaned up a publicplace.To settle the issue, I suggested that my students startfix<strong>in</strong>g the public problems they came across <strong>in</strong> the courseof their everyday activities. . . .My students picked up litter, fixed street signs, putknocked-over traffic cones back <strong>in</strong> place, cleaned <strong>and</strong>decorated communal lounges <strong>in</strong> their dorms, trimmedtrees that blocked visibility at <strong>in</strong>tersections, repairedpublic playground equipment, cleaned public restrooms,<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ok care of a hundred other public problems thatweren’t “their responsibility.”Most reported feel<strong>in</strong>g very uncomfortable do<strong>in</strong>gwhatever they did. They felt foolish, goody-goody,conspicuous. . . . In almost every case, their personalfeel<strong>in</strong>gs of discomfort were <strong>in</strong>creased by the reactionsof those around them. One student was remov<strong>in</strong>g adamaged <strong>and</strong> long-unused newspaper box from the bus502


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 503s<strong>to</strong>p where it had been a problem for months when thepolice arrived, hav<strong>in</strong>g been summoned by a neighbor.Another student decided <strong>to</strong> clean out a clogged s<strong>to</strong>rmdra<strong>in</strong> on his street <strong>and</strong> found himself be<strong>in</strong>g yelled at bya neighbor who <strong>in</strong>sisted the mess should be left for thestreet cleaners. Everyone who picked up litter wassneered at, laughed at, <strong>and</strong> generally put down. Oneyoung man was pick<strong>in</strong>g up litter scattered around atrashcan when a passerby sneered, “Clumsy!” 10The po<strong>in</strong>t of the above example, we hope, is obvious.What people th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>and</strong> say happens (or is likely <strong>to</strong> happen)often is not really the case. By go<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> theworld <strong>and</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g th<strong>in</strong>gs as they occur, we are (usually)better able <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> a more accurate picture. Thisis what ethnographers try <strong>to</strong> do—study people <strong>in</strong> theirnatural habitat <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> “see” th<strong>in</strong>gs that otherwisemight not even be anticipated. This is a major advantageof the ethnographic approach.Ethnographic ConceptsThere are a number of concepts that guide the work ofethnographers as they go about their research <strong>in</strong> thefield. Some of the most important <strong>in</strong>clude culture, aholistic outlook, contextualization, an emic perspective,multiple realities, thick description, member check<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> a nonjudgmental orientation. Let us give a briefdescription of each.Culture. The concept of culture is typically def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>one of two ways. Those who focus on behavior def<strong>in</strong>e it asthe sum of a social group’s observable patterns of behavior,cus<strong>to</strong>ms, <strong>and</strong> ways of life. 11 Those who concentrate onideas say that it comprises the ideas, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> knowledgethat characterize a particular group of people. 12<strong>How</strong>ever one def<strong>in</strong>es it, culture is the most important ofall ethnographic concepts. In Fetterman’s words, ithelps the ethnographer <strong>to</strong> search for a logical, cohesivepattern <strong>in</strong> the myriad, often ritualistic behaviors <strong>and</strong> ideasthat characterize a group. This concept becomes immediatelymean<strong>in</strong>gful after cross-cultural experience. Everyth<strong>in</strong>gis new <strong>to</strong> a student first enter<strong>in</strong>g a different culture.Attitudes or habits that natives espouse virtually withoutth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g are dist<strong>in</strong>ct <strong>and</strong> clear <strong>to</strong> the stranger. Liv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> aforeign community for a long period of time enables thefieldworker <strong>to</strong> see the power of dom<strong>in</strong>ant ideas, values,<strong>and</strong> patterns of behavior <strong>in</strong> the way people walk, talk,dress, eat, <strong>and</strong> sleep. The longer an <strong>in</strong>dividual stays <strong>in</strong> acommunity, build<strong>in</strong>g rapport, <strong>and</strong> the deeper they probe<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual lives, the greater the probability of hisor her learn<strong>in</strong>g about the sacred subtle elements of theculture: how people pray, how they feel about each other,<strong>and</strong> how they re<strong>in</strong>force their own cultural practices <strong>to</strong>ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tegrity of their system. 13The <strong>in</strong>terpretation of a group’s culture is consideredby many researchers <strong>to</strong> be the primary contribution ofethnographic research. Cultural <strong>in</strong>terpretation refers <strong>to</strong>the researcher’s ability <strong>to</strong> describe what he or she sees<strong>and</strong> hears from the po<strong>in</strong>t of view of the members of thegroup. A frequently cited example is that of the differencebetween a “w<strong>in</strong>k” <strong>and</strong> a “bl<strong>in</strong>k.” In one sense,there is no difference between the two. <strong>How</strong>ever, “anyonewho has ever mistaken a bl<strong>in</strong>k for a w<strong>in</strong>k is fullyaware of the significance of cultural <strong>in</strong>terpretation.” 14A Holistic Perspective. Ethnographers try <strong>to</strong> describeas much as they can about the culture of a group.Thus, they try <strong>to</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> some idea of the group’s his<strong>to</strong>ry,social structure, politics, religious beliefs, symbols,cus<strong>to</strong>ms, rituals, <strong>and</strong> environment. No s<strong>in</strong>gle study, ofcourse, can ever capture completely an entire culture,but ethnographic researchers do their best <strong>to</strong> see beyondthe immediate scene or event occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a classroom,<strong>in</strong> a neighborhood, on a particular street, or <strong>in</strong> a location<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the larger picture of which theparticular event may be a part. As you can imag<strong>in</strong>e,develop<strong>in</strong>g a holistic perspective dem<strong>and</strong>s that the ethnographerspend a great amount of time out <strong>in</strong> the fieldgather<strong>in</strong>g many different k<strong>in</strong>ds of data. Only by do<strong>in</strong>gso is he or she able <strong>to</strong> develop a picture of the social orcultural whole of that which he or she is study<strong>in</strong>g.Contextualization. When a researcher contextualizesdata, he or she places what was seen <strong>and</strong> heard<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a larger perspective. For example, the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rsof a large urban school district <strong>in</strong> which one ofthe authors of this text taught were about <strong>to</strong> term<strong>in</strong>atean after-school tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g project because of its lowattendance—about 50 percent. It was suggested <strong>to</strong> themthat an attendance rate of 50 percent was actually prettygood when one considered the students <strong>in</strong>volved (thestudents encouraged <strong>to</strong> attend the tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g sessions werethose who were do<strong>in</strong>g fail<strong>in</strong>g work <strong>in</strong> most, if not all, oftheir classes). This suggestion resulted <strong>in</strong> the districtcont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>g the program, as the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs were nowable <strong>to</strong> make a more <strong>in</strong>formed decision about the worthof the program. In other words, contextualization helped


504 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ema<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a worthwhile program that otherwise mighthave been elim<strong>in</strong>ated.An Emic Perspective. An emic perspective—that is, an “<strong>in</strong>sider’s” perspective of reality—is at theheart of ethnographic research. Ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g an emic perspectiveis essential <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g—<strong>and</strong> thus describ<strong>in</strong>gaccurately—the behaviors <strong>and</strong> situations an ethnographersees <strong>and</strong> hears. An emic perspective requires one <strong>to</strong>recognize <strong>and</strong> accept the idea of multiple realities.“Document<strong>in</strong>g multiple perspectives of reality <strong>in</strong> a givenstudy is crucial <strong>to</strong> an underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of why people th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>and</strong> act <strong>in</strong> the different ways they do.” 15An etic perspective, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is the externalobjective perspective on reality. Most ethnographicresearchers try <strong>to</strong> look at their data from both these perspectives.They may start collect<strong>in</strong>g data from an emicperspective, do<strong>in</strong>g their best <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the po<strong>in</strong>t ofview of those they are study<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> then try <strong>to</strong> makesense of what they have collected <strong>in</strong> terms of a moreobjective, scientific analysis. In short, they try <strong>to</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ean <strong>in</strong>sightful <strong>and</strong> sensitive cultural <strong>in</strong>terpretationwith a rigorous collection <strong>and</strong> analysis of what theyhave seen <strong>and</strong> heard.Thick Description. When ethnographers preparethe f<strong>in</strong>al report of their research, they engage <strong>in</strong> what isknown as thick description. In essence, this <strong>in</strong>volvesdescrib<strong>in</strong>g what they have seen <strong>and</strong> heard—their work<strong>in</strong> the field—<strong>in</strong> great detail, frequently us<strong>in</strong>g extensivequotations from the participants <strong>in</strong> their study. The<strong>in</strong>tent is, as mentioned earlier, <strong>to</strong> “pa<strong>in</strong>t a portrait” ofthe culture they have studied, <strong>to</strong> make it “come alive”for those who read the report.Member Check<strong>in</strong>g. As mentioned above, a majorobjective of ethnographic research is <strong>to</strong> represent as accuratelyas possible an emic perspective of reality—that is,reality as seen from the po<strong>in</strong>t of view of the participants<strong>in</strong> the study. One way that ethnographic researchers dothis is through what is known as member check<strong>in</strong>g—byhav<strong>in</strong>g the participants review what the researchers havewritten as a check for accuracy <strong>and</strong> completeness. It is oneof the primary strategies used <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research <strong>to</strong>validate the accuracy of the researcher’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.A Nonjudgmental Orientation. A nonjudgmentalorientation requires researchers <strong>to</strong> do their best<strong>to</strong> refra<strong>in</strong> from mak<strong>in</strong>g value judgments about unfamiliarpractices. None of us, of course, can be completelyneutral. But we can guard aga<strong>in</strong>st our most obviousbiases. <strong>How</strong>? By do<strong>in</strong>g our best <strong>to</strong> view another group’sbehaviors as impartially as we can. Fetterman gives anexample of how one of his biases might have been fatal:An experience I had with the Bedou<strong>in</strong> Arabs <strong>in</strong> the S<strong>in</strong>aidesert provides a useful example. . . . Dur<strong>in</strong>g my staywith the Bedou<strong>in</strong>s, I tried not <strong>to</strong> let my bias for Westernhygiene practices [show]. [M]y reaction <strong>to</strong> one of my firstacqua<strong>in</strong>tances, a Bedou<strong>in</strong> with a leathery face <strong>and</strong> feet,was far from neutral. . . . I admired his ability <strong>to</strong> survive<strong>and</strong> adapt <strong>in</strong> a harsh environment, mov<strong>in</strong>g from onewater hole <strong>to</strong> the next throughout the desert. <strong>How</strong>ever, mypersonal reaction <strong>to</strong> the odor of his garments (particularlyafter a camel ride) was far from impartial. He shared hisjacket with me <strong>to</strong> protect me from the heat. I thanked himof course, because . . . I did not want <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>sult him. ButI smelled like a camel for the rest of the day <strong>in</strong> the drydesert heat. I thought I didn’t need the jacket because wewere only a kilometer or two from our dest<strong>in</strong>ation. . . . Ilearned later that without his jacket I would have sufferedfrom sunstroke. The desert heat is so dry that perspirationevaporates almost immediately <strong>and</strong> an <strong>in</strong>experienced travelerdoes not always notice when the temperature climbsabove 130ºF. By slow<strong>in</strong>g down the evaporation rate, thejacket helped me reta<strong>in</strong> water. Had I rejected the jacket<strong>and</strong>, by implication, Bedou<strong>in</strong> hygiene practices, I wouldhave baked, <strong>and</strong> I would never have unders<strong>to</strong>od how muchtheir lives revolve around water. 16The most serious mistake an ethnographer can makeis <strong>to</strong> impose his or her own culture’s st<strong>and</strong>ards of behavior<strong>and</strong> values on<strong>to</strong> those of another culture.TOPICS THAT LEND THEMSELVES WELLTO ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHAs we have suggested, researchers who undertake anethnographic study want <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> as holistic a picture ofan educational sett<strong>in</strong>g as possible. Indeed, one of the keystrengths of ethnographic research is the comprehensivenessof perspective it provides. Because the researchergoes directly <strong>to</strong> the situation or sett<strong>in</strong>g that he or shewishes <strong>to</strong> study, deeper <strong>and</strong> more complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gbecomes possible. As a result, ethnographic researchis particularly suitable for <strong>to</strong>pics such as the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Those that by their very nature defy simple quantification(for example, the <strong>in</strong>teraction of students <strong>and</strong>teachers <strong>in</strong> classroom discussions).Those that can best be unders<strong>to</strong>od <strong>in</strong> a natural (asopposed <strong>to</strong> an artificial) sett<strong>in</strong>g (for example, thebehavior of students at a school event).


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 505Those that <strong>in</strong>volve the study of <strong>in</strong>dividual or groupactivities over time (such as the changes that occur<strong>in</strong> the attitudes of at-risk students as they participate<strong>in</strong> a specially designed, year-long, read<strong>in</strong>g program).Those <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the study of the roles that educa<strong>to</strong>rsplay, <strong>and</strong> the behaviors associated with those roles(for example, the behavior of classroom teachers,students, counselors, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, coaches, staff,<strong>and</strong> other school personnel as they fulfill their variousroles <strong>and</strong> how such behavior changes over time).Those that <strong>in</strong>volve the study of the activities <strong>and</strong>behavior of groups as a unit (such as classes, athleticteams, subject matter departments, adm<strong>in</strong>istrativeunits, work teams, etc.).Those <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the study of formal organizations <strong>in</strong>their <strong>to</strong>tality (for example, schools, school districts,<strong>and</strong> so forth).Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Ethnographic<strong>Research</strong>S<strong>in</strong>ce ethnographers attempt <strong>to</strong> observe everyth<strong>in</strong>gwith<strong>in</strong> the sett<strong>in</strong>g or situation they are observ<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> asense they do not sample at all. But as we have mentionedbefore, no researcher can observe everyth<strong>in</strong>g. Tothe extent that what is observed is only a portion of whatmight be observed, what a researcher observes is, therefore,a de fac<strong>to</strong> sample of all the possible observationsthat might be made.Also, the samples of persons studied by ethnographersare typically small (often only a few <strong>in</strong>dividuals,or a s<strong>in</strong>gle class) <strong>and</strong> do not permit generalization <strong>to</strong>a larger population. Many ethnographers, <strong>in</strong> fact, stateright at the outset of a study that they have no <strong>in</strong>tentionof generaliz<strong>in</strong>g the results of their study. What theyare after, they po<strong>in</strong>t out, is a more complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof a particular situation. The applicability of theirf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs can best be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by replication of theirwork <strong>in</strong> other sett<strong>in</strong>gs or situations by other researchers.Do Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>ersUse Hypotheses?Ethnographic researchers seldom <strong>in</strong>itiate their researchwith precise hypotheses. Rather, they attempt <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>an ongo<strong>in</strong>g situation or set of activities that cannotbe predicted <strong>in</strong> advance. They observe for a period oftime, formulate some <strong>in</strong>itial hypotheses that suggest <strong>to</strong>them additional k<strong>in</strong>ds of observations that may leadthem <strong>to</strong> revise their <strong>in</strong>itial conclusions, <strong>and</strong> so on.Ethnographic research, perhaps more so than any otherk<strong>in</strong>d of research, relies on both observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>gthat is cont<strong>in</strong>ual <strong>and</strong> susta<strong>in</strong>ed over time.An example of a question that might be <strong>in</strong>vestigatedthrough ethnographic research would be, “What is lifelike <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>ner-city high school?” The researcher’s goalwould be <strong>to</strong> document or portray the daily, ongo<strong>in</strong>gexperiences of the teachers, students, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs,<strong>and</strong> staff <strong>in</strong> such a school. The school would be regularlyvisited over a considerable length of time (a year wouldnot be uncommon). The researcher would observe classroomson a regular basis <strong>and</strong> attempt <strong>to</strong> describe, as fully<strong>and</strong> as richly as possible, what exists <strong>and</strong> what happens<strong>in</strong> those classrooms. He or she would also <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong>depth several teachers, students, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong>support staff.Descriptions (a better word might be portrayals)might depict the social atmosphere of the school; the<strong>in</strong>tellectual <strong>and</strong> emotional experiences of students; themanner <strong>in</strong> which adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> teachers (<strong>and</strong> staff<strong>and</strong> students) act <strong>to</strong>ward <strong>and</strong> react <strong>to</strong> others of differentethnic groups, sexes, or abilities; how the “rules” of theschool (<strong>and</strong> the classroom) are learned, modified, <strong>and</strong>enforced; the k<strong>in</strong>ds of concerns teachers (<strong>and</strong> students)have; the views students have of the school, <strong>and</strong> howthese compare with the views of the adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>and</strong>the faculty; <strong>and</strong> so forth.The data <strong>to</strong> be collected might <strong>in</strong>clude detailed h<strong>and</strong>writtenprose descriptions by the researcher-observer;audiotapes of pupil-student, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r-student, <strong>and</strong>adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r-faculty conferences; videotapes of classroomdiscussions <strong>and</strong> faculty meet<strong>in</strong>gs; examples ofteacher lesson plans <strong>and</strong> student work; sociogramsdepict<strong>in</strong>g “power” relationships that exist <strong>in</strong> a classroom;flowcharts illustrat<strong>in</strong>g the direction <strong>and</strong> frequencyof certa<strong>in</strong> types of comments (for example, the k<strong>in</strong>ds ofquestions asked by teachers <strong>and</strong> students of one another,<strong>and</strong> the responses that different k<strong>in</strong>ds produce); <strong>and</strong>anyth<strong>in</strong>g else the researcher th<strong>in</strong>ks would provide<strong>in</strong>sights <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> what goes on <strong>in</strong> this school. Notice that <strong>in</strong>this <strong>in</strong>stance, hypotheses would not be formulated at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the study.In short, then, the goal of researchers engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ethnographic research is <strong>to</strong> “pa<strong>in</strong>t a portrait” of a schoolor a classroom (or any other educational sett<strong>in</strong>g) as thoroughly,accurately, <strong>and</strong> vividly as possible so that otherscan also truly “see” that school or that classroom <strong>and</strong> itsparticipants <strong>and</strong> what they do. In fact, it can be viewedas an attempt <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e how a group gives mean<strong>in</strong>g


506 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>to</strong> its activities. Many believe that the ethnographic approachoffers a richness of description that is especiallyfruitful for underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g education.Data Collection <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>The two major means of data collection <strong>in</strong> ethnographicresearch are through participant observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g.Interview<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> fact, is the most important <strong>to</strong>olthat ethnographers use. Through <strong>in</strong>terviews, the researcheris able <strong>to</strong> put <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a larger context that which heor she has seen, heard, or experienced. As we described <strong>in</strong>Chapter 19, <strong>in</strong>terviews come <strong>in</strong> many forms: structured,semistructured, <strong>in</strong>formal, <strong>and</strong> retrospective. We won’texp<strong>and</strong> on the discussion here, except <strong>to</strong> say that <strong>in</strong>formal<strong>in</strong>terviews are the most common. To the <strong>in</strong>experienced,<strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews may seem <strong>to</strong> be the easiest <strong>to</strong> do, asthey require neither any particular type of question norany particular sequence <strong>in</strong> which questions must be asked.The <strong>in</strong>terviewer can pretty much follow the participant’s<strong>in</strong>terests. Often they seem <strong>to</strong> be no more than a casual conversation.Actually, however, they are quite difficult <strong>to</strong> dowell. The researcher must ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> a comfortable manner<strong>and</strong> establish a friendly situation, yet still attempt <strong>to</strong> learnabout another <strong>in</strong>dividual’s life <strong>in</strong> a fairly systematic fashion.This is not an easy th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> do. Experienced <strong>in</strong>terviewers,therefore, beg<strong>in</strong> with nonthreaten<strong>in</strong>g questionsposed <strong>in</strong> a conversational manner before they ask highlypersonal questions that <strong>in</strong>volve sensitive <strong>to</strong>pics.The other major technique that ethnographers use isparticipant observation, which we also discussed <strong>in</strong>some detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 19. Participant observation is crucial<strong>to</strong> effective fieldwork. As Fetterman suggests, participan<strong>to</strong>bservation “comb<strong>in</strong>es participation <strong>in</strong> the lives ofthe people under study with ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of a professionaldistance that allows adequate observation <strong>and</strong>record<strong>in</strong>g of data.” 17 An important aspect of participan<strong>to</strong>bservation is that it requires immersion <strong>in</strong> a culture. Typically,the researcher lives <strong>and</strong> works <strong>in</strong> the community of<strong>in</strong>terest for six months <strong>to</strong> a year or even longer <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalizethe basic beliefs, fears, hopes, <strong>and</strong> expectations of itspeople. In educational research, participant observation,however, is often noncont<strong>in</strong>uous <strong>and</strong> spread out over along period of time. Fetterman gives an example:In two ethnographic studies, of dropouts <strong>and</strong> gifted children,I visited the programs for only a few weeks everycouple of months over a three-year period. The visitswere <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cluded classroom observation, nons<strong>to</strong>p<strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews, occasional substitute teach<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>in</strong>teraction with community members, <strong>and</strong> the use of variousother research techniques, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g long-distancephone calls, d<strong>in</strong>ner with students’ families, <strong>and</strong> time spenthang<strong>in</strong>g out <strong>in</strong> the hallways <strong>and</strong> park<strong>in</strong>g lot with studentscutt<strong>in</strong>g classes. 18FIELD NOTESA major check on the accuracy of an ethnographer’s observationslies <strong>in</strong> the quality of his or her field notes. Toplace an ethnographic report <strong>in</strong> perspective, <strong>in</strong>terestedreaders need <strong>to</strong> know as much as possible about theideas <strong>and</strong> views of the researcher. That is why the researcher’sfield notes are so important. Unfortunately,this rema<strong>in</strong>s a major problem <strong>in</strong> the report<strong>in</strong>g of muchethnographic research, <strong>in</strong> that the readers of ethnographicreports seldom, if ever, have access <strong>to</strong> the researcher’sfield notes. Rarely do ethnographers tell ushow their <strong>in</strong>formation was collected, <strong>and</strong> hence it oftenis difficult <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the reliability of the researcher’sobservations.Field notes are just what their name implies—thenotes researchers take <strong>in</strong> the field. In educational research,this usually means the detailed notes researcherstake <strong>in</strong> the educational sett<strong>in</strong>g (classroom or school) asthey observe what is go<strong>in</strong>g on or as they <strong>in</strong>terview their<strong>in</strong>formants. They are the researchers’ written account ofwhat they hear, see, experience, <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>in</strong> the courseof collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> reflect<strong>in</strong>g on their data. 19Bernard suggests that field notes be dist<strong>in</strong>guishedfrom three other types of writ<strong>in</strong>g: field jott<strong>in</strong>gs, a fielddiary, <strong>and</strong> a field log. 20Field jott<strong>in</strong>gs refer <strong>to</strong> quick notes about someth<strong>in</strong>gthe researcher wants <strong>to</strong> write more about later. Theyprovide the stimulus <strong>to</strong> help researchers recall a lot ofdetails they do not have time <strong>to</strong> write down dur<strong>in</strong>g anobservation or an <strong>in</strong>terview.A field diary is, <strong>in</strong> effect, a personal statement ofthe researcher’s feel<strong>in</strong>gs, op<strong>in</strong>ions, <strong>and</strong> perceptionsabout others with whom he or she comes <strong>in</strong> contactdur<strong>in</strong>g the course of his or her work. It provides aplace where researchers can let their hair down, so <strong>to</strong>speak—an outlet for writ<strong>in</strong>g down th<strong>in</strong>gs that the researcherdoes not want <strong>to</strong> become part of the publicrecord. Here is part of a page from such a diary of oneof the authors of this book, written dur<strong>in</strong>g a semesterlongobservation of a social studies class <strong>in</strong> a suburbanhigh school.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 507Monday, 11/5. Cold, very ra<strong>in</strong>y day. Makes me feel sort ofdepressed. Phil, Felix, Alicia, Robert, <strong>and</strong> Susan came <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>classroom early <strong>to</strong>day <strong>to</strong> discuss yesterday’s assignment.Susan is look<strong>in</strong>g more disheveled than usual <strong>to</strong>day—seemspreoccupied while others are discuss<strong>in</strong>g ways <strong>to</strong> prepare thegroup report. She doesn’t speak <strong>to</strong> me, although all otherssay hello. I regret my failure <strong>to</strong> support her idea dur<strong>in</strong>gyesterday’s discussion when she asked me <strong>to</strong>. Hope that itwill not result <strong>in</strong> her refus<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>terviewed.Tuesday, 11/13. Susan <strong>and</strong> other members of committeesupposed <strong>to</strong> meet me <strong>in</strong> library before school <strong>to</strong>day forhelp with their report. Nobody showed. Feel that I’vedone someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> turn these kids off, especially Susan.Makes me angry <strong>to</strong>ward her, as this will now be the thirdtime that she has missed a meet<strong>in</strong>g with me. Only firsttime for the others. Perhaps she has more <strong>in</strong>fluence onthem than I thought? I don’t feel I am gett<strong>in</strong>g anywhere <strong>in</strong>underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g her, or why she has such <strong>in</strong>fluence on somany of the other kids.Thursday, 11/29. Wow! Mrs. R. (teacher) had extremelygood discussion <strong>to</strong>day. Seems like entire class participated(note: check discussion tally sheet <strong>to</strong> corroborate). I th<strong>in</strong>ksecret is <strong>to</strong> start off with someth<strong>in</strong>g that they perceive as<strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g. Why is it that sometimes they are so—sogood! so <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> ideas <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> other timesso apathetic? I can’t figure it out.Field work is often an <strong>in</strong>tense, emotionally dra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gexperience, <strong>and</strong> a diary can serve as a way for the researcher<strong>to</strong> let out his or her feel<strong>in</strong>gs, yet still keep themprivate.A field log is a sort of runn<strong>in</strong>g account of how researchersplan <strong>to</strong> spend their time compared <strong>to</strong> how theyactually spend it. It is, <strong>in</strong> effect, the researcher’s plan forcollect<strong>in</strong>g his or her data systematically. A field log consistsof books of blank, l<strong>in</strong>ed paper. Each day <strong>in</strong> the fieldis represented by two pages of the log. On the left page,the researcher lists what he or she plans <strong>to</strong> do that day—where <strong>to</strong> go, who <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview, what <strong>to</strong> observe, <strong>and</strong> soon. On the right side, the researcher lists what he or sheactually did that day. As the study progresses, <strong>and</strong>th<strong>in</strong>gs come <strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> know,the log provides a place for them <strong>to</strong> be scheduled.Bernard gives an example of how such a log is used.Suppose you’re study<strong>in</strong>g a local educational system. It’sApril 5 <strong>and</strong> you are talk<strong>in</strong>g with an <strong>in</strong>formant called MJR.She tells you that s<strong>in</strong>ce the military government <strong>to</strong>okover, children have <strong>to</strong> study politics for two hours everyday, <strong>and</strong> she doesn’t like it. Write a note <strong>to</strong> yourself <strong>in</strong>your log <strong>to</strong> ask other mothers about this issue, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>terview the school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal.Later on, when you are writ<strong>in</strong>g up your notes, youmay decide not <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal until after youhave accumulated more data about how mothers <strong>in</strong> thecommunity feel about the new curriculum. On the lefth<strong>and</strong>page for April 23 you note: “target date for <strong>in</strong>terviewwith school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal.” On the left-h<strong>and</strong> page ofApril 10 you note “make appo<strong>in</strong>tment for <strong>in</strong>terview on23rd with school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal.” For April 6 you note “needmore <strong>in</strong>terviews with mothers about new curriculum.” 21The value of ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a log is that it forces the researcher<strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k hard about the questions he or she trulywants answered, the procedures <strong>to</strong> be followed, <strong>and</strong> thedata really needed. Tak<strong>in</strong>g field notes is an art <strong>in</strong> itself.We can give only a brief <strong>in</strong>troduction here, but the po<strong>in</strong>tspresented below should give you some idea of the importance<strong>and</strong> complexity of the task.Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen state that field notes consist oftwo k<strong>in</strong>ds of materials—descriptive <strong>and</strong> reflective. 22Descriptive field notes attempt <strong>to</strong> describe the sett<strong>in</strong>g,the people, <strong>and</strong> what they do accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> what the researcherobserves. They <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Portraits of the subjects—their physical appearance,mannerisms, gestures, how they act, talk, <strong>and</strong> so on.Reconstruction of dialogue—conversations betweensubjects, as well as what they say <strong>to</strong> the researcher.Unique or particularly reveal<strong>in</strong>g statements shouldbe quoted.Description of the physical sett<strong>in</strong>g—a quick sketchof the room arrangements, placement of materials,<strong>and</strong> so on.Accounts of particular events—who was <strong>in</strong>volved,when, where, <strong>and</strong> how.Depiction of activities—a detailed description of whathappened, along with the order <strong>in</strong> which it happened.The observer’s behavior—the researcher’s actions,dress, conversations with participants, reactions,<strong>and</strong> so on.Reflective field notes present more of what the researcherhimself or herself is th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about as he or sheobserves. These <strong>in</strong>clude the follow<strong>in</strong>g:Reflections on analysis—the researcher’s speculationsabout what he or she is learn<strong>in</strong>g, ideas that aredevelop<strong>in</strong>g, patterns or connections seen, <strong>and</strong> so on.Reflections on method—procedures <strong>and</strong> materials theresearcher is us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the study, comments about the designof the study, problems that are aris<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> so on.


508 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eReflections on ethical dilemmas <strong>and</strong> conflicts—suchas any concerns that arise over responsibilities <strong>to</strong>subjects or value conflicts.Reflections on the observer’s frame of m<strong>in</strong>d—suchas on what the researcher is th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g as the study progresses(his or her attitudes, op<strong>in</strong>ions, <strong>and</strong> beliefs)<strong>and</strong> how these might be affect<strong>in</strong>g the study.Po<strong>in</strong>ts of clarification—notes <strong>to</strong> the researcher aboutth<strong>in</strong>gs that need <strong>to</strong> be clarified, checked later, etc.In no other form of research is the actual do<strong>in</strong>g of thestudy—the process itself—considered as consciously<strong>and</strong> deliberately as it is <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research. Thereflective aspect of field notes is the researcher’s way ofattempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> control for the danger of observer effectthat we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 19, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> rem<strong>in</strong>d us thatresearch, <strong>to</strong> be done well, requires ongo<strong>in</strong>g evaluation<strong>and</strong> judgment.An Example of Field Notes: Marge’s Room 23Date: March 24, 1980Joe McCloud11:00 A.M. <strong>to</strong> 12:30 P.M.Westwood High6th Set of NotesThe Fourth-Period Class <strong>in</strong> Marge’s RoomI arrived at Westwood High at five m<strong>in</strong>utes <strong>to</strong> eleven, atthe time Marge <strong>to</strong>ld me her fourth period started. I wasdressed as usual: sport shirt, ch<strong>in</strong>o pants, <strong>and</strong> Woolrichparka. The fourth period is the only time dur<strong>in</strong>g the daywhen all the students who are <strong>in</strong> the “neurologicallyimpaired/learn<strong>in</strong>g disability” program, better known as“Marge’s program,” come <strong>to</strong>gether. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the otherperiods, certa<strong>in</strong> students <strong>in</strong> the program, two or three orfour at most, come <strong>to</strong> her room for help with the workthey are gett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> other regular high school classes.It was a warm, fortyish, promise of a spr<strong>in</strong>g day.There was a police patrol wagon, the k<strong>in</strong>d that hasbenches <strong>in</strong> the back that are used for large busts, parked<strong>in</strong> the back of the big park<strong>in</strong>g lot that is <strong>in</strong> front of theschool. No one was sitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> it <strong>and</strong> I never heard itsreason for be<strong>in</strong>g there. In the circular drive <strong>in</strong> front ofthe school was parked a United States Army car. It had<strong>in</strong>signias on the side <strong>and</strong> was a khaki color. As I walkedfrom my car, a bald<strong>in</strong>g fortyish man <strong>in</strong> an Army uniformcame out of the build<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> went <strong>to</strong> the car <strong>and</strong> satdown. Four boys <strong>and</strong> a girl also walked out of theschool. All were white. They had on old dungarees <strong>and</strong>colored stenciled t-shirts with spr<strong>in</strong>g jackets over them.One of the boys, the tallest of the four, called out, “o<strong>in</strong>k,o<strong>in</strong>k, o<strong>in</strong>k.” This was done as he sighted the policevehicle <strong>in</strong> the back.O.C.: This was strange <strong>to</strong> me <strong>in</strong> that I didn’t th<strong>in</strong>k that thekids were <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> “the police as pigs.” Somehow I associatedthat with another time, the early 1970s. I’m go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> have<strong>to</strong> come <strong>to</strong> grips with the assumptions I have about highschool due <strong>to</strong> my own experience. Sometimes I feel likeWestwood is entirely different from my high school <strong>and</strong>yet this police car <strong>in</strong>cident rem<strong>in</strong>ded me of m<strong>in</strong>e.I walked <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> Marge’s class <strong>and</strong> she was st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>front of the room with more people than I had ever seen <strong>in</strong>the room save for her homeroom, which is right after secondperiod. She looked like she was talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the class orwas just about <strong>to</strong> start. She was dressed as she had beenon my other visits—clean, neat, well-dressed but casual.Today she had on a striped blazer, a white blouse <strong>and</strong> darkslacks. She looked up at me, smiled <strong>and</strong> said: “Oh, I havea lot more people here now than the last time.”O.C.: This was <strong>in</strong> reference <strong>to</strong> my other visits dur<strong>in</strong>g otherperiods where there are only a few students. She seemsself-conscious about hav<strong>in</strong>g such a small group of students<strong>to</strong> be responsible for. Perhaps she compares herself withthe regular teachers who have classes of thirty or so.There were two women <strong>in</strong> their late twenties sitt<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> the room. There was only one chair left. Marge said<strong>to</strong> me someth<strong>in</strong>g like: “We have two visi<strong>to</strong>rs from thecentral office <strong>to</strong>day. One is a vocational counselor <strong>and</strong>the other is a physical therapist,” but I don’t rememberif those were the words. I felt embarrassed com<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>late. I sat down <strong>in</strong> the only chair available, next <strong>to</strong> oneof the women from the central office. They had on skirts<strong>and</strong> carried their pocketbooks, much more dressed upthan the teachers I’ve seen. They sat there <strong>and</strong> observed.(The class seat<strong>in</strong>g arrangement is shown <strong>in</strong> thediagram at the <strong>to</strong>p of next page.). . . Marge walked about near her desk dur<strong>in</strong>g hertalk, which she started by say<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the class: “Now remember,<strong>to</strong>morrow is a fieldtrip <strong>to</strong> the Rollway Company.We all meet <strong>in</strong> the usual place, by the bus, <strong>in</strong> fron<strong>to</strong>f the ma<strong>in</strong> entrance at 8:30. Mrs. Sharp wanted me <strong>to</strong>tell you that the <strong>to</strong>ur of Rollway is not specifically foryou. It’s not like the trip <strong>to</strong> G.M. They <strong>to</strong>ok you <strong>to</strong>places where you were likely <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> get jobs.Here, it’s just a general <strong>to</strong>ur that everybody goes on.Many of the jobs that you will see are not for you. Someare just for people with eng<strong>in</strong>eer<strong>in</strong>g degrees. You’d


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 509AlfredPhil JeffPamMax<strong>in</strong>eLouMarkBobPhysicaltherapistMarge’s deskVocationalcounselorMeLeroyJasonbetter wear comfortable shoes because you may bewalk<strong>in</strong>g for two or three hours.” Max<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> Mark said:“Ooh,” <strong>in</strong> protest <strong>to</strong> the walk<strong>in</strong>g.She paused <strong>and</strong> said <strong>in</strong> a dem<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g voice: “OK, anyquestions? You are all go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be there. (Pause) I wantyou <strong>to</strong> take a piece of paper <strong>and</strong> write down some questionsso you have th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> ask at the plant.” She beganpass<strong>in</strong>g out paper <strong>and</strong> at this po<strong>in</strong>t Jason, who was sitt<strong>in</strong>gnext <strong>to</strong> me, made a tutt<strong>in</strong>g sound of disgust <strong>and</strong>said: “We got <strong>to</strong> do this?” Marge said: “I know this is<strong>to</strong>o easy for you, Jason.” This was said <strong>in</strong> a sarcasticway but not like a strong putdown.O.C.: It was like sarcasm between two people who knoweach other well. Marge has known many of these kids fora few years. I have <strong>to</strong> explore the implications of that forher relations with them.Marge cont<strong>in</strong>ued: “OK, what are some of the questionsyou are go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> ask?” Jason yelled out: “Insurance,”<strong>and</strong> Marge said: “I was ask<strong>in</strong>g Max<strong>in</strong>e, notJason.” This was said matter-of-factly without anger <strong>to</strong>wardJason. Max<strong>in</strong>e said: “Hours—the hours you work,the wages.” Somebody else yelled out: “Benefits.”Marge wrote these th<strong>in</strong>gs on the board. She got <strong>to</strong> Philwho was sitt<strong>in</strong>g there next <strong>to</strong> Jeff. I believe she skippedover Jeff. Mr. Armstrong was st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g right next <strong>to</strong> Phil.She said: “Have you got one?” Phil said: “I can’t th<strong>in</strong>kof one.” She said: “Honestly, Phil. Wake up.” Then shewent <strong>to</strong> Joe, the white boy. Joe <strong>and</strong> Jeff are the onlywhite boys I’ve seen <strong>in</strong> the program. The two girls arewhite. He said: “I can’t th<strong>in</strong>k of any.”She got <strong>to</strong> Jason <strong>and</strong> asked him if he could th<strong>in</strong>k ofanyth<strong>in</strong>g else. He said: “Yeah, you could ask ’em howmany of the products they made each year.” Marge said:“Yes, you could ask about production. <strong>How</strong> about Leroy,do you have any ideas, Leroy?” He said: “No.” . . . Jasonsaid out loud but not yell<strong>in</strong>g: “<strong>How</strong> much school<strong>in</strong>g youneed <strong>to</strong> get it.” Marge kept list<strong>in</strong>g them.O.C.: Marge was quite animated. If I hadn’t seen her likethis before I would th<strong>in</strong>k she was putt<strong>in</strong>g on a show forthe people from central office. . . .. . . I looked around the room, not<strong>in</strong>g the dress onsome of the students. Max<strong>in</strong>e had on a black t-shirt thathad some iron-on letter<strong>in</strong>g on it. It was a very well-doneiron-on <strong>and</strong> the shirt looked expensive. She had on Levijeans <strong>and</strong> Nike jogg<strong>in</strong>g sneakers. Mark is about 5'9" or5'10". He had on a long sleeve jersey with an alliga<strong>to</strong>ron the front, very stylish but his pants were wr<strong>in</strong>kled<strong>and</strong> he had on old muddy black basketball sneakers withboth laces broken, one <strong>in</strong> two places. Pam had on a lilaccoloredvelour sweater over a but<strong>to</strong>n-down striped shirt.Her hair looked very well-kept <strong>and</strong> looked like she hadhad it styled at an expensive hair place. Jeff sat next <strong>to</strong>her <strong>in</strong> his wheelchair. He had one foot up without a shoeon it as if it were spra<strong>in</strong>ed. . . .Phil had on a beige sweater over a white shirt <strong>and</strong>dark pants <strong>and</strong> low-cut basketball sneakers. The sneakerswere red <strong>and</strong> were dirty. He had a dirt r<strong>in</strong>g aroundthe collar. He is the least well-dressed of the crowd. . . .Jim is probably 5'9" or 5'10". He had on a red pullover.Jason had on a black golf cap <strong>and</strong> a beige spr<strong>in</strong>g jacke<strong>to</strong>ver a university t-shirt. He had on dark dress pants <strong>and</strong> ared university t-shirt with a v-neck. It was faded frombe<strong>in</strong>g washed. Jason’s eyes were noticeably red.O.C.: Two of the kids <strong>to</strong>ld me that Westwood High was afashion show. I have a difficult time figur<strong>in</strong>g out what’s <strong>in</strong>fashion. Jason used that expression. He seems <strong>to</strong> me <strong>to</strong> bethe most clothes-conscious. . . .I don’t know what got this started but she startedtalk<strong>in</strong>g about the social background of the kids <strong>in</strong> the


510 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eclass. She said: “Pam lives around here right up thereso she’s from a professional family. Now, Max<strong>in</strong>e,that’s different. She lives on the east side. She is one ofsix kids <strong>and</strong> her father isn’t that rich. As a matter offact, he’s <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>tenance, tak<strong>in</strong>g charge of clean<strong>in</strong>gcrews. Now, Jeff, he lives on Dogwood. He’s middleclass.” I asked about Lou. She said: “Pour Lou, talkabout be<strong>in</strong>g neurologically impaired. I don’t knowwhat <strong>to</strong> do about that guy. Now he has a sister whograduated two years ago. He worries me more thananybody. I don’t know what is go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> become of him.He is so slow. I don’t know any job that he could do.His father came <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> he looks just like him. What areyou go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> tell him? What is he go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong>do? What is he go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> do? Wash airplanes? I talked <strong>to</strong>the vocational counselor. She said that there were jobs<strong>in</strong> airports wash<strong>in</strong>g airplanes. I mean, how is he go<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> wash an airplane? <strong>How</strong> about sweep<strong>in</strong>g out thehangars? Maybe he could do that. The mother is someth<strong>in</strong>gelse. His mother th<strong>in</strong>ks that Lou is her punishment.Can you imag<strong>in</strong>e an attitude like that? I was justwonder<strong>in</strong>g what could she have done <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k that shedeserved Lou?“Now Luca Meta, he is upper class all the way.Leroy, there’s your low end of the spectrum. I don’tknow how many kids they have but they have a lot. Hismother just had a kidney removed. Everybody knows heis on parole. Matter of fact, whenever there is any steal<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> the school, they look at him. He used <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> gym<strong>and</strong> every time he went, someth<strong>in</strong>g was s<strong>to</strong>len. Nowthey don’t let him go <strong>to</strong> gym anymore. His parole officerwas down. He won’t be here next year.” . . .. . . She said: “By the way, I was talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> maybeyou overheard me about what we need is a competencybasedprogram here. I have already f<strong>in</strong>ished a competencybasedprogram if they ever <strong>to</strong>ok it. It is silly <strong>to</strong> have kidsspend four years sitt<strong>in</strong>g here, when it makes no sense <strong>in</strong>terms of them. They ought <strong>to</strong> be out work<strong>in</strong>g. If they’re notgo<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> graduate, what they ought <strong>to</strong> have is some liv<strong>in</strong>gskills like what we did with writ<strong>in</strong>g the checks. Peoplearen’t go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> teach them that out <strong>in</strong> the world so theycould do that. Once they had enough skills, liv<strong>in</strong>g skills, <strong>to</strong>make it on their own then they ought <strong>to</strong> go out. There is nosense <strong>to</strong> this.”...We left the room. Alfred <strong>and</strong> Marge walked up theempty hall with me. I asked her how the kids felt aboutbe<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> this class. She said: “Well, it varies. It really bothersPam. Like she failed his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> she has <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> summerschool. The reason she failed it was she wouldn’ttell them that she was <strong>in</strong> this program so she didn’t getany extra help <strong>and</strong> then she failed.” Marge walked me <strong>to</strong>the door. Alfred dropped off at the teachers’ room.On the way <strong>to</strong> the door she said: “Remember thatboy I <strong>to</strong>ld you about who’s go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> there? Thedentist’s son, the Swenson boy? Well, I have been hear<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>to</strong>ries about him. I come <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out that he is reallyE.M.H. (Educable Mentally H<strong>and</strong>icapped) <strong>and</strong> a hyperactivekid. I really am go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> have my h<strong>and</strong>s full withhim. If there is twenty <strong>in</strong> the program next year, I reallyam go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> need another aide.” I said good-bye <strong>and</strong>walked <strong>to</strong> my car.Data Analysis <strong>in</strong>Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Analysis is one of the most <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g aspects of ethnographicresearch. It beg<strong>in</strong>s from the first moment a researcherselects a problem <strong>to</strong> study <strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ues untilthe f<strong>in</strong>al report is written. Many techniques, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gcontent analysis (see Chapter 20) are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>gethnographic data. Some of the more important<strong>in</strong>clude triangulation, search<strong>in</strong>g for patterns, identify<strong>in</strong>gkey events, prepar<strong>in</strong>g visual representations, us<strong>in</strong>gstatistics, <strong>and</strong> crystallization. What follows is a briefdescription of each.Triangulation. Triangulation is fundamental <strong>in</strong>ethnographic research. Essentially, it establishes thevalidity of an ethnographer’s observations. It <strong>in</strong>volvescheck<strong>in</strong>g what one hears <strong>and</strong> sees by compar<strong>in</strong>g one’ssources of <strong>in</strong>formation—do they agree? Here’s an example:A researcher might compare a student’s oralstatements that he was a “good” student with a writtentranscript of his grades, his teacher’s comments <strong>in</strong> thisregard, <strong>and</strong>, perhaps, some unsolicited remarks fromhis fellow students. Triangulation here could verify—ornot—the student’s self-assessment.Triangulation can work with any subject, <strong>in</strong> any sett<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> at any level (Figure 21.1). It improves the qualityof the data that are collected <strong>and</strong> the accuracy of theresearcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations. It can occur naturally, even<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formal conversation. Consider this example:A prom<strong>in</strong>ent super<strong>in</strong>tendent, manag<strong>in</strong>g one of the largestdistricts <strong>in</strong> the nation, had just f<strong>in</strong>ished expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whyschool size made no difference <strong>in</strong> education. He said thathe had one 1500-pupil school <strong>and</strong> one 5000-pupil school<strong>in</strong> his district that he was particularly proud of, <strong>and</strong> that


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 511C<strong>and</strong>idate’scampaignspeechesC<strong>and</strong>idate’spersonal<strong>in</strong>terviewsFigure 21.1Triangulation <strong>and</strong>PoliticsC<strong>and</strong>idate’svot<strong>in</strong>grecordWhat areyour beliefs,Sena<strong>to</strong>r Bullw<strong>in</strong>kle?the school size had no effect on school spirit, the educativeprocess, or his ability <strong>to</strong> manage. He also expla<strong>in</strong>edthat he had <strong>to</strong> build two or three new schools next year,either three small schools or one small school <strong>and</strong> onelarge one. A colleague <strong>in</strong>terrupted <strong>to</strong> ask which hepreferred. The super<strong>in</strong>tendent replied, “Small ones, ofcourse; they are much easier <strong>to</strong> h<strong>and</strong>le.” He had betrayedhimself <strong>in</strong> one phrase. Although the adm<strong>in</strong>istrative partyl<strong>in</strong>e was that size made no difference—management ismanagement no matter how big or small the unit—thissuper<strong>in</strong>tendent revealed a very different personal op<strong>in</strong>ion<strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> a casual question. 24Patterns. Those who do ethnographic research lookfor patterns <strong>in</strong> the ways that people th<strong>in</strong>k <strong>and</strong> behave.They offer a means of check<strong>in</strong>g ethnographic reliabilitywhen they reveal consistencies <strong>in</strong> what people say <strong>and</strong>what they do. Typically, researchers start with a largemass of undifferentiated <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong> then, by compar<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> contrast<strong>in</strong>g what they collected, sort this <strong>in</strong>formationuntil a discernible l<strong>in</strong>e of thought or patternof behavior emerges. They then observe <strong>and</strong> listen somemore <strong>to</strong> see whether the new observations correspond<strong>to</strong> what they saw <strong>and</strong> heard before. This then requiresfurther sift<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> sort<strong>in</strong>g until the researchers are satisfiedthat what they are describ<strong>in</strong>g matches what wasobserved.Key Events. Key events occur <strong>in</strong> every social group<strong>and</strong> provide data that ethnographic researchers can use<strong>to</strong> describe <strong>and</strong> analyze an entire culture. They convey atremendous amount of <strong>in</strong>formation. They provide a“lens through which <strong>to</strong> view a culture.” 25 Examplesmight <strong>in</strong>clude the <strong>in</strong>troduction of computers <strong>in</strong> anelementary school, a fistfight between two girls dur<strong>in</strong>ga high school basketball game, the response of a groupof recovery room nurses <strong>to</strong> a medical emergency, a fire<strong>in</strong> a crowded apartment build<strong>in</strong>g, the <strong>in</strong>troduction of anew teach<strong>in</strong>g method <strong>in</strong> a social studies classroom, orthe return of a popular professor from sabbatical leave.Such events are especially useful for analysis, as theynot only help the researcher underst<strong>and</strong> the group he orshe is study<strong>in</strong>g, but they also help the researcher expla<strong>in</strong>the culture of the group <strong>to</strong> others.Visual Representations. These <strong>in</strong>clude suchth<strong>in</strong>gs as maps (e.g., of a classroom or school), flowcharts(e.g., of who says what <strong>to</strong> whom dur<strong>in</strong>g a classroomdiscussion), organizational charts (e.g., of how aschool library is organized), sociograms (e.g., of whichstudents receive the most <strong>in</strong>vitations <strong>to</strong> participate as amember of a classroom research team, matrices (e.g., achart <strong>to</strong> compare <strong>and</strong> cross-reference the various categoriesthat exist <strong>in</strong> a creative arts department <strong>in</strong> a university,such as music, dance, theatre, pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> thelike). The very process of prepar<strong>in</strong>g a visual representationoften can help a researcher crystallize his or herunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of an area, a system, a location, or evenan <strong>in</strong>teraction. Visual representations are very useful<strong>to</strong>ols <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research.Statistics. Although you might not expect it, ethnographersoften do use statistics <strong>in</strong> their work. Usually,however, they use nonparametric techniques (see Chapter11), such as a chi-square test, more often than parametricones. Typically, they are more likely <strong>to</strong> reportfrequencies than scores. They do use parametric statisticswhen they have large samples, however.


512 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eNevertheless, the use of statistics <strong>in</strong> ethnographicresearch presents a number of problems. Meet<strong>in</strong>g theassumptions that many <strong>in</strong>ferential tests require (e.g.,that the sample is r<strong>and</strong>om) often is virtually impossible.Typically, ethnographic studies use small samples <strong>and</strong>purposive samples. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, descriptivestatistics—means <strong>and</strong> medians—can at times be used <strong>to</strong>summarize the frequency of actions or events <strong>and</strong> are<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly found <strong>in</strong> ethnographic reports.Crystallization. Ethnographers try <strong>to</strong> pull <strong>to</strong>gethertheir thoughts at various stages throughout their research.Sometimes this results <strong>in</strong> only a summ<strong>in</strong>g upof <strong>in</strong>formation, but other times it results <strong>in</strong> a genu<strong>in</strong>e<strong>in</strong>sight. “Every study has classic moments when everyth<strong>in</strong>gfalls <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> place. After months of thought <strong>and</strong>immersion <strong>in</strong> the culture, a special configuration gels.All the sub<strong>to</strong>pics, m<strong>in</strong>iexperiments, layers of triangulatedeffort, key events, <strong>and</strong> patterns of behavior form acoherent <strong>and</strong> often cogent picture of what is happen<strong>in</strong>g.”26 Noth<strong>in</strong>g is more excit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> an ethnographerthan when this happens.The important th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> realize about the analysis ofethnographic data is that there is no s<strong>in</strong>gle stage or timewhen crystallization occurs. Multiple analyses <strong>and</strong>multiple forms are essential. Often it is cyclical—dataare collected, thought about, more data are collected,patterns are looked for, more data are collected, newpatterns are looked for, matrices <strong>and</strong> then more matricesare developed, <strong>and</strong> on <strong>and</strong> on. Data analysis <strong>in</strong> ethnographyis ongo<strong>in</strong>g, from start <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ish.Roger Harker <strong>and</strong> HisFifth-Grade ClassroomLet us look, then, at an example of ethnographic research.What follows is a short description, by the researcher, ofan ethnographic study of a fifth-grade classroom.I [the researcher] worked <strong>in</strong> depth with Roger Harker forsix months. I did an ethnography of his classroom <strong>and</strong> the<strong>in</strong>teraction between him <strong>and</strong> his pupils. This young manhad taught for three years <strong>in</strong> the elementary school. Hevolunteered for the study <strong>in</strong> order, he said, “<strong>to</strong> improvemy professional competence.”My collection of data fell <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g categories:(1) personal, au<strong>to</strong>biographical, <strong>and</strong> psychologicaldata on the teacher; (2) rat<strong>in</strong>gs of him by his pr<strong>in</strong>cipal<strong>and</strong> other superiors <strong>in</strong> the super<strong>in</strong>tendent’s office; (3) hisown self-estimates on the same po<strong>in</strong>ts; (4) observationsof his classroom, emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>teraction with children;(5) <strong>in</strong>terviews with each child <strong>and</strong> the elicitation of rat<strong>in</strong>gsof the teacher on many different dimensions, bothformally <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formally; (6) his rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> estimates foreach child <strong>in</strong> his classroom, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g estimates of popularitywith peers, academic performance <strong>and</strong> capacity,personal adjustment, home background, <strong>and</strong> lik<strong>in</strong>g forhim; (7) sociometric data from the children about eachother; <strong>and</strong> (8) <strong>in</strong>terviews with each person (super<strong>in</strong>tendent,pr<strong>in</strong>cipal, supervisors, children) who suppliedrat<strong>in</strong>gs of him.I also participated <strong>in</strong> the life of the school <strong>to</strong> the extentpossible, accompany<strong>in</strong>g the teacher where I could <strong>and</strong>“melt<strong>in</strong>g” <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the classroom as much as feasible. I wasalways there, but I had no authority <strong>and</strong> assumed none.I became a friend <strong>and</strong> confidant <strong>to</strong> the children.This teacher was regarded by his superiors as mostpromis<strong>in</strong>g—“clear <strong>and</strong> well-organized,” “sensitive <strong>to</strong>children’s needs,” “fair <strong>and</strong> just <strong>to</strong> all of the children,”“know<strong>in</strong>g his subject areas well.” I was not able <strong>to</strong> elicitwith either rat<strong>in</strong>gs scales or <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews any criticismsor negative evaluations. There were very few suggestionsfor change—<strong>and</strong> these were all <strong>in</strong> the area of subjectmatter <strong>and</strong> curriculum.Roger Harker described himself as “fair <strong>and</strong> just <strong>to</strong> allmy pupils,” as mak<strong>in</strong>g “fair decisions,” <strong>and</strong> as “play<strong>in</strong>g nofavorites.” This was a particular po<strong>in</strong>t of pride with him.His classroom was made up of children from a broadsocial stratum—upper-middle, middle, <strong>and</strong> lowerclasses—<strong>and</strong> the children represented Mexican-American,Anglo-European, <strong>and</strong> Japanese-American ethnic groups. Iwas particularly attentive <strong>to</strong> the relationships between theteacher <strong>and</strong> children from these various groups.One could go <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> much detail, but a few items willsuffice s<strong>in</strong>ce they all po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> the same direction, <strong>and</strong>that direction challenges both his perceptions of his ownbehavior <strong>and</strong> those of his superiors. He ranked highest onall dimensions, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g personal <strong>and</strong> academic fac<strong>to</strong>rs,those children who were most like himself—Anglo, middle<strong>to</strong> upper-middle social class, <strong>and</strong>, like him, ambitious(achievement-oriented). He also estimated that thesechildren were the most popular with their peers <strong>and</strong> werethe leaders of the classroom group. His knowledge aboutthe <strong>in</strong>dividual children, elicited without recourse <strong>to</strong> filesor notes, was distributed <strong>in</strong> the same way. He knew significantlymore about the children culturally like himself(on items concerned with home background as well asacademic performance) <strong>and</strong> least about those culturallymost different.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 513The children had quite different views of thesituation. Some children described him as not alwaysso “fair <strong>and</strong> just,” as “hav<strong>in</strong>g special pets,” as notbe<strong>in</strong>g easy <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> with their problems. On sociometric“maps” of the classroom show<strong>in</strong>g which childrenwanted <strong>to</strong> spend time with other specific children, orwork with them, sit near them, <strong>in</strong>vite them <strong>to</strong> a party ora show, etc., the most popular children were not at allthose the teacher rated highest. And his negative rat<strong>in</strong>gsproved <strong>to</strong> be equally <strong>in</strong>accurate. Children he rated asisolated or badly adjusted socially, most of whom werenon-Anglo or non-middle-class, more often than notturned out <strong>to</strong> be “stars of attraction” from the po<strong>in</strong>t ofview of the children.Observations of his classroom behavior supported thedata collected by other means. He most frequently calledon, <strong>to</strong>uched, helped, <strong>and</strong> looked directly at the childrenculturally like himself. He was never mean or cruel <strong>to</strong> theother children. It was almost as though they weren’t there.His <strong>in</strong>teraction with the children of Anglo-Europeanethnicity <strong>and</strong> middle <strong>and</strong> upper-middle social classbackground was more frequent than with the otherchildren, <strong>and</strong> the quality of the <strong>in</strong>teraction appeared <strong>to</strong>be differentiated <strong>in</strong> the same way.This young man, with the best of <strong>in</strong>tentions, wasconfirm<strong>in</strong>g the negative hypotheses <strong>and</strong> predictions (aswell as the positive ones) already made with<strong>in</strong> the socialsystem. He was <strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g Anglo middle-class childrenthat they were capable, had bright futures, were sociallyacceptable, <strong>and</strong> were worth a lot of trouble. He was also<strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g non-Anglo children that they were less capable,less socially acceptable, less worth the trouble. He wasdefeat<strong>in</strong>g his own declared educational goals.This young teacher did not know that he was discrim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g.He was rated very positively by his superiors onall counts, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g be<strong>in</strong>g “fair <strong>and</strong> just <strong>to</strong> all the children.”Apparently they were as bl<strong>in</strong>d <strong>to</strong> his discrim<strong>in</strong>ationas he was. The school system supported him <strong>and</strong> hisclassroom behavior without question<strong>in</strong>g or criticiz<strong>in</strong>ghim. And the dom<strong>in</strong>ant social structure of the communitysupported the school. 27Notice several th<strong>in</strong>gs about this descriptionThe study <strong>to</strong>ok place <strong>in</strong> a naturalistic sett<strong>in</strong>g—<strong>in</strong> theclassroom <strong>and</strong> school of Roger Harker.The researcher did not try <strong>to</strong> manipulate the situation<strong>in</strong> any way.There was no comparison of methods or treatments(as is often the case <strong>in</strong> experimental or causalcomparativeresearch).The study <strong>in</strong>volved only a s<strong>in</strong>gle classroom (an n ofone).The researcher was a participant observer, participat<strong>in</strong>g“<strong>in</strong> the life of the school <strong>to</strong> the extent possible.”The researcher used several different k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>struments<strong>to</strong> collect his data.The researcher tried <strong>to</strong> present a holistic descriptionof this teacher’s fifth-grade classroom.The study revealed much that would have beenmissed by researchers us<strong>in</strong>g other methodologies.No attempt was made <strong>to</strong> generalize the researcher’sf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> other sett<strong>in</strong>gs or situations. The “externalvalidity” of the study, <strong>in</strong> other words, was very limited.There is no way, unfortunately, <strong>to</strong> check the validityof the data or the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations (unlessanother researcher had <strong>in</strong>dependently observed thesame classroom).Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantagesof Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>Ethnographic research has a number of unique strengths,but also several weaknesses. A key strength is that it providesthe researcher with a much more comprehensiveperspective than do other forms of educational research.By observ<strong>in</strong>g the actual behavior of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> theirnatural sett<strong>in</strong>gs, one may ga<strong>in</strong> a much deeper <strong>and</strong> richerunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of such behavior. Ethnographic researchalso lends itself well <strong>to</strong> research <strong>to</strong>pics that are not easilyquantified. The thoughts of teachers <strong>and</strong> students,ideas, <strong>and</strong> other nuances of behavior that might escaperesearchers us<strong>in</strong>g other methodologies can often bedetected by ethnographic researchers.Furthermore, ethnographic research is particularlyappropriate <strong>to</strong> behaviors that are best unders<strong>to</strong>od byobserv<strong>in</strong>g them with<strong>in</strong> their natural sett<strong>in</strong>gs. Other typesof research can measure attitudes <strong>and</strong> behaviors <strong>in</strong>somewhat artificial sett<strong>in</strong>gs, but they frequently do notlend themselves well <strong>to</strong> naturalistic sett<strong>in</strong>gs. The “dynamics”of a faculty meet<strong>in</strong>g, or the “<strong>in</strong>teraction” betweenstudents <strong>and</strong> teacher <strong>in</strong> a classroom, for example,can probably best be studied through ethnographic <strong>in</strong>vestigation.F<strong>in</strong>ally, ethnographic research is especiallysuited <strong>to</strong> study<strong>in</strong>g group behavior over time. Thus, <strong>to</strong>underst<strong>and</strong> as fully as possible the “life” of an <strong>in</strong>ner-cityschool over a year-long period, an ethnographic approachmay well be the most appropriate methodology for aresearcher <strong>to</strong> use.


514 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eEthnographic research, like all research, however, isnot without its limitations. It is highly dependent on theparticular researcher’s observations <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations,<strong>and</strong> s<strong>in</strong>ce numerical data are rarely provided, there isusually no way <strong>to</strong> check the validity of the researcher’sconclusions. As a result, observer bias is almost impossible<strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate. Because usually only a s<strong>in</strong>gle situation(such as one classroom or one school) is observed, generalizabilityis almost nonexistent, except when it is possible<strong>to</strong> replicate the study <strong>in</strong> other sett<strong>in</strong>gs or situationsby other researchers. Because the researcher usually beg<strong>in</strong>shis or her observations without a specific hypothesis<strong>to</strong> confirm or deny, terms may not be def<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>and</strong> hencethe specific variables or relationships be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated(if any) may rema<strong>in</strong> unclear.Because of the <strong>in</strong>evitable ambiguity that accompaniesthis method, preplann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> review by others are muchless useful than <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies. While it is truethat no study is ever carried out precisely as planned,potential pitfalls are more easily identified <strong>and</strong> corrected<strong>in</strong> other methodologies. For this reason, we believeethnographic research <strong>to</strong> be a very difficult type of research<strong>to</strong> do well. It follows that beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g researchersus<strong>in</strong>g this method should receive close supervision.An Exampleof Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of ethnographic research, followed by a critiqueof its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> ourcritiques of the different types of research studies weanalyzed <strong>in</strong> other chapters, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Urban <strong>Education</strong>, 33: 218–242. Copyright © 1998 by Corw<strong>in</strong> Press, Inc. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission of Corw<strong>in</strong>Press, Inc.Dump<strong>in</strong>g Ground or EffectiveAlternative? Dropout-PreventionPrograms <strong>in</strong> Urban SchoolsCori GrothUniversity of UtahAbstractFocus<strong>in</strong>g on students’ perspectives, this article exam<strong>in</strong>es whether an urban dropoutpreventionprogram offers students effective alternatives <strong>to</strong> regular classes or if, <strong>in</strong>stead,the program is simply a dump<strong>in</strong>g ground for unwanted students. F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs generatedfrom <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> observations suggest both. Although the analysis of the students’perceptions <strong>in</strong>dicates that the program helps students <strong>to</strong> rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> acquirecredits, the program does little <strong>to</strong> help students create <strong>and</strong> apply practical knowledge <strong>in</strong>their everyday lives. Nor does the program help students <strong>to</strong> acquire the skills either forfitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture or for surviv<strong>in</strong>g as resisters of that culture. A discussionof policy implications is provided along with suggestions for future research.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 515Dropp<strong>in</strong>g out of high school reportedly has negative effects for both <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong>society. Dropouts tend <strong>to</strong> have lower-pay<strong>in</strong>g jobs, lower employment rates, <strong>and</strong>generally lower st<strong>and</strong>ards of liv<strong>in</strong>g (Catterall, 1985). Similarly, dropp<strong>in</strong>g out leads <strong>to</strong>social costs <strong>in</strong> the form of <strong>in</strong>creased crime rates, <strong>in</strong>creased dispensation of welfare <strong>and</strong>unemployment subsidies, <strong>in</strong>creased health care costs, <strong>and</strong> lowered tax revenues for thestate (Catterall, 1985; Lev<strong>in</strong>, 1972).The failure of ma<strong>in</strong>stream public schools <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> the issues of an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>glypluralistic context <strong>and</strong> the result<strong>in</strong>g problems of students who leave the educational systemearly have moved educa<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> provide alternatives <strong>to</strong> regular schools. A variety ofprograms have been designed <strong>to</strong> remedy the dropout problem. Despite the proliferationof such programs 1 (Reilly & Reilly, 1983), few researchers have studied the <strong>in</strong>teractionaldynamics between students <strong>and</strong> teachers <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the effectiveness of the programs.In this article, I address these issues by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g one dropout-prevention program<strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> how <strong>in</strong>teraction between students <strong>and</strong> faculty is def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> howthese <strong>in</strong>teractions relate <strong>to</strong> behavior associated with either stay<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> or leav<strong>in</strong>g school.Most researchers study<strong>in</strong>g the dropout problem have focused primarily on the at-riskstudents’ characteristics, while neglect<strong>in</strong>g their experiences (F<strong>in</strong>e, 1991; Kelly, 1993). Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe students’ experience is crucial for a critical analysis of educa<strong>to</strong>rs’ decisions <strong>to</strong>adopt remedial dropout-prevention programs. Without critically analyz<strong>in</strong>g the experiencesof students <strong>in</strong> these programs, educa<strong>to</strong>rs may perpetuate the patterns that <strong>in</strong>fluence students<strong>to</strong> drop out <strong>and</strong>, therefore, they will merely replicate the dysfunctional system thatconsistently loses about one out of four of its students (Catterall, 1985; LeCompte, 1987).JustificationNeed more detailPurpose?JustificationTHE “MISFITS”Often labeled as dropout-prevention programs or cont<strong>in</strong>uation schools, these programshave been created <strong>to</strong> extend the promise of education <strong>to</strong> students who have not fit <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>the ma<strong>in</strong>stream public schools (Kelly, 1996). In discuss<strong>in</strong>g the response of educa<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong> atriskpopulations, Kelly (1996) suggests that options provided <strong>to</strong> potential or presentdropouts should respond <strong>to</strong> two issues:(1) students’ need for flexibility <strong>and</strong> personalization, with the stated aim of“dropout prevention <strong>and</strong> recovery,” <strong>and</strong> (2) ma<strong>in</strong>stream comprehensive highschools’ need for mechanisms <strong>to</strong> isolate students identified as discipl<strong>in</strong>eproblems <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide specialized services efficiently. (p. 118)Kelly is pessimistic about these second-chance programs, particularly because theydo not address the larger structures both <strong>in</strong>side <strong>and</strong> outside the educational system(e.g., job programs, more <strong>and</strong> better fund<strong>in</strong>g for child care, funded access <strong>to</strong> contraception<strong>and</strong> abortion services, balanced hous<strong>in</strong>g development, social <strong>and</strong> health services)that currently replicate the traditional sort<strong>in</strong>g process (1996, p. 119).Schools have long been criticized for their normative culture, which may be thesource of student alienation <strong>and</strong> consequent disengagement, particularly for studentswho do not conform <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream st<strong>and</strong>ards of a typical student. Dei (1996) states:School “culture” embraces a unique set of core norms <strong>and</strong> values, well-def<strong>in</strong>edhierarchies of authority, <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itions of what is knowledge, what is goodpedagogy, <strong>and</strong> how students are best assessed <strong>and</strong> rewarded. Conventionalnorms about school<strong>in</strong>g rest on assimilationist assumptions. (p. 177)Def<strong>in</strong>ition?This educational culture becomes problematic as schools serve <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly diverse populations.Furthermore, this issue of diversity is especially relevant <strong>to</strong> the study of dropout


516 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAssumptionprevention because of the disproportionate numbers of m<strong>in</strong>ority youth <strong>and</strong> those fromlower socioeconomic status groups be<strong>in</strong>g served by alternative programs. To avoid acommon pitfall of much research on dropout prevention, I f<strong>in</strong>d it critical not <strong>to</strong> viewdropp<strong>in</strong>g out of school as a pathological <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual problem (Dei, 1996). Studentalienation <strong>and</strong> disengagement from school most certa<strong>in</strong>ly affects <strong>in</strong>dividuals, yet thesources of alienation <strong>and</strong> disengagement may be quite structural. In fact, F<strong>in</strong>e (1991) <strong>and</strong>Dei (1996) suggest that students disengag<strong>in</strong>g from school are social ac<strong>to</strong>rs, critiqu<strong>in</strong>g the“very foundations of school<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> our society” (Dei. 1996, p. 184). Students <strong>in</strong> alternativedropout-prevention programs may offer the k<strong>in</strong>d of marg<strong>in</strong>al voice that illum<strong>in</strong>atesstructural characteristics of systems that otherwise may be difficult <strong>to</strong> see.EDUCATIONAL SOLUTIONS TO THE DROPOUT PROBLEMNeed more detailPrior researchF<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs or op<strong>in</strong>ion?Dropout-prevention programs provide one strategy for address<strong>in</strong>g the dropout problem. Asnoted above, these programs provide alternative sett<strong>in</strong>gs where students can obta<strong>in</strong> credits<strong>to</strong>ward a high-school diploma. The grow<strong>in</strong>g popularity of such programs also addresseseduca<strong>to</strong>rs’ concern for accommodat<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly diverse student population (Ascher,1982; Catterall & Stern, 1986; Coll<strong>in</strong>s, 1987; Deal & Nolan, 1978; Duke & Perry, 1978; Frankl<strong>in</strong>,1992; Gold & Mann, 1982; Wehlage, Rutter, Smith, Lesko, & Fern<strong>and</strong>ez, 1989).Despite the <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g popularity of dropout-prevention programs <strong>and</strong> otheralternative programs, only a h<strong>and</strong>ful of researchers have addressed the issue. <strong>Research</strong>on alternative education is described by Frankl<strong>in</strong> (1992) as “generally terrible.” Compared<strong>to</strong> the copious attention paid <strong>to</strong> high-school dropouts, the attention researchers give <strong>to</strong>study<strong>in</strong>g dropout prevention <strong>and</strong> alternative education programs is <strong>in</strong>adequate.As a result, <strong>in</strong> part, of <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g governmental pressure <strong>to</strong> perform, educa<strong>to</strong>rs are <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g options for their students, particularly racial <strong>and</strong> ethnicm<strong>in</strong>orities. Provid<strong>in</strong>g alternative programs, however, does not completely solve the problem.<strong>Research</strong>ers criticize alternative programs on several fronts. They claim the programs serve asdump<strong>in</strong>g grounds for unwanted behavior problem students, especially m<strong>in</strong>ority students.Enrollment <strong>in</strong> alternative programs can stigmatize students. Gold <strong>and</strong> Mann (1982) state:Youth may be made <strong>to</strong> feel that they are different <strong>in</strong> a deroga<strong>to</strong>ry sense byhav<strong>in</strong>g been sent <strong>to</strong> a special school for “bad kids.” A substantial number ofadm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, teachers, <strong>and</strong> students did hold negative op<strong>in</strong>ions about thealternative programs <strong>and</strong> the young people who went there. (p. 311)<strong>Research</strong>ers suggest that plac<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>in</strong> dropout-prevention programs may be anextreme version of the track<strong>in</strong>g system, <strong>in</strong> which adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs disproportionately <strong>and</strong>cont<strong>in</strong>ually place low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g, low socioeconomic status (SES), <strong>and</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority students <strong>in</strong>lower academic level or vocational courses (Kelly, 1993). The same types of students occupy<strong>in</strong>glower academic level <strong>and</strong> vocational courses also occupy dropout-prevention programs.<strong>Research</strong>ers also claim that dropout-prevention programs require only m<strong>in</strong>imal scholasticst<strong>and</strong>ards. They say these programs have been criticized because “students earn scholasticcredits with less effort, that they receive pass<strong>in</strong>g grades for below-st<strong>and</strong>ard work, <strong>and</strong>that they are privileged <strong>to</strong> break ord<strong>in</strong>ary school rules” (Gold & Mann, 1982, p. 315).Educa<strong>to</strong>rs face the difficult task of provid<strong>in</strong>g effective alternatives for students thatwould give them access <strong>to</strong> the rewards a diploma supposedly offers. They attempt <strong>to</strong> createalternative programs by adapt<strong>in</strong>g policies <strong>and</strong> offer<strong>in</strong>g curriculum that will satisfy thestudents’ needs, while ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g conformity <strong>to</strong> the exist<strong>in</strong>g practices of the educationalsystem. They have not agreed, however, on the ways <strong>to</strong> do this. This is a very <strong>to</strong>ugh jobbut one that needs exam<strong>in</strong>ation.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 517METHODSThe design of this project is explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretive <strong>in</strong> nature. Assum<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>teractionistperspective, I placed primary emphasis on the students’ def<strong>in</strong>itions of their ownsituation <strong>in</strong> school, as well as their past experiences. This theoretical framework calls fora participant-observational approach <strong>to</strong> data collection, which <strong>in</strong>volves “be<strong>in</strong>g unusuallythorough <strong>and</strong> reflective <strong>in</strong> notic<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g everyday events <strong>in</strong> the field sett<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> identify the significance of actions <strong>in</strong> the events from the variouspo<strong>in</strong>ts of view of the ac<strong>to</strong>rs themselves” (Erickson, 1986, p. 121).Us<strong>in</strong>g this type of participant-observational approach, I attempted <strong>to</strong> capture theassertions students made about their experiences <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> their po<strong>in</strong>t of viewby record<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews with students, mak<strong>in</strong>g weekly <strong>and</strong> daily site visits <strong>to</strong> the school,observ<strong>in</strong>g participants <strong>in</strong> the program <strong>in</strong> their everyday sett<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g extensivefield notes of all the conversations <strong>and</strong> dynamics <strong>in</strong> the classroom. Data were collecteddur<strong>in</strong>g the 1994–1995 school year.Dur<strong>in</strong>g site visits, I observed students do<strong>in</strong>g their work, <strong>and</strong> I engaged <strong>in</strong> conversationswith them throughout the visit. In addition, I participated <strong>in</strong> bi-weekly focus groupmeet<strong>in</strong>gs conducted by the school counselor who was work<strong>in</strong>g with the AdvancedAchievement Academy (AAA) 2 program coord<strong>in</strong>a<strong>to</strong>r. These focus group meet<strong>in</strong>gs providedan excellent opportunity for me <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation about the students’ personallives as well as their perceptions of themselves <strong>in</strong> the larger school system.F<strong>in</strong>ally, I conducted <strong>in</strong>-class <strong>in</strong>terviews with the students. Dur<strong>in</strong>g each <strong>in</strong>terview,students were asked <strong>to</strong> discuss the follow<strong>in</strong>g issues: their experiences <strong>in</strong> AAA, how experiences<strong>in</strong> AAA differed from those <strong>in</strong> regular classes, their work on computers, theirrelationships with the teachers <strong>in</strong> AAA, how their relationship with the faculty <strong>in</strong> AAAdiffered from relationships with faculty <strong>in</strong> regular classes, their perceptions of what othersth<strong>in</strong>k of AAA, the reasons they enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA, their options for gett<strong>in</strong>g a diploma,how they knew about these options, their plans after graduation, what they th<strong>in</strong>k aboutschool, <strong>and</strong> what they perceive as the th<strong>in</strong>gs that limit them. These <strong>in</strong>terviews were taperecorded <strong>and</strong> transcribed.MethodData collectionProceduresUseful clarificationSett<strong>in</strong>g: Dropout-Prevention ProgramThe dropout-prevention program I studied, AAA, is located on the perimeter of a highschoolcampus <strong>in</strong> a southwestern metropolitan city. Established 4 years ago, the programoffers computer-assisted <strong>in</strong>struction. The students <strong>in</strong> AAA have either droppedout of school or were <strong>in</strong> the process of dropp<strong>in</strong>g out at the time of enrollment <strong>in</strong> theprogram. Students <strong>in</strong> the program receive credit for complet<strong>in</strong>g work on computerassistedtu<strong>to</strong>rials that are the equivalent of one regular semester course. The studentsparticipate <strong>in</strong> a focus group twice a week, <strong>in</strong> which a counselor leads them <strong>in</strong> discussionsrelated <strong>to</strong> their personal lives <strong>and</strong> school or community issues. A teacher is present <strong>in</strong>the classroom at all times, <strong>and</strong> a teacher’s aide is there twice a week. Enrollment <strong>in</strong> thisprogram is limited <strong>to</strong> 1 year, although some exceptions are made based on personalextenuat<strong>in</strong>g circumstances.The program direc<strong>to</strong>r selects students for the program from a wait<strong>in</strong>g list based onwho the direc<strong>to</strong>r th<strong>in</strong>ks will benefit most from the program. Students are not recruitedor placed <strong>in</strong> AAA by counselors or other adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs: rather, they must actively seekenrollment <strong>in</strong> the program on their own. Because of these policies, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs ofAAA are <strong>in</strong> an unusually good position <strong>to</strong> help students get back on track <strong>and</strong> start earn<strong>in</strong>gcredits for several reasons. First, the students who request enrollment already havereached a critical po<strong>in</strong>t at which they decided they did not want <strong>to</strong> be dropouts orProgram descriptionStudent selection


518 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7erema<strong>in</strong> dropouts. Second, the students have adopted a proactive strategy <strong>in</strong> try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>improve their academic st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.ParticipantsSampleUseful descriptionDur<strong>in</strong>g the course of my <strong>in</strong>vestigation, 25 students (4 Hispanic females, 6 Hispanic males,1 Native American female, 9 White females, <strong>and</strong> 5 White males) were enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA.The average enrollment was approximately 20 <strong>to</strong> 22 students, dropp<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> 15 students a<strong>to</strong>ne po<strong>in</strong>t. Students ranged <strong>in</strong> age from 16 <strong>to</strong> 18. The program direc<strong>to</strong>r tries <strong>to</strong> reflect theschool’s dropout population with<strong>in</strong> the program population by match<strong>in</strong>g the percentageof ethnic <strong>and</strong> racial distribution of the school <strong>in</strong> the program.The students come from a wide range of ethnic <strong>and</strong> class backgrounds <strong>and</strong> havedramatically different experiences with school. Many of the students have one or moreparents who did not graduate from high school, whereas others have parents whohave postsecondary degrees. Students have a variety of obligations outside school life,such as work<strong>in</strong>g several jobs or rais<strong>in</strong>g children. Despite such varied backgrounds, themost common reason given for enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the program is lack of credits.F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsCause/effectThe formally stated goal of AAA is that the program helps students stay <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong>eventually graduate. The implicit assumption of AAA, however, is that the program actsas a safety net for students who otherwise would drop out of school al<strong>to</strong>gether. The programis the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs’ attempt <strong>to</strong> temper the problem of dropouts. They do notattempt <strong>to</strong> reta<strong>in</strong> all students at risk of dropp<strong>in</strong>g out nor do they actively seek students<strong>to</strong> enroll <strong>in</strong> the program. Instead, the responsibility for enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the program is left <strong>to</strong>the students.This has several effects. First, because the students enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA take measures<strong>to</strong> pursue their education, they treat the program as a privilege. For most students, thisprogram is their last chance for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a diploma or for acquir<strong>in</strong>g credits <strong>to</strong>ward adiploma. For these reasons, students as well as faculty treat students’ participation <strong>in</strong> theprogram differently than they treat participation <strong>in</strong> regular classes. The follow<strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsexplore the various components of the attitudes of students <strong>in</strong> AAA <strong>to</strong>ward thesetwo different environments. The names presented here are changed <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> theanonymity of the students <strong>in</strong> the program.Contrast<strong>in</strong>g Features of the Dropout Program <strong>and</strong> Regular ClassesEmergent categoriesAs I learned more about the situation <strong>in</strong> the dropout-prevention program, I found thatseveral concepts reflected the experiences of the students. These concepts, divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>five categories, constitute the components of the educational experience that contrastbetween the dropout program <strong>and</strong> comprehensive school or regular classes. The categoriesare as follows:1. The pursuit of credits <strong>and</strong> diplomas,2. The relationships between students <strong>and</strong> teachers (<strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs),3. The construction of time frames,4. Classroom management <strong>and</strong> work context, <strong>and</strong>5. The effects of rules <strong>and</strong> policies.These concepts relate <strong>to</strong> various features of the students’ everyday life experiences. Thedivid<strong>in</strong>g l<strong>in</strong>es drawn among these categories, however, are not necessarily sharply def<strong>in</strong>ed.I discuss these categories by po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g out the differences <strong>in</strong> students’ perceptions


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 519of their past experiences with regular classes <strong>and</strong> their present experience <strong>in</strong> AAA. Thefollow<strong>in</strong>g discussion describes these five categories.Credits <strong>and</strong> Diploma PursuitIn the dropout-prevention program, students do all their work on computers. Thiscomputer-assisted <strong>in</strong>struction seems <strong>to</strong> be analogous <strong>to</strong> the use of Au<strong>to</strong>mated TellerMach<strong>in</strong>es (ATMs), which expedite the process of gett<strong>in</strong>g cash. When I asked students <strong>to</strong> describethe program <strong>to</strong> me, I was <strong>to</strong>ld repeatedly, “You can get credits quicker.” The dropoutpreventionprogram thus is like an “ATM for diplomas.” This is an especially salient notion,consider<strong>in</strong>g the exclusive use of computer-assisted <strong>in</strong>struction, test<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> grad<strong>in</strong>g. The follow<strong>in</strong>gexamples illustrate the relative ease <strong>and</strong> speed with which students acquire credits.I’m not a rocket scientist, but it’s pretty easy. It’s just manipulation; just push thebut<strong>to</strong>ns. (Brad, <strong>in</strong>terview, 12/1/94)You get <strong>to</strong> work at your own pace. You can work faster than <strong>in</strong> regular school.(Tammy, <strong>in</strong>terview, 11/29/94)F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsGood examplesI like how you can earn credits faster <strong>in</strong> here. You can earn a lot of credits <strong>in</strong> asemester if you want <strong>to</strong>. (Anthony, field notes, 10/4/94)The technological advances <strong>and</strong> changes, particularly the use of computers <strong>in</strong> theclassroom, affect the way students view their own education. Students actually are us<strong>in</strong>gthe computers <strong>to</strong> withdraw an education, just as bank cus<strong>to</strong>mers use computers <strong>to</strong>withdraw their money. In essence, the students are the clients (consumers), <strong>and</strong> the teachersare the bank tellers (facilita<strong>to</strong>rs) who simply moni<strong>to</strong>r a computer-human <strong>in</strong>teraction. Thisphenomenon is similar <strong>to</strong> the concepts discussed by Ritzer <strong>in</strong> The McDonaldization of Society(1993), <strong>in</strong> which he suggests that organizational systems are designed <strong>to</strong> exert the maximumamount of efficiency, calculability, predictability, <strong>and</strong> control as possible, <strong>in</strong>vad<strong>in</strong>gevery aspect of society, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g education. Ritzer argues that the “replacement of humanwith nonhuman technology is very often motivated by a desire for greater control” <strong>and</strong> forlower costs for capitalism, aimed primarily at people because they are most often the hardest<strong>to</strong> control (p. 100).The students see themselves as ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g control when pursu<strong>in</strong>g a diploma <strong>in</strong> AAA,because they set the pace of their learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> they m<strong>in</strong>imize distractions. In contrast, afull seven-period class schedule is quite complicated. Students limit these complicationsby focus<strong>in</strong>g their attention on obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g one class credit at a time.Every student, except one, said he or she def<strong>in</strong>itely wanted <strong>to</strong> get a high-schooldiploma. When asked how a diploma would help them, however, few students stated clearreasons. Rather, the students saw gett<strong>in</strong>g a diploma as an accomplishment <strong>to</strong> strive for, agoal, “you just need it.” This demonstration of high ambitions, but with little <strong>in</strong>dication ofthe rewards education offers, is common. Anthony, a Hispanic male, is a transfer studentfrom Southern California, where he was heavily <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> gangs. His parents relocatedwith the hope that Anthony might graduate from high school—the first from his family.CG: So why do you want <strong>to</strong> get a diploma?Anthony: Because it’s good <strong>to</strong> have a diploma. You know, you gotta go a stepforward than a step beh<strong>in</strong>d. It’s good I guess.CG: What do you th<strong>in</strong>k it will help you with?Anthony: I don’t know if it will help but, like, it’s a goal that I have. It’s someth<strong>in</strong>gI want, not someth<strong>in</strong>g that somebody wants for me. It’s what I want. (<strong>in</strong>terview,1/20/95)InterpretationResults orInterpretation?F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsGood Examples


520 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnother example of this contradiction between student aspirations <strong>and</strong> potentialbenefits of educational achievement is illustrated by Tammy, a White female who workspart-time at a local drugs<strong>to</strong>re, <strong>and</strong> recently has been hospitalized for methamphetam<strong>in</strong>eaddiction <strong>and</strong> multiple eat<strong>in</strong>g disorders.My mom was like, “Tammy, if you don’t want <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong> school, I’ll go down there<strong>and</strong> withdraw you from school. You won’t ever have <strong>to</strong> go. You can go out <strong>and</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d a full-time job at McDonald’s or someth<strong>in</strong>g. You’re never go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> get anoffice job.” (Tammy, <strong>in</strong>terview, 11/29/94)ResultsTammy’s <strong>and</strong> her mother’s association of hav<strong>in</strong>g a high-school diploma with be<strong>in</strong>g able<strong>to</strong> have an “office job” <strong>in</strong>dicates the recognition of the marked dist<strong>in</strong>ction between theemployment opportunities of high-school dropouts <strong>and</strong> graduates.The students’ vague descriptions of what a diploma represents seem <strong>to</strong> express thegeneral division between the knowledge obta<strong>in</strong>ed or created <strong>in</strong> the classroom <strong>and</strong>the piece of paper symboliz<strong>in</strong>g that knowledge. This division emphasizes quantity overquality. I rarely heard students talk about what they were actually learn<strong>in</strong>g. In contrast,I repeatedly heard students talk about the amount of credits they were earn<strong>in</strong>g. Thisattitude <strong>to</strong>ward learn<strong>in</strong>g seems <strong>to</strong> occur <strong>in</strong> both the regular classes <strong>and</strong> the alternativeprogram.Student/Teacher Relationships<strong>How</strong> often?Good examplesThe students <strong>in</strong> AAA often discussed problems with past teachers. For example, Lisa, aWhite female, is one of the more vocal members of the program. She comes from a middleclassbackground, with both parents <strong>in</strong> the household <strong>and</strong> both college educated.Dur<strong>in</strong>g a group discussion with the counselor, students were air<strong>in</strong>g their grievancesabout a particular assessment practice at the school. This conversation led <strong>to</strong>general compla<strong>in</strong>ts of teachers <strong>and</strong> policies.A Black teacher <strong>to</strong>ld a student, “I can underst<strong>and</strong> why you don’t underst<strong>and</strong>this, because you come from a lower class.” This is exactly what the teacher <strong>to</strong>lda student, <strong>and</strong> I was offended by this statement. (Lisa, field notes, 11/3/94)Although Lisa does not come from a “lower class,” it was apparent from my <strong>in</strong>teractionwith her that she is well aware of the differential treatment “lower-class” studentsreceive from teachers, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> students. It is possible that her awareness ofthis unequal treatment has, <strong>in</strong> part, developed from her own marg<strong>in</strong>alized status <strong>in</strong>the dropout program.Deborah, a Hispanic female, provides another example of problematic studentteacherrelationships.I remember one teacher who said, “You’re never go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> graduate. You’re nogood.” Right <strong>in</strong> front of my class. That made me feel really bad. I just didn’tcare. (<strong>in</strong>terview, 2/24/95)Deborah has experienced ongo<strong>in</strong>g conflict with teachers regard<strong>in</strong>g her parental <strong>and</strong>m<strong>in</strong>ority status. Deborah has two children <strong>and</strong> has had difficulties f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g child carefor them. Some days, she will br<strong>in</strong>g her youngest <strong>to</strong> class with her when he has a coldbecause her day care does not accept children with illnesses.The last example of problematic student-teacher relationships comes from anotherfemale student who seems <strong>to</strong> be marg<strong>in</strong>alized by her status, not as a Hispanic or mother,but rather as the object of stigmatiz<strong>in</strong>g labels. (She <strong>to</strong>ld me that she has been called a“druggie” <strong>and</strong> a “slut” for her association with current <strong>and</strong> past boyfriends.)


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 521Dur<strong>in</strong>g a group discussion with the counselor, students were offer<strong>in</strong>g their op<strong>in</strong>ionsabout who they thought were bad teachers <strong>in</strong> the high school. Denise po<strong>in</strong>ted outthe power differential between students <strong>and</strong> teachers.If you don’t get along with a teacher, it’s more of a problem for the student,because the teachers are the ones giv<strong>in</strong>g out the credit. (field notes, 11/21/94)Students recognize the hierarchical relationship between themselves <strong>and</strong> theadults with<strong>in</strong> the school, but they do not always submit <strong>to</strong> the adult authority. Rather,students actively resist. The follow<strong>in</strong>g example demonstrates the role of student associal critic.Dur<strong>in</strong>g a group discussion, the counselor expla<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> the class that Jill’s teacherfrom the high school came <strong>to</strong> her compla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g that Jill had not been <strong>in</strong> class, after tell<strong>in</strong>gthe teacher she would be there. Jill replied <strong>to</strong> the counselor that she had <strong>to</strong>ld the teachershe would try <strong>to</strong> be there if she could.Only one exampleShe (the teacher) gets an attitude with me, so I’m go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> get one with her.(field notes, 11/29/94)In this situation, Jill, a Hispanic female, does not submit <strong>to</strong> the teacher’s authority; <strong>in</strong>stead,she ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>s her au<strong>to</strong>nomy by determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for herself when she will attend class.Furthermore, she uses her own language (gett<strong>in</strong>g an “attitude”) <strong>to</strong> negate the legitimacyof the teacher’s authority. Jill, like Deborah, is a mother, for whom negotiat<strong>in</strong>g daycare <strong>and</strong> educational activities is problematic. Unlike Deborah, Jill has more educatedparents, from middle-class backgrounds, who can provide additional support as well asadvocate for their daughter. This seems <strong>to</strong> mediate the difficulties of the student-teacherrelationships for Jill.Furthermore, Jill does not exhibit the same type of animosity <strong>to</strong>ward adults <strong>in</strong> AAAas she does <strong>in</strong> regular classes. She <strong>to</strong>ld me that she has gotten <strong>to</strong> know the teacher <strong>in</strong>AAA better than any of the teachers <strong>in</strong> regular school. When I asked her how this typeof relationship changes th<strong>in</strong>gs for her, she replied, “It probably makes you act better.You’re a better student.” This sentiment was common among students <strong>in</strong> AAA.Student-teacher relationships <strong>in</strong> AAA were quite <strong>in</strong>formal. The adult faculty <strong>in</strong>cludedthe primary teacher, a teach<strong>in</strong>g aide, the program direc<strong>to</strong>r, <strong>and</strong> the counselor.The teacher did not lead any students <strong>in</strong> lessons; <strong>in</strong>stead, he moni<strong>to</strong>red students’ workon the computer <strong>and</strong> assisted students when they had questions about the programs.I noticed immediately that students <strong>in</strong> AAA appear <strong>to</strong> get along well with the ma<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r. He has a casual personable style, shar<strong>in</strong>g much of his private life with thestudents, as well as show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> theirs.You all know I’m a little different than a normal teacher. I don’t have a problemwith these th<strong>in</strong>gs [br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g pagers or Walkman-type sound systems <strong>to</strong> school].I cover for you guys a lot, believe it or not. (Mr. Brown, field notes, 12/6/94)Aside from his personal relationships with each student <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>formal counsel<strong>in</strong>ghe gives them, his primary role as a teacher is <strong>to</strong> moni<strong>to</strong>r the students’ progressthrough the tu<strong>to</strong>rials, help<strong>in</strong>g them where needed. He also manages classroom policies,such as com<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> class on time, mak<strong>in</strong>g up missed days or hours, announc<strong>in</strong>g workbreaks, <strong>and</strong> runn<strong>in</strong>g the computer programs. These tasks are not typical of the duties <strong>in</strong>regular classes, where the teachers traditionally have prepared lectures or exercises withwhich they lead a group.In AAA, the role of the teacher is more that of a coach or a guide, with the pass<strong>in</strong>gof <strong>in</strong>formation done by the computers. The students def<strong>in</strong>e their relationship with the


522 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eteacher <strong>in</strong> very positive terms, unlike <strong>in</strong> the regular classes. The follow<strong>in</strong>g is an exampleof students’ perceptions of student-teacher <strong>in</strong>teraction that occurs <strong>in</strong> the class.Well he’s do<strong>in</strong>g a good job, but it’s, I mean, he loves it. He <strong>to</strong>ld us he loved us.He’s a good guy . . . He’s not got, every hour another 30 students. He’s got abetter relationship because he sees us all the time. (Don, <strong>in</strong>terview, 12/6/94)Cause/effectThis type of student perception of the teacher elim<strong>in</strong>ates many of the conflicts that thestudents compla<strong>in</strong>ed of when they were <strong>in</strong> regular classes.Time Frame ConstructionsSemesters, <strong>and</strong> the school year they constitute, are socially constructed time frames, his<strong>to</strong>ricallydesigned <strong>to</strong> accommodate families who needed their children <strong>to</strong> work dur<strong>in</strong>gthe harvest season. Problems arise when the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> end of a semester do not co<strong>in</strong>cidewith the students’ personal activities or events, such as travel<strong>in</strong>g, work<strong>in</strong>g, mov<strong>in</strong>gfrom another city, or hav<strong>in</strong>g a child.If students cannot start school at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the semester, they often mustskip the entire semester or take night school classes. For example, Jose, a Hispanic male,had spent the summer out of <strong>to</strong>wn at a state-run ranch for juvenile offenders. Whenasked why he enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA, Jose expla<strong>in</strong>ed that the reason was that he was notallowed <strong>to</strong> enroll <strong>in</strong> school 2 weeks late, not because of lack of credits.The reason I’m <strong>in</strong> AAA is because I was <strong>in</strong> . . . [the ranch], <strong>and</strong> when I came backmy parents wanted me <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>ish school. And, so they brought me back. But, whenthey brought me back, they [the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs] <strong>to</strong>ld me it was <strong>to</strong>o late. I eitherhad <strong>to</strong> wait until another semester or go <strong>to</strong> night school. I didn’t want <strong>to</strong> go <strong>to</strong>night school <strong>and</strong> I didn’t want <strong>to</strong> wait until another semester. And, so they <strong>to</strong>ldme <strong>to</strong> go talk <strong>to</strong> Mr. Smith [the program direc<strong>to</strong>r of AAA]. (<strong>in</strong>terview, 1/23/95)This student cont<strong>in</strong>ued <strong>to</strong> tell me that because he enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA, he could stay <strong>in</strong>school <strong>and</strong> eventually graduate early. This prospect was appeal<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> him, consider<strong>in</strong>gthat he <strong>and</strong> his girlfriend recently had a child. He could now pursue his goal of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g afull-time job <strong>to</strong> support his new family.In contrast <strong>to</strong> the rigid time frames of semester courses <strong>in</strong> regular classes, studentsare not strictly bound by time frames <strong>in</strong> the alternative program. Courses are not set up ona semester basis; rather, students can enter the program at any time dur<strong>in</strong>g regular semestertime frames <strong>and</strong> complete courses at their own pace. The length of time it takes <strong>to</strong> completea course is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the amount of time <strong>and</strong> effort they contribute. Studentstherefore set their own time frames. The only restriction is a 1-year maximum enrollment<strong>in</strong> the program, after which students either graduate or enter regular classes aga<strong>in</strong>.Negotiat<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>in</strong> accordance with these time frames often is difficult whenextenuat<strong>in</strong>g circumstances prohibit or merely h<strong>in</strong>der students from comply<strong>in</strong>g with policies.For example, Bel<strong>in</strong>da, a White female, is severely limited medically as a result of diabetes,<strong>and</strong> she is able <strong>to</strong> attend classes only sporadically, when she feels well enough. Ifshe were <strong>to</strong> rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> regular classes, she would be expelled with<strong>in</strong> the first few weeksof the semester because of the rule limit<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong> 11 absences. She def<strong>in</strong>es herselfas a productive student, yet because of her medical condition, she is restricted from participat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> an education she would otherwise value greatly. She says AAA has helpedher <strong>to</strong> pursue her goal of graduat<strong>in</strong>g from high school <strong>and</strong> go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> college. She also hasthe advantage of parents, both college educated, who can assist her <strong>in</strong> negotiat<strong>in</strong>g withteachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 523Daily time frames constructed <strong>in</strong> the school are also sites of negotiation. AAA m<strong>in</strong>imizesthe problems for most students, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those with jobs <strong>and</strong> children, because itcondenses all the school time <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> 4 hours, compared <strong>to</strong> 7 hours <strong>in</strong> regular classes. I wassurprised when I overheard students ask<strong>in</strong>g the teacher if he would come early <strong>in</strong> themorn<strong>in</strong>gs or on Saturdays so they could spend time on the computer. When asked, noneof the students said they would change anyth<strong>in</strong>g about the times classes are held <strong>in</strong>AAA. All the work<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong>ld me their job schedules worked well with the classschedules. Similarly, the parents <strong>in</strong> the program have <strong>to</strong> leave their children <strong>in</strong> a day carefor only 4 hours rather than 7.Classroom ManagementAn issue relat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the work context <strong>in</strong> AAA is the construction of power relations betweenthe ac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong> the sett<strong>in</strong>g. The purported goal of the alternative program is <strong>to</strong> providean opportunity for students <strong>to</strong> get credits <strong>in</strong> a different manner from that offered<strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream classes. The staff assumes that the alternative is better because studentsare self-directed <strong>and</strong> have freedom <strong>to</strong> work at their own pace. Because of this, students<strong>in</strong> AAA have more positive perceptions of the student-teacher relationship than do those<strong>in</strong> regular classes.Cause/effectYou work at your own pace. Brown doesn’t tell you you have <strong>to</strong> get this workdone at this time. It’s like you decide how much work you want <strong>to</strong> do. (Brad,<strong>in</strong>terview 12/1/94)Because of the computer-assisted <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>and</strong> self-directed strategy, students donot feel as though they are be<strong>in</strong>g judged or treated unfairly by a teacher, as they did <strong>in</strong> regularclasses. In AAA, however, students cannot always expect <strong>to</strong> be self-directed. This oftenleads <strong>to</strong> students be<strong>in</strong>g treated <strong>in</strong> similar ways as <strong>in</strong> the traditional classrooms. Classroommanagement by the teacher sometimes conflicts with the au<strong>to</strong>nomy of the students. Forexample, on several occasions, I overheard the teacher tell<strong>in</strong>g students that if the talk<strong>in</strong>gcont<strong>in</strong>ues, he would write them up (a discipl<strong>in</strong>ary measure). This was especially perplex<strong>in</strong>gbecause the teacher also had <strong>to</strong>ld the students recently that he wanted them <strong>to</strong> use teamwork<strong>to</strong> solve problems <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d answers <strong>to</strong> questions about their work on the tu<strong>to</strong>rials.Contradict<strong>in</strong>g situations such as these illustrate the malleable power relations with<strong>in</strong> classroomhierarchies, which are usually characterized by the teacher’s control over students. 3One student demonstrated a technique for manag<strong>in</strong>g such ambiguous situations. Jillknows that the teacher usually allows students <strong>to</strong> talk quietly among themselves. After shewas <strong>to</strong>ld by the teacher not <strong>to</strong> talk, she turned <strong>to</strong> me <strong>and</strong> said, “Mr. Brown is <strong>in</strong> a bad mood<strong>to</strong>day.” She did not accept the teacher’s immediate statement <strong>to</strong> be quiet; rather, sheaccepted this as merely a temporary situation, one that would change once the teachergot <strong>in</strong> a “better mood.” Decid<strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> act so as <strong>to</strong> be successful <strong>in</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g their waythrough the educational maze becomes a complex, if not difficult, task, especially forstudents who do not normally conform <strong>to</strong> the narrow st<strong>and</strong>ards of acceptable behavior.Cause/effectDataGood exampleInterpretationEffects of Rules <strong>and</strong> PoliciesThe policies of the high school <strong>and</strong> the procedures with<strong>in</strong> AAA promote a work<strong>in</strong>g-classideology. This is illustrated best by the procedure of the timecards for students, on whichthe teacher keeps track of the exact time students arrive <strong>and</strong> leave class. The <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>gfeature of us<strong>in</strong>g time cards <strong>in</strong> the classroom is that it reflects the atmosphere <strong>in</strong> low-pay<strong>in</strong>gjobs, such as manufactur<strong>in</strong>g or service-<strong>in</strong>dustry jobs (Anyon, 1980; Bowles & G<strong>in</strong>tis, 1976).Students <strong>in</strong> AAA focus more on the amount of time they put <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their work than they doInterpretationOther evidence?


524 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eon the quality of time they put <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their work. Perhaps educa<strong>to</strong>rs have the same low expectationsfor dropouts or potential dropouts as work<strong>in</strong>g-class employers have for theiremployees. 4 If students th<strong>in</strong>k teachers have low expectations of them, they might producelower-quality work <strong>and</strong> consequently “get by” without do<strong>in</strong>g the k<strong>in</strong>d of work they arecapable of do<strong>in</strong>g.The emphasis on attendance was discussed frequently among the students, whomI saw regularly look<strong>in</strong>g at the posted times on the dry erase board. The board <strong>in</strong>dicatedwhich students were fall<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> their hours. Furthermore, the teacher asks students<strong>to</strong> telephone him if they are go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be absent, rather than simply br<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g anexcuse note when they return.In a conversation dur<strong>in</strong>g their 10-m<strong>in</strong>ute break, several students expressed theirawareness of the school’s strict attendance policies (both <strong>in</strong> regular classes <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> AAA).This was illustrated by their surprise at my description of large college courses.Anthony: <strong>How</strong> do they take roll?CG: They don’t.Anthony: You mean they can come when they want? They don’t have <strong>to</strong> showup? Ah, but they paid for it, so they want <strong>to</strong> go. (field notes, 12/6/94)Good exampleResultsIt seemed that Anthony realized as he was speak<strong>in</strong>g that students are not obligated <strong>to</strong>attend school for the same reasons <strong>in</strong> college as they are <strong>in</strong> high school.Disparate attendance policies po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> a subtle difference <strong>in</strong> students’ perceptionsof attendance <strong>in</strong> AAA compared <strong>to</strong> regular classes. Based on self-reports, the students <strong>in</strong>AAA have a much higher attendance rate than they did <strong>in</strong> regular classes. Studentsrepeatedly <strong>to</strong>ld me s<strong>to</strong>ries of skipp<strong>in</strong>g regular classes with little concern for the consequences,despite the rule that students can miss only 11 days of a class before be<strong>in</strong>g dismissedfor the semester. Conversely, students <strong>in</strong> AAA place a great deal of importance oncom<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> class, <strong>and</strong> especially on mak<strong>in</strong>g up missed hours, because they have no otheroptions if removed from AAA. In a discussion of students’ attendance deficits, Jennie<strong>and</strong> Jill, both Hispanic females with children, expressed their concern for a close friend,Deborah, who is deal<strong>in</strong>g with some personal problems (break<strong>in</strong>g up with a drugaddictedboyfriend, be<strong>in</strong>g kicked out of her home, <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g child care for her children).Jennie <strong>and</strong> Jill are concerned that Deborah is gett<strong>in</strong>g beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> her work <strong>and</strong> have a moregeneral concern about other students mak<strong>in</strong>g it through the AAA program.Jennie <strong>and</strong> Jill felt that people who really care will come after lunch <strong>to</strong> make uptheir hours: “Deborah just doesn’t care.“ They gave Anthony as an example of someonewho is really dedicated. “He is <strong>in</strong> here every day after lunch” (field notes, 10/18/94).The students stress the importance of <strong>in</strong>dividual responsibility when expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g behavior,another attitude dist<strong>in</strong>ct from their explanations of regular classes. This theme wascommon <strong>in</strong> the students’ explanations of their failure <strong>to</strong> follow school policy, or for “mess<strong>in</strong>gup.” “In the case of drop-outs, s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>dividualism means that everyone has an equal chance<strong>to</strong> succeed irrespective of their personal or family circumstances, failure can only be attributed<strong>to</strong> deficiencies <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual” (Stevenson & Ellsworth, 1993, p. 270). The studentsimplicitly def<strong>in</strong>e the rules as “right” <strong>and</strong> the people not follow<strong>in</strong>g the rules as “wrong.”Bank<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Education</strong>Regard<strong>in</strong>g the education of illiterates <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong> America, Paulo Freire said that “educationis suffer<strong>in</strong>g from narration sickness” (1993, p. 52). Furthermore,The teacher talks about reality as if it were motionless, static, compartmentalized,<strong>and</strong> predictable. Or else he expounds on a <strong>to</strong>pic completely alien <strong>to</strong> the existential


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 525experience of the students. His task is <strong>to</strong> “fill” the students with the contents ofhis narration—contents which are detached from reality, disconnected from the<strong>to</strong>tality that engendered them <strong>and</strong> could f<strong>in</strong>d them significant. (Freire, 1993, p. 52)These characteristics of education seem <strong>to</strong> describe not only education <strong>in</strong> Third Worldcountries but also the experiences of the students I encountered <strong>in</strong> AAA.The founders of comprehensive education envisioned public schools as placeswhere the “skills of democracy can be practiced, debated, <strong>and</strong> analyzed” (Giroux, 1988,p. 114). <strong>Education</strong> <strong>in</strong> AAA is noth<strong>in</strong>g like this vision. Students are expected <strong>to</strong> passivelyabsorb <strong>in</strong>formation they receive via computer programs, which act as the teachers. Students,therefore, do not play an active role <strong>in</strong> the production of knowledge; rather, theycomply with the ideological assumption that knowledge is not necessarily created buttransferred from one source <strong>to</strong> another. This assumption arises from the conservative notionof education <strong>in</strong> which knowledge is a “s<strong>to</strong>rehouse of artifacts” (Giroux, 1988).Somewhere, a computer programmer <strong>in</strong>puts a specified body of knowledge that allegedly<strong>in</strong>cludes the basic core of <strong>in</strong>formation that high-school students should know.The students then unquestion<strong>in</strong>gly accept this <strong>in</strong>formation as “true.”The knowledge students obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> this sett<strong>in</strong>g is structured more by the goals ofma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g order, m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g complications, <strong>and</strong> maximiz<strong>in</strong>g efficiency. Giroux (1988)states:InterpretationStudent voice <strong>and</strong> experience is reduced <strong>to</strong> the immediacy of its performance <strong>and</strong>exists as someth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be measured, adm<strong>in</strong>istered, registered, <strong>and</strong> controlled. Itsdist<strong>in</strong>ctiveness, its disjunctions, its lived quality are all dissolved under an ideologyof control <strong>and</strong> management. In the name of efficiency, the resources <strong>and</strong>wealth of student life his<strong>to</strong>ries are generally ignored. (p. 122)The lack of student participation <strong>in</strong> the construction of knowledge <strong>in</strong> AAA is evidencedby students’ descriptions of the work they do: “I mean there’s times I get bored with itbecause you get the same chair, the same screen, the same stuff every day.”The elim<strong>in</strong>ation of distractions <strong>and</strong> complicated <strong>in</strong>teractions makes the experiencesimpler; however, students also compla<strong>in</strong> that this lack of <strong>in</strong>teraction is frustrat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> bor<strong>in</strong>g. Students <strong>in</strong> AAA do not actively engage <strong>in</strong> the production ofknowledge by discuss<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> critically th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about ideas. By controll<strong>in</strong>g the paceof learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the amount of effort exerted, the students feel empowered. One studentsays, “I’m learn<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>in</strong> here, because this way I don’t really have a choice. Youcan’t just get up <strong>and</strong> walk out. I mean I could, but I don’t want <strong>to</strong>, because nobody istell<strong>in</strong>g me what <strong>to</strong> do.”Although the students do not participate <strong>in</strong> knowledge production, they have resistedthe status quo by leav<strong>in</strong>g the ma<strong>in</strong>stream classes <strong>and</strong> enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an alternativeeducational sett<strong>in</strong>g. By disengag<strong>in</strong>g from regular classes, they act as nonconformists, associal critics (F<strong>in</strong>e, 1991). They acquire a sense of ownership over their education, whichalso engenders a sense of power over their own lives. They have positive attitudes <strong>to</strong>wardthe program <strong>and</strong> “stick with it” because they feel, ironically, privileged for be<strong>in</strong>gable <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the program. They appreciate this second chance.CONCLUSIONSThe designers of AAA seem <strong>to</strong> have created an effective alternative for help<strong>in</strong>g at-riskstudents rema<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> school <strong>and</strong> acquire credits <strong>to</strong>ward a high-school diploma. Accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>the students, the program is a successful alternative. The students suggested both implicitly<strong>and</strong> explicitly that without AAA, they would not be <strong>in</strong> school. The program, however,


526 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eNot clear <strong>to</strong> usTrue of sample?Need dataWe agree.does little <strong>to</strong> help students create <strong>and</strong> apply practical knowledge <strong>in</strong> their everyday lives,nor does it help students <strong>to</strong> acquire the skills for either fitt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the dom<strong>in</strong>ant cultureor surviv<strong>in</strong>g as resisters of that culture. Furthermore, because the program is voluntary<strong>and</strong> has a limited enrollment, it is not catch<strong>in</strong>g every student who drops out. In these respects,it may be viewed as a dump<strong>in</strong>g ground.Students enroll<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> programs such as AAA often have problems <strong>in</strong>teract<strong>in</strong>g withothers <strong>in</strong> the conventional sett<strong>in</strong>g (Gold & Mann, 1982). These problems may stem from,among other reasons, lack of skill at negotiat<strong>in</strong>g with teachers or lack of compliancewith the rules of the classroom. If educa<strong>to</strong>rs provide an alternative sett<strong>in</strong>g for studentslack<strong>in</strong>g the social skills <strong>to</strong> get by <strong>in</strong> regular classes, then it seems counterproductive <strong>to</strong>place these students <strong>in</strong> a sett<strong>in</strong>g where they will not learn how <strong>to</strong> improve those skills.In essence, these programs act as hold<strong>in</strong>g tanks for those who do not “fit <strong>in</strong>” so thatconflict is reduced <strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream classes.Many alternative programs, particularly dropout-prevention programs, purport <strong>to</strong>offer students true alternatives <strong>to</strong> traditional classes or schools. To provide useful <strong>in</strong>formation<strong>to</strong> educa<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> researchers for adapt<strong>in</strong>g programs or creat<strong>in</strong>g new ones <strong>to</strong>better serve students, it would be beneficial <strong>to</strong> know whether a program is truly analternative or just a modification of the traditional format. Students <strong>in</strong> AAA def<strong>in</strong>e theprogram as markedly different from regular classes, primarily because of the breakdownof power relations between teachers <strong>and</strong> students. In contrast, the students describe thelearn<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>in</strong> AAA <strong>and</strong> regular classes similarly—the focus is on credits <strong>and</strong> thepassive absorption of <strong>in</strong>formation.Educa<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs of urban youth face a variety of challenges <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>ggenu<strong>in</strong>e educational alternatives. The larger problem stems from structural issuesrather than <strong>in</strong>dividual student issues (i.e., current economic structures <strong>in</strong> a post<strong>in</strong>dustrialera limit<strong>in</strong>g the job, health, <strong>and</strong> hous<strong>in</strong>g opportunities of urban youth).While many view schools <strong>and</strong> education as the keys <strong>to</strong> solv<strong>in</strong>g the problems ofdrugs, crime, poverty, <strong>and</strong> the myriad of urban ills, the opposite is more thereality: The success of urban schools is dependent upon our nation’s health,crim<strong>in</strong>al justice, economic, <strong>and</strong> other systems. (Raffel, Boyd, Briggs, Eubanks, &Fern<strong>and</strong>ez, 1992, p. 264)We must assume, nevertheless, that schools can make a difference for urban youth.Based on my <strong>in</strong>teraction with adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> teachers <strong>in</strong>volved with one alternativeprogram, it appears that educa<strong>to</strong>rs are limited <strong>in</strong> the types of alternatives theycan provide by their own lack of vision. We commonly hear compla<strong>in</strong>ts about district regulationsor other entities govern<strong>in</strong>g curriculum, staff<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> other matters (see Raffelet al., 1992). Without chang<strong>in</strong>g the educational strategies, however, we can expect thesame outcomes. In other words, if we do not alter the current practices, or at least offerstudents true alternatives, students will be alienated, disengage from schools, <strong>and</strong> dropout at the same rates, or more so. In discuss<strong>in</strong>g the reform of urban education, Lytle(1990) calls for a reth<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of educational practices that currently do not seem <strong>to</strong> bework<strong>in</strong>g for a grow<strong>in</strong>g number of youth.Even the most vocal of the advocates [of urban school reform] accept urbanschools <strong>in</strong> their present form; improvement is considered a matter of modify<strong>in</strong>gstaff<strong>in</strong>g, policy, <strong>and</strong> resource allocation practices. None of the advocates enterta<strong>in</strong>sthe possibility that urban schools as currently organized might be <strong>in</strong>appropriatefor educat<strong>in</strong>g disadvantaged students. (p. 219)Along these same l<strong>in</strong>es, provid<strong>in</strong>g alternative programs such as AAA seems <strong>to</strong> bea b<strong>and</strong>-aid approach <strong>to</strong> “solv<strong>in</strong>g the dropout problem.” Although it does appear


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 527successful at reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g students <strong>and</strong> aid<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g credits, it is unclearwhether it can give students the educational experiences needed <strong>to</strong> successfully copewith their problematic circumstances.To underst<strong>and</strong> better the challenges of creat<strong>in</strong>g true alternatives, we could greatlybenefit from research that exam<strong>in</strong>es the way alternative programs with<strong>in</strong> school systems<strong>in</strong>itially get created. Unfortunately, research <strong>in</strong> this area is <strong>in</strong>adequate. Furthermore, becauseI focused narrowly on student perceptions of their situation, I did not gather anysubstantial data on adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs’ perceptions. Acquir<strong>in</strong>g adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs’ views may aid <strong>in</strong>underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs’ <strong>in</strong>tentions <strong>and</strong>, thus, the dynamics of work<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> a se<strong>to</strong>f policies. Without critical analysis of school policies, little is done <strong>to</strong> promote the bestpossible environment for students <strong>to</strong> learn <strong>and</strong> become contribut<strong>in</strong>g members of society.Regard<strong>in</strong>g the usefulness of my f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, I th<strong>in</strong>k several issues are important. First,data obta<strong>in</strong>ed for my analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation came from a sample of 25 students <strong>and</strong>a few faculty members. A better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the situation could be obta<strong>in</strong>ed bygather<strong>in</strong>g more <strong>in</strong>formation from family members <strong>and</strong> from other students <strong>and</strong> teachers<strong>in</strong> regular classes, or by <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g other programs <strong>in</strong> other schools. Second, such asmall sample <strong>and</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle-case study limit the generalizability of the results.F<strong>in</strong>ally, study<strong>in</strong>g the longitud<strong>in</strong>al effects of participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> programs such as AAAcould greatly <strong>in</strong>form this discussion. Longitud<strong>in</strong>al data would enable a better underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof the degree <strong>to</strong> which students f<strong>in</strong>d the knowledge they ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> a programpractical, <strong>and</strong> of whether the students cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> experience the success, both academic<strong>and</strong> personal, they described hav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> AAA.<strong>Research</strong> on high-school dropouts <strong>and</strong> the programs designed <strong>to</strong> help this populationneed more attention. This project demonstrates the need <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e not only the characteristicsof students at risk of dropp<strong>in</strong>g out but also the perceptions <strong>and</strong> experiences of students<strong>and</strong> faculty. The <strong>in</strong>teraction among ac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>in</strong> public schools provides <strong>in</strong>valuable <strong>in</strong>formationfor underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the problems with<strong>in</strong> public schools, <strong>and</strong> it should not be overlooked.We agree.Suggestions forfuture researchRecognizeslimitationsNotes1. Prevent<strong>in</strong>g students from dropp<strong>in</strong>g out becomes <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly salientat the high school level because of the <strong>in</strong>creased visibility of the problemsresult<strong>in</strong>g from leav<strong>in</strong>g school early (Natriello, 1996). These programs,therefore, are aimed primarily at secondary students.2. I have changed the name of this program <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> anonymity for thesubjects of this study.3. Ritzer (1993) discusses this notion of control <strong>in</strong> educational sett<strong>in</strong>gs.He states:Not only are students taught <strong>to</strong> be obedient <strong>to</strong> authority, butalso <strong>to</strong> be receptive <strong>to</strong> the rationalized procedures of rotelearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> objective test<strong>in</strong>g. More important, spontaneity <strong>and</strong>creativity tend not be rewarded, <strong>and</strong> may even be discouraged,lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> what one expert calls “education for docility.” (p. 115)4. This type of treatment may be occurr<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the regular classes of thehigh school as well, but without extend<strong>in</strong>g this project <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e thosesett<strong>in</strong>gs, it is impossible <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e.ReferencesAnyon, J. (1980). Social class <strong>and</strong> the hidden curriculumof work. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>, 162: 67–92.Ascher, C. (1982). ERIC/CUE: Alternative schools—someanswers <strong>and</strong> questions. The Urban Review, 14: 65–69.Bowles, S., & G<strong>in</strong>tis, H. (1976). School<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> capitalistAmerica: <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> the contradictions of economiclife. New York: Basic Books.Catterall, J. S. (1985). A process model of dropp<strong>in</strong>g out ofschool: Implications for research <strong>and</strong> policy <strong>in</strong> an era ofraised academic st<strong>and</strong>ards (Grant No. OERIG860003).Los Angeles: University of California, Los Angeles,Center for the Study of Evaluation. (ERIC DocumentReproduction Service No. ED 271 837)Catterall, J. S., & Stern, D. (1986). The effects of alternative schoolprograms on high school completion <strong>and</strong> labor market outcomes.<strong>Education</strong>al Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Policy Analysis, 8: 77–86.Coll<strong>in</strong>s, R. L. (1987). Parents’ views of alternative publicschool programs: Implications for the use of alternativeprograms <strong>to</strong> reduce school dropout rates. <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong>Urban Society, 19: 290–302.


528 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eDeal, T. E., & Nolan, R. R. (1978). Alternative schools: Aconceptual map. School Review, 87(1): 29–49.Dei, G. J. S. (1996). Black youth <strong>and</strong> fad<strong>in</strong>g out of school. InD. Kelly & J. Gaskell (Eds.), Debat<strong>in</strong>g dropouts: Criticalpolicy <strong>and</strong> research perspectives on school leav<strong>in</strong>g(pp. 173–189). New York: Teachers College Record.Duke, D. L., & Perry, C. (1978). Can alternative schoolssucceed where Benjam<strong>in</strong> Spock, Spiro Agnew, <strong>and</strong>B. F. Sk<strong>in</strong>ner have failed? Adolescence, 13: 375–392.Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative methods <strong>in</strong> research onteach<strong>in</strong>g. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of researchon teach<strong>in</strong>g (3rd ed., pp. 119–161). New York: Macmillan.F<strong>in</strong>e, M. (1991). Fram<strong>in</strong>g dropouts: Notes on the politics ofan urban public high school. Albany: State University ofNew York Press.Frankl<strong>in</strong>, C. (1992). Alternative school programs for at-riskyouth. Social Work <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>, 14: 239–251.Freire, P. (1993). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York:Cont<strong>in</strong>uum.Giroux, H. A. (1988). School<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the struggle for publiclife. M<strong>in</strong>neapolis: University of M<strong>in</strong>nesota Press.Gold, M., & Mann, D. W. (1982). Alternative schools fortroublesome secondary students. The Urban Review,14: 305–316.Kelly, D. M. (1993). Last chance high: <strong>How</strong> girls <strong>and</strong> boysdrop <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> out of alternative schools. New Haven,CT: Yale University Press.Kelly, D. (1996). “Choos<strong>in</strong>g” the alternative: Conflict<strong>in</strong>gmissions <strong>and</strong> constra<strong>in</strong>ed choice <strong>in</strong> a dropout preventionprogram. In D. Kelly & J. Gaskell (Eds.), Debat<strong>in</strong>gdropouts: Critical policy <strong>and</strong> research perspectives onschool leav<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 101–122). New York: Teachers CollegeRecord.LeCompte, M. D. (1987). The cultural context of dropp<strong>in</strong>gout: Why remedial programs fail <strong>to</strong> solve the problem.<strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong> Urban Society, 19(3): 232–249.Lev<strong>in</strong>, H. M. (1972). The costs <strong>to</strong> the nation of <strong>in</strong>adequateeducation. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: U.S. Senate Select Committeeon Equal <strong>Education</strong> Opportunity.Lytle, J. H. (1990). Reform<strong>in</strong>g urban education: A review ofrecent reports <strong>and</strong> legislation. The Urban Review, 22(3):199–220.Natriello, G. (1996). Evaluation processes <strong>and</strong> student disengagementfrom high school. Sociology of <strong>Education</strong> <strong>and</strong>Socialization, 11: 147–172.Raffel, J. A., Boyd, W. L., Briggs, V. M., Eubanks, E. E., &Fern<strong>and</strong>ez, R. (1992). Policy dilemmas <strong>in</strong> urbaneducation: Address<strong>in</strong>g the needs of poor, at-risk children.Journal of Urban Affairs, 14(3–4): 263–289.Reilly, D. H., & Reilly, J. L. (1983). Alternative schools: Issues<strong>and</strong> directions. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 10(2):89–98.Ritzer, G. (1993). The McDonaldization of society. Thous<strong>and</strong>Oaks, CA: P<strong>in</strong>e Forge.Stevenson, R. B., & Ellsworth, J. (1993). Dropouts <strong>and</strong> thesilenc<strong>in</strong>g of critical voices. In L. Weis & M. F<strong>in</strong>e (Eds.),Beyond silenced voices: Class, race, <strong>and</strong> gender <strong>in</strong> UnitedStates schools (pp. 259–271). New York: State Universityof New York Press.Wehlage, G. G., Rutter, R. A., Smith, G. A., Lesko, N., &Fern<strong>and</strong>ez, R. R. (1989). Reduc<strong>in</strong>g the risk: Schools ascommunities of support. London: Falmer.Analysis of the StudyPURPOSEThe purpose is not clearly stated. By <strong>in</strong>ference, it is <strong>to</strong>clarify whether urban dropout-prevention programs areeffective alternatives <strong>to</strong> regular classes or simply“dump<strong>in</strong>g grounds for unwanted students.”JUSTIFICATIONThe justification is extensive <strong>and</strong> usually it is documented.Po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong>clude: negative effects of dropp<strong>in</strong>gout, the need for <strong>and</strong> variety of alternative programs, theimpact of the broader society, the paucity <strong>and</strong> poorquality of previous research, the tendency <strong>to</strong> blame thestudents, <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g diversity of students, <strong>and</strong> the allegationthat the programs are “dump<strong>in</strong>g grounds.”With regard <strong>to</strong> ethical issues, we see no problems ofrisk. Presumably, confidentiality was ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed. Itwould be helpful <strong>to</strong> know what participants were <strong>to</strong>ldregard<strong>in</strong>g the purpose of the study <strong>and</strong> potential uses ofthe f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. The author’s values are evident, as are“social consequences.” We th<strong>in</strong>k these are generallyclear <strong>to</strong> the reader.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 529DEFINITIONSNo def<strong>in</strong>itions are provided. The term dropout is probablysufficiently unders<strong>to</strong>od, though there are variations <strong>in</strong>usage. In this study, it apparently was taken <strong>to</strong> mean studentswho “have either dropped out of school or were <strong>in</strong>the process of dropp<strong>in</strong>g out at the time of the enrollment<strong>in</strong> the program.” Other key terms, some emerg<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>gthe study, as is common <strong>in</strong> such studies, are: effectivealternative, problematic student-teacher relationship,negotiat<strong>in</strong>g behavior, bank<strong>in</strong>g education, <strong>and</strong> practicalknowledge. Although these may be clear from the discussion<strong>and</strong> examples, def<strong>in</strong>itions would help.PRIOR RESEARCHMost prior research is <strong>in</strong>cluded as justification or discussion.The work of the “few researchers (who) havestudied the <strong>in</strong>teractional dynamics between students <strong>and</strong>teachers <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the effectiveness of the programs”should have been reported.HYPOTHESESAs is typical <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research, no hypotheses arestated at the outset, nor do any emerge dur<strong>in</strong>g the study.The tenor of the report, particularly the first three <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>rysections, suggests that a hypothesis could havebeen stated: Dropout programs are more like dump<strong>in</strong>ggrounds than effective alternatives <strong>to</strong> regular classes.SAMPLEThe purposive sample consisted of 25 students <strong>and</strong> theirteacher, aide, counselor, <strong>and</strong> program direc<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> a particularalternative program. Age <strong>and</strong> ethnicity of studentsare given. Students <strong>in</strong> the program must have activelysought entry <strong>and</strong> were selected from a wait<strong>in</strong>g list basedon potential benefit <strong>to</strong> them <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> match the ethnic characteristicsof the school.INSTRUMENTATIONThe author states that “participant observation” was themethod, though it is not always clear how participationoccurred. The primary source of <strong>in</strong>formation used <strong>in</strong>relation <strong>to</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs is <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>in</strong>terviews. No evidenceon reliability <strong>and</strong> validity is presented, though itappears that check<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation across differentsources (triangulation) was possible. The <strong>to</strong>pics addressed<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terviews are provided.PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYLittle procedural detail is given beyond “weekly <strong>and</strong>daily” (?) site visits (how many?), observ<strong>in</strong>g everydayactivities, attend<strong>in</strong>g biweekly “focus group” meet<strong>in</strong>gsled by the counselor, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual recorded <strong>in</strong>terviews.The program studied is described as consist<strong>in</strong>g entirelyof computer work. Descriptions of class activities <strong>and</strong>the role of the teacher are provided.Internal validity is largely irrelevant because relationshipswere not directly studied. Some, however, are<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs section <strong>and</strong> must be considered <strong>in</strong>the absence of clear-cut controls or data.All are subject <strong>to</strong>possible threats due <strong>to</strong> data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics<strong>and</strong>/orbias,subjectcharacteristics(e.g.,self-<strong>in</strong>sight),<strong>and</strong>subject attitude.DATA ANALYSISAs is common <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research, no formalanalysis was done; <strong>in</strong>stead a narrative is provided withexamples. Organization under five categories is helpful.More quantification, as is done with respect <strong>to</strong> “want<strong>in</strong>ga diploma” (all but one), would be helpful. For example,how many of the 25 students felt they were treatedfairly?RESULTS/DISCUSSIONResults are presented as “f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs” supported <strong>in</strong> part byexamples, <strong>in</strong> part by assertions such as “most students,”<strong>and</strong> partly by reference <strong>to</strong> other writers. It is virtually impossible<strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish results from <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Use ofexamples is often helpful <strong>in</strong> clarification, but the readermust trust that the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs accurately reflect the <strong>in</strong>formation.In some cases, <strong>in</strong>terpretation goes far beyond thedata, <strong>and</strong> this may not be clear <strong>to</strong> the reader. For example,although we agree that “bank<strong>in</strong>g education” is contrary <strong>to</strong>the goals of comprehensive education <strong>and</strong> that this programcannot prepare students for everyday life, theseop<strong>in</strong>ions do not follow closely from the <strong>in</strong>formation provided<strong>and</strong>, <strong>in</strong> our op<strong>in</strong>ion, require better argumentation.Some f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs describe cause-<strong>and</strong>-effect relationships<strong>and</strong> often are open <strong>to</strong> question. Examples are: “becausethe students enrolled <strong>in</strong> AAA take measures <strong>to</strong> pursuetheir education, they treat the program as a privilege”; “ifeduca<strong>to</strong>rs provide an alternative sett<strong>in</strong>g for students lack<strong>in</strong>gthe social skills <strong>to</strong> get by <strong>in</strong> regular classes [not, weth<strong>in</strong>k, demonstrated for these students], then it seemscounterproductive <strong>to</strong> place these students <strong>in</strong> a sett<strong>in</strong>gwhere they will not learn how <strong>to</strong> improve these skills”; theconclusion that “the policies of the high school <strong>and</strong> theprocedures with<strong>in</strong> AAA promote a work<strong>in</strong>g-class ideology”is supported only by evidence on use of timecards. This study illustrates both the richness of such


530 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eresearch <strong>and</strong> the difficulty <strong>in</strong> arriv<strong>in</strong>g at clear-cutconclusions. The author properly recognizes limitationsdue <strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> scope of data gather<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> recommendsa longitud<strong>in</strong>al approach for future research. Wewould add that the study also suggests many <strong>to</strong>pics formore direct <strong>in</strong>vestigation.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsTHE NATURE AND VALUE OF ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHEthnographic research is particularly appropriate for behaviors that are best unders<strong>to</strong>odby observ<strong>in</strong>g them with<strong>in</strong> their natural sett<strong>in</strong>gs.The key techniques <strong>in</strong> all ethnographic studies are <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> highlydetailed, almost cont<strong>in</strong>ual, ongo<strong>in</strong>g participant observation of a situation.A key strength of ethnographic research is that it provides the researcher with a muchmore comprehensive perspective than do other forms of educational research.ETHNOGRAPHIC CONCEPTSImportant concepts <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research <strong>in</strong>clude culture, holistic perspective,thick description, contextualization, a nonjudgmental orientation, emic perspective,etic perspective, member check<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> multiple realities.TOPICS THAT LEND THEMSELVES WELLTO ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHThese <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>to</strong>pics that defy simple quantification; those that can best be unders<strong>to</strong>od<strong>in</strong> a natural sett<strong>in</strong>g; those that <strong>in</strong>volve study<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual or group activities over time;those that <strong>in</strong>volve study<strong>in</strong>g the roles that <strong>in</strong>dividuals play <strong>and</strong> the behaviors associatedwith those roles; those that <strong>in</strong>volve study<strong>in</strong>g the activities <strong>and</strong> behaviors of groups as aunit; <strong>and</strong> those that <strong>in</strong>volve study<strong>in</strong>g formal organizations <strong>in</strong> their <strong>to</strong>tality.SAMPLING IN ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHThe sample <strong>in</strong> ethnographic studies is almost always purposive.The data obta<strong>in</strong>ed from ethnographic research samples rarely, if ever, permit generalization<strong>to</strong> a population.THE USE OF HYPOTHESES IN ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHEthnographic researchers seldom formulate precise hypotheses ahead of time.Rather, they develop them as their study emerges.


CHAPTER 21 Ethnographic <strong>Research</strong> 531DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS IN ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHThe two major means of data collection <strong>in</strong> ethnographic research are participan<strong>to</strong>bservation <strong>and</strong> detailed <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g.<strong>Research</strong>ers use a variety of <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong> ethnographic studies <strong>to</strong> collect data <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong> check validity. This is frequently referred <strong>to</strong> as triangulation.Analysis consists of cont<strong>in</strong>ual rework<strong>in</strong>g of data with emphasis on patterns, keyevents, <strong>and</strong> use of visual representations <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> observations.FIELDWORKField notes are the notes a researcher <strong>in</strong> an ethnographic study takes <strong>in</strong> the field. They<strong>in</strong>clude both descriptive field notes (what he or she sees <strong>and</strong> hears) <strong>and</strong> reflectivefield notes (what he or she th<strong>in</strong>ks about what has been observed).Field jott<strong>in</strong>gs refer <strong>to</strong> quick notes about someth<strong>in</strong>g the researcher wants <strong>to</strong> write moreabout later.A field diary is a personal statement of the researcher’s feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> op<strong>in</strong>ions aboutthe people <strong>and</strong> situations he or she is observ<strong>in</strong>g.A field log is a sort of runn<strong>in</strong>g account of how the researcher plans <strong>to</strong> spend his or hertime compared <strong>to</strong> how he or she actually spends it.ADVANTAGES AND DISADVANTAGES OF ETHNOGRAPHIC RESEARCHA key strength of ethnographic research is that it provides a much more comprehensiveperspective than other forms of educational research. It lends itself well <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>picsthat are not easily quantified. Also, it is particularly appropriate for study<strong>in</strong>g behaviorsbest unders<strong>to</strong>od <strong>in</strong> their natural sett<strong>in</strong>gs.Like all research, ethnographic research also has its limitations. It is highlydependent on the particular researcher’s observations. Furthermore, some observerbias is almost impossible <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate. Lastly, generalization is practicallynonexistent.contextualization 503crystallization 512culture 503descriptive fieldnotes 507emic perspective 504ethnographicresearch 501etic perspective 504field diary 506field jott<strong>in</strong>gs 506field log 507field notes 506holistic perspective 503<strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g 506key events 511member check<strong>in</strong>g 504multiple realities/perspectives 504participan<strong>to</strong>bservation 506reflective fieldnotes 507thick description 504triangulation 510Key Terms1. A major criticism of ethnographic research is that there is no way for the researcher<strong>to</strong> be <strong>to</strong>tally objective about what he or she observes. Would you agree? What mightan ethnographer say <strong>to</strong> rebut this charge?2. Ethnographic studies are rarely replicated. Why do you suppose this is so? Mightthey be? If so, how?3. What would you say is the most difficult aspect of ethnographic research? Why?For Discussion


532 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e4. What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the biggest advantage of ethnographic research? the biggestdisadvantage? Expla<strong>in</strong> your th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g.5. Would you be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be a participant <strong>in</strong> an ethnographic study? Why or why not?6. Supporters of qualitative research say that it can do someth<strong>in</strong>g that no other typeof research can do. If true, what might this be? Would this be especially true ofethnography?7. Are there any k<strong>in</strong>ds of <strong>in</strong>formation that other types of research can provide betterthan ethnographic research? If so, what might they be?8. <strong>How</strong> would you compare ethnographic research <strong>to</strong> the other types of research wehave discussed <strong>in</strong> this book <strong>in</strong> terms of difficulty? Expla<strong>in</strong> your reason<strong>in</strong>g.Notes1. H. R. Bernard (1994). <strong>Research</strong> methods <strong>in</strong> cultural anthropology, 2nd ed. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, p. 137.2. H. F. Wolcott (1966). Quoted <strong>in</strong> J. R. Creswell (2002). <strong>Education</strong>al research: Plann<strong>in</strong>g, conduct<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>evaluat<strong>in</strong>g qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative research. Columbus, OH: Merrill Prentice-Hall, p. 60.3. G. McPherson (1972). Small-<strong>to</strong>wn teacher. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.4. F. Erickson <strong>and</strong> G. Mohatt (1982). Cultural organization of participation structures <strong>in</strong> two classroomsof Indian students. In G. Sp<strong>in</strong>dler (1982), Do<strong>in</strong>g the ethnography of school<strong>in</strong>g. New York: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart &W<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>n, pp. 132–175.5. C. R. F<strong>in</strong>nan (1982). The ethnography of children’s play. In G. Sp<strong>in</strong>dler, op. cit., pp. 356–380.6. S. B. Palonsky (1975). Hempies <strong>and</strong> squeaks, truckers <strong>and</strong> cruisers: A participant observer study <strong>in</strong> a cityhigh school. <strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istration Quarterly, 11(2): 86–103.7. C. Ellis (1995) The other side of the fence: See<strong>in</strong>g black <strong>and</strong> white <strong>in</strong> a small southern <strong>to</strong>wn. QualitativeInquiry, 1(2): 147–167.8. P. A. Cusick (1973). Inside high school: The student’s perspective. New York: Holt, R<strong>in</strong>ehart & W<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>n.9. H. S. Becker, B. Greer, E. C. Hughes, <strong>and</strong> A. Strauss (1961). Boys <strong>in</strong> white: Student culture <strong>in</strong> medicalschool. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10. E. Babbie (1998). The practice of social research, 8th ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, pp. 241–242.11. M. Harris (1968). The rise of anthropological theory. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell, p. 16.12. David M. Fetterman (1989). Ethnography: Step by step. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage, p. 27.13. Ibid.14. Ibid., p. 28.15. Ibid., p. 31.16. Ibid., p. 33.17. Ibid., p. 45.18. Ibid., pp. 46–47.19. For a good description of field notes, see Chapter 4 <strong>in</strong> R. C. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> S. K. Biklen (2007). Qualitativeresearch <strong>in</strong> education: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> theory <strong>and</strong> practice, 5th ed. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon.20. Bernard, op. cit., pp. 181–186.21. Bernard, op. cit., p. 185.22. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> Biklen, op. cit., p. 120.23. Ibid., excerpted from pp. 260–270.24. Fetterman, op. cit., pp. 91–92.25. Ibid., p. 93.26. Ibid., p. 101.27. From the familiar <strong>to</strong> the strange <strong>and</strong> back aga<strong>in</strong>. In G. Sp<strong>in</strong>dler, op. cit.


His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>22"I’m analyz<strong>in</strong>gearly records <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e when men <strong>and</strong>women first startedhav<strong>in</strong>g disagreementsabout th<strong>in</strong>gs.""Good luck!I assume you’restart<strong>in</strong>g withGenesis?"OBJECTIVES Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly what his<strong>to</strong>rical research Dist<strong>in</strong>guish between external <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal<strong>in</strong>volves.criticism.State three purposes of his<strong>to</strong>rical research. Discuss when generalization <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalGive some examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds ofresearch is appropriate.questions <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalLocate examples of published his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch.studies <strong>and</strong> critique some of the strengthsName <strong>and</strong> describe briefly the major steps <strong>and</strong> weaknesses of these studies.<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research.Recognize an example of a his<strong>to</strong>rical studyGive some examples of his<strong>to</strong>rical sources. when you come across one <strong>in</strong> theDist<strong>in</strong>guish between primary <strong>and</strong>literature.secondary sources.What Is His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>?The Purposes of His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>What K<strong>in</strong>ds of Questions ArePursued Through His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>?Steps Involved <strong>in</strong>His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the ProblemLocat<strong>in</strong>g Relevant SourcesSummariz<strong>in</strong>g InformationObta<strong>in</strong>ed from His<strong>to</strong>ricalSourcesEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g His<strong>to</strong>rical SourcesData Analysis <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>Generalization <strong>in</strong>His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Advantages <strong>and</strong>Disadvantages ofHis<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>An Example ofHis<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisResults/Discussion


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Primary vs. Secondary SourcesAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 22.1: His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 22.2: Primary or Secondary Source?Activity 22.3: What K<strong>in</strong>d of His<strong>to</strong>rical Source?Activity 22.4: True or False?Hey Becky!”“Hey, Brent, where ya’ been?”“Library. Try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> come up with a <strong>to</strong>pic for my research study.”“<strong>How</strong>’s it go<strong>in</strong>g?”“Pretty good. I th<strong>in</strong>k I have an idea. You know, they want <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduce this new read<strong>in</strong>g program—sort of amodified ‘look-say’ approach—<strong>in</strong> some of the elementary schools <strong>in</strong> our district next year, but I sort of have mydoubts about it.”“<strong>How</strong> come?”“Well, the adm<strong>in</strong>istration keeps prais<strong>in</strong>g it <strong>to</strong> the skies, but I haven’t seen any evidence that it will work anybetter than the program we now use. Plus it’s pretty expensive. I’m on the curriculum advisory council, you know,<strong>and</strong> I’d like <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out whether it’s as effective as they say before we recommend spend<strong>in</strong>g a lot of money <strong>to</strong> buyall of the program materials. So . . .”“Hey. Sounds like you’ve found your <strong>to</strong>pic. Some k<strong>in</strong>d of study of the program’s past effectiveness (or lackthereof) might be just the ticket, eh?”“Right! A little his<strong>to</strong>rical research is called for here, I th<strong>in</strong>k.”We agree. Brent might <strong>in</strong>deed do a his<strong>to</strong>rical study, or perhaps locate one that has already been done. What this<strong>in</strong>volves is what this chapter is about.What Is His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>?His<strong>to</strong>rical research takes a somewhat different tackfrom much of the other research we have described.There is, of course, no manipulation or control of variableslike there is <strong>in</strong> experimental research, but moreparticularly, it is unique <strong>in</strong> that it focuses primarily onthe past. As we mentioned <strong>in</strong> Chapter 1, some aspect ofthe past is studied by perus<strong>in</strong>g documents of the period,by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g relics, or by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualswho lived dur<strong>in</strong>g the time. An attempt is then made <strong>to</strong>reconstruct what happened dur<strong>in</strong>g that time as completely<strong>and</strong> as accurately as possible <strong>and</strong> (usually) <strong>to</strong>expla<strong>in</strong> why it happened—although this can never befully accomplished s<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>formation from <strong>and</strong> aboutthe past is always <strong>in</strong>complete. His<strong>to</strong>rical research,then, is the systematic collection <strong>and</strong> evaluation of data<strong>to</strong> describe, expla<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong> thereby underst<strong>and</strong> actions orevents that occurred sometime <strong>in</strong> the past.THE PURPOSES OF HISTORICAL RESEARCH<strong>Education</strong>al researchers undertake his<strong>to</strong>rical studies fora variety of reasons:1. To make people aware of what has happened <strong>in</strong> thepast so they may learn from past failures <strong>and</strong> successes.A researcher might be <strong>in</strong>terested, for example,<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g why a particular curriculummodification (such as a new <strong>in</strong>quiry-oriented Englishcurriculum) succeeded <strong>in</strong> some school districtsbut not <strong>in</strong> others.534


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 5352. To learn how th<strong>in</strong>gs were done <strong>in</strong> the past <strong>to</strong> see ifthey might be applicable <strong>to</strong> present-day problems<strong>and</strong> concerns. Rather than “re<strong>in</strong>vent<strong>in</strong>g the wheel,”for example, it often may be wiser <strong>to</strong> look <strong>to</strong> thepast <strong>to</strong> see if a proposed <strong>in</strong>novation has been triedbefore. Sometimes an idea proposed as “a radical<strong>in</strong>novation” is not all that new. Along this l<strong>in</strong>e, thereview of literature that we discussed <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong>Chapter 5, which is done as a part of many otherk<strong>in</strong>ds of studies, is a k<strong>in</strong>d of his<strong>to</strong>rical research.Often a review of the literature will show that whatwe th<strong>in</strong>k is new has been done before (<strong>and</strong>, surpris<strong>in</strong>gly,many times!).3. To assist <strong>in</strong> prediction. If a particular idea orapproach has been tried before, even under somewhatdifferent circumstances, past results may offerpolicy makers some ideas about how present plansmay turn out. Thus, if language labora<strong>to</strong>ries havebeen found effective (or the reverse) <strong>in</strong> certa<strong>in</strong>school districts <strong>in</strong> the past, a district contemplat<strong>in</strong>gtheir use would have evidence on which <strong>to</strong> base itsown decisions.4. To test hypotheses concern<strong>in</strong>g relationships ortrends. Many <strong>in</strong>experienced researchers tend <strong>to</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k of his<strong>to</strong>rical research as purely descriptive <strong>in</strong>nature. When well designed <strong>and</strong> carefully executed,however, his<strong>to</strong>rical research can lead <strong>to</strong> the confirmationor rejection of relational hypotheses as well.Here are some examples of hypotheses that wouldlend themselves <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research:a. In the early 1900s, most female teachers camefrom the upper middle class, but most maleteachers did not.b. Curriculum changes that did not <strong>in</strong>volve extensiveplann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> participation by the teachers<strong>in</strong>volved usually failed.c. N<strong>in</strong>eteenth-century social studies textbooks show<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g reference <strong>to</strong> the contributions of women<strong>to</strong> the culture of the United States from 1800 <strong>to</strong>1900.d. Secondary school teachers have enjoyed greaterprestige than elementary school teachers s<strong>in</strong>ce1940.Many other hypotheses are possible, of course; theones above are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> illustrate only that his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch can lend itself <strong>to</strong> hypothesis-test<strong>in</strong>gstudies.5. To underst<strong>and</strong> present educational practices <strong>and</strong>policies more fully. Many current practices <strong>in</strong>education are by no means new. Inquiry teach<strong>in</strong>g,character education, open classrooms, an emphasison “basics,” Socratic teach<strong>in</strong>g, the use of case studies,<strong>in</strong>dividualized <strong>in</strong>struction, team teach<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>teach<strong>in</strong>g “labora<strong>to</strong>ries” are but a few of many ideasthat reappear from time <strong>to</strong> time as “the” salvation foreducation.WHAT KINDS OF QUESTIONS ARE PURSUEDTHROUGH HISTORICAL RESEARCH?Although his<strong>to</strong>rical research focuses on the past, thetypes of questions that lend themselves <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch are quite varied. Here are some examples:<strong>How</strong> were students educated <strong>in</strong> the South dur<strong>in</strong>g theCivil War?<strong>How</strong> many bills deal<strong>in</strong>g with education were passeddur<strong>in</strong>g the presidency of Lyndon B. Johnson, <strong>and</strong>what was the major <strong>in</strong>tent of those bills?What was <strong>in</strong>struction like <strong>in</strong> a typical fourth-gradeclassroom 100 years ago?<strong>How</strong> have work<strong>in</strong>g conditions for teachers changeds<strong>in</strong>ce 1900?What were the major discipl<strong>in</strong>e problems <strong>in</strong> schools<strong>in</strong> 1940 as compared <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>day?What educational issues has the general public perceived<strong>to</strong> be most important dur<strong>in</strong>g the last 20 years?<strong>How</strong> have the ideas of John Dewey <strong>in</strong>fluenced presentdayeducational practices?<strong>How</strong> have fem<strong>in</strong>ists contributed <strong>to</strong> education?<strong>How</strong> were m<strong>in</strong>orities (or the physically impaired)treated <strong>in</strong> our public schools dur<strong>in</strong>g the twentiethcentury?<strong>How</strong> were the policies <strong>and</strong> practices of school adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs<strong>in</strong> the early years of the twentieth centurydifferent from those <strong>to</strong>day?What has been the role of the federal government <strong>in</strong>education?Steps Involved <strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>There are four essential steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g ahis<strong>to</strong>rical study <strong>in</strong> education. These <strong>in</strong>clude def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g theproblem or question <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g theformulation of hypotheses if appropriate), locat<strong>in</strong>g relevantsources of his<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>in</strong>formation, summariz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>


536 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eevaluat<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from these sources,<strong>and</strong> present<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>formation as itrelates <strong>to</strong> the problem or question that orig<strong>in</strong>ated thestudy.DEFINING THE PROBLEMIn the simplest sense, the purpose of a his<strong>to</strong>rical study <strong>in</strong>education is <strong>to</strong> describe clearly <strong>and</strong> accurately some aspec<strong>to</strong>f the past as it related <strong>to</strong> education <strong>and</strong>/or school<strong>in</strong>g.As we mentioned previously, however, his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearchers aim <strong>to</strong> do more than just describe; they want<strong>to</strong> go beyond description <strong>to</strong> clarify <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong>sometimes <strong>to</strong> correct (as when a researcher f<strong>in</strong>ds previousaccounts of an action or event <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> error).His<strong>to</strong>rical research problems, therefore, are identified<strong>in</strong> much the same way as are problems studiedthrough other types of research. Like any researchproblem, they should be clearly <strong>and</strong> concisely stated,be manageable, have a defensible rationale, <strong>and</strong> (ifappropriate) <strong>in</strong>vestigate a hypothesized relationshipamong variables. A concern somewhat unique <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch is that a problem may be selected forstudy for which <strong>in</strong>sufficient data are available. Oftenimportant data of <strong>in</strong>terest (certa<strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>ds of documents,such as diaries or maps from a particular period) simplycannot be located <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. This is particularlytrue the further back <strong>in</strong> the past an <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rlooks. As a result, it is better <strong>to</strong> study <strong>in</strong> depth a welldef<strong>in</strong>edproblem that is perhaps more narrow than onewould like than <strong>to</strong> pursue a more broadly stated problemthat cannot be sharply def<strong>in</strong>ed or fully resolved. Aswith all research, the nature of the problem or hypothesisguides the study; if it is well def<strong>in</strong>ed, the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>ris off <strong>to</strong> a good start.Some examples of his<strong>to</strong>rical studies that have beenpublished are as follows:“The School<strong>in</strong>g Process <strong>in</strong> First Grade: Two Samplesa Decade Apart” 1“Teacher Survival Rates <strong>in</strong> St. Louis, 1969–1982” 2“Women Teachers on the Frontier” 3“Orig<strong>in</strong>s of the Modern Social Studies” 4“Miss<strong>in</strong>g the Mark: Intelligence Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> LosAngeles Public Schools, 1922–1932” 5“The Responses of American Indian Children <strong>to</strong>Presbyterian School<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the N<strong>in</strong>eteenth Century:An Analysis through Missionary Sources” 6“The 1960s <strong>and</strong> the Transformation of CampusCultures” 7“Emma Willard: Pioneer <strong>in</strong> Social Studies <strong>Education</strong>”8“Inquiry <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> <strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istration: The Last25 Years <strong>and</strong> the Next” 9“Bertr<strong>and</strong> Russell <strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong> <strong>in</strong> World Citizenship”10“The Decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>Age at Leav<strong>in</strong>g Home, 1920–1979” 11LOCATING RELEVANT SOURCESCategories of Sources. Once a researcher has decidedon the problem or question he or she wishes <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>vestigate, the search for sources beg<strong>in</strong>s. Just abouteveryth<strong>in</strong>g that has been written down <strong>in</strong> some form orother, <strong>and</strong> virtually every object imag<strong>in</strong>able, is a potentialsource for his<strong>to</strong>rical research. In general, however,his<strong>to</strong>rical source material can be grouped <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> four basiccategories: documents, numerical records, oral statements<strong>and</strong> records, <strong>and</strong> relics.1. Documents: Documents are written or pr<strong>in</strong>ted materialsthat have been produced <strong>in</strong> some form oranother—annual reports, artwork, bills, books, car<strong>to</strong>ons,circulars, court records, diaries, diplomas,legal records, newspapers, magaz<strong>in</strong>es, notebooks,school yearbooks, memos, tests, <strong>and</strong> so on. Theymay be h<strong>and</strong>written, pr<strong>in</strong>ted, typewritten, drawn, orsketched; they may be published or unpublished;they may be <strong>in</strong>tended for private or public consumption;they may be orig<strong>in</strong>al works or copies. In short,documents refers <strong>to</strong> any k<strong>in</strong>d of <strong>in</strong>formation thatexists <strong>in</strong> some type of written or pr<strong>in</strong>ted form.2. Numerical records: Numerical records can be consideredeither as a separate type of source <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> ofthemselves or as a subcategory of documents. Suchrecords <strong>in</strong>clude any type of numerical data <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>tedform: test scores, attendance figures, census reports,school budgets, <strong>and</strong> the like. In recent years, his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearchers are mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g use of computers<strong>to</strong> analyze the vast amounts of numerical datathat are available <strong>to</strong> them.3. Oral statements: Another valuable source of <strong>in</strong>formationfor the his<strong>to</strong>rical researcher are the statementspeople make orally. S<strong>to</strong>ries, myths, tales, legends,chants, songs, <strong>and</strong> other forms of oral expressionhave been used by people through the ages <strong>to</strong> leave arecord for future generations. But his<strong>to</strong>rians can alsoconduct oral <strong>in</strong>terviews with people who were a par<strong>to</strong>f or witnessed past events. This is a special form ofhis<strong>to</strong>rical research, called oral his<strong>to</strong>ry, which is currentlyundergo<strong>in</strong>g somewhat of a renaissance.


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 5374. Relics: The fourth type of his<strong>to</strong>rical source is therelic. A relic is any object whose physical or visualcharacteristics can provide some <strong>in</strong>formation aboutthe past. Examples <strong>in</strong>clude furniture, artwork, cloth<strong>in</strong>g,build<strong>in</strong>gs, monuments, or equipment.Follow<strong>in</strong>g are different examples of his<strong>to</strong>ricalsources.A primer used <strong>in</strong> a seventeenth-century schoolroomA diary kept by a woman teacher on the Ohio frontier<strong>in</strong> the 1800sThe written arguments for <strong>and</strong> aga<strong>in</strong>st a new schoolbond issue as published <strong>in</strong> a newspaper at a particulartimeA 1958 junior high school yearbookSamples of cloth<strong>in</strong>g worn by students <strong>in</strong> the earlyn<strong>in</strong>eteenth century <strong>in</strong> rural GeorgiaHigh school graduation diplomas from the 1920sA written memo from a school super<strong>in</strong>tendent <strong>to</strong> hisfacultyAttendance records from two different school districtsover a 40-year periodEssays written by elementary school children dur<strong>in</strong>gthe Civil WarTest scores atta<strong>in</strong>ed by students <strong>in</strong> various states atdifferent timesThe architectural plans for a school <strong>to</strong> be organizedaround flexible schedul<strong>in</strong>gA taped oral <strong>in</strong>terview with a secretary of educationwho served <strong>in</strong> the adm<strong>in</strong>istrations of three differentU.S. presidentsPrimary Versus Secondary Sources. As <strong>in</strong> allresearch, it is important <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish between primary<strong>and</strong> secondary sources. A primary source is one preparedby an <strong>in</strong>dividual who was a participant <strong>in</strong> or a directwitness <strong>to</strong> the event be<strong>in</strong>g described. An eyewitnessaccount of the open<strong>in</strong>g of a new school would be an example,as would a researcher’s report of the results ofhis or her own experiment. Other examples of primarysource material are as follows:A n<strong>in</strong>eteenth-century teacher’s account of what itwas like <strong>to</strong> live with a frontier familyA transcript of an oral <strong>in</strong>terview conducted <strong>in</strong> the1960s with the super<strong>in</strong>tendent of a large urban schooldistrict concern<strong>in</strong>g the problems his district facesEssays written dur<strong>in</strong>g World War II by students <strong>in</strong> response<strong>to</strong> the question, “What do you like most <strong>and</strong>least about school?”Songs composed by members of a high school gleeclub <strong>in</strong> the 1930sM<strong>in</strong>utes of a school board meet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> 1878, taken bythe secretary of the boardA paid consultant’s written evaluation of a newFrench curriculum adopted <strong>in</strong> 1985 by a particularschool districtA pho<strong>to</strong>graph of an eighth-grade graduat<strong>in</strong>g class<strong>in</strong> 1930Letters written between an American student <strong>and</strong> aJapanese student describ<strong>in</strong>g their school experiencesdur<strong>in</strong>g the Korean conflictA secondary source, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is a documentprepared by an <strong>in</strong>dividual who was not a directwitness <strong>to</strong> an event but who obta<strong>in</strong>ed his or her descriptionof the event from someone else. They are “one stepremoved,” so <strong>to</strong> speak, from the event. A newspaper edi<strong>to</strong>rialcomment<strong>in</strong>g on a recent teachers’ strike would bean example. Other examples of secondary source materialare as follows:An encyclopedia entry describ<strong>in</strong>g various types ofeducational research conducted over a 10-year periodA magaz<strong>in</strong>e article summariz<strong>in</strong>g Aris<strong>to</strong>tle’s views oneducationA newspaper account of a school board meet<strong>in</strong>gbased on oral <strong>in</strong>terviews with members of the boardA book describ<strong>in</strong>g school<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the New Engl<strong>and</strong>colonies dur<strong>in</strong>g the 1700sA parent’s description of a conversation (at whichshe was not present) between her son <strong>and</strong> his teacherA student’s report <strong>to</strong> her counselor of why herteacher said she was be<strong>in</strong>g suspended from schoolA textbook (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g this one) on educationalresearchWhenever possible, his<strong>to</strong>rians (like other researchers)want <strong>to</strong> use primary rather than secondarysources. Can you see why?* Unfortunately, primarysources are admittedly more difficult <strong>to</strong> acquire, especiallythe further back <strong>in</strong> time a researcher searches.Secondary sources are of necessity, therefore, usedquite extensively <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. If it is atall possible, however, the use of primary sources ispreferred.*When a researcher must rely on secondary data sources, he or she<strong>in</strong>creases the chance of the data be<strong>in</strong>g less detailed <strong>and</strong>/or less accurate.The accuracy of what is be<strong>in</strong>g reported also becomes moredifficult <strong>to</strong> check.


538 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSUMMARIZING INFORMATION OBTAINEDFROM HISTORICAL SOURCESThe process of review<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> extract<strong>in</strong>g data fromhis<strong>to</strong>rical sources is essentially the one described <strong>in</strong>Chapter 5—determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the relevancy of the particularmaterial <strong>to</strong> the question or problem be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated;record<strong>in</strong>g the full bibliographic data of the source; organiz<strong>in</strong>gthe data one collects under categories related <strong>to</strong>the problem be<strong>in</strong>g studied; <strong>and</strong> summariz<strong>in</strong>g pert<strong>in</strong>ent<strong>in</strong>formation (important facts, quotations, <strong>and</strong> questions)on note cards (see Chapter 5).For an example of organiz<strong>in</strong>g data, consider a study<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the daily activities that occurred <strong>in</strong> n<strong>in</strong>eteenth-centuryelementary schoolrooms. A researchermight organize his or her facts under such categories as“subjects taught,” “learn<strong>in</strong>g activities,” “play activities,”<strong>and</strong> “class rules.”Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> summariz<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>rical data is rarely, ifever, a neat, orderly sequence of steps <strong>to</strong> be followed,however. Often read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g are <strong>in</strong>terspersed.Edward J. Carr, a noted his<strong>to</strong>rian, provides the follow<strong>in</strong>gdescription of how his<strong>to</strong>rians engage <strong>in</strong> research:[A common] assumption [among lay people] appears <strong>to</strong>be that the his<strong>to</strong>rian divides his work <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two sharplydist<strong>in</strong>guishable phases or periods. First, he spends a longprelim<strong>in</strong>ary period read<strong>in</strong>g his sources <strong>and</strong> fill<strong>in</strong>g his notebookswith facts; then, when this is over he puts away hissources, takes out his notebooks, <strong>and</strong> writes his book frombeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> end. This is <strong>to</strong> me an unconv<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> unplausiblepicture. For myself, as soon as I have got go<strong>in</strong>gon a few of what I take <strong>to</strong> be the capital sources, the itchbecomes <strong>to</strong>o strong <strong>and</strong> I beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> write—not necessarilyat the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g, but somewhere, anywhere. Thereafter,read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> writ<strong>in</strong>g go on simultaneously. The writ<strong>in</strong>g isadded <strong>to</strong>, subtracted from, re-shaped, <strong>and</strong> cancelled, asI go on read<strong>in</strong>g. The read<strong>in</strong>g is guided <strong>and</strong> directed <strong>and</strong>made fruitful by the writ<strong>in</strong>g; the more I write, the moreI know what I am look<strong>in</strong>g for, the better I underst<strong>and</strong>the significance <strong>and</strong> relevance of what I f<strong>in</strong>d. 12EVALUATING HISTORICAL SOURCESPerhaps more so than <strong>in</strong> any other form of research, thehis<strong>to</strong>rical researcher must adopt a critical attitude <strong>to</strong>wardany <strong>and</strong> all sources he or she reviews. A researchercan never be sure about the genu<strong>in</strong>eness <strong>and</strong> accuracy ofhis<strong>to</strong>rical sources. A memo may have been written bysomeone other than the person who signed it. A lettermay refer <strong>to</strong> events that did not occur or that occurred ata different time or <strong>in</strong> a different place. A document mayhave been forged or <strong>in</strong>formation deliberately falsified.Key questions for any his<strong>to</strong>rical researcher are:Was this document really written by the supposedauthor (i.e., is it genu<strong>in</strong>e)?Is the <strong>in</strong>formation conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> this document true(i.e., is it accurate)?The first question refers <strong>to</strong> what is known as external criticism,the second <strong>to</strong> what is known as <strong>in</strong>ternal criticism.External Criticism. External criticism refers <strong>to</strong>the genu<strong>in</strong>eness of any <strong>and</strong> all documents the researcheruses. <strong>Research</strong>ers engaged <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research want <strong>to</strong>know whether or not the documents they f<strong>in</strong>d were reallyprepared by the (supposed) author(s) of the document.Obviously, falsified documents can (<strong>and</strong> sometimes do)lead <strong>to</strong> erroneous conclusions. Several questions come<strong>to</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the genu<strong>in</strong>eness of a his<strong>to</strong>ricalsource.Who wrote this document? Was the author liv<strong>in</strong>g atthat time? Some his<strong>to</strong>rical documents have beenshown <strong>to</strong> be forgeries. An article supposedly writtenby, say, Mart<strong>in</strong> Luther K<strong>in</strong>g, Jr., might actually havebeen prepared by someone wish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> tarnish hisreputation.For what purpose was the document written? Forwhom was it <strong>in</strong>tended? And why? (Toward whomwas a memo from a school super<strong>in</strong>tendent directed?What was the <strong>in</strong>tent of the memo?)When was the document written? Is the date on thedocument accurate? Could the details described haveactually happened dur<strong>in</strong>g this time? (Sometimespeople write the date of the previous year on correspondence<strong>in</strong> the first days of a new year.)Where was the document written? Could the detailsdescribed have occurred <strong>in</strong> this location? (A descriptionof an <strong>in</strong>ner-city school supposedly written by ateacher <strong>in</strong> Fremont, Nebraska, might well be viewedwith caution.)Under what conditions was the document written? Isthere any possibility that what was written mighthave been directly or subtly coerced? (A descriptionof a particular school’s curriculum <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istrationprepared by a committee of nontenured teachersmight give quite a different view from one written bythose who have tenure.)Do different forms or versions of the document exist?(Sometimes two versions of a letter are found with


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHShould His<strong>to</strong>rians Influence Policy?Arecurr<strong>in</strong>g controversy <strong>in</strong> the his<strong>to</strong>ry of education <strong>in</strong>volvesthe relationship of his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>to</strong> educational policy. Here iswhat one scholar recently had <strong>to</strong> say about the issue: “For his<strong>to</strong>riansof education, is political relevance achieved at theexpense of academic respectability? Should educational his<strong>to</strong>rians<strong>in</strong>volve themselves <strong>in</strong> discussions of policy <strong>and</strong>, if so,how?”* David Tyack, a noted his<strong>to</strong>rian, replied as follows:“Do his<strong>to</strong>rians have anyth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> educationalpolicy? Many th<strong>in</strong>k not, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g some educational his<strong>to</strong>rianswho fear that ‘presentism’ will corrupt the dis<strong>in</strong>terestedness ofthe scholar. That may be, but there is a problem: Everybodyuses some k<strong>in</strong>d of his<strong>to</strong>ry, if only personal memory, <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>gsense of the world. The question is not whether <strong>to</strong> use his<strong>to</strong>ry<strong>in</strong> policy-mak<strong>in</strong>g, but whether that his<strong>to</strong>ry is go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> be as accurateas possible. His<strong>to</strong>rians surely do not have policy genes.They do have special knowledge, however, that might proveuseful. In educational reform, for example, there is a wholes<strong>to</strong>rehouse of experiments <strong>to</strong> explore for a sense of whatworks <strong>and</strong> does not <strong>and</strong> why. Luckily, it is cheap <strong>to</strong> learn fromthose experiments <strong>and</strong> they don’t harm liv<strong>in</strong>g people.”† (Weassume this quote refers <strong>to</strong> naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g “experiments,”rather than true experiments.)What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? Should educational his<strong>to</strong>rians <strong>in</strong>volvethemselves <strong>in</strong> discussions of policy?*K. Mahoney (2000). New times, new questions. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 29:18–19.†D. Tyack (2000). Reflections on his<strong>to</strong>ries of U.S. education. <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>er, 29:19–20.nearly identical word<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> only slight differences<strong>in</strong> h<strong>and</strong>writ<strong>in</strong>g, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that one may be a forgery.)The important th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> remember with regard <strong>to</strong> externalcriticism is that researchers should do their best <strong>to</strong>ensure that the documents they are us<strong>in</strong>g are genu<strong>in</strong>e.The above questions (<strong>and</strong> others like them) are directed<strong>to</strong>ward this end.Internal Criticism. Once researchers have satisfiedthemselves that a source document is genu<strong>in</strong>e, theyneed <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if the contents of the document areaccurate. This <strong>in</strong>volves what is known as <strong>in</strong>ternalcriticism. Both the accuracy of the <strong>in</strong>formation conta<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> a document <strong>and</strong> the truthfulness of the authorneed <strong>to</strong> be evaluated. Whereas external criticism has <strong>to</strong>do with the nature or authenticity of the document itself,<strong>in</strong>ternal criticism has <strong>to</strong> do with what the documentsays. Is it likely that what the author says happenedreally did happen? Would people at that time havebehaved as they are portrayed? Could events haveoccurred this way? Are the data presented (attendancerecords, budget figures, test scores, <strong>and</strong> so on) reasonable?Note, however, that researchers should notdismiss a statement as <strong>in</strong>accurate just because it isunlikely—unlikely events do occur. What researchersmust determ<strong>in</strong>e is whether a particular event might haveoccurred, even if it is unlikely. As with external criticism,several questions need <strong>to</strong> be asked <strong>in</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> evaluate the accuracy of a document <strong>and</strong> the truthfulnessof its author.1. With regard <strong>to</strong> the author of the document:Was the author present at the event he or she isdescrib<strong>in</strong>g? In other words, is the document a primaryor a secondary source? As we mentionedbefore, primary sources are preferred over secondarysources because they usually (though notalways) are considered <strong>to</strong> be more accurate.Was the author a participant <strong>in</strong> or an observer ofthe event? In general, we might expect an observer<strong>to</strong> present a more detached <strong>and</strong> comprehensiveview of an event than a participant. Eyewitnessesdo differ <strong>in</strong> their accounts of the sameevent, however, <strong>and</strong> hence the statements of anobserver are not necessarily more accurate thanthose of a participant.Was the author competent <strong>to</strong> describe the event?This refers <strong>to</strong> the qualifications of the author. Washe or she an expert on whatever is be<strong>in</strong>g describedor discussed? an <strong>in</strong>terested observer? a passerby?Was the author emotionally <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the event?The wife of a fired teacher, for example, mightwell give a dis<strong>to</strong>rted view of the teacher’s contributions<strong>to</strong> the profession.Did the author have any vested <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the outcomesof the event? Might he or she have an ax ofsome sort <strong>to</strong> gr<strong>in</strong>d, for example, or possibly bebiased <strong>in</strong> some way? A student who cont<strong>in</strong>uallywas <strong>in</strong> disagreement with his teacher, for example,might tend <strong>to</strong> describe the teacher more negativelythan would the teacher’s colleagues.539


540 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e2. With regard <strong>to</strong> the contents of the document:Do the contents make sense (i.e., given the natureof the events described, does it seem reasonablethat they could have happened as portrayed)?Could the event described have occurred at thattime? For example, a researcher might justifiablybe suspicious of a document describ<strong>in</strong>g a WorldWar II battle that <strong>to</strong>ok place <strong>in</strong> 1946.Would people have behaved as described?A majordanger here is what is known as presentism—ascrib<strong>in</strong>g present-day beliefs, values, <strong>and</strong> ideas <strong>to</strong>people who lived at another time. A somewhatrelated problem is that of his<strong>to</strong>rical h<strong>in</strong>dsight.Just because we know how an event came outdoes not mean that people who lived before ordur<strong>in</strong>g the occurrence of an event believed anoutcome would turn out the way it did.Does the language of the document suggest a biasof any sort? Is it emotionally charged, <strong>in</strong>temperate,or otherwise slanted <strong>in</strong> a particular way?Might the ethnicity, gender, religion, politicalparty, socioeconomic status, or position of theauthor suggest a particular orientation (Figure22.1)? For example, a teacher’s account of aschool board meet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> which a pay raise wasvoted down might differ from one of the boardmember’s accounts.Do other versions of the event exist? Do theypresent a different description or <strong>in</strong>terpretationof what happened? But note that just becausethe majority of observers of an event agreeTHE DAILY•CHRONICLE•Investigationof Sena<strong>to</strong>rJones Defeatedby ConservativeBloc!Special <strong>to</strong> theChronicle byStephanie SmithFigure 22.1 What Really Happened?THE EVENINGNEWSInvestigationof Sena<strong>to</strong>rJones defeatedby radicalcoalition.By ThomasEnville, Jr.about what happened, this does not mean theyare necessarily always right. On more than oneoccasion, a m<strong>in</strong>ority view has proved <strong>to</strong> becorrect.Data Analysis<strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>As is the case with other types of qualitative research,his<strong>to</strong>rical researchers must f<strong>in</strong>d ways <strong>to</strong> make sense ou<strong>to</strong>f what is usually a very large amount of data <strong>and</strong> thensynthesize it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a mean<strong>in</strong>gful narrative of their own.Some prefer <strong>to</strong> operate from a theoretical model thathelps them organize the <strong>in</strong>formation they have collected<strong>and</strong> may even suggest categories for a content analysis.Others prefer <strong>to</strong> immerse themselves <strong>in</strong> their <strong>in</strong>formationuntil patterns or themes suggest themselves. A cod<strong>in</strong>gsystem may be useful <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g so. Recently, somehis<strong>to</strong>rians have used quantitative data, such as crime<strong>and</strong> unemployment rates, <strong>to</strong> validate <strong>in</strong>terpretationsderived from documents. 13Generalization<strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Can researchers engaged <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research generalizefrom their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs? It depends. As perhaps is obvious<strong>to</strong> you, his<strong>to</strong>rical researchers are rarely, if ever, able<strong>to</strong> study an entire population of <strong>in</strong>dividuals or events.They usually have little choice but <strong>to</strong> study a sample ofthe phenomena of <strong>in</strong>terest. And the sample studied isdeterm<strong>in</strong>ed by the his<strong>to</strong>rical sources that rema<strong>in</strong> fromthe past. This is a particular problem for the his<strong>to</strong>rian,because almost always certa<strong>in</strong> documents, relics, <strong>and</strong>other sources are miss<strong>in</strong>g, have been lost, or otherwisecannot be found. Those sources that are availableperhaps are not representative of all the possible sourcesthat did exist.Suppose, for example, that a researcher is <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g how social studies was taught <strong>in</strong> highschools <strong>in</strong> the late 1800s. She is limited <strong>to</strong> study<strong>in</strong>gwhatever sources rema<strong>in</strong> from that time. The researchermay locate several textbooks of the period, plus assignmentbooks, lesson plans, tests, letters <strong>and</strong> other correspondencewritten by teachers, <strong>and</strong> their diaries, all


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHImportant F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>Perhaps the best-known example of his<strong>to</strong>rical research thatis pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> education is a series of studies begun <strong>in</strong>1934 by the German sociologist Max Weber, who offered thetheory that religion was a major cause of social behavior <strong>and</strong>,<strong>in</strong> particular, of economic capitalism.* A more recent exampleof such studies is that of Robert N. Bellah, who exam<strong>in</strong>ed*M. Weber (1958). The Protestant ethic <strong>and</strong> the spirit of capitalism.Translated by T. Parsons. New York: Charles Scribner <strong>and</strong> Sons.his<strong>to</strong>rical documents perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> Japanese religion dur<strong>in</strong>g thelate 1800s <strong>and</strong> early 1900s.† He concluded that several emergentreligious beliefs, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the desirability of hard work<strong>and</strong> the acceptance of be<strong>in</strong>g a bus<strong>in</strong>essman, here<strong>to</strong>fore a lowstatusrole, were <strong>in</strong>strumental <strong>in</strong> sett<strong>in</strong>g the stage for the growthof capitalism <strong>in</strong> Japan. These conclusions paralleled those ofWeber’s earlier studies of Calv<strong>in</strong>ism <strong>in</strong> Europe. Weber alsoconcluded that capitalism failed <strong>to</strong> develop <strong>in</strong> the early societiesof Ch<strong>in</strong>a, Israel, <strong>and</strong> India because none of their religiousdoctr<strong>in</strong>es supported the essential capitalist idea of accumulation<strong>and</strong> re<strong>in</strong>vestment of wealth as a sign of worth<strong>in</strong>ess.†R. N. Bellah (1967). <strong>Research</strong> chronicle: Tokugawa religion. InP. E. Hammond (Ed.), Sociologists at work. Garden City, NY:Anchor Books, pp. 164–185.from this period. On the basis of a careful review of thissource material, the researcher draws some conclusionsabout the nature of social studies teach<strong>in</strong>g at that time.The researcher needs <strong>to</strong> take care <strong>to</strong> remember, however,that all of these are written sources—<strong>and</strong> they mayreflect quite a different view from that held by peoplewho were not <strong>in</strong>cl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> write down their thoughts,ideas, or assignments. What might the researcher do? Aswith all research, the validity of any generalizations thatare drawn can be strengthened by <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the size<strong>and</strong> diversity of the sample of data on which the generalizationsare based. For those his<strong>to</strong>rical studies that <strong>in</strong>volvethe study of quantitative records, the computerhas made it possible, <strong>in</strong> many <strong>in</strong>stances, for a researcher<strong>to</strong> draw a representative sample of data from largegroups of students, teachers, <strong>and</strong> others who are represented<strong>in</strong> school records, test scores, census reports, <strong>and</strong>other documents.Advantages <strong>and</strong> Disadvantagesof His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong>The pr<strong>in</strong>cipal advantage of his<strong>to</strong>rical research is that itpermits <strong>in</strong>vestigation of <strong>to</strong>pics <strong>and</strong> questions that can bestudied <strong>in</strong> no other way. It is the only research methodthat can study evidence from the past <strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> questionssuch as those presented earlier <strong>in</strong> the chapter. Inaddition, his<strong>to</strong>rical research can make use of a widerrange of evidence than most other methods (with thepossible exceptions of ethnographic <strong>and</strong> case-studyresearch). It thus provides an alternative <strong>and</strong> perhapsricher source of <strong>in</strong>formation on certa<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>pics that canalso be studied with other methodologies. A researchermight, for example, wish <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the hypothesisthat “curriculum changes that did not <strong>in</strong>volve extensiveplann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> participation by the teachers <strong>in</strong>volved usuallyfail(ed)” by collect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terview or observationaldata on groups of teachers who (1) have <strong>and</strong> (2) havenot participated <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g curricular changes(a causal-comparative study), or by arrang<strong>in</strong>g for variations<strong>in</strong> teacher participation (an experimental study).The question might also be studied, however, by exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gdocuments prepared over the past 50 years bydissem<strong>in</strong>a<strong>to</strong>rs of new curricula (their reports), by teachers(their diaries), <strong>and</strong> so forth.A disadvantage of his<strong>to</strong>rical research is that themeasures used <strong>in</strong> other methods <strong>to</strong> control for threats<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity are simply not possible <strong>in</strong> a his<strong>to</strong>ricalstudy. Limitations imposed by the nature of thesample of documents <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>strumentation process(content analysis) are likely <strong>to</strong> be severe. <strong>Research</strong>erscannot ensure representativeness of the sample, norcan they (usually) check the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity ofthe <strong>in</strong>ferences made from the data available. Depend<strong>in</strong>gon the question studied, all or many of the threats<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity we discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9 are likely<strong>to</strong> exist. The possibility of bias due <strong>to</strong> researcher characteristics(<strong>in</strong> data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis) is always541


542 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 22.2 His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong> Is Not as Easyas You May Th<strong>in</strong>k!"His<strong>to</strong>rical researchis easy—all you have <strong>to</strong>do is f<strong>in</strong>d the appropriatedocuments <strong>and</strong> makesense out of them.""Maybe it’s easyif you’re only try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>support a predeterm<strong>in</strong>edconclusion, but not if youreally want <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat happened!"present. The possibility that any observed relationshipsare due <strong>to</strong> a threat <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g subject characteristics(the <strong>in</strong>dividuals on whom <strong>in</strong>formation exists), implementation,his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, attitude, or locationalso is always present. Although any particular threatdepends on the nature of a particular study, methodsfor its control are unfortunately unavailable <strong>to</strong> theresearcher. Because so much depends on the skill <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>tegrity of the researcher—s<strong>in</strong>ce methodologicalcontrols are unavailable—we believe that his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch is among the most difficult of all types ofresearch <strong>to</strong> conduct (Figure 22.2).Do<strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>rical research requires much more th<strong>and</strong>igg<strong>in</strong>g up good material; done properly it can dem<strong>and</strong>a broader array of skills than other methods. The his<strong>to</strong>rianmay f<strong>in</strong>d she needs some of the skills of a l<strong>in</strong>guist,chemist, or archaeologist. Further, s<strong>in</strong>ce his<strong>to</strong>ry is admittedlyhighly <strong>in</strong>terpretive <strong>in</strong> a global sense, knowledgeof psychology, anthropology, <strong>and</strong> other discipl<strong>in</strong>esmay also be required.An Example of His<strong>to</strong>rical<strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of his<strong>to</strong>rical research, followed by a critique ofits strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> our critiquesof the different types of research studies we analyzed <strong>in</strong>other chapters, we use several of the concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: The Journal of Psychohis<strong>to</strong>ry, 28, no. 1 (Summer 2000): 62–71.Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w: Self-Actualization<strong>in</strong> a Period of TransitionVivian C. FoxWorcester State CollegeThis paper is concerned with a crucial period of self-actualization <strong>in</strong> the life of Lydia AnnS<strong>to</strong>w (1823–1904), an early n<strong>in</strong>eteenth century Massachusetts woman who illustrates the<strong>in</strong>teractions between adolescent development <strong>and</strong> the dynamics of reforms <strong>in</strong> education<strong>and</strong> fem<strong>in</strong>ism. The term “self-actualization” is adopted from Frederic L. Bender, whodef<strong>in</strong>es this Marxian concept as “the development of one’s talents <strong>and</strong> abilities <strong>and</strong>, thepursuit of one’s life <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> through one’s work.” 1 Although self-actualizationappears <strong>to</strong> be a highly <strong>in</strong>dividualized process, it always occurs <strong>in</strong> a larger social context. Itis crucial <strong>to</strong> emphasize this <strong>in</strong> Lydia S<strong>to</strong>w’s case s<strong>in</strong>ce the most relevant context for her selfactualizationwas highly transitional <strong>in</strong> two important respects, namely, the developmen<strong>to</strong>f educational theory <strong>and</strong> practice, <strong>and</strong> the evolution <strong>in</strong> the status of women.The major source for describ<strong>in</strong>g S<strong>to</strong>w’s self-actualization is the set of four Journalswhich she kept dur<strong>in</strong>g the period of her tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Massachusetts as a professionalteacher at the Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n Normal School, <strong>and</strong> for about two years thereafter (July 8,1839–February 23, 1843). 2In this paper I undertake a brief description of the contextual events beforeproceed<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> an analysis of the Journals. I would like <strong>to</strong> start with school reform.Def<strong>in</strong>itionsPurpose?Primary sourceSCHOOL REFORMThe process of school reform that played such an important role <strong>in</strong> Lydia’s life was itself areflection of a panoply of post-Revolution concerns. To some, the advent of technologywas alter<strong>in</strong>g New Engl<strong>and</strong>’s predom<strong>in</strong>antly rural work patterns through the constructionof fac<strong>to</strong>ries <strong>and</strong> railroads. Cities were grow<strong>in</strong>g larger, more varied, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>glys<strong>in</strong>ister with vast numbers of immigrant-strangers, prostitutes <strong>and</strong> salesmen of magicaldrug products. The new arrivals were, moreover, largely untra<strong>in</strong>ed, uneducated <strong>and</strong> non-Anglo-Saxon men who appeared quickly <strong>to</strong> acquire political power at the ballot box. Inview of these cascad<strong>in</strong>g changes, many wondered whether the glorious achievements ofthe Revolution could be ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed. 3To some, the appropriate response <strong>to</strong> these issues was <strong>in</strong> the direction of ensur<strong>in</strong>gan educated citizenry. Leaders <strong>in</strong> this movement emerged <strong>in</strong> the Northeast, particularly<strong>in</strong> Massachusetts. Such Massachusetts men as Horace Mann, James G. Carter, EdwardEverett, Edmund Dwight, Cyrus Peirce, <strong>and</strong> Henry Barnard who was from New York, supportedthe idea that a key <strong>to</strong> confront<strong>in</strong>g post-Revolutionary challenges was <strong>in</strong> the fieldof educational reform. 4James G. Carter, for example, while Chairman of the Committee on <strong>Education</strong> ofthe Massachusetts House of Representatives, successfully established himself as the architec<strong>to</strong>f an educational renaissance that <strong>in</strong>cluded creation of a state-wide Board of <strong>Education</strong>.Horace Mann was appo<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> 1837 as the first Secretary of the Board. 5Mann immediately launched a crusade, which cont<strong>in</strong>ued dur<strong>in</strong>g his twelve years of<strong>in</strong>cumbency, from which his ideas spread throughout the nation. His accomplishments <strong>in</strong>cludeda proliferation of the common schools, an expansion of their curriculum, <strong>and</strong> theIndependentvariableBackground543


544 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eReference neededPrimary sourceWe agree.tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g of teachers <strong>in</strong> new approaches <strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g which encompassed a new philosophyof learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> moral discipl<strong>in</strong>e. 6He accepted the Republican view, moreover, that popular education was necessaryfor the <strong>in</strong>tellectual <strong>and</strong> monetary enhancement of citizens which would contribute <strong>to</strong>the general well-be<strong>in</strong>g of the Republic. His beliefs emphasized that the new Republicrequired a high st<strong>and</strong>ard of morality <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate, as he put it, “the long catalogueof human ills.” 7Central <strong>to</strong> achiev<strong>in</strong>g educational reform <strong>and</strong> progress was the provision of professionaltra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for school teachers. Prior <strong>to</strong> this time, little or no tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g was required<strong>and</strong> persons with a m<strong>in</strong>imal amount of education could take charge of classrooms.Many of the ideas of Mann <strong>and</strong> his colleagues were obta<strong>in</strong>ed from Europe, especiallyfrom Prussia. Unlike its European counterparts, however, professional teacher tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> what were called the Normal Schools (a title derived from the French École Normale)was open <strong>to</strong> females. In 1838 Massachusetts adopted a law authoriz<strong>in</strong>g the establishmen<strong>to</strong>f three Normal Schools. The first appeared <strong>in</strong> Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n <strong>in</strong> 1839, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> accordance withthe statute it was open only <strong>to</strong> females. The other two, <strong>in</strong> Barre <strong>and</strong> Bridgewater <strong>in</strong> 1840,were co-educational. Lydia was a member of the first class <strong>to</strong> enroll <strong>in</strong> Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n.Speak<strong>in</strong>g for many reformers, Horace Mann emphasized the importance ofemploy<strong>in</strong>g female teachers.<strong>Education</strong> . . . is woman’s work. . . . Let woman, then be educated <strong>to</strong> the highestpracticable po<strong>in</strong>t; not only because it is her right, but because it is essential <strong>to</strong>the world’s progress. Let her voice be a familiar voice <strong>in</strong> the schools <strong>and</strong> theacademies, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> halls of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> science. 8Mann was not, of course, the first <strong>to</strong> recognize appropriate roles for women <strong>in</strong> theeducational enterprise. By the last part of the eighteenth century, for example, NewEngl<strong>and</strong> clergymen, struck by the greater church attendance of women, <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ned thatfemales were purer <strong>and</strong> more delicate than men, <strong>and</strong> advocated greater exposure <strong>to</strong>education for them as caretakers of the very young. 9 From the latter part of the eighteenthcentury, then, sons as well as daughters came <strong>to</strong> be under the pedagogy of theirmothers, unlike <strong>in</strong> the prior period when fathers became responsible for the educationof boys when they reached the age of seven. The assumption that women had specialmoral strengths—that they were “angels <strong>in</strong> the house”—gave them important credentialsfor both domestic <strong>and</strong> professional teach<strong>in</strong>g roles. 10The call for women’s education grew stronger as post-Revolutionary ideology expressedthe sentiment that <strong>in</strong> a Republic, school education must become available <strong>to</strong> allcitizens, both male <strong>and</strong> female. Bos<strong>to</strong>n, for example, allowed girls <strong>to</strong> be educated <strong>in</strong> itsgrammar school <strong>in</strong> 1789; <strong>and</strong> Dedham, Lydia’s home<strong>to</strong>wn, had already anticipated this asearly as the 1750s. In a highly unusual development, one Mary Green was so successful ateacher that she was added <strong>to</strong> the permanent Dedham teach<strong>in</strong>g staff. 11Clearly, when Lydia enrolled at Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n she was rid<strong>in</strong>g the crest of unique educationalopportunities. As detailed <strong>in</strong> the next section, this enhanced status of women aseduca<strong>to</strong>rs of the young was also strongly strengthened by demographic <strong>and</strong> economicconditions of the time.Now I want <strong>to</strong> discuss the matter of gender reform.GENDER REFORMAt the same time that Mann <strong>and</strong> the other reformers were reconstruct<strong>in</strong>g the field ofeducation so as <strong>to</strong> create new opportunities for women, their legal, social <strong>and</strong> economiccircumstances generally were, paradoxically, much aga<strong>in</strong>st the enhancement of their


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 545status. New Engl<strong>and</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ued <strong>to</strong> follow common law <strong>and</strong> Christian traditions. Theseacknowledged the husb<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be the head of the household who controlled the l<strong>and</strong>ed<strong>and</strong> personal property of the wife, as well as the wages she might earn. Although whitewomen were legally considered citizens, they were prohibited from most public activities.They could not vote, sit on juries, execute wills, or serve as guardians of their childrenupon the death of their husb<strong>and</strong>s; <strong>and</strong> most professions were not open <strong>to</strong> them. 12But there were currents of change as well, <strong>and</strong> noth<strong>in</strong>g illustrated this better thanthe opportunities presented <strong>to</strong> Lydia. In addition <strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g, the newly created NewEngl<strong>and</strong> textile fac<strong>to</strong>ries welcomed women, as did many of the develop<strong>in</strong>g reform movementssuch as temperance, abolition, <strong>and</strong> child welfare. Women such as Harriet BeecherS<strong>to</strong>we <strong>and</strong> Louisa May Alcott entered the ranks of professional writers. 13Much of this might be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by demography. From about the end of the eighteenthcentury, New Engl<strong>and</strong> generally <strong>and</strong> Massachusetts <strong>in</strong> particular experienced an imbalance<strong>in</strong> the demographic ratio of the sexes <strong>in</strong> favor of women. This presented the questionof how some of these “surplus women,” as they were called, would be supported. 14The problem was further exacerbated by the many new work opportunities for men, suchas those that opened <strong>in</strong> the west <strong>and</strong> were created by the <strong>in</strong>dustrial revolution. An appropriateresponse <strong>to</strong> the shortage of male workers was <strong>to</strong> provide the new opportunities forwork<strong>in</strong>g class women that have already been noted. 15But there were other less tangible forces at work as well that contributed <strong>to</strong> thegender evolution that Lydia found herself <strong>in</strong>. A number of women sensed that they wereexperienc<strong>in</strong>g a shift <strong>in</strong> their fortunes. Lucy Larcom, for example, a Massachusetts fac<strong>to</strong>ryworker dur<strong>in</strong>g Lydia’s time, expressed such a view <strong>in</strong> her au<strong>to</strong>biography.[In] the olden times it was seldom said <strong>to</strong> little girls, as it always has been <strong>to</strong> boys,that they ought <strong>to</strong> have some def<strong>in</strong>ite plan, while they were children, what <strong>to</strong> be<strong>and</strong> do when they were grown up. . . . But when I was grow<strong>in</strong>g up, we wereoften <strong>to</strong>ld that it was our duty <strong>to</strong> develop any talent one might possess, or atleast <strong>to</strong> learn how <strong>to</strong> do some one th<strong>in</strong>g which the world needed, or whichwould make it a pleasanter world. 16Although when Lydia enrolled at Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, legal changes <strong>in</strong> the status of womenwere still <strong>in</strong> the future, the social ecology of women was certa<strong>in</strong>ly different from what ithad been traditionally. Self-actualization was a possibility.Secondary sourceIndependent variableHypotheticalspeculationPrimary sourceInterpretationDependent variableLYDIA’S SELF-ACTUALIZATIONI have already mentioned that Lydia’s four Journals are the primary source for conclusionsconcern<strong>in</strong>g self-actualization. The first two of these chronologically were writtenwhile she was <strong>in</strong> residence at Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n. The latter two were penned after she returned<strong>to</strong> Dedham hav<strong>in</strong>g been graduated from Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n.The Journals were not a personal <strong>in</strong>dulgence. Keep<strong>in</strong>g them was a daily requirementfor all pupils, conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a summary of the day’s lectures <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g. Lydia’s Journalsappear <strong>to</strong> be unique <strong>in</strong> their <strong>in</strong>clusion of personal remarks concern<strong>in</strong>g her responses<strong>to</strong> the lectures <strong>and</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> evaluations of her own abilities <strong>and</strong> activities. 17 It was aweekly requirement that the Journals be turned <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the Pr<strong>in</strong>cipal, Cyrus Peirce, whowould return them with his comments.The four Journals as a whole reveal that the time she spent at Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n was crucial<strong>to</strong> the self-actualization Lydia achieved. She came <strong>to</strong> regard herself as a professionalteacher capable of express<strong>in</strong>g herself fully, able <strong>to</strong> love her pupils, hav<strong>in</strong>g the capacity <strong>to</strong>evaluate teach<strong>in</strong>g performances of herself <strong>and</strong> others, <strong>and</strong> contribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the moralArgues for validityInterpretation


546 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eJustification?InterpretationInterpretationInterpretationInterpretationInterpretationIndependent variablePrimary sourceprogress of the larger community. The outward manifestations of this self-actualization<strong>in</strong>cluded her election as the first woman <strong>to</strong> the Board of <strong>Education</strong> of the city of FallRiver. It was there that she married, lived with her husb<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> raised a child. It was alsothe city where she established a sew<strong>in</strong>g school for young women, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>sure that theycould earn a wage; where she became a member of the Women’s Suffrage League ofFall River; <strong>and</strong> where she began her work <strong>in</strong> the anti-slavery movement <strong>and</strong> theunderground railroad, often plac<strong>in</strong>g herself at personal risk. Her work <strong>in</strong> the abolitionistmovement led her <strong>to</strong> enterta<strong>in</strong> such leaders as William Garrison, William Douglas,Sojourner Truth, <strong>and</strong> Wendell Phillips. 18One would never expect such accomplishments from a read<strong>in</strong>g of her first twoJournals. Significant self-actualization did not appear a promis<strong>in</strong>g outcome, especially <strong>in</strong>the complexities of her family background. There was much <strong>to</strong> provide an anxiety aboutaccomplishment. Death had been a pervasive presence <strong>in</strong> her family. Her father diedwhen she was one year old <strong>and</strong> her mother when she was eleven. With the additionaldeaths of six sibl<strong>in</strong>gs, only Lydia <strong>and</strong> her older sister survived from the nuclear family.After the age of eleven, then, she was dependent upon the care of her k<strong>in</strong>. 19 It wouldnot be surpris<strong>in</strong>g if the pervasiveness of such primary loss surrounded her with uncerta<strong>in</strong>tyabout any accomplishment, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>duced compliant behavior <strong>to</strong> those will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>become responsible for her well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Some of this vulnerability, however, was likely <strong>to</strong>have been offset by the warmth <strong>and</strong> support of her k<strong>in</strong>.As a child <strong>in</strong> Dedham, she lived with a gr<strong>and</strong>mother <strong>and</strong> an aunt. In the same <strong>to</strong>wnor nearby vic<strong>in</strong>ity, her last two Journals reveal a rich k<strong>in</strong> group: it is possible <strong>to</strong> count twogr<strong>and</strong>mothers, eight aunts, six uncles, <strong>and</strong> numerous cous<strong>in</strong>s. Among the women therewere at least three teachers, one of them her sister, but only Lydia received professionaltra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. In her last two Journals she portrays her family as close, cont<strong>in</strong>uously <strong>in</strong>teractive,<strong>and</strong> as k<strong>in</strong> who supported one another <strong>in</strong> illnesses as well as <strong>in</strong> celebrations. Withher aunts <strong>and</strong> friends she attended lectures <strong>and</strong> studied French <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>ok s<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>glessons. 20 Thus, despite the many deaths <strong>in</strong> her immediate family, the Journals reveal ayoung woman who did not feel ab<strong>and</strong>oned nor did she act depressed. On the otherh<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> most strik<strong>in</strong>gly, while she under<strong>to</strong>ok many challenges dur<strong>in</strong>g her tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g atthe Normal School, she <strong>in</strong>variably expressed doubts as <strong>to</strong> whether she could performthem adequately. The experience at Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, however, made all the difference <strong>in</strong>develop<strong>in</strong>g the strengths that were manifested <strong>in</strong> the rest of her life. It also helped her<strong>to</strong> assuage her pervasive lack of confidence.The core of the Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n experience was Cyrus Peirce. 21 His <strong>in</strong>fluence on Lydiawas most s<strong>in</strong>gular. He belonged <strong>to</strong> a generation of school reformers who stressed moraldevelopment as a central goal of education, a belief that <strong>in</strong>cluded the fusion of mentaldiscipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong> Christian ideals that had already been a key part of Lydia’s upbr<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>g. 22His extraord<strong>in</strong>ary teach<strong>in</strong>g ability attracted the admiration of Horace Mann, whoengaged him as Pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>and</strong> then visited the school <strong>in</strong> its first weeks of operation. Mannrecorded:Highly as I had appreciated his talent, he surpassed the ideas I had formed of hisability <strong>to</strong> teach, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the prerequisite of all successful teach<strong>in</strong>g, the power ofw<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g the confidence of his pupils. This surpassed what I had ever seen before<strong>in</strong> any school. The exercises were conducted <strong>in</strong> the most thorough manner: thepr<strong>in</strong>ciple be<strong>in</strong>g stated, <strong>and</strong> then applied <strong>to</strong> various comb<strong>in</strong>ations of facts,however different, <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d the pr<strong>in</strong>ciple which underlies them all . . . 23Peirce’s abilities were not lost on Lydia, who developed an emotional <strong>and</strong> personalresponse <strong>to</strong> his work. Her first Journal reveals that her reaction was one of great remorse


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 547whenever she or any of her classmates caused him distress. “There is” she wrote, <strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>ghim about her feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> the Journal he would read, “noth<strong>in</strong>g that more affects myhapp<strong>in</strong>ess than this . . . <strong>to</strong> cause him [underl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> orig<strong>in</strong>al] unhapp<strong>in</strong>ess who has beenso forbear<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> patient with us.” 24Whenever such episodes happened, S<strong>to</strong>w <strong>in</strong>creased her effort <strong>to</strong> improve herself<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> be perfect if she could. This was a serious challenge for Lydia, who questioned herperformance <strong>in</strong> almost everyth<strong>in</strong>g she did as previously mentioned. She compla<strong>in</strong>ed, forexample, that she could not achieve a “balance between impulses <strong>and</strong> belief” 25 whenshe would f<strong>in</strong>ish eat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ffee or someth<strong>in</strong>g else sweet, or when she chatted with her fellowpupils aga<strong>in</strong>st the comm<strong>and</strong>s of her pr<strong>in</strong>cipal. 26More seriously, she questioned her own <strong>in</strong>telligence, us<strong>in</strong>g the language ofphrenology, a pseudo-psychological science which demonstrated a person’s talent basedupon the bumps or organs, or lack thereof, on her head. She expressed her frustrationwhen study<strong>in</strong>g algebra with: “Oh how I wish my organ of calculation was large.” 27 Peircewould have none of it. He directly challenged the prevail<strong>in</strong>g view that women were<strong>in</strong>capable of study<strong>in</strong>g mathematics. Some people, he wrote,have doubted if girls should be taught this branch, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>deed, some havequestioned the propriety of educat<strong>in</strong>g women for this study! Benevolentspirit <strong>in</strong>deed. The appropriateness of this study for women, how could it beasked? She fills <strong>and</strong> ought <strong>to</strong> fill those stations where this branch is requisite.The discipl<strong>in</strong>e of the m<strong>in</strong>d which this branch affords is important <strong>to</strong> theeduca<strong>to</strong>r. 28Her self-deprecation <strong>and</strong> doubts were ubiqui<strong>to</strong>us <strong>in</strong> the first two Journals. Compositionexercises did not escape. “Composition I almost despise [but] I must beg<strong>in</strong> now <strong>and</strong>do the best I can which is always poor.” 29 Peirce’s response was simply <strong>to</strong> write <strong>in</strong> largecapital letters across her Journal, “DESPISE !!”. But this expression of disgust was unusual.Normally, he complimented this often anxious <strong>and</strong> over-critical pupil. These complimentswere well deserved; for despite her own doubts, an exam<strong>in</strong>ation of her Journals<strong>in</strong> comparison with those of her classmates reveals their superiority <strong>in</strong> terms of comprehensiveness,underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> clarity. There may be one exception <strong>in</strong> the Journals of aMary Swift, although these were devoid of the personal comments found so often <strong>in</strong>Lydia’s Journals. 30Peirce’s impact on Lydia may be <strong>in</strong>ferred from a survey of the goals of his <strong>in</strong>teractionswith the Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n pupils. The most prom<strong>in</strong>ent of these were (1) <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>culcate newteach<strong>in</strong>g methodologies; (2) <strong>to</strong> challenge the prevail<strong>in</strong>g stereotypes about the nature ofwomen’s <strong>in</strong>telligence; (3) <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>spire them <strong>in</strong> the belief that women, compared <strong>to</strong> men,possessed at least equal <strong>in</strong>tellectual capabilities <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the case of teach<strong>in</strong>g skills, thatthey were superior. Peirce also shared Horace Mann’s oft-expressed belief <strong>in</strong> the moralsuperiority of women. 31Given the relationship of affection <strong>and</strong> respect that existed between Lydia <strong>and</strong>her men<strong>to</strong>r, it would not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g if many of her <strong>in</strong>itial feel<strong>in</strong>gs of <strong>in</strong>adequacy <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>feriority did not beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> be displaced as she entered the practice of professionalteach<strong>in</strong>g. Her later Journals reveal a confidence <strong>in</strong> critically assess<strong>in</strong>g the techniquesof fellow teachers, both male <strong>and</strong> female, whose classrooms she visited. More importantly,she developed an <strong>in</strong>dependence from the <strong>in</strong>fluence of Peirce, recogniz<strong>in</strong>g thatsome circumstances required a deviation from his teach<strong>in</strong>gs. Use of the ferule, for example,she found <strong>to</strong> be occasionally necessary when confronted by an oversized classof undiscipl<strong>in</strong>ed young men, even though Peirce had been <strong>in</strong>exorably opposed <strong>to</strong> thepractice. 32Supportive exampleInterpretationExampleInterpretationExampleInterpretationExampleInterpretationInterpretationQuote wouldhelp here


548 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eGood exampleIn a later teach<strong>in</strong>g position <strong>in</strong> Fall River, she found great joy, however, <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>gthe k<strong>in</strong>d of relationship with her pupils that Peirce had strongly emphasized <strong>and</strong> sheherself wanted <strong>to</strong> have. She wrote of this achievement: “My scholars are very tractable.I am becom<strong>in</strong>g more attached <strong>to</strong> them as the weeks glide on <strong>and</strong> may the lovestrengthen day by day dur<strong>in</strong>g our connection.” 33 It was <strong>in</strong> this experience that Lydiafulfilled the promise of the Normal School reform.CONCLUSIONInterpretationInterpretationIt is possible <strong>to</strong> conclude that Lydia’s self-actualization <strong>in</strong> the field of professional teach<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> as a concerned <strong>and</strong> active citizen, flows from diverse sources: those available becauseof the his<strong>to</strong>rical environmental circumstances as well as from her own childhoodexperiences. Her own efforts <strong>to</strong> achieve success were of major importance as well, particularlyher choice <strong>to</strong> undertake the new professional tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g even though members ofher own family demonstrated that it was not necessary <strong>to</strong> becom<strong>in</strong>g a teacher. Even asshe doubted her ability <strong>to</strong> meet the school’s st<strong>and</strong>ards, she persisted <strong>in</strong> seek<strong>in</strong>g selfimprovement.At this po<strong>in</strong>t fortune jo<strong>in</strong>ed her fate with the efforts of Cyrus Peirce whowas, at a time <strong>and</strong> at a place that was right for Lydia, crusad<strong>in</strong>g for the recruitment ofwomen like Lydia <strong>to</strong> the teach<strong>in</strong>g profession, <strong>and</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>spiration for females <strong>to</strong>strengthen their capacities <strong>to</strong> take an active part <strong>in</strong> the world’s affairs. Peirce’s men<strong>to</strong>rship<strong>to</strong> Lydia, a talented, discipl<strong>in</strong>ed, but anxious adolescent, provided her with <strong>in</strong>tellectual<strong>to</strong>ols, a moral <strong>and</strong> probably emotional guardianship, <strong>and</strong> an unswerv<strong>in</strong>g faith <strong>in</strong> theabilities of her sex. It was with these gifts that Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w underwent the process ofself-actualization. She developed her talents, <strong>and</strong> she pursued her life’s <strong>in</strong>terests whichwere <strong>to</strong> make moral contributions <strong>to</strong> her world.NotesSecondary sourcePrimary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourcePrimary sourceSecondary source1. Frederic L. Bender, edi<strong>to</strong>r, Karl Marx, The Communist Manifes<strong>to</strong> (New York, 1988): 21. Accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> Bender,Marx believed this important process could not be achieved under capitalism—where the worker experiencesalienation from her work.2. See the unpublished four volume Journal of Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w held at Fram<strong>in</strong>gham State College at Fram<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,Massachusetts. Fram<strong>in</strong>gham is a successor <strong>to</strong> the Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n Normal School. I would like <strong>to</strong> extend my appreciation<strong>to</strong> the library staff for their assistance, especially <strong>to</strong> the archivist Sally Phillips, who was so helpful when I first beganmy research.3. Several his<strong>to</strong>rians attribute this anxiety <strong>to</strong> the advance of commerce. See e.g., Charles Sellersk, The MarketRevolution: Jacksonian America, 1815–1840 (New York, 1991). For a more optimistic description of the period, seeDaniel Feller, The Jacksonian Promise: 1815–1846 (Baltimore, 1995).4. See, for example, Paul H. Matt<strong>in</strong>gly, The Classless Profession: American Schoolmen <strong>in</strong> the N<strong>in</strong>eteenth Century(New York: New York University Press, 1975).5. Lawrence A. Crem<strong>in</strong>, American <strong>Education</strong>: The National Experience, 1783–1876 (New York, 1980): 136; ArthurO. Nor<strong>to</strong>n, The First Normal School <strong>in</strong> America: The Journals of Cyrus Peirce <strong>and</strong> Mary Swift (Cambridge: HarvardUniversity Press, 1926), Introduction.6. Ibid, Crem<strong>in</strong>.7. Horace Mann, Common School Journal, III (1841) 15, <strong>in</strong> Crem<strong>in</strong>, p. 137.8. Horace Mann, “A Few Thoughts on the Powers <strong>and</strong> Duties of Women,” <strong>in</strong> Lectures on Various Subjects (NewYork, 1864) <strong>in</strong> Crem<strong>in</strong>, p. 143.9. W<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>n E. Langley <strong>and</strong> Vivian C. Fox, Women’s Rights <strong>in</strong> the United States: A Documentary His<strong>to</strong>ry, Parts I<strong>and</strong> II (Greenwood, 1994); Paula Baker, “The Domestication of Politics: Women <strong>and</strong> the American Political Society,1780–1920.” The American His<strong>to</strong>rical Review, 89:3, June, 1984: 620–647; Nancy Cott, The Bonds of Womanhood:Woman’s Sphere <strong>in</strong> New Engl<strong>and</strong> (New Haven, Yale University Press, 1977).10. Ibid, Cott; see also L<strong>in</strong>da K. Kerber, Women of the Republic.


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 54911. Thomas Woody, The His<strong>to</strong>ry of Women’s <strong>Education</strong> <strong>in</strong> The United States, Vol. I (New York: The Science Press,1929); Carlos Slafter, A Record of <strong>Education</strong>: The Schools <strong>and</strong> Teachers of Dedham, Massachusetts, 1644–1904(Dedham Transcript 1952):45.12. See Langley <strong>and</strong> Fox, Women’s Rights, especially parts I <strong>and</strong> II.13. Gerald<strong>in</strong>e Jonich Clifford, “Home <strong>and</strong> School <strong>in</strong> 19th Century America: Some Personal His<strong>to</strong>ry Reports From theUnited States,” His<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>Education</strong> Quarterly, 18:1 (Spr<strong>in</strong>g, 1978): 3–4.14. Maris A. V<strong>in</strong>ovskis, Fertility <strong>in</strong> Massachusetts from the Revolution <strong>to</strong> the Civil War (New York: Academic Press,1981): 221. Horace Mann said that <strong>in</strong> 1839 there were far many more female teachers than male teachers. SeeMary Swift’s Journals <strong>in</strong> Nor<strong>to</strong>n, The First Normal School, footnote 14.15. Barbara Myer Wertheimer, We Were There: The S<strong>to</strong>ry of Work<strong>in</strong>g Women <strong>in</strong> America (New York: PantheonBooks, 1961): Claudi Gold<strong>in</strong>, “The Economic Status of Women <strong>in</strong> the Early Republic: Quantitative Evidence,” Journalof Interdiscipl<strong>in</strong>ary His<strong>to</strong>ry, XVI:3 (W<strong>in</strong>ter, 1986):375–404.16. Lucy Larcom, A New Engl<strong>and</strong> Childhood.17. Lydia reveals that it was Professor Newman, pr<strong>in</strong>cipal of Barre Normal School, who <strong>in</strong>troduced her <strong>to</strong> the highst<strong>and</strong>ards of journal-keep<strong>in</strong>g she would ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>. She said, “He gave us some useful tips regard<strong>in</strong>g the importanceof writ<strong>in</strong>g abstracts of lectures, lessons or anyth<strong>in</strong>g else we might hear.” She recorded this <strong>in</strong> Volume IV, p. 16 of herJournals. Her summaries of lectures <strong>and</strong> of the books she read were remarkable especially <strong>in</strong> contrast <strong>to</strong> her classmates.Except for Mary Swift, all the other Journals from Lex<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n do not compare <strong>in</strong> quality or length with tha<strong>to</strong>f S<strong>to</strong>w’s.18. Most of this <strong>in</strong>formation was obta<strong>in</strong>ed from her obituary <strong>in</strong> the Fall River Even<strong>in</strong>g News written on Friday,August 16, 1904. Lydia was 81 years old when she died.19. Don Gleason Hill, edi<strong>to</strong>r, The Record of Baptisms, Marriages <strong>and</strong> Deaths <strong>and</strong> Admission <strong>to</strong> the Church,1638–1845 (Dedham, 1888):288. This record of the First Parish Church is listed as Dedham Cemetery Epitaphs.In the registry of Probate, Norfolk County, NP 17508, Lydia’s father, Timothy S<strong>to</strong>w, Jr., is listed as a “Hoosewright”or “Housewright.” The O.E.D. def<strong>in</strong>es these as housebuilders.20. These impressions are <strong>to</strong> be gleaned from her last two Journals when she was liv<strong>in</strong>g at home. <strong>How</strong>ever, whileat school Lydia wrote about her feel<strong>in</strong>gs regard<strong>in</strong>g her home. “There [mean<strong>in</strong>g home] is the true City of Refuge.Where are we <strong>to</strong> turn when it is shut from us or changed?” It is <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> note that Lydia is express<strong>in</strong>g twofeel<strong>in</strong>gs: one that she is lucky <strong>to</strong> have that true refuge, but also, secondly she exhibits an awareness of the pligh<strong>to</strong>ne would have without it. Probably this is because she is an orphan <strong>and</strong> probably contemplated what wouldhappen if she were not cared for by her relatives. Journal II, p. 72.21. For Peirce’s background, see Nor<strong>to</strong>n, Introduction, The First State Normal School.22. For an <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpretation of the personality fostered by the Congregationalist religion, see Philip Greven,The Protestant Temperament: Patterns of Child-Rear<strong>in</strong>g, Religious Experience, <strong>and</strong> the Self <strong>in</strong> Early America(New York, 1977):152–179. Of course read<strong>in</strong>g Lydia’s Journals also provides one with the most detailed viewof her religious <strong>and</strong> moral ideas.23. See Nor<strong>to</strong>n, Introduction, The First State Normal School.24. S<strong>to</strong>w, Journals, no. 1, p. 25.25. Ibid., p. 180. See also ibid at 217 <strong>and</strong> 241 for further examples of this belief.26. Ibid., p. 217, 241.27. Ibid., p. 25.28. Journals vol. II, p. 16.29. Ibid., p. 56.30. See Mary Swift’s published Journal <strong>in</strong> Nor<strong>to</strong>n, The First State Normal School.31. For Peirce’s views see Lydia’s Journals <strong>and</strong> Nor<strong>to</strong>n, which conta<strong>in</strong>s the Journals of Mary Swift <strong>and</strong> of CyrusPeirce. From them it is very clear that Peirce believed women <strong>to</strong> have many talents <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a highly developedmoral capacity. For Horace Mann, see his published lectures entitled, A Few Thoughts on the Powers <strong>and</strong> Dutiesof Woman, Two Lectures (Syracuse, Hall, Mills <strong>and</strong> Co., 1853).32. Journals vol. IV, p. 41.33. Ibid., p. 86.Secondary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourcePrimary sourcePrimary sourcePrimary sourcePrimary sourcePrimary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourceSecondary sourcePrimary sourcePrimary source


550 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONWe do not f<strong>in</strong>d a clear statement of purpose. In partbecause of the publication <strong>in</strong> which the study appears,The Journal of Psychohis<strong>to</strong>ry, we th<strong>in</strong>k the purposecould have been stated as, for example, “<strong>to</strong> enhance ourunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the ways <strong>in</strong> which societal conditions<strong>and</strong> personal characteristics <strong>in</strong>teract <strong>in</strong> produc<strong>in</strong>g valuedqualities such as ‘self-actualization.’ ” The justificationimplied <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>troduction is that the life of LydiaS<strong>to</strong>w is important <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>; this is elaborated laterunder “Gender Reform.”There are no problems of risk, deception, orconfidentiality.DEFINITIONSA clear def<strong>in</strong>ition of self-actualization is given <strong>in</strong> the<strong>in</strong>troduction. This is particularly important because notall def<strong>in</strong>itions of this term <strong>in</strong>clude “pursuit of one’s life<strong>in</strong>terests <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> through one’s work.” Other terms suchas self-improvement <strong>and</strong> concerned <strong>and</strong> active citizenare probably clear enough <strong>in</strong> context.PRIOR RESEARCHThere is no presentation of previous research, presumablybecause there is none that is directly relevant. If our<strong>in</strong>terpretation of the author’s purpose is correct, it maybe that other biographies would be pert<strong>in</strong>ent. There is nomention of other biographies of S<strong>to</strong>w. If they exist, theymight have provided additional evidence.HYPOTHESESNone are stated. The “<strong>in</strong>teraction” hypothesis is clearlyimplied; it appears likely that it conceptually precededthe analysis of the <strong>in</strong>formation.a population of persons could have been specified,though it’s not clear what its characteristics would be—perhaps “n<strong>in</strong>eteenth-century women who made a significantimpact on education.” A sample of such womenwould greatly <strong>in</strong>crease the generalizability of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsbut would, presumably, <strong>in</strong>volve major problems <strong>in</strong> locat<strong>in</strong>gsuitable source material.INSTRUMENTATIONThere is no <strong>in</strong>strumentation <strong>in</strong> the sense that we discussit <strong>in</strong> this text. The “<strong>in</strong>strument” <strong>in</strong> this case is theresearcher’s talent for locat<strong>in</strong>g, evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> analyz<strong>in</strong>gpert<strong>in</strong>ent sources. The concept of reliability usually haslittle relevance <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical data, because each item is notmean<strong>in</strong>gfully considered <strong>to</strong> be a sample across either conten<strong>to</strong>r time. In this study, however, comparison of journalstatements perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the same <strong>to</strong>pic (e.g., selfconfidence)could be made across the early two journals<strong>and</strong>, aga<strong>in</strong>, across the later two. These comparisons wouldgive an <strong>in</strong>dication of the consistency of these statements.Validity, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is paramount. It isaddressed by evaluat<strong>in</strong>g sources <strong>and</strong> by compar<strong>in</strong>g differentsources regard<strong>in</strong>g the same specifics. In thisstudy, data are from two types of source. Secondarysources are used extensively <strong>in</strong> the sections on schoolreform <strong>and</strong> gender reform. The source of <strong>in</strong>formationabout S<strong>to</strong>w is a primary one, her four journals. Some ofthe secondary sources could, it seems, have been usedas cross-checks for validity, but this apparently was notdone. The validity of the author’s summaries of this <strong>in</strong>formationis supported, <strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>stances, by quotationsfrom the journals <strong>and</strong> from other primary sources.External criticism does not appear <strong>to</strong> be an issue withrespect <strong>to</strong> the journals or, presumably, other references.The question of <strong>in</strong>ternal criticism is somewhat difficult<strong>to</strong> deal with, because the journals must be evaluated <strong>in</strong>terms of the writer’s feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> perceptions ratherthan events. Here, we are highly dependent on the researcher’ssummaries.SAMPLEThe sampl<strong>in</strong>g issue is quite different <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch as compared with other types of research.There typically is no population of persons <strong>to</strong> be sampled.It could be argued that a population of eventsexists, but if so, they are likely <strong>to</strong> be so different that selectionamong them makes more sense if done purposefully,<strong>in</strong> other words, a purposive sample. In this study,PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYThere is little <strong>to</strong> be said about procedures except thatsome discussion of the plans that the researcher developed<strong>and</strong> followed for analyz<strong>in</strong>g the documents, particularlythe journals, would be useful, especially so thatreaders could evaluate the presumed selection ofcontent. His<strong>to</strong>rical research is always subject <strong>to</strong> theallegation that the researcher has selected content based


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 551on personal bias. Internal validity concerns are justifiedregard<strong>in</strong>g this research because of the <strong>in</strong>tent <strong>to</strong> study therelationships among societal conditions, prior personalqualities, <strong>and</strong> personal development. In addition <strong>to</strong> datacollec<strong>to</strong>r (researcher) bias, other major threats <strong>in</strong>cludehis<strong>to</strong>ry (other events) <strong>and</strong> maturation. There is no way<strong>to</strong> control for these threats <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research.DATA ANALYSISData analysis procedures, as we have expla<strong>in</strong>ed them <strong>in</strong>this book, are not used <strong>in</strong> this study, nor do we see howmost of them could be. Use of the content analysis methods<strong>in</strong> Chapter 20 would serve <strong>to</strong> organize the <strong>in</strong>formation.Category-by-category tabulation of the frequency ofsimilar statements might have clarified <strong>in</strong>terpretations.RESULTS/DISCUSSIONThough we advocate keep<strong>in</strong>g the results of a study separatefrom the discussion of them, such separation isextremely difficult <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. The questionhere is whether the <strong>in</strong>formation (data) provided justifiesthe author’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>and</strong> conclusions. Thoughnot proven, we th<strong>in</strong>k the well-documented summariesof school <strong>and</strong> gender reforms dur<strong>in</strong>g S<strong>to</strong>w’s youngadulthood are persuasive. With respect <strong>to</strong> changes <strong>in</strong>S<strong>to</strong>w over a four-year period (ages 17 <strong>to</strong> 21), we arevery dependent on the author’s highly <strong>in</strong>ferential psychological<strong>in</strong>terpretations. Though we f<strong>in</strong>d them plausible(e.g., <strong>in</strong>terpretation of S<strong>to</strong>w’s factual family his<strong>to</strong>ry),more quotations from the journals wouldstrengthen such <strong>in</strong>terpretations, most importantly thather confidence <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependence <strong>in</strong>creased greatly dur<strong>in</strong>gthis time. Several are provided from the early journalsbut none from the later ones.The assertion that “Even as she doubted her ability <strong>to</strong>meet the school’s st<strong>and</strong>ards, she persisted <strong>in</strong> seek<strong>in</strong>gself-improvement” is reflected <strong>in</strong> quotations. We mustassume, however, that they are typical of both S<strong>to</strong>w’sstatements <strong>and</strong> her feel<strong>in</strong>gs. Similarly, the <strong>in</strong>fluence ofPeirce, <strong>in</strong> turn reflect<strong>in</strong>g social changes, seems persuasive,but, aga<strong>in</strong>, we must assume that the examples arerepresentative. We th<strong>in</strong>k there is a clear implication thatsocietal changes, family support, personal persistence,<strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>fluence of Peirce were all necessary <strong>to</strong> S<strong>to</strong>w’sself-actualization. While this is plausible, it is notdemonstrated by the study.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE NATURE OF HISTORICAL RESEARCHThe unique characteristic of his<strong>to</strong>rical research is that it focuses exclusively on the past.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsPURPOSES OF HISTORICAL RESEARCH<strong>Education</strong>al researchers conduct his<strong>to</strong>rical studies for a variety of reasons, but perhapsthe most frequently cited is <strong>to</strong> help people learn from past failures <strong>and</strong> successes.When well designed <strong>and</strong> carefully executed, his<strong>to</strong>rical research may lead <strong>to</strong> the confirmationor rejection of relational hypotheses.551


552 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSTEPS IN HISTORICAL RESEARCHFour essential steps are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a his<strong>to</strong>rical study: def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the problem orhypothesis <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated; search<strong>in</strong>g for relevant source material; summariz<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the sources the researcher is able <strong>to</strong> locate; <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the evidenceobta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> then draw<strong>in</strong>g conclusions about the problem or hypothesis.HISTORICAL SOURCESMost his<strong>to</strong>rical source material can be grouped <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> four basic categories: documents,numerical records, oral statements, <strong>and</strong> relics.Documents are written or pr<strong>in</strong>ted materials that have been produced <strong>in</strong> one form oranother sometime <strong>in</strong> the past.Numerical records <strong>in</strong>clude any type of numerical data <strong>in</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ted or h<strong>and</strong>written form.Oral statements <strong>in</strong>clude any form of statement spoken by someone.Relics are any objects whose physical or visual characteristics can provide some<strong>in</strong>formation about the past.A primary source is one prepared by an <strong>in</strong>dividual who was a participant <strong>in</strong> or adirect witness <strong>to</strong> the event that is be<strong>in</strong>g described.A secondary source is a document prepared by an <strong>in</strong>dividual who was not a direct witness<strong>to</strong> an event but who obta<strong>in</strong>ed his or her description of the event from someone else.EVALUATION OF HISTORICAL SOURCE MATERIALContent analysis is a primary method of data analysis <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research.External criticism refers <strong>to</strong> the genu<strong>in</strong>eness of the documents a researcher uses <strong>in</strong> ahis<strong>to</strong>rical study.Internal criticism refers <strong>to</strong> the accuracy of the contents of a document. Whereasexternal criticism has <strong>to</strong> do with the authenticity of a document, <strong>in</strong>ternal criticism has<strong>to</strong> do with what the document says.GENERALIZATION IN HISTORICAL RESEARCHAs <strong>in</strong> all research, researchers who conduct his<strong>to</strong>rical studies should exercise caution<strong>in</strong> generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from small or nonrepresentative samples.ADVANTAGES AND DISADVANTAGES OF HISTORICAL RESEARCHThe ma<strong>in</strong> advantage of his<strong>to</strong>rical research is that it permits the <strong>in</strong>vestigation of <strong>to</strong>picsthat could be studied <strong>in</strong> no other way. It is the only research method that can studyevidence from the past.A disadvantage is that controll<strong>in</strong>g for many of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity is notpossible <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research. Many of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity discussed <strong>in</strong>Chapter 9 are likely <strong>to</strong> exist <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical studies.Key Termsdocuments 536external criticism 538his<strong>to</strong>rical research 534<strong>in</strong>ternal criticism 539primary source 537relic 537secondary source 537


CHAPTER 22 His<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>Research</strong> 5531. A researcher wishes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate changes <strong>in</strong> high school graduation requirementss<strong>in</strong>ce 1900. Pose a possible hypothesis the researcher might <strong>in</strong>vestigate. Whatsources might he or she consult?2. Why might a researcher be cautious or suspicious about each of the follow<strong>in</strong>gsources?a. A typewriter impr<strong>in</strong>ted with the name “Chris<strong>to</strong>pher Columbus”b. A letter from Frankl<strong>in</strong> D. Roosevelt endors<strong>in</strong>g John F. Kennedy for the presidencyof the United Statesc. A letter <strong>to</strong> the edi<strong>to</strong>r from an eighth-grade student compla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g about the adequacyof the school’s advanced mathematics programd. A typed report of an <strong>in</strong>terview with a recently fired teacher describ<strong>in</strong>g theteacher’s compla<strong>in</strong>ts aga<strong>in</strong>st the school districte. A 1920 high school diploma <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g a student had graduated from the tenthgradef. A high school teacher’s attendance book <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g no absences by any memberof her class dur<strong>in</strong>g the entire year of 1942g. A pho<strong>to</strong>graph of an elementary school classroom <strong>in</strong> 18003. <strong>How</strong> would you compare his<strong>to</strong>rical research <strong>to</strong> the other methodologies we havediscussed <strong>in</strong> this book—is it harder or easier <strong>to</strong> do? Why?4. “<strong>Research</strong>ers cannot ensure representativeness of the sample” <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research.Why not?5. Which of the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research that we have described do youth<strong>in</strong>k would be the hardest <strong>to</strong> complete? the easiest? Why?6. Can you th<strong>in</strong>k of any <strong>to</strong>pic or idea that would not be a potential source for his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch? Why not? Suggest an example.7. His<strong>to</strong>rians usually prefer <strong>to</strong> use primary rather than secondary sources. Why? Canyou th<strong>in</strong>k of an <strong>in</strong>stance, however, where the reverse might be true? Discuss.8. Which do you th<strong>in</strong>k is harder <strong>to</strong> establish—the genu<strong>in</strong>eness or the accuracy of ahis<strong>to</strong>rical document? Why?For Discussion1. D. R. Entwisele et al. (1986). The school<strong>in</strong>g process <strong>in</strong> first grade: Two samples a decade apart. American<strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 23(4): 587–613.2. J. H. Mark <strong>and</strong> B. D. Anderson (1985). Teacher survival rates <strong>in</strong> St. Louis, 1969–1982. American <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> Journal, 22(2): 413–422.3. P. W. Kaufman (1985). Women teachers on the frontier. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.4. M. Lybarger (1983). Orig<strong>in</strong>s of the modern social studies. His<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>Education</strong> Quarterly, 23(4):445–468.5. J. R. Rafferty (1988). Miss<strong>in</strong>g the mark: Intelligence test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Los Angeles public schools, 1922–1932.His<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>Education</strong> Quarterly, 28(1): 73–93.6. M. C. Coleman (1987). The responses of American Indian children <strong>to</strong> Presbyterian school<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> then<strong>in</strong>eteenth century: An analysis through missionary sources. His<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>Education</strong> Quarterly, 27(4):473–498.7. H. L. Horowitz (1986). The 1960s <strong>and</strong> the transformation of campus cultures. His<strong>to</strong>ry of <strong>Education</strong>Quarterly, 26(1): 1–38.8. M. R. Nelson (1987). Emma Willard: Pioneer <strong>in</strong> social studies education. Theory <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social<strong>Education</strong>, 15(4): 245–256.9. D. J. Willower (1987). Inquiry <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> educational adm<strong>in</strong>istration: The last 25 years <strong>and</strong> the next. Journal of<strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istration, 15(1): 12–28.Notes


554 PART 6 Qualitative <strong>Research</strong> Methodologies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e10. S. D. Jespersen (1987). Bertr<strong>and</strong> Russell <strong>and</strong> education <strong>in</strong> world citizenship. Journal of Social Studies<strong>Research</strong>, 11(1): 1–6.11. F. K. Goldscheider <strong>and</strong> C. LeBourdars (1986). The decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> age at leav<strong>in</strong>g home, 1920–1979. Sociology<strong>and</strong> Social <strong>Research</strong>: An International Journal, 70(2): 143–145.12. E. J. Carr (1967). What Is his<strong>to</strong>ry? New York: R<strong>and</strong>om House, pp. 32–33.13. L. Isaac <strong>and</strong> L. Griff<strong>in</strong> (1989). Ahis<strong>to</strong>ricism <strong>in</strong> time-series analyses of his<strong>to</strong>rical process: Critique,redirection, <strong>and</strong> illustrations from U.S. labor his<strong>to</strong>ry. American Sociological Review, 54: 873–890.


Mixed-MethodsStudiesP A R T7Part 7 presents a discussion of mixed-methods studies, which comb<strong>in</strong>e quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative methods. Such studies have been receiv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>creased attention <strong>in</strong>recent years. Advocates po<strong>in</strong>t out the potential for us<strong>in</strong>g the strengths of bothapproaches, while critics discuss several limitations <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g ambiguity regard<strong>in</strong>g thedef<strong>in</strong>ition of the “method.” In Chapter 23, we present pros <strong>and</strong> cons <strong>and</strong> concludewith an example of a study that we have annotated <strong>and</strong> analyzed.


23 Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>What Is Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Why Do Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Drawbacks of Mixed-Methods StudiesA (Very) Brief His<strong>to</strong>ryTypes of Mixed-Methods<strong>Design</strong>sThe Explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>The Explana<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>The Triangulation <strong>Design</strong>Other Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong> <strong>Design</strong> IssuesSteps <strong>in</strong> Conduct<strong>in</strong>g aMixed-Methods StudyEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g a Mixed-Methods StudyEthics <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>SummaryAn Example of Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>Hypotheses <strong>and</strong> <strong>Design</strong>SampleInstrumentationInternal Validity/CredibilityData AnalysisResults/DiscussionDr. Graham“For my doc<strong>to</strong>raldissertation, I thought I might doa mixed-methods study.”“Oh?”“Yes. In the <strong>in</strong>itial phase I was th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>gof do<strong>in</strong>g an ethnographic study of a high school gang <strong>to</strong>try <strong>to</strong> learn why students jo<strong>in</strong> it. Then three years later, I will identifygroups of students who as freshmen jo<strong>in</strong>ed for differentreasons <strong>to</strong> see how they are different.”OBJECTIVES“I see, comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g an ethnography witha casual-comparative study. Not bad, but how manyyears do you have <strong>to</strong> spend do<strong>in</strong>g this?”Study<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> what a mixed-methods study is.Describe how mixed-methods researchdiffers from other types of research.Give at least three reasons why a researchermight want <strong>to</strong> do a mixed-methods study.Describe some of the drawbacks <strong>to</strong>conduct<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study.Name the three major types of mixedmethodsresearch designs <strong>and</strong> describebriefly how they differ.by Michael K. Gardner,Department of <strong>Education</strong>al Psychology,University of UtahList some of the steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>conduct<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study.List at least five questions that can be used<strong>to</strong> evaluate a mixed-methods study.Describe briefly how matters of ethicsaffect mixed-methods research.Recognize a mixed-methods study whenyou come across one <strong>in</strong> the educationalliterature.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About Mixed-Methods <strong>Design</strong>sAfter, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 23.1: Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 23.2: Identify<strong>in</strong>g Mixed-Methods <strong>Design</strong>sActivity 23.3: <strong>Research</strong> Questions <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods<strong>Design</strong>sActivity 23.4: Identify<strong>in</strong>g Terms <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods StudiesAlice Ochoa, the super<strong>in</strong>tendent of a large urban school district, is <strong>in</strong>formed by several of her pr<strong>in</strong>cipals thatthe use of drugs by elementary <strong>and</strong> middle school students <strong>in</strong> her district is <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g at an alarm<strong>in</strong>g rate.Worried, she asks Alfonso Mart<strong>in</strong>ez, a professor at a local university, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the problem. Mart<strong>in</strong>ez decides <strong>to</strong>beg<strong>in</strong> his research by look<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the situation at a nearby middle school where the use of drugs has been reportedas be<strong>in</strong>g especially high. He beg<strong>in</strong>s by obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g permission from the school pr<strong>in</strong>cipal <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the problem<strong>and</strong> by solicit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formed consent from the students <strong>and</strong> their parents <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> his research project.Mart<strong>in</strong>ez decides <strong>to</strong> conduct a mixed-methods study by first collect<strong>in</strong>g some data us<strong>in</strong>g a quantitative survey<strong>in</strong>strument <strong>and</strong> then follow<strong>in</strong>g up by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g a sample of the students who participated <strong>in</strong> the survey. Hehopes the <strong>in</strong>terviews will provide more details about students’ responses <strong>to</strong> the questionnaire <strong>and</strong> thereby suggestsome ways <strong>to</strong> combat the drug problem.What Is Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Mixed-methods research <strong>in</strong>volves the use of bothquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle study.Those who engage <strong>in</strong> such research argue that the use ofboth methods provides a more complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof research problems than does the use of either approachalone.Although mixed-methods research dates back <strong>to</strong> the1950s, only recently has it achieved a significant place <strong>in</strong>educational research—the first journal devoted <strong>to</strong> itbegan publication <strong>in</strong> 2005. It is not surpris<strong>in</strong>g, then, thatthere are different views as <strong>to</strong> what it is. For some, the essentialfeature is that mixed-methods research comb<strong>in</strong>esmethods of data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis from both quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative traditions. As we have <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong>earlier parts of this book, the former favors numericaldata <strong>and</strong> statistical analysis, whereas the latter prefers <strong>in</strong>depth<strong>in</strong>formation, often <strong>in</strong> narrative form, frequently obta<strong>in</strong>edthrough the analysis of written communications.For others, this description is not specific enough.They <strong>in</strong>sist that other features, particularly of qualitativemethods, be present. These <strong>in</strong>clude develop<strong>in</strong>g a holisticpicture <strong>and</strong> analysis of the phenomenon be<strong>in</strong>g studiedwith an emphasis on “thick” rather than “selective”description. We do not expect this matter of def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>to</strong>be resolved soon; <strong>in</strong> the meantime, examples of bothcan be found <strong>in</strong> the current literature.It should be noted that the type of <strong>in</strong>strument used <strong>to</strong>collect data is not a major difference between quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative methodologies. Observation <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g, prom<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>in</strong>struments used <strong>in</strong> qualitativeresearch, are also commonly found <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies.It is the manner, context, <strong>and</strong> sometimes <strong>in</strong>tent thatare different. 1Some actual examples of the k<strong>in</strong>ds of mixed-methodsstudies that have been conducted by educationalresearchers are as follows:“Investigat<strong>in</strong>g Classroom Environments <strong>in</strong> Taiwan<strong>and</strong> Australia with Multiple <strong>Research</strong> Methods” 2“Us<strong>in</strong>g Mixed-Methods <strong>to</strong> Explore Lat<strong>in</strong>o Children’sLiteracy Development” 3“Mix<strong>in</strong>g Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative Methods <strong>in</strong>Sports Fan <strong>Research</strong>” 4“Strengths-based Men<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Teacher <strong>Education</strong>: AMixed-Methods Study” 5557


558 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“Numbers <strong>and</strong> Words: Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Quantitative <strong>and</strong>Qualitative Methods <strong>in</strong> a S<strong>in</strong>gle Large-Scale EvaluationStudy” 6“Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g Qualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative Methods<strong>in</strong> Health <strong>Research</strong> with M<strong>in</strong>ority Elders: Lessonsfrom a Study of Dementia Caregiv<strong>in</strong>g” 7Why Do Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>?Mixed-methods research has several strengths. First,mixed-method research can help <strong>to</strong> clarify <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> relationshipsfound <strong>to</strong> exist between variables. For example,correlational data may <strong>in</strong>dicate a slight negative relationshipbetween the time students spend at home us<strong>in</strong>g acomputer <strong>and</strong> their grades—that is, as student computertime <strong>in</strong>creased, their grades suffered. The question israised as <strong>to</strong> why such a relationship exists. Interviewswith students might show that the students fell <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> twodist<strong>in</strong>ct groups: (a) a relatively large group who use thecomputer primarily for social <strong>in</strong>teraction (e.g., e-mail <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>stant messag<strong>in</strong>g) <strong>and</strong> whose grades are suffer<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong>(b) a smaller group who use the computer for gather<strong>in</strong>gschool-related <strong>in</strong>formation (e.g., through the use ofsearch eng<strong>in</strong>es) <strong>and</strong> whose grades are comparativelyhigh. When the two groups were <strong>in</strong>itially comb<strong>in</strong>ed, thelarger number of students <strong>in</strong> the first group produced thenegative relationship found <strong>to</strong> exist between computerusage <strong>and</strong> student grades. The subsequent <strong>in</strong>terviews,however, showed that the relationship was somewhatspurious, due more <strong>to</strong> the reasons why students used theircomputers, not <strong>to</strong> the use of computers per se.Second, mixed-methods research allows us <strong>to</strong> explorerelationships between variables <strong>in</strong> depth. In this situation,qualitative methods may be used <strong>to</strong> identify the importantvariables <strong>in</strong> an area of <strong>in</strong>terest. These variables may thenbe quantified <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>strument (such as a questionnaire)that is then adm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>to</strong> large numbers of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.The variables can then be correlated with other variables.For example, <strong>in</strong>terviews with students might reveal thatstudy problems can be categorized <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> three areas: (a) <strong>to</strong>olittle time spent study<strong>in</strong>g; (b) distractions <strong>in</strong> the study environment,such as television <strong>and</strong> radio; <strong>and</strong> (c) <strong>in</strong>sufficienthelp given by parents or sibl<strong>in</strong>gs. These problemscould be further <strong>in</strong>vestigated by construct<strong>in</strong>g a 12-itemquestionnaire, with four questions for each of the threestudy problem areas. After adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g this questionnaire<strong>to</strong> 300 students, researchers could correlate the studyproblem scores with other variables, such as studentgrades, st<strong>and</strong>ardized test performance, socioeconomiclevel, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> extracurricular activities, <strong>to</strong> seeif <strong>and</strong> how any of these other variables are related <strong>to</strong> particularstudy problems.Third, mixed-methods studies can help <strong>to</strong> confirm orcross-validate relationships discovered between variables,as when quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods arecompared <strong>to</strong> see if they converge on a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>terpretationof a phenomenon. If they do not converge, the reasonsfor the lack of convergence can be <strong>in</strong>vestigated.For example, a professor specializ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> mixed-methodsresearch might be asked <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the satisfactionof middle school students with their teachers’ grad<strong>in</strong>gpractices. He or she could prepare a questionnaire designed<strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the attitudes of students <strong>and</strong> thenconduct focus group with various samples of the students.If the survey responses generally reveal satisfactionwith the teachers’ grad<strong>in</strong>g practices, yet the focusgroup participants <strong>in</strong>dicate a considerable dissatisfactionwith them, a possible explanation might be that thestudents felt that their teachers would see the responses<strong>to</strong> the surveys (<strong>and</strong> thus they were reluctant <strong>to</strong> be critical).<strong>How</strong>ever, <strong>in</strong> the focus groups, with no teachers orother adults present, they could feel free <strong>to</strong> express theirtrue feel<strong>in</strong>gs. Thus, the apparent lack of convergence <strong>in</strong>this case might be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by a third variable: whetherteachers would have access <strong>to</strong> the results.Drawbacks of Mixed-MethodsStudiesAt this po<strong>in</strong>t you might wonder why all research problemsare not addressed us<strong>in</strong>g mixed-methods designs. Severaldrawbacks exist. First, mixed-methods studies are oftenextremely time-consum<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> expensive <strong>to</strong> carry out.Second, many researchers are experienced <strong>in</strong> only onetype of research. To conduct a mixed-methods study properly,one needs expertise <strong>in</strong> both types of research. Suchexpertise takes considerable time <strong>to</strong> develop.Indeed, the resources, time, <strong>and</strong> energy required <strong>to</strong> doa mixed-methods study may be prohibitive for a s<strong>in</strong>gleresearcher <strong>to</strong> undertake. This drawback can be avoided ifmultiple researchers, with differ<strong>in</strong>g areas of expertise,work as a team. <strong>How</strong>ever, if a s<strong>in</strong>gle researcher does nothave sufficient time, resources, <strong>and</strong> skills, he or shewould probably be better off do<strong>in</strong>g a purely quantitativeor qualitative study <strong>and</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g it well.


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 559Nevertheless, mixed-methods research rema<strong>in</strong>s a viableoption <strong>to</strong> consider. Increas<strong>in</strong>g numbers of mixedmethodsstudies are be<strong>in</strong>g done, <strong>and</strong> this type of researchshould be unders<strong>to</strong>od by all who are <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g research.A (Very) Brief His<strong>to</strong>ryMixed-methods research first came <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> play <strong>in</strong> the1950s when some <strong>in</strong>itial <strong>in</strong>terest developed <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>gmore than one research method <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle study. In1957, for example, Trow commented as follows:Every cobbler th<strong>in</strong>ks that leather is the only th<strong>in</strong>g. Mostsocial scientists . . . have their favorite methods withwhich they are familiar <strong>and</strong> have some skill <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g. AndI suspect we mostly choose <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate problems thatseem vulnerable <strong>to</strong> attack through these methods. But weshould at least try <strong>to</strong> be less parochial than cobblers. Letus be done with the arguments of “participant observation”versus <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g—as we have largely dispensedwith the arguments for psychology versus sociology—<strong>and</strong> get on with the bus<strong>in</strong>ess of attack<strong>in</strong>g our problemswith the widest array of conceptual <strong>and</strong> methodological<strong>to</strong>ols that we possess <strong>and</strong> they dem<strong>and</strong>. 8Campbell <strong>and</strong> Fiske (1959) 9 advocated measur<strong>in</strong>gtraits with multiple measures, so that it was possible <strong>to</strong>separate variance due <strong>to</strong> the trait from variance due <strong>to</strong>the method used <strong>to</strong> measure the trait. Campbell <strong>and</strong>Fiske were work<strong>in</strong>g strictly <strong>in</strong> the quantitative doma<strong>in</strong>,but their multitrait-multimethod matrix suggested theimportance of separat<strong>in</strong>g the phenomenon under studyfrom the <strong>to</strong>ols be<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>to</strong> study it. Denz<strong>in</strong> (1978) 10<strong>and</strong> Jick (1979) 11 both have been credited with apply<strong>in</strong>gthe term triangulation <strong>to</strong> research methods. Triangulation(or, more precisely, methodological triangulation)<strong>in</strong>volves us<strong>in</strong>g different methods <strong>and</strong>/or types of data <strong>to</strong>study the same research question. If the results are <strong>in</strong>agreement, they help validate the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g of each. Denz<strong>in</strong>used triangulation when he utilized multiple datasources <strong>to</strong> study the same phenomenon. Jick discussedthe use of triangulation with<strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle method (quantitativeor qualitative) <strong>and</strong> across methods (both quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative). He noted how the strengths of onemethod could offset the weaknesses of another. 12In Chapter 18, we po<strong>in</strong>ted out that quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative researchers differ <strong>in</strong> the set of beliefs or assumptionsthat guide the way they approach their <strong>in</strong>vestigations,<strong>and</strong> that these assumptions are related <strong>to</strong> theirworldviews—that is, the views they hold concern<strong>in</strong>g,among other th<strong>in</strong>gs, the nature of reality <strong>and</strong> the processof research. 13 As we mentioned there, the quantitative approachis associated with the philosophy of positivism.Qualitative methodologists, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, advocatea more “artistic” approach <strong>to</strong> research, adher<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> otherworldviews (such as postmodernism). 14These differences have caused many researchers <strong>to</strong>believe that quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative researchmethodologies were a dicho<strong>to</strong>my: an either-or propositionwith no middle ground. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the 1970s <strong>and</strong>1980s, <strong>in</strong> fact, many researchers on both sides of theissue argued strongly that the two methods (often referred<strong>to</strong> as “paradigms”) could not be comb<strong>in</strong>ed. Manyresearchers still hold <strong>to</strong> this view. In 1985, Rossman <strong>and</strong>Wilson 15 referred <strong>to</strong> those who stated that paradigmscould not be mixed as purists; those who could adapttheir methods <strong>to</strong> the particulars of a situation, theycalled situationists; <strong>and</strong> those who believed that multipleparadigms could be utilized <strong>in</strong> research, they calledpragmatists. Although the question of mix<strong>in</strong>g paradigmsstill exists, more researchers are embrac<strong>in</strong>g pragmatismas the best philosophical foundation for mixedmethodsresearch. 16Pragmatists proposed that researchers should usewhatever works. The most important element <strong>in</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>ga decision about which research method or methods <strong>to</strong>employ should be the research question at h<strong>and</strong>. Worldviews<strong>and</strong> preferences about methods should take aback seat, <strong>and</strong> the researcher should choose the researchapproach that most readily illum<strong>in</strong>ates the researchquestion. That research approach may be quantitative,qualitative, or a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of the two.Consider an example: The super<strong>in</strong>tendent of a largeschool district hires a consultant <strong>to</strong> carry out a phonesurvey <strong>to</strong> ask respondents a series of questions regard<strong>in</strong>ghow much they would be will<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> pay <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creasedtaxes for particular expenditures (e.g., such th<strong>in</strong>gs assmaller class size, pay raises for teachers, exp<strong>and</strong>ed athleticsprograms, <strong>and</strong> so forth). She is disappo<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>to</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d an unwill<strong>in</strong>gness on the part of those surveyed <strong>to</strong>fund any of the options they list at anywhere near theamounts that would be needed. So she decides <strong>to</strong> havethe consultant conduct focus groups <strong>to</strong> try <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhy. Are these two types of <strong>in</strong>formation fundamentally<strong>in</strong>compatible? By no means. Each type supplies the districtsuper<strong>in</strong>tendent with useful <strong>in</strong>formation. The quantitativedata tells her what the public will accept, whilethe focus groups tell her why they responded as they did,thereby help<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> clarify the negative response.


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCHAre Some Methods Incompatiblewith Others?Some researchers <strong>in</strong> education (as well as other discipl<strong>in</strong>es)argue that quantitative methods are <strong>in</strong>compatible withqualitative methods. They state that the basic assumptions ofeach method actually prevent the use of the other <strong>in</strong> the samestudy. Many qualitative researchers argue that qualitativemethods are based on a po<strong>in</strong>t of view about the nature of theworld—that reality is constructed, not revealed. S<strong>in</strong>ce every<strong>in</strong>dividual sees the world <strong>in</strong> his or her own way, there is nos<strong>in</strong>gle reality “out there” <strong>to</strong> be discovered; <strong>in</strong> fact, multiplerealities exist. Quantitative researchers, on the other h<strong>and</strong>,reject this po<strong>in</strong>t of view. Still other researchers would argue thatthis notion of <strong>in</strong>compatibility has been overblown. Krathwohl,for example, has stated that “quantitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs compress<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> summary numbers the trends <strong>and</strong> tendencies expressed<strong>in</strong> words <strong>in</strong> qualitative reports. In many <strong>in</strong>stances, counts ofcoded qualitative data might have produced data similar <strong>to</strong> thequantitative summaries. . . Many problems, <strong>in</strong> fact, actuallyrequire more than any one method can deliver; the answer, ofcourse, is a multiple-method approach.”**David R. Krathwohl (1998). Methods of educational <strong>and</strong> socialscience research: An <strong>in</strong>tegrated approach, 2nd ed. New York:Longman, p. 619.Types of Mixed-Methods<strong>Design</strong>sWhile quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods may becomb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> any way suitable <strong>to</strong> address a particularresearch question, certa<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods designs occurwith enough frequency for us <strong>to</strong> look at them <strong>in</strong> detail.Three major types of mixed-methods design exist: theexplora<strong>to</strong>ry design, the explana<strong>to</strong>ry design, <strong>and</strong> thetriangulation design. 17 Each <strong>in</strong>volves a comb<strong>in</strong>ation ofqualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative data.THE EXPLORATORY DESIGNIn this design, researchers first use a qualitative method<strong>to</strong> discover the important variables underly<strong>in</strong>g a phenomenonof <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>form a second, quantitative,method. (See Figure 23.1.) Next, they seek <strong>to</strong> discoverthe relationships among these variables. This typeof design is often used <strong>in</strong> the construction of questionnairesor rat<strong>in</strong>g scales designed <strong>to</strong> measure various <strong>to</strong>picsof <strong>in</strong>terest.In the explora<strong>to</strong>ry design, results of the qualitativephase give direction <strong>to</strong> the quantitative method, <strong>and</strong>quantitative results are used <strong>to</strong> validate or extend thequalitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. Data analysis <strong>in</strong> the explora<strong>to</strong>ry designis separate, correspond<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the first, qualitative,phase of the study <strong>and</strong> the second, quantitative, phase ofthe study. The rationale underly<strong>in</strong>g the explora<strong>to</strong>ry designis <strong>to</strong> explore a phenomenon or <strong>to</strong> identify importantthemes. In addition, it is especially useful when oneneeds <strong>to</strong> develop <strong>and</strong> test a particular type of <strong>in</strong>strument.The illustration at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of this chaptergives an example of an explora<strong>to</strong>ry design. The studentwants <strong>to</strong> use a qualitative method (ethnography), presumably<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g content analysis of <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>terviews<strong>and</strong> perhaps other narratives (such as essays), <strong>to</strong>identify students’ reasons for jo<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a high schoolgang <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> see how gang membership affected them.Subsequently, she would use a causal-comparativedesign <strong>to</strong> compare subgroups of students who haddifferent reasons for jo<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g when they were freshmen.To do this, she would have <strong>to</strong> sort out the subgroups,us<strong>in</strong>g her ethnographic data. She would then collectdata from them as seniors <strong>to</strong> see how these groupsdiffer <strong>in</strong> ways suggested by the ethnography. This willrequire additional data collection where the preferencewould be for quantitative <strong>in</strong>formation that may require<strong>in</strong>strument development.Figure 23.1Explora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>Source: Adapted fromCreswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark,2006.Qualitative study(higher priority)Quantitative study(lower priority)TimeComb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret results560


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 561Quantitative study(higher priority)Qualitative study(lower priority)TimeComb<strong>in</strong>e <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpret resultsFigure 23.2Explana<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>Design</strong>Source: Adapted fromCreswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark,2006.THE EXPLANATORY DESIGNSometimes a researcher will do a quantitative study, butwill require additional <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> flesh out the results.This is the purpose beh<strong>in</strong>d the explana<strong>to</strong>ry design. In thisdesign, the researcher first carries out a quantitativemethod <strong>and</strong> then uses a qualitative method <strong>to</strong> follow up<strong>and</strong> ref<strong>in</strong>e the quantitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (see Figure 23.2). Thetwo types of data are analyzed separately, with the resultsof the qualitative analysis used by the researcher <strong>to</strong>exp<strong>and</strong> upon the results of the quantitative study.For example, one of the authors was a co-<strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r,some years ago, <strong>in</strong> a study <strong>in</strong> which four fifth-grade teacherseach taught mathematics us<strong>in</strong>g ability group<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>non-group<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> alternate semesters <strong>in</strong> a counterbalancedexperiment. The study had the unusual feature, <strong>in</strong> schoolresearch, of r<strong>and</strong>om assignments of students <strong>to</strong> teachers.The major f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g was that one teacher achieved substantiallyhigher achievement ga<strong>in</strong>s with non-group<strong>in</strong>gwhereas the other three had greater ga<strong>in</strong>s with group<strong>in</strong>g. Afollow-up qualitative study us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> narrativedescription of classroom activities could have tested the<strong>in</strong>formal observation that the one teacher was more adeptat <strong>in</strong>dividualiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction than the three teacherswhose students learned more with group<strong>in</strong>g. 18THE TRIANGULATION DESIGNIn this design, the researcher uses both quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative methods <strong>to</strong> study the same phenomenon <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e if the two converge upon a s<strong>in</strong>gle underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof the research problem be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated. If theydo not, then the researcher must explore why the twomethods provide different pictures. Quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative methods are given equal priority, <strong>and</strong> all dataare collected simultaneously (see Figure 23.3). The datamay be analyzed <strong>to</strong>gether or separately. If analyzed <strong>to</strong>gether,data from the qualitative study may have <strong>to</strong> beconverted <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> quantitative data (e.g., assign<strong>in</strong>g numericalcodes <strong>in</strong> a process that is called quantiz<strong>in</strong>g) or thequantitative data may have <strong>to</strong> be converted <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> qualitativedata (e.g., provid<strong>in</strong>g narratives <strong>in</strong> a process that iscalled qualitiz<strong>in</strong>g). If the data are analyzed separately,the convergence or divergence of the results would thenbe discussed. The underly<strong>in</strong>g rationale for the use of thetriangulation design is that the strengths of the twomethods will complement each other <strong>and</strong> offset eachmethod’s respective weaknesses.Consider an example. One of the authors used amodified triangulation design <strong>to</strong> study four high schoolsocial studies teachers identified by their peers as outst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.19 He attempted <strong>to</strong> pa<strong>in</strong>t a portrait of what happenson a daily basis <strong>in</strong> their classrooms <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> identifyeffective teacher techniques <strong>and</strong> behaviors. To this end,he used several qualitative techniques, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g extensive<strong>in</strong>-class observation us<strong>in</strong>g a daily log <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewswith students <strong>and</strong> teachers. He also used a numberof quantitative <strong>in</strong>struments, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g performancechecklists, rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, <strong>and</strong> discussion flowcharts. Hedeveloped detailed descriptions of each teacher’s behaviors,teach<strong>in</strong>g style, <strong>and</strong> techniques <strong>and</strong> comparedthe teachers for similarities <strong>and</strong> differences. Triangulationwas achieved not only by compar<strong>in</strong>g teacher <strong>in</strong>terviews,student <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> observations, but also bycompar<strong>in</strong>g these with the quantitative measures ofclassroom <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>and</strong> achievement.One illustrative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g was that all four teachers emphasizedsmall-group work, as revealed by observation,teacher <strong>in</strong>terviews, <strong>and</strong> student rat<strong>in</strong>gs. Overall, thestudy’s f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs supported frequently recommendedteach<strong>in</strong>g strategies, but also suggested some that have notreceived much attention <strong>in</strong> the literature. These <strong>in</strong>cludedextensive personal <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> the lives of students,promot<strong>in</strong>g social <strong>in</strong>teraction both <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> outside of theclassroom, <strong>and</strong> consciously attend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> nonverbal cues.Far more <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>sight was obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> thisstudy through the use of both methods than if a purelyquantitative or qualitative method had been used.Qualitative study(equal priority)Quantitative study(equal priority)TimeFigure 23.3 Triangulation <strong>Design</strong>Comb<strong>in</strong>e results<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretSource: Adapted from Creswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark, 2006.


562 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e“I’m th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g of us<strong>in</strong>gan explora<strong>to</strong>ry, triangulation designfor my mixed-methods study.”“You’ve got <strong>to</strong> be kidd<strong>in</strong>g! I th<strong>in</strong>kyou’d better review those designs!”Other Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong> <strong>Design</strong> Issues 20Advocacy Lenses. A fac<strong>to</strong>r that can be can be used<strong>to</strong> categorize mixed-methods designs is the presence orabsence of an “advocacy lens.” An advocacy lensoccurs when the researcher’s worldview implies that thepurpose of research is <strong>to</strong> advocate for the improvedtreatment of research participants <strong>in</strong> the world outsideresearch. Examples of worldviews that <strong>in</strong>volve an advocacylens would be fem<strong>in</strong>ist theory, race-based theories,<strong>and</strong> critical theory. We have discussed the major mixedmethodsdesigns as if there were no advocacy lenspresent; however, each design can be approached withan explicit advocacy lens. A researcher might, for <strong>in</strong>stance,be <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> triangulat<strong>in</strong>g quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative methods concern<strong>in</strong>g student academicperformance <strong>in</strong> elementary school, compar<strong>in</strong>g performance<strong>in</strong> a primarily white suburban school with that ofa primarily black <strong>in</strong>ner-city school. The purpose of theresearch might be <strong>to</strong> improve conditions, <strong>and</strong> academicperformance, for black <strong>in</strong>ner-city students.Sampl<strong>in</strong>g. Sampl<strong>in</strong>g is as important <strong>in</strong> mixed methodsstudies as it is <strong>in</strong> any other type of research. Qualitativeresearchers typically use purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g,where<strong>in</strong> researchers <strong>in</strong>tentionally select participantswho are <strong>in</strong>formed about or have experience with thecentral concept(s) be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated. Usually samplesare small, the <strong>in</strong>tent be<strong>in</strong>g that a comparatively smallnumber of <strong>in</strong>dividuals can provide a considerableamount of detailed, <strong>in</strong>-depth <strong>in</strong>formation that large-sizesamples would not.Quantitative researchers typically want <strong>to</strong> choose <strong>in</strong>dividualswho are representative of a larger populationso that results can be generalized <strong>to</strong> that population.Generally, r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g strategies are preferred,but often this is not possible, especially <strong>in</strong> educationalsett<strong>in</strong>gs. Thus convenience, systematic, or purposivesamples must be used, with replication suggested <strong>and</strong>encouraged. Sample sizes are usually much larger than<strong>in</strong> qualitative studies.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, researchers must make a number ofdecisions with regard <strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g before beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g amixed-methods study, such as the relative size ofthe two samples <strong>in</strong>volved, whether they are <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 563the same participants, whether one sample is <strong>to</strong> be subsumedwith<strong>in</strong> the other, or whether the participantsshould be completely different for the two samples.Mixed-Model Studies. Tashakkori <strong>and</strong> Teddlie(1998) def<strong>in</strong>e mixed-model studies as those that “comb<strong>in</strong>ethe qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative approaches with<strong>in</strong> severaldifferent phases of the research process” (p. 19). In a s<strong>in</strong>glestudy, this might <strong>in</strong>volve an experimental study,followed by qualitative data collection, followed by quantitativeanalysis of the data after it had been converted <strong>to</strong>numbers. In mixed-model studies, the quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative approach <strong>to</strong> research may be addressed dur<strong>in</strong>geach of three phases of the research process: (1) the typeof <strong>in</strong>vestigation (confirma<strong>to</strong>ry [typically quantitative]versus explora<strong>to</strong>ry [typically qualitative]); (2) quantitativedata collection <strong>and</strong> operations versus qualitative datacollection <strong>and</strong> operations (3) statistical analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferenceversus qualitative analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ference. Indeed,Tashakkori <strong>and</strong> Teddlie (1998) use these dimensions <strong>to</strong>create a classification system for mixed-models research(see pp. 56–58). As may be obvious, this is a more complicatedsystem for classify<strong>in</strong>g research designs, <strong>and</strong> at leastsome of the comb<strong>in</strong>ations of the three phases of researchoccur very rarely <strong>in</strong> practice.Steps <strong>in</strong> Conduct<strong>in</strong>ga Mixed-Methods StudyDevelop a Clear Rationale for Do<strong>in</strong>g aMixed-Methods Study. A researcher should askhimself or herself why both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativemethods are needed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the problem at h<strong>and</strong>.If the reason<strong>in</strong>g is not clear, a mixed-methods studymay not be appropriate.Develop <strong>Research</strong> Questions for Both theQualitative <strong>and</strong> Quantitative Methods. As<strong>in</strong> all research, the nature of the research question orquestions will determ<strong>in</strong>e the type of design <strong>to</strong> be used.Many research questions can be addressed us<strong>in</strong>g eitheror both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research techniques.For example, suppose a researcher posed thisquestion: “Why don’t Asian-American college studentsmake greater use of college counsel<strong>in</strong>g centers?” He orshe might beg<strong>in</strong> by <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g a sample of Asian-American college students about their perceptions ofthe k<strong>in</strong>ds of students who use these centers. He or shemight then supplement these <strong>in</strong>terviews with survey <strong>in</strong>formationprovided by these centers about the proportionof students from different ethnic groups who usethe centers. The survey data might <strong>in</strong>dicate what degreeof underutilization exists, while the <strong>in</strong>terview datamight po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>to</strong> student perceptions that produce thisunderutilization.In many <strong>in</strong>stances the formation of a generalresearch question can lead <strong>to</strong> the development of some<strong>in</strong>dividual research hypotheses, some of which maylend themselves <strong>to</strong> a quantitative approach <strong>and</strong> some ofwhich may require a qualitative method. These “lowerlevel”hypotheses often can suggest specific analyses(either quantitative or qualitative) that will answer specificquestions. In the previous example, one suchhypothesis might be that Asian-American college studentsdo <strong>in</strong> fact underutilize college mental health counsel<strong>in</strong>gservices, which the survey data can address. If thesurvey results <strong>in</strong>dicate that Asian-American collegestudents utilize such centers less often than do studentsfrom other ethnic groups, the reasons can be addressed<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terviews. You will recall that qualitativeresearchers often prefer that hypotheses emerge as astudy progresses. This is much more likely <strong>to</strong> happenwith the explora<strong>to</strong>ry design.Decide If a Mixed-Methods Study Is Feasible.Mixed-methods studies, by their very nature, require theresearcher or research team <strong>to</strong> be experienced <strong>in</strong> bothquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research methods. It is rarethat a s<strong>in</strong>gle <strong>in</strong>dividual would have all of the requisiteskills necessary <strong>to</strong> conduct a mixed-methods researchstudy. The key question for anyone contemplat<strong>in</strong>g amixed-method study is this: Do you have the time, energy,<strong>and</strong> resources necessary <strong>to</strong> conduct such a study? Ifnot, can you collaborate with others who have the skills<strong>and</strong> expertise you lack? If you lack the necessary skillsor resources, it may <strong>in</strong>deed be better <strong>to</strong> re-conceptualizea study as basically a quantitative or qualitative <strong>in</strong>vestigationthan <strong>to</strong> beg<strong>in</strong> a mixed-methods study that cannotbe completed with<strong>in</strong> the time available.Determ<strong>in</strong>e the Mixed-Methods <strong>Design</strong> MostAppropriate <strong>to</strong> the <strong>Research</strong> Question orQuestions. As we mentioned earlier, there are essentiallythree mixed-methods designs from which aresearcher can choose. The triangulation design is appropriatewhen the researcher is try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> see if quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative methods converge on a s<strong>in</strong>gleunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of a phenomenon. The explana<strong>to</strong>ry design


***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSWhat <strong>to</strong> Do About Contradic<strong>to</strong>ryF<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsOn occasion, the quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs maycontradict each other. What does a researcher do if tha<strong>to</strong>ccurs? Three possible approaches suggest themselves.1. Present the two f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> parallel <strong>and</strong> state that moreresearch is needed.2. Collect additional data <strong>to</strong> resolve the contradiction,provided that this is both feasible <strong>and</strong> timely.3. View the problem as a spr<strong>in</strong>gboard for new directions of<strong>in</strong>quiry.is appropriate if one <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> use qualitative data <strong>to</strong>exp<strong>and</strong> upon the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of a quantitative study (orvice-versa). The explora<strong>to</strong>ry design is appropriate whenone is try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> first identify the relevant variables thatmay underlie a phenomenon <strong>and</strong> then later study<strong>in</strong>gthe relationships among these variables, or when <strong>in</strong>formationis needed <strong>to</strong> assist <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g quantitative<strong>in</strong>strumentation.Collect <strong>and</strong> Analyze the Data. Data collection<strong>and</strong> analysis procedures described earlier <strong>in</strong> this text areapplicable <strong>and</strong> appropriate <strong>to</strong> all mixed-methods studies,depend<strong>in</strong>g on the particular methods used. The differenceis that two different types of data are collected<strong>and</strong> analyzed, sometimes sequentially (as <strong>in</strong> theexplora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> explana<strong>to</strong>ry designs) <strong>and</strong> sometimesconcurrently (as <strong>in</strong> the triangulation design).Triangulation designs may also <strong>in</strong>volve the conversionof one type of data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the other type. As we mentionedearlier, the conversion of qualitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>quantitative data is referred <strong>to</strong> as quantiz<strong>in</strong>g. For<strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>in</strong>terviews may lead a researcher <strong>to</strong> believethere are three types of elementary science learners:(1) manipula<strong>to</strong>rs, who like <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>uch <strong>and</strong> change objects<strong>in</strong> their environments; (2) memorizers, who attempt <strong>to</strong>memorize rote facts from textbooks; <strong>and</strong> (3) cooperativelearners, who like <strong>to</strong> discuss <strong>to</strong>pics with other students<strong>in</strong> the class. By count<strong>in</strong>g the number of eachtype of learner <strong>in</strong> each of a number of science classes,the researcher could convert the qualitative data (thelearner types) <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> quantitative data (the numbers ofeach type).Aga<strong>in</strong>, as mentioned earlier, the conversion ofquantitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> qualitative data is referred <strong>to</strong> asqualitiz<strong>in</strong>g. For <strong>in</strong>stance, <strong>in</strong>dividuals who share variousquantitative characteristics may be grouped <strong>to</strong>gether<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> types. A researcher might categorize one group ofstudents that is never tardy, always turns <strong>in</strong> assignedwork, <strong>and</strong> writes long papers as “obsessive students.”By way of contrast, the researcher might categorize asecond group that is frequently tardy, often fails <strong>to</strong> turn<strong>in</strong> assigned work, <strong>and</strong> writes short papers as “un<strong>in</strong>terestedstudents.”Write Up the Results <strong>in</strong> a Manner Consistentwith the <strong>Design</strong> Be<strong>in</strong>g Used. In writ<strong>in</strong>g upthe results of a mixed-methods study, the ways <strong>in</strong> whichthe data were collected <strong>and</strong> analyzed are usually <strong>in</strong>tegrated<strong>in</strong> triangulation designs but treated separately forexplora<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong> explana<strong>to</strong>ry designs.Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g a Mixed-MethodsStudyEvaluation is necessary for all research, not just mixedmethodsresearch. <strong>How</strong>ever, given that mixed-methodsresearch <strong>in</strong>volves compar<strong>in</strong>g different methods, it is ofparticular importance here. Due <strong>to</strong> the fact that mixedmethodsstudies always <strong>in</strong>volve both quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative data <strong>and</strong> frequently two different phases ofdata collection, the evaluation of such studies is oftendifficult. Nevertheless, each method should be evaluatedaccord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the criteria we have suggested <strong>and</strong>used with other methods. 21Ask yourself if both qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativedata played a role <strong>in</strong> the conclusions reached. In goodmixed-methods research, these two methods shouldeither complement each other or address differentsub-questions related <strong>to</strong> the larger research questionaddressed by the study. Sometimes a researcher willcollect quantitative or qualitative data, but it will notplay a role <strong>in</strong> answer<strong>in</strong>g any of the important researchquestions. In these cases, the data is just an add-on(perhaps because the researcher “likes” that k<strong>in</strong>dof data), <strong>and</strong> the project is not truly a mixed-methodsapproach.564


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 565Second, ask yourself if the study conta<strong>in</strong>s threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity (as quantitative researchers refer <strong>to</strong> it)or credibility (as qualitative researchers refer <strong>to</strong> it). Arethere alternative explanations for the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, beyondthose given by the author? What steps have been taken<strong>to</strong> ensure that the design is tight <strong>and</strong> that high levels of<strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>and</strong> credibility have been achieved?Some of the appropriate steps have been described elsewhere<strong>in</strong> this text <strong>in</strong> discussions of quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative research.Third, ask yourself about the generalizability (asquantitative researchers refer <strong>to</strong> it) or transferability(as qualitative researchers refer <strong>to</strong> it) of the results. Dothe results found <strong>in</strong> the present study extend beyond thedoma<strong>in</strong> studied <strong>to</strong> other contexts <strong>and</strong> other <strong>in</strong>dividuals?Is the description of the qualitative results sufficient <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e if they would be useful <strong>to</strong> other researchers <strong>in</strong>other situations? The answers <strong>to</strong> these questions areessential because a study without generalizability(external validity) or transferability is of little <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong>anyone other than the study’s author.Ethics <strong>in</strong> Mixed-Methods<strong>Research</strong>Ethical concerns <strong>and</strong> questions affect mixed-methodsstudies just as much as they do any of the other k<strong>in</strong>ds ofresearch we have described <strong>and</strong> discussed <strong>in</strong> this text. Webriefly cover three of the most important—protect<strong>in</strong>gparticipant identity, treat<strong>in</strong>g participants with respect,<strong>and</strong> protect<strong>in</strong>g participants from both physical <strong>and</strong> psychologicalharm, on pages 433–434 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 18, aswell as discuss<strong>in</strong>g these ideas <strong>in</strong> some detail <strong>in</strong> Chapter 4.SummaryIn sum, it is apparent that mixed-methods studies are becom<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly common <strong>in</strong> educational research.Their value lies <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativemethods <strong>in</strong> ways that complement each other. Thestrengths of each approach <strong>to</strong> a large degree mitigate theweaknesses of the other. While mixed-methods researchdesigns are potentially quite attractive, however, theyshould be approached with the realization that <strong>to</strong> carrythem out well requires considerable time, energy, <strong>and</strong> resources.Furthermore, researchers need <strong>to</strong> be skilled <strong>in</strong>both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods, or <strong>to</strong> collaboratewith those who possess the skills they lack.An Example of Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong>In the rema<strong>in</strong>der of this chapter, we present a publishedexample of mixed-methods research, followed by a critiqueof its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> ourcritiques of the different types of research studies weanalyzed <strong>in</strong> earlier chapters, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 53(3) (2006): 279–287. Reproduced with permission.Perceived Family Support, Acculturation,<strong>and</strong> Life Satisfaction <strong>in</strong> MexicanAmerican Youth: A Mixed-MethodsExplorationL. M. EdwardsMarquette UniversityS. J. LopezUniversity of KansasIn this article, the authors describe a mixed-methods study designed <strong>to</strong> explore perceivedfamily support, acculturation, <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction among 266 Mexican Americanadolescents. Specifically, the authors conducted a thematic analysis of open-endedresponses <strong>to</strong> a question about life satisfaction <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> participants’ perceptionsof fac<strong>to</strong>rs that contributed <strong>to</strong> their overall satisfaction with life. The authors also conductedhierarchical regression analyses <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractivecontributions of perceived support from family <strong>and</strong> Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo acculturationorientations on life satisfaction. Convergence of mixed-methods f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs demonstratedthat perceived family support <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation were significant predic<strong>to</strong>rs oflife satisfaction <strong>in</strong> these adolescents. Implications, limitations, <strong>and</strong> directions for furtherresearch are discussed.JustificationPsychologists have identified <strong>and</strong> studied a number of challenges faced by Lat<strong>in</strong>oyouth (e.g., juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quency, gang activity, school dropout, alcohol <strong>and</strong> drug abuse),yet little scholarly time <strong>and</strong> energy have been spent on explor<strong>in</strong>g how these adolescentssuccessfully navigate their development <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> adulthood or how they experience wellbe<strong>in</strong>g(Rodriguez & Morrobel, 2004). <strong>Research</strong>ers have yet <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> the personalcharacteristics that play a role <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents’ satisfaction with life or how certa<strong>in</strong>cultural values <strong>and</strong>/or strengths <strong>and</strong> resources are related <strong>to</strong> their well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Answers <strong>to</strong>these questions can beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists with a deeper underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof how Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g, which can, <strong>in</strong> turn, hopefully allowresearchers <strong>to</strong> work <strong>to</strong> improve well-be<strong>in</strong>g for those who struggle <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d it.Lat<strong>in</strong>o 1 youth are a grow<strong>in</strong>g presence <strong>in</strong> most communities with<strong>in</strong> the UnitedStates. The U.S. Census Bureau projects that by the year 2010, 20% of young people betweenthe ages of 10 <strong>and</strong> 20 years will be of Hispanic orig<strong>in</strong>. Furthermore, it is projectedthat by the year 2020, one <strong>in</strong> five children will be Hispanic, <strong>and</strong> the Hispanic adolescentpopulation will <strong>in</strong>crease by 50% (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000, 2001). Whereas adolescenceis a unique developmental period for all youth, Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents <strong>in</strong> particular may faceadditional challenges as a result of their ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority status (Vazquez Garcia, Garcia1 In this article, the terms Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>and</strong> Hispanic have been used <strong>in</strong>terchangeably. Specifically, <strong>in</strong> cases <strong>in</strong>which research is summarized, the descrip<strong>to</strong>rs used by the authors were reta<strong>in</strong>ed. The participant sample,however, was restricted <strong>to</strong> adolescents who self-identified as “Mexican” or “Mexican American” <strong>and</strong> arethus described as such.566


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 567Coll, Erkut, Alarcon, & Tropp, 2000). These youth generally have undergone socializationexperiences of their Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture (known as enculturation) <strong>and</strong> also must learn <strong>to</strong>acculturate <strong>to</strong> the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture <strong>to</strong> some degree (Knight, Bernal, Cota, Garza, &Ocampo, 1993). Navigat<strong>in</strong>g the dem<strong>and</strong>s of these cultural contexts can be challeng<strong>in</strong>g,<strong>and</strong> yet many Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> positive outcomes. The <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gnumbers of Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth, along with the counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology field’s imperative <strong>to</strong> provideculturally competent services, require that professionals cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> thefull range of psychological function<strong>in</strong>g for members of this unique population.Counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists have cont<strong>in</strong>ually emphasized the importance of wellbe<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g client strengths <strong>in</strong> theory, research, <strong>and</strong> practice(Lopez et al., 2006; Walsh, 2003). This commitment <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the whole person,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal <strong>and</strong> contextual assets <strong>and</strong> challenges, has been one hallmark of thefield (Super, 1955; Tyler, 1973) <strong>and</strong> has <strong>in</strong>fluenced a variety of research about optimalhuman function<strong>in</strong>g (see D. W. Sue & Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003). More recent discussions <strong>in</strong> thisarea have underscored the importance of identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> nurtur<strong>in</strong>g cultural values <strong>and</strong>strengths <strong>in</strong> people of color (e.g., family, religious faith, biculturalism), be<strong>in</strong>g cautious <strong>to</strong>acknowledge that strengths are not universal <strong>and</strong> may differ accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> context or culturalbackground (Lopez et al., 2006; Lopez et al., 2002; D. W. Sue & Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003),<strong>and</strong> may be <strong>in</strong>fluenced by certa<strong>in</strong> with<strong>in</strong>-group differences such as acculturation level(Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003; Zane & Mak, 2003).As scholars respond <strong>to</strong> the emerg<strong>in</strong>g need <strong>to</strong> explore strengths among Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth,the importance of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g these resources <strong>and</strong> values with<strong>in</strong> a cultural context isevident. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g how Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents experience well-be<strong>in</strong>g from their ownperspectives <strong>and</strong> vantage po<strong>in</strong>ts is <strong>in</strong>tegral, as theories from other cultural worldviews maynot be applicable <strong>to</strong> their lives (Auerbach & Silverste<strong>in</strong>, 2003; Lopez et al., 2002; D. W. Sue& Constant<strong>in</strong>e, 2003). Furthermore, it is necessary <strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> test propositions aboutthe role of certa<strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultural values, such as the importance of family, <strong>in</strong> overall wellbe<strong>in</strong>g.Given that many Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents <strong>to</strong>day navigate bicultural contexts <strong>and</strong> adhere<strong>to</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o traditions <strong>and</strong> cus<strong>to</strong>ms <strong>to</strong> differ<strong>in</strong>g degrees (Romero & Roberts, 2003), it is likelythat the role family plays <strong>in</strong> adolescent well-be<strong>in</strong>g is complex <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluenced by <strong>in</strong>dividualdifferences such as acculturation. In this study, we sought <strong>to</strong> explore the relationships betweenthese variables by focus<strong>in</strong>g specifically on perceived family support, life satisfaction,<strong>and</strong> acculturation among Mexican American youth.Def<strong>in</strong>itionsJustificationPurposePERCEIVED FAMILY SUPPORT, ACCULTURATION,AND LIFE SATISFACTION AMONG LATINO YOUTHThe importance of family has been noted as a core Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultural value (Castillo, Conoley,& Brossart, 2004; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003; Paniagua, 1998; Sabogal, Mar<strong>in</strong>, Otero-Sabogal,Mar<strong>in</strong>, & Perez-Stable, 1987). Familismo (familism) is the term used <strong>to</strong> describe the importanceof extended family ties <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture as well as the strong identification <strong>and</strong>attachment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with their families (Tri<strong>and</strong>is, Mar<strong>in</strong>, Betancourt, Lisansky, &Chang, 1982). Familism is not unique <strong>to</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture <strong>and</strong> has been noted as an importantvalue for other ethnic groups such as African Americans, Asian Americans, <strong>and</strong> AmericanIndians (Cooper, 1999; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003). Nevertheless, it is considered a central aspec<strong>to</strong>f Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> some studies, it has been shown <strong>to</strong> be valued by Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>in</strong>dividualsmore than by non-Lat<strong>in</strong>o Whites (Ga<strong>in</strong>es et al., 1997; Mar<strong>in</strong>, 1993; M<strong>in</strong>del, 1980).In a study of familismo among Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents, Vazquez Garcia et al. (2000)found that the length of time youth had been <strong>in</strong> the United States did not affect their adherence<strong>to</strong> the value of familismo. These results demonstrated that the longer adolescentsDef<strong>in</strong>itionPrior research


568 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePrior researchDef<strong>in</strong>itionJustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionhad been <strong>in</strong> the United States, the less they endorsed the value of respe<strong>to</strong> (respect), buttheir endorsement of familismo did not change. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs highlight the central <strong>and</strong>endur<strong>in</strong>g role that family plays <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture, for both adults <strong>and</strong> adolescents.Most research about familismo has assessed the attitud<strong>in</strong>al dimension of this construct,which has been hypothesized <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude a sense of perceived support from family,family obligations, solidarity, reciprocity, <strong>and</strong> family as referents (Mar<strong>in</strong>, 1992; Mar<strong>in</strong> &Gamba, 2003; Sabogal et al., 1987). It appears that perceived family support may be thekey component of this value, as evidenced by research with Lat<strong>in</strong>o adults that <strong>in</strong>vestigateddifferences <strong>in</strong> aspects of familismo across acculturation levels. For example, Sabogalet al. found that as acculturation <strong>in</strong>creased, familial obligations <strong>and</strong> family as referentsdecreased <strong>in</strong> respondents. Perceived family support scores, however, did not differby acculturation level, place of birth or grow<strong>in</strong>g up, or generation.Taken <strong>to</strong>gether, research about the importance of family suggests that familismo isa core Lat<strong>in</strong>o cultural value <strong>and</strong> that perceived support from family is a crucial componen<strong>to</strong>f this value that is not affected by acculturation level <strong>in</strong> adults (Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003;Sabogal et al., 1987). In addition, research about family with Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth has demonstrateda relationship between aspects of familism <strong>and</strong> a lower risk of substance abuse(Unger et al., 2002), lower juvenile del<strong>in</strong>quency rates (Pabon, 1998), <strong>and</strong> other harmfulbehaviors (Mar<strong>in</strong>, 1993; Moore, 1970; Rodriguez & Kosloski, 1998). Less is known, however,about the relationship between perceived family support <strong>and</strong> well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>/orother positive psychological variables. Indeed, f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs about family <strong>and</strong> various negativeoutcomes cannot be generalized <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction or well-be<strong>in</strong>g because well-be<strong>in</strong>g ismore than just the absence of pathology or illness (Seligman, 2002). It is important <strong>to</strong>identify the variables that relate <strong>to</strong> positive outcomes <strong>in</strong> youth <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> those thatare related <strong>to</strong> negative outcomes <strong>and</strong> pathology (Gilman & Huebner, 2003).Life satisfaction, which has been identified as an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s appraisal of his or herlife, is a commonly used <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>r of well-be<strong>in</strong>g. As the cognitive, judgmental componen<strong>to</strong>f subjective well-be<strong>in</strong>g (SWB; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999), life satisfactioncan be dist<strong>in</strong>guished from affective components of well-be<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., positive <strong>and</strong> negativeaffect), <strong>and</strong> thus transcends the immediate effects of mood states (Diener et al.). Lifesatisfaction appears <strong>to</strong> relate <strong>to</strong> important <strong>in</strong>tra- <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpersonal outcomes (Gilman &Huebner, 2000), <strong>and</strong> numerous studies with adults suggest that life satisfaction is associatedwith marital quality, social <strong>in</strong>timacy, work engagement, positive illusions, selfefficacy,optimism, <strong>and</strong> goal striv<strong>in</strong>g (Diener & Suh, 2000; Myers & Diener, 1995).In contrast <strong>to</strong> the large body of literature about life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> adults,researchers are only beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> life satisfaction among adolescents(Gilman & Huebner, 2000). In a review of exist<strong>in</strong>g research, Gilman <strong>and</strong> Huebner (2003)noted that studies of adolescents have shown significant relationships between life satisfaction<strong>and</strong> positive <strong>and</strong> negative life experiences, parent-child conflict, substance use,stress <strong>and</strong> anxiety, <strong>and</strong> self-esteem <strong>in</strong> youth. With<strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>ority or Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth specifically,less is known about life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> its correlates. Underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g this variable <strong>in</strong> thecultural contexts <strong>in</strong> which Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents live is important, as life satisfaction can beconsidered central <strong>to</strong> decisions that these youth may make about work, education, <strong>and</strong>relationships (Bradley & Corwyn, 2004; Cooper, 1999; Romero & Roberts, 2003). In addition,underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g the role of acculturation <strong>in</strong> these relationships also is warranted asthe field beg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong> explore with<strong>in</strong>-group differences <strong>in</strong> psychological function<strong>in</strong>g amongLat<strong>in</strong>o youth <strong>and</strong> families (Castañeda, 1994).Acculturation has been def<strong>in</strong>ed as the process of change that results from cont<strong>in</strong>uouscontact between two different cultures (Berry, Trimble, & Olmedo, 1986). Severalmodels of acculturation have been proposed <strong>and</strong> used <strong>to</strong> guide measures of this


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 569construct. Most <strong>in</strong>itial research about acculturation adopted a unidimensional approach,which situated Lat<strong>in</strong>o <strong>in</strong>dividuals, for example, on a cont<strong>in</strong>uum of acculturation betweentwo opposite poles of European American <strong>and</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture. As <strong>in</strong>dividuals assimilated<strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream culture, this model suggested that they moved <strong>to</strong>ward the EuropeanAmerican end of the cont<strong>in</strong>uum <strong>and</strong> away from their Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture. A limitation of thisapproach, however, was that there was no acknowledgment of the possibility that acculturation<strong>to</strong>ward the dom<strong>in</strong>ant culture does not necessarily preclude the simultaneousretention of one’s culture of orig<strong>in</strong> (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Ger<strong>to</strong>n, 1993; Mar<strong>in</strong>, 1992;Szapocznik & Kurt<strong>in</strong>es, 1993; Zane & Mak, 2003).More recently, conceptualizations of acculturation have allowed for orthogonal,bidimensional measurements, such that acculturation <strong>to</strong> both Mexican <strong>and</strong> EuropeanAmerican culture can be assessed <strong>in</strong>dependently along two axes (e.g., Cuellar, Arnold,& Maldonado, 1995). Some researchers have <strong>in</strong>tegrated this approach <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> their measuremen<strong>to</strong>f acculturation (e.g., Cuellar et al., 1995; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 1996) <strong>and</strong>, assuch, have provided opportunities <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate acculturation <strong>in</strong> a more complex manner<strong>and</strong> clarify how <strong>in</strong>dividuals can identify <strong>to</strong> differ<strong>in</strong>g degrees with both dom<strong>in</strong>antculture <strong>and</strong> their cultures of orig<strong>in</strong> (S. Sue, 2003). It has been suggested that researchersattend <strong>to</strong> acculturation as an important variable that can <strong>in</strong>fluence a group’svalues <strong>and</strong> that <strong>in</strong>vestigations of acculturation use more multidimensional conceptualizations<strong>in</strong> an effort <strong>to</strong> better underst<strong>and</strong> cultural orientation <strong>and</strong> function<strong>in</strong>g (Berry,2003; Chun & Akutsu, 2003; Kim & Abreu, 2001; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 1996). In the case ofLat<strong>in</strong>o youth, therefore, an <strong>in</strong>vestigation of perceived family support <strong>and</strong> life satisfactionwarrants consideration of acculturation level as a possible fac<strong>to</strong>r that <strong>in</strong>fluencesthe relationship of these variables.One possibilitySecond possibilityTHE PRESENT STUDYThe overall purpose of this study was <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e the relationship between perceivedfamily support, acculturation, <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American adolescents.Specifically, this study was designed <strong>to</strong> empirically test assumptions about the importanceof perceived family support <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American youth <strong>and</strong><strong>to</strong> address the follow<strong>in</strong>g research questions: (a) What do Mexican American adolescentsdescribe as variables that contribute <strong>to</strong> their life satisfaction? (b) <strong>How</strong> do perceivedfamily support <strong>and</strong> acculturation relate <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction? <strong>and</strong> (c) Doesacculturation moderate the relationship between perceived family support <strong>and</strong> lifesatisfaction?To address these research questions, we used a mixed-methods approach comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gboth quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methodologies. Several researchers have discussedthe need for qualitative <strong>in</strong>vestigations of multicultural issues with<strong>in</strong> psychology (Choudhuri,2003; Morrow, Rakhsha, & Castaneda, 2001; Ponterot<strong>to</strong>, 2002; Umaña-Taylor &Bámaca, 2004), as they can provide an opportunity <strong>to</strong> better underst<strong>and</strong> new phenomenaor understudied populations without assum<strong>in</strong>g that there is “one universal truth <strong>to</strong> bediscovered” (Auerbach & Silverste<strong>in</strong>, 2003, p. 26). Mixed-methods research may be particularlyuseful for ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a more complex underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of a particular <strong>to</strong>pic while simultaneouslytest<strong>in</strong>g theoretical models (Hanson, Creswell, Plano Clark, Petska, & Creswell,2005). Greene, Caracelli, <strong>and</strong> Graham (1989) suggested that mixed-methods studies canserve several purposes, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g triangulation (seek<strong>in</strong>g convergence of results), complementarity(exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g overlapp<strong>in</strong>g or different facets of a phenomenon), <strong>in</strong>itiation (discover<strong>in</strong>gparadoxes <strong>and</strong> contradictions), development (us<strong>in</strong>g qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativemethods sequentially), <strong>and</strong> expansion (add<strong>in</strong>g breadth or scope <strong>to</strong> a project).PurposeImplies hypothesesMixed methods


570 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Design</strong> statedQualitative <strong>and</strong>quantitativeThe present mixed-methods study was conceptualized from a pragmatic theoreticalparadigm (Hanson et al., 2005; Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). We conceptualized<strong>and</strong> designed the study as a dom<strong>in</strong>antly quantitative, concurrent design, which is<strong>in</strong>dicated by the follow<strong>in</strong>g procedural notation (Morse, 1991): QUANT + qual. That is,both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative data were collected at the same time, <strong>and</strong> theprimary methodology was quantitative, with a lesser emphasis on the qualitativeportion (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998).For our purposes, qualitative methodology was used <strong>to</strong> address the first researchquestion, which sought <strong>to</strong> explore variables that youth described as contribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> theirlife satisfaction. Open-ended responses provided by participants were analyzed thematicallyby a collaborative research team, us<strong>in</strong>g several strategies from grounded theorymethodology (Strauss & Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1998), <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g open cod<strong>in</strong>g, category/theme generation,<strong>and</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>g patterns across categories. Themes about fac<strong>to</strong>rs that participantsbelieved contributed <strong>to</strong> their life satisfaction were derived <strong>and</strong> described through thisprocess. Quantitative methodology was used <strong>to</strong> answer the rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g research questionsabout the relationship between perceived family support, acculturation, <strong>and</strong> lifesatisfaction. We conducted a hierarchical multiple regression of perceived family support<strong>and</strong> Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo acculturation orientations on life satisfaction. We also added the<strong>in</strong>teractions of perceived family support <strong>and</strong> both acculturation orientations <strong>to</strong> seewhether these variables significantly predicted life satisfaction beyond the ma<strong>in</strong> effectsof perceived family support <strong>and</strong> Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo orientations alone.F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs from the quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative portions of the study were <strong>in</strong>tegrated<strong>to</strong> reveal areas of convergence as well as areas <strong>in</strong> which the data suggested discrepantf<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs or helped <strong>to</strong> provide a context for the data. Specifically, we sought <strong>to</strong>underst<strong>and</strong> the relation between life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> perceived family support by look<strong>in</strong>gfor areas of convergence as well as complementarity between our qualitative <strong>and</strong>quantitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (Greene et al., 1989).METHODParticipantsBy state?Participants <strong>in</strong> this study were 309 English-speak<strong>in</strong>g middle <strong>and</strong> high school students fromCalifornia, Kansas, <strong>and</strong> Texas. Because there is research <strong>to</strong> suggest that group<strong>in</strong>g Lat<strong>in</strong>oadolescents <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> one collective ethnic group may not appropriately capture the with<strong>in</strong>groupdifferences of this heterogeneous population (Umaña-Taylor & F<strong>in</strong>e, 2001), onlythe participants who self-identified as Mexican American (n 293) were <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> thepresent study. Furthermore, the small number of middle-school students (n 27) was removed<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> have a f<strong>in</strong>al sample with more homogeneity with respect <strong>to</strong> age group(e.g., all high school students). Of this f<strong>in</strong>al sample of 266 Mexican American high schoolstudents, 150 (56%) were girls <strong>and</strong> 116 (44%) were boys, <strong>and</strong> they had a mean age of15.74 years (SD 1.04, range 14–18 years). The majority of the sample was Catholic(78%), with 56% report<strong>in</strong>g that their parents had immigrated <strong>to</strong> the United States, <strong>and</strong>26% report<strong>in</strong>g that their gr<strong>and</strong>parents had immigrated <strong>to</strong> the United States.ProcedurePotential participants were solicited <strong>in</strong> various ways, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g contact<strong>in</strong>g the League ofUnited Lat<strong>in</strong> American Citizens (LULAC) National <strong>Education</strong>al Service Centers, public <strong>and</strong>private schools, afterschool programs, <strong>and</strong> selected Federal TRIO (i.e., Upward Bound,Upward Bound Math/Science, <strong>and</strong> <strong>Education</strong>al Talent Search) programs. The primary


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 571researcher discussed the project with adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> other staff <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial approval<strong>to</strong> solicit participants <strong>and</strong> provided all the materials for the schools <strong>and</strong> organizations.In some cases, the primary researcher went <strong>to</strong> the sites <strong>to</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister the surveysonce parental consent forms were obta<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> other cases, the site staff adm<strong>in</strong>isteredthe surveys <strong>and</strong> returned them, along with the consent forms, <strong>to</strong> the researcher viamail. Several sites met with large groups of students on a regular basis (e.g., TRIOprograms) <strong>and</strong> thus were able <strong>to</strong> moni<strong>to</strong>r the return of <strong>in</strong>formed consents <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong>ensure maximum participation by students. For these sites, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> those fromschools <strong>and</strong> community programs, the response rate was approximately 65%. At oneafterschool program, however, 100 consent forms were given <strong>to</strong> supervisors <strong>to</strong> pass out<strong>to</strong> students, <strong>and</strong> only 7 were returned. This was surpris<strong>in</strong>g consider<strong>in</strong>g the relatively highresponse rate we obta<strong>in</strong>ed from other sites, <strong>and</strong> we are unclear as <strong>to</strong> what extent thestudy was actually described <strong>to</strong> students as was <strong>in</strong>tended at this particular program.Packets conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g two <strong>in</strong>formed consent forms (one for the students/parents <strong>to</strong>keep <strong>and</strong> one <strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r), as well as an <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry letter, weredistributed <strong>to</strong> students <strong>to</strong> take home dur<strong>in</strong>g school or dur<strong>in</strong>g their program’s activitytime. Both consent forms <strong>and</strong> the letter were translated <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> Spanish such that all parentsreceived copies <strong>in</strong> English <strong>and</strong> Spanish. Parents were asked <strong>to</strong> send signed consentforms back <strong>to</strong> school (or the organization) with their children. Once consent was obta<strong>in</strong>edfrom parents, students who volunteered <strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> the study were asked <strong>to</strong>complete a student assent form <strong>and</strong> then were adm<strong>in</strong>istered a packet of materials dur<strong>in</strong>ga 45-m<strong>in</strong> period of school or of an afterschool program. Only students whose parentshad provided consent <strong>and</strong> who had themselves completed an assent form were allowed<strong>to</strong> complete the packet of questionnaires, <strong>and</strong> the questionnaires were only provided <strong>in</strong>English. This decision <strong>to</strong> only sample students who were proficient readers <strong>in</strong> English wasmade dur<strong>in</strong>g the development of the project because there was no exist<strong>in</strong>g data regard<strong>in</strong>gthe conceptual <strong>and</strong> functional equivalence of several of the measures for Lat<strong>in</strong>oadolescents <strong>in</strong> particular (American Psychological Association, 2002; Rogler, 1999).Limits generaliz<strong>in</strong>gInstrumentsDemographic questionnaire. A demographic questionnaire was <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>participants’ age, year <strong>in</strong> school, gender, race/ethnicity, generational status, <strong>and</strong> religiousaffiliation.GivensimultaneouslyOpen-ended question about well-be<strong>in</strong>g. At the bot<strong>to</strong>m of the first page (demographicform) of each packet of measures was the follow<strong>in</strong>g open-ended question: “Whatfac<strong>to</strong>rs do you th<strong>in</strong>k contribute <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> happ<strong>in</strong>ess?” Students were providedwith 10 l<strong>in</strong>es on which <strong>to</strong> write their responses <strong>and</strong> were encouraged <strong>to</strong> write onthe back of the page if they needed more space.QualitativePerceived social support. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support(MSPSS; Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988) is a 12-item scale that measures perceivedsupport from three doma<strong>in</strong>s: Family, Friends, <strong>and</strong> a Significant Other. Participantscomplet<strong>in</strong>g the MSPSS are asked <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate their agreement with items on a 7-po<strong>in</strong>tLikert scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (very strongly disagree) <strong>to</strong> 7 (very strongly agree). A sampleitem from the Family subscale is “I get the emotional help <strong>and</strong> support I need from myfamily.” Support for the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of the MSPSS has been found withsamples of college students, adolescents liv<strong>in</strong>g abroad, <strong>and</strong> adolescents on an <strong>in</strong>patientpsychiatry unit (Canty-Mitchell & Zimet, 2000).


572 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eOperationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionThe MSPSS has been used <strong>in</strong> several studies with young adults <strong>and</strong> adults <strong>in</strong> theUnited States <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> Europe (Kazarian & McCabe, 1991; Zimet et al., 1988; Zimet, Powell,Farley, Werkman, & Berkoff, 1990). In a recent study, Canty-Mitchell <strong>and</strong> Zimet (2000)<strong>in</strong>vestigated the MSPSS with a sample of urban adolescents, approximately 75% whowere ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority students. Results <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability estimates of .93 forthe <strong>to</strong>tal score, <strong>and</strong> .91, .89, <strong>and</strong> .91 for the Family, Friends, <strong>and</strong> Significant Other subscales.Fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis of the MSPSS with this sample confirmed the three-fac<strong>to</strong>r structureof the measure. In the present study, the four-item Perceived Support from Familysubscale was used, <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability for this scale was .88.Operationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionOnly globalscale given?Life satisfaction. The Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scale (MSLSS;Huebner, 1994) is a 47-item questionnaire that assesses life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> youth across sixspecific doma<strong>in</strong>s: Global, Family, Friends, School, Self, <strong>and</strong> Liv<strong>in</strong>g Environment. TheGlobal subscale, which comprises seven items, was used <strong>in</strong> the present study. Respondentswere asked <strong>to</strong> rate items on a 6-po<strong>in</strong>t Likert scale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (strongly disagree)<strong>to</strong> 6 (strongly agree). A sample item from this subscale is “My life is go<strong>in</strong>g well.”Support for validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of the MSLSS has been found with various samplesof children, middle- <strong>and</strong> high school students <strong>in</strong> the United States <strong>and</strong> Canada. TheMSLSS was found <strong>to</strong> correlate with other measures of well-be<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> its multidimensionalfac<strong>to</strong>r structure was confirmed (Huebner, 1994; Gilman, Huebner, & Laughl<strong>in</strong>,2000). Internal reliability coefficients <strong>in</strong> various studies with the MSLSS ranged from .77<strong>to</strong> .91 across all subscales. In the present study, the <strong>in</strong>ternal reliability was .86 for theGlobal subscale.Operationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionAcculturation level. The Acculturation Rat<strong>in</strong>g Scale for Mexican Americans-II(ARSMA-II; Cuellar et al., 1995), which comprises 30 items, was used <strong>to</strong> measure acculturation<strong>in</strong> this study. Individuals were asked <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> items about language preference,association, <strong>and</strong> identification with Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo cultures us<strong>in</strong>g a 5-po<strong>in</strong>t Likertscale rang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 (not at all) <strong>to</strong> 5 (extremely often or almost always). The Mexican<strong>and</strong> Anglo subscales comprise 17 <strong>and</strong> 13 items, respectively. Examples of items from theAnglo <strong>and</strong> Mexican subscales are “I enjoy listen<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> English language music,” <strong>and</strong> “Iassociate with Lat<strong>in</strong>os <strong>and</strong>/or Lat<strong>in</strong> Americans,” respectively. In this study, we used Anglo<strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation subscales <strong>in</strong>dependently <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> evaluate their uniquecontributions <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction.Studies with the ARSMA-II (Cuellar et al., 1995) revealed <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency estimatesfor the Anglo Orientation subscale (AOS) <strong>and</strong> the Mexican Orientation subscale(MOS) as .83 <strong>and</strong> .88, respectively. Flores <strong>and</strong> O’Brien (2002) reported reliability coefficientsof .77 <strong>and</strong> .91 for the AOS <strong>and</strong> MOS with Mexican American adolescent women <strong>in</strong>particular. Test–retest reliability estimates, over a 2-week period, were .94 for the AOS<strong>and</strong> .96 for the MOS (Cuellar et al.). Internal reliability estimates of the Mexican <strong>and</strong>Anglo orientation scales <strong>in</strong> the present study were .88 <strong>and</strong> .67, respectively.RESULTSQualitative AnalysesQualitative analyses were orig<strong>in</strong>ally conducted with responses from the <strong>to</strong>tal sample ofboth middle- <strong>and</strong> high school students, <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were then revised <strong>and</strong> verified afterthe removal of the small sample of middle-school students. Of the 266 participants, 260provided qualitative responses <strong>to</strong> the open-ended question, “What fac<strong>to</strong>rs do you th<strong>in</strong>k


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 573contribute <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> happ<strong>in</strong>ess?” Responses ranged from one word <strong>to</strong> shortparagraphs of six sentences. In this study, we used several strategies from groundedtheory (Strauss & Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1998) <strong>to</strong> analyze these responses, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g open cod<strong>in</strong>g,category/theme generation, <strong>and</strong> explor<strong>in</strong>g patterns across categories.Several strategies were used throughout the data analysis process <strong>to</strong> improve therigor <strong>and</strong> “trustworth<strong>in</strong>ess” of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs (Strauss & Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1998). First, a collaborativeteam, compris<strong>in</strong>g the primary researcher/Lisa M. Edwards (a biethnic Lat<strong>in</strong>a/Whitewoman) <strong>and</strong> two undergraduate Mexican American students, was convened. An externalaudi<strong>to</strong>r (a female European American graduate student) was asked <strong>to</strong> review emerg<strong>in</strong>gthemes <strong>and</strong> reflect on the analysis process. A second external audi<strong>to</strong>r (a male EuropeanAmerican graduate student) was asked <strong>to</strong> review the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs with the primary researcherafter the middle-school students had been removed <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> verify or revise the orig<strong>in</strong>althemes. Our reviews <strong>in</strong>dicated that the primary themes rema<strong>in</strong>ed the same.At the outset of data analysis, members of the research team discussed biases <strong>and</strong>assumptions, not<strong>in</strong>g that they believed family, friends, <strong>and</strong> religious beliefs would be themost frequent responses reported by participants. Together, the research team then decidedhow the qualitative data might best be unders<strong>to</strong>od. They engaged <strong>in</strong> “open cod<strong>in</strong>g,”or break<strong>in</strong>g down each participant’s responses <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> words, phrases, or sentencesthat represented mean<strong>in</strong>g units (Strauss & Corb<strong>in</strong>, 1998). These mean<strong>in</strong>g units were thenlabeled as concepts <strong>and</strong> were ref<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> discussed as necessary, eventually lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> af<strong>in</strong>al list of concepts. The f<strong>in</strong>al concept list <strong>in</strong>cluded the follow<strong>in</strong>g terms: family, friends,attitude, faith (e.g., God, spirituality), love, money, help<strong>in</strong>g others, work, home, education(e.g., teachers, school), physical health, <strong>and</strong> goals. Last, <strong>in</strong>terrelated concepts weregrouped <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> larger category themes (e.g., descriptive sentences), <strong>and</strong> the mostprom<strong>in</strong>ent themes were then selected <strong>and</strong> reviewed.Results from this analysis suggested one primary, core theme, as well as two additional,smaller themes. The core theme that emerged from participants’ open-ended responseswas Family is important for provid<strong>in</strong>g support <strong>and</strong> love <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cluded responsesthat described the significance of family <strong>and</strong> the ways <strong>in</strong> which family gives support <strong>and</strong>love <strong>to</strong> participants. Specifically, adolescents noted that their families provided unconditionalcare <strong>and</strong> encouragement as well as affection <strong>and</strong> support. Illustrative quotes <strong>in</strong>cludedthe follow<strong>in</strong>g: “Have a family that will be there for you <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>spire you <strong>to</strong> be abetter person <strong>in</strong> the future”; “Life satisfaction is <strong>to</strong> have your family be united <strong>and</strong> havelove for one another <strong>in</strong> the home”; “Above all family, because how you enjoy life isbe<strong>in</strong>g with those you care about”; “Hav<strong>in</strong>g your family with you—they are the best thatwe could have”; “Hav<strong>in</strong>g a car<strong>in</strong>g family who supports you”; <strong>and</strong> “Family—especially myfather who works hard <strong>to</strong> put food on the table everyday.”Two prom<strong>in</strong>ent additional themes emerged from the data as secondary <strong>to</strong> theimportance of family. The first theme, Friends provide help <strong>and</strong> fun, revealed participants’beliefs that friends contributed <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> several ways. Quotes describ<strong>in</strong>gthe role of friends <strong>in</strong>cluded “Hav<strong>in</strong>g friends <strong>to</strong> talk <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> hang around with, whowill help you out”; “. . . good friends who can be relied on”; <strong>and</strong> “. . . kick<strong>in</strong>’ it with myfriends—go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> school <strong>and</strong> talk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> my homeboys.”The second additional theme that emerged from the qualitative data related <strong>to</strong>the contribution of a positive approach or attitude <strong>to</strong>ward challenges. We labeled thistheme The importance of a positive attitude <strong>to</strong>ward life <strong>and</strong> problems. Illustrativequotes of this theme <strong>in</strong>cluded “Always be optimistic—there will be ups <strong>and</strong> downs, butlife will still go on”; “Life satisfaction comes when you are satisfied with who you are,but able <strong>to</strong> change what you can”; <strong>and</strong> “Do your best—even if you didn’t w<strong>in</strong> you couldsay that you tried your best.”Short responsesTrustworth<strong>in</strong>essdiscussedGood clarify<strong>in</strong>gexamples


574 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eQuantitative AnalysesAssumptionscheckedIncorrect statisticsDescriptive StatisticsPrelim<strong>in</strong>ary analyses <strong>in</strong>cluded check<strong>in</strong>g the data for outliers, normality, l<strong>in</strong>earity, <strong>and</strong>homoscedasticity as well as exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g potential differences on the basis of thelocation from which participants were sampled. The scatter plot of the studentizedresiduals aga<strong>in</strong>st the predicted values of life satisfaction revealed no violations ofassumptions of normality, l<strong>in</strong>earity, <strong>and</strong> homoscedasticity. Results based on Cook <strong>and</strong>Weisberg’s (1982) distance showed no serious outliers among the study variables(Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Miss<strong>in</strong>g data po<strong>in</strong>ts for an item on a subscale, which werefound <strong>in</strong> 14 cases, were h<strong>and</strong>led by substitut<strong>in</strong>g participants’ subscale or mean scalescores for the miss<strong>in</strong>g value.Independent samples t tests were conducted <strong>to</strong> see whether there were significantdifferences <strong>in</strong> the study variables for respondents who had completed surveys <strong>in</strong> California<strong>and</strong> Texas without the lead researcher present dur<strong>in</strong>g adm<strong>in</strong>istration (n 184) <strong>and</strong>those that had completed surveys <strong>in</strong> Kansas adm<strong>in</strong>istered by the lead researcher (n 82).Results <strong>in</strong>dicated that there were no significant differences <strong>in</strong> scores of life satisfaction,t(262) 1.65, p .10; perceived family support, t(261) 0.32, p .75; Mexicanorientation, t(259) 1.33, p .19; <strong>and</strong> Anglo orientation, t(263) 0.56, p .58.Because there were no differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>tal scores on the basis of the location of participants,all the data <strong>in</strong> this sample were analyzed <strong>to</strong>gether.The means, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations, <strong>and</strong> zero-order correlations for all study variables(perceived family support, Anglo <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientations, <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction) are presented<strong>in</strong> Table 1. As can be seen, life satisfaction was significantly, positively correlatedwith perceived support from family <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation. Thus, as scores ofperceived family support <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation <strong>in</strong>creased, scores of global life satisfaction<strong>in</strong>creased. Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo orientations also were significantly positivelycorrelated with perceived family support; as scores on each of the acculturation orientations<strong>in</strong>creased, perceived family support <strong>in</strong>creased.The ma<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractive effects of Mexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo acculturation orientations<strong>and</strong> perceived family support on life satisfaction were assessed us<strong>in</strong>g hierarchical multipleregression procedures described by Cohen, West, <strong>and</strong> Aiken (2003) <strong>and</strong> Lub<strong>in</strong>ski <strong>and</strong>Humphreys (1990). In order <strong>to</strong> reduce possible multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity, scales were st<strong>and</strong>ardizedbefore form<strong>in</strong>g cross-product terms <strong>and</strong> before runn<strong>in</strong>g the regression (Dunlap &Kemery, 1987; Jaccard, Wan, & Turrisi, 1990). Lub<strong>in</strong>ski <strong>and</strong> Humphreys described proceduresfor the detection of spurious modera<strong>to</strong>r effects, <strong>and</strong> they argued that modera<strong>to</strong>reffects <strong>and</strong> quadratic trends are likely <strong>to</strong> share a large proportion of the variance. Inother words, a significant <strong>in</strong>teraction between two variables may be observed only becausethe effect is correlated substantially with quadratic trends of the componentTABLE 1Means, St<strong>and</strong>ard Deviations, <strong>and</strong> Correlations Among Variables(N 266)Variable 1 2 3 41. Life satisfaction — .53*** .07 .22***2. Perceived family support — .15* .21**3. Anglo orientation — .074. Mexican orientation — —M 27.27 47.95 19.88 26.74SD 8.21 8.40 13.35 16.92*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 575TABLE 2 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Perceived FamilySupport, Mexican, <strong>and</strong> Anglo Orientations as Predic<strong>to</strong>rs of LifeSatisfaction (N 266)Variable B SE B t df R 2 R 2 F dfStep 1 (ma<strong>in</strong> effects) 254 0.28 0.28 32.17*** 3,254Perceived family support 0.51 0.07 .51 7.01***Anglo orientation 0.00 0.06 .00 0.02Mexican orientation 0.13 0.06 .13 2.05*Step 2 (quadratic effects) 251 0.28 0.00 0.1 3,251Step 3 (<strong>in</strong>teraction effects) 249 0.29 0.01 1.98 2,249Perceived family support 0.51 0.07 .51 7.01***Anglo orientation 0.00 0.06 .00 0.02Mexican orientation 0.13 0.06 .13 2.05*Family squared 0.00 0.05 .00 0.02Anglo squared 0.04 0.04 .05 0.82Mexican squared 0.02 0.05 .03 0.43Anglo x Family 0.07 0.06 .07 1.20Mexican x Family 0.11 0.06 .11 1.86*p .05. **p .01. ***p .001.Size of R 2Regression resultsvariables. Thus, enter<strong>in</strong>g quadratic trends <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the regression equation reduces the possibilityof observ<strong>in</strong>g such spurious modera<strong>to</strong>rs.In Lub<strong>in</strong>ski <strong>and</strong> Humphrey’s (1990) recommended procedure, ma<strong>in</strong> effects areentered first <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the regression equation; after this a priori entry, quadratic trendsare entered. In the first step, the ma<strong>in</strong> effects of perceived family support, Mexican orientation,<strong>and</strong> Anglo orientation were entered. Next, quadratic terms of the ma<strong>in</strong> effectvariables were entered <strong>to</strong> control for spurious modera<strong>to</strong>r effects. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the two-way<strong>in</strong>teractions of Mexican orientation <strong>and</strong> perceived family support, <strong>and</strong> Anglo orientation<strong>and</strong> perceived family support, were entered.Table 2 provides the results of the multiple regressions <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g our predic<strong>to</strong>r variables.The ma<strong>in</strong> effects of perceived family support ( .50, p .001) <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation( .12, p .05) were significant at Step 1, account<strong>in</strong>g for 28% of the variance<strong>in</strong> life satisfaction. Higher scores on the perceived family support <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientationvariables were associated with higher life satisfaction. After controll<strong>in</strong>g for quadratictrends (Step 2), the <strong>in</strong>teractions between Anglo <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientations <strong>and</strong> perceivedfamily support were not significant <strong>in</strong> Step 3, account<strong>in</strong>g for 1% of the variance<strong>in</strong> life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a <strong>to</strong>tal R 2 .29.DISCUSSIONBecause of the dearth of research exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g well-be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth, the presentmixed-methods study was conducted <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> researchers’ underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the relationshipbetween life satisfaction, acculturation, <strong>and</strong> perceived family support <strong>in</strong> MexicanAmerican adolescents. A contribution of this study was that a mixed-method approach wasused <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> qualitative perspectives on life satisfaction as well as quantitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsabout acculturation, perceived family support, <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction. An additional strengthof this study was that acculturation was measured <strong>and</strong> conceptualized <strong>in</strong> a bidimensionalmanner, such that questions about the <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>in</strong>fluences of both


576 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eMexican <strong>and</strong> Anglo orientations <strong>and</strong> family support on life satisfaction could be <strong>in</strong>vestigated(Zane & Mak, 2003). F<strong>in</strong>ally, rather than comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g ethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority groups or Lat<strong>in</strong>oethnic subgroups, or only sampl<strong>in</strong>g adults, we <strong>in</strong>vestigated Mexican American adolescents<strong>in</strong> particular (Castañeda, 1994; Umaña-Taylor & F<strong>in</strong>e, 2001).Integration of Mixed-Methods F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsConvergencediscussedThe quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative results converged <strong>to</strong> provide additional empirical supportfor the importance of family <strong>in</strong> the lives of Mexican American adolescents (Mar<strong>in</strong> &Gamba, 2003; Paniagua, 1998; Vazquez Garcia et al., 2000). The qualitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggestedthat youth identified their family as most important <strong>in</strong> contribut<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> their lifesatisfaction above other fac<strong>to</strong>rs such as friends, religion, or money. In addition, the qualitativef<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggested that the important role of family was <strong>to</strong> provide support specifically.These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs also suggest that previous research with adults about perceived supportas the critical aspect of familismo may also apply <strong>to</strong> youth (Sabogal et al., 1987).Two additional themes that emerged from our qualitative data, Friends providehelp <strong>and</strong> fun <strong>and</strong> The importance of a positive attitude <strong>to</strong>ward life <strong>and</strong> problems, suggestedaspects of participants’ lives that were important <strong>to</strong> them, though not as criticalas family. These additional f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, though not able <strong>to</strong> be explored by quantitativeanalyses <strong>in</strong> this particular study, help <strong>to</strong> identify additional fac<strong>to</strong>rs that contribute <strong>to</strong> lifesatisfaction from the perspective of Mexican American youth <strong>and</strong> can be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> futuremodels of life satisfaction.The quantitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs about the relationship between perceived family support<strong>and</strong> acculturation orientations revealed additional <strong>in</strong>formation regard<strong>in</strong>g the roleof these variables <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g overall life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American youth. Mexicanorientation, but not Anglo orientation, was significantly associated with life satisfaction<strong>in</strong> this sample, highlight<strong>in</strong>g the importance of <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g acculturation orientationsseparately (Kim & Abreu, 2001; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003). As noted by Ruelas,Atk<strong>in</strong>son, <strong>and</strong> Ramos-Sanchez (1998) <strong>in</strong> their study of counselor credibility, unidimensionalmeasures of acculturation may lead <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>accurate <strong>in</strong>ferences about cultural orientation.These authors used the ARSMA-II (Cuellar et al., 1995) <strong>and</strong> found that Mexicanorientation scores were significantly related <strong>to</strong> credibility of counselors, but not Angloscores. Although we <strong>in</strong>vestigated a different dependent variable, the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs were similar<strong>in</strong> that Mexican orientation appeared <strong>to</strong> be the <strong>in</strong>fluential acculturation orientation.Future studies about life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American youth should explore whyMexican orientation, <strong>in</strong> contrast <strong>to</strong> Anglo orientation, plays this important role.Good recognition oflimitationsLimitations, Directions for Future <strong>Research</strong>, <strong>and</strong> ImplicationsThere are several limitations <strong>to</strong> this study that should be noted. In the qualitative portionof the study, it was not clear whether the open-ended question that was presented <strong>to</strong>students was unders<strong>to</strong>od <strong>in</strong> the same way by each of the respondents. Furthermore, thequestion did not ask for an evaluation of participants’ life satisfaction. It is possible,therefore, that participants <strong>in</strong>terpreted this question <strong>to</strong> address fac<strong>to</strong>rs that contribute<strong>to</strong> life satisfaction for people <strong>in</strong> general. The study also was limited <strong>in</strong> that only Englishspeak<strong>in</strong>gparticipants were allowed <strong>to</strong> participate. By not <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Spanish-speak<strong>in</strong>gadolescents, <strong>and</strong> by recruit<strong>in</strong>g most participants from educational <strong>and</strong> cultural programs(e.g., TRIO programs <strong>and</strong> the like), we may have limited the representativeness of thesample across acculturation levels <strong>and</strong> degree of educational <strong>in</strong>volvement.One avenue for potential future <strong>in</strong>vestigation lies <strong>in</strong> underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g different typesof social support that Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents use or perceive <strong>in</strong> their lives. In the present study,


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 577only general perceived family support was assessed, <strong>and</strong> we were unable <strong>to</strong> measurehow specific types of support may contribute <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> these youth. A usefulconceptualization of social support was proposed by Weiss (1974), who described provisionsthat can result from relationships with others, such as guidance, reliable alliance, attachmentreassurance of worth, social <strong>in</strong>tegration, <strong>and</strong> opportunity <strong>to</strong> provide nurturance. Thisframework has been used <strong>and</strong> operationalized by many researchers (Aqu<strong>in</strong>o, Russell,Cutrona, & Altmaier, 1996; Cutrona & Russell, 1987) <strong>and</strong> can provide a beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g structurefor researchers <strong>to</strong> probe more specific types of social support <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth.In addition, <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g variables such as perceived support from family <strong>and</strong> acculturation<strong>and</strong> life satisfaction over time, rather than only focus<strong>in</strong>g on cross-sectionalviews, also will provide additional <strong>in</strong>formation about the chang<strong>in</strong>g role of family <strong>and</strong> its<strong>in</strong>fluence on the well-be<strong>in</strong>g of Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth as they transition <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> adulthood (Chun &Akutsu, 2003; Mar<strong>in</strong> & Gamba, 2003). The chang<strong>in</strong>g demographics of our present society,as well as the dem<strong>and</strong>s on the lives of adolescents, require closer <strong>in</strong>vestigations of howthe function <strong>and</strong> role of various resources such as family adapt over time <strong>and</strong> how acculturation(e.g., Mexican orientation) <strong>in</strong>fluences this process.The <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g presence of Mexican American youth <strong>in</strong> our schools <strong>and</strong> communitiesrequires that counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists purposefully work <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> these adolescents’experience of life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> obstacles that may be h<strong>in</strong>der<strong>in</strong>gtheir well-be<strong>in</strong>g. Low educational atta<strong>in</strong>ment as well as problems such as gang <strong>in</strong>volvement,substance abuse, <strong>and</strong> teenage pregnancy have been identified as significant concernsfaced by Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth (Chavez & Roney, 1990), <strong>and</strong> scholars have noted that fewmental health professionals are tra<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>to</strong> work with Mexican American adolescents(Castañeda, 1994). Investigat<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong>-group variability <strong>in</strong> the experience of life satisfaction<strong>and</strong> the role of perceived family support, such as the analyses <strong>in</strong> the presentstudy, provide a more balanced <strong>and</strong> detailed portrait of function<strong>in</strong>g with<strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>oadolescents (Villarruel & Montero-Sieburth, 2000). Our f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs suggest that family <strong>and</strong>Mexican orientation will be particularly important variables for counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychologists<strong>to</strong> consider when work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> promote life satisfaction with Mexican American youth.Future research can cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong> elucidate strengths <strong>and</strong> assets with<strong>in</strong> this important population<strong>and</strong> contribute <strong>to</strong> a grow<strong>in</strong>g body of knowledge about youth resources.ReferencesAmerican Psychological Association. (2002). Guidel<strong>in</strong>es onmulticultural education, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, research, practice, <strong>and</strong>organizational change for psychologists. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n DC:Author.Aqu<strong>in</strong>o, J. A., Russell, D., Cutrona, C. E,. & Altmaier, E. M.(1996). Employment status, social support, <strong>and</strong> lifesatisfaction among the elderly. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>gPsychology, 43, 480–489.Auerbach, C. F., & Silverste<strong>in</strong>, L. B. (2003). Qualitative data:An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> analysis. New York: NYUPress.Berry, J. W. (2003). Conceptual approaches <strong>to</strong> acculturation.In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, & G. Mar<strong>in</strong> (Eds.),Acculturation: Advances <strong>in</strong> theory, measurement, <strong>and</strong>applied research (pp. 17–37). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: AmericanPsychological Association.Berry, J. W., Trimble, J. E., & Olmedo, E. L. (1986). Assessmen<strong>to</strong>f acculturation. In W. J. Lonner & J. W. Berry (Eds.),Field methods <strong>in</strong> cross-cultural research (pp. 291–345).Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2004). Life satisfactionamong European American, African American, Ch<strong>in</strong>eseAmerican, Mexican American, <strong>and</strong> Dom<strong>in</strong>ican Americanadolescents. International Journal of BehavioralDevelopment, 28, 385–400.Canty-Mitchell, J., & Zimet, G. D. (2000). Psychometricproperties of the Multidimensional Scale of PerceivedSocial Support <strong>in</strong> urban adolescents. American Journalof Community Psychology, 28, 391–400.Castañeda, D. M. (1994). A research agenda for Mexican-American adolescent mental health. Adolescence, 29,225–240.


578 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eCastillo, L. G., Conoley, C. W., & Brossart, D. F. (2004).Acculturation, White marg<strong>in</strong>alization, <strong>and</strong> family supportas predic<strong>to</strong>rs of perceived distress <strong>in</strong> Mexican Americanfemale college students. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology,51, 151–157.Chavez, J. M., & Roney, C. E (1990). Psychocultural fac<strong>to</strong>rsaffect<strong>in</strong>g the mental health status of Mexican Americanadolescents. In A. R. Stiffman & L. E. Davis (Eds.), Ethnicissues <strong>in</strong> adolescent mental health (pp. 73–91). NewburyPark, CA: Sage.Choudhuri, D. D. (2003). Qualitative research <strong>and</strong> multiculturalcounsel<strong>in</strong>g competency. In D. B. Pope-Davis, H. L. K.Coleman, W. M. Liu, & R. L. Toporek (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book ofmulticultural competencies <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> psychology(pp. 267–282). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Chun, K. M., & Akutsu, P. D. (2003). Acculturation amongethnic m<strong>in</strong>ority families. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, &G. Mar<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances <strong>in</strong> theory,measurement <strong>and</strong> applied research (pp. 95–119).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Psychological Association.Cohen, J., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Appliedmultiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioralsciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Cook, R. D., & Weisberg, S. (1982). Residuals <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence<strong>in</strong> regression. New York: Chapman & Hall.Cooper, G. R. (1999). Multiple selves, multiple worlds:Cultural perspectives on <strong>in</strong>dividuality <strong>and</strong> connectedness<strong>in</strong> adolescent development. In A. S. Masten (Ed.), Culturalprocess <strong>in</strong> child development (pp. 25–57). Mahwah,NJ: Erlbaum.Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). AcculturationRat<strong>in</strong>g Scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of theorig<strong>in</strong>al ARSMA Scale. Hispanic Journal of BehavioralSciences, 17, 275–304.Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of socialrelationships <strong>and</strong> adaptation <strong>to</strong> stress. In W. H. Jones &D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances <strong>in</strong> personal relationships(Vol. I, pp. 37–68). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (2000). Measur<strong>in</strong>g subjective wellbe<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong> compare the quality of life of cultures. InE. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture aid subjective wellbe<strong>in</strong>g(pp. 3–12). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R., & Smith, H. (1999).Subjective well-be<strong>in</strong>g: Three decades of progress.Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 125, 276–302.Dunlap, W. P., & Kemery, E. R. (1987). Failure <strong>to</strong> detectmoderat<strong>in</strong>g effects. Is multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity the problem?Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 102, 418–420.Flores, L. Y., & O’Brien, K. M. (2002). The career developmen<strong>to</strong>f Mexican American adolescent women: A test of socialcognitive career theory. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology,49, 14–27.Ga<strong>in</strong>es, S. O., Marelich, W. D., Bledsoe, K. L., Steers, W. N.,Henderson, M. C., Granrose, C. S., et al. (1997). L<strong>in</strong>ksbetween race/ethnicity <strong>and</strong> cultural values as mediated byracial/ethnic identity <strong>and</strong> moderated by gender. Journal ofPersonality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 72, 1460–1476.Gilman, R., & Huebner, E. S. (2000). Review of life satisfactionmeasures for adolescents. Behaviour Change, 17, 178–195.Gilman, R., & Huebner, E. S. (2003). A review of life satisfactionresearch with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents. SchoolPsychology Quarterly, 18, 192–205.Gilman, R., Huebner, E. S., & Laughl<strong>in</strong>, J. (2000). A first studyof the Multidimensional Students’ Life Satisfaction Scalewith adolescents. Social Indica<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>Research</strong>, 52, 135–160.Greene, J. C., Caracelli, V. J., & Graham, W. F. (1989). Towarda conceptual framework for mixed-method evaluationdesigns. <strong>Education</strong>al Evaluation <strong>and</strong> Policy Analysis, 11,255–274.Hanson, W. E., Creswell, J. W., Plano Clark, V. L., Petska,K. S., & Creswell, J. D. (2005). Mixed methods researchdesigns <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>gPsychology, 52, 224–235.Huebner, E. S. (1994). Prelim<strong>in</strong>ary development <strong>and</strong> validationof a multidimensional life satisfaction scale forchildren. Psychological Assessment, 6, 149–158.Jaccard, J., Wan, C. K., & Turrisi, R. (1990). The detection<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation of <strong>in</strong>teraction effects betweencont<strong>in</strong>uous variables <strong>in</strong> multiple regression. MultivariateBehavioral <strong>Research</strong>, 25, 467–478.Kazarian, S. S., & McCabe, S. B. (1991). Dimensions of socialsupport <strong>in</strong> the MSPSS: Fac<strong>to</strong>rial structure, reliability, <strong>and</strong>theoretical implications. Journal of CommunityPsychology, 19, 150–160.Kim, B. S. K., & Abreu, J. M. (2001). Acculturation measurement.In J. G. Ponterot<strong>to</strong>, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, &C. M. Alex<strong>and</strong>er (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of multiculturalcounsel<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 394–424). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Knight, G. P., Bernal, M. E., Cota,M.K,Garza, C.A.,&Ocampo,K. A. (1993). Family socialization <strong>and</strong> Mexican Americanidentity <strong>and</strong> behavior. In M. E. Bernal & G. P. Knight (Eds.),Ethnic identity (pp. 105–129). New York: SUNY Press.LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H., & Ger<strong>to</strong>n, J. (1993). Psychologicalimpact of biculturalism: Evidence <strong>and</strong> theory.Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 114, 395–412.Lopez, S. J., Magyar-Moe, J. L., Petersen, S. E., Ryder, J. A.,Krieshok, T. S., O’Byme, K. K, et al. (2006). Contextualiz<strong>in</strong>ghuman strengths: A counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology agendafor <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the applicability of positive psychology. TheCounsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychologist, 34, 205–227.


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 579Lopez, S. J., Prosser, E. C, Edwards, L. M., Magyar-Moe, J. L.,Neufeld, J. E., & Rasmussen, H. N. (2002). Putt<strong>in</strong>g positivepsychology <strong>in</strong> a multicultural context. In C. R. Snyder &S. J. Lopez (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of positive psychology(pp. 700–714). New York: Oxford University Press.Lub<strong>in</strong>ski, D., & Humphreys, L. G. (1990). Assess<strong>in</strong>g spurious“modera<strong>to</strong>r effects”: Illustrated substantively with thehypothesized (“synergistic”) relation between spatial <strong>and</strong>mathematical ability. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 107,385–393.Mar<strong>in</strong>, G. (1992). Issues <strong>in</strong> the measurement of acculturationamong Hispanics. In K. F. Geis<strong>in</strong>ger (Ed.), Psychologicaltest<strong>in</strong>g of Hispanics (pp. 235–251). Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC:American Psychological Association.Mar<strong>in</strong>, G. (1993). Influence of acculturation on familialism<strong>and</strong> self-identification among Hispanics. In M. E. Bernal &G. P. Knight (Eds.), Ethnic identity (pp. 181–196). NewYork: SUNY Press.Mar<strong>in</strong>, G., & Gamba, R. J. (1996). A new measurement ofacculturation for Hispanics: The Bidimensional AcculturationScale for Hispanics (BAS). Hispanic Journal ofBehavioral Sciences, 18, 297–316.Mar<strong>in</strong>, G., & Gamba, R. J. (2003). Acculturation <strong>and</strong> changes<strong>in</strong> cultural values. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, &G. Mar<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances In theory,measurement, <strong>and</strong> applied research (pp. 83–94).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Psychological Association.M<strong>in</strong>del, C. H. (1980). Extended familism among urbanMexican Americans, Anglos <strong>and</strong> Blacks. Hispanic Journalof Behavioral Sciences, 2, 21–34.Moore, J. W. (1970). Mexican Americans. Englewood Cliffs,NJ: Prentice Hall.Morrow, S. L., Rakhsha, G., & Castaneda, C. L. (2001).Qualitative research methods for multicultural counsel<strong>in</strong>g.In J. G. Ponterot<strong>to</strong>, J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, & C. M.Alex<strong>and</strong>er (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of multicultural counsel<strong>in</strong>g(pp. 575–603). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Morse, J. M. (1991). Approaches <strong>to</strong> qualitative-quantitativemethodological triangulation. Nurs<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>, 40,120–123.Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? PsychologicalScience, 6, 10–19.Pabon, E. (1998). Hispanic adolescent del<strong>in</strong>quency <strong>and</strong>the family: A discussion of sociocultural <strong>in</strong>fluences.Adolescence, 33, 941–955.Paniagua, F. (1998). Assess<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> treat<strong>in</strong>g culturally diverseclients: A practical guide. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Ponterot<strong>to</strong>, J. G. (2002). Qualitative research methods: Thefifth force <strong>in</strong> psychology. The Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychologist,30, 394–406.Rodriguez, J. M., & Kosloski, K. (1998). The impact of acculturationon attitud<strong>in</strong>al familism <strong>in</strong> a community of Puer<strong>to</strong>Rican Americans. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences,20, 375–390.Rodriguez, M. C., & Morrobel, D. (2004). A review of Lat<strong>in</strong>oyouth development research <strong>and</strong> a call for an asset orientation.Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 26, 107–127.Rogler, L. H. (1999). Methodological sources of cultural<strong>in</strong>sensitivity <strong>in</strong> mental health research. AmericanPsychologist, 54, 424–433.Romero, A. J., & Roberts, R. E. (2003). Stress with<strong>in</strong> abiculturalcontext, for adolescents of Mexican descent. CulturalDiversity & Ethnic M<strong>in</strong>ority Psychology, 9, 171–184.Ruelas, S. R., Atk<strong>in</strong>son, D. R., & Ramos-Sanchez, L. (1998).Counselor help<strong>in</strong>g model <strong>and</strong> participant ethnicity, locusof control, <strong>and</strong> perceived counselor credibility. Journal ofCounsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 45, 98–103.Sabogal, F., Mar<strong>in</strong>, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., Mar<strong>in</strong>, B. V., &Perez-Stable, E. J. (1987). Hispanic familism <strong>and</strong> acculturation:What changes <strong>and</strong> what doesn’t? Hispanic Journalof Behavioral Sciences, 9, 397–412.Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Positive psychology, positiveprevention, <strong>and</strong> positive therapy. In C. R. Snyder & S. J.Lopez (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of positive psychology (pp. 3–9).New York: Oxford University Press.Strauss, A., & Corb<strong>in</strong>, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research:Techniques <strong>and</strong> procedures for develop<strong>in</strong>g groundedtheory (2nd ed.). Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Sue, D. W., & Constant<strong>in</strong>e, M. G. (2003). Optimal humanfunction<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> people of color <strong>in</strong> the United States. In B. W.Walsh (Ed.), Counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology <strong>and</strong> optimal humanfunction<strong>in</strong>g (pp. 151–169). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Sue, S. (2003). Foreword. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, &G. Mar<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances <strong>in</strong> theory,measurement, <strong>and</strong> applied research (pp. xvii–xxi).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Psychological Association.Super, D. E. (1955). Transition: From vocational guidance <strong>to</strong>counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology,2, 3–9.Szapocznik, J., & Kurt<strong>in</strong>es, W. (1993). Family psychology<strong>and</strong> cultural diversity. American Psychologist, 48,400–407.Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Us<strong>in</strong>g multivariatestatistics (3rd ed.). New York: HarperColl<strong>in</strong>s.Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed methodology.Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.Tri<strong>and</strong>is, H. C., Mar<strong>in</strong>, G., Betancourt, J., Lisansky, J., &Chang, B. (1982). Dimensions of familism amongHispanic <strong>and</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>stream Navy recruits. Chicago:Department of Psychology, University of Ill<strong>in</strong>ois.


580 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTyler, L. E. (1973). <strong>Design</strong> for a hopeful psychology. Americanpsychologist, 28, 1021–1029.Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Bámaca, M. Y. (2004). Conduct<strong>in</strong>gfocus groups with Lat<strong>in</strong>o populations: Lessons from thefield. Family Relations, 53, 261–272.Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & F<strong>in</strong>e, M. A. (2001). Methodologicalimplications of group<strong>in</strong>g Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> onecollective ethnic group. Hispanic Journal of BehavioralSciences, 23, 347–362.Unger, J. B., Ritt-Olson, A., Teran, L., Huang, T., Hoffman,B. R., & Palmer, P. (2002). Cultural values <strong>and</strong> substanceuse <strong>in</strong> a multiethnic sample of California adolescents.Addiction <strong>Research</strong> & Theory, 10, 257–279.U. S. Census Bureau. (2000). Annual projections of theresident population by age, sex, race, <strong>and</strong> Hispanic orig<strong>in</strong>:Lowest, middle, highest series <strong>and</strong> zero <strong>in</strong>ternationalmigration series, 1999 <strong>to</strong> 2100. Retrieved Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 7,2002, from http://www.census.gov/population/www/projections/natdet-D1A.htmlU. S. Census Bureau (2001). The Hispanic population:Census 2000 brief. Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: U.S. Departmen<strong>to</strong>f Commerce, Economics <strong>and</strong> Statistical Adm<strong>in</strong>istration.Vazquez Garcia, H. A., Garcia Coll, C., Erkut, S., Alarcon, O.,& Tropp, L. R. (2000). Family values of Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents.In M. Montero-Sieburth & F. A. Villarruel (Eds.), Mak<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>visible Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents visible (pp. 239–263). NewYork: Falmer Press.Villarruel, F. A., & Montero-Sieburth, M. (2000). Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth<strong>and</strong> America. In M. Montero-Sieburth & F. A. Villarruel(Eds.), Mak<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>visible Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents visible(pp. xiii–xxxii). New York: Falmer Press.Walsh, W. B. (Ed.). (2003). Counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology <strong>and</strong>optimal human function<strong>in</strong>g. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Weiss, R. S. (1974). The provisions of social relationships.In Z. Rub<strong>in</strong> (Ed.), Do<strong>in</strong>g un<strong>to</strong> others (pp. 17–26).Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Zane, N., & Mak, W. (2003). Major approaches <strong>to</strong> themeasurement of acculturation among ethnic m<strong>in</strong>oritypopulations: A content analysis <strong>and</strong> an alternativeempirical strategy. In K. M. Chun, P. B. Organista, &G. Mar<strong>in</strong> (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances <strong>in</strong> theory,measurement, <strong>and</strong> applied research (pp. 39–60).Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Psychological Association.Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K.(1988). The MultidimensionalScale of Perceived SocialSupport. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30–41.Zimet, G. D., Powell, S. S., Farley, G. K., Werkman, S., &Berkoff, K. A. (1990). Psychometric characteristics ofthe Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support.Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 610–617.Analysis of the StudyThe authors do a good job of stat<strong>in</strong>g the variables of <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong> this study <strong>and</strong> clearly stat<strong>in</strong>g that this study willemploy mixed-methods. The justification is somewhatlengthy but can be summarized by say<strong>in</strong>g that there isan <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g population of Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth <strong>and</strong> a need <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e the fac<strong>to</strong>rs that lead <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction amongthis population of adolescents. There seems <strong>to</strong> be a particularattempt <strong>to</strong> look at the research question from apositive perspective: What are the predic<strong>to</strong>rs of life satisfaction,as opposed <strong>to</strong> what are the predic<strong>to</strong>rs of problembehaviors (e.g., dropp<strong>in</strong>g out of school or drugusage), among Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth?The authors discuss the importance of family support,or familismo, <strong>and</strong> level of acculturation, or identification<strong>and</strong> comfort, with both the Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture <strong>and</strong> the Angloculture, as potential predic<strong>to</strong>rs of life satisfaction. Whilethese variables seem quite plausible, we wonder whetherother potential predic<strong>to</strong>rs might have been chosen <strong>in</strong> addition<strong>to</strong> these. It is not completely clear that these arethe only (or even the strongest) predic<strong>to</strong>rs of life satisfactionamong Lat<strong>in</strong>o adolescents.The purpose of the study is clearly stated as follows:“The overall purpose of the study was <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e therelationship between perceived family support, acculturation,<strong>and</strong> life satisfaction <strong>in</strong> Mexican American adolescents.”Specific questions <strong>and</strong> the research designfollow immediately afterwards.DEFINITIONSFamilismo is def<strong>in</strong>ed as the importance of extendedfamily ties <strong>in</strong> Lat<strong>in</strong>o culture as well as the strong identification<strong>and</strong> attachment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals with their families.From this def<strong>in</strong>ition the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable offamily support is derived.


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 581The def<strong>in</strong>ition of acculturation (also an <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable) is discussed <strong>in</strong> terms of its evolution from aunidimensional construct <strong>to</strong> a pair of <strong>in</strong>dependentdimensions. Orig<strong>in</strong>ally, acculturation was def<strong>in</strong>ed as thedegree <strong>to</strong> which one thought of oneself as Lat<strong>in</strong>o versusAnglo. Later theories, however, po<strong>in</strong>ted out that it waspossible <strong>to</strong> identify with both cultures rather than withonly a s<strong>in</strong>gle culture. This required measur<strong>in</strong>g identificationwith the two cultures separately.The concept of life satisfaction, the dependent variable<strong>in</strong> the study, is def<strong>in</strong>ed as an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s appraisalof his or her life, <strong>and</strong> is used as an <strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>r of wellbe<strong>in</strong>g.In particular, the authors note that life satisfaction<strong>in</strong>volves the cognitive, as opposed <strong>to</strong> the emotional,components of well-be<strong>in</strong>g.PRIOR RESEARCHPrior research on the <strong>in</strong>dependent <strong>and</strong> dependent variablesis cited extensively throughout the <strong>in</strong>troduction. Insome cases the research applies <strong>to</strong> populations otherthan Lat<strong>in</strong>o youth, which serves as a justification forstudy<strong>in</strong>g these variables <strong>in</strong> this population.HYPOTHESES AND DESIGNThe authors state no hypotheses, but rather three researchquestions are presented: (a) What do Mexican-American adolescents describe as variables that contribute<strong>to</strong> their life satisfaction? (b) <strong>How</strong> do perceivedfamily support <strong>and</strong> acculturation relate <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction?(c) Does acculturation moderate the relationshipbetween perceived family support <strong>and</strong> life satisfaction?The first of these questions is a qualitative one, the othertwo are quantitative. This demonstrates that the two differenttypes of research methods are tied <strong>to</strong> specificquestions—a good sign <strong>in</strong> a mixed-methods study, as itmakes clear which analyses are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> answerwhich questions. We th<strong>in</strong>k the latter two questionsclearly imply hypotheses, however—i.e., that the variablesare related.The authors present an argument for the importanceof mixed-methods research <strong>and</strong> describe the relative importanceof the two research method types: quantitativedata is given primary importance. The study used a triangulationdesign, all <strong>in</strong>struments were given at thesame time <strong>and</strong> the quantitative variables did not emergefrom the qualitative study (as <strong>in</strong> an explora<strong>to</strong>ry design).This is made clear at the outset both <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>itial questions<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the choice of quantitative <strong>in</strong>struments.The qualitative study was <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> elicit variablesimportant <strong>to</strong> life satisfaction, presumably <strong>to</strong> support thechoice of the quantitative variables—family support<strong>and</strong> acculturation. The quantitative method is a surveyadm<strong>in</strong>istered <strong>in</strong> groups followed by regression analysis.The qualitative method was based on grounded theory.SAMPLEThe target population is presumably all Mexican-American high school students <strong>in</strong> the United States. The<strong>in</strong>itial sample was a convenience sample of 309 Englishspeak<strong>in</strong>gmiddle <strong>and</strong> high school students from California,Kansas, <strong>and</strong> Texas recruited from various agencies.The sample was reduced <strong>to</strong> 266 by only <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g thosestudents who: (a) self-identified as Mexican-American<strong>and</strong> (b) who were <strong>in</strong> high school. The number of participantsis not listed by state. It probably should have been,as this would be a fac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> judg<strong>in</strong>g the generalizability/transferability of the results. It is unclear when theadditional criteria of student volunteer<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> parentalconsent were used but, <strong>in</strong> any case, they further reducegeneralizability.The authors do a good job of expla<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the recruitmentprocedures, <strong>and</strong> how consent was sought from:(a) the <strong>in</strong>stitutions used as recruitment centers, (b) theparents of the participants, <strong>and</strong> (c) the participantsthemselves. This explanation addresses certa<strong>in</strong> ethicalissues. <strong>How</strong>ever, the authors note that one recruitmentsite had an extremely low percentage of parental consent(7%) compared <strong>to</strong> most others (average of 65%).Why did this happen? The authors suggest that <strong>in</strong>adequatedescription of the study is the reason. We th<strong>in</strong>kdifferences <strong>in</strong> consent rate among sites <strong>and</strong> states shouldhave been reported <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> the average.INSTRUMENTATIONThe qualitative <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> the present study was as<strong>in</strong>gle question, “What fac<strong>to</strong>rs do you th<strong>in</strong>k contribute<strong>to</strong> life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> happ<strong>in</strong>ess?” Students were given10 blank l<strong>in</strong>es on which <strong>to</strong> respond (with the possibilityof us<strong>in</strong>g the back of the page). One problem with thistype of measure is that some participants may be reluctant<strong>to</strong> write a long answer, even if they have manyth<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> say. While an <strong>in</strong>terview might have beenpreferable, it would have been almost impossible <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview266 participants.The quantitative variables (perceived family support,life satisfaction, <strong>and</strong> Anglo <strong>and</strong> Mexican acculturation)are each operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> terms of scales or subscalesof various published <strong>in</strong>struments. Estimates of


582 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ereliability are given from the literature, as well as for thecurrent sample. One question did occur <strong>to</strong> us. Life satisfactionis operationally def<strong>in</strong>ed as the Global subscaleof the Multidimensional Students’ Life SatisfactionScale. We are unclear whether the entire scale was adm<strong>in</strong>istered<strong>and</strong> only the Global subscale used orwhether only the questions on the Global subscale wereadm<strong>in</strong>istered (perhaps <strong>to</strong> shorten the test<strong>in</strong>g time). Thelatter would raise questions about the validity of resultsbecause chang<strong>in</strong>g the context could affect the way itemswere answered.INTERNAL VALIDITY/CREDIBILITYInternal validity/credibility questions <strong>in</strong>volve thesoundness of the conclusions reached <strong>in</strong> the study,based on its design <strong>and</strong> execution. The quantitative variablesseem <strong>to</strong> have reasonable operational def<strong>in</strong>itions.One should remember, however, that regression analysisis a correlational technique. Therefore, direction ofcausation cannot be unequivocally determ<strong>in</strong>ed. Whileperceived family support <strong>and</strong> particular levels of acculturationmight cause high levels of life satisfaction, itcould be that a high degree of life satisfaction causesone <strong>to</strong> perceive higher levels of support <strong>and</strong> greateridentification with one or both cultures. With regard <strong>to</strong>credibility, the question arises as <strong>to</strong> whether respondentsmight have said more <strong>to</strong> the short-answer question ifthey hadn’t had <strong>to</strong> write their responses.Differences among respondent groups must be considereda possible threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. Given thevariety of sources students were recruited from, it seemslikely that socioeconomic status, for example, is related<strong>to</strong> both life satisfaction <strong>and</strong> perceived family support.Comparison of “sources” (e.g., private vs. publicschools) could clarify this issue. If such comparisonswere made, use of “t” tests as was done <strong>in</strong> some statecomparisons is not appropriate. Significance tests areappropriate for attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> generalize, not for assess<strong>in</strong>gequivalence of groups. Important differences canaffect outcomes, whether “significant” or not. In theabove <strong>in</strong>stance, the appropriate <strong>in</strong>dex is effect size(see page 244). When computed from data <strong>in</strong> the article,it is 0.22, a much better justification for comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ggroups.Atest<strong>in</strong>g threat may exist. If many students responded<strong>to</strong> the open-ended question with “family support,” itseems likely that they would recognize both this variable<strong>and</strong> “life satisfaction” on the questionnaire <strong>and</strong>, not necessarily<strong>in</strong>tentionally, make their responses consistent.DATA ANALYSISThe qualitative results were analyzed us<strong>in</strong>g groundedtheory, while the quantitative results were analyzedus<strong>in</strong>g correlations <strong>and</strong> hierarchical regression analysis.Both types of data (qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative) wereanalyzed separately <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> the discussion section.The analysis of open-ended responses followedrecommended procedures, although it seems <strong>to</strong> us thatthe use of audi<strong>to</strong>rs with closer ties <strong>to</strong> Mexican-Americanculture would have been preferable. Examples ofresponses clarify <strong>and</strong> support the emergent themes.RESULTS/DISCUSSIONThree themes emerged from the qualitative data: the importanceof family <strong>in</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g support <strong>and</strong> love (primarytheme), friends <strong>to</strong> provide help <strong>and</strong> fun (a secondarytheme), <strong>and</strong> the importance of a positive attitude<strong>to</strong>ward life (another secondary theme). The authors <strong>to</strong>okthe primary theme as support for their implied hypothesisthat Mexican-American high school students placehigh value on perceived family support.Two additional po<strong>in</strong>ts require mention<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> thequalitative analysis. As we expected, responses by students<strong>to</strong> the s<strong>in</strong>gle qualitative question were short.Would the results have been different had <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g(which would not have required the students <strong>to</strong> write responses)been used? Second, the authors explicitly addressthe issue of credibility or “trustworth<strong>in</strong>ess” of thequalitative results. It is good <strong>to</strong> see the steps taken <strong>to</strong> ensurehigh-quality qualitative f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.The authors began their analysis of the quantitativeresults by discuss<strong>in</strong>g the assumptions underly<strong>in</strong>g regression<strong>and</strong> demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g that the assumptions weremet. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the authors address the question of differences<strong>in</strong> their participants as a function of the state <strong>in</strong>which they resided. No differences were found, which isreassur<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>How</strong>ever, differences between participantsfrom California <strong>and</strong> Texas were not explored.The regression results revealed that perceived familysupport <strong>and</strong> Mexican orientation were significant predic<strong>to</strong>rsof life satisfaction, while Anglo orientation wasnot a significant predic<strong>to</strong>r. The amount of variance predictedwas 28 percent. While this is significant, the highreliability of life satisfaction scores of 0.86 suggests thatmany other fac<strong>to</strong>rs appear <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e life satisfactionamong Mexican-American high school students. Wehope that future research will shed some light on whatthese other fac<strong>to</strong>rs may be.


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 583The authors explicitly address the convergence <strong>and</strong>divergence of the qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative data.Specifically, the primary theme of family support (qualitativedata) re<strong>in</strong>forces the significance of family support<strong>in</strong> the regression analysis (quantitative data). Therole of friends <strong>and</strong> positive attitude (qualitative data)were not addressed <strong>in</strong> the regression. These may besome additional fac<strong>to</strong>rs account<strong>in</strong>g for life satisfactionthat can be studied <strong>in</strong> the future (quantitatively). Theauthors are <strong>to</strong> be commended for acknowledg<strong>in</strong>g some,although not all, of the study’s limitations.This article demonstrates how the results of qualitative<strong>and</strong> quantitative methodology <strong>to</strong>gether can providea more complete underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of a phenomenon thancould be achieved by us<strong>in</strong>g only one of the methodologiesby itself.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE NATURE AND VALUE OF MIXED-METHODS RESEARCHMixed-methods research <strong>in</strong>volves the use of both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative researchmethods <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle study. The results of these separate methods are comb<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>to</strong> present a more complete picture of the phenomenon under study than eithermethod could produce on its own.In mixed-methods research, the respective strengths of qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativemethods are seen as compensat<strong>in</strong>g for the respective weaknesses of each method.Disadvantages of mixed-methods research <strong>in</strong>volve the time, resources, <strong>and</strong> expertisenecessary <strong>to</strong> conduct this type of research well. With regard <strong>to</strong> expertise, if the researcheris not proficient <strong>in</strong> both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods, it is possible <strong>to</strong>team up with others who have expertise <strong>in</strong> the methods that the researcher is lack<strong>in</strong>g.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsWORLDVIEWS AND MIXED-METHODS RESEARCHQuantitative methods are usually associated with positivism.Qualitative methods are usually associated with postmodernism.Mixed-methods are usually associated with pragmatism.Pragmatists believe that one should use whatever methods best answer the researchquestion or questions at h<strong>and</strong>.TYPE OF MIXED-METHODS DESIGNSThe explora<strong>to</strong>ry design <strong>in</strong>volves first conduct<strong>in</strong>g a qualitative study <strong>to</strong> discover importantvariables underly<strong>in</strong>g a phenomenon <strong>and</strong> then conduct<strong>in</strong>g a quantitative study<strong>to</strong> discover relationships among the variables. This type of design is often used <strong>to</strong>develop rat<strong>in</strong>g scales <strong>in</strong> a new area of study.583


584 PART 7 Mixed-Methods Studies www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eThe explana<strong>to</strong>ry design <strong>in</strong>volves first conduct<strong>in</strong>g a quantitative study <strong>and</strong> then conduct<strong>in</strong>ga qualitative study <strong>to</strong> exp<strong>and</strong> upon the results of the quantitative study.The triangulation design <strong>in</strong>volves conduct<strong>in</strong>g both a qualitative study <strong>and</strong> a quantitativestudy (usually concurrently) <strong>and</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether the results of the two studiesconverge on a s<strong>in</strong>gle underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g of the underly<strong>in</strong>g phenomenon. If the resultsdo not converge, reasons for the lack of convergence need <strong>to</strong> be explored.All three of the mixed-methods designs may be conducted with an advocacy lens. Anadvocacy lens is present when the researcher’s worldview <strong>in</strong>volves advocat<strong>in</strong>g forthe improvement of conditions of the participants <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study.STEPS IN CONDUCTING MIXED-METHODS RESEARCHDevelop a clear rationale for the need for mixed-methods <strong>in</strong> the proposed project.Develop research questions that <strong>in</strong>volve both the qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative portionsof the study. Although a general research question may have led <strong>to</strong> the proposed project,sub-questions that <strong>in</strong>volve both qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative issues help show whymixed-methods are appropriate. These sub-questions also help guide data analysis.Before conduct<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study, one should decide if he or she has the time,resources, <strong>and</strong> expertise necessary <strong>to</strong> actually carry out the proposed project, <strong>and</strong> thendecide which mixed-methods research design applies <strong>to</strong> the proposed project.Triangulation designs often <strong>in</strong>volve the conversion of one type of data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the othertype. The conversion of qualitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> quantitative data is referred <strong>to</strong> as quantiz<strong>in</strong>g,while the conversion of quantitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> qualitative data is referred <strong>to</strong> asqualitiz<strong>in</strong>g.The results of a mixed-methods study should be written up <strong>in</strong> a manner consistentwith the research design selected.EVALUATING MIXED-METHODS RESEARCHThe <strong>in</strong>dividual quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative methods used should be evaluatedaccord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the criteria specific <strong>to</strong> these methods.One should check that both quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative data played a role <strong>in</strong> theconclusions; otherwise, one of the data types may simply be an “add-on.”Possible threats <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>and</strong>/or credibility as well as the externalvalidity or transferability of the study should always be considered.ETHICS IN MIXED-METHODS RESEARCHThe basic ethical concerns of protect<strong>in</strong>g participant identity, treat<strong>in</strong>g participantswith respect, <strong>and</strong> protect<strong>in</strong>g participants from both physical <strong>and</strong> psychological harmapply <strong>to</strong> mixed-methods studies as they do <strong>to</strong> other types of research.Key Termsadvocacy lens 562explana<strong>to</strong>ry design 560explora<strong>to</strong>ry design 560generalizability 565mixed-methodsresearch 557positivism 559postmodernism 559pragmatist 559qualitiz<strong>in</strong>g 561quantiz<strong>in</strong>g 561transferability 565triangulation 559triangulation design 560


CHAPTER 23 Mixed-Methods <strong>Research</strong> 5851. What do you see as the greatest strength of mixed-methods research? The greatestweakness?2. Are there any <strong>to</strong>pics that are particularly suitable <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated through amixed-methods study? If so, give an example.3. Mixed-methods research <strong>in</strong>volves the collection of both qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativedata. Which type of data do you th<strong>in</strong>k might be easiest <strong>to</strong> collect? Hardest? Why?4. Would it be possible <strong>to</strong> use r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a mixed-methods study? Why orwhy not?5. Is generalization possible <strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research?6. “Mixed-methods studies can help a researcher <strong>in</strong>vestigate questions that cannot beadequately researched through the use of quantitative or qualitative studies alone.”What might be some examples of such questions?7. Which of the mixed-methods research designs we describe <strong>in</strong> this chapter might bethe easiest <strong>to</strong> use? The hardest? Expla<strong>in</strong> why.8. What ethical concerns might possibly arise <strong>in</strong> do<strong>in</strong>g a mixed-methods study?For Discussion1. See Chapters 7, 17, 19, <strong>and</strong> 21.2. J. M. Aldridge, B. J. Fraser, <strong>and</strong> T. I. Huang (1999). Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 93(1): 48–62.3. C. Goldberg, R., Gallimore, <strong>and</strong> L. Reese (2005). In T. S. Weisner (ed.), Discover<strong>in</strong>g successful pathways<strong>in</strong> children’s development: Mixed methods <strong>in</strong> the study of childhood <strong>and</strong> family life (pp. 21–46). Chicago:University of Chicago Press.4. I. Jones (1997, December). Qualitative Report, 3(4). Retrieved April 8, 2006, from the Nova SoutheasternUniversity Web site: http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR3-4/jones.html.5. R. McEntarffer (2003). Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Nebraska–L<strong>in</strong>coln.6. G. B. Rossman <strong>and</strong> B. L. Wilson (1985). Evaluation Review, 9(5): 627–643.7. P. F. Weitzman <strong>and</strong> S. E. Levkoff (2000). Field Methods, 12(3): 195–208.8. M. Trow (1957). Comment of participant observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g: A comparison. HumanOrganization, 16: 33–35.9. D. T. Campbell <strong>and</strong> D. W. Fiske (1959). Convergent <strong>and</strong> discrim<strong>in</strong>ant validation by the multitraitmultimethodmatrix. Psychological Bullet<strong>in</strong>, 54: 297–312.10. N. K. Denz<strong>in</strong> (1978). The logic of naturalistic <strong>in</strong>quiry. In N. K. Denz<strong>in</strong> (ed.), Sociological methods: Asourcebook. New York: McGraw-Hill.11. T. D. Jick (1979). Mix<strong>in</strong>g qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitative methods: Triangulation <strong>in</strong> action. Adm<strong>in</strong>istrativeScience Quarterly, 24: 602–611.12. Jick, op. cit.13. See Chapter 18, pages 423–424.14. See the Controversies <strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong> box on page 426.15. G. B. Rossman <strong>and</strong> B. L. Wilson (1985). Numbers <strong>and</strong> words: Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativemethods <strong>in</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle large-scale evaluation study. Evaluation Review, 9(5): 627–643.16. A. Tashakkori <strong>and</strong> C. Teddlie (1998). Mixed methodology: Comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativeapproaches. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage, p. 41, <strong>and</strong> Creswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark, op. cit., Chapter 2.17. Creswell <strong>and</strong> Plano Clark also mention what they call an embedded design. See J. W. Creswell <strong>and</strong> V. L.Plano Clark (2007). <strong>Design</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g mixed methods research. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 67–71.18. N. E. Wallen <strong>and</strong> R. O. Vowles (1960). The effects of <strong>in</strong>tra-class ability group<strong>in</strong>g on arithmetic achievement<strong>in</strong> the sixth grade. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al Psychology, 51: 159–163.19. Jack R. Fraenkel (1994). A portrait of four social studies teachers <strong>and</strong> their classes: With special attentionpaid <strong>to</strong> identification of teach<strong>in</strong>g techniques <strong>and</strong> behaviors that contribute <strong>to</strong> student learn<strong>in</strong>g. In D. S.Tierney, ed., 1994 Yearbook of California <strong>Education</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. San Francisco, CA: Caddo Gap Press.20. These po<strong>in</strong>ts were made by Hanson, Creswell, Plano Clark, Petska, <strong>and</strong> Creswell, (2005). Mixed methodsresearch designs <strong>in</strong> counsel<strong>in</strong>g psychology. Journal of Counsel<strong>in</strong>g Psychology, 52(2): 224–235.21. See p. 281 <strong>in</strong> Chapter 13.Notes


<strong>Research</strong>by PractitionersP A R T8Part 8 presents a discussion of action research. Both similar <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> different from themore formal methodologies discussed earlier, action research has of late shown<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g popularity. We discuss this methodology <strong>in</strong> some detail <strong>and</strong> present severalexamples of how action research studies might actually be carried out <strong>in</strong> schools.Lastly, we present a published example of action research, followed by our analysis ofits strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses.


24Action <strong>Research</strong>What Is Action <strong>Research</strong>?Basic AssumptionsUnderly<strong>in</strong>g Action<strong>Research</strong>Types of Action<strong>Research</strong>Practical Action <strong>Research</strong>Participa<strong>to</strong>ry Action<strong>Research</strong>Levels of ParticipationSteps <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong>Identify<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>Research</strong>QuestionGather<strong>in</strong>g the NecessaryInformationAnalyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Interpret<strong>in</strong>gthe InformationDevelop<strong>in</strong>g an Action PlanSimilarities <strong>and</strong>Differences BetweenAction <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong>Formal Quantitative <strong>and</strong>Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong>Internal Validity <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong>Action <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong>External ValidityThe Advantagesof Action <strong>Research</strong>An Example ofAction <strong>Research</strong>Analysis of the StudyPurpose/JustificationDef<strong>in</strong>itionsPrior <strong>Research</strong>HypothesesSampleInstrumentationProcedures/Internal ValidityData AnalysisResults/Discussion/Interpretation"If I can’tgeneralize myresults from study<strong>in</strong>gjust one class, what’sthe po<strong>in</strong>t?"OBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Expla<strong>in</strong> the term “action research.”Describe the assumptions that underlieaction research.Expla<strong>in</strong> the purpose of action research.Describe the four steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>action research.Describe some of the advantages ofaction research.Describe some of the similarities <strong>and</strong>differences between action research <strong>and</strong>formal quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research.Describe the difference between practical<strong>and</strong> participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research."It should haveimmediate mean<strong>in</strong>gfor that situation; it maystimulate others <strong>to</strong> replicateyour study; <strong>and</strong> it maygenerate some ideas forfurther studies."Suggest some ways that other k<strong>in</strong>ds ofresearch methodologies might be used<strong>in</strong> action research.Name some of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity that exist <strong>in</strong> action researchstudies.Describe the k<strong>in</strong>ds of sampl<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>in</strong>action research.Expla<strong>in</strong> why action research studies areweak <strong>in</strong> external validity.Recognize an example of action researchwhen you come across it <strong>in</strong> theeducational literature.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About the Role of <strong>Research</strong>ers <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivities:Activity 24.1: Action <strong>Research</strong> QuestionsActivity 24.2: True or False?Robert Jackson is <strong>in</strong> his second year of teach<strong>in</strong>g at an elementary school <strong>in</strong> Sarasota, Florida. Recently, he hasbeen more <strong>and</strong> more bothered by a considerable amount of disruptive behavior <strong>in</strong> his fifth-grade class. Theboys <strong>in</strong> the class are particularly troublesome. Many take a long time <strong>to</strong> settle <strong>in</strong> their seats after the afternoonrecess, have trouble pay<strong>in</strong>g attention when he is giv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, <strong>and</strong> often punch out at other students forapparently no reason. The girls <strong>in</strong> the class never seem <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p talk<strong>in</strong>g. Robert is becom<strong>in</strong>g very concerned, as a lo<strong>to</strong>f valuable class time is taken up by his so-far-unsuccessful attempts <strong>to</strong> deal with these problems. Of specialconcern is that he feels his students are learn<strong>in</strong>g only a small amount of what they could were he able <strong>to</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong> amore orderly class.What might Robert do <strong>in</strong> this situation? Action research, the subject of this chapter, is an ideal methodology thathe might employ.What Is Action <strong>Research</strong>?Action research is conducted by one or more <strong>in</strong>dividualsor groups for the purpose of solv<strong>in</strong>g a problem or obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>formation <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>form local practice.Those <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> action research generally want <strong>to</strong>solve some k<strong>in</strong>d of day-<strong>to</strong>-day immediate problem, suchas how <strong>to</strong> decrease absenteeism or <strong>in</strong>cidents of v<strong>and</strong>alismamong the student body, motivate apathetic students,figure out ways <strong>to</strong> use technology <strong>to</strong> improve theteach<strong>in</strong>g of mathematics, or <strong>in</strong>crease fund<strong>in</strong>g.There are many k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions that lend themselves<strong>to</strong> action research <strong>in</strong> schools. What k<strong>in</strong>ds ofmethods, for example, work best with what k<strong>in</strong>ds of students?<strong>How</strong> can teachers encourage students <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>kabout important issues? <strong>How</strong> can content, teach<strong>in</strong>gstrategies, <strong>and</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g activities be varied <strong>to</strong> help studentsof differ<strong>in</strong>g ages, gender, ethnicity, <strong>and</strong> abilitylearn more effectively? <strong>How</strong> can subject matter bepresented so as <strong>to</strong> maximize underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g? What canteachers <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs do <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>crease the <strong>in</strong>terest ofstudents <strong>in</strong> school<strong>in</strong>g? What can counselors do? Whatcan other educational professionals do? <strong>How</strong> canparents become more <strong>in</strong>volved?Classroom teachers, counselors, supervisors, <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rscan help provide some answers <strong>to</strong> these(<strong>and</strong> other) important questions by engag<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> actionresearch. Such studies, taken <strong>in</strong>dividually, are seriouslylimited <strong>in</strong> generalizability. If, however, several teachers<strong>in</strong> different schools with<strong>in</strong> the same district, forexample, were <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the same question <strong>in</strong> theirclassrooms (thereby replicat<strong>in</strong>g the research of theirpeers), they could create a base of ideas that couldgeneralize <strong>to</strong> policy or practice.Action research often does not require completemastery of the major types of research we havedescribed <strong>in</strong> previous chapters. The steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>action research are actually pretty straightforward. Theimportant th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> remember is that such studies arerooted <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>terests <strong>and</strong> needs of practitioners.BASIC ASSUMPTIONSUNDERLYING ACTION RESEARCHA number of assumptions underlie action research.Those who do action research assume that those<strong>in</strong>volved, either s<strong>in</strong>gly or <strong>in</strong> groups, are <strong>in</strong>formed<strong>in</strong>dividuals who are capable of identify<strong>in</strong>g problems thatneed <strong>to</strong> be solved <strong>and</strong> of determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> go aboutsolv<strong>in</strong>g them. It is also assumed that those <strong>in</strong>volved areseriously committed <strong>to</strong> improv<strong>in</strong>g their performance <strong>and</strong>that they want cont<strong>in</strong>uously <strong>and</strong> systematically <strong>to</strong> reflec<strong>to</strong>n such performance. Further, it is assumed that teachers589


590 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 24.1Basic Assumptions Underly<strong>in</strong>g Action <strong>Research</strong>AssumptionTeachers <strong>and</strong> other education professionalshave the authority <strong>to</strong> make decisions.Teachers <strong>and</strong> other education professionalswant <strong>to</strong> improve their practice.Teachers <strong>and</strong> other education professionalsare committed <strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ual professionaldevelopment.Teachers <strong>and</strong> other education professionalswill <strong>and</strong> can engage <strong>in</strong> systematic research.ExampleA team of teachers, after discussions with the school adm<strong>in</strong>istration,decides <strong>to</strong> meet weekly <strong>to</strong> revise the mathematics curriculum <strong>to</strong> make itmore relevant <strong>to</strong> low-achiev<strong>in</strong>g students.A group of teachers decides <strong>to</strong> observe each other on a weekly basis <strong>and</strong>then discuss ways <strong>to</strong> improve their teach<strong>in</strong>g.The entire staff—adm<strong>in</strong>istration, teachers, counselors, <strong>and</strong> clerical staff—of an elementary school goes on a retreat <strong>to</strong> plan ways <strong>to</strong> improve theattendance <strong>and</strong> discipl<strong>in</strong>e policies for the school.Follow<strong>in</strong>g up on the example just listed above, the staff decides <strong>to</strong>collect data by review<strong>in</strong>g the attendance records of chronic absenteesover the past year, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of attendees <strong>and</strong>absentees <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e why they differ, <strong>to</strong> hold a series of after-schoolroundtable sessions between discipl<strong>in</strong>e-prone students <strong>and</strong> faculty <strong>to</strong>identify problems <strong>and</strong> discuss ways <strong>to</strong> resolve issues of contention, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>establish a men<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g system <strong>in</strong> which selected students can serve ascounselors <strong>to</strong> students need<strong>in</strong>g help with their assigned work.<strong>and</strong> others <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the schools want <strong>to</strong> engage <strong>in</strong> researchsystematically—<strong>to</strong> identify problems, decide on<strong>in</strong>vestigative procedures, determ<strong>in</strong>e data collection techniques,analyze <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret data, <strong>and</strong> develop plans ofaction <strong>to</strong> deal with problems. Lastly, it is assumed thatthose <strong>in</strong>tend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> carry out the research have the authority<strong>to</strong> undertake the necessary procedures <strong>and</strong> implementrecommendations. These assumptions are described a bitfurther <strong>and</strong> exemplified <strong>in</strong> Table 24.1.Types of Action <strong>Research</strong>Mills has identified two ma<strong>in</strong> types of action research,although variations <strong>and</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ations of the two arepossible. 1PRACTICAL ACTION RESEARCHPractical action research is <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> address a specificproblem with<strong>in</strong> a classroom, school, or other “community.”It can be carried out <strong>in</strong> a variety of sett<strong>in</strong>gs,such as educational, social service, or bus<strong>in</strong>ess locations.Its primary purpose is <strong>to</strong> improve practice <strong>in</strong> theshort term as well as <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>form larger issues. It can becarried out by <strong>in</strong>dividuals, teams, or even larger groups,provided the focus rema<strong>in</strong>s clear <strong>and</strong> specific. To bemaximally successful, practical action research shouldresult <strong>in</strong> an action plan that, ideally, will be implemented<strong>and</strong> further evaluated.PARTICIPATORY ACTION RESEARCHParticipa<strong>to</strong>ry action research, while shar<strong>in</strong>g the focuson a specific local issue <strong>and</strong> on us<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong>implement action, differs <strong>in</strong> important ways from practicalaction research. The first difference is that it has twoadditional purposes: <strong>to</strong> empower <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong> groups<strong>to</strong> improve their lives <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> br<strong>in</strong>g about social changeat some level—school, community, or society. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly,it deliberately <strong>in</strong>volves a sizable group of peoplerepresent<strong>in</strong>g diverse experiences <strong>and</strong> viewpo<strong>in</strong>ts, all ofwhom are focused on the same problem. The <strong>in</strong>tent is <strong>to</strong>have <strong>in</strong>tensive <strong>in</strong>volvement of all these stakeholders,who function as equal partners (Figure 24.1).Achiev<strong>in</strong>g this goal requires that the stakeholders,although they may not all be <strong>in</strong>volved at the outset,become active early <strong>in</strong> the process <strong>and</strong> jo<strong>in</strong>tly plan thestudy. This <strong>in</strong>cludes not only clarify<strong>in</strong>g purposes butalso agree<strong>in</strong>g on other aspects, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g data collection<strong>and</strong> analysis, <strong>in</strong>terpretation of data, <strong>and</strong> result<strong>in</strong>gactions. For this reason, participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research isoften referred <strong>to</strong> as collaborative research. In its “pure”form, participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research isa collaborative approach <strong>to</strong> research that provides peoplewith the means <strong>to</strong> take systematic action <strong>in</strong> an effort <strong>to</strong>


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 591"We need <strong>to</strong>be sure <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludeeveryone who has astake <strong>in</strong> ourproject.""Good idea, butwill they reallyparticipate?""I agree,but who are thestakeholders?Do we <strong>in</strong>cludetaxpayer groups?Unions?"Figure 24.1Stakeholdersresolve specific problems. [It] encourages consensual,democratic, <strong>and</strong> participa<strong>to</strong>ry strategies <strong>to</strong> encouragepeople <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e reflectively problems affect<strong>in</strong>gthem. ... Further, it encourages people <strong>to</strong> formulateaccounts <strong>and</strong> explanations of their situation, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>develop plans that may resolve these problems. 2Sometimes a tra<strong>in</strong>ed researcher identifies a problem<strong>and</strong> br<strong>in</strong>gs it <strong>to</strong> the attention of the stakeholders. But itis essential that the researcher realize that the problem<strong>to</strong> be studied must be a problem that is important <strong>to</strong>the stakeholders, <strong>and</strong> not simply of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> the researcher.The researcher <strong>and</strong> the stakeholders jo<strong>in</strong>tlyformulate the research problem (often through bra<strong>in</strong>s<strong>to</strong>rm<strong>in</strong>gor by conduct<strong>in</strong>g focus groups). This approachcontrasts with many of the more traditional <strong>in</strong>vestigations,<strong>in</strong> which the researchers formulate the problemby themselves (Figure 24.2). Berg describes the tra<strong>in</strong>edresearcher’s role as follows:The formally tra<strong>in</strong>ed researcher st<strong>and</strong>s with <strong>and</strong> alongsidethe community or group under study, not outside as anobjective observer or external consultant. The researchercontributes expertise when needed as a participant <strong>in</strong> theprocess. The researcher collaborates with local practitionersas well as stakeholders <strong>in</strong> the group or community.Other participants contribute their physical <strong>and</strong>/or <strong>in</strong>tellectualresources <strong>to</strong> the research process. The researcher isa partner with the study population; thus, this type ofresearch is considerably more value-laden than othermore traditional roles <strong>and</strong> endeavors. 3LEVELS OF PARTICIPATIONIn part because of the <strong>in</strong>fluence of participa<strong>to</strong>ry actionresearch, more attention has been paid <strong>in</strong> recent years <strong>to</strong>the role of <strong>in</strong>dividuals who participate <strong>in</strong> research projects.His<strong>to</strong>rically, <strong>in</strong> most educational <strong>and</strong> other research,the subjects <strong>in</strong> a study simply provided data—bybe<strong>in</strong>g tested, observed, <strong>in</strong>terviewed, <strong>and</strong> so forth. Theyreceived little or no benefit other than a thank-you (<strong>and</strong>sometimes not even that). The benefits of the study accrued<strong>to</strong> the researcher <strong>and</strong> (presumably) <strong>to</strong> the societyas a whole.Such use of <strong>in</strong>dividuals raises questions of ethics,even though there may be no risk, deception, or issues ofconfidentiality <strong>in</strong>volved. Consequently, more effort hasbeen directed <strong>to</strong>ward at least <strong>in</strong>form<strong>in</strong>g the participants<strong>in</strong> a study as <strong>to</strong> the purposes of the study. This may,however, create a threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of thestudy or the validity of the data. Participants may, <strong>in</strong>some cases, be provided the results of the study <strong>and</strong>,perhaps, be asked <strong>to</strong> review them. There is, <strong>in</strong> fact, acont<strong>in</strong>uum of participation (Figure 24.3). Higher levelsof <strong>in</strong>volvement may <strong>in</strong>clude help<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>strument development,data collection, <strong>and</strong> data analysis; participat<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong> data <strong>in</strong>terpretation; mak<strong>in</strong>g recommendations


592 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e"We need <strong>to</strong>decide what we want—then hire a consultant<strong>to</strong> do it!""I don’t th<strong>in</strong>kso. We may needan expert, but only<strong>to</strong> advise us.""No, anyconsultant has <strong>to</strong>be a real member ofour team!"(Pr<strong>in</strong>cipal)(Teacher)(Parent)Figure 24.2 The Role of the “Expert” <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong>987654321LEVELS OF PARTICIPATION IN ACTION RESEARCHInitiate studyParticipate <strong>in</strong> problem specificationParticipate <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g projectParticipate <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretationReview f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsAssist <strong>in</strong> data collection <strong>and</strong>/or analysisReceive f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsBecome <strong>in</strong>formed of purpose of the studyProvide <strong>in</strong>formationFigure 24.3 Levels of Participation <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong>for further research; actively participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> design<strong>in</strong>gthe study; formulat<strong>in</strong>g the problem of concern; even<strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g the research effort. In addition <strong>to</strong> degree ofparticipation, the nature of the participation varieswith participant <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>and</strong> background. It would beunusual, for example, for elementary students <strong>to</strong> participateat or beyond level three. Similarly, the stakeholders<strong>in</strong> participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research are unlikely <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>volved at all levels.Steps <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong>Action research <strong>in</strong>volves four basic stages: (1) identify<strong>in</strong>gthe research problem or question, (2) obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g thenecessary <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> answer the question(s), (3) analyz<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>formation that has beengathered, <strong>and</strong> (4) develop<strong>in</strong>g a plan of action. Let us discusseach of these stages <strong>in</strong> more detail.IDENTIFYING THE RESEARCH QUESTIONThe first stage <strong>in</strong> action research is clarify<strong>in</strong>g the problemof concern. An <strong>in</strong>dividual or group needs <strong>to</strong> carefullyexam<strong>in</strong>e the situation <strong>and</strong> identify the problem.Action research is most appropriate when teachers orothers <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> education wish <strong>to</strong> make someth<strong>in</strong>gbetter, improve their practice, deal with a troublesomeissue, or correct someth<strong>in</strong>g that is not work<strong>in</strong>g.An important th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> remember is that for an actionresearch project <strong>to</strong> be successful, it must be manageable.Thus, large-scale, complex issues are probablybest left <strong>to</strong> professional researchers. Action researchprojects are (usually) quite narrow <strong>in</strong> scope. <strong>How</strong>ever, ifa group of teachers, students, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, <strong>and</strong> so onhave decided <strong>to</strong> work <strong>to</strong>gether on some type of longtermproject, the research can be more extensive. Thus,a problem like “What might be a better way <strong>to</strong> teach


CONTROVERSIES IN RESEARCH<strong>How</strong> Much Should ParticipantsBe Involved <strong>in</strong> <strong>Research</strong>?The active <strong>in</strong>volvement of subjects <strong>in</strong> all aspects of plann<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out research has been advocated onthe grounds that participants not only have a right <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencethe direction <strong>and</strong> procedures of a study, but also thatthey can make major contributions <strong>to</strong> the research effort itself.Questions have been raised, however, as <strong>to</strong> whetherparticipation by <strong>in</strong>dividuals with only a limited background<strong>in</strong> research may result <strong>in</strong> errors <strong>and</strong>/or bias <strong>in</strong> the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>and</strong> perhaps be subverted <strong>to</strong> political ends;*,† there is addi-tional concern that community participants often could beexploited.‡Sclove contends that policy boards such as the National ScienceBoard should <strong>in</strong>clude nonexpert members as a way <strong>to</strong> democratizescience <strong>and</strong> enhance public support for research—ashas been done for years <strong>in</strong> other countries.§ Others argue thatthe active <strong>in</strong>volvement of participants can lead <strong>to</strong> social changeas “community members become self-sufficient researchers<strong>and</strong> activists.” || Some, however, see a danger <strong>in</strong> mix<strong>in</strong>g research<strong>and</strong> activism. S<strong>to</strong>ecker has explored three major, <strong>and</strong>controversial, roles that the academic expert might play <strong>in</strong> participantresearch: the <strong>in</strong>itia<strong>to</strong>r, the consultant, <strong>and</strong> the collabora<strong>to</strong>r,each appropriate <strong>to</strong> different community needs.#What do you th<strong>in</strong>k? To what extent should the participants<strong>in</strong> a study have a say <strong>in</strong> the plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> execution of the study?*A. Bowes. (1996). Evaluat<strong>in</strong>g an empower<strong>in</strong>g research strategy:Reflections on action research with South Asian women. Sociological<strong>Research</strong> Onl<strong>in</strong>e 1. Available at http//kennedy.soc.survey.ac.uk.sosresonl<strong>in</strong>e/1/1contents.html.†S. Nyoni. (1991). People power <strong>in</strong> Zimbabwe. In O. Fals-Border <strong>and</strong>M. A. Rahmar (Eds.), Action <strong>and</strong> knowledge: Break<strong>in</strong>g the monopolywith participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research. New York: Apex, pp. 109–120.‡Hall, B. (1992). “From marg<strong>in</strong>s <strong>to</strong> center? The development <strong>and</strong> purposeof participa<strong>to</strong>ry research,” American Sociologist, 23: 15–28.§R. E. Sclove (1998). “Better approaches <strong>to</strong> science policy.” Edi<strong>to</strong>rial<strong>and</strong> Letters, Science, 279 (February): 1283.|| R. S<strong>to</strong>ecker (1999). Are academics irrelevant? Roles for scholars <strong>in</strong>participa<strong>to</strong>ry research. American Behavioral Scientist, 42(5): 842.#Ibid., pp. 840–854.fractions?” is more suitable than “Is <strong>in</strong>quiry teach<strong>in</strong>gmore appropriate than more traditional teach<strong>in</strong>g?”While quite important, the latter is <strong>to</strong>o broad for easyresolution with a s<strong>in</strong>gle classroom or teacher.GATHERING THE NECESSARY INFORMATIONOnce a problem has been identified, the next step is <strong>to</strong>decide what sorts of data are needed <strong>and</strong> how <strong>to</strong> collectthem. Any of the methodologies we have describedearlier <strong>in</strong> this book can be used (although usually <strong>in</strong>a somewhat simplified <strong>and</strong> less sophisticated form)<strong>in</strong> action research. Experiments, surveys, causalcomparativestudies, observations, <strong>in</strong>terviews, analysisof documents, ethnographies—all are possible methodologies<strong>to</strong> consider. (We will present some examples ofhow these might be used later <strong>in</strong> the chapter).Teachers can be either active participants (e.g.,observ<strong>in</strong>g the computer strategies used by one’s studentswhile <strong>in</strong>struct<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong> computer usage) or nonparticipants(e.g., observ<strong>in</strong>g how students <strong>in</strong>teract withone another dur<strong>in</strong>g classroom study time). Whicheverrole is chosen, it is a good idea <strong>to</strong> record as much as possibledur<strong>in</strong>g the observations—<strong>in</strong> short, <strong>to</strong> take fieldnotes <strong>to</strong> describe what was seen <strong>and</strong> heard.In addition <strong>to</strong> observ<strong>in</strong>g, a second major category ofdata collection <strong>in</strong>volves <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g students or other<strong>in</strong>dividuals from whom <strong>in</strong>formation is desired. Data collectedthrough observations often can suggest questions<strong>to</strong> follow up on through <strong>in</strong>terviews or the adm<strong>in</strong>istrationof questionnaires. In fact, adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g questionnaires<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g the participants <strong>in</strong> a study can be avalid <strong>and</strong> productive way <strong>to</strong> assess the accuracy of observations.As is true of other aspects of action research,<strong>in</strong>terviews tend <strong>to</strong> be less formal <strong>and</strong> often a bit more unstructuredthan <strong>in</strong> more formal research studies.A third category of data collection <strong>in</strong>volves the exam<strong>in</strong>ation<strong>and</strong> analysis of documents. This method isperhaps the least time-consum<strong>in</strong>g of the three <strong>and</strong> theeasiest <strong>to</strong> commence. Attendance records, m<strong>in</strong>utes offaculty meet<strong>in</strong>gs, counselor records, school newspaperaccounts, student journals, lesson plans, adm<strong>in</strong>istrativelogs, suspension lists, detention records, seat<strong>in</strong>g charts,pho<strong>to</strong>graphs of class <strong>and</strong> school activities, studentportfolios—all are grist for the action researcher’s mill.Action research allows for the use of all of the typesof <strong>in</strong>struments discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 7—questionnaires,<strong>in</strong>terview schedules, checklists, rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, attitud<strong>in</strong>almeasures, <strong>and</strong> so forth. <strong>How</strong>ever, often the teachers,adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs, or counselors <strong>in</strong>volved (sometimes even593


594 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7estudents) develop their own <strong>in</strong>strument(s) <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong>make them locally appropriate. And they are usuallyshorter, simpler, <strong>and</strong> less formal than the <strong>in</strong>strumentsused <strong>in</strong> more traditional research studies.Some action research uses more than one <strong>in</strong>strumen<strong>to</strong>r other forms of triangulation (see pages 510–511).Thus, ask<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong> respond <strong>to</strong> carefully prepared<strong>in</strong>terview questions might be supplemented by videorecord<strong>in</strong>gs; data obta<strong>in</strong>ed through the use of observationalchecklists might be checked aga<strong>in</strong>st audio record<strong>in</strong>gsof classroom discussions; <strong>and</strong> so forth. Whatmethod(s) <strong>to</strong> use is dictated, as <strong>in</strong> any research <strong>in</strong>vestigation,by the nature of the research question.Action researchers must avoid collect<strong>in</strong>g merelyanecdotal data—that is, just the op<strong>in</strong>ions of people as <strong>to</strong>how the problem might be addressed. Although anecdotaldata are often valuable, we believe strongly thatmore substantive evidence of some sort (e.g., audiotaperecord<strong>in</strong>gs, videotapes, observations, written replies <strong>to</strong>questionnaires, <strong>and</strong> so forth) should be obta<strong>in</strong>ed.ANALYZING AND INTERPRETINGTHE INFORMATIONThis step focuses on analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the datagathered <strong>in</strong> step two. After be<strong>in</strong>g collected <strong>and</strong> summarized,the data need <strong>to</strong> be analyzed so that the participantscan decide what the data reveal. <strong>How</strong>ever, analysisof action research data is usually much less complex <strong>and</strong>detailed than other forms of research.What is important at this stage is that the data be exam<strong>in</strong>ed<strong>in</strong> relation <strong>to</strong> resolv<strong>in</strong>g the research question orproblem for which the research was conducted. With regard<strong>to</strong> participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research, Str<strong>in</strong>ger suggests anumber of questions that can provide a guid<strong>in</strong>g procedurefor analyz<strong>in</strong>g the gathered data.The first question, why, establishes a general focus forthe <strong>in</strong>vestigation, rem<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g everyone what the purposeof the study orig<strong>in</strong>ally was. The rema<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g questions—what, how, who, where, <strong>and</strong> when—enable participants<strong>to</strong> identify associated <strong>in</strong>fluences. The <strong>in</strong>tent is <strong>to</strong> betterunderst<strong>and</strong> the data <strong>in</strong> context of the sett<strong>in</strong>g or situation.What <strong>and</strong> how questions help <strong>to</strong> establish the problems<strong>and</strong> issues: What is go<strong>in</strong>g on that bothers people? <strong>How</strong>do these problems or issues <strong>in</strong>trude upon the lives of thepeople or the group? Who, where, <strong>and</strong> when questionsfocus on specific actions, events, <strong>and</strong> activities that relate<strong>to</strong> the problems or issues at h<strong>and</strong>. The purpose here isnot for participants <strong>to</strong> make quality judgments aboutthese elements; rather, it is <strong>to</strong> assess the data <strong>and</strong> clarify<strong>in</strong>formation that has been gathered. Additionally, ... thisprocess provides a means for participants <strong>to</strong> reflect onth<strong>in</strong>gs that they have themselves discussed (captured <strong>in</strong>the data) or that other participants have mentioned. 4When analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g data gathered <strong>in</strong> participa<strong>to</strong>ryaction research, it is important that the participantstry <strong>to</strong> reflect the perceptions of all the stakeholders<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study. Hence, they should work collaboratively<strong>to</strong> create descriptions of what the data reveal. Furthermore,the participants must make every effort <strong>to</strong> keepall of the stakeholders <strong>in</strong>formed of what is go<strong>in</strong>g on dur<strong>in</strong>gthe data gather<strong>in</strong>g stage <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> provide opportunitiesfor everyone <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>to</strong> read accounts of what is happen<strong>in</strong>gas they are prepared (not simply after the study is completed).This permits all of the stakeholders <strong>to</strong> give their<strong>in</strong>put cont<strong>in</strong>uously as the study progresses (Figure 24.4).DEVELOPING AN ACTION PLANFulfill<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tent of an action research study requirescreat<strong>in</strong>g a plan <strong>to</strong> implement changes based on the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.While it is desirable that a formal document beprepared, it is not essential; what is essential is that thestudy, at the very least, <strong>in</strong>dicate clear directions for furtherwork on the orig<strong>in</strong>al problem or concern.Similarities <strong>and</strong> DifferencesBetween Action <strong>Research</strong><strong>and</strong> Formal Quantitative<strong>and</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Action research is different <strong>in</strong> many ways from moreformal quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative research, but italso has a number of similarities. Both are shown <strong>in</strong>Table 24.2.SAMPLING IN ACTION RESEARCHAction research problems almost always focus on only aparticular group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals (a teacher’s class, some ofa counselor’s clients, an adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r’s faculty), <strong>and</strong>hence the sample <strong>and</strong> population are identical. R<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>g is often difficult <strong>in</strong> schools, but this is not as criticalas it would be <strong>in</strong> more traditional research endeavors,because generaliz<strong>in</strong>g is not necessarily likely nor desired.INTERNAL VALIDITY IN ACTION RESEARCHAction research studies are subject <strong>to</strong> all of the threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity we described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9, although <strong>in</strong>differ<strong>in</strong>g degrees. Such studies suffer particularly from


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 595"Here are myrecommendations.Take a look,will you?""Good!I’d like <strong>to</strong>.""That’s yourjob, Joe.""Whoa!I thought wewere a team. Weshould all lookat it!"Figure 24.4 Participation <strong>in</strong> Action <strong>Research</strong>the possibility of data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, because the datacollec<strong>to</strong>r is well aware of the <strong>in</strong>tent of the study. He orshe must take care not <strong>to</strong> overlook results or responseshe or she does not want <strong>to</strong> see. Implementation <strong>and</strong> attitud<strong>in</strong>aleffects are also a strong possibility, as either implementersor data collec<strong>to</strong>rs can, unwitt<strong>in</strong>gly, dis<strong>to</strong>rtthe results of a study.ACTION RESEARCHAND EXTERNAL VALIDITYAs is true of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experimental studies, actionresearch studies are weak when it comes <strong>to</strong> externalvalidity—generalizability. One cannot recommend us<strong>in</strong>ga practice found <strong>to</strong> be effective <strong>in</strong> only one classroom!Thus, action research studies that show a particularTABLE 24.2Action <strong>Research</strong>Similarities <strong>and</strong> Differences Between Action <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Formal Quantitative<strong>and</strong> Qualitative <strong>Research</strong>Systematic <strong>in</strong>quiry.Goal is <strong>to</strong> solve problems of local concern.Little formal tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g required <strong>to</strong> conductsuch studies.Intent is <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>and</strong> correct problems oflocal concern.Carried out by teacher or other localeducation professional.Uses primarily teacher-developed <strong>in</strong>struments.Less rigorous.Usually value-based.Purposive samples selected.Selective op<strong>in</strong>ions of researcher oftenconsidered as data.Generalizability is very limited.Formal <strong>Research</strong>Systematic <strong>in</strong>quiry.Goal is <strong>to</strong> develop <strong>and</strong> test theories <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> produce knowledgegeneralizable <strong>to</strong> wide population.Considerable tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g required <strong>to</strong> conduct such studies.Intent is <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate larger issues.Carried out by researcher who is not usually <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> local situation.Uses primarily professionally developed <strong>in</strong>struments.More rigorous.Frequently value-neutral.R<strong>and</strong>om samples (if possible) preferred.Selective op<strong>in</strong>ions of researcher never considered as data.Generalizability often appropriate.


596 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7epractice <strong>to</strong> be effective, that reveal certa<strong>in</strong> types of attitudes,or that encourage particular k<strong>in</strong>ds of changesneed <strong>to</strong> be replicated if their results are <strong>to</strong> be generalized<strong>to</strong> other <strong>in</strong>dividuals, sett<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> situations.The Advantagesof Action <strong>Research</strong>We can th<strong>in</strong>k of at least five advantages of do<strong>in</strong>g actionresearch. First, it can be done by almost any professional,<strong>in</strong> any type of school, at any grade level, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigatejust about any k<strong>in</strong>d of problem. It can be carried out byan <strong>in</strong>dividual teacher <strong>in</strong> his or her classroom. It can bedone by a group of teachers <strong>and</strong>/or parents, by a schoolpr<strong>in</strong>cipal or counselor, or by a school adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r atthe district level.Second, action research can improve educationalpractice. It helps teachers, counselors, <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rsbecome more competent professionals. Not only can ithelp them <strong>to</strong> become more competent <strong>and</strong> effective <strong>in</strong>what they do, but it can also help them be better able <strong>to</strong>underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> apply the research f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of others. Bydo<strong>in</strong>g action research themselves, teachers <strong>and</strong> other educationprofessionals not only can improve their skills,they can also improve their ability <strong>to</strong> read, <strong>in</strong>terpret, <strong>and</strong>critique more formal research when appropriate.Third, when teachers or other professionals design<strong>and</strong> carry out their own action research, they can developmore effective ways <strong>to</strong> practice their craft. Thiscan lead them <strong>to</strong> read formal research reports about similarpractices with greater underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g as <strong>to</strong> how theresults of such studies might apply <strong>to</strong> their own situations.More importantly, such research can serve as arich source of ideas about how <strong>to</strong> modify <strong>and</strong> perhapsenrich one’s own strategies <strong>and</strong> techniques.Fourth, action research can help teachers identifyproblems <strong>and</strong> issues systematically. Learn<strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> doaction research requires that <strong>in</strong>dividuals def<strong>in</strong>e a problemprecisely (often operationally), identify <strong>and</strong> try outalternative ways <strong>to</strong> deal with the problem, evaluatethese ways, <strong>and</strong> then share what they have learned withtheir peers. In effect, action research “shows practitionersthat it is possible <strong>to</strong> break out of the rut of <strong>in</strong>stitutionalized,taken-for-granted rout<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> develophope that seem<strong>in</strong>gly <strong>in</strong>tractable problems <strong>in</strong> the workplacecan be solved.” 5Fifth, action research can build up a small communityof research-oriented <strong>in</strong>dividuals with<strong>in</strong> the schoolitself. Action research, when systematically undertaken,can <strong>in</strong>volve several <strong>in</strong>dividuals work<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>gether <strong>to</strong>solve a problem or issue of mutual concern. This canhelp reduce the feel<strong>in</strong>g of isolation that many teachers,counselors, <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs experience as they goabout their daily tasks with<strong>in</strong> the school. One of the currentauthors, before becom<strong>in</strong>g a university professor,taught high school social studies. Dur<strong>in</strong>g his first yearof teach<strong>in</strong>g, he was assigned a class of particularly difficultstudents. Some of the other teachers <strong>in</strong> the schoolhad been work<strong>in</strong>g systematically as part of an action researchproject <strong>to</strong> test <strong>and</strong> evaluate various strategies fordeal<strong>in</strong>g with such students. They shared what they hadlearned (through their own action research). Their support<strong>and</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>formation proved <strong>in</strong>valuable <strong>to</strong> asomewhat overwhelmed beg<strong>in</strong>ner.Some Hypothetical Examplesof Practical Action <strong>Research</strong>Almost all of the methodologies described <strong>in</strong> the otherchapters <strong>in</strong> this book can be adapted (<strong>in</strong> a less formal<strong>and</strong> sophisticated form) by teachers <strong>and</strong> other educationprofessionals <strong>in</strong> the schools <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate problems <strong>and</strong>questions of <strong>in</strong>terest. Although we use school-based sett<strong>in</strong>gsfor the examples that follow, it takes only a littleimag<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>to</strong> conceptualize how action research canbe used elsewhere (e.g., mental health <strong>in</strong>stitutions, volunteerorganizations, community service agencies). Wenow present some examples of what could be done.INVESTIGATING THE TEACHINGOF SCIENCE CONCEPTS BY MEANSOF A COMPARISON-GROUP EXPERIMENTMs. Gonzales, a fifth-grade teacher, is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> thefollow<strong>in</strong>g question:Does us<strong>in</strong>g drama improve fifth-graders’ underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gof basic science concepts?<strong>How</strong> might Ms. Gonzales proceed?Although it could be <strong>in</strong>vestigated <strong>in</strong> a number ofways, this question lends itself particularly well <strong>to</strong> acomparison-group experiment (see Chapter 13).Ms. Gonzales could r<strong>and</strong>omly assign students <strong>to</strong> classes<strong>in</strong> which some teachers use dramatics <strong>and</strong> some teachersdo not. She could compare the effects of these contrast<strong>in</strong>gmethods by test<strong>in</strong>g the students <strong>in</strong> these classesat specified <strong>in</strong>tervals with an <strong>in</strong>strument designed <strong>to</strong>


MORE ABOUT RESEARCHAn Important Exampleof Action <strong>Research</strong>The early 1990s provide an example of effective participa<strong>to</strong>ryaction research. When a new powerhouse for theBonneville Dam was <strong>to</strong> be built <strong>in</strong> the center of the <strong>to</strong>wn ofNorth Bonneville, WA, all 470 residents faced eviction, relocation,<strong>and</strong> the probable demise of their <strong>to</strong>wn. Citizens ralliedaround the goal of relocat<strong>in</strong>g as an exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>wn where theychose. To do so, they had <strong>to</strong> oppose the U.S. Army Corps ofEng<strong>in</strong>eers. With help from faculty <strong>and</strong> students at the Universityof Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> Evergreen State College, a broadbasedcitizen group under<strong>to</strong>ok research <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>form themselves<strong>in</strong> detail about the assets <strong>and</strong> characteristics of their <strong>to</strong>wn aswell as the details of community plann<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> the politicalprocess. College students lived <strong>and</strong> worked <strong>in</strong> the <strong>to</strong>wn as theygathered data through documents, <strong>in</strong>formal discussions, <strong>and</strong>workshops accompanied by ongo<strong>in</strong>g feedback <strong>and</strong> discussionwith all sec<strong>to</strong>rs of the community. The <strong>to</strong>wn council providedf<strong>in</strong>ancial <strong>and</strong> logistical support. Citizens became <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly<strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out politicalaction. In the end, they not only atta<strong>in</strong>ed their goal but succeeded<strong>in</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g their design for their “new” <strong>to</strong>wn replace theone proposed by the Corps of Eng<strong>in</strong>eers.**F. Fischer (2000). Citizens, experts <strong>and</strong> the environment: Thepolitics of local knowledge. Durham <strong>and</strong> London: Duke UniversityPress, pp. 268–272.measure conceptual underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. The average scoreof the different classes on the test (the dependent variable)would give Ms. Gonzales some idea of the effectivenessof the methods be<strong>in</strong>g compared.Of course, Ms. Gonzales wants <strong>to</strong> have as much controlas possible over the assignment of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>to</strong> thevarious treatment groups. In most schools, the r<strong>and</strong>omassignment of students <strong>to</strong> treatment groups (classes)would be very difficult <strong>to</strong> accomplish. Should this bethe case, comparison still would be possible us<strong>in</strong>g aquasi-experimental design. Ms. Gonzales might, forexample, compare student achievement <strong>in</strong> two or more<strong>in</strong>tact classes <strong>in</strong> which some teachers agree <strong>to</strong> use thedrama approach. Because the students <strong>in</strong> these classeswould not have been assigned r<strong>and</strong>omly, the designcould not be considered a true experiment; but if the differencesbetween the classes <strong>in</strong> terms of what is be<strong>in</strong>gmeasured are quite large, <strong>and</strong> if students have beenmatched on pert<strong>in</strong>ent variables (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a pretest ofconceptual underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g), the results could still beuseful <strong>in</strong> show<strong>in</strong>g how the two methods compare.We would be concerned that the classes might differwith regard <strong>to</strong> important variables that could affect theoutcome of the study. If Ms. Gonzales is the data collec<strong>to</strong>r,she could un<strong>in</strong>tentionally favor one group when sheadm<strong>in</strong>isters the <strong>in</strong>strument(s).Ms. Gonzales should attempt <strong>to</strong> control for all extraneousvariables (student ability level, age, <strong>in</strong>structionaltime, teacher characteristics, <strong>and</strong> so on) that might affectthe outcome under <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Several control procedureswere described <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9: teach<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g thesame or closely connected periods of time, us<strong>in</strong>g equallyexperienced teachers for both methods, match<strong>in</strong>gstudents on ability <strong>and</strong> gender, hav<strong>in</strong>g someone elseadm<strong>in</strong>ister the <strong>in</strong>strument(s), <strong>and</strong> so forth.Ms. Gonzales might decide <strong>to</strong> use the causalcomparativemethod if some classes are already be<strong>in</strong>gtaught by teachers us<strong>in</strong>g the drama approach.STUDYING THE EFFECTS OF TIME-OUTON A STUDENT’S DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR BYMEANS OF A SINGLE-SUBJECT EXPERIMENTMs. Wong, a third-grade teacher, f<strong>in</strong>ds her class cont<strong>in</strong>ually<strong>in</strong>terrupted by a student who can’t seem <strong>to</strong> keepquiet. Distressed, she asks herself what she can do <strong>to</strong>control this student <strong>and</strong> wonders if some k<strong>in</strong>d of timeoutactivity might work. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, she asks:Would brief periods of removal from the classdecrease the frequency of this student’s disruptivebehavior?What might Ms. Wong do <strong>to</strong> get an answer <strong>to</strong> herquestion?This sort of question can best be answered by meansof a s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject A-B-A-B design (see Chapter 14).First, Ms. Wong needs <strong>to</strong> establish a basel<strong>in</strong>e of the student’sdisruptive behavior. Hence, she should observethe student carefully over a period of several days, chart<strong>in</strong>gthe frequency of the disruptive behavior. Once shehas recognized a stable pattern <strong>in</strong> the student’s behavior,she should <strong>in</strong>troduce the treatment—<strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>stance,time-out, or plac<strong>in</strong>g the student outside the classroom fora brief period of time—for several days <strong>and</strong> observe597


598 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ethe frequency of the student’s disruptive behavior afterthe treatment periods. She then should repeat the cycle.Ideally, the student’s disruptive behavior will decrease<strong>and</strong> Ms. Wong will no longer need <strong>to</strong> use a time-outperiod with this student.The ma<strong>in</strong> problem for Ms. Wong is be<strong>in</strong>g able <strong>to</strong> observe<strong>and</strong> chart the student’s behavior dur<strong>in</strong>g the timeoutperiod <strong>and</strong> still teach the other students <strong>in</strong> her class.She may also have difficulty mak<strong>in</strong>g sure the treatment(time-out) works as <strong>in</strong>tended (e.g., that the student is notw<strong>and</strong>er<strong>in</strong>g the halls). Both of these problems would begreatly dim<strong>in</strong>ished if she had a teacher’s aide <strong>to</strong> assistwith these concerns.DETERMINING WHAT STUDENTS LIKEABOUT SCHOOL BY MEANS OF A SURVEYMr. Abramson, a high school guidance counselor, is not<strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional methods. He is <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> how students feel about school <strong>in</strong> general.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, he asks the follow<strong>in</strong>g questions:What do students like about their classes? What dothey dislike? Why?What types of subjects do they like the best or least?<strong>How</strong> do the feel<strong>in</strong>gs of students of different ages,sexes, <strong>and</strong> ethnicities <strong>in</strong> our school compare?What might Mr. Abramson do <strong>to</strong> get some answers?These sorts of questions can best be answered by asurvey that measures student attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward theirclasses (see Chapter 17). Mr. Abramson will need <strong>to</strong>prepare a questionnaire, tak<strong>in</strong>g time <strong>to</strong> ensure that thequestions are directed <strong>to</strong>ward the <strong>in</strong>formation he wants<strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong>. Next, he should have some other members ofthe faculty look over the questions <strong>and</strong> identify any theyfeel will be mislead<strong>in</strong>g or ambiguous.There are two difficulties <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> such a survey.First, Mr. Abramson must ensure that the questions areclear <strong>and</strong> not mislead<strong>in</strong>g. He can accomplish this, <strong>to</strong> afair extent, by us<strong>in</strong>g objective or closed-ended questions,ensur<strong>in</strong>g that they all perta<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>pic under <strong>in</strong>vestigation,<strong>and</strong> then further elim<strong>in</strong>at<strong>in</strong>g ambiguity by pilottest<strong>in</strong>ga draft of the questionnaire with a small group ofstudents. Second, Mr. Abramson must be sure that a sufficientnumber of questionnaires are completed <strong>and</strong> returnedso that he can make mean<strong>in</strong>gful analyses. He canimprove the rate of return by giv<strong>in</strong>g the questionnaire <strong>to</strong>students <strong>to</strong> complete when they are all <strong>in</strong> one place.Once he collects the completed questionnaires, heshould tally the responses <strong>and</strong> see what he’s got.The big advantage of questionnaire research is that ithas the potential <strong>to</strong> provide a lot of <strong>in</strong>formation from quitea large sample of <strong>in</strong>dividuals. If more details about particularquestions are desired, Mr. Abramson can also conductpersonal <strong>in</strong>terviews with students. As we have mentionedbefore, the advantage here is that Mr. Abramsoncan ask open-ended questions (those giv<strong>in</strong>g the respondentmaximum freedom of response) with more confidence<strong>and</strong> pursue particular questions of special <strong>in</strong>teres<strong>to</strong>r value <strong>in</strong> depth. He would also be able <strong>to</strong> ask followupquestions <strong>and</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> any items that students f<strong>in</strong>dunclear.One problem here may be that some students maynot underst<strong>and</strong> the questions, or they may not returntheir questionnaire. Mr. Abramson has an advantageover many survey researchers <strong>in</strong> that he can probablyensure a high rate of return by adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g his questionnairedirectly <strong>to</strong> students <strong>in</strong> their classrooms. Hemust be careful <strong>to</strong> give directions that facilitate honest<strong>and</strong> serious answers <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> ensure the anonymity ofthe respondents. Although it is difficult, he also shouldtry <strong>to</strong> get data on both reliability (perhaps by giv<strong>in</strong>gthe questionnaire <strong>to</strong> a subsample a second time after anappropriate time <strong>in</strong>terval—say, two weeks) <strong>and</strong> validity(perhaps by select<strong>in</strong>g a subsample <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviewimmediately after they <strong>in</strong>dividually fill out the questionnaire).Check<strong>in</strong>g reliability <strong>and</strong> validity requiressacrific<strong>in</strong>g anonymity for those students <strong>in</strong> thesubsample, s<strong>in</strong>ce he must be able <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>in</strong>dividualquestionnaires.CHECKING FOR BIASIN ENGLISH ANTHOLOGIESBY MEANS OF A CONTENT ANALYSISMs. Hallowitz, an eighth-grade English teacher, is concernedabout the accuracy of the images or concepts thatare presented <strong>to</strong> her students <strong>in</strong> their literature anthologies.She asks the follow<strong>in</strong>g questions.Is the content presented <strong>in</strong> the literature anthologies<strong>in</strong> our district biased <strong>in</strong> any way? If so, how?What might Ms. Hallowitz do <strong>to</strong> get answers?To <strong>in</strong>vestigate these questions, content analysis iscalled for (see Chapter 20). Ms. Hallowitz decides <strong>to</strong>look particularly at the images of heroes that are presented<strong>in</strong> the literature anthologies used <strong>in</strong> the district.First, she needs <strong>to</strong> select the sample of anthologies <strong>to</strong>be analyzed—that is, <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e which texts she willperuse. (She restricts herself <strong>to</strong> only the current texts


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 599available for use <strong>in</strong> the district.) She then needs <strong>to</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k about the specific categories she wants <strong>to</strong> lookat. Let us assume she decides <strong>to</strong> analyze the physical,emotional, social, <strong>and</strong> mental characteristics of heroesthat are presented. She could then break these categoriesdown <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> smaller cod<strong>in</strong>g units such as thoseshown below.Physical Emotional Social MentalWeight Friendly Ethnicity WiseHeight Aloof Dress FunnyAge Hostile Occupation IntelligentBody type Un<strong>in</strong>volved Status Superhuman. . . .. . . .. . . .Ms. Hallowitz can prepare a cod<strong>in</strong>g sheet <strong>to</strong> tallythe data <strong>in</strong> each of the categories that she identifies <strong>in</strong>each anthology she studies. She can also readily compareamong categories <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e, for example,whether white men are portrayed as white-collarworkers <strong>and</strong> nonwhite people are portrayed as bluecollarworkers.A major advantage of content analysis is that it is unobtrusive.Ms. Hallowitz can “observe” without be<strong>in</strong>gobserved, s<strong>in</strong>ce the contents be<strong>in</strong>g analyzed are not <strong>in</strong>fluencedby her presence. Information that she mightf<strong>in</strong>d difficult or even impossible <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> through direc<strong>to</strong>bservation or other means can be ga<strong>in</strong>ed through acontent analysis of the sort sketched above.A second advantage is that content analysis is fairlyeasy for others <strong>to</strong> replicate. Lastly, the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>edthrough content analysis can be very helpful <strong>in</strong>plann<strong>in</strong>g for further <strong>in</strong>struction. Data of the type soughtby Ms. Hallowitz can suggest additional <strong>in</strong>formationthat may help students <strong>to</strong> ga<strong>in</strong> a more accurate <strong>and</strong> completepicture of the world they live <strong>in</strong>, the fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong>forces that exist with<strong>in</strong> it, <strong>and</strong> how these fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong>forces imp<strong>in</strong>ge on people’s lives.Ms. Hallowitz’s major problem lies <strong>in</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g able <strong>to</strong>specify clearly the categories that will suit her questions.If, for example, nonwhite males are less often portrayedas professionals, does this <strong>in</strong>dicate bias <strong>in</strong> the materials,or does it reflect reality (or both)? She should try <strong>to</strong> identifyall the anthologies be<strong>in</strong>g used <strong>in</strong> her district <strong>and</strong> theneither analyze each one or select a r<strong>and</strong>om sample.Ms. Hallowitz could, of course, survey teacher <strong>and</strong>/orstudent op<strong>in</strong>ions about bias, but that would answer adifferent question.PREDICTING WHICH KINDS OF STUDENTSARE LIKELY TO HAVE TROUBLELEARNING ALGEBRA BY MEANSOF A CORRELATIONAL STUDYLet’s turn <strong>to</strong> mathematics for our next example.Mr. Thompson, an algebra teacher, is bothered by the factthat some of his students have difficulty learn<strong>in</strong>g algebrawhile other students learn it with ease.As a result, he asks:<strong>How</strong> can I predict which sorts of <strong>in</strong>dividuals are likely<strong>to</strong> have trouble learn<strong>in</strong>g algebra?What might Mr. Thompson do <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate thisquestion?If Mr. Thompson could make fairly accurate predictions<strong>in</strong> this regard, he might be able <strong>to</strong> suggest some correctivemeasures that he or other teachers could use <strong>to</strong> helpstudents so that large numbers of “math-haters” are notproduced. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, correlational analysis would beappropriate (see Chapter 15). Mr. Thompson could use avariety of measures <strong>to</strong> collect different sorts of data on hisstudents: their performance on a number of “read<strong>in</strong>ess”tasks related <strong>to</strong> algebra learn<strong>in</strong>g (e.g., calculat<strong>in</strong>g, s<strong>to</strong>ryproblems); other variables that might be related <strong>to</strong> success<strong>in</strong> algebra (anxiety about math, critical th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g ability);familiarity with specific concepts (“constant,” “variable,”“distributed”); <strong>and</strong> any other variables that might conceivablypo<strong>in</strong>t out how those students who do better <strong>in</strong> algebradiffer from those who do more poorly.The <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed from such research canhelp Mr. Thompson predict more accurately which studentswill have learn<strong>in</strong>g difficulties <strong>in</strong> algebra <strong>and</strong>should suggest some techniques <strong>to</strong> help students learn.The ma<strong>in</strong> problem for Mr. Thompson is likely <strong>to</strong> begett<strong>in</strong>g adequate measurements on the different variableshe wishes <strong>to</strong> study. Some <strong>in</strong>formation should beavailable from school records; other variables will probablyrequire special <strong>in</strong>strumentation. (He must rememberthat this <strong>in</strong>formation must apply <strong>to</strong> students beforethey take the algebra class, not dur<strong>in</strong>g or after.)Mr. Thompson must, of course, have an adequatelyreliable <strong>and</strong> valid way <strong>to</strong> measure proficiency <strong>in</strong> algebra.He must also try <strong>to</strong> avoid <strong>in</strong>complete data (i.e.,miss<strong>in</strong>g scores for some students on some measures).COMPARING TWO WAYS OF TEACHINGCHEMISTRY BY MEANS OF A CAUSAL-COMPARATIVE STUDYMs. Perea, a first-year chemistry teacher, is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong>discover<strong>in</strong>g whether students <strong>in</strong> past classes achieved


RESEARCH TIPSTh<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> ConsiderWhen Do<strong>in</strong>g In-School <strong>Research</strong>Check the clarity of purpose <strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>itions with others.Give attention <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> describ<strong>in</strong>g your sample <strong>in</strong>a way that is clear <strong>to</strong> others <strong>and</strong>, it is hoped, permits generalizationof results.If appropriate, use exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struments; if it is necessary <strong>to</strong>develop your own, remember the guidel<strong>in</strong>es presented <strong>in</strong>Chapter 7.Make an effort <strong>to</strong> check the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of yourmeasures.Give thought <strong>to</strong> each of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. Takesteps <strong>to</strong> reduce these threats as much as possible.Use statistics where appropriate <strong>to</strong> clarify data. Use<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics only when justified—or as roughguides.Be clear about the population <strong>to</strong> which you are entitled <strong>to</strong>generalize. It may be only those you actually <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong>your study (i.e., your sample). It may be that you canprovide a rationale for broader generalization.more <strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> felt better about chemistry when they weretaught by a teacher who used “<strong>in</strong>quiry science” materials.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, she asks the follow<strong>in</strong>g question:<strong>How</strong> has the achievement of those students who havebeen taught with <strong>in</strong>quiry science materials comparedwith that of students who have been taught with traditionalmaterials?What might Ms. Perea do <strong>to</strong> get some answers <strong>to</strong> herquestion?If this question were <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated experimentally,two groups of students would have <strong>to</strong> be formed <strong>and</strong> theneach group taught differently by the teachers <strong>in</strong>volved(one teacher us<strong>in</strong>g a st<strong>and</strong>ard text, let’s say, <strong>and</strong> the otherus<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>quiry-oriented materials). The achievement<strong>and</strong> attitude of the two groups could then be compared bymeans of one or more assessment devices.To test this question us<strong>in</strong>g a causal-comparativedesign (see Chapter 16), however, Ms. Perea must f<strong>in</strong>da group of students who already have been exposed <strong>to</strong>the <strong>in</strong>quiry science materials <strong>and</strong> then compare theirachievement with that of another group taught with thest<strong>and</strong>ard text. Do the two groups differ <strong>in</strong> their achievement<strong>and</strong> attitude <strong>to</strong>ward chemistry? Suppose they do.Can Ms. Perea then conclude with confidence that thedifference <strong>in</strong> materials produced the difference <strong>in</strong>achievement <strong>and</strong>/or attitude? Alas, no, for other variablesmay be the cause. To the extent that she can ruleout such alternative explanations, she can have someconfidence that the <strong>in</strong>quiry materials are at least onefac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong> caus<strong>in</strong>g the difference between groups.Ms. Perea’s ma<strong>in</strong> problems are <strong>in</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g a goodmeasure of achievement <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g extraneousvariables. The latter is likely <strong>to</strong> be difficult, s<strong>in</strong>ce sheneeds <strong>to</strong> have access <strong>to</strong> prior classes <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> get therelevant <strong>in</strong>formation (such as student ability <strong>and</strong> teacherexperience). She might locate classes that were as similaras possible with regard <strong>to</strong> extraneous variables thatmight affect results.Unless she has a special reason for want<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> studyprevious classes, Ms. Perea might be advised <strong>to</strong> comparemethods that are be<strong>in</strong>g used currently. She might be able<strong>to</strong> use a quasi-experimental approach (by assign<strong>in</strong>gteachers <strong>to</strong> methods <strong>and</strong> controll<strong>in</strong>g the way <strong>in</strong> which themethods are carried out; see Chapter 13). If not, hercausal-comparative approach would permit easier controlof extraneous variables if current classes were used.FINDING OUTHOW MUSIC TEACHERS TEACHBY MEANS OF AN ETHNOGRAPHIC STUDYMr. Adams, the direc<strong>to</strong>r of curriculum <strong>in</strong> an elementaryschool district, is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> know<strong>in</strong>g more about howthe district’s music teachers teach their subject. Accord<strong>in</strong>gly,he asks:What do our music teachers do as they go about theirdaily rout<strong>in</strong>e—<strong>in</strong> what k<strong>in</strong>ds of activities do theyengage?What are the explicit <strong>and</strong> implicit rules of the game<strong>in</strong> music classes that seem <strong>to</strong> help or h<strong>in</strong>der theprocess of learn<strong>in</strong>g?What can Mr. Adams do <strong>to</strong> get some answers?To ga<strong>in</strong> some <strong>in</strong>sight <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> these questions, Mr. Adamscould choose <strong>to</strong> carry out an ethnography (see Chapter21). He could try <strong>to</strong> document or portray the activitiesthat go on <strong>in</strong> a music teacher’s classes as the teacher goesabout his or her daily rout<strong>in</strong>e. Ideally, Mr. Adams shouldfocus on only one classroom (or a small number of them at600


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 601most) <strong>and</strong> plan <strong>to</strong> observe the teacher <strong>and</strong> students <strong>in</strong> thatclassroom on as regular a basis as possible (perhaps oncea week for one semester). He should attempt <strong>to</strong> describe,as fully <strong>and</strong> as richly as possible, what he sees go<strong>in</strong>g on.The data <strong>to</strong> be collected might <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong>terviews withthe teacher <strong>and</strong> students, detailed prose descriptions ofclassroom rout<strong>in</strong>es, audiotapes of teacher-student conferences,videotapes of classroom discussions, examples ofteacher lesson plans <strong>and</strong> student work, <strong>and</strong> flowchartsthat illustrate the direction <strong>and</strong> frequency of certa<strong>in</strong> typesof comments (e.g., the k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions that teacher <strong>and</strong>students ask of one another <strong>and</strong> the responses that differentk<strong>in</strong>ds of questions produce).Ethnographic research can lend itself well <strong>to</strong> a detailedstudy of <strong>in</strong>dividuals as well as classrooms. Sometimesmuch can be learned from study<strong>in</strong>g just one <strong>in</strong>dividual.For example, some students learn how <strong>to</strong> play amusical <strong>in</strong>strument very easily. In hopes of ga<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sight<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> why this is the case, Mr. Adams might observe<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview one such student on a regular basis <strong>to</strong> seeif there are any noticeable patterns or regularities <strong>in</strong> thestudent’s behavior. Teachers <strong>and</strong> counselors, as well asthe student, might be <strong>in</strong>terviewed <strong>in</strong> depth. Mr. Adamsmight also conduct a similar series of observations <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terviews with a student who f<strong>in</strong>ds learn<strong>in</strong>g how <strong>to</strong> playan <strong>in</strong>strument very difficult, <strong>to</strong> see what differences canbe identified. As <strong>in</strong> the study of a whole classroom, asmuch <strong>in</strong>formation as possible (study style, attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward music, approach <strong>to</strong> the subject, behavior <strong>in</strong> class)would be collected. The hope here is that through thestudy of an <strong>in</strong>dividual, <strong>in</strong>sights can be ga<strong>in</strong>ed that willhelp the teacher with similar students <strong>in</strong> the future.In short, then, Mr. Adams’s goal should be <strong>to</strong> “pa<strong>in</strong>t aportrait” of a music classroom (or an <strong>in</strong>dividual teacheror student <strong>in</strong> such a classroom) <strong>in</strong> as thorough <strong>and</strong> accuratea manner as possible so that others can also “see”that classroom <strong>and</strong> its participants, <strong>and</strong> what they do.**Although it may appear that ethnographic research is relatively easy<strong>to</strong> do, it is, <strong>in</strong> fact, extremely difficult <strong>to</strong> do well. If you wish <strong>to</strong> learnmore about this method, consult one or more of the follow<strong>in</strong>g references:H. B. Bernard (2000). Social research methods. Thous<strong>and</strong>Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; J. P. Goetz <strong>and</strong> M. D. LeCompte(1993). Ethnography <strong>and</strong> qualitative design <strong>in</strong> educational research,2nd ed. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; Y. S. L<strong>in</strong>coln <strong>and</strong> E. G.Guba (1985). Naturalistic <strong>in</strong>quiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications;D. F. Lancy (2001). Study<strong>in</strong>g children <strong>and</strong> schools: Qualitativeresearch traditions. Prospect Heights, IL: Wavel<strong>and</strong> Press;D. M. Fetterman (1989). Ethnography: Step by step. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks,CA: Sage; R. C. Bogdan <strong>and</strong> S. K. Biklen (2007). Qualitativeresearch <strong>in</strong> education: An <strong>in</strong>troduction <strong>to</strong> theory <strong>and</strong> methods,5th ed. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon.One of the difficulties <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g an ethnographicstudy is that relatively little advice can be givenbeforeh<strong>and</strong>. The primary pitfall is allow<strong>in</strong>g personalviews <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence the <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> its<strong>in</strong>terpretation.Mr. Adams could elect <strong>to</strong> use a more structuredobservation system <strong>and</strong> a structured <strong>in</strong>terview. Thiswould reduce the subjectivity of his data, but it mightalso detract from the richness of what he reports. Webelieve that an ethnographic study should be done onlyunder the guidance of someone with prior tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>experience <strong>in</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g this methodology.An Example of Action<strong>Research</strong>At this po<strong>in</strong>t, we want <strong>to</strong> present a real-life example ofhow even one of the most difficult types of research <strong>to</strong>do <strong>in</strong> schools (a quasi-experiment) can be carried out <strong>in</strong>the context of ongo<strong>in</strong>g school activities <strong>and</strong> responsibilities.The follow<strong>in</strong>g study was done by one of ourstudents, Darlene DeMaria, <strong>in</strong> her special class forlearn<strong>in</strong>g-disabled students <strong>in</strong> a public elementary schoolnear San Francisco, California. 6 Ms. DeMaria hypothesizedthat male learn<strong>in</strong>g-disabled students <strong>in</strong> elementaryschools who receive a systematic program of relaxationexercises would show a greater reduction <strong>in</strong> off-taskbehaviors than students who do not receive such aprogram of exercise.Us<strong>in</strong>g an adaptation of an exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument,Ms. DeMaria selected 25 items (behaviors) from a 60-item scale previously designed <strong>to</strong> assess attentiondeficit. The 25 items selected were those most directlyrelated <strong>to</strong> off-task behavior. Each item was rated from 0<strong>to</strong> 4 on the basis of prior observation of the student, witha rat<strong>in</strong>g of 0 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that the behavior had never beenobserved <strong>and</strong> a rat<strong>in</strong>g of 4 <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that the behaviorhad been observed so frequently as <strong>to</strong> seriously <strong>in</strong>terferewith learn<strong>in</strong>g.Three weeks after school began, Ms. DeMaria <strong>and</strong>her aide <strong>in</strong>dependently filled out the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale foreach of the 18 students. The scores provided the basisfor assess<strong>in</strong>g improvement <strong>and</strong> for match<strong>in</strong>g two groupsprior <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention.Because the students were assigned <strong>to</strong> the “resourceroom” (where Ms. DeMaria taught) approximately onehour a day <strong>in</strong> groups of two <strong>to</strong> four <strong>and</strong> their scheduleshad been set previously, r<strong>and</strong>om assignment was not


602 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ePhase IPhase IIGroup I O M X 1 O X 1 OGroup II O M O X 1 OFigure 24.5 Experimental <strong>Design</strong> for theDeMaria Studypossible. It was, however, possible <strong>to</strong> match studentsacross groups on grade level <strong>and</strong> (roughly) on <strong>in</strong>itial rat<strong>in</strong>gsof off-task behavior. The class <strong>in</strong>cluded students <strong>in</strong>grades 1 through 6. Students selected <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong> the experimentalgroup received the relaxation program on adaily basis for four weeks (Phase I), after which bothMs. DeMaria <strong>and</strong> her aide aga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dependently rated all18 students. Comparison of the groups at this time providedthe first test of the hypothesis. Next, the relaxationprogram was cont<strong>in</strong>ued for the orig<strong>in</strong>al experimentalgroup <strong>and</strong> begun for the comparison group foranother four weeks (Phase II), permitt<strong>in</strong>g additionalcomparison of groups <strong>and</strong> resolv<strong>in</strong>g the ethical questionof exclud<strong>in</strong>g one group from a potentially beneficialexperience. At the end of this time, all students wereaga<strong>in</strong> rated <strong>in</strong>dependently by Ms. DeMaria <strong>and</strong> her aide.The experimental design is shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 24.5.The results showed that after Phase I, the experimentalgroup showed deterioration (more off-task behavior—contrary <strong>to</strong> the hypothesis), whereas the comparisongroup showed little change. At the end of Phase II, thescores for both groups rema<strong>in</strong>ed about the same as at theend of Phase I. Further analysis of the various subgroups(each <strong>in</strong>structed dur<strong>in</strong>g a different time period) showedlittle change <strong>in</strong> the groups that received only four weeksof tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Of the three subgroups that received eightweeks of tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, two showed a substantial decrease <strong>in</strong>off-task behavior <strong>and</strong> one showed a marked <strong>in</strong>crease. Theexplanation for the latter appears clear. One student whowas placed <strong>in</strong> the resource-room program just prior <strong>to</strong> theonset of relaxation tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g had an <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly disruptiveeffect on the other members of his subgroup, an <strong>in</strong>fluencethat the tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g was not powerful enough <strong>to</strong> counteract.This study demonstrates how research on importantquestions can be conducted <strong>in</strong> real-life situations<strong>in</strong> schools <strong>and</strong> can lead <strong>to</strong> useful, although tentative,implications for practice.Like any study, this one has several limitations. Thefirst is that agreement between Ms. DeMaria <strong>and</strong> heraide on the pretest was <strong>in</strong>sufficient <strong>and</strong> required furtherdiscussion <strong>and</strong> reconciliation of differences, thus mak<strong>in</strong>gthe pretest scores somewhat suspect. Agreement,however, was satisfac<strong>to</strong>ry (an r above .80) for theposttests.A second limitation is that the comparison groupscould not be precisely matched on the pretest, s<strong>in</strong>ce thecontrol group had more students at both extremes.Although neither group <strong>in</strong>itially showed more offtaskbehavior overall, this difference, as well as otheruncontrolled differences <strong>in</strong> subject characteristics, couldconceivably expla<strong>in</strong> the different outcomes for thetwo groups. Further, the fact that the implementer(Ms. DeMaria) was one of the raters could certa<strong>in</strong>ly have<strong>in</strong>fluenced the rat<strong>in</strong>gs. That this did not happen issuggested by the fact that Ms. DeMaria’s Phase II scoresfor the orig<strong>in</strong>al experimental group were <strong>in</strong> fact higher(contrary <strong>to</strong> her hypothesis) than <strong>in</strong> Phase I. Evidence ofretest reliability of scores could not be obta<strong>in</strong>ed dur<strong>in</strong>gthe time available for the study. Evidence for validityrests on the agreement between <strong>in</strong>dependent judges.Generalization beyond this one group of students <strong>and</strong>one teacher (Ms. DeMaria) clearly is not justified. Theanalysis of subgroups, although enlighten<strong>in</strong>g, is after thefact, <strong>and</strong> hence the results are highly tentative.Despite these limitations, the study does suggest thatthe relaxation program may have value for at least somestudents if it is carried out long enough. One or moreother teachers should be encouraged <strong>to</strong> replicate thestudy. An additional benefit was that the study clarified,for the teacher, the dynamics of each of the subgroups <strong>in</strong>her class.Classroom teachers <strong>and</strong> other professionals can (<strong>and</strong>should, we would argue) conduct studies like the one wehave summarized. As mentioned earlier, there is much<strong>in</strong> education about which we know little. Many questionsrema<strong>in</strong> unanswered; much <strong>in</strong>formation is needed.Classroom teachers, counselors, <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>rs canhelp <strong>to</strong> provide this <strong>in</strong>formation. We hope you will beone of those who do.A Published Exampleof Action <strong>Research</strong>To conclude this chapter, we present a published exampleof action research, followed by a critique of itsstrengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses. As we did <strong>in</strong> our critiques ofother types of research studies, we use concepts <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>in</strong> earlier parts of the book <strong>in</strong> our analysis.


RESEARCH REPORTFrom: Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Childhood <strong>Education</strong>, 14, no. 2 (2000): 232–238. Copyright 2000 by the Associationfor Childhood <strong>Education</strong> International.What Do You Mean“Th<strong>in</strong>k Before I Act”?:Conflict Resolution with ChoicesLonisa Brown<strong>in</strong>gBarbara DavisVirg<strong>in</strong>ia RestaSouthwest Texas State University, San MarcosAbstract.Twenty 1st-grade students participated <strong>in</strong> an eight-week action research study. Studentsparticipated <strong>in</strong> class meet<strong>in</strong>gs where they discussed problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g solutions, particularlypositive forms of conflict resolution. The “Wheel of Choice” provided the studentswith the choices for solutions that <strong>in</strong>cluded 1) make an apology, 2) tell the other person<strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p, 3) walk away, <strong>and</strong> 4) give an “I” message. Data was collected through 1) studentpre- <strong>and</strong> post-surveys, 2) student conflict-resolution journals, 3) behavior tally sheets, 4)observational notes, <strong>and</strong> 5) teacher reflective journal. Pre- <strong>and</strong> post-surveys <strong>in</strong>dicatedthat the students developed more positive strategies for solv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts. The studentconflict-resolution journals demonstrated that students were able <strong>to</strong> write down positiveforms of conflict resolution after th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about the problem. Analysis of the tally sheets<strong>in</strong>dicated that physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression decreased from the first week (22 <strong>in</strong>cidents)<strong>to</strong> the eighth week (4 <strong>in</strong>cidents) of the study. The teacher reflection journal <strong>and</strong>observational notes also documented that students used the “Wheel of Choice” whengiven the opportunity <strong>and</strong> time <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about the problem.“S<strong>to</strong>p laugh<strong>in</strong>g at me,” yelled Michael (all names are pseudonyms) as he shoved a classmatewith full force <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the bullet<strong>in</strong> board. His forehead was creased with l<strong>in</strong>es, hiseyes glared, <strong>and</strong> his mouth was one straight l<strong>in</strong>e. Michael’s clenched fists <strong>and</strong> stiff bodyshowed me (the first author) that he was not <strong>in</strong> the mood <strong>to</strong> talk about the problem.Dur<strong>in</strong>g my first year of teach<strong>in</strong>g 1st grade, I confronted <strong>in</strong>cidents like these on a dailybasis. My students did not seem <strong>to</strong> have the necessary skills <strong>to</strong> successfully solve conflictswith their peers. Like Michael, their first reaction was usually <strong>to</strong> hit, shove, push,<strong>and</strong>/or yell.As a teacher, I wanted <strong>to</strong> guide my students <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> solv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts productively.Most of my students witnessed verbal <strong>and</strong> physical aggression <strong>in</strong> their neighborhoods<strong>and</strong> homes, so they emulated that model as solutions <strong>to</strong> their problems. Therefore, help<strong>in</strong>gmy students learn positive methods of resolv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts became the focus of an actionresearch project I conducted <strong>in</strong> my classroom dur<strong>in</strong>g that first year of teach<strong>in</strong>g.The students <strong>in</strong> my 1st-grade class live <strong>in</strong> a world where violence, both gang <strong>and</strong>domestic, is commonplace. The majority of the students come from low-<strong>in</strong>come, m<strong>in</strong>orityfamilies. Many of them live <strong>in</strong> multi-family homes. They frequently move from home<strong>to</strong> home. These children act out what they see <strong>in</strong> their environment (e.g., hitt<strong>in</strong>g, yell<strong>in</strong>g,fight<strong>in</strong>g). Each week, I had <strong>to</strong> complete numerous referrals <strong>to</strong> the office for children whosolved conflicts <strong>in</strong> physically or verbally aggressive ways.Verbal onlySample description603


604 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>Research</strong> questionPurposePrior researchPrior researchPrior researchThe research question I developed <strong>to</strong> focus my <strong>in</strong>vestigation was, “Will physically<strong>and</strong> verbally aggressive students solve conflicts <strong>in</strong> a constructive manner if providedmodels of pro-social alternative solutions?” I wanted my young students <strong>to</strong> 1) decreasethe use of physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression <strong>to</strong> solve conflicts, 2) <strong>in</strong>crease the use of positivesolutions, <strong>and</strong> 3) be able <strong>to</strong> verbalize the problem, their feel<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> a constructivesolution. Ultimately, I wanted the children <strong>to</strong> become prepared for society-at-large bylearn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g constructive forms of conflict management.Numerous researchers have studied the effects of pro-social behavior <strong>and</strong> socialskills on young children. Children are very egocentric (i.e., they see the world from theirown perspective). They focus on an aspect of a situation that is important <strong>to</strong> them (Bailey,1994). Adults play an important role <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g children develop pro-social attitudes<strong>and</strong> behaviors. Children live what they learn (Nelsen, Lott, & Glenn, 1997; Wittmer &Honig, 1994). A major reason that children fight is that the adults <strong>in</strong> their lives have nottaught them other options for resolv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts (Nelsen, Duffy, Escobar, Or<strong>to</strong>lano, &Owen-Sohocki, 1996). Adults need <strong>to</strong> teach children <strong>to</strong> express their feel<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>others’ feel<strong>in</strong>gs. Children also need <strong>to</strong> be able <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k through a problem stepby-step<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> develop alternative solutions. If teachers take time <strong>to</strong> encourage, facilitate,<strong>and</strong> teach pro-social behaviors, children’s pro-social <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>and</strong>aggression decreases (Wittmer & Honig, 1994).Bailey (1994) states that children need <strong>to</strong> take responsibility for conflict resolution,which turns the problem over <strong>to</strong> the students a little at a time. Students develop problemsolv<strong>in</strong>gskills by f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g resolution ideas that work (Bailey, 1994; Sloane, 1998). Studentsbecome effective problem-solvers when teachers take the time <strong>to</strong> tra<strong>in</strong> students <strong>in</strong>problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> give them opportunities <strong>to</strong> practice these skills (Nelsen, Lott, & Glenn,1997). When teacher <strong>in</strong>tervention is not necessary, students should be able <strong>to</strong> follow theconflict resolution steps, which <strong>in</strong>clude: 1) def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the problem, 2) generat<strong>in</strong>g solutions, 3)reach<strong>in</strong>g agreements, 4) implement<strong>in</strong>g solutions, <strong>and</strong> 5) evaluat<strong>in</strong>g whether or not theproblem was solved (Sloane, 1998). Us<strong>in</strong>g these problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g steps <strong>and</strong> tak<strong>in</strong>g responsibilityfor their actions can help students learn <strong>to</strong> listen, underst<strong>and</strong> other perspectives, recognizeproblems, <strong>and</strong> look for alternative solutions (Bailey, 1994).Class meet<strong>in</strong>gs, as def<strong>in</strong>ed by Nelsen, Lott, <strong>and</strong> Glenn <strong>in</strong> Positive Discipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> theClassroom (1997), provide a designated time (e.g., 20 <strong>to</strong> 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes) dur<strong>in</strong>g the schoolday when students form a circle <strong>to</strong> discuss classroom issues <strong>and</strong> problems. Class meet<strong>in</strong>gsprovide an opportunity for students <strong>to</strong> practice us<strong>in</strong>g pro-social skills by mak<strong>in</strong>g decisions<strong>and</strong> solv<strong>in</strong>g problems <strong>to</strong>gether. They also help students develop a sense of value <strong>and</strong>belong<strong>in</strong>g by allow<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>to</strong> share concerns, anxieties, frustration, <strong>and</strong> celebrations.Through class meet<strong>in</strong>gs, students are able <strong>to</strong> express concerns <strong>and</strong> solve conflicts <strong>in</strong> anon-threaten<strong>in</strong>g environment. By develop<strong>in</strong>g solutions that are related, respectful, <strong>and</strong>reasonable, students learn skills that enable them <strong>to</strong> solve problems when they occur.The goal of class meet<strong>in</strong>gs is <strong>to</strong> help students learn skills they can use every day of theirlives (McEwan, Gathercoal, Donahue, Greenfield, & Strangio, 1998; Nelsen, Lott, &Glenn, 1997).METHODS AND PROCEDURESSampleThis action research project was conducted <strong>in</strong> my 1st-grade classroom at an elementaryschool located <strong>in</strong> the southwestern United States. N<strong>in</strong>ety-five percent of the studentsat this prek<strong>in</strong>dergarten through 5th-grade campus represent m<strong>in</strong>ority populations; themajority come from low-socioeconomic backgrounds. My 1st-grade class consisted of 20students (12 boys <strong>and</strong> 8 girls). One-fourth of the class was African American, approximately


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 605three-fourths were Hispanic, <strong>and</strong> one child was Caucasian. This eight-week project wasimplemented dur<strong>in</strong>g the second semester of the school year.At the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the school year, I <strong>in</strong>troduced the class meet<strong>in</strong>g format outl<strong>in</strong>edby Nelsen et al. (1997). The format consists of 1) form<strong>in</strong>g a circle, 2) giv<strong>in</strong>g compliments<strong>and</strong> appreciations, 3) follow<strong>in</strong>g up on prior solutions, 4) discuss<strong>in</strong>g agenda items(e.g., shar<strong>in</strong>g problems/concerns, problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g solutions), <strong>and</strong> 5) discuss<strong>in</strong>g futureplans (e.g., field trips, parties, projects).One aspect of Nelsen’s “Positive Discipl<strong>in</strong>e” procedures that I <strong>in</strong>troduced <strong>to</strong> my studentswas the use of the “Wheel of Choice” (see Sample A). The “Wheel of Choice” providesseveral alternative choices for solv<strong>in</strong>g disagreements. Some of the strategies on the“Wheel of Choice” <strong>in</strong>clude 1) make an apology, 2) tell the person <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p, 3) walk away,<strong>and</strong> 4) give an “I” message. Each step of the “Wheel of Choice” was <strong>in</strong>troduced throughrole play (by myself <strong>and</strong> the students) <strong>and</strong> discussion. A large “Wheel of Choice” poster,which <strong>in</strong>cluded pictures depict<strong>in</strong>g the problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g strategies (<strong>to</strong> assist emergent readers),was hung on the classroom door for the children <strong>to</strong> refer <strong>to</strong> whenever a conflictarose. By implement<strong>in</strong>g the “Wheel of Choice,” I hoped <strong>to</strong> engage my students <strong>in</strong> positiveforms of conflict resolution by giv<strong>in</strong>g them choices for solutions.SampleMethodData CollectionBoth quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative data were collected dur<strong>in</strong>g this study. While data wascollected on all of the class, I targeted five students <strong>to</strong> observe more closely dur<strong>in</strong>g theproject. These students were selected because I had observed them hav<strong>in</strong>g difficulty withproblem solv<strong>in</strong>g throughout the first semester. Their first reaction <strong>in</strong> times of conflict was<strong>to</strong> act out, either physically or verbally (e.g., hitt<strong>in</strong>g, push<strong>in</strong>g, fight<strong>in</strong>g, yell<strong>in</strong>g, or tattl<strong>in</strong>g).PurposivesubsampleWHEEL OF CHOICEIGNORE ITSHARE AND TAKE TURNSWALK AWAYCLASS MEETING AGENDATALK IT OUTUSE AN “I” MESSAGEAPOLOGIZEDO IT TOMORROWGO BACK AND TRY AGAINOFFER YOUR HELPTAKE A STOP TO COOL OFFTELL THEM TO STOPCOUNT TO TEN TO COOL OFFGO TO ANOTHER GAMESample A


606 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eReliability? Validity?Cognitive, notbehavioralPossible validitychecksRecall of behaviorBehavioral measureBehavioral evidenceof cognitive onlyAs the targets of my study, I hoped <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d that, dur<strong>in</strong>g times of anger, these five studentswould change their negative behavior <strong>to</strong> constructive problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g.In the follow<strong>in</strong>g sections, I will describe the various methods of data collection.These <strong>in</strong>cluded 1) student pre- <strong>and</strong> post-surveys, 2) student conflict-resolution journals,3) behavior tally sheets, 4) observational notes, <strong>and</strong> 5) teacher reflective journal.Student Surveys. Surveys were collected <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e how students viewed theirown problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g strategies (see Sample B). I read the survey questions <strong>to</strong> thestudents <strong>and</strong> asked them <strong>to</strong> circle the answers they felt were appropriate. For questions3 <strong>and</strong> 4, the students drew pictures <strong>and</strong> then dictated their responses <strong>to</strong> me. Thesesurveys were adm<strong>in</strong>istered at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> end of the study <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e thestudents’ progress <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> conflict resolution.Conflict-Resolution Journals. Data also were collected from student conflict-resolutionjournals. Follow<strong>in</strong>g a conflict, students wrote about what happened <strong>in</strong> their journals.They addressed the follow<strong>in</strong>g three questions <strong>in</strong> their journals: 1) What was the problem?2) <strong>How</strong> did you solve it? <strong>and</strong> 3) What is another way <strong>to</strong> solve it? The purpose of thejournals was <strong>to</strong> help the students remember what happened so they could discuss it withme later.Tally Sheets. A behavior tally sheet was used <strong>to</strong> collect data on the number of timesthe targeted students exhibited physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression. I also moved on the tallysheets <strong>to</strong> record how often students used the “Wheel of Choice” <strong>to</strong> solve problems. Byeach tally mark, I wrote the first letter of the student’s name <strong>and</strong> what he/she did. At theend of each week, I added up the tally marks.Observational Notes. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the class meet<strong>in</strong>gs, I kept observational notes <strong>in</strong> aspiral-bound notebook of the meet<strong>in</strong>g’s agenda. After the “Wheel of Choice” had been<strong>in</strong>troduced as a conflict-resolution strategy, I analyzed the class meet<strong>in</strong>g agenda noteseach week dur<strong>in</strong>g the project <strong>to</strong> see if the students were us<strong>in</strong>g the strategies <strong>to</strong> solveconflicts as a group. The agenda notes <strong>in</strong>cluded the problem or concern of an <strong>in</strong>dividualchild <strong>and</strong> the list of solutions the students developed collaboratively dur<strong>in</strong>g the classmeet<strong>in</strong>g. In my analysis, I was look<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> see if the types of resolutions studentsSample BStudent SurveyName: __________________________________ Date: _________________________________1. When you get mad at a friend you . . .a. hit your friendb. yell at your friendc. tell an adultd. tell your friend how you feel2. When I have a fight, I fix it myself.a. yesb. no3. Draw a picture of one time you had a fight with a friend. (The teacher will write thechild’s dictated responses.)4. Draw a picture of how you solved that problem. (The teacher will write the child’sdictated responses.)suggested were chang<strong>in</strong>g from punitive measures—such as “go <strong>to</strong> DIL” (<strong>in</strong>-school suspension),“take them <strong>to</strong> the pr<strong>in</strong>cipal,” “go <strong>to</strong> time out,” or “move their clothesp<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>red” (on a classroom discipl<strong>in</strong>ary color-change chart)—<strong>to</strong> more positive solutions.


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 607Teacher Journal. I also kept a teacher reflective journal dur<strong>in</strong>g this project, <strong>in</strong> which,each week, I wrote about the occurrences <strong>in</strong> my classroom. The follow<strong>in</strong>g questions wereaddressed: Were the students hav<strong>in</strong>g many conflicts? Were they us<strong>in</strong>g the “Wheel ofChoice”? Was there any particular student who was hav<strong>in</strong>g more trouble than others? AsI kept this journal, I looked for trends <strong>in</strong> students’ behavior <strong>and</strong> among the conflictresolutionsuggestions they chose <strong>to</strong> use.Analysis of Data <strong>and</strong> F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsAs shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 1, an analysis of the tally sheets <strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>in</strong>cidences of physical<strong>and</strong> verbal aggression decreased from the first <strong>to</strong> the eighth week of the study. At thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the study, verbal aggression was the method most frequently used <strong>to</strong>resolve a conflict. The <strong>to</strong>tal number of verbally aggressive <strong>in</strong>cidences recorded for weekone was 22, as compared <strong>to</strong> only 4 <strong>in</strong>cidences dur<strong>in</strong>g week eight. Physical aggressiondecreased the first three weeks, but then <strong>in</strong>creased dramatically dur<strong>in</strong>g week four (from4 times <strong>to</strong> 8 times). After see<strong>in</strong>g this <strong>in</strong>crease, I analyzed the names on the tally sheet <strong>to</strong>see which student(s) were solv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts <strong>in</strong> this way. The same student had used thisstrategy for all eight conflicts. Although it fluctuated, the number of physically aggressiveacts did decrease from the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the end of the study.While the number of <strong>in</strong>cidences of physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression did decrease bythe end of the study, the tally sheets did not <strong>in</strong>dicate that students used strategies fromthe “Wheel of Choice” dur<strong>in</strong>g the “heat” of a conflict. The most frequent use of the“Wheel of Choice” strategies occurred dur<strong>in</strong>g the second week, after the strategies hadbeen <strong>in</strong>troduced. Thereafter, the use of these strategies tapered off <strong>and</strong> began show<strong>in</strong>gup aga<strong>in</strong> at the conclusion of the study. This f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g suggests that students need <strong>to</strong> berem<strong>in</strong>ded <strong>to</strong> refer <strong>to</strong> the “Wheel of Choice” when conflicts arise.Results of the comparison of pre- <strong>and</strong> post-surveys <strong>in</strong>dicate that my students weredevelop<strong>in</strong>g the skills <strong>to</strong> use more positive strategies for solv<strong>in</strong>g conflicts. The majority ofthe responses on the pre-surveys <strong>in</strong>dicate that the students’ first reaction <strong>to</strong> conflict wasTargetedsubsample onlyClear results forverbal; less so forphysicalInterpretationf ?22201816PhysicalVerbalWheel of ChoiceFigure 1 Types ofConflict-ResolutionStrategies Used by TargetStudents (N-5)# of Times Observed141210864Title is wrong; thetally sheets recordaggression, notstrategies.201 2 3 4 5 6 7 8Weeks


608 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e3. Draw a picture of one time you had a fight with a friend. (Teacher will dictate.)Should state“record.”4. Draw a picture of how you solved that problem. (Teacher will dictate.)Figure 2 Karen’s Post-Surveyf ?Not clear <strong>in</strong> Figure 2Recall of behavior“And use”?By whom?Good detailDid they do it?<strong>to</strong> act out of anger rather than <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p <strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about the problem. The post-surveys,however, suggest that once students were given the chance <strong>to</strong> analyze the situation <strong>and</strong>th<strong>in</strong>k about how <strong>to</strong> solve it, they were able <strong>to</strong> generate more positive alternatives. Forexample, as demonstrated <strong>in</strong> Figure 2, when Karen had a conflict with Lisa, her <strong>in</strong>itialreaction was <strong>to</strong> laugh at her <strong>and</strong> push her. Karen then thought about the problem <strong>and</strong>solved the disagreement <strong>in</strong> a more productive fashion. This action suggests that Karenwas able <strong>to</strong> use what she had learned from the “Wheel of Choice,” as well as from theclass meet<strong>in</strong>gs.The conflict-resolution journals were used <strong>to</strong> guide students <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g aboutthe problem <strong>and</strong> the solution they chose <strong>to</strong> use. Through the journals, I was able <strong>to</strong> seewhat type of conflict-resolution strategies were be<strong>in</strong>g used (e.g., if students were able<strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k of alternative choices for conflict management). I was also able <strong>to</strong> see whichstudents had the most difficulty when conflicts arose. The journal notes reflect that studentswere able <strong>to</strong> write positive forms of conflict resolution down after th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g aboutthe problem. These solutions were usually based on the choices from the “Wheel ofChoice.” . . .I analyzed the content of my reflective journal <strong>and</strong> observational notes by colorcod<strong>in</strong>gresponse patterns. I was look<strong>in</strong>g for trends <strong>in</strong> types of conflict resolutions used.I also wanted <strong>to</strong> see which students were successful at us<strong>in</strong>g the “Wheel of Choice” <strong>and</strong>which students needed more assistance. My journal was a kaleidoscope of colors. Thefirst few entries showed only physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression. The “Wheel of Choice”was used at least once a day for one or two days. By the middle of the project, I hadrecorded that the “Wheel of Choice” poster was be<strong>in</strong>g used more by students, especiallydur<strong>in</strong>g class meet<strong>in</strong>gs. For example, the solutions my students began suggest<strong>in</strong>gdur<strong>in</strong>g class meet<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>cluded 1) tell the person <strong>to</strong> s<strong>to</strong>p, 2) tell him or her how you feel,3) walk away, <strong>and</strong> 4) ignore it. My students also learned how <strong>to</strong> ask someone else forhelp (e.g., counselor, teacher, other students) if any of these solutions did not work.Be<strong>in</strong>g given the opportunity <strong>to</strong> discuss problems <strong>and</strong> develop solutions as a grouphelped my young students become more effective problem solvers. Through my journal,I found that my students were capable of us<strong>in</strong>g the “Wheel of Choice” when given theopportunity <strong>and</strong> time <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about the problem.


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 609Based on the notes <strong>in</strong> my reflective journal <strong>and</strong> the observational data, I selectedone target student who appeared <strong>to</strong> be hav<strong>in</strong>g more difficulty than others when solv<strong>in</strong>gconflicts. Michael’s <strong>in</strong>itial response was <strong>to</strong> hit, shove, or yell “S<strong>to</strong>p!” I <strong>to</strong>ok observationalnotes on Michael as he <strong>in</strong>teracted with others <strong>in</strong> the class <strong>to</strong> see when he had the mostdifficulty. From the color-cod<strong>in</strong>g of physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression <strong>and</strong> by compar<strong>in</strong>g theconflicts <strong>in</strong> the observational notes <strong>and</strong> the student journals, I saw that Michael clearlyhad more difficulty with conflict when he was <strong>in</strong>dividually engrossed <strong>in</strong> an activity <strong>and</strong>another child disturbed him. For <strong>in</strong>stance, one day while Michael was read<strong>in</strong>g, Timbumped him. Michael’s <strong>in</strong>itial reaction was <strong>to</strong> hit Tim on the arm <strong>and</strong> yell, “You did that!”He then went back <strong>to</strong> his work, almost unaware of Tim’s feel<strong>in</strong>gs. What I noticed from mynotes was that when Michael had time <strong>to</strong> consider the choices for conflict resolution, hewould make a positive choice. This was especially obvious <strong>in</strong> his conflict-resolution journal,where he wrote “Wheel of Choice” solutions for all of his problems.Good exampleBehavioral orcognitiveCONCLUSIONSThrough this research, I learned that students need time <strong>and</strong> practice <strong>to</strong> change negativeconflict management habits. While their first reaction may be <strong>to</strong> resort <strong>to</strong> physical orverbal aggression, when given appropriate models <strong>and</strong> time <strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about alternativesolutions, even young children can learn <strong>to</strong> be effective problem solvers.Young children are eager <strong>to</strong> imitate the adults <strong>in</strong> their environment. Children roleplaywhat they observe the adult models <strong>in</strong> their lives do<strong>in</strong>g. My class came <strong>to</strong> me with abackground of violence <strong>and</strong> of us<strong>in</strong>g negative forms of conflict resolution. I could nottake away the students’ environment or negative role models, but I could teach thembetter ways of h<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g anger, frustration, <strong>and</strong> differences of op<strong>in</strong>ion.Although my students did not <strong>in</strong>itially solve conflicts <strong>in</strong> a productive manner, I knewthey unders<strong>to</strong>od the role of problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g as a group. At the end of the year, I askedthe students why we solved problems as a group dur<strong>in</strong>g class meet<strong>in</strong>gs. The success ofmy action research project became apparent when Lisa said, “When we solve problems<strong>to</strong>gether we have more than one idea. If one doesn’t work, we have more <strong>to</strong> try. If wetried <strong>to</strong> solve problems by ourselves, there would be only one th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> do.” By implement<strong>in</strong>gclass meet<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> the “Wheel of Choice,” my students were given additionalresources <strong>to</strong> assist them <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g constructive social skills.I believe my students are on the road <strong>to</strong> becom<strong>in</strong>g successful problem solvers, bothat school <strong>and</strong> beyond. Most important, they need cont<strong>in</strong>ued opportunities <strong>to</strong> practiceus<strong>in</strong>g constructive problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g strategies <strong>in</strong> a supportive environment <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong>further develop the skill of th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g before they act.Behavioral orcognitive habits?Cognitive, notbehavioralWe agreeReferencesBailey, B. (1994). There’s gotta be a better way: Discipl<strong>in</strong>ethat works. Oviedo, FL: Lov<strong>in</strong>g Guidance.McEwan, B., Gathercoal, P., Donahue, M., Greenfield, L., &Strangio, A. M. (1998, April). Empower<strong>in</strong>g students <strong>to</strong> takecontrol of their own decisions: The synthesis of judiciousdiscipl<strong>in</strong>e, class meet<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> issues of resiliency. Paperpresented at the annual meet<strong>in</strong>g of the American <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong> Association, San Diego, CA.Nelsen, J., Duffy, R., Escobar, L., Or<strong>to</strong>lano, K., & Owen-Sohocki,D. (1996). Positive discipl<strong>in</strong>e: A teacher’s A–Z guide.Rockl<strong>in</strong>, CA: Prima Publish<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> Communications.Nelsen, J., & Lott, L. (1997). Positive discipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> theclassroom: Teacher’s guide. Orem, UT: Empower<strong>in</strong>gPeople Books, Tapes, & Videos.Nelsen, J., Lott, L., & Glenn, H. S. (1997). Positive discipl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong>the classroom. Rockl<strong>in</strong>, CA: Prima Publish<strong>in</strong>g.Sloane, M. W. (1998). Scor<strong>in</strong>g conflict-resolution goals.Kappa Delta Pi Record, 35: 21–23.Wittmer, D. S., & Honig, A. S. (1994). Encourag<strong>in</strong>g positiveattitudes <strong>and</strong> behavior <strong>in</strong> young children. Young Children,49: 4–12.


610 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eAnalysis of the StudyPURPOSE/JUSTIFICATIONThe purpose is stated <strong>in</strong> the fourth paragraph <strong>in</strong> terms ofdesired changes <strong>in</strong> behavior of a current class, <strong>and</strong> presumablyof future classes: “Ultimately, I wanted the children<strong>to</strong> become prepared for society-at-large by learn<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g constructive forms of conflict management.”This is an excellent example of purpose <strong>in</strong> actionresearch. Extensive justification is provided, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gboth the exist<strong>in</strong>g problem <strong>and</strong> pert<strong>in</strong>ent research.There appear <strong>to</strong> be no problems regard<strong>in</strong>g risk,deception, or confidentiality.DEFINITIONSDef<strong>in</strong>itions are not provided. Although key terms mayseem obvious <strong>and</strong> are somewhat clarified by examples,we th<strong>in</strong>k def<strong>in</strong>ition of physical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression,positive solutions, constructive solution, <strong>and</strong> conflictwould be helpful.PRIOR RESEARCHNumerous relevant references are provided, both asbackground <strong>and</strong> on the specific <strong>in</strong>tervention. It is notclear whether they are comprehensive or representative,but this is less of a problem <strong>in</strong> action research.HYPOTHESESA directional hypothesis is clearly implied, i.e., that students,subsequent <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>tervention, will show decreasedphysical <strong>and</strong> verbal aggression, be better able <strong>to</strong> verbalizeconflicts <strong>and</strong> feel<strong>in</strong>gs, <strong>and</strong> implement more constructivesolutions <strong>to</strong> conflict.SAMPLEAs is <strong>to</strong> be expected <strong>in</strong> action research, the authors giveno <strong>in</strong>dication of <strong>in</strong>tend<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> generalize beyond theirsample, <strong>in</strong> which case the sample is the population. Ifgeneralization is <strong>in</strong>tended, theirs is a conveniencesample. The sample of 20 first-graders is described <strong>in</strong>terms of gender, ethnicity, general location, <strong>and</strong> socioeconomicstatus. Five of these, selected as need<strong>in</strong>g themost help, provided the data on aggression.INSTRUMENTATIONThe five <strong>in</strong>struments are, <strong>in</strong> general, adequately described.We must assume that dictated responses <strong>to</strong> thesurvey were done <strong>in</strong>dividually. Reliability <strong>and</strong> validityare not addressed; they are as important <strong>in</strong> actionresearch as <strong>in</strong> other types of research. While requir<strong>in</strong>gmore time <strong>and</strong> effort, it might have been possible <strong>to</strong> dothe student survey twice at pre- <strong>and</strong> posttest<strong>in</strong>g, with an<strong>in</strong>terval of a few days. A scor<strong>in</strong>g system could easilyhave been developed based on frequencies <strong>and</strong> used <strong>to</strong>check reliability. Similarly, across-time comparisonscould, we th<strong>in</strong>k, have been made with conflict-resolutionjournal entries, tally sheets, observational notes,<strong>and</strong>, perhaps, teacher journal entries. Validity checkscould have consisted of comparison across <strong>in</strong>struments(by <strong>in</strong>dividual or group) on common items such as “useof aggression <strong>to</strong> solve the problem.”PROCEDURES/INTERNAL VALIDITYProcedures are generally well described <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude areference for more detail on the <strong>in</strong>tervention. We th<strong>in</strong>k areader is given enough detail <strong>to</strong> permit replication, ifdesired. A part of the <strong>in</strong>tervention, class meet<strong>in</strong>gs, wasbegun at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the school year; the focus onconflict resolution, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g use of the “Wheel ofChoice,” was carried out for eight weeks dur<strong>in</strong>g the secondsemester. It is implied that this was implemented for20 <strong>to</strong> 30 m<strong>in</strong>utes (daily?). This study used a one-grouppretest-posttest experimental design. As such, it is subject<strong>to</strong> almost all of the threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity we discussed<strong>in</strong> Chapter 9. That is, subject characteristics(those not addressed by the pretest, such as ability level,family composition, <strong>and</strong> direct exposure <strong>to</strong> abuse), location,researcher bias, his<strong>to</strong>ry, maturation, attitude of subjects,<strong>and</strong> implementation are all possible alternative explanationsfor outcomes. It can be argued that becausethe primary purpose was (we th<strong>in</strong>k) <strong>to</strong> affect the students<strong>in</strong> the study, it doesn’t matter whether the <strong>in</strong>terventionitself or perhaps the teacher’s skill <strong>in</strong> work<strong>in</strong>g with childrenwas responsible for positive changes. It doesmatter, however, <strong>in</strong> assess<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention.DATA ANALYSISThe descriptive procedures used, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the bargraph, are appropriate (although the title for the graph is<strong>in</strong>correct). Scores based on frequency of occurrence foreach <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>and</strong> subsequent graph<strong>in</strong>g would haveprovided more <strong>in</strong>formation. Frequencies, rather thanterms such as the majority <strong>and</strong> tapered off, should havebeen reported more often throughout, particularly <strong>in</strong>pre/post comparisons. The examples provide usefulclarification.


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 611RESULTS/DISCUSSION/INTERPRETATIONAlthough results <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation are <strong>in</strong>termixed, thedist<strong>in</strong>ction is, we th<strong>in</strong>k, usually clear. The written resultsare, <strong>in</strong> general, supported by the data, though more detail,as noted above, would have strengthened the study.The discussion of Figure 2 is not consistent with theFigure itself. Results <strong>and</strong> their <strong>in</strong>terpretation seem appropriatewith one exception; the dist<strong>in</strong>ction betweencognitive <strong>to</strong>ols <strong>and</strong> behavior is not consistently ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed.The behavior of “verbal aggression” clearly decreasedfor the five targeted students; physical aggressiondid not, perhaps largely due <strong>to</strong> one student(Michael). For the class as a whole, the extent <strong>to</strong> whichthe “teacher journal” results reflect actual behavioralresolution of conflicts is not clear. The student survey,conflict-resolution journals, <strong>and</strong> observational notes apparentlyperta<strong>in</strong> ma<strong>in</strong>ly <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividual or group <strong>in</strong>tellectualuse of <strong>to</strong>ols, which is different from actual behavior.We th<strong>in</strong>k this study is a good example of actionresearch. It seems clear that the class <strong>and</strong> teacher benefitedfrom carry<strong>in</strong>g out the study. There is evidence thatthe students learned the <strong>in</strong>tervention content; less thatthey used it. Replication of the study by other teachers<strong>in</strong> the same locale or elsewhere would be valuable <strong>to</strong>them, we th<strong>in</strong>k, <strong>and</strong>, if comb<strong>in</strong>ed, would be useful <strong>in</strong>evaluat<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention.Go back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.THE NATURE OF ACTION RESEARCHAction research is conducted by a teacher, adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r, or other education professional<strong>to</strong> solve a problem at the local level.Each of the specific methods of research can be used <strong>in</strong> action research studies,although on a smaller scale.A given research question may often be <strong>in</strong>vestigated by any one of several methods.Some methods are more appropriate <strong>to</strong> a particular research question <strong>and</strong>/or sett<strong>in</strong>gthan other methods.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsASSUMPTIONS UNDERLYING ACTION RESEARCHSeveral assumptions underlie action research studies. These are that the participantshave the authority <strong>to</strong> make decisions, want <strong>to</strong> improve their practice, are committed<strong>to</strong> cont<strong>in</strong>ual professional development, <strong>and</strong> will engage <strong>in</strong> systematic <strong>in</strong>quiry.TYPES OF ACTION RESEARCHPractical action research addresses a specific local problem.Participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research, while also focused on address<strong>in</strong>g a specific local problem,attempts <strong>to</strong> empower participants or br<strong>in</strong>g about social change.


612 PART 8 <strong>Research</strong> by Practitioners www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eLEVEL OF PARTICIPATION IN ACTION RESEARCHParticipation can range from giv<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly greater <strong>in</strong>volvement<strong>in</strong> the various aspects of the study.STEPS IN ACTION RESEARCHThere are four steps <strong>in</strong> action research: identify<strong>in</strong>g the research question or problem,gather<strong>in</strong>g the necessary data, analyz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g the data <strong>and</strong> shar<strong>in</strong>g the resultswith the participants, <strong>and</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>g an action plan.In participa<strong>to</strong>ry research, every effort is made <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>volve all those who have a vested<strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the outcomes of the study—the stakeholders.ADVANTAGES OF ACTION RESEARCHThere are at least five advantages <strong>to</strong> action research. It can be done by just about anyone,<strong>in</strong> any type of school or other <strong>in</strong>stitution, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate just about any k<strong>in</strong>d ofproblem or issue. It can help <strong>to</strong> improve educational practice. It can help education<strong>and</strong> other professionals <strong>to</strong> improve their craft. It can help them learn <strong>to</strong> identify problemssystematically; <strong>and</strong> it can build up a small community of research-oriented<strong>in</strong>dividuals at the local level.Action research has both similarities <strong>to</strong> <strong>and</strong> differences from formal quantitative <strong>and</strong>qualitative research.SAMPLING IN ACTION RESEARCHAction researchers are most likely <strong>to</strong> choose a purposive sample.THREATS TO THE INTERNAL VALIDITY OF ACTION RESEARCHAction research studies suffer especially from the possibility of data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias,implementation, <strong>and</strong> attitud<strong>in</strong>al threats. Most others can be controlled <strong>to</strong> a considerabledegree.EXTERNAL VALIDITY AND ACTION RESEARCHAction research studies are weak <strong>in</strong> external validity. Replication is, therefore,essential <strong>in</strong> these studies.Key Termsaction plan 590action research 589participant 591participa<strong>to</strong>ry actionresearch 590practical actionresearch 590stakeholder 590For Discussion1. Are there any k<strong>in</strong>ds of questions that could not be <strong>in</strong>vestigated by means of anaction research study? If you th<strong>in</strong>k so, give an example.2. Do you th<strong>in</strong>k the assumptions that underlie action research are true? Expla<strong>in</strong> yourreason<strong>in</strong>g. Are any of them questionable?3. Which of the four stages of action research would be the hardest <strong>to</strong> carry out? Why?


CHAPTER 24 Action <strong>Research</strong> 6134. “The important th<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> action research is not <strong>to</strong> rely on collect<strong>in</strong>g merely anecdotaldata.” Would you agree? Why would this be <strong>in</strong>sufficent (if it would be)?5. All of the participants—the stakeholders—<strong>in</strong> an action research study must be <strong>in</strong>volved<strong>in</strong> the entire research process. Why not also require this <strong>in</strong> formal qualitative<strong>and</strong> quantitative studies?6. What do you th<strong>in</strong>k is the major advantage of action research? the major disadvantage?7. Which methodologies, other than the ones discussed, might be used <strong>in</strong> each of thehypothetical examples <strong>in</strong> this chapter?8. What other methods might have been used <strong>in</strong> the DeMaria study? Which, if any,would you recommend? Why?1. G. E. Mills (2000). Action research: A guide for the teacher researcher. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.2. Ibid., p. 6.3. B. L. Berg (2001). Qualitative methods for the social sciences. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Allyn & Bacon, p. 180.4. E. T. Str<strong>in</strong>ger (1999). Action research, 2nd ed. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage. Cited <strong>in</strong> Berg, op. cit., p. 183.5. Berg, op. cit., p. 182.6. D. DeMaria (1990). A study of the effect of relaxation exercises on a class of learn<strong>in</strong>g-disabled students.Master’s thesis. San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA.Notes


Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>P A R TProposals <strong>and</strong> Reports9Part 9 discusses how <strong>to</strong> prepare a research proposal or report. We describe the majorsections of such proposals <strong>and</strong> reports <strong>and</strong> then describe some sections that areunique <strong>to</strong> reports. We conclude with an example of a student’s research proposal,followed by our analysis of it.


25Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong>Proposals <strong>and</strong> ReportsThe <strong>Research</strong> ProposalThe Major Sections of a<strong>Research</strong> Proposal orReportProblem <strong>to</strong> Be InvestigatedBackground <strong>and</strong> Reviewof Related LiteratureProceduresBudgetGeneral CommentsSections Unique <strong>to</strong><strong>Research</strong> ReportsSome General Rules <strong>to</strong>ConsiderFormatA Few Comments AboutQualitative <strong>Research</strong>ReportsAn Outl<strong>in</strong>e of a <strong>Research</strong>ReportA Sample <strong>Research</strong>Proposal"Why all thisfuss about a detailedproposal for my studybefore I even beg<strong>in</strong>? Th<strong>in</strong>gsare go<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> change onceI get <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the study!""That’s true.Changes are <strong>in</strong>evitable.But a little thought nowwill save you a lot ofgrief later on!"OBJECTIVESStudy<strong>in</strong>g this chapter should enable you <strong>to</strong>:Describe briefly the ma<strong>in</strong> sections of aresearch proposal <strong>and</strong> a research report.Describe the major difference between aresearch proposal <strong>and</strong> a research report.Write a research proposal.Underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> critique a typicalresearch report or proposal.


INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNINGGo <strong>to</strong> the Onl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center atwww.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong>:Learn More About the Role of <strong>Research</strong>ers <strong>in</strong> Action<strong>Research</strong>After, or while, read<strong>in</strong>g this chapter:Go <strong>to</strong> your Student MasteryActivities book <strong>to</strong> do the follow<strong>in</strong>gactivity:Activity 25.1: Put Them <strong>in</strong> OrderBy now we hope you have learned many of the concepts <strong>and</strong> procedures <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> educational research.You may, <strong>in</strong> fact, have done considerable th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g about a research study of your own. To help you further,we discuss <strong>in</strong> this chapter the major components <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> proposal <strong>and</strong> report writ<strong>in</strong>g. A research proposal isnoth<strong>in</strong>g more than a written plan for conduct<strong>in</strong>g a research study. It is a generally accepted <strong>and</strong> commonly requiredprerequisite for carry<strong>in</strong>g out a research <strong>in</strong>vestigation.<strong>Research</strong> proposals <strong>and</strong> research reports are similar <strong>in</strong>many respects, the ma<strong>in</strong> difference be<strong>in</strong>g that a researchproposal is generated before a study beg<strong>in</strong>s, while aresearch report is prepared after a study is completed.In this chapter, we shall describe <strong>and</strong> illustrate what isexpected <strong>and</strong> usually <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> each section of thesedocuments. We shall also discuss what is appropriate <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> the two sections that are unique <strong>to</strong> researchreports—those <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g the results of the study <strong>and</strong> thesubsequent discussion of those results. We will highlightwhat we have found <strong>to</strong> be the most common mistakesmade by beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g researchers <strong>in</strong> prepar<strong>in</strong>g researchproposals. F<strong>in</strong>ally, we will present an example of aresearch proposal prepared by one of our students <strong>and</strong>comment on its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses.The <strong>Research</strong> ProposalAresearch proposal is noth<strong>in</strong>g more than a written plan forconduct<strong>in</strong>g a research study. It is a generally accepted <strong>and</strong>commonly required prerequisite for carry<strong>in</strong>g out a research<strong>in</strong>vestigation. It communicates a researcher’s <strong>in</strong>tentions,makes clear the purpose of the <strong>in</strong>tended study <strong>and</strong>its justification, <strong>and</strong> provides a step-by-step plan for conduct<strong>in</strong>gthe study. The research proposal identifies problems,states questions or hypotheses, identifies variables,<strong>and</strong> def<strong>in</strong>es terms. The subjects <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the sample,the <strong>in</strong>strument(s) <strong>to</strong> be used, the research design chosen,the procedures <strong>to</strong> be followed, how the data will beanalyzed—all are spelled out <strong>in</strong> some detail, <strong>and</strong> at least apartial review of previous related research is <strong>in</strong>cluded.A research proposal, then, is a written plan of a study.It spells out <strong>in</strong> detail what the researcher <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> do. Itpermits others <strong>to</strong> learn about the <strong>in</strong>tended research <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>offer suggestions for improv<strong>in</strong>g the study. It helps theresearcher clarify what needs <strong>to</strong> be done <strong>and</strong> helps himor her avoid un<strong>in</strong>tentional pitfalls or unknown problems.Such a written plan is highly desirable, s<strong>in</strong>ce it allows<strong>in</strong>terested others <strong>to</strong> evaluate the worth of a proposedstudy <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> make suggestions for improvement.Let us beg<strong>in</strong>, then, by describ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> illustrat<strong>in</strong>g themajor components that make up the research proposal.The Major Sections ofa <strong>Research</strong> Proposal or ReportPROBLEM TO BE INVESTIGATEDFour <strong>to</strong>pics usually are addressed <strong>in</strong> this section: (1) thepurpose of the study, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the researcher’s assumptions;(2) the justification for the study; (3) the researchquestion <strong>and</strong>/or hypotheses, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the variables <strong>to</strong>be <strong>in</strong>vestigated; <strong>and</strong> (4) the def<strong>in</strong>ition of terms.Purpose of the Study. Usually the first section <strong>in</strong>the proposal or report, the purpose states succ<strong>in</strong>ctly whatthe researcher proposes <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. The purposeshould be a concise statement, provid<strong>in</strong>g a framework<strong>to</strong> which details are added later. Generally speak<strong>in</strong>g, anystudy should seek <strong>to</strong> clarify some aspect of the field of<strong>in</strong>terest that is considered important, thereby contribut<strong>in</strong>gboth <strong>to</strong> overall knowledge <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> current practice.617


618 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eHere are some examples of statements of purpose <strong>in</strong>research reports taken from the literature.“The purpose of this study was <strong>to</strong> identify <strong>and</strong>describe the bedtime rout<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong> self-reported nocturnalsleep patterns of women over age 65 <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e the differences <strong>and</strong> relationships betweenthese rout<strong>in</strong>es <strong>and</strong> patterns accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> whether ornot the subject was <strong>in</strong>stitutionalized.” 1“The purpose of this study was <strong>to</strong> explore how youngadolescents portray the ideal person <strong>in</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong> response <strong>to</strong> a survey.” 2“This study attempts <strong>to</strong> identify some of the processesmediat<strong>in</strong>g self-fulfill<strong>in</strong>g prophecies <strong>in</strong> theclassroom.” 3The researcher should articulate any assumptionsthat are basic <strong>to</strong> the study. For example:It is assumed that, if found effective, the methodsstudied could be adopted by many teachers withoutspecial tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g.It is assumed that the descriptive <strong>in</strong>formation onfamily <strong>in</strong>teraction that is provided by this study,if dissem<strong>in</strong>ated, will have an <strong>in</strong>fluence on familyfunction<strong>in</strong>g.It is assumed that predictive <strong>in</strong>formation from thisstudy would be used by counselors <strong>in</strong> advis<strong>in</strong>gstudents.Justification for the Study. In the justification,researchers must make clear why this particular subjectis important <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate. They must present an argumentfor the “worth” of the study, so <strong>to</strong> speak. Forexample, if a researcher <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> study a particularmethod for modify<strong>in</strong>g student attitudes <strong>to</strong>ward government,he or she must make the case that such a study isimportant—that people are, or should be, concernedabout it. The researcher must also make clear why he orshe chooses <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the particular method. Inmany such proposals, there is the implication that currentmethods are not good enough; this should be madeexplicit, however.A good justification should also <strong>in</strong>clude any specificimplications that follow if relationships are identified. Inan <strong>in</strong>tervention study, for example, if the method be<strong>in</strong>gstudied appears <strong>to</strong> be successful, changes <strong>in</strong> pre-serviceor <strong>in</strong>-service tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g for teachers may be necessary;money may need <strong>to</strong> be spent <strong>in</strong> different ways; materials<strong>and</strong> other resources may need <strong>to</strong> be used differently, <strong>and</strong>so on. In survey studies, strong op<strong>in</strong>ions on certa<strong>in</strong>issues (such as peer op<strong>in</strong>ions about drug use) mayhave implications for teachers, counselors, parents, <strong>and</strong>others. Relationships found <strong>in</strong> correlational or causalcomparativestudies may justify predictive uses. Also,results of correlational or ethnographic studies may suggestpossibilities for subsequent experimental studies.These should be discussed.Here is an example of a justification. It is taken froma report of a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the relationship betweennarrative <strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> a literaturebasedsixth-grade his<strong>to</strong>ry program.Recent research on the development of his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>ghas focused on secondary students. For severaldecades research has rested on the premise that his<strong>to</strong>ricalunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g is demonstrated <strong>in</strong> the ability <strong>to</strong> analyze<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret passages of his<strong>to</strong>ry—or at least passagesconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>rical names, dates, <strong>and</strong> events. The resultshave <strong>in</strong>dicated that if his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g developsat all, it does not appear until late adolescence (Hallam,1970, 1979; Peel, 1967). From the perspective of thosewho work with younger children, however, this approachreflects an <strong>in</strong>complete view of his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.The <strong>in</strong>ference often drawn from the research is thatyoung children cannot underst<strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry; thereforehis<strong>to</strong>ry should not be part of their curriculum. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly,surveys have shown that young children do not <strong>in</strong>dicatemuch <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry as a school subject. Yet teachers<strong>and</strong> parents know that children ev<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the olddays, <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical events or characters, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> descriptionsof everyday life <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ric times, such as LauraIngalls Wilder’s Little House books (e.g., 1953). Childrenrespond <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry long before they are capable ofh<strong>and</strong>l<strong>in</strong>g current tests of his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Theresearch, however, has not taken his<strong>to</strong>rical response <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>account <strong>in</strong> the development of mature underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.The research on children’s response <strong>to</strong> literatureprovides some guidel<strong>in</strong>es for exam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g his<strong>to</strong>ricalresponse. <strong>Research</strong> by Applebee (1978), Favat (1977),<strong>and</strong> Schlager (1975) suggests that aspects of responseare developmental. Other scholars (Brit<strong>to</strong>n, 1978; Egan,1983; Rosenblatt, 1938) extend that suggestion <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ricalunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, argu<strong>in</strong>g that early, personal responses<strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry—especially his<strong>to</strong>ry embedded <strong>in</strong> narrative—are precursors <strong>to</strong> more mature <strong>and</strong> objective his<strong>to</strong>ricalunderst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g.Little has been done <strong>to</strong> study the form of such earlyhis<strong>to</strong>rical response. Kennedy’s (1983) study exam<strong>in</strong>ed therelationship between <strong>in</strong>formation-process<strong>in</strong>g capacity <strong>and</strong>his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g, but concentrated on adolescents.


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 619Reviews of research on his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g also fail<strong>to</strong> uncover studies of early response. There is noth<strong>in</strong>gdescrib<strong>in</strong>g how children respond <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical material <strong>in</strong> aregular classroom sett<strong>in</strong>g. <strong>How</strong> do children respond ontheir own, or <strong>in</strong> contact with peers? What forms of his<strong>to</strong>ryelicit the strongest responses? <strong>How</strong> do childrenexpress <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical material? Does the classroomcontext <strong>in</strong>fluence responses? What teacher behaviors<strong>in</strong>hibit or encourage response?These are important questions for the elementaryteacher faced with a social studies curriculum that cont<strong>in</strong>ues<strong>to</strong> emphasize his<strong>to</strong>ry, as well as for the theorist <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> the development of his<strong>to</strong>rical underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. Yetthese questions cannot easily be answered by traditionalempirical models. <strong>Research</strong> needs <strong>to</strong> be extended <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>clude focus on the range of evidence available throughnaturalistic <strong>in</strong>quiry. . . .Classroom observation suggests that narrative is apotent spur <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>in</strong>terest. Teachers note the <strong>in</strong>terestexhibited by students <strong>in</strong> such his<strong>to</strong>rical s<strong>to</strong>ries as TheDiary of Anne Frank (Frank, 1952) <strong>and</strong> Little House onthe Prairie (Wilder, 1953) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> the oral tradition of familyhis<strong>to</strong>ry (Huck, 1981). <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> discourse analysis<strong>and</strong> schema theory suggests that narrative may helpchildren make sense of his<strong>to</strong>ry. White <strong>and</strong> Gagne (1976),for <strong>in</strong>stance, found that connected discourse leads <strong>to</strong>better memory for mean<strong>in</strong>g. Such discourse providesa framework that improves recall <strong>and</strong> helps children recognizeimportant features <strong>in</strong> a text (K<strong>in</strong>tsch, Kozm<strong>in</strong>sky,Streby, McKoon, & Keenan, 1975). DeVilliers (1974) <strong>and</strong>Lev<strong>in</strong> (1970) found that readers processed words <strong>in</strong> connecteddiscourse more deeply than when the same wordsappeared <strong>in</strong> sentences or lists. Cull<strong>in</strong>an, Harwood, <strong>and</strong>Galda (1983) suggest that readers may be better able<strong>to</strong> remember th<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> narratives where the “connecteddiscourse allows the reader <strong>to</strong> organize <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terrelateelements <strong>in</strong> the text” (p. 31).One way <strong>to</strong> help children underst<strong>and</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry, then,may be <strong>to</strong> use the connected discourse of literature.Such an approach also allows the researcher <strong>to</strong> focuson response as the ongo<strong>in</strong>g construction of mean<strong>in</strong>g aschildren encounter his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>in</strong> literature. The follow<strong>in</strong>gstudy <strong>in</strong>vestigated children’s responses <strong>to</strong> a literaturebasedapproach <strong>to</strong> his<strong>to</strong>ry. 4Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:1. Have I identified the specific research problem Iwish <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate?2. Have I <strong>in</strong>dicated what I <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> do about thisproblem?3. Have I put forth an argument as <strong>to</strong> why this problemis worthy of <strong>in</strong>vestigation?4. Have I made my assumptions explicit?<strong>Research</strong> Questions or Hypothesis. The particularquestion <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated should be stated next.This is usually, but not always, a more specific form ofthe problem <strong>in</strong> question form. As you will recall, we,along with many other researchers, favor hypothesesfor reasons of clarity <strong>and</strong> as a research strategy. If aresearcher has a hypothesis <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d, it should be stated asclearly <strong>and</strong> as concisely as possible. It is unnecessarilyfrustrat<strong>in</strong>g for a reader <strong>to</strong> have <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fer what aresearcher’s hypothesis or hypotheses might be. (SeeChapter 2 for several examples of typical research questions<strong>and</strong> hypotheses <strong>in</strong> education.)Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:5. Have I asked the specific research question I wish <strong>to</strong>pursue?6. Do I have a hypothesis <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d? If so, have I expressedit?7. Do I <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate a relationship? If so, haveI <strong>in</strong>dicated the variables I th<strong>in</strong>k may be related?Def<strong>in</strong>itions. All key terms should be def<strong>in</strong>ed. In ahypothesis-test<strong>in</strong>g study, these are primarily the termsthat describe the variables of the study. The researcher’stask is <strong>to</strong> make his or her def<strong>in</strong>itions as clear as possible.If previous def<strong>in</strong>itions found <strong>in</strong> the literature are clear <strong>to</strong>all concerned, well <strong>and</strong> good. Often, however, they need<strong>to</strong> be modified <strong>to</strong> fit the present study. It is often helpful<strong>to</strong> formulate operational def<strong>in</strong>itions as a way of clarify<strong>in</strong>gterms or phrases. While it is probably impossible <strong>to</strong>elim<strong>in</strong>ate all ambiguity from def<strong>in</strong>itions, the clearer theterms are—<strong>to</strong> both the researcher <strong>and</strong> others—the fewerdifficulties will be encountered <strong>in</strong> subsequent plann<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g of the study.Here are some examples of def<strong>in</strong>itions taken fromthe literature. The first three are taken from a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gthe relationship between peer experiences<strong>and</strong> social self-perceptions among Canadian studentsfrom a variety of socioeconomic backgrounds <strong>in</strong> 10 elementaryschools:Social preference was assessed by ask<strong>in</strong>g each child<strong>to</strong> name three other children they would like most<strong>and</strong> like least for play<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>gether, <strong>in</strong>vit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> a birthdayparty, <strong>and</strong> sitt<strong>in</strong>g next <strong>to</strong> each other on a bus.Victimization by peers was measured by ask<strong>in</strong>g eachchild <strong>to</strong> nom<strong>in</strong>ate up <strong>to</strong> five other students who could


620 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ebe described as be<strong>in</strong>g made fun of, be<strong>in</strong>g callednames, <strong>and</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g hit <strong>and</strong> pushed by other kids.Lonel<strong>in</strong>ess was measured with a 16-item questionnairewith higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g greater lonel<strong>in</strong>ess. 5This next def<strong>in</strong>ition is taken from a study <strong>in</strong> which theresearcher <strong>in</strong>vestigated why students of color were notenter<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g:M<strong>in</strong>ority teacher was def<strong>in</strong>ed as “Lat<strong>in</strong>o/Hispanic,African-American/black, Asian American, or NativeAmerican.” 6This last def<strong>in</strong>ition comes from a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>ghow people see their work:People who have jobs was def<strong>in</strong>ed as be<strong>in</strong>g “only <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> the material benefits from work <strong>and</strong> do notseek or receive any other type of reward from it.” Peoplewho have careers was def<strong>in</strong>ed as hav<strong>in</strong>g “a deeperpersonal <strong>in</strong>vestment <strong>in</strong> their work <strong>and</strong> mark theirachievements not only through monetary ga<strong>in</strong>, butthrough advancement with<strong>in</strong> the occupational structure.”People who have call<strong>in</strong>gs was def<strong>in</strong>ed as peoplewho “f<strong>in</strong>d their work is <strong>in</strong>separable from their life. Aperson with a Call<strong>in</strong>g works not for f<strong>in</strong>ancial ga<strong>in</strong> orCareer advancement, but <strong>in</strong>stead for the fulfillmentthat do<strong>in</strong>g the work br<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual.” 7Key Question <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:8. Have I def<strong>in</strong>ed all key terms clearly (<strong>and</strong>, if possible,operationally)?BACKGROUND AND REVIEWOF RELATED LITERATUREIn a research report, the literature review may be alengthy section, especially <strong>in</strong> a master’s thesis or a doc<strong>to</strong>raldissertation. In a research proposal, it is a partialsummary of previous work related <strong>to</strong> the hypothesis orfocus of the study. The researcher is try<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> show herethat he or she is familiar with the major trends <strong>in</strong> previousresearch <strong>and</strong> op<strong>in</strong>ion on the <strong>to</strong>pic <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>stheir relevance <strong>to</strong> the study be<strong>in</strong>g planned. This reviewmay <strong>in</strong>clude theoretical conceptions, directly relatedstudies, <strong>and</strong> studies that provide additional perspectiveson the research question. In our experience, themajor weakness of many literature reviews is that theycite references (often many references) without <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>gtheir relevance or implications for the plannedstudy. (See Chapter 5 for details on literature reviews.)A portion of a literature review follows. It is taken froma study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the relationship between k<strong>in</strong>dergartenteachers’ theoretical orientation <strong>to</strong>ward read<strong>in</strong>g<strong>and</strong> student outcomes of children with different <strong>in</strong>itialread<strong>in</strong>g abilities.The whole language approach <strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g hascaptured the attention of many teachers <strong>and</strong> teachereduca<strong>to</strong>rs over the past 20 years. It . . . asserts thatchildren learn language most effectively at their owndevelopmental pace through social <strong>in</strong>teraction <strong>in</strong>language-rich environments <strong>and</strong> through exposure <strong>to</strong>quality literature. This approach is often contrasted with aphonics-oriented strategy <strong>in</strong> which children receiveformal <strong>in</strong>struction emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g sound-symbol correspondence.... Stahl <strong>and</strong> Miller (1989) <strong>and</strong> Stahl, McKenna,<strong>and</strong> Pagnucco (1994) conducted meta-analyses of studiesconducted <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten <strong>and</strong> first-grade classroomscompar<strong>in</strong>g the relative impact of whole language <strong>and</strong>traditional approaches <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. Both metaanalysesyielded the general conclusion that the overallimpact of the two approaches was “essentially similar”(Stahl et al., 1994, p. 175), a position disputed bySchickedanz (1990) <strong>and</strong> McGee <strong>and</strong> Lomaz (1990).In review<strong>in</strong>g the whole language/phonics debate, <strong>and</strong>the <strong>in</strong>ability of researchers <strong>to</strong> reach similar conclusionsafter review<strong>in</strong>g the same studies, several problematicareas emerge. First, the mean<strong>in</strong>g of the term whole language<strong>and</strong> a set of dist<strong>in</strong>ctive classroom practices represent<strong>in</strong>gits operationalization are difficult <strong>to</strong> specify (Stahl& Miller, 1989). This is exacerbated by the fact that someproponents conceive of whole language as a philosophyrather than an explicitly def<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>structional methodology(Edelsky, 1990; Goodman, 1986; McKenna,Rob<strong>in</strong>son, & Miller, 1990; Newman, 1985; Rich, 1985).Second, many—if not most—teachers are eclectic <strong>in</strong>their approach <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, <strong>and</strong> pure contrastsbetween whole language- <strong>and</strong> phonics-oriented <strong>in</strong>structionare generally difficult <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong> naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g,unmanipulated classroom environments (Slaughter,1988). Third, with the exception of Fisher <strong>and</strong> Hiebert(1990), relatively little research has documented differences<strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>structional behavior <strong>and</strong> practices of teacherssubscrib<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> whole language versus traditional approaches<strong>to</strong> early read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction (Feng & Etheridge,1993; Lehman, Allen, & Freeman, 1990; Stahl et al.,1994). F<strong>in</strong>ally, “relatively few studies” (Stahl et al.,1994, p. 175) compar<strong>in</strong>g whole language <strong>and</strong> traditionalread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction have used st<strong>and</strong>ardized achievementmeasures or <strong>in</strong>cluded large numbers of students (e.g.,Watson, Crenshaw, & K<strong>in</strong>g, 1984). ...


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 621A number of researchers have exam<strong>in</strong>ed the impac<strong>to</strong>f whole language approaches <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g development forstudents considered educationally at risk. Stahl <strong>and</strong> Miller(1989) concluded that “whole language/language experienceapproaches . . . produce weaker effects with populationslabeled specifically as disadvantaged” (p. 87). Thisconclusion is supported by the research of Gersten, Darch,<strong>and</strong> Gleason (1988), who reported positive effects for atrisk(economically disadvantaged) children of a direct <strong>in</strong>structionk<strong>in</strong>dergarten classroom, based largely on traditional,phonics-oriented pr<strong>in</strong>ciples. <strong>How</strong>ever, a number ofrecent studies (Milligan & Berg, 1992; Ot<strong>to</strong>, 1993; P<strong>in</strong>nell,Lyons, DeFord, Bryk, & Seltzer, 1994; Sulzby, Branz, &Buhle, 1993) present evidence consistent with Kasten <strong>and</strong>Clarke’s (1989) argument that whole language-basedread<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction should be especially beneficial for disadvantagedchildren. ...Ot<strong>to</strong> (1993) <strong>and</strong> Sulzby et al. (1993) presentedevidence suggest<strong>in</strong>g that s<strong>to</strong>rybook read<strong>in</strong>g, generallyassociated with developmentally sensitive, wholelanguage approaches <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction, was helpful<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the emergent read<strong>in</strong>g ability of <strong>in</strong>ner-cityk<strong>in</strong>dergartners (Ot<strong>to</strong>, 1993; Sulzby et al., 1993) <strong>and</strong> firstgraders (Sulzby et al., 1993). <strong>How</strong>ever, neither of thesestudies used control groups, either of children not seenas at-risk or of children receiv<strong>in</strong>g more traditional<strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> the same schools. Purcell-Gates, McIntyre,<strong>and</strong> Freppon (1995) reported that children <strong>in</strong> wellimplementedwhole language classes showed significantlygreater growth <strong>in</strong> their knowledge of writtenlanguage <strong>and</strong> more extensive breadth of knowledge ofwritten l<strong>in</strong>guistic features than their peers <strong>in</strong> skills-basedk<strong>in</strong>dergarten classes. Putnam (1990) found that <strong>in</strong>ner-cityk<strong>in</strong>dergarten students <strong>in</strong> a “Literate Environment” classroomga<strong>in</strong>ed more <strong>in</strong> vocabulary <strong>and</strong> syntactic complexitythan students <strong>in</strong> “Traditional” or “IBM Write <strong>to</strong> Read”classrooms.F<strong>in</strong>ally, research by P<strong>in</strong>nell et al. (1994) found that“Read<strong>in</strong>g Recovery,” a tu<strong>to</strong>r<strong>in</strong>g program for educationallydisadvantaged children, was more effective <strong>in</strong> improv<strong>in</strong>gthe read<strong>in</strong>g efficacy of high-risk first graders than a similarprogram (called “Read<strong>in</strong>g Success”) provided by teacherswho were more traditional (phonics- or skills-oriented)compared <strong>to</strong> the “Read<strong>in</strong>g Recovery” teachers. <strong>How</strong>ever,given that the “Read<strong>in</strong>g Recovery” <strong>and</strong> the “Read<strong>in</strong>gSuccess” teachers also differed <strong>in</strong> a number of otherways (previous experience <strong>and</strong> tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g timeschedule, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g activities), it is impossible <strong>to</strong> teaseout the effects of the teachers’ theoretical orientations<strong>to</strong>ward read<strong>in</strong>g. 8Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:9. Have I surveyed <strong>and</strong> described relevant studiesrelated <strong>to</strong> the problem?10. Have I surveyed exist<strong>in</strong>g expert op<strong>in</strong>ion on theproblem?11. Have I summarized the exist<strong>in</strong>g state of op<strong>in</strong>ion<strong>and</strong> research on the problem?PROCEDURESThe procedures section <strong>in</strong>cludes discussions of: (1)research design, (2) sample, (3) <strong>in</strong>strumentation, (4)procedural details, (5) <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, <strong>and</strong> (6) dataanalysis.<strong>Research</strong> <strong>Design</strong>. In experimental or correlationalstudies, the research design can be described us<strong>in</strong>g thesymbols presented <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 or 15. In causalcomparativestudies, the research design should be describedus<strong>in</strong>g the symbols presented <strong>in</strong> Chapter 16. Theparticular research design <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>in</strong> the study <strong>and</strong> itsapplication <strong>to</strong> the study should be identified. In moststudies, the basic design is fairly clear-cut <strong>and</strong> fits oneof the models we presented <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 <strong>to</strong> 17 <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>Chapters 20 <strong>to</strong> 22.Sample. In a proposal, a researcher should <strong>in</strong>dicate<strong>in</strong> considerable detail how he or she will obta<strong>in</strong> thesubjects—the sample—for the study. If generalizationis <strong>in</strong>tended, a r<strong>and</strong>om sample should be used. If a conveniencesample must be used, relevant demographics(gender, ethnicity, occupation, IQ, <strong>and</strong> so on) of thesample should be described. Lastly, the legitimate population<strong>to</strong> which the results of the study may be generalizedshould be <strong>in</strong>dicated. (See Chapter 6 for details onsampl<strong>in</strong>g.)Here is an example of a description of a conveniencesample. It was taken from the report of a study designed<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effects of behavior modification onthe classroom behavior of first- <strong>and</strong> third-graders.Thirty grade 1 (mean age 7 years, 1 month) <strong>and</strong> 25grade 3 children (mean age 9 years, 3 months) wereidentified by their classroom teachers as exhibit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>appropriateclassroom behavior, receiv<strong>in</strong>g no special services,<strong>and</strong> hav<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>telligence quotients between 85 <strong>and</strong>115. These children represented 23% of the grade 1 children<strong>in</strong> a large elementary school <strong>in</strong> the southeasternUnited States <strong>and</strong> 21% of the grade 3 children <strong>in</strong> the sameschool. All participants were from regular classrooms;


622 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7enone were receiv<strong>in</strong>g special educational services. Fifteengrade 1 subjects were assigned r<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>to</strong> the experimentaltreatment <strong>and</strong> 15 <strong>to</strong> the control condition; 25grade 3 subjects were assigned r<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>to</strong> each of thetwo conditions, with the experimental treatmentreceiv<strong>in</strong>g 13 <strong>and</strong> control, 12. The experimental group<strong>in</strong>cluded 22 boys, 6 girls; 11 black children, 17 whitechildren; 14 of low socioeconomic status, 14 of middle<strong>to</strong> high socioeconomic status. The control group wascomposed of 15 boys, 12 girls; 15 black children, 12 whitechildren; 7 of low socioeconomic status, 20 of middle <strong>to</strong>high socioeconomic status. No attrition occurred dur<strong>in</strong>gthis study. 9Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:12. Have I described my sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan?13. Have I described the relevant characteristics of mysample <strong>in</strong> detail?14. Have I identified the population <strong>to</strong> which the resultsof the study may legitimately be generalized?Instrumentation. Whenever possible, exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strumentsshould be used <strong>in</strong> a study, s<strong>in</strong>ce constructionof even the most straightforward test or questionnaireis often a very time-consum<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> difficult task. The useof an exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument, however, is not justified unlesssufficiently reliable <strong>and</strong> valid results can be obta<strong>in</strong>ed forthe researcher’s purpose. Too many studies are done with<strong>in</strong>struments that are merely convenient or well known.Usage is a poor criterion of quality, as shown by the cont<strong>in</strong>u<strong>in</strong>gpopularity of some widely used achievementtests despite years of scath<strong>in</strong>g professional criticism. (SeeChapter 7 for examples of the many types of <strong>in</strong>strumentsthat educational researchers can use.)In the event that appropriate ready-made <strong>in</strong>strumentsare not available, the procedures followed <strong>in</strong> develop<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>struments should be described with attention<strong>to</strong> how validity <strong>and</strong> reliability will (presumably) be enhanced.At least some sample items from the <strong>in</strong>strumentsshould be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the proposal.Even with <strong>in</strong>struments for which reliability <strong>and</strong> validityof scores are supported by impressive evidence,there is no guarantee that these <strong>in</strong>struments will function<strong>in</strong> the same way <strong>in</strong> the study itself. Differences <strong>in</strong>subjects <strong>and</strong> conditions may make previous estimates ofvalidity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong>applicable <strong>to</strong> the current context.Further, validity always depends on the <strong>in</strong>tent <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terpretation of the researcher. For all these reasons,the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of the scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed fromall <strong>in</strong>struments should be checked as a part of everystudy, preferably before the study beg<strong>in</strong>s.It is almost always feasible <strong>to</strong> check <strong>in</strong>ternal consistencyreliability s<strong>in</strong>ce no additional data are required.Check<strong>in</strong>g reliability of scores over time (stability) ismore difficult s<strong>in</strong>ce an additional adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the<strong>in</strong>strument is required. Even when feasible, repetition ofexactly the same <strong>in</strong>strument may be questionable, s<strong>in</strong>ce<strong>in</strong>dividuals may alter their responses as a result of tak<strong>in</strong>gthe <strong>in</strong>strument the first time.* Ask<strong>in</strong>g respondents <strong>to</strong>reply <strong>to</strong> a questionnaire or an <strong>in</strong>terview a second timeis often difficult, s<strong>in</strong>ce it seems rather foolish <strong>to</strong> them.Nonetheless, <strong>in</strong>genuity <strong>and</strong> the effort required <strong>to</strong> developa parallel form of the <strong>in</strong>strument(s) can oftenovercome these obstacles.†The most straightforward way <strong>to</strong> check validity is <strong>to</strong>use a second <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> measure the same variable.Often, this is not as difficult as it may seem, given thevariety of <strong>in</strong>struments that are available (see Chapter 7).Frequently, the judgment of knowledgeable persons(teachers, counselors, parents, <strong>and</strong> friends, for <strong>in</strong>stance),expressed as rat<strong>in</strong>gs or as a rank<strong>in</strong>g of themembers of a group, can serve as the second <strong>in</strong>strument.Sometimes a useful means of validat<strong>in</strong>g the responses<strong>to</strong> attitude, op<strong>in</strong>ion, or personality (such as self-esteem)scales filled out by subjects is <strong>to</strong> have a person whoknows each subject well fill out the same scale (as it applies<strong>to</strong> the subject) <strong>and</strong> then check <strong>to</strong> see how well therat<strong>in</strong>gs correspond. A f<strong>in</strong>al po<strong>in</strong>t is that reliability <strong>and</strong>validity data need not be obta<strong>in</strong>ed for the entire sample,although this is preferable. It is better <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> suchdata for only a portion of the sample (or even for a separate,although comparable, sample) than <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> nodata at all. (For a more detailed discussion of reliability<strong>and</strong> validity, see Chapter 8.)In some studies, especially his<strong>to</strong>rical <strong>and</strong> qualitativeones, there may be no formal <strong>in</strong>strument like a test or arat<strong>in</strong>g scale <strong>in</strong>volved. In such studies, the researcher isoften the “<strong>in</strong>strument” for obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g data. Even so, waysof maximiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> check<strong>in</strong>g on validity <strong>and</strong> reliabilityshould be set forth <strong>in</strong> the proposal <strong>and</strong> described later <strong>in</strong>the report.Here are some examples of <strong>in</strong>struments taken fromthe literature:Social class: “Socioeconomic status (SES) was determ<strong>in</strong>edon the basis of parental occupation of fatheror mother, whomever was higher. Occupations were*For example, they may look up the answers.†A compromise is <strong>to</strong> divide the exist<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two halves(as <strong>in</strong> the split-half procedure) <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister each half with a time<strong>in</strong>terval between adm<strong>in</strong>istrations.


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 623<strong>in</strong>dexed accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the Warner Revised OccupationalRat<strong>in</strong>g Scale. . . . The Warner Scale consists ofseven occupational categories with assigned valuesrang<strong>in</strong>g from 1 <strong>to</strong> 7, based on the skill requirements<strong>and</strong> social prestige of the job.” Higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicatedhigher social class st<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>g. 10Self-esteem: “We used the Coopersmith Self-EsteemInven<strong>to</strong>ry . . . , a 50-item scale, <strong>to</strong> measure global selfesteem.Adequate assessments of construct, concurrent,<strong>and</strong> predictive validity are reported <strong>in</strong> the manual.Higher scores <strong>in</strong>dicate higher self-esteem.” 11Psychological distress: “The Symp<strong>to</strong>m Checklist-90-Revised . . . , a 90-item self-report <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ry, wasused <strong>to</strong> assess psychological symp<strong>to</strong>ms.” 12Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:15. Have I described the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>to</strong> be used?16. Have I <strong>in</strong>dicated their relevance <strong>to</strong> the presentstudy?17. Have I stated how I will check the reliability ofscores obta<strong>in</strong>ed from all <strong>in</strong>struments?18. Have I stated how I will check the validity of scoresobta<strong>in</strong>ed from all <strong>in</strong>strument(s)?Procedural Details. Next, the procedures <strong>to</strong> befollowed <strong>in</strong> the study—what will be done, as well aswhen, where, <strong>and</strong> how—should be described <strong>in</strong> detail.In <strong>in</strong>tervention studies <strong>in</strong> particular, additional detailsare usually needed on the nature of the <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>and</strong>on the means of <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g the method or treatment.Keep <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that the goal here is <strong>to</strong> make it possible <strong>to</strong>replicate the study; another researcher should, on thebasis of the <strong>in</strong>formation provided <strong>in</strong> this section, be able<strong>to</strong> repeat the study <strong>in</strong> exactly the same way as the orig<strong>in</strong>alresearcher. Certa<strong>in</strong> procedures may change as thestudy is carried out, it is true, but a proposal shouldnonetheless have this level of clarity as its goal.The researcher should also make clear how the <strong>in</strong>formationcollected will be used <strong>to</strong> answer the orig<strong>in</strong>alquestion or <strong>to</strong> test the orig<strong>in</strong>al hypothesis.Here are some examples of procedural details takenfrom the literature:(From a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g why students of color arenot enter<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g): “Over a two-year period, Iconducted face-<strong>to</strong>-face, semi-structured <strong>in</strong>terviewswith 140 teachers of color <strong>in</strong> C<strong>in</strong>c<strong>in</strong>nati, Ohio; Seattle,Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n; <strong>and</strong> Long Beach, California. Semistructured,face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g was selected asthe most appropriate research strategy because of the<strong>in</strong>tense <strong>and</strong> critical nature of the <strong>to</strong>pic under scrut<strong>in</strong>y<strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>formants <strong>in</strong>volved.” 13(From a descriptive study of eleventh-grade U.S.His<strong>to</strong>ry classes): “Four 11th-grade United States his<strong>to</strong>ryclasses, located <strong>in</strong> a large urban high school(grades 9–12) on the west coast of the United States,were observed unobtrusively at least three times aweek for six weeks dur<strong>in</strong>g January <strong>and</strong> February of1993. In addition, each of the teachers of thoseclasses were <strong>in</strong>terviewed at length.” 14Key Question <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:19. Have I fully described the procedures <strong>to</strong> be followed<strong>in</strong> the study—what will be done, where,when, <strong>and</strong> how?Internal Validity. At this po<strong>in</strong>t, the essential plann<strong>in</strong>gfor a study should be nearly completed. It is nownecessary for the researcher <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>e the proposedmethodology for the presence of any feasible alternativeexplanations for the results should the study’s hypothesisbe supported (or should nonhypothesized relationshipsbe identified). We suggest that each of the threats<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity discussed <strong>in</strong> Chapter 9 be reviewed<strong>to</strong> see if any might apply <strong>to</strong> the proposed study. Shouldany troublesome areas be found, they should be mentioned<strong>and</strong> their likelihood discussed. The researchershould describe what he or she would do <strong>to</strong> elim<strong>in</strong>ate orm<strong>in</strong>imize them. Such an analysis often results <strong>in</strong> substantialchanges <strong>in</strong> or additions <strong>to</strong> the methodology ofthe study; if this occurs, realize that it is better <strong>to</strong>become aware of the need for such changes at this stagethan after the study is completed.Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:20. Have I discussed any feasible alternative explanationsthat might exist for the results of the study?21. Have I discussed how I will deal with these alternativeexplanations?Data Analysis. The researcher then should <strong>in</strong>dicatehow the data <strong>to</strong> be collected will be organized(see Chapter 7) <strong>and</strong> analyzed (see Chapters 10, 11,<strong>and</strong> 12).Key Questions <strong>to</strong> Ask Yourself at This Po<strong>in</strong>t:22. Have I described how I will organize the data I willcollect?23. Have I described how I will analyze the data, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>gstatistical procedures that will be used <strong>and</strong>why these procedures are appropriate?


***b_tt RESEARCH TIPSQuestions <strong>to</strong> Ask WhenEvaluat<strong>in</strong>g a <strong>Research</strong> ReportIs the literature review sufficiently comprehensive? Does it<strong>in</strong>clude studies that might be relevant <strong>to</strong> the problem under<strong>in</strong>vestigation?Was each of the variables <strong>in</strong> the study clearly def<strong>in</strong>ed?Was the sample representative of an identifiable population?If not, were limitations discussed?Was the methodology the researchers used appropriate <strong>and</strong>underst<strong>and</strong>able so that other researchers could replicate thestudy if they wished?Was each of the <strong>in</strong>struments sufficiently valid <strong>and</strong> reliablefor its <strong>in</strong>tended purpose?Were the statistical techniques, if used, appropriate <strong>and</strong>correct?Did the report <strong>in</strong>clude a thick description that revealedhow <strong>in</strong>dividuals responded (if appropriate)?Was the researchers’ conclusions supported by the data?Did the researchers draw reasonable implications for theory<strong>and</strong>/or practice from their f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs?BUDGET<strong>Research</strong> proposals are often submitted <strong>to</strong> government orprivate fund<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>stitutions <strong>in</strong> hopes of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g f<strong>in</strong>ancialsupport. Such <strong>in</strong>stitutions almost always require submissionof a tentative budget along with the proposal. Needless<strong>to</strong> say, the amount of money <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> a researchproposal can have a considerable impact on whether or notit is funded. Thus, great care should be given <strong>to</strong> prepar<strong>in</strong>gthe budget. Budgets usually <strong>in</strong>clude such items as salaries,materials, equipment costs, secretarial <strong>and</strong> other assistance,expenses (such as travel <strong>and</strong> postage), <strong>and</strong> overhead.GENERAL COMMENTSOne other comment may not seem necessary, but <strong>in</strong> ourexperience it is. Remember that all sections of a proposalmust be consistent. It is not uncommon <strong>to</strong> read a proposal<strong>in</strong> which each section by itself is quite acceptable butsome sections contradict others. The terms used <strong>in</strong> astudy, for example, must be used throughout as orig<strong>in</strong>allydef<strong>in</strong>ed. Any hypotheses must be consistent with the researchquestion. Instrumentation must be consistent withor appropriate for the research question, the hypotheses,<strong>and</strong> the procedures for data collection. The method of obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe sample must be appropriate for the <strong>in</strong>strumentsthat will be used <strong>and</strong> for the means of deal<strong>in</strong>g withalternative explanations for the results, <strong>and</strong> so forth.Sections Unique<strong>to</strong> <strong>Research</strong> ReportsOnce researchers have conducted <strong>and</strong> completed theirstudy, they must write a report of their procedures <strong>and</strong>f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. The unique features of a report describe whatwas done <strong>in</strong> the study, how it was done, what resultswere obta<strong>in</strong>ed, <strong>and</strong> what they mean. Although the detailsof a quantitative study may differ somewhat fromthose of a qualitative study, the emphasis <strong>in</strong> both shouldbe on accurate description so that the reader is quiteclear about what happened. The old st<strong>and</strong>bys—what,why, where, when, <strong>and</strong> how—are, as always, goodguides <strong>to</strong> follow.SOME GENERAL RULES TO CONSIDERA research report should be written as clearly <strong>and</strong> conciselyas possible. If at all possible, jargon is <strong>to</strong> beavoided. <strong>Research</strong> reports are always written <strong>in</strong> the pasttense. As might be expected, spell<strong>in</strong>g, punctuation, <strong>and</strong>grammar must be correct. (The spell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> grammarchecks on a computer are a big help here!)A style manual should be consulted before beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gthe report. A good source, recommended by most journaledi<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> used by many researchers when prepar<strong>in</strong>gtheir research reports, is the Publication Manual ofthe American Psychological Association (APA), 5th ed.(2001). Although various manuals emphasize differentrules, all have certa<strong>in</strong> ones <strong>in</strong> common. The use ofabbreviations <strong>and</strong> contractions, for example, is usuallydiscouraged, the only exceptions be<strong>in</strong>g those that arecommonly used <strong>and</strong> unders<strong>to</strong>od (such as IQ) or thosethat are repeated frequently <strong>in</strong> the report. Authors ofreferences cited <strong>in</strong> the report are usually referred <strong>to</strong> bylast name only (first name <strong>and</strong> middle <strong>in</strong>itials are givenonly <strong>in</strong> the bibliography; Table 25.1). Honorifics (e.g.,Dr., Professor, etc.) are not given.Once a report is completed, it is a good idea <strong>to</strong> havesomeone who is knowledgeable about the <strong>to</strong>pic reviewthe report for clarity <strong>and</strong> errors. Read<strong>in</strong>g the report aloud624


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 625TABLE 25.1Type of ReferenceBookEdited bookChapter <strong>in</strong> a bookJournal articleDissertation (unpublished)Book reviewElectronic sourceERIC referenceReferences APA StyleFormatFraenkel, J. R. & Wallen, N. E. (2009). <strong>How</strong> <strong>to</strong> design <strong>and</strong> evaluate research <strong>in</strong> education(7th ed.). San Francisco: McGraw-Hill.Jacoby, R., & Glauberman, N. (Eds.). (1995). The bell curve debate: His<strong>to</strong>ry, documents,op<strong>in</strong>ions. NY: R<strong>and</strong>om House.Gould, S. J. (1995). Mismeasure by any measure. In R. Jacoby & N. Glauberman (Eds.),The bell curve debate: His<strong>to</strong>ry, documents, op<strong>in</strong>ions. (pp. 3–13). NY: R<strong>and</strong>om House.Clarke, A. T., & Kurtz-Costes, B. (May/June, 1997). Television view<strong>in</strong>g, educationalquality of the home environment, <strong>and</strong> school read<strong>in</strong>ess. The Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>, 90(5), 279–285.Spitzer, S. L. (2001). No words necessary: An ethnography of daily activities with youngchildren who don’t talk. Unpublished doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertation, University of Southern California.Liss, A. (2004). Whose America? Culture wars <strong>in</strong> the public schools [Review of the bookWhose America? Culture wars <strong>in</strong> the public schools.] Social <strong>Education</strong>, 68, 238.Learnframe. (2000, August). Facts, figures, <strong>and</strong> forces beh<strong>in</strong>d e-learn<strong>in</strong>g. RetrievedOc<strong>to</strong>ber 15, 2002, from http://www.learnframe.com/aboutlearn<strong>in</strong>g/Mead, J. V. (1992). Look<strong>in</strong>g at old pho<strong>to</strong>graphs: Investigat<strong>in</strong>g the teacher tales that noviceteachers br<strong>in</strong>g with them (Report No. NCRTL-RR-92-4). East Lans<strong>in</strong>g, MI: National Centerfor <strong>Research</strong> on Teacher Learn<strong>in</strong>g. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 345 082)also can help check for mistakes <strong>in</strong> grammar as well asidentify unclearly written passages. These days, the useof a computer can help a great deal, as it provides theability <strong>to</strong> rearrange words <strong>and</strong> sentences easily, checkspell<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> grammar, <strong>and</strong> number pages au<strong>to</strong>matically.FORMATThe format of a report is the way it is organized. <strong>Research</strong>reports generally follow a format that reflects thesteps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the study itself; they also have manyof the same components <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> research proposals.Figure 25.1 illustrates the organization of a typicalresearch report. Let us address those components wehave not yet discussed.Abstract. The abstract is a brief summary of the entireresearch report. It is usually no longer than a paragraphor two <strong>and</strong> is typed on a separate page with theword Abstract centered at the <strong>to</strong>p of the page. Usually,an abstract conta<strong>in</strong>s a brief statement of the researchproblem, the hypothesis, a description of the sample,followed by a brief summary of the procedures, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>ga description of the <strong>in</strong>strument(s) used, how thedata were collected, the results of the study, <strong>and</strong> theresearcher’s conclusions.Results/F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs. As discussed previously, theresults of a study can be presented only <strong>in</strong> a researchreport; ord<strong>in</strong>arily there are no results <strong>in</strong> a proposal (unlessresults of some explora<strong>to</strong>ry research or a pilot study are<strong>in</strong>cluded as part of the background of the proposal). Areport of the results, sometimes called the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, is<strong>in</strong>cluded near the end of the report. The f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs ofthe study constitute the results of the researcher’s analysisof his or her data—that is, what the collected data reveal.In comparison-group studies, the means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationfor each group on the posttest measure(s) usuallyare reported. In correlational studies, correlation coefficients<strong>and</strong> scatterplots are reported. In survey studies, percentagesof responses <strong>to</strong> the questions asked, crossbreaktables, cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficients, etc., are given.The results section should describe any statistical techniquesthat were applied <strong>to</strong> the data <strong>and</strong> the results thatwere obta<strong>in</strong>ed. Each result should be discussed <strong>in</strong> relation<strong>to</strong> the <strong>to</strong>pic studied. The results of any statistical tests ofsignificance should be reported. Qualitative data analysisshould present clear descriptions (<strong>and</strong> sometimes quotations)<strong>to</strong> support <strong>and</strong>/or illustrate results obta<strong>in</strong>ed throughobservations <strong>and</strong>/or <strong>in</strong>terviews. Tables <strong>and</strong> figures shouldpresent clear summaries of the data analysis.It is particularly important <strong>in</strong> the results section of aresearch report that the data collection procedures be


626 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eclearly described, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g what k<strong>in</strong>ds of analyses weredone. Here are two examples taken from the literature.• (From a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the effects of cooperativelearn<strong>in</strong>g among Hispanic students <strong>in</strong> elementarysocial studies): “Means <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations ofraw scores for the social studies achievementpretests <strong>and</strong> posttests, as well as the adjusted meansfor the social studies achievement posttest, are reported.Results of the ANCOVA revealed a statisticallysignificant ma<strong>in</strong> effect for treatment, F(1,93) 25.72, p .001, favor<strong>in</strong>g cooperative learn<strong>in</strong>g overtraditional <strong>in</strong>struction; however, no statisticallysignificant effects were found for gender or for an<strong>in</strong>teraction between treatment <strong>and</strong> gender on socialstudies achievement. The correlation r between thepretest <strong>and</strong> the posttest was .67 (p .001).” 15• (From a study <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the relationship betweentime <strong>to</strong> completion <strong>and</strong> achievement on multiplechoiceitems): “The relationship between time <strong>to</strong>completion <strong>and</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ation achievement was exploredseparately for mid-semester <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>al exam<strong>in</strong>ations.The resultant correlation coefficients werelow <strong>and</strong> not statistically significant (p .05). Althoughthe range of coefficients extended from .27( .02) <strong>to</strong> .30, the coefficients of determ<strong>in</strong>ation forthese values suggest that 0.04% <strong>to</strong> 9% of variance <strong>in</strong>exam<strong>in</strong>ation performance could be expla<strong>in</strong>ed by differences<strong>in</strong> time <strong>to</strong> completion variables.” 16Discussion. The discussion section of a report presentsthe author’s <strong>in</strong>terpretation of what the resultsimply for theory <strong>and</strong>/or practice. This <strong>in</strong>cludes, <strong>in</strong>hypothesis-test<strong>in</strong>g studies, an assessment of the extent<strong>to</strong> which the hypothesis was supported.In the discussion section, researchers place their results<strong>in</strong> a broader context. Here they recapitulate anydifficulties that were encountered, make note of the limitationsof the study, <strong>and</strong> suggest further, related studiesthat might be done.To the extent possible, we believe the results <strong>and</strong> discussionsections of a study should be kept dist<strong>in</strong>ct fromeach other. A discussion section will typically go considerablybeyond the data <strong>in</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> place the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong> a broader perspective. It is important that thereader not be misled <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g that the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rhas obta<strong>in</strong>ed evidence for someth<strong>in</strong>g that is only speculation.To put it differently, there should be no room fordisagreement regard<strong>in</strong>g the statements <strong>in</strong> the results sectionof the report. The statements should follow clearly<strong>and</strong> directly from the data that were obta<strong>in</strong>ed. There maybe much argumentation <strong>and</strong> disagreement about thebroader <strong>in</strong>terpretation of these results, however.Let us consider the results of a study on teacher personality<strong>and</strong> classroom behavior. As hypothesized <strong>in</strong> thatstudy, correlations of .40 <strong>to</strong> .50 were found between a tes<strong>to</strong>f control need on the part of the teacher <strong>and</strong> (1) the exten<strong>to</strong>f controll<strong>in</strong>g behavior <strong>in</strong> the classroom as observed <strong>and</strong>(2) rat<strong>in</strong>gs by <strong>in</strong>terviewers as “less comfortable with self”<strong>and</strong> “hav<strong>in</strong>g more rigid attitudes of right <strong>and</strong> wrong.”These were the results of the study <strong>and</strong> should clearly beidentified as such <strong>in</strong> a report. In the discussion section,however, these f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs might be placed <strong>in</strong> a variety ofcontroversial perspectives. Thus, one <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r mightpropose that the study provides support for selection ofprospective teachers, argu<strong>in</strong>g that anyone scor<strong>in</strong>g high <strong>in</strong>control need should be excluded from a tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g programon the grounds that this characteristic <strong>and</strong> the classroombehavior it appears <strong>to</strong> predict are undesirable <strong>in</strong> teachers.In contrast, another <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r might <strong>in</strong>terpret the results<strong>to</strong> support the desirability of attract<strong>in</strong>g people withhigher control need <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> teach<strong>in</strong>g. This <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rmight argue that, at least <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>ner-city schools, teachersscor<strong>in</strong>g higher <strong>in</strong> control need are likely <strong>to</strong> have more orderlyclassrooms.Clearly, both of these <strong>in</strong>terpretations go far beyondthe results of the particular study. There is no reason the<strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>r should not make such an <strong>in</strong>terpretation,provided that it is clearly identified as such <strong>and</strong> does notgive the impression that the results of the study providedirect evidence <strong>in</strong> support of the <strong>in</strong>terpretation. Manytimes a researcher will sharply differentiate between results<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation by plac<strong>in</strong>g them <strong>in</strong> different sectionsof a report <strong>and</strong> label<strong>in</strong>g them accord<strong>in</strong>gly. At othertimes a researcher may <strong>in</strong>termix the two, mak<strong>in</strong>g it difficultfor the reader <strong>to</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>guish the results of thestudy from the researcher’s <strong>in</strong>terpretations. (For examplesof discussions, see any of the published researchreports presented <strong>in</strong> Chapters 13 through 17 <strong>and</strong> 19through 24.)Suggestions for Further <strong>Research</strong>. Normally,this is the f<strong>in</strong>al section of a report. Based on the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsof the present study, the researcher suggests some related<strong>and</strong> follow-up studies that might be conducted <strong>in</strong>the future <strong>to</strong> advance knowledge <strong>in</strong> the field.References. F<strong>in</strong>ally, the references (bibliography)should list all sources that were used <strong>in</strong> the writ<strong>in</strong>g ofthe report. Every (yes, every!) source cited <strong>in</strong> the report


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 627must be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the references, <strong>and</strong> every (yes,every!) report cited there must appear <strong>in</strong> the body of thereport. The reference section should beg<strong>in</strong> on a newpage. Usually a hang<strong>in</strong>g-<strong>in</strong>dent format is used, with allsources listed alphabetically by authors’ last names.Footnotes. Footnotes are numbered consecutively,us<strong>in</strong>g a superscript Arabic numeral, <strong>in</strong> the order <strong>in</strong>which they appear <strong>in</strong> the text of the report.Figures. Figures consist of draw<strong>in</strong>gs, graphs, charts,even pho<strong>to</strong>graphs or pictures. All figures should be numberedconsecutively <strong>and</strong> referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong> the text of the report.They should be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> a report only when theycan convey <strong>in</strong>formation better or more clearly than thetext itself or when they can summarize <strong>in</strong>formation thatwould require an extremely long explanation. Each figureshould be accompanied by a caption that captures theessence of the <strong>in</strong>formation illustrated.Tables. Tables also should be used only when theycan summarize or convey <strong>in</strong>formation better, more simply,or more clearly than the text alone. Tables (<strong>and</strong> figures)should always be viewed as supplements <strong>to</strong> text,never as provid<strong>in</strong>g new <strong>in</strong>formation meant <strong>to</strong> st<strong>and</strong>alone. They should always, however, be referred <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>the text. Like figures, each table should have a brief titlethat captures the essence of the <strong>in</strong>formation conta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>the table. It is a good idea <strong>to</strong> consult the APA PublicationManual for specifics regard<strong>in</strong>g the presentation offigures <strong>and</strong> tables <strong>in</strong> a research report.A FEW COMMENTSABOUT QUALITATIVE RESEARCH REPORTSMuch of the <strong>in</strong>formation that needs <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> aqualitative research report is similar <strong>to</strong> that <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong>a quantitative research report. At present, however,there is no commonly agreed-on format for a qualitativeresearch report. One currently f<strong>in</strong>ds a variety of formats,with researchers often <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g such th<strong>in</strong>gs as poems,s<strong>to</strong>ries, diaries, pho<strong>to</strong>graphs, essays, even song lyrics<strong>and</strong> draw<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> their reports.Two noticeable characteristics of qualitative reportsthat are rarely found <strong>in</strong> quantitative reports are that(1) qualitative researchers often write their reports <strong>in</strong> thefirst person (e.g., us<strong>in</strong>g the pronouns I or we rather thanthe researcher or the author) <strong>and</strong> (2) they often use theactive rather than the passive voice (“We observedclassroom X,” rather than “Classroom X was observedby the researcher.”)*Furthermore, the issue of confidentiality is of greaterconcern <strong>in</strong> qualitative than quantitative reports. Oftena considerable amount of <strong>in</strong>formation, much of it extremelyprivate, is obta<strong>in</strong>ed from the participants <strong>in</strong> aqualitative study. A simple guarantee of confidentialityis often <strong>in</strong>sufficient <strong>to</strong> protect their identity. As a result,fictitious names are frequently used <strong>in</strong> qualitative reportsbecause the sample <strong>in</strong>volved is usually so muchsmaller than that used <strong>in</strong> quantitative studies. If a researcheris conduct<strong>in</strong>g a series of <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>nercityhigh school, for example, over a period of weeks,many readers might be able <strong>to</strong> recognize who he or she<strong>in</strong>terviewed. The use of fictitious names, therefore, is afurther protection of their identity.AN OUTLINE OF A RESEARCH REPORTFigure 25.1 shows an outl<strong>in</strong>e of a research report. Althoughthe <strong>to</strong>pics listed are generally agreed <strong>to</strong> with<strong>in</strong> theresearch community, the particular sequence may vary <strong>in</strong>different studies. This is partly because of different preferencesamong researchers <strong>and</strong> partly because the head<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>and</strong> organization of the outl<strong>in</strong>e will be somewhatdifferent for different research methodologies. This outl<strong>in</strong>emay also be used for a research proposal, <strong>in</strong> whichcase sections IV <strong>and</strong> V would be omitted (<strong>and</strong> the futuretense used throughout). Also, a budget might be added.A Sample <strong>Research</strong> ProposalThe research proposal that follows was prepared by a student<strong>in</strong> one of our classes <strong>and</strong> is a good example of a beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>geffort. Such a proposal will normally go throughfurther revision based on the comments of faculty <strong>and</strong>others, but this will give you some idea of what a completedproposal by a student looks like. We comment onboth its strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses <strong>in</strong> the marg<strong>in</strong>s.Note that this proposal does not follow the organizationrecommended <strong>in</strong> Figure 25.1 exactly. It does, however,conta<strong>in</strong> all of the major components previouslydiscussed. It also <strong>in</strong>cludes a report of a pilot study—asmall-scale trial of the proposed procedures. Its purposeis <strong>to</strong> detect any problems so that they can be remediedbefore the study proper is carried out.*The APA Publication Manual recommends such practice now evenfor quantitative reports.


628 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure 25.1Organization of a<strong>Research</strong> ReportIntroduc<strong>to</strong>ry sectionTitle PageTable of ContentsList of FiguresList of TablesMa<strong>in</strong> BodyI. Problem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigatedA. Purpose of the study (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g assumptions)B. Justification of the studyC. <strong>Research</strong> question <strong>and</strong> hypothesesD. Def<strong>in</strong>ition of termsE. Brief overview of studyII. Background <strong>and</strong> review of related literatureA. Theory, if appropriateB. Studies directly relatedC. Studies tangentially relatedIII. ProceduresA. Description of the research designB. Description of the sampleC. Description of <strong>in</strong>struments used (scor<strong>in</strong>g procedures; reliability; validity)D. Explanation of the procedures followed (the what, when, where, <strong>and</strong> howof the study)E. Discussion of <strong>in</strong>ternal validityF. Discussion of external validityG. Description <strong>and</strong> justification of the statistical techniques or other methodsof analysis usedIV. F<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gsDescription of f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs pert<strong>in</strong>ent <strong>to</strong> each ofthe research hypotheses or questionsV. Summary <strong>and</strong> conclusionsA. Brief summary of the research question be<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>vestigated, the proceduresemployed, <strong>and</strong> the results obta<strong>in</strong>edB. Discussion of the implications of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs—their mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>significanceC. Limitations—unresolved problems <strong>and</strong> weaknessesD. Suggestions for further researchReferences (Bibliography)Appendixes


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 629THE EFFECTS OF INDIVIDUALIZED READINGUPON STUDENT MOTIVATION IN GRADE FOURNad<strong>in</strong>e DeLuca*RequiresdocumentationReplace with“better able”Could bemore specific<strong>to</strong> this studyAnoperationaldef<strong>in</strong>itionwould help.Good—clear<strong>and</strong> specificPurposeThe general purpose of this research is <strong>to</strong> add <strong>to</strong> the exist<strong>in</strong>gknowledge about read<strong>in</strong>g methods. Many educa<strong>to</strong>rs have become dissatisfiedwith general read<strong>in</strong>g programs <strong>in</strong> which teacher-directedgroup <strong>in</strong>struction means boredom <strong>and</strong> delay for quick students <strong>and</strong>embarrassment <strong>and</strong> lack of motivation for others. Although there hasbeen a great deal of writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> favor of an <strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>gapproach which is supposedly a highly-motivat<strong>in</strong>g method of teach<strong>in</strong>gread<strong>in</strong>g, sufficient data has not been presented <strong>to</strong> make the argumentfor or aga<strong>in</strong>st <strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g programs decisive. With thedata supplied by this study (<strong>and</strong> future ones), soon schools will befree <strong>to</strong> make the choice between implement<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>dividualizedread<strong>in</strong>g program or reta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a basal read<strong>in</strong>g method.Def<strong>in</strong>itionsMotivation: Motivation is <strong>in</strong>cit<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> susta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g action <strong>in</strong> anorganism. The motivation <strong>to</strong> learn could be thought of as be<strong>in</strong>g derivedfrom a comb<strong>in</strong>ation of several more basic needs such as the need <strong>to</strong>achieve, <strong>to</strong> explore, <strong>to</strong> satisfy curiosity.Individualization: Individualization is characteristic of an <strong>in</strong>dividualizedread<strong>in</strong>g program. Individualized read<strong>in</strong>g has as its basisthe concepts of seek<strong>in</strong>g, self-selection, <strong>and</strong> pac<strong>in</strong>g. An <strong>in</strong>dividualizedread<strong>in</strong>g program has the follow<strong>in</strong>g characteristics:1. Literature books for children predom<strong>in</strong>ate.2. Each child makes personal choices with regard <strong>to</strong> his read<strong>in</strong>gmaterials.3. Each child reads at his own rate <strong>and</strong> sets his own pace ofaccomplishment.4. Each child confers with the teacher about what he has read<strong>and</strong> the progress he has made.5. Each child carries his read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> some form of summariz<strong>in</strong>gactivity.6. Some k<strong>in</strong>d of record is kept by the teacher <strong>and</strong>/or thestudent.Demonstratesimportanceof studyIndicatesimplicationsif hypothesisis supported“Motivation<strong>to</strong> read” isreally thevariable.You shoulddelete thissentence.*Used by permission of the author.


630 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e7. Children work <strong>in</strong> groups for an immediate learn<strong>in</strong>g purpose<strong>and</strong> leave group when the purpose is accomplished.8. Word recognition <strong>and</strong> related skills are taught <strong>and</strong> vocabularyis accumulated <strong>in</strong> a natural way at the po<strong>in</strong>t of each child’sneed.OKOKPrior <strong>Research</strong>Abbott, J. L., “Fifteen Reasons Why Personalized Read<strong>in</strong>g InstructionDoesn’t Work.” Elementary English (January, 1972), 44:33–36.This article refutes many of the usual arguments aga<strong>in</strong>st<strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. It lists those cus<strong>to</strong>mary argumentsthen proceeds <strong>to</strong> expla<strong>in</strong> why the objections are not validones.It expla<strong>in</strong>s how such a program can be implemented by anord<strong>in</strong>ary classroom teacher <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> show the fallacy <strong>in</strong> thecompla<strong>in</strong>t that <strong>in</strong>dividualiz<strong>in</strong>g is impractical. Another fallacy <strong>in</strong>volvesthe argument that unless a traditional basal read<strong>in</strong>g programis used, children do not ga<strong>in</strong> all the necessary read<strong>in</strong>gskills.Barbe, Walter B., Educa<strong>to</strong>r’s Guide <strong>to</strong> Personalized Read<strong>in</strong>g Instruction.Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1961.Mr. Barbe outl<strong>in</strong>es a complete <strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g program.He expla<strong>in</strong>s the necessity of keep<strong>in</strong>g records of children’sread<strong>in</strong>g. The book <strong>in</strong>cludes samples of book-summariz<strong>in</strong>g activitiesas well as many checklists <strong>to</strong> ensure proper <strong>and</strong> completeskill development for read<strong>in</strong>g.Hunt, Lyman C., Jr., “Effect of Self-selection, Interest, <strong>and</strong> Motivationupon Independent, Instructional <strong>and</strong> Frustrational Levels.”Read<strong>in</strong>g Teacher (November, 1970), 24:146–151.Dr. Hunt expla<strong>in</strong>s how self-selection, <strong>in</strong>terest, <strong>and</strong> motivation(some of the basic pr<strong>in</strong>ciples beh<strong>in</strong>d <strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g),when used <strong>in</strong> a read<strong>in</strong>g program, result <strong>in</strong> greater read<strong>in</strong>gachievement.Miel, Alice, Ed., Individualiz<strong>in</strong>g Read<strong>in</strong>g Practices. New York: Bureauof Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1959.Veatch, Jeanette, Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Elementary School. New York: TheRol<strong>and</strong> Press Co., 1966.


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 631This is notreally aliterature review,although it isa goodbeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g atprepar<strong>in</strong>g one.Additionalmaterial needs Hypothesis<strong>to</strong> be added <strong>and</strong>summarized<strong>to</strong> justify thestudy.RightWest, Rol<strong>and</strong>, Individualized Read<strong>in</strong>g Instruction. Port Wash<strong>in</strong>g<strong>to</strong>n,New York: Kennikat Press, 1964.The three books listed above all provide examples of various<strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g programs actually be<strong>in</strong>g used by differentteachers. (The def<strong>in</strong>itions <strong>and</strong> items on the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale werederived from these three books.)The greater the degree of <strong>in</strong>dividualization <strong>in</strong> a read<strong>in</strong>g program,the higher will be the students’ motivation.PopulationAn ideal population would be all fourth graders <strong>in</strong> the UnitedStates. Because of different teacher-qualification requirements,different laws, <strong>and</strong> different teach<strong>in</strong>g programs, though, such ageneralization may not be justifiable. One that might be justifiablewould be a population of all fourth-grade classrooms <strong>in</strong> the SanFrancisco-Bay Area.Good—showsrelevance <strong>to</strong>present studyVariables are clear<strong>and</strong> hypothesis isdirectionalGoodsampl<strong>in</strong>gplanAdd“r<strong>and</strong>om”!Sampl<strong>in</strong>gThe study will be conducted <strong>in</strong> fourth-grade classrooms <strong>in</strong> theSan Francisco-Bay Area, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ner-city, rural, <strong>and</strong> suburbanschools. The sample will <strong>in</strong>clude at least one hundred classrooms.Ideally, the sampl<strong>in</strong>g will be done r<strong>and</strong>omly by identify<strong>in</strong>g allfourth-grade classrooms for the population described <strong>and</strong> us<strong>in</strong>g r<strong>and</strong>omnumbers <strong>to</strong> select the sample classrooms. As this would requireexcessive amounts of time, this sampl<strong>in</strong>g might need <strong>to</strong> be modifiedby tak<strong>in</strong>g a sample of schools <strong>in</strong> the area, identify<strong>in</strong>g all fourthgradeclassrooms <strong>in</strong> these schools only, then tak<strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>om samplefrom these classrooms.Two-stagesampl<strong>in</strong>gAppears <strong>to</strong>have goodcontentvalidity:items areconsistentwithdef<strong>in</strong>itionInstrumentationInstrumentation will <strong>in</strong>clude a rat<strong>in</strong>g scale <strong>to</strong> be used <strong>to</strong> rate thedegree of <strong>in</strong>dividualization <strong>in</strong> the read<strong>in</strong>g program <strong>in</strong> eachclassroom. A sample rat<strong>in</strong>g scale is shown below. Those itemson the left <strong>in</strong>dicate characteristics of classrooms with little<strong>in</strong>dividualization.Reliability: The rat<strong>in</strong>gs of the two observers who are observ<strong>in</strong>gseparately but at the same time <strong>in</strong> the same room will be compared <strong>to</strong>see how closely the rat<strong>in</strong>gs agree. The rat<strong>in</strong>g scale will be repeated foreach classroom on at least three different days.Three days maynot be sufficient <strong>to</strong>get reliable scores.Should statehow data ondifferent dayswill be used; itcan be used <strong>to</strong>check stability


632 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eWouldparents bequalified <strong>to</strong>judge this?Can’t use thesame item forboth variablesGood idea,but may be<strong>to</strong>o few items<strong>to</strong> give areliable <strong>in</strong>dexValidity: Certa<strong>in</strong> items on the student questionnaire (<strong>to</strong> be discussed<strong>in</strong> the next section) will be compared with the rat<strong>in</strong>gs on therat<strong>in</strong>g scale <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if there is a correlation between the degreeof <strong>in</strong>dividualization apparently observed <strong>and</strong> the degree <strong>in</strong>dicated bystudents’ responses. In the same manner, responses <strong>to</strong> questionsasked of teachers <strong>and</strong> parents can be used <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate whether therat<strong>in</strong>g scale is a true measure of the degree of <strong>in</strong>dividualization.Another means of <strong>in</strong>strumentation <strong>to</strong> be used is a studentquestionnaire. A sample questionnaire is <strong>in</strong>cluded. The follow<strong>in</strong>gquestions have as their purpose <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the degree of motivationby ask<strong>in</strong>g how many books read <strong>and</strong> how the child <strong>in</strong>dicateswhat he feels about read<strong>in</strong>g: questions numbered 1, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9,10, 11, 12, <strong>and</strong> 13. Questions 2, 3, 4, <strong>and</strong> 8 have as their purpose<strong>to</strong> help determ<strong>in</strong>e the validity of the items on the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale. Questions14 <strong>and</strong> 15 are <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the students’ attitudes<strong>to</strong>ward the questionnaire <strong>to</strong> help determ<strong>in</strong>e if their attitudes arepossible sources of bias for the study. Questions 8 <strong>and</strong> 9 have anadditional purpose which is <strong>to</strong> add knowledge about the novelty ofthe read<strong>in</strong>g situation <strong>in</strong> which the child now f<strong>in</strong>ds himself. This maybe used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e if there is a relationship between the noveltyof the situation <strong>and</strong> the degree of motivation.GoodMost items appear<strong>to</strong> have logical validity,but the lackof def<strong>in</strong>ition ofmotivation <strong>to</strong> readmakes it difficult<strong>to</strong> judge.Good idea, butmay not beenough items<strong>to</strong> give areliable <strong>in</strong>dexBut why? <strong>to</strong> control noveltyas an extraneous variable?RATING SCALE1. Basal readers or pro- 1 2 3 4 5 There is an obvious centergrammed readers pre-<strong>in</strong> the room conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g atdom<strong>in</strong>ate <strong>in</strong> room.least five library booksper child.2. Teacher teaches class 1 2 3 4 5 Teacher works with <strong>in</strong>diasa group.viduals or small groups.3. Children are all read- 1 2 3 4 5 Children are read<strong>in</strong>g vari<strong>in</strong>gfrom the same bookous materials at differentseries.levels.4. Teacher <strong>in</strong>itiates ac- 1 2 3 4 5 Student <strong>in</strong>itiates activities.tivities.5. No read<strong>in</strong>g records 1 2 3 4 5 Children or teacher areare <strong>in</strong> evidence.observed <strong>to</strong> be mak<strong>in</strong>gnotes or keep<strong>in</strong>g recordsof books read.


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 633RATING SCALE6. There is no evidence 1 2 3 4 5 There is evidence of bookof book summariz<strong>in</strong>g ac-summariz<strong>in</strong>g activitiestivities <strong>in</strong> the room.around room (e.g., studentmadebook jackets,pa<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>gs, draw<strong>in</strong>gs, modelsof scenes or charactersfrom books, class list ofbooks read, bullet<strong>in</strong> boarddisplays about booksread . . . ).7. Classroom is ar- 1 2 3 4 5 Classroom is arrangedranged with desks <strong>in</strong>with a read<strong>in</strong>g area sorows <strong>and</strong> no provisionthat children have opporfora special read<strong>in</strong>gtunities <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d quietarea.places <strong>to</strong> read silently.8. There is no confer- 1 2 3 4 5 There is a conference areaence area <strong>in</strong> the room forset apart from the rest ofthe teacher <strong>to</strong> work withthe class where thechildren <strong>in</strong>dividually.teacher works with children<strong>in</strong>dividually.9. Children are do<strong>in</strong>g 1 2 3 4 5 Children are do<strong>in</strong>g differthesame activities at theent activities from theirsame time.classmates.10. Teacher tells chil- 1 2 3 4 5 Children choose their owndren what they are <strong>to</strong>read<strong>in</strong>g materials.read dur<strong>in</strong>g class.11. Children read aloud 1 2 3 4 5 Children read silently at<strong>in</strong> turn <strong>to</strong> teacher astheir desks or <strong>in</strong> a read<strong>in</strong>gpart of a group us<strong>in</strong>g thearea or orally <strong>to</strong> the teachersame read<strong>in</strong>g textbook.on an <strong>in</strong>dividual basis.Student QuestionnaireAge _________ Grade _________ Father’s work _________________________Mother’s work _______________________Appears valid 1. <strong>How</strong> many books have you read <strong>in</strong> the last month? ______________2. Do you choose the books you read by yourself? __________________Appears valid If not, who does choose them for you? ___________________________Is your <strong>in</strong>tenthere <strong>to</strong> get atsocioeconomiclevel?


634 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eSome <strong>in</strong>dication of the scor<strong>in</strong>g system should be given. Open-ended questions mustrely on logical analysis of responses. You could use examples from your pilot study.AppearsvalidAppearsvalidQuestionablevalidity<strong>How</strong>scored?Appears valid3. Do you keep a record of what books you have read? _____________Does your teacher? _______________________________________________4. What different k<strong>in</strong>ds of read<strong>in</strong>g materials have you read thisyear?5. Do you feel you are learn<strong>in</strong>g very much <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g this year? _______Why or why not? _______________________________________________ __6. Complete these sentences:Books ________________________________________________________Read<strong>in</strong>g ______________________________________________________7. Do you enjoy read<strong>in</strong>g time? _______________________________________Appear valid as 8. Have you ever been taught read<strong>in</strong>g a different way? ________ _____<strong>in</strong>dications ofnovelty; generally When? __________________ <strong>How</strong> was it different? __________________not a good idea <strong>to</strong>9. Which way of learn<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> read do you like better? ________________have one item (9)dependent on _____________________________ Why? _______________________________another item (8)10. If you couldn’t come <strong>to</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g class for some reason, would youAppears valid be disappo<strong>in</strong>ted? ___________________ Why? _______________________Appears validQuestionablevalidity11. Is this classroom a happy place for you dur<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g time? _____12. Do most of the children <strong>in</strong> your classroom enjoy read<strong>in</strong>g?___________________________________________________________________Appears valid13. <strong>How</strong> much of your spare time at home do you spend read<strong>in</strong>g justfor fun? ______________________________________________________Goodidea14. Did you like answer<strong>in</strong>g these questions or would you have preferrednot <strong>to</strong>? _____________________________________________________15. Were any of the questions confus<strong>in</strong>g? ____________________________If so, which ones? ________________________________________________<strong>How</strong> were they confus<strong>in</strong>g? _______________________________________


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 635Why do youwant this<strong>in</strong>formation?GoodGoodWhy <strong>in</strong>clude?as a means ofcontroll<strong>in</strong>g“experience”?Why? <strong>to</strong>assess novelty?Why?Why?Student Questionnaire:Reliability: An attempt will be made <strong>to</strong> control item reliabilityby ask<strong>in</strong>g the same question <strong>in</strong> different ways <strong>and</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g theanswers.Validity: Validity may be questionable <strong>to</strong> some degree s<strong>in</strong>ceschool children may be reluctant <strong>to</strong> report anyth<strong>in</strong>g bad about theirteachers or the school. Observers will be rem<strong>in</strong>ded <strong>to</strong> establish rapportwith children as much as possible before adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g questionnaires<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> assure them that the purpose of the questionsdoes not affect them or their school <strong>in</strong> any way.A teacher questionnaire will also be adm<strong>in</strong>istered. A samplequestionnaire is <strong>in</strong>cluded. Some of the questions are <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>dicate if the approach be<strong>in</strong>g used by the teacher is new <strong>to</strong> her <strong>and</strong>what her attitude is <strong>to</strong>ward the method. These questions are numbered1, 2, 3, <strong>and</strong> 4. Question 5 is supposed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate how availableread<strong>in</strong>g materials are so that this can be compared <strong>to</strong> the degree ofstudent motivation. Questions 6 <strong>and</strong> 8 will provide validity checks forthe rat<strong>in</strong>g scale. Question 7 will help <strong>in</strong> determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a relationshipbetween socioeconomic levels <strong>and</strong> student motivation.Reliability: Reliability should not be <strong>to</strong>o great a problem withthis <strong>in</strong>strument s<strong>in</strong>ce most questions are of a factual nature.Validity: There may be a question as <strong>to</strong> validity depend<strong>in</strong>gupon how the questions are asked (if they are used <strong>in</strong> a structured<strong>in</strong>terview). The way they are asked may affect the answers. Anattempt has been made <strong>to</strong> state the questions so that the teacherdoes not realize what the purposes of this study are <strong>and</strong> so prejudiceher answers.Teacher Questionnaire1. <strong>How</strong> long have you been teach<strong>in</strong>g? _______________________________2. <strong>How</strong> long have you taught us<strong>in</strong>g the read<strong>in</strong>g approach you arenow us<strong>in</strong>g? _________________________________________________________3. What other approaches have you used? ___________________________4. If you could use any read<strong>in</strong>g approach you liked, which wouldyou use?____________________________________________________________________Why? _____________________________________________________________Which itemswill becompared?Good po<strong>in</strong>tGood ideaWhy? <strong>How</strong> is thisrelated <strong>to</strong> yourhypothesis?May be <strong>to</strong>o few items<strong>to</strong> give reliable <strong>in</strong>dexIncorrect. It is thereliability of<strong>in</strong>formation thatcounts. Persons may ormay not be consistent<strong>in</strong> giv<strong>in</strong>g factual<strong>in</strong>formation. It doesseem likely that thesequestions wouldprovide reliable data.


636 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eUnder procedures, you expla<strong>in</strong> that items 1–5 <strong>and</strong> 7 are <strong>in</strong>tended as attempts <strong>to</strong> control extraneousvariables. This is a very good idea, but the purpose should be made clear earlier (<strong>in</strong> this section).Why?Appearsvalid for<strong>in</strong>dividualizationTo assess socioeconomicstatusAppearsvalid for<strong>in</strong>dividualization5. In what manner do you obta<strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g materials? _____________ ________________________________________________________________________Where did you get most of those you now use? ________________________________________________________________________________________6. <strong>How</strong> often are the children grouped for read<strong>in</strong>g? _______________________________________________________________________________________7. From what neighborhood or area do most of the children <strong>in</strong> thisclass come?_____________________________________________________________________8. <strong>How</strong> do you decide when <strong>and</strong> how word recognition skills <strong>and</strong>vocabulary are taught <strong>to</strong> each child? ____________________________________________________________________________________________________Good idea;parents shouldbe able <strong>to</strong>judge“motivation<strong>to</strong> read.”Identify theresearchmethod <strong>to</strong>be used.GoodideaGoodGoodIf it were feasible, an excellent <strong>in</strong>strument would be a parentquestionnaire. The purpose of it would be <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e how much thechild reads at home, his general attitude <strong>to</strong>ward read<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>and</strong> anychanges <strong>in</strong> his attitude the parent has noticed.ProceduresS<strong>in</strong>ce the sample of one hundred classrooms is large <strong>and</strong> eachclassroom will need <strong>to</strong> be visited at least three times for thirty m<strong>in</strong>utes<strong>to</strong> one hour dur<strong>in</strong>g each visit on different weeks, quite a largeteam of observers—probably around twenty—will be needed. Theywill work <strong>in</strong> pairs observ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dependently. They will spend abou<strong>to</strong>ne-half hour each visit on the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale. The visits should takeplace between Monday <strong>and</strong> Thursday, s<strong>in</strong>ce activities <strong>and</strong> attitudesare often different on Fridays. The <strong>in</strong>vestigation will not beg<strong>in</strong> untilafter school has been <strong>in</strong> session for at least six weeks so that allprograms have had sufficient time <strong>to</strong> function smoothly.Control of extraneous variables: Sources of extraneous variablesmight <strong>in</strong>clude that teachers us<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualized read<strong>in</strong>g might be themore skillful <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>novative teachers. Also, <strong>in</strong> cases where the <strong>in</strong>dividualizedread<strong>in</strong>g program is a new one, teacher enthusiasm for thenew program might carry over <strong>to</strong> students. In this case it might bethe novelty of the approach <strong>and</strong> teacher enthusiasm rather than theprogram itself that is motivat<strong>in</strong>g. An attempt will be made <strong>to</strong> deter-


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 637You shoulddelete this.Isn’t it likely thatall classroomswould be affectedthe same?Further, it seemsunlikely that yoursecond variable(<strong>in</strong>dividualization)would be affected.If so, it’s noproblem so far as<strong>in</strong>ternal validityis concerned.Delete. This is<strong>in</strong>correct. Doyou see why?m<strong>in</strong>e if there is a relationship between novelty <strong>and</strong> teacher enthusiasm<strong>and</strong> student motivation by correlat<strong>in</strong>g the results of the teacherquestionnaire (show<strong>in</strong>g newness of program <strong>and</strong> teacher preference ofprogram), <strong>in</strong>dications from questions on student questionnaire, <strong>and</strong>statistics on motivation <strong>in</strong> a scatterplot. The <strong>in</strong>fluence of studentsocioeconomic levels on motivation will be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by compar<strong>in</strong>gthe answers <strong>to</strong> the question on the teacher questionnaire concern<strong>in</strong>gwhat area or neighborhood children live <strong>in</strong>, the question on parentaloccupations on the student questionnaire with student motivation. Theamount <strong>and</strong> availability of materials may <strong>in</strong>fluence motivation also.This <strong>in</strong>fluence will be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the answers of teachers concern<strong>in</strong>gwhere <strong>and</strong> how they get materials.“relate <strong>to</strong>”The presence of observers <strong>in</strong> the classroom may cause distraction<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluence the degree of motivation. By hav<strong>in</strong>g observers repeatprocedures three or more times, later observations may prove <strong>to</strong> benearly without this procedure bias. By keep<strong>in</strong>g observers <strong>in</strong> the darkabout the purpose of the study, it is hopeful that will control as muchbias <strong>in</strong> their observations <strong>and</strong> question-ask<strong>in</strong>g as possible. Good idea. <strong>How</strong>ever, s<strong>in</strong>cethey both observe (<strong>in</strong>dividualization) <strong>and</strong> adm<strong>in</strong>ister your questionnaire (motivation)they may well figure out the hypothesis. If there is concern that this “awareness”Data Analysis could <strong>in</strong>fluence their rat<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong>/or adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the questionnaire, it wouldObservations on the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale <strong>and</strong> answers on the questionnaireswill be given number rat<strong>in</strong>gs accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> the degree of <strong>in</strong>di-each <strong>in</strong>strumentbe preferable <strong>to</strong> havevidualization <strong>and</strong> amount of motivation respectively. The average of adm<strong>in</strong>istered bythe <strong>to</strong>tal rat<strong>in</strong>gs will then be averaged for the two observers on the different persons.rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, <strong>and</strong> the average of the <strong>to</strong>tal rat<strong>in</strong>gs will be averagedfor the questionnaires <strong>in</strong> each classroom <strong>to</strong> be used on a scatterplot<strong>to</strong> show the relationship between motivation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividualization <strong>in</strong>each classroom. Results of the teacher questionnaire will be comparedsimilarly with motivation on the scatterplot. The correlationwill be used <strong>to</strong> further <strong>in</strong>dicate relationships.PILOT STUDYThis section does a good job of identify<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> attempt<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>control variables likely <strong>to</strong> be detrimental <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.ProcedureThe pilot study was conducted <strong>in</strong> three primary grade schools <strong>in</strong>San Francisco. The pr<strong>in</strong>cipals of each school were contacted <strong>and</strong> wereasked if one or two read<strong>in</strong>g classes could be observed by the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rfor an hour or less. The pr<strong>in</strong>cipals chose the classrooms observed.About forty-five m<strong>in</strong>utes was spent <strong>in</strong> each of four thirdgradeclassrooms. No fourth grades were available <strong>in</strong> these schools.GoodWill you useall of theobservations?OK but couldbe clearerBetter <strong>to</strong> use term“relationship,” s<strong>in</strong>cewe aren’t sure aboutcausality, which isimplied by the word“<strong>in</strong>fluence”GoodGood, but howwill <strong>in</strong>formationbe scored?But teacherquestions lackcontent validity as<strong>in</strong>dica<strong>to</strong>rs of“motivation.”Items 6 <strong>and</strong> 8 cancheck “<strong>in</strong>dividualization,”however.


638 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eRoom#1#2#3#4Individualization1.42.13.03.2ScatterplotMotivation1.31.61.81.71.9Low Motivation High1.71.51.31.11.4 1.8 2.2 2.6 3.0 3.4Low Individualization HighThe <strong>in</strong>struments adm<strong>in</strong>istered were the student questionnaire<strong>and</strong> the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale.Both the questionnaire <strong>and</strong> rat<strong>in</strong>g scale were coded by school <strong>and</strong>by classroom so that the variables for each classroom might be compared.The rat<strong>in</strong>gs on the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale for each classroom were added<strong>to</strong>gether then averaged. Answers on items for the questionnaire wererated “1” for answers <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g low motivation <strong>and</strong> “2” for answers<strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g high motivation. (Note: Some items had as their purpose <strong>to</strong>test validity of rat<strong>in</strong>g scale or <strong>to</strong> provide data concern<strong>in</strong>g possiblebiases, so these items were not rated.) Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether answers


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 639<strong>in</strong>dicated high or low motivation created no problem except on Item #1.It was decided that fewer than eight books (two books per week) read <strong>in</strong>the past month <strong>in</strong>dicated low motivation, while more <strong>in</strong>dicated high motivation.The rat<strong>in</strong>gs for these questions were then added <strong>and</strong> averaged.Then these averaged numbers for all the questionnaires <strong>in</strong> each classroomwere averaged. The results were as follows:Although this pilot study could not possibly be said <strong>to</strong> uphold ordisprove the hypothesis, we might venture <strong>to</strong> say that if the actualstudy were <strong>to</strong> yield results similar <strong>to</strong> those shown on the graph,there would be a strong correlation (estimate: r = .90) between <strong>in</strong>dividualization<strong>and</strong> motivation. This correlation is much <strong>to</strong>o high <strong>to</strong> beattributed <strong>to</strong> chance with a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 100 classrooms. Ifthese were the results of the study described <strong>in</strong> the research proposal,the hypothesis would seem <strong>to</strong> be upheld.GoodobservationIndicationsUnfortunately, I was unable <strong>to</strong> conduct the pilot study <strong>in</strong> anyfourth-grade classrooms which immediately throws doubt upon thevalidity of the results. In adm<strong>in</strong>ister<strong>in</strong>g the student questionnaire, Idiscovered that many of the third-graders had difficulty underst<strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe questions. Therefore, the question<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>ok the form of <strong>in</strong>dividualstructured <strong>in</strong>terviews. Whether or not this difficulty would holdfor fourth-graders, <strong>to</strong>o, would need <strong>to</strong> be determ<strong>in</strong>ed by conduct<strong>in</strong>g amore extensive pilot study <strong>in</strong> fourth-grade classrooms.It was also discovered that Item #7 <strong>in</strong> the rat<strong>in</strong>g scale was difficult<strong>to</strong> rate. Perhaps it should be divided <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two separate items—one concern<strong>in</strong>g desk arrangement <strong>and</strong> one on the presence of a read<strong>in</strong>garea—<strong>and</strong> worded more clearly.Item #8 on the student questionnaire seemed <strong>to</strong> provide someproblems for children. Third-graders, at least, didn’t seem <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>the <strong>in</strong>tent of the question. There is also some uncerta<strong>in</strong>ty as <strong>to</strong>whether the answers on Item #15 reflected the students’ true feel<strong>in</strong>gs.S<strong>in</strong>ce it was adm<strong>in</strong>istered orally, students were probablyreluctant <strong>to</strong> answer negatively about the test <strong>to</strong> the adm<strong>in</strong>istra<strong>to</strong>r ofthe test. Aga<strong>in</strong>, a more extensive pilot study would be helpful <strong>in</strong>determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g if these <strong>in</strong>dications are typical.Although the results of the pilot study are not very valid due <strong>to</strong>its size <strong>and</strong> the circumstances, its value lies <strong>in</strong> the knowledge ga<strong>in</strong>edconcern<strong>in</strong>g specific items <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>and</strong> problems that can beanticipated for observers or participants <strong>in</strong> similar studies.RightRightRight


640 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eGo back <strong>to</strong> the INTERACTIVE AND APPLIED LEARNING feature at thebeg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of the chapter for a list<strong>in</strong>g of <strong>in</strong>teractive <strong>and</strong> applied activities. Go <strong>to</strong> theOnl<strong>in</strong>e Learn<strong>in</strong>g Center at www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e <strong>to</strong> take quizzes,practice with key terms, <strong>and</strong> review chapter content.Ma<strong>in</strong> Po<strong>in</strong>tsRESEARCH PROPOSAL VERSUS RESEARCH REPORT• A research proposal communicates a researcher’s plan for a study.• A research report communicates what was actually done <strong>in</strong> a study <strong>and</strong> whatresulted.MAJOR SECTIONS OF A RESEARCH PROPOSAL OR REPORT• The ma<strong>in</strong> body is the largest section of a proposal or a report <strong>and</strong> generally <strong>in</strong>cludesthe problem <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>vestigated (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the statement of the problem or question,the research hypotheses <strong>and</strong> variables, <strong>and</strong> the def<strong>in</strong>ition of terms); the review of theliterature; the procedures (<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g a description of the sample, the <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>to</strong>be used, the research design, <strong>and</strong> the procedures <strong>to</strong> be followed; an identification ofthreats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity; a description <strong>and</strong> a justification of the statistical proceduresused); <strong>and</strong> (<strong>in</strong> a proposal) a budget of expected costs.• All sections of a research proposal or a research report should be consistent with oneanother.SECTIONS UNIQUE TO RESEARCH REPORTS• The essential difference between a research proposal <strong>and</strong> a research report is that aresearch report states what was done rather than what will be done <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>cludes theactual results of the study. Thus, <strong>in</strong> a report, a description of the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs pert<strong>in</strong>ent<strong>to</strong> each of the research hypotheses or questions is presented, along with a discussionof what the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of the study imply for overall knowledge <strong>and</strong> currentpractice.• Normally, the f<strong>in</strong>al section of a report is the offer<strong>in</strong>g of some suggestions for furtherresearch.For Review1. Review the problem sheets that you have completed <strong>to</strong> see how they correspond <strong>to</strong>the suggestions made <strong>in</strong> this chapter.2. Review any or all of the critiques of studies <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the chapters on quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative research <strong>to</strong> see how they correspond <strong>to</strong> the suggestions made <strong>in</strong> thischapter.


CHAPTER 25 Prepar<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports 641abstract 625discussion 626f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs 625hypothesis 619justification (of astudy) 618literature review 620pilot study 627procedures 621purpose (of astudy) 617research design 621research proposal 617research report 617results (of a study) 625sample 621Key Terms1. To what extent should a researcher allow his or her personal writ<strong>in</strong>g style <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>fluencethe head<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong> organizational sequence <strong>in</strong> a research proposal (assum<strong>in</strong>g thatthere is no m<strong>and</strong>a<strong>to</strong>ry format prescribed by, for example, a fund<strong>in</strong>g agency)?2. To what common function do the problem statement, the research question, <strong>and</strong> thehypotheses all contribute? In what ways are they different?3. When <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>rs of <strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>to</strong>ry research courses evaluate research proposals ofstudents, they sometimes f<strong>in</strong>d logical <strong>in</strong>consistencies among the various parts. Whatdo you th<strong>in</strong>k are the most commonly found <strong>in</strong>consistencies?4. Why is it especially important <strong>in</strong> a study <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g a convenience sample <strong>to</strong> providea detailed description of the characteristics of the sample <strong>in</strong> the research report?Would this be necessary for a r<strong>and</strong>om sample as well? Expla<strong>in</strong>.5. Why is it important for a researcher <strong>to</strong> discuss threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong> (a) a researchproposal <strong>and</strong> (b) a research report?6. Often researchers do not describe their samples <strong>in</strong> detail <strong>in</strong> research reports. Why doyou suppose this is so?For Discussion1. J. E. Johnson (1988). Bedtime rout<strong>in</strong>es: Do they <strong>in</strong>fluence the sleep of elderly women? Journal of AppliedGeron<strong>to</strong>logy, 7: 97–110.2. D. A. Stiles, J. L. Gibbons, <strong>and</strong> J. Schnellman (1987). The smil<strong>in</strong>g sunbather <strong>and</strong> the chivalrousfootball player: Young adolescents’ images of the ideal woman <strong>and</strong> man. Journal of Early Adolescence,7: 411–427.3. L. M. Coleman, L. Jussim, <strong>and</strong> J. Abraham (1987). Students’ reactions <strong>to</strong> teachers’ evaluations: The uniqueimpact of negative feedback. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17: 1051–1070.4. L. S. Levstik (1986). The relationship between his<strong>to</strong>rical response <strong>and</strong> narrative <strong>in</strong> a sixth-grade classroom.Theory <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social <strong>Education</strong>, 14(1): 1–19. Repr<strong>in</strong>ted with permission of the National Council forthe Social Studies <strong>and</strong> the author.5. M. Boiv<strong>in</strong> <strong>and</strong> S. Hymel (1997). Peer experiences <strong>and</strong> social self-perceptions: A sequential model. DevelopmentalPsychology, 33: 135–143.6. June A. Gordon (1994). Why students of color are not enter<strong>in</strong>g teach<strong>in</strong>g: Reflections from m<strong>in</strong>ority teachers.Journal of Teacher <strong>Education</strong>, 45(1994): 220–227.7. Amy Wrzesniewski et al. (1997). Jobs, careers, <strong>and</strong> call<strong>in</strong>gs: People’s relations <strong>to</strong> their work. Journal of<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Personality, 31(1): 21–31.8. C. H. Sacks <strong>and</strong> J. R. Mergendoller (1997, W<strong>in</strong>ter). The relationship between teachers’ theoretical orientation<strong>to</strong>ward read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> student outcomes <strong>in</strong> k<strong>in</strong>dergarten children with different <strong>in</strong>itial read<strong>in</strong>g abilities.American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 34(4): 722–723.9. B. H. Mann<strong>in</strong>g (1988). Application of cognitive behavior modification: First <strong>and</strong> third graders’ selfmanagemen<strong>to</strong>f classroom behaviors. American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong> Journal, 25(2): 194.10. Anthony D. Norman et al. (1998). Moral reason<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> religious belief: Does content <strong>in</strong>fluence structure.Journal of Moral Reason<strong>in</strong>g, 27(1): 140–149.Notes


642 PART 9 Writ<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Proposals <strong>and</strong> Reports www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e11. Donna Bee-Gates et al. (1996). Help-seek<strong>in</strong>g behavior of Native American Indian high school students.Professional Psychology: <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Practice, 27: 495–499.12. Ibid.13. Gordon, op cit., pp. 220–227.14. Jack R. Fraenkel (1994). A portrait of four social studies teachers <strong>and</strong> their classes. In Dennis S. Tierney(ed.), 1994 yearbook of California education research. San Francisco: Caddo Gap Press, pp. 89–115.15. Judith R. Lampe, Gene R. Rooze, <strong>and</strong> Mary Tallent-Runnels (1996). Effects of cooperative learn<strong>in</strong>gamong Hispanic students <strong>in</strong> elementary social studies. Journal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 89: 187–191.16. Wayne E. Herman (1997). The relationship between time <strong>to</strong> completion <strong>and</strong> achievement on multiplechoiceitems. Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>and</strong> Development <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>, 30(2): 113–117.


AppendixesAPPENDIX A Portion of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>om Numbers A-2APPENDIX BSelected Values from a NormalCurve Table A-3APPENDIX C Chi-Square Distribution A-4APPENDIX D Us<strong>in</strong>g SPSS A-5A-1


APPENDIX APortion of a Table of R<strong>and</strong>om Numbers(a) (b) (c) (d) (e) (f) (g) (h) (i)83579 52978 49372 01577 62244 99947 76797 83365 0117251262 63969 56664 09946 78523 11984 54415 37641 0788905033 82862 53894 93440 24273 51621 04425 69084 5467102490 75667 67349 68029 00816 38027 91829 22524 6840351921 92986 09541 58867 09215 97495 04766 06763 8634131822 36187 57320 31877 91945 05078 76579 36364 5932640052 03394 79705 51593 29666 35193 85349 32757 0424335787 11263 95893 90361 89136 44024 92018 48831 8207210454 43051 22114 54648 40380 72727 06963 14497 1150609985 08854 74599 79240 80442 59447 83938 23467 4041357228 04256 76666 95735 40823 82351 95202 87848 8527504688 70407 89116 52789 47972 89447 15473 04439 1825530583 58010 55623 94680 16836 63488 36535 67533 1297273148 81884 16675 01089 81893 24114 30561 02549 6461872280 99756 57467 20870 16403 43892 10905 57466 3919478687 43717 38608 31741 07852 69138 58506 73982 3079186888 98939 58315 39570 73566 24282 48561 60536 3588529997 40384 81495 70526 28454 43466 81123 06094 3042921117 13086 01433 86098 13543 33601 09775 13204 7093450925 78963 28625 89395 81208 90784 73141 67076 5898663196 86512 67980 97084 36547 99414 39246 68880 7978754769 30950 75436 59398 77292 17629 21087 08223 9779469625 49952 65892 02302 50086 48199 21762 84309 5380894464 86584 34365 83368 87733 93495 50205 94569 2948452308 20863 05546 81939 96643 07580 28322 22357 59502A-2


APPENDIX BSelected Values from a Normal Curve TableColumn A lists the z-score values. Column B providesthe proportion of area between the mean <strong>and</strong> the z-scorevalue. Column C provides the proportion of area beyondthe z score.BCzMeanMeanNote: Because the normal distribution is symmetrical, areas for negative z-scores are the same as those for positive z-scores.z(A) (B) (C) (A) (B) (C)Area BetweenArea Betweenz Mean <strong>and</strong> z Area Beyond z z Mean <strong>and</strong> z Area Beyond z0.00 .0000 .50000.10 .0398 .46020.20 .0793 .42070.30 .1179 .38210.40 .1554 .34460.50 .1915 .30850.60 .2257 .27430.70 .2580 .24200.80 .2881 .21190.90 .3159 .18411.00 .3413 .15871.10 .3643 .13571.20 .3849 .11511.30 .4032 .09681.40 .4192 .08081.50 .4332 .06681.65 .4505 .04951.70 .4554 .04461.80 .4641 .03591.90 .4713 .02871.96 .4750 .02502.00 .4772 .02282.10 .4821 .01792.20 .4861 .01392.30 .4893 .01072.40 .4918 .00822.50 .4938 .00622.58 .4951 .00492.60 .4953 .00472.70 .4965 .00352.80 .4974 .00262.90 .4981 .00193.00 .4987 .00133.10 .4990 .00103.20 .4993 .00073.30 .4995 .00053.40 .4997 .00033.50 .4998 .00023.60 .4998 .00023.70 .4999 .00013.80 .49993 .000073.90 .49995 .000054.00 .49997 .00003From Table II of R. A. Fisher <strong>and</strong> F. Yates. Statistical tables for biological, agricultural, <strong>and</strong> medical research. London: Longman Group Ltd. (previously published byOliver & Boyd Ltd., Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the authors <strong>and</strong> publishers.A-3


APPENDIX CChi-Square DistributionThe table entries are critical values of χ 2 .Critical regionA-4Degrees ofProportion <strong>in</strong> Critical RegionFreedom(df) 0.10 0.05 0.025 0.01 0.0051 2.71 3.84 5.02 6.63 7.882 4.61 5.99 7.38 9.21 10.603 6.25 7.81 9.35 11.34 12.844 7.78 9.49 11.14 13.28 14.865 9.24 11.07 12.83 15.09 16.756 10.64 12.59 14.45 16.81 18.557 12.02 14.07 16.01 18.48 20.288 13.36 15.51 17.53 20.09 21.969 14.68 16.92 19.02 21.67 23.5910 15.99 18.31 20.48 23.21 25.1911 17.28 19.68 21.92 24.72 26.7612 18.55 21.03 23.34 26.22 28.3013 19.81 22.36 24.74 27.69 29.8214 21.06 23.68 26.12 29.14 31.3215 22.31 25.00 27.49 30.58 32.8016 23.54 26.30 28.85 32.00 34.2717 24.77 27.59 30.19 33.41 35.7218 25.99 28.87 31.53 34.81 37.1619 27.20 30.14 32.85 36.19 38.5820 28.41 31.41 34.17 37.57 40.0021 29.62 32.67 35.48 38.93 41.4022 30.81 33.92 36.78 40.29 42.8023 32.01 35.17 38.08 41.64 44.1824 33.20 36.42 39.36 42.98 45.5625 34.38 37.65 40.65 44.31 46.9326 35.56 38.89 41.92 45.64 48.2927 36.74 40.11 43.19 46.96 49.6428 37.92 41.34 44.46 48.28 50.9929 39.09 42.56 45.72 49.59 52.3430 40.26 43.77 46.98 50.89 53.6740 51.81 55.76 59.34 63.69 66.7750 63.17 67.50 71.42 76.15 79.4960 74.40 79.08 83.30 88.38 91.9570 85.53 90.53 95.02 100.42 104.2280 96.58 101.88 106.63 112.33 116.3290 107.56 113.14 118.14 124.12 128.30100 118.50 124.34 129.56 135.81 140.17From Table VII (abridged) of R. A. Fisher <strong>and</strong> F. Yates. Statistical tables for biological, agricultural, <strong>and</strong> medical research. London: Longman Group Ltd. (previouslypublished by Oliver & Boyd Ltd., Ed<strong>in</strong>burgh). Repr<strong>in</strong>ted by permission of the authors <strong>and</strong> publishers.


APPENDIX DUs<strong>in</strong>g SPSS*INTRODUCTIONSPSS is a powerful statistical program that can be used<strong>to</strong> perform a variety of statistical procedures. Like anysuch program, there are a number of techniques that youneed <strong>to</strong> master <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> use the program correctly <strong>and</strong>efficiently, but they are not difficult <strong>to</strong> learn. In this appendix,we shall provide you with a few worked-out,step-by-step examples that will enable you <strong>to</strong> see howthe program works, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> turn, use the program yourself.You will learn not only how <strong>to</strong> run some basicanalyses, but also <strong>to</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret the outputgenerated by the program. We used SPSS Version 15.0for W<strong>in</strong>dows for all examples <strong>and</strong> illustrations, butthere is also a version of SPSS that is compatible withMac<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>sh computers.GETTING STARTEDSPSS startup procedures differ slightly, depend<strong>in</strong>g onhow the program was <strong>in</strong>stalled. On most computers,the program is started by click<strong>in</strong>g on the SPSS iconor by choos<strong>in</strong>g it from a menu of options. The programshould then open au<strong>to</strong>matically with a blank dataw<strong>in</strong>dow, entitled SPSS Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r, that looks likeFigure D-1. 1In the second l<strong>in</strong>e at the <strong>to</strong>p of the screen, you willsee the words File, Edit, View, <strong>and</strong> so forth (this l<strong>in</strong>e iscalled the menu bar). Click<strong>in</strong>g on any of the words willproduce a pull-down menu of options that you canchoose <strong>to</strong> perform certa<strong>in</strong> tasks (we will show yousome examples a bit later). Most of the screen, however,is taken up by several cells for enter<strong>in</strong>g data ordisplay<strong>in</strong>g results.*Thanks <strong>to</strong> Mo<strong>in</strong> Syed at San Francisco State University for his helpon this material.1 When SPSS starts up, you may f<strong>in</strong>d that the screen shown <strong>in</strong> FigureD-1 is partially obscured by a small dialogue box that asks “What doyou want <strong>to</strong> do?” <strong>and</strong> which lists several options. If this happens,choose the option “Type <strong>in</strong> data,” then click OK.ENTERING DATAThe start<strong>in</strong>g po<strong>in</strong>t for everyth<strong>in</strong>g you do <strong>in</strong> SPSS isthe Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r (Figure D-1). As you can see, it is aspreadsheet <strong>in</strong> which you enter your data <strong>and</strong> specifythe names of the variables you <strong>in</strong>tend <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>clude <strong>in</strong> youranalysis. As with other spreadsheets, data are entered<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a matrix where the rows represent the names of <strong>in</strong>dividuals(or whatever entities are be<strong>in</strong>g measured), <strong>and</strong>the columns represent different variables (i.e., the variouscharacteristics about those <strong>in</strong>dividuals or entitiesthat are be<strong>in</strong>g measured).Here is an example <strong>to</strong> illustrate how <strong>to</strong> enter data<strong>and</strong> name variables. Imag<strong>in</strong>e that we have collected thefollow<strong>in</strong>g quiz scores for five studentsStudent Gender Quiz Score1 1 882 1 943 1 794 2 855 2 91Enter<strong>in</strong>g the data is quite simple. Highlight the upperleftcell (i.e., row 1, column 1) by click<strong>in</strong>g on it, <strong>and</strong> thentype <strong>in</strong> a “1” <strong>to</strong> represent the first student’s identificationnumber. Then hit Enter. The numeral “1” will appear <strong>in</strong>sidethe highlighted cell. Next, move one cell <strong>to</strong> the right<strong>and</strong> click on it, <strong>and</strong> enter “1” aga<strong>in</strong> (which represents thestudent’s gender). Then, move right one cell aga<strong>in</strong>, <strong>and</strong>enter <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> this cell the student’s quiz score (88). Thiscompletes the first row of data entry.Now, move <strong>to</strong> the second row of cells <strong>and</strong> enterthe values for the second student <strong>in</strong> the appropriatecolumns just as you did for the first student. Repeat thisprocedure until the data for all five students is entered.When you are f<strong>in</strong>ished, the screen should look like that<strong>in</strong> Figure D-2.NAMING VARIABLESOnce you have entered your data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> SPSS, you need<strong>to</strong> tell SPSS what names <strong>to</strong> assign <strong>to</strong> your variables soA-5


A-6 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure D-1 SPSS Data Edi<strong>to</strong>rthat you can refer <strong>to</strong> them later once you have decidedwhat analysis you want <strong>to</strong> have SPSS conduct. The onlylimitation here is that variable names must be between1 <strong>and</strong> 8 characters <strong>in</strong> length. They may be any comb<strong>in</strong>ationof letters <strong>and</strong> numbers, although they must beg<strong>in</strong>with a letter.As you can see <strong>in</strong> Figure D-2, we have three columnsof data, represent<strong>in</strong>g each student’s identification number,gender, <strong>and</strong> quiz score, but the columns are notnamed yet. Now, click on “Variable View” <strong>in</strong> the bot<strong>to</strong>mleft corner of the screen. A new screen will open up thatlooks like Figure D-3.As you can see, each variable is represented by a rowconta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g various bits of <strong>in</strong>formation about the variable.At this po<strong>in</strong>t, all we want <strong>to</strong> do is change the namesof the variables. You can see that the first variable isnamed “VAR00001.” This is the name SPSS assigns <strong>to</strong>the variable by default. To change this name, just clickon VAR00001, <strong>and</strong> type <strong>in</strong> the name you wish <strong>to</strong> assignthe variable, <strong>in</strong> this case, “student.” Next, move down<strong>to</strong> the second row <strong>and</strong> change “VAR00002” <strong>to</strong> “gender,”<strong>and</strong> then <strong>to</strong> the third row <strong>to</strong> change “VAR00003”<strong>to</strong> “score.” Now, click on “Data View” <strong>in</strong> the bot<strong>to</strong>mleft corner of the screen <strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> the screen conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gthe actual data values. Your screen should now looklike Figure D-4.SPECIFYING ANALYSESOnce the data have been entered <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r<strong>and</strong> your variables named, you are ready <strong>to</strong> tell SPSSwhat you want the program <strong>to</strong> do—that is, what type ofstatistical analysis you want SPSS <strong>to</strong> conduct. The procedureis really quite easy.


APPENDIXES Appendix D A-7Figure D-2 Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r W<strong>in</strong>dow with DataFirst, click on Analyze on the menu bar. A pull-downmenu will appear, list<strong>in</strong>g various types of statistical proceduresfrom which <strong>to</strong> choose. Click<strong>in</strong>g on any of theseoptions will produce yet another menu of options. Forexample, click<strong>in</strong>g on Descriptive Statistics will producea list of options, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Frequencies . . . <strong>and</strong> Descriptives. . . , among others, as shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-5. (Click<strong>in</strong>gon Compare Means would produce a list that would<strong>in</strong>clude Independent Samples T Test, <strong>and</strong> One-WayANOVA among others.)Click<strong>in</strong>g on one of these options (e.g., CompareMeans) will produce a dialog box where you can specifythe details of the analysis you have <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d. Hereyou would specify the names of the variables you wish<strong>to</strong> use <strong>in</strong> the analysis, any details about how the analysisis <strong>to</strong> be conducted, <strong>and</strong> what <strong>in</strong>formation you wish<strong>to</strong> have <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the output. Once all these choiceshave been made, click OK <strong>and</strong> SPSS will do the restfor you. That’s all there is <strong>to</strong> it. So, let’s look at someexamples.OBTAINING A FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTIONAND SOME DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICSTable 1 shows the scores for a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of thirtystudents chosen (from all of the statistics students at alarge University) <strong>to</strong> take a specially designed statisticsexam, along with their student identification number <strong>and</strong>gender (1 male, 2 female). Let us obta<strong>in</strong> some descriptivestatistics for the variables gender <strong>and</strong> score.First, enter the data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the first three columns of theData Edi<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> label the variables student, gender, <strong>and</strong>


A-8 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure D-3 Variable View Screenscore. Next, click on Analyze on the menu bar. When thepull-down menu appears, click on Descriptive Statistics.This will produce another menu (See Figure D-5).Click on Frequencies. This will produce a dialog box on<strong>to</strong>p of the other w<strong>in</strong>dows that looks like Figure D-6.Most of the dialog boxes <strong>in</strong> SPSS for W<strong>in</strong>dows willbe similar <strong>to</strong> the one shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-6 <strong>in</strong> severalrespects. On the left will be a box conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the namesof all of the variables you have specified <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded<strong>in</strong> the analysis. To the right will be an empty boxlabeled “Variables.” What we want <strong>to</strong> do is <strong>to</strong> move <strong>to</strong>the right box (from the left box) the names of the variable(s)for which we want a frequency distribution. Wedo this by click<strong>in</strong>g on the appropriate variable name <strong>to</strong>highlight it, <strong>and</strong> then click<strong>in</strong>g on the right-arrow but<strong>to</strong>n(located between the boxes) <strong>to</strong> move it. You repeat thisprocedure for each desired variable. In this <strong>in</strong>stance, wewant <strong>to</strong> select gender <strong>and</strong> score, which are our twovariables of <strong>in</strong>terest. Then click on OK <strong>and</strong> SPSS willrun the analysis.Descriptive Statistics. Should you desire <strong>to</strong> haveSPSS pr<strong>in</strong>t out some graphs or descriptive statistics <strong>in</strong>addition <strong>to</strong> the frequency distribution, however, do notclick on OK just yet. Look at the bot<strong>to</strong>m of the dialoguebox shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-6. There you will see threeother but<strong>to</strong>ns, labeled Statistics . . . , Charts . . . , <strong>and</strong>Format. . . . To obta<strong>in</strong> descriptive statistics <strong>in</strong> addition<strong>to</strong> the frequency distribution, click on Statistics . . .<strong>and</strong> a new dialog box will appear. This is shown <strong>in</strong>Figure D-7.As you can see, a variety of descriptive statistics arelisted. Simply click on the box <strong>to</strong> the left of each statisticthat you want SPSS <strong>to</strong> calculate for you. Let us selectall of the boxes under “Central Tendency” <strong>and</strong> also


APPENDIXES Appendix D A-9Figure D-4 Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r W<strong>in</strong>dowthose under “Dispersion.” Next, click on Cont<strong>in</strong>ue <strong>to</strong>return <strong>to</strong> the previous dialog box (Figure D-6) <strong>and</strong> thenOK <strong>to</strong> run the analysis. Table 2 shows the result.His<strong>to</strong>grams <strong>and</strong>/or bar graphs can also be obta<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>addition <strong>to</strong> frequency tables <strong>and</strong> (or <strong>in</strong>stead of) descriptivestatistics by click<strong>in</strong>g on Charts . . . at the bot<strong>to</strong>m ofthe dialog box shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-6. An example isshown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-8.CONDUCTING AN INDEPENDENTSAMPLES t-TESTLet us now conduct an <strong>in</strong>dependent samples t-test on thesame group of students shown <strong>in</strong> Table 1. Let us imag<strong>in</strong>ethat the <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r of this class hypothesizes that the femalestudents <strong>in</strong> the class (who are <strong>in</strong>dicated by thenumeral “2”) will perform differently on the speciallydesigned statistics exam<strong>in</strong>ation from the male students(who are <strong>in</strong>dicated by the numeral “1”). We want <strong>to</strong> testthe null hypothesis that there is no difference <strong>in</strong> studentperformance between the female <strong>and</strong> male students—that is, that the mean difference <strong>in</strong> the population ofstudents from which this sample is drawn is zero. Theresearch hypothesis is that the population means for thetwo groups of students are not equal.Analysis. If we had not already done this, we wouldenter the data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the first three columns of the Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r<strong>and</strong> label the variables student, gender, <strong>and</strong> score.Then we click on Analyze on the menu bar, <strong>and</strong> thenselect Compare Means. When the pull-down menuappears, we select Independent Samples T-Test. . . .This gives us a dialog box that looks like Figure D-9.Once aga<strong>in</strong>, the list of variables appears <strong>in</strong> the box onthe left. We now have <strong>to</strong> do two th<strong>in</strong>gs: (a) move one


A-10 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure D-5 Data View Screen with Pull-down MenuTABLE 1Data for Thirty Students Tak<strong>in</strong>g a Specially <strong>Design</strong>ed Statistics Exam<strong>in</strong>ationStudent123456789101112131415Gender111221111112222Score889479859184687369717783706580Student161718192021222324252627282930Gender222221112211111Score889274648195897363947582878691


APPENDIXES Appendix D A-11(or more) of the variables <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the box labeled “TestVariable(s)” <strong>to</strong> select our dependent variable(s), <strong>and</strong> (b)move one of the variables <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the box labeled “Group<strong>in</strong>gVariable” <strong>to</strong> identify the groups that are <strong>to</strong> becompared (i.e., <strong>to</strong> choose our <strong>in</strong>dependent variable). Wedo this by select<strong>in</strong>g the appropriate variables <strong>and</strong> thenclick<strong>in</strong>g on the correspond<strong>in</strong>g arrows that you see <strong>in</strong> thedialog box. Hence, let us click on score (our dependentvariable) <strong>to</strong> move it <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Test Variable(s)” box, <strong>and</strong>then on gender (our <strong>in</strong>dependent variable) <strong>to</strong> move it<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the “Group<strong>in</strong>g Variable” box.As soon as we select gender as the group<strong>in</strong>g variable,the but<strong>to</strong>n labeled Def<strong>in</strong>e Groups . . . becomes functional,that is, it is no longer fa<strong>in</strong>t, but now becomeFigure D-6 Frequencies Dialog BoxFigure D-7 Statistics Dialog BoxTABLE 2FrequenciesFrequency TableStatisticsGendergenderscoreN Valid 30 30Miss<strong>in</strong>g 0 0Mean 1.4333 80.37667Std. Error of Mean .09202 1.77238Median 1.0000 81.5000Mode 1.00 73.00(a)Std. Deviation .50401 9.70774Variance .254 94.240Range 1.00 32.00M<strong>in</strong>imum 1.00 63.00Maximum 2.00 95.00Sum 43.00 2411.00a. Multiple modes exists. The smallest value is shownCumulativeFrequency Percent Valid Percent PercentValid 1.00 17 56.7 56.7 56.72.00 13 43.3 43.3 100.0Total 30 100.0 100.0(Cont<strong>in</strong>ued)


A-12 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eTABLE 2Cont<strong>in</strong>uedFrequency TableScoreCumulativeFrequency Percent Valid Percent PercentValid 63.00 1 3.3 3.3 3.364.00 1 3.3 3.3 6.765.00 1 3.3 3.3 10.068.00 1 3.3 3.3 13.369.00 1 3.3 3.3 16.770.00 1 3.3 3.3 20.071.00 1 3.3 3.3 23.373.00 2 6.7 6.7 30.074.00 1 3.3 3.3 33.375.00 1 3.3 3.3 36.777.00 1 3.3 3.3 40.079.00 1 3.3 3.3 43.380.00 1 3.3 3.3 46.781.00 1 3.3 3.3 50.082.00 1 3.3 3.3 53.383.00 1 3.3 3.3 56.784.00 1 3.3 3.3 60.085.00 1 3.3 3.3 63.386.00 1 3.3 3.3 66.787.00 1 3.3 3.3 70.088.00 2 6.7 6.7 76.789.00 1 3.3 3.3 80.091.00 2 6.7 6.7 86.792.00 1 3.3 3.3 90.094.00 2 3.3 3.3 96.795.00 1 6.7 6.7 100.0Total 30 100.0 100.0dist<strong>in</strong>ct <strong>and</strong> easy <strong>to</strong> see. Click<strong>in</strong>g on it br<strong>in</strong>gs up anotherdialog box like the one shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-10.In this box, you must <strong>in</strong>dicate the two values of genderthat represent the two groups we want <strong>to</strong> compare, <strong>in</strong> thiscase, females <strong>and</strong> males. We coded them as “1” for males<strong>and</strong> “2” for females, so we type <strong>in</strong> a “1” opposite Group1 <strong>and</strong> a “2” opposite Group 2. We then click on Cont<strong>in</strong>ue<strong>to</strong> return <strong>to</strong> the dialog box shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-9. We clickon OK <strong>and</strong> SPSS will run the analysis.The output produced by SPSS is shown <strong>in</strong> FigureD-11.The first table, labeled Group Statistics, displaysthe sample size, mean, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>arderror of the mean separately for each of our two groups,males (“1.00”) <strong>and</strong> females (“2.00”). The second table,labeled Independent Samples Test, reveals the resultsof the t-test. Notice that there are two rows of <strong>in</strong>formationhere, one labeled Equal variances assumed <strong>and</strong>one labeled Equal variances not assumed. To knowwhich row we should look at, we must turn <strong>to</strong> the columnlabeled Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances.One of the assumptions of the t-test is that the populationvariances from the two groups we are compar<strong>in</strong>gare equal. Levene’s test is a test of this assumption. Thespecifics of this test are beyond the scope of this text,but <strong>in</strong> brief, a statistically significant Levene’s Test


APPENDIXES Appendix D A-134ScoreFigure D-8 His<strong>to</strong>gramof Table D-2 Data3Frequency21060.0070.0080.00Score90.00100.00Mean = 80.37Std. Dev. = 9.708N = 30Figure D-9 Independent Samples t-Test Dialog Box


A-14 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eFigure D-10 Def<strong>in</strong>e Groups Dialog Box<strong>in</strong>dicates that we have violated this assumption, <strong>and</strong> thatthe population variances are not equal. By look<strong>in</strong>g at thecolumn labeled Sig., you can see that the significancelevel for the Levene’s Test is .350, which is considerableygreater than .05, <strong>and</strong> hence not statistically significant.We can conclude, therefore, that the populationvariances of the two groups do not differ significantly<strong>and</strong> that we should look only at the first row labeledEqual variances assumed.You can see that SPSS reports the observed t-value, thedegrees of freedom (“df”), <strong>and</strong> the two-tailed p-value(“Sig. (2-tailed)”). Also reported on this l<strong>in</strong>e are the differencesbetween the means, the st<strong>and</strong>ard error of thedifference, <strong>and</strong> the 95% confidence <strong>in</strong>terval for the differencebetween population means. The observed t-value is.554, which results <strong>in</strong> a probability of .584, with degreesof freedom = 28. S<strong>in</strong>ce this is much greater than the valuerequired at .05, the result is considered <strong>to</strong> be NOT statisticallysignificant at the .05 level.CALCULATING A CORRELATIONLet us suppose that a psychology <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r is <strong>in</strong>terested<strong>in</strong> see<strong>in</strong>g if there is any relationship between a student’sachievement on her quizzes <strong>and</strong> his or her anxiety level.Accord<strong>in</strong>gly, she recruits a r<strong>and</strong>om sample of 30 students<strong>to</strong> participate <strong>in</strong> a study. She measures the anxietylevel of each student <strong>in</strong> the sample (us<strong>in</strong>g a specially designed“anxiety test” she has developed) as well as theirachievement scores on her mid-semester exam<strong>in</strong>ation.The data are shown <strong>in</strong> Table 3.As before, she enters the data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the first threecolumns of the Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r <strong>and</strong> labels the variables student,anxiety, <strong>and</strong> score. She is <strong>in</strong>terested <strong>in</strong> calculat<strong>in</strong>gthe Pearson product-moment correlation between thetwo variables, anxiety <strong>and</strong> score. In addition, she wants<strong>to</strong> test the null hypothesis that the correlation betweenthe variables <strong>in</strong> the population from which the samplewas drawn equals zero.


APPENDIXES Appendix D A-15Group StatisticsGENDERNMeanStd.DeviationStd. ErrorMeanSCORE 1.002.00171381.235379.23088.8213511.023872.139493.05747Independent Samples TestSCORE Equal variancesassumedEqual variancesnot assumedFigure D-11 T-Test ResultsLevene’s Test forEquality of VariancesF.904Sig..350t.554dfSig.(2-tailed)t-test for Equality of MeansMeanDifferenceStd. ErrorDifference95% Confidence Intervalof the DifferenceUpperLower28 .584 2.00452 3.62024 −5.41121 9.42026.537 22.570 .596 2.00452 3.73170 −5.72321 9.73226TABLE 3Student Anxiety Score1 1 24 882 2 36 943 3 40 794 4 31 855 5 50 916 6 32 847 7 30 688 8 28 739 9 36 6910 10 34 7111 11 18 7712 12 36 8313 13 21 7014 14 30 6515 15 40 80Student Anxiety Score16 16 34 8817 17 39 9218 18 35 7419 19 38 6420 20 40 8121 21 35 9522 22 39 8923 23 22 7324 24 20 6325 25 37 9426 26 35 7527 27 29 8228 28 20 8729 29 40 9630 30 30 91Analysis. The <strong>in</strong>struc<strong>to</strong>r clicks on Analyze on themenu bar, <strong>and</strong> then selects Correlate. From the menuthat then appears, she chooses Bivariate . . . This producesthe dialog box shown <strong>in</strong> Figure D-12. As before,the variables <strong>to</strong> be <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the analysis must bemoved from the box on the left <strong>to</strong> the box on the right(under “Variables”). In this case, we want <strong>to</strong> see thecorrelation between “anxiety” <strong>and</strong> “score,” so theseare moved <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> the right box by click<strong>in</strong>g on the righth<strong>and</strong>arrow. She then clicks on OK, <strong>and</strong> SPSS runs the


A-16 APPENDIXES Appendix D www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eanalysis, produc<strong>in</strong>g the results shown <strong>in</strong> Table 4. Asyou can see, the correlation between “anxiety” <strong>and</strong>“score” for this sample of 30 students is .364, significantat the 0.05 level.The examples we have presented here only beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>to</strong>show what SPSS can do. We have not even looked atthe many graph<strong>in</strong>g capabilities that SPSS possesses.Nevertheless, this should give you an idea of whatSPSS can do. We urge you <strong>to</strong> try out SPSS for yourselfso that you can discover the many types of analysesthat the program can deliver, as well as the manygraphs it can create.TABLE 4Correlationsanxiety scoreanxiety Pearson Correlation 1 .364*Sig. (2-tailed) .048N 30 30score Pearson Correlation .364* 1Sig. (2-tailed) .048N 30 30*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).Figure D-12 Data Correlation


GLOSSARY G-2GlossaryA-B design A s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experimental design <strong>in</strong> whichmeasurements are repeatedly made until stability is presumablyestablished (basel<strong>in</strong>e), after which treatment is <strong>in</strong>troduced<strong>and</strong> an appropriate number of measurements are made.A-B-A design Same as an A-B design, except that a secondbasel<strong>in</strong>e is added.A-B-A-B design Same as an A-B-A design, except that a secondtreatment is added.A-B-C-B design Same as an A-B-A-B design, except that thesecond basel<strong>in</strong>e phase is replaced by a modified treatmentphase.abstract A summary of a study that describes its most importantaspects, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g major results <strong>and</strong> conclusions.accessible population The population from which the researchercan realistically select subjects for a sample, <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> which theresearcher is entitled <strong>to</strong> generalize f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.achievement test An <strong>in</strong>strument used <strong>to</strong> measure the proficiencylevel of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> given areas of knowledge or skill.action plan A plan <strong>to</strong> implement change as a result of an actionresearch study.action research A type of research focused on a specific localproblem <strong>and</strong> result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> an action plan <strong>to</strong> address the problem.advocacy lens Exists when the researcher <strong>in</strong>dicates or impliesthat the purpose of the research is <strong>to</strong> improve conditions ofthe participant population.age-equivalent score A score that <strong>in</strong>dicates the age level forwhich a particular performance (score) is typical.alpha coefficient See Cronbach alpha.analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) A statistical technique forequat<strong>in</strong>g groups on one or more variables when test<strong>in</strong>g forstatistical significance; it adjusts scores on a dependentvariable for <strong>in</strong>itial differences on other variables, such aspretest performance or IQ.analysis of variance (ANOVA) A statistical technique fordeterm<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the statistical significance of differences amongmeans; it can be used with two or more groups.aptitude test An ability test used <strong>to</strong> predict performance <strong>in</strong> afuture situation.associational research/study A general type of research <strong>in</strong>which a researcher looks for relationships hav<strong>in</strong>g predictive<strong>and</strong>/or explana<strong>to</strong>ry power. Both correlational <strong>and</strong> causalcomparativestudies are examples.assumption Any important assertion presumed <strong>to</strong> be true but notactually verified; major assumptions should be described <strong>in</strong>one of the first sections of a research proposal or report.average A number represent<strong>in</strong>g the typical score atta<strong>in</strong>ed by agroup of subjects. See measures of central tendency.B-A-B design The same as an A-B-A-B design, except that the<strong>in</strong>itial basel<strong>in</strong>e phase is omitted.background questions Questions asked by an <strong>in</strong>terviewer or ona questionnaire <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation about a respondent’sbackground (age, occupation, etc.).bar graph A graphic way of illustrat<strong>in</strong>g differences.basel<strong>in</strong>e The graphic record of measurements taken prior <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>troduc<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong> a time-series design.behavior questions See experience questions.bias Occurs when the design of a study systematically favorscerta<strong>in</strong> outcomes.biography/biographical study A form of qualitative research <strong>in</strong>which the researcher works with the <strong>in</strong>dividual <strong>to</strong> clarifyimportant life experiences.boxplot A diagram portray<strong>in</strong>g a five-number summary.case study A form of qualitative research <strong>in</strong> which a s<strong>in</strong>gle<strong>in</strong>dividual or example is studied through extensive datacollection.categorical data/variables Data (variables) that differ only <strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>d, not <strong>in</strong> amount or degree.causal-comparative research <strong>Research</strong> <strong>to</strong> explore the causefor, or consequences of, exist<strong>in</strong>g differences <strong>in</strong> groups of<strong>in</strong>dividuals; also referred <strong>to</strong> as ex post fac<strong>to</strong> research.census An attempt <strong>to</strong> acquire data from every member of apopulation.chaos theory A theory <strong>and</strong> methodology of science thatemphasizes the rarity of general laws, the need for very largedata bases, <strong>and</strong> the importance of study<strong>in</strong>g exceptions <strong>to</strong>overall patterns.chi-square test (χ 2 ) Anonparametric test of statistical significanceappropriate when the data are <strong>in</strong> the form of frequency counts; itcompares frequencies actually observed <strong>in</strong> a study with expectedfrequencies <strong>to</strong> see whether they are significantly different.closed-ended question A question <strong>and</strong> a list of alternativeresponses from which the respondent selects; also referred <strong>to</strong>as a closed-form item.cluster sampl<strong>in</strong>g/cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g The selection ofgroups of <strong>in</strong>dividuals, called clusters, rather than s<strong>in</strong>gle<strong>in</strong>dividuals. All <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> a cluster are <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> thesample; the clusters are preferably selected r<strong>and</strong>omly fromthe larger population of clusters.cod<strong>in</strong>g The specification of categories <strong>in</strong> content analysisresearch. It may be done ahead of time or emerge fromfamiliarity with the raw data.cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme A set of categories an observer uses <strong>to</strong> record thefrequency of behaviors.coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation (r 2 ) The square of the correlationcoefficient. It <strong>in</strong>dicates the proportion of variance common <strong>to</strong>two variables.coefficient of multiple correlation (R) A numerical <strong>in</strong>dexdescrib<strong>in</strong>g the relationship between predicted <strong>and</strong> actualG-1


G-2 GLOSSARY www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7escores us<strong>in</strong>g multiple regression. The correlation betweena criterion <strong>and</strong> the “best comb<strong>in</strong>ation” of predic<strong>to</strong>rs.cohort study A design (<strong>in</strong> survey research) <strong>in</strong> which a particularpopulation is studied over time by tak<strong>in</strong>g different r<strong>and</strong>omsamples at various po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time. The population rema<strong>in</strong>sconceptually the same, but <strong>in</strong>dividuals change (for example,graduates of San Francisco State University surveyed 10, 20,<strong>and</strong> 30 years after graduation).comparison group The group <strong>in</strong> a research study that receives adifferent treatment from that of the experimental group.concurrent validity (evidence of) The degree <strong>to</strong> which thescores on an <strong>in</strong>strument are related <strong>to</strong> the scores on another<strong>in</strong>strument adm<strong>in</strong>istered at the same time, or <strong>to</strong> some othercriterion available at the same time.confidence <strong>in</strong>terval An <strong>in</strong>terval used <strong>to</strong> estimate a parameterthat is constructed <strong>in</strong> such a way that the <strong>in</strong>terval has apredeterm<strong>in</strong>ed probability of <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g the parameter.confirm<strong>in</strong>g sample In qualitative research, a sample selected <strong>to</strong>validate or extend previous f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.constant A characteristic that has the same value for all<strong>in</strong>dividuals.constitutive def<strong>in</strong>ition The explanation of the mean<strong>in</strong>g of aterm by us<strong>in</strong>g other words <strong>to</strong> describe what is meant.construct-related validity (evidence of) The degree <strong>to</strong> which an<strong>in</strong>strument measures an <strong>in</strong>tended hypothetical psychologicalconstruct, or nonobservable trait.content analysis A method of study<strong>in</strong>g human behavior<strong>in</strong>directly by analyz<strong>in</strong>g communications, usually through aprocess of categorization.content-related validity (evidence of) The degree <strong>to</strong> which an<strong>in</strong>strument logically appears <strong>to</strong> measure an <strong>in</strong>tended variable;it is determ<strong>in</strong>ed by expert judgment.contextualization Plac<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation/data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> a largerperspective, especially <strong>in</strong> ethnography.cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient An <strong>in</strong>dex of relationship derived from acrossbreak table.cont<strong>in</strong>gency question A question whose answer depends on theanswer <strong>to</strong> a prior question.cont<strong>in</strong>gency table See crossbreak table.control Efforts on the part of the researcher <strong>to</strong> remove theeffects of any variable other than the <strong>in</strong>dependent variablethat might affect performance on a dependent variable.control group The group <strong>in</strong> a research study that is treated“as usual.”convenience sample/sampl<strong>in</strong>g A sample that is easilyaccessible.correlational research <strong>Research</strong> that <strong>in</strong>volves collect<strong>in</strong>g data<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the degree <strong>to</strong> which a relationshipexists between two or more variables.correlation coefficient (r) A decimal number between .00 <strong>and</strong>±1.00 that <strong>in</strong>dicates the degree <strong>to</strong> which two quantitativevariables are related.counterbalanced design An experimental design <strong>in</strong> which allgroups receive all treatments. Each group receives thetreatments <strong>in</strong> a different order, <strong>and</strong> all groups are posttestedafter each treatment.credibility In qualitative research encompasses <strong>in</strong>strumentreliability <strong>and</strong> validity, as well as <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.criterion A second measurement used <strong>to</strong> evaluate <strong>in</strong>strumentvalidity.criterion-referenced <strong>in</strong>strument An <strong>in</strong>strument that specifies aparticular goal, or criterion, for students <strong>to</strong> achieve.criterion-related validity (evidence of) The degree <strong>to</strong> whichperformance on an <strong>in</strong>strument is related <strong>to</strong> performance onother <strong>in</strong>struments <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> measure the same variable, or<strong>to</strong> other variables logically related <strong>to</strong> the variable be<strong>in</strong>gmeasured.criterion variable The variable that is predicted <strong>in</strong> a predictionstudy; also any variable used <strong>to</strong> assess the criterion-relatedvalidity of an <strong>in</strong>strument.critical researchers <strong>Research</strong>ers who raise philosophical <strong>and</strong>ethical questions about the way educational research isconducted.critical sample In qualitative research, a sample considered <strong>to</strong>be enlighten<strong>in</strong>g because it is unusual.Cronbach alpha (α) An <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency or reliabilitycoefficient for an <strong>in</strong>strument requir<strong>in</strong>g only one testadm<strong>in</strong>istration.crossbreak table A table that shows all comb<strong>in</strong>ations of two ormore categorical variables <strong>and</strong> portrays the relationship (ifany) between the variables.cross-sectional survey A survey <strong>in</strong> which data are collectedat one po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> time from a predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed population orpopulations.crystallization Occasions, especially <strong>in</strong> ethnography, whendifferent k<strong>in</strong>ds of data “fall <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> place” <strong>to</strong> make a coherentpicture.culture The sum of a social group’s observable patterns ofbehavior <strong>and</strong>/or their cus<strong>to</strong>ms, beliefs, <strong>and</strong> knowledge.curvil<strong>in</strong>ear relationship A relationship shown <strong>in</strong> a scatterplot<strong>in</strong> which the l<strong>in</strong>e that best fits the po<strong>in</strong>ts is not straight.data Any <strong>in</strong>formation obta<strong>in</strong>ed about a sample or a population.data analysis The process of simplify<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> makeit comprehensible.data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias Un<strong>in</strong>tentional bias on the part of datacollec<strong>to</strong>rs that may create a threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validityof a study.degrees of freedom A number <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g how many <strong>in</strong>stancesout of a given number of <strong>in</strong>stances are “free <strong>to</strong> vary”—thatis, not predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed.demographic questions See background questions.dependent variable A variable affected or expected <strong>to</strong> beaffected by the <strong>in</strong>dependent variable; also called criterion oroutcome variable.derived score A score obta<strong>in</strong>ed from a raw score <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> aid <strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>terpretation. Derived scores provide a quantitative measureof each student’s performance relative <strong>to</strong> a comparison group.descriptive field notes The researchers’ attempt <strong>to</strong> record whatthey observe completely <strong>and</strong> objectively.descriptive research/study <strong>Research</strong> <strong>to</strong> describe exist<strong>in</strong>gconditions without analyz<strong>in</strong>g relationships among variables.descriptive statistics Data analysis techniques that enable theresearcher <strong>to</strong> mean<strong>in</strong>gfully describe data with numerical<strong>in</strong>dices or <strong>in</strong> graphic form.descrip<strong>to</strong>rs Terms used <strong>to</strong> locate sources dur<strong>in</strong>g a computersearch of the literature.design See research design.dicho<strong>to</strong>mous questions Questions that permit only a yes or noanswer.


GLOSSARY G-3directional hypothesis A relational hypothesis stated <strong>in</strong> such amanner that a direction, often <strong>in</strong>dicated by “greater than” or“less than,” is hypothesized for the results.discrim<strong>in</strong>ant function analysis A statistical procedure forpredict<strong>in</strong>g group membership (a categorical variable) fromtwo or more quantitative variables.discussion (of a study) A review of the results <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>glimitations of a study, plac<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a broaderperspective.distribution/distribution curves The real or theoreticalfrequency distribution of a set of scores.document Any written or pr<strong>in</strong>ted material.ecological generalizability The degree <strong>to</strong> which results can begeneralized <strong>to</strong> environments <strong>and</strong> conditions outside theresearch sett<strong>in</strong>g.effect size (ES) An <strong>in</strong>dex used <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the magnitude of anobta<strong>in</strong>ed result or relationship.emic perspective The view of reality of a cultural “<strong>in</strong>sider,”especially <strong>in</strong> ethnography.empirical Based on observable evidence.equivalent forms Two tests identical <strong>in</strong> every way except forthe actual items <strong>in</strong>cluded.errors of measurement Inconsistency of <strong>in</strong>dividual scores onthe same <strong>in</strong>strument.eta () An <strong>in</strong>dex that <strong>in</strong>dicates the degree of a curvil<strong>in</strong>earrelationship.ethnography/ethnographic research The collection of dataon many variables over an extended period of time <strong>in</strong> anaturalistic sett<strong>in</strong>g, usually us<strong>in</strong>g observation <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terviews.etic perspective The “outsider” or “objective” view of aculture’s reality, especially <strong>in</strong> ethnography.EXCEL A computer program for comput<strong>in</strong>g descriptive <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.expectancy table A chart compar<strong>in</strong>g predic<strong>to</strong>r categories withcriterion categories <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> evaluate <strong>in</strong>strument validity.experience questions Questions a researcher asks <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat sorts of th<strong>in</strong>gs an <strong>in</strong>dividual is do<strong>in</strong>g or has done.experiment A research study <strong>in</strong> which one or more <strong>in</strong>dependentvariables is/are systematically varied by the researcher <strong>to</strong>determ<strong>in</strong>e the effects of this variation.experimental group The group <strong>in</strong> a research study that receivesthe treatment (or method) of special <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the study.experimental research <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> which at least one<strong>in</strong>dependent variable is manipulated, other relevant variablesare controlled, <strong>and</strong> the effect on one or more dependentvariables is observed.experimental variable The variable that is manipulated(systematically altered) <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>tervention study by theresearcher.explana<strong>to</strong>ry mixed-methods design A study <strong>in</strong> whichquantitative data are collected first <strong>and</strong> further clarified withqualitative data.explora<strong>to</strong>ry mixed-methods design A study <strong>in</strong> whichqualitative data are collected first <strong>and</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are tested withsubsequent quantitative data.external audit A review of the methods <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretations of aqualitative study by an <strong>in</strong>dividual outside the study.external criticism Evaluation of the genu<strong>in</strong>eness of a document<strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research.external validity The degree <strong>to</strong> which results are generalizable,or applicable, <strong>to</strong> groups <strong>and</strong> environments outside theresearch sett<strong>in</strong>g.extraneous variable A variable that makes possible analternative explanation of results; an uncontrolledvariable.fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis A statistical method for reduc<strong>in</strong>g a set ofvariables <strong>to</strong> a smaller number of fac<strong>to</strong>rs.fac<strong>to</strong>rial design An experimental design that <strong>in</strong>volves twoor more <strong>in</strong>dependent variables (at least one of which ismanipulated) <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> study the effects of the variables<strong>in</strong>dividually, <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>teraction with each other, upon adependent variable.feel<strong>in</strong>gs questions Questions researchers ask <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out howpeople feel about th<strong>in</strong>gs.field diary A personal statement of a researcher’s op<strong>in</strong>ions aboutpeople <strong>and</strong> events he or she comes <strong>in</strong> contact with dur<strong>in</strong>gresearch.field jott<strong>in</strong>gs Quick notes taken by an ethnographer.field log A runn<strong>in</strong>g account of how an ethnographer plans <strong>to</strong>,<strong>and</strong> actually does, spend his or her time <strong>in</strong> the field.field notes The notes researchers take about what they observe<strong>and</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k about <strong>in</strong> the field.f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs See results (of a study).five-number summary A means of describ<strong>in</strong>g a skewedfrequency distribution giv<strong>in</strong>g the lowest, first quartile,median, third quartile, <strong>and</strong> highest score.focus group <strong>in</strong>terview An <strong>in</strong>terview conducted with a group <strong>in</strong>which respondents hear the views of each other.foreshadowed problems The problem or <strong>to</strong>pic that serves, <strong>in</strong> ageneral way, as the focus for a qualitative <strong>in</strong>quiry.frequency distribution A tabular method of show<strong>in</strong>g all of thescores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.frequency polygon A graphic method of show<strong>in</strong>g all of thescores obta<strong>in</strong>ed by a group of <strong>in</strong>dividuals.Friedman two-way analysis of variance A nonparametric<strong>in</strong>ferential statistic used <strong>to</strong> compare two or more groups thatare not <strong>in</strong>dependent.ga<strong>in</strong> score The difference between the pretest <strong>and</strong> posttest scoresof a measure.generaliz<strong>in</strong>g See ecological generalizability; populationgeneralizability.general references Sources that researchers use <strong>to</strong> identify morespecific references (e.g., <strong>in</strong>dexes, abstracts).grade equivalent score A score that <strong>in</strong>dicates the grade level forwhich a particular performance (score) is typical.grounded theory study A form of qualitative research thatderives <strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>in</strong>ductively from raw data withcont<strong>in</strong>ual <strong>in</strong>terplay between data <strong>and</strong> emerg<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terpretations.grouped frequency distribution A frequency distribution <strong>in</strong>which scores are grouped <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> equal <strong>in</strong>tervals.Hawthorne effect A positive effect of an <strong>in</strong>tervention result<strong>in</strong>gfrom the subjects’ knowledge that they are <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> astudy or their feel<strong>in</strong>g that they are <strong>in</strong> some way receiv<strong>in</strong>g“special” attention.


G-4 GLOSSARY www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7ehis<strong>to</strong>gram A graphic representation, consist<strong>in</strong>g of rectangles,of the scores <strong>in</strong> a distribution; the height of each rectangle<strong>in</strong>dicates the frequency of each score or group of scores.his<strong>to</strong>rical research The systematic collection <strong>and</strong> objectiveevaluation of data related <strong>to</strong> past occurrences <strong>to</strong> exam<strong>in</strong>ecauses, effects, or trends of those events that may helpexpla<strong>in</strong> present events <strong>and</strong> anticipate future events.his<strong>to</strong>ry threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong> an eventthat is not part of an <strong>in</strong>tervention but that may affectperformance on the dependent variable, thereby affect<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>ternal validity.holistic perspective The attempt <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>corporate all aspects of aculture <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> an ethnographic <strong>in</strong>terpretation.homogeneous sample In qualitative research, a sample selected<strong>in</strong> which all members are similar with respect <strong>to</strong> one or morecharacteristics.hypothesis A tentative, testable assertion regard<strong>in</strong>g theoccurrence of certa<strong>in</strong> behaviors, phenomena, or events; aprediction of study outcomes.implementation threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong>variations <strong>in</strong> the implementation of the treatment <strong>in</strong> an<strong>in</strong>tervention study, thereby affect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.<strong>in</strong>dependent variable A variable that affects (or is presumed<strong>to</strong> affect) the dependent variable under study <strong>and</strong> is<strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong> the research design so that its effect can bedeterm<strong>in</strong>ed; sometimes called the experimental ortreatment variable.<strong>in</strong>dex A general reference that gives the author, title, <strong>and</strong> placeof publication of a published work.<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics Data analysis techniques for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ghow likely it is that results based on a sample or samples aresimilar <strong>to</strong> results that would have been obta<strong>in</strong>ed for the entirepopulation.<strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terviews Less-structured forms of <strong>in</strong>terview, usuallyconducted by qualitative researchers. They do not <strong>in</strong>volveany specific type or sequence of question<strong>in</strong>g, but resemblemore the give-<strong>and</strong>-take of a casual conversation.Institutional Review Board (IRB) A research reviewboard required of all <strong>in</strong>stitutions receiv<strong>in</strong>g federalresearch funds.<strong>in</strong>strument Any device for systematically collect<strong>in</strong>g data, suchas a test, a questionnaire, or an <strong>in</strong>terview schedule.<strong>in</strong>strumental case study Study that focuses on a particular<strong>in</strong>dividual or situation with little effort <strong>to</strong> generalize.<strong>in</strong>strumentation Instruments <strong>and</strong> procedures used <strong>in</strong> collect<strong>in</strong>gdata <strong>in</strong> a study.<strong>in</strong>strumentation threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong>variations <strong>in</strong> the way data are collected, thereby affect<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>ternal validity.<strong>in</strong>strument decay Changes <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>strumentation over time thatmay affect the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of a study.<strong>in</strong>teraction An effect created by unique comb<strong>in</strong>ations of two ormore <strong>in</strong>dependent variables; systematically evaluated <strong>in</strong> afac<strong>to</strong>rial design.<strong>in</strong>ternal-consistency methods Procedures for estimat<strong>in</strong>greliability of scores us<strong>in</strong>g only one adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the<strong>in</strong>strument.<strong>in</strong>ternal criticism Determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g whether the contents of adocument are accurate.<strong>in</strong>ternal validity The degree <strong>to</strong> which observed differences onthe dependent variable are directly related <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable, not <strong>to</strong> some other (uncontrolled) variable.<strong>in</strong>terval scale A measurement scale that, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> order<strong>in</strong>gscores from high <strong>to</strong> low, also establishes a uniform unit <strong>in</strong> thescale so that any equal distance between two scores is ofequal magnitude.<strong>in</strong>tervention study/research A general type of research <strong>in</strong>which variables are manipulated <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> study the effec<strong>to</strong>n one or more dependent variables.<strong>in</strong>terview A form of data collection <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>dividuals orgroups are questioned orally.<strong>in</strong>terview schedule A set of questions <strong>to</strong> be asked by an<strong>in</strong>terviewer.<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic case study A study that attempts <strong>to</strong> generalize beyondthe particular case.justification (of a study) A rationale statement <strong>in</strong> which aresearcher <strong>in</strong>dicates why the study is important <strong>to</strong> conduct;<strong>in</strong>cludes implications for theory <strong>and</strong>/or practice.key ac<strong>to</strong>rs See key <strong>in</strong>formants.key events Events that provide unusually valuable data <strong>in</strong> anethnographic study.key <strong>in</strong>formants Individuals identified as expert sources of<strong>in</strong>formation, especially <strong>in</strong> qualitative research.knowledge questions Questions <strong>in</strong>terviewers ask <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat factual <strong>in</strong>formation a respondent possesses about aparticular <strong>to</strong>pic.Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance Anonparametric <strong>in</strong>ferential statistic used <strong>to</strong> compare twoor more <strong>in</strong>dependent groups for statistical significanceof differences.Kuder-Richardson approaches Procedures for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gan estimate of the <strong>in</strong>ternal consistency reliability of a tes<strong>to</strong>r other <strong>in</strong>strument from a s<strong>in</strong>gle adm<strong>in</strong>istration of the testwithout splitt<strong>in</strong>g the test <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> halves.latent content The underly<strong>in</strong>g mean<strong>in</strong>g of a communication.level of significance The probability that a discrepancy betweena sample statistic <strong>and</strong> a specified population parameter is due<strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, or chance. Commonly used significancelevels <strong>in</strong> educational research are .05 <strong>and</strong> .01.Likert scale A self-report<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>dividualresponds <strong>to</strong> a series of statements by <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g the extent ofagreement. Each choice is given a numerical value, <strong>and</strong> the<strong>to</strong>tal score is presumed <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicate the attitude or belief <strong>in</strong>question.l<strong>in</strong>ear relationship A relationship <strong>in</strong> which an <strong>in</strong>crease (ordecrease) <strong>in</strong> one variable is associated with a correspond<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>crease (or decrease) <strong>in</strong> another variable.literature review The systematic identification, location, <strong>and</strong>analysis of documents conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation related <strong>to</strong> aresearch problem.location threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong>characteristics of the sett<strong>in</strong>g or location <strong>in</strong> which a study isconducted, thereby produc<strong>in</strong>g a threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.longitud<strong>in</strong>al survey A study <strong>in</strong> which <strong>in</strong>formation is collected atdifferent po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> time <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> study changes over time


GLOSSARY G-5(usually of considerable length, such as several months oryears).manifest content The obvious mean<strong>in</strong>g of a communication.manipulated variable See experimental variable.Mann-Whitney U test A nonparametric <strong>in</strong>ferential statisticused <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether two uncorrelated groups differsignificantly.match<strong>in</strong>g design A technique for equat<strong>in</strong>g groups on one ormore variables, result<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> each member of one grouphav<strong>in</strong>g a direct counterpart <strong>in</strong> another group.maturation threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong> changesthat occur <strong>in</strong> subjects as a direct result of the passage of time<strong>and</strong> that may affect their performance on the dependentvariable, thereby affect<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.maximal variation sample In qualitative research, a sampleselected <strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> represent diversity <strong>in</strong> one or morecharacteristics.mean/arithmetic mean (X _ ) The sum of the scores <strong>in</strong> adistribution divided by the number of scores <strong>in</strong> thedistribution; the most commonly used measure of centraltendency.measures of central tendency Indices represent<strong>in</strong>g the averageor typical score atta<strong>in</strong>ed by a group of subjects; the mostcommonly used <strong>in</strong> educational research are the mean <strong>and</strong> themedian.mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g A process of pair<strong>in</strong>g two persons whosescores on one or more variables are similar.median That po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>in</strong> a distribution hav<strong>in</strong>g 50 percent of thescores above it <strong>and</strong> 50 percent of the scores below it.member check<strong>in</strong>g Procedure that <strong>in</strong>volves ask<strong>in</strong>g participants<strong>in</strong> a qualitative study <strong>to</strong> check the accuracy of the researchreport.meta-analysis A statistical procedure for comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the resultsof several studies on the same <strong>to</strong>pic.mixed-methods design research A study comb<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g quantitative<strong>and</strong> qualitative methods.mode The score that occurs most frequently <strong>in</strong> a distribution ofscores.modera<strong>to</strong>r variable A variable that may or may not becontrolled but has an effect on the research situation.mortality threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong> the factthat subjects who are for whatever reason “lost” <strong>to</strong> a studymay differ from those who rema<strong>in</strong> so that their absence hasan important effect on the results of the study.multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design A s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experimental design<strong>in</strong> which basel<strong>in</strong>e data are collected on several behaviors forone subject, after which the treatment is applied sequentiallyover a period of time <strong>to</strong> each behavior one at a time until allbehaviors are under treatment. Also used <strong>to</strong> collect data ondifferent subjects with regard <strong>to</strong> a s<strong>in</strong>gle behavior, or <strong>to</strong>assess a subject’s behavior <strong>in</strong> different sett<strong>in</strong>gs.multiple (collective) case study A study of multiple cases at thesame time.multiple realities/perspectives The recognition <strong>and</strong>acceptance of multiple views of reality, especially <strong>in</strong>ethnography.multiple regression A technique us<strong>in</strong>g a prediction equationwith two or more variables <strong>in</strong> comb<strong>in</strong>ation <strong>to</strong> predict acriterion (y = a + b 1 X 1 + b 2 X 2 + b 3 X 3 . . .).multiple-treatment <strong>in</strong>terference The carryover or delayedeffects of prior experimental treatments when <strong>in</strong>dividualsreceive two or more experimental treatments <strong>in</strong> succession.multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) Anextension of analysis of covariance that <strong>in</strong>corporates two ormore dependent variables <strong>in</strong> the same analysis.multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) An extension ofanalysis of variance that <strong>in</strong>corporates two or more dependentvariables <strong>in</strong> the same analysis.narrative research Study of the life experiences of an<strong>in</strong>dividual as <strong>to</strong>ld <strong>to</strong> the researcher or found <strong>in</strong> documents<strong>and</strong> archival material.naturalistic observation Observation <strong>in</strong> which the observercontrols or manipulates noth<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> tries not <strong>to</strong> affect theobserved situation <strong>in</strong> any way.negative case analysis In qualitative research, revis<strong>in</strong>g thepattern of <strong>in</strong>stances so it fits all cases.negatively skewed distribution A distribution <strong>in</strong> which thereare more extreme scores at the lower end than at the upper,or higher, end.nom<strong>in</strong>al scale A measurement scale that classifies elements<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> two or more categories, the numbers <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g that theelements are different, but not accord<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong> order ormagnitude.nondirectional hypothesis A prediction that a relationship existswithout specify<strong>in</strong>g its exact nature.nonequivalent control group design An experimental design<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g at least two groups, both of which may be pretested;one group receives the experimental treatment, <strong>and</strong> bothgroups are posttested. Individuals are not r<strong>and</strong>omly assigned<strong>to</strong> treatments.nonparametric technique A test of statistical significanceappropriate when the data represent an ord<strong>in</strong>al or nom<strong>in</strong>alscale, or when assumptions required for parametric testscannot be met.nonparticipant observation Observation <strong>in</strong> which the observeris not directly <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong> the situation <strong>to</strong> be observed.nonr<strong>and</strong>om sample/sampl<strong>in</strong>g The selection of a sample <strong>in</strong>which every member of the population does not have anequal chance of be<strong>in</strong>g selected.nonresponse Lack of response <strong>to</strong> some or all items on asurvey.normal curve A graphic illustration of a normal distribution.See normal distribution.normal distribution A theoretical “bell-shaped” distributionhav<strong>in</strong>g a wide application <strong>to</strong> both descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferentialstatistics. It is known or thought <strong>to</strong> portray many humancharacteristics <strong>in</strong> “typical” populations.norm group The sample group used <strong>to</strong> develop norms for an<strong>in</strong>strument.norm-referenced <strong>in</strong>strument An <strong>in</strong>strument that permitscomparison of an <strong>in</strong>dividual score <strong>to</strong> the scores of a groupof <strong>in</strong>dividuals on that same <strong>in</strong>strument.null hypothesis A statement that any difference betweenobta<strong>in</strong>ed sample statistics <strong>and</strong> specified populationparameters is due <strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, or chance.objectivity A lack of bias or prejudice.observational data Data obta<strong>in</strong>ed through direct observation.


G-6 GLOSSARY www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eobserver bias The possibility that an observer does not observeobjectively <strong>and</strong> accurately, thus produc<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>validobservations <strong>and</strong> a threat <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>ternal validity of a study.observer effect The impact of an observer’s presence on thebehavior observed.observer expectations The effect that an observer’s prior<strong>in</strong>formation can have on observational data.one-group pretest-posttest design A weak experimental design<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g one group that is pretested, exposed <strong>to</strong> a treatment,then posttested.one-shot case study design A weak experimental design<strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g one group that is exposed <strong>to</strong> a treatment <strong>and</strong> thenposttested.one-tailed test (of statistical significance) The use of only onetail of the sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of a statistic—used when adirectional hypothesis is stated.open-ended question A question giv<strong>in</strong>g the responder completefreedom of response.operational def<strong>in</strong>ition Def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g a term by stat<strong>in</strong>g the actions,processes, or operations used <strong>to</strong> measure or identifyexamples of it.op<strong>in</strong>ion questions Questions a researcher asks <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d out whatpeople th<strong>in</strong>k about a <strong>to</strong>pic.opportunistic sample In qualitative research, a sample chosen<strong>to</strong> take advantage of conditions that arise dur<strong>in</strong>g a study.oral his<strong>to</strong>ry Personal reflections of events <strong>and</strong> their causesgathered from one or more <strong>in</strong>dividuals.ord<strong>in</strong>al scale A measurement scale that ranks <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong>terms of the degree <strong>to</strong> which they possess a characteristic of<strong>in</strong>terest.outcome variable See dependent variable.outlier Score or other observation that deviates or fallsconsiderably outside most of the other scores or observations<strong>in</strong> a distribution or pattern.panel study A longitud<strong>in</strong>al design (<strong>in</strong> survey research) <strong>in</strong> whichthe same r<strong>and</strong>om sample is measured at different po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong>time.parameter A numerical <strong>in</strong>dex describ<strong>in</strong>g a characteristic of apopulation.parametric technique A test of significance appropriate whenthe data represent an <strong>in</strong>terval or ratio scale of measurement<strong>and</strong> other specific assumptions have been met.partial correlation A method of controll<strong>in</strong>g the subjectcharacteristics threat <strong>in</strong> correlational research by statisticallyhold<strong>in</strong>g one or more variables constant.participant observation Observation <strong>in</strong> which the observeractually becomes a participant <strong>in</strong> the situation <strong>to</strong> be observed.participants Individuals whose <strong>in</strong>volvement <strong>in</strong> a study canrange from provid<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> design<strong>in</strong>g thestudy.participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research Action research <strong>in</strong>tended no<strong>to</strong>nly <strong>to</strong> address a local problem but also <strong>to</strong> empower<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> br<strong>in</strong>g about social change.path analysis A type of sophisticated analysis <strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>gcausal connections among correlated variables.Pearson product-moment coefficient (Pearson r) An <strong>in</strong>dex ofcorrelation appropriate when the data represent either <strong>in</strong>tervalor ratio scales; it takes <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account each pair of scores <strong>and</strong>produces a coefficient between 0.00 <strong>and</strong> either ±1.00.peer debrief<strong>in</strong>g See external audit.percentile The score below which a given percent of a knowngroup scores, e.g., the 60th percentile is a score of 120.percentile rank An <strong>in</strong>dex of relative position <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g thepercentage of scores that fall at or below a given score.performance <strong>in</strong>strument An <strong>in</strong>strument designed <strong>to</strong> measureability <strong>to</strong> follow procedures or produce a product.phenomenology/phenomenological research/study A formof qualitative research <strong>in</strong> which the researcher attempts<strong>to</strong> identify commonalities <strong>in</strong> the perceptions of several<strong>in</strong>dividuals regard<strong>in</strong>g a particular phenomenon.pie chart A graphic method of display<strong>in</strong>g the breakdown of data<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> categories.pilot study A small-scale study adm<strong>in</strong>istered before conduct<strong>in</strong>gan actual study—its purpose is <strong>to</strong> reveal defects <strong>in</strong> theresearch plan.population The group <strong>to</strong> which the researcher would like theresults of a study <strong>to</strong> be generalizable; it <strong>in</strong>cludes all<strong>in</strong>dividuals with certa<strong>in</strong> specified characteristics.population generalizability The extent <strong>to</strong> which the resultsobta<strong>in</strong>ed from a sample are generalizable <strong>to</strong> a larger group.portraiture A form of qualitative research <strong>in</strong> which theresearcher <strong>and</strong> the <strong>in</strong>dividual be<strong>in</strong>g portrayed work <strong>to</strong>gether<strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong>e mean<strong>in</strong>g.positively skewed distribution A distribution <strong>in</strong> which there aremore extreme scores at the upper, or higher, end than at thelower end.positivism A philosophic viewpo<strong>in</strong>t emphasiz<strong>in</strong>g an “objective”reality that <strong>in</strong>cludes universal laws govern<strong>in</strong>g all th<strong>in</strong>gs<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g human behavior.postmodernism An <strong>in</strong>tensive criticism of scientific research.power of a statistical test The probability that the nullhypothesis will be rejected when there is a difference <strong>in</strong>the populations; the ability of a test <strong>to</strong> avoid a Type IIerror.practical action research Action research <strong>in</strong>tended <strong>to</strong> address aspecific local problem.practical significance A difference large enough <strong>to</strong> have somepractical effect. Contrast with statistical significance, whichmay be so small as <strong>to</strong> have no practical consequences.pragmatists A group of methodologists who propose us<strong>in</strong>gwhatever research methods work or will shed light on aproblem. Pragmatists believe that quantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitativemethods can be “mixed” <strong>in</strong> a research endeavor <strong>and</strong> might bemore <strong>in</strong>formative than us<strong>in</strong>g only a s<strong>in</strong>gle method.prediction The estimation of scores on one variable from<strong>in</strong>formation about one or more other variables.prediction equation A mathematical equation used <strong>in</strong> aprediction study.prediction study An attempt <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e variables that arerelated <strong>to</strong> a criterion variable.predictive validity (evidence of) The degree <strong>to</strong> which scores onan <strong>in</strong>strument predict characteristics of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> a futuresituation.predic<strong>to</strong>r variable(s) The variable(s) from which projectionsare made <strong>in</strong> a prediction study.pretest treatment <strong>in</strong>teraction The possibility that subjectsmay respond or react differently <strong>to</strong> a treatment because theyhave been pretested, thereby creat<strong>in</strong>g a threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity.


GLOSSARY G-7primary source Firsth<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation, such as the testimony ofan eyewitness, an orig<strong>in</strong>al document, a relic, or a descriptionof a study written by the person who conducted it.probability The relative frequency with which a particular even<strong>to</strong>ccurs among all events of <strong>in</strong>terest.problem statement A statement that <strong>in</strong>dicates the specificpurpose of the research, the variables of <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>to</strong> theresearcher, <strong>and</strong> any specific relationship between thosevariables that is <strong>to</strong> be, or was, <strong>in</strong>vestigated; <strong>in</strong>cludesdescription of background <strong>and</strong> rationale (justification) forthe study.procedures A detailed description by the researcher of what was(or will be) done <strong>in</strong> carry<strong>in</strong>g out a study.projective device An <strong>in</strong>strument that <strong>in</strong>cludes vague stimuli thatsubjects are asked <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret. There are no correct answersor replies.purpose (of a study) A specific statement by a researcher ofwhat he or she <strong>in</strong>tends <strong>to</strong> accomplish.purposive sample/sampl<strong>in</strong>g A nonr<strong>and</strong>om sample selectedbecause prior knowledge suggests it is representative, orbecause those selected have the needed <strong>in</strong>formation.qualitative research/study <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rattempts <strong>to</strong> study naturally occurr<strong>in</strong>g phenomena <strong>in</strong> all theircomplexity.qualitative variable A variable that is conceptualized <strong>and</strong>analyzed as dist<strong>in</strong>ct categories, with no cont<strong>in</strong>uum implied.qualitiz<strong>in</strong>g The process of convert<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>qualitative data.quantitative data Data that differ <strong>in</strong> amount or degree along acont<strong>in</strong>uum from less <strong>to</strong> more.quantitative research <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> which the <strong>in</strong>vestiga<strong>to</strong>rattempts <strong>to</strong> clarify phenomena through carefully designed<strong>and</strong> controlled data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis.quantitative variable A variable that is conceptualized <strong>and</strong>analyzed along a cont<strong>in</strong>uum. It differs <strong>in</strong> amount or degree.quantitiz<strong>in</strong>g The process of convert<strong>in</strong>g qualitative data <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>quantitative data.quasi-experimental design A type of experimental design <strong>in</strong>which the researcher does not use r<strong>and</strong>om assignment ofsubjects <strong>to</strong> groups.questionnaire A form for written or marked answers <strong>to</strong> questions.r<strong>and</strong>om assignment The process of assign<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividuals orgroups r<strong>and</strong>omly <strong>to</strong> different treatment conditions.r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only control group design Anexperimental design <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g at least two r<strong>and</strong>omly formedgroups; one group receives a treatment, <strong>and</strong> both groups areposttested.r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttest control group design Anexperimental design that <strong>in</strong>volves at least two groups; bothgroups are pretested, one group receives a treatment, <strong>and</strong>both groups are posttested. For effective control ofextraneous variables, the groups should be r<strong>and</strong>omlyformed.r<strong>and</strong>om sample/sampl<strong>in</strong>g A sample selected <strong>in</strong> such a way thatevery member of the population has an equal chance of be<strong>in</strong>gselected.r<strong>and</strong>om selection sampl<strong>in</strong>g The process of select<strong>in</strong>g a r<strong>and</strong>omsample.r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-group design An experimentaldesign that <strong>in</strong>volves r<strong>and</strong>om assignment of subjects <strong>to</strong> eachof four groups. Two groups are pretested, two are not, one ofthe pretested groups <strong>and</strong> one of the unpretested groupsreceive the experimental treatment, <strong>and</strong> all four groups areposttested.range The difference between the highest <strong>and</strong> lowest scores <strong>in</strong> adistribution; measure of variability.ratio scale A measurement scale that, <strong>in</strong> addition <strong>to</strong> be<strong>in</strong>g an<strong>in</strong>terval scale, also has an absolute zero <strong>in</strong> the scale.raw score The score atta<strong>in</strong>ed by an <strong>in</strong>dividual on the items on atest or other <strong>in</strong>strument.reflective field notes A record of the observer’s thoughts <strong>and</strong>reflections dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> after observation.regressed ga<strong>in</strong> score A score <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g amount of change thatis determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the correlation between scores on a posttest<strong>and</strong> a pretest (<strong>and</strong>/or other scores). It provides more stable<strong>in</strong>formation than a simple posttest-pretest difference.regression l<strong>in</strong>e The l<strong>in</strong>e of best fit for a set of scores plotted oncoord<strong>in</strong>ate axes (on a scatterplot).regression threat The possibility that results are due <strong>to</strong> atendency for groups, selected on the basis of extremescores, <strong>to</strong> regress <strong>to</strong>ward a more average score onsubsequent measurements, regardless of the experimentaltreatment.reliability The degree <strong>to</strong> which scores obta<strong>in</strong>ed with an<strong>in</strong>strument are consistent measures of whatever the<strong>in</strong>strument measures.reliability coefficient An <strong>in</strong>dex of the consistency of scores onthe same <strong>in</strong>strument. There are several methods of comput<strong>in</strong>ga reliability coefficient, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the type of consistency<strong>and</strong> characteristics of the <strong>in</strong>strument.relic Any object whose physical characteristics provide<strong>in</strong>formation about the past.replication Refers <strong>to</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g a study aga<strong>in</strong>; the secondstudy may be a repetition of the orig<strong>in</strong>al study, us<strong>in</strong>gdifferent subjects, or specified aspects of the study maybe changed.representativeness The extent <strong>to</strong> which a sample is identical (<strong>in</strong>all characteristics) <strong>to</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tended population.research The formal, systematic application of scholarship,discipl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong>quiry, <strong>and</strong> most often the scientific method <strong>to</strong>the study of problems.research design The overall plan for collect<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> order<strong>to</strong> answer the research question. Also the specific dataanalysis techniques or methods that the researcher <strong>in</strong>tends<strong>to</strong> use.research hypothesis A prediction of study outcomes. Often astatement of the expected relationship between two or morevariables.research proposal A detailed description of a proposed studydesigned <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>vestigate a given problem.research report A description of how a study was conducted,<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g results <strong>and</strong> conclusions.researcher reflexivity Record<strong>in</strong>g personal thoughts whileconduct<strong>in</strong>g observations or <strong>in</strong>terviews for later crosscheck<strong>in</strong>g.results (of a study) A statement that expla<strong>in</strong>s what is shown byanalysis of the data collected; <strong>in</strong>cludes tables <strong>and</strong> graphswhen appropriate.


G-8 GLOSSARY www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7eretrospective <strong>in</strong>terview A form of <strong>in</strong>terview <strong>in</strong> which theresearcher tries <strong>to</strong> get a respondent <strong>to</strong> reconstruct pastexperiences.sample The group on which <strong>in</strong>formation is obta<strong>in</strong>ed.sampl<strong>in</strong>g The process of select<strong>in</strong>g a number of <strong>in</strong>dividuals (asample) from a population, preferably <strong>in</strong> such a way that the<strong>in</strong>dividuals are representative of the larger group from whichthey were selected.sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution The theoretical distribution of allpossible values of a statistic from all possible samples of agiven size selected from a population.sampl<strong>in</strong>g error Expected, chance variation <strong>in</strong> sample statisticsthat occurs when successive samples are selected from apopulation.sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terval The distance <strong>in</strong> a list between <strong>in</strong>dividualschosen when sampl<strong>in</strong>g systematically.sampl<strong>in</strong>g ratio The proportion of <strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>in</strong> the populationthat are selected for the sample <strong>in</strong> systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g.scatterplot The plot of po<strong>in</strong>ts determ<strong>in</strong>ed by the crosstabulationof scores on coord<strong>in</strong>ate axes; used <strong>to</strong> represent<strong>and</strong> illustrate the relationship between two quantitativevariables.scientific method A way of know<strong>in</strong>g that is characterized by thepublic nature of its procedures <strong>and</strong> conclusions <strong>and</strong> byrigorous test<strong>in</strong>g of conclusions.scor<strong>in</strong>g agreement The percentage agreement among differentscorers or observers.search eng<strong>in</strong>e A comprehensive computer system for locat<strong>in</strong>greferences <strong>to</strong> specific <strong>to</strong>pics.search terms See descrip<strong>to</strong>rs.secondary source Secondh<strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>formation, such as adescription of his<strong>to</strong>rical events by someone not present whenthe event occurred.semantic differential An attitude scale us<strong>in</strong>g pairs of oppositessuch as hot-cold.semistructured <strong>in</strong>terview A structured <strong>in</strong>terview, comb<strong>in</strong>edwith open-ended questions.sensory questions Questions asked by a researcher <strong>to</strong> f<strong>in</strong>d outwhat a person has seen, heard, or experienced through his orher senses.sign test A nonparametric <strong>in</strong>ferential statistic used <strong>to</strong> comparetwo groups that are not <strong>in</strong>dependent.simple r<strong>and</strong>om sample See r<strong>and</strong>om sample/sampl<strong>in</strong>g.simulation <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> which an “artificial” situation is created<strong>and</strong> participants are <strong>to</strong>ld what activities they are <strong>to</strong> engage <strong>in</strong>.s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject design/research <strong>Design</strong> applied when the samplesize is one; used <strong>to</strong> study the behavior change that an<strong>in</strong>dividual exhibits as a result of some <strong>in</strong>tervention ortreatment.skewed distribution A nonsymmetrical distribution <strong>in</strong> whichthere are more extreme scores at one end of the distributionthan the other.snowball sample In qualitative research, a sample selected asthe need arises dur<strong>in</strong>g a study.split-half procedure A method of estimat<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>ternalconsistencyreliability of an <strong>in</strong>strument; it is obta<strong>in</strong>ed bygiv<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>strument once but scor<strong>in</strong>g it twice—for each oftwo equivalent “half tests.” These scores are then correlated.spreads Measures of variability.stability (of scores) The extent <strong>to</strong> which scores are reliable(consistent) over time.stakeholders Those who have a vested <strong>in</strong>terest <strong>in</strong> the outcomesof a study.st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation (SD) The most stable measure of variability;it takes <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> account each <strong>and</strong> every score <strong>in</strong> a distribution.st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference (SED) The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationof a distribution of differences between sample means.st<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate An estimate of the size of the error<strong>to</strong> be expected <strong>in</strong> predict<strong>in</strong>g a criterion score.st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean (SEM) The st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation ofsample means that <strong>in</strong>dicates by how much the sample meanscan be expected <strong>to</strong> differ if other samples from the samepopulation are used.st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement (SEMeas) An estimate of thesize of the error that one can expect <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>dividual’s score.st<strong>and</strong>ard score A derived score that expresses how far a givenraw score is from the mean, <strong>in</strong> terms of st<strong>and</strong>ard deviationunits.static-group comparison design A weak experimental designthat <strong>in</strong>volves at least two nonequivalent groups; one receivesa treatment <strong>and</strong> both are posttested.static-group pretest-posttest design The same as the static-groupcomparison design, except that both groups are pretested.statistic A numerical <strong>in</strong>dex describ<strong>in</strong>g a characteristic of a sample.statistical equat<strong>in</strong>g See statistical match<strong>in</strong>g.statistical match<strong>in</strong>g A means of equat<strong>in</strong>g groups us<strong>in</strong>g statisticalprediction.statistically significant The conclusion that results are unlikely<strong>to</strong> have occurred due <strong>to</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g error or “chance”; anobserved correlation or difference probably exists <strong>in</strong> thepopulation.Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) A computerprogram for calculat<strong>in</strong>g descriptive <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>ferential statistics.stem-leaf plot A method for show<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual scores <strong>in</strong> agrouped frequency distribution.stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g The process of select<strong>in</strong>g a sample<strong>in</strong> such a way that identified subgroups <strong>in</strong> the population arerepresented <strong>in</strong> the sample <strong>in</strong> the same proportion as they exist<strong>in</strong> the population.structured <strong>in</strong>terview A formal type of <strong>in</strong>terview, <strong>in</strong> whichthe researcher asks, <strong>in</strong> order, a set of predeterm<strong>in</strong>edquestions.subject characteristics threat The possibility that characteristicsof the subjects <strong>in</strong> a study may account for observedrelationships, thereby produc<strong>in</strong>g a threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity.subjects Individuals whose participation <strong>in</strong> a study is limited <strong>to</strong>provid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation.survey study/research An attempt <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> data from membersof a population (or a sample) <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e the current statusof that population with respect <strong>to</strong> one or more variables.systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g A selection procedure <strong>in</strong> which all sampleelements are determ<strong>in</strong>ed after the selection of the firstelement, s<strong>in</strong>ce each element on a selected list is separatedfrom the first element by a multiple of the selection <strong>in</strong>terval;e.g., every tenth element may be selected.table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers A table of numbers that provides thebest means of r<strong>and</strong>om selection or r<strong>and</strong>om assignment.tally sheet A form for record<strong>in</strong>g observed <strong>in</strong>stances of behavior.


GLOSSARY G-9target population The population <strong>to</strong> which the researcher,ideally, would like <strong>to</strong> generalize results.test<strong>in</strong>g threat A threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity that refers <strong>to</strong>improved scores on a posttest that are a result of subjectshav<strong>in</strong>g taken a pretest.test-retest method A procedure for determ<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g the extent <strong>to</strong>which scores from an <strong>in</strong>strument are reliable over time bycorrelat<strong>in</strong>g the scores from two adm<strong>in</strong>istrations of the same<strong>in</strong>strument <strong>to</strong> the same <strong>in</strong>dividuals.theme A means of organiz<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g data <strong>in</strong> a contentanalysis by group<strong>in</strong>g codes as the <strong>in</strong>terpretation progresses.theoretical sample In qualitative research, a sample that helps theresearcher underst<strong>and</strong> or formulate a concept or <strong>in</strong>terpretation.thick description In ethnography, the provision of great detailon the basic data/<strong>in</strong>formation.threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity An alternative explanation forresearch results, that is, that an observed relationship is anartifact of another variable.time-series design An experimental design <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g one groupthat is repeatedly pretested, exposed <strong>to</strong> an experimentaltreatment, <strong>and</strong> repeatedly posttested.transferability In qualitative research, the degree <strong>to</strong> which an<strong>in</strong>dividual can expect the results of a particular study <strong>to</strong> apply<strong>in</strong> a new situation or with new people. Transferability, <strong>in</strong> thequalitative doma<strong>in</strong>, is similar <strong>to</strong> generalizability <strong>in</strong> thequantitative doma<strong>in</strong>.treatment variable See experimental variable.trend study A longitud<strong>in</strong>al design (<strong>in</strong> survey research) <strong>in</strong> whichthe same population (conceptually but not literally) is studiedover time by tak<strong>in</strong>g different r<strong>and</strong>om samples.triangulation Cross-check<strong>in</strong>g of data us<strong>in</strong>g multiple datasources or multiple data-collection procedures.triangulation mixed-methods design A study <strong>in</strong> whichquantitative <strong>and</strong> qualitative data are collected simultaneously<strong>and</strong> used <strong>to</strong> validate <strong>and</strong> clarify f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs.T score A st<strong>and</strong>ard score derived from a z score by multiply<strong>in</strong>gthe z score by 10 <strong>and</strong> add<strong>in</strong>g 50.t-test for correlated means A parametric test of statisticalsignificance used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there is a statisticallysignificant difference between the means of two matched, ornon<strong>in</strong>dependent, samples. It is also used for pre-postcomparisons.t-test for correlated proportions A parametric test of statisticalsignificance used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there is a statisticallysignificant difference between two proportions based on thesame sample or otherwise non<strong>in</strong>dependent groups.t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent means A parametric test of significanceused <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there is a statistically significantdifference between the means of two <strong>in</strong>dependent samples.t-test for <strong>in</strong>dependent proportions A parametric test ofstatistical significance used <strong>to</strong> determ<strong>in</strong>e whether there is astatistically significant difference between two <strong>in</strong>dependentproportions.two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g A comb<strong>in</strong>ation of <strong>in</strong>dividualr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g.two-tailed test (of statistical significance) Use of both tails ofa sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution of a statistic—when a nondirectionalhypothesis is stated.Type I error The rejection by the researcher of a null hypothesisthat is actually true; also called an alpha error.Type II error The failure of a researcher <strong>to</strong> reject a nullhypothesis that is really false; also called a beta error.typical sample In qualitative research, a sample judged <strong>to</strong> berepresentative of the population of <strong>in</strong>terest.unit of analysis The unit that is used <strong>in</strong> data analysis(<strong>in</strong>dividuals, objects, groups, classrooms, etc.).unobtrusive measures Measures obta<strong>in</strong>ed without subjectsbe<strong>in</strong>g aware that they are be<strong>in</strong>g observed or measured, or byexam<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>animate objects (such as school suspension lists)<strong>in</strong> order <strong>to</strong> obta<strong>in</strong> desired <strong>in</strong>formation.validity The degree <strong>to</strong> which correct <strong>in</strong>ferences can be madebased on results from an <strong>in</strong>strument; depends not only on the<strong>in</strong>strument itself but also on the <strong>in</strong>strumentation process <strong>and</strong>the characteristics of the group studied.validity coefficient An <strong>in</strong>dex of the validity of scores; a specialapplication of the correlation coefficient.values questions See op<strong>in</strong>ion questions.variability The extent <strong>to</strong> which scores differ from oneanother.variable A characteristic that can assume any one of severalvalues, for example, cognitive ability, height, aptitude,teach<strong>in</strong>g method.variance (SD) 2 The square of the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation; a measureof variability.Web browser A computer program provid<strong>in</strong>g access <strong>to</strong> theWorld Wide Web.Wilk’s lambda The numerical <strong>in</strong>dex calculated when carry<strong>in</strong>gout MANOVA or MANCOVA.World Wide Web (WWW) An Internet reservoir of <strong>in</strong>formationused <strong>in</strong> search<strong>in</strong>g literature.written-response <strong>in</strong>strument An <strong>in</strong>strument requir<strong>in</strong>g writtenor marked responses.z score The most basic st<strong>and</strong>ard score that expresses how far ascore is from a mean <strong>in</strong> terms of st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation units.


IndexAA-B-A-B design, 302–303, 311, 597A-B-A design, 302–303A-B-C-B design, 303–304A-B design, 300–301abstract, of research proposal, 625accessible population, target versus,91–92achievement test, 125, 127, 128 (fig.).See also entries for specifictestsaction plan, 594action research, 13action plan, 594advantages of, 596assumptions underly<strong>in</strong>g, 589–590comparative study, hypotheticalexample of, 599–600comparison-group experiment,hypothetical example of,596–597content analysis, hypotheticalexample of, 598–599correlational study, hypotheticalexample of, 599data analysis <strong>and</strong>, 594data collection, 593–594def<strong>in</strong>ed, 589–590documents, analysis of, 593–594ethnographic study, hypotheticalexample of, 600–601example ofpractical, hypothetical, 596–601published, 602–611external validity <strong>and</strong>, 595–596field notes, 593f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 597<strong>in</strong>-school research, 600<strong>in</strong>struments, types of, 593–594<strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>, 594–595<strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g, 593participation, levels of, 591–592participa<strong>to</strong>ry, 590–591practical, 590qualitative research methods<strong>and</strong>, 594–596quantitative research method<strong>and</strong>, 594–596question formulation <strong>and</strong>, 592–593questionnaire, 563sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>, 594steps <strong>in</strong>, 592–594triangulation, 594types of, 590–592What Do You Mean “Th<strong>in</strong>k BeforeI Act?”: Conflict Resolutionwith Choices study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 603–611advocacy lenses, 562age <strong>and</strong> grade-level equivalents,135–136agreement with others, 4, 10 (fig.)alpha coefficient, 157–158AltaVista search eng<strong>in</strong>e, 81 (fig.), 84alternate forms method, 156. See alsoequivalent-forms methodalternat<strong>in</strong>g treatment design, 311American Council on <strong>Education</strong>, 123American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Association, 150American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Association BiographicalMembership Direc<strong>to</strong>ry, 75American <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>Journal, 74Amidon/Fl<strong>and</strong>ers scheme for cod<strong>in</strong>gcategories of <strong>in</strong>teraction,445 (fig.)analysis of covariance (ANCOVA),232, 264, 370–371. See alsoparametric techniquesanalysis of documents, 593analysis of variance (ANOVA),232–233, 234 (table). Seealso parametric techniquesANCOVA. See analysis of covariance(ANCOVA)anecdotal records, 112, 122–123ANOVA. See analysis of variance(ANOVA)APA. See Publication Manual of theAmerican PsychologicalAssociation (APA)applied research, 7aptitude tests, 127–129arithmetic mean, 192. See also meanaverageassociational research, 328. See alsocorrelational researchassociative research, 14–15assumption, def<strong>in</strong>ed, 16–18attitude of subjects threat, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,369–370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338def<strong>in</strong>ed, 174–175, 179 (fig.)experimental research <strong>and</strong>, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research <strong>and</strong>, 310attitude scale, 124–125attitud<strong>in</strong>al (demoralization) effect, 277audiotap<strong>in</strong>gfor <strong>in</strong>terview data, 452for observation, 445au<strong>to</strong>biography, 427–428averages, 192–194. See alsocentral tendencyBB-A-B design, 303–304background (or demographic)question, 448bar graph, 207–208basel<strong>in</strong>e condition, 300–301basel<strong>in</strong>e(s). See also entries formultiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designsdef<strong>in</strong>ed, 300–301number of, 309–310return <strong>to</strong>, 308–309basel<strong>in</strong>e period, 301. See also s<strong>in</strong>glesubjectresearchbasic research, 7Baxter International Inc., 57behavior rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, 117behaviors, <strong>in</strong>dependence of, 309Bellah, Robert N., 541bias, 168. See also data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias,as threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitydata collec<strong>to</strong>r, 170<strong>and</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 93selection, 167bibliographic cards, 72, 74 (fig.), 75bibliography, of researchproposal, 626bimodal distribution, 192biographical study, 427biography, 13Blum Sew<strong>in</strong>g Mach<strong>in</strong>e Test, 129,130 (fig.)Bonneville Dam, 597Boolean opera<strong>to</strong>r, 78–79, 79 (fig.)box-<strong>and</strong>-whiskers diagram, 194boxplots, 194–195Buckley Amendment (Family PrivacyAct), 62budget, <strong>and</strong> research proposal, 624Buros Institute, 114CCalifornia Achievement Test, 127California Test of Mental Maturity(CTMM), 128case study, 427def<strong>in</strong>ed, 13, 430<strong>in</strong>strumental, 431<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic, 430–431multiple- (or collective), 431categorical databar graphs <strong>and</strong> pie charts, 207–208compar<strong>in</strong>g groups, 251–253crossbreak table, 207–210def<strong>in</strong>ed, 186–187frequency table, 207–210<strong>in</strong>ferential techniques, commonlyused, 234 (table)<strong>in</strong>terpretation of, 252–253nonparametric techniques foranalyz<strong>in</strong>g, 233–234parametric techniques foranalyz<strong>in</strong>g, 233, 234 (table)quantitative data versus,186–187relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> group,253–254categorical variables, 365associations between, 372–373def<strong>in</strong>ed, 40versus quantitative variables,39–42causal-comparative research, 15, 27,363–365, 560. See alsocomparison-groupexperimentsaction research <strong>and</strong>, 599–600categorical variables <strong>and</strong>, 372–373correlational research <strong>and</strong>,364–365data analysis, 370–372data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias threat, 370data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristicsthreat, 370def<strong>in</strong>ed, 11–12evaluat<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity <strong>in</strong>, 369–370example <strong>and</strong> analysis of, 373–386experimental research <strong>and</strong>, 365f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 372his<strong>to</strong>ry threat, 370implementation threat, 370<strong>in</strong>strumentation, 370<strong>in</strong>strument decay threat, 370<strong>in</strong>terpretations of, 12location, 370maturation threat, 370mortality, 370Personal, Social, <strong>and</strong> FamilyCharacteristics of AngryStudents study <strong>and</strong> analysis,373–386<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 621steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>, 366–367subject characteristics, 367–370threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>,367–370types of, 364census, 391central tendency, 193–194, 243.See also averageschaos theory, 8–9character. See personality (orcharacter) <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>riesChild Development, 74children, research with, 59–60chi-square distribution, A-4chi-square test, 233–235. See alsononparametric techniquesCIJE. See Current Index <strong>to</strong> Journals<strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> (CIJE)cl<strong>in</strong>ical trial, 55, 57closed, fixed response <strong>in</strong>terview,447 (table)closed-ended questions, 16, 396–398,397 (table), 434cluster r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 94–97cluster sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 476cod<strong>in</strong>g, 140–141, 476–477cod<strong>in</strong>g scheme, 444coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation, 332–333coefficient of multiple correlation, 332cohort study, 391I-1


INDEX I-2collaborative research, 590. See alsoparticipa<strong>to</strong>ry action researchcomments, general, <strong>and</strong> researchproposal, 624Committee on Scientific <strong>and</strong>Professional Ethics of theAmerican PsychologicalAssociation, 53–54communication, question of, 16, 18comparison-group experiments, 602.See also causal-comparativeresearchaction research <strong>and</strong>, 596–597experimental research <strong>and</strong>, 262hypothetical example of, 596–597quantitative data <strong>and</strong>, 243–247complete observer, 441complete-participant, 441Comprehensive DissertationIndex, 76Comprehensive Tests of BasicSkills, 127computer<strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g observational data,444–445<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 482, 483for telephone surveys, 394computer search, of literatureconduct<strong>in</strong>g, 78–80database, decision on, 78descrip<strong>to</strong>rs, selection of, 78example of, 76, 77 (fig.), 78<strong>in</strong>dexes, 80–81pr<strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong>ut of desired references, 80problem formulation <strong>and</strong>, 78search, scope of, 78World Wide Web (WWW) search,80–84Computer Simulation Activity study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 282–293Comte, Auguste, 423conclusion, 5concrete or specific statement, 123concurrent validity, 150conditions, 300, 305–308confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals, 220–223,247, 255confidentiality, 56, 433. See also ethicsconfirm<strong>in</strong>g sample, 431consent form<strong>and</strong> disclosure of data, 62example of, 56 (fig.)for m<strong>in</strong>ors, 60 (fig.)<strong>and</strong> protect<strong>in</strong>g participantsfrom harm, 55consequential validity, 160constant comparative method, 429constants, versus variable, 39constitutive def<strong>in</strong>ition, 30construct-related evidence of validity,148–149, 153–154, 158, 161content analysis, 510action research <strong>and</strong>, 598advantages of, 483applications for, 473–474categorization <strong>in</strong>, 474cod<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>, 476–477computer <strong>in</strong>, us<strong>in</strong>g, 482–483data analysis, 479–480data collection <strong>and</strong>, 475def<strong>in</strong>ed, 472–473disadvantages of, 483–484example <strong>and</strong> analysis, 484–496f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 475illustration of, 480–482latent content <strong>and</strong>, 477–479manifest <strong>and</strong>, 477–479“Nuts <strong>and</strong> Dolts” study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 484–496objective, determ<strong>in</strong>ation of, 474rationale <strong>and</strong>, 475reliability <strong>and</strong>, 479sampl<strong>in</strong>g plan <strong>and</strong>, 475–476social behavior study, 480–483steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>, 474–480terms, def<strong>in</strong>ed, 474unit of analysis <strong>and</strong>, 475validity <strong>and</strong>, 479content-related evidence of validity,148–152, 154, 158, 161context sensitivity, 424 (table)contextualization, <strong>and</strong> ethnographicresearch, 503–504cont<strong>in</strong>gency coefficient, 234cont<strong>in</strong>gency table, 207. See alsocrossbreak tablecontrol group, 262convenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 98–100, 103,621–622<strong>and</strong> scatterplots, 203–206, 365correlational coefficient, 152–153,337. See also validitycoefficientcorrelational hypothesis, 475correlational research, 11, 15, 27, 172action research <strong>and</strong>, 599causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,364–365coefficient of determ<strong>in</strong>ation,332–333coefficient of multiplecorrelation, 332correlation coefficients <strong>and</strong>, 337data analysis <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terpretation, 337data collection, 336data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias threat, 340–342data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristicsthreat, 340, 342def<strong>in</strong>ed, 328design <strong>and</strong> procedures, 336discrim<strong>in</strong>ation function analysis,333–334evaluat<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>in</strong>, rationale forprocess of, 342–343evaluat<strong>in</strong>g threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity <strong>in</strong>, 341–343example of, 343–359<strong>and</strong> experimental research, 262explana<strong>to</strong>ry studies, 329–330fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis, 334f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 330<strong>in</strong>strumentation threats, 340, 342<strong>in</strong>strument decay, 340, 342<strong>in</strong>struments, 336location threat, 338–340, 342mortality threat, 341–342multiple regression, 331–332nature of, 328–329path analysis, 334–335prediction equation, 331prediction studies, 330problem selection, 335–337purpose of, 329–335<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 621sample, 335–336scatterplots, 329–331, 339steps <strong>in</strong>, 334–337structural model<strong>in</strong>g, 335subject characteristics threat, 338,341–342survey research <strong>and</strong>, 392test<strong>in</strong>g threat, 341–342threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>,337–341When Teachers’ <strong>and</strong> Parents’Values Differ study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 344–359correlation coefficients, 224, 230,247–249, 254correlations, 204, 224. See alsoscatterplotscalculat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> SPSS, A-14–A-16<strong>in</strong> everyday life, 209<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g, 247–251Pearson product-momentcoefficient, 206–207<strong>and</strong> regression equations, 205counterbalanced design, 271–272,276 (table)covariate, 232cover letter, for survey research,399–400covert participant, 441criterion group design, 367criterion-referenced <strong>in</strong>struments,136–137, 154, 158(table), 161criterion-related evidence of validity,148–149 (fig.), 152–153criterion variable, 261, 330, 367.See also dependent variablecritical researchers, 16critical sample, 431Cronbach alpha, 158crossbreak table, 207–210, 210 (fig.),224, 234 (table)crystallization, 510–512CTMM. See California Test of MentalMaturity (CTMM)culture, <strong>and</strong> ethnographic research, 503Current Index <strong>to</strong> Journals <strong>in</strong><strong>Education</strong> (CIJE), 67, 69,70 (fig.), 71–72, 74–75curve, power, 235–236curvil<strong>in</strong>ear, 247, 250Ddata. See also categorical data;numerical data, types of;quantitative data; scor<strong>in</strong>g;statisticsenter<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> SPSS, A-6tabulat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g, 140–141data analysis, 19–20action research <strong>and</strong>, 594causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,370–372<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 479–480correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 337<strong>in</strong> ethnographic research, 510–512his<strong>to</strong>rical research <strong>and</strong>, 540<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research, 564prepar<strong>in</strong>g for, 140qualitative research <strong>and</strong>, 426–427,452–453<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 623specify<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> SPSS, A-7survey research <strong>and</strong>, 404data collection<strong>in</strong> action research, 593–594choos<strong>in</strong>g mode of, for surveyresearch, 393<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 475correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 336<strong>in</strong> ethnographic research, 506–510<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research, 564<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 452–453qualitative research <strong>and</strong>, 426,454 (table)strategies, 454 (table)survey, 393 (table)data-collection <strong>in</strong>struments. Seealso <strong>in</strong>strumentationexamplesresearcher-completed, 116–123subject-completed, 123–134unobtrusive measures, 134<strong>in</strong>formation source, 112<strong>in</strong>strument source, 112–116rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, 116–117written response <strong>in</strong>strument versusperformance <strong>in</strong>struments, 116data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, as threat <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity, 170–171<strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research,368, 370<strong>in</strong> correlational research, 340–342<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 277, 279<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics, as threat<strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, 170causal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>,340, 342<strong>in</strong> experimental research,276 (table), 279<strong>and</strong> response rates, 404data correlation, A-15 (fig.)Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r, A-5 (fig.), A-8 (fig.)Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r w<strong>in</strong>dow with data,A-6 (fig.)data po<strong>in</strong>ts, 300Data View screen, A-8 (fig.)Def<strong>in</strong>e Groups dialog box, A-13 (fig.)def<strong>in</strong>itions, research, 19–20examples of, 619–620<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 619degrees of freedom (df), 230DeMaria, Darlene, 601–602demographic <strong>in</strong>formation,402–403, 621Department of Health <strong>and</strong> HumanServices (HHS), 60, 62dependent variable, 7, 261causal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 367<strong>in</strong>dependent versus, 42–43derived score, 135. See also raw scoredescriptive field notes, 507descriptive his<strong>to</strong>rical study, 15descriptive research, 12, 14descriptive statistics, 434, 453averages, 192–194bars <strong>and</strong> pie graphs, 207–208categorical data, 186–187categorical data, techniques forsummariz<strong>in</strong>g, 207–210causal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlation, 200–207crossbreak table, 207–210frequency polygons, 187–188frequency table, 207his<strong>to</strong>grams <strong>and</strong> stem-leaf plots,189–191<strong>in</strong>terpretation of, 243–247normal curve, 191–192numerical data, 185–187


I-3 INDEX www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7edescriptive statistics—Cont.obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, A-7–A-10quantitative data, 186–187quantitative data, <strong>in</strong> qualitativestudy, 434quantitative data, techniques forcollect<strong>in</strong>g, 187–207for relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> group,253–254skewed polygons, 189spreads, 194–197st<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>and</strong> normal curve,197–200statistics versus parameters, 185descrip<strong>to</strong>rs, 77–78design. See also entries for specificdesigns <strong>and</strong> specificresearch typesalternat<strong>in</strong>g treatment, 311causal-comparative research, 367correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 366flexibility of, 424 (table)mixed-methods, 560–564<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 621design <strong>and</strong> procedures. See specificresearch typesdesign flexibility, 424 (table)detached behavior, 15dicho<strong>to</strong>mous question, 450dictionary approach, <strong>to</strong> def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>gterms, 30difference <strong>in</strong> sample means, 230Digital Dissertations, 72, 73 (fig.)Digital Dissertations on Disc, 72direct adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>to</strong> a groupmethod, 393directional hypothesis, 47–48, 227discrim<strong>in</strong>ate function analysis, 333–334discussion-analysis tally sheet, 121discussion section, of researchproposal, 626distribution curve, 192distribution of sample means,218, 220 (fig.)documents, 536double-bl<strong>in</strong>d experiment, 277Dump<strong>in</strong>g Ground or EffectiveAlternative: Dropout-Prevention Programs <strong>in</strong>Urban Schools study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 514–530dynamic systems, 424 (table)EECER. See Exceptional Child<strong>Education</strong> Resources (ECER)ecological generalizability, 104<strong>Education</strong>al Adm<strong>in</strong>istrationQuarterly, 74<strong>Education</strong> Index, 69, 74–75effect size, 177, 244emic perspective, 504empathic neutrality, 424 (table)empirical (observable) referents, 28Encyclopedia of <strong>Education</strong>al<strong>Research</strong>, 68equivalent-forms method, 155–156,158 (table), 161ERIC Database, 115 (fig.)ERIC Document ReproductionService, 113ERIC onl<strong>in</strong>e, 71–72, 113, 114 (fig.)errors of measurement, 154–155, 158,183 (fig.)essay question, 133–134eta (), 206–207, 247, 250–251ethical questions, 29ethicschildren, research with, 59–60cl<strong>in</strong>ical trials, 55, 57confidentiality, 56–59, 433–434deception, issues of, 56–59def<strong>in</strong>ed, 53ensur<strong>in</strong>g confidentiality ofresearch data, 56examples <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g ethicalconcerns, 57–59examples of unethical practice,53, 58 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g, 452<strong>and</strong> material found on Internet, 83<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research, 565<strong>and</strong> pr<strong>in</strong>ciples, statement ofethical, 53–55protect<strong>in</strong>g participants from harm,55, 57–59<strong>and</strong> qualitative research, 433–434regulation of research, 60–63ethnicity subject characteristic, 369ethnographic researchaction research <strong>and</strong>, 600–601advantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages of,513–514<strong>and</strong> cod<strong>in</strong>g observational data, 444concepts guid<strong>in</strong>g, 503–505contextualization <strong>and</strong>, 503–504crystallization <strong>and</strong>, 512culture, 503data analysis, 510–512data collection <strong>in</strong>, 506–510def<strong>in</strong>ed, 12–13, 501–502descriptive field notes, 507Dump<strong>in</strong>g Ground or EffectiveAlternative: Dropout-Prevention Programs <strong>in</strong>Urban Schools study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 514–530emic perspective <strong>and</strong>, 504etic perspective <strong>and</strong>, 504example of, 514–530explora<strong>to</strong>ry design <strong>and</strong>, 560field diary <strong>and</strong>, 506–507field jott<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong>, 506–507field log <strong>and</strong>, 507field notes <strong>and</strong>, 506–510f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 502holistic perspective <strong>and</strong>, 503hypothesis <strong>and</strong>, 505–506key events <strong>and</strong>, 511member check<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>, 504multiple realities <strong>and</strong>, 504nonjudgmental orientation <strong>and</strong>, 504participant observation <strong>and</strong>, 506patterns <strong>and</strong>, 511<strong>and</strong> qualitative research, 427,431–432questions, 27reflective field notes <strong>and</strong>, 507Roger Harker <strong>and</strong> His Fifth GradeClassroom study, 512–513sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>, 505statistics <strong>and</strong>, 511–512thick description <strong>and</strong>, 504titles of studies, 502<strong>to</strong>pics lead<strong>in</strong>g <strong>to</strong>, 504–505triangulation <strong>and</strong>, 510–511value of, 502–503visual representation <strong>and</strong>, 511etic perspective, 504evaluative statement, 123ex post fac<strong>to</strong> research, 363. See alsocausal-comparative researchExceptional Child <strong>Education</strong>Resources (ECER), 71, 76Excite search eng<strong>in</strong>e, 81 (fig.)expectancy table, 153, 153 (table)experience (or behavior) question, 448experimental design. See alsoexperimental research;qualitative research; s<strong>in</strong>glesubjectresearchquasi-experimental design, 271–273true, 266–271, 621weak, 265experimental group, 262experimental researchattitude of subjects threat,174–175, 276 (table), 279causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,365, 369–370characteristics of, 262–264Computer Simulation Activitystudy <strong>and</strong> analysis, 282–293control designs <strong>in</strong>, 264–275counterbalanced designs, 271–272def<strong>in</strong>ed, 7, 10–11, 262examples of, 262, 281–293<strong>and</strong> experimental treatments,control of, 280–281extraneous variables, control of, 264fac<strong>to</strong>rial designs, 273–275group comparisons, 262group design <strong>in</strong>, 264–275his<strong>to</strong>ry threat, 276 (table), 279implementation threat, 276 (table),279–280<strong>in</strong>dependent variablemanipulation, 263<strong>in</strong>strumentation, 276 (table), 279location threat, 276 (table), 279match<strong>in</strong>g-only design, 271maturation threat, 276 (table), 279mortality threat, 276 (table), 279one-group pretest-posttest design,265–266one-shot case study, 265quasi-experimental designs,271–273questions, 27r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with match<strong>in</strong>g,268–271r<strong>and</strong>omization, 263r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only controlgroup design, 267, 269–270r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttest controlgroup design, 267–269r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-groupdesign, 268–269regression threat, 276 (table), 279s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 11static-group comparison design, 266static-group pretest-posttestdesign, 266subject characteristics threat,278–279test<strong>in</strong>g threat, 276 (table), 279threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity, controlof, 276–277threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity,evaluat<strong>in</strong>g likelihood of, 276(table), 277–280time-series designs, 272–273true designs, 266–271uniqueness of, 261–262value of, 14weak designs, 265experimental variable, 42, 261. See also<strong>in</strong>dependent variableexpert op<strong>in</strong>ion, 4–5, 10 (fig.)explana<strong>to</strong>ry design, 561, 563–564explana<strong>to</strong>ry study, 329–330explora<strong>to</strong>ry design, 560, 562(fig.), 564external criteria, 255external criticism, 538–539external validity, 565action research <strong>and</strong>, 595–596def<strong>in</strong>ed, 102generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from sample, 102–104<strong>and</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 311extraneous variables, 43–45,263–264extreme case sample, 431FF value, 232face-<strong>to</strong>-face <strong>in</strong>terviews, 400–402fac<strong>to</strong>r analysis, 334fac<strong>to</strong>rial design, 273–276Family Privacy Act of 1974 (BuckleyAmendment), 62feasibility questions, 29feel<strong>in</strong>gs question, 448–449field diary, 506–507field jott<strong>in</strong>gs, 506–507field log, 506–507field notes, 112<strong>and</strong> action research, 593def<strong>in</strong>ed, 506descriptive field notes, 507example of, 508–510field diary, 506–507field jott<strong>in</strong>gs, 506–507field log, 506–507reflective field notes, 507–508figures section, of researchproposal, 627first quartile (Q 1 ), 194five-number summary, 194–195flowchart, 120, 121 (fig.)focus group <strong>in</strong>terview, 451–452footnotes section, of researchproposal, 627foreshadowed problems, 425formal research, 595 (table)forma<strong>to</strong>f questionnaire, 398–399of research proposal, 625formulas KR20 <strong>and</strong> KR21, 156, 158frequencies dialog box, <strong>in</strong> SPSS,A-9 (fig.)frequency distribution, 187–188,A-7–A-10frequency polygons, 187–189,190 (fig.), 243, 370frequency table, 207–210Friedman two-way analysis ofvariance, 233, 234 (table)GGalaxy <strong>in</strong>dex, 80gender subject characteristic, 369general achievement tests, 127general comments, 624generalizability, 168, 565, 589.See also external validitygeneralization<strong>in</strong> his<strong>to</strong>rical research, 540–541<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 432–433value of, 432


INDEX I-4generalization tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 177generalized statement, 123generaliz<strong>in</strong>gecological generalizability, 104population generalizability,102–103from sample, 102–104general references, of literature, 67,69–72, 74general research types, 14–16Go Network <strong>in</strong>dex, 80Google, 81, 82 (fig.)Graduate Record Exam<strong>in</strong>ation, 127graphic rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, 117 (fig.)grounded-theory research, 427,429–430group adm<strong>in</strong>istration, 393group design, <strong>in</strong> experimentalresearch, 264. See alsocomparison-groupexperimentscounterbalanced design, 271–272,276 (table)fac<strong>to</strong>rial design, 273–275,276 (table)match<strong>in</strong>g-only design, 271,276 (table)one-group pretest-posttest design,265–266, 276 (table)one-shot case study, 265,276 (table)quasi-experimental design, 271–273r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with match<strong>in</strong>g,268–271r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only controlgroup design, 267r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttest controlgroup design, 267–269r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-groupdesign, 268–269, 276 (table)static-group comparison design,266, 276 (table)static-group pretest-posttestdesign, 266, 276 (table)time-series design, 272–273,276 (table)true experimental designs,266–271, 621weak experimental designs, 265grouped frequency distribution, 187,190–191group performance, 365“group play,” 130–132group studiescompar<strong>in</strong>g, 168categorical data, 251–253quantitative data, 243–247homogeneous subgroups, 368<strong>and</strong> regression threat, 175–176Gueron, Judith M., 61HH<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Research</strong> onTeach<strong>in</strong>g, 68Hawthorne effect, 174–175HemAssist trial, 57HHS. See Department of Health<strong>and</strong> Human Services (HHS)high-stakes test<strong>in</strong>g, 150his<strong>to</strong>gram, 189, 191 (fig.),A-12 (fig.)his<strong>to</strong>rical research, 427, 431–432.See also narrative researchadvantages <strong>and</strong> disadvantages <strong>in</strong>,541–542data analysis <strong>in</strong>, 540def<strong>in</strong>ed, 13, 15, 534–535examples of, 536, 542–551f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 541generalization <strong>in</strong>, 540–541Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w: SelfActualization <strong>in</strong> a Period ofTransition study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 543–551policy <strong>and</strong>, 539purposes of, 534–535questions pursed through, 535sources, 536–540categories of, 536–537documents <strong>and</strong>, 536evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, 538–540examples of, 537external criticism, 538–539<strong>in</strong>formation from,summariz<strong>in</strong>g, 538<strong>in</strong>ternal criticism, 539–540numerical records <strong>and</strong>, 536oral his<strong>to</strong>ry <strong>and</strong>, 536oral <strong>in</strong>terview, 536oral statements <strong>and</strong>, 536primary versussecondary, 537problem formulation <strong>and</strong>,535–536relics <strong>and</strong>, 537steps <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong>, 535–540his<strong>to</strong>ry threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity,172–173, 175, 178–179causal-comparativeresearch, 368causal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310holistic cultural portrait, 502holistic perspective, 424 (table), 503homogeneous sample, 431homogeneous subgroups, 368HotBot <strong>in</strong>dex, 80 (fig.)HotBot search eng<strong>in</strong>e, 81 (fig.)human sacrifice, 6hypothesis, 6, 19–20, 426. See alsoentries for specific researchtypesadvantages of stat<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> addition<strong>to</strong> research questions, 46conta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g modera<strong>to</strong>rvariables, 43def<strong>in</strong>ed, 45directional versus nondirectional,47–48disadvantages of stat<strong>in</strong>g, 46–47<strong>and</strong> ethnographic researchers,505–506<strong>and</strong> mixed-methods research, 563null, 224, 225 (fig.), 227–228, 235practical versus statistical,226–228qualitative research <strong>and</strong>, 426research, 224, 225 (fig.)<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 619significance level .05,224–225, 235significant, 47, 224–225s<strong>in</strong>gle research question suggestsseveral, 45test<strong>in</strong>g, 224–228hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g, 223–225Iimplementation threat, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338def<strong>in</strong>ed, 175–176, 178 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279–280m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310<strong>in</strong>dependent samples t-Test dialogbox, A-13 (fig.)<strong>in</strong>dependent variable, 7, 365causal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 367def<strong>in</strong>ed, 261versus dependent variables, 42–43examples of, 263manipulation of, 262–263Index of Consumer Sentiment, 403<strong>in</strong>dexes, 67, 69, 80–82 (fig.), 83<strong>in</strong>ductive analysis, 424 (table)<strong>in</strong>ference techniquesnonparametric, 229, 233–234parametric, 228–233power of statistical test, 235–236summary of, 234–235<strong>in</strong>ference test, 245, 370<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics, 255compar<strong>in</strong>g more than one sample,222–223comparison groups, quantitativedata, 243–247confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals, 220–222,223 (fig.)confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals <strong>and</strong>probability, 222def<strong>in</strong>ed, 216–217distribution of sample means,218, 219 (fig.)example of, 245–247hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g, 223–226<strong>in</strong>ference techniques, 228–236<strong>in</strong>terpretation of, 247–251logic of, 217–223<strong>and</strong> power analysis, 236practical versus statisticalsignificance, 226–228purpose of, 244–245for relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> group,253–254relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> groups <strong>and</strong>quantitative data, 247–251sample size, 230samples t-test exercise, 231sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, 217, 218 (fig.)st<strong>and</strong>ard error of differencebetween sample means, 223st<strong>and</strong>ard error of mean, 219–220techniques for answer<strong>in</strong>g questionsabout data, 228–236<strong>in</strong>formal conversational <strong>in</strong>terview,447 (table)<strong>in</strong>formal <strong>in</strong>terview, 446–447<strong>in</strong>formant, 112, 447–448<strong>in</strong>formant <strong>in</strong>struments, 112<strong>in</strong>formation overload, 6<strong>in</strong>formed consent, necessity for,452–453. See also ethicsInfoseek, 84<strong>in</strong>quiry method, versus lecturemethod, 172, 176<strong>in</strong>-school research, 600Institutional Review Boards (IRB), 60<strong>in</strong>strumental case study, 441<strong>in</strong>strumentation, 19–20. See also entriesfor specific research typesaction research <strong>and</strong>, 593–594causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,367–368, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 336, 342data, def<strong>in</strong>ed, 110data analysis preparation, 140–141data collec<strong>to</strong>r bias, 170–171data collec<strong>to</strong>r characteristics, 170data-collection <strong>in</strong>struments,examples of, 116–134def<strong>in</strong>ed, 110–111develop<strong>in</strong>g, 113importance of valid, 147<strong>in</strong>formant <strong>in</strong>struments, 112<strong>in</strong>strument decay, 169–170key questions, 110–111means of classify<strong>in</strong>g datacollection,112–116measurement scales, 137–140norm-referenced versus criterionreferenced<strong>in</strong>struments,136–137objectivity, 111reliability, 111researcher <strong>in</strong>struments, 112<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 622–623score types, 134–136social studies <strong>in</strong>struments, 113–115subject <strong>in</strong>struments, 112survey research <strong>and</strong>, 395–399, 404tally sheet, 112as threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity,169–171, 178–179, 276(table), 279, 340–341types of, 115–116usability, 111–112validity, 111<strong>in</strong>strument decay threat, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,368, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 340, 342def<strong>in</strong>ed, 169–170<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 277, 279<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310survey research <strong>and</strong>, 404<strong>in</strong>telligence test, 128–129, 159–161.See also entries forspecific tests<strong>in</strong>teraction effect, 273–275, 275 (fig.)<strong>in</strong>terlibrary loans, 84<strong>in</strong>ternal criticism, 539–540<strong>in</strong>ternal-consistency method, 158(table), 161alpha coefficient, 157–158def<strong>in</strong>ed, 156Kuder-Richardson approaches,156–157split-half procedure, 156<strong>in</strong>ternal validity. See also entriesfor specific research types<strong>in</strong> action research, 594–595def<strong>in</strong>ed, 166–167existence of threats <strong>to</strong>, 371 (fig.)fac<strong>to</strong>rs reduc<strong>in</strong>g likelihood off<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g relationship, 177–178his<strong>to</strong>ry threat <strong>to</strong>, 172–173, 178–179implementation threat <strong>to</strong>, 175–176,178–179<strong>in</strong>strumentation threat <strong>to</strong>, 169–171location threat <strong>to</strong>, 169, 175, 178–179


I-5 INDEX www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7e<strong>in</strong>ternal validity—Cont.maturation threat <strong>to</strong>, 173, 175,178–179mortality threat <strong>to</strong>, 167–168, 175,178–179<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 433regression threat <strong>to</strong>, 175–176,178–179<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 623subject attitude threat <strong>to</strong>, 174–175,178–179subject characteristics threat <strong>to</strong>,167–189survey research <strong>and</strong>, 404threats <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research,367–370<strong>in</strong> correlational research,337–341<strong>in</strong> everyday life, 175<strong>in</strong> experimental research,277–280m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179, 179 (table)test<strong>in</strong>g, 171–172, 175, 178–179International Journal of Science<strong>Education</strong>, 76<strong>in</strong>terpretive exercises, 123, 133<strong>in</strong>terpretive statement, 123<strong>in</strong>ter-quartile range (IQR), 194<strong>in</strong>terval measurement scale, 137–139<strong>in</strong>terval score, 136<strong>in</strong>tervention studies, 15, 168,174–175, 369<strong>in</strong>terview behavior, 449–451<strong>in</strong>terviewer. See also data collec<strong>to</strong>rbias, as threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity; data collec<strong>to</strong>rcharacteristics<strong>and</strong> survey research responserates, 404tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 400–401<strong>in</strong>terview guide approach, 447<strong>in</strong>terview research, 27<strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g. See alsoquestion(s); survey researchaction research <strong>and</strong>, 593closed, fixed response, 447 (table)closed-ended questions, 16ethics <strong>in</strong>, 452face-<strong>to</strong>-face, 400–401focus groups, 451–452how not <strong>to</strong>, 452<strong>in</strong>formal, 446–447<strong>in</strong>formal conversational, 447 (table)<strong>in</strong>formed consent, necessity for, 452<strong>in</strong>terview behavior, 449–451<strong>in</strong>terview guide approach,447 (table)key-ac<strong>to</strong>r, 447–448<strong>to</strong> measure ability, 401nonresponse <strong>to</strong> surveys, 402open-ended questions, 12, 16personal, 394–395purpose of, 445–446question types, 448–449record<strong>in</strong>g data from, 452retrospective, 447semistructural, 446st<strong>and</strong>ardized open-ended, 447 (table)structured, 446telephone, 400types of, 446–447<strong>in</strong>terview schedule, 112, 119, 395,399 (fig.)<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic case study, 430–431Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, 127IPAT Anxiety Scale, 125IQR. See <strong>in</strong>ter-quartile range (IQR)IQ scores, advantages of, 243IRB. See Institutional ReviewBoards (IRB)item formats, 132–134JJournal of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>,67, 74Journal of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> ScienceTeach<strong>in</strong>g, 67, 74Kkey-ac<strong>to</strong>r <strong>in</strong>terview, 447–448key ac<strong>to</strong>rs, 447–448key events, 511–512key <strong>in</strong>formant, 447–448K<strong>in</strong>sey, Alfred, 395knowledge question, 448Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis ofvariance, 233, 234 (table)Kuder Preference Record, 125Kuder-Richardson approaches,156–157, 158 (table)LL<strong>and</strong>on, Alf, 102latent content, 477–479, 483lecture method, <strong>in</strong>quiry versus,172, 176level of significance, 224Librarian’s Index <strong>to</strong> the Internet,80–81 (fig.)life his<strong>to</strong>ry, 428Likert scale, 124, 126 (fig.)l<strong>in</strong>ear relationship, 247literature review, 19–20computer search, 76–84def<strong>in</strong>ed, 67general references, 67, 69–72, 74<strong>in</strong>terlibrary loans, 84meta-analysis, 84–85<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 620–621search terms <strong>and</strong>, 72sourcesbibliographic card, 72, 74(fig.), 75note card, 75–76primary, 67, 74–76secondary, 67–68types of, 67–68steps <strong>in</strong> literature search, 68–76value of, 67writ<strong>in</strong>g report, 84–85location threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>,338–340, 342def<strong>in</strong>ed, 169<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imize, 175, 178–179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310survey research <strong>and</strong>, 404logic, 5, 10 (fig.)longitud<strong>in</strong>al study, 391LookSmart <strong>in</strong>dex, 80loss of subjects. See mortality threatLycos search eng<strong>in</strong>e, 81 (fig.)Lydia Ann S<strong>to</strong>w: Self Actualization <strong>in</strong>a Period of Transition study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 543–551Mmail survey, 393–394cover letter for, 399–400cover letter sample for, 400 (fig.)nonresponse <strong>to</strong>, 402major premise, 5“M<strong>and</strong>lebrot Bug,” 8–9manifest content, 477–479, 483manipulated variable, 42Mann-Whitney U test, 233, 234(table), 235. See alsononparametric techniquesMANOVA. See multivariate analysisof variance (MANOVA)marketable job skill subjectcharacteristic, 369–370match<strong>in</strong>gmechanical, 270–271r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with, 268–271statistical, 270–271, 368of subjects, 264, 368match<strong>in</strong>g-only design, 271, 276 (table)match<strong>in</strong>g questions, 123, 132maturation threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,368, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338def<strong>in</strong>ed, 173, 175, 178 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310maximal variation sample, 432Mead, Margaret, 502mean average, 192–194, 224.See also spread(s)exercise, 198probability areas between, <strong>and</strong>different z scores, 200 (fig.)“mean of the means,” 218measurement errors, 158, 236.See also errors ofmeasurement; st<strong>and</strong>ard errorof measurement (SEMeas)measurement scales, 139–140mechanical match<strong>in</strong>g, 270–271median, 192–193, 198. See alsospread(s)median average, 192–193member check<strong>in</strong>g, 453, 504Mental Measurements Yearbooks,The, 114Messick, Samuel, 160meta-analysis, 16<strong>and</strong> effect size, 177<strong>and</strong> literature review report, 84–85metaphysical question, 28Metropolitan Achievement Test, 127M<strong>in</strong>nesota Multiphasic PersonalityInven<strong>to</strong>ry, 125m<strong>in</strong>or premise, 5mixed-methods researchadvocacy lenses, 562classification system for, 563contradic<strong>to</strong>ry f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>and</strong>, 564data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis, 564def<strong>in</strong>ed, 16, 557–558design, choos<strong>in</strong>g appropriate,563–564design issues, 562–563drawbacks <strong>to</strong>, 558–559ethics <strong>in</strong>, 565evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, 564–565example <strong>and</strong> analysis of,565–583explana<strong>to</strong>ry design <strong>and</strong>, 561,563–564explora<strong>to</strong>ry design <strong>and</strong>, 560, 564feasibility of, 563his<strong>to</strong>ry of, 559<strong>in</strong>compatibility among somemethods, 560mixed-model studies, 563Perceived Family Support,Acculturation, <strong>and</strong> LifeSatisfaction <strong>in</strong> MexicanAmerican Youth: A MixedMethods Exploration study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 566–583purposes of, 558research questions for qualitative<strong>and</strong> quantitativemethods, 563results, write up of, 564sampl<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 562–563steps <strong>in</strong> conduct<strong>in</strong>g, 563–564studies, examples of, 557–558triangulation design <strong>and</strong>, 559,561–564types of, 560–561mixed-model studies, 563mode average, 192–193. See alsospread(s)modera<strong>to</strong>r variables, 43, 273–275mortality threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 368, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>,341–342def<strong>in</strong>ed, 167–168, 175, 178 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310survey research <strong>and</strong>, 404multiple- (or collective) casestudy, 431multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e designs, 305–307multiple choice, 123, 132, 396, 434multiple correlation, coefficien<strong>to</strong>f, 332multiple realities, 504multiple regression, 331–332multiprobe design, 311multitreatment design, 311multivariate analysis of variance(MANOVA), 232Nnarrative description, 186narrative research, 427–428National Commission Test<strong>in</strong>g <strong>and</strong>Public Policy, 160National <strong>Research</strong> Act of 1974, 60National Society for the Study of<strong>Education</strong> (NSSE)Yearbooks, 68naturalistic observation, 442natural laws, 8negative correlation, 203–205, 329negatively skewed, 189, 190 (fig.)nom<strong>in</strong>al measurement scale, 137–139nondirectional hypothesis, 47–48, 227non<strong>in</strong>tervention study, 368nonjudgmental orientation, 504nonl<strong>in</strong>ear (curvil<strong>in</strong>ear) relationship,206 (fig.)nonparametric techniques, 229,233–234, 255nonparticipant observation, 441


INDEX I-6nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g. See alsopurposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g; r<strong>and</strong>omsampl<strong>in</strong>gconvenience sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 98–99,100 (fig.)methods of, 100purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 99, 100 (fig.)r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g versus, 92–93systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 96–98,100 (fig.)nonrepresentation samples, 92 (fig.)nonresponse, <strong>to</strong> survey researchdef<strong>in</strong>ed, 401item, 403–404as threat <strong>to</strong> usefulness ofsurvey, 403<strong>to</strong>tal, 402–403normal curve, 191. See alsodistribution curvedef<strong>in</strong>ed, 192percentages under, 197 (fig.)probabilities under, 199 (fig.)selected values from table, A-3st<strong>and</strong>ard scores <strong>and</strong>, 197–200normal distribution, 192, 196–197, 219norm-referenced <strong>in</strong>struments,136–137note card, 75, 76 (fig.)notes. See field notesnull hypothesis, 224, 225 (fig.)evaluation, 227–228<strong>and</strong> power of statistical test, 235reject<strong>in</strong>g, 235 (fig.), 236numerical data, types of, 185–187numerical rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, 117numerical records, 536“Nuts <strong>and</strong> Dolts” of Teacher Images<strong>in</strong> Children’s PictureS<strong>to</strong>rybooks, The, study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 484–496Oobjectivity, def<strong>in</strong>ed, 111observable (empirical) referents, 28observational data, 443–445observational researchapproaches <strong>to</strong>, 441–442audiotap<strong>in</strong>g, 445cod<strong>in</strong>g observational data,444–445naturalistic, 442nonparticipant, 441observational data, 443–445observation effect, 443observer bias, 443–444observer effect, 443observer expectations, 444participant, 441, 506purpose of, 440versus rat<strong>in</strong>gs, 116–117simulation, 442–443technology, use of, 444–445videotap<strong>in</strong>g, 445observation forms, 119, 120 (fig.)observation techniques, scor<strong>in</strong>gagreement <strong>and</strong>, 159observer bias, 443–444observer effect, 443observer expectations, 444observer-participant, 441one-group pretest-posttest design,265–266, 276 (table)one-shot cast study design, 265,276 (table)one-tailed test, 227–228, 235–236open Direc<strong>to</strong>ry Project <strong>in</strong>dex, 80open-ended question, 12, 396–397,434, 450operational def<strong>in</strong>ition, <strong>in</strong> researchquestions, 31–32op<strong>in</strong>ion (or values) question, 448opportunistic sample, 431optical scanner, 483oral his<strong>to</strong>ry, 428, 536oral <strong>in</strong>terview, 536oral statement, 536ord<strong>in</strong>al measurement scale, 137–139Otis-Lennon, 128outcome variable, 261. See alsodependent variableoutliers, 203overt participant, 441Ppanel study, 391paradigms, 559parallel-forms method, 156. See alsoequivalent-forms methodparameters, 185parametric techniques, 235, 255for analyz<strong>in</strong>g categorical data, 233,234 (table)for analyz<strong>in</strong>g quantitative data,229–233, 234 (table)def<strong>in</strong>ed, 228–229partial correlation, 340 (fig.)participant-as-observer, 441participant observation, 441, 506participants. See also ethics; subjectsof action research, 591, 593as observers, 441<strong>and</strong> qualitative research, 433participation flowchart, 121 (fig.)participa<strong>to</strong>ry action research, 590–591path analysis, 334–335, 335 (fig.)patterns, 510–511Pearson correlation, 233, 247–249, 332Pearson r, 206, 210, 250Perceived Family Support,Acculturation, <strong>and</strong> LifeSatisfaction <strong>in</strong> MexicanAmerican Youth: A MixedMethods Exploration study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 566–583percentile ranks (percentiles), 135–136performance checklist, 120, 122 (fig.),129–130performance <strong>in</strong>struments, 116performance test, 129Personal, Social, <strong>and</strong> FamilyCharacteristics of AngryStudents study <strong>and</strong> analysis,373–386personal contact <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>sight,424 (table)personal <strong>in</strong>terview, 394–395, 434personality (or character) <strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries,125, 127 (fig.)phenomenological study, 427–429phenomenology, 13Piaget, Jean, 401, 442pic<strong>to</strong>rial attitude scale, 127 (fig.)Picture Situation Inven<strong>to</strong>ry, 129,130 (fig.)pie chart, 207–208Piers-Harris Children’s Self-ConceptScale (<strong>How</strong> I Feel AboutMyself), 125placebo effect, 277planned ignorance, 171population(s). See also samples <strong>and</strong>sampl<strong>in</strong>g; survey researchdef<strong>in</strong>ed, 90–91samples <strong>and</strong>, 90–92target versus accessible, 91–92population data form, 185. See alsoparameterspopulation generalizability, 102–103,104 (fig.)portraiture, 428portrayal, 12positive correlation, 203–205,328–329, 332, 338positively skewed, 189, 190 (fig.)positivism, 423–424, 559. See alsoquantitative researchpost hoc analysis, 232postmodernism, 424–426, 559power curve, 235, 236 (fig.)practical action research, 590practical significance, 226–228pragmatists, 559prediction equation, 331prediction study, 330predictive validity, 152–153pretest treatment <strong>in</strong>teraction threat,268, 369primary sources. See also sources ofhis<strong>to</strong>rical research, 537of literature, 69def<strong>in</strong>ed, 67examples of, 67locat<strong>in</strong>g, 74–75obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 74–76professional journals, 74read<strong>in</strong>g, 75reports, 74private experience, 6probabilityareas between mean <strong>and</strong> differentz scores, 200 (fig.)confidence <strong>in</strong>tervals <strong>and</strong>, 222def<strong>in</strong>ed, 199under normal curve, 199 (fig.)<strong>and</strong> z scores, 199–200prob<strong>in</strong>g, 301problem statement, 19–20procedures, 621–623product rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, 117, 118 (fig.)professional associations, 69. See alsoliterature reviewprofessional journal, 74Progress<strong>in</strong>g from Programmatic <strong>to</strong>Discovery <strong>Research</strong> study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 311–324projective devices, 129–130Proxmire, William, 33Psych Abstracts, 69psychoanalytical theory, 475Psychological Abstracts, 67, 69,71–72, 74–76psychological theory of development, 7PsycINFO, 71, 76, 80Publication Manual of the AmericanPsychological Association(APA), 84, 624–625, 627public evidence, 6purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 93, 99, 100 (fig.),103, 426purposive sampl<strong>in</strong>g designs, 475–476Qqualitative computer program,482–483qualitative data, 424 (table)qualitative research, 12–13, 15–16.See also case study; contentanalysis; ethnographicresearch; his<strong>to</strong>rical research;<strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g;mixed-methods research;observational researchapproaches <strong>to</strong>, 427–432au<strong>to</strong>biography, 427–428biographical study, 427case studies, 427, 430–431characteristics of, 424 (table)data analysis <strong>and</strong>, 426–427data collection <strong>and</strong>, 426, 454 (table)data collection <strong>and</strong> analysis <strong>in</strong>,452–453def<strong>in</strong>ed, 421–422ethics <strong>and</strong>, 433–434ethnographic, 427, 431–432,501–530example <strong>and</strong> analysis of, 455–465foreshadowed problems <strong>in</strong>, 425general characteristics of, 422–423generalization <strong>in</strong>, 432–433grounded theory, 427, 429–430his<strong>to</strong>rical research, 427, 431–432hypothesis <strong>and</strong>, 426<strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>, 433<strong>in</strong>terpretations <strong>and</strong> conclusions, 427life his<strong>to</strong>ry, 428<strong>and</strong> mixed-methods research, 557,559–560, 564narrative research, 427–428nonparticipant observation used<strong>in</strong>, 441oral his<strong>to</strong>ry, 428participants <strong>in</strong>, 426phenomenological studies <strong>and</strong>, 425phenomenology, 427–429postmodernism, 424–426purposive sample <strong>and</strong>, 326<strong>and</strong> quantitative research, 434–435quantitative research versus, 422(table), 423–425, 425 (table)questions, 454 (table)reports of, 627sampl<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>, 431–432steps <strong>in</strong>, 425–427validity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong>, 161,453–454Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk: Intervention forBehaviorally ChallengedYouths study <strong>and</strong> analysis,455–467qualitiz<strong>in</strong>g, 564quantitative data, 210averages, 192–194boxplots, 194–195versus categorical data, 186–187compar<strong>in</strong>g groups, 243–247correlation, 200–207correlation coefficients <strong>and</strong>scatterplots, 203–206def<strong>in</strong>ed, 186frequency polygons, 187–189his<strong>to</strong>gram <strong>and</strong> stem-leaf plots,189–191<strong>in</strong>ferential techniques, commonlyused, 234 (table)mean, 192–194mode, 192–193nonparametric techniques foranalyz<strong>in</strong>g, 233, 234 (table)normal curve, 191–192normal curve <strong>and</strong> z scores, 200


I-7 INDEX www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7equantitative data—Cont.parametric techniques for analyz<strong>in</strong>g,229–233, 234 (table)Pearson product-momentcoefficient, 206–207probability <strong>and</strong> z scores, 199–200quartiles <strong>and</strong> five-numbersummary, 194–195range, 195relat<strong>in</strong>g variables with<strong>in</strong> groups,247–251scatterplots, 201–206skewed polygons, 189spreads, 194–197st<strong>and</strong>ard curve <strong>and</strong> normal curve,197–200st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, 195–196st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of normaldistribution, 196–197summariz<strong>in</strong>g, 187–207T scores, 200z scores, 197–200quantitative research, 12, 15–16.See also mixed-methodsresearch; positivismaction research <strong>and</strong>, 594–596<strong>and</strong> mixed-methods research, 557,559–560, 564nonparticipant observationused <strong>in</strong>, 441qualitative research <strong>and</strong>, 434–435qualitative research versus, 422(table), 423–425, 425 (table)quantitative variables, 39–42, 365quantiz<strong>in</strong>g, 564quartiles, 194quasi-experimental design, 271–273question(s). See also <strong>in</strong>terviews <strong>and</strong><strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g; researchquestions<strong>and</strong> action research, 592–593background (or demographics), 448clear, 29closed-ended, 16, 396–398, 397(tabe), 434dicho<strong>to</strong>mous, 450essay, 133–134ethical, 29experience (or behavior), 448of feasibility, 29feel<strong>in</strong>gs, 448–449his<strong>to</strong>rical research, pursuedthrough, 535<strong>in</strong>terpretive-exercise questions, 123knowledge, 448match<strong>in</strong>g, 123, 132of metaphysical, 28<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods study, 563–564multiple choice, 123, 132, 396, 434open-ended, 12, 16, 396–397, 450op<strong>in</strong>ion (or values), 448pursued through his<strong>to</strong>ricalresearch, 535qualitative <strong>and</strong> quantitativeresearch, 434–435, 454of reality, 16, 18<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 619researchable versusnonresearchable, 28selection items on, 123sensory, 448–449short-answer, 396of significance, 29, 33–34of societal consequences, 17–19for survey research, 396–398true-false, 123, 132of unstated assumptions, 17–18of value, 28of values, 16–18questionnaire, 112<strong>and</strong> action research, 593<strong>and</strong> loss of subjects, 168, 395prepar<strong>in</strong>g, for survey research, 404pretest<strong>in</strong>g, for survey research, 398as subject-completed<strong>in</strong>strument, 123question of communication, 16, 18Rr<strong>and</strong>om assignment, 263r<strong>and</strong>om assignment with match<strong>in</strong>g,268–271r<strong>and</strong>omization, 264, 623r<strong>and</strong>omized posttest-only controlgroup, 267, 276 (table)r<strong>and</strong>omized pretest-posttest controlgroup design, 267–269,276 (table)r<strong>and</strong>omized Solomon four-groupdesign, 268–269, 276 (table)r<strong>and</strong>om numbers, table of, 93–94, A-2r<strong>and</strong>om replacement, 402r<strong>and</strong>om sample, 402, 426, 621r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 255. See alsononr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>gcluster, 94–97feasibility of, 103methods of, 93–96, 97 (fig.)versus nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 92–93simple, 93–94, 97 (fig.)stratified, 94–95, 97 (fig.)table of, 93, 94 (fig.)two-stage, 96, 97 (fig.)r<strong>and</strong>om selection, 263range, 195rat<strong>in</strong>g scale, 112, 116–118rat<strong>in</strong>gs, observations versus, 116–117ratio measurement scale, 137–139raw score, 134–136, 199 (fig.)Read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>Research</strong> Quarterly, 74reality, question of, 16, 28references, of research proposal,624–625, 626reflective field notes, 507–508regressed ga<strong>in</strong> score, 271regression equation, 205, 277regression l<strong>in</strong>e, 330–331regression threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycausal-comparative research<strong>and</strong>, 370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338def<strong>in</strong>ed, 175–176, 178 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310relationships. See also correlationsclarified by educational research, 41curvil<strong>in</strong>ear, 247fac<strong>to</strong>rs reduc<strong>in</strong>g likelihood off<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g, 177–178<strong>in</strong> hypothesis test<strong>in</strong>g, 223–224<strong>in</strong>ferential statistics <strong>to</strong> judgeimportance of, 249–251l<strong>in</strong>ear or straight-l<strong>in</strong>e, 247nonl<strong>in</strong>ear (curvil<strong>in</strong>ear), 206 (fig.)positive <strong>and</strong> negative correlations,203–205<strong>and</strong> research questions, 34, 38–39study<strong>in</strong>g, 15, 38–39reliability, 161<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 478–479<strong>and</strong> correlational coefficients, 337check<strong>in</strong>g, 157def<strong>in</strong>ed, 111, 147–148, 154equivalent-forms method, 156,158 (table)errors of measurement,154–155estimates of, 155<strong>in</strong>ternal-consistency methods,155–158, 158 (table)<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 161scor<strong>in</strong>g observer agreement,158–161, 158 (table)st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement(SEmeas), 158–159test-retest method, 155–156,158 (table)versus validity, 154, 155 (fig.)validity <strong>and</strong> reliability <strong>in</strong>qualitative research, 161<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 453–454<strong>and</strong> survey research, 395reliability coefficient, 155, 157reliability worksheet, 160 (fig.)relics, 537replicated, 6replication, 103external validity <strong>and</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subjectresearch, 311<strong>and</strong> meta-analysis, 177of qualitative studies, 432<strong>and</strong> survey research, 395reports, as primary source, 74representation samples, 92 (fig.)representativeness, 103research. See also specificresearch typesapproaches <strong>to</strong>, 242–243classify<strong>in</strong>g methodologies, 365critical analysis of, 16–19def<strong>in</strong>ed, 7educational concerns, 3formal, 595 (table)implications for educational, 8<strong>in</strong>-school, 600nature of, 3–20process of, 19–20regulation of, 60–63types of, 7, 10–16value of, 4, 13–14researcher-completed <strong>in</strong>strumentsanecdotal records, 122–123behavior rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, 117flowchart, 120<strong>in</strong>terview schedules, 119observation forms, 119–120performance checklist, 120, 122rat<strong>in</strong>g scales, 116–117tally sheet, 120time-<strong>and</strong>-motion log, 123–124researcher <strong>in</strong>struments, 112researcher reflexivity, 454researchers, regulation of research,60–63research hypothesis, 224, 225 (fig.)<strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Science <strong>and</strong>Technological <strong>Education</strong>, 76research proposalabstract, 625budget, 624data analysis, 623def<strong>in</strong>ed, 617def<strong>in</strong>ition <strong>in</strong>, 619–620discussion section of, 626figures, 627footnotes, 627format of, 625general comments, 624<strong>in</strong>strumentation, 622–623<strong>in</strong>ternal validity, 623justification of study, 618–619literature, background <strong>and</strong> reviewof, 620–621organization of, 628 (fig.)outl<strong>in</strong>e of, 627problem formulation <strong>and</strong>, 617procedural details, 623procedure, 621purpose of study, 617–618qualitative research reports, 627references section(bibliography), 627research design, 621research questions orhypothesis, 619results/f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, 625–627rules <strong>to</strong> consider, 624–625sample, 621–622, 627–639suggestions for further researchsection, 627tables, 627research questions. See also question(s)advantage of stat<strong>in</strong>g hypotheses, 46characteristics of good, 29–34def<strong>in</strong>ed, 27def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g terms <strong>in</strong>, 30–32examples of, 27–28<strong>in</strong>vestigate relationships <strong>and</strong>, 34relationships <strong>and</strong>, 38–39research report. See research proposalResources <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> (RIE), 69,70 (fig.), 71–72, 74–75results/f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs, of research proposal,625–627retrospective <strong>in</strong>terview, 447reversal designs, 302. See alsoA-B-A designReview of <strong>Education</strong>al <strong>Research</strong>, 68Review of <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong>, 68RIE. See Resources <strong>in</strong> <strong>Education</strong> (RIE)Roger Harder <strong>and</strong> His Fifth-GradeClassroom study, 512–513role-play<strong>in</strong>g simulation, 442Roosevelt, Frankl<strong>in</strong>, 102Rorschach Ink Blot Test, 129Russian <strong>and</strong> American CollegeStudents’Attitudes,Perceptions, <strong>and</strong> TendenciesToward Cheat<strong>in</strong>g study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 405–415SSamoan culture study, 502sample meansdifference <strong>in</strong>, 230distribution of, 218distribution of difference between,223 (fig.)st<strong>and</strong>ard error of differencebetween, 223samples <strong>and</strong> sampl<strong>in</strong>g. See alsor<strong>and</strong>om sample; statistics<strong>in</strong> action research, 594<strong>and</strong> causal-comparative research,366–367census or, 101compar<strong>in</strong>g more than one,222–223


INDEX I-8<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 475–476correlational research <strong>and</strong>,335–336def<strong>in</strong>ed, 90–93<strong>in</strong> ethnographic research, 505generaliz<strong>in</strong>g from sample, 102methods, review of, 99–101<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research,562–563nonr<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g methods,92–93, 96–100populations <strong>and</strong>, 90–92purposive, 93, 99, 431–432<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 431–432r<strong>and</strong>om versus nonr<strong>and</strong>om,92–93representation versusnonrepresentation, 92 (fig.)<strong>and</strong> research proposal, 621–622sample sizes, 101–102, 230select<strong>in</strong>g, for survey research, 395sample size, 101–102, 230sampl<strong>in</strong>g distribution, 218–219, 221sampl<strong>in</strong>g error, 217–218, 236scatterplotsfor comb<strong>in</strong>ation of variables,339 (fig.)construct, 201, 203<strong>and</strong> correlational coefficients, 365<strong>and</strong> correlational research, 329correlation coefficients <strong>and</strong>,203–206data used <strong>to</strong> construct, 202 (fig.)def<strong>in</strong>ed, 201example of, 203 (fig.), 204,206 (fig.)exercise, 206<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g, 203, 247–251outliers, 203Pearson correlation, 247–249prediction us<strong>in</strong>g, 330–331,330 (fig.)scientific research methodology, 4def<strong>in</strong>ed, 5experimental research, 7, 10–11steps <strong>to</strong>, 6scoresage- <strong>and</strong> grade-level equivalent,135–136choos<strong>in</strong>g, 136derived, 135<strong>in</strong>terval, 136percentile ranks (percentiles),135–136raw, 134–136st<strong>and</strong>ard, 136T, 136z, 136scor<strong>in</strong>g observer agreement, 158–161,158 (table)search eng<strong>in</strong>es, for research<strong>in</strong>gliterature, 80–82search terms, for literaturereview, 72secondary sources. See also sourcesof his<strong>to</strong>rical research, 537of literature, 67–69SED. See st<strong>and</strong>ard error of thedifference (SED)selection bias, 167. See also Subjectcharacteristicsselection item, 131–132self-checklist, 123–124, 125 (fig.)self-report data. See subject-completed<strong>in</strong>strumentsSEM. See st<strong>and</strong>ard error of themean (SEM)semantic differential, 125–126,1126 (fig.)SEMeas. See st<strong>and</strong>ard error ofmeasurement (SEMeas)semistructural <strong>in</strong>terview, 446sensory data, 4sensory experience, 4–5, 10 (fig.)sensory question, 448–449Sequential Tests of <strong>Education</strong>alProgress (STEP), 127short-answer questions, 396. See alsoclosed-ended questionsshotgun approach, 366significance, statistical, 255significance level .05, 224–225, 235.See also statisticalsignificancesignificant hypotheses, 47, 224–228significant research question, 29, 33–34sign test, 233, 234 (table). See alsononparametric techniquessimple r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 93–94,97 (fig.)simulation, 442–443s<strong>in</strong>gle-group (correlational) study, 172s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject designs, 299–305s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject experiment, 597s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject graph, 299, 300 (fig.)s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 11, 27A-B-A-B design, 302–303A-B-A design, 302–303A-B-C-B design, 303–304A-B design, 300–301B-A-B design, 303–304basel<strong>in</strong>e level, return of, 308–309basel<strong>in</strong>es, number of, 309–310behavior, <strong>in</strong>dependence of, 309change, degree <strong>and</strong> speed of, 308characteristics of, 299condition length, 305–306control of threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternalvalidity <strong>in</strong>, 310–311designs, 299–305example of, 311–324examples of designs, 310f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 300graph<strong>in</strong>g of designs, 299–300multiple-basel<strong>in</strong>e design, 305–307Progress<strong>in</strong>g from Programmatic <strong>to</strong>Discovery <strong>Research</strong> study<strong>and</strong> analysis, 311–324threats <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity <strong>in</strong>,305–311variables changed when mov<strong>in</strong>gfrom one condition <strong>to</strong>another, 306–30868-95-99.7 rule, 197size. See sample sizeskewed distribution, 192, 194–195skewed polygon, 189smooth curve, 191–192snowball sample, 432social behavior study, 480–483social consequences, 160Social Science Citation Index(SSCI), 71social studies competency-based<strong>in</strong>struments, 113social studies <strong>in</strong>struments, 113–115societal consequences, questionof, 17–19socioeconomic level of family subjectcharacteristic, 369sociogram, 130–131sociometric devices, 130–132sourcesof his<strong>to</strong>rical research, 536–540of literature, 67–76specific or concrete descriptivestatement, 123split-half procedure, 156spread(s), 193–197boxplots, 194–195<strong>and</strong> compar<strong>in</strong>g groups, 243range, 195st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, 195–196SPSSanalyses, specify<strong>in</strong>g, A-7correlation, calculat<strong>in</strong>g,A-14–A-16data, enter<strong>in</strong>g, A-6Data Edi<strong>to</strong>r, A-5 (fig.), A-8 (fig.)Data View screen with pull-downmenu, A-8 (fig.)descriptive statistics, A-7–A-10Frequencies dialog box, A-9 (fig.)frequency distribution, obta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g,A-7–A-10gett<strong>in</strong>g started, A-5samples t-test, conduct<strong>in</strong>g<strong>in</strong>dependent, A-10–A-14Statistics dialog box, A-10 (fig.)variables, nam<strong>in</strong>g, A-6–A-7SSCI. See Social Science CitationIndex (SSCI)st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation, 195exercise, 198of normal distribution, 196–197st<strong>and</strong>ard error of estimate, 331st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement(SEMeas), 158st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the difference(SED), 226between sample means, 223<strong>and</strong> test<strong>in</strong>g null hypothesis, 224st<strong>and</strong>ard error of the mean (SEM),219–220st<strong>and</strong>ardized open-ended <strong>in</strong>terview,447 (table)st<strong>and</strong>ard scores, 136def<strong>in</strong>ed, 197example of, 201 (fig.)<strong>and</strong> normal curve, 197–200normal curve <strong>and</strong> z scores, 200probability <strong>and</strong> z scores, 199–200T scores, 200z scores, 197–200Stanford Achievement Test, 127Stanford B<strong>in</strong>et Intelligence Scale, 128static-group comparison design, 266,276 (table)static-group pretest-posttestdesign, 266statistical equat<strong>in</strong>g, 270. See alsostatistical match<strong>in</strong>gstatistical <strong>in</strong>ference test, 370statistically significant, 224statistical match<strong>in</strong>g, 270–271statistical power analysis, 326statistical significance, 224–228, 255statistics. See also data; <strong>in</strong>ferentialstatisticsdef<strong>in</strong>ed, 185descriptive, 185–210<strong>and</strong> ethnographic research,511–512<strong>in</strong>ferential, 216–236<strong>in</strong>terpret<strong>in</strong>g, 254versus parameters, 185summary, 252statistics dialog box, <strong>in</strong> SPSS,A-10 (fig.)stem-leaf plot, 189–191STEP. See Sequential Tests of<strong>Education</strong>al Progress(STEP)straight-l<strong>in</strong>e relationship, 247stratified r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 94,95 (fig.), 97 (fig.)stratified sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 476stratify<strong>in</strong>g, 369structural model<strong>in</strong>g, 335structured <strong>in</strong>terview, 446student attentiveness, 6subject attitude threat, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validitydef<strong>in</strong>ed, 174–175, 178 (fig.)m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179subject characteristics threat<strong>to</strong> causal-comparative research, 367causal-comparative research <strong>and</strong>,369–370correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 338,341–344subject characteristics threat, <strong>to</strong><strong>in</strong>ternal validity, 167–180<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 278–279<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310subject-completed <strong>in</strong>strumentsachievement tests, 125, 127,128 (fig.)aptitude tests, 127–129attitude scales, 124–125item formats, 132–134performance tests, 129personality (or character)<strong>in</strong>ven<strong>to</strong>ries, 125projective devices, 129–130questionnaire, 123self-checklist, 123–124, 125 (fig.)sociometric devices, 130–132Subject Guide <strong>to</strong> Books <strong>in</strong> Pr<strong>in</strong>t, 69subject <strong>in</strong>struments, 112subjectsdeception, issues of, 56–57match<strong>in</strong>g, 264, 368<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 433sample, 19–20summary statistics, 252supply item, 132–134survey research, 27action research, 598advantage <strong>and</strong> disadvantages ofsurvey data collectionmethods, 393 (table)closed-ended questions, 396–398correlational research <strong>and</strong>, 392cover letter, prepar<strong>in</strong>g, 399–400cross-sectional, 391data analysis <strong>and</strong>, 404data collection, choos<strong>in</strong>g modeof, 393def<strong>in</strong>ed, 390difficulties <strong>in</strong>volved with, 12direct adm<strong>in</strong>istration <strong>to</strong> group, 393example <strong>and</strong> analysis of, 404–415f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong>, 395<strong>in</strong>strument, prepar<strong>in</strong>g, 395–399<strong>in</strong>strumentation process <strong>in</strong>,problems <strong>in</strong>, 404<strong>in</strong>terviewers, tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 400–401longitud<strong>in</strong>al, 391


I-9 INDEX www.mhhe.com/fraenkel7esurvey research—Cont.mail surveys, 393–394, 399,400 (fig.)nonresponse, 401–404open-ended questions, 396–397personal <strong>in</strong>terviews, 394–395problem, def<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g, 392purpose of, 390–391questionnaire, pretest<strong>in</strong>g, 398questionnaire format, 398–399question types, 396–398Russian <strong>and</strong> American CollegeStudents’Attitudes,Perceptions, <strong>and</strong> TendenciesToward Cheat<strong>in</strong>g study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 405–415sample, select<strong>in</strong>g, 395steps <strong>in</strong>, 393–401target population, identify<strong>in</strong>g,392–393telephone survey, 394threat <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity,evaluat<strong>in</strong>g, 404types of, 391Systematic sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 96–98, 100 (fig.)TTaba, Hilda, 123table of r<strong>and</strong>om numbers, 93, 94 (fig.)tables section, of researchproposal, 627tabulat<strong>in</strong>g, data, 140–141tally sheet, 112, 120, 121 (fig.),479 (table)target population, versus accessiblepopulation, 91–92TAT. See Thematic ApperceptionTest (TAT)team role play<strong>in</strong>g, 442technology, for observation, 444–445.See also computertelephone survey, 394, 400, 402Teoma search eng<strong>in</strong>e, 81 (fig.)test<strong>in</strong>g. See also achievement test;<strong>in</strong>telligence test; T-test<strong>in</strong> causal-comparative research,370–372chi-square, 233–234high-stakes, 150hypothesis, 224–228<strong>in</strong>ference, 245one-tailed, 227–228, 235–236power of, 235–236sign, 233two-tailed, 227–228test<strong>in</strong>g threat, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycorrelational research <strong>and</strong>, 341–342def<strong>in</strong>ed, 171–172, 175, 178 (fig.)<strong>in</strong> experimental research, 276(table), 279m<strong>in</strong>imiz<strong>in</strong>g, 179<strong>in</strong> s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, 310test-retest method, 155–156, 158(table), 161Thematic Apperception Test(TAT), 129theoretical sample, 431Theory <strong>and</strong> <strong>Research</strong> <strong>in</strong> Social<strong>Education</strong> (TRSE), 74, 480Thesaurus of ERIC Descriptions,72, 78thick description, 453, 504third quartile (Q 3 ), 194threats, <strong>to</strong> <strong>in</strong>ternal validity. See<strong>in</strong>ternal validity; <strong>and</strong> entriesfor specific research typestime-<strong>and</strong>-motion log, 123, 124 (fig.)time-series design, 272–273,276 (table)<strong>to</strong>tal nonresponse, 402–403transferability, 565treatment variable, 42, 261. See also<strong>in</strong>dependent variabletrend study, 391triangulation, 453, 510–511<strong>in</strong> action research, 594<strong>in</strong> mixed-methods research, 559,561–564true-false questions, 123, 132T score, 136, 200T-test. See also parametric techniques<strong>in</strong> causal-comparativeresearch, 370conduct <strong>in</strong>dependent samples, 231for correlated means, 230,234 (table)for correlated proportions, 233,234 (table)for difference <strong>in</strong> proportions, 233,234 (table)for <strong>in</strong>dependent means, 229,234 (table)for <strong>in</strong>dependent proportions, 233,234 (table)for means, 229–232, 234 (table)one-tailed, 227–228, 235for proportions, 233, 234 (table)for r, 232–233, 234 (table)results of, A-14 (fig.)two-stage r<strong>and</strong>om sampl<strong>in</strong>g, 96,97 (fig.)two-tailed test, 227–228Tyack, David, 539Type I error, 228–229Type II error, 228–229typical sample, 431Uunique care orientation, 424 (table)United States Census survey, 101unobtrusive measures, 134unstated assumptions, question of,16–18U.S. Census Bureau, 101, 391U.S. Department of <strong>Education</strong>, 14U.S. Job Corps program, 61usability, 111–112Vvalidation, 148validity, 161. See also <strong>in</strong>ternal validitycheck<strong>in</strong>g, 157concurrent, 152consequential, 160construct-related evidence ofvalidity, 148, 149 (fig.),153–154, 158 (table)<strong>in</strong> content analysis, 478–479content-related evidence of,148–152, 149 (fig.),158 (table)criterion-related evidence of,148–149, 149 (fig.),152–153, 158 (table)def<strong>in</strong>ed, 111, 147–148external, 102–104, 311, 565,595–596importance of valid <strong>in</strong>struments,147–154predictive, 152–153<strong>in</strong> qualitative research, 161, 453–454validity coefficient, 152, 337value (H), 233value (U), 233values, question of, 16–18variability, 194variables, 10. See also dependentvariable; <strong>in</strong>dependentvariable; <strong>and</strong> entries forspecific research types <strong>and</strong>variablesbuild<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong><strong>to</strong> design, 264versus constants, 39def<strong>in</strong>ed, 39extraneous, 43–45, 263–264fluctuation <strong>in</strong>, 155–156hold<strong>in</strong>g constant, 264<strong>in</strong>dependent versus dependent,42–43modera<strong>to</strong>r, 43nam<strong>in</strong>g, <strong>in</strong> SPSS, A-6–A-7quantitative versus categorical,39–42relat<strong>in</strong>g, with<strong>in</strong> group, 247–251variable view screen, <strong>in</strong> SPSS, A-7 (fig.)variance, 195–196videotap<strong>in</strong>g, for observation, 445visual representation, 511–512WWAIS-III. See Wechsler AdultIntelligence Scale(WAIS-III)Walk <strong>and</strong> Talk: Intervention forBehaviorally ChallengedYouths study <strong>and</strong> analysis,455–467Web, Max, 541web browser, 80WebCrawler <strong>in</strong>dex, 80Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale(WAIS-III), 128Wechsler Intelligence Scale forChildren (WISC-III), 128Western Electric Company, 174What Do You Mean “Th<strong>in</strong>k Before IAct?”: Conflict Resolutionwith Choices study <strong>and</strong>analysis, 603–611When Teachers’ <strong>and</strong> Parents’ ValuesDiffer study <strong>and</strong> analysis,344–359whiskers. See boxplotsWho’s Who <strong>in</strong> American <strong>Education</strong>, 75Wilk’s lambda, 232WISC-III. See Wechsler IntelligenceScale for Children (WISC-III)World Wide Web (WWW), search<strong>in</strong>gfor literature us<strong>in</strong>g. Computersearch, of literatureadvantages of search<strong>in</strong>g, 83def<strong>in</strong>ed, 80disadvantages of search<strong>in</strong>g, 83–84<strong>in</strong>dexes, 80–82 (fig.), 83search eng<strong>in</strong>es, 80–82written-response <strong>in</strong>struments, 116YYahoo!, 80, 81 (fig.)Zz score, 136advantage, 198–199comparisons of raw scores <strong>and</strong>, ontwo tests, 199 (fig.)def<strong>in</strong>ed, 197normal curve <strong>and</strong>, 199 (fig.)probability <strong>and</strong>, 199–200probability areas between mean<strong>and</strong>, 200 (fig.)st<strong>and</strong>ard error of mean <strong>and</strong>,219–220z scores, 197–200

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!