18.10.2021 Views

Applied English Phonology, Second Edition ( PDFDrive )

Book about English Phonology with exercises

Book about English Phonology with exercises

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

PHONOLOGY 33

we do not have the minimal pairs to prove that two or more sounds are in

contrast, we look for the ‘near-minimal pair’. This is a pair of words that would

be a minimal pair except for some irrelevant difference. What this rather

vague definition says is that the potentially influential elements in the linguistic

environment are kept constant, while others that are unlikely to influence a

change may be different. Essentially, we look at the immediately preceding and

immediately following environments, because these are the primary sources

of contextual conditioning for changes. For example, if we cannot find an exact

minimal pair to show the contrast between [S] and [Z] in English, we can use

the words vision [vIZ@n] and mission [mIS@n], or illusion [@luZ@n] and solution

[s@luS@n]. Although these pairs do not constitute minimal pairs (because the

difference is not solely in the suspicious pair of sounds, [S] and [Z], but also

related to others), the relevant ‘preceding’ and ‘following’ environments of the

suspicious pairs of sounds are kept identical. Similarly, pairs such as lethargy

[lET2dZi] and leather [lED2] for [T] and [D], and lesion [liZ@n] and heathen [hiD@n]

for [Z] and [D], would serve as near-minimal pairs. Thus, we can answer the

question “Do the two sounds occur in the same/similar environment?”

affirmatively, and conclude that the pairs of sounds considered above are in

contrast and belong to two separate phonemes.

2.2.2 Complementary distribution

The other distributional possibility, complementary distribution, presents the

diametrically opposing picture. Here we never find the two or more sounds

in the same environment. Stating it simply, we can say that two sounds are

in complementary distribution if /X/ never appears in any of the phonetic

environments in which /Y/ occurs. Having said that, we can now go back to

some of the examples we gave at the beginning of the chapter and re-examine

them. The first one concerns the dental and alveolar nasal sounds [6] and [n].

In English the distribution of these two sounds is such that they never appear

in the same environment (that is, they are mutually exclusive). We find the

dental only before /T/ or /D/, as in tenth [tE6T], in the game [I6 D@ . . . ], where

the other one never appears. When we find the sound /X/ only in a certain

environment, and the sound /Y/ in a completely different environment, then

it is impossible for the difference between these two sounds to be contrastive,

because a contrast requires an overlapping distribution. In such cases of

complementary distribution, we say that these sounds are allophones of one

and the same phoneme. We should be reminded, in passing, that the very

same two sounds are capable of occurring in the same environment, as we saw

in the case of Malayalam, and function contrastively (thus, belong to two

separate phonemes) in that language.

Another example of the complementary distribution of an allophonic

relationship can be given for [3] and [D] of Spanish. These two sounds can never

occur in the same environment in Spanish; [D] occurs between two vowels

or after a nasal, [3] occurs in the remaining environments. This is clearly

an example of a complementary distribution where the occurrences of the two

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!