05.01.2013 Views

Introductory notes to the Semiotics of Music - Philip Tagg's home page

Introductory notes to the Semiotics of Music - Philip Tagg's home page

Introductory notes to the Semiotics of Music - Philip Tagg's home page

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

36 P Tagg: Notes on <strong>Semiotics</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Music</strong> Musematic analysis<br />

cay’ has only <strong>of</strong>fered five (‘boring’, ‘1980s’, ‘video’, ‘urban’, ‘decay’). Since both respondents<br />

come from a similar background and since <strong>the</strong>y are reacting <strong>to</strong> <strong>the</strong> same<br />

music, it is reasonable <strong>to</strong> regard ‘abandoned warehouse’ and ‘urban decay’ as part <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> same paramusical semantic field. It would also be useful <strong>to</strong> know how many respondents<br />

associated <strong>to</strong> ‘grunge’ or <strong>the</strong> ‘eighties’, how many mentioned some kind <strong>of</strong><br />

social decay (e.g. unemployment) or violence (e.g. ‘chase’, ‘brandish’, ‘chain saw’).<br />

A useful second step in systematising responses is <strong>to</strong> divide <strong>the</strong>m in<strong>to</strong> three basic categories:<br />

[1] opinions, such as ‘boring’, ‘great music’, ‘bad taste’, etc; [2] intramusical associations,<br />

including musical works, styles and structural traits, as well as artists,<br />

composers, instruments, etc; [3] all o<strong>the</strong>r responses. Responses <strong>of</strong> type 2 are useful for<br />

interobjective comparison purposes (p. 36, ff.), while those <strong>of</strong> type 1, although <strong>of</strong> little<br />

direct relevance <strong>to</strong> <strong>the</strong> analysis <strong>of</strong> musical signification, may cast light on patterns <strong>of</strong> response<br />

at a later stage. 54 Responses <strong>of</strong> type 3 — all o<strong>the</strong>rs — will <strong>of</strong> course need fur<strong>the</strong>r<br />

categorisation. Whe<strong>the</strong>r observing behaviour in response <strong>to</strong> music or dealing with verbal<br />

and visual associations <strong>to</strong> music, a basic response taxonomy can be quite useful (see<br />

p. 44, ff.).<br />

There are two main advantages in using intersubjective observation in music semiotics:<br />

(i) it allows you <strong>to</strong> state that a certain proportion <strong>of</strong> a certain body <strong>of</strong> people at a certain<br />

time under certain conditions in a certain sociocultural context responded in a certain<br />

way <strong>to</strong> certain music; (ii) it calls for no musicological expertise whatsoever. Intersubjective<br />

observation provides vital information on many aspects <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basic communication<br />

process (fig. 1, p. 10) in relation <strong>to</strong> a particular body <strong>of</strong> music, especially in terms<br />

<strong>of</strong> reception and response. It also allows you <strong>to</strong> posit viable hypo<strong>the</strong>ses about <strong>the</strong> ‘intended<br />

message’, as well as about which structural elements within <strong>the</strong> ‘channel’ may<br />

or may not be related <strong>to</strong> which responses <strong>to</strong> <strong>the</strong> music. This final aspect — relating observations<br />

about musical reception <strong>to</strong> structural elements — requires considerable familiarity<br />

on <strong>the</strong> analyst’s part with <strong>the</strong> musical object giving rise <strong>to</strong> all those<br />

intersubjectively shared patterns <strong>of</strong> response. It is in this context that an interobjective<br />

approach comes in handy in helping us discover how particular patterns <strong>of</strong> musical<br />

structuration relate <strong>to</strong> particular patterns <strong>of</strong> response.<br />

Interobjectivity<br />

If an analytical approach establishing consistency <strong>of</strong> response <strong>to</strong> <strong>the</strong> same piece <strong>of</strong> music<br />

played <strong>to</strong> different listeners is called intersubjective, <strong>the</strong>n an interobjective approach<br />

is one which establishes consistency <strong>of</strong> structure between different pieces <strong>of</strong> music.<br />

While intersubjective inquiry allows you <strong>to</strong> relate a piece <strong>of</strong> music in general <strong>to</strong> a set <strong>of</strong><br />

responses <strong>to</strong> that music, <strong>the</strong> procedure <strong>of</strong> interobjective comparison relates a particular<br />

piece <strong>of</strong> music <strong>to</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r pieces <strong>of</strong> music.<br />

Now, relating music <strong>to</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r music may seem like a futile exercise because, at least in<br />

academic circles, only verbal and numeric symbols appear <strong>to</strong> own any validity as metalanguage.<br />

We would, in o<strong>the</strong>r words, seem <strong>to</strong> be no closer <strong>to</strong> an explanation <strong>of</strong> what<br />

<strong>the</strong> music under analysis is communicating. However, as we have already observed (p.<br />

18, points 2-3), musical discourse possesses a degree <strong>of</strong> relative au<strong>to</strong>nomy. This means<br />

that although music is most definitely related <strong>to</strong> society, in that it is so obviously connects<br />

with phenomena outside its own discourse, it also actually refers <strong>to</strong> itself on its<br />

own terms. In fact, people in <strong>the</strong> process <strong>of</strong> making music are far more influenced by<br />

54. For example, if <strong>the</strong>re is significant intersubjective agreement that a piece is boring and if <strong>the</strong> music in<br />

question also elicits a large number <strong>of</strong> blank responses, <strong>the</strong>n those respondents may well be unfamiliar<br />

with <strong>the</strong> s<strong>to</strong>re <strong>of</strong> symbols operating in that music (codal incompetence) or opposed <strong>to</strong> <strong>the</strong> sociocultural<br />

norms encoded in <strong>the</strong> music (codal interference). Of course, codal incompetence or interference<br />

may be equally in operation at <strong>the</strong> transmitting end.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!