15.02.2013 Views

La Décision de Devenir Chef d'Orchestre - EPHE

La Décision de Devenir Chef d'Orchestre - EPHE

La Décision de Devenir Chef d'Orchestre - EPHE

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

MINISTERE DE LA JEUNESSE, DE L’EDUCATION NATIONALE<br />

ET DE LA RECHERCHE<br />

ECOLE PRATIQUE DES HAUTES ETUDES<br />

Sciences <strong>de</strong> la Vie et <strong>de</strong> la Terre<br />

DIPLOME POSTDOCTORAL<br />

Présenté par:<br />

Ioannis MAKRIS<br />

Pour l’obtention du gra<strong>de</strong> <strong>de</strong> diplômé <strong>de</strong> l’Ecole Pratique <strong>de</strong>s Hautes Etu<strong>de</strong>s<br />

Discipline: Psychologie<br />

<strong>La</strong> <strong>Décision</strong> <strong>de</strong> <strong>Devenir</strong> <strong>Chef</strong> d’Orchestre :<br />

Un Inventaire Systématique <strong>de</strong>s Motifs<br />

Soutenu le 16 Décembre 2006 <strong>de</strong>vant le jury composé <strong>de</strong><br />

Prési<strong>de</strong>nt:<br />

Monsieur CELLIER Jean-Marie, DE E.P.H.E., Sorbonne -Paris<br />

Assesseur :<br />

Monsieur CHASSEIGNE Gérard, Professeur, Université <strong>de</strong> Champagne Monsieur<br />

RAUFASTE Eric, Professeur, Université du Mirail<br />

Examinateur :<br />

Madame MUNOZ SASTRE Maria Teresa,Professeur, Université du Mirail<br />

Monsieur MULLET Etienne, DE <strong>EPHE</strong>, MDR-Toulouse<br />

Diplôme préparé dans le laboratoire Ethique et Travail <strong>de</strong><br />

l’Ecole Pratique <strong>de</strong>s Hautes Etu<strong>de</strong>s<br />

Maison <strong>de</strong> la Recherche, UTM, F-31058, Toulouse ce<strong>de</strong>x 1, France.<br />

(Directeur d’Etu<strong>de</strong>s <strong>EPHE</strong>: Monsieur Etienne Mullet)<br />

Adresse éléctronique <strong>de</strong> M. le Professeur Etienne Mullet: mullet@univ-tlse2.fr<br />

Adresse électronique personnellle: makrisconductor@yahoo.gr<br />

L’objectif <strong>de</strong> la présente étu<strong>de</strong> consistait à examiner, les divers motifs qui pouvaient faire <strong>de</strong><br />

quelqu’un chef d’orchestre. En raison <strong>de</strong> la diversité <strong>de</strong>s motifs, nous avons décidé d’utiliser le cadre<br />

large <strong>de</strong> la Reversal Theory (Apter 2001). En tant que cadre théorique général pour la personnalité,<br />

nous avons utilisé la théorie du Big Five (Goldberg, 1992). Cent-un chefs d’orchestre, 17 femmes et<br />

81 hommes, ont complété un premier questionnaire <strong>de</strong> 92 items référant à d’éventuels motifs pour<br />

<strong>de</strong>venir chef d’orchestre, un second questionnaire <strong>de</strong> 50 items <strong>de</strong> personnalité, et un troisième qui<br />

comportait <strong>de</strong>s items se rapportant aux caractéristiques personnelles <strong>de</strong>s participants. Une analyse<br />

factorielle confirmatoire fut réalisée sur les données brutes au questionnaire <strong>de</strong> motifs. Le modèle<br />

testé a été un modèle à huit facteurs dérivé <strong>de</strong> la Reversal Theory. Il a aussi été montré que les motifs<br />

évoqués sont en relation notable avec la personnalité <strong>de</strong>s participants.<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 1


THE DECISION TO BECOME AN ORCHESTRA CONDUCTOR:<br />

A SYSTEMATIC INVENTORY OF MOTIVES<br />

The present study aimed at examining, the various motives that may lead an individual to<br />

becoming a classic orchestra conductor. In view of the diversity of potential motives, it was<br />

<strong>de</strong>ci<strong>de</strong>d to use the broad framework offered by the Reversal Theory (Apter). The theoretical<br />

framework for the examination of personality was based on the Big Five Theory (Goldberg, 1992).<br />

One hundred and one conductors (17 females and 81 males, 50 of them Greeks first filled in a<br />

questionnaire composed of 92 items referring to possible motives one may have for becoming an<br />

orchestra conductor. The same sample then filled in a 50-item questionnaire which <strong>de</strong>alt with<br />

personality matters and then filled a third questionnaire with questions salient to the personal<br />

characteristics of the participants. A confirmatory factor analysis was conducted on the raw data as<br />

that <strong>de</strong>rived from the Reversal Theory. The mo<strong>de</strong>l tested was correlated on the eight-factor mo<strong>de</strong>l.<br />

It was proven that much of what participants claimed in their answers was largely based on their<br />

personal characteristics.<br />

Discipline: Psychologie<br />

Mots clés: Musique, théorie <strong>de</strong> renversement, motif, chef d’orchestre.<br />

<strong>La</strong>boratoire Ethique et Travail <strong>de</strong> l’Ecole Pratique <strong>de</strong>s Hautes Etu<strong>de</strong>s, Maison <strong>de</strong> la Recherche,<br />

UTM, F-31058, Toulouse ce<strong>de</strong>x 1, France.<br />

Sommaire<br />

Remerciements…………………………………………………………… 3<br />

Sommaire……………………………………………………………………. 4<br />

Introduction………………………………………………………………….. 5<br />

Métho<strong>de</strong>………………………………………………………………………. 44<br />

Résultats……………………………………………………………………… 47<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 2


Discussion……………………………………………………………………... 61<br />

Références…………………………………………………………………….. 69<br />

Annexes………………………………………………………………………. 79<br />

Introduction<br />

L’objectif <strong>de</strong> la présente étu<strong>de</strong> était d’examiner, en nous appuyant sur un cadre théorique<br />

actuel et précis (la théorie du renversement <strong>de</strong> Apter et la théorie <strong>de</strong>s cinq facteurs <strong>de</strong> la<br />

personnalité - Big Five Factor -), les divers motifs (raisons) qui pouvaient faire quelqu’un déci<strong>de</strong><br />

<strong>de</strong> <strong>de</strong>venir un chef d’orchestre. L’objectif était également d’examiner les relations existant entre ces<br />

motifs et les facteurs <strong>de</strong> base <strong>de</strong> la personnalité.<br />

Références bibliographiques<br />

Apter, M. J. (1982). The experience of motivation: The theory of psychological reversals. New<br />

York: Aca<strong>de</strong>mic Press.<br />

Apter, M. J. (1989). Reversal Theory: Motivation, emotion and personality. London: Routledge.<br />

Apter, M. J. (2001) (Ed.). Motivational styles in everyday life: A gui<strong>de</strong> to Reversal Theory.<br />

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.<br />

Apter, M. J. & Batler, R. (1997). Gratuitous risk: A study of parachuting. In S. Svebak & M. J.<br />

Apter (Eds.), Stress and health: A reversal theory perspective (pp. 119-129). Washington D.C.:<br />

Taylor & Francis.<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 3


Bechne, K.-E. (1997). The <strong>de</strong>velopment of Musikerleben in adolescence: How and why children<br />

listen to music (pp. 143-159). In, I. Deliège, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Perception and cognition of<br />

music. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.<br />

Boal-Palheiros, G. M., & Hargreaves, D. J (2001). Listening to music at home and at school.<br />

British Journal of Music Education, 18, 103-118.<br />

Bower, G.H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In The handbook of emotion and<br />

memory: research and theory, (ed. S-A. Christianson), pp. 3-31. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Brand, M. (201). Chinese and American music majors: Cross-cultural comparisons in motivation<br />

and strategies for learning and studying. Psychology of Music, 29, 170-178.<br />

Clarke, E.F. (1988). Generative principles in music performance. In Generative principles in<br />

music. The psychology of performance, improvisation, and composition, (ed. J.A.Sloboda), pp. 1-26.<br />

Oxford, U.K: Clarendon Press.<br />

Clynes, M. (1989). Sentics: The touch of emotions. Bridport, UK: Prism Press.<br />

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (Neo-PI-R) and NEO<br />

Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. O<strong>de</strong>ssa, Florida: Psychological Assessment<br />

Resources.<br />

Cottencin, A., Mullet, E., & Sorum, P. (2006). Consulting an alternative practitioner: A systematic<br />

inventory of motives. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine.<br />

Davidson, J.W., Sloboda, J.A., Moore, D.G. and Howe, M.J.A. (1996). Characteristics of music<br />

teachers and the progress of young instrumentalists. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46.1,<br />

141-160.<br />

Diaz <strong>de</strong> Chumaceiro, C. L. (2004). Serendipity and pseudoserendipity in career paths of successful<br />

women: Orchestra conductors. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 345-356.<br />

Digman, J. M. (1996). The Curious History of the Five-Factor Mo<strong>de</strong>l. In J.S. Wiggins (Eds.),<br />

The Five-Factor Mo<strong>de</strong>l of Personality: Theoretical Perspective (pp. 1-20). Guilford Press.<br />

Gabrielsson, A. & Juslin, P.N. (1996). Emotional expression in music performance: between the<br />

performer’s intention and the listener’s experience. Psychology of Music, 24, 68-91.<br />

Friberg, A. (1991). Generative rules for music performance: a formal <strong>de</strong>scription of a rule system.<br />

Computer Music Journal, 15, 56-71.<br />

Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In p.n. Juslin, 7 J.A.<br />

Sloboda(Eds.), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research. New York: Oxford University Press, 431-<br />

449.<br />

Gagne, F. (1999). Nature or nurture? A re-examination of Sloboda and Howe’s (1991) interview<br />

study on talent <strong>de</strong>velopment in music. Psychology of Music, 27, 38-51.<br />

Gallup, G., Jr., & Castelli, J. (1989). The people’s religion. New York: Macmillan.<br />

Girau<strong>de</strong>au, C., Chasseigne, G, Apter, M. J., & Mullet, E. (2006). Adult’s lay views about<br />

intelligence : A Reversal Theory approach. Personality and Individual Differences.<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 4


Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The <strong>de</strong>velopment of markers of the Big-Five factor structure.<br />

Psychological Assessment, 4, 26-42.<br />

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, personality inventory measuring the<br />

lower-level facets of several Five-Factor mo<strong>de</strong>ls. In I. Merviel<strong>de</strong>, I. Deary, F., De Fruyt, and F.<br />

Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality Psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7-28). Tilburg, The Netherlands:<br />

Tilburg University Press.<br />

Haroutounian, J. (1995). Talent i<strong>de</strong>ntification and <strong>de</strong>velopment in the arts: An artistic/ educational<br />

dialogue. Roeper Review, 18, 112-17<br />

Hergraves D.J., North A.C.(Eds). (1997). The social psychology of music. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Hergraves D.J., North A.C., (1999). The functions in every day life: Re<strong>de</strong>fining the social in<br />

Music Psychology. Psychology of Music, 27, 71-83.<br />

Holbrook, M. & Anand, P. (1990). Effects of tempo and situtional arousal on the listener’s<br />

perceptual and affective responces to music. Psychology of Music, 18, 150-62.<br />

Howe, M.J.A., Davidson, J.W., Moore, D.G and Sloboda, J.A. (1995). Are there early childhood<br />

signs of musical ability? Psychology of Music, 23, 162-176.<br />

Juslin, P. N. (1997). Emotional communication in music performance: Afunctionalist perspective<br />

and some data. Music Perception, 14, 383-418.<br />

Juslin, P. N.(2000). Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music performance: Relating<br />

performance to perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and<br />

Performance, 26, 1797-813.<br />

Juslin, P. N., & <strong>La</strong>ukha, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A<br />

review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. Journal of New Music Research, 33, 217-238.<br />

Kamiya, J. (1990) Operant control of the EEG alpha rhythm and some of its reported effects on<br />

consciousness. In Altered states of consciousness, (3rd edn) (ed. C.T.Tart), pp. 600-11. San Fransisco,<br />

CA: Harper.<br />

Kenealy, P.(1988). Validation of a music mood induction procedure: Some preliminary findings.<br />

Cognition and Emotion 2, 41-8.<br />

Kelly, G.A.(1963). A theory of personality. The psychology of personal constructs. New York:<br />

W.W. Norton & Co.<br />

Kerr, J.H., Kawaguchi, C., Oiwa, M., Terayama, Y., & Zukawa, A. (2000) Stress, anxiety and<br />

other emotions in Japanese mo<strong>de</strong>rn dance performance. Pacific Journal of Psychology, 11, 1, 16-33.<br />

Konecni, V.J.(1982). Social interaction and musical preference. In D. Deutsch (Ed.). The<br />

psychology of music. San Diego, CA: Aca<strong>de</strong>mic Press.<br />

<strong>La</strong>khdar, M, Vinsonneau, G., Apter, M. J., & Mullet, E. (2006). Conversion in Islam among<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 5


French adolescents and adults: A systematic inventory of motives. International Journal of<br />

Psychology of Religion.<br />

<strong>La</strong>mont, A., Hargreaves, D. J., Marshall, N. A., & Tarrant, M. (2003). Young people’s music in<br />

and out of school. British Journal of Music Education, 20, 229-241.<br />

Lehmann, A.C. (1997). Acquired mental representations in music performance: Anecdotal and<br />

preliminary empirical evi<strong>de</strong>nce. In Does practice make perfect? Current theory and research on<br />

instrumental music practice, (es. H. Jorgensen & A.C.Lehmann), pp. 141-63. Oslo, Norway:<br />

Norwegien State Aca<strong>de</strong>my of Music.<br />

Merriam, A.P. (1964). The anthropology of music. Chicago: Northwestern University Press.<br />

Meyer, L.B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.<br />

Neill, S. L. (1998). Motivating factors for stu<strong>de</strong>nts participation in high school choral programs<br />

and vocal enrichment activities. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social<br />

Sciences, 59, p. 1105.<br />

North,A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. and O’Neill, S.A. (200). The importance of music to adolescents.<br />

British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 255-272.<br />

North,A.C., Hargreaves, D.J.(1997a). Experimental aesthetics in everyday life. In D.J.Hargreaves<br />

& A.C. North (Eds), The social psychology of music (pp.84-103). Oxford, UK: Oxford University<br />

Press.<br />

North,A.C., Hargreaves, D.J.(1999). Can music move people? The effects of musical complexity<br />

and silence on waiting time. Environment and behavior, Vol.31, No.1, January 1999, 136-149<br />

Ohgushi ,K. & Senju, M. (1987). How are the player’s i<strong>de</strong>as conveyed to the audience? Music<br />

Perception, 4, 311-24.<br />

Parker, G. B., & Brown, L.B. (1982). Coping behaviors that mediate between life events and<br />

<strong>de</strong>pression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1386-1391.<br />

Parncutt, P., Sloboda, J.A., & Clarke, E.F.(1997). Inter<strong>de</strong>pendance of right and left hands in sightread,<br />

written, and rehearsed fingerings of piano music in parallel octaves. In Proceedings of the Third<br />

Triennial ESCOM Conference, (ed. A. Gabrielsson), pp. 702-5. Uppsala, Swe<strong>de</strong>n: Department of<br />

Psychology, Uppsala University.<br />

Persson, R. S. (1993). The subjectivity of musical performance: An exploratory musicpsychological<br />

real world enquiry into the <strong>de</strong>terminants and education of musical reality. Unpublished<br />

dissertation, School of Human and Health sciences, Hud<strong>de</strong>rsfield University.<br />

Persson, R. S. (1995). Musical reality: Exploring the subjective word of performers. In Song and<br />

signification. Studies in music semiotics, (ed. R. Monelle & C.T. Gray), pp. 58-63. Edinburgh, UK:<br />

Faculty of Music, University of Edinburgh.<br />

Person, R.S., Pratt.g., & Robson,C.(1996). Motivational and influential components of musical<br />

performance: A qualitative analysis. In Fostering the growth of high ability: European perspectives,<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 6


(ed. A.J.Cropley & D. Dehn), pp.287-301. Norwood NJ:Ablex.<br />

Persson, R. S. (2001). The subjective world of the performer (pp. 275-289). In, P. N. Juslin, & J.<br />

A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.<br />

Rapoport, E. (1996). Emotional expression co<strong>de</strong> in opera and lied singing. Journal of New Music<br />

Research, 25, 109-49.<br />

Rigg, M.G. (1940). Speed as a <strong>de</strong>termination of musical mood. Journal of Experimental<br />

Psychology, 27, 566-71.<br />

Rippere, V.(1977)What’s the thing to do when you’re feeling <strong>de</strong>pressed? – A pilot study. Behavior<br />

Research and therapy, 15, 185-191.<br />

Roe, K. (1985). Swedish youth and music: Listening patterns and motivations. Communication<br />

Research, 12, 353-362.<br />

Robert, E., Thayer, J., Robert, N., and Tracey, M.McC. (1994). Self – regulation of Mood:<br />

Strategies for changing a Bad Mood, Raising Energy, and Reducing Tension. Journal of personality<br />

and Social Psychology, Vol. 67, No5, 910-925.<br />

Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2002). Assessing the Big Five: applications of 10 psychometric<br />

criteria to the <strong>de</strong>velopment of marker scales. In B. De Raad, & M. Perugini (Eds.), Big Five<br />

Assessment (pp. 29-58). Hogrefe and Huber Publishers.<br />

Saucier, G., & Ostendorf, F. (1999). Hierarchical sub-components of the Big-Five personality<br />

factors: A cross-language replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 613-627.<br />

Shore, R.E. (1990). Hypnosis and the concept of the general reality of orientation. In altered<br />

states of consciousness, (3rd edn) (ed. C.T.Tart), pp. 281-301, San Fransisco, CA: Harper.<br />

Sloboda, J.A., (1998). Does music mean anything? Musicae Scientiae, 2, 21-32.<br />

Sloboda, J.A. and Howe, M.J.A. (1999). Musical Talent and individual differences in musical<br />

achievement: Areply to Gagné (1999). Psychology of Music, 27, 52-54.<br />

Sloboda, J.A. and Howe, M.J.A. (1991). Biographical precursors of musical excellence: An<br />

interview study. Psychology of Music, 19, 3-21.<br />

Sloboda, J.A., Davidson, J.W., Howe, M.J.A. and Moore, D.G.(1996). The role of practice in the<br />

<strong>de</strong>velopment of expert musical performance. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 287-309.<br />

Stanislavski, C. (1988). An actor prepares. London: Methun Drama. (Originally published in<br />

1937).<br />

Steptoe, A. (2001). Negative emotions in music making: the problem of performance anxiety (pp.<br />

291-308). In P. N. Juslin, & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion: Research and Practice.<br />

Oxford: Oxford University Press.<br />

Stratton, V. N., & Zalanowski, A. H. (2003). Daily music listening habits in college stu<strong>de</strong>nts:<br />

Related moods and activities. Psychology and Education: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 40, 1-11.<br />

Tarrant, M., North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). English and American adolescents’ reasons<br />

for listening to music. Psychology of Music, 28, 166-173.<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 7


Thayer,R.E. (1978). Toward a psychological theory of multidimensional activation (arousal).<br />

Motivation and Emotion, 2,1-34.<br />

Thayer, R.E. (1987a). Energy, tiredness and tension effects of a sugar snak vs. mo<strong>de</strong>rate exercise.<br />

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 119-125.<br />

Thayer, R.E. (1987b). Problem perception, optimism, and related states as a function of time of<br />

day (diurnal rhythm), and mo<strong>de</strong>rate exercise: Two arousal states in interaction. Motivation and<br />

Emotion, 11, 19-36.<br />

Thayer, R.E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New York: Oxford University<br />

Press.<br />

Thayer, R.E. (1991). Practical short – term strategies for maintaining an optimal mood. In R.E.<br />

Thayer (Chair), Theoretical implications and practical applications of mo<strong>de</strong>rn mood theory.<br />

Symposium conducted at the 99th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,<br />

San Francisco.<br />

Thayer, R.E., Peters, D.P. III, Takahashi, P.J., & Birkhead – Flight, A.M. (1993). Mood and<br />

behavior (smoking and sugar snacking) following mo<strong>de</strong>rate exercise: A partial test of self – regulation<br />

theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 97-104.<br />

Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for<br />

changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social<br />

Psychology, 67, 910-925.<br />

Waterman, M. (1996). Emotional responses to music. Implicit and explicit effects in listeners and<br />

performers. Psychology of Music, 24, 53-67.<br />

Watt R.J. & Ash, R.L.(1998). A psychological investigation of meaning in music. Musicae<br />

Scientiae, 2, 33-54.<br />

Wedin, L. (1972). A multidimensional study of the perceptual-emotional qualities in music.<br />

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 13, 241-57.<br />

Wells, A, & Hakanen, E. A. (1991). The emotional use of popular music by adolescents.<br />

Journalism Quarterly, 68, 445-454.<br />

Wells, A, Hakanen, E. A., & Tokinoya, H. (2000). The emotional use of popular music by<br />

Japanese adolescents (pp. 174-181). In, S. Waksman (Ed.), Popular music: Intercultural<br />

interpretations. Bowling Green: Bowling Green State University.<br />

<strong>EPHE</strong> Banque <strong>de</strong> Monographies SVT 8

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!