24.02.2013 Views

annual report 2011 - Office for Research - Northwestern University

annual report 2011 - Office for Research - Northwestern University

annual report 2011 - Office for Research - Northwestern University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Loran Nordgren<br />

Kellogg School of Management<br />

The Empathy Gap between Perception<br />

and Pain<br />

Torture is prohibited by statutes worldwide, yet the legal<br />

definition of torture is almost invariably based on an<br />

inherently subjective judgment involving pain severity.<br />

Because most policy makers have never experienced<br />

waterboarding and other enhanced interrogation tactics<br />

<strong>for</strong> themselves (Senator McCain aside), they must rely on<br />

their inexperienced intuitions about how painful these<br />

practices seem. Yet work being done by Loran Nordgren,<br />

management and organizations, has shown that people<br />

who aren’t enduring a painful experience have a hard<br />

time estimating just how painful it is. This finding is<br />

known as an empathy gap, and it helps to explain why<br />

doctors underestimate their patients’ pain and why<br />

patients themselves underestimate how painful upcoming<br />

procedures will be.<br />

To test the idea that empathy gaps might distort the<br />

evaluation of torture policy, Nordgren and members<br />

of his lab asked people to evaluate common enhanced<br />

interrogation tactics such as sleep deprivation, solitary<br />

confinement, and exposure to cold temperatures. Some<br />

participants (the control group) evaluated these practices<br />

under normal conditions, while others (the experimental<br />

group) made their judgments while experiencing a mild<br />

version of the pain associated with those techniques. These<br />

participants submerged their hand in ice water (exposure to<br />

cold temperatures), felt social exclusion by being rejected<br />

by a group (solitary confinement), and were tested at the<br />

end of a three-hour night class (sleep deprivation).<br />

In each case the experimental group estimated the<br />

interrogation tactics to be significantly more painful, less<br />

ethical, and less acceptable as a practice than participants<br />

who made their judgments free of pain, as policy makers<br />

normally would. These findings suggest that policy makers<br />

(and the concerned public) suffer from a fundamental bias<br />

when evaluating waterboarding and other interrogation<br />

tactics. It appears that the legal standard <strong>for</strong> evaluating<br />

torture is psychologically untenable. According to<br />

Nordgren, policy makers simply cannot appreciate the<br />

severity of interrogation practices they themselves have<br />

not experienced, a psychological constraint that in effect<br />

encourages torture.<br />

Andrew Campbell<br />

Excellence in <strong>Research</strong> | Annual Report <strong>2011</strong> 47

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!