10.04.2013 Views

CONTENT - International Society of Zoological Sciences

CONTENT - International Society of Zoological Sciences

CONTENT - International Society of Zoological Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

S17 ICZ2008 - Abstracts<br />

Reproductive behavioral plasticity in Père David’s deer<br />

Zhigang Jiang 1,2 , Yan Zeng 1,2 , Zhenyu Zhong 3 , Chunwang Li 1 and<br />

Linyuan Zhang 3<br />

1<br />

Key Laboratory <strong>of</strong> Animal Ecology and Conservation Biology,<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Zoology, Chinese Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Sciences</strong>, Beijing,<br />

100101, China.<br />

2<br />

Graduated School <strong>of</strong> Chinese Academy <strong>of</strong> <strong>Sciences</strong>, Beijing,<br />

China 100049, China.<br />

3<br />

Beijing Père David’s Deer Park<br />

Père David’s deer is a polygamous species; one to several stags<br />

rut at the same time, a “Harem master” defends the receptive<br />

female herd and tries to monopolize the opportunity <strong>of</strong> mating.<br />

However, reproductive behavior in Père David’s deer is highly<br />

plastic. First, the males have different rut tactics: “Harem master<br />

tactic”, “Challenger tactic” and “Bachelor tactic”, rut season is<br />

extended for a long period, e.g. rut lasts as long as two months in<br />

the Beijing Père David deer population. Thus, the time to<br />

commence rut is plastic in stags. Female “estrus window”, which is<br />

composed <strong>of</strong> the individual females in estrus, drifts among years.<br />

That means unknown factor controls the estrus in the females, or<br />

females are able to adjust their estrus. We conducted a study on<br />

the reproductive behavior <strong>of</strong> Père David’s deer from 1994 to 2005.<br />

We observed the behaviors <strong>of</strong> males and females and used five<br />

polymorphic microstatellite loci which were screened out from 84<br />

pairs <strong>of</strong> species-transferred primers to study the mating system in<br />

the reintroduced Père David’s deer populations in China. The<br />

study identified multi stags sired <strong>of</strong>fspring in the population.<br />

“Harem masters” could not monopolize breeding opportunities,<br />

Challengers also had chance to breed. While the plasticity in<br />

female estrus window presumably is related to female cryptic sex<br />

choice, on the other hand, to be a “Harem master” is not the “The<br />

winner takes all” tactics. The multi-paternity in Père David’s deer<br />

indicates the rut tactics like “Challenger tactic” has its evolutionary<br />

fitness as well.<br />

Relationships between morphological traits, behavioural<br />

characteristics, and habitat selection in Iranian Wheatears<br />

using Fourth-Corner Problem analysis<br />

Mohammad Kaboli 1 , Mansour Aliabadian 2 , Saina Habibi 1 , Parisa<br />

Mehrandish 1 and Roger Prodon 3<br />

1<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Fishery and Environment, Faculty <strong>of</strong> Natural<br />

Resources, University <strong>of</strong> Tehran, Tehran, Iran<br />

2<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Biology, Faculty <strong>of</strong> Science, Ferdowsi University <strong>of</strong><br />

Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran<br />

3<br />

Laboratoire Ecologie et Biogéographie des Vertébrés (EPHE),<br />

Centre d’Ecologie Fonctionnelle et Evolutive, UMR 5175, 1919<br />

route de Mende, 34293 Montpellier cedex 5, France<br />

Ecological segregation in the Wheatear genus Oenanthe is a<br />

complex and intriguing question. Here we studied habitat variables,<br />

behavioural characteristics and morphological traits <strong>of</strong> 11<br />

Wheatear species that are distributed sympartrically in Iran. We<br />

recorded 19 behavioural characteristics and measured 36 habitat<br />

variables within a 100-m radius around observed birds. We also<br />

measured 21 biometrical variables in each species. We compared<br />

variable-to-variable correlation between these three data matrices<br />

using Legendre et al.’s Fourth-Corner Problem method. We first<br />

related, through the matrix A1 (presence/absence <strong>of</strong> species in<br />

sampling sites), the morphological traits <strong>of</strong> the species (mean<br />

values <strong>of</strong> the variables for each species; matrix Cmorphology) to the<br />

habitat features measured on each sampling site (matrix Bhabitat).<br />

We then related, through A1, the behavioural traits <strong>of</strong> the species<br />

(mean values <strong>of</strong> the variables for each species; matrix Cbehaviour) to<br />

the habitat features measured on each sampling site (matrix<br />

Bhabitat). We lastly related, through A2, the morphological traits <strong>of</strong><br />

the species (matrix Cmorphology) to the behavioural variables<br />

measured, on each sampling site, on the "principal" species <strong>of</strong><br />

Wheatears <strong>of</strong> the site (matrix Bbehaviour). Our results showed that<br />

morphological traits, particularly flight and foot-leg apparatuses,<br />

represent a good synthesis <strong>of</strong> overall behaviour traits. Since<br />

Morphological traits result from compromise between different<br />

selection pressures like foraging mode and migration pattern,<br />

seems plays an important role in determining the ecological range<br />

<strong>of</strong> a species. Likewise, habitat features are also correlated with<br />

behavioural traits, but less tightly.<br />

- 60 -<br />

This correlation between behaviour and habitat could be explained<br />

by this fact that many behavioural variables are defined on the<br />

habitat-basis (e.g. types <strong>of</strong> perch used). But the proportion <strong>of</strong><br />

significant relationships between habitat and behaviour remains<br />

the same when we consider only the "purely" behavioural variables<br />

(type, speed, and frequency <strong>of</strong> movements). Finally, The match<br />

between morphology and habitat is relatively loose that may partly<br />

be due to the fact that habitat variables are more likely depend on<br />

proximal factors, especially the sampling conditions (e.g., unequal<br />

frequency <strong>of</strong> different landscape features in different sampling<br />

areas, random component <strong>of</strong> the detection <strong>of</strong> the species).<br />

Control <strong>of</strong> copula duration: how males and females control<br />

sperm transfer<br />

Joshka Kaufmann, Jean-François Le Galliard and David Laloi<br />

Laboratoire Ecologie & Evolution - UMR 7625, Université Pierre et<br />

Marie Curie / ENS / AgroParisTech / CNRS, Paris, France<br />

In polygynandrous mating systems, sexual conflict can arise on the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> partners and on the reproductive investment <strong>of</strong> each<br />

sex. Particularly, traits that control sperm transfer can have quite<br />

different optima with regard to female and male fitness. Indeed,<br />

female behaviour may have evolved to ensure fertilization, favour<br />

preferred male through longer copulation or promote postcopulatory<br />

mate choice. Males may strategically allocate their<br />

sperm according to female reproductive quality, level <strong>of</strong> sperm<br />

competition and further mating opportunities. Behavioural<br />

mechanisms have been poorly studied in taxa, such as reptiles,<br />

where sperm transfer is driven by copula duration rather than by<br />

the number <strong>of</strong> copulations. In the present study, we investigate in<br />

the common lizard whether males and females control copula<br />

duration. In a sequential mating context, we study how mate<br />

choice and copulatory behaviours vary with individual quality (e.g.<br />

coloration, social dominance, fitness-related physical<br />

performances) and mating history. We principally disentangle the<br />

respective role <strong>of</strong> both sexes in this conflict for sperm transfer.<br />

Both sexes become more choosy in second mating opportunities,<br />

female preference being based on male ventral coloration while<br />

males prefer virgin females. Our results show that females control<br />

copula duration and hence sperm transfer and may therefore bias<br />

paternity. Nevertheless, no evidence was found that females<br />

favour high-quality males through longer copulation. Here, we<br />

suggest that female lizards may use both pre-copulatory mate<br />

choice and control <strong>of</strong> copula duration to maximize the genetic<br />

benefits <strong>of</strong> multiple mating.<br />

Is discrimination <strong>of</strong> song quality by shortened-wings female<br />

Canaries (Serinus canaria) reduced ?<br />

Alexandre Lerch and Laurent Nagle<br />

Laboratory <strong>of</strong> Ethology and Comparative Cognition, Université de<br />

Paris X-Nanterre ; Bat BSL, 1er étage; 200, avenue de la<br />

République, 92 000, Nanterre cedex 01, France<br />

Because <strong>of</strong> their greater investment in reproduction, females are<br />

supposed to be choosier than males when mating (Trivers, 1972).<br />

While numerous studies investigated this aspect <strong>of</strong> mate choice,<br />

most <strong>of</strong> them have focused on male traits that maintain female<br />

preferences (Jennion & Petrie, 1997).<br />

Nevertheless, consensus between females in the selection <strong>of</strong> male<br />

phenotypic features is not the rule, and more subtle mechanisms<br />

take place. Indeed, variations in female preferences do exist,<br />

especially in some contexts <strong>of</strong> predation (Simcox et al., 2005) or<br />

costly competition (Fawcett and Johnstone, 2003). In birds, a case<br />

<strong>of</strong> reduced discrimination for male coloration has been reported in<br />

female zebra Finches with shortened wings (Burley & Foster,<br />

2006).<br />

In canaries (Serinus canaria), song is known to be the main<br />

secondary sexual feature, and a special phrase (« A » phrase) is<br />

reported to elicit a high level <strong>of</strong> sexual responses by females<br />

(Vallet et al., 1998). In our study, we investigate the power <strong>of</strong><br />

discrimination toward songs <strong>of</strong> different quality, in a group <strong>of</strong><br />

female canaries tested in two conditions: 1) with intact wings<br />

(control group) and 2) with cut wings. Our preliminary results<br />

suggest that birds with shortened wings show a weaker preference<br />

toward high quality songs.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!