18.08.2013 Views

Long-term Memory

Long-term Memory

Long-term Memory

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Long</strong>-<strong>term</strong> <strong>Memory</strong><br />

• Tests of long-<strong>term</strong> memory<br />

– single item recognition<br />

• how to measure performance?<br />

– increased “hits” (saying old to targets) could be a bias to<br />

always say old<br />

– compare the hit rate to the false alarm rate (saying old to<br />

distractors / foils / lures)<br />

» assuming that recognition is performed based on<br />

familiarity, bias and memory can be separated<br />

» various techniques for separating bias and memory<br />

performance<br />

– free recall<br />

– cued-recall (paired associate learning)<br />

• study FLAG-SPOON, DRAWER-SWITCH, etc.<br />

• FLAG-______<br />

– results are similar to free recall


Theories<br />

• levels of processing: the only difference between STM<br />

and LTM is whether encoding is “shallow” or “deep”<br />

– memory is a continuum; not different components<br />

– deep processing is more semantic and self-referential and<br />

results in stronger, longer lasting memories<br />

– shallow processing is concerned with physical characteristics<br />

and results in weaker, temporary memories<br />

– Craik and Tulving (1975) had participants study adjectives with<br />

the following four tasks (listed from worst to best memory)<br />

• Is it in upper case?<br />

• Does it rhyme with ?<br />

• Does it fit in the sentence ?


• Modal view of memory (qualitative differences<br />

between STM and LTM)<br />

– Capacity<br />

• STM around 7 chunks, LTM nearly unlimited<br />

– Duration<br />

• STM around 30 seconds, LTM can last a lifetime<br />

–Coding<br />

• STM is acoustic<br />

– confuse “b” and “g”<br />

• LTM is semantic<br />

– Baddeley (1966a) – 20 minutes after studying a list<br />

» poor recall for semantically similar lists (HUGE, BIG, SIZE,<br />

etc.)<br />

» normal recall from acoustically similar lists (MAD, MAP,<br />

MAN, etc.)


Forgetting: Storage versus Retrieval<br />

• interference at retrieval<br />

– memory is like a cluttered desk<br />

– all memory traces are preserved in LTM, but it’s hard<br />

to retrieve them<br />

• can’t find the right retrieval cues<br />

– if all traces compete<br />

• both proactive and retroactive interference


• interference at storage (neural networks)<br />

– memory is like many pictures drawn on top of each<br />

other so its hard to see any individual picture<br />

– when memories are added, the representation is<br />

permanently changed<br />

• new memories overwrite old memories (retroactive<br />

interference)<br />

• old memories make it more difficult to encode new memories<br />

(proactive interference)<br />

– an example – adding memories to a Hopfield network<br />

• when memories are stored, links between the features of a<br />

memory are created<br />

• at retrieval, activated features serve as the retrieval cues and<br />

the system cycles until the activation no longer changes


Nurse<br />

Sick<br />

Study List<br />

Lawyer<br />

Medicine<br />

Health<br />

Hospital<br />

Dentist<br />

Physician<br />

cure<br />

Patient<br />

Office<br />

Surgeon<br />

Bed<br />

Rest<br />

Awake<br />

Tired<br />

Dream<br />

Snooze<br />

Blanket<br />

Doze<br />

Slumber<br />

Snore<br />

Nap<br />

Yawn


Results<br />

• Forgetting Curves<br />

– Ebbinghaus’s (1885) nonsense syllable experiment<br />

• over the course of days, he kept on relearning lists of nonsense<br />

syllables (e.g., RUR, HAL, BEIS, etc.)<br />

• forgetting was measured as percent savings (a comparison of<br />

immediate testing versus testing after a delay)<br />

• information is rapidly forgotten at first (non-linear)


• Bahrick’s (1983) Delaware, Ohio experiment<br />

– real-life demonstration of forgetting curves<br />

– students and alumni of Ohio Wesleyan were asked<br />

• free recall of streets, buildings, and landmarks<br />

• using a map, write in street names, buildings, and landmarks<br />

(visually cued)<br />

• recognition of streets, buildings, and landmarks (matching)<br />

– current students who had been there different<br />

amounts of time provided learning curves<br />

• buildings and landmarks learned faster<br />

– alumni of different ages provided forgetting curves<br />

• buildings and landmarks forgotten more slowly


Test List<br />

Lawyer<br />

Doctor<br />

Rest<br />

Sleep


• false memory<br />

effect (DRM)<br />

– higher false alarm<br />

rate for critical<br />

lures (sleep)<br />

Test List<br />

Lawyer<br />

Doctor<br />

Rest<br />

Sleep<br />

Nurse<br />

Sick<br />

Lawyer<br />

Medicine<br />

Health<br />

Hospital<br />

Dentist<br />

Physician<br />

cure<br />

Patient<br />

Office<br />

Surgeon<br />

Bed<br />

Rest<br />

Awake<br />

Tired<br />

Dream<br />

Snooze<br />

Blanket<br />

Doze<br />

Slumber<br />

Snore<br />

Nap<br />

Yawn


• memory is worse when similar items are studied<br />

– A,B,C, vs. A,A’,A’’ (false memory effect)<br />

• memory is better when context is kept the same<br />

– encoding specificity<br />

• study “spelling-BEE” or “honey-BEE” – cue as context<br />

• Golden and Baddeley (1975) – environmental context<br />

– words learned on land or underwater<br />

– Words tested on land or underwater<br />

memory<br />

performance<br />

LTM Effects<br />

study<br />

on land<br />

study<br />

underwater<br />

test<br />

underwater<br />

test<br />

on land


– state-dependent learning<br />

• same design as scuba experiment but using<br />

pharmacological states as a context (Eich, 1980)<br />

memory<br />

performance<br />

study<br />

drunk<br />

study<br />

sober<br />

test<br />

sober<br />

test<br />

drunk


• memory for longer lists is worse (length effect)<br />

– A,B,C vs. A,B,C,D,E,F<br />

• memory is better when stimuli are studied longer<br />

(strength effect)<br />

– A,B,C vs. A,A,A,B,B,B,C,C,C<br />

– distributed study is better than massed study (i.e., cramming)<br />

• spacing effect<br />

– A,A,A,B,B,B,C,C,C vs. C,A,BA,B,C,B,A,C<br />

– Encoding variability<br />

» More cues (many hooks one memory)<br />

» Cue overload (one hook many memories)<br />

• differences between recognition and recall<br />

– low frequency (obscure) items are recognized better but harder<br />

to recall<br />

– high frequency (common) items are harder to recognized but<br />

easier to recall


Narrative and Autobiographical <strong>Memory</strong><br />

• Do laboratory studies of memory apply to real life<br />

memory for stories and personal events?<br />

– Bartlett (1932) thought memory is often reconstructed based on<br />

schemata (constructive view of LTM).<br />

• serial reproduction: recall the same story on more than once<br />

– “War of the Ghosts” became more distorted with each reproduction<br />

– Autobiographical memory for ordinary events<br />

• Marjorie Linton (1975, 1982) spent six years recording the daily<br />

events in her life in a systematic fashion


• each day short descriptions of events<br />

– the date, how distinctive, how emotional, importance to<br />

life goals<br />

• tested herself at delays up to three years<br />

– order two events (which happened first)<br />

– provide exact dates of events<br />

• performance was good at all delays<br />

– real life memories are more durable<br />

» testing is similar to recognition and cued-recall<br />

– she often used problem solving to arrive at the dates of<br />

events<br />

» constructivist view of memory<br />

• events that she could not recall where often similar<br />

to other events she could not recall<br />

– repeated similar events can start to form a schema


– Barsalou (1988) interviewed undergrads for specific<br />

autobiographical events from the summer<br />

• only 21% of recollections were specific<br />

• the rest were “summarized events” (e.g., “I went to the beach<br />

everyday”)<br />

– Brewer (1988) used beepers to test randomly<br />

selected events<br />

• Linton studied the most important events of the day instead<br />

of the ability to recollect any particular event within the day<br />

• at random times (~2 hours), participants were alerted by a<br />

beeper and would write down:<br />

– location, time, actions, and thoughts<br />

– also a number of ratings<br />

» how often this event occurred<br />

» how pleasant<br />

» how trivial or significant<br />

– he also had them write down the most memorable event of<br />

each day


– memory was pretty good overall<br />

» did not fall off rapidly like Ebbinghaus’ studies<br />

– memory was better for actions than for thoughts<br />

– better for the most memorable events of the day<br />

– better for infrequent events and locations<br />

– better memory for rare actions<br />

» the more distinctive the event, the better the memory<br />

for that event


Flashbulb Memories<br />

(Brown & Kulik, 1977)<br />

• personal events at time of disaster<br />

– JFK assassination, Challenger explosion, start of gulf<br />

war, 9/11, etc.<br />

• physiological response<br />

– emotional responses cause hormone release<br />

• better encoding<br />

– Study of Ronald Reagan assassination attempt<br />

• stronger emotional responses = more details


• Neisser (1982b)<br />

– link to history<br />

• Story retelling<br />

– memory strengthening (rehearsal)<br />

– progressive distortion<br />

• Bonhannon’s (1988)<br />

– Challenger explosion<br />

• stronger emotions = greater memory<br />

• more retellings = greater memory<br />

• Weaver (1993) compared intentional memory to<br />

flashbulb memory<br />

– remember next meeting with roommate<br />

• Immediately, 3 months, 1 year<br />

– gulf war started at same time (direct comparison)<br />

• No accuracy differences<br />

– Ebbinghaus forgetting<br />

• Higher confidence for flashbulb<br />

– No relationship between accuracy and confidence


Eyewitness <strong>Memory</strong><br />

• memory integration (malleability)<br />

– Elizabeth Loftus (1979): series of accident slides<br />

• Viewed stop or yield sign<br />

• “Did another car pass the red Datsun while it was stopped at the<br />

stop/yield sign?”<br />

• recognition of slides<br />

– two alternative forced choice testing (2-AFC)<br />

» no need to compare hit and false alarm rates<br />

– 75% if the question was consistent<br />

– 41% if the question was inconsistent<br />

» More influenced by the misleading question


– Loftus (1975) had people view a film (no barn)<br />

• “How fast was the white sports car going when it passed the<br />

barn while traveling along the country road?”<br />

– One week later<br />

» Barn in sentence: 17% reported barn<br />

» No barn in sentence: 3% reported barn<br />

– Bransford and Franks (1971) had people read simple<br />

sentences such as:<br />

• The ants were in the kitchen<br />

• The jelly was on the table<br />

• The jelly was sweet<br />

• The ants ate the jelly<br />

• On a later test, they recognized with highest confidence the<br />

unseen combined sentence:<br />

– The ants in the kitchen ate the sweet jelly that was on the table


• does eyewitness confidence indicate accuracy?<br />

– courts place importance on confident eyewitness testimony<br />

– 36 of 40 DNA overturns were convictions on eyewitness<br />

identification<br />

• what’s the best way to conduct a lineup?<br />

– maximize identification of guilty suspects<br />

• similar to increasing the hit rate<br />

– minimize identification of innocent suspects<br />

• similar to decreasing the false alarm rate<br />

• lineup as a psychology memory experiment<br />

– same concerns<br />

• demand characteristics, bias to say ‘old’, etc.


• selecting distractors in a lineup (known-innocents)<br />

– nominal size: # in the lineup<br />

– functional size: # fit the suspect’s description<br />

– the use of mock eyewitnesses<br />

• if they prefer the suspect, the lineup is flawed<br />

– match-to-description, or also resemble-suspect?<br />

• either way, innocent suspect is protected against erroneous identification<br />

• if the distractors resemble-suspect, identification of guilty suspect is lower<br />

• demand characteristics<br />

– subtle cues<br />

– exert pressure to select (relative judgment)<br />

– double-blind eliminates these concerns<br />

• Not adopted by law enforcement<br />

• relative judgments versus absolute identification<br />

– more likely to convict innocent suspects w/ relative judgments<br />

– “none of the above”<br />

– sequential lineup<br />

• reduces ID of innocent suspects<br />

• doesn’t reduce ID of guilty suspects<br />

• however, might enhance demand characteristics


– is confidence related to accuracy?<br />

• weakly in real life<br />

– meta-analysis: 6% of the variance<br />

• reliable in in the laboratory<br />

– within-subjects correlation<br />

– between-subjects differences are larger<br />

• lineups: 1 observation per subject<br />

– can’t assess type of person<br />

• selection factor<br />

– people who actually select, likely to have high confidence<br />

• in summary<br />

– Confidence: social and personality factors<br />

» positive feedback from investigators<br />

– Accuracy: perceptual and memorial factors


False <strong>Memory</strong><br />

• “psychological problems indicate abuse or<br />

trauma as a child”<br />

– the self-help book, “The Courage to Heal”<br />

• “if you have a pattern of symptoms such as low self-esteem,<br />

depression, etc., then you were probably abused”<br />

– Court cases of recovered abuse<br />

– often memories arise in therapy<br />

• questionable therapy as source<br />

– hypnosis<br />

– sodium amytal<br />

– leading questions<br />

– imagination exercises


– Implanting new memories<br />

• Loftus and Pickrell (1995): 3 true events and one<br />

false “lost-in-the-mall”<br />

– interviews of relatives<br />

– between 4 and 6 years old at time of event<br />

– Booklet: description of the events<br />

» Fill in remembered details<br />

– remembered<br />

» 68% of the true events<br />

» 29% of false events<br />

– still recalled 1 and 2 weeks later<br />

– Differences<br />

» more words used to describe true memories<br />

» true memories rated as being more clear


• Hyman, Husband, and Billings (1995)<br />

– hospitalization, birthday/clown, wedding/punch, evacuate/store<br />

» 80-90% of true events<br />

» no false events recalled in first interview<br />

» 20% in a second interview<br />

» some participants elaborated upon their false memories<br />

(added details never presented)


• Forms of suggestion<br />

– Interviewing suspected criminals<br />

• imagine they participated in a crime<br />

– Therapists<br />

• imagine childhood events to recover them<br />

• self-help books: exercises<br />

– where would the abuse have been likely to occur?<br />

– who is the likely perpetrator?<br />

• Childhood Sexual Abuse (CSA)<br />

– Clancy et al. (2000) used DRM paradigm<br />

• Control group<br />

• Continuous memory of CSA<br />

• “repressed memory” of CSA<br />

• “recovered memory” of CSA<br />

– Recovered group showed much higher false DRM recall


• Loftus – imagination inflation<br />

– Likely to have occurred<br />

– imagination<br />

– Give likelihood ratings again<br />

– increased for imagined<br />

• imagination makes more familiar<br />

• familiarity is misattributed to<br />

childhood<br />

– source confusion


• Impossible memories (first year)<br />

– hippocampus and frontal lobes are underdeveloped<br />

– implanting impossible memories<br />

• good eye coordination comes from mobile<br />

• hypnotized and age regressed<br />

• “guided mnemonic restructuring”<br />

– imagination<br />

• false memory for<br />

– 46% of the hypnotic participants<br />

– 56% of the guided participants


• need experience/knowledge/script Pedzek et al.<br />

(1997)<br />

– Catholic and Jewish participants<br />

» 3 true memories<br />

» 1 false Shabbat and communion<br />

– no give-away words<br />

– 29 Catholic participants<br />

» 7 falsely recognized the Catholic event<br />

» 1 falsely recognized the Jewish event<br />

– 32 Jewish participants<br />

» 3 falsely recognized the Jewish event<br />

» 0 falsely recognized the Catholic event<br />

All these results only mean that recovered<br />

memories might be false


edspread<br />

betrayal<br />

donate<br />

escapade<br />

holly<br />

iceberg<br />

missile<br />

numeral<br />

outcast<br />

painless<br />

plumber<br />

plural<br />

priority<br />

rancher<br />

reversal<br />

skeleton<br />

trigger<br />

trinket<br />

waffle


Amnesia


• Bilateral damage to the medial temporal lobes<br />

– Hippocampus<br />

• memory for events and episodes<br />

– Amygdala<br />

• memory for emotions<br />

– patient H.M. (1953)<br />

– sources of damage<br />

• Surgery, stroke, hypoxia, head injury<br />

• Korsakoff’s syndrome, Alzheimer’s disease<br />

• electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), benzodiazepines<br />

– anterograde and retrograde amnesia<br />

brain<br />

damage<br />

PAST PRESENT<br />

retrograde<br />

anterograde


• Anterograde amnesia<br />

– Inability to form new long-<strong>term</strong> memories<br />

• general knowledge and personal history are frozen<br />

– recall, recognition, etc.<br />

– new vocabulary (e.g. jacuzzi, granola, etc.)<br />

• intact STM<br />

• all modalities are affected<br />

• Skill learning<br />

– Implicit learning, but no explicit memory (ch. 6 235-238)<br />

» procedural versus declarative<br />

– motoric or perceptual (i.e., cannot learn chess)<br />

» rotary pursuit<br />

» mirror tracing<br />

» mirror image reading<br />

» new words (not meaning)<br />

» long-<strong>term</strong> priming (stem and fragment completion)


ed__<br />

bet__<br />

don__<br />

esc___<br />

hol___<br />

ice___<br />

miss___<br />

num___<br />

out___<br />

pain___<br />

pl_m__r<br />

p_ur_l<br />

pr_o_ity<br />

ra_c_er<br />

re_e_sal<br />

s_el_ton<br />

t_ig_er<br />

t_in_et<br />

w_f_le


• Retrograde amnesia<br />

– loss of old memories<br />

• some retrograde amnesia is associated with<br />

anterograde amnesia<br />

– the time period just before the injury is forgotten<br />

(temporally graded)<br />

– depends upon abruptness<br />

• Korsakoff’s and Alzheimer’s<br />

–memory consolidation (Squire)<br />

• Hippocampally dependent<br />

– Memories are distributed throughout the cortex<br />

– Hippocampus ties together the various components<br />

– with time, the cortex can learn to do this on its own<br />

– this process is called memory consolidation<br />

• forgetting curves and amnesia


Improving <strong>Memory</strong> (ch. 8, 274-277)<br />

• Terms<br />

– Mnemosyne: Greek goddess<br />

– Mnemonic: memory aid<br />

– Mnemonist: skilled memory<br />

• variables that strengthen LTM<br />

– the spacing effect<br />

• encoding specificity<br />

• state-dependent learning<br />

– semantic encoding (a.k.a. deep encoding)<br />

• self-referential works best<br />

– rare or unusual<br />

• violate the schema<br />

– strong emotions (flashbulb memories)<br />

• physiological arousal<br />

– visual imagery


• Mnemonic Techniques<br />

– relate new to old information<br />

• chunking<br />

– N F L C B S I R A M T V<br />

• the method of loci<br />

– ordered landmarks and imagery


• technique of interacting images<br />

– in paired-associate learning (e.g., goat-pipe, tablehorse,...),<br />

forming a joint image<br />

• the pegword method<br />

– one is a bun<br />

– two is a shoe<br />

– three is a tree<br />

– four is a door<br />

– ....<br />

• recoding of material (similar to chunking)<br />

– HOMES = Huron, Ontario, Michigan, Erie, Superior

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!