04.09.2013 Views

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Microchiropteran <strong>bats</strong> use echolocation for orientation, and often for prey<br />

detection, and can thus operate <strong>in</strong> darkness and under unpredictable light<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(Griff<strong>in</strong> 1958). However, as high frequency sounds attenuate quickly <strong>in</strong> air, and<br />

limit the echolocation range (Kick 1982; Kalko & Schnitzler 1993; Fenton et al.<br />

1995), sonar cannot be effectively used for detection of small targets over long<br />

distances. Also, for echolocat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>bats</strong> forag<strong>in</strong>g close to vegetation, separation of<br />

prey from the background clutter (echoes from objects other than the target of<br />

<strong>in</strong>terest) is usually problematic (Schnitzler & Kalko1998; Arlettaz et al. 2001;<br />

Jensen et al. 2001). Therefore, some <strong>bats</strong> use for example prey-generated sounds<br />

(Ryan & Tuttle 1987; Arlettaz et al. 2001) and smell (Thies et al. 1998) as<br />

additional cues when search<strong>in</strong>g for prey. Few studies have addressed the<br />

possibility that visual cues may also be important for detection of prey. The<br />

aerial hawk<strong>in</strong>g northern bat (Eptesicus nilssonii: Vespertilionidae), is guided by<br />

visual cues when search<strong>in</strong>g for the large and conspicuously white ghost swifts<br />

Hepialus humuli (Lepidoptera), hover<strong>in</strong>g among high grass (clutter) at dusk<br />

(Eklöf et al. 2002). Other aerial hawk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>bats</strong>, such as Craseonycteris<br />

thonglongyai (Emballonuridae) might potentially use visual cues by mak<strong>in</strong>g use<br />

of the bright sky, aga<strong>in</strong>st which <strong>in</strong>sects are seen as silhouettes (Pettigrew 1980).<br />

The California leaf-nosed bat (Macrotus californicus) uses a glean<strong>in</strong>g forag<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tactic, and catches prey from the ground. This species is the only bat so far<br />

shown to be capable of catch<strong>in</strong>g prey by us<strong>in</strong>g vision alone (Bell 1985).<br />

The brown long-eared bat Plecotus auritus is also a glean<strong>in</strong>g bat,<br />

that sometimes takes <strong>in</strong>sects from leaves (Swift 1998). This means that it faces<br />

the problems of detect<strong>in</strong>g prey <strong>in</strong> a cluttered environment. Its echolocation calls<br />

are fa<strong>in</strong>t and short FM (frequency modulated) sweeps (Ahlén 1981; Parsons &<br />

Jones 2000) which may be an adaptation for forag<strong>in</strong>g close to vegetation or,<br />

alternatively, may be used only for spatial orientation (Arlettaz et al. 2001).<br />

Passive listen<strong>in</strong>g plays a major role for detect<strong>in</strong>g the prey <strong>in</strong> the long-eared bat<br />

(Anderson & Racey 1991). The ears are large and the hear<strong>in</strong>g is exceptionally<br />

sensitive to sounds around 15 kHz, close to the frequencies emitted by <strong>in</strong>sects<br />

mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> clutter (Coles et al. 1989).<br />

Plecotus auritus also has relatively big eyes compared to many<br />

other <strong>in</strong>sectivorous <strong>bats</strong> (Cranbrook 1963), suggest<strong>in</strong>g that these <strong>bats</strong> also have<br />

relatively good vision. Eisentraut (1950) was able to tra<strong>in</strong> brown long-eared <strong>bats</strong><br />

to discrim<strong>in</strong>ate between black and white 9-cm square shaped cards, but when he<br />

presented the <strong>bats</strong> with a circle and a cross they failed to make the right choice.<br />

This <strong>in</strong>dicates that P. auritus can discrim<strong>in</strong>ate between different targets by us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

vision, but not different patterns. This is <strong>in</strong> contrast to some phyllostomid <strong>bats</strong><br />

(Suthers & Chase 1966; Suthers et al. 1969), which show more sophisticated<br />

discrim<strong>in</strong>ation of patterns. However, Eisentraut’s experiments were carried out<br />

<strong>in</strong> bright light, and as subsequently shown by several authors, microchiropteran<br />

vision works better <strong>in</strong> dim ambient light (i.e. dusk and dawn illum<strong>in</strong>ation) than <strong>in</strong><br />

bright daylight (Bradbury & Nottebohm 1969; Ell<strong>in</strong>s & Masterson 1974; Hope &<br />

Bhatnagar 1979).<br />

The aim of this study is to <strong>in</strong>vestigate if brown long eared <strong>bats</strong><br />

use visual cues <strong>in</strong> addition to sonar cues when search<strong>in</strong>g for prey. Its large eyes<br />

and glean<strong>in</strong>g forag<strong>in</strong>g behaviour suggest that this may be the case. We quantified<br />

51

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!