04.09.2013 Views

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

Vision in echolocating bats - Fladdermus.net

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The brown long-eared bat Plecotus auritus responded to patterns equivalent to<br />

30’ of arc, which means that this species should be able to detect objects as small<br />

as 0.9 cm at a distance of 1 m. Among the Vespertilionidae, only Antrozous<br />

pallidus has been shown to have a better resolv<strong>in</strong>g power (15’; Bell & Fenton<br />

1986). These results and the fact that P. auritus has larger eyes than most<br />

Vespertilionids (Cranbrook 1963; Tab 1.) suggest that it should be possible for<br />

long-eared <strong>bats</strong> to detect prey sized objects visually. It typically feeds on<br />

relatively large <strong>in</strong>sects <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g many moths and beetles (Swift & Racey 1983,<br />

Rydell 1989). As P. auritus is a gleaner and sometimes takes <strong>in</strong>sects from leaves<br />

(Swift 1998), it faces potential problems with clutter and therefore use other<br />

sensory cues <strong>in</strong> addition to sonar. In fact, passive listen<strong>in</strong>g plays a major role <strong>in</strong><br />

prey detection by P. auritus (Anderson & Racey 1991). These <strong>bats</strong> are<br />

exceptionally sensitive to sounds around 15 kHz, which is close to the<br />

frequencies emitted by <strong>in</strong>sects mov<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> clutter (Coles et al. 1989). However,<br />

the long-eared <strong>bats</strong> may also use visual <strong>in</strong>formation when search<strong>in</strong>g for prey. In<br />

a recent study on feed<strong>in</strong>g behaviour, it was shown that P. auritus preferred to use<br />

visual cues to sonar when possible, and that they could detect ca. 2 cm long<br />

mealworms visually (Eklöf & Jones, <strong>in</strong> press).<br />

Visual acuity has previously been tested <strong>in</strong> a number of species (Table 2),<br />

both behaviourally by optomotor response tests (Suthers 1966, Bell & Fenton<br />

1986), and theoretically by count<strong>in</strong>g the number of ret<strong>in</strong>al ganglion cells (Koay<br />

et al. 1998, Heffner et al. 2001). Both methods give <strong>in</strong>dications of the m<strong>in</strong>imum<br />

separable angles, i.e. the m<strong>in</strong>imum distance between two po<strong>in</strong>ts that an animal<br />

need <strong>in</strong> order to be able to separate them. The acuity values estimated by<br />

count<strong>in</strong>g ret<strong>in</strong>al ganglion cells tend to be higher than those estimated from<br />

behavioural studies, suggest<strong>in</strong>g that the anatomical method gives a theoretical<br />

threshold, rather than what the <strong>bats</strong> actually respond to. Nevertheless, although<br />

the acuity values obta<strong>in</strong>ed from the different methods are roughly <strong>in</strong> the same<br />

order, comparisons across the two methods should be made with care.<br />

As shown by the literature data presented <strong>in</strong> Table 2, frugivorous and<br />

nectarivorous <strong>bats</strong> seem to have better spatial resolution than most <strong>in</strong>sectivorous<br />

species. Nevertheless, the f<strong>in</strong>est spatial resolution <strong>in</strong> any bat (3’38’’) is found <strong>in</strong><br />

the glean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sectivore Macrotus californicus (Phyllostomidae), and this<br />

happens to be the only bat known to f<strong>in</strong>d prey, us<strong>in</strong>g vision alone (Bell 1985,<br />

Bell & Fenton 1986). Indeed, glean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sectivores may have better visual acuity<br />

than aerial-<strong>in</strong>sectivores <strong>in</strong> general, and this suggests that the aerial-hawk<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>sectivores rely mostly on echolocation rather than vision for detection of small<br />

targets, while the opposite may be true <strong>in</strong> gleaners. At the same time it seems as<br />

if, among aerial-hawk<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>sectivores, Emballonuridae have better resolution<br />

than Vespertilionidae.<br />

The visual resolv<strong>in</strong>g power may depend on ambient light <strong>in</strong>tensity. In the<br />

common vampire bat Desmodus rotundus, for example, the acuity drops from<br />

48’ at a light <strong>in</strong>tensity of 31 mL (ca. 310 lux) to over 2° <strong>in</strong> 4*10 -4 mL (ca. 0.004<br />

lux) (Manske & Schmidt 1976). Other <strong>bats</strong>, such as Macrotus californicus and<br />

Antrozous pallidus reta<strong>in</strong> their visual acuity down to light levels as low as 2*10 -4<br />

mL (ca. 0.002 lux) (Bell & Fenton 1986). As a comparison, a light level of 0.1<br />

lux is equivalent to light levels at full moon, and similar to the conditions <strong>in</strong> this<br />

85

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!