10.01.2014 Views

Identifying Syntactically Relevant Units of Meaning

Identifying Syntactically Relevant Units of Meaning

Identifying Syntactically Relevant Units of Meaning

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Identifying</strong> <strong>Syntactically</strong><br />

<strong>Relevant</strong> <strong>Units</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>Meaning</strong><br />

Hans C. Boas<br />

The University <strong>of</strong> Texas at Austin<br />

hcb@mail.utexas.edu<br />

http://hcb.gmc.utexas.edu


Overview<br />

• Introduction<br />

• Using syntactic behavior for defining verb<br />

classes<br />

• Verb classes and limitations on<br />

predictability<br />

• A frame-semantic approach to verb<br />

classification<br />

• Conclusions and Outlook<br />

2


Introduction<br />

• Locative alternation<br />

(1) a. Sophie sprayed water on the bushes.<br />

(locative variant)<br />

b. Sophie sprayed the bushes with water.<br />

(with-variant)<br />

(See Fillmore 1968, Anderson 1971, Salk<strong>of</strong>f 1983, Rappaport & Levin 1985,<br />

Pinker 1989, Levin 1993, Goldberg 1995, Nemoto 1996, Dowty 2000, Boas<br />

2003, Iwata 2005, Levin & Rappaport Hovav 2005, among many others)<br />

3


Pustejovsky (1995: 8)<br />

It is the goal <strong>of</strong> any lexical semantic theory<br />

to adequately classify the lexical items <strong>of</strong><br />

a language into classes predictive <strong>of</strong> their<br />

syntactic and semantic expression.<br />

Furthermore, such a theory should not<br />

merely map the meanings <strong>of</strong> lexical items<br />

per sentence, on an individual basis.<br />

Rather, it should capture the semantic<br />

relations between words in such a way<br />

which facilitates this mapping.<br />

4


Using syntactic behavior for<br />

defining verb classes<br />

• Pinker’s (1989) lexical rule approach to<br />

locative alternation<br />

- Verbs <strong>of</strong> putting and covering<br />

- Semantic criteria for identifying two verb classes<br />

(force, dimensional geometry <strong>of</strong> solids, etc.)<br />

5


Content-oriented into/onto verbs<br />

(Pinker 1989: 126)<br />

6


Content-oriented with-verbs<br />

(Pinker 1989: 126-27)<br />

7


Pinker’s lexical rule approach<br />

The lexical rule takes a verb containing in<br />

its semantic structure the core ‘X causes Y<br />

to move into/onto Z,’ and converts it into a<br />

new verb whose semantic structure<br />

contains the core ‘X causes Z to change<br />

state by means <strong>of</strong> moving Y into/onto it.<br />

(Pinker 1989:79)<br />

8


Lexical Rule: Derivation<br />

• with-variant locative variant<br />

• locative variant with-variant<br />

• Directionality depends on ability <strong>of</strong> direct<br />

argument to stand as the sole complement<br />

9


Linking <strong>of</strong> underlying thematic roles<br />

to syntax<br />

The difference in argument structure follows from<br />

the linking rules: in the old verb, the moving<br />

thing was the theme and hence was linked to<br />

direct object; in the new verb, the location is the<br />

theme (<strong>of</strong> a state change) and hence is linked to<br />

object. The argument not linked to object gets<br />

linked to an oblique function or position by virtue<br />

<strong>of</strong> other linking rules in combination with lexical<br />

entries for specific prepositions.<br />

(Pinker 1989:79)<br />

10


Problem 1<br />

• Sole complement diagnostic does not make the correct<br />

predictions<br />

(5) a. John heaped books on the shelf.<br />

b. John heaped the shelf with books.<br />

c. *?John heaped the books.<br />

d. *John heaped the shelf. (Pinker 1989:38)<br />

(6) a. John packed books into the box.<br />

b. John packed the box with books.<br />

c. John packed the books.<br />

d. John packed the box. (Pinker 1989:39)<br />

Possibility <strong>of</strong> standing as a sole complement does not<br />

truly serve as a diagnostic for the derivational base<br />

11


Problem 2<br />

• Verbs such as drizzle, drip, and slop<br />

exhibit with-variants, contrary to Pinker.<br />

(7) I tried using a normal bit with a standard drill as I dripped it with oil.<br />

(re.antiques.radio+phono)<br />

(8) I made a fresh mashed-spider cake and drizzled it with raspberry sauce<br />

just for you. (acadia.chat)<br />

(9) I slopped it with Ketchup, smacked my lips, and took a bite.<br />

(alt.religion.christian.roman-catholic)<br />

12


Problem 3<br />

• Which verb sense is the “basic” variant?<br />

• E.g., stuff-verbs (cram, crowd, stuff, etc.)<br />

Stuff-verbs can trigger the formation <strong>of</strong><br />

corresponding content-oriented or<br />

into/onto forms” which “involve the notion<br />

<strong>of</strong> a container’s intended capacity (Pinker<br />

1989:234)<br />

13


Stuff-verbs<br />

• Verbs that fit the semantic characterization <strong>of</strong> stuff-verbs<br />

don’t exhibit similar syntactic behavior, e.g. squeeze<br />

(10) a. Dawn stuffed the turkey with breadcrumbs.<br />

b. Dawn stuffed breadcrumbs into the turkey.<br />

(11) a. *Christian squeezed the turkey with breadcrumbs.<br />

b. Christian squeezed breadcrumbs into the turkey.<br />

Why does lexical rule apply to stuff but not to squeeze?<br />

14


Summary – Problems with Pinker (1989)<br />

• Not clear which verb sense is “basic”<br />

• Lexical rules do not always have access to<br />

the proper types <strong>of</strong> inputs<br />

• Semantic classification not fine-grained<br />

enough<br />

Does not make the correct generalizations<br />

15


Levin’s (2003)<br />

Projectionist Approach<br />

Verb meaning<br />

Root<br />

(idiosyncratic part)<br />

Constant<br />

(Structural part, LCS, event structure)<br />

- Core meaning - combinations <strong>of</strong> primitive predicates<br />

- Not relevant for grammatical behavior - relevant for grammatical behavior<br />

- Number is in principle open-ended - limited set <strong>of</strong> possible event structures<br />

See also Rappaport Hovav & Levin (1998, 2001), Levin & Rappaport Hovav (2005)<br />

16


Simple vs. complex event structures<br />

(12) a. [x ACT ]<br />

b. [[ x ACT ] CAUSE [ BECOME [y ]]]<br />

(Rappaport Hovav and Levin 1998:108)<br />

(13) The argument-per-subevent condition<br />

There must be at least one argument XP in the syntax per<br />

subevent in the event structure.<br />

(Rappaport Hovav and Levin 2001: 779)<br />

Syntactic differences between run (manner verb) vs. break<br />

(result verb)<br />

17


“Generating” verb meaning<br />

(14) Template Augmentation<br />

Event structure templates may be freely<br />

augmented up to other possible templates<br />

in the basic inventory <strong>of</strong> event structure<br />

templates.<br />

(Rappaport Hovav and Levin 1998: 111)<br />

18


Locative Alternation (Levin 2003)<br />

• Simple event structures are found in complex event<br />

structures<br />

• Each verb is characterized by a particular type <strong>of</strong> end<br />

result<br />

• Only means or manner roots may exhibit multiple<br />

alternations<br />

a. [x ACT ]<br />

b. [[ x ACT ] CAUSE [ BECOME [y ]]]<br />

19


Why can some means and<br />

manner verbs alternate but not others?<br />

• The root <strong>of</strong> sew is associated with a simple event structure, and<br />

different end results are possible (template augmentation)<br />

Creating an object<br />

(15) a. Dale sewed the piece <strong>of</strong> silk into a ball gown.<br />

b. Dale sewed a ball gown out <strong>of</strong> the piece <strong>of</strong> silk.<br />

Covering a surface<br />

(16) a. Dale sewed bows on the costume.<br />

b. Dale sewed the costume with bows.<br />

Attaching things/impressing an image<br />

(17) a. Dale sewed the lining to the skirt.<br />

b. Dale sewed the lining and skirt together.<br />

(Levin 2003:10)<br />

20


Differences between sew and<br />

vacuum<br />

• The root <strong>of</strong> vacuum describes a means/manner used<br />

only to obtain a very specific result and can thus not be<br />

associated with various types <strong>of</strong> results<br />

(18) a. Avery vacuumed the dust <strong>of</strong>f the rug.<br />

b. Avery vacuumed the rug.<br />

(19) a.*Avery vacuumed the dust onto the rug.<br />

b.*Avery vacuumed the rug with the dust.<br />

(20) a.*Avery vacuumed the dust into a pile.<br />

b.*Avery vacuumed a pile from the dust.<br />

(Levin 2003:10-11)<br />

21


Event structures determines<br />

alternating behavior<br />

• Non-alternating verbs have roots associated with<br />

complex “result state” event structures, not with simple<br />

event structures<br />

(21) a. Ashley filled the bucket with water.<br />

b.*Ashley filled water into the bucket.<br />

(22) a. Ashley poured water into the bucket.<br />

b.*Ashley poured the bucket with water.<br />

(Levin 2003:11)<br />

22


Event structures determines<br />

alternating behavior<br />

Manner/Means Verbs<br />

(smear, sew)<br />

Associated with simple event<br />

structure<br />

Template augmentation<br />

possible<br />

Alternation possible<br />

Result Verbs<br />

(pour, fill)<br />

Associated with complex<br />

event structure<br />

Template augmentation not<br />

possible<br />

Alternation not possible<br />

23


Problem 1 with Projectionist Approach<br />

• Manner/means verbs closely related in meaning do not<br />

exhibit same alternation pattern (sew/stitch)<br />

• Which semantic factors are responsible?<br />

(23) a. *Joe stitched the piece <strong>of</strong> silk into a ball gown.<br />

b. *Joe stitched a ball gown out <strong>of</strong> the piece <strong>of</strong> silk.<br />

(24) a. Joe stitched bows on the costume.<br />

b. *Joe stitched the costume with bows.<br />

(25) a. Joe stitched the lining to the skirt.<br />

b. Joe stitched the lining and skirt together.<br />

24


Problem 2 with Projectionist Approach<br />

• Manner/verbs should be able to alternate to<br />

express different types <strong>of</strong> results, but they don’t<br />

(26)a. Collin {wiped/swept/rubbed} the dust <strong>of</strong>f the rug.<br />

b. Collin {wiped/swept/rubbed} the rug.<br />

(27)a. Russell {wiped/swept/rubbed} the dust onto the rug.<br />

b.*Russell {wiped/swept/rubbed} the rug with the dust.<br />

(28)a. Petra {wiped/swept/rubbed} the dust into a pile.<br />

b.*Petra {wiped/swept/rubbed} a pile from the dust.<br />

25


Levin’s event structure<br />

classification not precise enough<br />

• Despite our expectations, hang alternates<br />

(29) a. They hung pictures <strong>of</strong> the president onto their walls.<br />

b. They hung their walls with pictures <strong>of</strong> the president.<br />

• Irregular behavior <strong>of</strong> tape-verbs (anchor, band, belt, bolt, bracket,<br />

buckle, button, cement, chain, clamp, clasp, clip, epoxy, fetter, glue, gum,<br />

etc.)<br />

(30) a. Paul taped pictures onto the wall.<br />

b.*Paul taped the wall with pictures.<br />

27


Summary – Problems with Levin (2003)<br />

• Event structure classification is too coarsegrained<br />

• Template augmentation does not capture<br />

full distribution <strong>of</strong> verb senses<br />

• Similar problems as Goldberg (1995),<br />

where verbs and constructions “fuse” to<br />

generate new verb senses<br />

28


Levin (1993)<br />

• Classifications based on argument syntax<br />

• 193 verb classes covering 3024 verbs, or<br />

4,186 senses (Verbs <strong>of</strong> putting, verbs <strong>of</strong> removing,<br />

verbs <strong>of</strong> communication, etc.)<br />

• 79 alternations (Locative, Benefactive, Creation and<br />

Transformation, etc.)<br />

29


Multiple class membership and<br />

limitations on predictability<br />

• Levin (1993): verb classes based on syntactic behavior<br />

<strong>of</strong> verbs<br />

• E.g., classification <strong>of</strong> transitive verbs<br />

30


“Other verbs show same pattern <strong>of</strong> behavior”<br />

(31) a. Break Verbs: break, crack, rip, shatter, snap, …<br />

b. Cut Verbs: cut, hack, saw, scratch, clash, …<br />

c. Touch Verbs: pat, stroke, tickle, touch, …<br />

d. Hit Verbs: bash, hit, kick, pound, tap, whack, …<br />

(Levin 1993:7)<br />

What are the relevant meaning components common<br />

among the members <strong>of</strong> each class?<br />

Do all members <strong>of</strong> a verb class exhibit identical syntactic<br />

behavior?<br />

31


Verbs from same class show varied<br />

behavior in different alternations<br />

• Cut verbs participate in middle alternation and<br />

instrument subject alternation<br />

• Not all cut verbs participate in body-part-possessor<br />

ascension alternation<br />

(31) a. Carol cut herself on the thumb.<br />

b. Carol cut her thumb.<br />

(32) a. *Carol hewed herself on the thumb.<br />

b. Carol hewed her thumb.<br />

(see also Boas 2003 on resultative constructions)<br />

32


Syntactic frames not always<br />

heterogeneous<br />

(33)a. I ended up boxing with him.<br />

b. Tyson will box Lewis.<br />

(34) a. My son played/met with your son.<br />

b. My son played/met your son.<br />

33


World knowledge impacts syntactic distribution (Taylor 1996)<br />

34


Syntax = Semantics?<br />

• Not all verbal syntax is indicative <strong>of</strong> a verb’s<br />

lexical semantic properties<br />

• Syntactic classification gives only a limited<br />

picture <strong>of</strong> underlying semantics<br />

• Levin’s (1993) verb classes do not exhibit<br />

uniform meaning, cf. break vs. split verbs:<br />

• “It is possible that additional verbs from these<br />

other classes might qualify as split verbs.” (1993:<br />

167)<br />

35


More verb class problems<br />

• For each syntactic phenomenon there may be<br />

a different verb class definition<br />

• Verb classes are epiphenomenal<br />

• Lexicalist paradox: two contradictory goals for<br />

constructing lexical entries:<br />

(1) Factoring predictable information out <strong>of</strong> lexical<br />

entries (as minimal as possible)<br />

(2) Explain a verb’s varied syntactic behavior (as<br />

maximal as possible) – argument realization<br />

36


Alternative: A frame-semantic approach<br />

to verb classification<br />

• Frame Semantics (Fillmore 1982)<br />

• Lexical Unit (LU)<br />

• Semantic Frame, Frame Elements (FE)<br />

• Primacy <strong>of</strong> semantic information<br />

•FrameNet (Fillmore et al. 2003; http://framenet.icsi.berkeley.edu)<br />

37


Structure <strong>of</strong> Lexical Entry<br />

• Frame description<br />

• LUs evoking the frame: load, spray, etc.<br />

• Exhaustive inventory <strong>of</strong> how FEs are<br />

realized syntactically<br />

• Annotated example sentences<br />

38


Lexical Entry <strong>of</strong> load in the Filling Frame :<br />

Frame description<br />

39


Lexical Entry <strong>of</strong> load in the Filling Frame :<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> Valencies<br />

40


Lexical Entry <strong>of</strong> load in the Filling Frame :<br />

Annotated example sentences<br />

41


The Placing Frame: Frame description<br />

• Frame description: An AGENT places a<br />

THEME at a location (the GOAL), which is<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>iled (Langacker 1987). The THEME is under<br />

the control <strong>of</strong> the AGENT at the time <strong>of</strong> its<br />

arrival at the GOAL.<br />

• LUs evoking the frame: bag, brush, drape,<br />

load, pack, spray, etc.<br />

• Exhaustive inventory <strong>of</strong> how FEs are<br />

realized syntactically<br />

42


Lexical Entry <strong>of</strong> load in the Placing Frame :<br />

Summary <strong>of</strong> Valencies<br />

43


Lexical Entry <strong>of</strong> load in the Placing Frame :<br />

Annotated Example Sentences<br />

44


Baker & Ruppenh<strong>of</strong>er (2002)<br />

45


Advantages <strong>of</strong> frame-semantic<br />

classification<br />

• LUs are described with respect to the semantic frames<br />

they evoke (splitting approach, coherence)<br />

• More fine-grained classification<br />

• Full syntactic information is available, but is not taken as<br />

a necessary indicator for class membership<br />

• See also Faber and Mairal Usón (1999:2): “Semantic<br />

considerations are all-important because syntactic<br />

distinctions in themselves are not sufficient as a basis for<br />

the establishment <strong>of</strong> an inventory <strong>of</strong> semantic<br />

categories.”<br />

46


What is the relationship between the<br />

syntactic behavior <strong>of</strong> a verb and its<br />

meaning?<br />

• Syntactic alternations <strong>of</strong>fer limited insights<br />

• Refine Frame Semantics with the notion <strong>of</strong> “verb<br />

descriptivity” (Snell-Hornby 1983)<br />

• More fine-grained analysis <strong>of</strong> verb meaning<br />

• Analysis <strong>of</strong> participants and circumstances<br />

• Additional semantic elements implicitly associated<br />

with a verb’s meaning<br />

50


Verb descriptivity<br />

strut<br />

ANu (act nucleus) Mod (modificants)<br />

- walk - distinct physical characteristics (stiff, erect)<br />

- value judgments passed on the agent and<br />

his manner <strong>of</strong> walking (proud, pompous,<br />

self-satisfied, etc.)<br />

51


Verb descriptivity<br />

• strut: high level <strong>of</strong> verb-descriptivity<br />

• “descriptive verbs” (DV):<br />

DV = ANu + Mod (+x)<br />

x is an optional element without evaluative properties and not<br />

expressible in terms <strong>of</strong> adjectives or manner adverbs<br />

52


Two types <strong>of</strong> verb descriptivity<br />

• Direct descriptivity: modificant refers<br />

directly to the activity described by verb<br />

(e.g. shout)<br />

• Indirect descriptivity: modificant refers to a<br />

participant or circumstance behind the<br />

action or a combination <strong>of</strong> these (e.g.<br />

strut)<br />

53


Degrees <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

• The more semantic weight is taken by the<br />

modificant as against the act-nucleus, the higher<br />

the degree <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

• BUSTLE: high degree <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

– Modificant <strong>of</strong> bustle: excitedly, energetically, <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

with apparent purpose, but usually noisily or<br />

inefficiently, …<br />

– Act-nucleus <strong>of</strong> bustle: not clearly definable (activity<br />

such as behave, move about, etc.)<br />

54


Low degree <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

• Modificant <strong>of</strong> shout: loudly<br />

• Act-nucleus <strong>of</strong> shout: say, speak<br />

55


Importance <strong>of</strong> level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

• Level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity appears to influence<br />

syntactic distribution (range <strong>of</strong> argument<br />

realization) (see Hunston & Francis 2000)<br />

• Light verbs: come, go, make, take, get, …<br />

• Verbs with low level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity: shout, …<br />

• Verbs with high level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity: grovel,<br />

bustle, etc.<br />

56


Integrating “verb descriptivity” into<br />

Frame Semantics<br />

• How do different lexical units evoking the<br />

same frame differ in their degree <strong>of</strong><br />

descriptivity?<br />

• Is there a correlation between a verb’s<br />

level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity and the range <strong>of</strong><br />

syntactic patterns in which it occurs?<br />

57


Case study: Self_motion frame<br />

• Situations in which the SELF_MOVER, a<br />

living being, moves under its own power in<br />

a directed fashion, i.e. along a path what<br />

could be described as a PATH, with no<br />

separate vehicle. The presence <strong>of</strong> the FE<br />

PATH presupposes the presence <strong>of</strong> other<br />

FEs such as SOURCE, GOAL,<br />

DIRECTION, and AREA.<br />

58


First approximation <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

59


More motion verbs<br />

60


Comparison <strong>of</strong> motion verbs<br />

• They all evoke the Self_motion frame<br />

• Semantics <strong>of</strong> FEs are drastically different:<br />

–AGENT <strong>of</strong> walk, parade, totter, stagger<br />

– PATH <strong>of</strong> walk and parade vs. totter and<br />

stagger<br />

• Pr<strong>of</strong>iling <strong>of</strong> Frame Elements differs<br />

61


Differences in descriptivity<br />

62


Correlation between degree <strong>of</strong><br />

descriptivity and range <strong>of</strong> argument<br />

realization?<br />

• Syntactic alternations and grammatical<br />

constructions to test for range <strong>of</strong> argument<br />

realization<br />

63


Range <strong>of</strong> argument realization<br />

Inclusion <strong>of</strong> locative PP<br />

(33) a. Gerry {walked/paraded/staggered/tottered}.<br />

b. Gerry {walked/paraded/staggered/tottered} down the street.<br />

Zero-related nominal<br />

(34) a walk, a parade, a stagger, a totter<br />

64


Range <strong>of</strong> argument realization<br />

Resultative construction<br />

(35)a. Cathy {walked/?paraded/*staggered/*tottered} herself to exhaustion.<br />

b. Cathy {walked/*paraded/*staggered/*tottered} Pat <strong>of</strong>f the street.<br />

Locative preposition drop alternation<br />

(36) a. Julia {walked/paraded/staggered/tottered} across the town.<br />

b. Julia {walked/paraded/*staggered/*tottered} the town.<br />

65


Range <strong>of</strong> argument realization<br />

Induced action alternation<br />

(37) a. Claire {walked/paraded/*staggered/*tottered} the dog down the street.<br />

b. The dog {walked/paraded/staggered/tottered} down the street.<br />

Adjectival passive participles<br />

(38) the {walked/??paraded/*staggered/*tottered} dog<br />

66


Degree <strong>of</strong> descriptivity correlates<br />

with range <strong>of</strong> argument realization<br />

•LUssuch as walk: low level <strong>of</strong> descriptivity<br />

and broad range <strong>of</strong> argument realization<br />

•LUssuch as totter: high level <strong>of</strong><br />

descriptivity and narrow range <strong>of</strong> argument<br />

realization<br />

68


Combining Frame Semantics, Verb<br />

Descriptivity, and Componential Analysis<br />

71


Next steps<br />

• Analyze each semantic frame in terms <strong>of</strong> an act<br />

nucleus (abstract representation)<br />

• Develop fine-grained analysis <strong>of</strong> Frame<br />

Elements and their descriptive properties<br />

(componential analysis/verb descriptivity)<br />

• Compile set <strong>of</strong> test criteria that allow for a<br />

stream-lined catalogue <strong>of</strong> meaning elements<br />

• Investigate pr<strong>of</strong>iling properties and relate them<br />

to syntactic properties<br />

72


Next steps – locative alternation<br />

• Pr<strong>of</strong>iling properties <strong>of</strong> FEs AGENT, THEME, GOAL<br />

• Inherent meaning properties <strong>of</strong> verbs (components)<br />

archive<br />

load<br />

wallpaper<br />

Placing Frame<br />

YES<br />

YES<br />

NO<br />

Filling Frame<br />

NO<br />

YES<br />

YES<br />

73


Next steps – constructicon<br />

• Extension <strong>of</strong> FrameNet methodology to<br />

capture semantic and syntactic distribution<br />

<strong>of</strong> syntactic constructions<br />

• <strong>Identifying</strong> syntactically relevant units <strong>of</strong><br />

meaning in syntactic constructions<br />

74


Conclusions<br />

• Reliance on syntactic information for<br />

defining verb classes is problematic<br />

75

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!