28.03.2014 Views

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

4.3 Schemas keep the complexity of problem solving in check<br />

The discovery that the major difference between people who differed in ability was in terms<br />

of what they held in long-term memory changed our view of cognition. [. . . ] Long-term<br />

memory was not just used by humans to reminisce about the past but, rather, was a central<br />

component of problem solving and thought. (Sweller, 2010a, p. 32)<br />

4.3.1 Scripts and problem-solving schemas tell us what to do<br />

The literature discusses various more specific types of schemas. An often-cited construct is the script or<br />

event schema formulated by Schank and Abelson (1977). Scripts are schemas that encode knowledge of<br />

recurring events and their stereotypical stages and other properties, and allow us to act comfortably and<br />

efficiently in familiar situations. The event of a visit to a restaurant, which consists of typical stages like<br />

ordering, eating, and paying, is a classic example. Similarly, a problem-solving schema tells us what to do<br />

to accomplish a goal in a particular kind of situation. Sweller and Chandler provide a good example:<br />

Someone who is competent at algebra will have a schema for multiplying out a denominator.<br />

The schema will tell that person which of the infinite variety of algebraic equations is amenable<br />

to multiplying out a denominator and the procedure for doing so. When faced with a problem<br />

such as a/b = c, solve for a, we can immediately solve such a problem, despite the many<br />

forms in which it could be presented, because our schema for this type of algebra problem<br />

informs us, for example, that the solution requires multiplying out the denominator on the<br />

left-hand side, irrespective of the complexity of the term on the right-hand side. Schemas,<br />

stored in long-term memory, permit us to ignore the variety that would otherwise overwhelm<br />

our working memory. (Sweller and Chandler, 1994, p. 187)<br />

Schemas allow us to draw on our prior experience and general knowledge so as not to be overwhelmed by<br />

the details of what we are currently experiencing. When we see an unfamiliar person – a specific instance of<br />

a familiar, automated schema – we can easily tell that the person is a human and act accordingly, without<br />

having to store or process every tiny detail of the person that our senses barrage us with. Schemas are<br />

crucial to our success in whatever we do as they “allow us to carry out everyday activities with minimum<br />

effort and to capitalize on the regularities of events and situations” (Preece et al., 1994, p. 128).<br />

When faced with a complex problem to solve, we need to process various aspects of the problem and<br />

its solution in working memory simultaneously. A problem-solving schema such as the algebra schema<br />

described above allows us to ignore the specific details of the problem and recognize a more general<br />

pattern for which we have a schema. When a schema is available, it allows the entire problem or a part<br />

of the problem to be dealt with as a single chunk (see Section 4.1 above). In relation to one of their<br />

experimental studies, Sweller and Chandler (1994) discuss the solving of a simple problem: marking a<br />

point with given coordinates in a two-dimensional graphical coordinate system. They argue that for a<br />

complete novice, solving this problem requires an understanding of roughly seven distinct items, e.g.,<br />

the fact that x in P(x, y) refers to a location x on the x-axis. Where the complete beginner may be<br />

overwhelmed, someone who has experience with such problems can incorporate the entire problem into a<br />

single chunk that does not burden working memory greatly – or at all, if automated through practice.<br />

4.3.2 Schemas are a key ingredient of expertise<br />

Educational psychologists have long sought to understand the nature and development of expertise. On<br />

the one hand, the goal has been to characterize the mental representations that experts have and the<br />

ways in which experts put their knowledge to use when solving problems. On the other hand, researchers<br />

have investigated the mental representations and associated processes of novices, and contrasted them<br />

with those of experts. Through such comparisons, it is argued, we may better understand the changes<br />

that take place as we learn, and thereby improve teaching and learning.<br />

An easy explanation for the superior performance of experts in problem solving would be in their<br />

superior ‘cognitive hardware’ such as a larger short-term working memory or superior intelligence. Early<br />

37

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!