28.03.2014 Views

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

5.1.1 We use mental models to interact with causal systems<br />

Causal vs. logical mental models<br />

It is necessary at this point to differentiate between two influential threads of research on constructs called<br />

mental models.<br />

The term “mental model” received wide recognition after its introduction to cognitive psychology<br />

in the early 1980s. The two main threads of research on mental models are often attributed to the<br />

near-simultaneous publication of two books titled Mental Models, which elaborated in two different ways<br />

on Craik’s (1943) earlier notion of “models of reality”. The articles in the volume edited by Gentner<br />

and Stevens (1983) greatly influenced research into the kinds of mental representations people store<br />

about physical and software systems. The book by Johnson-Laird (1983) is seminal to a body of work<br />

that investigates a certain kind of situation-specific mental representation that people create in working<br />

memory to help them reason about logical problems. Markman and Gentner (2001) term mental models as<br />

described in the Gentner and Stevens book causal mental models and those in Johnson-Laird’s research<br />

tradition logical mental models. Logical mental models are not of interest for my present purposes;<br />

whenever I write about “mental models” in this thesis, I refer to causal mental models. 1<br />

Mental models of systems<br />

People form causal mental models of all manner of things. Schumacher and Czerwinski (1992) note that<br />

while one can have a mental model of a marriage or a social environment, not all topics are equally well<br />

represented on the research agenda. Much of the research on mental models has focused on the way people<br />

interact with and think about complex physical and software systems that involve causal mechanisms (e.g.,<br />

an electrical circuit, a word processor). The mental model is a theoretical construct posited to explain<br />

how people describe the purpose and underlying mechanisms of such systems to themselves and how they<br />

predict future system states. This emphasis stems partially from arbitrary historical reasons that arise<br />

from research tradition 2 and partially from practical reasons: “We would argue that experts in computer<br />

systems are easier to define than experts in marriage.” (Schumacher and Czerwinski, 1992)<br />

Research does exist on causal mental models that is not concerned with humans’ relationships with<br />

technical systems. However, I will limit my discussion to causal mental models of technical systems.<br />

Researchers have further sought to identify the features that mental models of systems have in general,<br />

the characteristics that distinguish between useful and not-so-useful mental models, the transferability<br />

of the knowledge stored in mental models, and the relationships between learning and mental model<br />

construction. I will comment on each of these topics below.<br />

5.1.2 Research has explored the characteristics of mental models<br />

According to Norman’s (1983) seminal description, mental models:<br />

• reflect people’s beliefs about the systems they use and about their own limitations, and include<br />

statements about the degree of uncertainty people feel about different aspects of their knowledge;<br />

• provide parsimonious, simplified explanations of complex phenomena;<br />

• often contain only incomplete, partial descriptions of operations, and may contain huge areas of<br />

uncertainty;<br />

• are ‘unscientific’ and imprecise, and often based on guesswork and naïve assumptions and beliefs,<br />

as well as “superstitious” rules that “seem to work” even if they make no sense;<br />

1 The body – or rather, bodies – of research on mental models make up a complex terminological and conceptual tangle.<br />

As with the word “schema” (Section 4.2), some authors use “mental model” as an umbrella term for all knowledge, a practice<br />

that has been criticized by authors such as Rouse and Morris (1986). There are numerous other more or less idiosyncratic<br />

ways of using the term in the literature, which I will not cover here.<br />

2 The focus on causal systems and devices can be traced back to human–machine interaction studies in the 1960s and<br />

a body of research that investigates process control: the manual control of complex machines and, later, the supervisory<br />

control of increasingly automatic machines (Rouse and Morris, 1986; Wickens, 1996, and references therein).<br />

53

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!