28.03.2014 Views

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

isbn9789526046266

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Skirmish 5: ignoring evidence<br />

Various authors have complained about the vagueness of constructivist ideas. For instance, Prior McCarty<br />

and Schwandt (2000, p. 42) describe knowledge construction as “a range of hazily imagined mental<br />

activities”, while Anderson et al. (2000b) claim that constructivists “refuse to focus on details and<br />

precise specifications”. Indeed, many constructivists seem to be satisfied with a highly abstract notion of<br />

construction, although conceptual change theorists (Section 6.5) and cognitive constructivists who rely<br />

on schema theory and mental models are exceptions. 9 The vagueness of instructional frameworks and<br />

a lack of rigorous studies and testable hypotheses have been acknowledged as weaknesses also by some<br />

scholars otherwise sympathetic to constructivism (Tobias and Duffy, 2009).<br />

A related criticism is that constructivisms are said to be oblivious to much that is known through<br />

research. Mayer (2004) and Kirschner et al. (2006) accuse constructivists of ignoring half a century of<br />

empirical evidence from cognitive psychology regarding the human cognitive apparatus and its effects on<br />

learning. Anderson et al. (2000b) pursue a similar line:<br />

This [constructivist] criticism of practice (called “drill and kill,” as if this phrase constituted<br />

empirical evaluation) is prominent in constructivist writings. Nothing flies more in the face of<br />

the last 20 years of research than the assertion that practice is bad. All evidence, from the<br />

laboratory and from extensive case studies of professionals, indicates that real competence<br />

only comes with extensive practice.<br />

Critics have called for radical constructivists and supporters of constructivist pedagogies to produce<br />

testable predictions and empirical data that supports their claims. At least part of the challenge may<br />

go unheeded, since some extreme forms of constructivism seem even to deny the possibility or relevance<br />

of empirical data to educational decisions (Prior McCarty and Schwandt, 2000; Anderson et al., 2000b).<br />

Skirmish 6: complex, authentic contexts<br />

An issue of disagreement between (some) cognitive psychologists and (some) constructivists is the<br />

constructivist claim that knowledge is context-dependent and effective learning requires rich contexts,<br />

authentic settings, and ill-structured problems which learners make sense of largely on their own. Mayer<br />

(2004), Anderson et al. (2000b) and Kirschner et al. (2006) cite numerous empirical studies whose results<br />

indicate that knowledge can be transferred between contexts (under the right conditions), and that illstructured<br />

problems, complex settings and other constructivist pedagogies tend to cognitively overload<br />

learners and are commonly ineffective.<br />

The psychologists that I have cited do not claim that authentic contexts are never useful or that no<br />

knowledge is context-dependent or best learned in a complex context. Anderson et al., for instance, do<br />

agree that some skills are best practiced in a complex setting, for motivational reasons or to learn special<br />

skills that are unique to the complex situation. However, they object to the sweeping statements on this<br />

topic that many constructivists make.<br />

Mayer (2004) warns against “the constructivist fallacy” of equating behavioral activity (hands-on tasks)<br />

with fruitful cognitive activity. According to Anderson et al. (2000b), situated learning and constructivism<br />

are movements which claim to oppose behaviorism, but which, through their neglect of cognition, have<br />

ironically ended up reiterating Burrhus F. Skinner’s utopian vision of behaviorist education from his novel<br />

Walden Two, in which people learn best from being subjected to the control of the community in which<br />

they participate.<br />

Situated learning theory places a particularly heavy emphasis on how effective learning is dependent<br />

on participation within a complex, authentic context. This makes situated learning a prime target for the<br />

critical cognitive scientists. One of the assertions of Kirschner et al. (2006) is a succinct antithesis of<br />

the situated theory of learning: “The practice of a profession is not the same as learning to practice the<br />

profession.”<br />

9 A colleague of mine once half-seriously commented that “constructivism can be used to justify any pedagogical<br />

innovation” and – on a separate occasion – that “constructivism can’t be used to justify anything”. This apparent contradiction<br />

is explained by the fact that constructivism, when taken in the broad sense, is so abstract when it comes to the processes of<br />

learning that it says everything and nothing at the same time.<br />

91

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!