25.05.2014 Views

Bat Echolocation Researc h - Bat Conservation International

Bat Echolocation Researc h - Bat Conservation International

Bat Echolocation Researc h - Bat Conservation International

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

108<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Much of our knowledge about bat ecology, behavior,<br />

and echolocation comes from studies conducted in the<br />

field. Owing to continuous technological improvements<br />

in equipment and an ever-increasing number of<br />

researchers, bat field ecology is a growing and yet still<br />

challenging area of research. In point, this symposium<br />

provides an overview of the current state in field-based<br />

bat echolocation studies. Another well of information<br />

for bat behavioral biologists are psychophysical experiments<br />

designed to investigate the performance and sensory<br />

capacities of echolocation systems. Often, a 2-alternative<br />

forced-choice paradigm is used in which a bat sits<br />

on a Y-shaped platform and crawls either onto the left or<br />

the right branch of the platform to indicate whether it<br />

perceives an object left or right. Such objects might be<br />

real targets like thin wires or virtual, so-called phantom<br />

targets (i.e., echoes modified by a computer and played<br />

back through a loudspeaker).<br />

Whereas field studies allow us to learn about the natural<br />

and complex behaviors of bats, they are limited by<br />

the difficulty of creating standardized experimental conditions<br />

that are necessary to answer many biological<br />

questions. In psychophysical tests, the experimental conditions<br />

can be controlled, and often reach a high level of<br />

sophistication. However, experiments with sitting rather<br />

than flying bats and highly specific, sometimes quite simplified<br />

psychophysical tasks (for the sake of experimental<br />

clarity) are different from a bat’s natural foraging situation,<br />

where it flies in 3-dimensional space and has to cope<br />

with complex echo scenes. In this paper, I discuss flightcage<br />

studies as a method to bridge the gap between field<br />

studies and psychophysical experiments. I outline the<br />

advantages of working with bats in a flight cage and then<br />

address more specifically the advantages and disadvantages<br />

of recording echolocation calls in a flight cage.<br />

Finally, I address the question of whether acoustic species<br />

identifications can be based on flight-cage recordings as<br />

a reference database.<br />

WHY WORK WITH BATS IN A FLIGHT CAGE?<br />

If one is interested in the study of hunting and associated<br />

echolocation behavior in bats in the field, one has<br />

to first know the natural foraging areas of the bats. This<br />

is feasible for bats who prefer a specific and delimited<br />

habitat type, such as the surface of calm water bodies.<br />

Not surprisingly, several detailed field studies report the<br />

prey-capture techniques and echolocation behavior of<br />

trawling Myotis species; those that capture insects from<br />

and at low heights above water surfaces (e.g., Britton et<br />

al.1997; Jones and Rayner 1988, 1991; Kalko and<br />

Schnitzler 1989). Likewise, bats that forage in open situations<br />

using echolocation calls with high sound-pressure<br />

level can, with considerable effort, be found,<br />

observed, and recorded in the field (e.g., Jensen and<br />

Miller 1999; Kalko 1995; Kalko and Schnitzler 1993).<br />

However, the natural hunting behavior of bats that<br />

habitually forage within or close to vegetation and who<br />

often use low sound-pressure level echolocation signals<br />

is difficult to see in the field. Even radio-tagged individuals<br />

are not easy to be directly observed (e.g., Arlettaz<br />

1999; Siemers et al. 1999). It is even more difficult to get<br />

close enough to obtain high-quality call recordings<br />

especially when the bats are actually capturing prey. For<br />

these species, flight-cage studies may provide a suitable<br />

approach to observe foraging behavior difficult to<br />

observe in the wild (e.g., Trachops cirrhosus (Phylostomidae):<br />

Barclay et al. 1981; Plecotus auritus (Vespertilionidae):<br />

Anderson and Racey 1991; Myotis sp.: Arlettaz et<br />

al. 2001; Faure and Barclay 1992, 1994; Siemers and<br />

Schnitzler 2000; Swift and Racey 2002).<br />

Most flight-cage studies focus on gleaning bats and<br />

on those which hunt in or close to vegetation. First, only<br />

anecdotal foraging observations are often obtainable for<br />

these species. Second, flight-cage studies closely emulate<br />

natural conditions for these kinds of bats. A flight cage is<br />

a restricted and echo-cluttered “habitat” from a bat’s point<br />

of view. To try and study prey capture behavior of open<br />

space foragers in such a restricted environment will not<br />

yield meaningful results. However, bats adapted to cluttered<br />

habitats should cope well with a flight-cage environment.<br />

They can be observed and experimentally tested<br />

while performing natural behaviors in this semi-natural<br />

setting. If necessary, flight cages can be equipped with<br />

habitat elements such as leaf litter, grasses, branches,<br />

small ponds, etc. (e.g., Arlettaz et al. 2001; Britton and<br />

Jones 1999; Siemers and Schnitzler 2000; Siemers et al.<br />

2001a, 2001b; Swift and Racey 2002).<br />

In a flight cage, it is also feasible to study behavior<br />

with a degree of detail difficult to achieve for animals in<br />

the wild. This, for example, applies to the study of the<br />

bats’ motor behavior during prey capture. Close-up photographic<br />

or video documentation can be used, as prey<br />

can be offered at a predefined location. Detailed behavioral<br />

documentation with sufficient sample sizes are relatively<br />

easy to obtain. For instance, the role of the tail<br />

membrane and feet during prey retrieval from the water<br />

surface by Myotis daubentonii was clarified in a flight cage<br />

(Britton and Jones 1999; Siemers et al. 2001a) complementing<br />

previous field observations (Jones and Rayner<br />

1988; Kalko and Schnitzler 1989).<br />

I referred to the difficulty of obtaining quantitative<br />

experimental data in the field although there are a range<br />

of excellent examples of experimental field studies<br />

assessing the sensory basis of food detection with wild<br />

bats (e.g., Barclay and Brigham 1994; Boonman et al.<br />

1998; Fuzessery et al. 1993; von Helversen and von Helversen<br />

1999). Two of these studies involved training wild<br />

trawling bats to forage in a defined experimental area,<br />

selecting and taking objects from the water surface. Von<br />

Helversen and von Helversen (1999) worked with flower-visiting<br />

glossophagine bats and experimentally<br />

manipulated their natural food resource, the inflorescences<br />

of the Neotropical liana, Mucuna holtonii. Where<br />

<strong>Bat</strong> <strong>Echolocation</strong> <strong>Researc</strong>h: tools, techniques & analysis

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!