17.06.2014 Views

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

and experienced in three time frames (past, present, and future). 3<br />

Consider some examples: guilt and shame are varieties of disgust<br />

in differing social contexts. When disgust is directed outward, it<br />

may take the form of disdain; when combined with anger, it<br />

may turn to recrimination and vengeance. The experience of primary<br />

emotions is modified by the virtually infinite variety of<br />

states of mind, particularly considering that the associations of<br />

long-term memory are unique representations of objects and<br />

events. One potential criticism of this taxonomy is that it is<br />

reductive or deterministic. I refute such criticism by calculating 4<br />

the unlimited variety of the precursors of suffering, values, and<br />

behavior. The present discussion is about suffering; but I cannot<br />

ignore the fact that creativity cannot be predicted by a reduction<br />

to finite elements. Our appetites for curiosity and mastery will<br />

sometimes motivate us to think and do outrageous and wonderful<br />

things. When considering the variety of experiences that<br />

result in suffering, most observers recall the opening of Tolstoy’s<br />

Anna Karenina: “All happy families resemble one another; every<br />

unhappy family is unhappy in its own fashion.”<br />

PREMISE 2<br />

When pain evokes fear and grief, the effect on the<br />

maps in the brain’s body-sensing regions is the<br />

same as for fear and grief alone.<br />

I PROPOSE THAT CHRONIC PAIN, OR SEVERE ACUTE PAIN,<br />

evokes fear and grief and that the pain-evoked fear and grief<br />

have the same effect on the maps in the body-sensing regions of<br />

the brain as do fear and grief alone. When pain evokes fear or<br />

grief by threatening the existential domains of the individual, as<br />

in Cassell’s formulation, the suffering that results is qualitatively<br />

no different from any other form of suffering. The differences<br />

in the varieties of the experience of suffering, I propose, arise<br />

from its context—what Cassell referred to as “the meaning” of<br />

suffering. The current technology of neuroscience does not have<br />

the resolution to address, no less to validate, this theoretical<br />

postulate. I have no doubt that it will.<br />

PREMISE 3<br />

Suffering is not an emotion, but is an “emotionally<br />

competent stimulus” capable of evoking emotions.<br />

FEAR AND GRIEF, THE PROPOSED ANTECEDENTS OF SUFFERING,<br />

ARE EMOTIONS. I propose that suffering is not. Emotions evoke<br />

action, somatic and visceral motor activity, and aversive behavior<br />

in the face of fear and disgust. I propose that if fear and<br />

grief do not evoke suffering, they do motivate corrective or<br />

aversive action in the face of danger or loss. 5<br />

I am persuaded to think that suffering does not evoke aversive<br />

or corrective behavior by itself. Suffering is involutional,<br />

resulting in withdrawal, stasis, and inaction. Suffering is interior<br />

and isolating. Suffering is experienced as distress, misery, agony,<br />

anguish, torment, wretchedness, despair, hopelessness, excruciation,<br />

and woe. When we are lonely, we seek companionship.<br />

When we “suffer” the death of a loved one, we shut ourselves<br />

off and rely on the cultural rituals of the funeral, wake, or<br />

receiving of friends and family to provide resilience and connection<br />

to others. We recognize and admire the resilience of those<br />

who console others around them, even though everyone has<br />

experienced the same loss or, in the case of danger, the same<br />

jeopardy and fear.<br />

I have portrayed suffering as leading to inaction and stasis.<br />

I propose that with the evolution of suffering, the emotions of<br />

disgust and anger took on the role of decreasing the intensity of<br />

suffering by motivating action. Such actions often appear to be<br />

maladaptive when they are resentful or hostile. The ancient role<br />

of disgust was to protect against injury from contamination by<br />

waste, decay, and contagion. To experience disgust at the perception<br />

of an abstract stimulus or of a benign object is a recent<br />

response. Rage once served for aggressive display and dominance<br />

in reproductive and survival conflicts. Now, exploitation<br />

and dominance, as expressed in revenge, are more common. 6<br />

Although Elisabeth Kubler-Ross’s work has been criticized after<br />

her death, her well-known monograph on loss and grief<br />

demonstrates how suffering evokes disgust and rage (11).<br />

3 Time frames include the “past” from long-term memory, both declarative<br />

and non-declarative; the “present” from short-term memory (the answer to<br />

Emily Webb’s question [from Thornton Wilder’s Our Town] is: no, we do<br />

not experience life as we live it, we experience it after it happens); and the<br />

“future” as representations of anticipated objects and events (5).<br />

4 The calculation of possible states of mind multiplies six primary emotions,<br />

10 drive states, three time frames, and an arbitrary ordinal scale of intensity<br />

for the contribution of each of the other three dimensions. Even if many of<br />

the cells of such a matrix are empty, the product is a number beyond<br />

comprehension.<br />

5 For example, if I am afraid of a bull moose, I wait until he leaves and do<br />

not cross the valley to fish where he is feeding. But I do not suffer for it,<br />

unless he is feeding at the edge of the best pool in the river.<br />

6 We easily intuit the ancient survival value of fear, surprise, disgust, and anger;<br />

however, we might consider the role of disgust and anger in meliorating suffering<br />

when we recall the media frenzy that accompanies any human tragedy.<br />

Accusation, scapegoating, recrimination, and revenge follow hard upon fear<br />

and grief. A good example is the Sago Mine explosion in January 2006. After<br />

days of intense rescue efforts, a rumor spread that 1 miner was dead and that<br />

12 survived. The opposite was true and the truth was not transmitted to the<br />

community for 3 hours. The disgust and rage that followed were severe,<br />

arguably out of proportion to the offence. For many, rage and disgust were<br />

easily turned against the media for exploiting the suffering of the lost miners’<br />

families. Perhaps we cleave to the suffering of others to validate our own<br />

resilience, but our reactions only reveal our vulnerability.<br />

T H E PA I N P R A C T I T I O N E R | V O L U M E 16 , N U M B E R 1 | 77

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!