06.11.2012 Views

Susan ayers cambridge handbook of psychology he

Susan ayers cambridge handbook of psychology he

Susan ayers cambridge handbook of psychology he

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

• Working memory (sometimes called short-term memory), which<br />

refers to processes for holding and manipulating material<br />

in a temporary store, usually for seconds or a few minutes<br />

(e.g. remembering a telephone number shortly after <strong>he</strong>aring it).<br />

• Procedural memory, which usually refers to t<strong>he</strong> ability to acquire<br />

new skills, or use previously acquired skills, be t<strong>he</strong>y motor or<br />

linguistic (e.g. learning to ride a bicycle).<br />

• Perceptual memory, which usually refers to t<strong>he</strong> ability to retain<br />

t<strong>he</strong> perceptual features <strong>of</strong> a stimulus, and is thought to underlie<br />

learning that leads to t<strong>he</strong> perceptual identification <strong>of</strong> stimuli<br />

(e.g. seeing a new model <strong>of</strong> a car and being able to identify it<br />

better t<strong>he</strong> second time round).<br />

In t<strong>he</strong> case <strong>of</strong> memory processes, while it is generally accepted that<br />

encoding, consolidation and retrieval represent important stages <strong>of</strong><br />

memory processing, t<strong>he</strong>re are divergent views as to t<strong>he</strong> specific<br />

mechanisms that underlie such stages (Meeter & Murre, 2004). T<strong>he</strong><br />

‘standard consolidation’ model (Squire et al., 2004) takes t<strong>he</strong> view<br />

that while t<strong>he</strong> medial temporal lobe plays a part in t<strong>he</strong> laying down <strong>of</strong><br />

new memories, t<strong>he</strong>ir stored representation is eventually subserved by<br />

cortical mechanisms, especially those in t<strong>he</strong> neocortex. By contrast,<br />

t<strong>he</strong> multiple-trace model (Nadel et al., 2000) regards t<strong>he</strong> medial temporal<br />

lobe as being critical for t<strong>he</strong> retention and recollection <strong>of</strong> all<br />

episodic memories, no matter how long ago t<strong>he</strong>y have been stored.<br />

Although precise figures are not available, t<strong>he</strong>re are considerable<br />

numbers <strong>of</strong> memory impaired people in society. Some 10% <strong>of</strong><br />

people over 65 years <strong>of</strong> age have dementia and some 36% <strong>of</strong><br />

people with severe <strong>he</strong>ad injury will have permanent memory<br />

impairments (see ‘Dementias’ and ‘Head injury’). Add to t<strong>he</strong>se<br />

figures those whose memory deficits result from Korsak<strong>of</strong>f’s syndrome,<br />

encephalitis, anoxia, AIDS and so forth, and one can begin<br />

to appreciate t<strong>he</strong> enormity <strong>of</strong> t<strong>he</strong> problem.<br />

Assessment <strong>of</strong> memory should include both neuropsychological<br />

and behavioural measures as it is important to identify cognitive<br />

strengths and weaknesses and to identify t<strong>he</strong> everyday problems<br />

arising from memory impairment. Neuropsychological tests<br />

should include general intellectual functioning, language, perception<br />

and executive functioning as well as detailed memory assessments.<br />

Immediate and delayed memory, nonverbal and verbal<br />

memory, recall and recognition, semantic and episodic memory,<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Baddeley, A. D. & Wilson, B. A. (1994).<br />

W<strong>he</strong>n implicit learning fails: amnesia and<br />

t<strong>he</strong> problem <strong>of</strong> error elimination.<br />

Neuropsychologia, 32, 53–68.<br />

Craik, F. I. M. & Tulving, E. (2000). Oxford<br />

<strong>handbook</strong> <strong>of</strong> memory. Oxford: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Kapur, N., Glisky, E. L. & Wilson, B. A. (2004).<br />

External memory aids and computers in<br />

memory rehabilitation. In A. D. Baddeley,<br />

M. D. Kopelman & B. A. Wilson. T<strong>he</strong><br />

essential <strong>handbook</strong> <strong>of</strong> memory disorders for<br />

clinicians (pp. 301–27). Chic<strong>he</strong>ster:<br />

John Wiley.<br />

Kapur, N. & Graham, K. S. (2002).<br />

Recovery <strong>of</strong> memory function in<br />

neurological disease. In A. D.<br />

Baddeley, M. Kopelman & B. A.<br />

Wilson (Eds.). Handbook <strong>of</strong> memory<br />

disorders (pp. 233–48). Chic<strong>he</strong>ster:<br />

John Wiley.<br />

Meeter, M. & Murre, J. M. (2004).<br />

Consolidation <strong>of</strong> long-term<br />

memory: evidence and alternatives.<br />

Psychological Bulletin, 130, 843–57.<br />

Nadel, L., Samsonovich, A., Ryan, L. &<br />

Moscovitch, M. (2000). Multiple<br />

trace t<strong>he</strong>ory <strong>of</strong> human memory:<br />

computational, neuroimaging and<br />

neuropsychological results. Hippocampus,<br />

10, 352–68.<br />

Rajah, M. N. & McIntosh, A. R. (2005).<br />

Overlap in t<strong>he</strong> functional neural systems<br />

involved in semantic and episodic memory<br />

implicit memory (remembering without awareness) and new learning<br />

will all need to be assessed (see Wilson, 2004 for furt<strong>he</strong>r discussion).<br />

More functional and behavioural measures will identify<br />

everyday problems causing concern and distress. T<strong>he</strong>se measures<br />

include observations in real life settings, interviewing patients, t<strong>he</strong>ir<br />

relatives and care staff and t<strong>he</strong> collection <strong>of</strong> information from such<br />

self report measures as diaries, c<strong>he</strong>cklists, questionnaires and rating<br />

scales (see ‘Neuropsychological assessment’).<br />

Restoration <strong>of</strong> memory functioning or retraining <strong>of</strong> memory<br />

following brain damage appear to be unachievable goals although<br />

some recovery may occur for a period <strong>of</strong> years (Kapur & Graham,<br />

2002; Wilson, 2003). Consequently, rehabilitation for memoryimpaired<br />

people focuses on environmental adaptations, compensatory<br />

strategies, improving learning and <strong>he</strong>lping t<strong>he</strong>m to make<br />

better use <strong>of</strong> t<strong>he</strong>ir residual skills. For those who are very severely<br />

intellectually impaired, structuring t<strong>he</strong> environment, to reduce<br />

t<strong>he</strong> need to remember, is probably t<strong>he</strong> most effective method.<br />

Examples include labelling doors, cupboards and drawers, drawing<br />

coloured lines from one place to anot<strong>he</strong>r and positioning material<br />

so it cannot be missed. External memory aids are probably t<strong>he</strong><br />

most beneficial <strong>of</strong> all t<strong>he</strong>rapeutic approac<strong>he</strong>s, although many<br />

memory-impaired people find it difficult to use t<strong>he</strong>se aids efficiently<br />

and it requires considerable ingenuity to teach t<strong>he</strong>ir use (Sohlberg<br />

& Mateer, 1989). Kapur et al. (2004) cover aspects <strong>of</strong> external and<br />

compensatory aids in some detail. One <strong>of</strong> t<strong>he</strong> recent developments<br />

for improving learning in amnesic subjects is t<strong>he</strong> errorless<br />

learning approach (Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Wilson et al., 1994)<br />

w<strong>he</strong>reby it was shown that preventing errors during t<strong>he</strong> learning<br />

process led to improved learning by amnesic subjects. Helping<br />

people to make better use <strong>of</strong> t<strong>he</strong>ir residual, albeit damaged,<br />

memory skills can be achieved through t<strong>he</strong> use <strong>of</strong> mnemonics<br />

and study or re<strong>he</strong>arsal strategies (see ‘Neuropsychological<br />

rehabilitation’).<br />

Finally, many memory-impaired people are anxious and isolated;<br />

so too are t<strong>he</strong>ir families. Rehabilitation should address t<strong>he</strong>se anxieties<br />

through anxiety management programmes, information and<br />

counselling and perhaps through groups for patients and/or t<strong>he</strong>ir<br />

relatives. Tate (2004) addresses t<strong>he</strong>se issues and ot<strong>he</strong>r aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

rehabilitation for people with amnesia.<br />

retrieval. Journal <strong>of</strong> Cognitive Neuroscience,<br />

17, 470–82.<br />

Schacter, D. L. & Tulving, E. (1994). Memory<br />

Systems 1994. Cambridge: MIT Press.<br />

Sohlberg, M. M. & Mateer, C. (1989).<br />

Training use <strong>of</strong> compensatory memory<br />

books: a three-stage behavioural<br />

approach. Journal <strong>of</strong> Clinical and<br />

Experimental Neuro<strong>psychology</strong>, 11,<br />

871–91.<br />

Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E. & Clark, R. E. (2004).<br />

T<strong>he</strong> medial temporal lobe. Annual Review<br />

<strong>of</strong> Neuroscience, 27, 279–306.<br />

Tate, R. (2004). Emotional and social<br />

consequences <strong>of</strong> memory disorders.<br />

In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman &<br />

B. A. Wilson (Eds.). T<strong>he</strong> essential <strong>handbook</strong><br />

Amnesia 547

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!