12.01.2015 Views

Slides

Slides

Slides

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Table 3. No. of verb types before inflections<br />

(Pine, Lieven and Rowland 1998)<br />

Subjects -ing -s -ed<br />

Rita 17 2 4<br />

Joey 32 5 8<br />

Julie 12 2 3<br />

Jean 30 2 4<br />

Ricky 8 2 1<br />

Eva 11 8 2<br />

Helen 4 2 -<br />

Carl 11 - -<br />

John 1 1 1<br />

Simon 22 - 2<br />

Olga 7 - -<br />

Laura 4 - 1<br />

Mean 13.3 2.0 2.2<br />

Example 2 : V + CP (Diessel 2004)<br />

A study of CHILDES data of five children covering the age range 1;7 to 5;1.<br />

For all five children (Naomi, Peter, Nina, Sarah, Adam) the most frequent<br />

complement taking verbs of the children's finite complement clauses are the following,<br />

which constituted 80% of the tokens:<br />

know, see, think, say, look (mean %: 22.9, 20.5, 19.0, 9.8, 8.8).<br />

(I) think it's a cow. [Peter 2;2]<br />

Know what that is [Nina 2;3]<br />

Let's see if we can fix them. [Peter 2;3]<br />

Diessel argues that most utterances that include a finite complement clause in early<br />

child speech are simple nonembedded sentences containing a single proposition.<br />

These 'matrix verbs' are epistemic markers, attention getters or markers of<br />

illocutionary force.<br />

2.4 Combinatorial versatility<br />

There should be a certain degree of overlap in the cooccurrence patterns of various<br />

members of the category.<br />

Example: Aux + V (Pine, Lieven and Rowland 1998)<br />

6

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!