23.07.2019 Views

YSM Issue 90.4

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

neuroscience<br />

NEWS<br />

NOW YOU HEAR ME, NOW YOU DON’T<br />

Testing susceptibility of people to hearing voices<br />

►BY SUNNIE LU<br />

PHOTOGRAPHY BY YASMIN ALAMDEEN<br />

►Dr. Powers and Dr. Corlett took MRI scans of people<br />

performing auditory tasks to understand auditory hallucinations.<br />

You hear footsteps coming down the hallway and voices chanting,<br />

“We’re coming.” A shadowy figure suddenly appears in the<br />

corner of your room, while a girl dressed up as a rat looms over<br />

your bed. Although these scenarios sound like they come from<br />

horror movies, they actually are real examples of hypnagogic hallucinations,<br />

which occur during the onset of sleep. Both having<br />

these experiences, Yale psychiatrist and neuroscientist Albert<br />

Powers and Yale neuroscientist Philip Corlett fascinated with<br />

auditory hallucinations. Their research, featured in Science, suggests<br />

that people who hear voices are more likely to experience<br />

induced hallucinations in a lab.<br />

It may seem concerning that both Powers and Corlett have<br />

experienced hallucinations while falling asleep, but these hallucinations<br />

are usually symptomatic of a neurological condition,<br />

not a psychiatric illness. However, people without a psychiatric<br />

condition can hear voices too. The two scientists wanted to figure<br />

out what produces auditory hallucinations and why some<br />

voice-hearing experiences are benign and others require medical<br />

attention.<br />

To explore these questions, they sought out four groups of<br />

test subjects: both psychotic and nonpsychotic voice-hearers<br />

and non-voice-hearers. After identifying potential subjects, the<br />

researchers separated the psychotic and nonpsychotic people<br />

using a questionnaire developed by forensic psychologists to<br />

distinguish between people who were actually experiencing hallucinations<br />

and those who only claimed to do so.<br />

After screening their subjects for hallucinations, Powers and<br />

Corlett induced auditory hallucinations in their subjects to identify<br />

whether psychotic people were more likely than non-psychotic<br />

people to hear conditioned sounds. Using a technique<br />

originally developed at Yale during the 1890s, the subjects were<br />

stimulated with a checkerboard image and a one-second long<br />

sound simultaneously and repeatedly, while getting their brains<br />

imaged by MRIs. This conditioned the subjects to associate the<br />

image with the tone. As the scientists changed the intensity of<br />

tone, sometimes turning it off, the subjects pressed a button when<br />

they thought they heard the tone, while changing the length of<br />

time they pressed the button to show their level of confidence.<br />

Many reported hearing a tone when only the checkerboard<br />

image appeared but no tone played. This inconsistency occurred<br />

more often with the two voice-hearing groups—the people with<br />

schizophrenia and self-identified clairaudient psychics. Both<br />

groups were almost five times more likely to report that they<br />

heard a nonexistent tone than the non-voice-hearing groups.<br />

Furthermore, the two non-voice-hearing groups were 28 percent<br />

more confident that they had heard the tone when no tone<br />

played. These results support a possible explanation for hallucinations.<br />

“The brain makes models for what the outside world is<br />

like,” said Corlett, noting that these models sometimes don’t always<br />

match reality. This study suggests that people hallucinate<br />

when their expectations overweigh what their senses tell them.<br />

Powers and Corlett further understood auditory hallucinations<br />

through analyzing the MRI scans collected: the parts of the<br />

brain that were responsive to the tone were active when people<br />

reported conditioned hallucinations, producing MRI scans of<br />

brains that looked like those of people actually hearing the tone.<br />

The images also revealed that both hallucinating and non-hallucinating<br />

people with psychosis exhibit abnormal brain activity in<br />

regions that monitor internal representations of reality. These results<br />

contribute to the idea that hallucinations stem from internal<br />

representations overruling actual sensory data.<br />

This study was able to distinguish not only between those who<br />

hallucinate and those who don’t, but also between psychotic and<br />

non-psychotic people. “The sooner you catch psychosis and the<br />

sooner you intervene, the better the general outcomes are,” said<br />

Powers. According to Powers, most people with the symptoms<br />

associated with increased chances of psychosis don’t even develop<br />

psychosis. The question then is, who should receive treatment?<br />

This new research may help to answer that key question<br />

by providing the basis for tests to diagnose patients who require<br />

psychiatric treatment early.<br />

“There is no one-size-fits-all anti-psychotic, so there should be<br />

different treatments for different people,” Corlett said. Although<br />

this sort of precision medicine has not existed in psychiatry so<br />

far, Corlett hopes that this study, along with future research, will<br />

lead to more personalized psychiatric treatments, which he believes<br />

would be more effective in helping people suffering with<br />

mental health issues.<br />

www.yalescientific.org<br />

October 2017<br />

Yale Scientific Magazine<br />

9

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!