10.01.2013 Views

Differential subject marking in Polish: The case of Genitive vs ...

Differential subject marking in Polish: The case of Genitive vs ...

Differential subject marking in Polish: The case of Genitive vs ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

imperfective viewpo<strong>in</strong>t is taken, the situation (or eventuality, to use Bach’s 1981 term<strong>in</strong>ology) is<br />

understood to hold throughout and potentially to extend beyond the Topic Time (i.e., a time for<br />

which the speaker wishes to make an assertion; recall footnote 4), that is, the situation is<br />

presented as unbounded. Now, given that byt’ <strong>in</strong> Russian and byc <strong>in</strong> <strong>Polish</strong> are stative verbs and<br />

given that the stative semantics is naturally compatible with the imperfective viewpo<strong>in</strong>t, it<br />

follows that byt’ and byc can naturally occur <strong>in</strong> “imperfective environments”, which gives rise to<br />

the impression that they are imperfective verbs. This impression notwithstand<strong>in</strong>g, they are – as<br />

far as their grammatical/morphological aspect is concerned – perfective verbs. It is this<br />

“perfective status” that determ<strong>in</strong>es their morphosyntactic behavior (recall the discussion above<br />

about the structure <strong>of</strong> the periphrastic future <strong>in</strong> Russian/<strong>Polish</strong>). Later, <strong>in</strong> section 5, it will be<br />

argued that it is precisely this “grammatical perfective aspect” which is relevant to the GEN-<br />

<strong>mark<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>in</strong> negated (existential)-locative sentences.<br />

Assum<strong>in</strong>g that the above observations are correct, the aspectual difference between byc<br />

and bywac can be described as follows: bywac has properties <strong>of</strong> an imperfective verb as far as<br />

both its lexical semantic properties (situation type aspect 33 ) and grammatical behavior<br />

(grammatical/morphological aspect) are concerned. In contrast, byc has semantic properties<br />

characteristic <strong>of</strong> an imperfective verb, but from the grammatical/morphological po<strong>in</strong>t <strong>of</strong> view it<br />

behaves as a perfective verb. 34<br />

3. Further factors responsible for the GEN/NOM split <strong>in</strong> (3)<br />

In the previous section, the first factor responsible for the GEN/NOM split <strong>in</strong> (3) has been<br />

identified, namely, the aspectual properties <strong>of</strong> the predicate: the GEN <strong>mark<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>of</strong> the “<strong>subject</strong>”<br />

appears to be dependent on the perfective aspect <strong>of</strong> the predicate (<strong>in</strong> the strictly<br />

grammatical/morphological sense), while the NOM <strong>mark<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>of</strong> the <strong>subject</strong> correlates with the<br />

clearly imperfective aspect (<strong>in</strong> terms <strong>of</strong> habituality/iterativity; cf. also footnote 29). <strong>The</strong> question<br />

33 Recall that bywac is habitual (or iterative) (Comrie 1976:27, fn. 1 remarks that “<strong>in</strong> Slavonic l<strong>in</strong>guistics,<br />

habitual forms are <strong>of</strong>ten referred to by the term ‘iterative’ (Russian mnogokratnyj), e.g. Russian pivat’,<br />

znavat’, the habitual counterparts <strong>of</strong> pit’ ‘dr<strong>in</strong>k’, znat’ ‘know’”). Habitual sentences are semantically stative.<br />

To see this, one should consider their truth conditions. As Smith (1997:34) po<strong>in</strong>ts out, when “<strong>in</strong>vestigat<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

truth <strong>of</strong> a habitual, one asks whether there is a pattern which holds over an <strong>in</strong>terval, not whether a particular<br />

situation occurred. <strong>The</strong> temporal schema holds for the <strong>in</strong>terval, as is typical <strong>of</strong> statives.”<br />

34 If this conclusion is correct, the situation <strong>in</strong> <strong>Polish</strong> described above would, <strong>in</strong> fact, <strong>in</strong> some sense resemble the<br />

situation found with stative verbs <strong>in</strong> F<strong>in</strong>nish. Kiparsky (1998:283) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that verbs like omistaa ‘to own’<br />

<strong>in</strong> F<strong>in</strong>nish behave – as far as their morphosyntactic properties (especially their <strong>case</strong> syntax) are concerned – as<br />

telic verbs, even though semantically they rema<strong>in</strong> atelic verbs.<br />

20

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!