03.03.2013 Views

Fourth International Orca Symposium and Workshop - CEBC - CNRS

Fourth International Orca Symposium and Workshop - CEBC - CNRS

Fourth International Orca Symposium and Workshop - CEBC - CNRS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Fourth</strong> <strong>International</strong> <strong>Orca</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Workshop</strong><br />

September 23-28, 2002<br />

<strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

© Frédérick Presle<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

1


<strong>Orca</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> & <strong>Workshop</strong><br />

<strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong><br />

79 360 Villiers en Bois<br />

France<br />

Tel 00 (33) (0) 5 49 09 78 39<br />

Fax 00 (33) (0) 5 49 09 65 26<br />

Organising Committee:Lance Barrett-Lennard (barrett@zoology.ubc.ca)<br />

John Ford (ford@zoology.ubc.ca)<br />

Christophe Guinet (guinet@cebc.cnrs.fr)<br />

Tiu Similä (iolaire@online.no)<br />

Fern<strong>and</strong>o Ugarte (fern<strong>and</strong>o_ugarte@hotmail.com)<br />

Contents:<br />

<strong>Workshop</strong>s 4<br />

Oral And Poster Presentations: 32<br />

Social determinants of population structure in killer whales: insights from association patterns,<br />

genetics <strong>and</strong> acoustics. 32<br />

Killer Whales At Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>, Southern Ocean 35<br />

Behavior <strong>and</strong> Ecology of Killer Whales in Monterey Bay, California 40<br />

Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) of Alaska: Gulf of Alaska to the Bering Sea. 45<br />

A REVIEW of Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) in Brazilian waters 46<br />

Sightings of killer whales off the Antarctic Peninsula from 1997/98 to 2001/02 50<br />

Click characteristics of killer whales feeding on Norwegian spring-spawning herring 54<br />

The BC Cetacean Sightings Network: working with the public to determine year-round<br />

distribution patterns 57<br />

Vocal Behaviour of Transient Killer Whales: Food Calling or Constrained Communication 59<br />

Mirror Image Processing in Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) 63<br />

Scaling Issues In Predator-Prey Interactions: Killer Whale Underwater Tail-Slaps 64<br />

Analysis of the discrete calls of killer whales from the Avacha Gulf of Kamchatka, Far East<br />

Russia 65<br />

Killer whales (Orcinus orca) in shetl<strong>and</strong> waters 68<br />

Reassessing The Social Organization Of Resident Killer Whales In British Columbia 72<br />

Behavioural <strong>and</strong> acoustic differences among belugas (Delphinapterus leucas) summer herds in<br />

the St. Lawrence estuary (Québec, Canada). 75<br />

Current Knowledge of Killer Whales in the Mexican Pacific 76<br />

Analysis of photo-identification data to make inferences about cetacean populations 77<br />

Assessing The Impacts Of Killer Whale Predation On Steller Sea Lions In Western Alaska 81<br />

World-wide genetic diversity in the killer whale (Orcinus orca); implications for demographic<br />

history. 83<br />

Socioecology of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Northern Patagonia 84<br />

Cooperative Hunting And Prey H<strong>and</strong>ling Of Killer Whales In Punta Norte, Patagonia,<br />

Argentina 85<br />

Killer whale (Orcinus orca) sightings in Alaska: Preliminary results from mariner survey data<br />

86<br />

Bioenergetic Changes From 1986 To 2002 In Southern Resident Killer Whales, Orcinus <strong>Orca</strong><br />

88<br />

Surface intervals of an adult male killer whale in norway 89<br />

Predatory Activity Of A Single Killer Whale, Orcinus <strong>Orca</strong>, At A Steller Sea Lion, Eumetopias<br />

Jubatus, Rookery In Alaska 92<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

2


Anatomy of a Long-term Study: Population Ecology of Killer Whales in Prince William<br />

Sound <strong>and</strong> Kenai Fjords, Alaska 1983-2002. 95<br />

Killer Whale Population Biology <strong>and</strong> Feeding Ecology in southeastern Alaska. 98<br />

unraveling the influence of social structure <strong>and</strong> prey type on vocal communication in killer<br />

whales 105<br />

O. orca abundance, distribution, seasonal presence, predation <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ings in the waters<br />

around Kamchatka <strong>and</strong> the Komm<strong>and</strong>er Isl<strong>and</strong>s: An assessment based on reported sightings<br />

1992-2000 106<br />

Killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Australian territorial <strong>and</strong> surrounding waters – are they<br />

secure ? 109<br />

Life history <strong>and</strong> decline of killer whales in crozet archipelago, southern indian ocean 119<br />

Life history <strong>and</strong> population dynamics of resident killer whales in Alaska 117<br />

Monitoring Whale Watching Activity In <strong>International</strong> Waters 120<br />

Life history <strong>and</strong> decline of killer whales in Crozet Archipelago, southern Indian Ocean 121<br />

Toxic chemical pollution <strong>and</strong> Pacific killer whales (Orcinus orca) 126<br />

The Biology <strong>and</strong> Status of an Endangered Transient Killer Whale Population in Prince William<br />

Sound, Alaska 131<br />

Matched Vocal Exchanges Of Shared Stereotyped Calls In Free-Ranging Resident Killer<br />

Whales 133<br />

Satellite Tracking Study Of Movements And Diving Behaviour Of Killer Whales In The<br />

Norwegian Sea 134<br />

Clicks, calls <strong>and</strong> underwater tail-slaps; sounds of norwegian killer whales. 136<br />

Aerial vocalisations of killer whales temporarily captured in northern norway. 137<br />

Interactions Between Killer Whales (Orcinus <strong>Orca</strong>) And Red Tuna (Thunnus Thynnus) Fishery<br />

In The Strait Of Gibraltar 138<br />

Observations Of Killer Whales During The Fall, Winter And Spring In Southeastern Alaska<br />

1980-2002 146<br />

What is the favourite prey ? Foraging ecology of killer whales at avacha gulf, russian far east.<br />

151<br />

Population Viability Analysis for the Southern Resident Population of Killer Whales 152<br />

Predation Behavior Of Transient Killer Whales In Monterey Bay, California 156<br />

Whistles <strong>and</strong> variable calls as close-range acoustic signals in wild killer whales off vancouver<br />

isl<strong>and</strong>, british columbia 160<br />

A Review Of Short- And Long-Term Effects Of Whale Watching On Killer Whales In British<br />

Columbia 165<br />

Associations <strong>and</strong> group size of killer whales in Kvæfjord, Norway, during October-November<br />

1997 168<br />

Norwegian killer whales feed on small herring schools close to the surface 174<br />

Can Routine Data Collection Benefit Killer Whale Research? 178<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca 183<br />

Pigmentation As An Indicative Feature For Populations Of Killer Whales 186<br />

First photo-identification matches for papua new guinea killer whales. 189<br />

Changes in call use in a resident orca-matriline with a new-born calf 190<br />

Spatial Modelling Of Antarctic Killer Whale Abundance And Distribution 192<br />

A Bioenergetic Model For Estimating The Food Requirements Of The Killer Whale (Orcinus<br />

<strong>Orca</strong>) 196<br />

Why do some killer whales talk so much? 199<br />

Authors index 207<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

3


<strong>Workshop</strong>s<br />

FORAGING ECOLOGY<br />

CONSERVATION<br />

ACOUSTIC<br />

POPULATION DYNAMICS, POPULATION STRUCTURE AND LIFE-HISTORY<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

4


KILLER WHALE WORKSHOP - FORAGING ECOLOGY<br />

Participants:<br />

Nancy Black<br />

Yves Cherel<br />

Luciano Dalla Rosa<br />

Paolo Domenici<br />

Graeme Ellis<br />

Christophe Guinet<br />

Dena Matkin<br />

Eva Saulitis<br />

Karina Tarasyan<br />

Alisa Schulman-Janiger<br />

Tiu Similä<br />

Jan Straley<br />

Richard Ternullo<br />

Victoria Turner<br />

Ingrid Visser<br />

Arliss Winship<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

The workshop participants discussed the following topics<br />

- How generalist/specialist are killer whale populations in their choice of prey? How<br />

flexible are killer whales to changes in prey abundance?<br />

- How can we identify the prey species of killer whales?<br />

- How does the choice of prey affect the behaviour <strong>and</strong> habitat use of killer whales ?<br />

- Recommendations for future work<br />

HOW GENERALIST/SPECIALIST ARE KILLER WHALES?<br />

In the previous <strong>Orca</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> in 1990 one of the main topics of discussions was the<br />

presence of two different types of killer whales in the coastal waters of eastern Pacific Ocean<br />

(from Alaska to California): the fish feeding residents <strong>and</strong> the marine mammal feeding<br />

transients. The difference in diet affects profoundly the behaviour, occurrence pattern, social<br />

organisation <strong>and</strong> acoustics of the two populations. The presence of two different types of<br />

killer whales within the same geographic area received much attention, <strong>and</strong> it was questioned<br />

if a similar situation existed elsewhere in the world <strong>and</strong> also if any killer whale populations<br />

have a more ”mixed” diet containing both marine mammals <strong>and</strong> fish. At that time there was<br />

evidence for the existence of separate marine mammal <strong>and</strong> fish feeding populations in the<br />

Antarctic waters, but information from other regions was considered preliminary.<br />

Since 1990, a third type of killer whale, ”offshore killer whales” have been described in<br />

the Northeastern Pacific. The offshore killer whales feed on fish <strong>and</strong> severe tooth wear<br />

suggests that an abrasive food source, such as elasmobranchs might be part of their diet. The<br />

offshore killer whales have not bee observed to mix with the residents <strong>and</strong> therefore it is<br />

suggested that sympatric, fish-feeding populations exist in these waters as well. In a recently<br />

started study in Kamchatka, Russia, the prey type of killer whales during summer season has<br />

been identified as fish (herring, mackerel <strong>and</strong> salmon). In Gibraltar Strait killer whales are<br />

feeding on tuna, also from the longlines used in the local fishery. Interaction with longline<br />

fishery has also been documented from the Crozet Isl<strong>and</strong>s, New Zeal<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Brazil. In the<br />

Northeast Atlantic killer whales feed on herring stocks both in Icel<strong>and</strong>ic <strong>and</strong> Norwegian<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

5


coastal waters. A satellite tracking study conducted in Norway indicates that the killer whales<br />

follow the yearly migration route of Norwegian spring-spawning herring. However, visual<br />

observations were not made of the killer whales during summer months <strong>and</strong> it is possible that<br />

the killer whales were preying on one of the predator species (fish, marine mammals) that<br />

follow herring to their feeding grounds during summer.<br />

One of the most important findings since the previous <strong>Orca</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong> has been the<br />

documentation of a mixed diet (i.e fish <strong>and</strong> marine mammals) for killer whales in the coastal<br />

waters of Argentina, Brazil, the Crozet Isl<strong>and</strong>s, New Zeal<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> Norway (for one group<br />

only). In all these areas further observations are needed for better documentation of how<br />

common a mixed diet is <strong>and</strong> to investigate the possibility of some killer whales specialising in<br />

marine mammals.<br />

In short, it is clear that there are both killer whales with very specialised diet <strong>and</strong> killer<br />

whales with a more varied diet. In addition, regardless of the degree of<br />

specialisation/generalisation in the population, group specific specialisations have been<br />

documented in many of the populations studied. The behaviour, especially the acoustic<br />

behaviour, of the populations with a mixed diet should be one of the focuses of future studies<br />

(more details in behaviour chapter).<br />

It should also be stressed that it is not sufficient to divide the prey of killer whales into<br />

just these two broad categories; fish <strong>and</strong> marine mammals. Killer whales do also feed on other<br />

type of prey (for example pelagic sharks). In addition, the behaviour <strong>and</strong> sensory abilities<br />

differ greatly among marine mammal <strong>and</strong> fish species.<br />

One of the obvious benefits of specialising on certain type of prey is in being able to<br />

develop <strong>and</strong> refine foraging techniques in a way that makes these populations very efficient<br />

hunters. However, the cost of being a specialist might be that the population is not flexible in<br />

adjusting to changes in prey abundance, ie. such populations are not likely to switch their prey<br />

type.<br />

The workshop participants suggested that predictability <strong>and</strong> abundance of prey could<br />

explain why some killer whales are specialised in their choice of prey while other populations<br />

are not. It was suggested that modelling prey abundance <strong>and</strong> occurrence for areas where there<br />

are specialists <strong>and</strong> areas where there are generalists might give us a better underst<strong>and</strong>ing of<br />

the underlying mechanisms.<br />

How to determine prey species?<br />

As the previous discussion shows, being able to identify killer whale prey species is<br />

important. In principle there are three different ways of studying the diet of killer whales;<br />

stomach content analysis of dead animals, observations of foraging killer whales <strong>and</strong> analysis<br />

of stable isotopes or fatty acids from biopsies.<br />

Identifying prey species through observing feeding killer whales can be very<br />

challenging. Identity of prey species is relatively easy to establish when feeding occurs close<br />

to the surface (eg. carousel feeding on schooling fish) or on the shore (eg. killer whales<br />

hunting on pinnipeds in Crozet <strong>and</strong> Patagonia). Observing killer whales feeding on fish or<br />

marine mammals below the surface is much more dem<strong>and</strong>ing. Underwater videocameras<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

6


(including crittercams) <strong>and</strong> high-frequency sonars can be helpful, but the use videocamera is<br />

restricted by visibility <strong>and</strong> sonar images do not necessarily allow for identifying prey species.<br />

Recent developments including a TDR (Time-depth recorder) which combines dive data to<br />

digital images of surroundings (including prey items) might prove useful. Developing<br />

observational skills is also important as was exemplified by how many years it took before<br />

marine mammal kills by transient killer whales in British Columbia were recognized.<br />

“Researcher exchange” has also proven very useful in several studies. Using same-observer<br />

skills in documenting the behaviour of different populations is recommended for several<br />

reasons; especially in new studies the presence of a skilled observer can be vital in identifying<br />

feeding behaviours <strong>and</strong> prey types <strong>and</strong> the use of “same pair of eyes” is also a way to make<br />

sure that the results from different studies are comparable.<br />

The use of a fine-mesh dipnet in collecting prey remains (fish scales, parts of marine<br />

mammals) among feeding killer whales is strongly recommended. This technique has been<br />

very useful in documenting the diet of killer whales in British Columbia.<br />

Biopsies containing killer whale blubber can be used for fatty acid analyse which are<br />

useful at least for in identifying trophic level of prey species. Fatty acid analysis conducted on<br />

transient <strong>and</strong> resident killer whales showed that these populations feed on prey from different<br />

trophic levels. Such analyse could be useful in identifying the trophic level of prey in other<br />

killer whale populations as well. More difficult is the use of this technique in identifying<br />

possible changes in prey species (ie. switching from one fish species to another). For better<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of how such changes can be studied by fatty acid profiles, an experimental<br />

study should be conducted on captive animals subjected to switch in their diet. It was stressed<br />

that any project collecting biopsy samples should make all possible effort to collect the<br />

samples in such a way that they can be used for different analysis; genetics, fatty acid profiles,<br />

toxic chemicals.<br />

Killer whale carcasses tend to sink, <strong>and</strong> therefore access to stomach contents is fairly<br />

limited. In addition, stomach content analysis is difficult to interpret given that the prey of the<br />

target species is taken simultaneously. Erosion of bones could be looked at to discern target<br />

prey of the killer whale (i.e. fish in stomachs of marine mammals <strong>and</strong> fish in stomachs of<br />

other fish, elephant seals with squid beaks in stomachs).<br />

How does choice of prey affect the foraging behaviour of killer whales?<br />

One of the main topics of the discussion in the workshop was how well killer whales are<br />

able to adjust their behaviour to feeding on different types of prey. In this context the<br />

populations feeding both on fish <strong>and</strong> marine mammals are of special interest. The studies<br />

conducted in Northeastern Pacific have clearly shown that the social <strong>and</strong> acoustic behaviour<br />

differs greatly between the marine mammal <strong>and</strong> fish feeding populations; silence is important<br />

when searching <strong>and</strong> hunting marine mammal prey while killer whales feeding on fish are<br />

vocal both while they are searching <strong>and</strong> feeding on fish. To date very little information exists<br />

on the acoustic behaviour of the killer whales which feed both on marine mammals <strong>and</strong> fish<br />

<strong>and</strong> future studies focusing on this are strongly recommended. One group of killer whales in<br />

the Monterey Bay area have been observed feeding on elasmobranchs <strong>and</strong> fish. When feeding<br />

on elasmobranchs the whales were in a small group <strong>and</strong> very silent, like transient killer<br />

whales while feeding on marine mammals. The group size was larger when these whales were<br />

feeding on fish, but it is not known if the whales are vocal when feeding on fish.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

7


Evidence for flexibility in vocal behaviour has been demonstrated for transients in<br />

Monterey Bay; the killer whales are silent while hunting small-sized marine mammal prey but<br />

are very vocal while hunting <strong>and</strong> feeding on grey whale calves. It is possible that the vocal<br />

activity increases to attract more whales to participate in the hunt for larger sized prey.<br />

Observations of transient killer whales in BC <strong>and</strong> Alaska also suggest that the transient killer<br />

whales which have been thought to occur in small groups may be living in larger social units<br />

that are in acoustic contact. They forage in small groups <strong>and</strong> start vocalising after kills to join<br />

for social behaviours.<br />

The group size of transients may vary depending on the size of prey. When feeding on<br />

harbour seals, the groups tend to be small. Sea lions are larger <strong>and</strong> also more dangerous for<br />

the killer whales to catch <strong>and</strong> therefore the group size is often larger when sea lions are<br />

caught. Although harbour porpoises, Dall’s porpoises <strong>and</strong> white-beaked dolphins are not very<br />

large, the transients need to operate in larger groups while hunting them since these small<br />

cetaceans are swift <strong>and</strong> able to evade predation unless pursued by several whales.<br />

The size of the prey can also be viewed in terms of mechanistic constraints. A smaller<br />

whale can turn quicker than a larger whale, <strong>and</strong> a small fish can turn faster than any whale.<br />

All strategies of hunting <strong>and</strong> feeding vary with many factors relating to prey size <strong>and</strong><br />

therefore the prey size <strong>and</strong> behaviour (for example schooling) will determine foraging<br />

behaviour<br />

Relatively little is known about potential age/sex related differences in the feeding<br />

behaviour of killer whales. In the marine mammal hunts where killer whales partially str<strong>and</strong><br />

themselves, such behaviour is not performed by adult males, probably due to their larger body<br />

size which gives them a greater risk of getting permanently str<strong>and</strong>ed. However, in New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong>, where killer whales feed on rays in very shallow waters, both sexes participate in<br />

feeding. Adult females seem to be the ones taking tuna from longlines in the Gibraltar strait,<br />

while both sexes are engaged in this behaviour in New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. It has been suggested that<br />

adult males would have an important function in feeding behaviours where deep <strong>and</strong>/or long<br />

dives are needed. In Norway killer whales occasionally chase up herring schools from 150-<br />

300 meters depth. However, the dive data collected in the satellite tracking study does not<br />

indicate that the adult males would make longer or deeper dives than subadult females. On the<br />

contrary, the deepest <strong>and</strong> longest dives have been made by 8-10 year old females.<br />

In almost all marine mammal kills by transients, food sharing is observed. This is often<br />

detected by observing gulls following different group members as they tear up prey <strong>and</strong> leave<br />

scraps behind. In New Zeal<strong>and</strong> killer whales are observed sharing food (including several<br />

observations of adult males sharing food with young calves) regardless of the age/sex of the<br />

animal killing the prey or the type of prey. The killer whales feeding on herring schools in<br />

Norway stun their prey with tailslaps before feeding; other whales than the ones performing<br />

the tailslaps also feed on the stunned prey.<br />

When killer whale prey species are known, research projects studying the foraging<br />

ecology of killer whales benefit from cooperation with scientists studying the behaviour of the<br />

prey species. In the Norwegian study, cooperation with herring biologists has been of vital<br />

importance in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the behaviour <strong>and</strong> habitat use of killer whales. One important<br />

aspect of the biology of the prey species is their sensory abilities; especially knowledge about<br />

hearing <strong>and</strong> vision are important in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the techniques killer whales use while<br />

hunting<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

8


The way prey avoids killer whale predation has been documented in several areas. Seals<br />

are known to move to higher ground, hide in kelp <strong>and</strong> dive close to seamounts <strong>and</strong> seafloor<br />

when killer whales are present <strong>and</strong> rays hide in shallow waters, between wharf pilings, in rock<br />

pools or even leave the water.. The preference for feeding on young marine mammals is<br />

probably related to the fact that naive prey is probably much easier to kill. Older individuals<br />

can be both more difficult <strong>and</strong> more risky to catch (pinnipeds, large cetaceans <strong>and</strong> some<br />

elasmobranchs are capable of injuring killer whales).<br />

The importance of habitat in foraging behaviour has received relatively little attention<br />

<strong>and</strong> it was suggested that physiographic features of different ocean basins should be<br />

investigated to underst<strong>and</strong> better the foraging strategies of killer whales. Killer whales feeding<br />

on fish are known to use underwater seamounts in herding their prey. Marine mammal<br />

feeding killer whales are known to use passive listening in locating prey <strong>and</strong> this behaviour<br />

must be influenced by sea surface state <strong>and</strong> ambient noise levels. In addition, underwater<br />

visibility can potentially affect the hunting success of killer whales. In this context it was also<br />

noted that the presence of a research vessel can affect the observed foraging behaviour. The<br />

engine noise can alert marine mammal prey to a potential hiding place <strong>and</strong> hinder foraging<br />

behaviour of transient killer whales by masking sounds made by prey. In Norway, herring<br />

schools have been observed reacting to engine noise (especially outboard engines).<br />

Future research<br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> the underlying mechanisms shaping specialist/generalist killer whale<br />

populations, modelling prey abundance <strong>and</strong> occurrence for areas where specialist <strong>and</strong><br />

generalist populations occur, would be useful.<br />

The fact that the behaviour of the killer whales feeding on marine mammals <strong>and</strong> fish in<br />

the Northeast Pacific is very different, raises some interesting questions regarding the killer<br />

whales with a mixed diet. Future studies should focus on describing the acoustic behaviour of<br />

the same killer whale groups while foraging on marine mammals <strong>and</strong> fish.<br />

To be able to compare the foraging behaviour of different populations, collaborative<br />

projects are encouraged. By using at least the same methodology, preferably also same<br />

observers, it would be much easier to compare the foraging behaviour of different<br />

populations.<br />

Research on fatty acid analysis is recommended to determine if it can be used to<br />

identify the diet of killer whales. Controlled experiments should be carried out on captive<br />

killer whales where diet can be manipulated to look for differences in fatty acid profiles.<br />

The way prey size affects hunting behaviour <strong>and</strong> food sharing should be studied in an<br />

area where transient killer whales are frequently observed feeding on different sized marine<br />

mammal prey (for example Monterey Bay area).<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong>ing the foraging behaviour of a killer whale population can benefit from<br />

cooperation with scientists studying the behaviour of the prey species.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

9


The use of satellite telemetry has shown to be a powerful tool in underst<strong>and</strong>ing the<br />

range, habitat use, movement pattern <strong>and</strong> diving behaviour of killer whales. However, the use<br />

of this technique is not recommended unless there is enough baseline information on the<br />

whales to be studied <strong>and</strong> there are important question/questions which can not be answered in<br />

another way.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

10


KILLER WHALE WORKSHOP - CONSERVATION<br />

27 SEPTEMBER 2002<br />

ANDREW TRITES, CHAIR<br />

Participants<br />

Andrew W Trites University of British Columbia Canada<br />

Lea David <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong> France<br />

Marthán Bester University of Pretoria South Africa<br />

Fabierre Delfour University of Grenoble France<br />

Miguel Iniguez Cethus Foundation Argentina<br />

Alex<strong>and</strong>ra Mironova Sevostrybvod Russia<br />

Birgit Kriete <strong>Orca</strong> Relief USA<br />

Stefan Jacobs Center for Whale Research USA<br />

Jodi Smith <strong>Orca</strong> Conservancy USA<br />

Nic Dedeluk Vancouver Aquarium Canada<br />

Marc Pakenham Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada<br />

Peter Ross Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada<br />

Margie Morrice Deakin University Australia<br />

WORKSHOP REPORT<br />

• The workshop participants recognized that we had broad geographic representation, with<br />

the exception of Norway<br />

• Conservation was assumed to reflect the maintenance of healthy abundant populations of<br />

killer whales now <strong>and</strong> in the future<br />

• Seven threats to the conservation of killer whales were identified. They included habitat<br />

loss, reductions in prey abundance, effects of climate change, lack of governance, whale<br />

watching, pollution <strong>and</strong> the possible effects of exotic species introduced into the marine<br />

environment.<br />

• <strong>Workshop</strong> participants were asked to rank the importance of these issues at local <strong>and</strong><br />

global scales (Appendix 1). Participants recognized that the 7 potential risks were not<br />

necessarily mutually exclusive.<br />

• Weighting the collective responses of the participants (Appendix 1) indicated the highest<br />

local conservation concerns were:<br />

o Changes in prey,<br />

o Pollution,<br />

o Whale watching, <strong>and</strong><br />

o Habitat loss.<br />

• Globally, the participants felt the highest concerns stemmed from:<br />

o Pollution,<br />

o Changes in prey,<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

11


o Lack of governance, <strong>and</strong><br />

o Climate change.<br />

• It was recognized that there are major overlaps <strong>and</strong> connections between these threats.<br />

• Given that killer whales are fairly adaptable, one means of identifying key threats is to<br />

consider what might affect the carrying capacity of killer whales (i.e., what controls their<br />

birth <strong>and</strong> death rates). As such, key threats are likely related to factors that affect:<br />

o Quality <strong>and</strong> quantity of food, <strong>and</strong><br />

o Direct mortality<br />

• We proceeded by defining the 7 key conservation concerns for killer whales <strong>and</strong> identified<br />

the key issues <strong>and</strong> information needs for each (i.e., what do we know <strong>and</strong> what do we not<br />

know in terms of what is needed for conservation, where are the gaps <strong>and</strong> what are the<br />

solutions <strong>and</strong> priorities).<br />

7 Threats to the Conservation of Killer<br />

Whales<br />

1. Prey Abundance <strong>and</strong> Fishing<br />

Killer whales need to be able to access a relative abundant supply of high quality prey<br />

unfettered by human activities in the ocean.<br />

Highest research priorities are to determine <strong>and</strong> monitor:<br />

• What do they eat?<br />

• How much?<br />

• What is the abundance of prey?<br />

Medium priorities are to determine:<br />

• Where do they eat?<br />

• Quality of prey <strong>and</strong> how does it vary by season?<br />

• What are the competitive interactions?<br />

Other Questions:<br />

• How do they eat/capture prey?<br />

• How dependant are killer whales on by-catch <strong>and</strong> fisheries caught prey?<br />

• What are the implications of killer whales predation on the food web? – develop<br />

ecosystem models<br />

• What is the status of killer whales prey?<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

12


• Are killer whales opportunistic or specialist feeders?<br />

2. POLLUTION<br />

Anthropogenic pollution knows no national borders, <strong>and</strong> many contaminants can affect the<br />

health <strong>and</strong> abundance of killer whales <strong>and</strong> their prey.<br />

Research priorities:<br />

• What are the short <strong>and</strong> long term affect of pollution on killer whale health?<br />

• Characterize <strong>and</strong> quantify the pollutant, <strong>and</strong> determine <strong>and</strong> predict the direct <strong>and</strong><br />

indirect effects on killer whales.<br />

o PBTs – old <strong>and</strong> new (high)<br />

o CLUPs (high) Coastal Localized Urban Pollutants<br />

o noise (high)<br />

o marine debris (high)<br />

o oil (medium)<br />

o air (medium)<br />

o eutrophication (medium)<br />

3. Whale watching<br />

Whale watching activities can introduce noise, pollutants <strong>and</strong> physical intrusions that impede<br />

the natural life processes of killer whales (feeding, resting, socializing, breeding <strong>and</strong> thriving).<br />

Research priorities:<br />

• Quantify <strong>and</strong> qualify the extent of commercial <strong>and</strong> recreational whale watching (numbers,<br />

time, distance <strong>and</strong> area).<br />

• Determine what qualifies as whale watching.<br />

• What is the economic, social <strong>and</strong> intrinsic value, <strong>and</strong> hence the conservation value of<br />

killer whales?<br />

• What effects does whale watching have on killer whales (at the individual, matrilineal<br />

line, clan level) in the short term <strong>and</strong> the long term? How do the following factors affect<br />

killer whales:<br />

o noise (high research priority)<br />

o pollution (high)<br />

o behaviour changes (high)<br />

o prey distribution (medium)<br />

o collision (medium)<br />

o physiological changes (medium)<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

13


4. Habitat loss<br />

Habitat encompasses inshore <strong>and</strong> offshore waters, including terrestrial watersheds needed<br />

by killer whales <strong>and</strong> their prey for normal life processes <strong>and</strong> can be reduced by human activities<br />

such as urbanization, fishing, pollution, climate change, etc.<br />

High Research Priorities:<br />

• Identify killer whales habitat <strong>and</strong> how it changes seasonally.<br />

• Underst<strong>and</strong> how human activity affects this killer whales habitat through:<br />

o Noise (high research priority)<br />

o prey reduction (eg. fishing, deforestation) (high)<br />

o pollution (toxics, turbidity, debris) (medium)<br />

o presence (medium)<br />

Medium Priorities:<br />

• Typify killer whales habitat according to use (eg. feeding, resting, socializing,<br />

traveling, breeding etc.).<br />

5. Introduction of Exotic Species<br />

Global transport of humans, their goods <strong>and</strong> animals leads to the introduction of exotic marine<br />

plants, organisms <strong>and</strong> pathogens into killer whale habitat, that might affect the health of killer whale<br />

populations <strong>and</strong> their prey.<br />

Research priorities:<br />

• What affects might exotic species have on killer whales?<br />

o disease (high research priority)<br />

o Competition (medium)<br />

o Prey (medium)<br />

o Aquaculture/hatcheries (medium)<br />

• What exotic species have been introduced? (medium)<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

14


6. Lack of Governance<br />

The wise application of knowledge at the individual, local, national <strong>and</strong> international levels to<br />

minimize human impacts on killer whales <strong>and</strong> their habitat.<br />

High research priorities:<br />

• Compile population specific Tables outlining what is known <strong>and</strong> not known about<br />

killer whale populations throughout the world.<br />

• Assess the status of killer whale populations throughout the world using<br />

internationally accepted st<strong>and</strong>ards, <strong>and</strong> identify information required for populations<br />

that are deemed data deficient.<br />

• Review <strong>and</strong> implement adequate planning <strong>and</strong> management mechanisms that are in<br />

keeping with the precautionary principle to manage human activities that affect killer<br />

whales.<br />

Medium priorities:<br />

• Develop an international set of stewardship protocols <strong>and</strong> ethics for the treatment of<br />

killer whales (i.e., concerning issues related to entrapment, emergency responses,<br />

research, captures <strong>and</strong> aquarium displays).<br />

• Develop <strong>and</strong> implement a means for the rapid exchange of information/knowledge<br />

between authorities/community that will lead to greater cooperation <strong>and</strong> coordination<br />

at all levels (e,g., researchers, public, governments, managers, fishers, commercial<br />

operators).<br />

7. Climate Change<br />

Climate change resulting from greenhouse gases will alter ocean productivity <strong>and</strong> may lead to<br />

potentially catastrophic changes in the abundance of preferred killer whale prey species.<br />

Research priorities:<br />

• Link climate change research to killer whales.<br />

• Develop ecosystem models to evaluate the effect of changes in prey numbers to killer<br />

whales.<br />

GENERAL COMMENTS<br />

• It is important to recognize that there are linkages between the 7 concerns outlined above,<br />

<strong>and</strong> that they are not mutually exclusive (e.g., whale watching occurs in urban areas where<br />

there are stresses from pollution, etc.).<br />

• Discussion ensued over how to better engage the public in the conservation needs of killer<br />

whales. From this stemmed the idea that we should establish <strong>and</strong> promote a Charter of<br />

Rights for Killer Whales.<br />

• Each workshop participant was asked to consider what should such a Charter of Rights<br />

include. Participants felt that killer whales should be able to:<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

15


o live in a clean, quiet <strong>and</strong> healthy environment<br />

o use their habitat freely <strong>and</strong> unharmed<br />

o have the freedom to travel <strong>and</strong> feed where they choose<br />

o consume prey that are abundant <strong>and</strong> free of toxins<br />

o rest without harassment<br />

o socialize <strong>and</strong> communicate freely<br />

o be free <strong>and</strong> protected by local, national <strong>and</strong> international laws<br />

o not be captured for human entertainment <strong>and</strong> unwillingly separated from family<br />

members<br />

• No consensus was sought on a final text for the Charter of Rights. Instead the<br />

Conservation <strong>Workshop</strong> participants decided to present the concept to the other workshop<br />

groups with the recommendation that it be further developed in time for the next Killer<br />

Whale <strong>Symposium</strong> for possible signing by researchers or governments.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

16


Appendix 1<br />

Top 3 threats to killer whales identified by workshop participants at local <strong>and</strong> global<br />

levels.<br />

Russia:<br />

Local Global<br />

1. Pollution, habitat loss,<br />

2. fisheries interactions (prey),<br />

3. captures (direct impact on killer whale<br />

population)<br />

British Columbia/Washington State:<br />

1. Whale watching,<br />

2. over fishing,<br />

3. pollution<br />

1. Habitat loss,<br />

2. whale watching,<br />

3. pollution.<br />

1. Pollution,<br />

2. habitat loss,<br />

3. whale watching<br />

1. Disturbance/harassment,<br />

2. prey (habitat loss/fisheries),<br />

3. pollution.<br />

1. Climate change (prey)<br />

2. PBT (prey),<br />

3. fishery interactions (over fishing).<br />

1. Fishing (prey availability),<br />

2. pollution,<br />

3. whale watching.<br />

1. Prey (natural <strong>and</strong> fishery),<br />

2. whale watching,<br />

3. toxics.<br />

Australia:<br />

1. Governance,<br />

2. habitat loss (prey),<br />

3. fishery interactions (shooting <strong>and</strong><br />

1. Climate change (prey <strong>and</strong> environment),<br />

2. pollution,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Pollution,<br />

2. fisheries (over fishing),<br />

3. whale watching.<br />

1. Pollution,<br />

2. climate change,<br />

3. habitat loss.<br />

1. Fishery interactions (prey),<br />

2. habitat loss ( urbanization),<br />

3. governance (lack of knowledge)<br />

1. Prey,<br />

2. pollution,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Fishing,<br />

2. pollution,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Prey,<br />

2. toxics,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Governance,<br />

2. climate change,<br />

3. habitat loss <strong>and</strong> pollution.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

17


ehavior change <strong>and</strong> prey).<br />

Mediterranean Sea:<br />

1. Pollution,<br />

2. fishery interactions,<br />

3. whale watching/ boat traffic<br />

1. Pollution (debris),<br />

2. disturbance/ harassment (boats),<br />

3. fishery interactions (prey <strong>and</strong> shooting).<br />

Prince Edward Isl<strong>and</strong>s:<br />

1. Fishery interaction (operational,<br />

shooting, entanglement),<br />

2. biological interactions (removal of prey),<br />

3. pollution.<br />

South Western Atlantic:<br />

1. Governance,<br />

2. fishery interactions (Antarctic cod,<br />

swordfish),<br />

3. habitat loss (cultural-core use areas)<br />

Alaska Prince William Sound:<br />

1. Prey,<br />

2. whale watching,<br />

3. toxics<br />

1. Pollution,<br />

2. fishery interactions,<br />

3. whale watching/ boat traffic<br />

1. Governance,<br />

2. prey,<br />

3. pollution<br />

1. Fisheries,<br />

2. pollution,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Habitat loss,<br />

2. climate change,<br />

3. governance.<br />

1. Prey,<br />

2. whale watching,<br />

3. toxics<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

18


KILLER WHALE WORKSHOP - ACOUSTIC<br />

Participants:<br />

John Ford fordjo@pac.dfo-mpo.gc.ca (Facilitator <strong>and</strong> Rapporteur)<br />

Harald Yurk yurk@zoology.ubc.ca (Assistant Rapporteur)<br />

Ari Shapiro smiling_whale@earthlink.net (Assistant Rapporteur)<br />

Volker Deecke deecke@zoology.ubc.ca<br />

Renaud de Stephanis renaud@teleline.es<br />

Olga Filatova alazor@rambler.ru<br />

Andrew Foote <strong>and</strong>rew.foote@hotmail.com<br />

Antoine Godefroid spiroutonio@hotmail.com<br />

Sarah Graham mickeylegs@yahoo.com<br />

Teo Leyssen teoleyssen@belgacom.net<br />

Nicola Rehn nicola.rehn@arcor.de<br />

Eva Saulitis saulitis@pobox.xyz.net<br />

Silvia Scali silvia_scali@yahoo.it<br />

Malene Juul Simon mjsimon@zi.ku.dk<br />

Frank Thomsen thomsen@zoologie.uni-hamburg.de<br />

Brigitte Weiss a9400355@unet.univie.ac.at<br />

_____________________________________________________________________<br />

The objective of the Acoustic <strong>Workshop</strong> was to focus discussion on a number of topics<br />

related to the recording, analysis, <strong>and</strong> interpretation of underwater acoustic signals of killer<br />

whales. These included discussion of recent advances in the technologies involved in field<br />

recording <strong>and</strong> laboratory analysis of killer whale sounds, the use of acoustics as a tool in field<br />

studies, the description <strong>and</strong> definition of acoustic signals, <strong>and</strong> the state of knowledge<br />

regarding the function of these signals. Discussions also focused on important gaps in our<br />

knowledge of killer whale acoustic behaviour, <strong>and</strong> how these might be filled. The following<br />

represents a synopsis of these discussions.<br />

Recording <strong>and</strong> Analysis:<br />

Advancing technology has provided many exciting new opportunities for the recording<br />

<strong>and</strong> analysis of killer whale acoustic signals. A number of hardware <strong>and</strong> software options are<br />

now available for all stages of acoustic data acquisition <strong>and</strong> analysis.<br />

Most field studies of killer whale acoustics involve recording signals from a small boat<br />

using a portable hydrophone <strong>and</strong> recorder system. To record social communication signals, a<br />

single omni-directional hydrophone with built-in preamplifier having a receiving sensitivity<br />

of at least -150 to -170 dB re 1 µPa over a frequency b<strong>and</strong> of 100 Hz to 20 kHz is quite<br />

adequate. Recordings where bearing <strong>and</strong> distance to sound sources require more complex,<br />

multi-element hydrophone arrays. Recordings of killer whales suitable for description of the<br />

basic structure of call types up to 15 kHz can be recorded using a professional-calibre audio<br />

cassette recorder. Few manufacturers currently make these types of recorders, although<br />

Marantz produces several good models in both mono <strong>and</strong> stereo formats. Reel-to-reel<br />

recorders are becoming scarce, but have the capability of making excellent recordings with<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

19


wide frequency response (up to 40 kHz or so) <strong>and</strong> good dynamic range. The Nagra IV is an<br />

example of a quality, field-capable reel-to-reel recorder.<br />

Most field researchers recording sounds of killer whales use Digital Audio Tape (DAT)<br />

recorders, which provide sampling rates up to 48 kHz, or an audio b<strong>and</strong>width to 24 kHz. The<br />

group recommended Sony models (e.g. D7, D8, D10 or D100) of DAT recorders. Tascam<br />

also produces good DAT recorders, although they have no time/date indexing, which could be<br />

a disadvantage in behavioural acoustic studies. Digital video recorders can record highquality<br />

16-bit audio tracks comparable to DAT recordings, <strong>and</strong> have the advantage of<br />

documenting concurrent surface behaviour of animals. However, most have Automatic Gain<br />

Controls (AGCs) rather than manual record level controls, which are not well suited to the<br />

loud, punctuated nature of killer whale clicks <strong>and</strong> calls. Mini-disc recorders are not<br />

recommended for scientific recordings, since they involve a lossy compression algorithm,<br />

which affects the accuracy of spectral analyses. Recording directly to hard disks on portable<br />

computers can yield high quality data, though many laptops are not robust enough for use in<br />

small boats in the marine environment. Larger ‘lunch-box’ style portable computers allow for<br />

the use of high-speed signal processing cards, which can yield extremely wideb<strong>and</strong> recordings<br />

of echolocation clicks when used with appropriate broadb<strong>and</strong> hydrophones.<br />

For data archiving, it should be noted that analogue tape media (reel-to-reel <strong>and</strong> audio<br />

cassette) are unlikely to survive more than 20-30 years. Many reel-to-reel tapes from the<br />

1980s <strong>and</strong> earlier cannot be read today due to sloughing of the magnetic coating on the tapes.<br />

Recordable CD <strong>and</strong> DVDs (CD-R <strong>and</strong> DVD-R) are expected to have a longevity of 50-75<br />

years, so digital transfer to this medium may be useful for archiving data. Other options for<br />

data archiving include contributing to institution-based acoustic archiving programs,<br />

including those at the British Museum of Natural History <strong>and</strong> Cornell University.<br />

In recent years, a variety of acoustic analysis software packages have become available<br />

that are very useful for killer whale studies. CoolEdit 2000 is a popular <strong>and</strong> inexpensive<br />

program that offers both spectrographic or waveform displays in real-time <strong>and</strong> a variety of<br />

other functions. One limitation is its inability to print spectrograms. Another popular tool,<br />

though far more expensive, is Avisoft-SASLab Pro, a program that is designed for<br />

bioacoustical analysis. It produces excellent spectrogram hardcopies. Canary, a bioacoustical<br />

software package from the Bioacoustical Research Program at Cornell University, is also<br />

widely used. It lacks real-time browsing capability <strong>and</strong> runs only on Macintosh computers.<br />

Other programs used in killer whale acoustic studies are Signal RTS, Spectralab, SeaWave<br />

(University of Pavia, Italy), <strong>and</strong> various custom programs that run with Matlab (Mathworks<br />

Inc.).<br />

Describing <strong>and</strong> defining signal structure<br />

Killer whales produce a wide variety of complex acoustic signals which can be difficult<br />

to describe <strong>and</strong> define in terms of physical structure <strong>and</strong> potential function. Clicks are<br />

thought to be emitted primarily for echolocation purposes, though their potential role in social<br />

communication requires further study. Pulses – similar in structure to echolocation clicks –<br />

are often produced at high repetition rates (typically 500 Hz to ≥ 2500 Hz) which give the<br />

aural impression of a continuous tone. Spectrographically, these signals are resolved as a<br />

series of sideb<strong>and</strong>s at intervals equivalent to the pulse repetition frequency. Most killer whale<br />

calls are composed of such rapidly produced pulses. Energy is not necessarily greatest in the<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

20


lowest b<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> burst-pulsing can result in complex sideb<strong>and</strong> patterns. Some killer whale call<br />

types show two concurrent but different pulsing frequencies. Whistles are continuous<br />

waveforms that are heard mostly in socializing contexts. Pure-tone whistles are represented<br />

spectrographically as a single b<strong>and</strong>, often with significant frequency modulation. Many<br />

whistles have one or two well defined harmonics at multiples of the fundamental tone<br />

frequency.<br />

Pulsed calls that are heard repetitively with various degrees of stereotypy are referred to<br />

as discrete call types. Discrete calls seem to be characteristic of killer whales in all global<br />

regions where they have been recorded. Often different discrete call types are easily<br />

distinguished by ear because of distinctive physical features (rapid modulations or abrupt<br />

shifts in pulse rates, overlapping pulse rates, etc.) <strong>and</strong> high stereotypy. In many cases,<br />

however, there are considerable variations in call structure, <strong>and</strong> clear definitions of call types<br />

are confounded by gradations between otherwise distinctive forms. There are several<br />

approaches to objectively <strong>and</strong> consistently defining call types. Repeated aural examination<br />

(listening carefully <strong>and</strong> often) combined with visual inspection of spectrograms can reliably<br />

identify most call types. Call types should also be defined quantitatively using measurements<br />

of duration <strong>and</strong> sideb<strong>and</strong> interval for each distinctive element (= components, segments or<br />

parts of calls defined by abrupt shifts in pulse rate). These measurements can be used in<br />

univariate or multivariate statistical analyses to distinguish similar call types. Other<br />

approaches to defining call types include the use of neural networks <strong>and</strong> other automated<br />

pattern recognition techniques. These are especially useful to distinguish fine-scale<br />

differences in shared calls, or in objectively defining categories within highly variable<br />

repertoires. It should be recognized that human-based call type classifications, regardless of<br />

how quantitatively valid they may be, are not necessarily the same as those perceived by the<br />

killer whales themselves.<br />

Function of killer whale social signals<br />

Field studies in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska have shown that pods (groups of closely<br />

related matrilines) of resident-type (fish feeding) killer whales have stable repertoires of 7-17<br />

discrete call types. These repertoires appear to be shared by all pods members, <strong>and</strong> are likely<br />

learned by individuals through mimicking older kin within the group, especially their<br />

mothers. Repertoires are thus perpetuated in maternal lineages by a process of cultural<br />

transmission across generations. Consistent differences, often referred to as dialects, exist in<br />

the call repertoires of pods within an area. Pods that share calls (i.e., have related dialects)<br />

belong to the same acoustic clan. Different clans appear to represent distinct, independent<br />

maternal lineages that have persisted for many generations, <strong>and</strong> have independent call<br />

traditions.<br />

Call repertoires, <strong>and</strong> the group-specific dialectal variations that exist within them, likely<br />

serve as a means of maintaining kin-group identity <strong>and</strong> cohesion, <strong>and</strong> coordinating group<br />

behaviours <strong>and</strong> activities. Recent genetic evidence indicates that dialects also influence<br />

selection of mating partners, thus serving as an outbreeding mechanism within killer whale<br />

communities. Although the frequency of use of call types varies with activity context in<br />

killer whale pods, no call has been shown to be tied exclusively to any particular behavioural<br />

context or circumstance.<br />

Dialects within clans appear to develop over time through the accumulation of<br />

consistent group-specific variations in call structure coincidental with a process of fission<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

21


within the lineage. The actual process of dialect change is poorly known. Recent studies<br />

have described fine changes in the structure of call types shared by two matrilines of northern<br />

residents in British Columbia. Each matriline had parallel changes, as if one matriline had<br />

copied changes from the other. This apparent synchrony of calling behaviour over time has<br />

implications of defining a “cultural clock”, or the rate of dialect change within clans. Dialect<br />

change may also occur in a punctuated manner, corresponding to sudden social or<br />

demographic changes within a lineage.<br />

Whistles in resident killer whales are strongly associated with socializing contexts, as<br />

are variable pulsed calls. These sounds appear to be used as close-range communication<br />

signals, often occurring together with physical interactions <strong>and</strong> visual displays. Whistles may<br />

also occur repetitively with levels of stereotypy approaching those of discrete calls.<br />

Mammal-hunting transient killer whales from Southeastern Alaska, British Columbia<br />

<strong>and</strong> California share a related call repertoire. The lack of kin-group-specific dialects in<br />

transients is no doubt related to their social system, which is more fluid <strong>and</strong> dynamic than that<br />

of resident killer whales. Nonetheless, there appear to be some regional variations in call<br />

repertoires within the west coast transient community.<br />

Studies of killer whales in regions other than the Northeastern Pacific have revealed<br />

similar patterns of acoustic behaviour, with signals being dominated by repetitive, stereotyped<br />

pulsed calls. Regions where discrete call types have been identified include Norway, Icel<strong>and</strong>,<br />

the Kamchatkan area of eastern Russia, Argentina, <strong>and</strong> Antarctica.<br />

Structure <strong>and</strong> Function of Echolocation Signals<br />

Echolocation has been relatively little studied in killer whales compared to the social<br />

signals of the species. Early captive studies demonstrated that, as with most odontocetes,<br />

killer whales are able to orient in their environment <strong>and</strong> detect objects through the use of<br />

series (or ‘trains’) of echolocation clicks. Field studies in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska have<br />

described major differences in the echolocation behaviour of resident <strong>and</strong> transient killer<br />

whales, apparently associated with the different hunting specializations of theses two<br />

ecotypes. Fish-feeding residents frequently emit echolocation click trains while foraging, but<br />

transients do not. Instead, transients forage either in silence, or sporadically produce isolated<br />

clicks. Suppressed echolocation activity in transients appears to be a tactic for hunting<br />

mammalian prey, which have excellent hearing abilities <strong>and</strong> would be alerted to the predators’<br />

approach if they detected echolocations signals.<br />

Recent field studies have focused on describing echolocation signal structure in killer<br />

whales through the use of wide-b<strong>and</strong> recording systems <strong>and</strong> arrays of hydrophones. Such<br />

studies in Norway <strong>and</strong> British Columbia have found that killer whale clicks are similar to<br />

those of other delphinids, though they tend to have lower frequency emphases than do the<br />

clicks of smaller dolphins. Further research is needed to describe the function of killer whale<br />

echolocation in better detail.<br />

Acoustics as a Tool<br />

Underwater acoustics not only provides a means of interpreting killer whale behaviour<br />

<strong>and</strong> social structure, it can also be used as a tool for other purposes. Fixed shore-based<br />

hydrophone installations at strategic coastal sites can provide a means of remotely monitoring<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

22


the presence of killer whales throughout the year, day <strong>and</strong> night in all weather conditions.<br />

Discrete or stereotyped calls can provide information on the identity of vocalizing killer<br />

whales groups. In British Columbia, remote monitoring installations at lighthouses have been<br />

used successfully for many years. These systems are monitored by lightkeepers, who make<br />

recordings when whales are heard. More recently, automated recording systems have been<br />

developed, which utilize call-recognition circuitry to trigger digitization <strong>and</strong> recording of<br />

acoustic signals when whale vocalizations are received.<br />

Underwater acoustic monitoring from boats can be used as an aid to locating groups of<br />

killer whales during field studies. Whale calls can be detected at ranges up to 20 kilometres<br />

in quiet conditions, well beyond the range of visual detection. To help locate vocalizing<br />

animals, an omni-directional hydrophone can be fixed in the mouth of a neoprene-lined bowl,<br />

which provides directionality when suspended over the boat’s side <strong>and</strong> scanned laterally (gas<br />

bubbles in the neoprene provide acoustic reflectivity underwater). Towed hydrophone arrays<br />

deployed from ships undertaking visual surveys may also detect distant killer whales, <strong>and</strong> can<br />

provide a bearing to their location.<br />

Playback of killer whale calls has been employed in field studies of the species <strong>and</strong> its<br />

prey. While potentially yielding insight into killer whale behaviour <strong>and</strong> ecology, playbacks<br />

have the potential to be very disruptive to the animals <strong>and</strong> may elicit violent responses.<br />

Experimental design of playback studies need to be well developed, <strong>and</strong> research questions<br />

must be clearly defined, in order for the study to have interpretable results. Close attention<br />

must be paid to the acoustic fidelity of the recordings being used <strong>and</strong> the playback apparatus.<br />

Playbacks could potentially be used to attract animals from a distance, particularly transient<br />

killer whales, which could aid in locating groups for study. They also have potential value as<br />

a management tool, to attract or repel whales from dangerous locations, such as during an oil<br />

spill.<br />

The Impacts of Noise on Acoustic Behaviour<br />

Increasing levels of anthropogenic noise, such as that associated with vessel traffic,<br />

seismic exploration, <strong>and</strong> military operations, are of growing concern. In some regions, killer<br />

whales frequent waters with intense levels of marine traffic, including fish boats, whale-watch<br />

vessels, <strong>and</strong> commercial shipping. Recent studies have suggested that typical levels of<br />

ambient noise in waters off southern Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia, are sufficient to<br />

cause temporary reduction in hearing acuity in resident killer whales, <strong>and</strong> permanent<br />

sensitivity shifts in extreme cases. Reduced hearing sensitivity could have major impacts on<br />

the ability of killer whales to function acoustically. Noise also has the potential to mask the<br />

calls of distant conspecifics, or the faint echoes from objects being investigated acoustically.<br />

Further research is needed to better underst<strong>and</strong> the hearing abilities of killer whales,<br />

particularly with respect to critical b<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> critical ratios. The effect of noise on the<br />

echolocation or social signal production also requires study, to determine if the animals<br />

compensate for ambient noise levels by changing vocal activity or source levels of sound<br />

production.<br />

Research Questions for Future Consideration<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

23


The following summarizes a variety of research questions <strong>and</strong> knowledge gaps that<br />

were identified <strong>and</strong> discussed by the Acoustics <strong>Workshop</strong> participants. Many of these relate<br />

to the general topics described above, while others do not.<br />

o How important is passive listening in killer whales for acoustic orientation <strong>and</strong><br />

discrimination?<br />

o What are the social functions of echolocation click production? Are clicks used to<br />

determine location of other matriline or pod members?<br />

o Do killer whales alter the frequency structure of echolocation clicks according to the<br />

nature of the target being searched for or examined acoustically? Does echolocation click rate<br />

vary with target range?<br />

o What is the origin of acoustic clans? Do the major acoustical differences seen among<br />

clans represent founder effects? Are clans typical of killer whale populations in regions other<br />

than the Northeast Pacific residents?<br />

o To what extent is the identity of vocalizing individuals encoded in discrete call types<br />

shared by kin members?<br />

o Are dialectal differences within clans the result of r<strong>and</strong>om drift, or are vocal changes<br />

directed?<br />

o Are call type classifications described in acoustic studies perceived in the same way<br />

by killer whales? Could the ability of killer whales to discriminate call types <strong>and</strong> variations<br />

be tested in a captive setting?<br />

o Are there anatomical or physiological influences or constraints on sound production in<br />

killer whales? Could structural features in signal structure encode sex, age, or populationspecific<br />

information?<br />

o Why are their such great differences in the sizes of call repertoires among resident<br />

killer whales?<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

24


KILLER WHALE WORKSHOP - POPULATION DYNAMICS, POPULATION<br />

STRUCTURE, AND LIFE-HISTORY<br />

Participants:<br />

Lance Barrett-Lennard <strong>and</strong> Rob Williams<br />

L. Barrett-Lennard, M. Chambellant, R. DeLeuwl, K. Delord, C. Gasparrou, A. van Ginneken, K. Heise,<br />

K. Irish, P. Mäkeläinen, C. Matkin, P. Olesiuk, E. Poncelet, A. Schaffar, M. Ugarte, R. Williams<br />

Introduction<br />

Systematic study of killer whales began in the early 1970’s, when Michael Bigg <strong>and</strong><br />

colleagues investigated the distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of the species in southern British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State (Bigg & Wolman 1975; Bigg 1982). Their research<br />

provided the impetus <strong>and</strong> foundation for a series of killer whale studies in the northeast<br />

Pacific that carry on to the present day (eg Bigg et. al. 1990, Olesiuk et al. 1990, Ford et al.<br />

1998, Matkin et al. 1999). Reliable photo-identification of individuals was <strong>and</strong> is at the core<br />

of these studies. However, additional methods relying on the analysis of behaviour, acoustic<br />

signals, movement patterns, <strong>and</strong>/or DNA are now routinely employed. The long-term studies<br />

in the northeastern Pacific have generated a wealth of information on killer whale population<br />

dynamics, population structure, <strong>and</strong> life history. Studies of killer whales in other areas are at<br />

an earlier stage, but are nonetheless producing many valuable insights as well (eg Guinet<br />

1992, Similä et al. 1996, Visser & Mäkeläinen 2000).<br />

At the beginning of this workshop, the participants agreed to focus on methods of killer<br />

whale field research <strong>and</strong> data analysis rather than on research results, since recent findings<br />

were covered thoroughly during the symposium that preceded this workshop. It was<br />

recognized that the research of Bigg <strong>and</strong> his British Columbian colleagues is being used as a<br />

general model for most other killer whale field studies. Therefore, much of the discussion<br />

revolved around the specifics of the British Columbian methods, their advantages <strong>and</strong><br />

disadvantages, contexts under which they do <strong>and</strong> do not work well, <strong>and</strong> how they can <strong>and</strong><br />

should be improved on in new studies. Also discussed were methods such as formal markrecapture<br />

analysis that are commonly used in other wildlife studies but that have not been<br />

widely used in killer whale studies. Finally, the participants had a brief discussion about the<br />

utility <strong>and</strong> validity of comparing population <strong>and</strong> life history parameter estimates between<br />

populations of killer whales.<br />

Methods of Field Research on Killer Whale Populations<br />

Photo-identification<br />

Killer whales are large animals in small populations, <strong>and</strong> as such do not lend themselves<br />

to many of the classical methods of population analysis used for other species. Fortunately,<br />

tracking the histories <strong>and</strong> fates of individuals directly using the technique of photoidentification<br />

is well-established <strong>and</strong> practical. Participants familiar with killer whale photoidentification<br />

offered the following recommendations:<br />

− good quality photographs are essential (see Bigg et al. 1986)<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

25


− film produces the highest-resolution images <strong>and</strong> is recommended over digital<br />

photography (although with technical improvements digital will likely take over as the<br />

medium of choice in the future)<br />

− images from video stills are almost worthless for killer whale photo-identification<br />

− when the film is analysed, it is recommended that every identifiable individual in<br />

every image be entered in a long-term database<br />

In British Columbia, identification photographs have been extremely useful for analyses<br />

of association patterns. In these analyses the frequency with which any two individuals are<br />

either identified together in the same image or appear in consecutive images is used to<br />

calculate an objective index of the strength of their social affiliation. A general discussion of<br />

the application of photo-identification to association analysis ensued. van Ginneken<br />

questioned the utility of association indices based on the number of photographic events,<br />

rather than the total time spent together. Olesiuk agreed, <strong>and</strong> noted that if the study were<br />

starting again, he would want to see proximity <strong>and</strong> time spent together measured, rather than<br />

simple presence in the same encounter/ consecutive image. He said that the method worked<br />

well in the BC study largely because of the enormous database: more than 50,000 frames—it<br />

would not work as well in smaller studies. Barrett-Lennard pointed out that in the first 15<br />

years of research in BC, researchers attempted to photograph groups of killer whales in single<br />

images, but in recent years there has been more emphasis on full-frame photographs as<br />

individuals--making association analysis by this method more difficult. According to the<br />

methods used to date, this will indicate a weakening of the strength of association for many<br />

pairs. Olesiuk agreed <strong>and</strong> noted that field researchers would do well to identify photographic<br />

sampling criteria <strong>and</strong> stick to them. Several researchers noted that 1) regardless of the criteria<br />

used regarding the number of whales per image, it is best to work systematically through<br />

each group of killer whales in every encounter <strong>and</strong> 2) although the models established in the<br />

early British Columbia studies are general helpful, researchers should establish photographic<br />

sampling protocols based on the objectives of their own studies.<br />

In addition to association analysis, identification photographs are used to estimate<br />

abundance. A discovery curve is a useful way of generating a rough abundance estimate. In<br />

this approach, one simply plots the total number of identified whales that have been<br />

catalogued in each year of an study. When the total number of identified whales levels off<br />

despite ongoing photo-identification work, it is assumed that most of the individuals in the<br />

population have been identified. A second approach is sight / re-sight analysis. Here, a<br />

researcher photographs as many whales as possible in the study area over a defined sighting<br />

period. At a later point, he or she repeats the exercise, <strong>and</strong> then analyses the photographs to<br />

determine how many of the individuals were sighted twice. In the method’s simplest form,<br />

the ratio of the number sighted twice to the total number sighted is assumed to be equal to the<br />

ratio of the total number sighted to the total population. Many sophisticated variants of this<br />

basic relationship are described in the literature. Care must be taken when using photoidentification<br />

data in sight / re-sight analysis because abundance estimates are strongly<br />

influenced by photo-quality <strong>and</strong> animal distinctiveness. A powerful way of eliminating the<br />

bias is to split the process into three stages: one observer or group of observers scores photoquality,<br />

another scores animal distinctiveness, <strong>and</strong> the most experienced observer scores<br />

matches between samples.<br />

Acoustic Analysis<br />

John Ford’s study of killer whale calls in late 1980’s <strong>and</strong> early 1990’s showed that<br />

British Columbian killer whales use group-specific call repertoires. Furthermore, he showed<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

26


that resident killer whales have both pod-specific <strong>and</strong> clan-specific dialects (Ford 1991).<br />

Transient killer whales have smaller repertoires than residents, but at least three populations<br />

of transients in the northeast Pacific can be distinguished acoustically. Although they are<br />

usually vocally active, resident killer whales occasionally travel silently. In contrast,<br />

transients are usually silent, vocalising most frequently after they make a kill. Little is known<br />

about the call repertoires of the offshore population of killer whale, but members of this group<br />

are usually vocal during their rare visits to the coast. Inter-population differences in call<br />

repertoires have proven to be so stable in British Columbia that they are now used to assign<br />

“new” groups of killer whale to existing populations.<br />

Hydrophones stationed in remote areas of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska have been used<br />

to monitor killer whale movements year-round for many years. While silence on the<br />

hydrophone does not necessarily indicate that killer whales are absent, calls provide proof that<br />

they are present. Indeed, calls not only indicate the presence of killer whales, but also reliably<br />

discriminate populations <strong>and</strong>, in some cases, specific groups of individuals. At the present<br />

time, the most reliable remote hydrophones are ones that broadcast their sounds continuously<br />

on a radio or microwave frequency so that they can be monitored by local residents.<br />

However, promising developments have been made with hydrophone systems that monitor<br />

continuously but broadcast <strong>and</strong>/or record only when killer whale called are detected. The<br />

workshop participants agreed that these methods could be very useful. A project is currently<br />

underway to investigate the practicality of using acoustic monitoring to assess transient killer whale<br />

predation on Steller sea lions in western Alaska.<br />

In general, the workshop participants agreed that acoustic analysis is a useful way to<br />

identify population structure, <strong>and</strong> recommended that it be incorporated into killer whale field<br />

studies wherever possible.<br />

Genetic Analysis<br />

Genetic analysis can shed light on both social organisation <strong>and</strong> population structure in<br />

killer whales <strong>and</strong> other species. For example, Stevens (1989) <strong>and</strong> Hoelzel & Dover (1991)<br />

showed fixed differences in mitochondrial DNA between sympatric <strong>and</strong> parapatric<br />

assemblages of killer whales in British Columbia, providing the first convincing evidence that<br />

they are, in fact, closed populations. Later, mating systems of resident killer whales in British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska were elucidated using two genetic techniques, one based on paternity<br />

screening <strong>and</strong> another on allele frequency analysis (Barrett-Lennard 2000). The latter study<br />

provided evidence of effective inbreeding avoidance despite the lack of permanent dispersal<br />

of members of either sex from their natal pods. Both Hoelzel et al. (1998) <strong>and</strong> Barrett-<br />

Lennard (2000) documented low levels of genetic diversity in killer whales in the northeast<br />

Pacific.<br />

Genetic analyses can be performed using small samples of skin procured with biopsy<br />

darts (Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996). While this procedure does not appear to have negative<br />

long-term consequences for killer whales, two factors need to be considered in deciding<br />

whether to incorporate biopsy sampling into a field study. First, acquiring samples involves<br />

repeated close approaches to whales, constituting a form of harassment. Permits may be<br />

difficult to procure in some jurisdictions. Second, biopsy sampling take considerable time,<br />

<strong>and</strong> has the potential to interfere with other field research methods such as photoidentification<br />

<strong>and</strong> acoustic recording. If biopsy sampling is incorporated into a study, plans<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

27


should be made to archive samples for posterity <strong>and</strong> great care should be taken to photoidentify<br />

each individual sampled.<br />

The following points were raised in a general discussion. Olesiuk pointed out that the<br />

genetic contribution of males is not reflected in current population models. Since paternal<br />

genetic contribution is likely to influence maternal reproductive success, he suggested that<br />

genetic techniques could reduce the noise in existing models. The question of using genetic<br />

techniques to estimate effective (breeding) population size was raised by Ugarte. Barrett-<br />

Lennard pointed out that conventional methods to directly extrapolate effective population<br />

size from allele frequencies are extremely sensitive to violations of a very restrictive set of<br />

assumptions (such as r<strong>and</strong>om breeding, which is known not to prevail in resident killer<br />

whales). Newer methods may improve the precision somewhat, but it is unlikely that<br />

genetics alone will provide a magic bullet for estimating population size. A question was<br />

raised about the conservation implications of mating system analysis. Barrett-Lennard<br />

provided an example. The southern resident killer whale population of British Columbia<br />

contains only three pods. Genetic analysis indicates that mating occurs between, not within<br />

pods (Barrett-Lennard 2000). This means that the females of the largest pods have very few<br />

potential mates---a situation that may be exacerbated by the general failure of young adult<br />

males to father calves. The results suggest that many calves in the next generation may be<br />

sibs <strong>and</strong> half-sibs, resulting in a rapid loss of genetic diversity.<br />

The possibility of doing a sight – resight population estimates of killer whales using<br />

genotypes instead of photographs was raised. There was general consensus that given the<br />

difficulty, expense, <strong>and</strong> invasiveness of biopsy sampling, <strong>and</strong> the expense of analzying DNA,<br />

photographic methods are much preferred for the species. However, it was conceded that in<br />

certain instances with relatively rarely-seen populations it could possibly be used along with<br />

photographic methods.<br />

Naming <strong>and</strong> Grouping Killer Whales in Field Studies<br />

One of the first problems faced in a new killer whale field study is how to name <strong>and</strong><br />

group identified individuals. The workshop participants felt that establishing an efficient,<br />

useful, flexible system early in a project was extremely helpful. Since most studies use the<br />

basic system developed by Bigg <strong>and</strong> colleagues for resident killer whales, the system is<br />

described below. A second system developed by the same researchers is also described, <strong>and</strong><br />

is followed by a general discussion.<br />

Bigg et al. (1990) established a system whereby a unique alpha-numeric code was used<br />

to designate each individual. A single letter designated the whale’s pod (defined as the largest<br />

group in a community that travelled together at least 50% of the time). The letter was<br />

followed by a two-digit number designating the order in which individuals in the pod were<br />

first identified. Thus, A25 was the twenty-fifth individual identified in A pod. Several years<br />

after this naming system was established, some of Bigg’s initial pod designations were shown<br />

to be incorrect. For example A pod in fact comprised three pods. Each of the “revised” pods<br />

kept the A-designator, but was given a name based on the most distinguishable individual in<br />

the group. Thus, A25 was reassigned to A5 pod. Recently Ford & Ellis (these proceedings)<br />

showed that pods are less permanent than originally thought <strong>and</strong> proposed that matrilines, not<br />

pods, be considered the fundamental units of social organisation in resident killer whales. In<br />

sum, although the resident naming system has worked for book-keeping, it does not serve as a<br />

reliable template of resident killer whale social organisation.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

28


Bigg also used the alphanumeric system described above to identify transient killer<br />

whales. However in the early 1990s, it became clear that some transient killer whales<br />

disperse between groups, <strong>and</strong> the naming system once again failed to serve as a template for<br />

social structure. A revised naming system was devised that gave every transient animal with<br />

an unknown mother a name beginning with T (for transient) followed by an arbitrary number.<br />

When calves are born to these animals they receive their mother’s name followed by a letter<br />

indicating their birth order. Thus, T106B is the second calf of T106. These names work well<br />

at present but will become unwieldy when strings of several letters are added to a name. In<br />

addition, if a calf is born <strong>and</strong> dies before being named, the birth order implied by the<br />

alphanumeric code may be misleading.<br />

Under both the resident <strong>and</strong> transient systems, killer whales are assigned names based<br />

on their relationships. When new killer whales are first seen, they are not named until these<br />

relationships are understood. This is not a problem in British Columbia, where it is rare to<br />

encounter new killer whales. It is a problem in a new study in western Alaska led by Matkin<br />

<strong>and</strong> Barrett-Lennard where the rate of discovery is high <strong>and</strong> where very little is known about<br />

relationships. In that study, abitrary numeric names are now being assigned as whales are<br />

identified. These may be replaced with relationship-based names as the study proceeds, or<br />

they may be used to track individuals indefinitely. A similar system is being used for the<br />

offshore population of killer whales, where individuals are named “Off” followed by an<br />

abitrary number.<br />

In the discussion, Mäkeläinen pointed out that studies are becoming international, <strong>and</strong><br />

she advocated the use of an alphanumeric code that tells researchers which population is<br />

implied, with an F or M to designate fish-eating or mammal-eating. However, several<br />

participants argued that our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of killer whale ecology may change, <strong>and</strong> it would<br />

be problematic to create a global system where names must be changed as new information is<br />

acquired. Olesiuk argued that a name should contain a fact, such as This is animal X, but not<br />

contain interpretations of data, such as This is the calf of animal X, <strong>and</strong> s/he eats only fish.<br />

These latter methods do not allow for mistakes. van Ginneken stated that females that adopt<br />

orphans, <strong>and</strong> naming systems that interpret association with relatedness may mislead future<br />

researchers. After further discussion the group reached consensus that the naming structure<br />

used in British Columbia is not necessarily the best way to name whales in new studies. The<br />

group agreed that whales in new areas should be given a unique identifier that contains no<br />

interpretation of relatedness or site-fidelity. The consensus is that there should be a central<br />

database, with an annual meeting to share photographs, <strong>and</strong> a common naming system.<br />

Comparisons between populations <strong>and</strong> studies<br />

The final part of the workshop asked when it is useful to extrapolate demographic <strong>and</strong><br />

life history parameters obtained from well-studied to less-well studied populations of killer<br />

whales. Olesiuk pointed out, in the way of an example, that there are very clear links between<br />

age- <strong>and</strong> sex-structure of residents under periods of increase <strong>and</strong> that we can now calculate<br />

similar values for declining populations. Olesiuk suggested that these values could be used to<br />

examine the status of transient killer whales, which are inherently harder to study than<br />

residents. At the very least, the comparisons would suggest areas for future research.<br />

Barrett-Lennard discussed the fact that in British Columbia no dispersal from resident<br />

matrilines had been described, while dispersal among transient matrilines definitely occurs.<br />

He asked whether there was any evidence of either of these systems from other areas.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

29


Poncelet noted that dispersal occurs among pods of elephant seal-eating killer whales at the<br />

Crozet Isl<strong>and</strong>s. Barrett-Lennard noted that transient dispersal is female-biased, <strong>and</strong> tends to<br />

occur after female offspring have their first calf. This is similar to the Crozet Isl<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

suggests that the primary function of dispersal in mammal-eating killer whales is to reduce<br />

food competition. It was agreed that, with the exception of resident killer whales in the<br />

northeast Pacific, social structure in killer whales is still poorly understood <strong>and</strong> should be a<br />

research priority in future studies.<br />

Bibliography<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L.G. (2000). Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer whales (Orcinus orca) as<br />

revealed by DNA analysis. Zoology. Vancouver, British Columbia, University of British<br />

Columbia: 97.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L.G., T.G. Smith, G.M. Ellis (1996). "A cetacean biopsy system using lightweight<br />

pneumatic darts, <strong>and</strong> its effect on the behaviour of killer whales." Marine Mammal Science 12: 14-<br />

27.<br />

Bigg, M.A., A.A. Wolman (1975). "Live-capture killer whale (Orcinus orca) fishery, British Columbia <strong>and</strong><br />

Washington, 1962-73." Journal of the Fisheries Research Board of Canada 32: 1213-1221.<br />

Bigg, M.A. (1982). "An assessment of killer whale (Orcinus orca) stocks off Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>, British<br />

Columbia." Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission 32: 625-666.<br />

Bigg, M.A., G.M. Ellis, K.C. Balcomb (1986). "The photographic identification of individual cetaceans."<br />

Whalewatcher 20: 10-12.<br />

Bigg, M.A., P.F. Olesiuk, G. M. Ellis (1990). "Social organization <strong>and</strong> genealogy of resident killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) in the coastal waters of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State." Report of the<br />

<strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission: 383-405.<br />

Ford, J.K. . (1991). "Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of<br />

British Columbia." Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 1454-1483.<br />

Ford, J.K.B., G.M. Ellis, L.G. Barrett-Lennard, A. B. Morton, K. C. Balcomb (1998). "Dietary<br />

specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters." Canadian Journal of Zoology 76: 1456-1471.<br />

Guinet, C. (1992). "Comportement de chasse des orques (Orcinus orca) autour des iles Crozet." Canadian<br />

Journal of Zoology 70: 1656-1667.<br />

Hoelzel, A.R., G.A. Dover (1991). "Genetic differentiation between sympatric killer whale populations."<br />

Heredity 66: 191-195.<br />

Hoelzel, A.R., M. Dahlheim, S.J. Stern (1998). "Low genetic variation among killer whales (Orcinus orca)<br />

in the Eastern North Pacific, <strong>and</strong> genetic differentiation between foraging specialists." Journal of<br />

Heredity 89: 121-128.<br />

Matkin, C.O., G.M. Ellis, E.L. Saulitis, L.G. Barrett-Lennard, D.R. Matkin (1999). Killer Whales of<br />

Southern Alaska. Homer, Alaska, North Gulf Oceanic Society.<br />

Olesiuk, P.F., M.A. Bigg, G.M. Ellis (1990). "Life history <strong>and</strong> population dynamics of resident killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) in the coastal waters of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State." Report of the<br />

<strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission(Special Issue 12): 209-243.<br />

Similä, T., J.C. Holst, I. Christensen (1996). "Occurrence <strong>and</strong> diet of killer whales in northern Norway:<br />

seasonal patterns relative to the distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of Norwegian spring-spawning<br />

herring." Canadian Journal of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Aquatic Sciences 53: 769-779.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

30


Stevens, T.A., D.A. Duffield, E.D. Asper, et al. (1989). "Preliminary findings of restriction fragment<br />

differences in mitochondrial DNA among killer whales (Orcinus orca)." Canadian Journal of<br />

Zoology 67: 2592-2595.<br />

Visser, I.N., P. Mäkeläinen (2000). "variation in eye-patch shape of killer whales (orcinus orca) in new<br />

zeal<strong>and</strong> waters." Marine Mammal Science 16(2): 459-469.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

31


Oral And Poster Presentations<br />

SOCIAL DETERMINANTS OF POPULATION STRUCTURE IN KILLER WHALES:<br />

INSIGHTS FROM ASSOCIATION PATTERNS, GENETICS AND ACOUSTICS.<br />

Barrett-Lennard L.G<br />

Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre <strong>and</strong> University of British Columbia. P.O. Box 3232,<br />

Vancouver, B.C. V6B 3X8, Canada, Canada & University of British Columbia, Department of<br />

Zoology, 6270 University Blvd, Vancouver, B.C. V6T 1Z4, Canada, barrett@zoology.ubc.ca<br />

Seven distinct populations of killer whales have been identified off the coasts of British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> southern Alaska. Three are fish-eating residents, three are mammal-eating<br />

transients, <strong>and</strong> one, offshores, have an unknown diet. Each population has from 75 to several<br />

hundred members, with the exception of one that is on the brink of extinction. Each of the<br />

resident populations shares its usual home range with one or more transient population <strong>and</strong><br />

vice versa, but populations with the same feeding specializations overlap very little. The<br />

offshores partially overlap the range of both other types. This situation is illustrated<br />

schematically in Figure 1.<br />

Figure 1: Approximate range of resident, transient <strong>and</strong> offshore killer whales in the northeastern Pacific ocean.<br />

Despite their adjacent or overlapping distributions, members of different populations<br />

avoid close contact. No emigration between populations has been documented, <strong>and</strong> genetic<br />

analysis indicates that it is rare. This segregation into extremely small sympatric <strong>and</strong><br />

parapatric populations has not been described in other highly motile animals <strong>and</strong> begs<br />

explanation. I hypothesize that it results from the concordance of four behavioural <strong>and</strong><br />

ecological attributes, outlined below.<br />

1. Killer whales in the northeast Pacific have highly effective inbreeding avoidance<br />

systems. In residents, most matings occur between individuals that belong to the same<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

32


population but that have markedly dissimilar acoustic repertoires (Barrett-Lennard,<br />

2000). Since relatedness <strong>and</strong> repertoire similarity are correlated, this mating system<br />

not only prevents incestuous matings but ensures that most occur between individuals<br />

that are less closely related than if mate choice was r<strong>and</strong>om. The mating systems of<br />

transients <strong>and</strong> offshores have not been described in detail, but allele frequency<br />

analysis suggest that they also achieve lower inbreeding levels than expected by<br />

r<strong>and</strong>om mating. Inbreeding avoidance within each killer whale population reduces the<br />

relative genetic benefits to individuals of dispersing between populations <strong>and</strong> allows<br />

smaller populations to be viable.<br />

2. Killer whales in the northeast Pacific live in an environment where prey are abundant<br />

<strong>and</strong> aggregated, but highly variable both spatially <strong>and</strong> temporally. Such an<br />

environment favours the transmission of information about the distribution of prey<br />

aggregations between related group members (Barrett-Lennard et al. 2001), which in<br />

turn benefits individuals that stay in their social groups.<br />

3. As long-lived, highly social animals, killer whales develop relationships not only<br />

within but between social groups. This allows linkages between social groups to<br />

develop, which may be reinforced <strong>and</strong> maintained by inter-group mating. These<br />

linkages may reduce inter-group conflict, improve access to resources, <strong>and</strong> provide a<br />

further advantage to individuals that remain in their natal populations compared to<br />

those that leave.<br />

4. Because water is an excellent conductor of sound <strong>and</strong> killer whales use group-specific<br />

calls, they have a reliable long-distance group identification system. This allows the<br />

social cohesion of physically-dispersed groups, <strong>and</strong> benefits groups that remain<br />

within acoustic contact over those that disperse widely. It also implies that killer<br />

whale populations are functional social units, vindicating Bigg’s description of them<br />

as “communities”.<br />

In combination, these factors reduce the competitive <strong>and</strong> genetic costs of non-dispersal<br />

experienced by most organisms <strong>and</strong> provide substantial benefits to remaining with kin for life.<br />

According to the hypothesis, population segregation is a natural consequence of limited<br />

dispersal. Population size, on the other h<strong>and</strong>, is likely a function of the frequency with which<br />

social groups associate, with fission occurring if the size <strong>and</strong> range of a population become<br />

such that some groups fail to encounter others at a threshold rate.<br />

The attributes of killer whales <strong>and</strong> their environment that predispose them to live in<br />

small, segregated populations may have evolved (both genetically <strong>and</strong> culturally) in response<br />

to the periodic events causing bottlenecks that might be expected to a top-level predator in a<br />

variable environment. However, the hypothesis does not preclude the possibility of<br />

coevolution. For example, the combination of the first three factors described above may<br />

have created strong selection pressure for the fourth.<br />

BIBLIOGRAPHY<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L. G. (2000). Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) as revealed by DNA analysis. PhD thesis. University of British<br />

Columbia, Vancouver.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L. G., V. B. Deecke, H. Yurk, J. K. B. Ford (2001). "A sound approach to<br />

the study of culture." Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Brain Sciences 24(2): 325-326.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

33


Bigg, M. A. (1982). "An assessment of killer whale (Orcinus orca) stocks off Vancouver<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia." Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission 32: 625-<br />

666.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

34


KILLER WHALES AT MARION ISLAND, SOUTHERN OCEAN<br />

M.N. Bester, M. Keith & P.A. Pistorius<br />

Mammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology <strong>and</strong> Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria,<br />

0002, South Africa.<br />

Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> (46°54’S, 37°45’E), the larger of the two isl<strong>and</strong>s within the Prince<br />

Edward Isl<strong>and</strong> group, is situated in the Southern Indian Ocean north of the Antarctic Polar<br />

Front. The isl<strong>and</strong> has an area of 296 km 2 <strong>and</strong> a circumference of approximately 72 km (Condy<br />

et al. 1978). Data on killer whales were collected on an opportunistic basis between 1973 <strong>and</strong><br />

2000 by researchers operating within the South African National Antarctic Programme<br />

(SANAP) as described in Condy et al. (1978), Keith et al. (2001) <strong>and</strong> Pistorius et al. (2002).<br />

Spatial distribution - distance nearshore - Most killer whales sighted off Marion Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

were predominantly in the nearshore area (Condy et al. 1978, Keith et al. 2001, Pistorius et al.<br />

2002). As southern elephant seal pups (Mirounga leonina) remain in near shore waters during<br />

play <strong>and</strong> local post-weaning dispersion (Lenglart & Bester 1982; Wilkinson & Bester 1990),<br />

<strong>and</strong> elephant seal females <strong>and</strong> penguins enter <strong>and</strong> exit the water here at the l<strong>and</strong>-sea boundary,<br />

it is likely to be the most rewarding hunting area for killer whales (Keith et al. 2001; Pistorius<br />

et al. 2002). Especially newly weaned southern elephant seal pups would be prone to<br />

predation near the shore since Guinet et al. (1992) found a high predation rate of killer whales<br />

on weaned elephant seal pups at the Îles Crozet. Swimming speeds of killer whales patrolling<br />

the beaches varied between 11.9 km h -1 <strong>and</strong> 14.44 km h -1 (Pistorius et al. 2002).<br />

Distribution around Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> - The most sightings of killer whales were recorded<br />

around the base station (Keith et al. 2001) possibly due to the high number of potential<br />

observers present there. Observer activity also possibly led to an increase in sightings at some<br />

sites in the vicinity of eight to ten field huts, focal points of human activity, situated along the<br />

coast of Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>. However, prey distribution <strong>and</strong> availability may also play a definite<br />

role in the frequency of occurrence of observations around the isl<strong>and</strong>. The areas with high<br />

percentage frequencies of sightings away from the base area support colonies of penguins <strong>and</strong><br />

elephant seals. By far the majority of breeding (<strong>and</strong> moulting) elephant seals occupy the<br />

eastern coasts (Wilkinson & Bester 1990). The consequent higher seal movement could<br />

explain the predominance of killer whales on the east coast. In addition, on 13 December<br />

2000, all of the 260 sightings of individuals which were recorded right around the isl<strong>and</strong><br />

(Pistorius et al. 2002) were made from the four observation sites along the northeast coast,<br />

except one killer whale that was seen at Kildalkey on the southeast coast (Fig. 1). Despite the<br />

large number of fur seals (Arctocephalus tropicalis) residing on the west <strong>and</strong> north coasts of<br />

Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> (Hofmeyr et al. 1997), relatively few sightings of killer whales were recorded<br />

there. This might be related to the behaviour, distribution <strong>and</strong> difference in the seasonal<br />

haulout cycle of these two (of three) seal species found at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Seasonal occurrence - The seasonal occurrence of killer whales at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> (Keith<br />

et al. 2001) follows that recorded by Condy et al. (1978) <strong>and</strong> Guinet et al. (1992) where the<br />

maximum frequency of observations occurred in October, November <strong>and</strong> December, on<br />

Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> (Fig. 2) <strong>and</strong> Îles Crozet respectively. Condy et al. (1978) <strong>and</strong> Keith et al.<br />

(2001) further found a steep decline in number of sightings during January <strong>and</strong> February, with<br />

another, less pronounced, increase in sightings occurring during March to May. Roux (1986),<br />

on the other h<strong>and</strong>, found a clear seasonal cycle of occurrence at the temperate Amsterdam<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong> with killer whales being rare in July to August <strong>and</strong> more common in February to March<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

35


(i.e. three to five months later than their peak in abundance around the sub-Antarctic isl<strong>and</strong>s).<br />

The observed difference in the timing of peak abundance in killer whales might be related to<br />

dispersing killer whales passing through different latitudes at different times (Mikhalev et al.<br />

1981).<br />

Group size - The overall mean group size for the whole study period was approximately<br />

four individuals (Keith et al. 2001; Condy et al. 1978). Elsewhere group sizes varied between<br />

1-15 individuals, with an average of three animals (Baird & Dill 1995), while Mikhalev et al.<br />

(1981) found the most frequent group size to be six to eight animals, although the numbers<br />

varied between one to several hundred animals.<br />

Sex <strong>and</strong> age composition - Adult females predominated (Keith et al. 2001) whereas<br />

Condy et al. (1978) calculated that killer whales had a sex/age composition of 28.7% adult<br />

males, 21.4% adult females, 24.7% subadults, 7.8% calves <strong>and</strong> an unidentified class of 17.4%<br />

at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>. However, comparison between studies is complicated due to the difficulty in<br />

distinguishing between a subadult male <strong>and</strong> an adult female.<br />

Diurnal sighting frequencies - Killer whales were only observable from dawn to dusk<br />

with a peak of opportunistic observations during late afternoon (Condy et al. 1978; Keith et<br />

al. 2001), a probable result of increased human activity around the base station at that<br />

particular time of the day. The dawn-to-dusk surveys (DDUs), on the other h<strong>and</strong>, produced a<br />

relatively constant distribution of sightings throughout the day (Keith et al. 2001; Pistorius et<br />

al. 2002).<br />

Photogrammetry - The Defran et al. (1990) method for photogrammetry could only be<br />

used on a few individuals that did possess unique, useable markings. The Heimlich-Boran<br />

(1986) method suffered from the varied placing of a base line (BL), but the methods devised<br />

(Keith et al. 2001) provide a quantitative way to position the BL. It may only become evident<br />

after obtaining improved photographs, whether the proposed new adapted methods (Keith et<br />

al. 2001) will improve the photogrammetric analysis method of Heimlich-Boran (1986).<br />

Photo-identification - Many of the photographs of the killer whales were of poor quality<br />

<strong>and</strong> taken from a great distance which made reliable identification difficult (Keith et al.<br />

2001). The opportunistic <strong>and</strong> sporadic nature of photographing the killer whales at Marion<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong> precluded all of the killer whales being photographed. Due to the nature of the<br />

photographs, which concentrated on individuals, groups could not be distinguished from the<br />

photographs. One female with distinct markings on the dorsal fin, first identified in 1973, was<br />

still present around the isl<strong>and</strong> in 1993, at an age in excess of 20 years.<br />

Population size - We identified 26 whales in seven pods during the early morning to late<br />

afternoon sightings on the isl<strong>and</strong>-wide vigil (13 December 2000). A second method indicated<br />

that 29 individuals moved north past the base station between 15h28 <strong>and</strong> 17h55, while a third<br />

estimate of abundance suggested that 20 killer whales were seen in front of the base station,<br />

three at Archway, <strong>and</strong> three were seen at 16h02 approaching from the west at Pinnacles<br />

(Pistorius et al. 2002). Therefore, we think that there were between 25 <strong>and</strong> 30 killer whales<br />

hunting around Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> during their peak presence in early December 2000.<br />

Human impact - Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> has recently been proposed as an eco-tourism destination<br />

(Heydenrych & Jackson 2000) <strong>and</strong> killer whale ‘watching’ could become a realistic tourist<br />

attraction. Looking solely at the seasonal <strong>and</strong> diurnal pattern of the occurrences, “orca”<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

36


watching from shore based vantage points could be a viable option in the peak season from<br />

October to December. If boat-based whale watching is ever implemented, care should be<br />

taken regarding contact with the killer whales as such an activity could have immediate<br />

behavioural effects as well as long term distribution adjustment effects (Corkeron 1995).<br />

Killer whales often take toothfish off longlines (Ashford et al. 1996) <strong>and</strong> the Marion Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

summer population may be attracted to longline fishing activity, which officially commenced<br />

in 1996 in the vicinity of the Prince Edward isl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

Funding for research at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong> was provided by the Department of<br />

Environmental Affairs <strong>and</strong> Tourism, on the advice of the South African Committee for<br />

Antarctic Research (SACAR).<br />

REFERENCES<br />

ASHFORD, J.R., RUBILAR, P.S. & MARTIN, A.R. 1996. Interactions between cetaceans <strong>and</strong><br />

longline fishery operations around South Georgia. Marine Mammal Science 12: 452-457.<br />

BAIRD, R.W. & DILL, L.M. 1995. Occurrence <strong>and</strong> behaviour of transient killer whales: seasonal <strong>and</strong><br />

pod-specific variability, foraging behaviour, <strong>and</strong> prey h<strong>and</strong>ling. Canadian Journal of Zoology<br />

73: 1300-1311.<br />

CONDY, P.R., VAN AARDE, R.J. & BESTER, M.N. 1978. The seasonal occurrence <strong>and</strong> behaviour<br />

of killer whales Orcinus orca, at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>. Journal of Zoology, London 184: 449-464.<br />

CORKERON, P.J. 1995. Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in Hervey Bay, Queensl<strong>and</strong>:<br />

behaviour <strong>and</strong> responses to whale-watching vessels. Canadian Journal of Zoology 73: 1290-<br />

1299.<br />

DEFRAN, R.H., SHULTZ, G.M. & WELLER, D.W. 1990. A technique for the photographic<br />

identification <strong>and</strong> cataloging of dorsal fins of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus).<br />

Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission (Special issue 12): 53-55.<br />

GUINET, C., JOUVENTIN, P. & WEIMERSKIRCH, H. 1992. Population changes, movements of<br />

southern elephant seals in Crozet <strong>and</strong> Kerguelen Archipelagos in the last decades. Polar<br />

Biology 12: 349-356.<br />

HEIMLICH-BORAN, J.R. 1986. Photogrammetric analysis of growth in Puget Sound Orcinus orca .<br />

In: Behavioural biology of killer whales, (eds) B.C. Kirkecold & J.S. Lockard, Vol. 1, Alan R.<br />

Liss, Inc., New York.<br />

HEYDENRYCH, R. & JACKSON, S. 2000. Environmental impact assessment of tourism on Marion<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>. Prince Edward Isl<strong>and</strong>s Management Committee, Department of Environmental Affairs<br />

<strong>and</strong> Tourism, South Africa.<br />

HOFMEYR, G.J.G., BESTER, M.N. & JONKER, F.C. 1997. Changes in population sizes <strong>and</strong><br />

distribution of fur seals at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>. Polar Biology 17: 150-158.<br />

KEITH, M., BESTER, M.N., BARTLETT, P.A. & BAKER, D. 2001. Killer whales (Orcinus orca) at<br />

Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>, Southern Ocean. African Zoology 36: 163-175.<br />

LENGLART, P.Y. & BESTER, M.N. 1982. Post-weaning dispersion of southern elephant seals<br />

Mirounga leonina underyearlings at Kerguelen. Revue de Ecologie (Terre et la Vie) 36: 175-<br />

186.<br />

MIKHALEV, Y.A., IVASHIN, M.V., SAVUSIN, V.P. & ZELENAYA, F.E. 1981. The distribution<br />

<strong>and</strong> biology of killer whales in the southern hemisphere. Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling<br />

Commission 31: 551-565.<br />

PISTORIUS, P.A., TAYLOR, F.E., LOUW, C., HANISE, B., BESTER, M.N., DE WET, C., DU<br />

PLOOY, A., GREEN, N., KLASEN, S., PODILE, S. & SCHOEMAN, J. 2002. Distribution,<br />

movement, <strong>and</strong> estimated population size of killer whales (Orcinus orca) at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

December 2000. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 32: 86-92.<br />

ROUX, J.-P 1986. Le cycle d’abondance des orques, Orcinus orca, aux îles Saint-Paul et Amsterdam.<br />

Mammalia 50: 5-8.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

37


WILKINSON, I.S. & BESTER, M.N. 1990. Duration of post-weaning fast <strong>and</strong> local<br />

dispersion in the southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina, at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>. Journal<br />

of Zoology, London 222: 591-600.<br />

N<br />

0%<br />

2km<br />

0%<br />

0%<br />

Mixed Pickle Cove<br />

0%<br />

0%<br />

Cape Davis<br />

Goodhope Bay<br />

4.8%<br />

0%<br />

Pinnacles<br />

16%<br />

Ship’s Cove<br />

Transvaal Cove (Base)<br />

Kildalkey Bay<br />

20%<br />

Archway Bay<br />

Figure 1. Observation points <strong>and</strong> percentage killer whale activity (black pie charts) recorded during 06h00 to<br />

18h00 observation on 13/12/2000 at Marion Isl<strong>and</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the percentage of elephant seal pups (grey pie<br />

charts) born at the main breeding beaches during the 2000 breeding season.<br />

8.3%<br />

0.5%<br />

14.2%<br />

12.1%<br />

10.7%<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

5.1%<br />

8.3%<br />

14.5%<br />

14.5%<br />

38%<br />

7.0%<br />

0.4%<br />

25%<br />

38


Frequency of occurrence (%)<br />

5<br />

4,5<br />

4<br />

3,5<br />

3<br />

2,5<br />

2<br />

1,5<br />

1<br />

0,5<br />

0<br />

oct-73<br />

oct-74<br />

oct-75<br />

oct-76<br />

oct-77<br />

oct-78<br />

oct-79<br />

oct-80<br />

oct-81<br />

oct-82<br />

oct-83<br />

oct-84<br />

oct-85<br />

Months<br />

Figure 2. Percentage frequencies of observations of killer whales for the 1973-1996 study period at Marion<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>. Data are represented on a monthly basis <strong>and</strong> were interpolated for months when no recordings were<br />

made. No data were available for the period October 1980 to November 1983.<br />

oct-86<br />

oct-87<br />

oct-88<br />

oct-89<br />

oct-90<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

oct-91<br />

oct-92<br />

oct-93<br />

oct-94<br />

oct-95<br />

39


BEHAVIOR AND ECOLOGY OF KILLER WHALES IN MONTEREY BAY,<br />

CALIFORNIA<br />

Black N. 1 , Ternullo R. 2 , Schulman-Janiger A. 3 , Ellis G. 4 , Dahlheim M. 5 . 1,2 Peggy Stap<br />

Monterey Bay Cetacean Project, P.O. Box 52001, Pacific Grove, CA 93950 Nyblack@aol.com 3<br />

American Cetacean Society, Los Angeles 4 Canadian Department of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans 5 National<br />

Marine Mammal Laboratory, NOAA.<br />

INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND<br />

Monterey Bay (36°N 122°W) is located along the central California coast within a<br />

highly productive region of major upwelling. The Monterey Submarine Canyon is the most<br />

prominent bathymetric feature within the Bay <strong>and</strong> beyond, allowing for unique opportunities<br />

to study deep-water cetaceans in a near-shore environment.<br />

Three known ecotypes (Residents, Transients, Offshore) of Killer Whales exist in the<br />

eastern North Pacific, <strong>and</strong> all have been sighted in Monterey Bay. Each can be distinguished<br />

by appearance <strong>and</strong> vocalizations at sea <strong>and</strong> have been differentiated genetically. Resident<br />

whales, which are well studied in the Pacific Northwest, travel <strong>and</strong> feed in predictable areas<br />

during summer months in inl<strong>and</strong> waters of Washington state, Vancouver, B.C., <strong>and</strong> Alaska.<br />

They prey primarily on fish, live in closely associated family groups, <strong>and</strong> are very vocal.<br />

Transients in Monterey Bay are part of the west coast community of about 300 whales that<br />

range from southern California to southeast Alaska, prey on marine mammals, travel in<br />

relatively small groups, travel over long ranges, <strong>and</strong> are often vocally quiet. The Offshore<br />

type is least known, often travels in large groups of over 100 animals, many have nicked fins,<br />

<strong>and</strong> probably prey on fish <strong>and</strong> squid. A fourth type that has occurred periodically in Monterey<br />

Bay is the “LA Pod”. No genetic samples have been obtained for this group, <strong>and</strong> they have<br />

never been seen in association with any other type.<br />

METHODS<br />

Our methods for reaching the killer whales included use of 55-70’ powerboats <strong>and</strong> a 22’<br />

inflatable. The study period for this report extended from 1987 through 2002. Our effort was<br />

both opportunistic, on whale watch vessels, <strong>and</strong> dedicated searches for killer whales on<br />

special projects funded by the BBC <strong>and</strong> National Geographic Society <strong>and</strong> on our own vessels<br />

throughout the study period. In addition a large sighting network is in place where various<br />

vessels reported killer whale sightings. Our research methods included photo-identifying<br />

individual whales <strong>and</strong> documenting their behavior, sighting locations <strong>and</strong> local movement<br />

patterns as well as collaborating with other scientists in the Pacific to document re-sightings<br />

in other areas.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Transients were most frequently sighted (223 sightings with photos from 1987-2002)<br />

<strong>and</strong> were highly associated with the canyon edge. Offshore (18 sightings) <strong>and</strong> LA Pod (9<br />

sightings) sightings were found along canyon edges <strong>and</strong> also in inshore shallower waters.<br />

Known prey of each killer whale type when feeding in Monterey Bay included for<br />

Transients: Gray whale calves, California sea lions, Harbor Seals, Elephant Seals, Dall’s<br />

Porpoise, Pacific White-Sided Dolphins, Long Beaked Common Dolphin, <strong>and</strong> seabirds;<br />

Offshores: salmon, small schooling fish, <strong>and</strong> blue shark; Residents: Chinook salmon; LA Pod:<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

40


Great White Shark (north of Monterey off Farallon Isl<strong>and</strong>s; Pyle et al. 1997). Prey was nonoverlapping<br />

among types except both offshore <strong>and</strong> resident whales preyed on salmon.<br />

As of September 2002, number of individual whales identified for each type included<br />

145 transients, 172 offshores (min. numbers with 230 maximum), <strong>and</strong> 8 LA pod members.<br />

Within the transients, 36 were adult males, 7 females with no calves (over 10 sighting years),<br />

24 reproductive females, <strong>and</strong> 78 juveniles/females/calves.<br />

The Resident K <strong>and</strong> L Pods (part of southern Residents), which occur predictably during<br />

summer in Washington <strong>and</strong> Vancouver, were sighted once in Monterey Bay on January 29,<br />

2000. This represents a distance of 1,251 km from their known summer range. During this<br />

day the whales foraged on Chinook salmon. This unusual event could represent declining<br />

food sources to the north <strong>and</strong> the whales' search for abundant prey elsewhere.<br />

LA Pod whales ranged from San Francisco (central California) to the upper Sea of<br />

Cortez, with individual movements of 2,847 km. These whales were frequently sighted off<br />

Los Angeles, CA during the 1980’s <strong>and</strong> a few times in Monterey during late 1980’s <strong>and</strong><br />

1990’s. They occurred off the West Coast of Baja <strong>and</strong> Sea of Cortez during this period as<br />

well. These whales were last sighted in December 1997 off San Diego, CA <strong>and</strong> may currently<br />

be residing in Mexico. Largest group size documented for these whales was 13, which is the<br />

known population for these individuals.<br />

The Offshore whales traveled the longest distance for any Killer Whale type in the<br />

Pacific, with individuals sighted in Los Angeles, Monterey Bay <strong>and</strong> off Kodiak Isl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

Alaska, a distance of 3,680 km. Group sizes of these whales while in Monterey Bay differed<br />

seasonally. During winter months mean group size was 52 with maximum group size around<br />

200 whales. Only one sighting occurred during spring with 22 whales, <strong>and</strong> during 8 sightings<br />

in fall, group sizes were small with a mean of 10 whales. No sightings occurred during<br />

summer, but offshores were sighted in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska during this period.<br />

Transient whales were more frequently sighted in Monterey Bay compared to other<br />

types <strong>and</strong> therefore, more information has been gathered on them. The discovery curve of new<br />

adult male <strong>and</strong> female whales identified beginning in1987 slowly increased until 1999 where<br />

it leveled off, assuming now that most adults in this population that travel through <strong>and</strong> feed in<br />

Monterey have been identified.<br />

Transient whales sighted in Monterey Bay have been identified from southern<br />

California to Southeast Alaska, with sightings within California, along the outer coasts of<br />

Oregon, Washington, British Columbia, <strong>and</strong> one sighting in inl<strong>and</strong> waters of Southeast<br />

Alaska. The longest-range movement of four whales occurred from Monterey to Southeast<br />

Alaska, a distance of 2,594 km. Occurrence varied by season, with peak occurrence during<br />

spring <strong>and</strong> fall <strong>and</strong> few sightings during summer. Of the whales identified in Monterey <strong>and</strong> resighted<br />

to the north, 12 were sighted off Washington, B.C., <strong>and</strong> Alaska during summer<br />

months. Group size also varied by season, with the largest groups occurring during spring <strong>and</strong><br />

smallest in summer. Many of the larger groups (32 was largest) were temporary associations<br />

of smaller subgroups over a day. Ninety-five percent of all identified whales were sighted in<br />

spring months over combined years. Whales that are rarely sighted were present more often<br />

during spring months <strong>and</strong> frequently sighted whales occurred more often during spring<br />

compared to other months. The higher occurrence, group sizes, <strong>and</strong> individual whale presence<br />

during spring corresponded with the migration of Gray Whale cow/calves through the Bay.<br />

The low sightings in summer could be due to Killer Whales shifting north with the pulse of<br />

mother/calf Gray Whales. As one example, an adult female sighted in Monterey on several<br />

occasions was also seen attacking a gray whale calf off Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong> during summer.<br />

Of the 24 documented reproductive females, 13 were sighted frequently enough to<br />

determine calving intervals. Since some whales may not be sighted for many months, there<br />

was a bias that calves may have been born <strong>and</strong> died before their mothers were re-sighted.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

41


Given this, years between calves ranged from 3 (n=1) to 12 (n=1) years; <strong>and</strong> in between: 4<br />

years (n=1), 5 yr (n=1), 6 yr (n=2), 7 yr (n=3), 8 yr (n=3), 9 yr (n=1). Compared to other<br />

populations this rate appeared low <strong>and</strong> could be related to high levels of PCB’s found in this<br />

group of killer whales.<br />

Association pattern analysis indicated that transient whales occurred in 18 core groups<br />

of frequently sighted whales (up to 36 if infrequently sighted whales were included). Core<br />

groups were usually composed of 2-4 reproductive females with juveniles <strong>and</strong> calves, <strong>and</strong><br />

some with one sprouter male or adult male. A few groups were composed of male pairs.<br />

These core groups of individuals were sighted together on more than 80% to 100% of the<br />

time. Two or more of these core groups joined together for temporary associations, especially<br />

during spring months while searching for <strong>and</strong> hunting Gray Whale calves. Three welldocumented<br />

young males were sighted over a period of 10 <strong>and</strong> 11 years <strong>and</strong> are now sprouters<br />

<strong>and</strong> young adults. All still travel with their probable mothers with estimated current ages of 15<br />

to 19 years.<br />

This study represents the only long-term database for Killer Whales south of<br />

Washington State but part of the same population as whales to the north. Monterey Bay is a<br />

key area for detailed studies of the three known ecotypes <strong>and</strong> possibly five, as the ranges of<br />

these types may overlap in the region.<br />

Study Area<br />

2000<br />

1500<br />

Depth in Meters<br />

Mont erey Submarine Canyon<br />

5 km<br />

Santa Cr uz<br />

Car mel Cany on<br />

122°20'W 122°00'W<br />

N<br />

n=140 transient sightings<br />

n=12 offshore sightings<br />

n=9 LA pod sightings<br />

1000<br />

500<br />

200<br />

Pt.<br />

Pinos<br />

Hopkins<br />

Cypr ess Pt.<br />

Pt. Lobos<br />

100 50<br />

Gr anite Canyon<br />

Pt. Sur<br />

37°00'N<br />

Monter ey<br />

Moss<br />

L<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

36°40'<br />

N<br />

36°20'N<br />

Figure 1. Distribution of Killer Whale types in Monterey Bay.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

42


white shark attack 1997<br />

40°<br />

30°<br />

8<br />

20°<br />

(Pyle et al.)<br />

50°<br />

40°<br />

145<br />

30°<br />

20°<br />

3<br />

last sighted 12/3/97<br />

Farallon Is.<br />

5<br />

8<br />

8<br />

Is. Cedros<br />

Monterey Bay<br />

Santa Barbara, CA<br />

Los Angeles, CA<br />

San Diego, CA<br />

5<br />

2,687 km<br />

2<br />

Canal de Ballenas, MX<br />

Figure 2. Distribution of LA Pod.<br />

27<br />

5<br />

1<br />

20<br />

4<br />

Farallon Is.<br />

5<br />

3<br />

Channel Is.<br />

4<br />

Catalina Is.<br />

0<br />

Southeast Alaska<br />

Queen Charlotte Is., B.C.<br />

Vancouver Is., B.C.<br />

Olympic Pen, WA<br />

Coos Bay, OR<br />

Monterey Bay<br />

2,594 km<br />

Santa Barbara, CA<br />

Los Angeles, CA<br />

Baja California, MX<br />

Figure 3. Distribution of Transients<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

43


50°<br />

40°<br />

164<br />

30°<br />

20°<br />

4<br />

3<br />

Kodiak Is. Alaska<br />

23<br />

2<br />

3<br />

3<br />

Farallon Is.<br />

Coos Bay, OR<br />

Monterey Bay<br />

Santa Barbara, CA<br />

24<br />

Los Angeles, CA<br />

0<br />

Southeast Alaska<br />

British Columbia<br />

Baja California, MX<br />

180-230 Identified off California<br />

as of 2002<br />

3,680 km<br />

Figure 4. Distribution of Offshores<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

44


KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) OF ALASKA: GULF OF ALASKA TO THE<br />

BERING SEA.<br />

Dahlheim M.E. 1 , Ellifrit D.K. 2 , Hoelzel A.R. 3 , DeLeuw R. 1 .<br />

1 Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Mammal Laboratory, 7600 S<strong>and</strong> Point Way,<br />

Seattle, Washington 98115; marilyn.dahlheim@noaa.gov; 2 Center for Whale Research, 1359<br />

Smuggler’s Cove Road, Friday Harbor, Washington 98250; 3 Dept. of Biological Sciences, Durham<br />

University, United Kingdom;<br />

The documented decline of Steller sea lions, harbor seals, <strong>and</strong> sea otters in the western<br />

Gulf of Alaska <strong>and</strong> Bering Sea has raised questions about the potential impact that killer<br />

whale predation may have on these populations. Predator population size is a key factor when<br />

determining impacts on prey populations. Although killer whale population size <strong>and</strong> stock<br />

structure is well documented for the waters of Southeast Alaska <strong>and</strong> Prince William Sound,<br />

this is not so for killer whales inhabiting the waters west of Kodiak Isl<strong>and</strong> to the Bering Sea.<br />

The first dedicated killer whale surveys in this area occurred in 1992 <strong>and</strong> 1993 where<br />

minimum counts of killer whales were obtained through photo-identification studies.<br />

Analyses of photographic data from both the dedicated surveys <strong>and</strong> fishery observer program<br />

(1980 to 2001) have resulted in the identification of approximately 400 individual killer<br />

whales. In 2001, a dedicated killer whale survey was completed in the waters from Kodiak<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong> west to the Eastern Aleutian Isl<strong>and</strong>s. In addition to the photo-identification study, killer<br />

whale biopsy samples were collected to enable us to identify resident, offshore, or transient<br />

eco-types. Based on the 2001 surveys, we have added approximately 240 whales to our<br />

photographic database. We have confirmed the presence of all three eco-types in this Alaskan<br />

region. At least 100 of these Alaskan killer whales have been seen multiple times over a 10year<br />

period allowing us to track movements of individual whales. In addition, resident killer<br />

whales from Southeast Alaska, Gulf of Alaska, <strong>and</strong> Bering Sea have been documented<br />

together linking killer whales throughout Alaskan waters. Offshore whales were found south<br />

of Kodiak Isl<strong>and</strong>, resulting in the northern most sighting of this eco-type.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

45


A REVIEW OF KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) IN BRAZILIAN WATERS<br />

Dalla Rosa, Luciano 1 , E.R. Secchi 1,2 , J. Lailson-Brito Jr. 3,4 <strong>and</strong> A.F. Azevedo 4,5<br />

1 Marine Mammals Laboratory, Museu Oceanográfico “Prof. Eliézer C. Rios”, Fundação Universidade<br />

Federal do Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e, Cx. Postal 379, Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e, RS, Brazil, 96200-970. e-mail: pgobldr@furg.br<br />

2 Marine Mammals Research Team, University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>.<br />

3 Projeto MAQUA – Depto. Oceanografia / Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro,<br />

RJ, Brazil.<br />

4Laboratório de Radioisótopos EPF - IBCCF, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro,<br />

RJ, Brazil.<br />

5 PPGB/IBRAG - Departamento de Ecologia - Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de<br />

Janeiro, RJ, Brazil.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

• Information on killer whales in Brazilian waters is still limited <strong>and</strong> based mostly on<br />

sporadic sightings <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ings.<br />

• In this study, we review the available literature <strong>and</strong> present new information on the<br />

occurrence, distribution <strong>and</strong> ecology of killer whales in Brazilian waters.<br />

METHODS<br />

Data were obtained from:<br />

• Scientific literature;<br />

• Unpublished information acquired from colleagues, museums <strong>and</strong> the media<br />

(newspapers <strong>and</strong> TV);<br />

• Information along with photographs donated by the public, including fishermen;<br />

• Personal observation during non systematic beach surveys or research cruises.<br />

RESULTS<br />

• The species has been recorded along the entire Brazilian coast, except for coastal<br />

waters of the northern region (Fig. 1). The southernmost records are near the border<br />

with Uruguay, while the northernmost is 10nm from St. Peter <strong>and</strong> St. Paul's<br />

Archipelago (00 o 55’N; 29 o 20’W).<br />

STRANDINGS<br />

• Str<strong>and</strong>ings were concentrated on the southern region, where they occurred mostly in<br />

spring <strong>and</strong> summer (Tab. 1). From 20 known str<strong>and</strong>ings, 16 occurred on the southern<br />

coast, 14 of which in Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e do Sul State.<br />

• The largest measured specimen was a 6.21m long female found in Cabo Frio, RJ. Two<br />

animals that str<strong>and</strong>ed in the southern region were calves, one estimated as<br />

approximately 2.60m long. Sex was determined for nine specimens: six females <strong>and</strong><br />

three males.<br />

SIGHTINGS<br />

• Sightings were reported for all seasons. Sighting positions are plotted in Fig. 1, <strong>and</strong><br />

include 29 new records, 19 in coastal waters of Rio de Janeiro State (RJ). Additional<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

46


sightings for RJ were obtained from Lodi <strong>and</strong> Hetzel (1998) <strong>and</strong> Siciliano et al.<br />

(1999). Other sources of sightings include Castello <strong>and</strong> Pinedo (1986), Best et al.<br />

(1986) <strong>and</strong> Secchi <strong>and</strong> Vasque Jr. (1998). Sightings not plotted due to lack of detailed<br />

information include those of Antonelle et al. (1987), regarding animals sighted off<br />

Paraíba State (6 o 29’S to 7 o 33’W) from 1980-1985, <strong>and</strong> those of Budylenko (1981) <strong>and</strong><br />

Mikhalev et al. (1981), for southern <strong>and</strong> northeastern-southern Brazil, respectively.<br />

• In the southeastern region, sightings were concentrated during spring <strong>and</strong> summer<br />

months along coastal waters.<br />

• In the southern region, most records are from winter <strong>and</strong> spring months <strong>and</strong> in<br />

offshore waters, where killer whale interactions with longline fisheries are common.<br />

• One adult male photographed at Ilha Gr<strong>and</strong>e Bay (RJ) in September 1993 was<br />

resighted five months later at Ubatuba (SP), ca. 100km from the first location.<br />

DIET<br />

• Stomach contents from 8 animals have been analyzed. A variety of prey items were<br />

recorded: bony fish (weakfish, Cynoscion guatucupa), elasmobranchs (eagle stingray,<br />

Myliobatis sp.), cetaceans (Burmeister’s porpoise, Phocoena spinipinnis; franciscana,<br />

Pontoporia blainvillei), cephalopods (Suborder Oegopsida) <strong>and</strong> salps (Iasis zonaria)<br />

(Castello, 1977; Dalla Rosa, 1995; Ott <strong>and</strong> Danilewicz, 1996).<br />

• Killer whales also prey on: tunas (Thunnus spp.) <strong>and</strong>, mainly, sworfish, Xiphias<br />

gladius, caught by the longline fishery off southern <strong>and</strong> southeastern Brazil (Dalla<br />

Rosa, 1995; Secchi <strong>and</strong> Vasque Jr., 1998); manta rays, Manta birostris (Lodi <strong>and</strong><br />

Hetzel, 1998).<br />

HUMAN IMPACTS<br />

• Six individuals were captured by Soviet pelagic fleets from 1969/70-1978/79 between<br />

10 <strong>and</strong> 20 o S (Mikhalev et al., 1981);<br />

• In February 1975, a female killer whale was incidentally captured on a beach seine at<br />

Cassino Beach, southern Brazil;<br />

• In July 1994, a female orca was incidentally caught in a monofilament longline off<br />

southern Brazil, but escaped alive (Dalla Rosa, 1995);<br />

• Harpooning <strong>and</strong> shooting have also been reported by longline fishermen.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

• Most records are from the southeastern <strong>and</strong> southern regions. Although higher<br />

densities might be expected in temperate than in tropical waters, the lower research<br />

efforts in the northeastern <strong>and</strong>, especially, northern region could have accentuated the<br />

lack of information for these areas.<br />

• Occurrence of killer whales in coastal waters of Rio de Janeiro is seasonal (Lodi <strong>and</strong><br />

Hetzel, 1998; Siciliano et al., 1999). In other areas, however, additional information is<br />

necessary to draw any conclusions. Summer <strong>and</strong> autumn surveys would help to verify<br />

if occurrence is seasonal in offshore waters of southern Brazil.<br />

• The same killer whale population possibly occurs in southern Brazil, Uruguay <strong>and</strong><br />

northern Argentina, as suggested by the saddle patch pigmentation pattern (Iñiguez et<br />

al., 1994) <strong>and</strong> the interactions with longline fisheries. Further photo-identification <strong>and</strong><br />

genetic studies are urged to determine the population identity of killer whales in the<br />

southwest Atlantic.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

47


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

We wish to thank Projeto Baleia Jubarte-Abrolhos, GEMARS, Marcos C.O. Santos,<br />

Hugo P. Castello, Paulo H. Ott, Lis<strong>and</strong>ro Almeida, Regina Zanellato, Bia Hetzel, Fábio C.S.<br />

Costa <strong>and</strong> all other contributors who kindly shared information.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Antonelle, H.H., Lodi, L. <strong>and</strong> Borobia, M. 1987. Avistagens de cetáceos no período de 1980 a 1985 no litoral da<br />

Paraíba, Brasil. In: Reunião de Especialistas em Mamíferos Acuáticos da América do Sul, 2., Rio de Janeiro,<br />

August 4-8, 1986. Anais: resumos. p.114.<br />

Budylenko, G.A. 1981. Distribution <strong>and</strong> some aspects of the biology of killer whales in the South Atlantic. Rep.<br />

int. Whal. Commn 31: 523-525.<br />

Castello, H.P. 1977. Food of a killer whale: eagle sting-ray, (Myliobatis) found in the stomach of a str<strong>and</strong>ed Orcinus<br />

orca. Sci. Rep. Whales Res. Inst., 29: 107-111.<br />

Castello, H.P. <strong>and</strong> Pinedo, M.C. 1986. Sobre unos avistages en el mar de distintas espécies de cetáceos en el sur de<br />

Brasil. In: Reunión de Trabajo de Especialistas en Mamíferos Acuáticos de América del Sur, 1., Buenos Aires,<br />

25-29 junio, 1984. Actas. pp 61-68.<br />

Dalla Rosa, L. 1995. Interações com a pesca de espinhel e informações sobre a dieta alimentar de orca, Orcinus<br />

orca Linnaeus 1758 (Cetacea, Delphinidae), no sul e sudeste do Brasil. Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e, 40p.(Bachelor thesis)<br />

Iñiguez, M.A., Secchi, E.R., Tossenberger, V. <strong>and</strong> Dalla Rosa, L. 1994. <strong>Orca</strong>s, Orcinus orca, en la Argentina y<br />

Brasil: informe preliminar (<strong>Orca</strong>s in Argentina <strong>and</strong> Brazil: preliminary report). In: Reunião de Trabalho de<br />

Especialistas em Mamíferos Aquáticos da América do Sul, 6., Florianópolis, 24-28 outubro. Resumos. p.<br />

103.<br />

Lodi, L. <strong>and</strong> Hetzel, B. 1998. Orcinus orca (Cetacea; Delphinidae) em águas costeiras do Estado do Rio de Janeiro.<br />

Bioikos 12(1): 46-54.<br />

Mikhalev, Y.A., Ivashin, M.V., Sausin, V.P. <strong>and</strong> Zelemaya, F.E. 1981. The distribution <strong>and</strong> biology of killer whales<br />

in the Southern Hemisphere. Rep. int. Whal. Commn 31: 551-566.<br />

Ott, P.H. <strong>and</strong> Danilewicz, D. 1996. Presence of franciscanas (Pontoporia blainvillei) in the stomach of a killer<br />

whale (Orcinus orca) str<strong>and</strong>ed in southern Brazil. Mammalia 62(4): 605-609.<br />

Secchi, E.R. <strong>and</strong> Vaske JR., T. 1998. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) sightings <strong>and</strong> depredation on tuna <strong>and</strong> swordfish<br />

longline catches in southern Brazil. Aquatic Mammals, 24(2): 117-122.<br />

Siciliano, S., Lailson Brito Jr., J. <strong>and</strong> Azevedo, A.F. 1999. Seasonal occurrence of killer whales (Orcinus orca)<br />

in waters of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. Z. Säugetierkunde 64: 251-255.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

48


Figure 1. Sightings (o) <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ings (+) of killer whales along the Brazilian coast.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

49


SIGHTINGS OF KILLER WHALES OFF THE ANTARCTIC PENINSULA FROM<br />

1997/98 TO 2001/02<br />

Dalla Rosa, L. 1 , Danilewicz, D. 2 , Bassoi, M. 3 , Moreno, I.B. 2 , Santos, M.C.O. 4 & Flores, P.A.C. 5<br />

1 Projeto Baleias/Brazilian Antarctic Programme, Marine Mammals Laboratory, Museu<br />

Oceanográfico “Prof. Eliézer C. Rios”, Fundação Universidade Federal do Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e, Cx. Postal<br />

379, Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e, RS, 96200-970, Brazil. E-mail: pgobldr@furg.br<br />

2 Grupo de Estudos de Mamíferos Aquáticos do Rio Gr<strong>and</strong>e do Sul (GEMARS), Rua Felipe Neri,<br />

382/203, Porto Alegre, RS, 90440-150, Brazil.<br />

3 School of Ocean <strong>and</strong> Earth Science, Southampton Oceanography Centre, European Way, SO14 3ZH,<br />

Southampton, United Kingdom.<br />

4 Instituto de Biociências, Universidade de São Paulo, Rua do Matão 321, São Paulo, SP, 055088-900,<br />

Brazil.<br />

5 <strong>International</strong> Wildlife Coalition Brazil, Cx. Postal 5087, Florianópolis, SC, 88040-970, Brazil.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Killer whales, Orcinus orca, are a common species around Antarctic waters (e.g. Jehl<br />

et al., 1980; Mikhalev et al., 1981), however information on their abundance, distribution,<br />

population identity <strong>and</strong> taxonomic status remains to be better assessed. Mikhalev et al. (1981)<br />

proposed a new species of killer whale, Orcinus nanus, for the Southern Hemisphere, based<br />

on morphological <strong>and</strong> biological data. Berzin <strong>and</strong> Vladimirov (1983) also proposed a new<br />

species of killer whale, Orcinus glacialis, for Antarctic waters, based on morphological <strong>and</strong><br />

ecological differences in relation to O. orca. Both proposals probably refer to the same<br />

population of smaller individuals. Antarctic killer whales commonly present lighter<br />

pigmentation with a conspicuous dorsal “cape” (Jehl et al., 1980; Thomas et al., 1981; Evans<br />

et al., 1982).<br />

Since the 1997/98 austral summer, the Projeto Baleias/Brazilian Antarctic<br />

Programme (PROANTAR) has conducted ship surveys in the Antarctic Peninsula region with<br />

the following objectives: (1) photo-identify humpback whales for comparison with<br />

international catalogues; (2) biopsy humpback whales for DNA <strong>and</strong> pollution analyses; (3)<br />

determine cetacean distribution <strong>and</strong> density estimates; <strong>and</strong> (4) record all cetacean sightings. In<br />

this paper we present data on killer whales observed off the Antarctic Peninsula in the<br />

summer seasons of 1997/98 through 2001/02.<br />

MATERIAL AND METHODS<br />

During five consecutive summer seasons (1997/98 – 2001/2002), cetacean research<br />

was conducted in the waters of the Antarctic Peninsula region, especially in the Gerlache <strong>and</strong><br />

Bransfield Straits, from the 75m Oceanographic <strong>and</strong> Supply Ship (NApOc) ‘Ary Rongel’. In<br />

the Gerlache Strait, cetacean dedicated surveys were performed for distribution studies <strong>and</strong><br />

density estimates. A total of 20 transects were performed. Whale search was made with naked<br />

eye <strong>and</strong> with 7X50 binoculars. The observers watched from the exterior wing bridges<br />

approximately 14m above sea level. Data recorded on the killer whales included date, time,<br />

coordinates, pod size/composition <strong>and</strong> behavior. Whenever possible, individuals were photoidentified<br />

from the ship or from a small inflatable boat, following Bigg et al. (1987).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

50


RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Sightings<br />

Overall, killer whales were the third most frequently sighted cetacean species in the<br />

region, after humpback <strong>and</strong> minke whales. A total of 41 sightings were registered (Fig. 1).<br />

Twenty-seven sightings (66%) occurred in the Gerlache Strait, where cetacean dedicated<br />

surveys were performed. Killer whale encounter rates in the Gerlache Strait ranged from zero<br />

to 0.37 individual per nautical mile, with an average of 0.10 (CV = 125%; n = 20 surveys),<br />

while humpback <strong>and</strong> minke encounter rates averaged 0.46 (CV = 90%) <strong>and</strong> 0.09 (CV =<br />

115%), respectively. Group size estimates varied from 1 to about 35 individuals ( X = 9.32;<br />

SD = 7.50), with larger groups composed of two or more subgroups. Calves were present on<br />

at least 61% of the sightings.<br />

Pigmentation pattern/Photo-identification<br />

The overall gray pigmentation with a conspicuous dorsal “cape” (Fig. 2) was observed<br />

on all but one of the closer encounters, suggesting that it is the predominant pattern off the<br />

Antarctic Peninsula. The existence of groups with different levels of lighter pigmentation<br />

could not be discarded. The strikingly distinctive coloration pattern of these Antarctic killer<br />

whales may be indicative of a specialized population (among ice floes, this light coloration<br />

might help to improve feeding efficiency, serving as a camouflage against prey) <strong>and</strong>, possibly,<br />

even a new species. Genetic <strong>and</strong> photo-identification studies are necessary to assess their<br />

taxonomic status <strong>and</strong> population identity. The yellowish hue due to diatom deposits was also<br />

observed, in varying amounts, on most encounters, including the one where the animals did<br />

not present a visible dorsal “cape”. The exception were some of the distant sightings, when<br />

animals might have presented this pigmentation but it could not be seen.<br />

Twenty-five killer whales have been photo-identified. Due to the overall lighter<br />

pigmentation, body scars are usually conspicuous <strong>and</strong> may be useful on individual<br />

identification, however care must be taken as at least some of the marks may not be<br />

permanent.<br />

Interactions with other marine mammals<br />

Killer whales were observed approaching <strong>and</strong> encircling humpback whales on four<br />

occasions, <strong>and</strong> a group of minke whales once, however no effective predation was observed.<br />

According to Dolphin (1987) <strong>and</strong> Jefferson et al. (1991), non-predatory interactions between<br />

killer whales <strong>and</strong> other marine mammal species are relatively common. On the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

predatory interactions may last for hours, making it more difficult to witness the whole event.<br />

During two consecutive sightings made on 4 December 1997, near the Elephant Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

(61 o 26’S-55 o 00’W <strong>and</strong> 61 o 24’S-54 o 60’W), killer whales were seen throwing seals into the air<br />

with the tail flukes, possibly leopard seals, Hydrurga leptonyx. This type of behavior has also<br />

been observed in Peninsula Valdés (Iñiguez, 1993) <strong>and</strong> in Alaska (Dolphin, 1987). Killer<br />

whales were also seen chasing <strong>and</strong> presumably feeding on fur seals, Arctocephalus gazella, on<br />

1 March 2001 in the Schollaert Channel (64 o 31’S – 62 o 46’W). Killer whale tooth rake marks<br />

were present on about 4% of the flukes of humpback whales photo-identified in the Antarctic<br />

Peninsula region (n = 300). Katona et al. (1980) report that 33% of the humpbacks photoidentified<br />

in the western North Atlantic presented such scars.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

51


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

The Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) provided<br />

the financial support. SECIRM/Brazilian Navy provided the logistical support. We also wish<br />

to thank Paul Kinas, Eduardo Secchi, Glauco Caon, Alex<strong>and</strong>re Zerbini, Márcio Martins <strong>and</strong><br />

Paulo Ott for help during the course of this study.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Berzin, A.A. <strong>and</strong> Vladimirov, V.L. 1983. A new species of killer whale (Cetacea, Delphinidae) from<br />

Antarctic waters. Zool. Zh. 62(2): 287-295 [traduzido por S. Pearson].<br />

Bigg, M.A., EllisG.M., Ford, J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Balcomb, K.C. 1987. Killer whales – A Study of their<br />

Identification, Genealogy <strong>and</strong> Natural History in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State. Phantom Press <strong>and</strong><br />

Publishers, Nanaimo, British Columbia, 79p.<br />

Dolphin, W.F. 1987. Observations of humpback whale (Megaptera novaengliae) - killer whale (Orcinus<br />

orca) interactions in Alaska: comparison with terrestrial predator-prey relationships. Can. Field-Nat. 101(1):<br />

70-75.<br />

Iñiguez, M.A. 1993. <strong>Orca</strong>s de la Patagonia Argentina. Buenos Aires, Propulsora literaria. 88p.<br />

Jefferson, T.A., Stacey, P.J. <strong>and</strong> Baird, R. 1991. A review of killer whale interactions with other marine<br />

mammals: predation to co-existence. Mamm. Rev. 21(4): 151-180.<br />

Jehl, J.R., Evans, W.E., Awbrey, F.T. <strong>and</strong> Drieschmann, W.S. 1980. Distribution <strong>and</strong> geographic<br />

variation in the killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations of the Antarctic <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. Antarctic Journal of the<br />

United States 15(5): 161-163.<br />

Katona, S., Harcourt, P.M., Perkins, J.S. e Kraus, S. 1980. Humpback whales of the western North<br />

Atlantic: a catalog of identified individuals. 2 nd Ed., College of the Atlantic, Maine. 169p.<br />

Mikhalev, Y.A., Ivashin, M.V., Sausin, V.P. <strong>and</strong> Zelemaya, F.E. 1981. The distribution <strong>and</strong> biology of<br />

killer whales in the Southern Hemisphere. Rep. int. Whal. Commn 31: 551-566.<br />

Thomas, J.A., Leatherwood, S., Evans, W.E. <strong>and</strong> Jehl, J.R. 1981. Ross sea killer whale distribution,<br />

color pattern <strong>and</strong> vocalizations. Antarctic Journal of the United States 16: 157-158.<br />

Fig. 1 - Study area <strong>and</strong> killer whale sighting locations off the Antarctic Peninsula.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

52


Fig. 2 - The overall gray pigmentation is characteristic of most killer whales from the Antarctic<br />

Peninsula region.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

53


CLICK CHARACTERISTICS OF KILLER WHALES FEEDING ON NORWEGIAN<br />

SPRING-SPAWNING HERRING<br />

Damsgård, B. 1 , Similä, T. 2 & Leyssen, T. 3<br />

1) Fiskeriforskning, 9291 Tromsø, Norway; borge.damsgaard@fiskforsk.norut.no<br />

2) NORCA, Box 181, 8465 Straumsjøen, Norway; iolaire@online.no<br />

3) NORCA, Rozenlaan 8, 3650 Dilsen-Stokkem, Belgium; teoleyssen@hotmail.com<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

During the period 1998-2001 we recorded sounds from killer whales, Orcinus orca,<br />

feeding on Norwegian spring-spawning herring, Clupea harengus, during the winter season in<br />

the coastal waters of northern Norway. The study revealed that most click series used during<br />

feeding were broadb<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> with relatively few clicks (mean 13.2 clicks) <strong>and</strong> short click<br />

intervals (mean 69 ms).<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

The use of underwater sounds play a key role in the feeding behaviour of many marine<br />

mammals. Killer whales have a large acoustic variation, including pulsed sounds, tonal<br />

whistles <strong>and</strong> short clicks (Ford 1989). Pulsed sounds are probably used for calls <strong>and</strong><br />

communication, while clicks are directional forward-projected sounds with high intensities. It<br />

is well known that killer whales like other odontocetes may echolocate, but the mechanisms<br />

<strong>and</strong> ecological functions of clicking whales in the wild are still only partly described. Clicks<br />

of resident <strong>and</strong> transient killer whales in British Columbia, Canada, are well documented by<br />

Barrett-Lennard et al. (1996), while whales from other parts of the world are only anecdotally<br />

described.<br />

In the present study we describe click characteristics from killer whales in Vestfjorden,<br />

northern Norway (Similä & Ugarte, 1993), feeding on Norwegian spring-spawning herring.<br />

The recordings were conducted during hunting, herding <strong>and</strong> ‘carousel’ feeding of killer<br />

whales.<br />

MATERIALS AND METHODS<br />

The portable equipment was based on Brüel & Kjær hydrophones (8103 <strong>and</strong> 8105), a<br />

Nexus amplifier (Copenhagen, Denmark), <strong>and</strong> a DAT-recorder (Sony TCD-D100). One<br />

channel was frequency modulated (Pettersson D200, Uppsala, Sweden), enabling us to<br />

simultaneously record audio range sounds <strong>and</strong> high frequency sounds up to 100 kHz.<br />

Of totally approx. 8 h recordings we selected 329 sounds (with approx. 4200 clicks)<br />

based on the following criteria: The recordings should have a low background noise <strong>and</strong> no<br />

overload, <strong>and</strong> the sounds should be complete series of clicks that could be separated from<br />

other click series. Numbers of clicks, total time, click intervals (accuracy < 0.1 ms) <strong>and</strong><br />

frequency range were analysed, using Avisoft SAS-Lab (Berlin, Germany).<br />

RESULT AND DISCUSSION<br />

The study revealed that most click series used during feeding were broadb<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>and</strong><br />

with relatively few clicks <strong>and</strong> short click intervals. The click series consisted of 2-109 clicks,<br />

with a mean of 13.2 clicks per sound. Most of the click series (62%) had less than 10 clicks.<br />

The mean total time was 0.59 s (range 0.05-6.7 s), <strong>and</strong> most of the click series (70%) lasted<br />

less than 0.5 s. In comparison, fish eating resident killer whales in Canada produced on<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

54


average click sounds with 51 clicks <strong>and</strong> a total time of 7.2 s, while mammal eating transient<br />

killer whales on average used 17 clicks <strong>and</strong> a total time of 2.8 s (Barrett-Lennard et al., 1996).<br />

The click interval ranged from 6 ms to 750 ms, with a mean click interval of 69 ms,<br />

which corresponds to a click rate of 14.5 s -1 . In comparison the resident <strong>and</strong> transient killer<br />

whales in British Columbia had a mean click rate of 7.1 <strong>and</strong> 6.1 s -1 , respectively (Barrett-<br />

Lennard et al., 1996).<br />

In some click sounds the click interval were constant, while other sounds had a variable<br />

click interval. The sound in Fig. 1 A had more than 100 clicks <strong>and</strong> a click interval of 9.6 ms<br />

that remains nearly constant throughout the sound. Each single click had a double structure<br />

with a constant peak to peak interval of 0.32 ms. The click sound in Fig. 1 B <strong>and</strong> C are the<br />

high <strong>and</strong> audio component of the same sound. The click interval varies from 26 to 119 ms,<br />

<strong>and</strong> each click had a single click structure. Canadian click sounds did also have constant or<br />

variable click intervals (Barrett-Lennard et al., 1996), but the ecological significance of these<br />

acoustic features is still unknown. A killer whale may echolocate differently depending on<br />

whether it have a specific target or not, or the whale may use clicks for several ecological<br />

purposes.<br />

Frequency (kHz)<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

100<br />

50<br />

0<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1<br />

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6<br />

Low frequency<br />

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6<br />

Time (seconds)<br />

A)<br />

High frequency<br />

Figure 1. Examples of clicks from killer whales feeding on herring in Norway. A) A click sound with short <strong>and</strong><br />

constant click intervals; B) High frequency component of a click sound with long <strong>and</strong> variable click intervals<br />

(frequency limits between the two dotted lines); <strong>and</strong> C) Audio frequency component of sound B.<br />

Experimental studies of bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus, have clearly<br />

demonstrated the relationship between click intervals <strong>and</strong> distance between the whale <strong>and</strong> its<br />

target (Au & Nachticall, 1997). If the whale active listen to each click before emitting the next<br />

click, <strong>and</strong> if we assume that the processing time lag before the next click is 20 ms (Au &<br />

Nachticall, 1997), the click intervals in the present study indicate distances less than 40 m<br />

between the whale <strong>and</strong> the herring. If so, killer whales feeding on herring use clicks within a<br />

visible range between predators <strong>and</strong> prey. However, click intervals may also vary with other<br />

factors such as difficulties in detecting a target, or the present or absent of a target of interest<br />

(Au & Nachticall, 1997). The longest click interval in the present study (750 ms) correspond<br />

with a distance of more than 500 m. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, whales such as beluga,<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

B)<br />

C)<br />

55


Delphinapterus leucas, probably emit long series of clicks without listening to each single<br />

click (Turl & Penner, 1989), <strong>and</strong> it is not possible to rule out the possibility of the same<br />

mechanism in killer whales feeding on herring.<br />

An analysis of the frequency range of each click revealed that most clicks were<br />

broadb<strong>and</strong>ed, covering both the audio range <strong>and</strong> the high frequency range. The result indicate<br />

that many clicks had an energy peak between 20 <strong>and</strong> 30 kHz, while some of the energy<br />

ranged to 80-100 kHz. This corresponds well with the audiogram for killer whale, which<br />

range from 1 to 100 kHz, with a maximum hearing ability of approx. 20 kHz (Szymanski et<br />

al., 1999). In the sound example in Fig. 1 B <strong>and</strong> C, the frequency range were between 4 <strong>and</strong><br />

86 kHz, with a peak energy about 22 kHz in the beginning of the sound. Frequency<br />

distribution is dependent of the distance between the hydrophone <strong>and</strong> the animal, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

position of the animal (Au & Nachticall, 1997), <strong>and</strong> the changes in frequencies during the<br />

sound may be due the movement of the whale.<br />

The study raises questions about the ecological function of clicks during feeding in<br />

killer whales. The click intervals <strong>and</strong> underwater observations indicate that most of the sounds<br />

are produced within the visible range of their preys. Independent of the question whether<br />

herring may hear high frequency sounds (Astrup, 1999), the fish may easily hear the low<br />

frequency component of the sound. One might speculate if the whales use clicks only for<br />

echolocation or in addition for other purposes.<br />

In summery, the study indicated that click series may play an important ecological role<br />

when killer whales feed on herring, potentially both as an close range echolocation <strong>and</strong> as an<br />

behavioural herding of fish in smaller groups.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Astrup, J., 1999. Ultrasound detection in fish – a parallel to the sonar-mediated detection of<br />

bats by ultrasound-sensitive insects? Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 124: 19-27.<br />

Au, W.W.L., & Nachtigall, P.E., 1997. Acoustics of echolocating dolphins <strong>and</strong> small whales.<br />

Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 29: 127-162.<br />

Szymanski, M.D., Bain, D.E., Kiehl, K., Pennington, S., Wong, S. & Henry, K.R., 1999.<br />

Killer whale (Orcinus orca) hearing: Auditory brainstem response <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

audiograms. J. Acoustic Soc. Am. 106: 1134-1141.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L.G., Ford, J.K.B. & Heise, K.A., 1996. The mixed blessing of echolocation:<br />

differences in sonar use by fish-eating <strong>and</strong> mammal-eating killer whales. Anim. Behav.<br />

51: 553-565.<br />

Ford, J. K.B., 1989. Acoustic behavior of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 67: 727-745.<br />

Similä, T. & Ugarte, F., 1993. Surface <strong>and</strong> underwater observations of cooperatively feeding<br />

killer whales in Northern Norway. Can. J. Zool. 71: 1494-1499.<br />

Turl, C.W. & Penner, R.H., 1989. Differences in echolocation click patterns of the beluga<br />

(Delphinapterus leucas). J. Acoustic Soc. Am. 68: 497-502<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

56


THE BC CETACEAN SIGHTINGS NETWORK: WORKING WITH THE PUBLIC<br />

TO DETERMINE YEAR-ROUND DISTRIBUTION PATTERNS<br />

Nicola B. Dedeluk 1, Lance G. Barrett-Lennard 1,2, John K.B. Ford 2,3<br />

1 Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre, P.O. Box 3232, Vancouver, B.C. V6B 3X8 Canada,<br />

2 Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, 6270 University Boulevard, Vancouver,<br />

B.C. V6T 1Z4, 3 Pacific Biological Station, Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada, Nanaimo, B.C., V9T 6N7<br />

Canada<br />

British Columbia’s 800 kilometer coastline is sparsely populated by humans but home<br />

to at least 17 species of cetaceans. Most of the systematic research on these species has been<br />

conducted during the summer near Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>. In an attempt to learn more about the<br />

relative abundance <strong>and</strong> year-round distribution of cetaceans in British Columbia, the<br />

Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre <strong>and</strong> Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada established<br />

the B.C. Cetacean Sightings Network in 2000. In addition to conducting basic research, the<br />

Network educates the public about conservation issues affecting cetaceans <strong>and</strong> actively<br />

promotes the stewardship of marine habitats. Participants file cetacean sightings on a WEB<br />

form, by e-mail, fax or in special logbooks. All the sightings reports received are entered into<br />

a comprehensive database. The Network achieves its educational objectives <strong>and</strong> recruits new<br />

participants through media interviews, public meetings, paid advertising, <strong>and</strong> an extensive<br />

website (www.wildwhales.org). It also co-sponsors a yearly course in marine mammal<br />

biology, ecology, <strong>and</strong> conservation for the staff of commercial whale watching companies.<br />

The website contains information to assist untrained observers with species identification <strong>and</strong><br />

contains an online sighting report form.<br />

The Network currently has over 200 active participants from most of B.C.’s coastal<br />

communities. These observers include researchers, vessel operators, commercial <strong>and</strong> sport<br />

fishermen, whale watching guides, lighthouse keepers, <strong>and</strong> other members of the public. To<br />

date, we have received over 2000 sightings reports, half of which concern killer whales,<br />

(Orcinus orca). As was expected at the outset most of our sightings are from locations around<br />

populated areas (Southern <strong>and</strong> Eastern Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>) <strong>and</strong> sixty two percent were filed in<br />

the summer (Table I). This likely represents a bias in sighting effort, <strong>and</strong> we are actively<br />

working to increase effort in the winter <strong>and</strong> in remote areas.<br />

Table I: Reported cetacean sightings observation by month from April 2000 to May 2002.<br />

Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun July Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec<br />

# 66 35 120 186 133 306 387 344 271 149 48 58<br />

% 3.1 1.7 5.7 8.8 6.3 14.5 18.3 16.3 12.8 7.1 2.3 2.8<br />

Sixty two percent of the 1083 sightings reports of killer whales included information on<br />

the population (resident, transient, <strong>and</strong> offshore). Figure 1 shows the breakdown of sightings<br />

reports by population <strong>and</strong> month.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

57


250<br />

200<br />

150<br />

100<br />

50<br />

0<br />

Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec<br />

Figure 1: Number of reported sightings of identified killer whale populations by month.<br />

Of f shore<br />

Transient<br />

Nort hern Resident<br />

Sout hern Resident<br />

Unknown Type<br />

Southern resident killer whales represent 47% of the sightings received. Northern<br />

residents accounted for 38% of sightings; transients 15% <strong>and</strong> offshore killer whales represent<br />

less than .5%. Like most killer whale studies the B.C. Cetacean Sightings Network is an<br />

ongoing project. It is only with the long-term gathering of data that we will be able to make<br />

any predictions about the year-round habitat use by cetaceans on our coast.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

58


VOCAL BEHAVIOUR OF TRANSIENT KILLER WHALES: FOOD CALLING OR<br />

CONSTRAINED COMMUNICATION<br />

Volker B. Deecke*†, John K.B. Ford‡† & Peter J.B. Slater*<br />

*School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, Scotl<strong>and</strong>, UK<br />

†Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre, Vancouver BC, Canada<br />

‡Pacific Biological Station, Department of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans, Nanaimo BC, Canada<br />

The cost of vocal behaviour is usually expressed in energetic terms; however, animals may pay<br />

ecological costs for vocalizing by alerting their predators or prey. The Northeast Pacific is home to two<br />

distinct ecotypes of killer whales (Orcinus orca): resident killer whales feed on fish, a prey with poor<br />

hearing abilities whereas transient killer whales hunt marine mammals, which have sensitive underwater<br />

hearing at the frequencies of killer whale vocal communication. In this study, we investigate whether the<br />

vocal behaviour of the two ecotypes was shaped by the different hearing abilities of their prey. We<br />

recorded the vocalizations <strong>and</strong> associated behavioural context of groups of transient <strong>and</strong> resident killer<br />

whales in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Southeast Alaska. Transient killer whales emitted pulsed calls<br />

significantly less frequently than residents. The vocal activity of transients increased after a successful<br />

attack on a marine mammal, <strong>and</strong> the levels of vocal activity after an attack were significantly higher than<br />

for all other behaviour categories except socializing. Since at least some marine mammals respond to the<br />

calls of transient killer whales, the reduced vocal activity of transients is likely due to a greater ecological<br />

cost for calling in this ecotype. The fact that transients call after a successful attack could represent food<br />

calling (informing other animals in the area about the presence of food) or could be due to the fact that the<br />

ecological cost for vocal behaviour is relatively small after a successful attack. Since the calls after a kill<br />

are often very faint <strong>and</strong> since we failed to observe other animals joining a feeding group, a reduced<br />

constraint on vocal behaviour is the more likely explanation for calling in this behavioural context.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

The primary function of vocal behaviour in mammals <strong>and</strong> birds is communication<br />

between individuals or, in the form of echolocation, orientation <strong>and</strong> prey detection. Vocal<br />

behaviour clearly generates benefits but it also has associated costs: in addition to the energy<br />

required to generate the sound, vocalizing animals may experience costs from passing on<br />

information to unintended receivers. In the case of predators that specialize on prey with<br />

sensitive hearing, these costs can be substantial, since the prey is likely to react to the<br />

predator’s vocalization thus reducing the probability of capture.<br />

In a variety of animals, vocal behaviour is associated with the discovery or manipulation<br />

of food. In many cases such vocalizations serve to detect prey (e.g. echolocation) or to<br />

manipulate prey behaviour. In other cases, however, food-related vocalizations may be<br />

directed at conspecifics <strong>and</strong> have communicative function. By attracting conspecifics, such<br />

food calling may decrease the risk of predation. Alternatively, food calling could attract<br />

potential mates, or lead to improved access to or defense of a food source.<br />

The Northeast Pacific is home to two distinct forms of killer whales (Orcinus orca) that<br />

differ in their behaviour <strong>and</strong> diet (Ford et al. 1998; Saulitis et al. 2000). The two forms do not<br />

interbreed <strong>and</strong> rarely interact. Resident killer whales live in large stable groups <strong>and</strong> feed<br />

exclusively on fish, a prey with poor hearing capabilities (Hawkins & Johnstone 1978).<br />

Transient killer whales live in smaller social groups <strong>and</strong> prey only on marine mammals with<br />

acute underwater hearing (e.g. Renouf 1992; Au et al. 2000) that respond to killer whale calls<br />

(Deecke et al. in press). Barrett-Lennard et al. (1996) showed that transient killer whales use<br />

echolocation clicks much less frequently than residents.<br />

In this study, we wanted to investigate the behaviour context in which transient killer<br />

whales use communicative vocalizations. We wanted to test whether rates of vocal behaviour<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

59


are significantly elevated after a successful attack on a marine mammal, <strong>and</strong> whether transient<br />

use communicative vocalizations less frequently that the sympatric resident killer whales.<br />

METHODS<br />

The data for this study were collected in the summers of 1999-2002 off British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> Southeast Alaska. Groups of killer whales were identified photographically <strong>and</strong><br />

followed in a small boat. We stopped the boat ahead of the whales <strong>and</strong> as they passed we<br />

recorded underwater vocalizations on DAT tape <strong>and</strong> measured the distance to the animals<br />

with laser rangefinders. For each pass, we classified the group’s behaviour as travel, slow<br />

travel, milling, or socializing using easily quantified variables (swim speed, orientation <strong>and</strong><br />

synchronicity of the surfacing, <strong>and</strong> the presence of aerial or percussive behaviours). Milling<br />

after a kill was a separate behaviour category when a marine mammal kill was confirmed. To<br />

avoid missing faint calls, we only used the sections of recordings when the animals were<br />

within 500m of the boat. We quantified the level of vocal activity (vocal rate) by dividing the<br />

number of calls recorded while within 500m of the animals by the amount of time spent<br />

within this range <strong>and</strong> the number of animals in the group.<br />

To identify the behaviour context of vocal activity, we compared vocal rates for the<br />

different behaviour categories. To test whether vocal activity was related to the presence of<br />

food (i.e. increased after a successful attack), we compared vocal rates after the kill to the<br />

vocal rates for all other behaviour categories observed in encounters where a kill could be<br />

confirmed. Finally, to test for differences in vocal activity between the two killer whale<br />

ecotypes, we compared vocal rates across all behaviour categories for encounters with<br />

resident <strong>and</strong> transient killer whales.<br />

RESULTS<br />

The vocal rates showed significant differences between behaviour categories (Kruskall-<br />

Wallis Test: χ 2 = 16.43, p = 0.002). Vocal behaviour was only common when the animals<br />

were milling after a kill or socializing. The whales were usually silent (median vocal rate = 0<br />

calls/individual/minute) during all other behaviours (Fig. 1). Kills of a marine mammal could<br />

be confirmed in six encounters. In these encounters vocal rates after the kill were higher than<br />

during all other behaviour categories (Fig. 2) <strong>and</strong> this difference is significant (Wilcoxon<br />

Test: Z 6,1 = -2.02, p = 0.043). The comparison of vocal rates during encounters with residents<br />

<strong>and</strong> transients (Fig 3) showed that transients use pulsed calls significantly less often that<br />

resident killer whales (Mann-Whitney test: U = 46, p = 0.029).<br />

Call rate (calls per individual per min)<br />

3.5<br />

3.0<br />

2.5<br />

2.0<br />

1.5<br />

1.0<br />

0.5<br />

0.0<br />

Resident<br />

N=8<br />

Ecotype<br />

Figure 1: Differences in the rate of pulsed calls by fish-eating (resident) <strong>and</strong> mammal-eating (transient) killer<br />

whales across all behaviour categories.<br />

Transient<br />

N=24<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

60


Call rate (calls per individual per min)<br />

1.4<br />

1.2<br />

1.0<br />

0.8<br />

0.6<br />

0.4<br />

0.2<br />

0.0<br />

After Kill<br />

N=6<br />

Behaviour Category<br />

Other Behaviours<br />

N=6<br />

Figure 2: Differences in the rate of pulsed calls when milling after a kill compared to all other behaviour<br />

categories from the 6 encounters during which confirmed kills were observed<br />

Call rate (calls per individual per min)<br />

3.5<br />

3.0<br />

2.5<br />

2.0<br />

1.5<br />

1.0<br />

0.5<br />

0.0<br />

After Kill<br />

N=6<br />

Socializing<br />

N=5<br />

Slow Travel<br />

N=10<br />

Travel<br />

N=15<br />

Behaviour Category<br />

Figure 3: Differences in the rate of pulsed calls across behaviour categories. Horizontal bars give median call<br />

rate, boxes show the interquartile range <strong>and</strong> whiskers give the range. Horizontal lines delineate homogeneous<br />

subsets (p < 0.05).<br />

CONCLUSIONS<br />

The results of this study show that transient killer whales vocalize only infrequently.<br />

Transients vocalized most often while milling after a marine mammal kill, <strong>and</strong> in the 6<br />

encounters where kills could be confirmed, the levels of vocal activity were significantly<br />

elevated in this behaviour context. This shows a strong link between vocal activity <strong>and</strong> the<br />

presence of food in transient killer whales indicating that vocal behaviour in transient killer<br />

whales is to a high degree food-associated (in the sense of Janik, 2000).<br />

In contrast to the findings of Guinet (1992), there is no evidence that the vocal<br />

behaviour recorded in this study served to attract other killer whales. In no instance were<br />

other whales observed to join the focal group when it was vocal, although other groups were<br />

within vocal range on several occasions. In addition, there is no obvious benefit for food<br />

calling in transient killer whales, <strong>and</strong> it is therefore unlikely that the animals recorded in this<br />

Milling<br />

N=4<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

61


study called to signal of the presence of food to conspecifics. The most parsimonious<br />

explanation for why transients vocalize after a kill is the relatively low cost for vocal<br />

behaviour after a successful attack. After a kill the animals are satiated, <strong>and</strong> may not need to<br />

hunt again for some time, so that alerting potential prey animals in the immediate area does<br />

not carry a high cost. In addition, the attacks on marine mammals are noisy since they are<br />

usually accompanied with fast swimming, aerial behaviours <strong>and</strong> hitting or ramming the prey,.<br />

Other potential prey animals in the area may therefore already know that killer whales are<br />

nearby, so that there is no additional cost for vocalizing.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

C. Brignall, N. Dedeluk, D. Matkin, P.-A. Presi, E. Saulitis, G.M. Weingartner, R.M.C.<br />

Williams, M. Wong, <strong>and</strong> H. Yurk provided essential help with the field work. In addition, we<br />

would like to thank L.G. Barrett-Lennard, J.+M. Borrowman, J. DeBoeck, G.M. Ellis, B.+R.<br />

Lamont, B.+D. MacKay, C. Matkin, D. Matkin, A. Morton, E. Saulitis, J. Watson, <strong>and</strong><br />

R.M.C. Williams for valuable logistic support. Financial <strong>and</strong> in kind support came from the<br />

Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre, The BC Wild Killer Whale Adoption Program,<br />

Glacier Bay National Park, <strong>and</strong> the BBC Natural History Unit. During part of this study, VBD<br />

was supported by a DAAD-Doktor<strong>and</strong>enstipendium aus Mitteln des Dritten<br />

Hochschulsonderprogramms.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

Au, W. W. L., Popper, A. N. & Fay, R. R. (ed.) 2000: Hearing by Whales <strong>and</strong> Dolphins.<br />

New York NY: Springer Verlag.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L. G., Ford, J. K. B. & Heise, K. A. 1996: The mixed blessing of<br />

echolocation: Differences in sonar use by fish-eating <strong>and</strong> mammal-eating killer whales.<br />

Animal Behaviour 51:553-565.<br />

Deecke, V. B., Slater, P. J. B. & Ford, J. K. B. in press: Selective habituation shapes<br />

acoustic predator recognition in harbour seals. Nature .<br />

Ford, J. K. B., Ellis, G. M., Barrett-Lennard, L.G., Morton, A. B., Palm, R. & Balcomb,<br />

K. C. 1998: Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whales (Orcinus<br />

orca) in coastal British Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. Canadian Journal of Zoology 76:1456-<br />

1471.<br />

Guinet, C. 1992: Comportement de chasse des orques (Orcinus orca) autour des îles<br />

Crozet. Canadian Journal of Zoology 70:1656-1667.<br />

Hawkins, A. D. & Johnstone, A. D. F. 1978: The hearing of the Atlantic salmon, Salmo<br />

salar. Journal of Fish Biology 13:655-674.<br />

Janik, V. M. 2000: Food-related bray calls in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops<br />

truncatus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267:923-<br />

927.<br />

Renouf, D. 1992: Sensory reception <strong>and</strong> processing in Phocidae <strong>and</strong> Otariidae. In<br />

Behaviour of Pinnipeds (ed. D. Renouf), pp. 345-394. London: Chapman & Hall.<br />

Saulitis, E. L., Matkin, C. O., Barrett-Lennard, L.G., Heise, K. A. & Ellis, G. M. 2000:<br />

Foraging strategies of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William<br />

Sound. Marine Mammal Science 16:94-109.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

62


MIRROR IMAGE PROCESSING IN KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA)<br />

Delfour F..<br />

13 imp. A. Marfaing, 31400 Toulouse, France, fabienne_delfour@yahoo.com.<br />

The mirror self-recognition paradigm has been used with many species of primates to assess the<br />

existence of a cognitive ability allowing individuals to underst<strong>and</strong> mirrored information about the self.<br />

Dolphins <strong>and</strong> their relatives might be expected to show mirror-induced contingency checking, a<br />

prerequisite to self-recognition, because of their high brain development, their complex social life <strong>and</strong><br />

their demonstrated abilities in bodily imitation.<br />

A study of killer whales’ (Orcinus orca) behaviour in front of a mirror is presented here,<br />

including a mark-test. The results showed the presence of contingency checking behaviours in<br />

these animals: repetitive/rhythmical head movement, showing tongue (mouth wide opened)<br />

<strong>and</strong> shaking head, playing with fish in front of the mirror. Moreover the mark test suggested<br />

that the mark individual anticipated that its image would look different.<br />

This study shows that killer whales, like bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), appear to<br />

possess the cognitive abilities required for self-recognition. However we still do not know how an<br />

individual perceives itself. In order to go further in the investigation it would be valuable not just to<br />

search for self-recognition per se but also to explore the functions of self-awareness.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

63


SCALING ISSUES IN PREDATOR-PREY INTERACTIONS: KILLER WHALE<br />

UNDERWATER TAIL-SLAPS<br />

Domenici, P. 1 , Similä, T. 2 , Batty, R.S. 3 .<br />

1 CNR, Località Sa Mardini; Torregr<strong>and</strong>e, (Or) Italy, domenici@barolo.icb.ge.cnr.it;<br />

2 Norwegian Killer Whale Project, Box 181, 8465 Straumsjøen, Norway;<br />

3 Dunstaffnage Marine Laboratory, PO Box 3, Oban , Scotl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Cooperative hunting by killer whales (Orcinus orca) has been reported in a number of<br />

descriptive studies. However, no previous study has provided a quantitative analysis of the<br />

kinematics of killer whales’ attacks on fish. Killer whales feeding on herring (Clupea<br />

harengus) in a fjord in northern Norway were observed using underwater remote-controlled<br />

video. The whales herded herring into a tight school close to the surface, while periodically<br />

lunging at it <strong>and</strong> stunning the herring by slapping them with the underside of their flukes<br />

while completely submerged. Killer whales then ate the stunned herring one by one.<br />

Successful tail-slaps occurred in synchrony with a loud noise. This noise was not heard when<br />

the tail-slaps occasionally “missed” the target, suggesting that the herring were stunned by<br />

physical contact. The kinematics of tail-slapping were analysed in detail. Tail-slaps consisted<br />

of a biphasic behaviour , i.e. two phases with opposite angles of attack, a preparatory phase<br />

<strong>and</strong> a slap phase. During the slap phase, the maximum angle of attack of the flukes was 47°<br />

on average. The maximum speed of the flukes was 2.2 Length s -1 (14 m s -1 ) while the<br />

maximum acceleration of the flukes was size-independent <strong>and</strong> was 48 m s -2 . The theoretical<br />

maximum number of herring hit by a tail-slap ranged between 10-47 individual herring.<br />

Given the high performance of the tail-slaps in terms of speed <strong>and</strong> acceleration, we suggest<br />

that tail-slapping by killer whales when feeding on schooling herring is a more efficient<br />

strategy of prey capture than whole-body attacks, since acceleration <strong>and</strong> manoeuvrability are<br />

likely to be poor in such large vertebrates. Scaling issues of vertebrate predator-prey<br />

interactions involving killer whales will be discussed.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

64


ANALYSIS OF THE DISCRETE CALLS OF KILLER WHALES FROM THE<br />

AVACHA GULF OF KAMCHATKA, FAR EAST RUSSIA<br />

Olga A.Filatova(1), Alex<strong>and</strong>er M.Burdin(2), Haruko Sato(3), Erich Hoyt(4), Karina K.Tarasyan(1),<br />

Alex<strong>and</strong>ra M.Mironova(5)<br />

Moscow State University, Russia (1); Kamchatka Institute of Ecology <strong>and</strong> Nature Management<br />

Petropavlovsk-Kamchatsky, Russia (2); Alaska SeaLife Center, Seward, USA (2); Tokyo, Japan (3);<br />

WDCS, North Berwick, Scotl<strong>and</strong> (4); Sevvostrybvod, Petropavlovsk-Kamchatsky, Russia (5)<br />

Acoustic repertoires of killer whales are well studied in several parts of the world. The<br />

long-term investigations of O. orca acoustic behavior in different parts of the world (Canada<br />

(Ford 1984, 1991), Alaska (Yurk et al., 2002), Norway (Moore et al., 1988)) provide evidence<br />

for the existence of a system of vocal dialects unique for each population. But till recently<br />

Kamchatkian orca population has been unexplored in this way. Our study of acoustic behavior<br />

of killer whales in Avacha Gulf of Kamchatka at the Russian Far East coast of the North<br />

Pacific is the first one of this kind in the area. In this study we tried to make a preliminary<br />

description <strong>and</strong> classification of discrete call types of the Kamchatkian orcas in order to<br />

investigate the structure of the acoustic repertoire <strong>and</strong> to find out whether they have the<br />

system of pod-specific vocal dialects.<br />

We obtained <strong>and</strong> analyzed the sound materials from the three field seasons (September<br />

1999, 2000, <strong>and</strong> August-September 2001), from the groups, which were recognized using the<br />

method of photographic identification. Through these three seasons, we worked for 23 days in<br />

total <strong>and</strong> held 39 orca encounters on our surface activities. We have preliminary Photo-<br />

Identified total of 39 (1999), 86(2000), <strong>and</strong> 151 individuals in 2001.<br />

The preliminary classification of calls was made according to the structural features of<br />

sonograms <strong>and</strong> my own impressions while hearing them. The following parameters were<br />

measured from the sonogram: total sound duration; duration of the main component; initial,<br />

middle <strong>and</strong> final frequency of the main component; minimum <strong>and</strong> maximum frequency of the<br />

main component. The discriminant analysis was made using the program Statistica 5.0. Then<br />

the results of sound classification were compared with information obtained by<br />

photoidentification of each group that we met.<br />

We have found that the acoustic repertoire of Kamchatka O. orca is organized according<br />

to a hierarchical principle: the majority of sounds can be classified into discrete categories<br />

with greater or lesser variability in sound structure, allowing us to discern several subtypes.<br />

We discriminated 27 types <strong>and</strong> 8 subtypes of discrete calls. In the table 1 you can see the<br />

distribution of discrete calls recorded from different groups <strong>and</strong> multi-group aggregations in<br />

2001. Call repertoires differ from each other, but none of the groups has entirely unique<br />

repertoire. Hence we suppose that all the groups we met in this area are the members of one<br />

clan <strong>and</strong> mate with each other, so we consider them to be the one population.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

65


Date Groups<br />

KB, 0017a, 0028a,<br />

q eq eqsh ea eonlybt bwi bd ba clow esv sve chigh svist vizg vlong 13 wv wavc vzw iu i ix in f chavk oa chir kwa<br />

28.8 0028b, 0028c<br />

KA6, KB, 0017a,<br />

+ + + + + + + + + + +<br />

28.8 0028b, 0028c + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

29.8 0129a + +<br />

31.8 KB, 0129b + + + + + + +<br />

31.8 KA6, 0128c, 0017a + + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

31.8 KA6<br />

0017a, 0128c,<br />

31.8 0129b, ?+<br />

+ + +<br />

0017a, 0128c,<br />

31.8 0129b, ?+ + + + + +<br />

2.9 KB + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

2.9 KB, 0128c + + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

2.9 0129a, 0104b + +<br />

4.9 0104a, 0104b + + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

5.9 0104a, 0105 +<br />

5.9 0104a, 0106<br />

5.9 0104a, 0107<br />

+ +<br />

6.9 KB, 0128b +<br />

KB, 9929, 0128b,<br />

+ + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

6.9 0129b + + + + + + + + + + + + + +<br />

6.9 0106 + + + + + + + +<br />

7.9 KB, 9929a +<br />

KB, 9929, 9929a,<br />

+ + + + + +<br />

11.9 0111<br />

0017a, ?9929a,<br />

+ + + + + + + + + + +<br />

12.9 0128c + +<br />

12.9<br />

0017a, ?9929a,<br />

0128c + + + + +<br />

Table 1. Distribution of discrete call types recorded in presence of different groups in 2001<br />

We met supposedly multi-group aggregations on 23 (59%) of our total encounters, so it<br />

was impossible to determine the repertoire of each pod. We have chosen two groups (KB <strong>and</strong><br />

0106) we met alone for the longest time to describe their repertoires <strong>and</strong> compare them with<br />

each other. We discriminated 20 types from the two chosen groups: 18 from the pod KB <strong>and</strong><br />

12 from the pod 0106. Comparison of the repertoires of these groups showed that they shared<br />

9 call types <strong>and</strong> 10 types were unique (8 for the pod KB <strong>and</strong> 2 for the pod 0106)(table2). This<br />

allows us to predict the existence of a system of vocal dialects for the Kamchatka O. orca<br />

population.<br />

KB 0106<br />

q +<br />

ea + +<br />

ex +<br />

bt + +<br />

bwi + +<br />

bd + +<br />

clow + +<br />

ce +<br />

esv + +<br />

vizg + +<br />

vlong +<br />

wvizg +<br />

wavc +<br />

i +<br />

ix +<br />

f + +<br />

d + +<br />

chavk +<br />

g + +<br />

unn +<br />

Table 2. Discrete call repertoires of groups KB <strong>and</strong> 0106<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

66


REFERENCES<br />

Ford J.K.B. Call traditions <strong>and</strong> dialects of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in British<br />

Columbia: University of British Columbia; 1984.<br />

Ford J.K.B. Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal<br />

waters of British Columbia. Can.J.Zool. 1991; 69:1454-1483.<br />

Moore S.E., Francine J.K., Bowles A.E. <strong>and</strong> Ford J.K.B. 1988. North Athlantic killer<br />

whales. Journan of the Marine Research Institute, Reykjavik. Vol. XI<br />

Yurk H., Barrett-Lennard L., Ford J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Matkin C.O. 2002. Cultural transmission<br />

within maternal lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southeastern Alaska. Animal<br />

behavior 63, 1103-1119<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

67


KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) IN SHETLAND WATERS<br />

Fisher P. , Ellis P., Gillham K., Taylor A., Harvey P.<br />

Shetl<strong>and</strong> Sea Mammal Group, Upper Quoy, Billister, North Nesting, Shetl<strong>and</strong> ZE2 9PR, UK,<br />

prfisher@lineone.net<br />

The Shetl<strong>and</strong> Sea Mammal Group is conducting photoidentification work <strong>and</strong> recording<br />

sightings of killer whales (Orcinus orca) around the Shetl<strong>and</strong> Isl<strong>and</strong>s (60°N, 1-2°W). The aim<br />

is to study patterns of occurrence of killer whales in coastal waters, their prey preferences,<br />

hunting strategies <strong>and</strong> social organisation. The number of reported sightings of killer whales<br />

has increased markedly in recent years, from less than 20 sightings per year prior to 1993,<br />

averaging over 50 sightings per year in 1994 - 2000. Pods are seen almost year-round, with<br />

peaks in sightings in most years during June <strong>and</strong> July. This may reflect pods returning to<br />

favoured feeding sites (e.g. seal haul-out sites), or an inshore movement of killer whales from<br />

different aggregations in the North Atlantic. Observed group sizes range from one to 15<br />

animals, with a tendency for smaller group size during winter than summer. The killer whales<br />

appear to live in stable family groups. Marine mammals contribute an important component<br />

of their diet, with as many as four seals taken during one feeding episode <strong>and</strong> most l<strong>and</strong>-based<br />

sightings close to seal haul-outs. There is also one record of a harbour porpoise (Phocoena<br />

phocoena) taken by killer whales. A juvenile minke whale (Balenoptera acutorostrata) was<br />

reportedly chased <strong>and</strong> later str<strong>and</strong>ed in Levenwick Bay in July 1997. Killer whales also feed<br />

on shoals of mackerel (Scomber scombrus) found inshore, occasionally close to line fishing<br />

boats. Preliminary acoustic studies suggest that pods are not very vocal whilst transiting along<br />

the coast in search of marine mammals. Despite concern from environmental groups, no<br />

studies have been conducted on potential impacts of increased aquaculture activity on marine<br />

mammals in the area. Whale watching is becoming an important part of local tourism industry<br />

<strong>and</strong> killer whales may be seen as a flagship species to promote eco-tourism in the isles.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

68


IS KILLER WHALE (ORCINUS ORCA) BEHAVIOUR DIURNAL? (Poster)<br />

Foote A. 42 Barfield Avenue, London, N20 0DD, UK, <strong>and</strong>rew_foote@hotmail.com<br />

ACOUSTIC RESEARCH PROJECTS FROM THE SEASOUND WORKING<br />

GROUP, SAN JUAN ISLAND, WASHINGTON, USA.<br />

Foote, A. Jones , R. Osbourne., V. Veirs., <strong>and</strong> M. Yoder Williams.<br />

Contact: a.d.foote@durham.ac.uk<br />

The Whale Museum, First Street, Friday Harbour, WA, USA. 98250.<br />

The SeaSound remote sensing network is centred around the installation of remote<br />

video cameras <strong>and</strong> a passive underwater acoustic network for sensing the marine environment<br />

of Haro Strait along the border of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State, (fig 1.).<br />

Fig 1.<br />

Live video images originate from digital remote cameras at Lime Kiln State Park <strong>and</strong><br />

the Oval Research Station on San Juan Isl<strong>and</strong>. The underwater acoustic signals are broadcast<br />

from an FM radio station (89.1). The primary mission of the SeaSound Remote Sensing<br />

Network had been to facilitate scientific research, education <strong>and</strong> stewardship of the whales<br />

<strong>and</strong> the ecosystem that supports them through the use of cutting edge technology.<br />

Independent of the individual collaborative studies described below The Whale<br />

Museum’s focus over the summer of 2002 has been to maintain its archive of every orca passby<br />

recorded by the SeaSound systems so that they are available to any interested investigators.<br />

The other focus of the museum’s efforts this summer has been on improving performance <strong>and</strong><br />

obtaining finer calibration of the remote sensing technology. On the horizon is potential<br />

funding to collect systematic sound-source measurements on various types of vessels under<br />

varying conditions using the SeaSound systems. The results of the study would then be<br />

applied to further developing guidelines for whale watching vessels that minimize noise<br />

interference to the whales. This summer’s effort to have a truly calibrated system at the<br />

O.V.A.L. array has been a necessary preliminary step in being able to undertake the accurate<br />

measurements such a study requires.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

69


PRESENT PROJECTS.<br />

Val Veirs, Professor of Physics, Colorado College, Colorado Springs, USA.<br />

I direct the Colorado College OVAL project (<strong>Orca</strong> Vocalisation <strong>and</strong> Localisation.) Each<br />

spring a group of 8-10 Colorado College students come to San Juan Isl<strong>and</strong> to study spatial<br />

relationships between vocalising orcas. We use an array of eight international transducer<br />

corporation (ITC) hydrophones deployed on fixed underwater st<strong>and</strong>s at the locations shown.<br />

Time differences are measured <strong>and</strong> localisations are performed using computer codes written<br />

by the OVAL team.<br />

Results of one pass by are shown here.<br />

On June 25, at 10 p.m., orcas vocalized with calls <strong>and</strong> echolocation clicks interspersed<br />

as they passed by southbound. The graphs below show the location <strong>and</strong> sequence of selected<br />

echolocation clicks <strong>and</strong> calls from this pass by.<br />

E's show the reconstructed locations of echolocation clicks <strong>and</strong> C's show the locations<br />

of calls. The colour coding shows the time sequence over the 25 min of this pass by. Notice<br />

that most of the 83 localized echolocations <strong>and</strong> calls are in the first half of the pass by (red<br />

shading to green). The calls come from a region that is about 150m in length <strong>and</strong> ~40 m wide<br />

while the echolocation clicks come from a much larger region which includes the calling area.<br />

Hence the active hunting for fish (echolocation) is being performed over a domain that is<br />

much larger than the localisations of the interspersed calling whales.<br />

Andrew Foote, MSc. student, Durham University.<br />

I am collaborating with The Whale Museum <strong>and</strong> The Center For Whale Research,<br />

using data collected over the last several decades to investigate the relationship between<br />

changes in association patterns over time, pod repertoires <strong>and</strong> the degree of call sharing<br />

among Southern Resident pods. I am also assessing possible correlations between call<br />

function <strong>and</strong> the degree of within call variability <strong>and</strong> investigating these data in the context of<br />

changes in the call's structural parameters over time. Assessment of temporal variation in<br />

acoustic behaviour over shorter time scales is being undertaken to look for possible<br />

correlations with foraging behaviour, diurnal activity, <strong>and</strong> background noise levels.<br />

Guenevere Jones, PhD. Student, University of Washington.<br />

I am interested in writing a computer program that can learn to recognize salient<br />

features in killer whale vocalizations. I will be using the SeaSound hydrophone array to<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

70


localize the vocalizations of individual whales. I will then feed the whale vocalizations into<br />

various computer learning programs (e.g. neural nets, genetic programs) along with the visual<br />

IDs of the whales to determine if there are features of the vocalizations that are individually<br />

distinctive. In addition, other information on the whales, such as age, sex, <strong>and</strong> activity, may be<br />

fed into the computer program to be correlated with the whale vocalizations.<br />

Molly Yoder Williams, Undergraduate, Colorado College.<br />

I am doing a simple analysis of archived recordings from Lime Kiln Lighthouse. My<br />

study is focused specifically on call trains of a particular call commonly used within the<br />

Southern Resident community. By examining <strong>and</strong> comparing a number of basic parameters, I<br />

hope to better classify <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong> one aspect of this species’ acoustic behaviour. I<br />

addition, correlation will be made with older recordings, to show change over time, <strong>and</strong> others<br />

from different locations within the populations range, for locality based comparisons.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

71


REASSESSING THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF RESIDENT KILLER WHALES<br />

IN BRITISH COLUMBIA<br />

Ford J.K.B.; Ellis, G.M.<br />

Pacific Biological Station, Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada, Nanaimo, BC V9R 5K6 ;<br />

ford@zoology.ubc.ca<br />

Killer whales in coastal waters of British Columbia have been studied intensively since<br />

the early 1970s by means of individual photo-identification. Using data collected during<br />

1973-87, M.A. Bigg et al. (1990) classified the social organization of fish-feeding ‘resident’<br />

whales into five groupings according to 1) the relative strength of bonds of individuals <strong>and</strong><br />

groups from direct observations <strong>and</strong> co-occurrence in frames of identification photographs, or<br />

2) similarity in acoustic repertoire. These levels of social organization were defined as<br />

follows:<br />

- Matrilineal group: Group of individuals that always travel together <strong>and</strong> in close<br />

proximity to one another. The groups are matrifocal. Genealogically, a matrilineal<br />

group is a matriline of 1 to 4 generations (most 2-3), from which there is no dispersal<br />

of individuals.<br />

- Subpod: Closely related matrilineal groups that almost always (>95%) travel with one<br />

another. Most subpods contain one or two matrilineal groups.<br />

- Pod: Subpod(s) that travel with one another the majority of the time (> 50%)<br />

- Clan: An acoustic grouping of pods that share one or more discrete calls, indicating<br />

that they share a common distant ancestor. Most pods exhibit little preference for<br />

travelling with other pods within their clan<br />

- Community: Pods that share a common range <strong>and</strong> associate with one another.<br />

Bigg et al. also proposed that new subpods <strong>and</strong> pods form through a gradual process of<br />

matrilineal fission, which may take many decades to complete. In exp<strong>and</strong>ing populations,<br />

most pods would be undergoing this process of fission. In populations that are at equilibrium<br />

or are declining, most social groups would be stable or dying out.<br />

Our main objective in the current analysis is to examine the fate of matrilineal groups (=<br />

matrilines), subpods <strong>and</strong> pods in the northern resident community over an extended study<br />

period (1973-2000), in order to assess the validity <strong>and</strong> utility of social organization as defined<br />

by Bigg et al. 1990. We also attempt to describe the process of fission <strong>and</strong> fusion in the<br />

community, <strong>and</strong> identify possible demographic factors that affect social evolution within<br />

lineages. Data for this analysis were compiled from a number of sources, <strong>and</strong> included<br />

encounters with either photographically or visually identified killer whale groups.<br />

Association analyses were undertaken at the level of the matriline (= matrilineal group of<br />

Bigg et al. 1990), which is the basic unit of social structure <strong>and</strong> has consistent membership.<br />

Associations of matrilines were determined from their co-occurrence in encounters.<br />

Matrilines travelling in the same direction <strong>and</strong> within acoustic range of one another (typically<br />

< 5 km apart), were considered to be associating. Strength of associations were determined<br />

from the proportion of encounters in which a pod’s matrilines were together, <strong>and</strong> with the<br />

Simple Ratio index of association (calculated with the program Socprog 1.2, provided by H.<br />

Whitehead, Dalhousie University).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

72


A total of 16 pods were described for the northern resident community by Bigg et al.<br />

(1990), comprised of 36 matrilines. The community exp<strong>and</strong>ed by 60% over the course of the<br />

study, from 129 animals in 1975 to 208 in 2000. Northern residents were encountered <strong>and</strong><br />

identified on 2979 occasions between 1973-2000. A mean of 4.4 matrilines (± 2.78 SD) were<br />

present per encounter. Association analyses for the 16 northern resident pods revealed<br />

considerable fluidity in the bonds among member matrilines across years, <strong>and</strong> many pods<br />

underwent fission during the study period, particularly during the late 1980s <strong>and</strong> 1990s. An<br />

example is illustrated in the figure below, which shows the proportion of encounters with pod<br />

A1 in which all three member matrilines were together. Although this pod generally met the<br />

definition of a pod during the first half of the study period, this was clearly not the case after<br />

the mid 1980s.<br />

Proportion of total encounters<br />

1<br />

0.75<br />

0.5<br />

0.25<br />

0<br />

1972 1976 1980 1984 1988 1992 1996 2000<br />

Proportion of A1 pod encounters with all three matrilines (A12, A30, A36) together, 1973-2000. n = 1912<br />

encounters<br />

More dynamic patterns of fission <strong>and</strong> fusion were seen within other northern resident<br />

pods. In some pods, member matrilines were together the majority of encounters in certain<br />

years, <strong>and</strong> completely apart in others. Preferred associates of numerous matrilines were often<br />

matrilines from other pods, particularly in recent years. Overall, of the 16 northern resident<br />

pods described by Bigg et al. 1990, only 8 (50%) still appeared to the meet the definition by<br />

the year 2000. Of these, 4 pods were composed of single matrilines, which would not be<br />

expected to undergo fission. All pods that contained two or more matrilines <strong>and</strong> increased in<br />

size underwent fission between the mid 1980s <strong>and</strong> 2000. Demographic factors influencing<br />

pod splitting may include the death of a matriarch in the matriline, maturation of offspring,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the proportion of males in the matriline.<br />

We conclude that existing definitions of social structure based on group associations are<br />

neither reliable nor particularly useful because 1) bonds among groups are often dynamic <strong>and</strong><br />

ephemeral, 2) definitions are artificial because they depend on arbitrary measures of bond<br />

Year<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

73


strength <strong>and</strong> time scales, <strong>and</strong> 3) bonds are often independent of kinship. An exception to this<br />

is the community, which includes all matrilines that associate with one another <strong>and</strong> which<br />

forms the breeding population. We suggest that definitions of resident society based on<br />

maternal genealogy are preferable, as such groupings are 1) temporally stable, 2) independent<br />

of fluctuating group affiliations, <strong>and</strong> 3) reflect the true population structure. Thus, the most<br />

useful definitions of social structure within a resident killer whale community are the<br />

matriline <strong>and</strong> clan. The term pod should perhaps be used generically to describe any<br />

aggregation of whales, without implication as to structure, or alternatively as a synonym for<br />

the matriline.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

This long-term study would not have been possible without the assistance of many<br />

individuals <strong>and</strong> organizations over the years. For important contributions to the database of<br />

northern resident encounters, we thank D. Bain, L. Barrett-Lennard, D. Briggs, J. Borrowman,<br />

V. Deecke, B. Falconer, C. Guinet, K. Hansen, K. Heise, S. Hutchings, J. Jacobsen, B.<br />

Mackay, A. Morton, L Nichol, R. Palm, A. Spong, P. Spong, H. Symonds, F. Thomsen, J.<br />

Watson, E. White, S. Wichniowski, <strong>and</strong> R. Williams.<br />

REFERENCE<br />

Bigg, M.A., P.F. Olesiuk, G.M. Ellis, J.K.B. Ford, <strong>and</strong> K.C. Balcomb III. 1990. Social organization <strong>and</strong><br />

genealogy of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the coastal waters of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington<br />

State. Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission (Special Issue 12): 383-405.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

74


BEHAVIOURAL AND ACOUSTIC DIFFERENCES AMONG BELUGAS<br />

(DELPHINAPTERUS LEUCAS) SUMMER HERDS IN THE ST. LAWRENCE<br />

ESTUARY (QUEBEC, CANADA).<br />

Godefroid A. 1 , Cloutier R. 2 , Michaud R. 3 , Desrosiers G. 1 .<br />

1 Institut des Sciences de la Mer (ISMER), Rimouski (Québec, Canada ), spiroutonio@hotmail.com;<br />

2 Université du Québec à Rimouski, Département de biologie, Rimouski (Québec, Canada);<br />

3 Centre d’Interprétation des Mammifères marins, Tadoussac (Québec, Canada).<br />

The estuary <strong>and</strong> gulf of the St. Lawrence river represent the southern limit of the world<br />

distribution of the belugas. During the summer, belugas are distributed along the St. Lawrence<br />

estuary whereas, they are wintering in the gulf gathered in large herds. During the summer,<br />

they gathered in three types of herds : (1) adult, (2) adult <strong>and</strong> juvenile <strong>and</strong> (3) mixed. A recent<br />

photo-identification study has found a strong social segregation between different female’s<br />

«communities» <strong>and</strong> male’s «networks», each exhibing strong site fidelity. Our purpose was to<br />

examine variations in behavioural activity <strong>and</strong> vocal repertoire of the beluga in different<br />

sectors frequented by different communities or networks at three specific sites (i.e., Baie-<br />

Sainte-Marguerite, Cap-Chien, <strong>and</strong> Isle of Kamouraska). Swimming behavioural activities<br />

(i.e., directional, multidirectional, <strong>and</strong> milling) <strong>and</strong> the vocal repertoire (i.e., 7 types of<br />

whistles, 3 burst pulsed sounds categories, 2 types of echolocation clics <strong>and</strong> 3 categories for<br />

mixed <strong>and</strong> noisy vocalisations, <strong>and</strong> jaw claps) have been quantified for each type of herd.<br />

Behavioural <strong>and</strong> acoustical (i.e., 32 hours of recordings) data were collected from June to<br />

September, from l<strong>and</strong> sites, in each one of the sites. Analyses of behavioural data show that<br />

one type of herd <strong>and</strong> one swimming behaviour are predominant on each of the three sites (i.e.,<br />

64% of milling at Baie-Sainte-Marguerite with 73,5% of adult herds encountered, 44% of<br />

directional swimming at Cap-Chien with 44.5% of adult <strong>and</strong> juvenile herds encountered, <strong>and</strong><br />

41% of multidirectional swimming at Kamouraska with 56% of mixed herds encountered).<br />

Acoustical data show some differences concerning the proportion of sounds for some<br />

categories of whistles, clics, burst pulsed sounds <strong>and</strong> jaw claps that is differing from one site<br />

to an other. These results will permit a more precise sight on the intra-population organisation<br />

of St. Lawrence river’s belugas during the summer.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

75


CURRENT KNOWLEDGE OF KILLER WHALES IN THE MEXICAN PACIFIC<br />

Guerrero-Ruiz M., Urbán-R.J., Gendron D., Flores de Sahagún V.<br />

Programa de Investigación de Mamíferos Marinos. Departamento de Biología Marina. Universidad<br />

Autónoma de Baja California Sur. A.P. 19-B. La Paz, B.C.S. 23081. México, megr@uabcs.mx<br />

Killer whales are widely distributed along the Pacific coast of Mexico, although they are<br />

only occasionally seen in some areas. Because the population status of killer whales in<br />

Mexican waters is not clear, the purpose of this report is to present current information on the<br />

distribution, movements, communities, <strong>and</strong> feeding habits of killer whales in the Mexican<br />

Pacific. This long-term study has been based on sighting data <strong>and</strong> individual photoidentification,<br />

using the shape <strong>and</strong> size of the dorsal fin <strong>and</strong> saddle patch of killer whales. We<br />

have compiled more than 200 sightings in the Mexican Pacific from published <strong>and</strong><br />

unpublished records from 1858-2002. More than 850 individuals have been sighted with<br />

group sizes that have ranged from 1-40. More than 90 individuals have been photo-identified<br />

<strong>and</strong> more than 30 have been documented more than once. The longest period between reencounters<br />

of a known individual has been more than 13 years. Based on the individual<br />

associations, we have suggested four communities of killer whales as temporal inhabitants of<br />

the Gulf of California. Some individuals belonging to these communities have been moving<br />

inside <strong>and</strong> outside the Gulf of California, along the Mexican Pacific, up to California. Killer<br />

whales in Mexican waters have been seen feeding on baleen whales, dolphins, seals, sea lions,<br />

sea turtles, sharks, sunfishes, <strong>and</strong> manta rays. Also, throughout these years, six individual<br />

str<strong>and</strong>ings <strong>and</strong> one live mass str<strong>and</strong>ing were recorded as well as a sighting of killer whales<br />

with remoras. Even though these killer whales have not been linked to any type of killer<br />

whales they might be similar to the transient type because of their feeding habits, nevertheless<br />

genetic <strong>and</strong> acoustic studies are needed to confirm this statement, as well as to determine<br />

whether or not these animals belong to a different population of killer whales.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

76


ANALYSIS OF PHOTO-IDENTIFICATION DATA TO MAKE INFERENCES<br />

ABOUT CETACEAN POPULATIONS<br />

Philip Hammond<br />

Sea Mammal Research Unit, University of St Andrews, Fife KY16 8LB, UK<br />

This presentation explores the use of statistical analysis of photo-identification data to<br />

provide information on abundance, survival, reproduction <strong>and</strong> movements of cetaceans. The<br />

examples given relate to humpback whales <strong>and</strong> bottlenose dolphins, but the methods used <strong>and</strong><br />

the information obtainable are highly relevant to killer whale research in many parts of the<br />

world.<br />

ABUNDANCE OF WEST GREENLAND HUMPBACK WHALES<br />

Most humpback whales in the North Atlantic calve <strong>and</strong> mate around the West Indies in<br />

winter <strong>and</strong> all feed in high latitudes in summer. Although animals from all feeding areas mix<br />

on the breeding grounds, maternally directed migration leads to high site fidelity of animals to<br />

particular feeding areas (Gulf of Maine, Eastern Canada, West Greenl<strong>and</strong>, Icel<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Norway). The West Greenl<strong>and</strong> feeding aggregation can therefore be considered as a separate<br />

population. This population has been subject to a traditional subsistence hunt by native<br />

Greenl<strong>and</strong>ers <strong>and</strong> to commercial whaling operations in the 1920s. An estimated 800+ animals<br />

have been taken from the population since 1886.<br />

To estimate abundance, a photo-identification study was conducted in coastal waters off<br />

West Greenl<strong>and</strong> in 1988-1993 (Larsen & Hammond, in review); the final two years as part of<br />

project YoNAH (Smith et al. 1999). During systematic coverage in July <strong>and</strong> August each year<br />

670 groups of humpbacks were encountered <strong>and</strong> a 348 individuals were identified. Animals<br />

were mostly seen along the inshore edges of banks, particularly along the 200m depth<br />

contour.<br />

With these data there are two options for analysis: an open population model, such as<br />

the Jolly-Seber model applied to all the data simultaneously; or calculating sequential<br />

estimates for pairs of years using the closed population Petersen model. The latter method was<br />

adopted for two reasons. First it gives more precise estimates of abundance if the assumptions<br />

of the method are met, which they were judged to have been (with one exception - see<br />

following point). Second, it allowed a new method of incorporating matching errors to be<br />

taken into account (Stevick et al. 2001), which was believed to be important. This method<br />

utilises data on the false negative error rate in matching as a function of photographic quality.<br />

A series of analyses were run for each pair of years including increasingly poor<br />

photographs in the data sets. This was done to find a balance between (a) using only very<br />

good quality photographs, being certain of not missing matches (no bias), but utilising a small<br />

proportion of the available data (poor precision) <strong>and</strong> (b) using all photographs to increase<br />

precision but risking a bias because of missed matches resulting from poor quality images.<br />

The Mean Square Error of the estimates (bias 2 + variance) was used to determine the best<br />

estimates.<br />

Results showed that all photographs should be included for the best estimate, probably<br />

because the bias had been largely eliminated by incorporation of the matching error<br />

correction. The estimates for each pair of years were highly consistent (four were between<br />

348-376) with one outlier (566 in 1990-91). The outlier had poor precision resulting from a<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

77


low number of recaptures <strong>and</strong> may also have been positively biased due to small sample bias.<br />

There was no evidence of a trend <strong>and</strong> average abundance for the period was estimated at 360<br />

animals with a coefficient of variation of 7%.<br />

SURVIVAL OF BOTTLENOSE DOLPHINS IN THE SADO ESTUARY<br />

The Sado Estuary in Portugal is home to a very small resident population of bottlenose<br />

dolphins (Gaspar et al. in prep). There are other bottlenose dolphins outside the Estuary <strong>and</strong><br />

animals from inside <strong>and</strong> outside are sometimes seen together, but the Sado animals are<br />

effectively a separate population. The Sado Estuary is a National Nature Reserve <strong>and</strong> the<br />

dolphins have been studied since 1981. Eighty-nine animals have been identified in the 20<br />

years of study; 29 calves <strong>and</strong> 60 adults. Residence has been determined by examining<br />

monthly sighting rates of individuals. The distribution is clearly bimodal with a hiatus<br />

between animals seen rarely (classified as non-residents) <strong>and</strong> animals seen frequently<br />

(residents).<br />

Almost all individually identified Sado bottlenose dolphins are seen almost every year<br />

so there is no need to estimate abundance; the data are effectively a census. Numbers of<br />

resident animals declined from around 40 in the late 1980s to a low of 30 in 1997. This trend,<br />

combined with a lack of any sub-adult animals maturing into adults prompted concern about<br />

the viability of the population. Attempts to explain the decline have focussed on using the<br />

photo-identification data to estimate survival rates (a) for adults, sub-adults <strong>and</strong> calves, <strong>and</strong><br />

(b) for different periods of time during the study period (Gaspar & Hammond, in prep).<br />

Data on births were also examined. The number of births <strong>and</strong> the crude birth rate<br />

(births/total population size) were lower prior to 1994 than from 1994 onwards. These data<br />

must be treated with caution, however, because differences in data collection methods mean<br />

that very young animals are more likely to have been missed in the earlier years of the study.<br />

Survival models estimate, at minimum, survival probabilities (rates) <strong>and</strong> capture<br />

probabilities. These can be time independent (in which case the estimated values are averages<br />

for the study period) or can be allowed to vary with time. In this analysis, models were<br />

applied in which both these parameters were time independent or varied over two or three<br />

periods. For young animals, for which the year of birth was known or could be reliably<br />

assigned, age-specific survival rates were estimated (1, 2, 3, <strong>and</strong> 4+ years). Analyses were run<br />

using program MARK (http://www.cnr.colostate.edu/~gwhite/mark/mark.htm) <strong>and</strong> Akaike’s<br />

Information Criterion (AIC) was used for model selection.<br />

Two models had most support from the data on adult survival. One showed a period of<br />

high survival during 1986-89 <strong>and</strong> a period of lower survival 1990-2000. The other showed<br />

variation over three periods with survival increasing again in 1999-2000. There is thus strong<br />

evidence for a period of low adult survival at least in the period 1990-1998.<br />

For young animals, one model had most support from the data. This showed survival<br />

varying over two periods of time 1984-1993 <strong>and</strong> 1994-2000. In the early period, first year<br />

survival was very high which concurs with the likelihood of missed calves in this period.<br />

Second <strong>and</strong> third year survival were similar <strong>and</strong> were combined into a single estimate, which<br />

was low in the early period <strong>and</strong> high in the later period. Sub-adult (age 4+) survival was also<br />

considerably lower in the early than the later period. This latter result concurs with the<br />

observed lack of recruitment to the adult population. Thus there is strong evidence for a<br />

period of low calf <strong>and</strong> sub-adult survival through 1993.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

78


Overall, there is some evidence for low reproduction <strong>and</strong> strong evidence for low calf<br />

<strong>and</strong> sub-adult survival in 1984-1993. This may have triggered the decline in numbers in the<br />

late 1980s. The decline may then have continued because of low adult survival 1990-1998.<br />

Reasons for the decline are still unknown. From 1999 onwards, the results show higher<br />

reproduction <strong>and</strong> survival rates all round, which may auger well for the future.<br />

RECENT RANGE EXPANSION OF BOTTLENOSE DOLPHINS OFF<br />

EASTERN SCOTLAND<br />

The recent establishment of a Special Area of Conservation (SAC) for bottlenose<br />

dolphins in the Moray Frith (NE Scotl<strong>and</strong>) has focussed attention on this small, resident, high<br />

latitude population. Relevant to how well the SAC will provide protection for the population<br />

is a suspected range expansion indicated by an increase in incidental sightings of dolphins in<br />

the outer Moray Firth <strong>and</strong> then south to St Andrews Bay during the late 1990s. Because this<br />

could be indicative of the discovery of an existing range driven by an increased awareness <strong>and</strong><br />

interest following the establishment of the SAC, rather than a true range expansion, photoidentification<br />

data were used to investigate this systematically (Wilson et al., in review). A<br />

subset of 54 well-marked animals that were alive in 1990-92 <strong>and</strong> still alive in 1998-2000 was<br />

used in analysis. Animals were divided into three groups: (a) inner Moray Firth only; (b) inner<br />

+ outer Moray Firth; (c) inner + outer Moray Firth + south to St Andrews Bay.<br />

Previous work had shown a clear spatial structuring of the population with animals seen<br />

most frequently also seen closer to the head of the inner Moray Firth (Inverness), with this<br />

pattern being maintained seasonally (Wilson et al. 1997). This pattern has been conserved<br />

with the inner Moray Firth only animals seen closer to Inverness <strong>and</strong> more frequently than the<br />

others. Both these difference are highly significant. So there is clear evidence of a spatially<br />

structured population with some individuals consistently staying close to the head of the inner<br />

Moray Firth <strong>and</strong> others consistently spending less time there. If the range had truly exp<strong>and</strong>ed,<br />

we would expect to see the “outer Moray Firth” animals (groups b <strong>and</strong> c) less <strong>and</strong> less<br />

frequently through the 1990s because these are the animals that would be moving further <strong>and</strong><br />

further afield over time. This pattern was indeed observed.<br />

A second, less obvious, line of evidence involves investigating the cause of death <strong>and</strong><br />

distribution of str<strong>and</strong>ed harbour porpoises. Bottlenose dolphins attack <strong>and</strong> kill harbour<br />

porpoises (Ross & Wilson, 1996) <strong>and</strong> animals that die in this way can be identified in post<br />

mortem examinations through characteristic signs (multiple fractures, ruptured internal<br />

organs, distinctive cuts <strong>and</strong> bruises). We compared the distribution of str<strong>and</strong>ed porpoises<br />

dying as a result of violent interactions with bottlenose dolphins with the distribution of those<br />

dying from other causes (a total of >200 str<strong>and</strong>ings). In 1990-95, “other causes” str<strong>and</strong>ings<br />

were distributed all down the coast to St Andrews Bay (<strong>and</strong> beyond). This pattern was the<br />

same in 1996-2000. There was no difference in the median distance of str<strong>and</strong>ings from<br />

Inverness. “Violent interactions with bottlenose dolphin” str<strong>and</strong>ings were distributed similarly<br />

in 1996-2000 but in the period 1990-1995 very few were seen south to St Andrews Bay. And<br />

there was a significant difference in the median distance from Inverness. This implies that<br />

there were few bottlenose dolphins in the southern part of the area in the early 1990s.<br />

In summary, there is clear evidence for range expansion. All animals seen outside the<br />

inner Moray Firth were also seen in the inner Moray Firth so all animals belong to the same<br />

population. Outer Moray Firth animals are still being seen in the inner Moray Firth so this is a<br />

range expansion, not a range shift. And the spatial stratification in distribution in the inner<br />

Moray Firth has been conserved through the 1990s; the dolphins seen farthest from Inverness<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

79


are the outer Moray Firth animals. For the str<strong>and</strong>ings, the distribution of porpoises that died as<br />

result of violent interactions with bottlenose dolphins has extended south during the 1990s.<br />

The observed range expansion is most likely related to food availability. If prey had<br />

declined in the inner Moray Firth, animals may have been forced to range more widely to find<br />

food, or new prey resources may have been discovered by wide ranging individuals, or both.<br />

The spatial stratification is interesting in this context. If some animals now spend more time<br />

outside the inner Moray Firth, why do some animals stay all the time in the inner Moray<br />

Firth? Is this because of partitioning of prey resources, or competition for resources?<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Gaspar, R., Silva, A., Harzen, S. & dos Santos, M.E. (in prep). Long-term photoidentification<br />

study of bottlenose dolphins in the Sado Estuary: residency, population size <strong>and</strong><br />

reproductive parameters.<br />

Gaspar, R. & Hammond, P.S. (in prep). Changes in survival <strong>and</strong> other population<br />

parameters in a very small bottlenose dolphin population.<br />

Larsen, F. & Hammond, P.S. (in review). Distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of West<br />

Greenl<strong>and</strong> humpback whales. Canadian Journal of Zoology.<br />

Ross, H.M. <strong>and</strong> Wilson, B. (1996). Violent interactions between bottlenose dolphins<br />

<strong>and</strong> harbour porpoises. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, B. 263: 283-286.<br />

Smith, T.D., Allen, J., Clapham, P.J., Hammond, P.S., Katona, S.K., Larsen, F., Lien, J.,<br />

Mattila, D.K., Sigurjónsson, J., Stevick, P., Øien, N. (1999). A ocean-basin-wide markrecapture<br />

study of the North Atlantic humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae). Marine<br />

Mammal Science 15: 1-32.<br />

Stevick, P.T., Palsbøll, P., Smith, T.D., Bravington, M.V. & Hammond, P.S. (2001).<br />

Errors in identification using natural markings: rates, sources <strong>and</strong> effects on capture-recapture<br />

estimates of abundance. Canadian Journal of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Aquatic Sciences 58: 1861-1870.<br />

Wilson, B., Thompson, P.M. & Hammond, P.S. (1997). Habitat use by bottlenose<br />

dolphins: seasonal distribution <strong>and</strong> stratified movement patterns in the Moray Firth, Scotl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Journal of Applied Ecology 34: 1365-1374.<br />

Wilson, B., Hammond, P.S., Reid, R.J., Grellier, K. & Thompson, P.M. (in review).<br />

Recent range expansion in North Sea bottlenose dolphins: evidence <strong>and</strong> management<br />

implications. Conservation Biology.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

80


ASSESSING THE IMPACTS OF KILLER WHALE PREDATION ON STELLER SEA<br />

LIONS IN WESTERN ALASKA<br />

Heise, K. 1 , Barrett-Lennard, L.G. 1,2 , Martell, S. 3 , Saulitis, E. 4 , Matkin, C. 4 , DeMaster, D. 5 , Trites, A. 6<br />

1 Dept. of Zoology, University of British Columbia, 6270 University Blvd., Vancouver, BC., V6T<br />

1K8; : heise@zoology.ubc.ca<br />

2 Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre, Box 3232., Vancouver, B.C. V6B 3X8;<br />

3 Fisheries Centre, 6248 Biological Sciences Road., Vancouver, V6T 1Z4;<br />

4 North Gulf Oceanic Society, 60920 Mary Allen Ave., Homer, AK 99603;<br />

5 National Marine Mammal Laboratory, Alaska Fisheries Science Centre, 7600 S<strong>and</strong> Point Way NE,<br />

Seattle WA 98115;<br />

6 Marine Mammal Research Consortium, Fisheries Centre, 6248 Biological Sciences Road, Vancouver,<br />

BC V6T 1Z4.<br />

The western Alaskan population of Steller sea lion (Eumetopias jubatus) has declined<br />

dramatically from approximately 264,000 in the late 1970's to as few as 35,000 to 42,000<br />

animals in 2000. The discovery of flipper tags from 14 Steller sea lions in the stomach of a<br />

dead killer whale (Orcinus orca) in 1992 focused attention on the possible role that killer<br />

whale predation may have played in this 85% decline. We compiled killer whale stomach<br />

content information <strong>and</strong> surveyed mariners in order to better underst<strong>and</strong> the evidence for<br />

killer whale predation on Steller sea lions. We then used simulation models to test the<br />

hypothesis that killer whale predation could have caused the decline.<br />

We began by reporting the results of stomach content analyses of 8 transient/ transient<br />

like killer whales from Alaska. One stomach was empty, <strong>and</strong> only 2 contained Steller sea lion<br />

remains. Harbour seals appeared to be the predominant prey, <strong>and</strong> were found in all 7<br />

stomachs containing remains (one was empty). Two stomachs contained Dall’s porpoise, 1<br />

contained beluga, 1 contained river otter <strong>and</strong> 1 contained bird feathers. We also surveyed<br />

mariners to determine the frequency <strong>and</strong> outcome of observed attacks on sea lions, the age<br />

classes of sea lions taken, <strong>and</strong> the areas where predatory attacks occurred. We received 126<br />

responses to our survey from mariners from Alaska <strong>and</strong> British Columbia. They described<br />

492 interactions between killer whales <strong>and</strong> sea lions, of which 32 were predatory attacks.<br />

Most of the described attacks were of adult sea lions, however repeated visits by transient<br />

killer whales to rookeries <strong>and</strong> haulouts suggest that pups <strong>and</strong> juveniles may be attacked <strong>and</strong><br />

consumed underwater.<br />

We used this information, along with Steller sea lion life history data, to build stochastic<br />

age-structured predation models designed to test the hypothesis that the decline was driven by<br />

killer whale predation. We then generated likelihood profiles to estimate the number of<br />

transient killer whales <strong>and</strong> the proportion of Steller sea lions in their diet that would have been<br />

sufficient to cause the decline. Our results indicate that a population of 250 transient killer<br />

whales could account for the observed decline of sea lions in western Alaska if a dietary shift<br />

occurred in the early 1970’s such that an additional 28% of their caloric intake was provided<br />

by sea lions (Figure 1). The model further indicates that as few as 120 transients, with 20%<br />

more of their diet supplied by sea lions than prior to the decline, could effectively trap the<br />

present population of western Alaskan sea lions in a predator pit (Walters 1986) <strong>and</strong> prevent it<br />

from recovering.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

81


It is plausible that earlier declines of alternative prey (e.g. northern fur seals <strong>and</strong> harbor<br />

seals) led killer whales to shift a greater proportion of their diet to Steller sea lions in the<br />

1970’s. Our models indicate that a substantial impact of killer whale predation on the western<br />

Steller sea lion populations cannot be ruled out with available data. We therefore recommend<br />

that future field studies should focus on determining the number of transient killer whales in<br />

western Alaska <strong>and</strong> the proportion of Steller sea lions in their diet. We also think it is<br />

important to investigate the impact of killer whale predation on other species such as harbour<br />

seals, fur seals <strong>and</strong> porpoises in western Alaska, <strong>and</strong> on sea lion stocks in other areas.<br />

Figure 1. Likelihood profile for the proportion of Steller sea lions in the diet of 125 <strong>and</strong><br />

250 transient killer whales. Likelihoods were rescaled to values between 0 <strong>and</strong> 1. If the<br />

proportion of sea lions in the diet of 250 killer whales increased 0.28 (CI 0.26-0.3) beginning<br />

in 1974, this would be sufficient to cause the decline. The decline can also be explained if the<br />

proportion of sea lions in the diet of 125 killer whales increased 0.56 (CI 0.52-0.6).<br />

Acknowledgements: We thank Eva Saulitis for her involvement in this project in its early<br />

stages, <strong>and</strong> Arliss Winship for his help in completing the figures. We also thank the many<br />

mariners who replied to our survey. We are very grateful to David Bain, Rich Ferrero, Donna<br />

Willoya, Kate Wynne, <strong>and</strong> Liana Jack for contributing stomach content information. We also<br />

thank the late Bud Fay, Elaine Humphries, William A. Walker <strong>and</strong> Pacific Identifications<br />

(Victoria BC) for identifying stomach contents, <strong>and</strong> Graeme Ellis for photo-identifying killer<br />

whales. We are also very grateful for funding provided from the North Gulf Oceanic Society,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to NOAA <strong>and</strong> the North Pacific Marine Science Foundation for grants to the North<br />

Pacific Universities Marine Mammal Research Consortium.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

82


WORLD-WIDE GENETIC DIVERSITY IN THE KILLER WHALE (ORCINUS<br />

ORCA); IMPLICATIONS FOR DEMOGRAPHIC HISTORY.<br />

Hoelzel R.A. 1 , Natoli A. 1 , Dahlheim M.E. 2 , Olavarria C. 3 , Baird R.W. 4 , Nicholson C. 1 , Black N.A. 5 .<br />

1 Department of Biological Sciences, Durham University, South Road, Durham, DH1 3LE, Engl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

a.r.hoelzel@dur.ac.uk;<br />

2 National Marine Mammal Lab., NMFS, 7600 S<strong>and</strong> Point Way N.E., Seattle, WA, 98115, USA;<br />

3 School of Biological Sciences, University of Auckl<strong>and</strong>, Private Bag 92019, Auckl<strong>and</strong>, New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong>;<br />

4 Biology Department, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, B3H 4J1, Canada;<br />

5 Monterey Bay Cetacean Project, P.O. Box 52001, Pacific Grove, CA 93950, USA.<br />

A low level of genetic variation in mammalian populations where the census population<br />

size is relatively large has been attributed to various factors, such as a naturally small<br />

effective population size, historical bottlenecks, <strong>and</strong> social behaviour. The killer whale is an<br />

abundant, highly social species with reduced genetic variation. We investigated both mtDNA<br />

(sequence data for up to 1,820bp) <strong>and</strong> microsatellite DNA loci among samples from the<br />

eastern North Pacific, western South Pacific, western North Atlantic, eastern North Atlantic,<br />

western South Atlantic, <strong>and</strong> Antarctic <strong>and</strong> found no consistent geographic pattern of global<br />

diversity, <strong>and</strong> no mtDNA variation within some regional populations. For example, the<br />

overall level of nucleotide diversity for the mtDNA control region was π = 0.005, a level<br />

comparable to that commonly seen for species known to have undergone a population<br />

bottleneck. Genetic distance was in some cases greater between populations in sympatry than<br />

between populations in separate oceans. The regional lack of mtDNA variation is likely to be<br />

due to the strict matrilineal expansion of local populations, a consequence of their matrifocal<br />

social behaviour. Related mechanisms could in turn lead to reduced mtDNA variation worldwide,<br />

however the pattern of variation among putative populations <strong>and</strong> among haplotypes<br />

suggests the possibility of an historical population bottleneck as an additional factor.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

83


SOCIOECOLOGY OF KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) IN NORTHERN<br />

PATAGONIA<br />

Iñíguez M.A., Tossenberger V.P., Gasparrou C.<br />

Fundación Cethus. Juan de Garay 2861 Dto “3”, Olivos, (1636), Pcia de Buenos Aires, Argentina,<br />

tovera@sanjulian.com.ar<br />

Observations on killer whales in Northern Patagonia were done using photoidentification<br />

techniques between 1985 <strong>and</strong> 1999. Thirty killer whales have been identified in<br />

the study area since 1975 but a core group of 17 have been found to return to the area each<br />

year. Boat <strong>and</strong> shore observations were conducted during both the austral summer-autumn<br />

season 1985-1999 <strong>and</strong> the austral winter season 1992-1993, using the individual follow<br />

protocol. The objective of this paper is to study the long-term association <strong>and</strong> composition of<br />

killer whales pods in Northern Patagonia, Argentina. The 17 individuals regularly seen in the<br />

region were classified at the beginning of the field programme into three pods PNA, B <strong>and</strong> C.<br />

The pods contain 6,2 <strong>and</strong> 9 killer whales respectively. Group size ranged from 1 to 10.<br />

Between 1991 <strong>and</strong> 1993, A1, A2, A4, A6, B1 <strong>and</strong> C2 were no longer sighted. After 1993,<br />

changes in the pod composition were observed. First, association between A <strong>and</strong> B pods was<br />

observed <strong>and</strong> since 1994 between A <strong>and</strong> C pods. B pod interacted with all the other pods.<br />

Dispersal of both sexes from groups larger than three individuals was observed <strong>and</strong> it may be<br />

related to foraging strategies <strong>and</strong> provisioning. Individuals from different pods ab<strong>and</strong>oned<br />

their maternal groups to made up new aggregations.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

84


COOPERATIVE HUNTING AND PREY HANDLING OF KILLER WHALES IN<br />

PUNTA NORTE, PATAGONIA, ARGENTINA<br />

Iñíguez M.A., Tossenberger V.P., Gasparrou C.<br />

Fundación Cethus. Juan de Garay 2861 Dto “3”, Olivos, (1636), Pcia de Buenos Aires, Argentina,<br />

tovera@sanjulian.com.ar<br />

Field observations carried out between 1989 <strong>and</strong> 1999 were used to describe the prey<br />

species <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour of killer whales in Northern Patagonia. They fed almost<br />

exclusively upon pinnipeds including southern sea lions (Otaria flavescens) <strong>and</strong> southern<br />

elephant seals (Mirounga leonina). Southern sea lions comprised 85.71% <strong>and</strong> of them,<br />

82.25% were pups. Killer whale diet in Northern Patagonia also included southern elephant<br />

seals (3.89%), magellanic penguins (Spheniscus magellanicus) (3.89%) <strong>and</strong> fish (6.06%). A<br />

record of a silvery grebe (Podiceps occipitalis) killed by killer whales was also reported.<br />

Killer whales used mainly intentional str<strong>and</strong>ing techniques to hunt pinnipeds (n=174).<br />

Intentional str<strong>and</strong>ing events were timed, <strong>and</strong> each one was found to last between 8 <strong>and</strong> 81<br />

seconds ( X =29.22 sec). The feeding behaviour that followed the str<strong>and</strong>ing events lasted from<br />

1 to 82 minutes ( X =18.00´). A1, A3, A5, B1, B2, C1, C4, C5 individuals killer whales were<br />

involved in hunting events as hunters. Seven different hunting techniques were observed<br />

including intentional str<strong>and</strong>ing, pursuit, circling, strike with the fluke, close approach, breach<br />

<strong>and</strong> semi-intentional str<strong>and</strong>ing. Intergroup conflicts over prey patches or prey carcasses were<br />

observed. Prey sharing was registered on 109 occasions. Foraging association between all<br />

three pods (A, B <strong>and</strong> C) were observed on 29 occasions. Eleven teaching events, where calves<br />

were tutored on hunting techniques were identified.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

85


KILLER WHALE (ORCINUS ORCA) SIGHTINGS IN ALASKA: PRELIMINARY<br />

RESULTS FROM MARINER SURVEY DATA<br />

Kerry E. Irish 1 , John K.B. Ford 2,4 , Lance G. Barrett-Lennard 3,4 <strong>and</strong> Andrew W. Trites 1<br />

(1) Marine Mammal Research Unit, University of British Columbia, Room 18 Hut B-3 6248<br />

Biological Sciences Road, Vancouver, B.C. CANADA V6T 1Z4<br />

(2) Pacific Biological Station, Department of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans, Nanaimo, B.C. CANADA V9R<br />

5K6<br />

(3) Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre, P.O. Box 3232, Vancouver, B.C. CANADA V6B<br />

3X8<br />

(4) Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, 6270 University Boulevard Vancouver,<br />

B.C. CANADA V6T 1Z4<br />

Long term, systematic studies of the natural histories <strong>and</strong> population status of resident<br />

<strong>and</strong> transient killer whale populations have been conducted in the coastal waters of the<br />

northeastern Pacific from Washington State to the Kenai Fjords. These studies have provided<br />

reliable population estimates <strong>and</strong> a good underst<strong>and</strong>ing of life history <strong>and</strong> ecology for killer<br />

whales in these regions. Such comprehensive studies have not yet been undertaken in the<br />

northern Pacific west of the Kenai Fjords, although initial field efforts are currently underway.<br />

There is thus a paucity of data on the abundance, distribution, population structure <strong>and</strong><br />

foraging behaviour of killer whales in this region.<br />

Although killer whales have a cosmopolitan distribution throughout the world’s oceans,<br />

they are far more abundant in some regions than others. This makes initial investigations of<br />

killer whale biology in new areas challenging, as few parameters exist to focus field efforts<br />

<strong>and</strong> many of these areas are remote <strong>and</strong> difficult to survey. While line transect protocols are<br />

an effective method for assessing the density <strong>and</strong> distribution of some odontocete species, this<br />

approach has not proven useful for killer whales. Previous efforts to use line transect surveys<br />

have been difficult because of the non-r<strong>and</strong>om distribution of killer whales - due to the<br />

existence of distributional “hot spots”, <strong>and</strong> furthermore these hotspots are ephemeral on both<br />

annual <strong>and</strong> multi-annual time scales. Thus a key element in many killer whale studies around<br />

the globe is the role of the lay observer in reducing the time researchers spend looking for<br />

whales. The prominence of killer whale mythology in the local culture, the relatively low<br />

probability of species misidentification <strong>and</strong> the high levels of motivation <strong>and</strong> enthusiasm<br />

combine to make Alaska an ideal area in which to employ lay observers. In addition, the<br />

logistical <strong>and</strong> fiscal constraints of embarking upon dedicated surveys indicate that there may<br />

be considerable advantage in using an alternative means of collecting baseline information on<br />

killer whales of this region.<br />

The late Michael Bigg surveyed local mariners in British Columbia over a weekend for<br />

three successive summers in the early 1970’s to investigate killer whale distribution. By<br />

engaging mariners who were already on the water to look for killer whales allowed him to<br />

quickly <strong>and</strong> economically establish a ‘snapshot’ of killer whale distribution by increasing the<br />

survey area coverage. This method provided a minimum estimate of killer whale abundance<br />

for British Columbia that was later confirmed by photo-identification studies as relatively<br />

accurate. Bigg also found that while widely distributed, there were key "hot spots" where<br />

killer whales would aggregate. Mariner survey data from western Alaska should also help to<br />

locate such “hot spots”, which may be useful for guiding future research efforts in this region.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

86


Using this method established by Bigg, killer whale sighting forms were disseminated<br />

widely to mariners throughout Alaska via print <strong>and</strong> audio media, a dedicated web site<br />

(www.AlaskaKillerWhales.org), a notice to mariners, local contacts <strong>and</strong> commercial interests.<br />

Alaskan mariners were asked to look for killer whales during the weekend of July 19 – 21,<br />

2002 <strong>and</strong> fill in a survey form whether they saw killer whales or not. On the survey form, they<br />

were asked three sets of questions; to describe the route taken <strong>and</strong> general weather conditions;<br />

if killer whales were seen or not; <strong>and</strong> general information about previous encounters with<br />

killer whales. One hundred-sixty survey forms were collected during that weekend from<br />

mariners throughout Alaska. Participation was fairly evenly distributed with 38.8% of the<br />

surveys collected from south-central Alaska, 34.1% from the Aleutians <strong>and</strong> 27.1% from<br />

southeastern Alaska.<br />

The largest number of surveys were submitted by recreational mariners (32.3%),<br />

followed by commercial fishermen (27.7%) <strong>and</strong> charter operators (26.2%). Other notable<br />

contributors included Alaska State Ferries, US Coast Guard, <strong>and</strong> various government <strong>and</strong><br />

independent researchers. The majority of participants mailed in their surveys (39%), with a<br />

notable number using the web interface (28%) or facsimile (25%), <strong>and</strong> a few emailed (6.25%)<br />

or phoned (1.6%) in their survey results.<br />

Forty of the surveys reported sightings of killer whales. The greatest number of those<br />

sightings came from Southeast Alaska (46.5%), the second greatest number of sightings came<br />

from South Central Alaska (37.2%) with the fewest number of sightings reported from the<br />

Aleutians (16.3%). The majority of sightings reported group sizes of 1– 8 individuals with a<br />

mean of 4 (80%) ten percent reported group sizes of 11-15 <strong>and</strong> 10% reported groups sizes of<br />

30-40.<br />

Initial spatial analysis (uncorrected for effort) shows three potential hot spots of killer<br />

whale sightings; Unimak Pass (Aleutians); Prince William Sound (South Central Alaska) <strong>and</strong><br />

near Juneau (Southeast Alaska). Additional analysis will consider survey effort, bathymetric,<br />

fisheries catches <strong>and</strong> marine mammal distributions with both killer whale sightings <strong>and</strong> areas<br />

surveyed. A comparison of these data with those collected by Bigg in the 1970’s British<br />

Columbia surveys will be undertaken as well. The next survey is planned for the first week of<br />

March 2003, in an effort to increase sample size <strong>and</strong> look for potential differences in seasonal<br />

distribution.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

87


BIOENERGETIC CHANGES FROM 1986 TO 2002 IN SOUTHERN RESIDENT<br />

KILLER WHALES, ORCINUS ORCA<br />

Kriete B.<br />

53 <strong>Orca</strong> Relief, Limestone Point Road, Friday Harbor, WA, 98250, USA Kriete, birgit@orcarelief.org<br />

From 1996 to 2001 the population of the southern resident population of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) in northern Puget Sound, WA, USA, decreased from a high of 98 individuals<br />

to a low of 79 animals, a reduction of almost 20% in only 6 years (Bain 2002). At the same<br />

time, whale watching ecotourism in the area increased from 2 commercial whale watch<br />

operations in 1987 to over 90 commercial boats in 2000 (Bain 2002). This study provides a<br />

comparison of the physiological changes of the southern resident killer whale population from<br />

a period of very little boat traffic to an era of increased marine vessel commerce.<br />

Data on energy expenditure <strong>and</strong> daily caloric requirements were measured in the<br />

southern resident killer whale population in the mid-1980’s (Kriete 1995). Swimming<br />

velocities <strong>and</strong> respiration rates in different age/sex classes of traveling wild killer whales were<br />

collected in all research seasons. These data were combined with oxygen consumption<br />

measured during different activities of captive orcas to determine bioenergetic requirements<br />

for killer whales.<br />

The same study on wild killer whales was repeated in 2001 in the same location.<br />

Swimming speeds <strong>and</strong> breathing rates showed statistically significant increases over those of<br />

the mid-1980’s. Adult killer whales expended close to 20% more energy due to increased<br />

swimming velocity in 2001 compared to the 1980’s. Increases in metabolic rates must be<br />

balanced by an increase in food consumption <strong>and</strong> additional feeding behavior was observed in<br />

2001. While environmental conditions were very similar between the observation eras, the<br />

only change was the exorbitant increase in both commercial <strong>and</strong> private boats following the<br />

whales for more than 14 hours per day. We believe, therefore, that there is a direct<br />

relationship between the increase in metabolic rates in southern resident killer whales <strong>and</strong> the<br />

increase in whale watch operations.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

88


SURFACE INTERVALS OF AN ADULT MALE KILLER WHALE IN NORWAY<br />

Leyssen, T. (1), Similä, T. (2), Hanson M. Bradley (3), Holst, J.C. (4), Øien, N.(5)<br />

(1) NORCA, Rozenlaan 8 3650 Dilsen-Stokkem, Belgium.<br />

(2,4,5) Institute of Marine Research, P.O.Box 1870 Nordnes, 5817 Bergen, Norway<br />

(3) NOAA, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Mammal Laboratory, 7600 S<strong>and</strong> Point<br />

Way NE, Seattle, WA 98115 USA<br />

Surface intervals are frequently used in cetacean studies to describe surface activity <strong>and</strong><br />

energy consumption. During fall 2000 surface intervals of killer whales in northern Norway<br />

were studied visually by keeping track of a recognizable individual during different<br />

behaviours (1). Because these whales only enter the fjords during early winter, weather <strong>and</strong><br />

light conditions limited these observations to few hours per individual.<br />

In December 2001 an adult male killer whale was equipped with a satellite tag. Before<br />

release a suction cup TDR (Time-Depth recorder, Wildlife Computers) was also deployed<br />

which recorded depth <strong>and</strong> swimming speed for 60 hours. The TDR stayed on for longer than<br />

any TDR deployed on killer whales before. The reason for this must be because the whale was<br />

restrained during the tagging <strong>and</strong> the tag could be attached with care <strong>and</strong> in a good location.<br />

The collected data set contained 4885 surfacings. The data was analysed for possible<br />

diurnal changes in the surfacing pattern <strong>and</strong> also for possible differences between the TDR<br />

data <strong>and</strong> earlier data on adult males collected as visual observations.<br />

BOUTS<br />

Whales’ surface intervals typically occur as bouts. To distinguish between the withinbout<br />

surface intervals <strong>and</strong> the inbetween-bouts surface intervals, we need to calculate the bout<br />

criterion interval (BCI)<br />

The Log-Survival function shows that the distinction between short (within bouts) <strong>and</strong><br />

long (in between bouts) dives, has a cut-off point (BCI) at 44 seconds<br />

This agrees well with the previous study (1), where this was put at 41 seconds.<br />

SURFACE INTERVALS<br />

To compare the surface intervals with visually acquired data from previous studies, the<br />

surface intervals in this study were grouped per 10 minutes.<br />

An overview of the results from the previous study (1)<br />

median surface No of blows mean duration of<br />

interval (sec) per 10 min long dives (sec)<br />

subsurface feeding 13,1 27,3 72,9<br />

travel feeding 26,7 17,6 157,5<br />

travel / foraging 22,2 11,9 138,5<br />

resting 18,8 11,3 180,9<br />

The median surface intervals, as calculated per 10 minutes, have a mean of 26.6<br />

seconds, with a minimum of 14 seconds <strong>and</strong> a maximum of 186 seconds. This maximum<br />

value far exceeds anything recorded during visual observations. However, 349 of the 363<br />

median values were smaller than 44 seconds. 359 values smaller than 80 seconds.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

89


Some of the exhalation intervals were longer than 2 minutes, with as little as 2<br />

surfacings in 10 minutes. This kind of surface intervals were not recorded through visual<br />

observations. This is to be expected. With visual observations it is very important not to miss<br />

any surfacings. With very long surface intervals, the whale might move a long way between<br />

the surfacings, <strong>and</strong> therefore you can’t be sure you missed none. As a consequence, the<br />

datasets with such long surface intervals will mostly be discarded. This explains the<br />

discrepancy in maximum value for median surface intervals.<br />

nr of 10 min periods<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22<br />

number of surfaces per 10 min<br />

In this study: the mean number of blows is 13.4 per 10 minutes, with a minimum of 2<br />

<strong>and</strong> a maximum of 22. If we compare these numbers with the table above, we see that this<br />

corresponds with travel <strong>and</strong> resting behaviour. Non of this data corresponds with subsurface<br />

feeding.<br />

seconds<br />

Number of intervals<br />

200<br />

150<br />

100<br />

50<br />

0<br />

400<br />

300<br />

200<br />

100<br />

0<br />

1<br />

12<br />

18<br />

14<br />

35<br />

16<br />

52<br />

18<br />

69<br />

Mean Surface Intervals per 10 minutes<br />

86<br />

20<br />

103<br />

120<br />

137<br />

154<br />

171<br />

188<br />

205<br />

10 minute periods<br />

222<br />

Within-bouts surface intervals<br />

22<br />

24<br />

26<br />

28<br />

30<br />

seconds<br />

32<br />

239<br />

256<br />

273<br />

290<br />

307<br />

324<br />

341<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

34<br />

36<br />

38<br />

40<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

42<br />

44<br />

358<br />

All surface<br />

intervals<br />

mean = 44.6 sec<br />

median = 23 sec<br />

mode = 19 sec<br />

maximum = 392 sec<br />

minimum = 6 sec<br />

Within-bout<br />

surface intervals:<br />

mean = 22.8<br />

seconds<br />

median = 21.5<br />

seconds<br />

mode = 21 seconds<br />

90


VARIABILITIES IN SURFACE INTERVALS OVER TIME<br />

There were large changes in the surfacing pattern throughout the 60 hour period, but no<br />

diurnal pattern could be observed. Sixty hours of recording is substantial data collected with a<br />

TDR but it is a limited time span in a whales’ life. However if there was a rigorous diurnal<br />

rhythm, it would have shown in 60 hours.<br />

The mean surface intervals per 10 minute period was 48.4 seconds, with a st<strong>and</strong>ard<br />

deviation of 18.7 seconds, a minimum of 24.2 seconds <strong>and</strong> a maximum of 175.7seconds.<br />

As can be seen from the graph on the left, there were 2 stretches of longer dives (period<br />

60 to 120, or about 10 hours).<br />

If we exclude these 10 hours, we have about 50 hours where the mean surface intervals<br />

per 10 minute period was 41.9 seconds, with a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of 9.7 seconds, a minimum<br />

of 24.2 seconds <strong>and</strong> a maximum of 67.8 seconds. These 10 hours have a mean surface interval<br />

per 10 minute period of 72.1 seconds, with a st<strong>and</strong>ard deviation of 42.1 seconds. This shows<br />

not only that during this period of 10 hours the diving was prolonged but also much more<br />

variable.<br />

THE EFFECTS OF TAGGING.<br />

This whale was caught <strong>and</strong> held for about an hour in order to attach a satellite <strong>and</strong> radio<br />

tag. Then the suction cup TDR was attached <strong>and</strong> the whale was released.<br />

As in any tagging, it is important to consider the effects of the tagging on the behaviour<br />

of the animal. There are 2 possible effects:<br />

1. The effect of moving around with the tags attached to the body, which will cause<br />

extra drag (was kept to a minimum by shaping the tags).<strong>and</strong> might cause annoyance This<br />

effect is very difficult to quantify as we don’t have any information about swimming speed<br />

<strong>and</strong> diving depth of a non-tagged whale. However, the fact that the results of this study<br />

correspond well with earlier visual data, lets us assume that this effect will not be very large.<br />

2. The effect of the tagging process. Here we can look at the surface intervals, dive<br />

depth <strong>and</strong> swimming speed after release <strong>and</strong> compare this data with a period sufficiently later.<br />

After release the whale made deep dives with long surface intervals for 10 minutes. After this<br />

there is no discernable effect on surface intervals <strong>and</strong> dive depth. The swimming speed stayed<br />

relatively high for 1 hour. It is impossible to know how much of this is caused by the tagging,<br />

but after 1 hour the data becomes indistinguishable from the rest of the recording period. It<br />

would therefore be safe for us to assume that this effect lasts for maximum 1 hour. There are<br />

more occurrences of fast swimming later on in the dataset. Compared to those, this first period<br />

of fast swimming is characterised by lower maximum swimming speed but more sustained<br />

speed. To err on the safe site, we excluded the first 2 hours of data from the analysis.<br />

CONCLUSION:<br />

The study shows that visual observations are a reliable way of observing surfacing<br />

patterns of individual killer whales, but a TDR is more reliable than visual observations in<br />

studying long surfacing intervals.<br />

There was no diurnal pattern to the surface intervals of this whale during the 60 hour<br />

recording period.<br />

In our study area, discerning potential diurnal or other patterns in surfacing behaviour is<br />

not possible through visual observations. <strong>and</strong> therefore the use of a TDR gave us a new<br />

insight into this behaviour<br />

(1) Turunen, Sanna. 2001. The feeding behaviour of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the wintering grounds of Norwegian springspawning<br />

herring. BSc thesis, University of Aberdeen.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

91


PREDATORY ACTIVITY OF A SINGLE KILLER WHALE, ORCINUS ORCA, AT A<br />

STELLER SEA LION, EUMETOPIAS JUBATUS, ROOKERY IN ALASKA<br />

Daniela Maldini, John Maniscalco, Alex<strong>and</strong>er Burdin<br />

Alaska SeaLife Center, PO Box 1329, Seward, AK 99664<br />

Eastern North Pacific Steller sea lions of the Western stock have dramatically<br />

declined. These estimates prompted their listing as Endangered in the Western stock in 1997.<br />

To determine the reasons for such decline, the US Federal government has recently<br />

committed considerable funding to the scientific community.<br />

One of the possible factors being explored by scientists to explain the decline is<br />

the importance of predation by other marine mammals. The effects of predation pressure have<br />

been so far investigated with conflicting results. Part of the problem has been the scarcity of<br />

data available on both the rate of predation <strong>and</strong> on the number of predators exacting pressure<br />

on Steller sea lions.<br />

Apart from man, there are only two types of predators thought to take Steller sea<br />

lions: Pacific sleeper sharks <strong>and</strong> killer whales of the transient type, <strong>and</strong> the predation by<br />

sharks has only been postulated based on few stomach contents since an attack has never been<br />

witnessed. This leaves killer whales as the most likely c<strong>and</strong>idate for a predator-prey theory.<br />

The Alaska SeaLife Center (ASLC), in cooperation with the North Gulf Oceanic<br />

Society (NGOS), is operating in the area of the Prince-William Sound/Kenai Fjords, in<br />

Southcentral Alaska where some of the largest declines in Steller Sea Lions have occurred.<br />

The ASLC is concentrating on the area around Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong>, a well monitored SSL rookery<br />

where pupping occurs.<br />

Transient killer whales are known to visit SSL rookeries to forage. They travel in<br />

small pods (1-7 individuals) over large areas, hunting on various kinds of marine mammals<br />

<strong>and</strong> birds. Based on long-range studies on the life history of transient killer whales around the<br />

world, particular pods appear to specialize in a specific type of marine mammal (some are<br />

seal specialists, others sea lion specialists, etc). This specialization is not exclusive of other<br />

prey types if the main prey is not available, but does appear to dictate the particular hunting<br />

strategies of a pod.<br />

In the Kenai Fjords/Prince William Sound region there are two types of transients:<br />

the AT1 group, a genetically separate group currently numbering 12 whales which specializes<br />

on harbor seals, <strong>and</strong> the less known Gulf of Alaska transients (whose population size is<br />

unknown although at least 60 whales have been identified so far), which appear to specialize<br />

on Steller Sea Lions.<br />

From data on killer whale stomach contents, observations of attacks <strong>and</strong><br />

circumstantial evidence collected by various authors, it has become clear that killer whales<br />

may exercise a significant predation pressure on already depleted populations such as Steller<br />

sea lions <strong>and</strong> harbor seals in Alaskan waters. In turn, the depletion of key prey populations<br />

may result in a shift in diet by the predator or other alterations in the predator’s life style. In<br />

light of this, the study of such interactions is vital to the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the population<br />

dynamics of both the predator <strong>and</strong> the prey.<br />

From the urgent perspective of addressing the causes of Steller sea lion decline, it<br />

is necessary to move past the realization that killer whales do prey on them <strong>and</strong> attempt to<br />

quantify the significance of this predation. It is also important to realize that predation<br />

pressure by killer whales may vary by rookery, by geographic locale <strong>and</strong> by time of year.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

92


The lack of underst<strong>and</strong>ing of transient killer whale ecology is due to the difficulties<br />

inherent in finding <strong>and</strong> tracking an animal that is elusive by nature (because of its predatory<br />

strategies) <strong>and</strong> its apparently wide range which has been estimated to be 180,000 square<br />

miles.<br />

In an attempt to monitor transient killer whale behavior near the Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

rookery we are using a series of remotely operated cameras located on the isl<strong>and</strong>. The signal<br />

from these cameras is sent via microwaves to an antenna on the roof at the ASLC <strong>and</strong> to a<br />

series of televisions in the research area where researchers monitor <strong>and</strong> record SSL behavior.<br />

In addition to the camera system, two hydrophones are constantly transmitting the underwater<br />

sounds around Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> to the ASLC. Using a combination of video <strong>and</strong> sounds, we<br />

have been able to monitor killer whale activity in the vicinity of Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> we<br />

hope, in the near future, to be able to capture on both audio <strong>and</strong> video the predation activity of<br />

transient killer whales in this area.<br />

Between 2000 <strong>and</strong> 2001 we have been able to remotely detect the presence of a<br />

single transient killer whale which frequents the Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> rookery with some<br />

regularity. AT109 is a transient killer whale which was first photographed in 1987 in nearby<br />

Prince William Sound. Since she has never been sighted with a calf, it is suspected she may<br />

beyond reproductive age (40 or more). Based on remote monitoring of the video cameras,<br />

AT109 visited Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> during at least 19 time slots encompassing periods of one to 11<br />

consecutive days patrolling the rookery between 2000 <strong>and</strong> the beginning of 2002. Many of<br />

these dates have also been confirmed by reports from tour operators in the Kenai Fjords area<br />

some of whom know AT109 well, <strong>and</strong> by our own surveys by boat. The whale’s activity near<br />

the rookery is concentrated in the late summer when sea lion pups are starting to swim.<br />

Interestingly, this summer AT109 was no longer alone but was swimming with<br />

another female <strong>and</strong> her calf. In the company of this female she altered her behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

although she did visit the Kenai Fjords area, she was never seen at Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong>, but at a<br />

different Steller sea lion haul out near Cape Resurrection. This raises some interesting<br />

questions such as whether the number of whales in a group may be a factor in determining the<br />

hunting strategy of the group, or whether individual whale preferences may influence prey<br />

choice to the point that social relationships shape the hunting strategy of a group or, again,<br />

whether the population size at the Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> haul-out is too small to support three killer<br />

whales versus only one.<br />

Steller sea lion mother/pup pairs found on Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> throughout the pupping<br />

season, are monitored around the clock <strong>and</strong> censused hourly by ASLC scientists. When a<br />

mother is censused without her pup on several occasions the pup is presumed lost. If we<br />

believe that killer whale AT109 preys on Steller sea lion pups, her rate of predation on this<br />

age class can be inferred from their reduced numbers after AT109’s visits, <strong>and</strong> especially by<br />

the confirmed loss of a pup by a known sea lion mother still present at the rookery after the<br />

whale’s visit. In late July 2001, daily sea lion pup numbers at Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> averaged 50 (±<br />

1 S.E.) during four days prior to a nine-day visit by AT109 <strong>and</strong> dropped to 38 (± 1 S.E.)<br />

afterward; a significant decline of 24% (U = 218.5, P < 0.001). During the same period in<br />

2000, when no killer whale was present near the rookery there was no change in pup numbers<br />

(U=34.5, P > 0.10). In addition, out of 31 identifiable mothers with pup being monitored daily<br />

from the video cameras, only 27 still had their pups after AT109’s visit (a 14% decline).<br />

During a six-day, late August visit by AT109 to the rookery, an increase in overall<br />

pup numbers was presumably caused by an influx of sea lions from a nearby rookery. Yet, of<br />

known female sea lions with pups during that time, 13% had lost their pup during the orca’s<br />

visit.<br />

On one occasion we intercepted AT109 in the vicinity of the rookery after<br />

noticing her presence on the video camera. In addition to her movement patterns around the<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

93


ookery we recorded 111 dives during 3 ½ hours of observation. The pattern was consistent,<br />

with several short dives (one minute or less) followed by longer dives (up to 10 minutes).<br />

Dive times were longer near the rookery (< 500 m) as the whale patrolled the haul-out back<br />

<strong>and</strong> forth underwater, <strong>and</strong> shorter away from the rookery, but the difference was not<br />

significant (U=1624, P =0.169). Although we did not witness any predation event while<br />

following AT109, predation appears to have occurred during that same visit, which lasted six<br />

days, based on reports from one reliable tour operator who witnessed a kill. Judging from the<br />

whale’s behavior <strong>and</strong> dive pattern it is possible that predation events occur underwater.<br />

Data here presented support the possibility that predation pressure by one killer<br />

whale at Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> may have resulted in a predation rate on Steller sea lion pups as high<br />

as 13-24% in 2001. If this is the case, one single killer whale preying regularly on a rookery<br />

as small as Chiswell Isl<strong>and</strong> may exert a significant impact on its recovery.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

94


ANATOMY OF A LONG-TERM STUDY: POPULATION ECOLOGY OF KILLER<br />

WHALES IN PRINCE WILLIAM SOUND AND KENAI FJORDS, ALASKA 1983-<br />

2002.<br />

Craig Matkin, Graeme Ellis, Eva Saulitis, Lance Barrett Lennard, Harald Yurk, Kathy Heise, Peter<br />

Olesiuk<br />

North Gulf Oceanic Society, 60920 Mary Allen Ave., Homer, Alaska 99603, comatkin@xyz.net<br />

In choosing a study design we elected to use methodology that did not involve<br />

systematic vessel transects, but was based on finding “hotspots” where we could maximize our<br />

chances of encountering whales through opportunistic encounters using a local sighting network<br />

that was developed during the study. This allowed the use of small boats, but sometimes<br />

necessitated waiting for encounters. All aspects of the study were based on photoidentification<br />

or every individual. Our population estimates are all minimums based on photoidentified<br />

individuals only. The study is by necessity long term <strong>and</strong> emphasizes all aspects of live history<br />

of the animals. The study area for our core project is Kenai Fjords <strong>and</strong> Prince William Sound<br />

(Figure 1). What I present here, is a synopsis <strong>and</strong> overview of the project, several other talks<br />

will detail aspects of the project I touch on here.<br />

Figure 1 : Study Area<br />

The field components of this study included:<br />

• Photoidentification of every individual<br />

• Recording whale <strong>and</strong> vessel tracklines<br />

• Whale behavior by time <strong>and</strong> location<br />

• Acoustic recordings<br />

• Biopsy sampling<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

95


skin for genetics<br />

blubber for contaminants<br />

• Monitoring of vessel interactions with whales<br />

• Prey observation <strong>and</strong> sample collection<br />

Our first <strong>and</strong> most important tool, photoidentification, has been used to determine the total<br />

number of killer whales that used the study area over the period of 18 years. It also gives a<br />

reasonable indication of the frequency with which each group used the area <strong>and</strong> the patterns<br />

of use, at least seasonally.<br />

Repeated photography of the major resident pods over a period of 10 years provided the<br />

data for the analysis of associations between individuals <strong>and</strong> resulted in the creation of<br />

genealogical trees for each matriline. Now after 18 years, or approximately one killer whale<br />

generation, we are developing a comprehensive population model for the well known resident<br />

pods. This includes mortality rates by sex <strong>and</strong> age, age estimates by various methods, calving<br />

intervals <strong>and</strong> fecundity rates, <strong>and</strong> rate of onset of post-reproduction in females. This<br />

information is incorporated into a matrix model <strong>and</strong> life tables.<br />

Photoidentification is also essential for our genetic analysis. Every biopsied whale is<br />

photographed <strong>and</strong> identified at time of sampling to determine population affiliation of its<br />

group or pod <strong>and</strong> to investigate social structure <strong>and</strong> breeding systems. Both mtDNA <strong>and</strong><br />

nuclear DNA analysis have been used in the investigations. Accurate identification is also<br />

important in to interpret contaminant analysis where varying levels of contaminants can be<br />

traced to particular age <strong>and</strong> sex classes of whales. When documenting feeding habits<br />

photoidentification also provides the identity <strong>and</strong> population affiliation of the whales<br />

observed. Feeding habits vary by group <strong>and</strong> population.<br />

We are now using a Nobletech system integrated with the ships GPS that maps the vessel<br />

track as well as the trackline when with whales. Behaviors are linked to time <strong>and</strong> location on<br />

a nautical chart. This allows the analysis of the whale’s patterns of use within the study area.<br />

Directional hydrophones are used to locate whales <strong>and</strong> complement the visual search <strong>and</strong><br />

observer networks. Whales are recorded after identifications are made <strong>and</strong> each various pods<br />

(residents) <strong>and</strong> populations can be identified by their calls. Our call catalogues allow<br />

identification of whales from calls alone. Calls recorded from remote hydrophones yield<br />

information on the identities of the whales that use the area in winter when fieldwork is<br />

difficult or impossible. Acoustics provide a shorter term picture of relationships between<br />

whale groups which complements the historic view offered by genetic analysis.<br />

Biopsy sampling is conducted using Pneudart air powered rifle <strong>and</strong> lightweight aluminum<br />

darts. The velocity is adjustable <strong>and</strong> samples are typically 5mm indiameter <strong>and</strong> 30mm in<br />

length <strong>and</strong> consist of skin <strong>and</strong> blubber. Genetic analysis confirms that the separation of<br />

residents <strong>and</strong> transients is a long st<strong>and</strong>ing, population level separation. In addition it has<br />

indicated that pods are not inbred; fathers of calves are apparently very distantly related to the<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> are not found within the natal pod of the calf. Separation of subpopulations<br />

within resident <strong>and</strong> transient ecotypes has also been possible. Genetic analysis has shown two<br />

non-associating transient populations exist within our study area, the AT1 group <strong>and</strong> the Gulf<br />

of Alaska transients. Although we have considerable information on the rapidly declining<br />

AT1 transient population, the Gulf of Alaska transients appear to be a small, widely<br />

distributed population that has been difficult to study. Genetics has also revealed that the<br />

mitochondrial haplotypes for both the southern residents <strong>and</strong> the northern residents exist in<br />

the Prince William Sound These genotypes are specific to particular pods which can be<br />

separated into two acoustic clans. It is likely that two founding events occurred following the<br />

last ice age in southern Alaska.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

96


Blubber from biopsy samples were analyzed for PCBs <strong>and</strong> DDTs using HPLC/PDA<br />

method. It was found that transients have 15-20 times higher contaminant levels than<br />

residents, apparently due to their position higher on the food chain. In both populations<br />

lowest levels were found in reproductive females due to loss of contaminants via lactation<br />

(passed on to the calves). Highest levels were found in first born offspring, particularly older<br />

males that were unable to transfer contaminants via lactation. PCB levels in transients<br />

averaged over 250ppm total PCBs <strong>and</strong> 350 total DDTs with much higher levels in specific<br />

individuals.<br />

Finally, we have examined feeding habits by collecting scales from fish kills or prey<br />

pieces or by visual observation of prey within the mouths of the whales. When unsure if a kill<br />

took place (no physical evidence or clear observation) the interaction was treated as an<br />

harassment. Specific salmon species were targeted by the resident whales. In the July-<br />

September period silver or coho salmon are the primary prey while in the May-June period it<br />

appears that king or Chinook salmon are the primary prey, although samples from that period<br />

have not been completely analyzed. We suspect that king salmon are also important during<br />

the winter months.<br />

The AT1 transient diet is focused on harbor seals <strong>and</strong> Dall’s porpoise (with no<br />

predation on Steller sea lions). Although we have only a few feeding observations of the<br />

Gulf of Alaska transients, most are from two groups of animals ( 7 individuals) <strong>and</strong> are not<br />

necessarily representative of the entire population. These whales seem to specialize in<br />

predation on Steller sea lions, an endangered species in our region. One of the current<br />

research tasks is to determine how many of the Gulf of Alaska transients actually feed on sea<br />

lions <strong>and</strong> what the impact may be on the population of sea lions. Conversely, the decline of<br />

Steller sea lions may be having a significant impact on these transient killer whales.<br />

In summation:<br />

• By choice <strong>and</strong> necessity we used a small boat based surveys within a limited study<br />

area<br />

• Integrated approach-- all aspects are based on photoidentification of each individual<br />

which is essential for the other aspects of the study.<br />

• Long-term study is necessary to underst<strong>and</strong> population dynamics (15 yrs+) <strong>and</strong><br />

monitor annual changes (ie changes due to the Exxon Valdez Oil Spill)<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

97


KILLER WHALE POPULATION BIOLOGY AND FEEDING ECOLOGY IN<br />

SOUTHEASTERN ALASKA.<br />

Matkin D. 1 , Sautilis E. 2 .<br />

1 PO Box 22, Gustavus, Alaska, USA 99 826, denamatkin@hotmail.com<br />

; 2 60920 Mary Allen Ave Homer, Alaska, USA 99603<br />

Killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in southeastern Alaska have been photoidentified<br />

since 1984 (Leatherwood et al. 1984). Year-round work in the Glacier Bay/Icy<br />

Strait area began in 1988, <strong>and</strong> research efforts here have increased steadily up to the present<br />

(Matkin 1990). The study area is in the northern portion of southeastern Alaska <strong>and</strong> primarily<br />

includes Glacier Bay <strong>and</strong> the contiguous waters of Icy Strait <strong>and</strong> Cross Sound (Fig.1). Prince<br />

William Sound is to the north <strong>and</strong> British Columbia to the south.<br />

This paper presents a synthesis of the results of a 14-year study. It mainly includes<br />

photo-identification <strong>and</strong> feeding data from 1988 through 2001 collected by this author plus<br />

data collected by biologists working for Glacier Bay National Park <strong>and</strong> Preserve. In northern<br />

southeastern Alaska, we have identified 92 residents, 144 transients, <strong>and</strong> 14 individuals out of<br />

a pod of about 30 offshores. Populations that inhabit Prince William Sound, Alaska <strong>and</strong><br />

populations that frequent British Columbia, Canada overlap in southeastern Alaska.<br />

Residents <strong>and</strong> transients have been reproductively isolated from one another for<br />

thous<strong>and</strong>s of years, although they travel <strong>and</strong> forage through the same waters. The offshores<br />

are more closely related to the residents <strong>and</strong> are found in large fish-eating groups of about 30<br />

individuals (Matkin et al. 1999). The transients are mainly divided into AT1 transients (from<br />

Prince William Sound), Gulf of Alaska transients <strong>and</strong> West Coast transients. West Coast<br />

transients are the most commonly encountered type in Glacier Bay <strong>and</strong> Icy Strait. In 2001,<br />

two Gulf of Alaska transient males were documented swimming in Glacier Bay with West<br />

Coast transients for the first time. However, it is not known if the two types interbreed.<br />

From the pioneering photo-identification work of Dr. Mike Bigg in British Columbia,<br />

we know that residents form large stable matrilineal pods that eat fish, vocalize frequently,<br />

have more hooked dorsal fins <strong>and</strong> more black inside their white saddle patches (Bigg et al.<br />

1987). Several pods can join <strong>and</strong> form super-pods of over 100 individuals. The two main<br />

resident pods in southeastern Alaska (AF <strong>and</strong> AG pods) are most closely related to the<br />

northern residents of British Columbia.<br />

AF <strong>and</strong> AG pods travel regularly between southeastern Alaska <strong>and</strong> Prince William<br />

Sound, Alaska, <strong>and</strong> they intermingle most frequently with the Prince William Sound resident<br />

pods that are also most closely related to the northern residents of British Columbia. AF<br />

individuals made two visits to Prince William Sound in two months, <strong>and</strong> they traveled north<br />

750 km in just four days. These late July <strong>and</strong> August multi-pod associations are when coho<br />

salmon (Onchorhynchus kisutch) runs are at their peak, <strong>and</strong> social <strong>and</strong> sexual activity at these<br />

times indicate that successful matings occur (Matkin et al. 1997). AF <strong>and</strong> AG pods share<br />

many of the same discrete calls with each other as well as with Prince William Sound pods,<br />

further indicating a genetic relationship among these resident pods.<br />

AG pod has been photographed in southeastern Alaska every month of the year<br />

(sometimes at the faces of tidewater glaciers). AG pod consists of 29 individuals that feed on<br />

salmon <strong>and</strong> Pacific halibut (Hippoglossus stenolepis). AG pod females with young juveniles<br />

harassed to injury a common loon (Gavia immer) <strong>and</strong> harassed into flight a pigeon guillemot<br />

(Cepphus columbia). If residents mill, splash or breach close to humpback whales<br />

(Megaptera novaeangliae), humpbacks react with wheeze-blows, pectoral fin slaps, breaches,<br />

or rapid travel in the opposite direction.<br />

This study mainly focused on West Coast transients, 130 of which were photoidentified<br />

in the Glacier Bay/Icy Strait study area. These transients range a minimum of 2600<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

98


km from southeastern Alaska, south to California (Goley <strong>and</strong> Straley 1994). The same<br />

transient individuals have been documented traveling a distance of 700 km from the Glacier<br />

Bay area to British Columbia 3 times in less than 10 months (Ford <strong>and</strong> Ellis 1999). They<br />

forage primarily for harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) from inter-tidal lagoons to the icebergs at<br />

the faces of the tidewater glaciers where harbor seals give birth to their pups in May <strong>and</strong> begin<br />

to wean them in June. Traveling <strong>and</strong> foraging in groups of one to 23 individuals, transient use<br />

of Glacier Bay peaks in June <strong>and</strong> July each year.<br />

A line graph (Fig.2) of the minimum number of transient individuals identified each<br />

year in the Glacier Bay/Icy Strait study area shows an average of 40 transients per year. The<br />

early years on this graph indicate increasing effort. The dip in 1991 has to do with problems<br />

with data analysis that year, <strong>and</strong> the peak in year 2000 is in part due to more effort.<br />

Identifications since 1992 are thanks to Graeme Ellis at the Pacific Biological Station in<br />

Nanaimo, British Columbia.<br />

The transients that hunt in the Glacier Bay region use it on a very regular basis. Some<br />

of the individuals have been documented in regular associations for more than a decade. T85<br />

is a cow that has been photo-documented every year (between June <strong>and</strong> September) since<br />

1988. She had a calf in 1992 <strong>and</strong> another in 1995, <strong>and</strong> was identified in the Glacier Bay area<br />

5 times in June of 2000 with both of these offspring. One of her preferred hunting partners is<br />

T40, an adult male she has been in frequent association with since 1988. T40 was the first<br />

animal photo-identified in this study, <strong>and</strong> he has been photo-identified in the study area from<br />

June through October every year since 1986 except two. T87 is another adult male transient<br />

that has been photo-identified in association with his constant hunting partner T88. Since<br />

1988, they have been in Glacier Bay between May <strong>and</strong> September every year but two.<br />

Representing 33 kill incidents, a pie chart (Fig.3) of percents of prey species taken by<br />

transients shows that at 43%, the harbor seal (Phoca vitulina) is the primary prey in the<br />

Glacier Bay/Icy Strait region. Harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) come second, <strong>and</strong><br />

combined with Dall’s porpoise Phocoenoides dalli) are 27% of prey taken. Seabirds are 15%,<br />

Steller sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus) 12% <strong>and</strong> minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata)<br />

3% of West Coast transients’ diet.<br />

Attacks on harbor seals included all killer whale age <strong>and</strong> sex groups <strong>and</strong> lasted from 5<br />

minutes to 2 hours. Transient groups that killed seals ranged from 2 to 14 individuals,<br />

averaging 5 individuals. In 50% of seal kills, there was just milling with no surface activity.<br />

In the other 50%, they lobtailed on the seal first, sometimes up to 6 times. They rubbed<br />

against seals <strong>and</strong> carried them in their mouths before killing them. Seals that hid alongside or<br />

under the boat were caught <strong>and</strong> carried away from the boat. One seal climbed into the<br />

research boat to escape being eaten. After seal kills, transients may spyhop, breach or lobtail.<br />

In British Columbia, John Ford et al. (1998) found harbor seals to be the primary<br />

transient prey as well, representing 53% of the West Coast transient diet. In Prince William<br />

Sound, Eva Saulitis et al. (2000) found harbor seals (31%) second to Dall’s <strong>and</strong> harbor<br />

porpoise (45%) in those transients’ diet.<br />

In Glacier Bay/Icy Strait, harbor porpoise represented all but one of the porpoise kills.<br />

the british columbia study showed dall’s <strong>and</strong> harbor porpoise combined made up 23%, which<br />

again is similar to glacier bay (27%). groups of 3 to11 killer whales attacked porpoise from<br />

20 minutes up to 3 hours. all age <strong>and</strong> sex transients close pursued, repeatedly caught <strong>and</strong><br />

released, hit porpoise into the air <strong>and</strong> carried them skinned in their mouths. After porpoise<br />

kills, they may leave behind lungs, heart, <strong>and</strong> in one case an almost whole skinned skeleton.<br />

After successful kills, transients may breach, have erections, spyhop, pectoral fin <strong>and</strong> tail slap,<br />

headst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> vocalize.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

99


Some seabirds in Glacier Bay become easy prey for transients when the birds molt in<br />

mid-summer, losing their flight feathers <strong>and</strong> rafting into groups of hundreds of flightless<br />

birds. Surf, white-winged <strong>and</strong> black scoters (Melanitta perspicillata, M. fusca, M. nigra) are<br />

primarily eaten, followed by common mergansers (Mergus merganser). Transients also<br />

harassed marbled murrelet (Brachyramphus mormoratus), common murre (Uria aalge) <strong>and</strong><br />

pelagic cormorant (Phalacrocorax pelagicus).<br />

Groups of 3 to 7 females <strong>and</strong> young chased, bubbled under, hit into the air, lobtailed,<br />

lunged <strong>and</strong> breached on, repeat caught <strong>and</strong> released birds. After seabird kills, transients may<br />

spyhop, breach or lobtail. Whereas seabirds represented 15% of kills in Glacier Bay, they<br />

were only 6% in British Columbia, where in 3 out of 8 seabird kills, the carcass was<br />

ab<strong>and</strong>oned, <strong>and</strong> there were more than twice as many harassments than kills. In British<br />

Columbia, common murre were primarily chosen. No attacks on birds were seen in Prince<br />

William Sound.<br />

Sea lions represented 12% <strong>and</strong> 13% of transient predation in the Glacier Bay <strong>and</strong><br />

British Columbia studies, respectively. Sea lions represented 15% of the transient diet in this<br />

author’s previous feeding study (Matkin <strong>and</strong> Dahlheim 1995) <strong>and</strong> in Lance Barrett-Lennard’s<br />

study on “The impact of killer whale predation on Steller sea lion populations in British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska,” (Barrett-Lennard et al. 1995). In all areas, transients ignored or just<br />

harassed sea lions more than twice as often than they achieved successful kills. All age <strong>and</strong><br />

sex killer whales participated in sea lion kills in group sizes of 4 to 6 individuals.<br />

Attacks lasted 15 to 65 minutes in which killer whales chased, breached on, tail<br />

slashed, dorsal slapped, pushed, rubbed against, hit sea lions into the air, carried them in their<br />

mouths or held them underwater. Sea lions responded by porpoising rapidly away, playing<br />

dead at the surface or attempting to inflict injury with teeth, nails <strong>and</strong> a ferocious disposition.<br />

Sea lion individuals smaller than adult males were chosen for prey items. After these long<br />

attacks with more surface activity needed to subdue the Steller sea lion, transients may breach<br />

with erections, spyhop, lobtail, pectoral fin slap <strong>and</strong> vocalize. Humpback whales investigated<br />

these attacks by rubbing past or lightly tail-slapping the sea lion <strong>and</strong> mingling with the killer<br />

whales underwater.<br />

Only one minke whale kill occurred in the study area, lasting 90 minutes. Thirteen<br />

transients lead by an adult male chased, surrounded, rammed <strong>and</strong> bit the minke’s underside<br />

<strong>and</strong> blocked its escape. It ultimately bled to death <strong>and</strong> sank. A single humpback whale came<br />

over to investigate the attack twice, whereupon the lead transient male rammed it broadside.<br />

The humpback defended itself by turning on its back <strong>and</strong> thrashing its tail up <strong>and</strong> down.<br />

Humpback whales also responded to transients’ close proximity by ignoring them, leaving the<br />

immediate area, having longer dive times, pectoral fin slapping, breaching, groaning, wheezeblowing<br />

<strong>and</strong> vocalizing.<br />

Large whale kills are considered uncommon or rare in British Columbia, <strong>and</strong> have<br />

never been documented in Prince William Sound. Since large portions of the large whales are<br />

not eaten, these episodes may occur to reinforce cooperative social bonds among transients,<br />

<strong>and</strong> provide practice sessions for their young. Out of 12 humpback whale attacks worldwide,<br />

8 occurred in southeastern Alaska <strong>and</strong> none were confirmed kills. Killer whale group sizes<br />

ranged from 1 to 17, attacks included all age <strong>and</strong> sex groups, lasting from 26 minutes to 4<br />

hours. Killer whales cooperated by keeping humpback adults separate from their calves<br />

(Sharpe 1990).<br />

Attacks on minke whales worldwide were more successful. Out of 8 attacks, all died.<br />

All killer whale age <strong>and</strong> sex groups participated in groups of 3 to 15 individuals. Attacks<br />

lasted 30 minutes to 3 ½ hours, <strong>and</strong> were frequently described as calm <strong>and</strong> deliberate. Minkes<br />

that could not escape did not fight back. They were pulled underwater, bitten, skinned, <strong>and</strong><br />

had their tongue, jaw, baleen, blubber <strong>and</strong> ribs eaten (Hancock 1965).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

100


Most large whale kills from Mexico to Alaska were of gray whales (Eschrichtius<br />

robustus). Four transients identified in Glacier Bay in 1989, were identified killing gray<br />

whale calves off Monterey, California in 1992. Out of 9 kills along the west coast, 5 were of<br />

calves. Eight attacks resulted in escape. Both attacks <strong>and</strong> successful kills were lead by adult<br />

males or females with young. In groups of 2 to 17, killer whales attacked gray whales from 1<br />

½ to 5 ½ hours (Baldridge 1972). After taking turns attacking, killer whales may depart the<br />

area up to 35 minutes, then return to eat <strong>and</strong> vocalize. They eat tongue, jaw, rostrum, baleen,<br />

m<strong>and</strong>ible, thorax <strong>and</strong> ventral surface blubber. Gray whales sometimes escaped attack by<br />

remaining motionless at the surface, retreating to shallows <strong>and</strong> kelp beds, exhaling below the<br />

surface, forming compact groups <strong>and</strong> calves climbing onto their mothers’ head (Marks 1998).<br />

A lack of predatory behavior of killer whales toward sea otters (Enhydra lutris) was<br />

noted in British Columbia, southeastern Alaska <strong>and</strong> Prince William Sound studies. Normal<br />

behavior for both residents <strong>and</strong> transients was to pass by or under sea otters with neither<br />

species seeming to notice the other. During 13 encounters in Glacier Bay or Icy Strait, with<br />

both residents <strong>and</strong> transients, this was the case. In 3 other encounters, sea otters reacted to<br />

residents’ spyhopping, <strong>and</strong> transients’ milling within 20 meters. Sea otters reacted by diving<br />

<strong>and</strong> resurfacing inside the kelp, looking around, or porpoising <strong>and</strong> diving away from the killer<br />

whales. Two Gulf of Alaska transient males identified in Glacier Bay in 1998 passed by 75<br />

sea otters (some singles, some mothers with pups) with no visible interactions.<br />

Unidentified killer whales also took <strong>and</strong> ate a swimming moose (Alces alces) in Icy<br />

Strait in 1992. They tried unsuccessfully to scare a second moose out of a nearby kelp bed,<br />

but they soon gave up, <strong>and</strong> the second moose later entangled itself in the kelp <strong>and</strong> drowned.<br />

Transients in Prince William Sound harassed (by chasing) salmon on one occasion, <strong>and</strong><br />

transients in Glacier Bay/Icy Strait harassed salmon on two occasions.<br />

Although the wide variety of prey presented here for transients indicates that they can<br />

be opportunistic, the actual diet of each small sub-group of transients may be more simple. It<br />

reflects a combination of prey resource availability in a given area <strong>and</strong> cultural transmission<br />

of specific hunting skills for that prey. Killer whale diet overall is the result of behavior<br />

learned within a structure of deep social bonds which is passed from one generation to the<br />

next.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Baldridge, A. 1972. Killer whales attack <strong>and</strong> eat a gray whale. Journal of<br />

Mammalogy 53: 898-900.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L., K. Heise, E. Saulitis, G. Ellis <strong>and</strong> C. Matkin. 1995. The impact<br />

of killer whale predation on Steller sea lion populations in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska.<br />

Report for the North Pacific Universities Marine Mammal Research Consortium Fisheries<br />

Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C.<br />

Bigg, M.A., G.M. Ellis, J.K.B. Ford <strong>and</strong> K.C. Balcomb III. 1987. Killer Whales: A<br />

study of their identification, genealogy <strong>and</strong> natural history in British Columbia <strong>and</strong><br />

Washington state. Phantom Press <strong>and</strong> Publishers, Inc., Nanaimo, B.C. 79 pp.<br />

Ford, J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> G.M. Ellis. 1999. Transients: Mammal-hunting killer whales. UBC<br />

Press, Vancouver, B.C.<br />

Ford, J.K.B., G.M. Ellis, L.G Barrett-Lennard, A.B. Morton, R.S. Palm <strong>and</strong> K.C.<br />

Balcomb III. 1998. Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whales<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

101


(Orcinus orca) in coastal British Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. Canadian Journal of Zoology<br />

76: 1456-1471.<br />

Goley, P.D. <strong>and</strong> J.M. Straley. 1994. Attack on gray whales (Eschrichtius robustus) in<br />

Monterey Bay, California, by killer whales (Orcinus orca) previously identified in Glacier<br />

Bay, Alaska. Canadian Journal of Zoology 72: 1528-1530.<br />

Hancock, D. 1965. Killer whales kill <strong>and</strong> eat a minke whale. Journal of Mammalogy<br />

46: 341-342.<br />

Leatherwood, S., K.C. Balcomb III, C. Matkin <strong>and</strong> G. Ellis. 1984. Killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) of southern Alaska. Hubbs Sea World Research Institute Technical Report<br />

No. 84-175. 59 pp.<br />

Marks, J. 1998. Two calves killed on migration through bay. The Monterey County<br />

Herald (April 22, 1998).<br />

Matkin, D.R. 1990. Killer whales in Glacier Bay <strong>and</strong> Icy Strait, Alaska. In: A.M.<br />

Milner <strong>and</strong> J.D. Wood, Jr. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Second Glacier Bay Science<br />

<strong>Symposium</strong>, 1988. National Park Service, Anchorage, Alaska. pp. 96-100.<br />

Matkin, D.R. <strong>and</strong> M.E. Dahlheim. 1995. Feeding behaviors of killer whales in<br />

northern southeastern Alaska. In: D.R. Engstrom (Ed.), Proceedings of the Third Glacier Bay<br />

Science <strong>Symposium</strong>, 1993. NPS, Anchorage, Alaska. pp.246-253.<br />

Matkin, C.O., D.R. Matkin, G.M. Ellis, E. Saulitis <strong>and</strong> D. McSweeney. 1997.<br />

Movements of resident killer whales between southeastern Alaska <strong>and</strong> Prince William Sound,<br />

Alaska. Marine Mammal Science, 13 (3) (July 1997).<br />

Matkin, C.O., G.M. Ellis, E. Saulitis, L.G. Barrett-Lennard<strong>and</strong> D.R. Matkin. 1999.<br />

Killer whales of southern Alaska. North Gulf Oceanic Society, Homer, Alaska.<br />

Saulitis, E., C. Matkin, L. Barrett-Lennard, K. Heise <strong>and</strong> G. Ellis. 2000. Foraging<br />

strategies of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William Sound,<br />

Alaska. Marine Mammal Science 16 (1): 94-109.<br />

Sharpe, F.A. C.G. D’Vincent <strong>and</strong> R.M. Nilson. 1990. Interactions between orcas <strong>and</strong><br />

cooperatively foraging humpback whales in southeastern Alaska. Abstract presented at the<br />

Third <strong>International</strong> <strong>Orca</strong> <strong>Symposium</strong>, Victoria, B.C.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

102


Figure 1<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

103


80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1984<br />

1986<br />

Glacier Bay/West Coast<br />

Transients Identified by Year<br />

1988<br />

1990<br />

Seabirds<br />

15%<br />

1992<br />

Figure 2<br />

1994<br />

Transient Prey by Percent<br />

in Glacier Bay/Icy Strait<br />

Minke Whale<br />

3%<br />

Steller Sea Lion<br />

12%<br />

Harbor/Dall's<br />

Porpoise<br />

27%<br />

Figure 3<br />

1996<br />

Harbor Seal<br />

43%<br />

1998<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

2000<br />

104


UNRAVELING THE INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL STRUCTURE AND PREY TYPE ON<br />

VOCAL COMMUNICATION IN KILLER WHALES<br />

Miller P..<br />

WHOI, MS#34; Woods Hole, MA 02543; USA, pmiller@whoi.edu.<br />

Killer whales are a cosmopolitan species with a wide range of different social structures<br />

that are strongly influenced by the temporal <strong>and</strong> spatial patterns of available prey. The<br />

diversity of social structures in this species offers a model to explore how social systems<br />

influence vocal behaviour <strong>and</strong> ontogeny through vocal learning. However, it is likely that a<br />

primary function of communication is to coordinate foraging behaviour, which will differ<br />

depending on prey-type, <strong>and</strong> sound production is affected strongly by the hearing abilities of<br />

prey (Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996). Therefore, a difficult but important challenge in comparing<br />

the vocal behavior of different killer whale populations will be to distinguish effects of prey<br />

behaviour <strong>and</strong> hearing sensitivity from effects of social structure. In addition to studies on<br />

prey hearing abilities, descriptions of the role of communication in foraging are needed. In<br />

residents, vocal behaviour is fairly well-described <strong>and</strong> the design <strong>and</strong> use of acoustic signals<br />

support Ford’s (1991) thesis that calling serves to maintain group stability <strong>and</strong> coordinate<br />

behavior, including foraging. However, because it is impossible to observe whales <strong>and</strong> their<br />

fish prey from the surface, we have no direct information on the role of acoustic<br />

communication in foraging. The social structure <strong>and</strong> sound production of herring-feeding<br />

killer whales off Norway appear quite similar to that of the salmon-feeding whales (Simila et<br />

al. 1996; Strager 1995). However, there are likely to be important differences in how group<br />

members coordinate behavior, <strong>and</strong> role of communication, during foraging on this smaller<br />

schooling prey. In fish-eating species, underwater tracking of individuals <strong>and</strong> prey using tags,<br />

sonar <strong>and</strong> hydrophone arrays is needed to elucidate how sounds are used in foraging. The role<br />

of communication during foraging on mammal prey may be somewhat easier to study because<br />

such feeding is more conspicuous <strong>and</strong> closer to the surface.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

105


O. ORCA ABUNDANCE, DISTRIBUTION, SEASONAL PRESENCE, PREDATION<br />

AND STRANDINGS IN THE WATERS AROUND KAMCHATKA AND THE<br />

KOMMANDER ISLANDS: AN ASSESSMENT BASED ON REPORTED SIGHTINGS<br />

1992-2000<br />

Mironova A.M.(1), Burdin A.M.(2), Hoyt E.(5), Jikiya E.L.(3), Nikulin V.S.(1), Pavlov N.N.(1), Sato<br />

H.(4), Tarasyan K.K.(3), Filatova O.A.(3),<br />

Sevvostrybvod, Petropavlovsk-Kamchatsky, Russia (1) ; Kamchatka Institute of Ecology <strong>and</strong> Nature<br />

Managment Petropavlovsk-Kamchatsky, Russia (2); Moscow State University, Russia (3); Tokyo,<br />

Japan (4); WDCS, North Berwick, Scotl<strong>and</strong> (5)<br />

In spite of wide-ranging O. orca presence in the waters of Kamchatka <strong>and</strong> the<br />

Comm<strong>and</strong>er Isl<strong>and</strong>s, published information is scarce due to the high cost <strong>and</strong> difficulty of<br />

working from ships in the North Pacific off far eastern Russia. In this work, we used Arc<br />

View software to analyze data about O. orca collected mainly by Sevvostrybvod surveyors<br />

<strong>and</strong> observers from 1992-2000 inclusive. Over 9 years, we report 274 encounters with 1,619<br />

animals around Kamchatka <strong>and</strong> the Comm<strong>and</strong>er Isl<strong>and</strong>s. O. orca seasonal presence was as<br />

follows: 14 encounters (61 animals) during the winter period, 44 encounters (196) in spring,<br />

126 encounters (726) in summer <strong>and</strong> 90 encounters (636 animals) in autumn months.<br />

However, without the calculation of level of effort throughout the year, the seasonal O. orca<br />

distribution is only approximate. Most common group size was 2-5 individuals (42.5% of<br />

encounters), while lone animals were 13.3% of encounters. The largest group consisted of 57<br />

individuals. As a rule, observers do not report the sexual composition of groups.<br />

Kamchatka peninsula:<br />

Most often, O. orca were found in areas along eastern Kamchatka with intensive marine<br />

traffic from the cape Bezymiannyi on the north to the cape Piratkov in the south (37 sightings<br />

of 507 animals) including Starichkov Isl<strong>and</strong>, bays Sarannaya, Viluchinskaya, Jirovaya,<br />

Falshivaya, Russkaya, Asacha <strong>and</strong> the capes of Opasniy, Kekurniy, Piramidniy, Polosatiy; in<br />

the region of cape Shipunskiy (4 sightings of 31 animals), Vestnik bay (15 sightings of 48<br />

animals).<br />

. There were 7 reported events of O. orca predation on sea mammals including walrus,<br />

Odobenus rosmarus, northern fur-seal, Callorhinus ursinus, , two attacks on northern minke<br />

whale, Balaenoptera acutorostrata, <strong>and</strong> three attacks on humpback whales, Megaptera<br />

novaeangliae. A total of 4 O. orca carcasses were found: an adult male (1999), a young<br />

female (2000) <strong>and</strong> two O. orca of unknown sex <strong>and</strong> age (1997 <strong>and</strong> 1999).<br />

Comm<strong>and</strong>er Isl<strong>and</strong>s:<br />

Here, O. orca are most often observed around northern fur seal (Callorhinus ursinus)<br />

rookeries; the most reported sightings were around capes Yushina (7 sightings of 18 orcas),<br />

Severo-Zapadnyi (4 sightings of 10 animals), South-east rookery (9 sightings of 94 animals),<br />

bays Nikolskaya (8 sightings of 23 orcas), Poludennaya (6 sightings of 94 orcas) <strong>and</strong><br />

Gladkovskaya (6 sightings of 9 animals).<br />

There were 3 recorded events of O. orca predation on marine mammals, twice on<br />

northern fur-seals, Callorhinus ursinus. Two dead adult O. orca: male (1994) <strong>and</strong> female<br />

(1997) were found on Bering Isl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

In addition, off Kamchatka, since 1999, O. orca have been preying on Pacific halibut,<br />

Hippoglossus stenolepsis <strong>and</strong> black halibut, Reinchardtins hippoglossoides, stealing from the<br />

bottom nets <strong>and</strong> longlines of fishermen. The scale of the damage has increased annually, <strong>and</strong><br />

this problem requires urgent attention.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

106


On the basis of these reported sightings alone (without considering current photo-ID<br />

or other work on O. orca), a preliminary estimate of abundance in summer for both<br />

Kamchatka <strong>and</strong> the Comm<strong>and</strong>er Isl<strong>and</strong>s would be at least 700-800 animals.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

107


FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

108


KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) IN AUSTRALIAN TERRITORIAL AND<br />

SURROUNDING WATERS – ARE THEY SECURE ?<br />

Margie Morrice 1 , Catherine Bell 1 , John van den Hoff 2 , Debbie Thiele 1 , Peter Gill 1 , Dave Paton 3 ,<br />

Magaly Chambellant 4<br />

1 School of Ecology & Environment, Deakin University, PO Box 423 Warrnambool, VIC 3280,<br />

Australia<br />

2 Australian Antarctic Division, Channel Hwy, Kingston TAS 7050, Australia<br />

3 Southern Cross Centre for Whale Research, Noosa, QLD, Australia<br />

4 Centre D’etudes Biologiques De Chize – <strong>CNRS</strong>, 79360 Beauvoir-sur-Niort, France<br />

BACKGROUND<br />

Australian waters contain a diverse array of marine habitats. The 11 million sq km of<br />

ocean under Australian jurisdiction extends from warm tropical waters to the coldest<br />

Antarctic waters. The area consists of Exclusive Economic Zones (EEZs) extending 200<br />

nautical miles from the Australian continent, Tasmania <strong>and</strong> its offshore isl<strong>and</strong>s - Christmas<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>, the Cocos <strong>and</strong> Keeling Group, Lord Howe Isl<strong>and</strong>, Norfolk Isl<strong>and</strong>, Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Territory of Heard Isl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> McDonald Isl<strong>and</strong>s. The waters off the Australian<br />

Antarctic Territory (AAT) are managed in accordance with the requirements of CCAMLR of<br />

which Australia is a leading member. The region supports locally productive marine<br />

communities that provide for a number of large marine predators including pelagic fish,<br />

sharks, squid, <strong>and</strong> toothed whales, the largest being sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus)<br />

<strong>and</strong> killer whales (Orcinus orca).<br />

Killer whales are often described as cosmopolitan in distribution, <strong>and</strong> the published<br />

literature for the Australian region supports this to some extent. Sightings of killer whales<br />

have been documented from most Australian states <strong>and</strong> territories, with the southern regions<br />

(South Australia, Victoria, Tasmania <strong>and</strong> Antarctic) having support from cetacean reporting<br />

programs (Cotton 1943, Chittleborough 1953, Aitken 1971, Guiler 1978, Parker 1978, TFDA<br />

1981, McManus et al. 1984, Anderson & Prince 1985, Nicol 1986, Kemper <strong>and</strong> Ling 1991,<br />

Ling 1991, Copson 1994, Dixon <strong>and</strong> Frigo 1994, Gill <strong>and</strong> Thiele 1997, Stewardson <strong>and</strong> Child<br />

1997, Thiele <strong>and</strong> Gill 1999, Chatto <strong>and</strong> Warneke 2000, Shaughnessy 2000, Janetzki <strong>and</strong><br />

Paterson 2001, Morrice <strong>and</strong> van den Hoff 2001, Paterson <strong>and</strong> Paterson 2001). However, many<br />

records remain unpublished.<br />

It was recognised that there would be value in bringing together the unpublished<br />

information on killer whales for the Australian region to provide baseline information. This<br />

would allow assessments of trends in killer whale ecology such as distribution, evidence for<br />

migration <strong>and</strong> foraging, <strong>and</strong> which could put ‘Australian’ killer whale populations in a world<br />

context. This information would then be available to base <strong>and</strong> support conservation <strong>and</strong><br />

management decisions for killer whales in Australian waters, <strong>and</strong> would provide a wider<br />

awareness of this species <strong>and</strong> its issues.<br />

METHODS<br />

Incidental records<br />

Information relating to killer whale sightings, str<strong>and</strong>ings, museum specimens <strong>and</strong><br />

fishery interactions were requested from Australian government agencies, museums, research<br />

institutions, non-government organisations <strong>and</strong> interested individuals. The management of<br />

this information varied from formal sightings <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ing databases to ad-hoc collections.<br />

These records were collected by observers with a range of cetacean observation experience as<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

109


part of regular conservation <strong>and</strong> museum activities, field research, fisheries operations, other<br />

platforms of opportunity programs or recreational activity. As such, the level of detail <strong>and</strong><br />

reliability of this information varied considerably. Any observable missing data or<br />

discrepancies in the data were queried <strong>and</strong> corrected.<br />

Records <strong>and</strong> anecdotal information, including reports of nil sightings, were obtained<br />

from the Museum <strong>and</strong> Art Gallery of the Northern Territory (NT), Queensl<strong>and</strong> Museum<br />

(QLD), Queensl<strong>and</strong> Parks <strong>and</strong> Wildlife Service (QLD), Queensl<strong>and</strong> Boating <strong>and</strong> Fisheries<br />

Patrol (QLD), Rob Paterson (QLD), James Cook University (QLD), Sea World (QLD), New<br />

South Wales National Parks <strong>and</strong> Wildlife Service (NSW), Australian Museum (NSW),<br />

Southern Cross Centre for Whale Research (NSW), Macleay Museum - University of Sydney<br />

(NSW), Taronga Park Zoo (NSW), Biological Sciences - Museum Macquarie University<br />

(NSW), Environment Australia (ACT), Australian Fisheries Management Authority (ACT),<br />

CSIRO (ACT), Dept. Natural Resources <strong>and</strong> Environment (VIC), Museum Victoria (VIC),<br />

Australocetus Research (VIC), Marequus (VIC), Deakin University (VIC), LaTrobe<br />

University (VIC), Phillip Isl<strong>and</strong> Nature Park (VIC), Applied Ecology Solutions (VIC), K. Lott<br />

(VIC), Dept. Primary Industries Water <strong>and</strong> Environment (TAS), Tasmanian Museum <strong>and</strong> Art<br />

Gallery (TAS), University of Tasmania (TAS), Warneke Marine Mammal Services (TAS),<br />

South Australian Museum (SA), Dept. Conservation <strong>and</strong> L<strong>and</strong> Management (WA), West<br />

Australian Museum (WA), John Bannister (WA), Centre for Whale Research (WA), <strong>and</strong><br />

Joanne Tilbury (WA). There are many other anecdotes, records, photographic <strong>and</strong> other<br />

material relating to killer whales for this region, however they are not easily accessible <strong>and</strong><br />

will need collation in the future (eg. aerial <strong>and</strong> boat-based surveys for marine wildlife <strong>and</strong><br />

surveillance).<br />

Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> sighting program<br />

A killer whale sighting program for Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> its surrounding waters was<br />

initiated in 1994 (Morrice <strong>and</strong> van den Hoff 2001). Killer whales were recorded by Australian<br />

National Antarctic Research Expedition (ANARE) personnel onto specifically designed<br />

sighting sheets. This allowed records of sightings, behaviour <strong>and</strong> environmental information<br />

to be collected in a consistent <strong>and</strong> detailed manner, <strong>and</strong> extended to the collection of baseline<br />

photographic <strong>and</strong> behavioural catalogues. Additional historical information prior to 1994 was<br />

collated from station log books <strong>and</strong> expeditioner’s field note books. The reliability of this data<br />

is considered good, as the personnel were provided with killer whale identification guides <strong>and</strong><br />

experienced marine mammal scientists were often on h<strong>and</strong> to help confirm <strong>and</strong> detail the<br />

sightings. Of all the observations relating to each sighting, only spatial <strong>and</strong> temporal data was<br />

assessed for this study. Behaviour, habitat-use <strong>and</strong> photo-identification information will be<br />

published separately.<br />

SOCEP program<br />

The Southern Ocean Cetacean Ecosystem Program (SOCEP, D. Thiele) has been<br />

conducting cetacean surveys in the AAT annually since the 1995 austral winter season. More<br />

recently the program has incorporated components other than visual survey (photo<br />

identification, biopsy <strong>and</strong> passive acoustics) <strong>and</strong> focussed effort on the multidisciplinary<br />

marine science voyages under what is now known as the Antarctic Marine Living Resources<br />

(AMLR program). SOCEP has conducted st<strong>and</strong>ardized annual cetacean surveys using<br />

experienced observers on-board a total of 17 ANARE voyages to the AAT (longitudinal range<br />

~70°E - 155°E ) between 1995 <strong>and</strong> 2001. A wide range of data were collected for all species<br />

encountered, including killer whales.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

110


Southern Oceans <strong>Orca</strong> Database<br />

The Southern Oceans <strong>Orca</strong> Database (SOOD) was established to merge all the available<br />

Australian killer whale information into a uniform format for querying <strong>and</strong> analysis. Data<br />

exchange agreements were set-up to manage the ownership <strong>and</strong> future exchange of<br />

information between the interested parties. Environmental data, including bathymetry,<br />

coastline <strong>and</strong> marine jurisdictional boundaries, were accessed from Geoscience Australia.<br />

ArcView Geographic Information Systems software (ESRI, 1996) was used to display <strong>and</strong><br />

analyse the sighting records. Remaining data was used to describe distribution, pod<br />

composition, other animals present, feeding <strong>and</strong> other behaviour including fishery<br />

interactions. The SOOD also summarises killer whale specimens that are currently held in<br />

institutions across Australia, including those that may be available for future analysis (eg.<br />

genetic, stomach contents, pollutant).<br />

RESULTS<br />

The results are preliminary. To date a total of 910 killer whale records have been<br />

collected from most regions in Australia (Figure 1). The majority of these records are<br />

incidental (793), <strong>and</strong> over 62% of all records were from Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong>. Other regions<br />

where sightings are common include South Australia, Victoria, south-east Tasmania <strong>and</strong> in<br />

waters surrounding the Australian Antarctic Territory. These aggregations likely reflect<br />

increased killer whale activity combined with a disproportionate effort in recording sightings.<br />

No sightings have been recorded in the Australian territories of the Cocos Keeling Isl<strong>and</strong><br />

Group, Christmas Isl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> the Heard <strong>and</strong> McDonald Isl<strong>and</strong> Group, however this does not<br />

mean killer whales are not there.<br />

Records for the tropical latitudes (0-25° S) are rare, mid-latitudes (25-50° S) are more<br />

common during the austral winter <strong>and</strong> spring, in the sub-Antarctic (50-60° S) there is a<br />

definite seasonal peak over the austral spring <strong>and</strong> summer months, <strong>and</strong> in Antarctic waters<br />

(60-80° S) killer whales are observed more commonly during the mid to late austral summer.<br />

In south-east Australian regions <strong>and</strong> at Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> the whales are seen all year round<br />

(Figure 2).<br />

Mean group size was approximately 4 (SD±3.07), ranging from 1 to 100. Mean group<br />

size for killer whales sighted in Antarctic waters was larger at approximately 6 (SD±5.15,<br />

range 1-60), <strong>and</strong> for Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> the mean was 4 (SD±1, range 1-20) whales per group.<br />

Some group size may have been under-estimated when the killer whales were widely<br />

dispersed.<br />

From the total number of killer whales seen around Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> at any one time, a<br />

minimum estimated population size of 20 was calculated. The typical group composition for<br />

this region is a single adult male with three females/juveniles. A photo-identification<br />

catalogue has recorded 13 individuals, with only one re-sight. This highlights the difficulty in<br />

collecting this type of information in extreme environments where shore-based observations<br />

dominate the data, <strong>and</strong> where dedicated research is lacking.<br />

The distribution of killer whale sightings correlates to areas where their prey are<br />

concentrated <strong>and</strong> fishing activities are seasonally active. These sightings are also probably<br />

biased by the increased reporting effort in these areas at these times. For the mid latitudes (25-<br />

50° S), killer whale sightings have been “naturally” associated with migrating humpback<br />

whales (Megaptera novaeangliae), sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus), dugong (Dugong<br />

dugon) feeding areas, Australian <strong>and</strong> New Zeal<strong>and</strong> fur seal breeding colonies (Arctocephalus<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

111


pusillus doriferus, Arctocephalus forsteri), <strong>and</strong> large groups of bottlenose <strong>and</strong> common<br />

dolphins (Tursiops truncates sp., Delphinus delphis). An “artificial” association exists<br />

between killer whales <strong>and</strong> the dropline <strong>and</strong> longline fisheries for tuna. It is not known whether<br />

fish taken from these lines (bait, target <strong>and</strong> bycatch) form part of the natural diet of these<br />

whales.<br />

At Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong> killer whales were most often sighted inshore during the peak<br />

breeding periods of southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) (particularly when weaned<br />

seals are learning to swim <strong>and</strong> leave their natal site for feeding grounds); New Zeal<strong>and</strong>,<br />

Subantarctic <strong>and</strong> Antarctic fur seals (Arctocephalus forsteri, A. tropicalis, A. gazella); <strong>and</strong><br />

king <strong>and</strong> royal penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus, Eudyptes schlegeli). In the AAT waters<br />

killer whales were associated with minke whales (Balaenoptera acutorostrata), crabeater<br />

seals (Lobodon carcinophagus) <strong>and</strong> emperor penguins (Aptenodytes forsteri), all of which are<br />

known prey for killer whales. These killer whales have been observed with diatom films on<br />

their skin <strong>and</strong> a distinctive cape pigmentation on their dorsal surface. Other wildlife<br />

associated with killer whale groups include seabirds such as petrels, albatross, skuas, gulls<br />

<strong>and</strong> prions; <strong>and</strong> fish, dolphins, white sharks (Carcharadon carcharius) <strong>and</strong> false killer whales<br />

(Pseudorca crassidens).<br />

KEY ISSUES<br />

The current key issues for killer whales in waters under Australian jurisdiction include:<br />

− a lack of baseline knowledge,<br />

− conservation status <strong>and</strong> management approaches which do not reflect the current state of<br />

knowledge overseas,<br />

− potential changes in prey availability associated with over-fishing <strong>and</strong> global climate<br />

change,<br />

− direct fishery interactions, <strong>and</strong><br />

− pollution ie. ingestion of marine debris <strong>and</strong> their susceptibility to accumulating high levels<br />

of heavy metals <strong>and</strong> organochlorines.<br />

The current conservation status, protection mechanisms <strong>and</strong> the management of<br />

information relating to killer whales in Australian waters reflects the paucity of available<br />

information in this region. In the Action Plan for Australian Cetaceans killer whales are<br />

defined under adopted IUCN categories as ‘insufficiently known‘ but ‘probably secure’, the<br />

only species of the 45 recognised cetacean taxa in Australian waters listed under this category,<br />

<strong>and</strong> which is based on estimates of this species in Antarctic waters (Bannister et al. 1996).<br />

Clearly, there is a requirement to take some positive action on the ‘insufficiently known‘<br />

status for this species to more accurately determine its current status <strong>and</strong> future survival<br />

probabilities.<br />

Interactions between killer whales <strong>and</strong> fisheries are common to longline <strong>and</strong> dropline<br />

fisheries in south-east Australia. To some extent these interactions have been documented by<br />

dedicated fishery observers <strong>and</strong> to a lesser extent from anecdotal accounts by fishing crew<br />

(Tasmanian Fisheries Development Authority 1981, McManus et al. 1984, Bell et al. in press,<br />

Morrice unpublished data).<br />

The direct nature of this type of interaction is detrimental to both the fishery <strong>and</strong> the<br />

whales themselves. Among the effects of interactions are possible direct competition for food,<br />

changes in whale behaviour, damage to fish gear, increased time <strong>and</strong> effort to fill quotas; <strong>and</strong><br />

direct mortality of whales from shooting, entanglement <strong>and</strong> drowning (Bell et al. in prep.).<br />

The stomach contents from a str<strong>and</strong>ed young male killer whale in South Australia provides<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

112


evidence that it had ingested pieces of at least three dolphin species (likely Delphinus delphis,<br />

Tursiops truncatus, unknown spp.) that had been cut with a knife. This suggests the dolphin<br />

had been used by a fisher as bait for the whale, whether to harm the whale or not is uncertain<br />

as the direct cause of death of this killer whale is unknown (Gibbs 2001). Recommended<br />

strategies to address the issue of fisheries interactions include a more detailed assessment <strong>and</strong><br />

quantification of fishery interactions, <strong>and</strong> the development of mitigation measures based on<br />

experience <strong>and</strong> successful programs developed in Australia <strong>and</strong> overseas.<br />

Killer whales rarely str<strong>and</strong>. To date 3 mass <strong>and</strong> 16 single str<strong>and</strong>ings have been<br />

confirmed. Although reporting of these events has improved with str<strong>and</strong>ing networks <strong>and</strong> an<br />

increase in public awareness, opportunities for the collection of crucial scientific information<br />

are not maximised. The collection of sightings, photo-identification information from freeranging<br />

killer whales, post-mortem samples <strong>and</strong> the need for dedicated research at key<br />

locations such as Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong>, the Australian Antarctic Territory, Coffs Harbour <strong>and</strong><br />

Tasmania is critical to the future management of this species.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Aitken, P. F. (1971). Whales from the coast of South Australia. Trans. R. Soc. S. Aust. 95(2): 95-103.<br />

Anderson, P. K. <strong>and</strong> Prince, R. I. T. (1985). Predation on dugongs: attack by killer whales. J. Mamm. 66<br />

(3): 554-556.<br />

Bannister, J. L., Kemper, C. M. <strong>and</strong> Warneke, R. M. (1996). The Action Plan for Australian Cetaceans.<br />

Australian Nature Conservation Agency. 242pp.<br />

Bell, C., Shaughnessy, P. Morrice, M. <strong>and</strong> Stanley, B. (in prep.). Marine mammals <strong>and</strong> Japanese long-line<br />

fishing vessels in Australian waters: interactions <strong>and</strong> other sightings.<br />

Chatto, Ray <strong>and</strong> Warneke, Robert M. (2000). Records of cetacean str<strong>and</strong>ings in the Northern Territory of<br />

Australia.<br />

The Beagle, Records of the Museums <strong>and</strong> Art Galleries of the Northern Territory 16: 163-175.<br />

Chittleborough, R. G. (1953). Aerial observations on the humpback whale, Megaptera nodosa<br />

(Bonnaterre), with notes on other species. Aust J. Marine <strong>and</strong> Fresh. Res. 4: 219-226.<br />

Copson, G. R. (1994). Cetacean sightings <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ings at subantarctic Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong>, 1968-1990.<br />

ANARE Research Notes 91.<br />

Cotton, B. C. (1943). Killer whales in South Australia. The South Australian Naturalist 22: 2-3.<br />

Dixon, J. M. <strong>and</strong> Frigo, L. (1994). The cetacean collection of the Museum of Victoria. An annotated<br />

catalogue.<br />

Australian Deer Research Foundation: Melbourne.<br />

Gibbs, Susan <strong>and</strong> Long, Martine (2001). Stomachs contents of a killer whale (Orcinus orca) implicate<br />

human<br />

interaction in South Australia. Poster paper for the Southern Hemisphere Marine Mammal Conference,<br />

Phillip Isl<strong>and</strong>, Australia.<br />

Gill, P. C. <strong>and</strong> Thiele, D. (1997). A winter sighting of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Antarctic sea ice.<br />

Polar Biol. 17: 401-404.<br />

Guiler, Eric R. (1978). Whale str<strong>and</strong>ings in Tasmania since 1945 with notes on some seal reports. Papers<br />

<strong>and</strong> Proceedings of the Royal Society of Tasmania 112: 189-213.<br />

Janetzki, H. A. <strong>and</strong> R. A. Paterson (2001). Aspects of humpback whale, Megaptera novaeangliae, calf<br />

mortality in Queensl<strong>and</strong>. Memoirs of the Queensl<strong>and</strong> Museum 47 (2): 431-435.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

113


Kemper, C. M. <strong>and</strong> Ling, J. K. (1991). Whale str<strong>and</strong>ings in South Australia (1881-1989). Transactions of<br />

the Royal<br />

Society of South Australia 115(1): 37-52.<br />

Ling, J. K. (1991). Recent sightings of killer whales, Orcinus orca (Cetacea: Delphinidae), in South Australia.<br />

Trans. R. Soc. S. Aust. 15(2): 95-98.<br />

McManus, T. J.; Wapstra, J. E.; Guiler, E. R.; Munday, B. L. <strong>and</strong> Obendorf, D. L. (1984). Cetacean str<strong>and</strong>ings in<br />

Tasmania<br />

from February 1978 to May 1983. Papers <strong>and</strong> Proceedings of the Royal Society of Tasmania 118: 117-<br />

135.<br />

Morrice, Margaret <strong>and</strong> van den Hoff, John (2001). Preliminary investigations of killer whales (Orcinus<br />

orca) from inshore waters around sub-Antarctic Macquarie Isl<strong>and</strong>. Society for Marine Mammalogy Twelfth<br />

Biennial Conference, Hawaii, 1999 (poster); <strong>and</strong> Southern Hemisphere Marine Mammal Conference, Phillip<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>, Australia, May 2001(poster).<br />

Nicol, D. J. (1986). A review <strong>and</strong> update of the Tasmanian cetacean str<strong>and</strong>ing record to the end of<br />

February 1986. Environmental Studies Working Paper 21. University of Tasmania. 93 pp.<br />

Parker, A. A. (1978). Observations of whales on ANARE voyages between Australia <strong>and</strong> Antarctica.<br />

Aust. Wildl. Res. 5: 25 39.<br />

Paterson, R. A. <strong>and</strong> Paterson, P. (2001). A presumed killer whale (Orcinus orca) attack on humpback<br />

whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) at Point Lookout, Queensl<strong>and</strong>. Memoirs of the Queensl<strong>and</strong> Museum 47(2):<br />

436.<br />

Shaughnessy, P. D. (ed.) (2000). Antarctic seals, whales <strong>and</strong> dolphins of the early twentieth century:<br />

Marine mammals of the Australasian Antarctic Exhibition, 1911-14 (AAE) <strong>and</strong> the British, Australian <strong>and</strong> New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> Antarctic Research Expedition, 1929-31 (BANZARE). ANARE Reports 142: 154 pp.<br />

Stewardson, C. L. <strong>and</strong> Child, P. L. (1997). Mammals of the ice: an introductory guide to the seals, whales<br />

<strong>and</strong> dolphins in the Australian subantarctic <strong>and</strong> Antarctica, based on records from ANARE voyages, 1977-90.<br />

Australian Antarctic Division, Hobart. Sedona Publishing, Canberra. 183 pp.<br />

Tasmanian Fisheries Development Authority (1981). Assessment of impact of interference from Orcinus<br />

orca (killer whale) on Tasmanian dropline fishery: preliminary report. Consultancy report for the Australian<br />

National Parks <strong>and</strong> Wildlife Service, Tasmania. 11 pp.<br />

Thiele, D. <strong>and</strong> Gill, P. C. (1999). Cetacean observations during a winter voyage into Antarctic sea ice<br />

south of Australia. Antarctic Science 11(1): 48-53.<br />

.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

114


Figure 1. Distribution of killer whale sightings <strong>and</strong> str<strong>and</strong>ings from both incidental <strong>and</strong> on-effort records for the<br />

Australian territorial region. Lines represent the 200 nm limit of waters under Australia’s jurisdiction<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

115


Figure 2. Seasonal <strong>and</strong> latitudinal patterns in killer whale sightings for the Australian territorial region.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

116


LIFE HISTORY AND POPULATION DYNAMICS OF RESIDENT KILLER<br />

WHALES IN ALASKA<br />

Olesiuk, Peter F. 1 , Craig O. Matkin 2 , Graeme M. Ellis 1 , <strong>and</strong> Eva L. Saulitis 2<br />

1 Department of Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans, Pacific Biological Station, Nanaimo, B.C., Canada V9R 5K6;<br />

2 North Gulf Oceanic Society, 60920 Mary Allen Avenue, Homer, Alaska, USA 99603<br />

Life history parameters were derived for resident killer whales in Alaska based on longterm<br />

photo-identification studies. Field work was conducted annually during 1984-2001 in<br />

the waters between the Kodiak Archipelago <strong>and</strong> SE Alaska, but was concentrated in PWS /<br />

Kenai Fjords during summer months. Although the total population numbers in excess of 450<br />

whales, our analysis was based on the 10 best-known pods comprised of 318 individuals, 152-<br />

229 of which were alive at any given time. All animals were not resighted in all years – in<br />

83% of instances animals were seen in consecutive years, in 14% there were gaps of 1-2 years<br />

in resightings, <strong>and</strong> in 3% there were gaps of 3-5 years. As a result, the exact year of death<br />

may not have been known in a few cases, <strong>and</strong> we amortized these over the period in question.<br />

Although newly recruited calves were not always seen in the year they were born, the year of<br />

birth could usually be established based on their size when first seen. To account for viable<br />

calves that might have been born during gaps but died prior to being seen, we applied a small<br />

correction (1.2 calves out of 141 observed births) based on the survival rates of calves that<br />

were observed every year <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>and</strong> length of gaps between consecutive encounters<br />

with reproductive females. The population included AB-pod, which was observed swimming<br />

in oil following the Exxon Valdez oil spill in 1989 <strong>and</strong> suffered very high mortality in that <strong>and</strong><br />

the following year. We excluded AB-pod in development of the baseline population model,<br />

<strong>and</strong> utilized the model to assess its dynamics during <strong>and</strong> following the oil spill.<br />

Following the approach developed by Olesiuk, Bigg <strong>and</strong> Ellis (1990) <strong>and</strong> updated by<br />

Olesiuk, Ellis, Ford <strong>and</strong> Balcomb (2000) for BC <strong>and</strong> WA resident killer whales, <strong>and</strong> using<br />

genealogies developed for southern Alaskan killer whales (Matkin, Ellis, Olesiuk <strong>and</strong> Saulitis<br />

1999; Matkin, Ellis, Saulitis, Barrett-Lennard <strong>and</strong> Matkin 1999) we employed various<br />

methods were used to estimate the ages of animals. A total of 178 whales were born during or<br />

just prior to the start of the study, <strong>and</strong> the exact year of birth could be established based on<br />

their small size in the year they were first observed. We refer to these as known-aged<br />

animals. Another 12 animals were smaller juveniles when first seen, <strong>and</strong> only the<br />

approximate year of birth could be determined by their size. The error associated with these<br />

estimates was judged to range from ±1 years for the youngest to ±5 years for the oldest<br />

juveniles. The 29 females that were juvenile-sized when first seen (but too large to accurately<br />

age based on size) were aged in reference to the year in which they gave birth to their first<br />

viable calf. The mean age at first birth was determined from known-aged animals. However,<br />

since the known-aged animals in AK study were too young to fully represent the maturation<br />

curve, we adopted the maturation curve for BC-WA residents, which indicated that the first<br />

viable calf was born at a mean age of 15.4 years (range 10-21 years). The uncertainty<br />

associated with these age estimates is about ±5 years, the observed range in age at which<br />

females first give birth. The 49 females that were adult-sized <strong>and</strong> first seen <strong>and</strong> gave birth to<br />

their first known viable calf during or prior to the study, were also aged in reference to the<br />

year of birth of their first viable calf. Since these adult-sized females may actually have given<br />

birth <strong>and</strong> lost younger progeny prior to the study, we applied a correction to account for<br />

potential calf loss based on calving intervals <strong>and</strong> survival rates of calves (see Olesiuk, Bigg<br />

<strong>and</strong> Ellis 1990). The corrections ranged from 0.6 years for females giving birth to their first<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

117


known offspring early in the study, to 1.9 years for females whose oldest offspring was aged<br />

15 years at the beginning of the study, to 5.1 years for females whose oldest offspring was<br />

aged 30 years at the start of the study. The 25 males that were juvenile-sized when first seen<br />

(but too large to estimate accurately based on size) were aged in reference to the year they<br />

attained sexual maturity. Age at sexual maturity was determined based on the year the dorsal<br />

fin first attained a height-to-width ratio of 1.4. For the same reasons outlined above for<br />

females, the maturation curve for northern BC resident males was employed, which indicated<br />

that males typically reached maturity at a mean age of 14.2 years (range 10-18 years). The<br />

uncertainty for these ages was thus about ±4 years. The 8 males that were sexually but not<br />

physically mature were aged in reference to the year the fin was completely developed. Data<br />

form BC-WA indicated that completion of fin development typically required 5.4 years (range<br />

3-7 years), which thus added an additional ±2 years to the uncertainty in the age estimate. For<br />

the 19 oldest males that were physically mature when first seen, we could only estimate their<br />

minimum age by assuming they had attained physical maturity in the year they were first<br />

seen.<br />

Thus far the rate of maturation for females in the Alaska population seems consistent<br />

with the maturation curve for BC-WA, which suggests that females typically give birth to<br />

their first viable calf at about age 15 years of age. Calves were subsequently produced at<br />

intervals that usually ranged from 3-7 years (mean 4.9 years), but were occasionally as short<br />

as 2 years or as long as 12 years. Mean calving intervals increased slightly but significantly<br />

with age of the mother, from 4.3 years at age 15 to about 5.6 years by age 40. Fecundity rates<br />

declined much more sharply with age, mainly because older females stopped calving. We<br />

estimated the rate of onset of post reproduction from the ratio of fecundity of reproductivelyactive<br />

females (defined as those that had given birth within the last 10 years) to all females.<br />

The median age of onset of reproductive senescence was 45 years, with virtually all female<br />

being post-reproductive by age 55 years. Southern Alaska resident killer whales would be<br />

expected to produce 5.7 calves over a 30-year reproductive lifespan, which was slightly<br />

greater than the 4.3 calves produced over a 23-year reproductive lifespan for BC northern<br />

residents.<br />

Survivorship for both males <strong>and</strong> females conformed with the classic mammalian Ushaped<br />

curve, indicating that the youngest <strong>and</strong> oldest animals experienced the highest<br />

mortality; however, the curve was narrower for males than females. Mortality rates for<br />

juveniles could not be estimated separately for each sex, but virtual equal numbers of males<br />

<strong>and</strong> females matured during the study suggesting that rates were similar (assuming an equal<br />

sex ratio at birth). The survivorship curve for males was very similar to those observed in<br />

northern BC residents, but the curve for females was narrower indicating that mortality<br />

increased more rapidly following the onset of reproductive senescence.<br />

The life history parameters were incorporated into life tables <strong>and</strong> a Lesile-type matrix<br />

population model to determine population parameters <strong>and</strong> model its dynamics. The life tables<br />

indicated that females had a mean life expectancy of 39 years <strong>and</strong> maximum longevity of<br />

roughly 60-70 years, <strong>and</strong> (assuming mortality of oldest males was the same as BC northern<br />

residents) males a mean life expectancy of 31 years <strong>and</strong> maximum longevity of roughly 50-60<br />

years. The reproductive value of females (i.e. the expected subsequent calf production)<br />

increased from 4.0 calves at birth to 4.4 calves at age 15 (because not all juveniles survive to<br />

reproduce), <strong>and</strong> then declined to zero by age 55 as they reached the end of their reproductive<br />

lifespan.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

118


The population model predicted that the population should increase at 2.7% per annum<br />

<strong>and</strong> be comprised of 51% juveniles, 23% mature males, 22% reproductive females <strong>and</strong> 5%<br />

post-reproductive females. The population actually grew at 3.3% per annum, <strong>and</strong> was<br />

comprised of 51% juveniles, 19% mature males, 24% reproductive females, <strong>and</strong> 7% postreproductive<br />

females. The population biology of Alaskan killer whales was remarkably<br />

similar to that observed in B.C. <strong>and</strong> Washington State during the 1970s <strong>and</strong> 80s, which<br />

increased at 2.9% <strong>and</strong> was comprised of 50% juveniles, 19% mature males, 21% reproductive<br />

females, <strong>and</strong> 10% post-reproductive females. One notable difference was that females in<br />

Alaska appeared to experience a more abrupt increase in mortality as they approached<br />

reproductive senescence, resulting in reduced longevity. During the Alaskan study, however,<br />

the proportion of post-reproductive females declined from 11% to 5%, suggesting it<br />

represented a period of atypically high morality for older females, <strong>and</strong> as a result we may<br />

have underestimated average female life expectancy <strong>and</strong> longevity.<br />

The population model was used to assess the dynamics of AB-pod, <strong>and</strong> the impact of<br />

the Exxon Valdez spill. This pod declined from 36 individuals in 1988 to 22 by 1990, which<br />

was nearly 10 times the expected mortality rate. Although other pods have increased at a<br />

mean rate of 3.3% per annum in the decade following the spill, AB-pod has shown little<br />

growth. Nevertheless, the population model indicated that the number of births in AB-pod<br />

since 1990 has been close to the number expected based on its sex- <strong>and</strong> age-structure (14<br />

observed births versus 13 predicted), although the number of deaths has been somewhat<br />

greater than expected (10 observed versus 6 expected). Thus, a reduced birth rate or greatly<br />

elevated death rate does not explain the lack of recovery of AB-pod. However, the<br />

reproductive value of the pod declined disproportionately to its reduction in size (49% versus<br />

38% respectively), implying that the animals lost following the spill tended to be those with<br />

higher than average reproductive values, i.e. young females. Had these females not been lost,<br />

the model indicated that an additional 12 calves would have been recruited into the pod in the<br />

decade following the spill. It thus appears the lack of recruitment due to the loss of females<br />

with high reproductive value is the primary reason for the lack of recovery of AB-pod<br />

following the Exxon Valdez oil spill.<br />

References:<br />

Olesiuk, P. F, M. A. Bigg, <strong>and</strong> G. M. Ellis. 1990. Life history <strong>and</strong> population dynamics of<br />

resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia <strong>and</strong><br />

Washington State. Rep. Int. Whal. Commn. (Special Report 12): 209-243.<br />

Olesiuk, P. F., G. M. Ellis, J. K. B. Ford <strong>and</strong> K. C. Balcomb. 2000. An update on the<br />

population dynamics of southern <strong>and</strong> northern resident whales in B.C. <strong>and</strong> Washington<br />

State. Presentation at the Southern Resident Killer Whale <strong>Workshop</strong>, National Marine<br />

Mammal Laboratory, Seattle, WA, 1-2 April 2000.<br />

Matkin, C. O., G. M. Ellis, P. Olesiuk, <strong>and</strong> E. L. Saulitis. 1999. Association patterns <strong>and</strong><br />

inferred genealogies of resident killer whales, Orcinus orca, in Prince William Sound,<br />

Alaska. Fishery Bulletin 97(4): 900-919.<br />

Matkin, C. O., G. M. Ellis, E. L. Saulitis, L. G. Barrett-Lennard, <strong>and</strong> D. R. Matkin. 1999.<br />

Killer whales of southern Alaska. North Gulf Oceanic Society, Homer, Alaska. 96 pp.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

119


MONITORING WHALE WATCHING ACTIVITY IN INTERNATIONAL WATERS<br />

Pakenham M.<br />

Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans 25 Huron St. Victoria, B.C. Canada V8V 4V9, Pakenhamm@pac.dfo-mpo.gc.ca<br />

The waters of Canada’s Pacific coast provide habitat for an extraordinary abundance <strong>and</strong><br />

diversity of marine mammals <strong>and</strong> seabirds. The Committee on the Status of Endangered Wildlife in<br />

Canada (COSEWIC) in November 2001, listed the southern resident population of north-eastern<br />

Pacific orcas as “Endangered”. The seemingly endless opportunities to encounter marine<br />

wildlife around southern British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington state have given rise to<br />

tremendous growth in commercial <strong>and</strong> recreational whale watching activities; estimated at<br />

USD$68 million annually in British Columbia alone. This economic growth has increased the<br />

pressure on the resident orca population dramatically. To raise awareness <strong>and</strong> help reduce<br />

potentially harmful impacts from these threats, the Marine Mammal Monitoring Project (M3)<br />

has collaborated with several Canadian <strong>and</strong> US agencies to develop international guidelines<br />

for marine mammal viewing. The M3 stewardship patrol vessel provides an on-water<br />

presence around whale watching activities <strong>and</strong> allows crew members to record observations<br />

on vessels engaged in wildlife viewing. Compiled into a database, this information provides<br />

the basis for feedback reporting to industry, <strong>and</strong> allows for the accurate characterisation of<br />

marine mammal viewing activities to assist resource managers <strong>and</strong> key-decision-makers. In a<br />

dynamic environment with up to fifty vessels actively viewing as many as seventy-eight<br />

orcas, the challenges for accurate observation have lead to new <strong>and</strong> innovative techniques for<br />

monitoring. As Canada <strong>and</strong> the U.S. move towards the development of marine mammal<br />

viewing regulations, issues such as jurisdiction, human behaviour change, licensing of<br />

vessels, intensity of activity, acoustical implications, sanctuaries, bio-accumulation of toxics,<br />

recovery plans <strong>and</strong> enforcement are ahead.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

120


LIFE HISTORY AND DECLINE OF KILLER WHALES IN CROZET<br />

Introduction<br />

ARCHIPELAGO, SOUTHERN INDIAN OCEAN<br />

Poncelet E. 1,2 , Guinet C. 1 , Mangin S. 1 , Barbraud C. 1 .<br />

1 Centre d’Etudes Biologiques de Chizé, 79360 Villiers-en-Bois, France,<br />

2 1, allée des Oliviers, 06400 Cannes, France, eponcelet@ifrance.com<br />

Killer whales in the coastal waters of Possession Isl<strong>and</strong>, Crozet Archipelago, southern<br />

Indian Ocean, are the subject of a long term monitoring study which started in 1987,<br />

additional opportunistic pictures back to the sixties <strong>and</strong> seventies were also analysed (Guinet,<br />

1988). Latest population parameters were presented by Guinet in 1991. For the period 1987-<br />

1990, 76 individuals were identified <strong>and</strong> the population declined by 7%. Since 1991,<br />

additional photo-identification pictures were collected through dedicated field work but also<br />

opportunistically. In the present study, we analysed the whole data set available to estimate<br />

population parameters <strong>and</strong> trends with mark-recapture models.<br />

Methods<br />

With regard to killer whales’ occurrence pattern in the coastal waters of Possession<br />

Isl<strong>and</strong>, mainly in October-December <strong>and</strong> in some extent in January (Guinet, 1991), we<br />

worked with shifted years running from April to March to include this peak in a single year<br />

(e.g., year 1998 started in April 1998 <strong>and</strong> ended in March 1999).<br />

Photo-identification<br />

The photo-identification field work was conducted from several locations on the shore<br />

of Possession Isl<strong>and</strong>, one of the main isl<strong>and</strong> of Crozet Archipelago. Sighting histories were<br />

extracted from the photo-identification database i) to apply mark-recapture models <strong>and</strong> ii) to<br />

calculate gross fecundity rates.<br />

Mark-recapture modelling<br />

We considered sets of near years (consecutive when possible) with high photoidentification<br />

effort, <strong>and</strong> eventually high number of new identifications, to estimate a trend in<br />

abundance with Robust Design mark-recapture models. The program CAPTURE (Rexstad <strong>and</strong><br />

Burnham, 1991) was used to h<strong>and</strong>le different models featuring an individual heterogeneity in<br />

the recapture probability (h) <strong>and</strong>/or a behavioural response to capture (b) <strong>and</strong>/or a timedependent<br />

recapture probability (t), or none of the previous (M(0)).<br />

Years with low photo-identification effort (less than 15 pictures) were discarded to estimate<br />

survival without excessive bias. The program U-CARE (Choquet et al., 2001) was used to test<br />

the goodness-of-fit of the general Cormack-Jolly-Seber (CJS) model to our data, <strong>and</strong> CJSderived<br />

models were h<strong>and</strong>led with the program MARK (White <strong>and</strong> Burnham, 1999).<br />

Gross fecundity calculation<br />

The average annual fecundity rate was calculated after the method used by Bigg in 1982.<br />

It was defined as the proportion of the summed number of years for which identified mature<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

121


females were monitored (the “cow years”), against the number of viable neonates (about 12<br />

months of age) born to these females during the cow years.<br />

Results<br />

Photo-identification<br />

The photo-identification effort was highly variable, ranging from 1 to 783 pictures/year<br />

(fig. 1). Years with high effort correspond to years when dedicated field work has been carried<br />

out. In 2001, the cumulated number of identified individuals was 93, including animals<br />

assumed dead. An undetermined number of individuals were not identified because they were<br />

poorly marked <strong>and</strong> did not show up close enough to the shore to get good identification<br />

pictures.<br />

Abundance modelling <strong>and</strong> estimation<br />

We selected the years 1988, 1989, 1998 <strong>and</strong> 2000 as two sets of near years representing<br />

two primary recapture occasions with a 9-year interval, each including two secondary<br />

recapture occasions. After these data, the program CAPTURE provided a selection criteria for<br />

each model available (table 1)<br />

Cumulated number of identified inviduals<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

5 1 5 9 1 2 3 7 27 10<br />

0<br />

11 6<br />

44<br />

61<br />

4 4<br />

40 40<br />

1964<br />

1965<br />

1966<br />

1967<br />

Effort (number of pictures)<br />

Cumulated number of identified individuals<br />

1968<br />

1969<br />

1970<br />

1971<br />

1972<br />

1973<br />

1974<br />

1975<br />

1976<br />

1977<br />

1978<br />

1979<br />

1980<br />

1981<br />

1982<br />

Years<br />

1983<br />

1984<br />

1985<br />

1986<br />

445<br />

1987<br />

397<br />

330<br />

1988<br />

1989<br />

72<br />

1990<br />

1991<br />

1992<br />

1993<br />

1 1 1 13<br />

Fig. 1. Photo-identification effort (number of pictures) <strong>and</strong> cumulated number of<br />

identified individuals from 1964 to 2001.<br />

Table 1. Model selection criteria computed by the program CAPTURE. The higher the<br />

value is, the better the model fits the data.<br />

M(0) M(h) M(b) M(bh<br />

)<br />

1994<br />

1995<br />

1996<br />

395<br />

1997<br />

1998<br />

83<br />

783<br />

1999<br />

2000<br />

7<br />

2001<br />

1000<br />

900<br />

800<br />

700<br />

600<br />

500<br />

400<br />

300<br />

200<br />

100<br />

M(t) M(th) M(tb) M(tb<br />

h)<br />

0.84 0.16 0 0.85 0.41 0.18 0.09 1<br />

The first highest ranked model is M(tbh) <strong>and</strong> has no estimator. The second highest<br />

ranked model is M(bh) for which the program CAPTURE proposes two estimators: Pollock<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

0<br />

Effort (number of picutres)<br />

122


<strong>and</strong> Otto’s <strong>and</strong> Generalised Removal. Pollock <strong>and</strong> Otto’s estimator provided estimates of 93<br />

(SE = 6.78, approximate 95% CI = 84-110) individuals in 1988-1989 <strong>and</strong> 43 (SE = 4.47,<br />

approximate 95% CI = 38-56) in 1998-2000 (fig. 2). The Generalised Removal estimator<br />

couldn’t estimate the abundances by lack of sufficient recapture occasions. Finally the third<br />

highest ranked model, M(0), provided estimates of 83 (SE = 6.22, approximate 95% CI = 76-<br />

101) individuals in 1988-1989, <strong>and</strong> 36 individuals (SE = 2.58, approximate 95% CI = 34-46,<br />

fig.2) in 1998-2000.<br />

Survival modelling <strong>and</strong> estimation<br />

The years considered for survival modelling were 1977, 1980, 1982, 1985 to 1989, 1990<br />

<strong>and</strong> 1998 to 2000. The goodness of fit tests indicated that there was no departure from the<br />

general CJS model. However, the model did not fit the data very well (χ²=47.40, df=27,<br />

p=0.009). The overdispersion factor ( ĉ ) measuring the lack of fit as the ratio of the degrees of<br />

freedom against the χ² statistic was 1.755. It is commonly admitted that a c ˆ inferior to 3 is<br />

acceptable <strong>and</strong> can be used in the model selection (Lebreton et al., 1992).<br />

Abundance estimates<br />

(with 95% CI)<br />

120<br />

100<br />

80<br />

60<br />

40<br />

20<br />

0<br />

M(bh) model<br />

M(0) model<br />

1988-1989 1998-2000<br />

Periods<br />

Fig. 2. Outputs of the Robust Designs models M(bh) <strong>and</strong> M(0) for the years1988, 1989,<br />

1998 <strong>and</strong> 2000.<br />

The process of model selection is illustrated in table 2. Our initial model was the general<br />

CJS model (1), which features time-dependent survival (φ) <strong>and</strong> recapture probabilities (p). In a<br />

first step, we simplified the survival modelling. A model with constant survival fitted the data<br />

better than the initial model (model 1 vs. 2), but a model with trend-dependent survival was<br />

even more acceptable (model 1 vs. 3). In a second step, we simplified the modelling of the<br />

recapture probability. A model with a constant recapture probability did not fit the data better<br />

than the initial model (model 1 vs. 4), but a model with a photo-identification effort-dependent<br />

recapture probability fitted better (model 1 vs. 5). Finally, in a third step, we simplified both<br />

survival <strong>and</strong> recapture probability <strong>and</strong> tested a model with a trend-dependent survival <strong>and</strong> an<br />

effort-dependent recapture probability. This model was more acceptable than the initial model<br />

(model 1 vs. 6).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

123


Table 2. Process of model selection for the analysis of survival (models are adjusted for<br />

ĉ = 1.755). QAICc is the corrected Akaike’s Information Criterion: the lower the QAICc is,<br />

the more parsimonious the model is.<br />

Model QAICc ∆QAICc #Param.<br />

(3) φtrend,pt 280.479 0.000 12<br />

(2) φ,pt 287.295 6.816 12<br />

(5) φt,peffort 289.086 8.607 7<br />

(6) φtrend,peffort 291.908 11.429 4<br />

(1) φt,pt 299.199 18.720 22<br />

(4) φt,p 311.271 30.792 12<br />

The most parsimonious model was model 3 featuring a time trend-dependent survival<br />

<strong>and</strong> a time-dependent recapture probability, <strong>and</strong> estimating a declining survival from 0.979<br />

(SE = 0.016, 95% CI = 0.907-0.995) in 1977-1980 to 0.815 (SE = 0.048, 95% CI = 0.701-<br />

0.892) in 1999-2000 (fig. 3).<br />

Average fecundity rate<br />

From 1985 to 1990, only two viable neonates were identified in 42 cow years, resulting<br />

in a average annual fecundity rate of 4.76%. From 1998 to 2000, no viable neonate was<br />

observed, resulting in a null fecundity.<br />

Survival rate (with 95% CI)<br />

1<br />

0.95<br />

0.9<br />

0.85<br />

0.8<br />

0.75<br />

0.7<br />

0.65<br />

Periods<br />

Fig 3. Output of the φtrend,pt model: annual survival rates for all individuals between<br />

1977 <strong>and</strong> 2000 (confidence intervals corrected for ĉ = 1.755).<br />

Discussion<br />

The modelling of the abundance <strong>and</strong> survival of the killer whales in the coastal waters of<br />

Possession Isl<strong>and</strong> indicate strong declines between 1988 <strong>and</strong> 2000. Several factors may have<br />

combined <strong>and</strong> resulted in this situation: i) a low <strong>and</strong> decreasing fecundity, possibly impacted<br />

by a density dependence (Allee effect); ii) the decline of the main preys: large baleen whales<br />

due to past whaling, <strong>and</strong> southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) from the 1970 to 1990<br />

which remained in low numbers up to 1997 at least (Guinet et al., 1999); iii) the possible<br />

mortality induced by recent interactions with the Patagonian toothfish (Dissostichus<br />

eleginoides) longline fishery; <strong>and</strong> iv) the possible dispersion of individuals or groups from the<br />

coastal waters. A few individuals were observed with poorly known “offshore” killer whales<br />

interacting with longliners, but presently, there is no evidence of mixing with surrounding<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

124


killer whale concentrations in Prince Edward Isl<strong>and</strong>s, south Africa or Antarctic. A preliminary<br />

toxicological study indicates that PCBs levels are considerably lower than in British Columbia<br />

transients, however the burdens are not negligible (Ross, pers. com.) <strong>and</strong> the effects of PCBs<br />

on health at the observed concentrations are unknown. We fear that the killer whales of<br />

Possession Isl<strong>and</strong> might disappear with unique genetic diversity <strong>and</strong> social culture, like AT1<br />

transients in Alaska (Matkin et al., 1999).<br />

References<br />

Bigg, M.A. 1982. An assessment of killer whale (Orcinus orca) stocks off Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

British Columbia. Reports of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission, 32: 655-666.<br />

Guinet, C. 1988. Historique de la présence des orques autour de l'île de la Possession, archipel<br />

Crozet: photo-identification 1964-1986. Mammalia, 52(2): 285-289<br />

Guinet, C. 1991. L'orque (Orcinus orca) autour de l'Archipel Crozet, comparaison avec d'autres<br />

localités. Revue d'Ecologie (Terre et Vie), 46: 18-34.<br />

Guinet, C., Jouventin, P., Weimerskirch, H. 1999. Recent population change of the southern<br />

elephant seal at Îles Crozet <strong>and</strong> Îles Kerguelen: the end of the decrease? Antarctic Science,<br />

11(2): 193-197.<br />

Lebreton, J.-D., Burnham, K.P., Colbert J., Anderson D.R. 1992. Modeling survival <strong>and</strong> testing<br />

biological hypotheses using marked animals: a unified approach with case studies. Ecological<br />

Monographs, 62(1): 67-118.<br />

Matkin, C.O., Saulitis, E.L., Ellis, G.M., Barrett-Lennard, L.G. 1999. The AT1 group of<br />

transient killer whales in southern Alaska: a unique population in decline. In: Abstracts of 13 th<br />

Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals, Maui, Hawaii, USA, 28 November -<br />

3 December 1999. The Society for Marine Mammalogy.<br />

Rexstad, E., Burnham, K.P. 1991. Users’ guide for Interactive Program CAPTURE. Colorado<br />

Cooperative Fish & Wildlife Research Unit, Colorado State University, Fort Collins,<br />

Colorado.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

125


TOXIC CHEMICAL POLLUTION AND PACIFIC KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS<br />

ORCA)<br />

Peter S. Ross 1 , Graeme Ellis 2 , John K.B. Ford 2 , Lance-Barrett-Lennard 3<br />

1 Institute of Ocean Sciences, P.O. Box 6000, Sidney BC V8L 4B2, Canada (rosspe@pac.dfompo.gc.ca)<br />

2 Pacific Biological Station, Hammond Bay Road, Nanaimo BC Canada<br />

3 Vancouver Aquarium, P.O. Box 3232, Vancouver BC V6B 3X8 Canada<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

We recently found that killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations frequenting the coastal<br />

waters of British Columbia (Canada) <strong>and</strong> Washington (USA) are among the most<br />

contaminated marine mammals in the world. Our study benefited from (i) knowledge of<br />

individual identities, relationships <strong>and</strong> feeding preferences for all resident <strong>and</strong> most transient<br />

killer whales (documented through a long-term photo-identification catalogue); (ii) a biopsy<br />

dart system which sampled skin <strong>and</strong> blubber (approximately 250 mg); <strong>and</strong> (iii) comprehensive<br />

chemical analysis, which detected over 200 different Persistent Organic Pollutants (POPs),<br />

including polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs). We are currently attempting to answer two<br />

major questions; namely: ‘Where are these contaminants coming from?’ <strong>and</strong> ‘Are these high<br />

contaminant levels affecting the health of killer whales?’. To this end, we are carrying out a<br />

food chain-based study to track the movement <strong>and</strong> fate of contaminants in coastal British<br />

Columbia <strong>and</strong> the NE Pacific Ocean. We are also developing a suite of skin- <strong>and</strong> blubberbased<br />

biomarkers of contaminant effects. Our harbour seal (Phoca vitulina) research has<br />

contributed to an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of chemical sources, contaminant trends in the region <strong>and</strong><br />

POP-related health effects in marine mammals. Our work suggests that while Puget Sound<br />

(Washington State) represents a PCB “hotspot”, the preferred prey of resident killer whales<br />

(Chinook salmon) accumulates 99% of its POP burden from the open Pacific Ocean. While<br />

contaminated sites along the west coast of North America may continue to introduce<br />

chemicals into killer whale food chain, the deposition of atmospherically-transported<br />

chemicals of Asian origin into the NE Pacific represents an additional concern. The 20%<br />

decline in southern resident killer whale numbers in recent years highlights the need for<br />

conservation-based management which addresses such findings in the context of diminishing<br />

prey abundance <strong>and</strong> disturbance from heavy boat traffic.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Killer whales (Orcinus orca) represent, without question, charismatic megafauna,<br />

attracting the interest <strong>and</strong> attention of millions of people worldwide. British Columbia is no<br />

exception, where aboriginal legends <strong>and</strong> new world ecotourism have cemented the status of<br />

killer whales as the principal icons of the NE Pacific Ocean. In addition to the public interest<br />

in killer whales, there has been considerable scientific research on this marine mammal<br />

species in British Columbia (B.C.). The pioneering work of the late Dr. Michael Bigg <strong>and</strong> his<br />

colleagues in establishing individual-based photo-identification catalogues (Bigg et al.<br />

1990;Ford et al. 2000;Ford <strong>and</strong> Ellis 1999;Ford et al. 1994) has provided an invaluable basis<br />

for studies of killer whale population abundance, distribution, feeding ecology,<br />

communication, <strong>and</strong> genetic relationships (Ford et al. 1998;Barrett-Lennard 2000;Ford <strong>and</strong><br />

Fisher 1982).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

126


TOXICOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF TOXIC CHEMICALS IN MARINE<br />

MAMMALS<br />

There are several classes of environmental contaminants of concern in the context of<br />

marine mammals. In general, the persistent (P), bioaccumulative (B) <strong>and</strong> toxic (T) or PBT<br />

chemicals represent the greatest health risk to high trophic level marine mammals. This is due<br />

to the fact that such chemicals do not readily break down in the environment or in organisms;<br />

they are lipophilic (fat-soluble) <strong>and</strong> therefore become increasingly concentrated at each level<br />

in aquatic food chains as fats are metabolized; <strong>and</strong> are hormone mimics. Such “endocrine<br />

disrupting” compounds can present a health risk to highly exposed wildlife.<br />

Chemicals of particular concern on a global scale include the “legacy” (i.e. regulated in<br />

the industrialized world) compounds such as the polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), the<br />

polychlorinated dibenzo-p-dioxins (PCDDs or dioxins), <strong>and</strong> the organochlorine pesticide<br />

DDT. Current or “new” chemicals of concern (i.e. largely unregulated in the industrialized<br />

world) include the polybrominated diphenyl ethers (PBDEs) <strong>and</strong> pharmaceutical products.<br />

The endocrine disrupting properties of many members of these chemical classes<br />

represent a profound concern to marine mammals. Several populations of pinnipeds <strong>and</strong><br />

cetaceans inhabiting or frequenting the industrial waters of northern Europe, the<br />

Mediterranean Sea, the St Lawrence estuary in eastern Canada, <strong>and</strong> Puget Sound in NW<br />

U.S.A. have been found to be highly contaminated with industrial <strong>and</strong> agricultural chemicals<br />

such as PCBs <strong>and</strong> DDT (Ross et al. 1996a; Aguilar <strong>and</strong> Borrell 1994; Muir et al. 1999; Ross<br />

et al. 2001). High contaminant levels have been associated with reproductive impairment <strong>and</strong><br />

skeletal lesions in free-ranging marine mammals (Helle et al. 1976; Bergman et al. 1992).<br />

Captive feeding studies have implicated PCBs in reproductive, immune function <strong>and</strong><br />

endocrine effects in harbour seals (Reijnders 1986; De Swart et al. 1996; Ross et al. 1996a;<br />

Ross et al. 1996b).<br />

CONTAMINANTS IN KILLER WHALES<br />

Two major factors explain the relative contamination of many marine mammals: i)<br />

trophic level (the higher trophic level species being more contaminated because of<br />

biomagnification through the food chain); <strong>and</strong> ii) proximity to industrial areas. Fish-eating or<br />

mammal-eating marine mammals are therefore almost always more contaminated with PBT<br />

contaminants than baleen whales. Despite findings that marine mammals frequenting<br />

industrial coastal waters have a tendency to be contaminated with e.g. PCBs, it has become<br />

increasingly evident that marine mammal inhabiting even remote (“pristine”) environments<br />

have become contaminated as a result of long-range transport of atmospheric pollutants (Muir<br />

et al. 2000).<br />

More recently, the photo-identification catalogues have served as a foundation for<br />

studies of toxic chemicals in killer whales (Ross et al. 2000). The development of a biopsy<br />

technique to obtain micro-samples of skin <strong>and</strong> underlying blubber from known killer whales<br />

at a distance (Barrett-Lennard et al. 1996) enabled an assessment of the effects of such factors<br />

as age, sex <strong>and</strong> dietary preferences on contaminant concentrations. The inherent value <strong>and</strong><br />

ultimate impact of our contaminant studies in killer whales reflects a combination of i)<br />

knowledge of age, sex <strong>and</strong> diet of sampled individuals, ii) the ability to obtain microsamples<br />

in a minimally-invasive manner, iii) a highly sensitive method to measure contaminants in<br />

samples obtained, <strong>and</strong> iv) our ability to set the results in the context of biological, ecological<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

127


<strong>and</strong> toxicological sciences. Our findings can therefore provide us with a robust overview of<br />

the degree of toxic chemical contamination in healthy, free-ranging Pacific killer whales.<br />

Our recent research indicated that the transient <strong>and</strong> southern resident killer whale<br />

communities of the NE Pacific Ocean can now be considered among the most contaminated<br />

marine mammals in the world, surpassing the endangered St Lawrence Beluga whales by a<br />

factor of two to five times (Ross et al. 2000). Concentrations of PCBs in all killer whale<br />

populations studied in British Columbia exceeded immunotoxicity <strong>and</strong> endocrine disruption<br />

threshold levels established for captive harbour seals (Ross et al. 1995; De Swart et al. 1994).<br />

While interspecies comparison must be made with caution, the free-ranging killer whales in<br />

the NE Pacific Ocean must be considered at risk for adverse health effects.<br />

WHERE ARE THESE CONTAMINANTS COMING FROM?<br />

Upon discovering that NE Pacific killer whales can now be considered among the most<br />

contaminated marine mammals in the world, we asked ourselves ‘where are these chemicals<br />

coming from?’. We are exploring this question in different ways. Firstly, our harbour seal<br />

research suggests that Puget Sound (northern Washington State) represents a ‘hotspot’ for<br />

PCB contamination (Ross et al. 2001). This suggests that local (i.e. resident) fish species (<strong>and</strong><br />

occasional prey items for resident killer whales) are highly contaminated with PCBs. In<br />

addition, the principal prey for resident killer whales (salmonids) are likely obtaining most of<br />

their contaminants in offshore areas where they grow <strong>and</strong> feed in the North Pacific Ocean<br />

(O'Neill et al. 1998; Ewald et al. 1998). This suggests that resident killer whales are, in fact,<br />

providing a signature of both “local” <strong>and</strong> “global” contamination.<br />

As long-lived, high trophic level species, killer whales are vulnerable to contamination<br />

by persistent <strong>and</strong> bioaccumulative contaminants. While the “legacy” chemicals have been<br />

regulated in North America <strong>and</strong> Europe, many are still in production <strong>and</strong> use in the<br />

developing world. In addition, “new” chemicals have been found at increasing concentrations<br />

in arctic ringed seals (Ikonomou et al. 2002), suggesting that the global environment<br />

continues to be contaminated by PBT compounds. Levels of most “legacy” contaminant<br />

decreased steadily during the 1970s <strong>and</strong> 1980s, although these trends have since stalled<br />

(Addison <strong>and</strong> Stobo 2001). Killer whales will continue to face risks from exposure to many of<br />

these compounds. Our current assessment using a modelling-based approach to predicting<br />

concentrations in killer whales over time (Hickie et al. 2001) suggests that these risks are<br />

likely to continue for decades to come.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

1. Addison,R.F. <strong>and</strong> Stobo,W.T. 2001. Trends in organochlorine residue concentrations<br />

<strong>and</strong> burdens in grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) from Sable Is., N.S., Canada, between<br />

1974 <strong>and</strong> 1994. Environ.Pollut. 112(3): 505-513.<br />

2. Aguilar,A. <strong>and</strong> Borrell,A. 1994. Abnormally high polychlorinated biphenyl levels in<br />

striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) affected by the 1990-1992 Mediterranean<br />

epizootic. Sci.Total Environ. 154: 237-247.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

128


3. Barrett-Lennard,L.G. 2000. Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) as revealed by DNA analysis. University of British Columbia, Ph.D.<br />

thesis.<br />

4. Barrett-Lennard,L.G., Smith,T.G., <strong>and</strong> Ellis,G.M. 1996. A cetacean biopsy system<br />

using lightweight pneumatic darts, <strong>and</strong> its effect on the behavior of killer whales.<br />

Mar.Mammal Sci. 12: 14-27.<br />

5. Bergman,A., Olsson,M., <strong>and</strong> Reil<strong>and</strong>,S. 1992. Skull-bone lesions in the Baltic grey<br />

seal (Halichoerus grypus). Ambio 21: 517-519.<br />

6. Bigg,M.A., Olesiuk,P.F., Ellis,G.M., Ford,J.K.B., <strong>and</strong> Balcomb,K.C. 1990. Social<br />

organization <strong>and</strong> geneology of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the coastal<br />

waters of British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State. Rep.Int.Whaling Comm. 12: 383-<br />

405.<br />

7. De Swart,R.L., Ross,P.S., Vedder,L.J., Timmerman,H.H., Heisterkamp,S.H., Van<br />

Loveren,H., Vos,J.G., Reijnders,P.J.H., <strong>and</strong> Osterhaus,A.D.M.E. 1994. Impairment of<br />

immune function in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) feeding on fish from polluted waters.<br />

Ambio 23: 155-159.<br />

8. De Swart,R.L., Ross,P.S., Vos,J.G., <strong>and</strong> Osterhaus,A.D.M.E. 1996. Impaired<br />

immunity in harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) exposed to bioaccumulated environmental<br />

contaminants: review of a long-term study. Environ.Health Perspect. 104 (suppl. 4):<br />

823-828.<br />

9. Ewald,G., Larsson,P., Linge,H., Okla,L., <strong>and</strong> Szarzi,N. 1998. Biotransport of organic<br />

pollutants to an inl<strong>and</strong> Alaska lake by migrating sockeye salmon (Oncorhyncus<br />

nerka). Arctic 51: 40-47.<br />

10. Ford,J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Ellis,G.M. 1999. Transients: Mammal-hunting killer whales. UBC<br />

Press, Vancouver.<br />

11. Ford,J.K.B., Ellis,G.M., <strong>and</strong> Balcomb,K.C. 1994. Killer whales. UBC Press,<br />

Vancouver.<br />

12. Ford,J.K.B., Ellis,G.M., <strong>and</strong> Balcomb,K.C. 2000. Killer whales. UBC Press,<br />

Vancouver.<br />

13. Ford,J.K.B., Ellis,G.M., Barrett-Lennard,L.G., Morton,A.B., Palm,R.S., <strong>and</strong><br />

Balcomb,K.C. 1998. Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of killer<br />

whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal British Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. Can.J.Zool.<br />

76: 1456-1471.<br />

14. Ford,J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Fisher,H.D. 1982. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) dialects as an indicator<br />

of stocks in British Columbia. Rep.Int.Whaling Comm. 32: 671-679.<br />

15. Helle,E., Olsson,M., <strong>and</strong> Jensen,S. 1976. DDT <strong>and</strong> PCB levels <strong>and</strong> reproduction in<br />

ringed seal from the Bothnian Bay. Ambio 5: 188-189.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

129


16. Hickie, B. D., Ross, P. S., <strong>and</strong> Macdonald, R. An examination of the history of PCB<br />

accumulation by north Pacific killer whales. Society for Marine Mammalogy<br />

Vancouver, Canada. 2001.<br />

17. Ikonomou,M.G., Rayne,S., <strong>and</strong> Addison,R.F. 2002. Exponential increases of the<br />

brominated flame retardants, polybrominated diphenyl ethers, in the Canadian Arctic<br />

from 1981 to 2000. Environmental Science <strong>and</strong> Technology 36: 1886-1892.<br />

18. Muir,D., Riget,F., Cleemann,M., Skaare,J., Kleivane,L., Nakata,H., Dietz,R.,<br />

Severinsen,T., <strong>and</strong> Tanabe,S. 2000. Circumpolar trends of PCBs <strong>and</strong> organochlorine<br />

pesticides in the Arctic marine envrionment inffered from levels in ringed seals.<br />

Environmental Science <strong>and</strong> Technology 34: 2431-2438.<br />

19. Muir,D.C.G., Koczanski,K., Rosenberg,B., <strong>and</strong> Bél<strong>and</strong>,P. 1999. Persistent<br />

organochlorines in beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas) from the St Lawrence river<br />

estuary-II. Temporal trends, 1982-1994. Environ.Pollut. 93(2): 235-245.<br />

20. O'Neill,S.M., West,J.E., <strong>and</strong> Hoeman,J.C. 1998. Spatial trends in the concentration of<br />

polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) in Chinook (Oncorhyncus tshawytscha) <strong>and</strong> Coho<br />

salmon (O. kisutch) in Puget Sound <strong>and</strong> factors affecting PCB accumulation: Results<br />

from the Puget Sound Ambient Monitoring Program. Puget Sound Research '98 312-<br />

328.<br />

21. Reijnders,P.J.H. 1986. Reproductive failure in common seals feeding on fish from<br />

polluted coastal waters. Nature 324: 456-457.<br />

22. Ross,P.S., De Swart,R.L., Addison,R.F., Van Loveren,H., Vos,J.G., <strong>and</strong><br />

Osterhaus,A.D.M.E. 1996a. Contaminant-induced immunotoxicity in harbour seals:<br />

wildlife at risk? Toxicology 112: 157-169.<br />

23. Ross,P.S., De Swart,R.L., Reijnders,P.J.H., Van Loveren,H., Vos,J.G., <strong>and</strong><br />

Osterhaus,A.D.M.E. 1995. Contaminant-related suppression of delayed-type<br />

hypersensitivity <strong>and</strong> antibody responses in harbor seals fed herring from the Baltic<br />

Sea. Environ.Health Perspect. 103: 162-167.<br />

24. Ross,P.S., De Swart,R.L., Van Loveren,H., Osterhaus,A.D.M.E., <strong>and</strong> Vos,J.G. 1996b.<br />

The immunotoxicity of environmental contaminants to marine wildlife: A review.<br />

Ann.Rev.Fish Dis. 6: 151-165.<br />

25. Ross,P.S., Ellis,G.M., Ikonomou,M.G., Barrett-Lennard,L.G., <strong>and</strong> Addison,R.F. 2000.<br />

High PCB concentrations in free-ranging Pacific killer whales, Orcinus orca: effects<br />

of age, sex <strong>and</strong> dietary preference. Mar.Pollut.Bull. 40: 504-515.<br />

26. Ross,P.S., Jeffries,S., Yunker,M., Addison, R.F., Ikonomou,M., <strong>and</strong> Calambokidis,J.<br />

2003: Harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington reveal both<br />

'local' <strong>and</strong> 'global' PCB, PDD <strong>and</strong> PCDF signals. submitted.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

130


THE BIOLOGY AND STATUS OF AN ENDANGERED TRANSIENT KILLER<br />

WHALE POPULATION IN PRINCE WILLIAM SOUND, ALASKA<br />

Eva Saulitis, Craig Matkin <strong>and</strong> Graeme Ellis<br />

North Gulf Oceanic Society, 60920 Mary Allen Ave., Homer, Alaska 99603, saulitis@pobox.xyz.net.<br />

The AT1 transient population of southern Alaska is on the verge of extinction. The<br />

AT1s are mammal-eating killer whales inhabiting a limited range, between Prince William<br />

Sound <strong>and</strong> the Kenai Fjords. They share this range with several resident (fish-eating) killer<br />

whale pods <strong>and</strong> with the Gulf of Alaska transient population; however, they do not associate<br />

with those groups.<br />

The AT1 killer whales are genetically isolated from both the west coast transient<br />

population that ranges from southeastern Alaska through southern California, from the<br />

sympatric Gulf of Alaska transient population, <strong>and</strong> from Prince William Sound residents<br />

(Barrett-Lennard 2000). The amount of genetic diversity within the AT1 genome suggests<br />

that these animals were recently part of a much larger population (Barrett-Lennard, pers.<br />

comm.)<br />

The AT1 diet consists primarily of harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) <strong>and</strong> Dall’s porpoises<br />

(Phocoenoides dalli). Unlike Gulf of Alaska transients, AT1 transients are not known to feed<br />

on western Steller sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus) (Saulitis et al. 2000).<br />

AT1 transients utilize three foraging strategies: nearshore foraging, offshore foraging<br />

<strong>and</strong> foraging in glacial fjords. Almost all prey killed during offshore foraging are porpoises.<br />

Porpoise attacks involve a coordinated effort of group members (mean size = 5.0<br />

whales/group) lasting up to 40 minutes. Nearshore <strong>and</strong> glacial fjord foraging are primarily for<br />

harbor seals. Group size for nearshore foraging was smaller than for offshore foraging (3.3<br />

killer whales/group.) Single males <strong>and</strong> male pairs were often observed hunting seals in<br />

glacial fjords, where seals congregate to give birth, molt <strong>and</strong> haul out.<br />

The acoustic repertoire of the AT1s mirrors their genetic separation from other<br />

transients. Their 12 stereotyped pulsed calls are entirely different than those of Gulf of<br />

Alaska or west coast transients. Use of calls by AT1 transients is context-specific <strong>and</strong> closely<br />

tied to their foraging strategies. Three calls are statistically correlated with foraging; these are<br />

low amplitude, short vocalizations easily masked by underwater background noise.<br />

Otherwise, foraging <strong>and</strong> traveling whales are silent, using a strategy of passive listening to<br />

locate prey.<br />

AT1 transients commonly travel in foraging groups of 2-6 that represent long-term<br />

associations among individuals, including male-male <strong>and</strong> female-offspring associations.<br />

Males commonly travel alone for periods of time, using specific stereotyped vocalizations to<br />

relocate other AT1s. These particular high amplitude calls are emitted almost exclusively for<br />

the purpose of long-distance communication <strong>and</strong> can be detected several km away.<br />

Regularly, larger groups of up to 22 AT1s form for social purposes. In these situations,<br />

whistles are the most common vocalization. High frequency whistles are considered close<br />

contact calls as they attenuate rapidly over distance.<br />

Since 1990, the AT1 transient population has declined from 22 animals to nine in 2002.<br />

This decline may reflect the interaction of several anthropogenic <strong>and</strong> biological factors. In<br />

1984, when the study began, the sex ratio of AT1s <strong>and</strong> their small population size already<br />

suggested a population in decline from a loss of reproductive females. Adult males made up<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

131


41% of the population, double that of other transient populations <strong>and</strong> of resident pods.<br />

Nevertheless, between 1978 <strong>and</strong> 1984, the AT1s produced three viable offspring. Who were<br />

the fathers of those calves? Were they males who died in 1990, before biopsy work was<br />

begun? Or did females have access to other AT1-type individuals? Since 1984, no new AT1<br />

calves have been documented.<br />

The population of harbor seals, the AT1s’ primary prey, has declined by 80% since the<br />

1970s (Frost et al. 1999). The effect is more marked in the oil-impacted part of Prince<br />

William Sound. Nine AT1s disappeared during the winter of 1990, suggesting that those<br />

mortalities were oil spill related. Two AT1 males, one in his thirties, <strong>and</strong> one twenty-one<br />

year old who never physically matured, beached <strong>and</strong> died in 2000 <strong>and</strong> 2001.<br />

Analysis of blubber samples indicates that environmental toxin (PCB <strong>and</strong> DDT) levels<br />

in the blubber of AT1 transients are twenty to thirty times higher than those of residents,<br />

within the range known to cause reproductive problems in other marine mammal species<br />

(Ylitalo et al. 2001).<br />

As the apex predators in the marine food chain of Prince William Sound, the demise of<br />

the AT1 transient population points to persistent problems in the environment. The AT1s are<br />

symbolic of the fragility of ocean ecosystems. Their inevitable extinction will be a genetic<br />

loss, a further change in the ecological structure of Prince William Sound, <strong>and</strong> a cultural loss,<br />

as they are part of the traditional mythology of the region. Even today, Chugachmiut <strong>and</strong><br />

Alutiiq Eskimo elders <strong>and</strong> tradition-bearers believe that when killer whales enter a bay, death<br />

will come to someone in their village, that whales are calling someone, as humans become<br />

killer whales when they die. Though there is little hope for the survival of the ATs, their fate<br />

illustrates the vulnerability inherent in the social structure of killer whale populations.<br />

REFERENCES CITED<br />

Barrett-Lennard, Lance. 2000. Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer<br />

whales,<br />

Orcinus orca, as revealed by DNA analysis. PhD dissertation, University of British<br />

Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.<br />

Frost, K.J, L.F. Lowry & J. Ver Hoef. 1999. Monitoring the trend of harbor seals in<br />

Prince William Sound after the Exxon Valdez oil spill. Marine Mammal Science: 15.2:<br />

494-506.<br />

Saulitis, E.L., C.O. Matkin, L. Barrett-Lennard, K. Heise & G. Ellis. 2000. Foraging<br />

strategies of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William Sound,<br />

Alaska. Marine Mammal Science: 16.1: 94-109.<br />

Ylitalo, G., C.O. Matkin, J. Buzitis, M.M. Krahn, L.L. Jones, T. Rowles & J.E. Stein.<br />

2001. Influence of life-history parameters on organochlorine concentrations in freeranging<br />

killer whales (Orcinus orca) from Prince William Sound, Alaska. The Science of the<br />

Total Environment: 281: 183-203.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

132


MATCHED VOCAL EXCHANGES OF SHARED STEREOTYPED CALLS IN FREE-<br />

RANGING RESIDENT KILLER WHALES<br />

Shapiro, A. D., Miller, P. J. O., Tyack, P. L., Solow, A. R..<br />

Gatty Marine Laboratory; University of St. Andrews; Fife KY16 8LB; UK, as64@st-<strong>and</strong>rews.ac.uk.<br />

Previous recordings of resident killer whale groups reveal matrilineal group-specific call<br />

repertoires <strong>and</strong> a strong tendency for calls of the same type to be produced in series. Vocal<br />

interactions between individual free-ranging animals, however, had remained unexplored<br />

because it had not been possible to identify signalers reliably with a single hydrophone. Here<br />

we link acoustic arrivals of calls on a towed hydrophone array with visual tracking of photoidentified<br />

individuals to ascribe calls to a focal animal when separated by >35 m, <strong>and</strong> thereby<br />

out of visual range, from other members of its natal group. We confirm that individual<br />

members of a matrilineal natal group share a repertoire of stereotyped calls, <strong>and</strong> statistically<br />

examine aspects of call timing between group members. Because analysis of the intervals<br />

between stereotyped calls indicated that calls were produced in bouts with a bout criterion<br />

interval of 19.6s, we developed r<strong>and</strong>omisation tests that preserved call interval structure. A<br />

rotation test revealed focal whales were four times more likely than chance to produce a call<br />

within 5 s of a call from a non-focal animal. Consecutive calls produced by different<br />

individuals during group calling bouts matched type more than expected by chance, providing<br />

further evidence that individual calling behaviour is influenced by the calling behaviour of<br />

group members. Matched exchanges of stereotyped calls may allow receivers to respond<br />

specifically to the signals of the previous signaler. By communicating individual position <strong>and</strong><br />

possibly orientation, these exchanges may function to co-ordinate <strong>and</strong> synchronise movement<br />

patterns <strong>and</strong> group behaviour<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

133


SATELLITE TRACKING STUDY OF MOVEMENTS AND DIVING BEHAVIOUR<br />

OF KILLER WHALES IN THE NORWEGIAN SEA<br />

Similä, Tiu (1), Holst, Jens Christian (2), Øien, Nils (3) <strong>and</strong> Hanson M. Bradley (4)<br />

1,2,3 Institute of Marine Research, P.O.Box 1870, Nordnes 5817 Bergen, Norway<br />

4 NOAA, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Mammal Laboratory, 7600 S<strong>and</strong> Point<br />

Way NE, Seattle, WA 98115 USA<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

A long term photoidentification study on behavioural ecology of killer whales (Orcinus<br />

orca) was initiated in Norway in 1987. The occurrence of killer whales in coastal waters is<br />

related to the seasonal migrations of Norwegian spring-spawning herring (Clupea harengus)<br />

(Christensen 1988). This herring stock winters in a fjord system north of the arctic circle <strong>and</strong><br />

an estimated 500-600 killer whales are present in this area from October to January (Similäet<br />

al. 1996). The behaviour <strong>and</strong> habitat use of killer whales has been studied in the wintering<br />

grounds of herring (Similä 1997) but the short period of daylight in fall <strong>and</strong> winter has not<br />

allowed for studies on possible diurnal patterns <strong>and</strong> group specific differences in area use <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviour. After the killer whales <strong>and</strong> herring leave the wintering grounds, little is known<br />

about the movements or behaviour of killer whales (Similä et al. 1996). The lack of a core<br />

area for killer whale occurrence, especially during summer months, has made it difficult to<br />

locate killer whales between January <strong>and</strong> October. Recent developments in tag design has<br />

made it possible to study the ranging <strong>and</strong> diving behaviour of small odontocetes (for<br />

references see Hooker <strong>and</strong> Baird 2001).<br />

In 2000-2001 seven killer whales were equipped with satellite- <strong>and</strong> VHF tags in the<br />

wintering grounds of herring. The study was conducted in cooperation with studies on<br />

Norwegian spring-spawning herring, which is at least seasonally the main type of prey of<br />

killer whales in these waters. The aim of the project is to study seasonal movements <strong>and</strong><br />

diving behaviour of killer whales, focusing on interactions between herring <strong>and</strong> killer whales.<br />

This abstract summarises preliminary results on range <strong>and</strong> habitat use of killer whales during<br />

wintering period of herring.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Two whales were captured <strong>and</strong> tagged in 2000 <strong>and</strong> five whales in 2001. All whales were<br />

tagged in Vestfjord <strong>and</strong> Tysfjord in the core wintering grounds of herring in northern Norway.<br />

The transmitters deployed on the whales consisted of pair of tags mounted on the sides of the<br />

dorsal fin. A satellite transmitter was mounted on one side <strong>and</strong> VHF transmitter on the<br />

opposite side using a four pin attachment arrangement (Hanson 2001). Five of the satellite<br />

transmitters were were duty cycled to transmit daily while two of the tags transmitted every 3<br />

days. One of whales, an adult male, had a time-depth recorder attached with a suction cup<br />

providing 60 hours of detailed dive data after it floated free <strong>and</strong> was recovered. The<br />

performance of the five tags attached in 2001 was better than of the two tags attached in 2000.<br />

Based on observations made of one the tagged whales in 2000, the positions of the tag <strong>and</strong><br />

its antenna, were modified which improved tag performance in 2001. All individuals were<br />

radio-tracked <strong>and</strong> observed after their release. No long-term reaction to the tags or tagging<br />

could be detected.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

134


The total number of good quality locations received from the tagged whales during the<br />

wintering period of herring varied between 12-56 positions. Most of the positions were<br />

received from the wintering grounds of herring. However, five of the tagged whales made<br />

long distance movements away from this area; the swimming <strong>and</strong> diving behaviour of the<br />

whales as well as information on prey items suggests that the function of these trips was to<br />

survey areas where herring is abundant during other seasons than winter.<br />

Based on photoidentification data collected since 1987 the range of killer whales during<br />

October-January had been estimated to be 13 583 km 2 (estimated as a minimum convex<br />

polygon). The satellite tracking study has exp<strong>and</strong>ed the known range of killer whales during<br />

this season considerably. The ranges varied between the individuals; the smallest estimated<br />

Kernel home range was 3566 km 2 (95 % isopleth) <strong>and</strong> the largest 288 284 km 2 (95 %<br />

isopleth).<br />

In addition to new knowledge about the range of the whales during this season, the data<br />

set has also given the first detailed insight into the habitat use of killer whale individuals<br />

within the wintering grounds of herring. There were differences in the habitat use of the pods<br />

<strong>and</strong> the core area where killer whales have been photoidentified was not used equally by the<br />

different groups. This data is currently being compared to the long-term photoidentification<br />

data set for analysis for the areas where the pods have been identified <strong>and</strong> for differences in<br />

the number of observations/pod.<br />

REFERENCES:<br />

Christensen, I. 1988. Distribution, movements <strong>and</strong> abundance of killer whales (Orcinus<br />

orca) in Norwegian coastal waters 1982-1987 based on questionnaire surveys. Rit Fiskideildar<br />

XI, 79-88.<br />

Hanson, M. B. 2001. An evaluation of the relationship between small cetacean tag<br />

design <strong>and</strong> attachment durations: a bioengineering approach. Unpubl. Ph.D. dissertation,<br />

Univ. of Washington, Seattle, WA. 208 pp.<br />

Hooker, S <strong>and</strong> Baird, R. 2001. Diving <strong>and</strong> ranging behaviour of odontocetes: a<br />

methodological review <strong>and</strong> critique. Mammal Rev 31 (1): 81-105.<br />

Similä, T., Holst, J.C <strong>and</strong> Christensen, I 1996. Occurrence <strong>and</strong> diet of killer whales in<br />

northern Norway: seasonal patterns relative to the distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of Norwegian<br />

spring-spawning herring. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 53: 769-779.<br />

Similä, T. 1997. Behavioural ecology of killer whales in northern Norway. PhD thesis,<br />

NFH, University of Tromsø 1997.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

135


CLICKS, CALLS AND UNDERWATER TAIL-SLAPS; SOUNDS OF NORWEGIAN<br />

KILLER WHALES.<br />

1 1 2 1<br />

Simon M. , Miller L. , Ugarte F. , Wahlberg M. .<br />

1 Center for Sound Communication, Institute of Biology, SDU-Odense, Denmark;<br />

msimon@post.tele.dk<br />

2 Allégade 23B 3.th, 2000 Frederiksberg, Denmark.<br />

The presentation concerns the sounds produced by Norwegian killer whales during the<br />

herring wintering season. There were two goals; first to measure source levels <strong>and</strong> spectral<br />

characteristics of sounds produced by killer whales while feeding on herring <strong>and</strong> secondly to<br />

describe the relationship between the vocalisations <strong>and</strong> the activity of Norwegian killer<br />

whales. For the first, killer whales were recorded using a four-hydrophone array (recording<br />

b<strong>and</strong>width: 1Hz to either 150 kHz or 300 kHz). In order to estimate apparent source levels<br />

(ASL), the distance to the whales was estimated from the differences in the time of arrival of<br />

the sound to the hydrophones. For the second, sound recordings in the proximity of whales<br />

were made using a recording system with a frequency range of 20Hz to 20kHz. One out of<br />

four activity states (feeding, travelling, resting or socialising) were assigned to the whales by<br />

an experienced observer. Five minute samples of 40 recording sessions (10 samples for each<br />

activity state) were analysed. Preliminary results suggest that the presumed echolocation<br />

clicks of these killer whales are broadb<strong>and</strong>, with centre frequencies of 26-57 kHz. The 10 dB<br />

b<strong>and</strong>width was 12-32 kHz <strong>and</strong> the average click duration was 38 µs. ASL’s ranged from 187<br />

to 213 dB re. 1µPa (p-p) @ 1m (n = 52 clicks, including clicks on- <strong>and</strong> off-axis). The<br />

underwater tail-slaps, used by the whales to stun herring, produced multi-pulsed broadb<strong>and</strong><br />

sounds with ASL of 187 dB re. 1µPa (p-p) @ 1m (n = 1). Feeding behaviour was<br />

characterised by high vocal activity <strong>and</strong> was the only activity where tail-slaps occurred. In<br />

general, killer whales travelled in silence. Most whistles were heard during socialising.<br />

Resting behaviour was not clearly defined acoustically.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

136


AERIAL VOCALISATIONS OF KILLER WHALES TEMPORARILY CAPTURED<br />

IN NORTHERN NORWAY.<br />

Simon M.J. 1 , Ugarte F. 2 .<br />

1 Inst. Biology, University of Odense, Campusvej 55, 5230 Odense M., Denmark.,<br />

msimon@post.tele.dk; 2 Allégade 23 B, 3.th. 2000 Frederiksberg. Denmark.<br />

The aim of this study is to describe the vocalisations of two temporarily captured killer<br />

whales. During satellite tagging of killer whales in northern Norway, two killer whales were<br />

lifted to the deck of a tagging vessel. On deck, the vocalisations <strong>and</strong> blows of the whales were<br />

recorded using a DAT recorder <strong>and</strong> a directional microphone placed 0,5 m. in front of the<br />

whale. 65 minutes of sounds were analysed using Avisoft Lab software. No whistles were<br />

produced <strong>and</strong> only a few isolated echolocation clicks appeared. Each whale repeated one or<br />

two compound calls made of two to three elements. The elements could appear alone, in pairs<br />

or in triads. When in pairs or triads, the elements always followed the same order (e.g.: abc,<br />

ab, ac or bc). To our knowledge, compound calls among odontocetes have only been<br />

described for Norwegian killer whales. Under water recordings of the pod of one of the<br />

whales are being analysed for call matches. So far, the call produced by the captured whale<br />

has not been found in these recordings. Being cautious about small sample size (n = two<br />

captures), the fact that both whales used only compound calls, suggest that compound calls<br />

can be used in emotional contexts, such as stress. The fact that the vocalisation emitted by one<br />

whale has not been heard during the recordings of its pod while engaged in different<br />

behaviours, suggests that this particular vocalisation is used during specific contexts. The fact<br />

that two whales caught, belonging to two different pods, produced different calls, indicate that<br />

these calls belong to the pod specific dialect.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

137


INTERACTIONS BETWEEN KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) AND RED<br />

TUNA (THUNNUS THYNNUS) FISHERY IN THE STRAIT OF GIBRALTAR<br />

de Stephanis R. 1 , Pérez Gimeno N. 1 , Salazar Sierra J. 1 , Poncelet E. 2 , Guinet C. 3,1 .<br />

1 CIRCé Cabeza de Manzaneda 3, Pelayo, 11390 Algeciras, Cadiz, Spain, renaud@teleline.es ;<br />

2 1, allée des Oliviers, 06400 Cannes, France; 3 <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, 79360 Villiers-en-Bois, France<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Sightings of Killer Whales have been reported in the area of The Strait of Gibraltar for<br />

more than 500 years. (Bayed <strong>and</strong> Beaubrun, 1987, Aloncle, 1964, Morcillo, pers comm).<br />

This area is also very important for tuna fisheries. The Red Tuna (Thunnus thynnus)<br />

migrates every year throughout The Strait of Gibraltar entering the Mediterranean sea in<br />

spring to breed, <strong>and</strong> leaving the Mediterranean sea in summer (Rodriguez, J. 1964). For the<br />

last 500 years, the traditionnal way of fishing Red Tuna has been the Almdraba (pound nets),<br />

where the Killer Whales were interacting in the Strait <strong>and</strong> in close tuna fisheries areas<br />

(Morcillo, M., pers. com.).<br />

This large fast swimming fish species appears to be the main fish prey of Killer Whales<br />

in the area in spring <strong>and</strong> summer.<br />

In the last decade fishermen have been starting to use drop lines to catch the Red Tuna,<br />

<strong>and</strong> it is just this interaction that is the topic of the study.This research project started in 1998<br />

in The Strait of Gibraltar using different whale-watching boats. In 2001 <strong>and</strong> 2002 the project<br />

starts to works from the research boat ELSA.<br />

METHODOLOGY<br />

During the year of 1998, interviews to fishermen were carried out in order to know<br />

exactly where the fishing boats were seeing the Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> the possible interactions<br />

with them in the area of Tarifa. During the years 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2000, whale watching trips were<br />

carried along the Strait of Gbraltar, <strong>and</strong> the killer whales were observed around the fishermen,<br />

in the West area of the Strait. In the summers of 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2000, 16 dedicated Killer Whale<br />

whale-watching trips with one or two experienced observers onboard were carried out in the<br />

study region. These trips had an average duration of 3:50 hrs <strong>and</strong> two different boats ,of 7 <strong>and</strong><br />

9-m long, were used for this purpose between 22th July <strong>and</strong> 20th August of both years. Data<br />

concerning number of individuals, social structure, <strong>and</strong> general behaviour were recorded, <strong>and</strong><br />

pictures of the dorsal fins were taken for identification purposes in each one of the sightings,<br />

although not all the animals were photographed in each sighting due to the whale watching<br />

conditions. During the years 2001 <strong>and</strong> 2002 aleatoric research trips were carried along the<br />

Strait of Gibraltar. Those transects can be seen in FIG 1.-2<br />

Observations regarding to the depredation of tuna from the drop line by the Killer<br />

Whales, as well as the reactions of the fishermen were also recorded. Furthermore, the<br />

number <strong>and</strong> type of fishing vessels observed around the group of animals were also identified.<br />

RESULTS SIGHTINGS<br />

Between 1998 <strong>and</strong> 2002 around 14 000 nautical milles ahve been sailed in the Strait of<br />

Gibraltar resulting in 1898 sightings of Common Dolphins (Delphinus delphis), Striped<br />

Dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba), Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), Long Finned<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

138


Pilot Whales (Globicephala melas), Sperm Whales (Physeter macrocephalus) Fin Whales<br />

(Balaenoptera physalus) <strong>and</strong> Killer Whales.<br />

Killer Whales were only observed in the western past of the Strait.This area is centred 5<br />

miles north of Tanger, next to the sea mountains "Monte Tartesos", "Cañón de Bolonia" <strong>and</strong><br />

"Cresta Kmara"<br />

Between 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2002, 35 sightings of Killer Whales were recorded in this area, <strong>and</strong> 1<br />

sighting was also made in at the south of Barbate, at the north-west of the Strait. (See FIG<br />

3). This last sighting was made during the month of April, while the rest of the sighting were<br />

made during the month of July <strong>and</strong> Augus t.<br />

Results Behaviour<br />

The animals were observed during a total of 105 hours <strong>and</strong> 19 minutes. The behaviours<br />

recorded were socialising, for 36min ( 0,6 %), <strong>and</strong> feeding during 104 <strong>and</strong> 43 min (99,4 %).<br />

Killer Whales were observed in the presence of fishing boats where the average abundance<br />

was 102 within a radius of 700-1000 meters in 30 ocasions. The rest of the sightings (6) the<br />

killer whales were observed feeding without fishing boats around. Witnessed interactions<br />

consisted of either removing the fish from the drop line hooks or biting the caught fishes.<br />

Results Identification<br />

Photos of the animals' dorsal fin were taken in 30 of the sightings, which were classified<br />

in three categories: bad, good, <strong>and</strong> excellent, <strong>and</strong> only the pictures included in the last two<br />

categories were taken into account for photo-identification purposes. Between 17 <strong>and</strong> 18<br />

Killer Whales were captured Between 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2002. Between 12 <strong>and</strong> 13 individuals were<br />

observed interacting with Tuna fisherys, <strong>and</strong> 5 individuals were observed at the south of<br />

Barbate, in April 2002..<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

The Group Of Killer Whales<br />

The data reveals the presence of a stable group of at least 12 individuals of Killer<br />

Whales in the western part of the Strait, while the identification of a 13th needs confirmation.<br />

The observation of the feeding behaviour <strong>and</strong> the fact that no attack of the Killer<br />

Whales to other prey was observed, suggest that the main diet during the summer<strong>and</strong> spring<br />

period should be Red Tuna.<br />

Interactions With Fisheries<br />

According to the records, when the fish is being lifted, the Killer Whales try to steal or<br />

bite it. This competition for the same resource (Red Tuna Fish), is the main reason why so<br />

many interactions between Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> fisheries have been described in the region for a<br />

long time. In certain times, it seems that fishermen really dislike these attacks of the Killer<br />

Whales, <strong>and</strong> they can even throw stones over them, or try to scare them by riding the boat<br />

over them. On 26th July 2000, a shoot-like sound was recorded, but it was not possible to<br />

clarify if it was only to threat the animals or to hurt them.<br />

Beside this, the fast development of the whale watching platforms in the area (5 to 6<br />

boats are awaited for this summer 2003) (Urquiola <strong>and</strong> de Stephanis, 2000) the presence of<br />

some research vessels (3 boats are awaited for the summer 2003), the interests for the mass<br />

media (local <strong>and</strong> international T.V. channels), <strong>and</strong> the local political problems regarding the<br />

fishing international agreements could create management problems between these sectors<br />

<strong>and</strong> the fishing community, <strong>and</strong> could interfere in this Killer Whales group.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

139


CONCLUSIONS<br />

In the summers 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2002, a group of between 12 <strong>and</strong> 13 animals at least regularly<br />

took advantage of the presence of a drop line fishery west to The Strait of Gibraltar to obtain<br />

easy food by stealing hooked fishes. This group has probably specialised in this feeding<br />

strategy. Clear interactions between fishing boats <strong>and</strong> Killer Whales exist in the area during<br />

the summers.<br />

These interactions <strong>and</strong> the depletion of the Red Tuna stocks due to over fishing is likely<br />

to result in negative impacts on both Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> fishermen in this area. Management<br />

procedures should be developed, to preserve Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> the interest of fishermen.<br />

AKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

Special Thanks to all the institutions that sponsorised this project:<br />

1998-2000: FIRMM: foundation for Information <strong>and</strong> research on marine mammals,<br />

Whale watching platform of the Strait of Gibraltar, 2001: Autonomous City of Ceuta, 2002:<br />

BBC: British Braodcast Corp <strong>and</strong> LIFE02NAT/E/8610. Special Thanks also to all the<br />

volunteers that colaborated in the data colection <strong>and</strong> data analyse. Thanks to all the fishermen<br />

that had to see us during those 4 summers. They were really patient with us... Thanks to the<br />

captains who patiently supported us in the campaigns we carried out, in particular to Antonio,<br />

Andres, Juan, Miguel <strong>and</strong> Kiko. To all the staff members of the different whale watching<br />

platforms, in special to Walti, Keti <strong>and</strong> all the volunteers of those platforms. Last but not<br />

least, to the “Torre de Salvamento Marítimo de Tarifa Tráfico”.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

BAYED, A. & BEAUBRUN, P.-Ch., 1987. Les mammifères marins du Maroc :<br />

inventaire préliminaire. Mammalia, 51(3): 437-446.<br />

ALONCLE, H., 1964. Premières observations sur les petits cétacés des côtes<br />

marocaines. Bulletin de l'Institut des Pêches Maritimes du Maroc, 12: 21-42.<br />

URQUIOLA, E., DE STEPHANIS, D, 2000. "Growth of whale watching in Spain. The<br />

Sucsses of the platforms in south mainl<strong>and</strong>. New rules". In European Research on Cetaceans<br />

14. G. Donovan (ed.). Proc. 14 th Ann. Meeting European Cetacean Society, Cork, Irel<strong>and</strong>, 5-<br />

9 April. In press.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

140


FIG.1: Research Area<br />

FIG.2: Aleatoric transects realised along the Strait of Gibraltar durring 2001 <strong>and</strong> 2002 from the research boat<br />

ELSA<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

141


FIG.3: Sigtings of Killer whales during 1998-2002 in the Strait of Gibraltar<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

142


THEODOLITE STUDY OF THE EFFECTS OF VESSEL TRAFFIC ON<br />

KILLER WHALES (ORCINUS ORCA) IN THE NEAR-SHORE WATERS OF<br />

WASHINGTON STATE, USA (POSTER)<br />

Smith J.C.¹, Bain D.E.²<br />

¹<strong>Orca</strong> Conservancy, 219 1st Avenue South, Suite #315 Seattle, WA 98104, USA,<br />

dyesqueen@rockisl<strong>and</strong>.com ;<br />

²Six Flags Marine World Vallejo, Vallejo, CA 94589, USA, dbain@u.washington.edu<br />

Beginning in 1999 a shore-based theodolite tracking study was set up to look at the<br />

relationship between whale watching vessels <strong>and</strong> killer whale behavior, as well as a way of<br />

measuring compliance of the self imposed no-boat corridor within a ¼ mile of shoreline when<br />

whales are present. Measurements included dive times, swim speeds, surface behaviors, <strong>and</strong><br />

distance offshore of whale watch operators.<br />

RESULTS<br />

This study tested the null hypothesis that whale indices are independent of vessel<br />

activity. Null hypothesis being rejected would indicate whale response may be related to the<br />

intensity of whale-watching in a non-linear manner.<br />

Directness Index<br />

A statistical finding (Table 1) resulted from looking at the directness index with <strong>and</strong><br />

without boats present. The mean with boats was 83.28 while the mean without boats was<br />

91.47. Whale paths were direct with no boats, more deviation occurred with boats numbering<br />

between 6 <strong>and</strong> 25, gradually becoming more direct as boat numbers climbed towards 30<br />

(Figure 1).<br />

Distances Offshore<br />

Whales were tracked offshore with a median distance of less than 400 meters of the<br />

shoreline each year of the study. Year 2001 showed the continuation of this.<br />

¼ Mile Compliance<br />

Numbers of observations of commercial whale watching boats showed their distribution<br />

peak just outside the ¼ mile line of 400 meters. When compared to Soundwatch present or<br />

absent, commercial compliance went down for 2001. 22.54% were within 350 meters when<br />

Soundwatch was present, while 13.16% were inside 400 meters while Soundwatch was<br />

absent.<br />

Non commercial private boats stayed within the ¼ mile zone roughly half of the time.<br />

These boat types encompass fishing vessels <strong>and</strong> kayaks, that aren’t asked to move offshore by<br />

Soundwatch, or ocean-liners that naturally stay offshore.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

Tracking whales in the absence of boats has proven to be significantly difficult due to<br />

the increased numbers of hours boats are around whales. Boats appear to be accompanying<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

143


whales for up to 10 hours a day, seven days a week. Length of time spent with whales varied<br />

between the hours of 0600 until after dusk, (~2100 h). For each of the past two years of the<br />

study only 9% of whale passes were without boats, although 2001 held a higher percentage of<br />

‘no-boat’ tracks than previous years. In addition, in year 2000 data there seems to be a slight<br />

yet significant trend for animals to increase distance traveled in the presence of a few boats.<br />

Whales were direct with zero boats, <strong>and</strong> then adopted a less direct path with boats numbering<br />

between 4 <strong>and</strong> 17. This trend was found to exist in the 2001 data as well.<br />

The assumption that boats do not affect whale behavior was violated. However, due to the<br />

small sample size, other assumptions such as tidal <strong>and</strong> current states, time of day, time of<br />

year, age, sex <strong>and</strong> group dynamics, were not important factors, but may have been violated in<br />

addition. More tracks would allow for the breakdown of these variables <strong>and</strong> more confidence<br />

in assumptions. Samples of ‘boat’ tracks numbering around 40 didn’t result in significant<br />

change from year to year but ‘no-boat’ type tracks changed going from 8-12 samples.<br />

Clearly it would be of tremendous value to get a large number of no-boat tracks in a single<br />

season. Because of the small number of low directness index tracks, it is unsure if the<br />

quantitative results of the statistical test overstate the probability that this is true.<br />

Compliance regarding the ¼ mile has been easily determined during this study.<br />

For 1999 <strong>and</strong> 2000, private vessels complied with the ¼ mile ‘no-boat’ zone 55% of the<br />

time; commercial whale watchers were just under 80 of the time. Year 2000 showed the<br />

effectiveness of the Soundwatch Boater Education Program, with a 90% compliance by<br />

commercial whale watchers, when Soundwatch was present. Unfortunately year 2001<br />

showed decreased time on the water by Soundwatch <strong>and</strong> a decrease in distance offshore by<br />

commercial whale watch operators by roughly 50 meters.<br />

The results presented here show a statistically significant trend for boat traffic to disrupt<br />

short-term behavior of individual killer whales. If boat traffic generally forces animals to<br />

swim further, then this carries an obvious metabolic cost. Paired with a decreasing food<br />

source <strong>and</strong> heavy contaminant load, short-term responses should not be dismissed.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

144


FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

145


OBSERVATIONS OF KILLER WHALES DURING THE FALL, WINTER AND<br />

SPRING IN SOUTHEASTERN ALASKA 1980-2002<br />

Janice M. Straley 1 <strong>and</strong> Graeme Ellis 2<br />

1 University of Alaska Southeast Sitka Campus, 1332 Seward Ave., Sitka, Alaska 99835 USA<br />

2 Pacific Biological Station. Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada, Nanaimo, BC Canada V9T 6N7<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

Killer whales frequent the coastal rim of the North Pacific during all months of the year,<br />

however, little is know of their distribution during the fall, winter <strong>and</strong> spring. Data reported<br />

here were collected in the waters of southeastern Alaska across the years1980-2002. These<br />

data resulted in 20 encounters with killer whales in 11 of the 22 survey years. Most (18)<br />

encounters were observed from 1993 to 2002. The number of encounters per year ranged from<br />

one to four. Observations occurred from September through May with no killer whales<br />

observed in December. There were 99 sightings of killer whales during these encounters<br />

representing 44 different whales. Most (25) of these 44 killer whales were sighted only once.<br />

Group size ranged from 1 to 14 with an average of 5.0 (se=0.68) whales per group. Killer<br />

whales observed during this study were associated with mammal eating or ‘transient’<br />

populations that range along the western coast of the United States <strong>and</strong> Canada. Behavior<br />

included foraging near shore (40%), traveling (35%), predation or feeding (15%), milling<br />

(10%). Milling occurred near feeding humpback whales, Steller sea lions, harbor porpoises<br />

<strong>and</strong> Dall’s porpoises. All predation events (n=3) were upon marine mammals (harbor seal or<br />

Steller sea lion or Dall’s porpoise). In summary, this study has provided insights into the<br />

importance of southeastern Alaska, a complex coastal fjord habitat, by killer whales during<br />

the fall, winter <strong>and</strong> spring.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Killer whales frequent the coastal rim of the North Pacific during all months of the year,<br />

however, little is known of their distribution <strong>and</strong> foraging behavior in the fall, winter <strong>and</strong><br />

spring in the eastern North Pacific. The killer whales that roam the waters of southeastern<br />

Alaska are closely linked with the whales seen to the south, in British Columbia, Canada<br />

(Ford <strong>and</strong> Ellis 1999). This ongoing study of killer whales is a first step towards<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing the distribution <strong>and</strong> foraging behavior in Alaskan waters during the fall, winter<br />

<strong>and</strong> spring.<br />

METHODS<br />

The results presented in this paper summarize data collected on killer whales across 22<br />

years, from 1980 to 2002. During 1980 to 2000 opportunistic studies occurred in conjunction<br />

with research on humpback whales <strong>and</strong> in 2001 <strong>and</strong> 2002 directed studies were conducted<br />

specifically on killer whales. All observations were during the fall, winter <strong>and</strong> spring in<br />

southeastern Alaska (Figure 1). Small boats less than 8m were used to record observations of<br />

killer whales <strong>and</strong> 35mm SLR cameras were used to obtain photographs of dorsal fins <strong>and</strong><br />

saddle patches of individual killer whales. Killer whales were identified as to ecotype based<br />

upon association with other known killer whales in the same population.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

146


Southeaster<br />

n Alaska Study<br />

Figure 1. Map of North Pacific Ocean with arrow pointing to the southeastern Alaska study<br />

area.<br />

RESULTS<br />

A total of 44 different killer whales were seen 99 times. The majority of the killer<br />

whales were observed once (25 whales) <strong>and</strong> one killer whale was seen six times (Figure 2).<br />

New killer whales continued to be identified in the study area. Three new killer whales were<br />

identified in 2002. There were 20 encounters with killer whales with group size ranging from<br />

1 to 14 whales (mean=5.0 whales, se=0.68, Table 1). All killer whales observed were<br />

transients or the mammal-eating ecotype.<br />

Table 1. Group size <strong>and</strong> date of encounter for killer whales seen during fall, winter <strong>and</strong> spring in southeastern<br />

Alaska, 1980 to 2002.<br />

Date Group size Date<br />

Group<br />

size<br />

11-Feb-80 6 11-Nov-98 3<br />

07-Sep-84 3 10-Mar-00 6<br />

10-Jan-93 3 17-Mar-00 2<br />

16-Feb-94 7 04-Apr-00 8<br />

24-Mar-95 7 28-May-01 6<br />

04-Jan-96 14 24-Feb-02 6<br />

29-Oct-96 2 23-Mar-02 3<br />

09-Jan-97 6 06-Sep-02 2<br />

04-Feb-97 5 07-Sep-02 2<br />

05-Nov-97 7 12-Sep-02 1<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

147


NUMBER OF SIGHTINGS<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6<br />

NUMBER OF WHALES<br />

Figure 2. Number of times individual killer whales were sighted across the study period, 1980 to<br />

2002.<br />

Most of the encounters (18 out of 20) occurred from 1993 to 2002 <strong>and</strong> the number of<br />

killer whales encounters increased in 2002 (Figure 3).<br />

ENCOUNTERS<br />

6<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

1980<br />

1982<br />

1984<br />

1986<br />

1988<br />

1990<br />

1992<br />

YEAR<br />

Figure 3. Number of killer whale encounters each year in southeastern Alaska, 1980 to 2002.<br />

There were discernable peaks in the presence of killers whales by month <strong>and</strong> the most<br />

encounters occurred in the winter months <strong>and</strong> in September (Figure 4).<br />

1994<br />

1996<br />

1998<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

2000<br />

2002<br />

148


ENCOUNTERS<br />

5<br />

4<br />

3<br />

2<br />

1<br />

0<br />

SEP OCT NOV DEC JAN FEB MAR APR MAY<br />

MONTH<br />

Figure 4. Killer whale encounters each month during fall, winter <strong>and</strong><br />

spring in southeastern Alaska, 1980 to 2002.<br />

The primary behavior observed during the 20 encounters of killer whales was foraging<br />

within 1.6km of shore, occurring 40% of the time. The next most common behavior observed<br />

was fast travel, which was seen in 35% of the encounters. Milling occurred 10% of the time<br />

near feeding humpback whales, Dall’s <strong>and</strong> harbor porpoises, Steller sea lions <strong>and</strong> harbor<br />

seals. Feeding was observed three times (15% of the time); once on a Dall’s porpoise, once<br />

on a Steller sea lion <strong>and</strong> once on either a sea lion or seal.<br />

CONCLUSIONS<br />

Transient killer whales were they only type of killer whale encountered during the fall,<br />

winter <strong>and</strong> spring in southeastern Alaska from 1980 to 2002. Observations of higher number<br />

of transients during the fall, winter <strong>and</strong> spring were consistent with what has been observed in<br />

British Columbia, however, they do see smaller resident (fish eating) groups occasionally<br />

(Ford et al. 1998). The occurrence of transient killer whales near coastal areas coincides with<br />

the habits of the prey of the different killer whales, transient prey is available year round <strong>and</strong><br />

salmon, a migratory species <strong>and</strong> primary prey of residents, are less available.<br />

There were discernable peaks in the presence of killers whales by month. The<br />

September peak could be due to better sighting conditions <strong>and</strong> more vessels on the water to<br />

report whales before the fall <strong>and</strong> winter storms begin. The January through March peaks are<br />

likely due to an increase in Steller sea lions which inhabit Sitka Sound feeding on herring<br />

which are present in the deep coastal fjords in winter. The increase in the number of killer<br />

whales observed in 2002 could be due to a change in the number of killer whales or a shift in<br />

habitat use. However, the most likely reason for this increase is the start of dedicated survey<br />

effort on killer whales in 2001, which began in earnest in January 2002 when a<br />

comprehensive reporting network was established.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

149


REFERENCES<br />

Ford, J.K.B <strong>and</strong> G. Ellis. 1999. Transients: Mammal-Hunting Killer Whales of British<br />

Columbia, Washington <strong>and</strong> Southeastern Alaska. University of British Columbia Press,<br />

Vancouver, BC 96pp.<br />

Ford, J.K.B., G.Ellis, L.G.Barrett-Lennard, A.B. Morton, R.S. Palm, K.C. Balcomb III. 1998.<br />

Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of killer whale (Orcinus orca) in<br />

coastal British Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent water. Can. J. Zool. 76:1456-1471<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

150


WHAT IS THE FAVOURITE PREY ? FORAGING ECOLOGY OF KILLER<br />

WHALES AT AVACHA GULF, RUSSIAN FAR EAST.<br />

Tarasyan K., Jikiya E.<br />

Russia, 119899, Moscow,Moscow State University,Biology Faculty, Department of Zoology<br />

Vertebrate, karen@ntl.ru<br />

Kamchatka killer whale population is studied since 1999 up to a present year by the<br />

scientific expedition organized by Kamchatka Institute of Ecology <strong>and</strong> Nature Management<br />

<strong>and</strong> Moscow State University. Methods of our research are traditional for investigations of<br />

cetacean: we use photo-identification technique, acoustic analyzing <strong>and</strong> l<strong>and</strong>-based behavioral<br />

observations simultaneously.<br />

This report represents some aspects of foraging ecology of killer whales at Avacha Gulf,<br />

Russian Far East, based on two-year behavioral data. It is known that killer whales use<br />

different methods for different fish prey. We has distinguished two hunting methods: long<br />

diving of single killer whale, which is not coordinated with other group members, used for<br />

catching single fish, <strong>and</strong> “karusel” method, described for killer whales <strong>and</strong> other cetacean<br />

during chasing fish schools by the group of dolphins. Perquisition of local fishermen has<br />

allowed finding out the basic species of the fishes, which inhabit at Avacha Gulf. Some of<br />

these species, such as walleye pollock, greenlings, codfish or flounder, have commercial<br />

value. Behavior <strong>and</strong> position of orcas during foraging let us to predict fish species, which are<br />

hunted by observed killer whales.<br />

Varying ratio of two types of fish chasing <strong>and</strong> changing places of foraging from season<br />

to season allows to assume, that the basic food objects, attracting killer whale to Avacha Gulf,<br />

are changing from year to year, depending basically on large number <strong>and</strong> availability of<br />

separate possible prey species.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

151


POPULATION VIABILITY ANALYSIS FOR THE SOUTHERN RESIDENT<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

POPULATION OF KILLER WHALES<br />

Taylor M., Plater<br />

Center For Biological Diversity, B. PO Box 40090, Berkeley, CA 94704,<br />

bplater@biologicaldiversity.org<br />

Population viability analysis by stochastic population modeling suggests that the<br />

Southern Resident population of killer whales is likely to become extinct in the foreseeable<br />

future unless ongoing habitat degradation is halted. Only the most optimistic models free of<br />

adverse impacts to habitat resulted in predictions of population stability. Incorporation of<br />

plausible impacts such as oil spills, epizootics, <strong>and</strong> reduced salmon food stocks greatly<br />

increased predicted extinction risk, even under the optimistic assumption that the long-term<br />

average fecundity <strong>and</strong> mortality observed in the population would otherwise continue<br />

unchanged. If the reduced fecundity <strong>and</strong> adult survival seen in the population since 1996 is<br />

assumed to continue indefinitely rather than return to the lower levels seen over the previous<br />

22 years, the risk of extinction of the Southern Resident population within 100 years is highly<br />

likely, even in the absence of additional threats to the population.<br />

The most plausible scenario, incorporating risks of inbreeding depression <strong>and</strong><br />

mate limitation at small population sizes, risks of oil spills, epizootics, <strong>and</strong> reduced food<br />

supply predicted a median time to extinction of 74 years with a 95% confidence interval of<br />

33-121 years. However, adult mortality may increase further if recent trends continue. If<br />

adult mortality continues to increase at the same rate, then extinction risk would be even<br />

higher.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Once abundant throughout the waters of the Pacific Northwest region of the United<br />

States <strong>and</strong> Canada, the Southern Resident killer whale has been declining since 1996 <strong>and</strong> is<br />

now one of the most imperiled killer whale populations in the world. The decline has been<br />

attributed to several anthropogenic sources, primarily depletion of preferred food stocks, toxic<br />

pollution, <strong>and</strong> disturbance from whale watching boats <strong>and</strong> other shipping traffic. We<br />

conducted a Population Viability Analysis to determine the likelihood of the population going<br />

extinct if these threats continue unabated.<br />

Killer whales in the Pacific Northwest are divided into three forms: Residents,<br />

Transients, <strong>and</strong> Offshores. The forms are distinct genetically, behaviorally, <strong>and</strong><br />

morphologically, but occupy adjacent or overlapping ranges (Baird 1999). Although the<br />

forms are distinct enough to be classified as separate subspecies or even species, all are<br />

currently classified as one species.<br />

Resident killer whales in the Pacific Northwest are divided into two populations:<br />

Northern Residents <strong>and</strong> Southern Residents. Each stock has distinct patterns of association,<br />

pigmentation <strong>and</strong> genetics (Bigg et al. 1987, Baird <strong>and</strong> Stacey 1988, Bain 1989, Ford et al.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

152


1998, Hoelzel et al. 1998, Matkin et al. 1998, Barrett-Lennard 2000). Although the Resident<br />

populations have partly overlapping ranges, behavioral interactions are rare. Genetic <strong>and</strong><br />

morphological differences suggest that they have been reproductively isolated for thous<strong>and</strong>s<br />

of years (Baird <strong>and</strong> Stacey 1988, Stevens et al. 1989, Hoelzel <strong>and</strong> Dover 1991, Hoelzel et al.<br />

1998, Barrett-Lennard 2000).<br />

POPULATION TRENDS, 1973-2002<br />

The Southern Residents have declined precipitously since 1996. This ongoing decline<br />

is unlike any earlier decline seen in this population because it involves elevated mortality of<br />

young adults <strong>and</strong> reduced fecundity. The decline has caused Canada to list the Southern<br />

Residents as “endangered.” However, the United States has declined to protect the whales,<br />

despite the fact that government scientists found that the population is both discrete <strong>and</strong><br />

endangered.<br />

METHODS<br />

Population size<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

67<br />

71 71 71<br />

81<br />

80<br />

79<br />

83<br />

81<br />

78<br />

76<br />

74<br />

76<br />

81<br />

84 84 85 84<br />

90 91<br />

96<br />

94<br />

96 97<br />

60<br />

1973 1975 1977 1979 1981 1983 1985 1987 1989 1991 1993 1995 1997 1999 2001<br />

FIGURE 1. Population Trends for Southern Residents Since 1973<br />

The published record of births <strong>and</strong> deaths for the period 1973-2002 from the Center for<br />

Whale Research with corrections by University of Washington whale biologist Dr. David<br />

Bain were used to calculate age <strong>and</strong> sex distributions, <strong>and</strong> annual mortality <strong>and</strong> fecundity<br />

statistics.<br />

Best estimates of life history variables were then used for successive simulations of the<br />

Vortex modeling algorithm (Lacey et al., 2000). Stochastic population simulations were run<br />

for 18 different sets of parameters. All models, however, assumed that no trends in<br />

parameters exist; that the Southern Residents are freely polygamous, mating in a single<br />

panmictic population without social structure; that the age of first breeding is 16 years for<br />

both sexes; that the maximum breeding age for each sex is 40 years; that no twinning occurs;<br />

<strong>and</strong> that there is no concordance in environment variance in mortality & fecundity.<br />

The most plausible model: sets inbreeding effects at 2 lethal equivalents, a conservative<br />

estimate; has a 50% primary sex ratio, a conservative ratio in light of the observed sex ratio<br />

of 57% males in the population; maintains fecundity <strong>and</strong> juvenile mortality levels at the long-<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

91<br />

89<br />

84<br />

82<br />

78 79<br />

153


term averages for the population; sets adult mortalities at the elevated levels seen between<br />

1996-2001 because the elevated mortality of adults is a novelty in the 28 year record of<br />

observation; incorporates a declining carrying capacity to model food scarcity; incorporates<br />

an Allee effect to model the likely effects of mate limitation at low population sizes; <strong>and</strong><br />

models oils spill <strong>and</strong> epizootic risks based on impacts seen in other killer whale <strong>and</strong> marine<br />

mammal populations that have been exposed to these threats. Results of 17 other model runs<br />

are not shown here.<br />

RESULTS: MOST PLAUSIBLE MODEL SHOWS EXTINCTION IMMINENT<br />

For this scenario, predicted extinction risk was high within 100 years, with<br />

median time to extinction of only 74 years with a 95% confidence interval of 33 - 121 years.<br />

The population trajectories of the simulations showed wide variability (Fig. 2). Note that time<br />

at which median predicted population size reaches zero is not the same as median time to<br />

extinction. Extinction is defined as extinction of one sex, <strong>and</strong> so is expected to occur earlier<br />

than death of all individuals.<br />

Population<br />

110<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160<br />

FIGURE 2. Extinction Trajectory of Southern Resident Killer Whales<br />

The results suggest that the Southern Resident population is very likely to become<br />

extinct in the foreseeable future unless action is taken to arrest declining habitat quality.<br />

Protection for this population is urgently needed so that the population can survive any future<br />

catastrophes or perturbations. Effort should therefore be directed at identifying the causes of<br />

the recent increases in adult mortality <strong>and</strong> declining fecundity <strong>and</strong> at finding ways of halting<br />

or mitigating any human impacts that might be implicated. If the higher risks of death <strong>and</strong><br />

impaired reproduction created by oil spills, salmon reduction, pollution <strong>and</strong> disease are<br />

reversed, then there is a low likelihood of extinction in the foreseeable future. If these risks<br />

are not substantially removed, extinction is certain. Governments should engage protective<br />

measures such as the United States Endangered Species Act to adequately address the threats<br />

facing the Southern Residents.<br />

Time<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

154


LITERATURE CITIED<br />

Bain, D.E. 1989. An evaluation of evolutionary processes: studies of natural selection,<br />

dispersal, <strong>and</strong> cultural evolution: I Killer Whales (Orcinus orca). Ph.D. Thesis, University of<br />

California, Santa Cruz.<br />

Baird, R.W. 1999. Status of Killer Whales in Canada. Report to the Committee on the<br />

Status of Endangered Wildlife in Canada (COSEWIC), Ottawa.<br />

Baird, R.W. <strong>and</strong> Stacey, P.J. 1988. Variation in saddle patch pigmentation in<br />

populations of Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) from British Columbia, Alaska, <strong>and</strong> Washington<br />

State. Can. J. Zool. 66, 2582-2585.<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L.G. 2000. Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) as revealed by DNA analysis. PhD Thesis, University of British Columbia,<br />

Vancouver, B.C.<br />

Bigg, M.A., G.M. Ellis, J.K.B. Ford, <strong>and</strong> K.C. Balcomb. 1987. Killer Whales: a study of<br />

their identification, genealogy, <strong>and</strong> natural history in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State.<br />

Phantom Press, Nanaimo, B.C.<br />

Ford, J.K.B., G.M. Ellis, L.G. Barrett-Lennard, A.B. Morton, R.S. Palm, <strong>and</strong> K.C.<br />

Balcomb. 1998. Dietary specialization in two sympatric populations of Killer Whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) in coastal British Columbia <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. Canadian Journal of Zoology<br />

77: in press.<br />

Hoelzel, A.R., <strong>and</strong> G.A. Dover, 1991. Genetic Differentiation Between Sympatric Killer<br />

Whale Populations. Journal of Heredity, 66:191-195.<br />

Hoelzel, A.R., M. E. Dahlheim <strong>and</strong> S.J. Stern, 1998. Low genetic variation variation<br />

among killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the eastern North Pacific <strong>and</strong> differentiation between<br />

foraging specialists. J. Heredity 89, 121-128.<br />

Lacey, RC, Hughes, KA <strong>and</strong> Miller, PS (2000) Vortex: a stochastic simulation of the<br />

extinction process. Chicago Zoological Soc.<br />

Matkin, C.O., D. Schel, G. Ellis, L. Barrett-Lennard, H. Jurk <strong>and</strong> E. Saulitis. 1998.<br />

Comprehensive Killer Whale investigation, Exxon Valdez oil spill restoration project annual<br />

report (Restoration Project 97012). North Gulf Oceanic Society, Homer, Alaska.<br />

Stevens, T.A., D.A. Duffield, E.D. Asper, K.G. Hewlett, A. Bolz, L.J. Gage, <strong>and</strong> G.D.<br />

Bossart. 1989. Preliminary findings of restriction fragment differences in mitochondrial DNA<br />

among Killer Whales (Orcinus orca). Canadian Journal of Zoology 67:2592-2595.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

155


PREDATION BEHAVIOR OF TRANSIENT KILLER WHALES IN MONTEREY<br />

BAY, CALIFORNIA<br />

Ternullo R., Black N.<br />

Monterey Bay Cetacean Project, P.P. Box 52001, Pacific Grove, CA 93950, rternullo@aol.com.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Monterey Bay, located along the central California coast, is affected by strong<br />

seasonal upwelling, providing rich primary production. This in turn provides a reliable food<br />

source that supports four species of pinnipeds, two species of porpoise, six species of<br />

dolphins, at least four species of baleen whales, <strong>and</strong> numerous species of seabirds. Of the<br />

three ecotypes of Killer Whales known for the eastern North Pacific, transients (marine<br />

mammal predators) are most frequently sighted.<br />

METHODS<br />

For each Killer Whale sighting, we attempted to photo-identify all whales within a<br />

group. Data collected for each group included sighting location, movements, <strong>and</strong> behavior of<br />

transient groups. If prey species were encountered, we attempted to identify them, <strong>and</strong> noted<br />

if they were harassed, attacked, or consumed. When possible, predation events were<br />

videotaped to analyze hunting strategies <strong>and</strong> roles of specific Killer Whales.<br />

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION<br />

Proportion of prey items observed for 84 events documented since 1987 included<br />

California Sea Lion (35%), Gray Whale calf (30%), <strong>and</strong> 10 % or less in descending order for<br />

Dall’s Porpoise, Northern Elephant Seal, seabird, Harbor Seal, Pacific White-Sided Dolphin,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Common Dolphin. Killer Whales occurred in greatest numbers either when prey items<br />

were in abundance or their young were setting out to sea for the first time. A peak in transient<br />

Killer Whale sightings occurred during late spring when Gray Whale calves <strong>and</strong> their mothers<br />

are transiting Monterey Bay on their northward migration. Northern Elephant Seals <strong>and</strong><br />

Pacific Harbor Seals were also weaning their young at approximately the same time. There<br />

was then a decline in Killer Whale sightings until late August <strong>and</strong> sightings increased<br />

throughout the fall corresponding with an influx of California Sea Lions <strong>and</strong> Elephant Seals at<br />

sea, then declined again until early spring. On average there were about forty to fifty sightings<br />

per year. Killer Whale group sizes varied by prey type with an average group size of three for<br />

Dall’s porpoise attacks to 13 for attacks on Gray Whale calves.<br />

California Sea Lions were the most numerous prey items, <strong>and</strong> all age classes of Sea<br />

Lions were taken. Predation events on this species were very visible due to the relatively long<br />

process of incapacitating the Sea Lion by tossing, body <strong>and</strong> tail slams, <strong>and</strong> then in most cases,<br />

ending the attack by drowning the prey. In some cases, the Sea Lion was battered, drowned<br />

<strong>and</strong> killed, then towed along with the Killer Whales for several hours.<br />

Harbor Seals have been identified as prey items from fur fragments or brief glimpses<br />

of the seal’s presence before it was killed. They are possibly under-reported because of the<br />

cryptic <strong>and</strong> quick killing process used on these seals.<br />

All age classes of Northern Elephant Seals were taken. Adult male Elephant Seals<br />

appeared to be prevented from taking deep dives by the Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> drowning was the<br />

suspected cause of death, often lasting up to an hour. Weaners were often taken quickly at the<br />

surface <strong>and</strong> a round of pummeling with flukes led to eventual drowning. There has not<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

156


een an observed attack on Northern Fur Seals, but they are known as prey for transients in<br />

this population. Their behavior of resting on the surface or in kelp paddies with little<br />

movement for long periods of time may be an advantageous strategy to avoid detection by<br />

Killer Whales.<br />

Both Dall’s <strong>and</strong> Harbor Porpoise are present in Monterey Bay, but only Dall’s Porpoise<br />

have been recorded as prey items. Killer Whales travelling through the Bay were closely<br />

associated with the edge of the Monterey Submarine Canyon, an area where Dall’s Porpoise<br />

occur, whereas Harbor Porpoise inhabit near-shore shallow waters where Killer Whales rarely<br />

occur. Attacks on Dall’s porpoise involved a quick rush by the Killer Whales, then the<br />

porpoise was often popped up in the air by the head of the whale, or else there was a quick<br />

chase <strong>and</strong> bite which mortally wounded the porpoise. Porpoise were taken by surprise <strong>and</strong><br />

quickly killed.<br />

Of six dolphin species commonly seen in Monterey Bay, only Pacific White-Sided<br />

Dolphin <strong>and</strong> Long-Beaked Common Dolphin have been attacked <strong>and</strong> killed by transients.<br />

These dolphins often travel in large groups of 100-2000+, <strong>and</strong> Killer Whales often quietly<br />

trail the group for awhile before choosing one that lags behind. Both species exhibited a<br />

strong flight response whenever Killer Whales were detected, therefore proving a difficult<br />

prey item to catch.<br />

Four species of baleen whales are commonly seen in Monterey Bay: Humpback<br />

Whales, Blue Whales, Gray Whales, <strong>and</strong> Minke Whales. Of these only Gray Whales were<br />

observed during attacks by Killer Whales. Humpback <strong>and</strong> Blue Whales both bear many<br />

indications of struggles with Killer Whales on their bodies <strong>and</strong> flukes; i.e. tooth rake marks<br />

from Killer Whales on tail flukes. One beached Minke Whale had evidence of Killer Whale<br />

predation. Due to the extreme damage on some Humpback Whale flukes, predation attempts<br />

obviously take place, but they may not occur in California waters. Several observations of<br />

Killer Whales pursuing <strong>and</strong> harassing Blue Whales exist for the study area.<br />

Gray Whales <strong>and</strong> particularly their calves seem to provide a significant food resource<br />

on a seasonal basis. During the mother/calf phase of the northern Gray Whale migration there<br />

appears to be a location of high vulnerability associated with particular areas of Monterey Bay<br />

<strong>and</strong> its complex canyon system.<br />

Gray Whales that migrate through Monterey Bay either pass near shore or cut straight<br />

across the canyon. Killer Whale predation events occurred most often on the north side of Bay<br />

<strong>and</strong> began over the deep canyon before the Gray Whales reached shelf waters on the north<br />

side of the Bay. The killer whales often chased the Grays further north or east into shelf<br />

waters.<br />

W. Perryman (NMFS) conducted surveys of Gray Whale calves from 1994 to 2002<br />

from a shore station south of Monterey. The number of Gray Whale calves born each year<br />

varied from a peak of 501 calves counted on survey to a low of 87 calves. In years with high<br />

Gray Whale calf counts there were more attacks by Killer Whales in Monterey Bay. During<br />

1999, the first year of low calf numbers, there were many Killer Whale sightings over the<br />

spring migration months but Killer Whales were not finding calves. Even so there were three<br />

known attacks, possibly due to the continued presence of Killer Whales in the area.<br />

The steep bathymetry of the canyon edge seemed to provide some advantage to the<br />

Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> provoked a radical change in surfacing <strong>and</strong> travel behavior in the Gray<br />

Whales. Passive listening by Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> possible orienting vocalizations near canyon<br />

edges by the Gray Whales may serve to increase vulnerability. There is also the possibility<br />

that communication occurs between the Gray Whale mother <strong>and</strong> calf, or the calf is<br />

inappropriately vocalizing. The Killer Whales seemed to use the Bay as a prime hunting zone<br />

<strong>and</strong> may form small groups of 5-10 individuals to converge on the location of a mother/calf<br />

within minutes to hours of their detection.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

157


Once a mother/calf Gray Whale pair was detected, the Killer Whales grouped up <strong>and</strong><br />

pursued them until the Grays slowed down <strong>and</strong> were surrounded by the Killer Whales. As<br />

much as six hours may pass from initial attack to kill with ramming, biting, pulling on the<br />

pectoral fins, <strong>and</strong> attempts to separate the mother from the calf. During this period the mother<br />

<strong>and</strong> calf try to dash for the safety of shallow water <strong>and</strong> the mother Gray will often roll belly<br />

up <strong>and</strong> her calf will get on top of her for brief periods of safety from the intense onslaught. If<br />

the Killer Whales are successful in driving away the mother, the calf is swiftly drowned <strong>and</strong><br />

feeding commences. The protracted struggle involves female Killer Whales, juveniles, <strong>and</strong><br />

calves. Adult males appear to be able to kill successfully on their own but as a male team of<br />

two, they also will participate in feeding events resulting from the attack <strong>and</strong> kill of a Gray<br />

from cooperating females.<br />

The amounts of the carcasses that the whales fed on ranged from just the tongue <strong>and</strong><br />

blubber around the lower jaw to extensive feeding with complete stripping of the blubber. The<br />

tongue weighs as much as 400 Kg <strong>and</strong> the blubber over several thous<strong>and</strong> Kg. Sometimes the<br />

force exerted by the Killer Whales was enough to decapitate a calf.<br />

Even though nearly the entire transient population sighted off Monterey Bay has been<br />

present during a Gray Whale attack, they were not all directly involved. In over 60% of welldocumented<br />

attacks, three core groups of whales that were frequently sighted in the study area<br />

were present. Adult reproductive females within these core groups worked together <strong>and</strong><br />

engaged in specific roles; for example, one whale acted as separator between the Gray Whale<br />

mother <strong>and</strong> calf <strong>and</strong> others assisted around the periphery of the Grays to overcome the calf.<br />

The ecosystem found off Central California provides a diverse prey base available to<br />

Killer Whales <strong>and</strong> mastery of a variety of hunting techniques by the whales is essential. This<br />

applies to both temporal <strong>and</strong> spatial distribution of a food resource. This implies that a young<br />

Killer Whale needs an extended period of instruction, such as locating key areas at the correct<br />

time of year, determining suitable hunting habitat, overcoming prey items of vastly different<br />

sizes, working within a group cooperatively, <strong>and</strong> absorbing cultural nuance from other group<br />

members.<br />

California Sea Lion<br />

Gray Whale Calf<br />

Dall's Porpoise<br />

Elephant Seal<br />

Seabird<br />

Harbor Seal<br />

Pacific White-Sided Dolphin<br />

Common Dolphin (Dc)<br />

0 10 20<br />

Percent<br />

30 40<br />

Fig 1. Proportion of prey items observed.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

158


percent number of sightings<br />

16<br />

14<br />

12<br />

10<br />

8<br />

6<br />

4<br />

2<br />

0<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec<br />

<br />

<br />

month (1987-2001)<br />

Fig. 2 Percent number of sightings of Killer Whales by month.<br />

Key Whales Involved in Gray Whale Attacks<br />

Adult Females<br />

All whales with 1 or more calves<br />

Adult Males<br />

Work in pairs<br />

Whale # 39 40 49 50 51 24 25 30<br />

Attack Date* Group Size<br />

4/27/92 X X<br />

2<br />

5/2/92 X X X X X<br />

18<br />

5/15/93 X X X X X<br />

20<br />

4/11/97 X X X X<br />

30<br />

4/21/97 14<br />

4/19/98(1) X X X X X<br />

12<br />

4/19/98(2) X X 2<br />

5/24/99 X X<br />

11<br />

6/5/99 X X X X<br />

20<br />

*only dates for best observations <strong>and</strong> some or all IDs<br />

Table 1. Key killer whales (by ID #) involved<br />

in 60% of attacks on Gray Whales.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

159


WHISTLES AND VARIABLE CALLS AS CLOSE-RANGE ACOUSTIC SIGNALS IN<br />

WILD KILLER WHALES OFF VANCOUVER ISLAND, BRITISH COLUMBIA<br />

Thomsen F. 1 , Teichert S. 1 , Riesch R. 1 , Rehn N. 1 , Ford J. K.B. 2<br />

1 Zoologisches Institut und Zoologisches Museum, Arbeitsbereich Ethologie, Martin-Luther-King-Platz<br />

3, D-20146 Hamburg, Germany,thomsen@zoologie.uni-hamburg.de;<br />

2 Marine Mammal Resarch Program, Pacific-Biological-Station, Nanaimo, Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans,<br />

Canada, B.C. Canada V9R 5K6.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

In a variety of terrestrial social mammals sounds used in close-range signalling are<br />

graded. Graded sounds are highly variable with fluid transitions between basic signal<br />

types. The complexity or intensity of the graded sound correlates closely with the arousal<br />

level of the signaller, facilitating a subtle information transfer between interacting<br />

individuals ( Marler 1976; Fox <strong>and</strong> Cohen 1977; Zimen 1978; Goodall 1986). Further, in<br />

mammals <strong>and</strong> birds, close-range sounds follow a ‘motivation-structural-code’, where high<br />

pitched tonal sounds signal friendly motivations <strong>and</strong> low frequency broad-b<strong>and</strong> sounds<br />

signal agonistic ones (Owings <strong>and</strong> Morton 1998). Most delphinids emit a great variety of<br />

whistles <strong>and</strong> burst-pulses during close-range interactions (review in Herzing 2000).<br />

However, only limited attempts have been made in a detailed behavioural <strong>and</strong> structural<br />

analysis of those signals. Therefore close-range acoustic communication in delphinids is<br />

only poorly understood.<br />

Resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off the coast of British Columbia produce<br />

whistles <strong>and</strong> variable pulsed calls at high rates during close-ranges, for example during<br />

socializing (Ford 1989; Thomsen et al. 2002). Earlier studies indicate that variable calls are<br />

graded <strong>and</strong> that both sound types are used to signal motivation during close-range<br />

interactions (Ford 1989). However, no study has focussed on a detailed analysis of the<br />

structure of whistles <strong>and</strong> variable calls. Our ongoing <strong>and</strong> long-term study investigates in<br />

the structure of whistles <strong>and</strong> variable calls in northern resident killer whales off British<br />

Columbia.<br />

METHODS<br />

Since 1994 we collect underwater sound recordings <strong>and</strong> surface behavioural<br />

observations of northern resident killer whales in Johnstone Strait <strong>and</strong> adjacent waters. We<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

160


also analyse earlier recordings obtained between 1978-1983 by one of us (J.K.B. Ford).<br />

We use SIGNAL, version 3.0, <strong>and</strong> RTS, version 2.0 (Engineering Design), for<br />

bioacoustical analysis. For a detailed description of data collection- <strong>and</strong> analysis see<br />

Thomsen et al. (2001, 2002).<br />

RESULTS<br />

Whistles are physically diverse signals with the majority being harmonical sounds.<br />

Parameter measurements indicate that they are very complex: they range in duration<br />

between 0.06 <strong>and</strong> 18 s (average 1.8 s) <strong>and</strong> contain between 0 <strong>and</strong> 71 frequency<br />

modulations (Thomsen et al. 2001). Our observations in the field indicate that they have a<br />

relatively short range of detectability (< 500 m). This corresponds closely with Millers<br />

(2000) findings, who measured an average ‘soft’ source level of 140 dB re 1µPa at 1 m<br />

(range 129-148 dB) for whistles of northern resident killer whales. About 25 % of all<br />

whistles appear to be stereotyped <strong>and</strong> stable for almost two decades (Fig. 1). We found six<br />

stereotyped whistles forms (W1-W6) that are often emitted in sequences (Fig. 2). In a<br />

sequence stereotyped whistles of the same type follow more frequently each other than<br />

different types.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, we could confirm that variable calls are structurally graded<br />

sounds. Teichert (2000) found variable call types that differed in carrier frequencies. These<br />

call types can be arranged on a graded scale from very low-frequency calls to high<br />

frequency ones.<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

We conclude that both whistles <strong>and</strong> variable calls play a very important role in<br />

coordinating close-range interactions in wild killer whales. It is possible that variable<br />

whistles indicate the motivation directly through the duration <strong>and</strong> number of frequency<br />

modulations. Stereotyped whistles are evidently used for close-range signalling too <strong>and</strong> we<br />

suspect that sequences of stereotyped whistles play an important role here. Sequences<br />

could indicate motivation over time with long <strong>and</strong> complex sequences indicating a higher<br />

motivation of the sender (s) than shorter <strong>and</strong> simple ones. Thus, sequences might be of<br />

great importance in coordinating <strong>and</strong> maintaining interactions at close-ranges. The results<br />

on variable calls further support earlier observations that variable calls in wild killer<br />

whales are organised similar to graded calls in canids <strong>and</strong> primates. The different forms of<br />

variable calls probably indicate subtle variations in motivation in accordance to<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

161


motivation-structural-rules. Then, low frequency calls would signal agonistic motivations,<br />

whereas high-frequency types signal friendly ones.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

We would like to thank Jim Borrowman <strong>and</strong> Bill <strong>and</strong> Donna Mackay for their support<br />

for this study. Thanks to Dave Tyre, Brian Sylvester, Wayne Garton, Rolf Hicker <strong>and</strong> Steve<br />

Wischniowski for their help in the field. Dave Briggs, Helena Symonds, Paul Spong, Rob<br />

Williams <strong>and</strong> Anna Spong provided important information on whale locations. Many thanks<br />

to Prof. Dr. Andreas Elepf<strong>and</strong>t <strong>and</strong> Dr. Karl-Heinz Frommolt (Humboldt-University, Berlin)<br />

for their cooperation. F. Thomsen would like to thank Jakob Parzefall, Cord Crasselt, Ralf<br />

Wanker, Massoud Yasseri <strong>and</strong> Ingo Schlupp for their help during the analysis at Hamburg<br />

University. We thank Christina Jakisch, Simone Hinzpeter, Stephanie Stempell, Rouven<br />

Schmidt <strong>and</strong> Stefan Froschauer for their help in whistle classification. The study was partly<br />

funded by a graduate scholarship of the University of Hamburg <strong>and</strong> by a scholarship of the<br />

German Academic Exchange Fund (Bonn).<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Ford, J.K.B. (1989). Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off<br />

Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia. Can. J. Zool., 67: 727-745.<br />

Fox, M.W. <strong>and</strong> Cohen, J.A. (1977). Canid communication. In: Sebeok, T.A. (ed.), How<br />

animals communicate. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, London, pp. 729-747.<br />

Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of behavior. The Belknap<br />

Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge.<br />

Herzing, D. (2000). Acoustics <strong>and</strong> social behavior of wild dolphins: implications for a<br />

sound society. In: Au, W.W.L., Popper, A.N. <strong>and</strong> Fay, R.R. (ed.) Hearing by whales <strong>and</strong><br />

dolphins. Springer, pp. 225-273.<br />

Miller, P.J.O. (2000). Maintaining contact: design <strong>and</strong> use of acoustic signals in killer<br />

whales, Orcinus orca. Ph.D. dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Woods Hole<br />

Oceanographic Institution.<br />

Marler, P. (1976). Social organization, communication <strong>and</strong> graded signals: the<br />

chimpanzee <strong>and</strong> the gorilla. In: Bateson, P.P.G. <strong>and</strong> Hinde, R.A. (ed.), Growing points in<br />

ethology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 239-280.<br />

Owings, H.O. <strong>and</strong> Morton, E.S. (1998). Animal vocal communication: a new approach.<br />

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

162


Thomsen, F., Franck, D. <strong>and</strong> Ford, J.K.B. (2001). Characteristics of whistles from the<br />

acoustic repertoire of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>, British<br />

Columbia. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109: 1240-1246.<br />

Thomsen, F., Franck, D. <strong>and</strong> Ford, J.K.B. (2002). On the communicative significance of<br />

whistles in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca). Naturwissenschaften 89: 404-407.<br />

74.<br />

Zimen, E. (1978). Der Wolf. Mythos und Verhalten. Meyster, Wien, München, pp. 35-<br />

Fig. 1 Exemplary spectrograms of whistle types W1-W3 recorded in different years (DT = 20.5 ms, DF = 48.8<br />

Hz, FFT size = 1024 points). Above: W1 1983, 1996, 1997; mid: W2 1979, 1996, 1997; below: W3 1979, 1996<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

163


Fig. 2 Spectrograms of three sequences of stereotyped whistles recorded from socializing killer whales Above:<br />

A5, D1 1980; mid: A1 1996; below A1, A5 1997. (DT = 20.5 ms, DF = 48.8 Hz, FFT size = 1024 points).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

164


A REVIEW OF SHORT- AND LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF WHALE WATCHING<br />

ON KILLER WHALES IN BRITISH COLUMBIA<br />

Andrew W. Trites 1 , David E. Bain 2 , Robert M. Williams 1 & John K.B. Ford 1,3<br />

1.Marine Mammal Research Unit, Fisheries Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC<br />

2. Friday Harbor Labs, University of Washington, Friday Harbor, WA 98250<br />

3. Marine Mammal Research Program, Pacific Biological Station, Fisheries & Oceans Canada,<br />

Nanaimo, BC<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Whale watching is often promoted as an alternative to whaling or fishing. It is a<br />

growing industry around the world <strong>and</strong> one that raises questions about its effects on marine<br />

mammals. In particular—can the whales sustain it? This is the fundamental question we<br />

explored.<br />

In British Columbia, killer whales have been studied for 25 years, <strong>and</strong> watched<br />

commercially <strong>and</strong> recreationally for about 20 years. They seem to receive more attention than<br />

any other species of cetacean.<br />

Of the three populations of killer whales in British Columbia, it is the residents that are<br />

the focus of whale watching. The two core areas for commercial whale watching are Haro<br />

Strait <strong>and</strong> Johnstone Strait. Most of the studies of the effects of whale watching on killer<br />

whales have been conducted in Johnstone Strait.<br />

For the past 7 years, numbers of killer whales in the northern resident population have<br />

been relatively stable, while those in the southern residents have declined. Some have argued<br />

that whale watching has contributed to this population decline.<br />

There has been a sharp increase in the numbers of commercial boats engaged in whale<br />

watching in Haro Strait, while those in the northern region have remained stable. There has<br />

also undoubtedly been a significant growth in numbers of private boats engaged in watching<br />

the whales.<br />

SHORT TERM EFFECTS<br />

The first studies of the effects of whale watching began in the 1980s when David Briggs<br />

watched the response to killer whales to vessels at the rubbing beaches of Robson Bight in<br />

Johnstone Strait. At about the same time, Susan Kruze observed the whales form a cliff<br />

across from the Bight, while David Duffus <strong>and</strong> Nicole Adimey made observations from the<br />

water.<br />

Their findings can be essentially summarized under two categories of responses: ‘near<br />

shore’ <strong>and</strong> ‘away from shore’.<br />

David Briggs found that whales stopped rubbing when approached by boats, while<br />

Susan Kruse reported that whales swam 1.5 times faster when boats were present. David<br />

Duffus was unable to note any affect, while Nicole Adimey noted a change in behavior—<br />

killer whales were 3-4 times more percussive when a single boat was present, but showed no<br />

apparent response when more than one boat was present.<br />

During the 1990s, researchers had a new tool to further their underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the<br />

effects of whale watching on killer whales. It was the Killer Whale Catalogue, which enabled<br />

them to identify the response of known individuals. There were also large numbers of<br />

volunteers prepared to put in thous<strong>and</strong>s of hours of observation.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

165


BC Parks established an ecological reserve at Robson Bight to protect the killer whale<br />

rubbing beaches. It is a reserve that works on the honor’s system, whereby boaters are asked<br />

to voluntarily restrain from entering the reserve. Observations made in the 1990s from the<br />

cliff across from the Reserve noted numbers of vessels <strong>and</strong> whales that were inside <strong>and</strong><br />

outside of the Reserve. Analyzing these thous<strong>and</strong>s of observations showed that killer whales<br />

left or stopped rubbing when boats were present. They also showed that the probability of<br />

leaving increased with an increase in the number of vessels that entered the reserve.<br />

One of the problems with all of the analyses done up to this point was that there were no<br />

real controls <strong>and</strong> the studies relied on opportunistic observations. This lead to Rob Williams<br />

conducting an experiment to track a single whale when no boat was present, then to have an<br />

experimental boat approach <strong>and</strong> follow the same individual according to accepted whale<br />

watching guidelines. Observations were made using a theotolite, a spotting scope <strong>and</strong> a<br />

laptop computer from the cliff top.<br />

The data collected during this experiment allowed the X-Y coordinates of where whales<br />

surfaced to be determined. From this, the angle of deviation <strong>and</strong> the overall directness of<br />

swimming were calculated. Thus the diving <strong>and</strong> swimming behavior of whales was compared<br />

before <strong>and</strong> after a boat was present.<br />

In the presence of one boat, Williams found that females swam faster, <strong>and</strong> had greater<br />

angles between their dives, than did males. Males on the other h<strong>and</strong> swam in a less direct<br />

path. However, when more than one boat was present, females made longer dives <strong>and</strong> swam<br />

in a more predictable direction. Males also swam in a more predictable direction, but at<br />

slower speeds.<br />

In summary, short-term immediate effects of whale watching were clearly<br />

demonstrated. They showed that killer whales are particularly sensitive to vessels when close<br />

to shore; <strong>and</strong> that males <strong>and</strong> females use different strategies in response to the presence of<br />

vessels when they are away from shore. And finally, they showed that the number <strong>and</strong><br />

proximity of vessels alters their response.<br />

LONG TERM EFFECTS<br />

While short-term responses do occur, the question that remains is “so what”? Are there<br />

any long-term effects? Has anything been detected after 25 years of research?<br />

One can postulate that whale watching might increase mortality, or reduce birth rates, or<br />

change the distribution of killer whales. But the reality is that there is no direct evidence that<br />

such changes can be attributed to whale watching, despite 25 years of research <strong>and</strong> 20 years of<br />

whale watching. However, just because such effects have not been measured, does not mean<br />

that they are not occurring.<br />

One means of trying to actively evaluate the possible long-term effects of whale<br />

watching is to compare changes in killer whales over time <strong>and</strong> space such as whether the<br />

dynamics <strong>and</strong> life tables of populations exposed to different levels of whale watching differ,<br />

or whether populations have different physiological <strong>and</strong> behavioral responses by region or<br />

time of year. Another approach is to develop mathematical models to predict the population<br />

effects of vessels on hearing <strong>and</strong> energetics. Such models can show that it is theoretically<br />

possible for whale watching to have population effects on killer whales.<br />

CONCLUSIONS<br />

In conclusion, it is apparent that on a short-time scale, whales employ a multivariate<br />

array of responses that are a function of vessel numbers, proximity <strong>and</strong> distance to shore, <strong>and</strong><br />

no doubt numerous other factors. However, on a long-time scale, we can only postulate<br />

possible effects <strong>and</strong> may never be able to conclusively demonstrate them. This begs the<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

166


question of whether whale watching activities should be curtailed if there is no absolute proof<br />

that it has a negative long-term effect on killer whales.<br />

It is debatable whether such long-term effects should be conclusively demonstrated<br />

when simply reducing the short-term effects may ultimately reduce the likelihood or severity<br />

of long-term effects. A number of approaches might be taken. The first is what is being tried<br />

in many places – namely to educate the mariner. Another is to establish whale reserves where<br />

boats do not enter. Thought should also be given to temporal restrictions that reduce the<br />

number of days that whales are actively pursued by vessels engaged in active whale watching.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

167


ASSOCIATIONS AND GROUP SIZE OF KILLER WHALES IN KVÆFJORD,<br />

INTRODUCTION:<br />

NORWAY, DURING OCTOBER-NOVEMBER 1997<br />

Fern<strong>and</strong>o Ugarte<br />

Whale Works. Allégade 23B 3.th, 2000 Frederiksberg, Denmark.<br />

Photo identification of killer whales in the coastal waters of northern Norway during<br />

October-November has taken place since 1983 (Lyrholm 1988, Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1997).<br />

During this time of the year, a large number of killer whales follow the migration of the<br />

Norwegian spring-spawning herring stock, which normally winters in the waters of Vestfjord<br />

<strong>and</strong> its tributary fjords, including Tysfjord (Similä et al 1996). During 1997-1998, part of the<br />

Norwegian spring-spawning herring stock wintered in the area of Kvæfjord, Norway, <strong>and</strong><br />

some killer whales followed (fig.1). This atypical wintering area contained an aggregation of<br />

killer whales smaller than the one normally found in Vestfjord.<br />

Photo identification fieldwork in Kvæfjord yielded more within-season re-sightings than<br />

the usual fieldwork in Vestfjord. The present study examines the associations of the killer<br />

whales observed in Kvæfjord, to complement the previously suggested model of social<br />

relationships of the Norwegian killer whales (Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1997).<br />

METHODS:<br />

Observations were made between October 25 <strong>and</strong> November 14, 1997, from a 10 m<br />

cabin cruiser. Once a killer whale group was localised, the first 10 minutes of the encounter<br />

were used to observe from a distance <strong>and</strong> record group size <strong>and</strong> group activity. After the<br />

observation period, the whales were photographically identified. An encounter ended when it<br />

was believed that all the whales with conspicuous marks had been photographed. In order to<br />

avoid bias due to repeated samplings of the same situation, each consecutive encounter was<br />

made either on a different group or during a different group activity.<br />

When there were several groups in the area, a killer whale group was defined as a<br />

cluster of animals that appeared to be independent from others. When a group was divided in<br />

clear sub-groups, special emphasis was made in observing the behaviour <strong>and</strong> counting the<br />

number of individuals in the sub-group that was closer to the boat during the 10 minutes<br />

observation period.<br />

Individual whales were identified from photographs of their left side showing the dorsal<br />

fin <strong>and</strong> saddle patch (Bigg et al 1987). Each identified whale received an individual<br />

alphanumerical code. Pictures of the identified whales were compared with identification<br />

pictures of killer whales in northern Norway taken during 1983-1996 (Similä, unpublished<br />

catalogue).<br />

A cluster analysis was made in order to investigate the existence of groups formed by<br />

whales with strong associations. The analysis was made using Cystat © 7.0.1 software, with<br />

average linkage method <strong>and</strong> Euclidean distances from a matrix containing the association<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

168


values of all possible pairs of whales. Simple ratio indexes of associations were used (Wilson<br />

1995). Only the whales observed more than once were included in the analysis.<br />

RESULTS:<br />

Killer whales were observed during 11 days (n = 60 encounters). Photo identification<br />

efforts were done during 36 encounters <strong>and</strong> 56 whales were identified, of which 14 (25%)<br />

were seen in Tysfjord during 1990-1993.<br />

There were no relationships between killer whale group size <strong>and</strong> activity state. 25% of<br />

the groups were divided into two or more sub-groups (n = 60 group counts). Group sizes<br />

ranged 1-25 individuals (median = 8 individuals) <strong>and</strong> sub-groups ranged 1-18 (median = 6<br />

individuals).<br />

The size of whole groups changed only twice during the 600 minutes of observations,<br />

which corresponded to one occasion in which one whale left <strong>and</strong> one in which one joined a<br />

group. Re-arrangements of the constellations of whales within a group were more frequent.<br />

The number of subgroups within the group changed 4 times <strong>and</strong> whales switched from one<br />

subgroup to another 11 times (n = 600 minutes).<br />

63 % of the animals (n = 35 individuals) were identified during more than one<br />

encounter. On only 7 occasions killer whale individuals were observed two consecutive times<br />

in groups of the same size (n = 87 successive values for group size of known individuals).<br />

This lack of stability in the group size is illustrated by the group sizes of NE1, the whale<br />

identified more times during this study (n = 11 observations). The group sizes of NE1, in the<br />

order observed, were: 11, 7, 12, 1, 7, 20, 9, 8, 15, 10 <strong>and</strong> 25 whales.<br />

An average of 60 % of the whales present were identified on each encounter (n = 36<br />

encounters, SD 27). The cluster analysis showed 10 groups of whales with association<br />

distances closer than 0.5, suggesting strong bonds between individuals (fig. 2). Some of the<br />

animals that showed the strongest associations had also been identified together in previous<br />

years (table 1).<br />

DISCUSSION:<br />

The fact that not all the animals present in each group were always identified can have<br />

caused underestimations of association indexes. Despite this problem, several whales showed<br />

high probabilities of being identified together. Some of the animals that showed strong<br />

associations have also been identified together in previous years, indicating that Norwegian<br />

killer whales associate with the same partners during time scales of few weeks <strong>and</strong> during tens<br />

of years. Long-term associations do not seem to depend on the sex of the animal, since males<br />

associated with males, males with females <strong>and</strong> females with females. Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte<br />

(1997) showed that males identified as juveniles in the 1980s continued to swim close to their<br />

mothers after reaching sexual maturity in the 1990s, suggesting that Norwegian killer whales<br />

form stable matrilineal groups. All these results indicate that killer whales in Norway do form<br />

very stable bonds.<br />

Advantages of living in stable matrilineal groups include: learned behaviour <strong>and</strong><br />

information can be transmitted by more experienced whales to genetically related animals<br />

(Boran <strong>and</strong> Hemlich 1999); by cooperatively foraging with kin, food sharing <strong>and</strong> provisioning<br />

can increase fitness (Dawkins 1976); animals that know each other well can coordinate the<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

169


search <strong>and</strong> hunt better <strong>and</strong> increase the per-capita rate of food consumption (Heishorn <strong>and</strong><br />

Packer 1995); allomaternal care (Woodroffe <strong>and</strong> Vincent 1994).<br />

Despite the potential advantages of living among close kin, matrilineal groups where<br />

males remain after reaching sexual maturity are very rare among animals. This may be due to<br />

the difficulty of avoiding inbreeding. Killer whales may get around this problem if females<br />

are able to decide whom to mate with after assessing the genetic proximity of potential<br />

partners with the help of vocal dialects (Barret-Lennard 2000).<br />

During this study, killer whale groups were sometimes arranged into subgroups in<br />

constellations that changed either because the subgroups split or joined or because individuals<br />

moved between subgroups. Group sizes changed seldom during an observation period, while<br />

individuals typically formed part of groups of different size during two consecutive<br />

observations. These results suggest that the whales within a group rearranged positions within<br />

minutes <strong>and</strong> that group sizes are stable during few hours <strong>and</strong> change after several hours.<br />

The existence of individuals with strong bonds <strong>and</strong> groups of flexible size could be<br />

explained if Norwegian killer whales were socially organised in small, stable units that split<br />

<strong>and</strong> join with similar units. Such units could be equivalent to the matrilineal units described<br />

for the resident killer whales in the west coast of North America (Bigg et al 1987).<br />

The searching efficiency of killer whales as predators depends on factors such as their<br />

ability to echolocate <strong>and</strong> / or to find prey by listening, either to the prey itself or to the sound<br />

of other predators feeding (Barret-Lennard et al 1996). Large groups can spread <strong>and</strong> search in<br />

larger areas, while it may be easier to coordinate activities in small groups. In Norway, killer<br />

whale group size may be an adaptation to the abundance of suitable herring schools, with<br />

larger groups being better when such schools are scarce. The existence of foraging groups that<br />

change size according to the density of food patches has been described for several species<br />

(Chapman et al 1995). A social organisation as described above, composed of small, stable<br />

groups that split <strong>and</strong> join, would be ideal to achieve group sizes that fit the density of suitable<br />

herring schools, while at the same time keeping the advantages of having at least some stable<br />

companions.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS:<br />

Funding was provided by NFH (Tromsø University),WWF Sweden <strong>and</strong> ANCRU. Tiu<br />

Similä <strong>and</strong> Tore Haug gave essential advice. Ben Wilson, Peter Van der Gullik <strong>and</strong> Volker<br />

Ratmeyer helped in the field. Godstrek made the figures. Tiu Similä allowed access to the<br />

sighting histories of whales identified during 1988-1993.<br />

REFERENCES:<br />

Barret-Lennard, L. 2000. Population Structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) as revealed by DNA analysis. PhD thesis. University of British Columbia<br />

Barret-Lennard, L., Ford, J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Heise, K. 1996. The mixed blessing of<br />

echolocation: differences in sonar use by fish-eating <strong>and</strong> mammal-eating killer whales. Anim.<br />

Behav. 51:533-565<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

170


Bigg, M.A, Ellis, G.M., Ford J.K.B. <strong>and</strong> Balcomb, K.C. 1987. Killer whales, a study of<br />

their identification, genealogy <strong>and</strong> natural history in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Washington State.<br />

Phantom Press <strong>and</strong> Publishers Inc. Nanaimo, B.C.<br />

Boran J.R., <strong>and</strong> Hemlich S.L. 1999. Social learning in cetaceans: hunting, hearing <strong>and</strong><br />

hierarchies. In: Mammalian Social Learning. Ed: Box, H.O. <strong>and</strong> Gibson, K.R. Cambridge<br />

Univ. press: 282-307<br />

Chapman, C.A., Wrangham, R.W. <strong>and</strong> Chapman, L.J. 1995. Ecological constraints on<br />

group size: an analysis of spider monkey <strong>and</strong> chimpanzee subgroups. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.<br />

36:59-70<br />

Dawkins, R. 1976. The Selfish Gene. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford.<br />

Heishorn, R., <strong>and</strong> Packer, C. 1995. Complex cooperative strategies in group territorial<br />

African lions. Science. 269(5228): 1260-1262.<br />

Lyrholm, T. 1988. Photoidentification of individual killer whales, Orcinus orca, off the<br />

coast of Norway, 1983-1986. Rit. Fiskideildar. 11:89-94.<br />

Similä T. <strong>and</strong> Ugarte F. 1997. Social organisation of north Norwegian killer whales. In:<br />

Similä, T. PhD thesis. NFH, University of Tromsø<br />

Similä, T., Holst, J.C., <strong>and</strong> Christensen, I. 1996. Occurrence <strong>and</strong> diet of killer whales in<br />

Northern Norway: seasonal patterns relative to the distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of Norwegian<br />

spring-spawning herring. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 53:769-779<br />

Woodrofe, R. <strong>and</strong> Vincent, A. 1994. Mother's little helpers: patterns of male care in<br />

mammals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9:294-297<br />

Wilson, D.R.B. 1995. The ecology of bottlenose dolphins in the Morray Firth, Scotl<strong>and</strong>:<br />

a population at the northern extreme of the species range. PhD thesis. University of Aberdeen.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

171


Table 1. Whales seen together in the same group during this study, during the period 1983-1986 (Lyrholm 1998)<br />

<strong>and</strong> during 1988-1993 (dataset from Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1997). Alphanumerical codes (e.g.: NB-6, NG-22) are<br />

names assigned to identified individuals. In parentheses are the sex of the whale <strong>and</strong> the number of times it has<br />

been seen in all three studies, m = male; f = female.<br />

Figure 1. Map of the study area<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

172


Figure 2. Cluster tree of associations of whales seen twice or more during this study. Alphanumerical codes (e.g.:<br />

NP9, NG29, etc.) are identification numbers of individual killer whales. Potential groups of whales with strong<br />

associations are highlighted.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

173


NORWEGIAN KILLER WHALES FEED ON SMALL HERRING SCHOOLS CLOSE<br />

TO THE SURFACE<br />

Ugarte F.<br />

Whale Works Allégade 23 B, 3.th. 2000 Frederiksberg. Denmark, fern<strong>and</strong>o_ugarte@hotmail.com<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Herring is a physostomus fish <strong>and</strong> can perform rapid vertical movements, which are<br />

useful to avoid predators <strong>and</strong> to undergo diurnal vertical migrations (Blaxter 1985). In its<br />

wintering area, the herring of the Norwegian spring spawning stock is typically found at<br />

depths of 0 - 70 meters during night <strong>and</strong> 150 - 500 meters during daytime (Dommasnes et al<br />

1994).<br />

The majority of Norwegian killer whales are predators specialised on herring of the<br />

Norwegian spring-spawning stock (Similä et al 1996). Although killer whales can dive at<br />

depths larger than 350 meters, they seem to spend by far most of their time in the upper 20<br />

meters of the water column (Baird et al. 1998, Schorr et al 2001, Similä et al 2002). The<br />

deepest interaction recorded between killer whales <strong>and</strong> herring so far was at a depth of 170<br />

meters, close to the upper range where herring is typically found during daylight. During<br />

several occasions, Norwegian killer whales have been observed feeding in the upper 20<br />

meters of the water column (Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1993, Similä 1998, Nøttestad <strong>and</strong> Similä<br />

2001).<br />

Herring have a strong schooling habit that can be used as a defence against predators<br />

(Magurran 1990, Parrish 1992). Most of the reported observations of Norwegian killer whales<br />

preying on herring involved small herring schools, rather than the more typical large herring<br />

aggregations (Similä <strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1993, Similä 1998, Nøttestad <strong>and</strong> Axelsen 1999, Nøttestad<br />

<strong>and</strong> Similä 2001).<br />

Killer whales seem to prefer feeding on small herring schools at shallow depths <strong>and</strong><br />

therefore the depth <strong>and</strong> size of the herring schools may be important factors influencing the<br />

availability of this fish as a prey item. The present study investigated the relationship between<br />

the depth <strong>and</strong> size of herring aggregations <strong>and</strong> the activity of Norwegian killer whales.<br />

METHODS:<br />

The present study was made during October <strong>and</strong> November of 1997 in the waters off<br />

Vestfjord, Andfjord, <strong>and</strong> some of their tributary fjords.<br />

A fish finder (Simrad Skiper 603) was used to take echo-sounder samples directly<br />

under killer whales (fig. 1). The herring aggregations were categorised as either schools or<br />

layers. Layers are shoals with a thickness of about 10 meters <strong>and</strong> several hundred meters of<br />

diameter. Previous to the samples, it was recorded whether the killer whales were engaged in<br />

one of the following activities: Feeding, Travelling, Resting, Socialising (Similä 1997, Ugarte<br />

2001). Night observations were made using night vision goggles.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

174


RESULTS:<br />

A total of 55 echo-sounder traces under killer whales were obtained: 46 during daylight<br />

<strong>and</strong> 9 during night.<br />

During daytime <strong>and</strong> in the absence of killer whales, herring were typically found as<br />

layers deeper than 150 m. At nightfall, the herring layers ascended to the surface <strong>and</strong> the killer<br />

whales ab<strong>and</strong>oned the area.<br />

During daytime, the herring under the killer whales was at depths of either 0-15 meters<br />

(n = 16 observations) or more than 50 meters (n = 20 observations).<br />

During daylight, herring layers beneath killer whales were often at depths larger than 60<br />

meters, while herring schools were at depths of either 0 - 15 or 60 - 200 meters.<br />

All feeding observations (n= 13) were made during daylight <strong>and</strong> involved small schools<br />

at depths shallower than 15 m (fig. 2).<br />

DISCUSSION:<br />

This study supports the idea that killer whales prefer to feed close to the surface (Similä<br />

<strong>and</strong> Ugarte 1993, Similä 1998, Nøttestad <strong>and</strong> Similä 2001).<br />

The lack of herring observed at 15-50 meters of depth can be explained if all herring<br />

shallower than 50 m was herded to the surface by the whales.<br />

Killer whales must be extremely efficient at driving small schools of herring to the<br />

surface, since no intermediate states between such schools <strong>and</strong> the typical deep herring layer<br />

were observed.<br />

The killer whales may have avoided the herring at night because the sheer amounts of<br />

fish presented an obstacle for efficient hunting. However, a larger sample would be needed to<br />

draw clear concussions.<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS:<br />

Funding was provided by WWF Sweden, The Andenes Cetacean Research Unit, The<br />

Royal <strong>International</strong> Whale Safari Club. The Roald Amundsen Centre, The Columbus Zoo <strong>and</strong><br />

Pilkington Optronics. This work was part of an MSc for the Norwegian College of Fisheries<br />

Science, University of Tromsø. Supervisors were Tiu Similä <strong>and</strong> Tore Haug. Ben Wilson<br />

provided ideas for this study <strong>and</strong> helped during fieldwork. Peter Van der Gullik <strong>and</strong> Volker<br />

Ratmeyer also helped in the field. Godstrek made the figures.<br />

REFERENCES:<br />

Baird, R.W., Dill, L.M. <strong>and</strong> Hanson, M.B. 1988. Diving behaviour of killer whales. In:<br />

abstracts of the World Marine Mammal Science Conference. Monaco, 20-24 Jan. The Society<br />

for Marine Mammalogy <strong>and</strong> the European Cetacean Society.<br />

Blaxter, J.H.S. 1985. The herring: a successful species? Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci.<br />

42(1):1-30<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

175


Dommasnes, A., Rey, F. <strong>and</strong> Røttingen, I. 1994. Reduced oxygen concentration in<br />

herring wintering areas. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 51:63-69<br />

Magurran, A. 1990. The adaptative significance of schooling as an anti-predator defence<br />

in fish. Ann. Zool. Fennici. 27:5-66<br />

Nøttestad, L. <strong>and</strong> Axelsen, B.E. 1999. Herring schooling manoeuvres in response to<br />

killer whale attack. Can. J. Zool. 68:1209-1215<br />

Nøttestad, L., <strong>and</strong> Similä, T. 2001. Killer whales attacking schooling fish: why force<br />

herring from deep water to the surface? Mar. Mamm. Sci. 17(2):343-352<br />

Parrish, J. 1992. Do predators "shape" fish schools? Interactions between predators <strong>and</strong><br />

their schooling prey. Neth. J. Zool. 42 82-3):358-370<br />

Similä, T. 1997. Behaviour <strong>and</strong> habitat use of killer whales in northern Norway. In:<br />

Simila, T. PhD thesis. NFH, University of Tromsø<br />

Simila, T. 1998. Sonar observations f killer whales feeding on herring schools. Aq.<br />

Mam. 23(3):119-126<br />

Similä T., <strong>and</strong> Ugarte F. 1993. Surface <strong>and</strong> underwater observations of cooperatively<br />

feeding killer whales (Orcinus orca) in northern Norway. Can. J. Zool. 71:1494-1499<br />

Similä, T., Holst, J.C. <strong>and</strong> Christensen, I. 1996. Occurrence <strong>and</strong> diet of killer whales in<br />

northern Norway: seasonal patterns relative to the distribution <strong>and</strong> abundance of Norwegian<br />

spring-spawning herring. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 53:769-779<br />

Similä, T., Holst, J.C., Øien, N., <strong>and</strong> Hanson, B. 2002. Satellite tracking study of<br />

movements, range <strong>and</strong> diving behaviour of killer whales in the Norwegian Sea. In: abstracts<br />

of the 16th annual conference of the European Cetacean Society, Liege. Pp. 18-19<br />

Schorr, J.L., Baird, R.W., Foster, J.F. <strong>and</strong> Hanson, M.B. 2001. Diving behaviour of<br />

fish-eating killer whales off Southern Icel<strong>and</strong>. In: abstracts of the 14 th biennial conference on<br />

the biology of marine mammals. Victoria.<br />

Ugarte, F. 2001. Behaviour <strong>and</strong> social organisation of killer whales in northern<br />

Norway. MSc thesis. NFH, University of Tromsø.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

176


Figure texts for the summary of: "Norwegian killer whales feed on small herring<br />

schools close to the surface", by Fern<strong>and</strong>o Ugarte:<br />

Figure 1: Trajectories of the boat while taking echosounder samples. The goal was to minimise the risk of<br />

missing small herring schools among the whales. A) When the whales moved forward, the boat sampled close<br />

behind the group. B) When the whales remained in the same area, the boat sampled through the group.<br />

Figure 2: Killer whale activity state <strong>and</strong>: herring depth (top); type of herring aggregation (bottom). N = 45<br />

encounters.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

177


CAN ROUTINE DATA COLLECTION BENEFIT KILLER WHALE RESEARCH?<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

A.M. van Ginneken, KC Balcomb III<br />

Center for Whale Research, Friday Harbor, Washington, U.S.A.<br />

Research data are ideally collected prospectively in a controlled setting, but killer<br />

whales cannot be controlled. In addition, public pressure makes observation <strong>and</strong> data<br />

collection increasingly difficult, even more so in the context of a specific research question.<br />

Another frequent problem is lack of ‘control’ data: when a research question arises because of<br />

a subjectively observed change or trend, one can no longer measure the past. A longterm<br />

collection of data on a routine basis may provide control data for the future, <strong>and</strong> may reveal<br />

relationships between parameters of orca life <strong>and</strong> human impact that can only be detected<br />

with large amounts of data collected in a variety of circumstances.<br />

Researchers, however, who attempt to analyze routinely collected data retrospectively,<br />

are often confronted with limitations due to incompleteness <strong>and</strong> lack of st<strong>and</strong>ardization.<br />

Photo-ID is a well established method that has produced many years of analyzable data.<br />

Behavior is much more elusive to document. The challenge is to get most out of each<br />

opportunity to observe killer whales as systematically as possible. How systematic can we be<br />

when we have to work with what the orca <strong>and</strong> its environment provide?<br />

This summary adresses the collection, representation, <strong>and</strong> extraction of data in the<br />

context of the <strong>Orca</strong> Survey, an ongoing study on killer whales by the Center for Whale<br />

Research (CWR) since 1976. Specific emphasis will be on parameters concerning behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> the environment.<br />

DATA COLLECTION<br />

Since 1976, the CWR has taken identification photographs, according to the method that<br />

Mike Bigg established in 1973 [1,2]. The analysis of these photographs have revealed much<br />

about the population dynamics <strong>and</strong> association behavior of the Southern resident population<br />

of orcas [3].<br />

Since 1994, we introduced routine collection of data on behavior <strong>and</strong> environmental<br />

parameters, such as wind (Beaufort scale), tide, <strong>and</strong> number of vessels present. In our attempt<br />

to make the method as systematic as possible, we based it on three main principles:<br />

1. Recording of observations rather than interpretations<br />

2. Defining behaviors with a high level of consensus among observers<br />

3. Explicit recording of detail <strong>and</strong> lack of detail<br />

Typical examples of interpretation are behavior terms like ‘foraging’, ‘feeding’, <strong>and</strong><br />

‘play’. Examples of observation terms are: ‘travel medium’, ‘groups spread out’, ‘breach’,<br />

<strong>and</strong> ‘taillob’. Data collection based on observations has two advantages:<br />

One can define criteria for ‘play’ <strong>and</strong> ‘foraging’ in a more objective way <strong>and</strong> extract<br />

more homogeneous sets of data, that fulfill these criteria. With recordings at the interpretation<br />

level one cannot go back to the source data for the actual observations.<br />

Consensus about observations is much easier to achieve than consensus on<br />

interpretations. Recognition of a type of behavior can effectively be trained in a short period<br />

of time as has been proven among <strong>Orca</strong> Survey staff <strong>and</strong> Earthwatch volunteers, who spend<br />

only 10 days on site.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

178


The <strong>Orca</strong> Survey staff has accentuated the definitions of behaviors that can be observed<br />

in the wild. These behaviors are devided in ‘duration’ behaviors that last for a certain period<br />

of time, <strong>and</strong> ‘instantaneous’ behaviors that occur in a moment <strong>and</strong> can be counted. The<br />

duration behaviors encompass travel speed <strong>and</strong> travel formation, whereas the instantaneous<br />

behaviors include the breach, spyhop, taillob, etc.These well-defined behavior types are<br />

illustrated on a reference tape of orca behaviors, showing fragments of wild footage. The tape<br />

has been scrutinized by staff <strong>and</strong> serves as a teaching instrument for volunteers <strong>and</strong> new staff.<br />

Behavior is difficult to record: much of what is going on escapes our attention, we often<br />

cannot visually identify which individual displays a behavior, or name all the members in a<br />

group, if it were only for the distance we have to the whales. We do not want to pretend<br />

completeness by entering ‘guesses’ for what we cannot asses. We explicitly record what we<br />

can <strong>and</strong> cannot not observe. The core of our records on behavior is: Who did What When. For<br />

this purpose we show the main fields on our Behavior log form with some data:<br />

Date: 20-Jun-2002<br />

Time Id(s) Behavior Number of whales Frequency<br />

10:20 J2,J30,unk Slow 3<br />

10:24 unk Breach 1 2<br />

10:26 unk Taillob 2 5<br />

Line 1 means: a group of three whales, of which only J2 <strong>and</strong> J30 can be identified,<br />

travels slowly.<br />

Line 2 means: an unknown whale did two breaches in succession.<br />

Line 3 means: two whales did 5 taillobs, but it was unclear how many each whale did.<br />

Uncertainty in the recognition of individuals is represented by ‘unknown’, sometimes<br />

the number of whales is blank, because the number of whales engaged in the behavior cannot<br />

be assessed.<br />

Beside behavior, we record GPS coordinates, <strong>and</strong> every 15 minutes we document the<br />

seastate on a Beaufort scale, the tide, <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>and</strong> type of vessels present. All data are<br />

recorded under staff supervision on paper forms, which are checked prior to data entry in a<br />

database, <strong>and</strong> rechecked by staff after data entry.<br />

THE DATABASE<br />

Currently, we have a database with photo-ID data <strong>and</strong> life-history parameters, such as<br />

matrilines, gender, <strong>and</strong> age-estimates. The behavior database contains the parameters<br />

mentioned above.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

179


Fig. 1 Structure of <strong>Orca</strong> Survey data in Access, including photographic, behavior, <strong>and</strong> GPS data.<br />

The first author designed a relational data model in third normal form, <strong>and</strong> applied<br />

extensive referential integrity [4], ensuring that all data are linked via the encounter date,<br />

encounter sequence, <strong>and</strong> time of observation. The encounter sequence makes all data unique<br />

for situations when crew on more than one vessel are simultaneously recording data. The<br />

main tables <strong>and</strong> their relationships are shown in Figure 1.<br />

Data from a h<strong>and</strong>held GPS is directly loaded into the computer. The GPS data are<br />

imported in Excel <strong>and</strong> new attributes are added to represent date <strong>and</strong> time in proper format for<br />

the Access database. The Excel data are then imported into Access <strong>and</strong> linked with the main<br />

table Boatmain.<br />

DATA EXTRACTION<br />

The Access database is the basis for all data extractions through SQL queries. Query<br />

results can then be used for descriptive graphs <strong>and</strong> tables of for statistic analysis. In the<br />

presented poster, we do not focus on the statistical confirmation or rejection of a specific<br />

scientific hypothesis, but we show examples of data extraction, resulting in data that may be<br />

subjected to such analysis. The examples of data extraction, as illustrated in the poster, are<br />

based on query results that have been exported to Excel:<br />

Population parameters:<br />

Population size since 1976<br />

Births <strong>and</strong> losses since 1976<br />

Age – gender distribution since 1973 (1973-1975 data from M. Bigg ang G.Ellis)<br />

Age at delivery in relation to calving order<br />

Age <strong>and</strong> gender at death / disappearance<br />

Matrilines showing gender <strong>and</strong> age category (illustrating reproductive capacity per pod)<br />

Behavior:<br />

Travel speed in relation to number of boats present<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

180


Travel formation in relation to number of boats present<br />

Plot of boattracks in the presence of orcas<br />

Plot of locations where milling behavior was observed<br />

For plotting of query results involving GPS data, a conversion of these data for plotting<br />

in Ozi-explorer was performed with Microsoft Excel, followed by a few simple manipulations<br />

in Wordpad.<br />

CONCLUSION<br />

We showed our methods for data collection, representation, <strong>and</strong> extraction. Although<br />

we are aware of the limitations of retrospective research, we believe that longterm collection<br />

of these data will help to detect changes overtime <strong>and</strong> may reveal relationships between<br />

parameters that we have not yet been aware of.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

[1] Bigg MA. An assessment of killer whale (Orcinus orca) stocks off Vancouver isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia.<br />

Rep. Int. Whal.Comm. 1982;32:655-66.<br />

[2] Bigg MA, MacAskie IB, Ellis G. Photoidentification of individual killer whales. Whalewatcher<br />

1983;17(1):3-5.<br />

[3] van Ginneken AM, Ellifrit DK. <strong>Orca</strong> Survey All Whale Guide to the Southern Resident Community 1976-<br />

2000 . Center for Whale Research, Friday Harbor WA. 2000.<br />

[4] Date C. Introduction to Database Systems. 1995. Addison-Wesley, New York.<br />

E-mail address of the first author: vanginneken@mi.fgg.eur.nl<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

181


PHOTOIDENTIFICATION OF KILLER WHALES IN ICELANDIC WATERS<br />

Vikingsson, G 1 , Sigurjonsson, J. 1 , Similä, T 2 .<br />

1 Marine Research Institute, P.O.Box 1390, 121 Reykjavik, Icel<strong>and</strong>, gisli@hafro.is ;<br />

2 Wild Idea, P.O.Box 181, 8465 Straumsjøen, Norway<br />

A long-term photoidentification study of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Icel<strong>and</strong>ic<br />

coastal waters was initiated in 1981. The study focuses on occurrence pattern of killer whales,<br />

stability of associations among the identified whales <strong>and</strong> possible movements of killer whales<br />

between Icel<strong>and</strong>ic <strong>and</strong> Norwegian coastal waters. Herring (Clupea harengus) is apparently the<br />

main type of prey of killer whales in Icel<strong>and</strong>ic coastal waters <strong>and</strong> photoidentification work<br />

has been carried out both in the wintering grounds of Icel<strong>and</strong>ic herring off the east coast of<br />

Icel<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> in the spawning grounds of herring off the south coast of Icel<strong>and</strong> during summer.<br />

In 1985-86 dedicated field work was carried out for several months, during other years cruises<br />

have been of shorter duration or identification pictures have been obtained on a more<br />

opportunistic basis. 366 killer whales have been identified. Most of the identification data is<br />

from the wintering grounds of herring but 56 different individuals have been identified in the<br />

summer spawning grounds of herring. The identification data indicates that at least some of<br />

the whales exploit herring both at the winter <strong>and</strong> summer grounds. In addition, two whales<br />

have been identified from a group of killer whales observed harrassing a grey seal<br />

(Halichoerus grypus); these whales have not been identified in the herring grounds. The<br />

opportunistic collection of identification pictures has resulted in relatively few resightings;<br />

only 25 % of the whales have been sighted during more than one season. This has made the<br />

analysis of group structure <strong>and</strong> stability difficult, however, the resighting data shows that<br />

stable bonds exist between the adult individuals. No matches have been found between the<br />

Icel<strong>and</strong>ic <strong>and</strong> Norwegian identification catalogues<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

182


NEW ZEALAND ORCA<br />

Visser I.N.<br />

<strong>Orca</strong> Research Trust P.O. Box 1233, Whangarei, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>, ingrid@orca.org.nz<br />

ABSTRACT.<br />

Between 1992 – 1997 a study was established to determine baseline information on<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca, <strong>and</strong> to provide recommendations for future management <strong>and</strong> conservation.<br />

Photo identification was used to determine distribution around New Zeal<strong>and</strong> waters <strong>and</strong> range<br />

use by individuals. The mean sighting period was 3.8 years. One orca was seen over a 20<br />

year period, <strong>and</strong> one resighted 30 times. Fifty orca were resighted five or more times. There<br />

was an apparent sub-division of the population into at least three geographic sub-populations.<br />

Association indices were compared within <strong>and</strong> between the proposed sub-populations as a test<br />

of possible sub-divisions. The mean associations within the sub-populations were<br />

significantly greater than between the sub-populations. Preliminary mtDNA analysis supports<br />

the hypothesis that some New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca do not mix. Sex ratios for the total population<br />

<strong>and</strong> for possible sub-populations do not differ from 1:1. Young are present in all populations,<br />

although ratios appear to differ. Population estimates show the total New Zeal<strong>and</strong> population<br />

of orca is at a critically low level (range 65-167 animals, with 115 calculated as alive in<br />

1997). There was a need to designate a conservation status for New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca that<br />

recognised their threats <strong>and</strong> low population levels. The New Zeal<strong>and</strong> government has<br />

recently changed their status from ‘common’ to ‘critical’.<br />

METHODS<br />

The study area covered the coast of New Zeal<strong>and</strong> out to approximately 20 miles<br />

offshore <strong>and</strong> for the purpose of this study, was divided into six Regions. Sighting data of<br />

individually identifiable animals came exclusively from photographs, not from individuals<br />

recognised in the field without being photographed. The information from the photo-id<br />

techniques was used to identify the distribution <strong>and</strong> range of individual orca <strong>and</strong> general<br />

distribution trends for the population. This information was then used to assess if there were<br />

potential sub-populations, perhaps divided geographically. A simple cumulative total was<br />

used (Discovery Curve) <strong>and</strong> calculations for the Total Enumeration (TE) <strong>and</strong> a stochastic<br />

model (Jolly-Seber) were both used for estimating the population of New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca.<br />

Associations were calculated using the Half-Weight Index. where the higher the Association<br />

Index (i.e., the closer the number is to 1), the more time the animals spend together. A value<br />

of zero would indicate that two animals were never seen together. Of the 50 orca seen more<br />

than five times, 41 had an Association Index of 0.33 or above with at least one other orca.<br />

These Association Indices were plotted in a graphic ‘circle-plot’, whereby the codes for the<br />

orca (e.g., NZ1, NZ50) were placed in a circle <strong>and</strong> lines of different thickness drawn to<br />

represent the Association Index value.<br />

RESULTS<br />

During the period December 1992 - December 1997, 117 individual orca were photoidentified.<br />

Of these, 75 % (n = 88) were seen on more than two occasions <strong>and</strong> 42 % (n = 50)<br />

were seen on more than five occasions. Twelve percent of orca (n = 14) were photo-identified<br />

on more than 10 occasions, <strong>and</strong> one orca on 30 occasions. The mean number of sightings for<br />

the 117 photo-identified animals was 5.4. Other individuals (n = 29) have been seen only<br />

once. Although some individuals were seen irregularly, others were seen regularly (up to 11<br />

times per year). The mean number of years over which individuals were sighted was 3.8<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

183


years (mode 4, median 8). However, if the 29 animals that were only sighted once were not<br />

included, the mean number of years over which individuals were sighted rises to 4.7 years<br />

(mode = 4, median = 8.5). There were three main patterns of distribution of New Zeal<strong>and</strong><br />

orca, however, the sighting locations for individual orca varied within each general trend of<br />

North-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only, North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong>, South-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only. Taking the 50 individuals who<br />

had been seen more than five times, it was possible to suggest three potential sub-populations.<br />

Overall, discovery rates were slow, with the maximum number of orca catalogued in<br />

one year being 37. Using an average orca mortality estimate from overseas of approximately<br />

2% per year, it is possible to enumerate the population. Considering the longevity of<br />

individuals <strong>and</strong> infrequent resightings data, the low frequency of resightings, or even failure<br />

to resight for up to 12 years, a lack of a resighting may not necessarily predict whether an<br />

animal was alive or dead in 1997. However, taking all individuals seen in 1997, plus all<br />

others last seen in previous years (devalued for mortality), a minimum estimate for most of<br />

the New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca population was derived for December 1997, (n = 115 orca). For the<br />

Jolly-Seber population model, the statistical program POPAN-4 was utilised. The size of the<br />

population in the last six months of 1996 was 119 ± 24 (s.e.) orca (95% confidence intervals<br />

based on a normal distribution [71, 167]). Of the 117 orca photo-identified, 47 were<br />

presumed to be females, 35 were confirmed as adult males, 13 were confirmed as SAM’s, two<br />

were juveniles, 11 were calves <strong>and</strong> the age or sex class could not be determined for a further<br />

nine orca. Adding the adult males <strong>and</strong> SAM’s together gives a sex ratio of 47 females to 50<br />

males. This sex ratio is not significantly different from a 1:1 sex ratio (X 2 = 0.67, df = 1, p <<br />

0.5). New Zeal<strong>and</strong> orca tend to travel in small to medium sized groups, where group size<br />

ranged from two to 22 individuals (mean = 4.5), of which 65% (n = 36) were comprised of<br />

eleven or less individuals, with groups of 12 (24%, n = 13) being the most common.<br />

Association Indices were calculated for the 50 orca seen more than five times. The<br />

highest Association Index value was (0.93), which was calculated for four dyads. The next<br />

highest Indices (0.84) <strong>and</strong> (0.83) were calculated for one dyad each, <strong>and</strong> (0.80) was calculated<br />

for four dyads. The mean Association Index value (for all 50 orca seen more than five times)<br />

was 0.25, the mode was 0.13 <strong>and</strong> the median was 0.18. However, it should be noted that this<br />

included 857 instances where there was no association at all (i.e., an Association Index of 0).<br />

Associations within two of the three proposed sub-populations were high. Both the<br />

‘within North-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only’ (NN) <strong>and</strong> ‘within South-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only’ (SS) were significantly<br />

higher than between the three sub-populations <strong>and</strong> ‘within North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong>’(NS).<br />

Associations were low between North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> both the North-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only <strong>and</strong><br />

South-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only sub-populations. As expected, no associations were recorded between the<br />

North-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only <strong>and</strong> South-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only sub-populations. There was a significant difference<br />

between the North-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only <strong>and</strong> the North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong> sub-populations. In addition,<br />

there was a significant difference between the South-Isl<strong>and</strong>-only sub-population <strong>and</strong> the<br />

North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong> sub-population. The Association Index within the supposed<br />

North+South-Isl<strong>and</strong> population (NS) was not significantly different to both of the betweenpopulation<br />

indices (N NS) <strong>and</strong> (NS S). By limiting the plotted Association Indices to 0.33<br />

<strong>and</strong> above (<strong>and</strong> therefore removing occasional associations), there were a number of distinct<br />

groupings of orca visible, based on their associations on the circle plots.<br />

Reclassification of the New Zeal<strong>and</strong> population of orca from the erroneous New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> governmental category of ‘common’, to the recognised IUCN status of ‘Threatened’<br />

or ‘Rare’ was proposed by Visser (2002). Although this would hopefully see more effort put<br />

into their management, ‘red listing’ the population does not in itself confer protection.<br />

However, it would impose moral pressure on the New Zeal<strong>and</strong> Government to act<br />

accordingly. The status has recently been changed within the New Zeal<strong>and</strong> classification<br />

system to ‘critical’.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

184


Visser, I. N. (2000). <strong>Orca</strong> (Orcinus orca) in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> waters. Ph. D. Dissertation.<br />

University of Auckl<strong>and</strong>, Auckl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

185


PIGMENTATION AS AN INDICATIVE FEATURE FOR POPULATIONS OF<br />

KILLER WHALES<br />

Ingrid N. Visser.<br />

<strong>Orca</strong> Research Trust, P.O. Box 1233, Whangarei, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. ; ingrid@orca.org.nz<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

Worldwide, the typical diagnostic pigmentation pattern for killer whales (Orcinus orca)<br />

is a black body with postocular white patches (eye-patches), a white chin <strong>and</strong> venter<br />

extending onto a white ventral fluke pattern, <strong>and</strong> a post-dorsal grey patch (saddle-patch).<br />

However variations in this basic pigmentation pattern have been observed, beyond the<br />

infrequently reported albino or ‘white morphs’. These include an overall lighter colouration,<br />

in some cases lightening to grey, a clearly visible darker ‘dorsal cape’ <strong>and</strong> dark under-flukes.<br />

In addition, there appears to be a wide variation in eye-patch size, shape <strong>and</strong> orientation.<br />

For instance, in one str<strong>and</strong>ing in New Zeal<strong>and</strong>, all 17 animals had eye-patches less than half<br />

the size of the typical New Zeal<strong>and</strong> eye-patches. During another encounter with<br />

approximately 40 animals, similar small-sized eye-patches were apparent, yet the angular<br />

orientation was different. These 40 animals also showed light to dark grey dorsal<br />

pigmentation <strong>and</strong> demarcation of ‘dorsal capes’. On yet another encounter animals were<br />

noted to have very large eye-patches (approximately three times the size of typical New<br />

Zeal<strong>and</strong> eye-patches). In over 5000 photographs of New Zeal<strong>and</strong> killer whales, collected both<br />

opportunistically <strong>and</strong> historically (from as early as 1915), no other images show these types of<br />

eye-patches, a dorsal cape or pale pigmentation.<br />

Variations in pigmentation may be an indicative feature to distinguish killer whale<br />

populations. Further non-lethal research such as photo documentation <strong>and</strong> genetic sampling<br />

may support the theory that distinct populations or even separate species of killer whales may<br />

exist in different geographical regions.<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

• The typical diagnostic pigmentation pattern for killer whales (Orcinus orca) includes;<br />

~ black body<br />

~ white postocular patches (eye-patches)<br />

~ white chin<br />

~ white venter extending onto flanks<br />

~ white ventral fluke pattern<br />

~ grey post-dorsal saddle-patch<br />

• ‘White morphs’ <strong>and</strong> partially albino killer whales have been described (Fertl et al.<br />

1999) but these seem to be isolated occurrences.<br />

RESULTS<br />

• Variations in the basic black <strong>and</strong> white pigmentation pattern do appear to occur to<br />

whole populations, or groups (e.g., Berghan & Visser 2001).<br />

~ A group of eight killer whales sighted off New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 1997 had grey<br />

pigmentation <strong>and</strong> ‘dorsal capes’ – areas of darker pigment on dorsal area.<br />

~ A group of approximately 40 killer whales sighted off New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 2001 had grey<br />

pigmentation <strong>and</strong> ‘dorsal capes’.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

186


~ Some Antarctic killer whales have been described with grey pigmentation <strong>and</strong> ‘dorsal<br />

capes’ (Evans et al., 1982).<br />

• The underside of killer whale tail flukes are typically described as white (Fig. 2),<br />

however, some killer whales in Papua New Guinea have been noted as having grey<br />

under-flukes.<br />

• Variation in eye-patch size, shape <strong>and</strong> orientation have been described (Visser &<br />

Mäkeläinen 2000).<br />

~ Seventeen killer whales which str<strong>and</strong>ed in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 1955 had eye-patches less<br />

than half the size of the typical New Zeal<strong>and</strong> killer whale eye-patches.<br />

~ Approximately 40 killer whales sighted off New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 2001 had small-sized eyepatches,<br />

yet the angular orientation was different from the 1955 animals.<br />

~ Eight killer whales sighted off New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 1997 had eye-patches at least twice the<br />

size of the typical New Zeal<strong>and</strong> eye-patches.<br />

• Over 5000 photographs have been collected of New Zeal<strong>and</strong> killer whales between<br />

1915 <strong>and</strong> 2002, yet no other images show these variations in eye-patches, a ‘dorsal<br />

cape’ or pale pigmentation.<br />

• A group of approximately 40 killer whales with light grey pigmentation patterns,<br />

photographed off New Zeal<strong>and</strong> in 2001, had ‘lean’ profiles when compared to typical<br />

New Zeal<strong>and</strong> killer whales.<br />

DISCUSSION POINTS<br />

• Worldwide, the typical diagnostic pigmentation pattern for killer whales is a black<br />

body with white areas.<br />

• Partially albino <strong>and</strong> ‘white morph’ animals do not necessarily represent group<br />

pigmentation.<br />

• Variations in pigmentation may be an indicative feature to distinguish groups,<br />

populations, sub-species or even separate species of killer whales.<br />

• Variations in the basic black <strong>and</strong> white pigmentation pattern include grey bodies with<br />

grey ‘dorsal capes’.<br />

• Eye-patch size <strong>and</strong> orientation appear to differ between groups <strong>and</strong> possibly<br />

populations of killer whales.<br />

SUMMARY<br />

• Killer whale groups, populations, sub-species or species may be separated by<br />

distinctive pigmentation patterns.<br />

• Further non-lethal research, such as photo-documentation <strong>and</strong> genetics, may support<br />

this theory.<br />

REFERENCES<br />

Fertl, D., Pusser, L. T., & Long, J. J. (1999). First record of an albino bottlenose<br />

dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) in the Gulf of Mexico, with a review of anomalously white<br />

cetaceans. Marine Mammal Science. 15, (1), 227-234.<br />

Berghan, J., & Visser, I. N. (2001). Antarctic Killer Whale Identification Catalogue.<br />

14th biennial conference on the biology of marine mammals, Vancouver, Canada November<br />

28 - December 3, 2001, 22.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

187


Evans, W. E., Yablokov, A. V., & Bowles, A. E. (1982). Geographic variation in the<br />

color pattern of killer whales (Orcinus orca). Report of the <strong>International</strong> Whaling<br />

Commission. 32, 687-694.<br />

Visser, I. N., & Mäkeläinen, P. (2000). Variation in eye-patch shape of killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca) in New Zeal<strong>and</strong> waters. Marine Mammal Science. 16, (2), 459-469.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

188


FIRST PHOTO-IDENTIFICATION MATCHES FOR PAPUA NEW GUINEA<br />

KILLER WHALES.<br />

Ingrid N. Visser<br />

<strong>Orca</strong> Research Trust, P.O. Box 1233, Whangarei, New Zeal<strong>and</strong>. ingrid@orca.org.nz<br />

Photo-identification images were collected for 14 killer whales in Papua New Guinea<br />

waters, <strong>and</strong> a catalogue established. Matches were made for two animals – a female sighted<br />

two days <strong>and</strong> approximately 30 nautical miles apart, <strong>and</strong> a sub-adult male sighted<br />

approximately three years apart in the same region. While the IUCN does not list killer<br />

whales as present in Papua New Guinea waters, the records collected so far indicate it is<br />

found in the area for at least 10 months of the year. Thirty-three sightings from 1987 to July<br />

2002 were confirmed, with a further 44 sightings of unknown date or exact location recorded.<br />

The earliest reference to killer whales in this region was from 1956, when they were recorded<br />

taking fish off long-lines. Killer whales from these waters have been observed feeding on<br />

four species of elasmobranchs (scalloped-hammerhead shark, Sphyrna lewini; grey reef shark,<br />

Carcharhinus amblyrhynchos; manta ray, Manta birostris; <strong>and</strong> blue-spotted eagle ray<br />

Dasyatis kuhlii) <strong>and</strong> three fin-fish (yellow-fin tuna, Thunnus albacares; big-eye tuna, Thunnus<br />

obesus <strong>and</strong> Indo-Pacific sailfish, Istiophorus platypterus). Killer whales in Papua New<br />

Guinea waters have been reported in association with two species of cetaceans (sperm whales,<br />

Physeter macrocephalus <strong>and</strong> spinner dolphins, Stenella longirostris).<br />

Key words <strong>Orca</strong>, killer whale, Papua New Guinea, photo-identification, foraging,<br />

elasmobranchs, fin-fish.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

189


CHANGES IN CALL USE IN A RESIDENT ORCA-MATRILINE WITH A NEW-<br />

BORN CALF<br />

Brigitte Weiss & Friedrich Ladich<br />

Corresponding author: Weiss<br />

e-mail: a9400355@unet.univie.ac.at<br />

Institute of Zoology, University of Vienna, Althanstrasse 14, A-1090 Vienna, Austria<br />

Studies of the vocal behaviour of resident type orcas, Orcinus orca, in British Columbia<br />

(Ford1989, Ford 1991) showed that pods - social units comprised of one or more closely<br />

related matrilines - have unique repertoires of up to 17 discrete calls. Repertoire differences<br />

among pods consist of different call types <strong>and</strong> of subtypes that differ consistently in certain<br />

structural variables. Similar, but less pronounced differences can be found in the calling<br />

behaviour of matrilines belonging to the same pod (Miller & Bain 2000). None of the call<br />

types could be attributed exclusively to specific behaviours, <strong>and</strong> Ford (1989) argues that their<br />

function lies in the context of social organisation <strong>and</strong> group cohesion. Also, dialects may be<br />

used to discriminate between relatives <strong>and</strong> nonrelatives <strong>and</strong> might thus function as a<br />

mechanism for avoiding inbreeding (Treisman 1978).<br />

In the course of a study on the social significance of calls in resident orcas we<br />

investigated short-time changes in call use of a matriline before <strong>and</strong> up to three weeks after<br />

the birth of a calf. The birthdate is known precisely (± 1 day) <strong>and</strong> the focal matriline (the<br />

A30s, belonging to A-Clan of the Northern Resident Community) spent the weeks following<br />

the birth in the visually <strong>and</strong> acoustically well observed area of Johnstone Strait <strong>and</strong> adjacent<br />

waters in British Columbia, Canada. A network of radio-transmitting hydrophone stations,<br />

operated by the l<strong>and</strong>-based research station <strong>Orca</strong>Lab, enables the underwater acoustic<br />

environment of the area to be monitored continuously, 24 hours a day <strong>and</strong> year-round.<br />

Recordings with an acceptable signal to noise ratio were available for days when the calf was<br />

1 - 5, 15, 19 respectively 20 days old. During all selected recordings, whales were visually<br />

identified <strong>and</strong> the only matriline within range of a hydrophone. Calls were classified by<br />

simultaneous aural <strong>and</strong> visual inspection of sonagrams, produced with Cool Edit 2000 or<br />

S_Tools 3.55. Classification into discrete call types, variable calls <strong>and</strong> aberrant versions of<br />

discrete calls mainly followed that of Ford (1989 <strong>and</strong> 1991).<br />

Recordings of the A30 matriline after the birth of the calf A75 in September 2001 were<br />

split into 5 minute samples <strong>and</strong> the entire amount of samples available per day was used for<br />

statistical analysis. To increase the number of samples, data for days 2 <strong>and</strong> 3, 4 <strong>and</strong> 5 <strong>and</strong> for<br />

days 19 <strong>and</strong> 20 after birth were pooled. As a reference, fourteen samples from nine different<br />

days between July 18th, 1996, <strong>and</strong> September 4th, 2001, were r<strong>and</strong>omly chosen. To increase<br />

the number of calls taken into account 10 minutes samples were chosen. From the first day<br />

after birth until the family left the core area three weeks later, distinct changes in call use were<br />

apparent. Most apparent, call type N47 was used more than 10 times more frequently when<br />

the calf was one day old (Mann-Whitney: U = 11, p < 0.001, n = 30). During the following<br />

two weeks, the use of N47s gradually declined <strong>and</strong> recovered to values found before the birth<br />

(n = 77, rs = - 0.704, p < 0.001, Mann-Whitney: before vs. two weeks: U = 107.5, p = 0.448, n<br />

= 32). Also, within the first three weeks after the birth, call “A12 special”, a version of the N5<br />

call, reached values significantly higher than before the birth (Mann-Whitney: before vs. day<br />

19: A12 special: U = 14, p < 0.02, n = 20). Two weeks after birth the number of aberrant <strong>and</strong><br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

190


variable calls was significantly higher <strong>and</strong> the number of N7s <strong>and</strong> N8s significantly lower<br />

than compared to pre-birth recordings (Mann-Whitney: before vs. two weeks: n = 32, N7: U =<br />

71.5, p < 0.05, N8: U = 69, p < 0.02, aberrant calls: U = 16.5, p < 0.001, variable calls: U =<br />

69,<br />

p < 0.05). The other calls used more or less frequently immediately after birth had<br />

returned to normal values. Apart from a slight increase in N4s, no more significant changes in<br />

call use occurred in the third week after birth (Mann-Whitney: two weeks vs. three weeks: U<br />

= 57.5, p = 0.017, n = 13).<br />

Call type N47 is almost exclusive to the A30 matriline, <strong>and</strong> “A12 special” to the closely<br />

related A12 matriline. Their increased use after the calf's birth may facilitate the learning<br />

process for the "acoustic family badge" of its own <strong>and</strong> closely related matrilines <strong>and</strong> thereby<br />

help to recognize <strong>and</strong> maintain cohesion with family members.<br />

REFERENCES:<br />

Ford, J. K. B. 1989: Acoustic behaviour of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off<br />

Vancouver Isl<strong>and</strong>, British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 67, 727 – 745.<br />

Ford, J. K. B. 1991: Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in<br />

coastal waters of British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 69, 1454 – 1483.<br />

Miller, P. J. O. & Bain, D. E. 2000: Within-pod variation in the sound production of a<br />

pod of killer whales, Orcinus orca. Anim. Behav. 60, 617 – 628.<br />

Treisman, M. 1978: Bird song dialects, repertoire size, <strong>and</strong> kin association. Anim.<br />

Behav. 26, 814 – 817.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

191


SPATIAL MODELLING OF ANTARCTIC KILLER WHALE ABUNDANCE AND<br />

DISTRIBUTION<br />

Williams R., Hammond P<br />

Sea Mammal Research Unit, Gatty Marine Lab, University of St Andrews, St Andrews Fife KY16<br />

8LB, rmcw@smru.ac.uk<br />

The British Columbia (BC) killer whale study was initiated by Dr. Michael Bigg in<br />

response to an urgent need to estimate the number of killer whales in Canada’s Pacific waters<br />

(Ford et al. 2000). As governments attempt to manage resources on the ecosystem level, it<br />

will become increasingly important to estimate the abundance of top predators in the marine<br />

environment.<br />

Ford et al. (2000) calculated abundance of BC’s resident killer whales through longterm<br />

compilation of a photo-identification (photo-ID) catalogue, but not all populations lend<br />

themselves to complete enumeration. The northeast Pacific census works well, not only<br />

because investigators have dedicated years to their studies, but also because, as these longterm<br />

studies have shown, the whale populations are small, closed, accessible in inshore<br />

waters, <strong>and</strong> show no dispersal from the natal unit.<br />

This last condition is unknown in any other mammalian population, even in the BC<br />

transient killer whale community. Therefore, it may be appropriate to begin killer whale<br />

studies in other ocean basins with the expectation that a population does not follow the<br />

northeast Pacific resident model. In these new studies, abundance estimation may yield just<br />

that: an estimate, rather than a count.<br />

Capture-recapture analyses can be used to estimate abundance from identification<br />

photographs, but they become increasingly complex when a study area spans an ocean basin<br />

<strong>and</strong> contains an unknown number of populations (see Hammond et al. 1990). One such<br />

example is the Southern Ocean, where killer whale abundance has been estimated to be in the<br />

tens of thous<strong>and</strong>s (Branch <strong>and</strong> Butterworth 2001), <strong>and</strong> where the number of ecotypes remains<br />

unknown.<br />

Line-transect surveys are the most commonly used method among a family of<br />

abundance estimation techniques called Distance Sampling (Buckl<strong>and</strong> et al. 2001). In a<br />

shipboard sightings survey, ships follow a grid of systematically spaced tracklines placed<br />

r<strong>and</strong>omly in a study area to provide representative coverage. In addition to effort, observers<br />

record distance <strong>and</strong> angle to each sighting of a cetacean (or group). This enables modelling<br />

the probability of detecting a whale as a function of perpendicular distance from the ship, to<br />

estimate the width of the strip effectively searched, <strong>and</strong> therefore, whale density – the number<br />

of animals seen per unit area searched. This mean whale density in the samples is used to<br />

calculate abundance, which is why design-unbiased surveys must provide representative<br />

coverage.<br />

Conventional Distance Sampling (CDS) methods allow relatively fast abundance<br />

estimation, but they can be very expensive. Furthermore, they calculate mean density of<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

192


whales, rather than providing information on distribution. Distance sampling techniques are<br />

not designed to provide the additional life-history data that photo-ID studies can yield.<br />

Recent developments in line-transect survey design <strong>and</strong> analysis are termed Advanced<br />

Distance Sampling (ADS) techniques. These enable modelling effective strip width with<br />

environmental <strong>and</strong> biological covariates, <strong>and</strong> incorporation of geographic information for<br />

automated survey design (Buckl<strong>and</strong> et al. 2001). A third type of ADS is Spatial Modelling, in<br />

which heterogeneity in whale density along transects (Hedley et al. 1999) is modelled as a<br />

function of environmental <strong>and</strong> spatial variables, <strong>and</strong> used to predict density throughout a<br />

study area. Spatial modelling allows abundance estimation for any subset of a study area<br />

(unlike CDS, where abundance can be estimated only for predefined strata). Similarly, they<br />

can identify ‘hotspots,’ areas of predicted high density. Spatial modelling techniques offer<br />

additional appeal in that they make no assumption about placement of tracklines.<br />

We believe that these two families of methods, photo-ID <strong>and</strong> spatial modelling of linetransect<br />

data, could play complementary roles in studying killer whale populations. Such<br />

studies can be conducted simultaneously, by going off-effort after a sighting is made, <strong>and</strong><br />

closing in on killer whale groups to confirm school size, <strong>and</strong> to obtain identification<br />

photographs <strong>and</strong> biopsy samples. We use killer whales in the Southern Ocean as an example<br />

where preliminary work shows high killer whale abundance, while the number of populations<br />

present is unknown.<br />

We collected line-transect data from Platforms of Opportunity in the Scotia Sea during<br />

the austral summers of 2000-1 <strong>and</strong> 2001-2. By recording effort, as well as distance <strong>and</strong> angle<br />

to each sighting, we calculated killer whale density in 187 data recording sessions along<br />

9650km of trackline. Killer whales were sighted during 13 of these sessions, leaving 174<br />

tracklines along which killer whales were not seen. In total, 14 groups, totalling 61 animals,<br />

were spotted while on-effort.<br />

We used a Generalised Additive Model (GAM) to express heterogeneity of whale<br />

sightings as a function of spatial variables. Segments of tracklines where no killer whales<br />

were seen were given a 0 value for the response variable, while segments where killer whales<br />

were seen had a value of 1. The logit link was used to build a model describing heterogeneity<br />

in killer whale sightings with a logistic equation. We chose the GAM framework due to its<br />

flexibility in fitting the response variable (i.e., presence or absence of killer whales in a<br />

sampling unit) as non-linear functions of a suite of simple spatial variables (latitude, longitude<br />

<strong>and</strong> distance from the nearest coastline).<br />

Model selection was determined by Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC), which<br />

selects the most parsimonious model, the one that fits the data best with the least number of<br />

parameters. The chosen model was used to predict killer whale density throughout the study<br />

area, since a value for each predictor variable could be calculated for every square in a grid<br />

overlaying the study area.<br />

The selected model predicted two areas of relatively high density, one in the Scotia Sea,<br />

<strong>and</strong> another in the protected waters of the Antarctic Peninsula (Fig. 1). Integrating under the<br />

entire density surface yielded a predicted abundance of 415 schools in the study area. This<br />

represents a predicted total abundance of approximately 1800 killer whales, since mean group<br />

size in the study was 4.3 animals.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

193


Several sources of bias may influence this estimate. The model appeared to highly<br />

sensitive to the few sightings made in the middle of the Scotia Sea, where relatively little<br />

survey effort was conducted. This calls into question the need for objective means of<br />

determining when a survey has provided reasonable coverage of a study area, <strong>and</strong> for means<br />

other than AIC for model selection. Secondly, the effective strip width component of the<br />

density estimate was similarly influenced by small sample size.<br />

However, with additional survey effort, we believe that our study will highlight the<br />

utility of this valuable technique, whereby line-transect survey data from Platforms of<br />

Opportunity are used to model patchiness of whale sightings to predict areas of high density.<br />

Such areas could then be targeted, increasing the efficiency of subsequent line-transect<br />

surveys (by a priori stratification into areas of high vs. low density) or photo-ID surveys, as<br />

well as other methods to investigate population structure (e.g. biopsy <strong>and</strong> acoustics).<br />

In practice, long-term killer whale studies have progressed by identifying <strong>and</strong> targeting<br />

hotspots. Spatial modelling of line-transect data from inexpensive platforms provides an<br />

objective means of identifying likely hotspots in new study areas, based on where animals<br />

were seen, <strong>and</strong> where they were not seen, in pilot studies. In a spatial modelling framework,<br />

line-transect data become much more informative than sightings alone, <strong>and</strong> the techniques are<br />

likely to have wide application in areas where killer whale studies are just beginning.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

Branch, T. A. <strong>and</strong> D. S. Butterworth (2001). “Estimates of abundance south of 60°S for cetacean species<br />

sighted frequently on the 1978/79 to 1997/98 IWC/IDCR-SOWER sighting surveys.” Journal of Cetacean<br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Management 3(3): 251-270.<br />

Buckl<strong>and</strong>, S., D. R. Anderson, et al. (2001). Introduction to Distance Sampling: Estimating abundance of<br />

biological populations, Oxford University Press.<br />

Ford, J. K. B., G. M. Ellis, et al. (2000). Killer whales. 2nd Ed. Vancouver, UBC Press.<br />

Hammond, P. S., S. A. Mizroch <strong>and</strong> G.P. Donovan. (1990). Individual recognition of cetaceans: Use of<br />

photo-identification <strong>and</strong> other techniques to estimate population parameters. Reports of the <strong>International</strong><br />

Whaling Commission (Special Issue 12). Cambridge, <strong>International</strong> Whaling Commission.<br />

Hedley, S. L., Buckl<strong>and</strong>, S.T. <strong>and</strong> Borchers, D.L. (1999). “Spatial modelling from line transect data.” J.<br />

Cetacean Res. Manage. 1(3): 255-264.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

194


Figure 1. Predicted density of killer whale schools, based on a spatial model linking probability of sighting killer<br />

whales to smoothed functions of latitude, longitude <strong>and</strong> distance from the nearest coastline. Purple squares have<br />

low predicted densities of killer whale schools (except outside the study area defined by the black polygon,<br />

where predicted values have been coerced to zero). Abundance is calculated by integrating under the entire<br />

density surface. Additionally, areas of predicted high density (the red squares) could be targeted for photo-ID,<br />

biopsy or acoustic surveys.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

195


A BIOENERGETIC MODEL FOR ESTIMATING THE FOOD REQUIREMENTS OF<br />

THE KILLER WHALE (ORCINUS ORCA)<br />

Arliss J. Winship <strong>and</strong> Andrew W. Trites<br />

Department of Zoology <strong>and</strong> Marine Mammal Research Unit, Fisheries Center, University of British<br />

Columbia, Room 18, Hut B-3, 6248 Biological Sciences Road, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4.<br />

Telephone – (604) 822-8183, FAX – (604) 822-8180, e-mail: winship@zoology.ubc.ca<br />

INTRODUCTION<br />

Killer whales (Orcinus orca) are top predators in their ecosystems. Quantitative<br />

estimates of the amount of food consumed by killer whales are critical to underst<strong>and</strong>ing the<br />

impact of this species on populations of its prey. For example, a modeling study by Barrett-<br />

Lennard et al. (1995) suggested that predation by killer whales may be impeding the recovery<br />

of some populations of the endangered Steller sea lion (Eumetopias jubatus) in Alaska. A key<br />

parameter in their model was the amount of food that killer whales eat each day. Estimates of<br />

killer whale food consumption are also important for assessing the levels of competition<br />

between killer whales <strong>and</strong> other marine predators (including humans). However, it is difficult<br />

to quantify the food consumption of cetaceans in the wild. One technique that has commonly<br />

been used to estimate the food consumption of cetaceans is bioenergetic modeling (Hinga<br />

1979; Lockyer 1981; Markussen et al. 1992).<br />

The first objective of our study is to develop a bioenergetic model for the killer whale<br />

by adapting an existing bioenergetic model (Winship et al. 2002). The model will predict the<br />

food requirements of killer whales, but also the uncertainty in these predictions. Our second<br />

objective is to use this model to estimate the number of Steller sea lions consumed by<br />

transient killer whales in Alaska.<br />

MODEL<br />

The model is based on the principle that, on some time scale, the energy expended by an<br />

organism must equal the energy consumed by the organism. Thus, by estimating all of the<br />

energy expenditures of an organism, one can predict the amount of energy required to meet<br />

those energetic dem<strong>and</strong>s. Information about the organism’s diet can then be used to convert<br />

predicted caloric requirements to estimates of prey intake. The model’s outputs are<br />

predictions of the amounts of prey consumed by killer whales of each age <strong>and</strong> sex.<br />

The parameters in the model can be grouped into two categories: bioenergetic <strong>and</strong> diet.<br />

Bioenergetic parameters include body size, body composition, activity budgets, resting <strong>and</strong><br />

active metabolic rates, <strong>and</strong> digestive efficiency. Diet parameters include diet composition (the<br />

proportion of diet biomass comprised of each prey species), energy density of prey, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

parts of prey that are consumed. Data are available for some parameters (e.g., body size—<br />

Clark et al. 2000; metabolic rates—Kriete 1995; <strong>and</strong> activity budgets—Saulitis et al. 2000),<br />

however there are very few data currently available for other parameters. To incorporate this<br />

uncertainty in parameter values the model has a Monte Carlo r<strong>and</strong>om sampling routine that<br />

calculates (explicitly <strong>and</strong> quantitatively) the uncertainty in the model’s predictions (Fig. 1).<br />

This allows us to examine the relative impact of uncertainty in different parameters on the<br />

precision of the model’s predictions.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

196


Probability<br />

R<strong>and</strong>omly choose parameter<br />

values from specified<br />

sampling distributions<br />

Parameter value<br />

Frequency<br />

1,000,000 times<br />

Food requirements<br />

Run model to obtain<br />

point estimate of<br />

food requirements<br />

Figure 1. Monte Carlo r<strong>and</strong>om sampling technique used to estimate error in model predictions. The model<br />

output is a distribution of point estimates of food requirements (rather than a single point estimate) from which<br />

we can calculate the error in the model’s predictions.<br />

OUTCOME<br />

The predictions from our model will provide estimates of the food requirements of killer<br />

whales <strong>and</strong> the uncertainty in these estimates. These estimates can then be incorporated into<br />

predator-prey or multi-species models to examine the role of killer whales in their<br />

ecosystems. Our specific application of the model will provide an estimate of the number of<br />

Steller sea lions that are consumed by transient killer whales in Alaska.<br />

Importantly, the results of our modeling study will indicate key areas of killer whale<br />

biology that need more research to further refine estimates of killer whale prey consumption.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L. G., K. Heise, E. Saulitis, G. Ellis, <strong>and</strong> C. Matkin. 1995. The impact of<br />

killer whale predation on Steller sea lion populations in British Columbia <strong>and</strong> Alaska.<br />

North Pacific Universities Marine Mammal Research Consortium, Fisheries Center,<br />

University of British Columbia.<br />

Clark, S. T., D. K. Odell, <strong>and</strong> C. T. Lacinak. 2000. Aspects of growth in captive killer whales<br />

(Orcinus orca). Marine Mammal Science 16:110-123.<br />

Hinga, K. R. 1979. The food requirements of whales in the southern hemisphere. Deep-Sea<br />

Research A26:569-577.<br />

Kriete, B. 1995. Bioenergetics in the killer whale, Orcinus orca. Ph.D. thesis, University of<br />

British Columbia, Vancouver, 138 pp.<br />

Lockyer, C. 1981. Growth <strong>and</strong> energy budgets of large baleen whales from the southern<br />

hemisphere. Pp. 379-487 in Mammals in the seas: general papers <strong>and</strong> large cetaceans.<br />

FAO Fisheries Series 5, Rome, 4:1-504.<br />

Markussen, N. H., M. Ryg, <strong>and</strong> C. Lydersen. 1992. Food consumption of the NE Atlantic<br />

minke whale (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) population estimated with a simulation<br />

model. ICES Journal of Marine Science 49:317-323.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

197


Saulitis, E., C. Matkin, L. Barrett-Lennard, K. Heise, <strong>and</strong> G. Ellis. 2000. Foraging strategies<br />

of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William Sound,<br />

Alaska. Marine Mammal Science 16:94-109.<br />

Winship, A. J., A. W. Trites, <strong>and</strong> D. A. S. Rosen. 2002. A bioenergetic model for estimating<br />

the food requirements of Steller sea lions Eumetopias jubatus in Alaska, USA. Marine<br />

Ecology Progress Series 229:291-312.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

198


WHY DO SOME KILLER WHALES TALK SO MUCH?<br />

Harald Yurk 1, John K.B. Ford 2, Craig O. Matkin 3, Eva S. Saulitis 3,Lance G. Barrett-Lennard 4, &<br />

Kathy Heise 1<br />

1 University of British Columbia,<br />

2 Fisheries <strong>and</strong> Oceans Canada<br />

3 North Gulf Oceanic Society,<br />

4 Vancouver Aquarium Marine Science Centre<br />

Killer whales show great variety in repertoire size <strong>and</strong> vocal rates of their discrete<br />

vocalizations. In contrast to many smaller delphinids, killer whales do not show linearly<br />

increasing vocal rates with bigger group sizes. There are a number of possible reasons why<br />

vocal rates differ among killer whale groups, sub-populations <strong>and</strong> populations. Vocal rates<br />

might be adapted to the foraging on a particular prey type, (e.g. fish with limited hearing<br />

abilities versus mammal with well developed broad range hearing abilities), or vocal rates<br />

reflect long term associations among individuals, in which case a constant exchange of<br />

vocalizations to synchronize behaviour is only necessary when different groups mix. If<br />

learning <strong>and</strong> maintaining a particular repertoire of calls were costly than repertoire size would<br />

usually depend on the effects it has on breeding success. This appears to be true for some<br />

songbirds, where bigger song repertoires of male birds equal greater mating success although<br />

repertoire size might not be the selected trait but a side effect of selection on repertoire<br />

sharing (Beecher 2000).<br />

In the Northeastern Pacific, two populations of killer whales, transients <strong>and</strong> residents are<br />

strikingly different in their vocal behaviour. Transients appear to vocalize infrequently<br />

(Ford1984) <strong>and</strong> have small vocal repertoires of discrete calls. Call rate <strong>and</strong> repertoire size of<br />

transients could be seen as an adaptation to their foraging method on marine mammals (Ford<br />

1984; Deecke, pers. comm., see also chapter in this volume). Residents, which prey on fish,<br />

vocalize more frequently <strong>and</strong> also have greater repertoires of discrete calls than transients<br />

(Ford 1984). Residents are also characterized by long-term associations of matrilinealy related<br />

individuals.<br />

In the following pages we will take a closer look at the vocal behaviour of residents<br />

with regard to the following issues: 1. Call repertoire size, 2. Call structure diversity, <strong>and</strong> 3.<br />

Call rates <strong>and</strong> repertoire use. Because this manuscript is based on work-in-progress, the<br />

trends reported here should be considered as tentative.<br />

CALL REPERTOIRE SIZE<br />

The number of discrete call types that each pod or group of closely related matrilines<br />

uses ranges from 7-17. All members of the matriline use the same set of learned calls (Ford<br />

1991; Ford 1991; Miller <strong>and</strong> Bain 2000; Yurk, Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002) <strong>and</strong> the repertoire<br />

size appears to be more closely correlated with the number of matrilines within a pod than to<br />

the number of individual whales within that same group.<br />

Ford (1991) <strong>and</strong> Deecke, Ford et al. (2000) proposed that cultural drift by which<br />

accumulated copying errors change the structure of call types is the main mechanism to<br />

explain repertoire divergence. However, this process does not explain rapid changes in<br />

repertoire size after group-fission (Yurk, Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002), <strong>and</strong> the lack of gradual<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

199


change in call structure for call types that are more complex (Deecke, Ford et al. 2000; Yurk,<br />

Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002). (Yurk, Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002) proposed instead that<br />

repertoire differences could be achieved by selectively dropping calls <strong>and</strong> changing the<br />

frequency of others during matriline fission, while similarity is maintained by keeping the<br />

majority of calls stable, <strong>and</strong> only allowing certain call structures to change gradually over<br />

time. This process requires that the call structure of discrete calls is hierarchical (Ford 1984;<br />

Yurk, Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002), <strong>and</strong> that the process of call development is based on the<br />

transmission of structural parts rather than whole calls.<br />

CALL STRUCTURE DIVERSITY<br />

Fig. 1: Spectrographic example of a discrete call. Calls often consist of two components, a) a lower frequency component –<br />

LFC (duration : 0.5 to 2 sec/ pulse repetition rate: 0.2 to 2.5 kHz), <strong>and</strong> b) an upper frequency component – UFC (duration:<br />

0.5 –2.5 sec/ frequency range: 4 to 8 kHz) . Abrupt shifts of the pulse frequency in the lower frequency component<br />

distinguish elements. Elements differ from call segments that are characterized by silent intervals between them.<br />

The structure of call types is divided hierarchically into parts (Fig.1) (Ford 1984). Yurk,<br />

Barrett-Lennard et al. (2002) named different parts according to frequency contour shifts <strong>and</strong><br />

temporal discontinuities as components, segments, <strong>and</strong> elements. The call organization<br />

appears to be preserved in the process of call transmission between generations. Groups of<br />

matrilineal related residents in Alaska show structural integrity although each part differs<br />

between groups (Fig. 2).<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

200


Figure2: Spectrographic examples of a shared call type of four resident pods in Southern Alaska. The structural similarity<br />

decreases from left to right mainly within one element, while all segments <strong>and</strong> elements are present in all four calls.<br />

Repertoire similarity also decreases from left to right between the four pods.<br />

The structural variation of the same call type is more prominent within segments <strong>and</strong><br />

elements of the lower frequency component of the calls, while the upper frequency<br />

components appear to be less affected by transmission related changes. This reduced variation<br />

of the upper frequency components could imply a selected functional purpose of those<br />

components, e.g. to coordinate group movements because of a greater directionality of this<br />

component as proposed by Miller (2002).<br />

When frequency contours of calls are compared between different clans that do not<br />

share complete call types, some elements <strong>and</strong> segments still appear to be similar. Even<br />

between sub-populations those similarities persist although they are more obvious in call<br />

segments than in call elements. Therefore, it might be better to define call types on the basis<br />

of the frequency contours of their structural parts rather than complete contours, <strong>and</strong> compare<br />

elements <strong>and</strong> segments between clans <strong>and</strong> sub-populations to determine acoustic<br />

relationships.<br />

CALL RATES AND REPERTOIRE USAGE<br />

Call repertoires, or dialects (Ford 2002), are cultural indicators of relatedness (Yurk,<br />

Barrett-Lennard et al. 2002), <strong>and</strong> could function as an inbreeding avoidance mechanism if<br />

used in mate choice (Ford 1991; Barrett-Lennard 2000). Because repertoire size varies among<br />

groups <strong>and</strong> is correlated with the number of matrilines in a dialect group or clan, repertoire<br />

acquisition <strong>and</strong> maintenance could be a true signal of rank <strong>and</strong> success of a clan. Repertoire<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

201


use <strong>and</strong> call rate should reflect such a ranking system during vocal exchanges between<br />

members of different clans <strong>and</strong>/or by mediating access to rich food sources.<br />

There is some evidence for such an acoustically mediated ranking system among<br />

Alaskan residents. During the summer months residents usually prey on returning salmon in<br />

inlets or passages that create funnels for returning fish. Members of the repertoire rich ABclan<br />

prefer foraging in a passage that usually contains higher prey densities than most of the<br />

surrounding areas. Members of this clan appear to consistently use a greater proportion of<br />

their repertoire even when by themselves (Fig. 3a), but especially when in contact with<br />

members of the AD-clan (Fig. 3b), the other clan within the Southern Alaskan resident<br />

community. However, some members of the AD-clan, the AE matrilines frequent an adjacent<br />

area to the one mentioned above, <strong>and</strong> are highly vocal in the presence of AB-clan whales.<br />

Members of the AB matrilines appear to use fewer call types than when alone <strong>and</strong> call less<br />

frequently when in direct contact with AE matrilines (Fig.3c).<br />

A)<br />

30.000<br />

25.000<br />

20.000<br />

15.000<br />

10.000<br />

5.000<br />

0.000<br />

l<br />

AB<br />

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8<br />

18.000<br />

16.000<br />

14.000<br />

12.000<br />

10.000<br />

8.000<br />

6.000<br />

4.000<br />

2.000<br />

0.000<br />

AB<br />

B)<br />

30.000<br />

25.000<br />

20.000<br />

15.000<br />

10.000<br />

5.000<br />

0.000<br />

AB w. AD<br />

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1<br />

AB with AD matrilines<br />

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7<br />

C)<br />

Figure 3: Calling behaviour of AB matrilines during encounters with matrilines from another clan or when alone.<br />

Proportional repertoire usage on x-Axis versus call rate on y-Axis. A) AB matrilines alone (14 encounters), B) AB matrilines<br />

in contact with AD clan whales (7 encounters), C) Changes of repertoire use <strong>and</strong> call rate during encounters of AB matrilines<br />

with members of AD clan (7 encounters).<br />

Repertoire size, use <strong>and</strong> call rate might be a cultural means by which resident killer<br />

whales mediate access to food sources. Access to food sources will ultimately affect the<br />

survival of clans.<br />

LITERATURE CITED<br />

Barrett-Lennard, L. G. (2000). Population structure <strong>and</strong> mating patterns of killer<br />

whales, Orcinus orca, as revealed by DNA analysis. Zoology. Vancouver, BC,<br />

Canada, University of British Columbia.<br />

Beecher, M. D. (2000). "Territory tenure in song sparrows is related to song sharing with<br />

neighbours, but not to repertoire size." Animal Behaviour 59(1): 29-37.<br />

AE<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

202


Deecke, V. B., J. K. B. Ford, et al. (2000). "Dialect change in resident killer whales:<br />

implications for vocal learning <strong>and</strong> cultural transmission." Animal Behaviour 60(5):<br />

629-638.<br />

Ford, J. K. B. (1984). Call traditions <strong>and</strong> dialects of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in British<br />

Columbia. Zoology. Vancouver, University of British Columbia.<br />

Ford, J. K. B. (1991). "Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal<br />

waters of British Columbia." Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 1454-1483.<br />

Ford, J. K. B. (2002). Dialects. The Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. W. F. Perrin, B.<br />

Wursig <strong>and</strong> H. G. M. Thewissen. New York, Academic Press.<br />

Miller, P. J. O. (2002). "Mixed-directionality of killer whale stereotyped calls: a direction of<br />

movement cue?" Behavioral Ecology <strong>and</strong> Sociobiology 52(3): 262-270.<br />

Miller, P. J. O. <strong>and</strong> D. E. Bain (2000). "Within-pod variation in the sound production of a<br />

pod of killer whales, Orcinus orca." Animal Behaviour 60(5): 617-628.<br />

Yurk, H., L. G. Barrett-Lennard, et al. (2002). "Cultural transmission within maternal<br />

lineages: vocal clans in resident killer whales in southern Alaska." Animal Behaviour<br />

63(6): 1103-1119.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

203


Index of Authors<br />

Azevedo, 47<br />

Bain, 146, 168<br />

Baird, 86<br />

Balcomb, 181<br />

Barbraud, 124<br />

Barrett Lennard, 98<br />

Barrett-Lennard, 27, 33, 58, 83, 89, 129,<br />

202, 206<br />

Bassoi, 51<br />

Batty, 66<br />

Bell, 112<br />

Bester, 36<br />

Black, 41, 86, 159<br />

Bradley, 92<br />

Burdin, 67, 95, 109<br />

Chambellant, 112<br />

Cloutier, 77<br />

Dahlheim, 41, 46, 86<br />

Dalla Rosa, 47, 51<br />

Damsgård, 55<br />

Danilewicz, 51<br />

de Stephanis, 141<br />

Dedeluk, 58<br />

Deecke, 60<br />

DeLeuw, 46<br />

Delfour, 65<br />

DeMaster, 83<br />

Desrosiers, 77<br />

Domenici, 66<br />

Ellifrit, 46<br />

Ellis, 31, 41, 70, 74, 98, 105, 120, 129,<br />

132, 134, 149<br />

Filatova, 67, 109<br />

Flores, 51<br />

Flores de Sahagún, 78<br />

Foote, 71<br />

Ford, 58, 60, 74, 89, 129, 163, 168, 202<br />

Gasparrou, 87, 88<br />

Gendron, 78<br />

Gill, 112<br />

Gillham, 70<br />

Godefroid, 77<br />

Guerrero-Ruiz, 78<br />

Guinet, 124, 141<br />

Hammond, 79, 195<br />

Hanson, 92, 137<br />

Harvey, 70<br />

Heise, 83, 98, 202<br />

Hoelzel, 46, 86<br />

Holst, 92, 137<br />

Hoyt, 67, 109<br />

Iñíguez, 87, 88<br />

Irish, 89<br />

Jikiya, 109, 154<br />

Jones, 71<br />

Keith, 36<br />

Kriete, 91<br />

Ladich, 193<br />

Lailson-Brito, 47<br />

Leyssen, 55, 92<br />

Maldini, 95<br />

Mangin, 124<br />

Maniscalco, 95<br />

Martell, 83<br />

Matkin C., 98, 120, 134, 202<br />

Matkin D., 101<br />

Matkin, C., 83<br />

Michaud, 77<br />

Miller, 108, 136, 139<br />

Mironova, 67, 109<br />

Moreno, 51<br />

Morrice, 112<br />

Natoli, 86<br />

Nicholson, 86<br />

Nikulin, 109<br />

Øien, 92, 137<br />

Olavarria, 86<br />

Olesiuk, 98, 120<br />

Osbourne, 71<br />

Pakenham, 123<br />

Paton, 112<br />

Pavlov, 109<br />

Pérez Gimeno, 141<br />

Peter, 120<br />

Pistorius, 36<br />

Plater, 155<br />

Poncelet, 124, 141<br />

Rehn, 163<br />

Riesch, 163<br />

Ross, 129<br />

Salazar Sierra, 141<br />

Santos, 51<br />

Sato, 67, 109<br />

Saulitis, 83, 98, 120, 134, 202<br />

Sautilis, 101<br />

Schulman-Janiger, 41<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

204


Secchi, 47<br />

Shapiro, 136<br />

Sigurjonsson, 185<br />

Similä, 55, 66, 92, 137, 175, 185<br />

Simon, 139, 140<br />

Slater, 60<br />

Smith, 146<br />

Solow, 136<br />

Stap, 41<br />

Straley, 149<br />

Tarasyan, 67, 109, 154<br />

Taylor, 70, 155<br />

Teichert, 163<br />

Ternullo, 41, 159<br />

Thiele, 112<br />

Thomsen, 163<br />

Tossenberger, 87, 88<br />

Trites, 83, 89, 168, 199<br />

Tyack, 136<br />

Ugarte, 139, 140, 171, 177<br />

Urbán-R, 78<br />

van den Hoff, 112<br />

van Ginneken, 181<br />

Veirs, 71<br />

Vikingsson, 185<br />

Visser, 186, 189, 192<br />

Wahlberg, 139<br />

Weiss, 193<br />

Williams, 71, 168, 195<br />

Winship, 199<br />

Yurk, 98, 202<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

208


REMERCIEMENTS<br />

Le quatrième symposium international et groupes de travail sur les orques<br />

s’est déroulé au Centre d’Études Biologiques de Chizé du lundi 23 septembre au<br />

samedi 28. Le quatrième symposium s’intégrait dans le cadre du programme des<br />

colloques intitulés "Les entretiens de Chizé".<br />

Ces rencontres ont donné l’opportunité à 70 chercheurs et étudiants de se<br />

rencontrer, d’échanger des informations et de mettre en place des collaborations.<br />

Nous avons rassemblé les personnes qui travaillent sur toutes les études en cours<br />

dans le monde. Il y a eu 21 pays représentés et plusieurs études dans de nouveaux<br />

sites ont été présentées lors du colloque : notamment au Kamtchatka, Brésil,<br />

Nouvelle Guinée ou le détroit de Gibraltar. Plusieurs problèmes de conservation ont<br />

été abordés lors de ce colloque, interaction avec les pêcheries, pollution…<br />

Ce symposium et les groupes de travail qui ont suivi n’ont pu être organisés<br />

que grâce au soutient de l’ensemble du personnel du <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong> qui se sont<br />

considérablement investis pour accueillir, héberger et restaurer les participants. Nous<br />

souhaitons aussi remercier vivement Karine Delord pour le travail remarquable<br />

effectué pour l’organisation et la gestion de ce colloque, Renaud de Stephanis pour<br />

l’important travail de mise en page de ces proceedings.<br />

Ces réunions n’auraient pas pu avoir lieu sans le soutient important apporté<br />

par le programme Com’Science du Conseil Régional du Poitou-Charentes, du<br />

Conseil Général des Deux-Sèvres et du Marinel<strong>and</strong> d’Antibes.<br />

Enfin, nous remercions l’ensemble des participants pour la qualité de leur<br />

contribution. C’était un plaisir de partager ces moments avec vous.<br />

Merci à vous tous<br />

Le comité organisateur.<br />

FOURTH INTERNATIONAL ORCA SYMPOSIUM AND WORKSHOPS<br />

SEPTEMBER 23-28 2002, <strong>CEBC</strong>-<strong>CNRS</strong>, France<br />

209

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!