Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...
Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...
Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...
You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles
YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.
Developmental Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong><br />
2006, Vol. 42, No. 1, 153–163 0012-1649/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.153<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>Influence</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>: <strong>The</strong> <strong>Role</strong> <strong>of</strong> Motivati<strong>on</strong>al Factors<br />
and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Characteristics<br />
Carolyn McNamara Barry<br />
Loyola College in Maryland<br />
Kathryn R. Wentzel<br />
University <strong>of</strong> Maryland<br />
This study examined motivati<strong>on</strong> (prosocial goals), individual characteristics (sex, ethnicity, and grade),<br />
and friendship characteristics (affective quality, interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, and friendship stability) in relati<strong>on</strong><br />
to middle adolescents’ prosocial behavior over time. Ninth- and 10th-grade students (N 208) attending<br />
a suburban, mid-Atlantic public high school and having at least 1 reciprocated friendship were followed<br />
for 1 year. Hierarchical regressi<strong>on</strong> analyses revealed that a friend’s behavior is related to an individual’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit, which in turn, is related to an individual’s prosocial behavior. Further, the<br />
affective quality <strong>of</strong> a friendship and the frequency with which friends interact moderate relati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior to an individual’s prosocial goal pursuit.<br />
Keywords: friendship, prosocial behavior, motivati<strong>on</strong>, friendship quality, adolescence<br />
<strong>The</strong>ory as well as research supports the noti<strong>on</strong> that friends can<br />
be important socializers <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior. According to Piaget<br />
(1932/1965), friendships are more egalitarian than any other type<br />
<strong>of</strong> peer relati<strong>on</strong>ship or the parent–child relati<strong>on</strong>ship. <strong>The</strong>refore,<br />
friendships afford a unique c<strong>on</strong>text for interacti<strong>on</strong> and collaborati<strong>on</strong><br />
wherein children socially c<strong>on</strong>struct their morality <strong>of</strong> interpers<strong>on</strong>al<br />
resp<strong>on</strong>sibility and mutual c<strong>on</strong>cern (e.g., Corsaro & Eder,<br />
1990; Dam<strong>on</strong>, 1988; Youniss, 1981). Others (Berndt, 1999; Hartup<br />
& Stevens, 1997) have argued that friends can have motivati<strong>on</strong>al<br />
significance by modeling positive forms <strong>of</strong> social behavior; as<br />
friends interact with and observe each other, change in their<br />
behavior can be explained by characteristics <strong>of</strong> the friendship.<br />
Research provides some support for these theoretical asserti<strong>on</strong>s in<br />
that, compared with acquaintances, friends engage in more frequent<br />
and positive social interacti<strong>on</strong>s that facilitate the development<br />
<strong>of</strong> moral acti<strong>on</strong>s (Azmitia & M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, 1993; Dam<strong>on</strong> &<br />
Killen, 1982; Nels<strong>on</strong> & Aboud, 1985). In additi<strong>on</strong>, children who<br />
observe prosocial behavior <strong>of</strong> peers are likely to behave similarly<br />
(Bryan & Walbek, 1970; Elliott & Vasta, 1970; Hartup & Coates,<br />
1967), in part because friends tend to reinforce each other for<br />
behavior that is like their own (Hartup, 1983).<br />
Of interest in the present study is the fact that few researchers<br />
have examined the positive influence <strong>of</strong> friends <strong>on</strong> social devel-<br />
Carolyn McNamara Barry, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Loyola College<br />
in Maryland; Kathryn R. Wentzel, Department <strong>of</strong> Human Development,<br />
University <strong>of</strong> Maryland.<br />
<strong>The</strong> first wave <strong>of</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> was used for Carolyn McNamara<br />
Barry’s fulfillment <strong>of</strong> the doctoral dissertati<strong>on</strong> at the University <strong>of</strong> Maryland.<br />
Porti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> this study were presented at the biennial meetings <strong>of</strong> the<br />
Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, Minnesota,<br />
April 2001, and Tampa, Florida, April 2003. We extend our gratitude for<br />
a research grant from the Department <strong>of</strong> Human Development, University<br />
<strong>of</strong> Maryland, given to Carolyn McNamara Barry.<br />
Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be addressed to Carolyn<br />
McNamara Barry, Department <strong>of</strong> Psychology, Loyola College in Maryland,<br />
4501 North Charles Street, Baltimore, MD 21210-2699. E-mail:<br />
cbarry@loyola.edu<br />
153<br />
opment, especially prosocial behavior during adolescence. Much<br />
more is known about how peers influence <strong>on</strong>e another to engage in<br />
various forms <strong>of</strong> risk-taking behaviors (Pleyd<strong>on</strong> & Schner, 2001;<br />
Prinstein, Meade, & Cohen, 2003). In additi<strong>on</strong>, studies <strong>of</strong> positive<br />
friend influence have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted primarily with younger<br />
children. However, adolescents spend increasingly more time with<br />
their friends (Crockett, Los<strong>of</strong>f, & Petersen, 1984) and are more<br />
dependent <strong>on</strong> their peers than are younger children (Lars<strong>on</strong> &<br />
Richards, 1991). In light <strong>of</strong> these issues, the focus <strong>of</strong> the present<br />
study was <strong>on</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>s between friends’ and individuals’ prosocial<br />
behavior as adolescents make their way through high school.<br />
Toward this end, we examined individuals’ pursuit <strong>of</strong> goals to<br />
behave prosocially as a motivati<strong>on</strong>al process that links their percepti<strong>on</strong><br />
<strong>of</strong> a friend’s prosocial behavior and their own prosocial<br />
behavior (as assessed by the collective peer group). In additi<strong>on</strong>, we<br />
studied affective quality, interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, and stability<br />
am<strong>on</strong>g friends as characteristics that might explain significant<br />
relati<strong>on</strong>s between an individual’s percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior and an individual’s prosocial goal pursuit.<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships and <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong><br />
Although the research is limited, significant relati<strong>on</strong>s between<br />
friendships and prosocial behavior have been documented. First,<br />
friends are similar in the degree to which they display prosocial<br />
behavior and are motivated to do so (Wentzel, Barry, & Caldwell,<br />
2004; Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). In additi<strong>on</strong>, prosocial behavior<br />
occurs more frequently between friends than between peers who<br />
are not friends (Berndt, 1985; Berndt, Hawkins, & Hoyle, 1986).<br />
Young adolescents who have friends are more likely to be prosocial<br />
than are those without friends (McGuire & Weisz, 1982).<br />
Also, similarity <strong>of</strong> levels <strong>of</strong> prosocial intenti<strong>on</strong>s and prosocial<br />
behavior between friends increases over time (Berndt, 1981a).<br />
Finally, not <strong>on</strong>ly is prosocial behavior more comm<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g<br />
friends than am<strong>on</strong>g n<strong>on</strong>friends, but children c<strong>on</strong>ceptualize friendships<br />
as involving prosocial interacti<strong>on</strong>s such as sharing and cooperating<br />
(Berndt, 1981b; Bigelow & LaGaipa, 1980).
154<br />
It is likely that these significant relati<strong>on</strong>s partly reflect the fact<br />
that adolescents who dem<strong>on</strong>strate <strong>on</strong>e type <strong>of</strong> social competence<br />
(i.e., making friends) <strong>of</strong>ten display other types as well. Of additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />
interest, however, is whether these relati<strong>on</strong>s also reflect a<br />
process <strong>of</strong> friend influence (Berndt, 1999; Hartup, 1996). As noted<br />
by Hartup and Stevens (1997), theoretical models to explain the<br />
process <strong>of</strong> friend influence are scant. However, some scholars<br />
(e.g., Berndt, 1999; Wentzel et al., 2004) have adopted a sociallearning<br />
perspective to explain this process. For instance, individuals<br />
might develop specific behavioral styles or interests because<br />
these are c<strong>on</strong>sidered to be desirable characteristics <strong>of</strong> their close<br />
friends (Bukowski & Hoza, 1989; Hartup, 1996; Hartup &<br />
Stevens, 1997). Specifically, the tenets <strong>of</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning<br />
(e.g., Bandura, 1986) support the noti<strong>on</strong> that if a friend models<br />
certain types <strong>of</strong> behavior, an individual is likely to behave like the<br />
friend subsequently, because exposure to modeled behavior is<br />
frequent and affectively charged.<br />
Adolescents report greater frequency in observing their friends’<br />
behavior than their n<strong>on</strong>friends’ behavior (Crockett et al., 1984).<br />
Moreover, friendships typically are characterized by str<strong>on</strong>g emoti<strong>on</strong>al<br />
b<strong>on</strong>ds, thereby increasing the likelihood that friends will<br />
imitate each other’s behavior (Berndt & Perry, 1986). Given the<br />
str<strong>on</strong>g emoti<strong>on</strong>al b<strong>on</strong>ds between friends (Berndt & Perry, 1986),<br />
especially during adolescence, it is plausible that friends might<br />
mimic each other’s behavior. <strong>The</strong>refore, friend influence by way<br />
<strong>of</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning is most likely to occur when (a) the<br />
affective quality <strong>of</strong> a friendship is high, such that a friend provides<br />
an important source <strong>of</strong> nurturance to the individual, and (b) the<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency and friendship stability between an individual<br />
and a friend is high, such that they spend a significant amount<br />
<strong>of</strong> time together.<br />
Bandura (1986) further argued that although behavior can be<br />
learned by observing others, it is likely to be enacted to the extent<br />
that an individual is motivated to do so. <strong>The</strong>refore, an individual’s<br />
behavior might become more similar to a friend’s behavior because<br />
<strong>of</strong> a change in underlying motivati<strong>on</strong>al processes, such as<br />
goals and values. In the current study, therefore, we predicted that<br />
the affective quality, interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, and stability <strong>of</strong> a<br />
friendship would moderate the relati<strong>on</strong> between a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior and an individual’s goal pursuit. In turn, an individual’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit was predicted to relate to an individual’s<br />
prosocial behavior, thereby providing a pathway between<br />
friends’ and individuals’ prosocial behavior. A prosocial goal was<br />
defined as a cognitive representati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the extent to which an<br />
individual would like to display prosocial behavior in a given<br />
situati<strong>on</strong> (Ford, 1992; Wentzel, 1994); goal pursuit marks the<br />
frequency with which individuals see themselves as trying to<br />
achieve the goal. Pers<strong>on</strong>al goals are <strong>on</strong>e comp<strong>on</strong>ent <strong>of</strong> a larger<br />
system <strong>of</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> that includes the directi<strong>on</strong> (pers<strong>on</strong>al goals),<br />
energizati<strong>on</strong> (emoti<strong>on</strong>al arousal), and regulati<strong>on</strong> (pers<strong>on</strong>al agency)<br />
<strong>of</strong> behavior (see Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998; Ford, 1992).<br />
In summary, although the similarity <strong>of</strong> friends’ behavior has<br />
been noted for some time (Kandel, 1978a, 1978b), the process by<br />
which friends influence <strong>on</strong>e another is less well understood. In the<br />
current study, we examined how friendship stability and the affective<br />
quality and interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency between friends increased<br />
the likelihood that a friend’s prosocial behavior would motivate an<br />
individual to behave prosocially. In turn, motivati<strong>on</strong> to behave<br />
prosocially was posited to be related directly to displays <strong>of</strong> proso-<br />
BARRY AND WENTZEL<br />
cial behavior (e.g., Wentzel, 1994), as shown in Figure 1. According<br />
to Kandel (1978a), the similar behavior exhibited by friends is<br />
likely the result <strong>of</strong> two distinct processes: selecti<strong>on</strong> and influence.<br />
Similarity am<strong>on</strong>g friends explains, in part, why individuals become<br />
friends (Hallinan & Williams, 1990); however, friends’<br />
characteristics explain change in adolescents’ behavior over time<br />
(Berndt & Keefe, 1995). Research designs typically have c<strong>on</strong>founded<br />
selecti<strong>on</strong> with influence by not accounting for the extent<br />
to which two friends are similar at the point <strong>of</strong> friendship formati<strong>on</strong>.<br />
<strong>The</strong>refore, to study influence, <strong>on</strong>e must c<strong>on</strong>trol for selecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />
In the current l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study, we did this by examining the<br />
extent to which friends’ behavior at Time 1 predicted individuals’<br />
behavior at Time 2 while taking into account individuals’ behavior<br />
at Time 1.<br />
Research Questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />
In the current study, we examined the processes by which a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior is related to change in an individual’s<br />
prosocial behavior over 1 year. Two specific questi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />
addressed:<br />
1. Does prosocial goal pursuit provide a pathway that links a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior and an individual’s (i.e., target’s)<br />
prosocial behavior? Target’s prosocial goal pursuit was believed to<br />
explain the associati<strong>on</strong> between a friend’s prosocial behavior at<br />
Time 1 and change in target’s prosocial behavior over 1 year.<br />
2. To what extent do friendship characteristics <strong>of</strong> affective<br />
quality, interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, and friendship stability moderate<br />
the relati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a friend’s prosocial behavior to an individual’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit? <strong>The</strong>se relati<strong>on</strong>s (depicted in Figure 1) were<br />
examined while taking into account sex, grade, and ethnicity;<br />
research has documented that prosocial behavior varies as a functi<strong>on</strong><br />
<strong>of</strong> these variables (Carlo, Hausmann, Christiansen, & Randall,<br />
2003; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Fabes & Eisenberg, 1996 [as cited<br />
in Fabes, Carlo, Kupan<strong>of</strong>f, & Laible, 1999]).<br />
Participants<br />
Method<br />
Students attending a suburban public high school in the New York<br />
metropolitan area participated in the 2-year l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal study. <strong>The</strong> median<br />
annual household incomes for the two towns that comprised the high<br />
school student populati<strong>on</strong> were $106,208 and $101,991 (U.S. Bureau <strong>of</strong> the<br />
Census, 2000). <strong>The</strong> Time 1 sample c<strong>on</strong>sisted <strong>of</strong> 287 9th-grade (n 145)<br />
and 10th-grade (n 142) students, and the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />
<strong>of</strong> 265 9th-grade (n 134) and 10th-grade (n 131) students. Data were<br />
collected in the spring <strong>of</strong> each c<strong>on</strong>secutive year. All students participated<br />
unless parental permissi<strong>on</strong> was denied (n 26), the students were absent<br />
Figure 1. Proposed c<strong>on</strong>ceptual model <strong>of</strong> friend influence.
<strong>on</strong> the day <strong>of</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> (n 24), or the students themselves chose not<br />
to participate <strong>on</strong> the day <strong>of</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> (n 1). At both time points, the<br />
demographic characteristics <strong>of</strong> the sample were as follows: 58% female,<br />
42% male, 90% Caucasian, 6% Asian, 2% Hispanic, and 1% members <strong>of</strong><br />
other minority groups. Students who did not participate in the sec<strong>on</strong>d data<br />
collecti<strong>on</strong> because <strong>of</strong> attriti<strong>on</strong> or absence (n 21) did not differ significantly<br />
from those in the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample with respect to demographic<br />
characteristics or study variables.<br />
Procedure<br />
At each <strong>of</strong> the two data collecti<strong>on</strong>s, C.M.B. collected data from all<br />
participating 9th-grade and 10th-grade students in their 43-min social<br />
studies classes (18 classes for Time 1, 17 classes for Time 2) in the spring<br />
<strong>of</strong> each year. Data collecti<strong>on</strong> that occurs later in the school year is<br />
advantageous, because students’ familiarity with their peers is maximized.<br />
Students were assured c<strong>on</strong>fidentiality for all resp<strong>on</strong>ses and informed that<br />
they did not have to answer any questi<strong>on</strong> that they did not want to answer.<br />
<strong>The</strong> researcher remained in the classroom and clarified instructi<strong>on</strong>s as<br />
needed. Classroom teachers (N 6 at Time 1 and Time 2) remained in the<br />
classroom throughout the data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />
Measures<br />
All measures were administered at both time points.<br />
Background informati<strong>on</strong>. Students were asked to complete a demographic<br />
questi<strong>on</strong>naire that specified their sex (0 male, 1 female),<br />
grade level (9 or 10 for Time 1, 10 or 11 for Time 2), and ethnicity (1 <br />
Caucasian, 2 African American, 3 Asian, 4 Hispanic, 5 <br />
American Indian, 6 other).<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship. Students were instructed to write the first and last names <strong>of</strong><br />
their three best friends from their high school; however, they were informed<br />
that they could choose fewer names if they so desired. Only same-sex friendships<br />
were investigated in the current study because these capture the majority<br />
<strong>of</strong> best friendships for adolescents (Hartup, 1992), and they differ in quality<br />
from opposite-sex friendships (Sharabany, Gersh<strong>on</strong>i, & H<strong>of</strong>man, 1981). Samegrade<br />
friendships were examined to maximize the interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency in<br />
these school-based relati<strong>on</strong>ships. From the names provided, reciprocated<br />
friendships were determined by matching students’ school-based best friend<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong>s with each other. At Time 1, 21% <strong>of</strong> the students had no reciprocated<br />
friendships, 34% had <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated friendship, 30% had two reciprocated<br />
friendships, and 15% had three reciprocated friendships. At Time 2,<br />
16% <strong>of</strong> the students had no reciprocated friendships, 31% had <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated<br />
friendship, 36% had two reciprocated friendships, and 17% had three reciprocated<br />
friendships. Data from students who had at least <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated<br />
friendship were used in all analyses (n 228, Time 1; n 221, Time 2); cases<br />
in which nominated best friendships were not reciprocated (n 59, Time 1;<br />
n 44, Time 2) were disregarded.<br />
All data corresp<strong>on</strong>ding to a target individual’s reciprocated friends were<br />
obtained from either the sole reciprocated friendship or a randomly selected<br />
reciprocated friend (if the target had more than <strong>on</strong>e). For all analyses, listwise<br />
deleti<strong>on</strong> was used, resulting in a final l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample <strong>of</strong> 208. Am<strong>on</strong>g the<br />
original sample <strong>of</strong> ethnic minority students at Time 1, 42% did not have a<br />
reciprocated friendship. Am<strong>on</strong>g ethnic minority students with a reciprocated<br />
friendship, <strong>on</strong>ly 4 (i.e., 29%) had a same-race friendship, and all <strong>of</strong> these were<br />
between Asian Americans. <strong>The</strong> remaining 71% <strong>of</strong> the reciprocated friendships<br />
am<strong>on</strong>g ethnic minorities were cross-race, with most (50%) <strong>of</strong> those friendships<br />
between Asian American and Caucasian students.<br />
Affective quality. Two measures were used to assess affective quality:<br />
friendship closeness and friendship importance. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship closeness was<br />
assessed with the Affective B<strong>on</strong>d and Reflected Appraisal subscales <strong>of</strong> the<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Qualities Scale (Bukowski, Hoza, & Boivin, 1994). Students<br />
were asked to indicate how true five statements were about each <strong>of</strong> their<br />
nominated friendships <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (not true) to5(really true). Because<br />
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />
students could nominate up to three friends, three copies <strong>of</strong> this questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />
were provided to each student. Students were asked to write the name<br />
<strong>of</strong> their respective friend <strong>on</strong> a line labeled <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #2, or<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />
#3; each item referred to <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1, #2, or #3, respectively. A sample item<br />
from the Affective B<strong>on</strong>d subscale is “I feel happy when I am with <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />
#1,” and a sample item from the Reflected Appraisal subscale is “When I<br />
do a good job at something, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 is happy for me.” <strong>The</strong> five scores<br />
for each friend were averaged to form a single closeness score; <strong>on</strong>ly Time<br />
1 scores were used for analyses (M 4.00, SD 0.75, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s <br />
.85). According to Bukowski et al. (1994), reciprocated friendships have<br />
higher closeness scores than do unreciprocated friendships, and stable<br />
friendships over a 6-m<strong>on</strong>th period have been associated with significantly<br />
higher ( p .05) closeness scores than have unstable friendships.<br />
Affective quality was also assessed by a target individual’s perceived<br />
importance <strong>of</strong> a friendship. After students indicated the names <strong>of</strong> their three<br />
school-based best friends, they were instructed to rate each friendship <strong>on</strong> a<br />
scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (important) to3(extremely important); <strong>on</strong>ly Time 1 scores were<br />
used for analyses (M 2.48, SD 0.65). <strong>The</strong> two indicators <strong>of</strong> affective<br />
quality were related significantly (r .55, p .001), and thus, scores were<br />
averaged to form a composite (M 3.24, SD 0.62).<br />
Interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency. Students completed the Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency<br />
subscale <strong>of</strong> Berndt and Keefe’s friendship features questi<strong>on</strong>naire (T. J.<br />
Berndt, pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>, October 1997). Students were asked to<br />
indicate how frequently they interacted with their friend by rating six items<br />
(see the Appendix) <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (never) to5(every day). (With Berndt’s<br />
permissi<strong>on</strong>, we made a minor modificati<strong>on</strong> to the wording <strong>of</strong> the scale such<br />
that “this friend” was replaced with “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1,” “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #2,” or “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />
#3” so as to remind the students <strong>of</strong> the particular friendship about which<br />
they were answering questi<strong>on</strong>s.) <strong>The</strong> six scores for each friend were then<br />
averaged to form a single interacti<strong>on</strong>-frequency score; <strong>on</strong>ly Time 1 scores<br />
were used for analyses (M 3.44, SD 0.70, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .78).<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship stability. As noted above, the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />
<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>of</strong> individuals with a reciprocated friend at Time 1. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship-stability<br />
scores reflected whether the target maintained a reciprocated friendship over<br />
time. If target’s reciprocated friendship at Time 1 was also reciprocated at<br />
Time 2, then friendship stability was coded as 3; if target’s reciprocated<br />
friendship at Time 1 was unreciprocated (but menti<strong>on</strong>ed) at Time 2, friendship<br />
stability was coded as 2; if target’s reciprocated friendship at Time 1 was not<br />
menti<strong>on</strong>ed as a friend at Time 2, then friendship stability was coded as 1. On<br />
the basis <strong>of</strong> the sample <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly those with reciprocated friendships at Time 1,<br />
the frequencies for friendship stability were as follows: 31.8% for 1, 9.8% for<br />
2, and 58.4% for 3.<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> goal pursuit. Students were asked to complete the <strong>Prosocial</strong><br />
Goal Pursuit scale (Wentzel, 1991), which c<strong>on</strong>sists <strong>of</strong> academic (e.g.,<br />
“How <strong>of</strong>ten do you try to share what you’ve learned with your classmates?”)<br />
and social (e.g., “How <strong>of</strong>ten do you try to cheer some<strong>on</strong>e up when<br />
something has g<strong>on</strong>e wr<strong>on</strong>g?”) subscales. Students rated <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1<br />
(rarely) to5(almost always) how <strong>of</strong>ten they tried to accomplish various<br />
prosocial behaviors. <strong>The</strong> two subscales were correlated significantly (r <br />
.46, p .001), and thus, scores were averaged to form a composite (M <br />
3.78, SD 0.56, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .75).<br />
Target’s prosocial behavior. Peer nominati<strong>on</strong>s were used to assess<br />
target’s prosocial behavior (see Wentzel, 1994). Students were given three<br />
sheets <strong>of</strong> paper, with a different behavioral descripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> each, al<strong>on</strong>g with<br />
a list <strong>of</strong> 15 randomly generated names <strong>of</strong> same-sex students from their<br />
grade. Lists <strong>of</strong> names were randomly generated to obtain independent<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong>s for each behavior, to gain the perspective <strong>of</strong> the peer group at<br />
large, and to minimize the amount <strong>of</strong> time necessary for data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />
<strong>The</strong> three questi<strong>on</strong>s were as follows:<br />
1. Who cooperates and shares?<br />
2. Who helps other kids when they have a problem?<br />
3. Who shares things with you when you need something?<br />
155
156<br />
Each student’s name appeared <strong>on</strong> other students’ lists approximately 12<br />
times for each questi<strong>on</strong>. Students were asked to circle the names <strong>of</strong><br />
classmates who fit each behavioral descripti<strong>on</strong> and to cross out all names<br />
<strong>of</strong> classmates whom they did not know. Scoring c<strong>on</strong>sisted <strong>of</strong> computing a<br />
ratio <strong>of</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> nominati<strong>on</strong>s a student received divided by the<br />
number <strong>of</strong> times the student’s name appeared minus the number <strong>of</strong> times<br />
the student was not known by classmates. Because these items were<br />
correlated significantly (for Time 1, r .60 for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 1 and 2, r .62<br />
for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 1 and 3, and r .56 for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 2 and 3; for Time 2, r <br />
.58 for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 1 and 2, r .65 for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 1 and 3, and r .64<br />
for Questi<strong>on</strong>s 2 and 3; for all six correlati<strong>on</strong>s, p .001), the three<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong> scores were averaged to form a prosocial behavior composite<br />
at each time point.<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial behavior. Each participant was asked to complete<br />
the <strong>Prosocial</strong> Interacti<strong>on</strong> scale <strong>of</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong>naire <strong>on</strong> friendship features<br />
(Berndt & Keefe, 1995) and the Help scale <strong>of</strong> the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Qualities<br />
Scale (Bukowski et al., 1994). A sample item for the <strong>Prosocial</strong> Interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
scale is “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 helps me if I can’t do something by myself.” <strong>The</strong> Help<br />
scale c<strong>on</strong>sists <strong>of</strong> two subscales: Aid (e.g., “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 would help me when<br />
I needed it”) and Protecti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 would stick up for me if<br />
another kid was causing me trouble”). <strong>The</strong> original scales have two<br />
different rating systems; to keep the directi<strong>on</strong>s as clear and simple as<br />
possible for the participants, we adopted the Bukowski et al. (1994) rating<br />
system. Thus, students were asked to rate <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (not true) to5<br />
(really true) how <strong>of</strong>ten the specified friend exhibits various indicators <strong>of</strong><br />
prosocial behavior. Each participant was asked to resp<strong>on</strong>d to these prosocial<br />
items for each nominated friend. <strong>The</strong>n the nine scores for each friend<br />
were averaged to form a single prosocial-behavior score (for Time 1, M <br />
4.04, SD 0.68, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .76; for Time 2, M 4.18, SD 0.76,<br />
Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .75).<br />
Results<br />
In this study, we sought to identify the process by which friends’<br />
prosocial behavior relates to targets’ prosocial behavior by addressing<br />
two research questi<strong>on</strong>s. For the first research questi<strong>on</strong>,<br />
goals were posited to provide a pathway between a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior and changes in a target’s prosocial behavior. Specifically,<br />
we predicted that a friend’s behavior would be related to<br />
changes in goal pursuit over time; in turn, a target individual’s goal<br />
pursuit would be related to that target’s change in prosocial behavior.<br />
To examine this questi<strong>on</strong>, we applied the most stringent<br />
test <strong>of</strong> mediati<strong>on</strong>. As described by Bar<strong>on</strong> and Kenny (1986),<br />
Table 1<br />
Intercorrelati<strong>on</strong>s Am<strong>on</strong>g Variables<br />
support for mediati<strong>on</strong> is found when (a) the independent variable<br />
significantly predicts both the mediator and the outcome (i.e.,<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior significantly predicts both target’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit and target’s prosocial behavior), (b) the<br />
mediator significantly predicts the outcome (i.e., target’s prosocial<br />
goal pursuit significantly predicts target’s prosocial behavior), and<br />
(c) the independent variable predicts the outcome less (partial<br />
mediati<strong>on</strong>) or n<strong>on</strong>significantly (complete mediati<strong>on</strong>) when the<br />
mediator also is included as a predictor (i.e., friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior is a less significant or n<strong>on</strong>significant predictor <strong>of</strong> target’s<br />
prosocial behavior when target’s prosocial goal pursuit is included<br />
in the regressi<strong>on</strong> equati<strong>on</strong>).<br />
We also examined the role <strong>of</strong> friendship characteristics (affective<br />
quality, interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency) in moderating the relati<strong>on</strong><br />
between friend’s prosocial behavior at Time 1 (T1) and change in<br />
target’s prosocial goal pursuit over time; and we examined the role<br />
<strong>of</strong> friendship stability in moderating the relati<strong>on</strong> between friend’s<br />
prosocial behavior at Time 2 (T2) and change in target’s prosocial<br />
goal pursuit over time (in this case, the T2 score for friend’s<br />
behavior was used, because friendship stability occurred over two<br />
time points). According to Bar<strong>on</strong> and Kenny (1986), support for<br />
moderati<strong>on</strong> is found when interacti<strong>on</strong> terms entered at the final<br />
step <strong>of</strong> a hierarchical regressi<strong>on</strong> are significant. Support for moderati<strong>on</strong><br />
may also be found when an individual interacti<strong>on</strong> term<br />
(e.g., Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>) is a significant<br />
predictor, even if the overall step for the interacti<strong>on</strong> term<br />
is not significant. A power analysis was c<strong>on</strong>ducted in accordance<br />
with Cohen (1992), and the final l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample size was<br />
found to be adequate to detect medium effect sizes.<br />
Questi<strong>on</strong> 1: Does <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit Provide a<br />
Pathway That Links a <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong><br />
and an Individual’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>?<br />
Target’s prosocial goal pursuit was predicted to explain the<br />
associati<strong>on</strong> between a friend’s prosocial behavior (T1) and a<br />
target’s prosocial behavior 1 year later, as depicted in Figure 1. To<br />
examine this hypothesis, we first computed zero-order correlati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />
As shown in Table 1, friend’s prosocial behavior was related<br />
significantly and positively to target’s prosocial goal pursuit at<br />
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9<br />
1. Sex —<br />
2. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial behavior, T1<br />
(target’s percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> behavior) .36*** —<br />
3. Affective quality, T1 .42*** .73*** —<br />
4. Interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, T1 .15* .39*** .49*** —<br />
5. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship stability .03 .06 .19*** .12 —<br />
6. Target’s prosocial goal pursuit, T1 .35*** .34*** .25*** .17** .01 —<br />
7. Target’s prosocial goal pursuit, T2 .35*** .32*** .24*** .09 .00 .59*** —<br />
8. Target’s prosocial behavior, T1<br />
(peer-nominated behavior) .26*** .01 .09 .06 .09 .25*** .18** —<br />
9. Target’s prosocial behavior, T2<br />
(peer-nominated behavior) .43*** .14* .22** .07 .08 .37*** .28*** .75*** —<br />
Note. n 208. T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />
* p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.<br />
BARRY AND WENTZEL
oth time points and to target’s prosocial behavior at T2 (<strong>on</strong> the<br />
basis <strong>of</strong> peer nominati<strong>on</strong>s) but not to target’s prosocial behavior at<br />
T1 (also <strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> peer nominati<strong>on</strong>s). Moreover, target’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit at both time points was related significantly<br />
and positively to target’s prosocial behavior at both time points.<br />
Sex was related significantly and positively to friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior, target’s prosocial goal pursuit (T1 and T2), and target’s<br />
prosocial behavior (T1 and T2). In other words, girls were more<br />
likely to perceive their friends as prosocial, to report frequent<br />
prosocial goal pursuit, and to be nominated by same-sex, samegrade<br />
classmates as more prosocial than were boys. 1<br />
<strong>The</strong> significant correlati<strong>on</strong>s indicated that testing for mediati<strong>on</strong><br />
had merit, in that variables <strong>of</strong> interest were related in the expected<br />
directi<strong>on</strong>s. To test for mediati<strong>on</strong> more formally, we next examined<br />
the pathway from friend’s prosocial behavior to target’s prosocial<br />
goal pursuit with a series <strong>of</strong> regressi<strong>on</strong> models. First, we regressed<br />
prosocial goal pursuit at T1 <strong>on</strong> friend’s prosocial behavior (C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />
a <strong>of</strong> Bar<strong>on</strong> & Kenny, 1986). As shown in Table 2, friend’s<br />
prosocial behavior significantly and positively predicted target’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit, F(2, 174) 16.35, p .0001. (Step 3 was<br />
included to test moderati<strong>on</strong>, which is described in the next secti<strong>on</strong>.)<br />
We next examined relati<strong>on</strong>s between friend’s prosocial behavior<br />
and change in goal pursuit over time by regressing prosocial<br />
goal pursuit at T2 <strong>on</strong> friend’s prosocial behavior at T1, after<br />
c<strong>on</strong>trolling for prosocial goal pursuit at T1 (an extensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> Bar<strong>on</strong><br />
& Kenny’s, 1986, C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> a). As shown in Table 3, friend’s<br />
prosocial behavior was not a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> target’s prosocial<br />
goal pursuit (T2), although target’s prosocial goal pursuit (T1)<br />
significantly predicted target’s prosocial goal pursuit (T2). Finally,<br />
we regressed prosocial behavior at T2 <strong>on</strong> both friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior at T1 and prosocial goal pursuit at T2 (C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s b and<br />
c <strong>of</strong> Bar<strong>on</strong> & Kenny, 1986), after c<strong>on</strong>trolling for prosocial goal<br />
pursuit at T1. As shown in Table 4, target’s prosocial goal pursuit<br />
(T1, but not T2) predicted target’s prosocial behavior at T2 with<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior at T1 c<strong>on</strong>trolled for.<br />
Taken together, the results <strong>of</strong> the regressi<strong>on</strong> analyses indicate<br />
that target’s prosocial goal pursuit (T1) served as a pathway that<br />
links friend’s prosocial behavior (T1; <strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> target’s<br />
percepti<strong>on</strong>) to target’s prosocial behavior (T2; <strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> peer<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong>s), in that friend’s prosocial behavior (T1) was related<br />
to target’s prosocial goal pursuit (T1), and target’s prosocial goal<br />
Table 2<br />
Results <strong>of</strong> Hierarchical Regressi<strong>on</strong> Analysis <strong>on</strong> Target’s<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit, Time 1<br />
Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 <br />
1. Sex<br />
2. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial behavior<br />
.30*** .20** .19**<br />
(FPB), T1 .28*** .35***<br />
3. Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency FPB, T1 .16*<br />
Affective Quality FPB, T1 .21**<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Stability FPB, T2 .01<br />
R 2<br />
.09*** .07*** .05*<br />
Total R 2<br />
.21***<br />
Note. n 177. <strong>The</strong> standard errors for each step are as follows: .55 (Step<br />
1), .53 (Step 2), and .52 (Step 3). T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />
* p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.<br />
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />
pursuit at T1 was related to the change in target’s prosocial<br />
behavior over time. However, complete mediati<strong>on</strong>, as defined by<br />
Bar<strong>on</strong> and Kenny (1986), was not supported given that friend’s<br />
prosocial behavior (T1) was a n<strong>on</strong>significant predictor <strong>of</strong> change<br />
in target’s prosocial goal pursuit, and target’s prosocial goal pursuit<br />
(T2) was a n<strong>on</strong>significant predictor <strong>of</strong> target’s prosocial behavior<br />
(T2).<br />
Questi<strong>on</strong> 2: How Do <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Characteristics <strong>of</strong><br />
Affective Quality, Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency, and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship<br />
Stability Moderate the Relati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong><br />
<strong>Behavior</strong> to a Target Individual’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goals?<br />
157<br />
In keeping with social-learning theory (Bandura, 1986), we<br />
hypothesized that behavior is learned, in part, through observing<br />
best friends. Moreover, we predicted that friend influence is most<br />
likely to occur by way <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit when (a) the affective<br />
quality <strong>of</strong> a friendship is high, and (b) the interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency<br />
and friendship stability between an individual and a friend are high<br />
such that the individual and friend spend a significant amount <strong>of</strong><br />
time together.<br />
To test for these moderating effects, we included interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
terms at the last step <strong>of</strong> the regressi<strong>on</strong> analyses presented in Tables<br />
2 and 3: Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>, Interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
Frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>, and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship<br />
Stability <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>. Affective quality, interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
frequency, and friend’s prosocial behavior were mean centered<br />
to reduce the redundancy <strong>of</strong> each interacti<strong>on</strong> term.<br />
As shown in Table 2, two interacti<strong>on</strong> terms (involving affective<br />
quality and interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency) included at Step 3 significantly<br />
predicted target’s prosocial goal pursuit at T1; however, friendship<br />
stability was not a significant predictor. As shown in Table 3, the<br />
Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> term<br />
was not a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> T2 goal pursuit with T1 goal<br />
pursuit c<strong>on</strong>trolled for. (We tested models in which each interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
term was entered as a separate step, and the results were not<br />
significantly different from those reported in Tables 2 and 3).<br />
To explore further the significant Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> for target’s prosocial goal pursuit at<br />
T1, we regressed goal pursuit (T1) <strong>on</strong> friend’s prosocial behavior<br />
for each <strong>of</strong> three levels <strong>of</strong> affective quality, as determined by a<br />
subdivisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the affective quality variable al<strong>on</strong>g the 33rd and<br />
67th percentiles (see Aiken & West, 1991). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s behavior was<br />
a significant, positive predictor ( .38, p .001) <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit<br />
at T1 <strong>on</strong>ly when affective quality was high, F(1, 68) 11.43,<br />
SE 0.43, p .001 (see Figure 2). <strong>The</strong>se findings thereby suggest<br />
that friend’s prosocial behavior is related to target’s prosocial goal<br />
pursuit at T1 when the target perceives the friendship to have high<br />
levels <strong>of</strong> affective quality.<br />
To explore further the significant Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency <br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> for target’s prosocial goal<br />
pursuit at T1, we regressed goal pursuit (T1) <strong>on</strong> friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior for each <strong>of</strong> three levels <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency, as<br />
determined by a subdivisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency variable<br />
1 Initially, grade and ethnicity were included within the first step for all<br />
three regressi<strong>on</strong>s; however, the two variables were n<strong>on</strong>significant and,<br />
thus, omitted from what is reported here.
158<br />
Table 3<br />
Results <strong>of</strong> Hierarchical Regressi<strong>on</strong> Analysis <strong>on</strong> Target’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit, Time 2<br />
al<strong>on</strong>g the 33rd and 67th percentiles. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s behavior was a<br />
significant, positive predictor ( .42, p .0001) <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit<br />
at T1 <strong>on</strong>ly when interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency was low, F(1, 76) 16.67,<br />
SE 0.57, p .0001 (see Figure 3). Thus, friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior was related to target’s prosocial goal pursuit at T1 when<br />
the target had low levels <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency with the friend.<br />
Finally, as shown in Table 3, Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> was a significant, positive predictor <strong>of</strong> change<br />
in target’s prosocial goal pursuit over time. Because the latter<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong> term was positive, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to the negative interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
term shown in Table 2, a close examinati<strong>on</strong> through follow-up<br />
analyses is required to understand such significant results, because<br />
inspecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the sign <strong>of</strong> the interacti<strong>on</strong> term can be misleading<br />
(Mossholder, Kemery, & Bedeian, 1990). As a result, we further<br />
explored this finding, in accordance with Aiken and West (1991),<br />
by regressing target’s goal pursuit (T2) <strong>on</strong> both goal pursuit (T1)<br />
and friend’s prosocial behavior for each <strong>of</strong> the three levels <strong>of</strong><br />
interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s behavior was a significant, positive<br />
predictor ( .29, p .002) <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit at T2 <strong>on</strong>ly when<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency was high, F(2, 72) 30.97, SE 0.41, p <br />
.0001 (see Figure 4). <strong>The</strong>refore, low frequency <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
appeared to have a significant and positive impact <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current<br />
Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 <br />
1. Sex .32*** .15* .11† .11†<br />
2. <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit, T1 .57*** .54*** .57***<br />
3. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial behavior (FPB), T1 .12† .05<br />
4. Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency FPB, T1 .13*<br />
Affective Quality FPB, T1 .06<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Stability FPB, T2 .09<br />
R 2<br />
.10*** .30*** .01† .02<br />
Total R 2<br />
.43***<br />
Note. n 177. <strong>The</strong> standard errors for each step are as follows: .57 (Step 1), .47 (Step 2), .46 (Step 3), and<br />
.46 (Step 4). T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />
† p .10. * p .05. *** p .001.<br />
Table 4<br />
Results <strong>of</strong> Hierarchical Regressi<strong>on</strong> Analysis <strong>on</strong> Target’s<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>, Time 2<br />
Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 <br />
1. Sex .43*** .21*** .20*** .20***<br />
2. Target’s prosocial<br />
behavior, T1<br />
.65*** .66*** .66***<br />
Target’s prosocial goal<br />
pursuit, T1<br />
.14** .12** .13*<br />
3. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial<br />
behavior, T1<br />
.04 .04<br />
4. Target’s prosocial goal<br />
pursuit, T2<br />
.01<br />
R 2<br />
.18*** .45*** .00 .00<br />
Total R 2<br />
.63***<br />
Note. n 206. <strong>The</strong> standard errors for each step are as follows: .16 (Step<br />
1) and .11 (Steps 2, 3, and 4). T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />
* p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.<br />
BARRY AND WENTZEL<br />
relati<strong>on</strong>s between friends’ prosocial behavior and targets’ goal<br />
pursuit, whereas high interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency appeared to have a<br />
significant and positive impact <strong>on</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>s between friends’<br />
prosocial behavior and targets’ goal pursuit over time.<br />
Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />
In this study, we examined the process by which a best friend<br />
might influence an individual’s prosocial behavior. As predicted, a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior (as observed by the target individual)<br />
was related to a target’s prosocial behavior (<strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> peer<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong>s) over time by way <strong>of</strong> a target’s pursuit <strong>of</strong> goals to be<br />
prosocial at T1. In additi<strong>on</strong>, friendship characteristics moderated<br />
the relati<strong>on</strong> between a friend’s prosocial behavior and a target’s<br />
goal pursuit such that relati<strong>on</strong>s differed as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the<br />
affective quality and frequency <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> between friends.<br />
<strong>The</strong>refore, our findings provide further c<strong>on</strong>firmati<strong>on</strong> that motivati<strong>on</strong>al<br />
processes can explain significant associati<strong>on</strong>s between the<br />
behaviors <strong>of</strong> two friends, highlighting the importance <strong>of</strong> friendship<br />
characteristics in defining the nature <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships between a<br />
friend’s behavior and an individual’s goal pursuit. Finally, the<br />
significant gender differences in prosocial behavior and prosocial<br />
goal pursuit found in our adolescent sample extend empirical<br />
research (e.g., Parker & Asher, 1993) that has found such differences<br />
in early and middle childhood (e.g., Eisenberg & Mussen,<br />
1989), and they support theoretical c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong>s (see Eisenberg &<br />
Figure 2. Effect <strong>of</strong> Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />
target’s prosocial goal pursuit at Time 1 (T1).
Figure 3. Effect <strong>of</strong> Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency (Int Freq) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong><br />
<strong>Behavior</strong> <strong>on</strong> target’s prosocial goal pursuit at Time 1 (T1).<br />
Fabes, 1998) that girls are expected to behave in prosocial ways to<br />
a greater extent than are boys.<br />
<strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit as an Intermediary Process<br />
According to the tenets <strong>of</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning (Bandura,<br />
1986), an individual is likely to behave similarly to a friend<br />
because exposure to modeled behavior is frequent and affectively<br />
charged. In additi<strong>on</strong>, Bandura (1986) proposed that although behavior<br />
can be learned by observing others, it is likely to be enacted<br />
to the extent that an individual is motivated to do so. <strong>The</strong> findings<br />
<strong>of</strong> the current study support these noti<strong>on</strong>s, in that an individual’s<br />
pursuit <strong>of</strong> prosocial goals (at both time points) was related to his or<br />
her percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> his or her friend’s prosocial behavior. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />
changes in prosocial behavior (observed by the peer group)<br />
over time were related to levels <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit at T1. <strong>The</strong>se results<br />
also replicate previous findings linking the prosocial behavior <strong>of</strong><br />
friends by way <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit (Wentzel et al., 2004), and they<br />
provide further dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> that friendships can have a significant<br />
influence <strong>on</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong>al processes (e.g., Rose & Asher,<br />
1999). <strong>The</strong> findings also affirm that prosocial goal setting is related<br />
to displays <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior over the course <strong>of</strong> 1 year (e.g.,<br />
Nels<strong>on</strong> & Crick, 1999; Wentzel et al., 2004).<br />
Our findings indicate that the amount <strong>of</strong> change in an individual’s<br />
goal pursuit explained by a friend’s behavior is fairly small.<br />
Indeed, the prosocial behavior <strong>of</strong> the students in our study was<br />
highly stable over time. This was not surprising given that the<br />
sample was composed <strong>of</strong> adolescents who enjoyed close friendships<br />
and, therefore, were socially competent at the outset. In<br />
additi<strong>on</strong>, given the potential influence <strong>of</strong> adults (e.g., parents,<br />
teachers, relatives), other children (e.g., siblings, rivals), and the<br />
media <strong>on</strong> an adolescent’s behavior, our findings support the noti<strong>on</strong><br />
that a friend’s behavior can be meaningful in the life <strong>of</strong> a student.<br />
It also is important to note, however, that measures <strong>of</strong> a single<br />
best friend’s prosocial behavior also might be a proxy for behavior<br />
displayed by a larger group <strong>of</strong> friends and close acquaintances. For<br />
example, Sage and Kindermann (1999) found that adolescents’<br />
classroom motivati<strong>on</strong> varies as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> peer-group approval<br />
and disapproval. <strong>The</strong>refore, influence processes within friendship<br />
cliques (see Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997) and peer networks (see<br />
Kindermann, 1993) might be important targets for future research.<br />
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />
In additi<strong>on</strong>, an individual might observe and become motivated to<br />
display the behavior <strong>of</strong> peers with whom he or she wants to<br />
become friends. Pairings am<strong>on</strong>g potential or new friends have yet<br />
to be examined in research <strong>on</strong> friend influence. However, research<br />
<strong>on</strong> friendship-making strategies has shown that the display <strong>of</strong><br />
prosocial behavior (e.g., sharing) is a successful strategy for making<br />
friends (Wentzel & Erdley, 1993). <strong>The</strong>refore, changes in<br />
prosocial behavior over time might reflect, in part, changes in<br />
investments in forming and maintaining new friendships.<br />
An unexpected finding was the n<strong>on</strong>significant c<strong>on</strong>current relati<strong>on</strong><br />
between a friend’s and a target’s prosocial behavior at T1<br />
(depicted in Table 1). This lack <strong>of</strong> similarity in friends’ prosocial<br />
behavior has not been reported elsewhere (cf. Wentzel et al.,<br />
2004). A methodological explanati<strong>on</strong> for this finding is that a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior was established <strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />
<strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e individual, whereas a target’s prosocial behavior<br />
was established <strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> a larger set <strong>of</strong> peer nominati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />
<strong>The</strong>refore, a friend’s behavior scores reflected a relatively subjective<br />
and less reliable assessment <strong>of</strong> a close friend, whereas a<br />
target’s behavior scores were based <strong>on</strong> a more objective set <strong>of</strong><br />
assessments. In additi<strong>on</strong>, items used to assess a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior were much more specific, reflecting behavior that is<br />
likely to be observed more frequently by a friend than by the larger<br />
peer group. For these reas<strong>on</strong>s, the lack <strong>of</strong> significant overlap<br />
between these two scores is understandable.<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Characteristics as Moderators<br />
159<br />
<strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> our study also extend work in this area by<br />
documenting the importance <strong>of</strong> friendship characteristics in explaining<br />
relati<strong>on</strong>s between a friend’s behavior and an individual’s<br />
goal pursuit. As suggested by the literature <strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning,<br />
the affective quality <strong>of</strong> a friendship and the frequency <strong>of</strong><br />
friends’ interacti<strong>on</strong>s, as perceived by an individual, appear to<br />
influence relati<strong>on</strong>s between a friend’s behavior and the individual’s<br />
goal pursuit. However, this influence <strong>on</strong>ly seems to occur<br />
under specific c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. For example, findings indicate that a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior is related positively to an individual’s<br />
goal pursuit <strong>on</strong>ly when the target perceives the affective quality <strong>of</strong><br />
a friendship to be high. In other words, a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior is most likely to be associated with an individual’s<br />
Figure 4. Effect <strong>of</strong> Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency (Int Freq) <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong><br />
<strong>Behavior</strong> <strong>on</strong> target’s prosocial goal pursuit at Time 2 (T2).
160<br />
motivati<strong>on</strong> to be helpful and cooperative when the individual has<br />
a str<strong>on</strong>g, positive b<strong>on</strong>d with that friend.<br />
Significant moderating effects also were found for the frequency<br />
<strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>s between friends. In this case, at T1, a friend’s<br />
observed behavior was related positively to an individual’s goal<br />
pursuit <strong>on</strong>ly when the individual interacted infrequently with the<br />
friend. However, over time, a friend’s prosocial behavior was<br />
related positively to an individual’s goal pursuit when interacti<strong>on</strong><br />
with that friend was frequent. This latter finding would be expected<br />
<strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> tenets <strong>of</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al learning; behavior is<br />
likely to be modeled if an individual has more frequent and stable<br />
exposure to it over time. In c<strong>on</strong>trast, the moderating effect at T1<br />
might suggest that over the short term, modeling effects are not as<br />
str<strong>on</strong>g. One alternative explanati<strong>on</strong> is that individuals who interact<br />
with their best friends infrequently might expend greater effort to<br />
behave prosocially simply to establish and maintain these friendships.<br />
Although suggestive, our findings cannot support causal c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s<br />
c<strong>on</strong>cerning the effects <strong>of</strong> friendship qualities <strong>on</strong> an individual’s<br />
prosocial goal pursuit. Indeed, it is possible that these<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong> effects may reflect perceived similarity, such that perceiving<br />
high levels <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior in a friend to whom <strong>on</strong>e<br />
is emoti<strong>on</strong>ally attached might lead to percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e’s self as<br />
behaving in similar ways and, thus, reporting high frequencies <strong>of</strong><br />
goal pursuit. However, some adolescents reported high levels <strong>of</strong><br />
prosocial goal pursuit even when their friends did not display<br />
corresp<strong>on</strong>dingly high levels <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior (e.g., the high<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong>-frequency group in Figure 3), suggesting that an explanati<strong>on</strong><br />
based <strong>on</strong> similarity <strong>of</strong> friends does not hold for all cases.<br />
Other factors bey<strong>on</strong>d the friendship dyad might also be important<br />
for understanding the link between a friend’s behavior and an<br />
individual’s goal pursuit. For instance, cognitive processes described<br />
by Bandura (1986)—such as how observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> prosocial<br />
behavior are interpreted and stored in memory—and strategy<br />
knowledge c<strong>on</strong>cerning how to actually produce prosocial behavior<br />
might be particularly critical processes to examine in this regard.<br />
Moreover, other motivati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>structs, such as an individual’s<br />
attributi<strong>on</strong>al style (Weiner, 1985) and perceived efficacy for prosocial<br />
behavior (Bandura, 1994), and affective factors such as empathy<br />
(Eisenberg, Wolchik, Goldberg, & Engel, 2002) should be<br />
c<strong>on</strong>sidered to understand more fully the circumstances under<br />
which a target individual’s percepti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a friend’s behavior might<br />
influence an individual’s goal pursuit.<br />
Methodological Issues<br />
Several methodological aspects <strong>of</strong> our study also are important<br />
to note. First, our measure <strong>of</strong> friendship placed restricti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> who<br />
could be nominated as a friend: same-grade and same-sex individuals<br />
who attended the same public high school. In reality, adolescents<br />
<strong>of</strong>ten have friends who are <strong>of</strong> the opposite sex or are in a<br />
different grade or school than they are. It is important to note,<br />
however, that all <strong>of</strong> the study’s participants, even those without<br />
reciprocated friendships, were able to list at least <strong>on</strong>e best friend<br />
who met our criteria. In additi<strong>on</strong>, a limit <strong>of</strong> three best-friend<br />
nominati<strong>on</strong>s was established. Although it is possible that students<br />
had more than three best friends, it is highly unlikely that a<br />
significant porti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> students had more than three reciprocated<br />
best friendships (<strong>on</strong>ly 15% <strong>of</strong> the study participants had three<br />
reciprocated best friendships at T1). In general, however, our<br />
BARRY AND WENTZEL<br />
findings must be tempered with the reality that restricti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong><br />
friendship nominati<strong>on</strong>s might result in identificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> reciprocated<br />
friendships that do not represent the best friendships or the<br />
friendships <strong>of</strong> greatest quality for all participants.<br />
In additi<strong>on</strong>, our measure <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency did not include<br />
interacti<strong>on</strong>s that take place via e-mail, instant messaging, text<br />
messaging, or other venues that do not require physical proximity.<br />
Indeed, these forms <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> are becoming increasingly comm<strong>on</strong><br />
am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents (B<strong>on</strong>ebrake, 2002; C<strong>of</strong>fey & Stipp, 1997;<br />
Odell, Korgen, Schumacher, & Delucchi, 2000). As a result, the<br />
extent to which interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency might influence how a<br />
friend’s prosocial behavior is related to an individual’s motivati<strong>on</strong><br />
is likely to be underreported in this study, especially given the high<br />
socioec<strong>on</strong>omic status (SES) <strong>of</strong> the sample and the participants’<br />
likely access to these technologies. Finally, a friend’s prosocial<br />
behavior and a target’s prosocial-behavior scores were assessed <strong>on</strong><br />
the basis <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> from the same individual. Although this<br />
practice likely inflated the relati<strong>on</strong> between these two variables,<br />
our model was based <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that an adolescent’s<br />
observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> his or her friend’s behavior (rather than observati<strong>on</strong>s<br />
made by the larger peer group) had the most proximal<br />
influence <strong>on</strong> his or her subsequent behavior.<br />
Sampling issues also are important to note, in that these friendships<br />
occurred in fairly affluent and predominantly European<br />
American communities. <strong>The</strong> prep<strong>on</strong>derance <strong>of</strong> upper-middle-class<br />
European American students in our sample allows for c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s<br />
about this populati<strong>on</strong>. However, the current investigati<strong>on</strong> does not<br />
further the understanding <strong>of</strong> friendship influence am<strong>on</strong>g ethnic<br />
minority populati<strong>on</strong>s or students from lower SES communities. A<br />
small literature suggests that friendships differ as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />
ethnicity. For instance, African Americans tend to report denser<br />
networks <strong>of</strong> neighborhood friends but talk to few friends during<br />
school (DuBois & Hirsch, 1990). Moreover, Asian American<br />
adolescents are more likely to have friends <strong>of</strong> the same race than<br />
are both Latino and African American adolescents; however, African<br />
Americans report greater close friendship support than do<br />
Asian Americans (Way & Chen, 2000). <strong>Prosocial</strong> behavior also<br />
varies as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> ethnicity, such that Mexican American<br />
adolescents tend to prefer cooperative to competitive behaviors<br />
(see Knight, Bernal, & Carlo, 1995) more than do European<br />
Americans. Further, cross-cultural research has documented variati<strong>on</strong>s<br />
in prosocial behavior (for a review, see Carlo, Fabes, Laible,<br />
& Kupan<strong>of</strong>f, 1999). <strong>The</strong>refore, research that uses more ethnically<br />
diverse samples is needed to investigate the generalizability <strong>of</strong> the<br />
influence processes suggested by our findings to other groups <strong>of</strong><br />
n<strong>on</strong>-Caucasian adolescent students.<br />
Although our sample was limited, our findings indicate that<br />
compared with almost 80% <strong>of</strong> European American students who<br />
had reciprocated friendships, <strong>on</strong>ly 58% <strong>of</strong> the ethnic minority<br />
students in the sample had a reciprocated friend. It is possible that<br />
these students were more likely to have friendships outside <strong>of</strong> the<br />
school c<strong>on</strong>text. However, the majority <strong>of</strong> the Asian American<br />
students (the largest ethnic minority group in the school studied)<br />
reported having either a same-race (25%) or cross-race (50%)<br />
friendship (with the remaining 25% reporting no reciprocated<br />
friendship). <strong>The</strong>refore, adolescents in at least <strong>on</strong>e minority group<br />
appeared to be integrated into the social culture <strong>of</strong> the school to a<br />
greater extent than were other ethnic minority adolescents. <strong>The</strong>se<br />
differences in friendship formati<strong>on</strong> as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> ethnicity
clearly deserve further study, with a focus <strong>on</strong> possible c<strong>on</strong>founding<br />
effects <strong>of</strong> SES <strong>of</strong> individual students, heterogeneity <strong>of</strong> values<br />
within ethnic groups, and the degree to which these groups are a<br />
minority within a particular school. Given that not all adolescents<br />
are able to form friendships <strong>of</strong> high quality, scholars should also<br />
examine influence processes am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents with low levels <strong>of</strong><br />
friendship quality.<br />
C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong><br />
<strong>The</strong> results <strong>of</strong> the present study suggest that the influence <strong>of</strong> best<br />
friends <strong>on</strong> prosocial behavior might be explained, in part, by<br />
observati<strong>on</strong>al learning processes. Specifically, goal pursuit appears<br />
to provide a pathway that links a friend’s prosocial behavior to a<br />
target’s prosocial behavior, with friendship characteristics (i.e.,<br />
affective quality and interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency) determining the nature<br />
<strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>s between a friend’s prosocial behavior and a target’s<br />
goal pursuit. To date, the body <strong>of</strong> literature <strong>on</strong> friend influence has<br />
focused <strong>on</strong> numerous negative behaviors, such as alcohol (e.g.,<br />
Urberg, Degˇirmenciogˇlu, & Pilgrim, 1997) and cigarette use (e.g.,<br />
Ennett & Bauman, 1994). <strong>The</strong>refore, the current findings also<br />
extend the literature <strong>on</strong> adolescent friend influence to more positive<br />
behaviors. Immediate directi<strong>on</strong>s for future research should<br />
include the examinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> individual characteristics (e.g., empathy),<br />
other motivati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>structs, and the roles <strong>of</strong> multiple<br />
friends within an ethnically and socioec<strong>on</strong>omically diverse sample.<br />
<strong>The</strong> study <strong>of</strong> friend influence could also be broadened to include<br />
other peer socializers (e.g., members <strong>of</strong> cliques and peer networks)<br />
to yield more ecological validity in the study <strong>of</strong> how and why<br />
adolescents are motivated to display prosocial behavior.<br />
References<br />
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regressi<strong>on</strong>: Testing and<br />
interpreting interacti<strong>on</strong>s. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.<br />
Azmitia, M., & M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, R. (1993). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship, transactive dialogues,<br />
and the development <strong>of</strong> scientific reas<strong>on</strong>ing. Social Development, 2,<br />
202–221.<br />
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> thought and acti<strong>on</strong>: A social<br />
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.<br />
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy: <strong>The</strong> exercise <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol. New York:<br />
Freeman.<br />
Bar<strong>on</strong>, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). <strong>The</strong> moderator–mediator variable<br />
distincti<strong>on</strong> in social psychological research: C<strong>on</strong>ceptual, strategic, and<br />
statistical c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s. Journal <strong>of</strong> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology,<br />
51, 1173–1182.<br />
Berndt, T. J. (1981a). Age changes and changes over time in prosocial<br />
intenti<strong>on</strong>s and behavior between friends. Developmental Psychology, 17,<br />
408–416.<br />
Berndt, T. J. (1981b). Relati<strong>on</strong>s between social cogniti<strong>on</strong>, n<strong>on</strong>-social<br />
cogniti<strong>on</strong>, and social behavior: <strong>The</strong> case <strong>of</strong> friendship. In J. H. Flavell &<br />
L. D. Ross (Eds.), Social cognitive development: Fr<strong>on</strong>tiers and possible<br />
futures (pp. 176–199). New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />
Berndt, T. J. (1985). <strong>Prosocial</strong> behavior between friends in middle childhood<br />
and early adolescence. Journal <strong>of</strong> Early Adolescence, 5, 307–317.<br />
Berndt, T. J. (1999). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>s’ influence <strong>on</strong> students’ adjustment to school.<br />
Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychologist, 34, 15–28.<br />
Berndt, T. J., Hawkins, J. A., & Hoyle, S. G. (1986). Changes in friendship<br />
during a school year: Effects <strong>on</strong> children’s and adolescents’ impressi<strong>on</strong>s<br />
<strong>of</strong> friendship and sharing with friends. Child Development, 57, 1284–<br />
1297.<br />
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />
161<br />
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>s’ influence <strong>on</strong> adolescents’<br />
adjustment to school. Child Development, 66, 1312–1329.<br />
Berndt, T. J., & Perry, T. B. (1986). Children’s percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> friendships<br />
as supportive relati<strong>on</strong>ships. Developmental Psychology, 22, 640–648.<br />
Bigelow, B. J., & LaGaipa, J. J. (1980). <strong>The</strong> development <strong>of</strong> friendship<br />
values and choice. In H. C. Foot, A. J. Chapman, & J. R. Smith (Eds.),<br />
<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship and social relati<strong>on</strong>s in children (pp. 15–44). New York:<br />
Wiley.<br />
B<strong>on</strong>ebrake, K. (2002). College students’ Internet use, relati<strong>on</strong>ship formati<strong>on</strong>,<br />
and pers<strong>on</strong>ality correlates. Cyberpsychology and <strong>Behavior</strong>, 5, 551–<br />
557.<br />
Bryan, J. H., & Walbek, N. H. (1970). <strong>The</strong> impact <strong>of</strong> words and deeds<br />
c<strong>on</strong>cerning altruism up<strong>on</strong> children. Child Development, 41, 747–757.<br />
Bukowski, W. M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularity and friendship: Issues in<br />
theory, measurement, and outcome. In T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.),<br />
Peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships in child development (pp. 15–45). New York: Wiley.<br />
Bukowski, W. M., Hoza, B., & Boivin, M. (1994). Measuring friendship<br />
quality during pre- and early adolescence: <strong>The</strong> development and psychometric<br />
properties <strong>of</strong> the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Qualities Scale. Journal <strong>of</strong> Social<br />
and Pers<strong>on</strong>al Relati<strong>on</strong>ships, 11, 471–484.<br />
Carlo, G., Fabes, R. A., Laible, D., & Kupan<strong>of</strong>f, K. (1999). Early adolescence<br />
and prosocial/moral behavior II: <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> social and c<strong>on</strong>textual<br />
influences. Journal <strong>of</strong> Early Adolescence, 19, 133–147.<br />
Carlo, G., Hausmann, A., Christiansen, S., & Randall, B. A. (2003).<br />
Sociocognitive and behavioral correlates <strong>of</strong> a measure <strong>of</strong> prosocial<br />
tendencies for adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong> Early Adolescence, 23, 107–134.<br />
C<strong>of</strong>fey, S., & Stipp, H. (1997). <strong>The</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong>s between computer and<br />
televisi<strong>on</strong> usage. Journal <strong>of</strong> Advertising Research, 37, 61–67.<br />
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.<br />
Corsaro, W. A., & Eder, D. (1990). Children’s peer cultures. Annual<br />
Review <strong>of</strong> Sociology, 16, 197–220.<br />
Crockett, L., Los<strong>of</strong>f, M., & Petersen, A. C. (1984). Percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> the peer<br />
group and friendship in early adolescence. Journal <strong>of</strong> Early Adolescence,<br />
4, 155–181.<br />
Dam<strong>on</strong>, W. (1988). <strong>The</strong> moral child: Nurturing children’s natural moral<br />
growth. New York: Free Press.<br />
Dam<strong>on</strong>, W., & Killen, M. (1982). Peer interacti<strong>on</strong> and the process <strong>of</strong><br />
change in children’s moral reas<strong>on</strong>ing. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 28,<br />
347–367.<br />
DuBois, D. L., & Hirsch, B. J. (1990). School and neighborhood friendship<br />
patterns <strong>of</strong> Blacks and Whites in early adolescence. Child Development,<br />
61, 524–536.<br />
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivati<strong>on</strong> to succeed.<br />
In W. Dam<strong>on</strong> (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child<br />
psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emoti<strong>on</strong>al, and pers<strong>on</strong>ality development (5th<br />
ed., pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.<br />
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). <strong>Prosocial</strong> development. In W.<br />
Dam<strong>on</strong> (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child<br />
psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emoti<strong>on</strong>al, and pers<strong>on</strong>ality development (5th<br />
ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley.<br />
Eisenberg, N., & Mussen, P. H. (1989). <strong>The</strong> roots <strong>of</strong> prosocial behavior in<br />
children. New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />
Eisenberg, N., Wolchik, S., Goldberg, L., & Engel, I. (1992). Parental<br />
values, reinforcement, and young children’s prosocial behavior: A l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />
study. Journal <strong>of</strong> Genetic Psychology, 153, 19–36.<br />
Elliott, R., & Vasta, R. (1970). <strong>The</strong> modeling <strong>of</strong> sharing: Effects associated<br />
with vicarious reinforcement, symbolizati<strong>on</strong>, age, and generalizati<strong>on</strong>.<br />
Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Child Psychology, 10, 8–15.<br />
Ennett, S. T., & Bauman, K. E. (1994). <strong>The</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> influence and<br />
selecti<strong>on</strong> to adolescent peer group homogeneity: <strong>The</strong> case <strong>of</strong> adolescent<br />
cigarette smoking. Journal <strong>of</strong> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology, 67,<br />
653–663.<br />
Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Kupan<strong>of</strong>f, K., & Laible, D. (1999). Early adoles-
162<br />
cence and prosocial/moral behavior I: <strong>The</strong> role <strong>of</strong> individual processes.<br />
Journal <strong>of</strong> Early Adolescence, 19, 5–16.<br />
Ford, M. E. (1992). Motivating humans: Goals, emoti<strong>on</strong>s, and pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />
agency beliefs. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.<br />
Hallinan, M. T., & Williams, R. A. (1990). Students’ characteristics and<br />
the peer-influence process. Sociology <strong>of</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 63, 122–132.<br />
Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relati<strong>on</strong>s. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & E. M.<br />
Hetheringt<strong>on</strong> (Vol. Ed.), Handbook <strong>of</strong> child psychology, Vol. 4: Socializati<strong>on</strong>,<br />
pers<strong>on</strong>ality, and social development (pp. 103–196). New York:<br />
Wiley.<br />
Hartup, W. W. (1992). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships and their developmental significance.<br />
In H. McGurk (Ed.), Childhood social development: C<strong>on</strong>temporary<br />
perspectives (pp. 175–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />
Hartup, W. W. (1996). <strong>The</strong> company they keep: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships and their<br />
developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1–13.<br />
Hartup, W. W., & Coates, B. (1967). Imitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> a peer as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />
reinforcement from the peer group and rewardingness <strong>of</strong> the model.<br />
Child Development, 38, 1003–1016.<br />
Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships and adaptati<strong>on</strong> in the life<br />
course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355–370.<br />
Kandel, D. B. (1978a). Homophily, selecti<strong>on</strong>, and socializati<strong>on</strong> in adolescent<br />
friendships. American Journal <strong>of</strong> Sociology, 64, 427–436.<br />
Kandel, D. B. (1978b). Similarity in real-life adolescent friendship pairs.<br />
Journal <strong>of</strong> Pers<strong>on</strong>ality and Social Psychology, 36, 306–312.<br />
Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as c<strong>on</strong>texts for individual<br />
development: <strong>The</strong> case <strong>of</strong> children’s motivati<strong>on</strong> in school. Developmental<br />
Psychology, 29, 970–977.<br />
Knight, G. P., Bernal, M., & Carlo, G. (1995). Socializati<strong>on</strong> and the<br />
development <strong>of</strong> cooperative, competitive, and individualistic behaviors<br />
am<strong>on</strong>g Mexican American children. In E. E. Garcia & B. McLaughlin<br />
(Eds.), Meeting the challenge <strong>of</strong> linguistic and cultural diversity in early<br />
childhood educati<strong>on</strong> (pp. 85–102). New York: Teachers College Press.<br />
Lars<strong>on</strong>, R., & Richards, M. H. (1991). Daily compani<strong>on</strong>ship in late<br />
childhood and early adolescence: Changing developmental c<strong>on</strong>texts.<br />
Child Development, 62, 284–300.<br />
McGuire, K. D., & Weisz, J. R. (1982). Social cogniti<strong>on</strong> and behavior<br />
correlates <strong>of</strong> preadolescent chumship. Child Development, 53, 1478–<br />
1484.<br />
Mossholder, K. W., Kemery, E. R., & Bedeian, A. G. (1990). On using<br />
regressi<strong>on</strong> coefficients to interpret moderator effects. Educati<strong>on</strong>al and<br />
Psychological Measurement, 50, 255–263.<br />
Nels<strong>on</strong>, D. A., & Crick, N. R. (1999). Rose-colored glasses: Examining the<br />
social informati<strong>on</strong>-processing <strong>of</strong> prosocial young adolescents. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />
Early Adolescence, 19, 17–38.<br />
Nels<strong>on</strong>, J., & Aboud, F. E. (1985). <strong>The</strong> resoluti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> social c<strong>on</strong>flict<br />
between friends. Child Development, 56, 1009–1017.<br />
Odell, P. M., Korgen, K. O., Schumacher, P., & Delucchi, M. (2000).<br />
BARRY AND WENTZEL<br />
Internet use am<strong>on</strong>g female and male college students. Cyberpsychology<br />
and <strong>Behavior</strong>, 3, 855–862.<br />
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship and friendship quality in<br />
middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings <strong>of</strong><br />
l<strong>on</strong>eliness and social dissatisfacti<strong>on</strong>. Developmental Psychology, 29,<br />
611–621.<br />
Piaget, J. (1965). <strong>The</strong> moral judgment <strong>of</strong> the child. New York: Free Press<br />
(Original work published 1932)<br />
Pleyd<strong>on</strong>, A. P., & Schner, J. G. (2001). Female adolescent friendship and<br />
delinquent behavior. Adolescence, 36, 189–205.<br />
Prinstein, M. J., Meade, C. S., & Cohen, G. L. (2003). Adolescent oral sex,<br />
peer popularity, and percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> best friends’ sexual behavior. Journal<br />
<strong>of</strong> Pediatric Psychology, 28, 243–249.<br />
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (1999). Children’s goals and strategies in<br />
resp<strong>on</strong>se to c<strong>on</strong>flicts within a friendship. Developmental Psychology, 35,<br />
69–79.<br />
Sage, N. A., & Kindermann, T. A. (1999). Peer networks, behavior<br />
c<strong>on</strong>tingencies, and children’s engagement in the classroom. Merrill–<br />
Palmer Quarterly, 45, 143–171.<br />
Sharabany, R., Gersh<strong>on</strong>i, R., & H<strong>of</strong>man, J. E. (1981). Girlfriend, boyfriend:<br />
Age and sex differences in intimate friendship. Developmental<br />
Psychology, 17, 800–808.<br />
U.S. Bureau <strong>of</strong> the Census. (2000). DP-3. Pr<strong>of</strong>ile <strong>of</strong> selected ec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />
characteristics: 2000. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: U.S. Department <strong>of</strong> Commerce.<br />
Urberg, K. A., Degˇirmenciogˇlu, S. M., & Pilgrim, C. (1997). Close friend<br />
and group influence <strong>on</strong> adolescent cigarette smoking and alcohol use.<br />
Developmental Psychology, 33, 834–844.<br />
Way, N., & Chen, L. (2000). Close and general friendships am<strong>on</strong>g African<br />
American, Latino, and Asian American adolescents from low-income<br />
families. Journal <strong>of</strong> Adolescent Research, 15, 274–301.<br />
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributi<strong>on</strong>al theory <strong>of</strong> achievement motivati<strong>on</strong> and<br />
emoti<strong>on</strong>. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573.<br />
Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Relati<strong>on</strong>s between social competence and academic<br />
achievement in early adolescence. Child Development, 62, 1066–1078.<br />
Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> social goal pursuit to social acceptance,<br />
classroom behavior, and perceived social support. Journal <strong>of</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />
Psychology, 86, 173–182.<br />
Wentzel, K. R., Barry, C. M., & Caldwell, K. (2004). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships in middle<br />
school: <str<strong>on</strong>g>Influence</str<strong>on</strong>g>s <strong>on</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> and school adjustment. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />
Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 96, 195–203.<br />
Wentzel, K. R., & Caldwell, K. (1997). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ships, peer acceptance, and<br />
group membership: Relati<strong>on</strong>s to academic achievement in middle<br />
school. Child Development, 68, 1198–1209.<br />
Wentzel, K. R., & Erdley, C. A. (1993). Strategies for making friends:<br />
Relati<strong>on</strong>s to social behavior and peer acceptance in early adolescence.<br />
Developmental Psychology, 29, 819–826.<br />
Youniss, J. (1981). An analysis <strong>of</strong> moral development through a theory <strong>of</strong><br />
social c<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 27, 384–403.
Appendix<br />
Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency Subscale (T. J. Berndt, Pers<strong>on</strong>al Communicati<strong>on</strong>, October 1997)<br />
1. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you talk to <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 <strong>on</strong> the ph<strong>on</strong>e?<br />
2. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 get together <strong>on</strong> weekends or after school?<br />
3. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 go places together, like a movie,<br />
skating, shopping or a sport’s event?<br />
4. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 go over to each other’s houses?<br />
5. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you and <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 just sit around and talk about things<br />
like school, sports, or anything else?<br />
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />
Members <strong>of</strong> Underrepresented Groups:<br />
Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted<br />
6. How <strong>of</strong>ten do you spend time with <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 when you have free time<br />
during the school day?<br />
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publicati<strong>on</strong>s and<br />
Communicati<strong>on</strong>s Board would like to invite your participati<strong>on</strong>. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the<br />
publicati<strong>on</strong>s process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. <strong>The</strong> P&C<br />
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members <strong>of</strong> underrepresented groups to participate<br />
more in this process.<br />
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to the address below. Please note the<br />
following important points:<br />
To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. <strong>The</strong><br />
experience <strong>of</strong> publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective<br />
review.<br />
To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader <strong>of</strong> the five to six empirical journals that are most<br />
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge <strong>of</strong> recently<br />
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submissi<strong>on</strong><br />
within the c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>of</strong> existing research.<br />
To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you<br />
are interested in, and describe your area <strong>of</strong> experitise. Be as specific as possible. For example,<br />
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cogniti<strong>on</strong>” or “attitude<br />
change” as well.<br />
Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1–4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to<br />
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript<br />
thoroughly.<br />
Write to Journals Office, American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, 750 First Street, NE, Washingt<strong>on</strong>,<br />
DC 20002-4242.<br />
163<br />
Received August 25, 2004<br />
Revisi<strong>on</strong> received June 3, 2005<br />
Accepted June 9, 2005