25.03.2013 Views

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

158<br />

Table 3<br />

Results <strong>of</strong> Hierarchical Regressi<strong>on</strong> Analysis <strong>on</strong> Target’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit, Time 2<br />

al<strong>on</strong>g the 33rd and 67th percentiles. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s behavior was a<br />

significant, positive predictor ( .42, p .0001) <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit<br />

at T1 <strong>on</strong>ly when interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency was low, F(1, 76) 16.67,<br />

SE 0.57, p .0001 (see Figure 3). Thus, friend’s prosocial<br />

behavior was related to target’s prosocial goal pursuit at T1 when<br />

the target had low levels <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency with the friend.<br />

Finally, as shown in Table 3, Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s<br />

<strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> was a significant, positive predictor <strong>of</strong> change<br />

in target’s prosocial goal pursuit over time. Because the latter<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> term was positive, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to the negative interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

term shown in Table 2, a close examinati<strong>on</strong> through follow-up<br />

analyses is required to understand such significant results, because<br />

inspecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the sign <strong>of</strong> the interacti<strong>on</strong> term can be misleading<br />

(Mossholder, Kemery, & Bedeian, 1990). As a result, we further<br />

explored this finding, in accordance with Aiken and West (1991),<br />

by regressing target’s goal pursuit (T2) <strong>on</strong> both goal pursuit (T1)<br />

and friend’s prosocial behavior for each <strong>of</strong> the three levels <strong>of</strong><br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s behavior was a significant, positive<br />

predictor ( .29, p .002) <strong>of</strong> goal pursuit at T2 <strong>on</strong>ly when<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency was high, F(2, 72) 30.97, SE 0.41, p <br />

.0001 (see Figure 4). <strong>The</strong>refore, low frequency <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

appeared to have a significant and positive impact <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current<br />

Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 <br />

1. Sex .32*** .15* .11† .11†<br />

2. <strong>Prosocial</strong> Goal Pursuit, T1 .57*** .54*** .57***<br />

3. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial behavior (FPB), T1 .12† .05<br />

4. Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency FPB, T1 .13*<br />

Affective Quality FPB, T1 .06<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Stability FPB, T2 .09<br />

R 2<br />

.10*** .30*** .01† .02<br />

Total R 2<br />

.43***<br />

Note. n 177. <strong>The</strong> standard errors for each step are as follows: .57 (Step 1), .47 (Step 2), .46 (Step 3), and<br />

.46 (Step 4). T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />

† p .10. * p .05. *** p .001.<br />

Table 4<br />

Results <strong>of</strong> Hierarchical Regressi<strong>on</strong> Analysis <strong>on</strong> Target’s<br />

<strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong>, Time 2<br />

Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 <br />

1. Sex .43*** .21*** .20*** .20***<br />

2. Target’s prosocial<br />

behavior, T1<br />

.65*** .66*** .66***<br />

Target’s prosocial goal<br />

pursuit, T1<br />

.14** .12** .13*<br />

3. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s prosocial<br />

behavior, T1<br />

.04 .04<br />

4. Target’s prosocial goal<br />

pursuit, T2<br />

.01<br />

R 2<br />

.18*** .45*** .00 .00<br />

Total R 2<br />

.63***<br />

Note. n 206. <strong>The</strong> standard errors for each step are as follows: .16 (Step<br />

1) and .11 (Steps 2, 3, and 4). T1 Time 1; T2 Time 2.<br />

* p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.<br />

BARRY AND WENTZEL<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>s between friends’ prosocial behavior and targets’ goal<br />

pursuit, whereas high interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency appeared to have a<br />

significant and positive impact <strong>on</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>s between friends’<br />

prosocial behavior and targets’ goal pursuit over time.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

In this study, we examined the process by which a best friend<br />

might influence an individual’s prosocial behavior. As predicted, a<br />

friend’s prosocial behavior (as observed by the target individual)<br />

was related to a target’s prosocial behavior (<strong>on</strong> the basis <strong>of</strong> peer<br />

nominati<strong>on</strong>s) over time by way <strong>of</strong> a target’s pursuit <strong>of</strong> goals to be<br />

prosocial at T1. In additi<strong>on</strong>, friendship characteristics moderated<br />

the relati<strong>on</strong> between a friend’s prosocial behavior and a target’s<br />

goal pursuit such that relati<strong>on</strong>s differed as a functi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> the<br />

affective quality and frequency <strong>of</strong> interacti<strong>on</strong> between friends.<br />

<strong>The</strong>refore, our findings provide further c<strong>on</strong>firmati<strong>on</strong> that motivati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

processes can explain significant associati<strong>on</strong>s between the<br />

behaviors <strong>of</strong> two friends, highlighting the importance <strong>of</strong> friendship<br />

characteristics in defining the nature <strong>of</strong> relati<strong>on</strong>ships between a<br />

friend’s behavior and an individual’s goal pursuit. Finally, the<br />

significant gender differences in prosocial behavior and prosocial<br />

goal pursuit found in our adolescent sample extend empirical<br />

research (e.g., Parker & Asher, 1993) that has found such differences<br />

in early and middle childhood (e.g., Eisenberg & Mussen,<br />

1989), and they support theoretical c<strong>on</strong>tenti<strong>on</strong>s (see Eisenberg &<br />

Figure 2. Effect <strong>of</strong> Affective Quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>’s <strong>Prosocial</strong> <strong>Behavior</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

target’s prosocial goal pursuit at Time 1 (T1).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!