25.03.2013 Views

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

Friend Influence on Prosocial Behavior: The Role of Motivational ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>on</strong> the day <strong>of</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> (n 24), or the students themselves chose not<br />

to participate <strong>on</strong> the day <strong>of</strong> data collecti<strong>on</strong> (n 1). At both time points, the<br />

demographic characteristics <strong>of</strong> the sample were as follows: 58% female,<br />

42% male, 90% Caucasian, 6% Asian, 2% Hispanic, and 1% members <strong>of</strong><br />

other minority groups. Students who did not participate in the sec<strong>on</strong>d data<br />

collecti<strong>on</strong> because <strong>of</strong> attriti<strong>on</strong> or absence (n 21) did not differ significantly<br />

from those in the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample with respect to demographic<br />

characteristics or study variables.<br />

Procedure<br />

At each <strong>of</strong> the two data collecti<strong>on</strong>s, C.M.B. collected data from all<br />

participating 9th-grade and 10th-grade students in their 43-min social<br />

studies classes (18 classes for Time 1, 17 classes for Time 2) in the spring<br />

<strong>of</strong> each year. Data collecti<strong>on</strong> that occurs later in the school year is<br />

advantageous, because students’ familiarity with their peers is maximized.<br />

Students were assured c<strong>on</strong>fidentiality for all resp<strong>on</strong>ses and informed that<br />

they did not have to answer any questi<strong>on</strong> that they did not want to answer.<br />

<strong>The</strong> researcher remained in the classroom and clarified instructi<strong>on</strong>s as<br />

needed. Classroom teachers (N 6 at Time 1 and Time 2) remained in the<br />

classroom throughout the data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Measures<br />

All measures were administered at both time points.<br />

Background informati<strong>on</strong>. Students were asked to complete a demographic<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>naire that specified their sex (0 male, 1 female),<br />

grade level (9 or 10 for Time 1, 10 or 11 for Time 2), and ethnicity (1 <br />

Caucasian, 2 African American, 3 Asian, 4 Hispanic, 5 <br />

American Indian, 6 other).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship. Students were instructed to write the first and last names <strong>of</strong><br />

their three best friends from their high school; however, they were informed<br />

that they could choose fewer names if they so desired. Only same-sex friendships<br />

were investigated in the current study because these capture the majority<br />

<strong>of</strong> best friendships for adolescents (Hartup, 1992), and they differ in quality<br />

from opposite-sex friendships (Sharabany, Gersh<strong>on</strong>i, & H<strong>of</strong>man, 1981). Samegrade<br />

friendships were examined to maximize the interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency in<br />

these school-based relati<strong>on</strong>ships. From the names provided, reciprocated<br />

friendships were determined by matching students’ school-based best friend<br />

nominati<strong>on</strong>s with each other. At Time 1, 21% <strong>of</strong> the students had no reciprocated<br />

friendships, 34% had <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated friendship, 30% had two reciprocated<br />

friendships, and 15% had three reciprocated friendships. At Time 2,<br />

16% <strong>of</strong> the students had no reciprocated friendships, 31% had <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated<br />

friendship, 36% had two reciprocated friendships, and 17% had three reciprocated<br />

friendships. Data from students who had at least <strong>on</strong>e reciprocated<br />

friendship were used in all analyses (n 228, Time 1; n 221, Time 2); cases<br />

in which nominated best friendships were not reciprocated (n 59, Time 1;<br />

n 44, Time 2) were disregarded.<br />

All data corresp<strong>on</strong>ding to a target individual’s reciprocated friends were<br />

obtained from either the sole reciprocated friendship or a randomly selected<br />

reciprocated friend (if the target had more than <strong>on</strong>e). For all analyses, listwise<br />

deleti<strong>on</strong> was used, resulting in a final l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample <strong>of</strong> 208. Am<strong>on</strong>g the<br />

original sample <strong>of</strong> ethnic minority students at Time 1, 42% did not have a<br />

reciprocated friendship. Am<strong>on</strong>g ethnic minority students with a reciprocated<br />

friendship, <strong>on</strong>ly 4 (i.e., 29%) had a same-race friendship, and all <strong>of</strong> these were<br />

between Asian Americans. <strong>The</strong> remaining 71% <strong>of</strong> the reciprocated friendships<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g ethnic minorities were cross-race, with most (50%) <strong>of</strong> those friendships<br />

between Asian American and Caucasian students.<br />

Affective quality. Two measures were used to assess affective quality:<br />

friendship closeness and friendship importance. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship closeness was<br />

assessed with the Affective B<strong>on</strong>d and Reflected Appraisal subscales <strong>of</strong> the<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship Qualities Scale (Bukowski, Hoza, & Boivin, 1994). Students<br />

were asked to indicate how true five statements were about each <strong>of</strong> their<br />

nominated friendships <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (not true) to5(really true). Because<br />

PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR<br />

students could nominate up to three friends, three copies <strong>of</strong> this questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

were provided to each student. Students were asked to write the name<br />

<strong>of</strong> their respective friend <strong>on</strong> a line labeled <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #2, or<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

#3; each item referred to <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1, #2, or #3, respectively. A sample item<br />

from the Affective B<strong>on</strong>d subscale is “I feel happy when I am with <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

#1,” and a sample item from the Reflected Appraisal subscale is “When I<br />

do a good job at something, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1 is happy for me.” <strong>The</strong> five scores<br />

for each friend were averaged to form a single closeness score; <strong>on</strong>ly Time<br />

1 scores were used for analyses (M 4.00, SD 0.75, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s <br />

.85). According to Bukowski et al. (1994), reciprocated friendships have<br />

higher closeness scores than do unreciprocated friendships, and stable<br />

friendships over a 6-m<strong>on</strong>th period have been associated with significantly<br />

higher ( p .05) closeness scores than have unstable friendships.<br />

Affective quality was also assessed by a target individual’s perceived<br />

importance <strong>of</strong> a friendship. After students indicated the names <strong>of</strong> their three<br />

school-based best friends, they were instructed to rate each friendship <strong>on</strong> a<br />

scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (important) to3(extremely important); <strong>on</strong>ly Time 1 scores were<br />

used for analyses (M 2.48, SD 0.65). <strong>The</strong> two indicators <strong>of</strong> affective<br />

quality were related significantly (r .55, p .001), and thus, scores were<br />

averaged to form a composite (M 3.24, SD 0.62).<br />

Interacti<strong>on</strong> frequency. Students completed the Interacti<strong>on</strong> Frequency<br />

subscale <strong>of</strong> Berndt and Keefe’s friendship features questi<strong>on</strong>naire (T. J.<br />

Berndt, pers<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong>, October 1997). Students were asked to<br />

indicate how frequently they interacted with their friend by rating six items<br />

(see the Appendix) <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1 (never) to5(every day). (With Berndt’s<br />

permissi<strong>on</strong>, we made a minor modificati<strong>on</strong> to the wording <strong>of</strong> the scale such<br />

that “this friend” was replaced with “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #1,” “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g> #2,” or “<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

#3” so as to remind the students <strong>of</strong> the particular friendship about which<br />

they were answering questi<strong>on</strong>s.) <strong>The</strong> six scores for each friend were then<br />

averaged to form a single interacti<strong>on</strong>-frequency score; <strong>on</strong>ly Time 1 scores<br />

were used for analyses (M 3.44, SD 0.70, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .78).<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship stability. As noted above, the l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal sample c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>of</strong> individuals with a reciprocated friend at Time 1. <str<strong>on</strong>g>Friend</str<strong>on</strong>g>ship-stability<br />

scores reflected whether the target maintained a reciprocated friendship over<br />

time. If target’s reciprocated friendship at Time 1 was also reciprocated at<br />

Time 2, then friendship stability was coded as 3; if target’s reciprocated<br />

friendship at Time 1 was unreciprocated (but menti<strong>on</strong>ed) at Time 2, friendship<br />

stability was coded as 2; if target’s reciprocated friendship at Time 1 was not<br />

menti<strong>on</strong>ed as a friend at Time 2, then friendship stability was coded as 1. On<br />

the basis <strong>of</strong> the sample <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly those with reciprocated friendships at Time 1,<br />

the frequencies for friendship stability were as follows: 31.8% for 1, 9.8% for<br />

2, and 58.4% for 3.<br />

<strong>Prosocial</strong> goal pursuit. Students were asked to complete the <strong>Prosocial</strong><br />

Goal Pursuit scale (Wentzel, 1991), which c<strong>on</strong>sists <strong>of</strong> academic (e.g.,<br />

“How <strong>of</strong>ten do you try to share what you’ve learned with your classmates?”)<br />

and social (e.g., “How <strong>of</strong>ten do you try to cheer some<strong>on</strong>e up when<br />

something has g<strong>on</strong>e wr<strong>on</strong>g?”) subscales. Students rated <strong>on</strong> a scale <strong>of</strong> 1<br />

(rarely) to5(almost always) how <strong>of</strong>ten they tried to accomplish various<br />

prosocial behaviors. <strong>The</strong> two subscales were correlated significantly (r <br />

.46, p .001), and thus, scores were averaged to form a composite (M <br />

3.78, SD 0.56, Cr<strong>on</strong>bach’s .75).<br />

Target’s prosocial behavior. Peer nominati<strong>on</strong>s were used to assess<br />

target’s prosocial behavior (see Wentzel, 1994). Students were given three<br />

sheets <strong>of</strong> paper, with a different behavioral descripti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> each, al<strong>on</strong>g with<br />

a list <strong>of</strong> 15 randomly generated names <strong>of</strong> same-sex students from their<br />

grade. Lists <strong>of</strong> names were randomly generated to obtain independent<br />

nominati<strong>on</strong>s for each behavior, to gain the perspective <strong>of</strong> the peer group at<br />

large, and to minimize the amount <strong>of</strong> time necessary for data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

<strong>The</strong> three questi<strong>on</strong>s were as follows:<br />

1. Who cooperates and shares?<br />

2. Who helps other kids when they have a problem?<br />

3. Who shares things with you when you need something?<br />

155

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!