13.08.2013 Views

GENERATIVE CHARACTERISTICS AS ANTECEDENTS OF ...

GENERATIVE CHARACTERISTICS AS ANTECEDENTS OF ...

GENERATIVE CHARACTERISTICS AS ANTECEDENTS OF ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>GENERATIVE</strong> <strong>CHARACTERISTICS</strong> <strong>AS</strong> <strong>ANTECEDENTS</strong> <strong>OF</strong> ENGAGEMENT<br />

By<br />

Thomas H. Carlson<br />

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Quantitative Research Report<br />

in the Executive Doctor of Management Program<br />

at the Weatherhead School of Management<br />

Advisors:<br />

Richard Boland, Ph.D.<br />

Jagdip Singh, Ph.D.<br />

Antoinette Somers, Ph.D.<br />

Martin Zummersch, E.D.M.<br />

C<strong>AS</strong>E WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY<br />

March 2009


<strong>GENERATIVE</strong> <strong>CHARACTERISTICS</strong> <strong>AS</strong> <strong>ANTECEDENTS</strong> <strong>OF</strong> PERFORMANCE<br />

ABSTRACT<br />

Organizations and their members at all levels are increasingly expected to perform,<br />

adapt, and to do ‘more with less’. Positive psychology and appreciative inquiry inform us<br />

that as organizations strive to become more productive, innovative, and agile their interests<br />

and objectives are well-served by understanding that they have at-hand greater capacity for<br />

performance and learning than perhaps they had realized. Positive psychology emphasizes<br />

the role of character strengths and virtues suggesting that individual characteristics, whether<br />

dispositional or acquired, differentiate between those who are better able to contribute to<br />

organizational performance and learning and those who are less so. Appreciative inquiry<br />

emphasizes relational characteristics as key distinctions. These characteristics are generative<br />

in nature; they are pragmatic and theoretical, positive and normative. This study examines<br />

the effects of selected individual and relational characteristics on organization member<br />

psychological engagement in work performance and self-development, and offers some<br />

insights to intervention design. Analysis of data captured from a not-for-profit healthcare<br />

organization suggests that: (1) individual characteristics have limited and negative effects on<br />

psychological engagement, (2) relational characteristics have positive effects on<br />

psychological engagement, and (3) women and men use distinctly different mechanisms to<br />

translate individual and relational characteristics into psychological engagement. This paper<br />

addresses these relationships in one organization at one point in time and therefore readers<br />

should be cautious in generalizing the results.<br />

Key words: Engagement, shared values, peer trust, leadership, gratitude, generative.<br />

2


TABLE <strong>OF</strong> CONTENTS<br />

Abstract..................................................................................................................................... 2<br />

Introduction............................................................................................................................... 4<br />

Theory and Hypotheses Development...................................................................................... 6<br />

Research Method .................................................................................................................... 12<br />

Analysis and Findings............................................................................................................. 22<br />

Discussion............................................................................................................................... 26<br />

Limitations .............................................................................................................................. 29<br />

Implications............................................................................................................................. 30<br />

Conclusions............................................................................................................................. 31<br />

Appendices<br />

Appendix A: Survey Instrument Items and Sources.................................................. 32<br />

Appendix B: Item Correlations and Descriptive Statistics: Pearson Correlations.... 35<br />

Appendix C: Exploratory Factor Analysis................................................................. 36<br />

Appendix D: Final CFA Measurement Model........................................................... 37<br />

Appendix E: Analysis of Method Bias ...................................................................... 38<br />

Appendix F: Female Engagement.............................................................................. 39<br />

Appendix G: Male Engagement................................................................................. 40<br />

References............................................................................................................................... 41<br />

List of Figures<br />

Figure 1: Conceptual Model ...................................................................................... 12<br />

Figure 2: Hypothesized Model Constrained for Method Bias................................... 18<br />

List of Tables<br />

Table 1: Data Collection Sources .............................................................................. 13<br />

Table 2: Description of Respondent Profile............................................................... 14<br />

Table 3: Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations among the Constructs:<br />

Pearson Correlations .................................................................................... 16<br />

Table 4: Analysis of Convergent and Discriminate Validity: Unconstrained .......... 17<br />

Table 5: Factor Loadings and Measurement Properties Derived from CMV-<br />

Constrained CFA Model.............................................................................. 20<br />

Table 6: Construct Reliability Comparison ............................................................... 21<br />

Table 7: Mediation Tests: Mathieu & Taylor (2006)................................................. 22<br />

Table 8: Hypothesis Test Results............................................................................... 23<br />

Table 9: Estimated Coefficients and Nomological Relationships for the Female<br />

and Male Models.......................................................................................... 26<br />

3


INTRODUCTION<br />

“The twentieth-century organization has not operated well in a rapidly changing<br />

environment. Structure, systems, practices and culture have often been more of a drag on<br />

change than a facilitator” (Kotter, 1996: 161). The ability to maintain competitive, and even<br />

achieve superior, levels of performance and learning are necessary organizational<br />

competencies. Bushe and Kassam (2005) and others have observed that a significant majority<br />

of change and adaptation efforts (estimated as 70% to 80%) fail to meet expectation.<br />

Neither methodology nor technology has offered effective solutions to this<br />

fundamental challenge. Perhaps, neither is the issue. The underlying issue is the organization<br />

itself; its socially constructed, culturally embedded, and unexamined norms of accepted<br />

behavior, interaction and practice which reproduce the conditions that frustrate their efforts<br />

to learn and perform.<br />

The fundamental curiosity which motivates this paper is exploration of the antecedent<br />

characteristics which facilitate psychological engagement in work performance and self-<br />

development. The intent is to provide insight into design and conduct of interventions which<br />

result in alternative forms of practice, organizational learning and improved performance.<br />

Traditional interventions are framed as clarifying and improving ‘who does what,<br />

when, and how’. Thinking and action are focused on operational aspects organization<br />

function; job description, role interpretation, process and procedure, monitoring, metrics, and<br />

training. These traditional approaches fail to ‘fix’ the root-cause problem.<br />

The underlying problem is that the organization lacks the internal vitality to function<br />

effectively (Whitney & Trosten-Bloom, 2003). It is not a purposeful community of practice<br />

(Wenger, 1998). It lacks a pervasive sense of shared values, beliefs and visions (Amis, Slack,<br />

4


& Hinings, 2002), and trust in peers and leadership (Maister, Green, & Galford, 2000;<br />

McEvily, Perrone, & Zaheer, 2003) . It neglects the importance of relationship and fails to<br />

develop heightened levels productive interaction, and quality communications (Dutton &<br />

Heaphy, 2003; Gittell, 2003) which are the collaborative foundation for organizational<br />

learning (Argyris, 1982; Barrett, 1995; Berger & Luckmann, 1966; Bohm, 1996; Friedlander,<br />

1983; Hansen, 1999; Hansen, Mors, & Lovas, 2005)., and higher performance (Ouchi &<br />

Johnson,1978; Arthur, 1994; Ichniowski & Shaw, 1999). Group alignment of this type<br />

provides the basis for focused collective participation in pursuit of common objectives.<br />

Appreciative Inquiry is offered as a positive and even revolutionary theory of change<br />

(Bushe, 1995; Cooperrider & Whitney, 2000b; Srivastva & Cooperrider, 1990). Advocates of<br />

AI attribute its effectiveness to: creation and use of positive images (Cooperrider, 2000;<br />

Srivastva & Cooperrider, 1990; Weick, 2003) and generative questions (Adams, Schiller, &<br />

Cooperrider, 2004), identifying and building on strength (Clifton & Harter, 2003;<br />

Cooperrider, Sorensen, Yaeger, & Whitney, 2001), establishing perspective of abundance<br />

rather than deficiency (Cooperrider & Sekerka, 2003), and focusing on possibilities rather<br />

than problems (Luechauer, 2000; Thatchenkery, 2004).<br />

Research in positive psychology also provides supports the principles and<br />

perspectives of appreciative inquiry; ‘positive spirals’ (Fredrickson, 2003), broaden and build<br />

(Fredrickson, 2005) character strengths and virtues (Peterson & Park, 2004; Peterson &<br />

Seligman, 2003; Peterson, 2004), building and strengths (Cameron, 2003; Clifton & Harter,<br />

2003; Clifton & Hodges, 2004) and the role of constructive discourse (Cooperrider & Avital,<br />

2004).<br />

Testimony to the effectiveness of AI-based interventions is becoming more prevalent<br />

5


(Fry, Barrett, Seiling, & Whitney, 2002; Prowley, Fry, Barrett, & Bright, 2004; Srivastva &<br />

Cooperrider, 1990). However, testimony is not research. It isn’t surprising that Appreciative<br />

Inquiry has its critics. AI has been characterized as failing to provide linkages to other theory<br />

and practice (Golembiewski, 2001), as being based more on speculation and enthusiasm than<br />

empirical research (Head, 1997), and as having limited and only specific applicability (Bushe<br />

& Coetzer, 1995).<br />

The following sections of this paper discuss theories and constructs used to frame the<br />

research question, describe the research design, methodology, data collection, and analysis;<br />

present the findings as pre-intervention and post-intervention comparisons, and discuss those<br />

findings and their implications for both future research and practice.<br />

THEORY AND HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT<br />

This section discusses the constructs upon which this study is based. This research<br />

accepts that organizations are the result of ongoing social construction and attempts to inform<br />

our understanding of how individual and shared generative characteristics influence<br />

employee engagement in work performance and self-development.<br />

Engagement<br />

The fundamental interest in this study is to inform our understanding of the individual<br />

and relational generative characteristics which are, or could be, antecedents to the ways in<br />

which work group members engage themselves in the performance of their work and their<br />

continued development. This is a somewhat different perspective than those of role<br />

engagement, task engagement, or organizational commitment. Rather, the interest here is in<br />

why group members decide to bring their real and whole ‘self’ to work and dedicate their<br />

attention and energy to achieving higher levels of performance and to the development of<br />

6


subject knowledge and craft skill (Kahn, 1990; Kahn, Cross, & Parker, 2003). That<br />

dedication is expressed as awareness (Seligman, 2002), heightened concentration and<br />

cognitive processes (Seligman, 2005), positive affectivity and positive emotional states<br />

(Seligman, 1990; Fredrickson, 2005), and greater productivity (Harter, Schmidt, & Hayes,<br />

2002; Clifton & Harter, 2003). The constructs used here, engaged performance and engaged<br />

development, are subsets of the thirteen-item scale developed by Buckingham and Coffman<br />

(1999) and later used by Harter (Harter et al., 2002). This study uses those constructs to<br />

capture the influences of antecedent individual and relational generative characteristics.<br />

Generative characteristics are attributes of individuals and groups that capture the<br />

best of ‘what is’ (pragmatic), and provoke collective projection and visioning of the best of<br />

‘what might be’ (theoretical), ‘what can be’ (positive), and ‘what should be’ (normative).<br />

Generative characteristics promote dialogue, collaboration, collective experimentation, and<br />

seek practical actionable knowledge (Cooperrider, 2000; Cooperrider & Sekerka, 2003;<br />

Watkins & Mohr, 2001).<br />

Relational Generative Characteristics<br />

The body of literature which explores commitment and group alignment is extensive.<br />

The interest here is in the perspectives drawn from Appreciative Inquiry and Positive<br />

Psychology. Both accept the underlying premise that both social and practice norms are<br />

socially constructed. Social construction is a process of continuous negotiation and change in<br />

which identity, meaning, sense of group and individual belonging are formed and maintained<br />

(Berger & Luckmann, 1966; Wenger, 1998; Gergen & Thatchenkery, 2004). Essential to<br />

these notions are identification, articulation, and enactment of shared values, and<br />

development of trust in others, both peers and leadership. Shared values provide the<br />

7


fundamental connection of commonality and ‘community’ among group members (Amis,<br />

Slack, & Hinings, 2002; Kahn et al., 2003; Wrzesniewski, 2003). Shared values and meaning<br />

provide the basis for articulation of the vision of positive futures, shared goals and objectives,<br />

and positive perspectives regarding strengths, abundance, and possibility (Cameron, 2003;<br />

Clifton & Harter, 2003; Cooperrider, 2000; Fredrickson, 2003).<br />

Peer trust provides the basis for group members to interconnect and interact in ways<br />

which are open, authentic, and free of undo hesitation or condition (Maister et al., 2000;<br />

McEvily et al., 2003; Whitney & Trosten-Bloom, 2003). It is an essential, if implicit, element<br />

of psychological safety (Edmondson, 1999) which has a positive influence on interpersonal<br />

risk taking, learning, and performance.<br />

Other qualitative research and field reports (Fry et al., 2002) provide support to these<br />

notions and to the power of their interaction. Further, qualitative research previously<br />

conducted within the organization, which is the subject of this study, indicated that<br />

participants of a newly formed group actively sought to form and deliberately articulated a<br />

‘manifesto’ of shared values, articulated their vision of how they would work together in<br />

pursuit of their objectives, and consciously and publicly agreed to enact behaviors which<br />

fostered trust and psychological safety. The effect of their effort was dramatically increased<br />

information sharing, knowledge sharing, learning, and performance (Carlson, 2007).<br />

These understandings lead to the first four hypotheses:<br />

Hypothesis 1. Shared values have a direct and positive influence on engaged<br />

performance.<br />

Hypothesis 2. Shared values have a direct and positive influence on engaged<br />

development.<br />

Hypothesis 3. Peer trust has a direct and positive influence on engaged performance.<br />

8


Hypothesis 4. Peer trust has a direct and positive influence on engaged development.<br />

Individual Generative Characteristics<br />

Individual generative characteristics are traits that are viewed by many researchers in<br />

AI and positive psychology as essential to effective personal interaction and relationship<br />

development (Barrett, Thomas, & Hocevar, 1995; Bohm, 1996; Cooperrider & Srivastva,<br />

2000a). The specific individual traits chosen for inclusion in this study are communicative<br />

adaptability (Duran, 1983) and gratitude (Emmons, 2003; Emmons, McCullough, & Bono,<br />

2004; Peterson & Seligman, 2003).<br />

Communicative adaptability is represented by the social experience dimension of the<br />

original construct (Duran, 1983). It is intended to measure respondent ability to perceive<br />

social cues and circumstances, and adapt interaction goals and behaviors accordingly. As<br />

such it provides insight into group members’ perceived and self-assessed ability to accurately<br />

sense and appropriately reconcile tension which exists among self, others, situation, and<br />

objective. It measures respondent comfort and facility at ‘fitting in’ and functioning<br />

harmoniously as part of a group. It was selected because it is a learned skill, an acquired trait,<br />

that reflects to some degree respondent social and emotional intelligence (Boyatzis & McKee,<br />

2005; Goleman, 1995).<br />

Gratitude is a relatively recently developed construct which derives from research in<br />

positive psychology. Regarded as a dispositional trait (Emmons, 2003; Emmons & Shelton,<br />

2005a), gratitude is the ‘generalized tendency to recognize and respond with positive<br />

emotions to others’ benevolence’ (Lopez & Snyder; 2005). It is conceptualized as both<br />

conscious and active (Emmons, McCullough, & Tsang, 2005b; Emmons et al., 2004). The<br />

notion of gratitude is particularly interesting for two reasons. It is regarded by some as the<br />

9


core virtue from which others stem (Peterson & Seligman, 2003). The more practical reason,<br />

and the one which applies most strongly here, is that research has shown that gratitude has<br />

strong positive correlations with other character strengths and dispositional attributes which<br />

are generally viewed as positive and therefore desirable in oneself and in other group<br />

members (Biswas-Diener & Dean, 2007). Among the attributes with which gratitude is<br />

highly correlated are altruism and generosity, hope, helping, sharing, empathy, life<br />

satisfaction, and positive emotional affect. For that reason gratitude, to a degree, serves as<br />

proxy for that set of character traits and associated behaviors. These models suggest the<br />

following hypotheses:<br />

Hypothesis 5. Communicative Adaptability has a direct and positive effect on<br />

engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 6. Communicative Adaptability has a direct and positive effect on<br />

engaged development.<br />

Hypothesis 7. Gratitude has a direct and positive effect on engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 8. Gratitude has a direct and positive effect on engaged development.<br />

Trust in Leadership<br />

Numerous and recent studies have sought to inform our understanding of the role of<br />

leaders and leadership in organizations. Shein (2004) speaks to the role of leader and<br />

leadership in creating and maintaining culture. Others speak to the leaders’ role in creating<br />

inspirational vision and messages of hope and possibility which galvanized the thinking and<br />

efforts of the work group (Boyatzis & McKee, 2005; Goleman, Boyatzis & McKee, 2002;<br />

Cooperrider, 2000; Srivastva & Cooperrider, 1990). McEvily, Perrone, & Zaheer (2003)<br />

discuss the importance of trust as a shared and valued organizing and leadership principle.<br />

Regardless the perspective, leaders and leadership are evaluated by the work group in large<br />

10


part based on perceptions and observations of behaviors seen as authentic; caring about,<br />

listening to, and including the work group in their thinking, planning, and action. For those<br />

reasons the construct which represents trust in leadership is procedural justice (Moorman,<br />

1991). The construct overall addresses very fundamental group member questions about<br />

inclusion and confidence. As such trust in leadership is conceptualized as having a<br />

moderating effect on the direct relationships to engagement, and that provides the basis for<br />

the final hypotheses:<br />

Hypothesis 9. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between shared values and engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 10. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between shared values and engaged development.<br />

Hypothesis 11. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between peer trust and engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 12. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between peer trust and engaged development.<br />

Hypothesis 13. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between communicative adaptability and engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 14. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between communicative adaptability and engaged development.<br />

Hypothesis 15. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between gratitude and engaged performance.<br />

Hypothesis 16. Trust in leadership has a partial mediating effect on the relationship<br />

between gratitude and engaged development.<br />

Control Variables<br />

Based on similar studies in the literature and prior research in the same organization<br />

(Carlson, 2007) tenure and gender were used as control variables. Their inclusion leads to the<br />

final hypotheses.<br />

11


Hypothesis 17. Tenure moderates the effects of antecedent variables on both engaged<br />

performance and engaged development.<br />

Hypothesis 18. Gender moderates the effects of antecedent variables on both<br />

engaged performance and engaged development.<br />

The conceptual model is represented in Figure 1 below.<br />

Peer Trust<br />

Shared Values<br />

Communicative<br />

Adaptability<br />

Gratitude<br />

Setting and Sample<br />

Leadership Trust<br />

FIGURE 1:<br />

Conceptual Model<br />

Gender Tenure<br />

RESEARCH METHOD<br />

Engaged<br />

Performance<br />

Engaged<br />

Development<br />

This study was conducted in cooperation with the medical operations division of a<br />

not-for-profit health care organization. Areas which participated in the data collection<br />

include: the Clinical Leadership group which is comprised of directors, managers, physicians,<br />

and other key clinical staff from across the division; pharmacists and technicians from four<br />

large pharmacy locations; physicians, nurses and other patient care providers from each of<br />

the Specialty Care departments; and the applications development staff from the information<br />

technology group which provides support to the medical operation division. A total of 316<br />

12


esponses were received. Appendix A illustrates the demographic characteristics of the<br />

respondents.<br />

Data Collection and Preparation<br />

Development of the survey instrument was guided by the results of a previous<br />

qualitative study conducted within the same organization, prior review of relevant literature,<br />

and selected previously validated constructs. The instrument was tested in ‘walk through’<br />

sessions to ensure the appropriateness of the questions.<br />

In order to mitigate the likelihood of method error the survey instrument was<br />

administered in three different modes and at different times (Babbie, 2007). Table 1 below<br />

summarizes the data collection sources.<br />

TABLE 1:<br />

Data Collection Sources<br />

Population Online Meetings Mail % Participation<br />

Clinical leadership 176 134 76.1<br />

Other Clinicians 190 53 61 60.0<br />

Pharmacy 72 33 45.9<br />

Info. Tech. 36 35 97.2<br />

Total 474 134 88 94 66.7<br />

The Clinical Leadership Group received qualified invitations and the necessary link via email.<br />

Those choosing to participate completed the survey online (SurveyMonkey). A total of 134<br />

responses were received (102 in the first wave, 32 in the second) from a population of 176<br />

(76.1%). In the cases of the IT group and four clinical departments (Neurology, G/I, Podiatry<br />

and Orthopedics) potential participants were invited, hardcopy survey instruments distributed,<br />

completed and collected during regularly scheduled monthly meetings. From the IT group 35<br />

responses were received from 36 attendees (97.2%; four staff members were not in<br />

attendance). From the clinical groups 53 responses were received from 55 attendees (96.4%;<br />

13


in total 12 staff were not in attendance). The remainder of the clinical staff (including the 12<br />

not in attendance at the monthly meetings), as well as staff members in the pharmacy units<br />

received invitations, hardcopy survey instruments, and stamped return envelops by mail. A<br />

total of 61 responses were received from the remaining clinical population of 135 (45.2%).<br />

From the pharmacy units a total of 33 responses were received from a population of 72<br />

(44.4%). Table 2 below describes the respondent profile.<br />

TABLE 2:<br />

Description of Respondent Profile<br />

Age Gender Tenure<br />

29 or younger 2.85 Female 75.63 2 yrs. or less 16.14<br />

30 - 39 13.29 Male 23.10 3 to 5 yrs. 12.03<br />

40 -49 30.39 Missing 1.27 6 to 10 yrs. 14.87<br />

50 and older 51.59 >10 yrs. 55.70<br />

Missing 1.90 Missing 1.27<br />

Fifty-seven of the 316 responses had missing data; two of those (both online<br />

responses) were unusable at any level of analysis. Subsequently eight were removed as<br />

outliers and influentials.<br />

In order to minimize the likelihood of measurement error all questions were<br />

converted to 7-point Likert scales, reverse coding of some items was eliminated, and the data<br />

were collected in different forms and forums (Babbie, 2007)The response for each item was<br />

anchored by strongly disagree, neutral, and strongly agree. Sampling error was controlled by<br />

question structure and question order. Pre-testing of the survey instrument identified possible<br />

instances of difficulty in question interpretation; question wording and length were modified<br />

and retested (Babbie, 2007).<br />

Variables and Measurement<br />

All the constructs and items used in the study were taken from existing validated<br />

14


scales (refer to Appendix A). The construct for engagement was adapted from Buckingham<br />

& Coffman (1999) and Harter, Schmidt & Hayes (2002). Those authors note that this<br />

thirteen-item construct can be multidimensional and is sensitive to the context in which it’s<br />

used. The interest in this study is in the influence of individual and relational generative<br />

traits on employee engagement in the performance of their work and in self-development. All<br />

thirteen items were included in the initial analysis and were reduced to three for the<br />

dependent variable engaged performance. The values alignment (Cable & Judge, 1996),<br />

vision alignment (O'Reilly & Chatman, 1986), and peer trust (Dunham & Smith, 1979)<br />

constructs were selected and adapted for use in this study and are viewed here as the basis for<br />

derivation of socially constructed belief mechanisms, commitment, cohesion, development<br />

and acceptance of shared objectives, and collective action.<br />

The constructs communicative adaptability (Duran, 1983) and gratitude (Emmons,<br />

McCullough, & Tsang, 2005; Emmons & Shelton, 2005; Emmons, McCullough, & Bono,<br />

2004) were selected for use as representative of individual generative characteristics. The<br />

first is a learned personal skill which embodies cognitive, social, and emotional awareness<br />

and agility; the ability to perceive and interpret interpersonal and situational cues and<br />

successfully ‘fit’ personal conversational style to the environment and participants. Gratitude<br />

represents individual character strength and ‘virtuous behavior’ (Peterson & Seligman, 2004).<br />

Gratitude is used in this study due to its theoretical and practical alignment with the<br />

principles of appreciative inquiry and positive psychology, and due to its strong positive<br />

correlation with other character strengths and behaviors which include helping, sharing,<br />

giving, positive emotional affect, and satisfaction with life (Biswas-Deiner & Dean, 2007).<br />

The construct used to measure Leadership Trust is that of Procedural Justice;<br />

15


(Moorman, 1999) which was selected because it captures the notions of inclusion and<br />

confidence; ‘Am I heard?’, ‘Do they care?’, ‘Can I rely on their processes and judgment?’<br />

Together these constructs form a model which is intended to inform our<br />

understanding of individual and relational factors which influence the formation of employee<br />

engagement in work performance and self-development.<br />

Data Analysis<br />

The data were analyzed using SPSS and AMOS for exploratory factor analysis (EFA)<br />

and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).<br />

Seven factors emerged through exploratory factor analysis (refer to Appendix C).<br />

Some of the constructs included in the original conceptualization of the model were trimmed<br />

as the result of persistent crossloading and lack of convergence within the context of the<br />

other constructs. Some items comprising the remaining seven constructs were trimmed (those<br />

with loadings < 0.50) in order to improve the fit of the model. Intercorrelations and<br />

descriptive statistics among the items are illustrated in Appendix B. Table 3 below provides<br />

descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among the constructs used in the final model.<br />

TABLE 3:<br />

Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations among the Constructs:<br />

Pearson Correlations<br />

Mean Std. Dev. CommAdapt Gratitude PeerTrust SharedValues LeaderTrust EngagedPerf EngagedDev<br />

CommAdapt 5.94 1.016 1<br />

Gratitude 6.27 .824 .510** 1<br />

PeerTrust 5.27 1.111 .356** .272** 1<br />

SharedValues 5.41 1.089 .373** .432** .552** 1<br />

LeaderTrust 4.46 1.384 .263** .297** .467** .636** 1<br />

EngagedPerf 5.69 1.044 .263** .399** .467** .527** .449** 1<br />

EngagedDev 4.78 1.626 .142* .172** .417** .439** .546** .421** 1<br />

** - Correlations significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed)<br />

* - Correlations significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed)<br />

16


Adequacy of the model was established with confirmatory factor analysis in AMOS<br />

(refer to Appendix D) by assessing goodness of fit statistics, and individual construct<br />

reliabilities as well as the convergent validity of the individual constructs, and the<br />

discriminate validity among the constructs. Convergent validity of the constructs is assessed<br />

using the composite reliability scale and average variance extracted. Results from<br />

examination of the adequacy of the model are presented in Table 4 below.<br />

Constructs/Items<br />

TABLE 4:<br />

Analysis of Convergent and Discriminate Validity:<br />

Unconstrained<br />

Loading<br />

(Standatrized) P-Value<br />

Composite<br />

Reliability<br />

Average<br />

Variance<br />

Extracted<br />

Maximum<br />

Variance<br />

Shared<br />

Average<br />

Shared<br />

Variance<br />

Commuticative Adaptability 0.872 0.631 0.356 0.148<br />

C2 0.884 < 0.001<br />

C3 0.818 < 0.001<br />

C4 0.789 < 0.001<br />

C5 0.715 < 0.001<br />

Gratitude 0.856 0.600 0.356 0.173<br />

G1 0.83 < 0.001<br />

G2 0.824 < 0.001<br />

G3 0.623 < 0.001<br />

G4 0.755 < 0.001<br />

G6 0.675 < 0.001<br />

Leadership Trust 0.914 0.679 0.486 0.285<br />

L1 0.893 < 0.001<br />

L2 0.812 < 0.001<br />

L3 0.761 < 0.001<br />

L4 0.829 < 0.001<br />

L5 0.817 < 0.001<br />

Peer Trust<br />

0.819 0.533 0.368 0.259<br />

P1 0.711 < 0.001<br />

P2 0.728 < 0.001<br />

P3 0.832 < 0.001<br />

P4 0.631 < 0.001<br />

Shared Values 0.929 0.653 0.486 0.308<br />

Val1 0.835 < 0.001<br />

Val3 0.834 < 0.001<br />

Vis1 0.719 < 0.001<br />

Vis2 0.715 < 0.001<br />

Vis3 0.722 < 0.001<br />

Vis5 0.898 < 0.001<br />

Vis6 0.772 < 0.001<br />

Vis7 0.778 < 0.001<br />

Vis8 0.757 < 0.001<br />

Engaged Performance 0.759 0.517 0.377 0.267<br />

E2 0.577 < 0.001<br />

E3 0.743 < 0.001<br />

E4 0.815 < 0.001<br />

Engaged Development 0.780 0.543 0.440 `.232<br />

E5 0.715 < 0.001<br />

E7 0.777 < 0.001<br />

E12<br />

Goodness of Fit Statistics<br />

0.712 < 0.001<br />

Chi-Sq. (df) 808.1 (437)<br />

NFI 0.851<br />

IFI 0.926<br />

TLI 0.915<br />

CFI 0.925<br />

RMSEA I 0.058<br />

90% CI .052 - .065<br />

SRMR 0.052<br />

17


Further analysis the data demonstrated strong and significant method variance (CMV).<br />

Following Podsakoff (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Podsakoff, & Lee, 2003) a model using a<br />

single latent method factor to control for unmeasured effects (refer to Appendix E) is used to<br />

separate the variance of the responses into three components; trait, method, and random error.<br />

The presence of significant method variance is attributed to the method of measurement<br />

rather than the constructs themselves, and reflected in the correlations among the items and<br />

among the constructs. Further, that bias complicates interpretation of the data, and increases<br />

the likelihood of misleading conclusions (Type I error). The hypothesized model was<br />

constrained for common method variance (refer to Figure 2 below).<br />

d7<br />

d8<br />

d9<br />

d10<br />

d11<br />

d12<br />

d13<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d22<br />

1<br />

d23<br />

1<br />

d24<br />

1<br />

d25<br />

1<br />

d14<br />

1<br />

d15<br />

1<br />

d16<br />

1<br />

d17<br />

d18<br />

d19<br />

d20<br />

d21<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

ZVAL1<br />

ZVAL3<br />

ZVIS1<br />

ZVIS3<br />

FIGURE 2:<br />

Hypothesized Model Constrained for Method Bias<br />

ZVIS5<br />

ZVIS6<br />

ZVIS7<br />

ZP1<br />

ZP2<br />

ZP3<br />

ZP4<br />

ZC2<br />

ZC3<br />

ZC4<br />

ZC5<br />

ZG1<br />

ZG2<br />

ZG4<br />

ZG6<br />

SharedValues<br />

PeerTrust<br />

CommAdapt<br />

LeadershipTrust<br />

Gratitude<br />

Hypothesized Model<br />

ZL1<br />

1<br />

d26<br />

ZL2<br />

1<br />

Common<br />

Method<br />

Variance<br />

ZL3<br />

1<br />

d27 d28<br />

EngagedPerf<br />

EngagedDev<br />

ZL4<br />

1<br />

d29<br />

ZL5<br />

1<br />

d30<br />

ZE2<br />

ZE3<br />

ZE4<br />

ZE5<br />

ZE7<br />

ZE12<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d1<br />

d2<br />

d3<br />

d4<br />

d5<br />

d6<br />

18


Estimated Coefficients and Nomological Relationships in the<br />

Hypothesized Model<br />

Dependent Variable<br />

Hypothesized<br />

Independent Variable<br />

Model<br />

Coefficient<br />

t-Value R 2<br />

Leadership Trust (LT) .392<br />

Shared Values -> LT .554*** 6.236<br />

Peer Trust -> LT .239** 3.255<br />

Communicative Adaptability -<br />

> LT<br />

.008 .091<br />

Gratitude -> LT .083 .871<br />

Engaged Performance (EP) .241<br />

Shared Values -> EP .013 .115<br />

Peer Trust -> EP .009 .130<br />

Communicative Adaptability - -.316*** -3.338<br />

> EP<br />

Gratitude -> EP -.082 -.844<br />

Leadership Trust -> EP .243 1.827<br />

Engaged Development (ED) .458<br />

Shared Values -> ED -.147 -1.334<br />

Peer Trust -> ED .095 1.425<br />

Communicative Adaptability - -.249** 2.860<br />

> ED<br />

Gratitude ->ED -.241* -2.491<br />

Leadership Trust -> ED .540*** 4.050<br />

Goodness of Fit Statistics<br />

Chi-sq. (df) 683.9 (379)<br />

NFI .865<br />

IFI .935<br />

TLI .924<br />

CFI .934<br />

SMSR .070<br />

RMSEA .057<br />

90% CI .050 - .063<br />

Note: Correlation significance: *** = 0.001; ** = 0.01; * = 0.05.<br />

The previous analysis of adequacy, goodness of fit, convergent validity, and<br />

discriminate validity were rerun using results from the constrained model. Results of that<br />

analysis are presented in Table 5.<br />

19


Constructs/Items<br />

TABLE 5:<br />

Factor Loadings and Measurement Properties Derived from<br />

CMV-Constrained CFA Model<br />

Average Maximum Average<br />

Loading Variance Variance Shared<br />

(Standardized) P-Value Reliability Extracted Shared Variance<br />

Commuticative Adaptability 0.744 0.427 0.308 0.109<br />

C2 0.627 < 0.001<br />

C3 0.557 < 0.001<br />

C4 0.409 < 0.001<br />

C5 0.459 < 0.001<br />

Gratitude 0.650 0.321 0.309 0.118<br />

G1 0.593 < 0.001<br />

G2 0.399 < 0.001<br />

G4 0.412 < 0.001<br />

G6 0.401 < 0.001<br />

Leadership Trust 0.885 0.606 0.276 0.130<br />

L1 0.766 < 0.001<br />

L2 0.702 < 0.001<br />

L3 0.613 < 0.001<br />

L4 0.67 < 0.001<br />

L5 0.73 < 0.001<br />

Peer Trust<br />

0.710 0.387 0.216 0.088<br />

P1 0.54 < 0.001<br />

P2 0.517 < 0.001<br />

P3 0.671 < 0.001<br />

P4 0.401 < 0.001<br />

Shared Values 0.878 0.517 0.276 0.087<br />

Val1 0.616 < 0.001<br />

Val3 0.698 < 0.001<br />

Vis1 0.376 < 0.001<br />

Vis3 0.518 < 0.001<br />

Vis5 0.715 < 0.001<br />

Vis6 0.677 < 0.001<br />

Vis7 0.535 < 0.001<br />

Engaged Performance 0.572 0.322 0.308 0.101<br />

E2 0.298 < 0.001<br />

E3 0.505 < 0.001<br />

E4 0.58 < 0.001<br />

Engaged Development 0.711 0.452 0.309 0.167<br />

E5 0.648 < 0.001<br />

E7 0.618 < 0.001<br />

E12 0.564 < 0.001<br />

Goodness of Fit Statistics<br />

Chi-Sq. (df) 726.0 (398)<br />

NFI .857<br />

IFI .927<br />

TLI .916<br />

CFI .926<br />

RMSEA .060<br />

90% CI .053 - .066<br />

SRMR .086<br />

The bias-corrected model produced construct reliability, AVE, MVS, and <strong>AS</strong>V results which<br />

were consistently lower than those produced by the unconstrained model. Composite scale<br />

reliability for five of the constructs range from .710 to .875, exceeding the recommended<br />

20


cutoff value (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). The exceptions are gratitude (.650) and engaged<br />

performance (.572). Average variance extracted ranges from .321 to .606 and fell below the<br />

recommended threshold of .50 (reference) for all but two of the constructs, however in all<br />

instances average variance extracted is greater than maximum variance shared and average<br />

shared variance.<br />

The constrained CFA model achieved acceptable goodness of measures (CFI > 0.90;<br />

SRMR < 0.90; RMSEA < .060), indicating that the model provided a good representation of<br />

the variance-covariance nature of the measures. All seven constructs which comprise the<br />

final model demonstrate adequate convergent and discriminate validity. Overall, the results<br />

of CFA specify that the modeled constructs appear to have adequate explanatory properties<br />

for analysis and interpretation using Structured Equation Modeling (SEM). Table 6<br />

compares the reliability of each construct from the source article, prior to CMV correction,<br />

and after CMV correction.<br />

TABLE 6:<br />

Construct Reliability Comparison<br />

Construct Reliability Measures<br />

Construct: Source With CMB Corrected<br />

Communicative Adaptability .82 .872 .774<br />

Gratitude .82 .856 .650<br />

Leadership Trust .94 .914 .885<br />

Peer Trust .83 .819 .710<br />

Shares Values .86-.91 .929 .878<br />

Engaged Performance .83-.91* .759 .572<br />

Engaged Development .83-.91* .780 .711<br />

* Based on original 13 item scale<br />

The presence of the embedded common method variance anomaly required the<br />

correction mechanism to be carried forward throughout the remainder of the analysis.<br />

21


ANALYSIS AND FINDINGS<br />

A structural model constrained for method bias was specified in AMOS to test the<br />

hypothesized model and assess its overall fit. SEM is the preferred technique for analyzing<br />

models with latent variables and possible measurement error, and is an effective means of<br />

testing simultaneous equations. The hypothesized model achieved reasonable overall fit<br />

indices.<br />

Mediation Analysis<br />

Mediation analysis, guided by Mathieu & Taylor (2006), was conducted to test the<br />

influence of Leadership Trust on the relationships between the independent variables and<br />

engaged performance and engaged development; hypotheses 9 through 16. Results of testing<br />

are presented in Table 7. Only H16: Trust in leadership has a mediating effect on the<br />

relationship between gratitude and engaged development, was supported by the indication of<br />

partial mediation. All others, H8 through H15 were not supported.<br />

TABLE 7:<br />

Mediation Tests: Mathieu & Taylor (2006)<br />

Hypothesis Mediation Path<br />

Hypothesized<br />

Effect p‐Value Indication<br />

H9 SV‐>LT‐>EPerf Partial<br />

βyx NS<br />

βyx →βmx NS Conclude Null<br />

H10 SV‐>LT‐>EDev Partial<br />

βmx →βym NS<br />

βyx *** Conclude Indirect<br />

H11 PT‐>LT‐>EPerf Partial<br />

βmx →βym NS<br />

βyx NS Conclude Null<br />

H12 PT‐>LT‐>EDev Partial<br />

βyx NS<br />

βyx →βmx *** Conclude Indierct<br />

H13 CA‐>LT‐>EPerf Partial<br />

βmx →βym **<br />

βyx NS Conclude Direct<br />

H14 CA‐>LT‐>EDev Partial<br />

βyx **<br />

βyx →βmx NS Conclude Direct<br />

H15 Grat‐>LT‐>EPerf Partial<br />

βyx NS<br />

βyx →βmx NS Conclude Null<br />

H16 Grat‐>LT‐>EDev Partial<br />

βmx →βym **<br />

βyx ** Conclude Partial<br />

22


The re-specified model produced overall goodness of fit indices which were virtually<br />

identical (SRMR = .070, NFI = .864, IFI = .935, TLI = .925, CFI = .934, and RMSEA<br />

= .056) to those of the hypothesized model.<br />

Moderation Analysis<br />

Moderation testing revealed that tenure did not significantly influence the<br />

relationships of the antecedent variables on either engaged performance or engaged<br />

development; H17 is disconfirmed. However the moderating effect of gender was significant,<br />

indicating invariance between female and male respondents, and required further analysis;<br />

H18 is supported.<br />

The effects of gender were examined by running separate SEM analysis for each<br />

group (Byrne & Stewart, 2006; Podsakoff et al., 2003). Results of hypothesis testing are<br />

presented in Table 8.<br />

TABLE 8:<br />

Hypothesis Test Results<br />

Hypothesis:<br />

Result<br />

Female Male<br />

H1: SV=>EP direct and positive Supported Disconfirmed<br />

H2: SV=>ED direct and positive Disconfirmed Supported<br />

H3: PT=> EP direct and positive Disconfirmed Supported<br />

H4: PT=>ED direct and positive Supported Disconfirmed<br />

H5: CA=>EP direct and positive Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H6: CA=>ED direct and positive Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H7: G=>EP direct and positive Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H8: G=>ED direct and positive Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H9: SV=>LT=>EP partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H10: SV=>LT=>ED partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H11: PT=>LT=>EP partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H12: PT=>LT=>ED partial mediation Supported Disconfirmed<br />

H13: CA=>LY=>EP partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H14: CA=>LT=>ED partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H15:G=>LT=>EP partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H16: G=.LT=>ED partial mediation Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H17: Tenure moderates EP Disconfirmed Disconfirmed<br />

H18: Gender modetates ED Supported Supported<br />

SV = Shared Values; PT = Peer Trust; ; CA = Commumicative Adaptability<br />

G = Gratitude; LT = leadership Trust; EP = Enaed Performance;<br />

ED = Enaged Development<br />

23


Hypotheses 1 through 8 were tested for both the female and male groups. Models<br />

were run sequentially with an unconstrained model until all parameters, except structural<br />

weights, were had been constrained. Leaving the structural weights unconstrained attributed<br />

any differences between the groups to the groups themselves and not to the method bias. The<br />

comparison across groups resulted in significant differences in the mechanisms by which<br />

female and male participants engage in performance and development. The models are<br />

illustrated in Appendices F and G. The model for females produced acceptable fit indices; the<br />

model for males did not.<br />

The models indicate that in the female group communications adaptability has a<br />

significant but negative effect on both engaged performance (-0.58) and engaged<br />

development (-0.36), while gratitude had no significant effect on either. Among the male<br />

group gratitude had significant but negative effect on engaged development (-0.24) and no<br />

significant effect on engaged performance, while communicative adaptability had no<br />

significant effect on either dependent variable. These findings refute hypotheses 5 through 8<br />

in both groups. Further, the findings fail to support the notion that individual generative traits<br />

enhance engagement in either performance or self-development.<br />

However, the findings indicate significant and positive effects of peer trust on<br />

leadership trust (female 0.37, male 0.50), leadership trust on engaged development (0.38,<br />

0.33), and leadership trust on shared values (0.52, 0.55) for both groups. The effect of<br />

leadership trust on shared values had not been hypothesized and was entirely unanticipated.<br />

Further, among females the direct effects of shared values on engaged performance and peer<br />

trust on engaged development were hypothesized as H1 and H4 in the original model, were<br />

significant and positive, and therefore are supported by the findings for the female group. H2<br />

24


and H3 were not supported for the female group. In addition, leadership trust partially<br />

mediates the relationship between peer trust and engaged development (direct effect = 0.17,<br />

indirect effect = (0.37) x (0.38) = 0.141, total effect = 0.311). That partial mediation accounts<br />

for 45.3% [0.141/ (0.311)] of the total effect and therefore supports H12 within the female<br />

group; no other mediation hypotheses are supported (Shrout & Bolger, 2002).<br />

Among males direct effect of peer trust on engaged performance was hypothesized in the<br />

original model as H3, was positive and significant, and therefore is supported by the findings<br />

for the male group. H1, H2, and H4 were not supported for the male group. Shared values<br />

also partially mediate the relationship between leadership trust and engaged development<br />

among the male sample accounting for 30.2% of the total effect. Also, the analysis revealed<br />

that engaged development has a positive and significant effect on engaged performance<br />

(0.58) in the male group; another finding not hypothesized in the original model and<br />

completely unanticipated. No mediated relationships exist in the model; none of the<br />

hypotheses, H9 through H16 are supported for the male group. Estimated coefficients and<br />

nomological relationships for the final female (refer to Appendix F) and final male (refer to<br />

Appendix G) are presented in Table 9 below.<br />

25


TABLE 9:<br />

Estimated Coefficients and Nomological Relationships for the Female and Male Models<br />

Estimated Coefficients and Nomological Relationships for the Female and Male Models<br />

Female Model Male model<br />

Dependent Variable<br />

Independent Variable<br />

Estimated<br />

Coefficient t-Value R<br />

s<br />

2<br />

Estimated<br />

Coefficient t-Value R<br />

s<br />

2<br />

Leadership Trust (LT) .184 .108<br />

Peer Trust -> LT .372*** 4.724 .474 1.891<br />

Shared Values (SV) .347 .241<br />

Leadership Trust -> SV .523*** 6.754 .416*** 3.341<br />

Engaged Performance (EP) .377 .409<br />

Shared Values -> EP .271** 2.972<br />

Peer Trust -> EP .432* 2.023<br />

Communicative Adaptability -> EP -.585*** -3.481<br />

Engaged Development -> EP .459* 2.424<br />

Engaged Development (ED) .441 .469<br />

Shared Values -> ED<br />

Peer Trust -> ED .172** 2.609<br />

Communicative Adaptability -> ED -.354** -2.742<br />

Gratitude ->ED -.284* -2.198<br />

Leadership Trust -> ED .383*** 4.576 .405*** 3.181<br />

Goodness of Fit Statistics Female Male<br />

Chi-sq. (df) 639.2 (394) 697.6 (396)<br />

NFI .832 .636<br />

IFI .928 .802<br />

TLI .920 .776<br />

CFI .927 .796<br />

SMSR .098 .140<br />

RMSEA .058 .109<br />

90% CI .050 - .066 .096 - .122<br />

DISCUSSION<br />

The literature in positive psychology focuses on the role of character strengths and<br />

virtues (Peterson & Seligman, 2003; Peterson & Park, 2004; Seligman, 2005), individual<br />

generative characteristics, in improving personal perspective and social environments.<br />

Persons of good character and positive perspective are happier, more adaptive, and more<br />

productive. The literature in appreciative inquiry stresses the importance of the generative<br />

nature of relationships, the ‘webs of connectedness’ (Cooperrider & Sekerka, 2003;<br />

Cooperrider & Whitney, 2000b; Fredrickson, 2005; Gergen & Thatchenkery, 2004), as the<br />

26


asis for personal and organizational learning and performance. Persons who are better, more<br />

effectively and efficiently, connected to others learn faster and perform at higher levels.<br />

Organizations which have well-developed and active networks of connection among their<br />

members are more adaptive and higher performing (Hansen et al., 2005; Mohrman, Tenkasi,<br />

& Mohrman, 2003; Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998). This study draws on constructs from both<br />

areas of inquiry and examines their influence on individual engagement (Csikszentmihalyi,<br />

1990; Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 2006; Kahn, 1990, 1992) in performance and<br />

development.<br />

The results of this inquiry identify several interesting findings. The proposition that<br />

individual character strengths and virtues positively influence performance and development<br />

was not supported. In fact, where significant effect existed it was negative. All the related<br />

hypotheses (H5 through H8) were disconfirmed. The cause of this surprising outcome is<br />

unclear, but perhaps is attributable, in part, to the use of self-report data and inherent<br />

difficulties associated with accurate self-assessment.<br />

Equally interesting was the emergence of the causal link between leadership trust and<br />

shared values. Not originally hypothesized, this effect and its strength (0.52 female, 0.55<br />

male) and significance in both female and male samples supports the literature on leadership<br />

and organizational culture (Amis et al., 2002; McEvily et al., 2003; Shein, 2004). As<br />

organizational culture is a subject of ongoing interaction and exchange among members,<br />

leadership trust and associated behaviors are an essential part of that conversation.<br />

The role of trust in leadership as mediator of the causal relationships between<br />

independent and dependent variables was far less prevalent than hypothesized. The only two<br />

instances of partial mediation were revealed in the findings. The first of these is in the peer<br />

27


trust => leadership trust => engaged development relationships for the female sample only.<br />

The second is the in the leadership trust => shared values => engaged development<br />

relationships for the male sample only. These causal relationships emphasize the importance<br />

of the direct connection between leadership trust and engaged development for groups, and<br />

significant difference in the ways in which engaged development is achieved by each group.<br />

Within the female group the direct path is between peer trust and engaged development;<br />

leadership trust partially mediates that relationship via the weaker although significant<br />

indirect path. In contrast, within the male group the stronger direct path is between leadership<br />

trust and engaged development; shared values mediate via the weaker although significant<br />

indirect path. The implications are that members of the female group place greater emphasis<br />

on the practices and behavior of coworkers as antecedent to self-development. They invest in<br />

themselves as a result of connection with, and to their benefit within the context of, their<br />

immediate work group and those with whom they share it. In contrast, males place greater<br />

emphasis on the practices and behaviors of leadership as antecedent to self-development.<br />

They invest in themselves as a result of connection with, and to their benefit within the<br />

context of, the broader organization and those who guide it.<br />

A final point of interest is the difference in the antecedent relationships to engaged<br />

performance. Here again the mechanisms are distinct. The only relational characteristic<br />

which has significant effect on engaged performance in the female group is shared values.<br />

The implication is that members of the female group engage in the performance of their work<br />

as a result of being compelled by, or in service to, a shared and unifying purpose. Again, this<br />

dynamic contrasts with that of the male respondents. There the antecedents are engaged<br />

development and the relational characteristic of peer trust. The implications are two. Male<br />

28


espondents engage themselves in the performance of their work because they’ve made the<br />

self-investment to do so – preparation and performance are strongly linked. The other<br />

implication is that they derive motivation to engage in performance of their work because<br />

they trust that their peers are doing the same - they want to meet the standard of their work<br />

group.<br />

LIMITATIONS<br />

The findings of this study should be viewed within the context of its limitations. Data<br />

for the study was gathered from a single organization over a brief period of time. While the<br />

data was collected by different means and in different forums, the survey instrument,<br />

particularly the communicative adaptability and gratitude constructs, relied heavily on the<br />

respondent’s ability to accurately self-assess and reply to questions which tend to provoke a<br />

certain level of personal discomfort and invite socially desirable responses. The subject<br />

organization is a very survey-savvy culture in which, in the normal case, respondent<br />

confidentiality is seldom taken for granted. The most substantial limitation to this study is the<br />

presence of significant method bias. While appropriate techniques were applied to correct for<br />

the bias, they resulted in weaker construct reliabilities, particularly for gratitude and engaged<br />

performance.<br />

Construct reliabilities and AVE results were acceptable in the unconstrained model.<br />

However after controlling for common method bias all construct reliability, AVE, MVS, and<br />

<strong>AS</strong>V results were lower. Construct reliability for gratitude and engaged performance fell<br />

below the accepted threshold of 0.7. AVE results for five of the constructs fell below the<br />

accepted 0.5 threshold. While the results are derived from the bias constrained model, the<br />

findings of this study should be interpreted with these outcomes in mind.<br />

29


The sample used in CFA and SEM was relatively small consisting of a total of 251<br />

cases. Analysis was further impacted during SEM due to the asymmetrical female-male (186<br />

female; 65 male) composition of the sample. Therefore, as with any study, the results should<br />

be interpreted with caution, and understanding of the limitations of the data.<br />

IMPLICATIONS<br />

This study suggests that generative relational characteristics have positive effects on<br />

organization member’s willingness to engage themselves in the performance of their work<br />

and in self-development. The results have clear implications for organization leaders and<br />

consultants concerned with organizational performance and learning. Effective AI-based<br />

interventions, particularly in small groups, are designed to address first the identification and<br />

articulation of fundamental shared values and beliefs of the organization members.<br />

Consistently enacted those values guide day-to-day behaviors, and are woven into the fabric<br />

of individual and collective practice. Organizational leadership need to be mindful of the<br />

power of their influence and lead by example; by adhering personally to those values, and by<br />

not settling for less from themselves or others.<br />

The techniques of enacting shared values can be taught. Building trustworthiness and<br />

trust, using appropriate conversational modes and styles, and developing social networks are<br />

learned and essential skills which are appropriate to address in subsequent phases of an<br />

intervention. Shared values and their enactment, trust in leadership, and trust in peers have<br />

both direct and indirect effects on engagement in both performance and learning.<br />

The results of this study also suggest that individual generative characteristics,<br />

character strengths and virtues, have negative direct effects on engaged performance and<br />

engaged development. This finding is counterintuitive and more than somewhat difficult to<br />

30


accept. It is hoped and intended that this will serve as a call to further empirical research in<br />

this area. Arguments in the extant literature are far too compelling, and the potential benefits<br />

far too great to do otherwise.<br />

CONCLUSIONS<br />

Organizations and their members at all levels are increasingly expected to perform,<br />

adapt, and to do ‘more with less’. As they strive to become more productive, innovative, and<br />

agile their interests and objectives will be well-served by understanding that they have at-<br />

hand greater capacity for performance and learning than perhaps they had realized.<br />

Identifying, articulating, and ‘living’ shared values provides foundation for building trust at<br />

all levels. In that environment organization members have the psychological safety to bring<br />

their whole self, their real self, to their work and to the practice and development of their<br />

knowledge, skills and performance. They are freer to engage.<br />

This study suggests that ‘who you are’, individual characteristics whether<br />

dispositional or learned, is far less important than ‘how you are’, cognitive-behavioral<br />

relational characteristics. Further, it provides empirical support to the qualitative research<br />

and testimonial evidence which are commonly offered in support of appreciative inquiry.<br />

Shared values matter. Trust, the pervasive expectation of values-based generalized trust,<br />

matters. Relationship matters. Further exploration of these concepts and their interaction will<br />

further our understanding of individual and collective performance, organizational learning,<br />

and change.<br />

31


APPENDIX A:<br />

Survey Instrument Items and Sources<br />

Trust – Peers in D. Fields, p. 38; Dunham & Smith (1979); Co-worker dimension (only):5 items, Likert 5<br />

(converted to Likert 7 for this paper), Cronbach = 0.83.<br />

1.) The employees I work with are the best group I could ask for.<br />

2.) My attitude toward my job is favorably influenced by the people I work with.<br />

3.) The example my fellow employees set encourages me to work hard.<br />

4.) The way in which my coworkers handle their jobs contributes greatly to the success of our organization.<br />

Removed from analysis:<br />

5.) In this organization there is very little interpersonal friction.<br />

Values Alignment – in D. Fields, p227; Cable & Judge (1996); 3 item instrument, Likert 5 (converted to Likert<br />

7 for this paper); Cronbach = 0.87.<br />

1.) My values ‘match’ or fit this organization and those of the current employees.<br />

3.) The values and ‘personality’ of this organization reflect my own.<br />

Removed from analysis:<br />

2.) My values match those of the current employees of this organization.<br />

Vision Alignment – in D. Fields, p54; O’Reilly & Chatman, (1986); Identification items (only), 8 items,<br />

Likert7, Cronbach = 0.86 to 0.91.<br />

1.) What this organization stands for is important to me.<br />

3.) If the values of this organization were different, I would not be as attached to this organization.<br />

5.) The reason I prefer this organization to others is because of what it stands for – its values.<br />

6.) My attachment to this organizations is primarily based on the similarity of my values and those represented<br />

by the organization<br />

7.) I’m proud to tell others that I am a part of this organization.<br />

Removed form analysis:<br />

2.) I ‘talk up’ this organization to my friends as a great organization to work for.<br />

4.) Since joining this organization, my personal values and those of the organization have become more similar.<br />

8.) I feel a sense of ‘ownership’ for this organization rather than being just an employee.<br />

Communicative Adaptability - Duran & Zakahi (1984, 1987, 1990), Social Experience items (only), 5 items,<br />

Likert 5 (converted to Likert 7 for this paper), Cronbach = 0.82.<br />

1.) I like to be active in different social groups.<br />

2.) I enjoy socializing with diverse groups of people.<br />

4.) I find it easy to get along with new people.<br />

5.) I ‘mix’ well at social functions.<br />

Removed from analysis:<br />

3.) I enjoy meeting new people.<br />

Gratitude – in Lopez & Snyder, p.336; McCullough, et. al. (2002), 6 items, Likert 7, Cronbach = 0.82<br />

1.) I have so much in life to be thankful for.<br />

2.) If I had to list everything I felt grateful for, it would be a very long list.<br />

4.) When I look at the world, I don’t see much to be grateful for. (Reverse scored).<br />

6.) I feel grateful to someone or for something every day.<br />

Removed from analysis:<br />

3.) As I get older I find myself more able to appreciate the people, events, and situations that have been part of<br />

my life history.<br />

5.) I’m grateful to a wide variety of people.<br />

Trust – Leadership – in D. Fields, p 175; Moorman (1999), Procedural justice items (only), 9 items, Likert 7,<br />

Cronbach = 0.94.<br />

32


1.) Leadership in this organization collects necessary accurate information before making decisions.<br />

2.) Leadership in this organization provides opportunities to appeal or challenge its decisions.<br />

3.) Leadership in this organization has all sides which will be affected by a decision represented when making<br />

the decisions.<br />

4.) Leadership in this organization generates standards so decisions can be made with consistency.<br />

5.) Leadership in this organization hears the concerns of all those affected by the decision.<br />

Engagement – in Buckingham & Coffman (1999) and Harter, Schmidt & Hayes (2002); 13 items; Likert 5<br />

(converted to Likert 7 for this paper); Cronbach = 0.83 to 0.91.<br />

Engaged Performance<br />

2.) I know what is expected of me at work.<br />

3.) I have the materials and equipment I need to do my work right.<br />

4.) At work, I have the opportunity to do what I do best everyday.<br />

Engaged Development<br />

5.) In the last seven days, I have received recognition or praise for doing good work.<br />

7.) There is someone at work who encourages my development.<br />

12.) In the last six months, someone at work has talked to me about my progress.<br />

Removed from analysis:<br />

1.) How satisfied are you with [organization name] as a place to work?<br />

6.) My manager, or someone at work, seems to care about me as a person.<br />

8.) At work, my opinions seem to count.<br />

9.) The mission/purpose of my company makes me feel my job is important.<br />

10.) My fellow employees are committed to doing quality work.<br />

11.) I have a best friend at work.<br />

13.) This last year, I have had opportunities at work to learn and grow.<br />

Constructs removed from the model:<br />

Job Interdependence – in D. Fields, p. 103; Pierce & Gregersen (1991), 5 items; Likert 5 (converted to Likert<br />

7 for this paper); Cronbach = 0.76.<br />

1.) I work closely with others in doing my work.<br />

2.) I frequently must coordinate my efforts with others<br />

3.) My own performance is dependent on receiving accurate information from others.<br />

4.) The way I perform my job has a significant impact on others.<br />

5.) My work requires me to consult with others frequently.<br />

Task Independence – in D. Fields, p. 113; Spreitzer (1995), Self-determination: 3 items, Likert 7; Cronbach =<br />

0.79; Impact: 3 items, Likert 7; Cronbach = 0.85.<br />

Self-determination items:<br />

1.) I have significant autonomy in determining how to do my job.<br />

2.) I can decide on my own how to go about doing my work.<br />

3.) I have considerable opportunity for independence and freedom in how I do my job.<br />

Impact items:<br />

4.) I have a large impact on what happens in my department.<br />

5.) I have a great deal of control over what happens in my department.<br />

6.) I have significant influence over what happens in my department.<br />

Trust – Manager – in D. Fields, p 106; Oldham & Cummings (1996), Supportive: 8 items, Likert 7, Cronbach<br />

= 0.86; Non-controlling: 4 items, Likert 7, Cronbach =0.67.<br />

Supportive:<br />

1.) My manager helps me solve work related problems.<br />

2.) My manager encourages me to develop my skills.<br />

3.) My manager keeps me informed about how employees think and feels about things.<br />

33


4.) My manager encourages employees to participate in important decisions.<br />

5.) My manager praises good work.<br />

6.) My manager encourages employees to speak up when they disagree with a decision.<br />

7.) My manager explains her/his actions.<br />

8.) My manager rewards me for good performance.<br />

Non-controlling:<br />

9.) My manager trusts me to keep her/him informed.<br />

10.) My manager trusts me to determine what will be done and how.<br />

11.) My manager always gives me a chance to make important decisions on my own.<br />

12.) My manager leaves it up to me to decide how to go about doing my job.<br />

34


C2 1<br />

APPENDIX B:<br />

Item Correlations and Descriptive Statistics:<br />

Pearson Correlations<br />

C2 C3 C4 C5 E2 E3 E4 E5 E7 E12 G1 G2 G4 G6<br />

C3 .717(**) 1<br />

C4 .636(**) .651(**) 1<br />

C5 .645(**) .547(**) .564(**) 1<br />

E2 .176(**) .173(**) .225(**) .182(**) 1<br />

E4 .176(**) .191(**) .268(**) .165(**) .473(**) .613(**) 1<br />

E5 0.051 0.007 0.056 0.082 .177(**) .354(**) .271(**) 1<br />

E7 0.115 0.059 .183(**) .130(*) .184(**) .317(**) .373(**) .553(**) 1<br />

E12 .143(*) .152(*) .154(*) 0.116 .155(*) .357(**) .323(**) .539(**) .540(**) 1<br />

G1 .337(**) .413(**) .472(**) .348(**) .385(**) .302(**) .262(**) 0.067 .143(*) 0.096 1<br />

G2 .458(**) .459(**) .568(**) .407(**) .254(**) .249(**) .205(**) 0.069 .162(*) .158(*) .687(**) 1<br />

G4 .210(**) .305(**) .352(**) .256(**) .393(**) .249(**) .289(**) 0.115 .134(*) .166(**) .634(**) .477(**) 1<br />

G6 .296(**) .329(**) .327(**) .203(**) .269(**) .273(**) .210(**) 0.076 0.12 .132(*) .589(**) .565(**) .492(**) 1<br />

L1 .246(**) .181(**) .191(**) .231(**) .209(**) .353(**) .349(**) .325(**) .474(**) .379(**) .243(**) .235(**) .245(**) .138(*)<br />

L2 .178(**) .167(**) .212(**) 0.098 .208(**) .356(**) .383(**) .392(**) .480(**) .360(**) .194(**) .216(**) .261(**) .128(*)<br />

L3 .273(**) .234(**) .256(**) .229(**) .226(**) .338(**) .364(**) .298(**) .420(**) .353(**) .234(**) .204(**) .248(**) .137(*)<br />

L4 .244(**) .220(**) .205(**) .214(**) .278(**) .382(**) .388(**) .366(**) .405(**) .388(**) .249(**) .260(**) .260(**) .212(**)<br />

L5 .130(*) .128(*) .137(*) 0.097 .134(*) .307(**) .362(**) .384(**) .517(**) .407(**) .214(**) .159(*) .223(**) .169(**)<br />

P1 .222(**) .212(**) .255(**) .157(*) .285(**) .362(**) .354(**) .263(**) .307(**) .244(**) .161(*) .191(**) .125(*) 0.101<br />

P2 .292(**) .227(**) .304(**) .275(**) .170(**) .333(**) .361(**) .206(**) .254(**) .343(**) .173(**) .242(**) 0.123 .155(*)<br />

P3 .250(**) .180(**) .278(**) .200(**) .194(**) .324(**) .383(**) .390(**) .404(**) .353(**) .161(*) .248(**) .156(*) .169(**)<br />

P4 .282(**) .175(**) .325(**) .285(**) .239(**) .224(**) .394(**) .207(**) .203(**) .138(*) .225(**) .260(**) .201(**) .159(*)<br />

VAL1 .321(**) .249(**) .269(**) .334(**) .294(**) .331(**) .382(**) .310(**) .369(**) .335(**) .335(**) .301(**) .361(**) .263(**)<br />

VAL3 .298(**) .205(**) .251(**) .172(**) .299(**) .380(**) .408(**) .340(**) .413(**) .281(**) .265(**) .272(**) .257(**) .288(**)<br />

VIS1 .441(**) .417(**) .420(**) .292(**) .346(**) .347(**) .379(**) .245(**) .269(**) .269(**) .471(**) .430(**) .409(**) .292(**)<br />

VIS3 .266(**) .233(**) .219(**) .218(**) .255(**) .360(**) .334(**) .201(**) .325(**) .274(**) .306(**) .254(**) .320(**) .260(**)<br />

VIS5 .272(**) .248(**) .222(**) .223(**) .324(**) .387(**) .417(**) .286(**) .413(**) .288(**) .315(**) .299(**) .326(**) .256(**)<br />

VIS6 .315(**) .246(**) .225(**) .176(**) .212(**) .349(**) .366(**) .291(**) .331(**) .280(**) .257(**) .234(**) .260(**) .266(**)<br />

VIS7 .281(**) .321(**) .245(**) .169(**) .340(**) .488(**) .507(**) .303(**) .319(**) .332(**) .311(**) .304(**) .317(**) .232(**)<br />

N=251<br />

** Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two tailed)<br />

* Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two tailed)<br />

L1 L2 L3 L4 L5 P1 P2 P3 P4 VAL1 VAL3 VIS1 VIS3 VIS5 VIS6 VIS7<br />

L1 1<br />

L2 .715(**) 1<br />

L3 .697(**) .649(**) 1<br />

L4 .772(**) .630(**) .610(**) 1<br />

L5 .722(**) .715(**) .598(**) .671(**) 1<br />

P1 .345(**) .300(**) .304(**) .312(**) .248(**) 1<br />

P2 .342(**) .283(**) .350(**) .282(**) .297(**) .468(**) 1<br />

P3 .455(**) .376(**) .369(**) .438(**) .429(**) .623(**) .615(**) 1<br />

P4 .259(**) .202(**) .190(**) .309(**) .304(**) .426(**) .504(**) .506(**) 1<br />

VAL1 .562(**) .482(**) .479(**) .583(**) .509(**) .376(**) .394(**) .368(**) .359(**) 1<br />

VAL3 .513(**) .492(**) .497(**) .590(**) .471(**) .413(**) .371(**) .402(**) .366(**) .733(**) 1<br />

VIS1 .387(**) .390(**) .327(**) .354(**) .358(**) .300(**) .326(**) .296(**) .346(**) .599(**) .579(**) 1<br />

VIS3 .425(**) .411(**) .386(**) .407(**) .466(**) .453(**) .470(**) .473(**) .334(**) .627(**) .571(**) .470(**) 1<br />

VIS5 .515(**) .525(**) .453(**) .533(**) .519(**) .391(**) .347(**) .393(**) .301(**) .747(**) .766(**) .649(**) .677(**) 1<br />

VIS6 .413(**) .411(**) .361(**) .443(**) .477(**) .362(**) .353(**) .374(**) .350(**) .670(**) .788(**) .548(**) .634(**) .719(**) 1<br />

VIS7 .445(**) .471(**) .362(**) .494(**) .448(**) .382(**) .329(**) .384(**) .313(**) .602(**) .652(**) .610(**) .539(**) .718(**) .644(**) 1<br />

35


APPENDIX C:<br />

Exploratory Factor Analysis<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6<br />

C2 .114 .939 -.138<br />

C3 .813<br />

C4 -.119 .667 .179 .101<br />

C5 .705<br />

E2 .209 .552<br />

E3 .121 .652<br />

E4 .101 .813<br />

E5 .802<br />

E7 .156 .626<br />

E12 .681<br />

G1 .918<br />

G2 .216 .687<br />

G4 .692 .111<br />

G6 -.109 .679<br />

L1 .995<br />

L2 .741<br />

L3 .764 .126<br />

L4 .768<br />

L5 .726 -.115 .114<br />

P1 .102 .647 .110<br />

P2 .688<br />

P3 .862 .119<br />

P4 .578<br />

VAL1 .727 .185<br />

VAL3 .863<br />

VIS1 .582 .205 .162<br />

VIS3 .621 .301<br />

VIS5 .885<br />

VIS6 .949 -.110<br />

VIS7 .652 .276<br />

Maximum Likelihood Extraction; Promax Rotation; 7 iterations<br />

7<br />

36


d2 1<br />

d3 1<br />

d4<br />

d5<br />

d6 1<br />

d7 1<br />

d9 1<br />

1<br />

d8<br />

d10<br />

d11<br />

d12<br />

d13<br />

d14<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

ZC2<br />

ZC3<br />

ZC4<br />

ZC5<br />

ZG1<br />

ZG2<br />

ZG4<br />

ZG6<br />

ZL1<br />

ZL2<br />

ZL3<br />

ZL4<br />

ZL5<br />

ZP1<br />

1<br />

d15<br />

APPENDIX D:<br />

Final CFA Measurement Model<br />

AMOS Output Graphic<br />

1<br />

CommAdapt<br />

1<br />

Gratitude<br />

1<br />

LeaderTrust<br />

ZP2<br />

1<br />

d16<br />

CFA Model<br />

ZP3<br />

1<br />

d17<br />

ZP4<br />

1<br />

d18<br />

Goodness of Fit Statistics;<br />

Chi-square (df) = 686.8 (377)<br />

SRMR = .050<br />

NFI =.864<br />

IFI = .934<br />

TLI = .923<br />

CFI = .933<br />

RMSEA = .057<br />

1<br />

PeerTrust<br />

1<br />

Values<br />

1<br />

EngagePerf<br />

1<br />

EngageDev<br />

ZE2<br />

ZE3<br />

ZE4<br />

ZE5<br />

ZE7<br />

ZE12<br />

1<br />

d27<br />

1<br />

d28<br />

1<br />

d29<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d30<br />

d31<br />

d32<br />

1<br />

ZVAL1 d20<br />

1<br />

ZVAL3 d21<br />

1<br />

ZVIS1 d22<br />

1<br />

ZVIS3 d23<br />

1<br />

ZVIS5 d24<br />

1<br />

ZVIS6 d25<br />

1<br />

ZVIS7 d26<br />

37


1<br />

d1<br />

1<br />

d2<br />

d3<br />

d4<br />

1<br />

d5<br />

d6 1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d7 1<br />

d8<br />

d9<br />

1<br />

d10 1<br />

1<br />

d11 1<br />

1<br />

d12<br />

d13 1<br />

ZC2<br />

ZC3<br />

ZC4<br />

ZC5<br />

ZG1<br />

ZG2<br />

ZG4<br />

ZG6<br />

ZL1<br />

ZL2<br />

ZL3<br />

ZL4<br />

ZL5<br />

Gooodness of Fit Statistics<br />

APPENDIX E:<br />

Analysis of Method Bias<br />

AMOS output Graphic<br />

Chi-square (df) SRMS NFI IFI TLI CFI RMSEA 90% CI<br />

Unconstrained 637.1 (354) .038 .874 .940 .925 .939 .057 .049-.064<br />

Constrained 726.0 (383) .086 .857 .927 .916 .926 .060 .053-.066<br />

Beta m = .563<br />

ZP1<br />

1<br />

d14<br />

1<br />

CommAdapt r4<br />

r9<br />

1<br />

r10<br />

Gratitude<br />

r11<br />

r12<br />

r13<br />

1<br />

LeadershipTrust<br />

1<br />

PeerTrust<br />

ZP2 ZP3<br />

1 1<br />

d15<br />

r2<br />

r3<br />

r5<br />

r6<br />

r7<br />

r8<br />

r1<br />

r14 r15 r16 r17<br />

d16<br />

Method Bias<br />

(Constrained)<br />

ZP4<br />

1<br />

d17<br />

1<br />

METHOD<br />

r28<br />

r29<br />

r30<br />

r25<br />

1<br />

EngagePerf<br />

r26<br />

r27<br />

r18<br />

r19<br />

r201<br />

r21<br />

EngageDev<br />

r22<br />

r23<br />

r24<br />

1<br />

SharedValues<br />

1<br />

ZE2 d28<br />

ZE3<br />

ZE4<br />

1<br />

ZE5 d25<br />

ZE7<br />

ZE12<br />

1<br />

ZVAL1 d18<br />

ZVAL3<br />

ZVIS1<br />

ZVIS3<br />

ZVIS5<br />

ZVIS6<br />

ZVIS7<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d26<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d29<br />

1<br />

d30<br />

d27<br />

d19<br />

1<br />

d20<br />

d21<br />

d22<br />

d23<br />

d24<br />

38


-.09<br />

APPENDIX F:<br />

Female Engagement<br />

SEM_Female (only)<br />

(Constrained for Method Bias)<br />

.22<br />

1<br />

d1 ZC2<br />

.31<br />

d2 ZC3<br />

ZC4<br />

ZC5<br />

ZG1<br />

ZG2<br />

ZG4<br />

ZG6<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

1.00<br />

.36<br />

1<br />

d3<br />

.50<br />

1<br />

d4<br />

.18<br />

1<br />

d5<br />

.28<br />

d6 1<br />

.58<br />

d7 1<br />

.28<br />

.82<br />

.54 CommAdapt<br />

.63<br />

1.00<br />

.41<br />

.72<br />

.72 Gratitude<br />

.46<br />

1<br />

d8<br />

.24<br />

1<br />

d9 ZL1<br />

.34<br />

d10 ZL2<br />

ZL3<br />

ZL4<br />

ZL5<br />

1<br />

.43<br />

d11 1<br />

.28<br />

d12 1<br />

.28<br />

d13 1<br />

1.00 .51<br />

METHOD<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.73<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

ZE2 d28<br />

.47<br />

.51<br />

.28<br />

.95<br />

1<br />

ZE3 d29<br />

.51<br />

EngagePerf 1.00<br />

.30<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

ZE4 d30<br />

.51<br />

.52<br />

.51<br />

1<br />

1.00 ZE5 d25<br />

.76<br />

.51<br />

.46<br />

.51<br />

1.05 1<br />

EngageDev<br />

.98<br />

ZE7 d26<br />

.38<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

ZE12 d27<br />

.34<br />

1<br />

1.00<br />

ZVAL1 d18<br />

.90<br />

.30<br />

.93<br />

1.00<br />

1<br />

.76<br />

ZVAL3 d19<br />

LeadershipTrust<br />

.59<br />

SharedValues<br />

.42<br />

.88<br />

1<br />

.73 ZVIS1 d20<br />

.96<br />

.49<br />

1.16<br />

1<br />

ZVIS3 d21<br />

.88<br />

.83<br />

.16<br />

.86<br />

1<br />

ZVIS5 d22<br />

PeerTrust<br />

.62 .39<br />

.40<br />

1.00<br />

1<br />

ZP1<br />

ZVIS6 d23<br />

ZP4<br />

.31<br />

1<br />

.53 ZP2 ZP3 1 .67<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d14 .60<br />

1<br />

ZVIS7 d24<br />

.02 d17<br />

d15 d16<br />

.51 .51<br />

.22 .51<br />

.51<br />

-.58 f1<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

-.35<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.51<br />

.27 .51<br />

.51<br />

1<br />

.51<br />

.24.51<br />

.51 .38<br />

.51<br />

f2<br />

f3<br />

1<br />

.17<br />

.52<br />

1<br />

.32<br />

.37<br />

f4<br />

.51<br />

.14<br />

.10<br />

Goodness of Fit<br />

Chi-sq. (df) 639.2 (394)<br />

SRMR .098<br />

NFI .832<br />

IFI .928<br />

TLI .920<br />

CFI .927<br />

RMSEA .058<br />

90% CI .050-.066<br />

39


1<br />

d1<br />

d2 1<br />

d3<br />

d4<br />

1<br />

d5<br />

d6 1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d7 1<br />

d8<br />

d9<br />

1<br />

d10 1<br />

1<br />

d11 1<br />

1<br />

d12<br />

d13 1<br />

ZC2<br />

ZC3<br />

ZC4<br />

ZC5<br />

ZG1<br />

ZG2<br />

ZG4<br />

ZG6<br />

ZL1<br />

ZL2<br />

ZL3<br />

ZL4<br />

ZL5<br />

ZP1<br />

1<br />

d14<br />

APPENDIX G:<br />

Male Engagement<br />

SEM_Male (only)<br />

(Constrained for Method Bias)<br />

r1<br />

1 r2<br />

r3<br />

CommAdapt r4<br />

1<br />

1<br />

r9<br />

r10<br />

Gratitude<br />

r11<br />

r12<br />

r13<br />

LeadershipTrust<br />

1<br />

PeerTrust<br />

ZP2 ZP3<br />

1 1<br />

d15<br />

r5<br />

r6<br />

r7<br />

r8<br />

r14<br />

r15 r16<br />

f3<br />

1<br />

d16<br />

ZP4<br />

1<br />

d17<br />

Gooodness of Fit<br />

Chi-sq. (df) 697.6 (396)<br />

SRMR .140<br />

NFI .636<br />

IFI .802<br />

TLI .776<br />

CFI .796<br />

RMSEA .109<br />

90% CI .096 - .122<br />

1<br />

METHOD<br />

r17<br />

f1<br />

1<br />

EngagePerf<br />

r18<br />

r19<br />

r20 1<br />

r21<br />

EngageDev<br />

r22<br />

1 r23<br />

r24<br />

f2<br />

SharedValues<br />

1<br />

f4<br />

r28<br />

r29<br />

r30<br />

r25<br />

r26<br />

r27<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

ZE2 d28<br />

1<br />

ZE3 d29<br />

1<br />

ZE4 d30<br />

1<br />

ZE5 d25<br />

1<br />

ZE7 d26<br />

1<br />

ZE12 d27<br />

1<br />

ZVAL1 d18<br />

1<br />

ZVAL3 d19<br />

ZVIS1<br />

ZVIS3<br />

ZVIS5<br />

ZVIS6<br />

ZVIS7<br />

1<br />

d20<br />

1<br />

d21<br />

1<br />

1<br />

1<br />

d22<br />

d23<br />

d24<br />

40


REFERENCES<br />

Adams, M. G., Schiller, M., & Cooperrider, D. 2004. with our questions we make the world.<br />

In D. Cooperrider & M. Avital (Eds.), Constructive discourse and human<br />

organization: 105-124. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.<br />

Amis, J., Slack, T., & Hinings, C. R. 2002. Values and organizational change. The Journal<br />

of Applied Behavioral Science, 38(4): 436.<br />

Argyris, C. 1982. Reasoning, learning, and action. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Arthur, J. B. 1994. The effects of human resource systems on manufacturing performance<br />

and turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 670-687.<br />

Babbie, E. 2007. The practice of social research (11th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thompson-<br />

Wadsworth.<br />

Barrett, F. 1995. Creating appreciative learning cultures. Organizational Dynamics, 24(2):<br />

36-49.<br />

Barrett, F., Thomas, G. F., & Hocevar, S. P. 1995. The central role of discourse in large-scale<br />

change: A social construction perspective. The Journal of Applied Behavioral<br />

Science, 31(3): 353-372.<br />

Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. 1966. The social construction of reality: A treatise in the<br />

sociology of knowledge. New York, NY: Anchor Books.<br />

Biswas-Diener, R. D., & Dean, B. 2007. Positive psychology coaching. Hoboken, NJ: John<br />

Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Bohm, D. 1996. On dialogue. New York, NY: Routledge Classics.<br />

Boyatzis, R. M., & McKee, A. 2005. Resonant leadership. Boston, MA.: Harvard Business<br />

School Press.<br />

Buckingham, M., & Coffman, C. 1999. First, break all the rules. New York, NY: Simon &<br />

Schuster.<br />

Bushe, G. R. 1995. Advances in appreciative inquiry as an organization development<br />

intervention. Organization Development Journal, 13(3).<br />

Bushe, G. R., & Coetzer, G. 1995. Appreciative inquiry as a team development intervention:<br />

A controlled experiment. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 31(1): 13-30.<br />

41


Bushe, G. R., & Kassam, A. F. 2005. When is appreciative inquiry transformational? A metacase<br />

analysis. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 41(2): 161.<br />

Byrne, B. M., & Stewart, S. 2006. The MACS approach to testing for multigroup invariance<br />

of second-order structure: A walk through the process. Structural Equation<br />

Modeling, 13(2): 287-321.<br />

Cable, D. M., & Judge, T. A. 1996. Person-organization fit, job choice decisions, and<br />

organizational entry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 67(3):<br />

294-311.<br />

Cameron, K. S. 2003. Organizational virtuousness and performance. In K. S. Cameron, J.<br />

Dutton & R. A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: Foundations of a<br />

new discipline: 33-47. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler Publishing.<br />

Clifton, D. O., & Harter, J. K. 2003. Investing in strengths. In K. S. Cameron, J. Dutton & R.<br />

A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: 122-137. San Francisco, CA:<br />

Berrett-Koehler Publishers, Inc.<br />

Clifton, D. O., & Hodges, T. D. 2004. Strength-based development in practice. In P. A.<br />

Linley, S. Joseph, & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice.<br />

Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Cooperrider, D. 2000. Positive image, positive action: The affirmative basis for organizing.<br />

In D. L. Cooperrider, P. F. Sorensen, D. Whitney & T. Yaeger (Eds.), Appreciative<br />

inquiry: Rethinking human organization toward a positive theory of change.<br />

Champaign, IL: Stipes Publishing.<br />

Cooperrider, D., & Srivastva, S. 2000a. Appreciative inquiry in organizational life. In D.<br />

Cooperrider & J. Peter F. Sorensen & T. Yaeger (Eds.), Appreciative inquiry:<br />

Rethinking human organization toward a positive theory of change: 35-97.<br />

Champaign, IL: Stipes Publishing.<br />

Cooperrider, D., & Whitney, D. 2000b. A positive revolution in change: Appreciative inquiry.<br />

In D. Cooperrider, P. F. Sorensen, D. Whitney & T. Yaeger (Eds.), Appreciative<br />

inquiry: Rethinking human organization toward a positive theory of change: 3-28.<br />

Champaign, IL: Stipes Publishing.<br />

Cooperrider, D., Sorensen, P. F., Yaeger, T. F., & Whitney, D. (Eds.). 2001. Appreciative<br />

inquiry: An emerging direction for organizational development. Champaign, IL:<br />

Stipes Publishing.<br />

42


Cooperrider, D., & Sekerka, L. E. 2003. Toward a theory of positive organizational change.<br />

In K. S. Cameron, J. Dutton & R. A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational<br />

scholarship: 225-240. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.<br />

Cooperrider, D., & Avital, M. (Eds.). 2004. Constructive discourses in human organization:<br />

Advances in appreciative inquiry, Vol. 1. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.<br />

Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1990. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY:<br />

Harper-Collins.<br />

Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. 2006. A life worth living: Contributions to<br />

positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford university Press.<br />

Dunham, R. B., & Smith, F. J. 1979. Organizational surveys: An internal assessment of<br />

organizational health. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.<br />

Duran, R. L. 1983. Communicative adaptability: A measure of social competence.<br />

Communications Quarterly, 31(4): 320-326.<br />

Dutton, J. E., & Heaphy, E. D. 2003. The power of high-quality connections. In K. S.<br />

Cameron & J. E. Dutton & R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship:<br />

Foundations of a new discipline: 263-278. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler<br />

Publishing.<br />

Edmondson, A. 1999. Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams.<br />

Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(2): 350-383.<br />

Emmons, R. A. 2003. Acts of gratitude in organizations. In K. Cameron, J. Dutton & R. E.<br />

Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: 81-93. San Francisco, CA:<br />

Berrett-Koehler Publishers, Inc.<br />

Emmons, R. A., McCullough, M. E., & Bono, G. 2004. Gratitude in practice and the practice<br />

of gratitude. In P. A. Linley, S. Joseph, & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Positive<br />

psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Emmons, R. A., & Shelton, C. 2005a. Gratitude and the science of positive psychology. In C.<br />

R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez, (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology. New York:<br />

Oxford Press.<br />

Emmons, R. A., McCullough, M., & Tsang, J. 2005b. The assessment of gratitude. In S. J.<br />

Lopez & C. R. Snyder, (Eds.), Positive psychology assessment. Washington, D.C.:<br />

American Psychological Association.<br />

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. 1981. Evaluating structural equation models with unobserved<br />

variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 19: 39-50.<br />

43


Fredrickson, B. L. 2003. Positive emotions and upward spirals in organizations. In K. S.<br />

Cameron & J. E. Dutton & R. A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship:<br />

Foundations of a new discipline: 163-175. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler<br />

Publishing.<br />

Fredrickson, B. L. 2005. Positive emotions. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez, (Eds.), Handbook<br />

of positive psychology. New York: Oxford Press.<br />

Friedlander, F. 1983. Patterns of individual and organizational learning. In S. Srivastva (Ed.),<br />

The executive mind: 192-220. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, Inc.<br />

Fry, R., Barrett, F., Seiling, J., & Whitney, D. 2002. Appreciative inquiry and<br />

organizational transformation: Reports from the field. Westport, CT: Quorum<br />

Books.<br />

Gergen, K. J., & Thatchenkery, T. J. 2004. Organization science as social construction:<br />

Postmodern potentials. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 40(2): 228-249.<br />

Gittell, J. H. 2003. A theory of relational coordination. In K. S. Cameron & J. E. Dutton & R.<br />

A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: Foundations of a new<br />

discipline: 279-295. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.<br />

Goleman, D. 1995. Emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.<br />

Goleman, D., Boyatzis, R., & McKee, A. 2002. Primal leadership. Boston, MA: Harvard<br />

Business School Press.<br />

Golembiewski, R. T. 2001. Three perspectives on appreciative inquiry. In D. Cooperrider, P.<br />

F. Sorensen, T. Yaeger & D. Whitney (Eds.), Appreciative inquiry: An emerging<br />

direction for organizational development: 389-396. Champaign, IL: Stipes<br />

Publishing.<br />

Hansen, M. T. 1999. The search-transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing<br />

information across organization subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44: 82-<br />

111.<br />

Hansen, M. T., Mors, M. L., & Lovas, B. 2005. Knowledge sharing in organizations:<br />

Multiple networks, multiple phases. Academy of Management Journal, 48(5): 776-<br />

793.<br />

Harter, J. K., F.L. Schmidt, & Hayes, T. L.. 2002. Business-unit level relationships between<br />

employee satisfaction, employee engagement, and business outcomes: A metaanalysis.<br />

Journal of Applied Psychology, 87: 268-279.<br />

44


Head, T. C. 1997. Why does appreciative inquiry work?: Speculation and a call for<br />

empirical research. Tennessee State University, Nashville, TN.<br />

Ichniowski, C., & Shaw, K. 1999. The effects of human resource management systems on<br />

economic performance: An international comparison of U. S. and Japanese plants.<br />

Management Science, 45(5): 704-721.<br />

Kahn, W. A. 1990. Psychological conditions of personal engagement and disengagement at<br />

work. Academy of Management Journal, 33(4): 692-724.<br />

Kahn, W. A. 1992. To be full there: Psychological presence at work. Human Relations, 45:<br />

692-724.<br />

Kahn, W. A., Cross, R., & Parker, A. 2003. Layers of diagnosis for planned relational change<br />

in organizations. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 39(3): 259-280.<br />

Kotter, J. P. 1996. Leading change. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.<br />

Lopez, S. J., & Snyder, C. R. 2005. Handbook of positive psychology. New York, NY:<br />

Oxford University Press.<br />

Luechauer, D. L. 2000. Applying appreciative inquiry instead of problem-solving techniques<br />

to facilitate change. Management Development Forum, 2(1).<br />

Maister, D. H., Green, C. H., & Galford, R. M. 2000. The trusted advisor. New York, NY:<br />

The Free Press.<br />

Mathieu, J. E., & Taylor, S. R.. 2006. Clarifying conditions and decision points for<br />

mediational type inferences in organizational behavior. Journal of Organizational<br />

Behavior, 27: 1031-1056.<br />

McEvily, B., Perrone, V., & Zaheer, A. 2003. Trust as an organizing principle.<br />

Organizational Science, 14(1), (January-February 2003): 91-103.<br />

Mohrman, S. A., Tenkasi, R. V., & Mohrman, A. M. 2003. The role of networks in<br />

fundamental organizational change: A grounded analysis. The Journal of Applied<br />

Behavioral Science, 39(3): 301.<br />

Moorman, R. H. 1991. Relationship between organizational justice and organizational<br />

citizenship behaviors: Do fairness perceptions influence employee citizenship?<br />

Journal of Applied Psychology, 76(6): 845-855.<br />

Nahapiet, J., & Ghoshal, S. 1998. Social capital, intellectual capital, and the organizational<br />

advantage. Academy of Management Review, 23(2): 242-256.<br />

45


O'Reilly, C., III, & Chatman, J. 1986. Organizational commitment and psychological<br />

attachment: The effects of compliance, identification, and internalization on prosocial<br />

behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71: 492-499.<br />

Ouchi, W. G., & Johnson, J. B. 1978. Types of organizational control and their relationship<br />

to emotional well being. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23.<br />

Peterson, C., & Park, N. 2004. Classification and measurement of character strengths:<br />

implications for practice. In P. A. Linley, S. Joseph, & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.),<br />

Positive psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.<br />

Peterson, C. M., & Seligman, M. E. 2003. Positive organizational studies: Lessons from<br />

positive psychology. In K. S. Cameron & J. E. Dutton & R. A. Quinn (Eds.), Positive<br />

organizational scholarship: Foundations of a new discipline: 14-27. San Francisco,<br />

CA: Berrett-Koehler.<br />

Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. 2004. Character strengths and virtues. New York, NY:<br />

Oxford University Press.<br />

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, N. P., & Lee, J. Y. 2003. The<br />

mismeasurement of man(agement) and its implications for leadership research. The<br />

Leadership Quarterly, 14: 615-656.<br />

Prowley, E. H., Fry, R., Barrett, F., & Bright, D. S. 2004. Dialogic democracy meets<br />

command and control: Transformation through the appreciative inquiry summit.<br />

Academy of Management Executive, 18(3): 67-80.<br />

Seligman, M. E. 1990. Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your life. New<br />

York, NY: Vintage books.<br />

Seligman, M. E. 2002. Authentic happiness. New York, NY: Free Press.<br />

Seligman, M. E. 2005. Positive psychology, positive prevention, and positive therapy. In C.<br />

R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez, (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology. New York:<br />

Oxford Press.<br />

Shein, E. H. 2004. Organizational culture and leadership. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Shrout P. E. & Bolger, N. 2002. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies:<br />

New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4): 422-445.<br />

Srivastva, S. & Cooperrider, D. (Eds.). 1990. Appreciative management and leadership:<br />

The power of positive thought and action in organizations. San Francisco CA:<br />

Jossey-Bass, Inc.<br />

46


Thatchenkery, T. J. 2004. Paradox and organizational change: The transformative of<br />

hermeneutic appreciation. In D. Cooperrider & M. Avital (Eds.), Constructive<br />

discourse in human organization: 77-101. Oxford UK: Elsevier.<br />

Watkins, J. M., & Mohr, B. J. 2001. Appreciative inquiry: Change at the speed of<br />

imagination. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer.<br />

Weick. 2003. Positive organizing and organizational tragedy. In K. S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton<br />

& R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: 66-80. San Francisco,<br />

CA: Berrett-Koehler.<br />

Wenger, E. 1998. Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity. Cambridge,<br />

UK: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Whitney, D. & Trosten-Bloom, a. A. 2003. The power of appreciative inquiry: A practical<br />

guide to positive change. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler Publishing, Inc.<br />

Wrzesniewski, A. 2003. Finding positive meaning in work. In K. S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton,<br />

& R. E. Quinn, (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship: 296-308. San Francisco,<br />

CA: Berrett-Koehler Publishers, Inc.<br />

47

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!