10.01.2014 Views

In press: In: Dimitrova-Vulchanova, M - NTNU

In press: In: Dimitrova-Vulchanova, M - NTNU

In press: In: Dimitrova-Vulchanova, M - NTNU

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

1. Background<br />

An important characteristic of language is that it imposes structure on the ‘reality’ it<br />

represents (Jackendoff 1996, Talmy 1985, 2000) by selecting for ex<strong>press</strong>ion only specific<br />

concepts and features. As a consequence, the linguistic ex<strong>press</strong>ion of spatial scenes, including<br />

motion, is constrained by a schema consisting of a closed class of elements like Motion, Path,<br />

Figure and Ground. Motion-event typology focuses on how these elements are encoded crosslinguistically.<br />

One of the most widely applied formats recently is Talmy’s (1985, 2000),<br />

which approaches motion encoding in language from two perspectives, that of linguistic form,<br />

and that of semantic content. <strong>In</strong> terms of linguistic form, the verb is viewed as the main<br />

lexical item encoding the motion event. Languages differ as to what elements of the motion<br />

situation they choose to bundle into their most frequently occurring basic motion verbs.<br />

According to these preferences, languages can be divided into several types, the most<br />

common being path-type languages and manner-type languages. From the perspective of<br />

semantic content and its interface with syntax, languages can be divided into two types,<br />

according to how they ex<strong>press</strong> the path of motion. <strong>In</strong> the so-called verb-framed languages<br />

(e.g. Spanish, Turkish, and Japanese), path is encoded in the verb in the main clause (enter,<br />

exit, ascend, etc.), while manner is specified most commonly in a subordinate clause. <strong>In</strong><br />

satellite-framed languages path is encoded in ‘satellites’ (e.g. adpositions in Germanic<br />

languages and verb prefixes in Slavic languages) which accompany the verb. Manner in such<br />

languages is encoded in the main verb of the clause, and typically those languages have a<br />

large number of commonly occurring manner of motion verbs. Although Talmy’s<br />

classification is useful and captures important insights in motion encoding, the rigidity of the<br />

system has engendered much debate. Many recent studies (e.g. Strömqvist & Verhoeven<br />

2004, Ameka & Essegbey 2000) have demonstrated that the data are more complex. Slobin<br />

(2004) argues that the preferential encoding of path vs. manner in verb roots is not a matter of<br />

belonging to one of two poles, but rather a cline along which manner salience can vary.<br />

Furthermore, there appears to be at least one more distinct type of language, where manner<br />

and path are ex<strong>press</strong>ed by equivalent semantic forms (e.g. verbs in serial-verb languages, cf.<br />

Ameka & Essegbey 2000, Zlatev & Yangklang 2004, among others).<br />

Typological research and Language Acquisition research (cf. Slobin 2001) indicate that<br />

the most frequent verbs of motion cross-linguistically and the ones that tend to be acquired<br />

early on in the process of language acquisition are generalized items denoting default motion<br />

scenes (e.g. scenes which are underspecified for global or local path shape, or parameters,<br />

2

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!