01.05.2015 Views

o_19k6fnuqsqg41mt7rqp1c8pkueo.pdf

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

F. R. Balteş, M. Miclea, A. C. Miu<br />

109<br />

to music (Baltes et al., 2011; Levitin, 2006; Rickard, 2004). Therefore, the aim of<br />

the present study was to explore the affective space of an entire musical<br />

composition. We chose to use Vivaldi’s Four Seasons because we suspected that<br />

the extensive popularity of this composition may be related to its emotional content.<br />

The Four Seasons has been previously used in cognitive research that investigated<br />

the effects of music on memory or categorization tasks in older adults<br />

(Mammarella, Fairfield, & Cornoldi, 2007; Thompson, Moulin, Hayre, & Jones,<br />

2005). Clearly, in light of the rapidly developing literature on music and cognition,<br />

mapping the affective space of everyday music will become increasingly necessary.<br />

The affective space of select musical stimuli has started to be mapped on<br />

the two dimensions of emotional arousal and valence (Grewe, Nagel, Kopiez, &<br />

Altenmuller, 2007; Vieillard et al., 2008). In Russell’s circumplex model, the<br />

conceptual distance between different emotions and the structure of affective<br />

experience are represented by a circle that has pleasure and displeasure (i.e.,<br />

valence) on the extremes of the left-right axis, and activation and sleepiness (i.e.,<br />

arousal) on the extremes of the upper-lower axis (Russell, 1980). Using the<br />

multidimensional scale method, which allows for emotions to be investigated<br />

without the use of linguistic labels, another study confirmed that emotional<br />

activation and valence are representative dimensions of music-induced emotions<br />

(Bigand et al., 2005). For instance, excerpts from Bach and Mahler were both<br />

perceived as pleasant, but they were distinguished by different degrees of emotional<br />

arousal (Flores-Gutierrez et al., 2007). Therefore, the present study used self-report<br />

measures of music-induced emotional arousal and valence.<br />

Another important aim of this study was to investigate whether there are<br />

differences in music-induced emotions between musicians and non-musicians. The<br />

traditional view is that in comparison to non-musicians, musicians have subtler and<br />

more complex knowledge about the musical tonalities typical to their culture, and<br />

they use it in order to improve their musical perception and memory (Dowling,<br />

1978; Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979). This perspective has been supported by studies<br />

from cognitive neuroscience, which identified various neuroanatomical and<br />

neurophysiological differences between musicians and non-musicians. For instance,<br />

musicians displayed increased grey matter density in Heschl’s gyrus (i.e., primary<br />

auditory cortex) and early auditory N19-P30 evoked potentials; these differences<br />

correlated with the musicality score on the Advance Measures of Music Audiation<br />

test (Hutchinson, Lee, Gaab, & Schlaug, 2003; Schlaug, Jancke, Huang, Staiger, &<br />

Steinmetz, 1995). However, non-musicians are also able to learn the tonal<br />

principles of their cultural musical idiom, and they accurately use this knowledge in<br />

music processing tasks in order to differentiate tonal from atonal melodies, for<br />

instance (Bartlett & Dowling, 1980; Frances, 1988). The emerging view is that all<br />

music listeners, whether musicians or non-musicians, may share a certain form of<br />

musical knowledge, which gives meaning to the music that they listen to (Halpern,<br />

Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal<br />

16 (2012) 107-119

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!